SlideShare a Scribd company logo
HOME CAGE CONSUMPTION OF 20% ETHANOL PREDICTS REWARD SEEKING BEHAVIOR
INTRODUCTION
Alcohol
• 3rd preventable cause of death in the US [5]
• In 2010, economic costs of excessive alcohol consumption was $249 billion [2]
• Individual preference for alcohol is predictive of alcohol use disorder [7]
Orbitofrontal Cortex
• Influential in motivation, predicting reward, and relapse in addicts [3, 4, 6]
• Regional differences in neuronal firing in the medial and lateral OFC (mOFC and lOFC) [1, 4]
There are very few studies examining the role of the mOFC and lOFC in alcohol seeking.
Siegal, R.E., Hernandez, J.S. and Moorman, D. E.
Psychological and Brain Sciences, Neuroscience and Behavior Program, University of Massachusetts Amherst
OPERANT TASK BEHAVIORAL DATA
Reward
and end
of trial
Longer
than
500 ms
= Long
nose
poke
Tone
signals
type of
reward
Shorter
than
100 ms
= Short
nose
poke
Start of
Trial
House
light
turns off
Nose
poke for
100 ms
1, 5 or
10 kHz
tone
Exit nose
poke in
500 ms
Reward
delivered
METHODS
Figure 3. Intermittent Access to 20% Ethanol
A.
A.
Figure 7. Individual variability in home cage drinking
Figure 10. There was a significant difference in number and percent of rewards earned between sessions
Figure 8. There was a significant difference in licking behavior between sessions
HOMECAGE IA TO 20% ETHANOL
Figure 11. There was a significant difference in number and percent of rewards retrieved between sessions.
Figure 2. Procedure Overview
A.
Rats initiated significantly more trials and
triggered significantly more tones in
sessions containing sucrose rewards (Fig.
9A and 9B; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p <
0.001, ****p < 0.0001).
B.
Summary
• Individual rats expressed variability in home cage ethanol consumption
• Individual preference for ethanol may be a predictor of future ethanol seeking behavior in rats
• Rats prefer sucrose over ethanol rewards; however rats will risk receiving ethanol rewards when there is a possibility
that they may receive a sucrose reward instead
Future Directions
• Analyze the neural activity in the mOFC and lOFC when presented with cues predicting ethanol or sucrose rewards
• Analyze the effect of previous ethanol or sucrose trials in interwoven sessions
SUMMARY and FUTURE DIRECTIONS
1. Burton, A. C., Kashtelyan, V., Bryden, D. W., & Roesch, M. R. (November 07, 2013). Increased Firing to Cues That Predict Low-Value Reward in the Medial Orbitofrontal Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 12, 3310-3321.
2. http://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/alcohol-use.htm
3. Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (March 01, 2010). Neurocircuitry of Addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 4.)
4. Moorman, D. E., & Aston-Jones, G. (January 01, 2014). Orbitofrontal cortical neurons encode expectation-driven initiation of reward-seeking. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34, 31,
10234-46.
5. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Alcohol Facts and Statistics. (n.d.). Retrieved December 19, 2015, fromhttp://www.niaaa.nih.gov/alcohol-health/overviewalcohol-consumption/alcohol-facts-and-statistics
6. Schoenbaum, G., Roesch, M. R., Stalnaker, T. A., & Takahashi, Y. K. (January 01, 2009). A new perspective on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in adaptive behaviour. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10, 12, 885-92.
7. Spoelder, M., Hesseling, P., Baars, A. M., Lozeman-van, . K. J. G., Rotte, M. D., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., & Lesscher, H. M. B. (December 01, 2015). Individual Variation in Alcohol Intake Predicts Reinforcement, Motivation, and
Compulsive Alcohol Use in Rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39, 12, 2427-2437.
8. Verschure, P. F. M. J., Verschure, P. F. M. J., Pennartz, C. M. A., & Pezzulo, G. (January 01, 2014). The why, what, where, when and how of goal-directed choice: Neuronal and computational principles. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 1655.)
9. Mormede, P., Colas, A., & Jones, B. C. (January 01, 2004). High ethanol preferring rats fail to show dependence following short- or long-term ethanol exposure. Alcohol and Alcoholism (oxford, Oxfordshire), 39, 3.)
Thank you to Jessica Feliciano, Aidan Leith, Kathy Tran and Kerrin Bersani. A special thank you to John Hernandez for figures 3A, 4A and 6A, but more importantly for his guidance throughout
my undergraduate research. Thank you to Dr. Moorman for his mentorship and support. Thank you to the Commonwealth Honors College at the University of Massachusetts Amherst for their
financial contributions.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Figure 1. Hypotheses
A.
HOME CAGE
INTERMITTENT
ACCESS TO ETHANOL
Figure 3A
• 40 ml 20% ethanol
3x/week TRAINING
• 40 ml water 2x/week
• 4 weeks
TRAINING
Figure 4A
• 15% sucrose
solution
• 85% reward
retrieval
SURGERY
• 32 electrodes
(16 in mOFC
16 in lOFC)
• A/P: 3.6-4.6 mm
D/V: -5.0-5.2 mm
TESTING
Figure 5A and 5B
• Electrophysiology
recordings
• Operant task with
reward
HISTOLOGY
Figure 6A
• Perfuse
rats
• Stain brain
slices (40
um)
Figure 4. Operant Box Setup
Figure 5. Operant Task
Figure 6. Recording sites
A.
A.
C. Low, medium and high drinkers
Figure 14. There was a significant difference in the time taken to retrieve the reward during sucrose and ethanol trials
References
OFC Activity
for Ethanol
Ethanol
Predicting
Cue
Sucrose
Predicting
Cue
Home Cage
Ethanol
Consumption
Rats
High
Drinkers
Reward
Seeking
Reward
Seeking
more mOFC
less lOFC
Low
Drinkers
Reward
Seeking
No Reward
Seeking
less mOFC
more lOFC
A.
Rewards
earned: All
trials
B.
Rewards
earned :
Sucrose,
10% and
20%
ethanol
C.
Rewards
retrieved:
All trials
D.
Rewards
retrieved:
Sucrose,
10% and
20%
ethanol
These graphs suggest that although not significant, rats with high levels of home cage drinking tend to earn and retrieve more
sucrose and ethanol rewards (Fig. 15A, 15B, 15C and 15D; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001*).
Figure 9. There was a significant difference in number of initiated trials and tones played between sessions
Figure 12. Home cage drinking predicts future ethanol seeking behavior
These graphs suggest that home
cage drinking is an indicator of
future ethanol consumption
(Fig. 12A and 12B; *p < 0.05).
B. Home cage drinking and rewards retrieved
A. Total initiated trials: All sessions B. Total tones played: All sessions
A. Licking behavior: Blocked sucrose
and ethanol sessions
B. Licking behavior: Interleaved sucrose
and ethanol sessions
C. Licking behavior: Interleaved
ethanol sessions
Rats licked significantly more for sucrose when compared to ethanol (Fig. 8A, 8B, and 8C; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001,
****p < 0.0001).
A. Total rewards retrieved: All sessions
C. Percent of rewards retrieved:
blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions
Average home cage drinking increased from 2.68 g/kg on day 1 to 4.18 g/kg on day 13 (Fig. 7A). Individual variability was
seen in rats’ home cage drinking (Fig. 7B). Rats were split into low, medium and high drinkers based on previous
determinants of high drinkers (Fig. 7C)
B.
A. House light off to initial nose
poke: All sessions
B. Tone off to leave nose poke:
Blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions
C. Tone off to leave nose poke: Interleaved
sucrose and ethanol sessions
Rats were significantly faster at initiating trials during session containing sucrose rewards (Fig. 13A; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p <
0.001, ****p < 0.0001). Rats were also significantly faster at leaving the nose poke to retrieving the reward during interleaved
sucrose and 10% ethanol sessions (Fig. 13B and 13C; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).
A. Time taken to retrieve the reward: Blocked sucrose and
ethanol sessions
B. Time taken to retrieve the reward: Interleaved sucrose and
ethanol sessions
Rats were significantly faster at retrieving sucrose rewards compared to an ethanol rewards (Fig. 14A and 14B; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01,
***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).
Figure 15. Rewards earned and retrieved for low, medium and high drinkers
A. Home cage drinking and licking behavior
Figure 13. There was a significant difference in the time taken to initiate trials and to leave the nose poke during
blocked and interwoven sucrose and ethanol trials
B. Home cage drinking by animalA. Home cage drinking by day
*r = 0.58
B. Total rewards retrieved: Interleaved sucrose and
ethanol sessions
D. Percent of rewards retrieved: Interleaved sucrose and
ethanol sessions
Rats retrieved significantly more rewards in sessions where sucrose was a possible reward (Fig. 11A, and 11B; *p < 0.05, **p <
0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). The percent of rewards retrieved (of rewards earned) was significantly higher for blocked
sucrose sessions (Fig. 11C and 11D; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).
A. Total rewards earned: All sessions B. Total rewards earned: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol
sessions
C. Percent of rewards earned: blocked
sucrose and ethanol sessions
D. Percent of rewards earned: Interleaved sucrose and
ethanol sessions
Rats earned significantly more rewards in sessions where sucrose was a possible reward (Fig. 10A, and 10B; *p < 0.05, **p <
0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). The percent of rewards earned (of tones played) was significantly higher for blocked
sucrose sessions (Fig. 10C and 10D; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).

More Related Content

Similar to Thesis Poster BCRC

Poster
PosterPoster
Poster
Anant Naik
 
Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...
Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...
Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...
US Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), Center for Computational Toxicology and Exposure
 
76 Lab Review
76 Lab Review76 Lab Review
76 Lab Review
flguppie
 
Slas talk 2016
Slas talk 2016Slas talk 2016
Slas talk 2016
Sean Ekins
 
Lab review
Lab reviewLab review
Lab review
ktanaka2
 
Lab review
Lab reviewLab review
Lab review
ktanaka2
 
Alcohol Presentation2
Alcohol Presentation2Alcohol Presentation2
Alcohol Presentation2
AndrewRosenthal
 
Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)
Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)
Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)
Elizabeth Ventura
 
Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472
Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472
Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472
Reza Chowdhury, MSEE, EIT
 
U4D7waterCHEMISTRY
U4D7waterCHEMISTRYU4D7waterCHEMISTRY
U4D7waterCHEMISTRY
Cindy Brown
 
Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...
Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...
Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...
Mithil Fal Desai
 
Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...
Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...
Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...
Arthur Stem
 
Lab Report 1
Lab Report 1Lab Report 1
Lab Report 1
Blayr Downey
 
33133 article text-60219-2-10-20211104
33133 article text-60219-2-10-2021110433133 article text-60219-2-10-20211104
33133 article text-60219-2-10-20211104
nalayranc
 
Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)
Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)
Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)
Stephen Taylor
 
Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)
Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)
Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)
Stephen Taylor
 
Be 4101 project
Be 4101 projectBe 4101 project
Be 4101 project
TyWilliams24
 
poster v5 (1)
poster v5 (1)poster v5 (1)
poster v5 (1)
Wilfried Tadjou
 
Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016 Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016
Fontem Ventures
 
Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016
Fontem Ventures
 

Similar to Thesis Poster BCRC (20)

Poster
PosterPoster
Poster
 
Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...
Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...
Why High-Throughput Screening Data quality is important: Ephrin pharmacophore...
 
76 Lab Review
76 Lab Review76 Lab Review
76 Lab Review
 
Slas talk 2016
Slas talk 2016Slas talk 2016
Slas talk 2016
 
Lab review
Lab reviewLab review
Lab review
 
Lab review
Lab reviewLab review
Lab review
 
Alcohol Presentation2
Alcohol Presentation2Alcohol Presentation2
Alcohol Presentation2
 
Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)
Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)
Research Days Poster - 2015-3 (2)
 
Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472
Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472
Reza H Chowdhury CEE 472
 
U4D7waterCHEMISTRY
U4D7waterCHEMISTRYU4D7waterCHEMISTRY
U4D7waterCHEMISTRY
 
Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...
Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...
Extraction of photosynthetic pigments from different parts of the plants and ...
 
Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...
Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...
Sugarcane Ash and Sugarcane Ash-Derived Silica Nanoparticles Alter Cellular M...
 
Lab Report 1
Lab Report 1Lab Report 1
Lab Report 1
 
33133 article text-60219-2-10-20211104
33133 article text-60219-2-10-2021110433133 article text-60219-2-10-20211104
33133 article text-60219-2-10-20211104
 
Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)
Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)
Essential Biology 11.3 The Kidney (AHL)
 
Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)
Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)
Essential Biology: Making ATP Workbook (SL Core only)
 
Be 4101 project
Be 4101 projectBe 4101 project
Be 4101 project
 
poster v5 (1)
poster v5 (1)poster v5 (1)
poster v5 (1)
 
Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016 Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016
 
Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016Prasauskas et al 2016
Prasauskas et al 2016
 

Thesis Poster BCRC

  • 1. HOME CAGE CONSUMPTION OF 20% ETHANOL PREDICTS REWARD SEEKING BEHAVIOR INTRODUCTION Alcohol • 3rd preventable cause of death in the US [5] • In 2010, economic costs of excessive alcohol consumption was $249 billion [2] • Individual preference for alcohol is predictive of alcohol use disorder [7] Orbitofrontal Cortex • Influential in motivation, predicting reward, and relapse in addicts [3, 4, 6] • Regional differences in neuronal firing in the medial and lateral OFC (mOFC and lOFC) [1, 4] There are very few studies examining the role of the mOFC and lOFC in alcohol seeking. Siegal, R.E., Hernandez, J.S. and Moorman, D. E. Psychological and Brain Sciences, Neuroscience and Behavior Program, University of Massachusetts Amherst OPERANT TASK BEHAVIORAL DATA Reward and end of trial Longer than 500 ms = Long nose poke Tone signals type of reward Shorter than 100 ms = Short nose poke Start of Trial House light turns off Nose poke for 100 ms 1, 5 or 10 kHz tone Exit nose poke in 500 ms Reward delivered METHODS Figure 3. Intermittent Access to 20% Ethanol A. A. Figure 7. Individual variability in home cage drinking Figure 10. There was a significant difference in number and percent of rewards earned between sessions Figure 8. There was a significant difference in licking behavior between sessions HOMECAGE IA TO 20% ETHANOL Figure 11. There was a significant difference in number and percent of rewards retrieved between sessions. Figure 2. Procedure Overview A. Rats initiated significantly more trials and triggered significantly more tones in sessions containing sucrose rewards (Fig. 9A and 9B; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). B. Summary • Individual rats expressed variability in home cage ethanol consumption • Individual preference for ethanol may be a predictor of future ethanol seeking behavior in rats • Rats prefer sucrose over ethanol rewards; however rats will risk receiving ethanol rewards when there is a possibility that they may receive a sucrose reward instead Future Directions • Analyze the neural activity in the mOFC and lOFC when presented with cues predicting ethanol or sucrose rewards • Analyze the effect of previous ethanol or sucrose trials in interwoven sessions SUMMARY and FUTURE DIRECTIONS 1. Burton, A. C., Kashtelyan, V., Bryden, D. W., & Roesch, M. R. (November 07, 2013). Increased Firing to Cues That Predict Low-Value Reward in the Medial Orbitofrontal Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 12, 3310-3321. 2. http://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/alcohol-use.htm 3. Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (March 01, 2010). Neurocircuitry of Addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 4.) 4. Moorman, D. E., & Aston-Jones, G. (January 01, 2014). Orbitofrontal cortical neurons encode expectation-driven initiation of reward-seeking. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34, 31, 10234-46. 5. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Alcohol Facts and Statistics. (n.d.). Retrieved December 19, 2015, fromhttp://www.niaaa.nih.gov/alcohol-health/overviewalcohol-consumption/alcohol-facts-and-statistics 6. Schoenbaum, G., Roesch, M. R., Stalnaker, T. A., & Takahashi, Y. K. (January 01, 2009). A new perspective on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in adaptive behaviour. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10, 12, 885-92. 7. Spoelder, M., Hesseling, P., Baars, A. M., Lozeman-van, . K. J. G., Rotte, M. D., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., & Lesscher, H. M. B. (December 01, 2015). Individual Variation in Alcohol Intake Predicts Reinforcement, Motivation, and Compulsive Alcohol Use in Rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39, 12, 2427-2437. 8. Verschure, P. F. M. J., Verschure, P. F. M. J., Pennartz, C. M. A., & Pezzulo, G. (January 01, 2014). The why, what, where, when and how of goal-directed choice: Neuronal and computational principles. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 1655.) 9. Mormede, P., Colas, A., & Jones, B. C. (January 01, 2004). High ethanol preferring rats fail to show dependence following short- or long-term ethanol exposure. Alcohol and Alcoholism (oxford, Oxfordshire), 39, 3.) Thank you to Jessica Feliciano, Aidan Leith, Kathy Tran and Kerrin Bersani. A special thank you to John Hernandez for figures 3A, 4A and 6A, but more importantly for his guidance throughout my undergraduate research. Thank you to Dr. Moorman for his mentorship and support. Thank you to the Commonwealth Honors College at the University of Massachusetts Amherst for their financial contributions. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Figure 1. Hypotheses A. HOME CAGE INTERMITTENT ACCESS TO ETHANOL Figure 3A • 40 ml 20% ethanol 3x/week TRAINING • 40 ml water 2x/week • 4 weeks TRAINING Figure 4A • 15% sucrose solution • 85% reward retrieval SURGERY • 32 electrodes (16 in mOFC 16 in lOFC) • A/P: 3.6-4.6 mm D/V: -5.0-5.2 mm TESTING Figure 5A and 5B • Electrophysiology recordings • Operant task with reward HISTOLOGY Figure 6A • Perfuse rats • Stain brain slices (40 um) Figure 4. Operant Box Setup Figure 5. Operant Task Figure 6. Recording sites A. A. C. Low, medium and high drinkers Figure 14. There was a significant difference in the time taken to retrieve the reward during sucrose and ethanol trials References OFC Activity for Ethanol Ethanol Predicting Cue Sucrose Predicting Cue Home Cage Ethanol Consumption Rats High Drinkers Reward Seeking Reward Seeking more mOFC less lOFC Low Drinkers Reward Seeking No Reward Seeking less mOFC more lOFC A. Rewards earned: All trials B. Rewards earned : Sucrose, 10% and 20% ethanol C. Rewards retrieved: All trials D. Rewards retrieved: Sucrose, 10% and 20% ethanol These graphs suggest that although not significant, rats with high levels of home cage drinking tend to earn and retrieve more sucrose and ethanol rewards (Fig. 15A, 15B, 15C and 15D; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001*). Figure 9. There was a significant difference in number of initiated trials and tones played between sessions Figure 12. Home cage drinking predicts future ethanol seeking behavior These graphs suggest that home cage drinking is an indicator of future ethanol consumption (Fig. 12A and 12B; *p < 0.05). B. Home cage drinking and rewards retrieved A. Total initiated trials: All sessions B. Total tones played: All sessions A. Licking behavior: Blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions B. Licking behavior: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions C. Licking behavior: Interleaved ethanol sessions Rats licked significantly more for sucrose when compared to ethanol (Fig. 8A, 8B, and 8C; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). A. Total rewards retrieved: All sessions C. Percent of rewards retrieved: blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions Average home cage drinking increased from 2.68 g/kg on day 1 to 4.18 g/kg on day 13 (Fig. 7A). Individual variability was seen in rats’ home cage drinking (Fig. 7B). Rats were split into low, medium and high drinkers based on previous determinants of high drinkers (Fig. 7C) B. A. House light off to initial nose poke: All sessions B. Tone off to leave nose poke: Blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions C. Tone off to leave nose poke: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions Rats were significantly faster at initiating trials during session containing sucrose rewards (Fig. 13A; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). Rats were also significantly faster at leaving the nose poke to retrieving the reward during interleaved sucrose and 10% ethanol sessions (Fig. 13B and 13C; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). A. Time taken to retrieve the reward: Blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions B. Time taken to retrieve the reward: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions Rats were significantly faster at retrieving sucrose rewards compared to an ethanol rewards (Fig. 14A and 14B; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). Figure 15. Rewards earned and retrieved for low, medium and high drinkers A. Home cage drinking and licking behavior Figure 13. There was a significant difference in the time taken to initiate trials and to leave the nose poke during blocked and interwoven sucrose and ethanol trials B. Home cage drinking by animalA. Home cage drinking by day *r = 0.58 B. Total rewards retrieved: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions D. Percent of rewards retrieved: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions Rats retrieved significantly more rewards in sessions where sucrose was a possible reward (Fig. 11A, and 11B; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). The percent of rewards retrieved (of rewards earned) was significantly higher for blocked sucrose sessions (Fig. 11C and 11D; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). A. Total rewards earned: All sessions B. Total rewards earned: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions C. Percent of rewards earned: blocked sucrose and ethanol sessions D. Percent of rewards earned: Interleaved sucrose and ethanol sessions Rats earned significantly more rewards in sessions where sucrose was a possible reward (Fig. 10A, and 10B; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). The percent of rewards earned (of tones played) was significantly higher for blocked sucrose sessions (Fig. 10C and 10D; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).