SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 271
Journal of Family Violence
(2011) 26:163–169 DOI
10.1007/s10896-010-9346-0
Predicting Officer Physical Assaults at Domestic Assault Calls
Richard R. Johnson
Published online: 18 January 2011 # Springer Science +
Business Media, LLC 2011
Abstract Police work is rife with the potential for physical
harm, and domestic assault
calls are one source of assaults on officers. Inability to
accurately predict what circumstances
might lead to an attack is one cause of officer cynicism and
paranoia. Having the ability to
identify which suspects pose the greatest threat of assault would
allow officers to take
appropriate protective measures. Using data collected from
1,951 domestic assault calls across
three cities, the present study compared characteristics and
behaviors of the batterers with
whether or not the batterers physically assaulted the responding
officers. Findings revealed five
significant batterer characteristics (employment status, shared
residence with abuse victim,
alcohol consumption, property damage, and hostile demeanor
toward officers) that
successfully predicted officer assaults. These risk factors may
be incorporated into police safety
training in the response to family violence calls.
Keywords Domestic violence. Officer safety. Officer assaults
The potential for assaults on police officers handling domestic
assault calls has been the
subject of some debate over the last few decades. While some
studies have found that
domestic assault calls only contribute to a very small portion of
the assaults that officers
experience (Garner and Clemmer 1986; Hirschel et al. 1994;
Konstantin 1984), studies in
different communities have suggested that domestic assault
calls are one of the three most
common situations where officer assaults occur (Ellis et al.
1993;
Stanford and Mowry 1990; Uchida et al. 1987). Even if other
types of calls have higher rates of
officer assault, this does not necessarily mean that domestic
assault calls are “safe.” On the
contrary, as FBI statistics indicate that from 1980 through 2003
more than 224,000 police
officers in the U.S. were assaulted while handling family
disturbance calls (Johnson 2007, 2008).
Furthermore, while survival rate for officers assaulted in the
U.S. has been steadily increasing
over the last 30 years, the survival rate for officers assaulted at
domestic violence calls has
failed to increase for officers assaulted at domestic assault
calls. It is the unpredictability and
constant potential for danger in the form of a violent physical
attack that distinguishes the work
environment of the police officer. Inability to accurately predict
what circumstances might lead
to an attack is suggested to be a primary cause of police
cynicism and officer mistrust of the
public, causing officers to emphasize keeping their guard up at
all times (Barker 1999; Skolnick
1966; Wilson 1968). This is impossible to achieve, however,
and could lead to paranoia or
citizen complaints for overly aggressive behavior. Therefore,
increasing an officer’s ability to
predict which domestic assault situations pose the highest
potential for assault could be very
helpful. Being able to identify for officers which circumstances
pose threats of assault would
allow officers to take protective measures. The present study
sought to utilize characteristics of
the domestic batterer and the situational circumstances of the
incident to predict assaults on
police officers attending domestic assault calls. This study
extended the previous research on
police officer assaults by moving beyond simply searching for
correlates of assault and
developing prediction odds ratios for assaults. A simple
correlate usually refers to a factor that
occurs at the same time as the outcome, while predictor
variables predict an increased
probability of the outcome (Loeber 1990). This study
Page 164
sought to predict increased probabilities of assaults on officers.
Furthermore, this research
targeted assaults at domestic calls, which some previous
research has suggested occur
somewhat differently than assaults on officers performing other
duties (Johnson 2007, 2008). It
was hoped that key characteristics could be identified that
would be useful in predicting the
likelihood of an officer assault and permit the utilization of
proactive safety tactics by officers.
Literature Review
While there has been a fair amount of research on the correlates
of police use of force
against citizens (see for example Alpert and Dunham 1999;
Geller and Toch 1995; Terrill and
Mastrofski 2002), there has only been limited research on the
correlates of physical assaults
against police officers. Meyer et al. (1979) reviewed the
characteristics of assailants in 1,143
assaults on police officers from 37 municipal law enforcement
agencies in five U.S. states. The
majority of the assailants in these assaults were young (between
ages 18 and 25), male,
members of racial minority groups, unemployed, had been
consuming alcohol, and had
displayed an angry or belligerent demeanor prior to committing
the assault. A number of these
individuals also had personal problems such as a lack of a high
school education, a criminal
record, and family relationship difficulties (Meyer et al. 1979).
More recently, Pinizzotto et al.
(1998) reviewed the assailant characteristics in all law
enforcement officer murders reported to
the FBI from 1985 to 1994. Again they found that the majority
of the assailants were young
(between the ages of 15 and 29), male, members of racial
minority groups, unemployed, low
socioeconomic status, unmarried, under the influence of drugs
or alcohol, and had a criminal
record.
Other Western nations have also conducted similar studies and
identified similar
assailant characteristics. Brown (1994), Moxey and McKenzie
(1993), and Noaks and
Christopher (1990) analyzed data from officer assaults in the
United Kingdom. In all three of
these English studies the assailants were found to be
predominantly lower income males who
were unemployed, had a criminal record, and were consuming
alcohol at the time of the
assault. Most were between the ages of 17 and 25, with an
average age of 22 years old. In
Australia, Mayhew (2001) analyzed official police reports and
found similar characteristics were
prevalent in those who assaulted officers in that nation. The
Australian assailants were
generally young males of lower socioeconomic status who were
intoxicated, with aboriginals
being over-represented among the assailants. In Iceland,
Bragason (2007) conducted a survey
of assaulted police officers who described the characteristics of
their assailants. Again it was
found that those who assaulted the officers appeared to be
predominantly young indigenous
males who were unemployed, had a criminal record, and had
been consuming alcohol. While
the previous research has done a fairly good job of describing
the characteristics of the
assailants who attack police officers in many nations, they all
failed to make any attempt to
predict officer assaults. While the person who is most likely to
assault police officers is
described as a lower class, adolescent male who has been
consuming alcohol, it could be
argued that this description fits a significant proportion of the
citizens with whom the police
routinely interact. Yet only small fractions of the intoxicated,
lower class, adolescent males the
police encounter actually assault them. So how do the assailants
differ from the non-assailants
that officers typically encounter? The previous literature has
been silent on this question.
Another weakness to the previous literature is an assumption
that all assault incidents are
similar. Assailant characteristics are pooled across all types of
officer assault incidents. Recently
there has been evidence to suggest that there may be differences
in both offender
characteristics, and how assaults occur, across different types of
officer assault incidents
(Johnson 2007). Previous studies failed to look specifically at
physical assaults of officers
handling domestic assault calls to determine what
characteristics offenders in these specific
situations have, and how these characteristics differ from
batterers in domestic assault calls
who did not assault the police. The present study attempted to
fill this void in the literature.
Method
Participants
Data used came from the pooling of datasets from three
previous studies. A sample of
domestic assault calls was needed that contained descriptive
information about the batterer
involved, and contained both cases that involved an assault on
an officer and cases that did not.
Data from three well known domestic violence arrest
experiment studies were used, the first
being the Minneapolis Domestic Violence Experiment (Sherman
and Rogan 1984) which
involved official police reports in 1,250 domestic assault cases
handled by the Minneapolis
Police Department. The second was a replication of the
Minneapolis experiment that was
conducted in Milwaukee, Wisconsin, involving 921 domestic
assault cases handled by the
Milwaukee Police Department (Sherman et al. 1991). The last
study was a replication study in
Miami, Florida that involved 907
Page 165
domestic assault cases handled by the Metro-Dade Police
Department (Pate et al. 1991). 1
Even though these data were collected in the 1980s, they remain
relevant today for
several reasons. First, they continue to be used in research on
family violence published in
prestigious social science research journals (Garner and
Maxwell 2000; Hickman 2003; Maxwell
et al. 2002; Piquero et al. 2006). Second, since empirical
evidence suggests that assaults on
officers at domestic calls, and officer survival rates from these
assaults, have remained stable
for more than 30 years (Johnson 2007, 2008), there is little
likelihood that any possible changes
in police tactics since the 1980s has occurred with respect to
handling domestic calls. Finally,
the emphasis of this investigation rested on offender
characteristics to predict offender
behavior, relating to innate human behavioral traits that are
unlikely to have evolved
significantly over a few decades.
Data collection method used in all three of these studies was
identical. The data about
the domestic assault incident came from official reports by
police officers and from interviews
with the domestic abuse victims conducted by the research team
within a week after the
incident. Information about the batterer’s behaviors before the
arrival of the police was also
obtained in the abuse victim interviews. Finally, data about the
batterer’s prior criminal record
was obtained from the state crime computer database (Pate et al.
1991; Sherman and Rogan
1984; Sherman et al. 1991).
After merging the 3,078 cases from these three studies it was
discovered that 98 cases
(3.2%) involved a female batterer, none of whom had assaulted
an officer. Because of this lack
of variation in the dependent variable for female batterers in the
sample, the decision was
made to limit this analysis to only male batterers. In another
33.3% of the cases the batterer
had fled the scene prior to the arrival of the police, thus
eliminating the possibility of an assault
on the officers. These 1,026 cases were therefore eliminated
from the analysis, as were three
additional cases that involved an assault on officers by the
abuse victim rather than the
batterer.2 The final dataset for the present study consisted of
data on 1,951 incidents of
domestic assault that were investigated by the police.
The fact that so many cases were eliminated from the sample for
this analysis could
raise questions of validity, especially if the batterers excluded
from the analysis differed
significantly from those that remained. It order to determine the
extent of this potential
weakness, basic demographic characteristics available for the
batterers who remained in the
sample were compared with those who were excluded from the
sample. These demographic
characteristics are displayed in Table 1 and suggest that there
were surprisingly few differences
between the cases that were excluded and those that remained in
the sample. In general, those
batterers that were excluded from the analyses were slightly
more likely to be male, were
slightly younger, and less likely to be married.
Procedure
The dependent variable in this analysis was whether or not the
official report indicated
the suspect had physically assaulted the officers handling the
domestic assault call. Because the
data collected was focused primarily on the batterer and the
abuse victim, details were not
available on whether or not the officers sustained injuries, the
seriousness of the injuries,
whether or not the assailant used a weapon, or when during the
encounter this attack took
place. While having this information would have permitted a
richer analysis, the emphasis of
this study, however was the prediction of assaults, no matter
how minor. Just as with victims of
abuse, assaults of any kind are a crime and place police officers
at risk of injury and increased
stress. Data were available on 12 descriptive characteristics
about the batterers in the domestic
assaults that served as the independent variables in this
analysis. These variables and their
descriptive statistics are displayed in Table 2. The first
independent variable considered was the
batterer’s age, and it was hypothesized that younger batterers
would be more likely to assault
officers than older batterers. Second, whether the batterer was
married to the abuse victim
was
Table 1 Comparison between sample and excluded cases
Sample Cases Excluded Cases
N 1,951 1,127
Percent male 100% 91%
Mean age 33.4 30.4
Percent married 51% 47%
Percent consuming alcohol 51% 49%
Percent using drugs 22% 23%
Percent damaged property 18% 16%
Percent struck abuse victim 94% 91%
Page 166
Table 2 Variable descriptive statistics (N=1,951)
Dependent
Variable N %
N % %
Batterer
physically
assaulted
officers
117 6%
Independent
Variables
Batterer’s age
Min 18 Max 79
Mean 33.4 SD
9.01
Min 18 Max 79 Mean 33.4 SD 9.01
Batterer married
to abuse victim
995 51%
Batterer’s prior
DV arrests
Min 0 Max 10 Mean 0.89 SD 1.08
Batterer
unemployed
702 36%
Batterer has
HS/GED
1,093 56% 56%
Batterer residing
with abuse
victim
741 38%
Batterer
consuming
alcohol
429 22%
Batterer using
drugs
429 22%
Batterer
damaged
property
351 18%
Batterer struck
abuse victim
1,834 94%
Batterer knew
police were
responding
1,034 53%
Batterer
displayed hostile
demeanor
1,073 55%
considered, under the assumption that married batterers may
hold stronger views of privilege
to control their spouses and defend against outside interference
by the police. Similarly, the
third independent variable was whether or not the batterer and
abuse victim resided together.
It was hypothesized that batterers who reside with their abuse
victims would be more
defensive of this territory being invaded by the arrival of an
outside authority, such as the
police.
The fourth and fifth independent variables dealt with substance
use issues, whether or
not the batterer had consumed alcohol prior to the arrival of the
police, and whether the
batterer had used an illegal drug before the arrival of the police.
Each of these variables was
expected to have a positive relationship with assaulting police
officers.
The next three independent variables were believed to be
measures of the batterer’s
level of aggression at the time of the incident. It was reasoned
that whether or not the batterer
had damaged property as part of the domestic assault incident,
and whether or not the abuse
victim had been struck by the batterer as part of the incident,
would suggest how violently
aggressive the batterer was at the time of the incident. Whether
or not the batterer displayed a
hostile demeanor toward the officers upon their arrival at the
scene was also included in the
model to measure if the batterer’s level of aggression was still
high when the officers finally
arrived.
Whether or not the batterer had been told the police had been
summoned was also
considered as a possible predictor. When offenders are surprised
by the appearance of the
police they may be more likely to resist or be antagonistic
toward the officers. It was therefore
hypothesized that the batterer would be more likely to assault
the officers if their arrival on the
scene was unexpected, allowing him less time to compose him,
flee the scene, or formulate a
plan for how to respond to the officers.
The last three predictors in the model related to the batterer’s
background, specifically
his prior history of domestic assault arrests, his employment
status, and his education level.
Regarding the batterer’s prior record, all three datasets used for
this analysis recorded the
number of prior domestic assault arrests the batterer had
received. Unfortunately, not all of
the datasets included information on each batterer’s total
number of prior criminal offense
arrests, (which may have been a better predictor of past
criminality), or all prior violent
offenses (which may have been a better predictor of violent
propensity). Whether or not the
batterer was employed was included in the model, as was
whether or not the batterer had
attained a high school diploma or general education
development (GED) diploma. It was
hypothesized that batterers with prior domestic assault arrests,
those who were unemployed,
and those who had less than a high school education would be at
greater risk for assaulting an
officer.
Results
Only 117 cases involved an assault on officers, thus supporting
the previous literature
that domestic assault calls are not necessarily frequent events
(Hirschel et al. 1994; Konstantin
1984; Garner and Clemmer 1986). Because the dependent
variable was a dichotomous nominal
level measure, binary logistic regression was utilized to
determine the significance of the
influence the independent variables had on the probability of
whether or not the batterer
assaulted the police officers on the scene. The logistic
regression technique also permitted the
determination of the odds each predictor had of increasing the
likelihood of assault, while
controlling for the influence of all of the other predictors in the
model. The results of this
logistic regression analysis are presented in Table 3.
As can been seen in Table 3, the model chi-square was highly
significant (p<.001),
indicating that the combined predictors in the model did a fair
job of predicting whether or not
the batterer would assault the officers. The Nagelkerke pseudo
R2 suggests that the
independent variables in the model explained more than a
quarter of the variation between
cases in the odds of the batterer physically attacking the police.
Within the model, however,
Page 167
Table 3 Logistic regression of batterer characteristics on officer
assault (N=1,951)
Variable Coefficient SE Odds Ratio
Batterer age −0.016 .020 0.984
Batterer married 0.070 .327 1.073
Prior DV arrests −0.126 .219 0.882
Batterer unemployed 0.542* .308 1.582
High school/GED 0.047 .308 3.165
Resides with abuse
victim
1.152** .342 3.165
Using alcohol 1.706*** .431 5.507
Using drugs −0.562 .459 0.570
Property damaged 0.652* .307 1.919
Abuse victim struck −0.656 .582 0.519
Hostile demeanor 2.530*** .480 12.555
Knew Police were
coming
−0.161 .298 0.852
(Constant) −5.792*** 1.029 0.003
Model Chi-square 122.843***
Nagelkerke Pseudo
R2
.276
Significance Levels: * p<.05; ** p<.01; *** p<.001
only five of the 12 independent variables were significant in
predicting the odds of an assault
occurring.
The strongest predictor was whether or not the batterer
displayed a hostile demeanor
at the arrival of the police. When the suspect displayed a hostile
demeanor toward the police,
the batterer was almost 13 times more likely to assault the
officers than if he did not display a
hostile demeanor. The next significant predictor in strength was
whether or not the batterer
had been consuming alcohol prior to the arrival of the police.
Batterers who had been
consuming alcohol were five-and-a-half times more likely to
assault officers than those who had
not. If the batterer shared a residence with the victim he was
more than three times more likely
to physically assault the police. Batterers who had reportedly
damaged property during the
domestic incident were almost twice as likely to assault officers
as those who had not damaged
property. Those batterers who were unemployed were about
one-and-a-half times more likely
to assault the police as batters who were employed. Finally, all
of the independent variables
that revealed significant relationships to the dependent variable
displayed relationships in the
predicted directions.
To reveal the substantive significance these five variables had
on predicting whether or
not an assault on the police would occur, the cases that bore all
five predictors were compared
with the cases that lacked all five significant predictors. A total
of 43 cases involved a batterer
who displayed a hostile demeanor, had been consuming alcohol,
resided with his abuse victim,
had damaged property in the incident, and was unemployed. Of
these cases, 25.6% (n=11)
resulted in an assault on the police. On the other hand, 263
incidents in the sample failed to
have any of the five significant predictor characteristics, and
none of these incidents resulted in
an officer assault. Therefore, it appears that simultaneous
presence of all five of these
predictors increases the chances of an officer being assaulted to
better than one in four, while
the absence of all of these predictors reduces the chances of
assault to less than one in 263.
Discussion and Conclusion
The purpose of this study was to attempt to identify domestic
batterer characteristics
that helped predict whether or not batterers would assault police
officers who responded to
their acts of domestic abuse. It was hoped that this exploratory
analysis would reveal
characteristics that had predictive value in order to help police
officers identify when they are
at increased risk of being assaulted by a male domestic batterer.
It appears that this study took
a significant first step in identifying such risk factor
characteristics.
As hostile demeanor and alcohol consumption were significant
predictors of an officer
assault. Prior research has suggested that domestic batterers
frequently act composed when
officers arrive and tend to act as if the abuse victim is the one
who is acting irrationally (Buzawa
and Buzawa 2003; Walker 1989). Therefore, batterers who
remain hostile in their demeanor
even after the police arrive appear likely to let their hostility
turn to physical violence against
the officers. Thus encountering a batterer who still displays a
hostile demeanor when the
officers arrive is an uncommon event and appears highly
predictive of an impending assault.
As for batterer alcohol consumption, the prior research on
police officer assaults
suggested that alcohol consumption is correlated with attacks on
officers (Brown 1994; Meyer
et al. 1979; Moxey and McKenzie 1993; Noaks and Christopher
1990; Pinizzotto et al. 1998),
and the same was found here. Alcohol serves as a depressant,
contributing to the batterer’s
negative mood, and may reduce the batterer’s inhibitions about
using force against the police.
Alcohol consumption frequently coexists with domestic
violence (Buzawa and Buzawa 2003;
Saunders 1995; Straus et al. 1980), and appears to increase
likelihood of the batterer assaulting
the responding police officers.
Batterers who reside with their abuse victim are more likely to
assault officers than
those who live apart from their victim. This may be due to
impressions of territorial control.
Prior research has suggested that many batterers have
heightened needs for dominance and
control in their relationships and their home (Buzawa and
Buzawa 2003; Holtzworth-Munroe
and Stuart 1994; Walker 1989). They
Page 168
are threatened when they cannot successfully control the
behavior of their intimate
partners, and appear to be even more threatened when the police
enter their home and
interject themselves into the situation. In essence, the batterer
has lost all influence over what
he views as his human and physical property when the police
intervene. While this police
interference may be difficult for the batterer to accept at any
location, it appears to be an even
stronger insult when it occurs in his own residence.
It may also be this emphasis on dominance, power, and control
that results in
unemployed batterers being more likely to assault officers than
batterers who are employed.
Unemployed batterers may be already struggling to maintain
dominance in their home. Being
unable to provide for their families, they no longer have the
option of using economic means to
control their abuse victims. Some abuse victims are willing to
tolerate their abuse because of
the economic rewards afforded to her and her children if she
stays in the relationship (Buzawa
and Buzawa 2003; Straus et al. 1980), but if there are very few
financial rewards due to the
batterer’s unemployment, there are fewer incentives to stay. The
batterer begins to lose some
of his power and control in the relationship. This situation can
be aggravated further for the
batterer if it results in his abuse victim seeking employment to
support the family, thus gaining
some economic independence that further threatens that
batterer’s control. This situation may
make the batterer more volatile than normal when the police
arrive to interject themselves into
that situation, and limit the batterer’s control to an even greater
extent.
Destruction of property by the batterer prior to the arrival of the
police was another significant
predictor of officer assaults. This may be an indicator of the
degree of the batterer’s rage. While
whether or not the victim was struck was not a significant
predictor, it is important to
remember how many batterers view their use of force. Their
abusive behavior is used to
control their victim. When the abuse victim is completely
compliant the abuse is usually very
minor, often only limited to insults and innuendo. When the
abuse victim’s behavior is
perceived as less compliant, the severity of the abuse increases
proportionally (Buzawa and
Buzawa 2003; Saunders 1995; Walker 1989). When the batterer
damages property, especially
his own possessions, it is not a control technique as much as it
is an expression of rage. This
rage is then turned on the police when they arrive.
These five significant predictors could be utilized by law
enforcement agencies to screen
domestic assault calls for their level of danger. In many
situations, police dispatchers can collect
details about four of these elements from the victim or witness
who reports the crime. Even if
the caller is a neighbor, the dispatcher can often ascertain from
the caller if the suspect resides
with the victim, if he has been drinking, and if he has damaged
any property. If the caller is a
friend or family member, he/she should also be able to reveal if
the batterer is unemployed.
The dispatcher could then warn the responding officers if these
four assault predictors are
already present at the scene. The dispatcher can also assign
additional units when these
elements are present. When the officers arrive, they can
determine for themselves if the fifth
element, a hostile demeanor toward them, is present. If so, the
officers can take defensive
measures to isolate the batterer and place him in a position of
disadvantage (such as on his
knees with his hands interlaced on his head) before questioning
him.
Finally, it is important to note for police training purposes that
female victims and female
batterers in domestic assault situations assault the police only
very rarely. Of the 3,078
domestic assault calls in the original sampling frame, only 98
(3.2%) involved a female batterer,
illustrating how rarely law enforcement officers encounter
female perpetrators of domestic
assault. Of these 98 incidents of female perpetrated domestic
assault, none of them resulted in
an assault on an officer. Three of the male perpetrated domestic
assault calls did result in a
female abuse victim assaulting an officer. Of the 2,980
incidents of male perpetrated domestic
assault calls in the original sampling frame, less than 0.1%
involved a victim assault on an
officer. In this dataset, only one in 1,026 domestic assault calls
handled by the police resulted in
a female victim assault on an officer.
Several inferences can be drawn from the findings in this study.
First, attacks on police officers
while they are handling domestic assault calls do not occur very
frequent- ly. In the present
study, officers were assaulted in only 6% of cases in the sample.
Nevertheless, due to the sheer
volume of domestic assault calls handled by the police in the
U.S., there still is a significant risk
of assault. For example, if the odds of assault are truly only one
in 33 (3%), and a patrol officer
in an urban area responds to at least two domestic assault calls
per week, the officer is likely to
be assaulted at least twice per year. Obviously other factors are
also at work here, however, as
the variables in the present study left more than 70% of the
variation unexplained. While an
explained variance of 30% is common among social science
studies of human behavior (Loeber
1990; Rosenthal 2001), more work needs to be done. Certain
officer characteristics (such as
skills in interpersonal communication), or situational
characteristics (such as the number of
backup officers present) may help further reduce the odds of
being assaulted. Possibilities
should be pursued in future research.
Another inference to be drawn from this study is that the five
predictors that were significantly
correlated with assaulting an officer may be reliable risk factors
that officers may use to predict
their risk of assault. Whether or not the batterer at a domestic
assault incident has most of
these five characteristics could easily be determined by a
Page 169
skilled police 911 dispatcher, provided that the caller has
firsthand knowledge about the
incident and is being cooperative. If the batterer appears to have
any of these five
characteristics, the responding officers could possibly be
warned of this fact prior to their
arrival. Policies could also be developed to send more backup
units than normally would be
sent if it is determined that the batterer has most of these
characteristics. However, before
these characteristics could be reliably established as risk
factors, replication of these findings
would be necessary.
As with all studies, this one had its limitations. First, because of
the age of the data it may not
have accounted for any changes in police tactics that may have
occurred over the last few
decades. Second, the data lacked information on the seriousness
of the assaults on the officers.
It was unknown, therefore, if the victim officers sustained any
injuries from these assaults, and
if the predictors varied by the seriousness of the assault.
Finally, the data lacked information on
the interpersonal interactions between the officers and the
batterer upon their arrival. What
the officers did and said at the scene may have had an
aggravating of mitigating influence on
the potential for assault. These weaknesses need to be addressed
in future research regarding
assaults on officers attending domestic assault calls.
References
R. R. Johnson (*) Department of Criminal Justice, University of
Toledo, 2801 W. Bancroft St.,
Mail Stop 119, Toledo, OH 43606-3390, USA e-mail:
[email protected]
1 Data obtained from Sherman, L. W. and Berk, R. A., Specific
Deterrent Effects of Arrest for
Domestic Assault: Minneapolis, 1981– 1982: Sherman, L. W.,
Schmidt, J. D., Rogan, D. P.,
Milwaukee Domestic Violence Experiment, 1987–1989: Pate,
A., Hamilton, E. E., and Sampson,
A., Spouse Abuse Replication Project in Metro-Dade County,
Florida 1987–1989: computer file
datasets; Ann Arbor, Michigan: Inter-university Consortium for
Political and Social Research,
2001. 2 While police lore frequently provides examples of
incidents where it was the abuse
victim at the domestic violence call who assaulted the officers,
the data here suggests that this
is a truly rare event. An attack on officers by a victim occurred
in less than one in a thousand
domestic violence calls recorded in these three datasets.
Alpert, G. P., & Dunham, R. G. (1999). Use of force: Overview
of national and local data.
Washington: National Institute of Justice. Barker, J. C. (1999).
Danger, duty, and disillusion: The
worldview of
Los Angeles police officers. Prospect Heights: Waveland.
Bragason, O. O. (2007). Assaults against police officers: A self-
report study among Icelandic
police officers. Reykjavik: The Office of the National Police
Commissioner.
Brown, B. (1994). Assaults on police officers: An examination
of the circumstances in which
such incidents occur. London: Home Office Research Group.
Buzawa, E., & Buzawa, C. (2003). Domestic violence: The
criminal justice response (3rd ed.).
Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Ellis, D., Choi, A., & Blaus, C. (1993). Injuries to police
officers attending domestic disturbances:
an empirical study. Canadian Journal of Criminology, 34, 149–
168.
Garner, J., & Clemmer, E. (1986). Danger to police in domestic
disturbances: A new look.
Washington: National Institute of Justice. Garner, J., &
Maxwell, C. (2000). What are the lessons
of the police arrest studies? Journal of Aggression,
Maltreatment and Trauma,
4, 83–114.
Geller, W. A., & Toch, H. (1995). And justice for all:
Understanding and controlling police abuse
of force. Washington: Police Executive Research Forum.
Hickman, L. J. (2003). Fair treatment or preferred outcome?
The impact of police behavior on
victim reports of domestic violence incidents. Law and Society
Review, 37, 607–633.
mailto:[email protected]
Hirschel, J. D., Dean, C. W., & Lumb, R. C. (1994). The
relative contribution of domestic violence
to assault and injury of police officers. Justice Quarterly, 11,
99–117.
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (1994). Typologies of
male batterers: three subtypes and
the differences among them. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 476–
497.
Johnson, R. R. (2007). Surviving firearm assaults at domestic
violence calls. Law Enforcement
Executive Forum, 7(7), 155–167.
Johnson, R. R. (2008). Assessing the true dangerousness of
domestic violence calls. Law
Enforcement Executive Forum, 8(5), 19–29.
Konstantin, D. (1984). Homicides of American law enforcement
officers. Justice Quarterly, 1,
29–45.
Loeber, R. (1990). Development and risk factors of juvenile
antisocial behavior and delinquency.
Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 1–41. Maxwell, C., Garner, J.,
& Fagan, J. (2002). The preventive
effects of arrest on intimate partner violence: research, policy
and theory.
Criminology and Public Policy, 2, 51–79.
Mayhew, C. (2001). Occupational health and safety risks faced
by police officers. Trends and
Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice, 196, 1–6. Meyer, C. K.,
Magedanz, D. C., Kieselhorst, D., &
Chapman, S. (1979). Violence and the police: the special case
of the police assailant.
Journal of Police Science and Administration, 7, 161–171.
Moxey, M., & McKenzie, I. (1993). Assaults on police.
Policing: An International Journal of Police
Strategies and Management, 9, 172–186.
Noaks, L., & Christopher, S. (1990). Why are police assaulted?
Policing: An International Journal
of Police Strategies and Management, 6, 625–638.
Pate, A. M., Hamilton, E. E., & Annan, S. (1991). Metro-Dade
spouse abuse replication project:
Final report. Washington: Police Foundation.
Pinizzotto, A., David, E., & Miller, C. (1998). In the line of
fire: learning from assaults on law
enforcement officers. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 67(2), 15–
23.
Piquero, A., Brame, R., Fagan, J., & Moffitt, T. (2006).
Assessing the offending activity of criminal
domestic violence suspects: offense specialization, escalation,
and de-escalation evidence from
the spouse assault replication program. Public Health Reports,
121, 409–418.
Rosenthal, J. A. (2001). Statistics and data interpretation.
Belmont: Brooks/Cole.
Saunders, D. G. (1995). Prediction of wife assault. In J.
Campbell (Ed.), Assessing dangerousness:
Violence by sexual offenders, batterers, and child abusers (pp.
9–34). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Sherman, L. W., & Rogan, D. P. (1984). The specific deterrent
effects of arrest for domestic
assault. American Sociological Review, 49, 261–272.
Sherman, L. W., Schmidt, J. D., Rogan, D. P., Gartin, P. R.,
Cohn, E. G., Collins, D. J., et al. (1991).
From initial deterrence to long- term escalation: short-custody
arrest for poverty ghetto
domestic violence. Criminology, 29(4), 821–850.
Skolnick, J. (1966). Justice without trial: Law enforcement in a
democratic society. New York:
Wiley.
Stanford, R. M., & Mowry, B. L. (1990). Domestic disturbance
danger rate. Journal of Police
Science and Administration, 17, 244–249.
Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind
closed doors: Violence in the
American family. Garden City: Anchor.
Terrill, W., & Mastrofski, S. D. (2002). Reassessing situational
and officer based determinants of
police coercion. Justice Quarterly, 19, 215–248.
Uchida, C. D., Brooks, L. W., & Koper, C. S. (1987). Danger to
police during domestic encounters:
assaults on Baltimore county police, 1984–86. Criminal Justice
Policy Review, 2, 357–371.
Walker, L. E. (1989). Terrifying love: Why battered women kill
and how society responds. New
York: Harper and Row.
Wilson, J. Q. (1968). Varieties of police behavior: The
management of law and order in eight
communities. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Copyright of Journal of Family Violence is the property of
Springer Science &
Business Media B.V. and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple
sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual
use.
Predicting Officer Physical Assaults at Domestic Assault
CallsLiterature ReviewMethodProcedureTable 1 Comparison
between sample and excluded casesTable 2 Variable descriptive
statistics (N=1,951)ResultsTable 3 Logistic regression of
batterer characteristics on officer assault (N=1,951)Discussion
and ConclusionReferences
REVIEW
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence
in the LGBT Community Using a Bystander
Framework
Sharyn J. Potter, PhD, MPH, Kim Fountain, PhD, and Jane G.
Stapleton, MA
Sexual and relationship violence are two major public health
issues that affect an alarming number
of undergraduate students. As a result, many colleges and
universities have protocols to serve
victims of these forms of violence. Despite federal legislation
stating that all students should have
equitable experiences, current protocols and programs focus
primarily on heterosexual
students. College student victims of sexual and relationship
violence who identify as lesbian, gay,
bisexual, or transgender can face particular challenges,
including disclosure of their sexual
and gender orientations, and revictimization when seeking
services. In recent years an
increasing number of campuses have adopted bystander
prevention strategies to address sexual
and relationship violence. These strategies seek to engage
community members in the prevention
of sexual and relationship violence by training them to identify
and safely intervene in situations
where sexual or relationship violence is about to occur, is
occurring, or has occurred. In this article
we review published bystander prevention strategies that focus
on preventing sexual and
relationship violence in the campus community, and discuss
how bystander strategies are
addressing or can address relationship and sexual violence in
the LGBT community. (HARV REV
PSYCHIATRY 2012;20:201–208.)
Keywords: bisexual. bystander prevention strategies,
college, gay, lesbian, relationship and sexual
violence, transgender
From the Department of Sociology (Dr. Potter) and Women’s
Stud-
ies Program (Dr. Fountain and Ms. Stapleton), University of
New
Hampshire.
Original manuscript received 18 August 2011; revised
manuscript
received 17 December 2011, accepted for publication subject to
re-
vision 17 January 2012; revised manuscript received 9 February
2012.
Correspondence: Sharyn J. Potter, PhD, MPH, Department of
Sociol-
ogy, University of New Hampshire, 20 Academic Way, Durham,
NH
03824. Correspondence: [email protected]
© 2012 President and Fellows of Harvard College
DOI: 10.3109/10673229.2012.712838
Sexual assault of women is the most common violent crime
committed on college campuses today; one in four college
women experiences completed or attempted rape during
their college years.1 The majority of these assaults are
perpetrated by the victim’s acquaintances (e.g., classmates,
residence hall neighbors, dates) or intimate partners.1−3
College women are at higher risk for sexual assault than
peers who are not in college.4 Studies show that college men
also report unwanted sexual experiences.5,6 Recent research
indicates that 25% of women and 14% of men will experience
relationship violence over the course of their lifetimes.7
Exposure to sexual and relationship violence∗ is a key public
health issue and is associated with a multiplicity of negative
outcomes, including increased substance use, depressive
symptoms, health-risk behaviors, and symptoms of
posttraumatic stress disorder among various samples of
survivors.∗ ,9−13
*We use the term sexual violence to refer to a range of
behaviors that are unwanted by the recipient, including, but not
limited to, the
following: derogatory or insulting remarks about physical
appearance; persistent sexual advances that are undesired by the
recipient;
unwanted touching; and unwanted oral, anal, or vaginal
penetration. These behaviors could be initiated by someone
either known or
unknown to the recipient, including someone in an existing
relationship with the recipient. We use the term relationship
violence to refer to
a range of behaviors experienced in the con- text of any type of
intimate relationship or friendship. These behaviors include use
of physical
force or threats of force against a partner, such as slapping,
punching, throwing objects, threatening with weapons, or
threatening any
kind of physical harm. It can also include extreme emotional
abuse such as intimidation, blaming, putting down, making fun
of, and name
calling.
∗ Although the terms victim and survivor are often used
interchangeably,7 in the legal definition the term victim rather
than survivor is
used to describe a person following an assault.8
201
mailto:[email protected]
Harv Rev Psychiatry
July/August 2012
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT
Community 202
The extent to which campuses are working to prevent
these problems varies widely.4 Most efforts are aimed at
heterosexual women—statistically, the most highly
represented group. But this population is not the only one
for which sexual and relationship violence is an issue.
Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender (LGBT) students
represent a marginalized population at the same14 or at
higher risk for sexual and relationship violence15−18 as their
heterosexual peers. Yet the 5%–10% of undergraduate
student populations who identify as LGBT19,20 are signif-
icantly underserved by colleges’ prevention strategies for
sexual and relationship violence.
Campus efforts to combat such violence have demon-
strated mixed results regarding effectiveness, particularly
over time.21 Some prevention strategies have been viewed as
directly or indirectly victim blaming.22 Others have focused
too much on individuals or small groups (such as athletes or
fraternity members), or on criminal justice policies rather
than wider social change.23,24 Some campuses have therefore
begun to utilize a bystander approach to engage the broader
campus community—in particular, bystanders—in efforts to
reduce sexual and relationship violence on campus. In this
article we examine the extent to which the published by-
stander prevention strategies have addressed sexual and re-
lationship violence in the LGBT community, and we provide
suggestions to improve bystander-intervention frameworks.
SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE
EXPERIENCED BY LGBT COLLEGE STUDENTS
Campus Climate for Students Who Identify as LGBT
Sociocultural bias against LGBT people and cultures, known
as heterosexism and homophobia, can frame LGBT identi-
ties as sinful, sick, perverted, and a threat to the traditional
family.25−27 When heterosexuality is posited as the norm,
acts of violence at every level of society perpetrated against
LGBT people are justified or explained through reference to
such negative constructions.28 The climate of LGBT intol-
erance on college campuses has been well documented by
researchers.29−34 For LGBT students, college campuses are
often unwelcoming, unsafe environments, where they expe-
rience harassment,29−31 isolation, ostracization,32 and phys-
ical violence.33 LGBT students can face discrimination or
harassment from their peers (e.g., roommates, classmates,
teammates) and other members of the campus community,
including the professional staff (e.g., coaches, hall directors,
professors, administrators, campus safety, health services
staff). Harassment and discrimination from campus com-
munity members can not only make LGBT students feel
unwelcome but also reduce their self-esteem and prevent
them from seeking help from campus professionals.32,33,35
Perpetrator Tactics
Perpetrators most commonly use two tactics to control their
victims: threatening to or actually revealing the victim’s sex-
uality to others (“outing” the victim), or isolating the victim.
For many lesbians who report abuse, the abuse occurs dur-
ing their first relationship, when they are most vulnerable to
batterers who have the capacity to control or manipulate
information.36 For students the impact of outing may be dev-
astating and may occur on several different levels. Many stu-
dents experiment with their sexuality or come out in college.
They may not tell their parents for any number of reasons,
including being cut off from financial and emotional support,
losing parental housing during school breaks, or losing jobs
that might be their only form of support.37−40 The college
environment can offer students the opportunity to explore
same-sex relationships,36 but many such first relationships
are at high risk for relationship violence because the victims
want both to confirm their sense of self and sexual identity,
and to “fit in.” The victims may also lack confidence in what
behaviors are acceptable in intimate same-sex relationships
and may have no or minimal contact with LGBT friend-
ship/community networks, within which they could air their
concerns. The desire to fit in leaves victims vulnerable be-
cause they may take risks, explain away the violence, or lack
the vocabulary or life skills to identify, name, and act on the
violence. For those who might possess such skills and sup-
port and who might even be out, the embarrassment behind
feeling that they somehow are not expressing their sexual or
gender identities “correctly” contributes to their silence
around relationship violence and sexual assaults. These fac-
tors and the perpetrators’ awareness of them increase the
likelihood of relationship violence.41
Isolation—one of the most effective and common tactics
that batterers resort to in heterosexual relationships42—is
often easy to use against LGBT victims because they may
not be open about their sexuality and are therefore socially
isolated. Alternatively, they may have come out but been
rejected by their social groups or families. A variation on this
theme is for batterers to limit the circle of people who are
allowed to know about the relationship, as the batterer
claims that they are not “out.”41
Harv Rev Psychiatry
Volume 20, Number 4
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT
Community 203
Barriers to Seeking Help and Reporting
The consequences and implications of sexual and relation-
ship violence in LGBT communities are both similar to, and
different from, those of the heterosexual community. Both
heterosexual and LGBT victims of sexual and relationship
violence who seek help must disclose the crime. Students
who identify as LGBT, however, may feel uncomfortable do-
ing so; the disclosure of the crime may also involve the dis-
closure of their sexual identities or gender orientations. For
students who are not out, their perceived need to hide their
sexual identities can be a barrier to seeking services. Peo-
ple who are just coming out or who are not accustomed to
talking about their sexual orientations may find the lan-
guage required to discuss their experiences inaccessible.43
Indeed, the language of sexual and relationship violence
tends to be extraordinarily gendered; female victims are
“attacked” or “abused” by male perpetrators, but fewer ex-
pressions are available to describe same-sex assaults. This
linguistic shortfall reflects common thinking; for instance,
some people cannot fathom or may explicitly deny that a
man can sexually assault another man. Even those who are
accustomed to speaking about same-sex situations on a va-
riety of levels may find it difficult to cross the linguistic gap
after a traumatic event. It can be all the more difficult for
someone who has not yet developed the confidence, let alone
the language related to his or her LGBT identity, to make a
report.
Male victims of sexual and relationship violence are often
blamed for not stopping an attack—which makes it difficult
for young men to speak of sexual or relationship violence
perpetrated by other men. Reporting requires them not only
to reveal very personal information but to frame acts of vio-
lence in a manner that makes sense in heterosexist culture,
in which victims are presumed to be heterosexual women.
Indeed, the current reauthorization of the Violence Against
Women Act faces opposition—for the first time—in large
part due to the addition of provisions for services address-
ing the LGBT communities. A gay man just coming to terms
with his sexuality may find it overwhelming to negotiate a
linguistic terrain that clearly marks him as gay and as a
victim of sexual assault.
LGBT individuals who identify as members of a racial or
ethnic minority can face additional burdens in facing stereo-
types of LGBT people as white and from Western cultures
when seeking help.44,45 Similarly, they face accusations of
being traitors to their racial or cultural identities for taking
on the LGBT identity.
Although all victims may face retribution from their
friends for seeking help or reporting the crime, on many
campuses the LGBT communities are small and insulated,
and the fear of retribution can be compounded. The actual
or perceived homophobia on college campuses may
prevent LGBT undergraduate victims of sexual and
relationship violence from seeking help.
As with other marginalized groups, LGBT victims may
choose not to report violence, thereby avoiding
further stigmatization of the LGBT community.46,47
Victims may be concerned that the identification and
labeling of same-sex perpetrators will further compromise
the perception of relationships that already involve
negative stereotyping.48 As members of marginalized
communities, LGBT individuals—for fear of
discrimination or harassment—often have serious
reservations about accessing authority figures or disclosing
their sexuality.49 Many LGBT youth attempt to access
services or safety nets such as religious figures, school
counselors, or the police, only to be told that their sexuality
—not the actual perpetration of the violence—is the issue.
For some victims the inability to obtain support from the
system only underscores their isolation and vulnerability.47
Existing Treatments
Services for LGBT and heterosexual victims of relationship
and sexual violence commonly take the form of one-on-
one counseling, advocacy efforts, and education. On college
cam- puses, collaborations with medical facilities,
law enforcement, and campus judicial boards may be
part of these services. In the United States, services for
victims of sexual and relationship violence are rarely fully
inclusive, and the funding of services to meet the needs
of LGBT victims, in particular, is typically
inadequate.47 At the present time lesbian victims of
relationship violence have fewer options than
heterosexual victims for accessing safe and effective
services.49 Other studies have shown that some crisis staff
view same-sex relationship violence as less harmful
than heterosexual violence—a result of the
stereotypes that men can defend themselves and women
are not violent.50 Because LGBT victims are often unwilling
to report violence, statistical data reflect lower rates
of incidence than would otherwise be the case, leading
to minimal funding for direct services, advocacy, and
prevention for LGBT communities. To be most responsive
and effective, direct services, advocacy, and prevention
efforts must proactively respond to the range of
vulnerabilities that offenders exploit.51,52
Harv Rev Psychiatry
July/August 2012
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT
Community 204
USING BYSTANDER PROGRAMS TO ADDRESS
SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE IN THE
LGBT COMMUNITY
In the social psychology literature, bystanders are defined
as individuals who witness criminal acts, emergency
situations, or instances where community norms are
violated.53−55 A bystander’s action or lack of action can
worsen, maintain, or improve the situation. Using research
on the effectiveness of prevention efforts, scholars and
practitioners have tried to engage bystanders as allies in
pre- venting sexual and relationship violence. Further
impetus for engaging bystanders comes from
research on perpetrator characteristics (e.g., hostility
toward women, victimization experiences)56 and situational
factors, such as community norms or community tolerance
toward sexual and relationship violence that can facilitate
or inhibit perpetrator behaviors;57−60 indeed, violence
occurs when there are motivated perpetrators, vulnerable
potential victims, and the absence of community
members who can or will intervene.61 Attention to both
perpetrator behavior and community responsibility can
facilitate effective community-level prevention efforts.
Bystander models focus on teaching bystanders active,
helpful behaviors to safely intervene in situations that
involve sexual and relationship violence.62
Although the use of bystander prevention strategies on
college campuses is growing, the majority of bystander
programs have not been formally evaluated, largely as a
result of limited funding and administrative time.63,64 Five
bystander programs for preventing sexual violence, as well
as one social-marketing campaign, have had evaluations
published in the peer-reviewed literature.65 The programs,
each of which utilizes a different approach, are currently in
place on college campuses in the United States and Canada.
The Mentors in Violence Prevention (MVP) model developed
by Katz in 1993 is one of the first violence prevention
programs using a bystander framework.66 The MVP pro-
gram, in addition to being used on college campuses, is used
with sports teams and with members of the U.S. military.
The program utilizes sports metaphors (e.g., a playbook) to
engage men in preventing violence against women.66 Since
its inception, the program has looked at the harassment of
gays and lesbians, and also at other heterosexist behaviors.
The occurrence of domestic violence and sexual assault in
same-sex couples is addressed, but the scenarios in the
play- book do not yet deal with bystander intervention
when the abuse is within LGBT relationships themselves
—but only when heterosexuals are abusing, harassing, or
talking in degrading ways about LGBT people (Jackson
Katz, personal communication).
The InterACT Sexual Assault Prevention Program is an
interactive skill-building program that seeks to increase
participants’ knowledge on the importance and effec-
tiveness of bystander interventions in preventing sexual
assault.67 While relationship and sexual violence within the
LGBT community is not a main focus, this phenomenon is
addressed during the program’s facilitated-discussion
component. InterACT has a separate program that is used to
address homophobia; its main focus is violence and bullying
aimed at LGBT people by heterosexuals, rather than
violence within the LGBT community (Marc Rich and
Courtney Ahrens, personal communication).
The Men’s Project68 incorporates discussion of bystander
techniques and utilizes a social-norms model of change,69,70
where peers’ perceptions of their peers’ attitudes and actions
are used to influence behavioral change. At the present time
this program does not address relationship and sexual vio-
lence in the LGBT community (Christine Gidycz, personal
communication).
The Men’s Program,71 a bystander program that utilizes
a film discussing a male-on-male rape as its focal point, does
not address sexual and relationship violence in same-sex
relationships (John Foubert, personal communication).
The Bringing in the BystanderTM in-person prevention
program trains participants to safely intervene when sexual
assault or relationship abuse is about to occur, is occurring,
or has occurred.62 The facilitator guide for this program in-
structs peer facilitators to explain that perpetrators and vic-
tims of sexual and relationship violence are not restricted to
particular sexual identities, relationships, or socioeconomic
backgrounds. “Its perpetrators and victims may be women or
men, young or old, gay, lesbian, straight or bisexual. Re-
lationship abuse affects people of all races, socioeconomic
backgrounds, and educational levels.”72
Most of the educational strategies for preventing sexual
and relationship violence are in the form of in-person pre-
vention programs, and the Know Your PowerTM Bystander
Social Marketing Campaign (developed in 2004) utilizes the
main tenets of the Bringing in the Bystander in-person
prevention program, in particular. The model of a social-
marketing campaign engages bystander behaviors when
sexual assault, relationship violence, or stalking is occur-
ring, is about to occur, and has occurred. The campaign, ad-
ministered campus-wide for a six-week period, utilizes 11' x
17' posters, bookmarks, table tents, full-side bus wraps,
computer pop-up screens, and products distributed with the
campaign logo.73 Studies have shown that participants who
have been exposed to, and who identified with, the
campaign images (compared to participants who report not
identifying with the images) were more likely both to believe
that they had a role to play in reducing sexual and
relationship violence and to have engaged in bystander
behaviors.73−75
During the running of a campaign, the images that ad-
dress relationship and sexual violence in the heterosexual
community (16 images) and the LGBT community (4 images)
are displayed together; the goal is to recognize that sexual
and relationship violence is not limited to heterosexual
relationships and to engage all community members to
Harv Rev Psychiatry
Volume 20, Number 4
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT
Community 205
acknowledge and safely intervene in these situations. The
first LGBT image, developed in 2006, has two scenes. In the
first scene two friends realize that another friend is lying
about the source of the bruises on his arms. The friends
realize that the bruising is not the result of a skateboard-
ing accident but has been inflicted by the victim’s abusive
boyfriend. In the next scene one friend offers to take the
victim to the campus rape crisis center. In the second LGBT
image, developed in 2009, there are three scenes that il-
lustrate bystanders supporting their friends after the oc-
currence of a sexual assault. The first two scenes highlight
female victims. In the third scene a male victim discloses
that he has been sexually assaulted. His male friend tells
him that he believes him. In the third LGBT image, devel-
oped in 2011, a college party scene is taking place; a female
is being emotionally abusive to her girlfriend. In the image
the bystanders identify and label the abuse and devise a
strategy to safely intervene to help their friend. In the
fourth LGBT image, also developed in 2011, two friends are
sitting in a local pizza shop. One of them, a potential per-
petrator, describes how he has met a man online and that
he plans on “hooking up” with this man, regardless of what
the man wants. The friend labels his friend’s plan as the
perpetration of a sexual assault and tries to convince him to
change his mind.
The nascent field of engaging community members as
active bystanders to recognize and safely intervene when
they anticipate or witness sexual and relationship violence
provides an opportunity to prevent violence not only in the
majority campus community but also in marginalized cam-
pus communities (e.g., LGBTs and the disabled). Programs
that engage community members as both bystanders and
victims work to break down the isolation that threatens
potential victims.76 Anti-LGBT stigma may prevent non-
LGBT students from intervening, because of either fears of
association or an inability to identify violence in a situation
involving LGBT students. As college and university officials
continue to develop strategies to address relationship and
sexual violence in the heterosexual and non-heterosexual
communities, care must be taken that all members of the
larger community feel comfortable providing and seeking
help.
PROPOSED DIRECTIONS FOR THE FIELD, AND
CONCLUSION
Any bystander, whether LGBT or heterosexual, who en-
counters an instance of sexual or relationship violence in
the LGBT community should be able to recognize the vi-
olence and to intervene in situations where violence is
occurring—regardless of the sexual orientation of the vic-
tims and perpetrators. Bystanders who intervene must also
be taught, however, how to explain their empathic behavior
to their peers and family members (by suggesting, for exam-
ple, that LGBT relationships can be healthy and should be
respected), who may otherwise ridicule their choice to inter-
vene and may even see their advocacy as going against social
norms and supporting such relationships.77,78 Intervening
bystanders may be required to cope with guilt by association,
including potential violence directed toward them. In fact,
because of the potential for violence, agencies such as the
New York City Gay and Lesbian Anti-violence Project send
outreach workers into the field in pairs (or more) as a safety
measure.
As college administrators implement prevention strate-
gies to reduce sexual and relationship violence in the LGBT
community, it must be remembered that these two types of
violence are not the same as hate violence. That said, within
marginalized communities, all three types of violence can be
intertwined; research indicates the prevalence of hate
crime–related sexual assault among gay or bisexual men
ranges from 3.0% to 19.8% of all such assaults and among
lesbian or bisexual women, from 1% to 12.3%.79 Sexual and
relationship violence and hate violence require unique
approaches, each with its own appropriate messaging and
effective interventions. While the strategies addressing the
different forms of violence cannot be interchanged, neither
should they be developed in isolation. Only if campus
administrators address these issues will all students be free
to pursue their optimal intellectual and emotional growth
during their college or university years.
Although some of the bystander prevention strategies ad-
dress the discrimination and bullying that is faced by LGBT
students,66,67 most do not. There are a few exceptions.62,73
These findings are problematic considering the prevalence of
sexual and relationship violence in the LGBT undergraduate
community and the unique barriers that LGBT students face
when seeking help. The use of strategies to engage members
of the broader community in preventing sexual and
relationship violence within and against the LGBT com-
munity needs to be increased. Furthermore, when victims
who identify as LGBT seek help, they often find counselors
that are ill equipped to offer support;14,48,80 counselors must
be trained to provide professional and culturally competent
services to LGBT victims. Finally, since the LGBT
community is heterogeneous, efforts need to be made to
understand why some groups access support and others do
not.81
Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of
interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content
and writing of the article.
REFERENCES
1. Fisher BS, Cullen FT, Turner MG. The sexual victimization
of
college women. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice
and Bureau of Justice, 2000 [Report No. NIJ 182369].
Harv Rev Psychiatry
July/August 2012 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence
in the LGBT Community 206
2. Koss M, Harvey M. The rape victim: clinical and community.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1991.
3. Sampson R. Acquaintance rape of college students.
Washington,
DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Community Oriented
Policing Services, 2002.
4. Karjane HM, Fisher BS, Cullen FT. Sexual assault on
campus:
what colleges and universities are doing about it. Washing-
ton, DC: National Institute of Justice, 2005 [Report No. NCJ
205521].
5. Banyard VL, Ward S, Cohn ES, Moorhead C, Walsh W. Un-
wanted sexual contact on campus: a comparison of women’s
and men’s experiences. Violence Vict 2007;22:52–70.
6. Isley P. Sexual assault of men: college-age victims. NASPA J
1998;35:305–17.
7. Koziol-McLain J, Campbell JC. Universal screening and
mandatory reporting: an update on two important issues for
victims/survivors of intimate partner violence. J Emerg Nurs
2001;27:602–6.
8. University of North Carolina, Greensboro. Safe
Zone/Violence:
legal terms. http://cap.uncg.edu/safezone/topics/violence/#legal
9. Black MC, Basile KC, Breiding MJ, et al. The National Inti-
mate Partner and Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2010 sum-
mary report. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Injury Preven-
tion and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
2011.
10. Acierno R, Brady K, Gray M, Kilpatrick D, Resnick H, Best
CL.
Psychopathology following interpersonal violence: a compari-
son of risk factors in older and younger adults. J Clin Geropsy-
chol 2002;8:13–23.
11. Arata CM, Burkhart BR. Post-traumatic stress disorder
among
college student victims of acquaintance assault. J Psychol Hu-
man Sex 1996;8:79–92.
12. Brener ND, McMahon PM, Warren CW, Douglas KA.
Forced
sexual intercourse and associated health-risk behaviors among
female college students in the United States. J Consult Clin
Psychol 1999;67:252–9.
13. Larimer ME, Lydum AR, Anderson BK, Turner AP. Male
and
female recipients of unwanted sexual contact in a college stu-
dent sample: prevalence rates, alcohol use, and depression
symptoms. Sex Roles 1999;40:295–308.
14. Duke A, Davidson M. Same-sex intimate partner violence:
les-
bian, gay, and bisexual affirmative outreach and advocacy. J
Aggression Maltreat Trauma 2009;18:795–816.
15. Rothman E, Silverman J. The effect of a college sexual
assault
prevention program on first-year students’ victimization rates.
J Am Coll Health 2007;55:283–90.
16. Duncan DF. Prevalence of sexual assault victimization
among
heterosexual and gay lesbian university-students. Psychol Rep
1990;66:65–6.
17. Rhodes SD, McCoy TP, Wilkin AM, Wolfson M. Behavioral
risk
disparities in a random sample of self-identifying gay and non-
gay male university students. J Homosex 2009;56:1083–100.
18. Bowman R, Mogan H. Comparison of rates of verbal and
phys-
ical abuse on campus by gender and sexual orientation. Coll
Stud J 1998;32:43–52.
19. Gonyea RM, Moore JV. Gay, lesbian, bisexual, and
transgen-
der students and their engagement in educationally purpose-
ful activities in college. 2007.
http://cpr.iub.edu/uploads/GLBT%
20and%20Engagement-%20ASHE%202007.pdf
20. Weiss S. About 12 percent of students identify as gay or
bisexual. Brown Daily Herald 2010;12 Nov. http://www.
browndailyherald.com/about-12-percent-of-students-identify-
as-gay-or-bisexual-1.2403002
21. Anderson L, Whiston S. Sexual education programs: a meta-
analytic examination of their effectiveness. Psychol Women Q
2005;29:374–88.
22. Schewe PA, O’Donohue W. Rape prevention:
methodologi- cal
problems and new directions. Clin Psychol Rev 1993;13: 667–
82.
23. Potter RH, Krider JE, McMahon PM. Examining elements
of
campus sexual violence policies. Violence Against Women
2000;6:1345–62.
24. Swift CF, Ryan-Finn K. Perpetrator prevention: stopping the
development of sexually abusive behavior. In: Swift CF, ed.
Sexual assault and abuse: sociocultural context of prevention.
New York: Haworth, 1995.
25. Jackson S, Scott S. Sexual antinomies in late modernity.
Sexu-
alities 2004;7:233–48.
26. Seidman S. From polluted homosexual to the normal gay:
changing patterns of sexual regulation in America. In: Ingra-
ham C, ed. Thinking straight: new work in critical heterosexu-
ality studies. New York: Routledge, 2005.
27. Bryant K. In defense of gay ghildren? “Progay” homo-
phobia
and the production of homonormativity. Sexualities
2008;11:455–75.
28. Herek GM. Hate crimes and stigma-related experiences
among
sexual minority adults in the United States: prevalence esti-
mates from a national probability sample. J Interpers Violence
2009;24:54–74.
29. Brown R, Clarke B, Gortmaker V, Robinson-Keilig R.
Assessing
the campus climate for gay, lesbian, bisexual and transgender
(LGBT) students using a multiple perspectives approach. J Coll
Stud Dev 2004;45:8–26.
30. D’Augelli AR. Lesbians’ and gay men’s experiences of
discrimi-
nation and harassment in a university community. Am J Com-
munity Psychol 1989;17:317–21.
31. Rankin S, Weber G, Blumenfeld W, Frazer S. State of
higher
education for lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender people.
Charlotte, NC: Campus Pride, 2010.
32. Hamilton L. Trading on heterosexuality: college women’s
gen-
der strategies and homophobia. Gend Soc 2007;21:145–72.
33. Rankin, SR. Campus climate for gay, lesbian, bisexual, and
transgender people: a national perspective. New York: National
Gay and Lesbian Task Force Policy Institute, 2003.
34. Waldo CR. Out on campus: sexual orientation and academic
climate in a university context. Am J Community Psychol
1998;26:745–74.
35. Boysen GA, Vogel DL, Cope MA, Hubbard A. Incidents of
bias in
college classrooms: instructor and student perceptions. J Divers
Higher Educ 2009;4:219–34.
36. Ristock J. No more secrets: violence in lesbian
relationships.
London: Routledge, 2002.
37. Turell S, Herman M. “Family” support for family vio-
lence:
exploring community support systems for lesbian and
http://cap.uncg.edu/safezone/topics/violence/#legal
http://cpr.iub.edu/uploads/GLBT%25
http://www/
Harv Rev Psychiatry
Volume 20, Number 4
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT
Community 207
bisexual women who have experienced abuse. J Lesbian Stud
2008;12:211–24.
38. Floyd FJ, Stein TS. Sexual orientation identity formation
among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: multiple patterns of
milestone experiences. J Res Adolesc 2002;12:167–91.
39. Burke TW, Owen SS. Same-sex domestic violence: is
anyone
listening? Gay Lesbian Rev Worldwide 2006;13:6–7.
40. Chutter K. Opening our awareness to heterosexist and
homo-
phobic attitudes in society. Relational Child Youth Care Pract
2007;20:22–7.
41. Donovan C, Hester M, Holmes J, McCarry M. Comparing
domestic abuse in same sex and heterosexual relationships
[Initial report from study funded by the Economic and So- cial
Research Council, award no. RES-000-23-0650]. 2006.
http://www.broken-rainbow.org.uk/cohsar report.pdf
42. Stark, E. Coercive control: the entrapment of women in per-
sonal life. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007.
43. Kulick D. Gay and lesbian language. Annu Rev Anthropol
2000;29:243–85.
44. Barrett DC, Pollack LM. Whose gay community? Social
class,
sexual self-expression, and gay community involvement. Sociol
Q 2005;46:437–56.
45. Crawford I, Allison KW, Zamboni BD, Soto T. The
influence of
dual-identity development on the psychosocial functioning of
African-American gay and bisexual men. J Sex Res 2002;
39:179–89.
46. McClennen J, Summers A, Vaughan C. Gay men’s domestic
violence: dynamics, help-seeking behaviors and correlates. J
Gay Lesbian Soc Serv 2002;14:23–49.
47. National Coalition of Anti-Violence Programs. Hate
violence
against lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender people in the
United States: a report by the National Coalition of Anti-
Violence Programs. New York: NCAVP, 2009.
48. Bornstein D, Fawcett J, Sullivan M, Senturia K, Shiu-
Thornton
S. Understanding the experiences of lesbian, bisexual and trans
survivors of domestic violence: a qualitative study. J Homosex
2006;51:159–81.
49. Irwin J. (Dis)counted stories: domestic violence and
lesbians.
Qual Soc Work 2008;7:199–215.
50. Brown M, Groscup J. Perceptions of same-sex domestic
violence
among crisis center staff. J Fam Violence 2009;24:87–93.
51. Healy SA. Advocating for health and human services: the
New
York experience. J Gay Lesbian Psychother 2004;8:43–66.
52. Frazer S. LGBT health and human services needs in New
York
State. New York, New York: Empire State Pride Agenda Foun-
dation and New York State Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Trans-
gender Health and Human Services Network, 2009.
53. Darley JM, Latane B. Bystander intervention in
emergencies:
diffusion of responsibility. J Pers Soc Psychol 1968;8:377–83.
54. Latane B, Darley JM. Help in a crisis: bystander response to
an emergency. Morristown, NJ: General Learning, 1976.
55. Moriarity T. Crime, commitment, and the responsive
bystander:
two field experiments. J Pers Soc Psychol 1975;31:370–6.
56. Lisak D, Miller PM. Repeat rape and multiple offending
among
undetected rapists. Violence Vict 2002;17:73–84.
57. Schwartz MD, DeKeseredy W. Sexual assault on the college
campus: the role of male peer support. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage, 1997.
58. Berkowitz AD. Fostering men’s responsibility for
preventing
sexual assault. In: Schewe PA, ed. Preventing violence in rela-
tionships: interventions across the lifespan. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association, 2002.
59. Schwartz MD, DeKeseredy WS. Aggregation bias and
woman
abuse. J Interpers Violence 2000;15:555–65.
60. DeKeseredy WS, Schwartz MD, Alvi S. The role of pro-
feminist
men in dealing with woman abuse on the Cana- dian college
campus. Violence Against Women 2000;6: 918–35.
61. Schwartz MD, DeKeseredy WS, Tait D, Alvi S. Male peer
support and a feminist routine activities theory: understand-
ing sexual assault on the college campus. Justice Q 2001;18:
623–49.
62. Banyard V, Moynihan M, Plante E. Sexual violence
prevention
through bystander education: an experimental evaluation. Am
J Community Psychol 2007;35:463–81.
63. Potter S, Stapleton J. Bringing in the target audience
in
bystander social marketing materials for communi- ties:
suggestions for practitioners. Violence Against Women
2011;17:797–812.
64. Potter S, Banyard V. Guest editors’ introduction. Violence
Against Women 2011;17:679–83.
65. Potter SJ, Banyard VL, eds. Special issue: engaging com-
munities to end sexual violence: current research on by-
stander focused prevention. Violence Against Women 2011;
17(6).
66. Katz J, Heisterkamp H, Fleming W. The social justice roots
of
the Mentors in Violence Prevention Model and its ap- plication
in a high school setting. Violence Against Women 2011;17:684–
702.
67. Ahrens C, Rich M, Ullman J. Rehearsing for real life:
the
impact of the InterACT sexual assault prevention program on
self-reported likelihood of engaging in by- stander
interventions. Violence Against Women 2011;17:760– 76.
68. Gidycz C, Orchowski L, Berkowitz A. Preventing sexual ag-
gression among college men: an evaluation of a social norms
and bystander intervention program. Violence Against Women
2011;17:720–42.
69. Berkowitz, A. An overview of the social norms approach.
In:
Lederman L, Stewart L, eds. Changing the culture of college
drinking. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton, 2005.
70. Fabiano PM, Perkins HW, Berkowitz A, Linkenbach J, Stark
C.
Engaging men as social justice allies in ending violence against
women: evidence for a social norms approach. J Am Coll Health
2003;52:105–12.
71. Foubert J. In practice—anti-rape on the road: men hit
the
highways to share a powerful message. About Campus
2005;10:24–7.
72. Moynihan MM, Banyard VL, Plante EG, Eckstein RP.
Bringing
in the Bystander:TM establishing a community of responsibility.
Facilitator’s guide. Durham: University New Hampshire, 2010.
73. Potter SJ. Using a multi-media social marketing campaign to
increase active bystanders on the college campus. J Am College
Health 2012;60;282–95.
74. Potter SJ, Moynihan MM, Stapleton JG. Using social self-
identification in social marketing materials aimed at
http://www.broken-rainbow.org.uk/cohsar
Harv Rev Psychiatry
July/August 2012
Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT
Community 208
reducing violence against women on campus. J Interpers
Violence 2011;26:971–90.
75. Potter SJ, Stapleton JG. Translating sexual assault and
stalk-
ing prevention from a college campus to a US military post:
piloting the Know Your Power Social Marketing Campaign. J
Interpers Violence 2012;27:1593–1621.
76. Horn SS, Romeo KE. Peer contexts for lesbian, gay,
bisexual,
and transgender students: reducing stigma, prejudice, and dis-
crimination. Prev Res 2010;17:7–10.
77. Herek GM. Heterosexuals’ attitudes toward lesbians and gay
men: correlates and gender differences. J Sex Res 1988;25:
451–77.
78. Schulte L. Similarities and differences in homophobia
among
African Americans and Caucasians. Race Gend Class
2002;9:71–93.
79. Rothman EF, Exner D, Baughman AL. The prevalence of
sexual
assault against people who identify as gay, lesbian, or bisexual
in the United States: a systematic review. Trauma Violence
Abuse 2011;12:55–66.
80. Renzetti C. Violent betrayal: partner abuse in lesbian
relation-
ships. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1992.
81. Turell S, Cornell-Swanson L. Not all alike: within-group
differ-
ences in seeking help for same-sex relationship abuses. J Gay
Lesbian Soc Serv 2005;18:71–88.
Copyright of Harvard Review of Psychiatry is the property of
Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE EXPERIENCED
BY LGBT COLLEGE STUDENTSCampus Climate for Students
Who Identify as LGBTPerpetrator TacticsBarriers to Seeking
Help and ReportingExisting TreatmentsUSING BYSTANDER
PROGRAMS TO ADDRESS SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP
VIOLENCE IN THE LGBT COMMUNITYPROPOSED
DIRECTIONS FOR THE FIELD, AND
CONCLUSIONREFERENCES
Received 06/19/12
Revised 12/18/12
Accepted 01/09/13
DOI: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2014.00154.x
Research
Perceived Racism as Moderator
Between Self-Esteem/Shyness
and Psychological Distress
Among African Americans
Ruth Chu-Lien Chao, Joseph Longo, Canzi Wang,
Deepta Dasgupta, and Jessica Fear
In the spirit of the mission of social justice in counseling, this
study examined perceived racism as a moderator on the
association between self-concept (i.e., self-esteem, shyness) and
distress among African Americans. The authors
examined whether perceived racism changed the relationship
between self-esteem/shyness and psychological dis-
tress by investigating a sample of 394 African Americans. With
a moderation analysis, the authors found signifcant
interaction effects of Self-Esteem × Perceived Racism and
Shyness × Perceived Racism on distress.
Keywords: self-esteem, shyness, perceived racism,
psychological distress, African Americans
Given the importance placed on individualism in Western
culture, self-concept has been critical in understanding
people’s psychological issues from the Western perspective.
Moreover, it is hardly surprising that self-concept has been
described as one of the most pivotal personality attributes
related to psychological distress (Marsh, Trautwein, Lüdtke,
Köller, & Baumert, 2006). Self-concept has two indexes, self-
esteem and shyness, which indicate how much people believe
in themselves in terms of their sense of personal value and
comfortableness with others (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Specif-
cally, individuals with higher self-esteem tend to have better
mental health and are more resilient in the face of adversity
compared with those with low self-esteem (Marcussen, 2006).
Shyness, the other component of self-concept, is defned as
“one’s reaction to being with strangers or casual acquain-
tances: tension . . . feelings of awkwardness and discomfort”
(Cheek & Buss, 1981, p. 330). Shyness is associated with high
negative emotionality, negative affectivity, personal distress,
low positive affect, and low constructive coping (Eisenberg,
Fabes, & Murphy, 1995).
Many scholars (e.g., Cowden, 2005; Harris, 2010; Mar-
cussen, 2006) have found that self-concept can positively or
negatively relate to psychological distress, which refers to
the current or past level of overall experiences of symptoms,
intensity of symptoms, and number of reported symptoms
(Derogatis, 1993). With regard to the association between
self-concept and psychological distress, there are two types
of studies: one investigates the relationship between self-
esteem and psychological distress (e.g., Abe, 2004; Cassidy,
O’Connor, Howe, & Warden, 2004), and the other explores
the relationship between shyness and mental health (e.g.,
Cowden, 2005).
According to Ratts (2011), one important component in
the social justice mission in counseling is to alter or recognize
oppressive environmental conditions such as racism. Unfor-
tunately, the context of the prior studies shows that a gap still
exists between the prior studies and the call from the social
justice mission. The prior research used a univariate perspec-
tive, which could only answer the direct association between
self-concept (e.g., self-esteem, shyness) and psychological
distress, but may not ft with the emerging commitment of
the social justice mission in counseling-related professions
(e.g., Goodman et al., 2004; Lum, 2003; Ratts, 2011; Ratts,
Toporek, & Lewis, 2010). Among the burgeoning topics
most directly relevant to social justice are correlates and
consequences of injustices, including racism and prejudice.
The harmful nature of racism has been a crucial experience
for African Americans. Perceived racism could be a proxy
for actual experiences of racism, and it refers to one’s ap-
praisals of the stressfulness of the racist events (Landrine
& Klonoff, 1996). Indeed, evidence has accumulated during
the past decade to suggest that reports of perceived racism
Ruth Chu-Lien Chao, Joseph Longo, Canzi Wang, Deepta
Dasgupta, and Jessica Fear, Department of Counseling Psychol-
ogy, University of Denver. Correspondence concerning this
article should be addressed to Ruth Chu-Lien Chao, Department
of
Counseling Psychology, Morgridge College of Education,
University of Denver, 1999 East Evans Avenue, #260, Denver,
CO 80208
(e-mail: [email protected]).
© 2014 by the American Counseling Association. All rights
reserved.
Journal of Counseling & Development ■ July 2014 ■ Volume 92
259
mailto:[email protected]
Chao, Longo, Wang, Dasgupta, & Fear
are related to mental health and symptoms of psychological
distress (e.g., Fischer & Shaw, 1999). There is evidence to
suggest that, in addition to being harmful, perceived racism
varies among African Americans (Clark, Anderson, Clark,
& Williams, 1999; Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). Levels of
perceived racism relate to how African Americans manage
the association between stress and mental health (Kaiser,
Major, & McCoy, 2004).
Mahalik, Pierre, and Wan (2006) suggested that self-con-
cept, including self-esteem and shyness, was commingled
with perceived racism to such a degree that it was diffcult
to determine the association between self-concept and psy-
chological distress without considering perceived racism.
For example, among African Americans, perceived racism
was associated with lower self-esteem and higher psycho-
logical distress, which subsequently may make it diffcult
to initiate a conversation with strangers (Clark et al., 1999;
Fischer & Shaw, 1999). African Americans with higher
perceived racism had lower use of counseling services and
greater diffculty in trusting culturally different counselors.
Applying the current literature of the relationships between
self-esteem/shyness and psychological distress to African
Americans without considering their perceived racism would
miss the mission of social justice in counseling (Fischer &
Shaw, 1999; Kaiser et al., 2004; Ratts, 2011). To fulfll the
mission of social justice, which recognizes and alters op-
pressive environmental conditions, we examined the role of
an oppressive environment condition (i.e., perceived racism)
to better understand the relationship between self-esteem/
shyness and psychological distress.
Self-Esteem, Perceived Racism, and
Psychological Distress
Self-esteem refers to an individual’s overall evaluation or
appraisal of his or her worth and encompasses both beliefs
about oneself and emotions. It refers to how people feel
about themselves, such as feeling competent and pos-
sessing a positive self-attitude (Marcussen, 2006). Theo-
retically, self-esteem infuences the way people are and
their sense of personal value. Self-esteem also affects the
way people feel and act in the world and how they relate
to others. Those with high self-esteem may believe that
they have good qualities, a perception that is critical in
maintaining low psychological distress. In recent years, a
growing body of research has supported the vulnerability
model, which states that low self-esteem operates as a
risk factor for psychological distress (Harris, 2010; Orth,
Robins, & Roberts, 2008). Individuals with low self-esteem
are assumed to have fewer coping resources and thus are
prone to distress, whereas those with high self-esteem
are assumed to have suffcient coping strategies, which
prevent them from spiraling downward into psychologi-
cal distress. Empirically, high self-esteem has been found
to be negatively associated with psychological distress,
but low self-esteem may signifcantly exacerbate people’s
psychological maladjustment (Orth et al., 2008).
Although the relationship between self-esteem and psy-
chological distress has been established, when applying this
relationship to African Americans, the precise mechanisms
of in what conditions and among whom self-esteem may
lead to distress have not yet been determined. In other words,
conceptualizing a direct association between self-esteem and
psychological distress for African Americans may lead to two
diffculties. First, Mahalik et al. (2006) found that African
Americans’ development of self-esteem closely relates to
their racial identity and discrimination. Szymanski and Gupta
(2009) found that among African Americans, perceived rac-
ism was negatively associated with self-esteem but positively
associated with distress. Our study attempted to further ad-
vance the literature on self-esteem, perceived racism, and
psychological distress by conceptualizing perceived racism
as a moderator that may change the mechanism between
self-esteem and distress.
Second, Klonoff, Landrine, and Ullman (1999) reported
that perceived racism is a ubiquitous part of many Afri-
can Americans’ experiences. More than 90% of African
Americans have experienced racist events in their lifetime
(Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). Indeed, racism is insidious in
society and permeates many aspects of African American
life. Jones (1997) suggested that the experience of racism is
multidimensional and can be classifed by using a tripartite
typology: individual racism, institutional racism, and cultural
racism. Because racism could be experienced by African
Americans on almost a daily basis, Landrine and Klonoff
(1996) suggested that the assessment of the degrees of racism
or appraisal of the racist experience may be more important
than measuring the types of racism. For example, two African
Americans may experience the same racist event, and yet
one may fnd it very stressful or be severely hurt whereas the
other dismisses it (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). In addition,
perceived racism can signifcantly affect African Americans’
mental health. The more racism that African Americans ex-
perience, the higher the levels of psychological distress they
report (Fischer & Shaw, 1999).
An understanding of the role of perceived racism would
seem to be of great importance to counselors when they serve
African Americans. In other words, while we continue to work
toward the social justice mission (i.e., a better understanding
of racism), it is inevitable to examine how perceived racism
may change the established association between self-esteem
and distress. Furthermore, we also work to identify under what
conditions (i.e., high vs. low perceived racism) is self-esteem
negatively associated with the distress of African Americans.
Understanding the role of perceived racism could allow coun-
selors to determine among whom (i.e., African Americans
Journal of Counseling & Development ■ July 2014 ■ Volume 92
260
Perceived Racism Among African Americans
who perceive high vs. low racism) psychological distress
can be minimized regardless of the levels of self-esteem. We
thus conceptualized that the association between self-esteem
and distress among African Americans may depend on the
extent of racism that the individual perceives. When African
Americans perceive high levels of racism, their self-esteem
may be vulnerable to psychological distress. Yet, when African
Americans perceive low levels of racism, they may be able
to maintain low psychological distress across different levels
of self-esteem. Therefore, our frst hypothesis was that per-
ceived racism moderates the relation between self-esteem and
psychological distress among African Americans. However,
when perceiving less racism, African Americans would be
able to maintain low psychological distress across different
levels of self-esteem. Taken together, although prior studies
have addressed perceived racism, almost none addressed it as
a moderator variable. Thus, we examined perceived racism as
a moderator, rather than simply repeating past studies on the
direct effects between self-esteem and psychological distress.
Shyness, Perceived Racism, and
Psychological Distress
According to Cheek and Buss (1981), shyness is a person-
ality characteristic or a component of self-concept and is
characterized by the discomfort and inhibition that may occur
in the presence of others. Conceptually, shy people tend to
be aware of themselves as social objects more than people
with low shyness. Given that shy people tend to feel tense
and awkward, they may have higher levels of psychological
distress (Eisenberg et al., 1995). Empirically, shyness is pre-
dictive of later psychological and social functioning (Beer,
2002). Shyness is positively associated with neuroticism
and negatively associated with extroversion, two personality
traits of the Big Five scale (Ryan & Xenos, 2011). Shyness
is often thought to be associated with anxiety or generalized
negative affect, and its relation to psychological distress has
been reasonably well documented. Research has also found
that shyness is associated with depression (Beer, 2002), low
self-esteem, and loneliness (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Thus,
based on the current literature, shyness could be a risk factor
for psychological distress.
Although shyness could be a vulnerability factor for psy-
chological distress, it is still unknown or questionable if this
association is valid among African Americans. For example,
African Americans may feel uncomfortable when encounter-
ing strangers because of shyness or perceived racism from an
unwelcoming stranger. Or perhaps the extent of the racism
that African Americans perceive exacerbates their shyness on
distressed feelings. To date, there has been no rigorous test
of the association between shyness and psychological distress
among African Americans. Joiner (1997) suggested that
the association between shyness and psychological distress
should be considered within the context of a targeted popula-
tion. Given the fact that many African Americans encounter
racist events (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996) and that the way in
which it is experienced is a unique stressor for this popula-
tion (Clark et al., 1999; Harrell, 2000), it seems appropriate
to include perceptions of racism in the association between
shyness and distress among African Americans. Thus, a goal
of our study was to explain, at least in part, when and how
shy African Americans become psychologically distressed,
by using interactive views between shyness and perceived
racism on symptoms of distress.
The reason that perceived racism could be a moderator
that changes the association between shyness and distress
could be due to a variety of issues (Eisenberg et al., 1995).
First, when African Americans perceive more racism, they
may be more vulnerable to shyness, which has been associ-
ated with distress. African Americans who perceive more
racism tend to have less resilience and fewer resources
to manage daily stress than those who perceive less rac-
ism (Beer, 2002). Second, although shyness is associated
with psychological distress, more important for African
Americans, it is suggested that higher level of perceived
racism may make shy African Americans more vulner-
able to distress. For African Americans, shyness involves
an internal experience of emotional isolation, whereas
perceived racism creates an even deeper sense of social
marginalization and relational disconnection. Moreover,
when African Americans are disturbed by racism, they
may become lonely and frustrated and may be unable
to manage their shyness, which in turn leads to greater
psychological distress (Cowden, 2005). That is, African
Americans’ perceived high levels of racism are predicted
to worsen the association between shyness and distress.
Finally, because shyness does not assume high levels of
perceived racism, some shy African Americans may still
perceive low levels of racism. Such low levels of perceived
racism may make African Americans more confdent and
relaxed when encountering strangers.
Thus, low perceived racism may buffer against shyness on
negative social and health consequences, and African Ameri-
cans reporting low perceived racism may be able to maintain
low psychological distress no matter how shy they are. The
present reconceptualization thus asserts that shyness will
buffer against psychological distress when African Ameri-
cans perceive low levels of racism-related stress (i.e., per-
ceived racism moderates the relation between shyness and
psychological distress among African Americans). In other
words, when feeling shy, African Americans who perceive
low levels of racism may have a buffer that differentially
affects the association between shyness and psychological
distress (Clark et al., 1999). Thus, our second hypothesis was
that perceived racism moderates the relationship between
shyness and psychological distress among African Ameri-
Journal of Counseling & Development ■ July 2014 ■ Volume 92
261
Chao, Longo, Wang, Dasgupta, & Fear
cans. The less racism perceived by African Americans, the
more buffered the relation is; however, those who perceive
high levels of racism would lack the buffer against shyness
on distress. Therefore, we examined perceived racism as a
moderator between shyness and psychological distress rather
than simply addressing the direct effects between shyness
and psychological distress.
The Present Study
In an effort to advance the social justice mission of counseling
and psychology, we expanded the current literature on the
associa-
tion between self-concept and psychological distress for African
Americans. Contextualizing this association based on African
Americans’ experiences with racism serves two purposes: First,
it addresses the emergent social justice mission imperative in
counseling; and second, it facilitates counselors’ cultural
sensitivity
when serving this population. Because perceived racism affects
the lives of many African Americans, we constructively tested
our
two hypotheses with a sample of nonclinical African Americans.
Our frst hypothesis aimed to determine whether perceived
racism
moderates the association between self-esteem and
psychological
distress. Our second hypothesis examined whether perceived
rac-
ism moderates the association between shyness and
psychological
distress. We hope that our fndings will assist African Americans
in dealing with the association between self-concept and
distress,
as well as help advance the current literature on self-esteem and
shyness in the face of racism. We placed the association
between
self-esteem/shyness and psychological distress in the mission of
social justice via conceptualizing perceived racism as a
moderator
to this association.
Method
Power Analysis
A power analysis was conducted by using G*Power 3.1.2
(Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996) to estimate the sample
size. A sample size of 386, 55, and 26 was needed for a small,
medium, and large effect size, respectively. In our study, a
sample size of 394 would detect a two-way interaction effect
with a small to medium effect size and a power of .80.
Participants
Our study participants were 394 African American students
from one historically Black university in the mid-Atlantic
region of the United States. Specifcally, there were 212 (54%)
women and 177 (45%) men; fve participants did not identify
their gender. Most participants were undergraduate students
(17% freshmen, 14% sophomores, 23% juniors, 26% seniors,
and 20% graduate school). The participants had declared ma-
jors in biology, engineering, education, or psychology. About
77% of participants were 25 years of age or younger. All par-
ticipants were informed that their participation was voluntary
and reassured that all information would be kept anonymous.
Instruments
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965).
The RSES is a 10-item scale that assesses an individual’s
self-esteem. It uses a 4-point Likert-type response, ranging
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree), with higher
scores indicating higher self-esteem. The RSES has a more
than adequate coeffcient alpha of .92 and a test–retest cor-
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx
Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx

150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx
150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx
150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docxherminaprocter
 
250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx
250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx
250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docxnovabroom
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017PubliAnastaciaShadelb
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017PubliKiyokoSlagleis
 
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publiUMAR48665
 
Farrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime drop
Farrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime dropFarrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime drop
Farrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime dropGraham Farrell
 
KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3Kresenda Keith
 
Literary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law Enforcement
Literary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law EnforcementLiterary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law Enforcement
Literary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law EnforcementStewart Fountain
 
Vanessa Schoening February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx
 Vanessa Schoening      February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx Vanessa Schoening      February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx
Vanessa Schoening February 13, 2014Module 2 .docxMARRY7
 
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on SocietyPolice Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on SocietyAutumn Moody
 
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docxModule 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docxbunnyfinney
 
Vanessa Schoening January 24, 2014Module 1.docx
Vanessa Schoening        January 24, 2014Module 1.docxVanessa Schoening        January 24, 2014Module 1.docx
Vanessa Schoening January 24, 2014Module 1.docxdickonsondorris
 
FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP
FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP
FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP Louise Grove
 
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docxI have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docxsamirapdcosden
 
CJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docx
CJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docxCJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docx
CJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docxclarebernice
 
Presented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docx
Presented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docxPresented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docx
Presented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docxharrisonhoward80223
 
A Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In America
A Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In AmericaA Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In America
A Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In AmericaLisa Muthukumar
 
Running Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE .docx
Running Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE                            .docxRunning Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE                            .docx
Running Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE .docxjeanettehully
 
Police Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson RichardPolice Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson RichardRichard Anderson
 

Similar to Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx (20)

150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx
150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx
150 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.  Issues a.docx
 
250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx
250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx
250 words agree or disagree to each questions Q 1.An int.docx
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
 
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publi
 
Farrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime drop
Farrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime dropFarrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime drop
Farrell 2015 Attempted crime and the crime drop
 
KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3
 
Literary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law Enforcement
Literary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law EnforcementLiterary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law Enforcement
Literary Analysis on Solving Problems in America's Law Enforcement
 
FINAL+DRAFT-2
FINAL+DRAFT-2FINAL+DRAFT-2
FINAL+DRAFT-2
 
Vanessa Schoening February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx
 Vanessa Schoening      February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx Vanessa Schoening      February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx
Vanessa Schoening February 13, 2014Module 2 .docx
 
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on SocietyPolice Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
 
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docxModule 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
 
Vanessa Schoening January 24, 2014Module 1.docx
Vanessa Schoening        January 24, 2014Module 1.docxVanessa Schoening        January 24, 2014Module 1.docx
Vanessa Schoening January 24, 2014Module 1.docx
 
FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP
FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP
FIVE TESTS FOR A THEORY OF THE CRIME DROP
 
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docxI have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
 
CJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docx
CJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docxCJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docx
CJ-4880 Police Brutality Increase in the unjustified .docx
 
Presented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docx
Presented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docxPresented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docx
Presented byJohn Lynch, Yamhill Co. Juv. Dept.SUPERVISION .docx
 
A Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In America
A Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In AmericaA Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In America
A Race And Power Perspective On Police Brutality In America
 
Running Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE .docx
Running Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE                            .docxRunning Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE                            .docx
Running Head ETHICAL ISSUES IN POLICE .docx
 
Police Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson RichardPolice Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson Richard
 

More from croysierkathey

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docxcroysierkathey
 

More from croysierkathey (20)

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxJisc
 
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdfFICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdfPondicherry University
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxmarlenawright1
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Pooja Bhuva
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.christianmathematics
 
dusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learning
dusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learningdusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learning
dusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learningMarc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...Poonam Aher Patil
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxannathomasp01
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSCeline George
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsMebane Rash
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxJisc
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxHow to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxCeline George
 
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptxREMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptxDr. Ravikiran H M Gowda
 
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Pooja Bhuva
 
Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111
Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111
Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111GangaMaiya1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
 
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdfFICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
Call Girls in Uttam Nagar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in  Uttam Nagar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7Call Girls in  Uttam Nagar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Uttam Nagar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 
dusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learning
dusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learningdusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learning
dusjagr & nano talk on open tools for agriculture research and learning
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
 
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxHow to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
 
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptxREMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
 
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
 
Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111
Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111
Details on CBSE Compartment Exam.pptx1111
 

Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26163–169 DOI 10.10.docx

  • 1. Journal of Family Violence (2011) 26:163–169 DOI 10.1007/s10896-010-9346-0 Predicting Officer Physical Assaults at Domestic Assault Calls Richard R. Johnson Published online: 18 January 2011 # Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2011 Abstract Police work is rife with the potential for physical harm, and domestic assault calls are one source of assaults on officers. Inability to accurately predict what circumstances might lead to an attack is one cause of officer cynicism and paranoia. Having the ability to identify which suspects pose the greatest threat of assault would allow officers to take appropriate protective measures. Using data collected from 1,951 domestic assault calls across three cities, the present study compared characteristics and behaviors of the batterers with whether or not the batterers physically assaulted the responding officers. Findings revealed five significant batterer characteristics (employment status, shared residence with abuse victim, alcohol consumption, property damage, and hostile demeanor toward officers) that successfully predicted officer assaults. These risk factors may be incorporated into police safety
  • 2. training in the response to family violence calls. Keywords Domestic violence. Officer safety. Officer assaults The potential for assaults on police officers handling domestic assault calls has been the subject of some debate over the last few decades. While some studies have found that domestic assault calls only contribute to a very small portion of the assaults that officers experience (Garner and Clemmer 1986; Hirschel et al. 1994; Konstantin 1984), studies in different communities have suggested that domestic assault calls are one of the three most common situations where officer assaults occur (Ellis et al. 1993; Stanford and Mowry 1990; Uchida et al. 1987). Even if other types of calls have higher rates of officer assault, this does not necessarily mean that domestic assault calls are “safe.” On the contrary, as FBI statistics indicate that from 1980 through 2003 more than 224,000 police officers in the U.S. were assaulted while handling family disturbance calls (Johnson 2007, 2008). Furthermore, while survival rate for officers assaulted in the U.S. has been steadily increasing over the last 30 years, the survival rate for officers assaulted at domestic violence calls has failed to increase for officers assaulted at domestic assault calls. It is the unpredictability and constant potential for danger in the form of a violent physical attack that distinguishes the work environment of the police officer. Inability to accurately predict what circumstances might lead
  • 3. to an attack is suggested to be a primary cause of police cynicism and officer mistrust of the public, causing officers to emphasize keeping their guard up at all times (Barker 1999; Skolnick 1966; Wilson 1968). This is impossible to achieve, however, and could lead to paranoia or citizen complaints for overly aggressive behavior. Therefore, increasing an officer’s ability to predict which domestic assault situations pose the highest potential for assault could be very helpful. Being able to identify for officers which circumstances pose threats of assault would allow officers to take protective measures. The present study sought to utilize characteristics of the domestic batterer and the situational circumstances of the incident to predict assaults on police officers attending domestic assault calls. This study extended the previous research on police officer assaults by moving beyond simply searching for correlates of assault and developing prediction odds ratios for assaults. A simple correlate usually refers to a factor that occurs at the same time as the outcome, while predictor variables predict an increased probability of the outcome (Loeber 1990). This study Page 164 sought to predict increased probabilities of assaults on officers. Furthermore, this research targeted assaults at domestic calls, which some previous research has suggested occur
  • 4. somewhat differently than assaults on officers performing other duties (Johnson 2007, 2008). It was hoped that key characteristics could be identified that would be useful in predicting the likelihood of an officer assault and permit the utilization of proactive safety tactics by officers. Literature Review While there has been a fair amount of research on the correlates of police use of force against citizens (see for example Alpert and Dunham 1999; Geller and Toch 1995; Terrill and Mastrofski 2002), there has only been limited research on the correlates of physical assaults against police officers. Meyer et al. (1979) reviewed the characteristics of assailants in 1,143 assaults on police officers from 37 municipal law enforcement agencies in five U.S. states. The majority of the assailants in these assaults were young (between ages 18 and 25), male, members of racial minority groups, unemployed, had been consuming alcohol, and had displayed an angry or belligerent demeanor prior to committing the assault. A number of these individuals also had personal problems such as a lack of a high school education, a criminal record, and family relationship difficulties (Meyer et al. 1979). More recently, Pinizzotto et al. (1998) reviewed the assailant characteristics in all law enforcement officer murders reported to the FBI from 1985 to 1994. Again they found that the majority of the assailants were young (between the ages of 15 and 29), male, members of racial
  • 5. minority groups, unemployed, low socioeconomic status, unmarried, under the influence of drugs or alcohol, and had a criminal record. Other Western nations have also conducted similar studies and identified similar assailant characteristics. Brown (1994), Moxey and McKenzie (1993), and Noaks and Christopher (1990) analyzed data from officer assaults in the United Kingdom. In all three of these English studies the assailants were found to be predominantly lower income males who were unemployed, had a criminal record, and were consuming alcohol at the time of the assault. Most were between the ages of 17 and 25, with an average age of 22 years old. In Australia, Mayhew (2001) analyzed official police reports and found similar characteristics were prevalent in those who assaulted officers in that nation. The Australian assailants were generally young males of lower socioeconomic status who were intoxicated, with aboriginals being over-represented among the assailants. In Iceland, Bragason (2007) conducted a survey of assaulted police officers who described the characteristics of their assailants. Again it was found that those who assaulted the officers appeared to be predominantly young indigenous males who were unemployed, had a criminal record, and had been consuming alcohol. While the previous research has done a fairly good job of describing the characteristics of the
  • 6. assailants who attack police officers in many nations, they all failed to make any attempt to predict officer assaults. While the person who is most likely to assault police officers is described as a lower class, adolescent male who has been consuming alcohol, it could be argued that this description fits a significant proportion of the citizens with whom the police routinely interact. Yet only small fractions of the intoxicated, lower class, adolescent males the police encounter actually assault them. So how do the assailants differ from the non-assailants that officers typically encounter? The previous literature has been silent on this question. Another weakness to the previous literature is an assumption that all assault incidents are similar. Assailant characteristics are pooled across all types of officer assault incidents. Recently there has been evidence to suggest that there may be differences in both offender characteristics, and how assaults occur, across different types of officer assault incidents (Johnson 2007). Previous studies failed to look specifically at physical assaults of officers handling domestic assault calls to determine what characteristics offenders in these specific situations have, and how these characteristics differ from batterers in domestic assault calls who did not assault the police. The present study attempted to fill this void in the literature. Method Participants
  • 7. Data used came from the pooling of datasets from three previous studies. A sample of domestic assault calls was needed that contained descriptive information about the batterer involved, and contained both cases that involved an assault on an officer and cases that did not. Data from three well known domestic violence arrest experiment studies were used, the first being the Minneapolis Domestic Violence Experiment (Sherman and Rogan 1984) which involved official police reports in 1,250 domestic assault cases handled by the Minneapolis Police Department. The second was a replication of the Minneapolis experiment that was conducted in Milwaukee, Wisconsin, involving 921 domestic assault cases handled by the Milwaukee Police Department (Sherman et al. 1991). The last study was a replication study in Miami, Florida that involved 907 Page 165 domestic assault cases handled by the Metro-Dade Police Department (Pate et al. 1991). 1 Even though these data were collected in the 1980s, they remain relevant today for several reasons. First, they continue to be used in research on family violence published in prestigious social science research journals (Garner and
  • 8. Maxwell 2000; Hickman 2003; Maxwell et al. 2002; Piquero et al. 2006). Second, since empirical evidence suggests that assaults on officers at domestic calls, and officer survival rates from these assaults, have remained stable for more than 30 years (Johnson 2007, 2008), there is little likelihood that any possible changes in police tactics since the 1980s has occurred with respect to handling domestic calls. Finally, the emphasis of this investigation rested on offender characteristics to predict offender behavior, relating to innate human behavioral traits that are unlikely to have evolved significantly over a few decades. Data collection method used in all three of these studies was identical. The data about the domestic assault incident came from official reports by police officers and from interviews with the domestic abuse victims conducted by the research team within a week after the incident. Information about the batterer’s behaviors before the arrival of the police was also obtained in the abuse victim interviews. Finally, data about the batterer’s prior criminal record was obtained from the state crime computer database (Pate et al. 1991; Sherman and Rogan 1984; Sherman et al. 1991). After merging the 3,078 cases from these three studies it was discovered that 98 cases (3.2%) involved a female batterer, none of whom had assaulted an officer. Because of this lack of variation in the dependent variable for female batterers in the sample, the decision was made to limit this analysis to only male batterers. In another
  • 9. 33.3% of the cases the batterer had fled the scene prior to the arrival of the police, thus eliminating the possibility of an assault on the officers. These 1,026 cases were therefore eliminated from the analysis, as were three additional cases that involved an assault on officers by the abuse victim rather than the batterer.2 The final dataset for the present study consisted of data on 1,951 incidents of domestic assault that were investigated by the police. The fact that so many cases were eliminated from the sample for this analysis could raise questions of validity, especially if the batterers excluded from the analysis differed significantly from those that remained. It order to determine the extent of this potential weakness, basic demographic characteristics available for the batterers who remained in the sample were compared with those who were excluded from the sample. These demographic characteristics are displayed in Table 1 and suggest that there were surprisingly few differences between the cases that were excluded and those that remained in the sample. In general, those batterers that were excluded from the analyses were slightly more likely to be male, were slightly younger, and less likely to be married. Procedure
  • 10. The dependent variable in this analysis was whether or not the official report indicated the suspect had physically assaulted the officers handling the domestic assault call. Because the data collected was focused primarily on the batterer and the abuse victim, details were not available on whether or not the officers sustained injuries, the seriousness of the injuries, whether or not the assailant used a weapon, or when during the encounter this attack took place. While having this information would have permitted a richer analysis, the emphasis of this study, however was the prediction of assaults, no matter how minor. Just as with victims of abuse, assaults of any kind are a crime and place police officers at risk of injury and increased stress. Data were available on 12 descriptive characteristics about the batterers in the domestic assaults that served as the independent variables in this analysis. These variables and their descriptive statistics are displayed in Table 2. The first independent variable considered was the batterer’s age, and it was hypothesized that younger batterers would be more likely to assault officers than older batterers. Second, whether the batterer was married to the abuse victim was Table 1 Comparison between sample and excluded cases Sample Cases Excluded Cases N 1,951 1,127 Percent male 100% 91% Mean age 33.4 30.4 Percent married 51% 47% Percent consuming alcohol 51% 49%
  • 11. Percent using drugs 22% 23% Percent damaged property 18% 16% Percent struck abuse victim 94% 91% Page 166 Table 2 Variable descriptive statistics (N=1,951) Dependent Variable N % N % % Batterer physically assaulted officers 117 6% Independent Variables Batterer’s age Min 18 Max 79 Mean 33.4 SD 9.01 Min 18 Max 79 Mean 33.4 SD 9.01 Batterer married to abuse victim
  • 12. 995 51% Batterer’s prior DV arrests Min 0 Max 10 Mean 0.89 SD 1.08 Batterer unemployed 702 36% Batterer has HS/GED 1,093 56% 56% Batterer residing with abuse victim 741 38% Batterer consuming alcohol 429 22% Batterer using drugs 429 22% Batterer damaged
  • 13. property 351 18% Batterer struck abuse victim 1,834 94% Batterer knew police were responding 1,034 53% Batterer displayed hostile demeanor 1,073 55% considered, under the assumption that married batterers may hold stronger views of privilege to control their spouses and defend against outside interference by the police. Similarly, the third independent variable was whether or not the batterer and abuse victim resided together. It was hypothesized that batterers who reside with their abuse victims would be more defensive of this territory being invaded by the arrival of an outside authority, such as the police. The fourth and fifth independent variables dealt with substance
  • 14. use issues, whether or not the batterer had consumed alcohol prior to the arrival of the police, and whether the batterer had used an illegal drug before the arrival of the police. Each of these variables was expected to have a positive relationship with assaulting police officers. The next three independent variables were believed to be measures of the batterer’s level of aggression at the time of the incident. It was reasoned that whether or not the batterer had damaged property as part of the domestic assault incident, and whether or not the abuse victim had been struck by the batterer as part of the incident, would suggest how violently aggressive the batterer was at the time of the incident. Whether or not the batterer displayed a hostile demeanor toward the officers upon their arrival at the scene was also included in the model to measure if the batterer’s level of aggression was still high when the officers finally arrived. Whether or not the batterer had been told the police had been summoned was also considered as a possible predictor. When offenders are surprised by the appearance of the police they may be more likely to resist or be antagonistic toward the officers. It was therefore hypothesized that the batterer would be more likely to assault the officers if their arrival on the scene was unexpected, allowing him less time to compose him, flee the scene, or formulate a plan for how to respond to the officers.
  • 15. The last three predictors in the model related to the batterer’s background, specifically his prior history of domestic assault arrests, his employment status, and his education level. Regarding the batterer’s prior record, all three datasets used for this analysis recorded the number of prior domestic assault arrests the batterer had received. Unfortunately, not all of the datasets included information on each batterer’s total number of prior criminal offense arrests, (which may have been a better predictor of past criminality), or all prior violent offenses (which may have been a better predictor of violent propensity). Whether or not the batterer was employed was included in the model, as was whether or not the batterer had attained a high school diploma or general education development (GED) diploma. It was hypothesized that batterers with prior domestic assault arrests, those who were unemployed, and those who had less than a high school education would be at greater risk for assaulting an officer. Results Only 117 cases involved an assault on officers, thus supporting the previous literature that domestic assault calls are not necessarily frequent events (Hirschel et al. 1994; Konstantin 1984; Garner and Clemmer 1986). Because the dependent variable was a dichotomous nominal level measure, binary logistic regression was utilized to determine the significance of the
  • 16. influence the independent variables had on the probability of whether or not the batterer assaulted the police officers on the scene. The logistic regression technique also permitted the determination of the odds each predictor had of increasing the likelihood of assault, while controlling for the influence of all of the other predictors in the model. The results of this logistic regression analysis are presented in Table 3. As can been seen in Table 3, the model chi-square was highly significant (p<.001), indicating that the combined predictors in the model did a fair job of predicting whether or not the batterer would assault the officers. The Nagelkerke pseudo R2 suggests that the independent variables in the model explained more than a quarter of the variation between cases in the odds of the batterer physically attacking the police. Within the model, however, Page 167 Table 3 Logistic regression of batterer characteristics on officer assault (N=1,951) Variable Coefficient SE Odds Ratio Batterer age −0.016 .020 0.984 Batterer married 0.070 .327 1.073 Prior DV arrests −0.126 .219 0.882 Batterer unemployed 0.542* .308 1.582 High school/GED 0.047 .308 3.165 Resides with abuse victim
  • 17. 1.152** .342 3.165 Using alcohol 1.706*** .431 5.507 Using drugs −0.562 .459 0.570 Property damaged 0.652* .307 1.919 Abuse victim struck −0.656 .582 0.519 Hostile demeanor 2.530*** .480 12.555 Knew Police were coming −0.161 .298 0.852 (Constant) −5.792*** 1.029 0.003 Model Chi-square 122.843*** Nagelkerke Pseudo R2 .276 Significance Levels: * p<.05; ** p<.01; *** p<.001 only five of the 12 independent variables were significant in predicting the odds of an assault occurring. The strongest predictor was whether or not the batterer displayed a hostile demeanor at the arrival of the police. When the suspect displayed a hostile demeanor toward the police, the batterer was almost 13 times more likely to assault the officers than if he did not display a
  • 18. hostile demeanor. The next significant predictor in strength was whether or not the batterer had been consuming alcohol prior to the arrival of the police. Batterers who had been consuming alcohol were five-and-a-half times more likely to assault officers than those who had not. If the batterer shared a residence with the victim he was more than three times more likely to physically assault the police. Batterers who had reportedly damaged property during the domestic incident were almost twice as likely to assault officers as those who had not damaged property. Those batterers who were unemployed were about one-and-a-half times more likely to assault the police as batters who were employed. Finally, all of the independent variables that revealed significant relationships to the dependent variable displayed relationships in the predicted directions. To reveal the substantive significance these five variables had on predicting whether or not an assault on the police would occur, the cases that bore all five predictors were compared with the cases that lacked all five significant predictors. A total of 43 cases involved a batterer who displayed a hostile demeanor, had been consuming alcohol, resided with his abuse victim, had damaged property in the incident, and was unemployed. Of these cases, 25.6% (n=11) resulted in an assault on the police. On the other hand, 263 incidents in the sample failed to have any of the five significant predictor characteristics, and none of these incidents resulted in an officer assault. Therefore, it appears that simultaneous presence of all five of these
  • 19. predictors increases the chances of an officer being assaulted to better than one in four, while the absence of all of these predictors reduces the chances of assault to less than one in 263. Discussion and Conclusion The purpose of this study was to attempt to identify domestic batterer characteristics that helped predict whether or not batterers would assault police officers who responded to their acts of domestic abuse. It was hoped that this exploratory analysis would reveal characteristics that had predictive value in order to help police officers identify when they are at increased risk of being assaulted by a male domestic batterer. It appears that this study took a significant first step in identifying such risk factor characteristics. As hostile demeanor and alcohol consumption were significant predictors of an officer assault. Prior research has suggested that domestic batterers frequently act composed when officers arrive and tend to act as if the abuse victim is the one who is acting irrationally (Buzawa and Buzawa 2003; Walker 1989). Therefore, batterers who remain hostile in their demeanor even after the police arrive appear likely to let their hostility turn to physical violence against the officers. Thus encountering a batterer who still displays a hostile demeanor when the
  • 20. officers arrive is an uncommon event and appears highly predictive of an impending assault. As for batterer alcohol consumption, the prior research on police officer assaults suggested that alcohol consumption is correlated with attacks on officers (Brown 1994; Meyer et al. 1979; Moxey and McKenzie 1993; Noaks and Christopher 1990; Pinizzotto et al. 1998), and the same was found here. Alcohol serves as a depressant, contributing to the batterer’s negative mood, and may reduce the batterer’s inhibitions about using force against the police. Alcohol consumption frequently coexists with domestic violence (Buzawa and Buzawa 2003; Saunders 1995; Straus et al. 1980), and appears to increase likelihood of the batterer assaulting the responding police officers. Batterers who reside with their abuse victim are more likely to assault officers than those who live apart from their victim. This may be due to impressions of territorial control. Prior research has suggested that many batterers have heightened needs for dominance and control in their relationships and their home (Buzawa and Buzawa 2003; Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart 1994; Walker 1989). They Page 168 are threatened when they cannot successfully control the behavior of their intimate partners, and appear to be even more threatened when the police
  • 21. enter their home and interject themselves into the situation. In essence, the batterer has lost all influence over what he views as his human and physical property when the police intervene. While this police interference may be difficult for the batterer to accept at any location, it appears to be an even stronger insult when it occurs in his own residence. It may also be this emphasis on dominance, power, and control that results in unemployed batterers being more likely to assault officers than batterers who are employed. Unemployed batterers may be already struggling to maintain dominance in their home. Being unable to provide for their families, they no longer have the option of using economic means to control their abuse victims. Some abuse victims are willing to tolerate their abuse because of the economic rewards afforded to her and her children if she stays in the relationship (Buzawa and Buzawa 2003; Straus et al. 1980), but if there are very few financial rewards due to the batterer’s unemployment, there are fewer incentives to stay. The batterer begins to lose some of his power and control in the relationship. This situation can be aggravated further for the batterer if it results in his abuse victim seeking employment to support the family, thus gaining some economic independence that further threatens that batterer’s control. This situation may make the batterer more volatile than normal when the police arrive to interject themselves into
  • 22. that situation, and limit the batterer’s control to an even greater extent. Destruction of property by the batterer prior to the arrival of the police was another significant predictor of officer assaults. This may be an indicator of the degree of the batterer’s rage. While whether or not the victim was struck was not a significant predictor, it is important to remember how many batterers view their use of force. Their abusive behavior is used to control their victim. When the abuse victim is completely compliant the abuse is usually very minor, often only limited to insults and innuendo. When the abuse victim’s behavior is perceived as less compliant, the severity of the abuse increases proportionally (Buzawa and Buzawa 2003; Saunders 1995; Walker 1989). When the batterer damages property, especially his own possessions, it is not a control technique as much as it is an expression of rage. This rage is then turned on the police when they arrive. These five significant predictors could be utilized by law enforcement agencies to screen domestic assault calls for their level of danger. In many situations, police dispatchers can collect details about four of these elements from the victim or witness who reports the crime. Even if the caller is a neighbor, the dispatcher can often ascertain from the caller if the suspect resides with the victim, if he has been drinking, and if he has damaged any property. If the caller is a friend or family member, he/she should also be able to reveal if the batterer is unemployed. The dispatcher could then warn the responding officers if these
  • 23. four assault predictors are already present at the scene. The dispatcher can also assign additional units when these elements are present. When the officers arrive, they can determine for themselves if the fifth element, a hostile demeanor toward them, is present. If so, the officers can take defensive measures to isolate the batterer and place him in a position of disadvantage (such as on his knees with his hands interlaced on his head) before questioning him. Finally, it is important to note for police training purposes that female victims and female batterers in domestic assault situations assault the police only very rarely. Of the 3,078 domestic assault calls in the original sampling frame, only 98 (3.2%) involved a female batterer, illustrating how rarely law enforcement officers encounter female perpetrators of domestic assault. Of these 98 incidents of female perpetrated domestic assault, none of them resulted in an assault on an officer. Three of the male perpetrated domestic assault calls did result in a female abuse victim assaulting an officer. Of the 2,980 incidents of male perpetrated domestic assault calls in the original sampling frame, less than 0.1% involved a victim assault on an officer. In this dataset, only one in 1,026 domestic assault calls handled by the police resulted in a female victim assault on an officer. Several inferences can be drawn from the findings in this study. First, attacks on police officers while they are handling domestic assault calls do not occur very frequent- ly. In the present
  • 24. study, officers were assaulted in only 6% of cases in the sample. Nevertheless, due to the sheer volume of domestic assault calls handled by the police in the U.S., there still is a significant risk of assault. For example, if the odds of assault are truly only one in 33 (3%), and a patrol officer in an urban area responds to at least two domestic assault calls per week, the officer is likely to be assaulted at least twice per year. Obviously other factors are also at work here, however, as the variables in the present study left more than 70% of the variation unexplained. While an explained variance of 30% is common among social science studies of human behavior (Loeber 1990; Rosenthal 2001), more work needs to be done. Certain officer characteristics (such as skills in interpersonal communication), or situational characteristics (such as the number of backup officers present) may help further reduce the odds of being assaulted. Possibilities should be pursued in future research. Another inference to be drawn from this study is that the five predictors that were significantly correlated with assaulting an officer may be reliable risk factors that officers may use to predict their risk of assault. Whether or not the batterer at a domestic assault incident has most of these five characteristics could easily be determined by a Page 169
  • 25. skilled police 911 dispatcher, provided that the caller has firsthand knowledge about the incident and is being cooperative. If the batterer appears to have any of these five characteristics, the responding officers could possibly be warned of this fact prior to their arrival. Policies could also be developed to send more backup units than normally would be sent if it is determined that the batterer has most of these characteristics. However, before these characteristics could be reliably established as risk factors, replication of these findings would be necessary. As with all studies, this one had its limitations. First, because of the age of the data it may not have accounted for any changes in police tactics that may have occurred over the last few decades. Second, the data lacked information on the seriousness of the assaults on the officers. It was unknown, therefore, if the victim officers sustained any injuries from these assaults, and if the predictors varied by the seriousness of the assault. Finally, the data lacked information on the interpersonal interactions between the officers and the batterer upon their arrival. What the officers did and said at the scene may have had an aggravating of mitigating influence on the potential for assault. These weaknesses need to be addressed in future research regarding assaults on officers attending domestic assault calls. References
  • 26. R. R. Johnson (*) Department of Criminal Justice, University of Toledo, 2801 W. Bancroft St., Mail Stop 119, Toledo, OH 43606-3390, USA e-mail: [email protected] 1 Data obtained from Sherman, L. W. and Berk, R. A., Specific Deterrent Effects of Arrest for Domestic Assault: Minneapolis, 1981– 1982: Sherman, L. W., Schmidt, J. D., Rogan, D. P., Milwaukee Domestic Violence Experiment, 1987–1989: Pate, A., Hamilton, E. E., and Sampson, A., Spouse Abuse Replication Project in Metro-Dade County, Florida 1987–1989: computer file datasets; Ann Arbor, Michigan: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research, 2001. 2 While police lore frequently provides examples of incidents where it was the abuse victim at the domestic violence call who assaulted the officers, the data here suggests that this is a truly rare event. An attack on officers by a victim occurred in less than one in a thousand domestic violence calls recorded in these three datasets. Alpert, G. P., & Dunham, R. G. (1999). Use of force: Overview of national and local data. Washington: National Institute of Justice. Barker, J. C. (1999). Danger, duty, and disillusion: The worldview of Los Angeles police officers. Prospect Heights: Waveland. Bragason, O. O. (2007). Assaults against police officers: A self-
  • 27. report study among Icelandic police officers. Reykjavik: The Office of the National Police Commissioner. Brown, B. (1994). Assaults on police officers: An examination of the circumstances in which such incidents occur. London: Home Office Research Group. Buzawa, E., & Buzawa, C. (2003). Domestic violence: The criminal justice response (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Ellis, D., Choi, A., & Blaus, C. (1993). Injuries to police officers attending domestic disturbances: an empirical study. Canadian Journal of Criminology, 34, 149– 168. Garner, J., & Clemmer, E. (1986). Danger to police in domestic disturbances: A new look. Washington: National Institute of Justice. Garner, J., & Maxwell, C. (2000). What are the lessons of the police arrest studies? Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 4, 83–114. Geller, W. A., & Toch, H. (1995). And justice for all: Understanding and controlling police abuse of force. Washington: Police Executive Research Forum. Hickman, L. J. (2003). Fair treatment or preferred outcome? The impact of police behavior on victim reports of domestic violence incidents. Law and Society Review, 37, 607–633. mailto:[email protected]
  • 28. Hirschel, J. D., Dean, C. W., & Lumb, R. C. (1994). The relative contribution of domestic violence to assault and injury of police officers. Justice Quarterly, 11, 99–117. Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (1994). Typologies of male batterers: three subtypes and the differences among them. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 476– 497. Johnson, R. R. (2007). Surviving firearm assaults at domestic violence calls. Law Enforcement Executive Forum, 7(7), 155–167. Johnson, R. R. (2008). Assessing the true dangerousness of domestic violence calls. Law Enforcement Executive Forum, 8(5), 19–29. Konstantin, D. (1984). Homicides of American law enforcement officers. Justice Quarterly, 1, 29–45. Loeber, R. (1990). Development and risk factors of juvenile antisocial behavior and delinquency. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 1–41. Maxwell, C., Garner, J., & Fagan, J. (2002). The preventive effects of arrest on intimate partner violence: research, policy and theory. Criminology and Public Policy, 2, 51–79. Mayhew, C. (2001). Occupational health and safety risks faced by police officers. Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice, 196, 1–6. Meyer, C. K.,
  • 29. Magedanz, D. C., Kieselhorst, D., & Chapman, S. (1979). Violence and the police: the special case of the police assailant. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 7, 161–171. Moxey, M., & McKenzie, I. (1993). Assaults on police. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 9, 172–186. Noaks, L., & Christopher, S. (1990). Why are police assaulted? Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 6, 625–638. Pate, A. M., Hamilton, E. E., & Annan, S. (1991). Metro-Dade spouse abuse replication project: Final report. Washington: Police Foundation. Pinizzotto, A., David, E., & Miller, C. (1998). In the line of fire: learning from assaults on law enforcement officers. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 67(2), 15– 23. Piquero, A., Brame, R., Fagan, J., & Moffitt, T. (2006). Assessing the offending activity of criminal domestic violence suspects: offense specialization, escalation, and de-escalation evidence from the spouse assault replication program. Public Health Reports, 121, 409–418. Rosenthal, J. A. (2001). Statistics and data interpretation. Belmont: Brooks/Cole. Saunders, D. G. (1995). Prediction of wife assault. In J. Campbell (Ed.), Assessing dangerousness: Violence by sexual offenders, batterers, and child abusers (pp.
  • 30. 9–34). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Sherman, L. W., & Rogan, D. P. (1984). The specific deterrent effects of arrest for domestic assault. American Sociological Review, 49, 261–272. Sherman, L. W., Schmidt, J. D., Rogan, D. P., Gartin, P. R., Cohn, E. G., Collins, D. J., et al. (1991). From initial deterrence to long- term escalation: short-custody arrest for poverty ghetto domestic violence. Criminology, 29(4), 821–850. Skolnick, J. (1966). Justice without trial: Law enforcement in a democratic society. New York: Wiley. Stanford, R. M., & Mowry, B. L. (1990). Domestic disturbance danger rate. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 17, 244–249. Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the American family. Garden City: Anchor. Terrill, W., & Mastrofski, S. D. (2002). Reassessing situational and officer based determinants of police coercion. Justice Quarterly, 19, 215–248. Uchida, C. D., Brooks, L. W., & Koper, C. S. (1987). Danger to police during domestic encounters: assaults on Baltimore county police, 1984–86. Criminal Justice Policy Review, 2, 357–371. Walker, L. E. (1989). Terrifying love: Why battered women kill
  • 31. and how society responds. New York: Harper and Row. Wilson, J. Q. (1968). Varieties of police behavior: The management of law and order in eight communities. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Copyright of Journal of Family Violence is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Predicting Officer Physical Assaults at Domestic Assault CallsLiterature ReviewMethodProcedureTable 1 Comparison between sample and excluded casesTable 2 Variable descriptive statistics (N=1,951)ResultsTable 3 Logistic regression of batterer characteristics on officer assault (N=1,951)Discussion and ConclusionReferences REVIEW Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence
  • 32. in the LGBT Community Using a Bystander Framework Sharyn J. Potter, PhD, MPH, Kim Fountain, PhD, and Jane G. Stapleton, MA Sexual and relationship violence are two major public health issues that affect an alarming number of undergraduate students. As a result, many colleges and universities have protocols to serve victims of these forms of violence. Despite federal legislation stating that all students should have equitable experiences, current protocols and programs focus primarily on heterosexual students. College student victims of sexual and relationship violence who identify as lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgender can face particular challenges, including disclosure of their sexual and gender orientations, and revictimization when seeking services. In recent years an increasing number of campuses have adopted bystander prevention strategies to address sexual and relationship violence. These strategies seek to engage community members in the prevention of sexual and relationship violence by training them to identify and safely intervene in situations where sexual or relationship violence is about to occur, is occurring, or has occurred. In this article we review published bystander prevention strategies that focus on preventing sexual and relationship violence in the campus community, and discuss how bystander strategies are addressing or can address relationship and sexual violence in the LGBT community. (HARV REV PSYCHIATRY 2012;20:201–208.)
  • 33. Keywords: bisexual. bystander prevention strategies, college, gay, lesbian, relationship and sexual violence, transgender From the Department of Sociology (Dr. Potter) and Women’s Stud- ies Program (Dr. Fountain and Ms. Stapleton), University of New Hampshire. Original manuscript received 18 August 2011; revised manuscript received 17 December 2011, accepted for publication subject to re- vision 17 January 2012; revised manuscript received 9 February 2012. Correspondence: Sharyn J. Potter, PhD, MPH, Department of Sociol- ogy, University of New Hampshire, 20 Academic Way, Durham, NH 03824. Correspondence: [email protected] © 2012 President and Fellows of Harvard College DOI: 10.3109/10673229.2012.712838 Sexual assault of women is the most common violent crime committed on college campuses today; one in four college women experiences completed or attempted rape during their college years.1 The majority of these assaults are perpetrated by the victim’s acquaintances (e.g., classmates, residence hall neighbors, dates) or intimate partners.1−3 College women are at higher risk for sexual assault than peers who are not in college.4 Studies show that college men also report unwanted sexual experiences.5,6 Recent research indicates that 25% of women and 14% of men will experience
  • 34. relationship violence over the course of their lifetimes.7 Exposure to sexual and relationship violence∗ is a key public health issue and is associated with a multiplicity of negative outcomes, including increased substance use, depressive symptoms, health-risk behaviors, and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder among various samples of survivors.∗ ,9−13 *We use the term sexual violence to refer to a range of behaviors that are unwanted by the recipient, including, but not limited to, the following: derogatory or insulting remarks about physical appearance; persistent sexual advances that are undesired by the recipient; unwanted touching; and unwanted oral, anal, or vaginal penetration. These behaviors could be initiated by someone either known or unknown to the recipient, including someone in an existing relationship with the recipient. We use the term relationship violence to refer to a range of behaviors experienced in the con- text of any type of intimate relationship or friendship. These behaviors include use of physical force or threats of force against a partner, such as slapping, punching, throwing objects, threatening with weapons, or threatening any kind of physical harm. It can also include extreme emotional abuse such as intimidation, blaming, putting down, making fun of, and name calling. ∗ Although the terms victim and survivor are often used interchangeably,7 in the legal definition the term victim rather than survivor is used to describe a person following an assault.8
  • 35. 201 mailto:[email protected] Harv Rev Psychiatry July/August 2012 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 202 The extent to which campuses are working to prevent these problems varies widely.4 Most efforts are aimed at heterosexual women—statistically, the most highly represented group. But this population is not the only one for which sexual and relationship violence is an issue. Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender (LGBT) students represent a marginalized population at the same14 or at higher risk for sexual and relationship violence15−18 as their heterosexual peers. Yet the 5%–10% of undergraduate student populations who identify as LGBT19,20 are signif- icantly underserved by colleges’ prevention strategies for sexual and relationship violence. Campus efforts to combat such violence have demon- strated mixed results regarding effectiveness, particularly over time.21 Some prevention strategies have been viewed as directly or indirectly victim blaming.22 Others have focused too much on individuals or small groups (such as athletes or fraternity members), or on criminal justice policies rather than wider social change.23,24 Some campuses have therefore begun to utilize a bystander approach to engage the broader campus community—in particular, bystanders—in efforts to reduce sexual and relationship violence on campus. In this article we examine the extent to which the published by- stander prevention strategies have addressed sexual and re-
  • 36. lationship violence in the LGBT community, and we provide suggestions to improve bystander-intervention frameworks. SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE EXPERIENCED BY LGBT COLLEGE STUDENTS Campus Climate for Students Who Identify as LGBT Sociocultural bias against LGBT people and cultures, known as heterosexism and homophobia, can frame LGBT identi- ties as sinful, sick, perverted, and a threat to the traditional family.25−27 When heterosexuality is posited as the norm, acts of violence at every level of society perpetrated against LGBT people are justified or explained through reference to such negative constructions.28 The climate of LGBT intol- erance on college campuses has been well documented by researchers.29−34 For LGBT students, college campuses are often unwelcoming, unsafe environments, where they expe- rience harassment,29−31 isolation, ostracization,32 and phys- ical violence.33 LGBT students can face discrimination or harassment from their peers (e.g., roommates, classmates, teammates) and other members of the campus community, including the professional staff (e.g., coaches, hall directors, professors, administrators, campus safety, health services staff). Harassment and discrimination from campus com- munity members can not only make LGBT students feel unwelcome but also reduce their self-esteem and prevent them from seeking help from campus professionals.32,33,35 Perpetrator Tactics Perpetrators most commonly use two tactics to control their victims: threatening to or actually revealing the victim’s sex- uality to others (“outing” the victim), or isolating the victim. For many lesbians who report abuse, the abuse occurs dur- ing their first relationship, when they are most vulnerable to
  • 37. batterers who have the capacity to control or manipulate information.36 For students the impact of outing may be dev- astating and may occur on several different levels. Many stu- dents experiment with their sexuality or come out in college. They may not tell their parents for any number of reasons, including being cut off from financial and emotional support, losing parental housing during school breaks, or losing jobs that might be their only form of support.37−40 The college environment can offer students the opportunity to explore same-sex relationships,36 but many such first relationships are at high risk for relationship violence because the victims want both to confirm their sense of self and sexual identity, and to “fit in.” The victims may also lack confidence in what behaviors are acceptable in intimate same-sex relationships and may have no or minimal contact with LGBT friend- ship/community networks, within which they could air their concerns. The desire to fit in leaves victims vulnerable be- cause they may take risks, explain away the violence, or lack the vocabulary or life skills to identify, name, and act on the violence. For those who might possess such skills and sup- port and who might even be out, the embarrassment behind feeling that they somehow are not expressing their sexual or gender identities “correctly” contributes to their silence around relationship violence and sexual assaults. These fac- tors and the perpetrators’ awareness of them increase the likelihood of relationship violence.41 Isolation—one of the most effective and common tactics that batterers resort to in heterosexual relationships42—is often easy to use against LGBT victims because they may not be open about their sexuality and are therefore socially isolated. Alternatively, they may have come out but been rejected by their social groups or families. A variation on this theme is for batterers to limit the circle of people who are allowed to know about the relationship, as the batterer claims that they are not “out.”41
  • 38. Harv Rev Psychiatry Volume 20, Number 4 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 203 Barriers to Seeking Help and Reporting The consequences and implications of sexual and relation- ship violence in LGBT communities are both similar to, and different from, those of the heterosexual community. Both heterosexual and LGBT victims of sexual and relationship violence who seek help must disclose the crime. Students who identify as LGBT, however, may feel uncomfortable do- ing so; the disclosure of the crime may also involve the dis- closure of their sexual identities or gender orientations. For students who are not out, their perceived need to hide their sexual identities can be a barrier to seeking services. Peo- ple who are just coming out or who are not accustomed to talking about their sexual orientations may find the lan- guage required to discuss their experiences inaccessible.43 Indeed, the language of sexual and relationship violence tends to be extraordinarily gendered; female victims are “attacked” or “abused” by male perpetrators, but fewer ex- pressions are available to describe same-sex assaults. This linguistic shortfall reflects common thinking; for instance, some people cannot fathom or may explicitly deny that a man can sexually assault another man. Even those who are accustomed to speaking about same-sex situations on a va- riety of levels may find it difficult to cross the linguistic gap after a traumatic event. It can be all the more difficult for someone who has not yet developed the confidence, let alone the language related to his or her LGBT identity, to make a
  • 39. report. Male victims of sexual and relationship violence are often blamed for not stopping an attack—which makes it difficult for young men to speak of sexual or relationship violence perpetrated by other men. Reporting requires them not only to reveal very personal information but to frame acts of vio- lence in a manner that makes sense in heterosexist culture, in which victims are presumed to be heterosexual women. Indeed, the current reauthorization of the Violence Against Women Act faces opposition—for the first time—in large part due to the addition of provisions for services address- ing the LGBT communities. A gay man just coming to terms with his sexuality may find it overwhelming to negotiate a linguistic terrain that clearly marks him as gay and as a victim of sexual assault. LGBT individuals who identify as members of a racial or ethnic minority can face additional burdens in facing stereo- types of LGBT people as white and from Western cultures when seeking help.44,45 Similarly, they face accusations of being traitors to their racial or cultural identities for taking on the LGBT identity. Although all victims may face retribution from their friends for seeking help or reporting the crime, on many campuses the LGBT communities are small and insulated, and the fear of retribution can be compounded. The actual or perceived homophobia on college campuses may prevent LGBT undergraduate victims of sexual and relationship violence from seeking help. As with other marginalized groups, LGBT victims may choose not to report violence, thereby avoiding further stigmatization of the LGBT community.46,47 Victims may be concerned that the identification and
  • 40. labeling of same-sex perpetrators will further compromise the perception of relationships that already involve negative stereotyping.48 As members of marginalized communities, LGBT individuals—for fear of discrimination or harassment—often have serious reservations about accessing authority figures or disclosing their sexuality.49 Many LGBT youth attempt to access services or safety nets such as religious figures, school counselors, or the police, only to be told that their sexuality —not the actual perpetration of the violence—is the issue. For some victims the inability to obtain support from the system only underscores their isolation and vulnerability.47 Existing Treatments Services for LGBT and heterosexual victims of relationship and sexual violence commonly take the form of one-on- one counseling, advocacy efforts, and education. On college cam- puses, collaborations with medical facilities, law enforcement, and campus judicial boards may be part of these services. In the United States, services for victims of sexual and relationship violence are rarely fully inclusive, and the funding of services to meet the needs of LGBT victims, in particular, is typically inadequate.47 At the present time lesbian victims of relationship violence have fewer options than heterosexual victims for accessing safe and effective services.49 Other studies have shown that some crisis staff view same-sex relationship violence as less harmful than heterosexual violence—a result of the stereotypes that men can defend themselves and women are not violent.50 Because LGBT victims are often unwilling to report violence, statistical data reflect lower rates of incidence than would otherwise be the case, leading to minimal funding for direct services, advocacy, and prevention for LGBT communities. To be most responsive
  • 41. and effective, direct services, advocacy, and prevention efforts must proactively respond to the range of vulnerabilities that offenders exploit.51,52 Harv Rev Psychiatry July/August 2012 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 204 USING BYSTANDER PROGRAMS TO ADDRESS SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE IN THE LGBT COMMUNITY In the social psychology literature, bystanders are defined as individuals who witness criminal acts, emergency situations, or instances where community norms are violated.53−55 A bystander’s action or lack of action can worsen, maintain, or improve the situation. Using research on the effectiveness of prevention efforts, scholars and practitioners have tried to engage bystanders as allies in pre- venting sexual and relationship violence. Further impetus for engaging bystanders comes from research on perpetrator characteristics (e.g., hostility toward women, victimization experiences)56 and situational factors, such as community norms or community tolerance toward sexual and relationship violence that can facilitate or inhibit perpetrator behaviors;57−60 indeed, violence occurs when there are motivated perpetrators, vulnerable potential victims, and the absence of community members who can or will intervene.61 Attention to both perpetrator behavior and community responsibility can facilitate effective community-level prevention efforts. Bystander models focus on teaching bystanders active,
  • 42. helpful behaviors to safely intervene in situations that involve sexual and relationship violence.62 Although the use of bystander prevention strategies on college campuses is growing, the majority of bystander programs have not been formally evaluated, largely as a result of limited funding and administrative time.63,64 Five bystander programs for preventing sexual violence, as well as one social-marketing campaign, have had evaluations published in the peer-reviewed literature.65 The programs, each of which utilizes a different approach, are currently in place on college campuses in the United States and Canada. The Mentors in Violence Prevention (MVP) model developed by Katz in 1993 is one of the first violence prevention programs using a bystander framework.66 The MVP pro- gram, in addition to being used on college campuses, is used with sports teams and with members of the U.S. military. The program utilizes sports metaphors (e.g., a playbook) to engage men in preventing violence against women.66 Since its inception, the program has looked at the harassment of gays and lesbians, and also at other heterosexist behaviors. The occurrence of domestic violence and sexual assault in same-sex couples is addressed, but the scenarios in the play- book do not yet deal with bystander intervention when the abuse is within LGBT relationships themselves —but only when heterosexuals are abusing, harassing, or talking in degrading ways about LGBT people (Jackson Katz, personal communication). The InterACT Sexual Assault Prevention Program is an interactive skill-building program that seeks to increase participants’ knowledge on the importance and effec- tiveness of bystander interventions in preventing sexual assault.67 While relationship and sexual violence within the LGBT community is not a main focus, this phenomenon is
  • 43. addressed during the program’s facilitated-discussion component. InterACT has a separate program that is used to address homophobia; its main focus is violence and bullying aimed at LGBT people by heterosexuals, rather than violence within the LGBT community (Marc Rich and Courtney Ahrens, personal communication). The Men’s Project68 incorporates discussion of bystander techniques and utilizes a social-norms model of change,69,70 where peers’ perceptions of their peers’ attitudes and actions are used to influence behavioral change. At the present time this program does not address relationship and sexual vio- lence in the LGBT community (Christine Gidycz, personal communication). The Men’s Program,71 a bystander program that utilizes a film discussing a male-on-male rape as its focal point, does not address sexual and relationship violence in same-sex relationships (John Foubert, personal communication). The Bringing in the BystanderTM in-person prevention program trains participants to safely intervene when sexual assault or relationship abuse is about to occur, is occurring, or has occurred.62 The facilitator guide for this program in- structs peer facilitators to explain that perpetrators and vic- tims of sexual and relationship violence are not restricted to particular sexual identities, relationships, or socioeconomic backgrounds. “Its perpetrators and victims may be women or men, young or old, gay, lesbian, straight or bisexual. Re- lationship abuse affects people of all races, socioeconomic backgrounds, and educational levels.”72 Most of the educational strategies for preventing sexual and relationship violence are in the form of in-person pre- vention programs, and the Know Your PowerTM Bystander Social Marketing Campaign (developed in 2004) utilizes the main tenets of the Bringing in the Bystander in-person
  • 44. prevention program, in particular. The model of a social- marketing campaign engages bystander behaviors when sexual assault, relationship violence, or stalking is occur- ring, is about to occur, and has occurred. The campaign, ad- ministered campus-wide for a six-week period, utilizes 11' x 17' posters, bookmarks, table tents, full-side bus wraps, computer pop-up screens, and products distributed with the campaign logo.73 Studies have shown that participants who have been exposed to, and who identified with, the campaign images (compared to participants who report not identifying with the images) were more likely both to believe that they had a role to play in reducing sexual and relationship violence and to have engaged in bystander behaviors.73−75 During the running of a campaign, the images that ad- dress relationship and sexual violence in the heterosexual community (16 images) and the LGBT community (4 images) are displayed together; the goal is to recognize that sexual and relationship violence is not limited to heterosexual relationships and to engage all community members to Harv Rev Psychiatry Volume 20, Number 4 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 205 acknowledge and safely intervene in these situations. The first LGBT image, developed in 2006, has two scenes. In the first scene two friends realize that another friend is lying about the source of the bruises on his arms. The friends realize that the bruising is not the result of a skateboard- ing accident but has been inflicted by the victim’s abusive
  • 45. boyfriend. In the next scene one friend offers to take the victim to the campus rape crisis center. In the second LGBT image, developed in 2009, there are three scenes that il- lustrate bystanders supporting their friends after the oc- currence of a sexual assault. The first two scenes highlight female victims. In the third scene a male victim discloses that he has been sexually assaulted. His male friend tells him that he believes him. In the third LGBT image, devel- oped in 2011, a college party scene is taking place; a female is being emotionally abusive to her girlfriend. In the image the bystanders identify and label the abuse and devise a strategy to safely intervene to help their friend. In the fourth LGBT image, also developed in 2011, two friends are sitting in a local pizza shop. One of them, a potential per- petrator, describes how he has met a man online and that he plans on “hooking up” with this man, regardless of what the man wants. The friend labels his friend’s plan as the perpetration of a sexual assault and tries to convince him to change his mind. The nascent field of engaging community members as active bystanders to recognize and safely intervene when they anticipate or witness sexual and relationship violence provides an opportunity to prevent violence not only in the majority campus community but also in marginalized cam- pus communities (e.g., LGBTs and the disabled). Programs that engage community members as both bystanders and victims work to break down the isolation that threatens potential victims.76 Anti-LGBT stigma may prevent non- LGBT students from intervening, because of either fears of association or an inability to identify violence in a situation involving LGBT students. As college and university officials continue to develop strategies to address relationship and sexual violence in the heterosexual and non-heterosexual communities, care must be taken that all members of the larger community feel comfortable providing and seeking
  • 46. help. PROPOSED DIRECTIONS FOR THE FIELD, AND CONCLUSION Any bystander, whether LGBT or heterosexual, who en- counters an instance of sexual or relationship violence in the LGBT community should be able to recognize the vi- olence and to intervene in situations where violence is occurring—regardless of the sexual orientation of the vic- tims and perpetrators. Bystanders who intervene must also be taught, however, how to explain their empathic behavior to their peers and family members (by suggesting, for exam- ple, that LGBT relationships can be healthy and should be respected), who may otherwise ridicule their choice to inter- vene and may even see their advocacy as going against social norms and supporting such relationships.77,78 Intervening bystanders may be required to cope with guilt by association, including potential violence directed toward them. In fact, because of the potential for violence, agencies such as the New York City Gay and Lesbian Anti-violence Project send outreach workers into the field in pairs (or more) as a safety measure. As college administrators implement prevention strate- gies to reduce sexual and relationship violence in the LGBT community, it must be remembered that these two types of violence are not the same as hate violence. That said, within marginalized communities, all three types of violence can be intertwined; research indicates the prevalence of hate crime–related sexual assault among gay or bisexual men ranges from 3.0% to 19.8% of all such assaults and among lesbian or bisexual women, from 1% to 12.3%.79 Sexual and relationship violence and hate violence require unique approaches, each with its own appropriate messaging and
  • 47. effective interventions. While the strategies addressing the different forms of violence cannot be interchanged, neither should they be developed in isolation. Only if campus administrators address these issues will all students be free to pursue their optimal intellectual and emotional growth during their college or university years. Although some of the bystander prevention strategies ad- dress the discrimination and bullying that is faced by LGBT students,66,67 most do not. There are a few exceptions.62,73 These findings are problematic considering the prevalence of sexual and relationship violence in the LGBT undergraduate community and the unique barriers that LGBT students face when seeking help. The use of strategies to engage members of the broader community in preventing sexual and relationship violence within and against the LGBT com- munity needs to be increased. Furthermore, when victims who identify as LGBT seek help, they often find counselors that are ill equipped to offer support;14,48,80 counselors must be trained to provide professional and culturally competent services to LGBT victims. Finally, since the LGBT community is heterogeneous, efforts need to be made to understand why some groups access support and others do not.81 Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the article. REFERENCES 1. Fisher BS, Cullen FT, Turner MG. The sexual victimization of college women. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice and Bureau of Justice, 2000 [Report No. NIJ 182369].
  • 48. Harv Rev Psychiatry July/August 2012 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 206 2. Koss M, Harvey M. The rape victim: clinical and community. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1991. 3. Sampson R. Acquaintance rape of college students. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Community Oriented Policing Services, 2002. 4. Karjane HM, Fisher BS, Cullen FT. Sexual assault on campus: what colleges and universities are doing about it. Washing- ton, DC: National Institute of Justice, 2005 [Report No. NCJ 205521]. 5. Banyard VL, Ward S, Cohn ES, Moorhead C, Walsh W. Un- wanted sexual contact on campus: a comparison of women’s and men’s experiences. Violence Vict 2007;22:52–70. 6. Isley P. Sexual assault of men: college-age victims. NASPA J 1998;35:305–17. 7. Koziol-McLain J, Campbell JC. Universal screening and mandatory reporting: an update on two important issues for victims/survivors of intimate partner violence. J Emerg Nurs 2001;27:602–6. 8. University of North Carolina, Greensboro. Safe Zone/Violence: legal terms. http://cap.uncg.edu/safezone/topics/violence/#legal
  • 49. 9. Black MC, Basile KC, Breiding MJ, et al. The National Inti- mate Partner and Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2010 sum- mary report. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Injury Preven- tion and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2011. 10. Acierno R, Brady K, Gray M, Kilpatrick D, Resnick H, Best CL. Psychopathology following interpersonal violence: a compari- son of risk factors in older and younger adults. J Clin Geropsy- chol 2002;8:13–23. 11. Arata CM, Burkhart BR. Post-traumatic stress disorder among college student victims of acquaintance assault. J Psychol Hu- man Sex 1996;8:79–92. 12. Brener ND, McMahon PM, Warren CW, Douglas KA. Forced sexual intercourse and associated health-risk behaviors among female college students in the United States. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999;67:252–9. 13. Larimer ME, Lydum AR, Anderson BK, Turner AP. Male and female recipients of unwanted sexual contact in a college stu- dent sample: prevalence rates, alcohol use, and depression symptoms. Sex Roles 1999;40:295–308. 14. Duke A, Davidson M. Same-sex intimate partner violence: les- bian, gay, and bisexual affirmative outreach and advocacy. J Aggression Maltreat Trauma 2009;18:795–816. 15. Rothman E, Silverman J. The effect of a college sexual
  • 50. assault prevention program on first-year students’ victimization rates. J Am Coll Health 2007;55:283–90. 16. Duncan DF. Prevalence of sexual assault victimization among heterosexual and gay lesbian university-students. Psychol Rep 1990;66:65–6. 17. Rhodes SD, McCoy TP, Wilkin AM, Wolfson M. Behavioral risk disparities in a random sample of self-identifying gay and non- gay male university students. J Homosex 2009;56:1083–100. 18. Bowman R, Mogan H. Comparison of rates of verbal and phys- ical abuse on campus by gender and sexual orientation. Coll Stud J 1998;32:43–52. 19. Gonyea RM, Moore JV. Gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgen- der students and their engagement in educationally purpose- ful activities in college. 2007. http://cpr.iub.edu/uploads/GLBT% 20and%20Engagement-%20ASHE%202007.pdf 20. Weiss S. About 12 percent of students identify as gay or bisexual. Brown Daily Herald 2010;12 Nov. http://www. browndailyherald.com/about-12-percent-of-students-identify- as-gay-or-bisexual-1.2403002 21. Anderson L, Whiston S. Sexual education programs: a meta- analytic examination of their effectiveness. Psychol Women Q 2005;29:374–88.
  • 51. 22. Schewe PA, O’Donohue W. Rape prevention: methodologi- cal problems and new directions. Clin Psychol Rev 1993;13: 667– 82. 23. Potter RH, Krider JE, McMahon PM. Examining elements of campus sexual violence policies. Violence Against Women 2000;6:1345–62. 24. Swift CF, Ryan-Finn K. Perpetrator prevention: stopping the development of sexually abusive behavior. In: Swift CF, ed. Sexual assault and abuse: sociocultural context of prevention. New York: Haworth, 1995. 25. Jackson S, Scott S. Sexual antinomies in late modernity. Sexu- alities 2004;7:233–48. 26. Seidman S. From polluted homosexual to the normal gay: changing patterns of sexual regulation in America. In: Ingra- ham C, ed. Thinking straight: new work in critical heterosexu- ality studies. New York: Routledge, 2005. 27. Bryant K. In defense of gay ghildren? “Progay” homo- phobia and the production of homonormativity. Sexualities 2008;11:455–75. 28. Herek GM. Hate crimes and stigma-related experiences among sexual minority adults in the United States: prevalence esti- mates from a national probability sample. J Interpers Violence 2009;24:54–74. 29. Brown R, Clarke B, Gortmaker V, Robinson-Keilig R.
  • 52. Assessing the campus climate for gay, lesbian, bisexual and transgender (LGBT) students using a multiple perspectives approach. J Coll Stud Dev 2004;45:8–26. 30. D’Augelli AR. Lesbians’ and gay men’s experiences of discrimi- nation and harassment in a university community. Am J Com- munity Psychol 1989;17:317–21. 31. Rankin S, Weber G, Blumenfeld W, Frazer S. State of higher education for lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender people. Charlotte, NC: Campus Pride, 2010. 32. Hamilton L. Trading on heterosexuality: college women’s gen- der strategies and homophobia. Gend Soc 2007;21:145–72. 33. Rankin, SR. Campus climate for gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender people: a national perspective. New York: National Gay and Lesbian Task Force Policy Institute, 2003. 34. Waldo CR. Out on campus: sexual orientation and academic climate in a university context. Am J Community Psychol 1998;26:745–74. 35. Boysen GA, Vogel DL, Cope MA, Hubbard A. Incidents of bias in college classrooms: instructor and student perceptions. J Divers Higher Educ 2009;4:219–34. 36. Ristock J. No more secrets: violence in lesbian relationships. London: Routledge, 2002.
  • 53. 37. Turell S, Herman M. “Family” support for family vio- lence: exploring community support systems for lesbian and http://cap.uncg.edu/safezone/topics/violence/#legal http://cpr.iub.edu/uploads/GLBT%25 http://www/ Harv Rev Psychiatry Volume 20, Number 4 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 207 bisexual women who have experienced abuse. J Lesbian Stud 2008;12:211–24. 38. Floyd FJ, Stein TS. Sexual orientation identity formation among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: multiple patterns of milestone experiences. J Res Adolesc 2002;12:167–91. 39. Burke TW, Owen SS. Same-sex domestic violence: is anyone listening? Gay Lesbian Rev Worldwide 2006;13:6–7. 40. Chutter K. Opening our awareness to heterosexist and homo- phobic attitudes in society. Relational Child Youth Care Pract 2007;20:22–7. 41. Donovan C, Hester M, Holmes J, McCarry M. Comparing domestic abuse in same sex and heterosexual relationships [Initial report from study funded by the Economic and So- cial Research Council, award no. RES-000-23-0650]. 2006. http://www.broken-rainbow.org.uk/cohsar report.pdf
  • 54. 42. Stark, E. Coercive control: the entrapment of women in per- sonal life. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007. 43. Kulick D. Gay and lesbian language. Annu Rev Anthropol 2000;29:243–85. 44. Barrett DC, Pollack LM. Whose gay community? Social class, sexual self-expression, and gay community involvement. Sociol Q 2005;46:437–56. 45. Crawford I, Allison KW, Zamboni BD, Soto T. The influence of dual-identity development on the psychosocial functioning of African-American gay and bisexual men. J Sex Res 2002; 39:179–89. 46. McClennen J, Summers A, Vaughan C. Gay men’s domestic violence: dynamics, help-seeking behaviors and correlates. J Gay Lesbian Soc Serv 2002;14:23–49. 47. National Coalition of Anti-Violence Programs. Hate violence against lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender people in the United States: a report by the National Coalition of Anti- Violence Programs. New York: NCAVP, 2009. 48. Bornstein D, Fawcett J, Sullivan M, Senturia K, Shiu- Thornton S. Understanding the experiences of lesbian, bisexual and trans survivors of domestic violence: a qualitative study. J Homosex 2006;51:159–81. 49. Irwin J. (Dis)counted stories: domestic violence and lesbians.
  • 55. Qual Soc Work 2008;7:199–215. 50. Brown M, Groscup J. Perceptions of same-sex domestic violence among crisis center staff. J Fam Violence 2009;24:87–93. 51. Healy SA. Advocating for health and human services: the New York experience. J Gay Lesbian Psychother 2004;8:43–66. 52. Frazer S. LGBT health and human services needs in New York State. New York, New York: Empire State Pride Agenda Foun- dation and New York State Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Trans- gender Health and Human Services Network, 2009. 53. Darley JM, Latane B. Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. J Pers Soc Psychol 1968;8:377–83. 54. Latane B, Darley JM. Help in a crisis: bystander response to an emergency. Morristown, NJ: General Learning, 1976. 55. Moriarity T. Crime, commitment, and the responsive bystander: two field experiments. J Pers Soc Psychol 1975;31:370–6. 56. Lisak D, Miller PM. Repeat rape and multiple offending among undetected rapists. Violence Vict 2002;17:73–84. 57. Schwartz MD, DeKeseredy W. Sexual assault on the college campus: the role of male peer support. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1997. 58. Berkowitz AD. Fostering men’s responsibility for
  • 56. preventing sexual assault. In: Schewe PA, ed. Preventing violence in rela- tionships: interventions across the lifespan. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2002. 59. Schwartz MD, DeKeseredy WS. Aggregation bias and woman abuse. J Interpers Violence 2000;15:555–65. 60. DeKeseredy WS, Schwartz MD, Alvi S. The role of pro- feminist men in dealing with woman abuse on the Cana- dian college campus. Violence Against Women 2000;6: 918–35. 61. Schwartz MD, DeKeseredy WS, Tait D, Alvi S. Male peer support and a feminist routine activities theory: understand- ing sexual assault on the college campus. Justice Q 2001;18: 623–49. 62. Banyard V, Moynihan M, Plante E. Sexual violence prevention through bystander education: an experimental evaluation. Am J Community Psychol 2007;35:463–81. 63. Potter S, Stapleton J. Bringing in the target audience in bystander social marketing materials for communi- ties: suggestions for practitioners. Violence Against Women 2011;17:797–812. 64. Potter S, Banyard V. Guest editors’ introduction. Violence Against Women 2011;17:679–83. 65. Potter SJ, Banyard VL, eds. Special issue: engaging com- munities to end sexual violence: current research on by- stander focused prevention. Violence Against Women 2011;
  • 57. 17(6). 66. Katz J, Heisterkamp H, Fleming W. The social justice roots of the Mentors in Violence Prevention Model and its ap- plication in a high school setting. Violence Against Women 2011;17:684– 702. 67. Ahrens C, Rich M, Ullman J. Rehearsing for real life: the impact of the InterACT sexual assault prevention program on self-reported likelihood of engaging in by- stander interventions. Violence Against Women 2011;17:760– 76. 68. Gidycz C, Orchowski L, Berkowitz A. Preventing sexual ag- gression among college men: an evaluation of a social norms and bystander intervention program. Violence Against Women 2011;17:720–42. 69. Berkowitz, A. An overview of the social norms approach. In: Lederman L, Stewart L, eds. Changing the culture of college drinking. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton, 2005. 70. Fabiano PM, Perkins HW, Berkowitz A, Linkenbach J, Stark C. Engaging men as social justice allies in ending violence against women: evidence for a social norms approach. J Am Coll Health 2003;52:105–12. 71. Foubert J. In practice—anti-rape on the road: men hit the highways to share a powerful message. About Campus 2005;10:24–7. 72. Moynihan MM, Banyard VL, Plante EG, Eckstein RP.
  • 58. Bringing in the Bystander:TM establishing a community of responsibility. Facilitator’s guide. Durham: University New Hampshire, 2010. 73. Potter SJ. Using a multi-media social marketing campaign to increase active bystanders on the college campus. J Am College Health 2012;60;282–95. 74. Potter SJ, Moynihan MM, Stapleton JG. Using social self- identification in social marketing materials aimed at http://www.broken-rainbow.org.uk/cohsar Harv Rev Psychiatry July/August 2012 Addressing Sexual and Relationship Violence in the LGBT Community 208 reducing violence against women on campus. J Interpers Violence 2011;26:971–90. 75. Potter SJ, Stapleton JG. Translating sexual assault and stalk- ing prevention from a college campus to a US military post: piloting the Know Your Power Social Marketing Campaign. J Interpers Violence 2012;27:1593–1621. 76. Horn SS, Romeo KE. Peer contexts for lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender students: reducing stigma, prejudice, and dis- crimination. Prev Res 2010;17:7–10. 77. Herek GM. Heterosexuals’ attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: correlates and gender differences. J Sex Res 1988;25:
  • 59. 451–77. 78. Schulte L. Similarities and differences in homophobia among African Americans and Caucasians. Race Gend Class 2002;9:71–93. 79. Rothman EF, Exner D, Baughman AL. The prevalence of sexual assault against people who identify as gay, lesbian, or bisexual in the United States: a systematic review. Trauma Violence Abuse 2011;12:55–66. 80. Renzetti C. Violent betrayal: partner abuse in lesbian relation- ships. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1992. 81. Turell S, Cornell-Swanson L. Not all alike: within-group differ- ences in seeking help for same-sex relationship abuses. J Gay Lesbian Soc Serv 2005;18:71–88. Copyright of Harvard Review of Psychiatry is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE EXPERIENCED BY LGBT COLLEGE STUDENTSCampus Climate for Students Who Identify as LGBTPerpetrator TacticsBarriers to Seeking Help and ReportingExisting TreatmentsUSING BYSTANDER PROGRAMS TO ADDRESS SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE IN THE LGBT COMMUNITYPROPOSED DIRECTIONS FOR THE FIELD, AND CONCLUSIONREFERENCES
  • 60. Received 06/19/12 Revised 12/18/12 Accepted 01/09/13 DOI: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2014.00154.x Research Perceived Racism as Moderator Between Self-Esteem/Shyness and Psychological Distress Among African Americans Ruth Chu-Lien Chao, Joseph Longo, Canzi Wang, Deepta Dasgupta, and Jessica Fear In the spirit of the mission of social justice in counseling, this study examined perceived racism as a moderator on the association between self-concept (i.e., self-esteem, shyness) and distress among African Americans. The authors examined whether perceived racism changed the relationship between self-esteem/shyness and psychological dis-
  • 61. tress by investigating a sample of 394 African Americans. With a moderation analysis, the authors found signifcant interaction effects of Self-Esteem × Perceived Racism and Shyness × Perceived Racism on distress. Keywords: self-esteem, shyness, perceived racism, psychological distress, African Americans Given the importance placed on individualism in Western culture, self-concept has been critical in understanding people’s psychological issues from the Western perspective. Moreover, it is hardly surprising that self-concept has been described as one of the most pivotal personality attributes related to psychological distress (Marsh, Trautwein, Lüdtke, Köller, & Baumert, 2006). Self-concept has two indexes, self- esteem and shyness, which indicate how much people believe in themselves in terms of their sense of personal value and comfortableness with others (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Specif- cally, individuals with higher self-esteem tend to have better mental health and are more resilient in the face of adversity compared with those with low self-esteem (Marcussen, 2006). Shyness, the other component of self-concept, is defned as “one’s reaction to being with strangers or casual acquain- tances: tension . . . feelings of awkwardness and discomfort” (Cheek & Buss, 1981, p. 330). Shyness is associated with high negative emotionality, negative affectivity, personal distress, low positive affect, and low constructive coping (Eisenberg, Fabes, & Murphy, 1995). Many scholars (e.g., Cowden, 2005; Harris, 2010; Mar- cussen, 2006) have found that self-concept can positively or negatively relate to psychological distress, which refers to the current or past level of overall experiences of symptoms, intensity of symptoms, and number of reported symptoms (Derogatis, 1993). With regard to the association between
  • 62. self-concept and psychological distress, there are two types of studies: one investigates the relationship between self- esteem and psychological distress (e.g., Abe, 2004; Cassidy, O’Connor, Howe, & Warden, 2004), and the other explores the relationship between shyness and mental health (e.g., Cowden, 2005). According to Ratts (2011), one important component in the social justice mission in counseling is to alter or recognize oppressive environmental conditions such as racism. Unfor- tunately, the context of the prior studies shows that a gap still exists between the prior studies and the call from the social justice mission. The prior research used a univariate perspec- tive, which could only answer the direct association between self-concept (e.g., self-esteem, shyness) and psychological distress, but may not ft with the emerging commitment of the social justice mission in counseling-related professions (e.g., Goodman et al., 2004; Lum, 2003; Ratts, 2011; Ratts, Toporek, & Lewis, 2010). Among the burgeoning topics most directly relevant to social justice are correlates and consequences of injustices, including racism and prejudice. The harmful nature of racism has been a crucial experience for African Americans. Perceived racism could be a proxy for actual experiences of racism, and it refers to one’s ap- praisals of the stressfulness of the racist events (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). Indeed, evidence has accumulated during the past decade to suggest that reports of perceived racism Ruth Chu-Lien Chao, Joseph Longo, Canzi Wang, Deepta Dasgupta, and Jessica Fear, Department of Counseling Psychol- ogy, University of Denver. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ruth Chu-Lien Chao, Department of Counseling Psychology, Morgridge College of Education, University of Denver, 1999 East Evans Avenue, #260, Denver, CO 80208
  • 63. (e-mail: [email protected]). © 2014 by the American Counseling Association. All rights reserved. Journal of Counseling & Development ■ July 2014 ■ Volume 92 259 mailto:[email protected]
  • 64. Chao, Longo, Wang, Dasgupta, & Fear are related to mental health and symptoms of psychological distress (e.g., Fischer & Shaw, 1999). There is evidence to suggest that, in addition to being harmful, perceived racism varies among African Americans (Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). Levels of perceived racism relate to how African Americans manage the association between stress and mental health (Kaiser, Major, & McCoy, 2004). Mahalik, Pierre, and Wan (2006) suggested that self-con- cept, including self-esteem and shyness, was commingled with perceived racism to such a degree that it was diffcult to determine the association between self-concept and psy- chological distress without considering perceived racism. For example, among African Americans, perceived racism was associated with lower self-esteem and higher psycho- logical distress, which subsequently may make it diffcult to initiate a conversation with strangers (Clark et al., 1999; Fischer & Shaw, 1999). African Americans with higher perceived racism had lower use of counseling services and greater diffculty in trusting culturally different counselors. Applying the current literature of the relationships between self-esteem/shyness and psychological distress to African Americans without considering their perceived racism would miss the mission of social justice in counseling (Fischer & Shaw, 1999; Kaiser et al., 2004; Ratts, 2011). To fulfll the mission of social justice, which recognizes and alters op- pressive environmental conditions, we examined the role of an oppressive environment condition (i.e., perceived racism) to better understand the relationship between self-esteem/ shyness and psychological distress.
  • 65. Self-Esteem, Perceived Racism, and Psychological Distress Self-esteem refers to an individual’s overall evaluation or appraisal of his or her worth and encompasses both beliefs about oneself and emotions. It refers to how people feel about themselves, such as feeling competent and pos- sessing a positive self-attitude (Marcussen, 2006). Theo- retically, self-esteem infuences the way people are and their sense of personal value. Self-esteem also affects the way people feel and act in the world and how they relate to others. Those with high self-esteem may believe that they have good qualities, a perception that is critical in maintaining low psychological distress. In recent years, a growing body of research has supported the vulnerability model, which states that low self-esteem operates as a risk factor for psychological distress (Harris, 2010; Orth, Robins, & Roberts, 2008). Individuals with low self-esteem are assumed to have fewer coping resources and thus are prone to distress, whereas those with high self-esteem are assumed to have suffcient coping strategies, which prevent them from spiraling downward into psychologi- cal distress. Empirically, high self-esteem has been found to be negatively associated with psychological distress, but low self-esteem may signifcantly exacerbate people’s psychological maladjustment (Orth et al., 2008). Although the relationship between self-esteem and psy- chological distress has been established, when applying this relationship to African Americans, the precise mechanisms of in what conditions and among whom self-esteem may lead to distress have not yet been determined. In other words, conceptualizing a direct association between self-esteem and psychological distress for African Americans may lead to two
  • 66. diffculties. First, Mahalik et al. (2006) found that African Americans’ development of self-esteem closely relates to their racial identity and discrimination. Szymanski and Gupta (2009) found that among African Americans, perceived rac- ism was negatively associated with self-esteem but positively associated with distress. Our study attempted to further ad- vance the literature on self-esteem, perceived racism, and psychological distress by conceptualizing perceived racism as a moderator that may change the mechanism between self-esteem and distress. Second, Klonoff, Landrine, and Ullman (1999) reported that perceived racism is a ubiquitous part of many Afri- can Americans’ experiences. More than 90% of African Americans have experienced racist events in their lifetime (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). Indeed, racism is insidious in society and permeates many aspects of African American life. Jones (1997) suggested that the experience of racism is multidimensional and can be classifed by using a tripartite typology: individual racism, institutional racism, and cultural racism. Because racism could be experienced by African Americans on almost a daily basis, Landrine and Klonoff (1996) suggested that the assessment of the degrees of racism or appraisal of the racist experience may be more important than measuring the types of racism. For example, two African Americans may experience the same racist event, and yet one may fnd it very stressful or be severely hurt whereas the other dismisses it (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). In addition, perceived racism can signifcantly affect African Americans’ mental health. The more racism that African Americans ex- perience, the higher the levels of psychological distress they report (Fischer & Shaw, 1999). An understanding of the role of perceived racism would seem to be of great importance to counselors when they serve African Americans. In other words, while we continue to work
  • 67. toward the social justice mission (i.e., a better understanding of racism), it is inevitable to examine how perceived racism may change the established association between self-esteem and distress. Furthermore, we also work to identify under what conditions (i.e., high vs. low perceived racism) is self-esteem negatively associated with the distress of African Americans. Understanding the role of perceived racism could allow coun- selors to determine among whom (i.e., African Americans Journal of Counseling & Development ■ July 2014 ■ Volume 92 260
  • 68. Perceived Racism Among African Americans who perceive high vs. low racism) psychological distress can be minimized regardless of the levels of self-esteem. We thus conceptualized that the association between self-esteem and distress among African Americans may depend on the extent of racism that the individual perceives. When African Americans perceive high levels of racism, their self-esteem may be vulnerable to psychological distress. Yet, when African Americans perceive low levels of racism, they may be able to maintain low psychological distress across different levels of self-esteem. Therefore, our frst hypothesis was that per- ceived racism moderates the relation between self-esteem and psychological distress among African Americans. However, when perceiving less racism, African Americans would be able to maintain low psychological distress across different levels of self-esteem. Taken together, although prior studies have addressed perceived racism, almost none addressed it as a moderator variable. Thus, we examined perceived racism as a moderator, rather than simply repeating past studies on the direct effects between self-esteem and psychological distress. Shyness, Perceived Racism, and Psychological Distress According to Cheek and Buss (1981), shyness is a person- ality characteristic or a component of self-concept and is characterized by the discomfort and inhibition that may occur in the presence of others. Conceptually, shy people tend to be aware of themselves as social objects more than people with low shyness. Given that shy people tend to feel tense and awkward, they may have higher levels of psychological distress (Eisenberg et al., 1995). Empirically, shyness is pre- dictive of later psychological and social functioning (Beer,
  • 69. 2002). Shyness is positively associated with neuroticism and negatively associated with extroversion, two personality traits of the Big Five scale (Ryan & Xenos, 2011). Shyness is often thought to be associated with anxiety or generalized negative affect, and its relation to psychological distress has been reasonably well documented. Research has also found that shyness is associated with depression (Beer, 2002), low self-esteem, and loneliness (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Thus, based on the current literature, shyness could be a risk factor for psychological distress. Although shyness could be a vulnerability factor for psy- chological distress, it is still unknown or questionable if this association is valid among African Americans. For example, African Americans may feel uncomfortable when encounter- ing strangers because of shyness or perceived racism from an unwelcoming stranger. Or perhaps the extent of the racism that African Americans perceive exacerbates their shyness on distressed feelings. To date, there has been no rigorous test of the association between shyness and psychological distress among African Americans. Joiner (1997) suggested that the association between shyness and psychological distress should be considered within the context of a targeted popula- tion. Given the fact that many African Americans encounter racist events (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996) and that the way in which it is experienced is a unique stressor for this popula- tion (Clark et al., 1999; Harrell, 2000), it seems appropriate to include perceptions of racism in the association between shyness and distress among African Americans. Thus, a goal of our study was to explain, at least in part, when and how shy African Americans become psychologically distressed, by using interactive views between shyness and perceived racism on symptoms of distress. The reason that perceived racism could be a moderator
  • 70. that changes the association between shyness and distress could be due to a variety of issues (Eisenberg et al., 1995). First, when African Americans perceive more racism, they may be more vulnerable to shyness, which has been associ- ated with distress. African Americans who perceive more racism tend to have less resilience and fewer resources to manage daily stress than those who perceive less rac- ism (Beer, 2002). Second, although shyness is associated with psychological distress, more important for African Americans, it is suggested that higher level of perceived racism may make shy African Americans more vulner- able to distress. For African Americans, shyness involves an internal experience of emotional isolation, whereas perceived racism creates an even deeper sense of social marginalization and relational disconnection. Moreover, when African Americans are disturbed by racism, they may become lonely and frustrated and may be unable to manage their shyness, which in turn leads to greater psychological distress (Cowden, 2005). That is, African Americans’ perceived high levels of racism are predicted to worsen the association between shyness and distress. Finally, because shyness does not assume high levels of perceived racism, some shy African Americans may still perceive low levels of racism. Such low levels of perceived racism may make African Americans more confdent and relaxed when encountering strangers. Thus, low perceived racism may buffer against shyness on negative social and health consequences, and African Ameri- cans reporting low perceived racism may be able to maintain low psychological distress no matter how shy they are. The present reconceptualization thus asserts that shyness will buffer against psychological distress when African Ameri- cans perceive low levels of racism-related stress (i.e., per- ceived racism moderates the relation between shyness and psychological distress among African Americans). In other
  • 71. words, when feeling shy, African Americans who perceive low levels of racism may have a buffer that differentially affects the association between shyness and psychological distress (Clark et al., 1999). Thus, our second hypothesis was that perceived racism moderates the relationship between shyness and psychological distress among African Ameri- Journal of Counseling & Development ■ July 2014 ■ Volume 92 261 Chao, Longo, Wang, Dasgupta, & Fear cans. The less racism perceived by African Americans, the
  • 72. more buffered the relation is; however, those who perceive high levels of racism would lack the buffer against shyness on distress. Therefore, we examined perceived racism as a moderator between shyness and psychological distress rather than simply addressing the direct effects between shyness and psychological distress. The Present Study In an effort to advance the social justice mission of counseling and psychology, we expanded the current literature on the associa- tion between self-concept and psychological distress for African Americans. Contextualizing this association based on African Americans’ experiences with racism serves two purposes: First, it addresses the emergent social justice mission imperative in counseling; and second, it facilitates counselors’ cultural sensitivity when serving this population. Because perceived racism affects the lives of many African Americans, we constructively tested our two hypotheses with a sample of nonclinical African Americans. Our frst hypothesis aimed to determine whether perceived racism moderates the association between self-esteem and psychological distress. Our second hypothesis examined whether perceived rac- ism moderates the association between shyness and psychological distress. We hope that our fndings will assist African Americans in dealing with the association between self-concept and distress, as well as help advance the current literature on self-esteem and shyness in the face of racism. We placed the association between self-esteem/shyness and psychological distress in the mission of
  • 73. social justice via conceptualizing perceived racism as a moderator to this association. Method Power Analysis A power analysis was conducted by using G*Power 3.1.2 (Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996) to estimate the sample size. A sample size of 386, 55, and 26 was needed for a small, medium, and large effect size, respectively. In our study, a sample size of 394 would detect a two-way interaction effect with a small to medium effect size and a power of .80. Participants Our study participants were 394 African American students from one historically Black university in the mid-Atlantic region of the United States. Specifcally, there were 212 (54%) women and 177 (45%) men; fve participants did not identify their gender. Most participants were undergraduate students (17% freshmen, 14% sophomores, 23% juniors, 26% seniors, and 20% graduate school). The participants had declared ma- jors in biology, engineering, education, or psychology. About 77% of participants were 25 years of age or younger. All par- ticipants were informed that their participation was voluntary and reassured that all information would be kept anonymous. Instruments Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965). The RSES is a 10-item scale that assesses an individual’s self-esteem. It uses a 4-point Likert-type response, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree), with higher scores indicating higher self-esteem. The RSES has a more than adequate coeffcient alpha of .92 and a test–retest cor-