SlideShare a Scribd company logo
This article was downloaded by: [Joanna Huxster]
On: 29 June 2015, At: 15:13
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Click for updates
The Journal of Environmental Education
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjee20
Undergraduate Understanding of Climate
Change: The Influences of College Major
and Environmental Group Membership
on Survey Knowledge Scores
Joanna K. Huxster
a
, Ximena Uribe-Zarain
b
& Willett Kempton
b
a
Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
b
University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware, USA
Published online: 29 Jun 2015.
To cite this article: Joanna K. Huxster, Ximena Uribe-Zarain & Willett Kempton (2015) Undergraduate
Understanding of Climate Change: The Influences of College Major and Environmental Group
Membership on Survey Knowledge Scores, The Journal of Environmental Education, 46:3, 149-165,
DOI: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1021661
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00958964.2015.1021661
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION, 46(3), 149–165, 2015
Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0095-8964 / 1940-1892 online
DOI: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1021661
Undergraduate Understanding of Climate Change: The
Influences of College Major and Environmental Group
Membership on Survey Knowledge Scores
Joanna K. Huxster
Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
Ximena Uribe-Zarain and Willett Kempton
University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware, USA
A survey covering the scientific and social aspects of climate change was administered to examine
U.S. undergraduate student mental models, and compare knowledge between groups based on major
and environmental group membership. A Knowledge Score (scale 0–35, mean score = 17.84) was
generated for respondents at two, central East Coast, U.S. universities (n = 465). Elements of student
mental models examined include environmental issue confusion, skepticism, and self-reported under-
standing. This study finds that students frequently confuse climate change with other environmental
issues, and that a substantial majority of students do not have an understanding of climate change
that closely matches the scientific model. These misconceptions extend to their understanding of
mitigation actions. Environmental group membership is shown to be a greater determinant of climate
change knowledge than enrollment in a science major.
Keywords climate change, mental models, public understanding of science, science literacy
The scientific community has reached a high level of consensus about the occurrence of anthro-
pogenic global climate change (IPCC, 2013; Oreskes, 2004; Rosenberg, Vedlitz, & Cowman,
2010). In the United States, outside of the scientific community, there continues to be consid-
erable debate and confusion surrounding the topic. Studies of public understanding of climate
change conducted in the 1990s by Kempton, Boster, & Hartley (1995) and Bostrom, Morgan,
Fischoff, & Read (1994) found that members of the general public frequently held inaccurate
mental models regarding the processes of climate change. Participants in these studies had of-
ten gathered misinformation and combined multiple environmental problems into their mental
models. Since the 1990s, climate change discussion and science have both advanced; however,
the level of public understanding has not noticeably increased. A study by the Yale Project on
Climate Change Communication (Leiserowitz, Smith, & Marlon, 2010) and a retest of Bostrom
et al.’s 1994 study (Reynolds, Bostrom, Read, & Morgan, 2010), both revealed little change in the
Correspondence should be sent to Joanna K. Huxster, Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA. E-mail:
jkh49@drexel.edu
Color versions of one or more figures in this article are available online at www.tandfonline.com/vjee.
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
150 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
understanding of the United States public. The new findings again included persistent confusion
between climate change and other environmental issues such as ozone depletion.
This article examines the knowledge and mental models of an important indicator popula-
tion, undergraduate students. They represent the future leaders and thinkers of the country, and,
as voters, they are capable of contributing to political and social change in the present. Their
understanding of this urgent, socioscientific issue is critical to the future of the global climate.
Significant recent work, such as that of Markowitz, Hodge, & Harp (2014), has been done to
increase climate change communication efforts. Understanding the mental models of respon-
dents in higher education will help evaluate the efficacy of the current education and science
communication efforts and identify ways in which they can be improved.
This study samples students nearing the end of their undergraduate programs and evaluates
the understanding they have accumulated and retained throughout their academic careers. Semi-
structured interviews (n = 18) were conducted to develop and refine survey questions at one
public, central East Coast, U.S. university. The resulting survey was administered to respondents
from two public, central East Coast, U.S. universities (n = 853, completed to end of survey n
= 465) and Knowledge Scores for these respondents were generated based on their responses.
In this article, these Knowledge Scores, as well as the knowledge elements of participant mental
models observed through interview and survey data, are analyzed.
AMERICAN ENVIRONMENTAL VALUES AND SCIENTIFIC
UNDERSTANDING
In their book Environmental Values in American Culture, Kempton et al. (1995) conduct semi-
structured interviews and surveys to understand American cultural models of the environment,
and of specific issues, including climate change. Kempton et al. (1995) define a cultural model or
mental model as a set of guidelines or an imaginary map each individual builds for his or herself
about any particular subject through which he or she can run scenarios, analyze information, and
reach conclusions (1995, p. 10). A further examination of the literature on mental models and
their implications can be found in the next section of this literature review.
In “Cultural Models of Weather and the Atmosphere” from Kempton et al. 1995, the authors
closely examine their informants’ cultural models of climate change (or as it was more commonly
called in 1995, the greenhouse effect). Overall, they find that the responses of their laypersons
do not correspond to scientific models. The authors describe ways in which informants modeled
climate change that are based upon models for previously existing concepts, all of which deviate
from the scientific understanding. Some of these concepts include greenhouse gases as pollution,
or the “pollution model,” and ozone depletion as the mechanism of climate change, or the “ozone
depletion model” (Kempton, Boster, & Hartley, 1995).
A 2010 study done by the Yale Project on Climate Change Communication reveals some
similar results to the Kempton et al. studies (Leiserowitz et al., 2010). In the Yale study, 2,030
adult members of the American public were surveyed on their knowledge of climate change and
were given grades (A–F) on their performance. A majority of those surveyed received Bs, Cs,
and Ds on their knowledge, with the largest proportion scoring Cs and only 2% earning an A.
The majority of the adults surveyed had heard of the “greenhouse effect” and could correctly
identify carbon dioxide as a greenhouse gas, but few understood how great the increase in carbon
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 151
dioxide in the atmosphere has been since 1850. A majority were found to correctly understand
that burning of fossil fuels contributes to climate change, but the study also finds that “majorities
of Americans. .. incorrectly believe that the hole in the ozone layer, toxic wastes, aerosol spray
cans, volcanic eruptions, the sun, and acid rain contribute to global warming” (2010, p. 11). The
results of their understanding could be seen in the respondents’ listed solutions. These included
both correct (reducing driving, switching to renewable energy sources) and incorrect (reducing
toxic waste, banning aerosol spray cans) actions.
The Yale study also documents the pervasiveness of climate skepticism in the American
publics’ understanding of climate change. Many of those surveyed did not believe that scientists
can predict future weather or that computer models are reliable. A third of those surveyed stated
that they did not believe that humans cause climate change. More than half of the respondents said
that they aren’t sure if climate change is happening, or that they do not believe it to be happening
at all. Over one third said that there is much disagreement among scientists about whether climate
change is happening. The respondents listed television, printed media, and the Internet as their
main sources of information about climate change, and a majority stated that they needed more
and better information on the subject (Leiserowitz et al., 2010).
Overall, the literature reveals a lack of scientific understanding of the processes of climate
change within the general public. The consequences of this lack of understanding tend to be
misinformation on the possible effects and severity of the issue, and the adoption of ineffective
mitigation actions, as well as possibly weakened support for governmental initiative on the issue.
RESEARCH QUESTIONS
1. What levels of knowledge, as measured by a calculated Knowledge Score, do respondents
at these two universities hold to explain climate change?
2. What knowledge elements of mental models are represented, and how do the respondents’
models compare to the scientific model of climate change?
3. How does the participants’ knowledge of climate change, as defined by the Knowl-
edge Score, differ between respondents in climate-related science majors, respondents
belonging to environmental groups, and respondents in neither category?
MENTAL MODEL THEORY AND CLIMATE CHANGE
Mental models are an important component of this study, as they are one of the tools by which
respondents’ understanding will be described and measured. Mental models, also called cultural
models or cognitive models, come from a subfield of anthropology known as cognitive anthro-
pology. D’Andrade (1995, p. 151) defines cognitive models as a “small-scale model” of external
reality and possible personal actions that people carry around in their heads. It “consists of an
interrelated set of elements which fit together to represent something” and this model is used to
reason or to calculate by mentally manipulating its parts to solve a problem. In cognitive science,
mental models are commonly used to explain human reasoning. It is theorized, “people do not
reason using abstract rules, but rather they construct and combine mental models and generate
inferences consistent with those models” (Bly & Rumelhart, 1999, p. 156). People can “run”
their mental models to come up with conclusions or solutions to a problem. Since mental models
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
152 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
are constructions of human thought, they often include simplifying assumptions (Mudditt, 1996).
Errors in conclusions, therefore, will likely not be random, but will instead correspond with gaps
or errors in mental models. Studies show that mental models, complete with gaps or distortions,
can be passed on, taught and transferred (Bly & Rumelhart, 1999).
Mental models are not only seen as agents of reasoning, but also are linked to motivation and
action. D’Andrade (1992, p. 23–24), in an essay from the book Human Motives and Cultural
Models, describes mental models as having a “motivational force” because they describe the
world, set forth both conscious and unconscious goals, and elicit desires. Holland and Quinn
(1987, p. 9) specifically address how mental models motivate behavior, expressing that the basis
of mental models’ directive force is in the authority and expertise with which they are invested
and within the “intrinsic persuasiveness” the models hold for us. Indeed, the influence of mental
models on the actions and perceptions of their holders can be seen in a variety of studies (Gentner
& Stevens, 2014). The examples range from people’s interactions with their thermostat settings,
which is influenced by mental models informants hold for how the thermostat works (Kempton,
1987) to middle school students’ understanding of the greenhouse effect (Shepardson, Choi,
Niyogi, & Charusombat, 2011) to students’ models of their personal affect on the environment
and the behaviors those mental models produce (Liu & Lin, 2015).
This research will use respondent survey responses to reconstruct and examine the knowledge
elements of participant mental models of climate change. Mental and cultural model theory has
been used as the theoretical framework for numerous studies regarding public understanding
of climate change. In Kempton (1991) and Kempton et al. (1995), the studies most influencing
the methodology of the present research, semi-structured interviews and survey results were
used to infer mental models of climate change held by members of the general public. The
Kempton et al. (1995) study found that “laypersons” hold several overarching models to ex-
plain climate change. These same models were found in research conducted by Bostrom et al.
(1994) and in a retest by Reynolds et al. (2010) using a similar survey instrument. In 2010, Zia
and Todd used mental and cultural model theory to examine and map the belief networks of
members of the public as related to their ideological, religious, socioeconomic, and educational
background.
These and other studies set the methodological and conceptual precedent for the use of mental
model theory to explain understanding of climate change in this study. This article analyzes the
knowledge portion of a larger dissertation study (Huxster, 2013).
DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY
The methodology of this research contains elements of both ethnography (from cultural an-
thropology) and survey research, like the work done by Kemptonand colleagues in their 1995
book. Kempton et al. (1995) conducted semi-structured interviews of members of the general
public. The ideas, reasoning, and mental models expressed in these interviews were then used
to construct survey statements to be administered to a larger population. This methodology, also
described in Bernard (2002), allows the researcher to test the existence of the mental models
seen in semi-structured interviews across a larger population. The mixed methods analysis of
Kempton et al.’s (1995) data included the examination of informant quotations and survey re-
sponses to infer the mental models of the sampled population regarding climate change. This
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 153
methodology is applied in the present study to illuminate the mental models of U.S. undergraduate
students, assess their knowledge, and understanding of climate change, and answer the research
questions.
Prior to the development of the survey instrument, a pilot study was conducted consisting
of semi-structured interviews of 18 university students. Semi-structured interviews include a
pre-determined set of questions, but the interviewer is able to ask further questions and probe
for more complete answers, depending on how the informants respond (e.g., Agar, 1980). The
interviews conducted for this study were constructed of open-ended questions aimed at gathering
information about students’ understanding, knowledge, and lines of reasoning regarding climate
change. Transcripts of these semi-structured interviews were reviewed, and the mental models
of these informants were inferred from the lines of reasoning displayed. This process involved
the identification and systematic counting of keywords, statements, and ideas, and reconstruction
of participant perceptions and models based on inferred logic. The comments and arguments of
the informant mental models were then condensed into one-sentence statements on the survey.1
Representative statements from all of the displayed mental models were included to insure that
even the models seen infrequently in the 18 interviews were represented and could be tested in
the overall student population. Survey respondents were asked to indicate their agreement to the
survey statements on a 5-point Likert scale.2
Using this methodology for survey creation results in some multi-part statement wording not
used in simple opinion surveys. Since the statements on this survey draw from the interviews
with the same population, the wording can be informal yet appropriate to the student interview
subjects. Also, since we are attempting to elicit mental models rather than isolated beliefs, some
survey statements include multipart ideas that attempt to capture the mental models and lines
of reasoning displayed in the pilot study interviews. As noted by Bernard (2002) and Kempton
et al. (1995), this survey item methodology is used for testing for lines of reasoning or multi-part
mental models; the items are not intended to elicit individual components facts. The included
survey questions can be seen in Table 1 and the full survey and interview instruments can be
found in Huxster (2013).
The informants in the semi-structured interviews held several different mental models, but
their reasoning and understanding could be categorized into three overarching models: predom-
inantly scientific understanding, environmental issue confusion, and natural causes and cycles.
After the survey was administered, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were performed
to provide evidence of instrument and construct validity, and it was found that the survey did
measure the three main components of mental models observed in the pilot study. In the confir-
matory factor analysis performed with varimax rotation, three factors were identified (Table 1).
These three factors fit closely with the models seen in the semi-structured interviews. The first
factor includes almost all of the scientifically correct statements about the mechanisms, causes,
scientific consensus, and severity of climate change and has been labeled “Predominantly Sci-
entific Understanding.” The items loading into the second factor are statements that attribute the
causes or mechanisms of various environmental issues to climate change, but these causes are not
actually significant to the issue. This factor is labeled “Issue Confusion.” The statements in the
third factor all refer to skepticism about the severity and scientific certainty of climate change, as
well as statements expressing a belief that natural causes are responsible for climate change. This
factor has been labeled “Natural Causes and Skepticism.” One survey item was discarded after
the factor analyses owing to a misprint in the Likert scale wording. A more detailed explanation
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
154 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
TABLE 1
Factor Loadings of the Rotated Factors for Knowledge Variables
Sci Und.
Issue
Conf
Nat Cause
and Skep
2.40 We need to change our transportation system, like creating more public
transportation, in order to make a big impact on reducing climate change
.710
2.37 Individuals can reduce climate change by not buying gas-guzzling cars
and instead maybe owning a more fuel-efficient car or hybrid
.687
2.43 The government could put more resources into developing alternative
and renewable sources of energy in order to reduce climate change
.672
2.42 I’ve heard that using efficient light bulbs, turning off electric appliances
and insulating my house are all ways I can reduce my contribution to
climate change
.624
2.25 Even though it’s sometimes debated, I feel that it’s pretty well
documented that humans have released too much carbon dioxide into the
atmosphere
.619
2.32 The government could tax carbon emissions or introduce a system of cap
and trade in an effort to reduce climate change
.616
2.30 The best way to deal with climate change would be to reduce or
eliminate carbon-based fuel sources
.611
2.23 Burning fossil fuels releases carbon dioxide into the atmosphere, which
traps heat and causes climate change
.600
2.31 Individuals can give political support to clean energy initiatives to help
deal with climate change
.571
2.39 Cutting down on waste and pollution will help stop the melting of the ice
caps
.512 .401
2.74 Even if the science isn’t clear to me, I think it’s better to act and be safe
rather than sorry
.508
2.27 I’ve heard that driving less and using bicycles and public transportation
are ways to reduce climate change
.507
2.44 Recycling is essential to reducing climate change because it keeps
plastics out of landfills and reduces production
.506
2.18 Climate change is caused by an increased amount of gases like carbon
dioxide and methane in the atmosphere
.502
2.75 It’s hard for scientists to be certain about anything, but I think the patterns
and trends that they’re seeing in climate change have them pretty convinced
.443
2.68 I think that it’s generally pretty well accepted that climate change is
occurring
.425
2.14 Gas and chemical emissions from our cars deplete the ozone and cause
climate change
.782
2.11 Climate change is happening because we are depleting the ozone layer,
and that lets in more heat from the sun
.778
2.16 Greenhouse gases make the hole in the ozone layer worse .724
2.15 Toxic pollution in the atmosphere causes climate change .710
2.21 Climate change happens because we release chemicals from aerosol cans
into the atmosphere
.681
2.36 I can help prevent climate change by not buying or using aerosol cans .625
2.22 Nuclear power plants create pollution that contributes to climate change .608
2.35 The government can reduce climate change by regulating toxic pollution .444 .561
(Continued on next page)
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 155
TABLE 1
Factor Loadings of the Rotated Factors for Knowledge Variables (Continued)
Sci Und.
Issue
Conf
Nat Cause
and Skep
2.19 Climate change involves magnetic field and fluctuations in the sun, and
other things beyond our control, and those things are bigger factors than
greenhouse gas emissions
.672
2.70 The climate has changed a lot in the past, so I think we might be making
too big a deal about how it’s changing now
.667
2.62 From my sources I’ve learned that too much emphasis is put on the
human element of climate change, rather than the natural elements
.654
2.9 I think that climate change concerns are exaggerated more than the science
supports
.637
2.69 I think that scientists are certain that the climate is changing, but not
about who or what is responsible
.616
2.20 Natural thing like volcanoes and water vapor have more of an impact on
climate change than human actions
.613
2.73 I don’t think that climate change is the most pressing environmental issue
we face today, and I think that it gets too much press
.612
2.10 Scientists are highly certain that humans are definitely the cause of
current, rapid climate change
.535
2.12 Climate change is caused by the greenhouse effect –.450
Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.
Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
of the confirmatory factor analyses can be found in the supplementary material accompanying
the online article.
The survey was administered to a sample of junior and senior students at two public, central
East Coast universities. These respondents were selected for the survey as they have spent three
or four years studying their individual majors and therefore better represent the influence that a
student’s major may have on their understanding of climate change. Systematic sampling was
used to gather a representative sample of students and the survey was conducted online using
Qualtrics survey software. Eight hundred fifty-three respondents from the two universities took
the survey, with 465 respondents from the two universities completing the entire survey. For
the comparison of climate-related science majors and non-science majors, a selection of majors
was made including majors relating to environmental science and policy, chemistry, biology, and
geology.
In order to acquire sufficient response rates from majors of interest with fewer students, the
geology majors and the Environmental Science and Policy Group were sampled 100%. For
all other majors, proportional stratified sampling was used. Within the departmental subgroups,
systematic sampling was used to select the final sample, with an interval of N = 4. In the statistical
analysis of the survey results, those majors that were sampled at 100% with high response rates
were weighted at .25 when used in conjunction with the other majors. Following the researchers’
sampling, the registrar’s office of each University performed the selection of students from the
appropriate majors, and sent them e-mails asking them to take the online survey. The identities
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
156 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
of the individual respondents sampled were not revealed to the researcher and no incentives
were offered to the students for participation. To ensure higher numbers of participants in the
selected majors of interest, respondents from capstone courses in those respective majors were
also sampled based on identification of those students by their professors. Beyond the initial
survey invitation and reminder emails sent to the entire sample, the researcher did not contact
individual respondents.
The survey’s Knowledge Variables, analyzed here, include both correct and incorrect state-
ments about the mechanisms of climate change, as well as the actions that can be taken to
prevent or mitigate climate change. In a socioscientific issue such as climate change, the so-
cial components cannot be completely separated from the science (Klosterman & Sadler, 2009).
The survey Knowledge Variables also involve those social components most closely associated
with the scientific model and that can be coded as correct and incorrect in comparison with the
scientific model. The social aspects of these Knowledge Variables are most clearly represented
in statements dealing with scientific consensus and government action. A complete list of the
Knowledge Variables is included in the factor analysis in Table 1.
As part of the analysis of the Knowledge Variables, a “Knowledge Score” was generated
for each respondent based on respondents’ agreement and disagreement with scientifically cor-
rect and incorrect statements. The Knowledge Score was calculated from 35 survey statements.
Students received 1 “point” for each statement matching the scientific model of climate change
to which they agreed. Students also received 1 “point” for each statement not matching the
scientific model to which they disagreed. This created a score on a scale of 0 to 35. Only
the answers of the 465 participants who completed the survey in its entirety were included
in the calculation of the Knowledge Score. The mean Knowledge Scores of science and non-
science students were compared using a t-test, as were the mean knowledge scores of environ-
mental group members and respondents not belonging to an environmental group. Two-Way
Analysis of Variance was used to compare mean Knowledge Scores between four “Sets” of
respondents:
1. Science major and Environmental group
2. Science major and Non-environmental group
3. Non-Science major and Environmental group
4. Non-science major and Non-environmental group.
Thus, we can compare the influence of major and environmental group on climate change
knowledge.
In addition to the calculation and analysis of the Knowledge Scores, responses to indi-
vidual survey items dealing with the three overarching types of mental models seen in the
interviews are analyzed. The first set of statements examined are those similar to the scien-
tific model, and are included as examples of respondents’ responses to individual parts of the
model. Statements showing confusion between climate change and other environmental issues
were included to illustrate ways in which respondents’ mental models differ from the scientific
model. Incorrect statements attributing current, rapid climate change3
to natural causes were
included in this analysis to show the role skepticism plays in respondents’ mental models. Fi-
nally, survey statements asking participants to self-report their knowledge and understanding
of climate change were included to examine how their self-assessment fits into their mental
models.
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 157
TABLE 2
Descriptive Statistics for Two Public, Central East Coast
Universities Knowledge Scores
N 465
Mean 17.84
Median 18.00
Mode 16.00
Std. Deviation 5.40
Variance 29.26
RESULTS AND ANALYSIS
The creation of the Knowledge Score allowed for the evaluation of climate change knowledge of
the respondents, and comparison of knowledge across specific groups of respondents. Incomplete
surveys were excluded from this analysis, as their scores skewed the results. The mean Knowledge
Score (scale 0–35) of the 465 respondents (both universities) was 17.84 with a standard deviation
of 5.40. The highest score received was a 35, indicating all answers matched the scientific model,
and the lowest was a 0, indicating that no answers matched the scientific model. The statistics for
the Knowledge scores can be seen in Table 2 and the score distribution can be seen in Figure 1.
Comparison of Knowledge Scores Between Groups
Descriptive statistics were performed and compared between groups of respondents based on
their science education and their environmental group membership. The first comparison was the
science majors as compared with non-science majors. An independent samples t-test was run and
FIGURE 1 Student respondent knowledge score distribution.
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
158 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
FIGURE 2 Mean knowledge scores for four “core groups.”
a significant difference was found between the means of the two groups (t(448) = 3.46, p < .05).
The mean Knowledge Score of the science majors was significantly higher (m = 19.60, sd =
5.98) than the non-science majors (m = 17.43, sd = 5.19). Based on these results, it can be said
that respondents in the selected science majors, unsurprisingly, have a greater understanding of
climate change science than respondents who were not science majors.
The next comparison was between participants who belong to an environmental group with
those who do not. An independent samples t-test comparing the two found a significant difference
between the means (t(456) = 4.44, p < .05). The mean Knowledge Score of environmental group
respondents was significantly higher (m = 20.11, sd = 5.04) than that of the non-environmental
group respondents (m = 17.36, sd = 5.35).
In order to examine the effects of both environmental group membership and science major, a
2 (Environmental Group Membership) X 2(Science Major) between-subjects factorial ANOVA
was calculated comparing the Knowledge Scores of the respondents. Environmental group mem-
bership and science major were each shown to have significant main effects. The respondent Sets
and their corresponding mean scores are as follows:
1. Science major and Environmental group (m = 21.38),
2. Science major and Non-environmental group (m = 18.36),
3. Non-Science major and Environmental group (m = 19.5),
4. Non-science major and Non-environmental group (m = 17.13).
A significant difference was found between the groups F(3, 447) = 9.60, p < .05) and
post hoc analysis shows that environmental group membership is a greater indicator of a high
Knowledge Score than enrollment in a science major. The interaction between environmental
group membership and science major was not significant (F(1, 447) = 6.36, p > .05). A graph
of the mean scores (Figure 2) shows an obvious difference between the mean of Core Group
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 159
1, Science major and Environmental group, and that of Core Group 4, Non-science major and
Non-environmental group.
Specific Elements of Respondent Mental Models
Specific statements from the survey were of particular interest in inferring elements of respon-
dent’s mental models of climate change. The selection of these specific statements was based on
the mental models observed in the responses of students in the pilot study interviews. During
the interview process, students were able to describe correctly some aspects of climate change,
but also showed significant issue confusion, in which they conflated climate change with other
environmental issues such as ozone depletion. A few of the students interviewed expressed that
natural causes play a significant role in recent climate change, while others indicated that they
know little about how climate change occurs (Huxster, 2013). Representative statements from
these lines of reasoning are presented here to test the prevalence of these specific aspects of student
mental models across the student population. Separating and presenting these contradictory sets
of statements also illustrates some of the interesting overlaps in student agreement seen between
scientifically correct and scientifically incorrect statements. This serves to further explain and fill
out the outline of their mental models of climate change.
Statements Matching the Scientific Model
First, responses to survey statements matching different aspects of the scientific model are
examined to gain a clear picture of how respondents understand the causes of climate change
and the actions that can be taken. Next, responses to statements of different models of climate
change, specifically those that confuse climate change with other environmental issues and those
attributing climate change to natural causes, are listed to investigate respondents’ subscription
to those models. Finally, respondents’ responses to statements that self-identify confusion or
ignorance about climate change are considered.
Table 3a shows response percentages to survey items matching the scientific model of climate
change.4
The responses to statement 2.10 show that just over half of the respondents understand
the high level of scientific consensus that exists about climate change. Almost 30% of the
respondents are unsure about the scientific agreement, and almost 20% do not agree that scientific
agreement exists. This shows some lack of understanding of the scientific community’s views.
A more positive indicator for respondents’ understanding of climate change is that 77% of the
respondents agree with statement 2.23, that the burning of fossil fuels contributes to climate
change.
Complete comprehension of the scientific model of climate change also includes understanding
of the mitigation actions that can be taken. Responses to survey statements matching the scientific
model of actions to help prevent or lessen climate change are shown in Table 3b. The majority of
the respondents have heard of energy efficiency measures they can enact in their own homes, and
know that alternative and renewable energy sources are important parts to the solution to climate
change. A smaller majority was able to identify that the reduction or elimination of carbon-based
fuel sources is the best way to mitigate climate change. The difference in responses between
2.30 and 2.43 suggests that although respondents have retained the information that renewable
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
160 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
TABLE 3
Responses to Survey Statements Grouped by Model Element
3a. Responses to Statements Matching the Scientific Model of Climate Change
Item Agree Neither Disagree N
2.10 Scientists are highly certain that humans are definitely
the cause of current, rapid climate change
53.2% 29.0% 17.8% 682
2.18 Climate change is caused by an increased amount of
gases like carbon dioxide and methane in the atmosphere
75.1% 21.5% 3.4% 610
2.23 Burning fossil fuels releases carbon dioxide into the
atmosphere, which traps heat and causes climate change
77.0% 19.6% 3.3% 566
3b. Responses to Statements Matching the Scientific Model of Mitigation Actions
2.30 The best way to deal with climate change would be to
reduce or eliminate carbon-based fuel sources
59.2% 32.0% 8.8% 522
2.42 I’ve heard that using efficient light bulbs, turning off
electric appliances and insulating my house are all ways I
can reduce my contribution to climate change
91.7% 4.9% 3.4% 494
2.43 The government could put more resources into
developing alternative and renewable sources of energy in
order to reduce climate change
84.9% 10.8% 4.2% 489
3c. Responses to Statements Confusing Climate Change with Ozone Depletion
2.11 Climate change is happening because we are depleting
the ozone layer, and that lets in more heat from the sun.
43.0% 26.9% 30.1% 672
2.14 Gas and chemical emissions from our cars deplete the
ozone and cause climate change.
66.8% 17.4% 15.7% 648
2.16 Greenhouse gases make the hole in the ozone layer worse 54.8% 23.2% 21.9% 633
2.36 I can help prevent climate change by not buying or using
aerosol cans
50.1% 28.7% 21.1% 505
3d. Responses to Statements Confusing Climate Change with Toxic Pollution
2.15 Toxic pollution in the atmosphere makes climate change
worse
65.2% 24.0% 10.7% 641
2.35 The government can reduce climate change by regulating
toxic pollution
65.7% 21.7% 12.7% 507
2.39 Cutting down on waste and pollution will help stop the
melting of the ice caps
54.4% 31.8% 13.8% 500
3e. Responses to Statements attributing Climate Change to Natural Causes
2.19 Climate change involves magnetic field and fluctuations
in the sun, and other things beyond our control, and those
things are bigger factor than greenhouse gas emissions.
25.4% 47.1% 27.5% 594
2.20 Natural things like volcanoes and water vapor have more
of an impact on climate change than human action.
16.3% 41.7% 42.0% 588
3f. Responses to Statements Expressing Confusion or Ignorance about Climate Change
2.13 I know that changes in the atmosphere cause climate
change, but I don’t know very much about the scientific
processes that make it happen
45.3% 18.2% 36.5% 658
2.24 No one has ever really explained to me how the
greenhouse effect works, or if they have I can’t really
remember
27.6% 11.6% 60.8% 562
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 161
resources are an important solution, fewer respondents understand why these alternatives are
important or why non-renewable energy sources exacerbate the problem.
Statements Displaying Issue Confusion
Despite the high number of responses indicating that fossil fuels play a role in climate change,
responses to several statements dealing with the confusion of climate change with other envi-
ronmental issues reinforce the mental models observed in the pilot study interviews. Some of
the statements dealing with the confusion of climate change and ozone depletion are seen in
Table 3c. The percentages of agreement with these statements indicate significant confusion in
respondents’ mental models between climate change and ozone depletion. The responses were
relatively split on statement 2.11, with a slight majority agreeing that depletion of the ozone
layer causes climate change. However, more than half of the respondents incorrectly believed
that greenhouse gases contribute to the hole in the ozone layer. Two thirds of the respondents
have incorporated new information about the contribution of car emissions to climate change into
their ozone depletion model of climate change, as can be seen in statement 2.14. The confusion
of climate change and ozone depletion affects not only respondents’ understanding of causes of
climate change, but also understanding of prevention measures. Half of the respondents mistak-
enly believe that limiting their use of aerosol cans will help to prevent climate change. The issue
confusion between climate change and ozone depletion as seen in this study is similar to issues
encountered in previous studies of public understanding of climate change (Bostrom et al., 1994;
Kempton et al., 1995). The confusion surprisingly has persisted over 20 years, despite public
coverage and despite these respondents having taken science courses, presumably with lessons
to the contrary, (Reynolds et al., 2010).
Another environmental issue commonly confused with climate change in the pilot study and
in previous literature is that of toxic pollution or waste. In Table 3d, statements illustrating this
confusion are listed. For both statements 2.15 and 2.35, nearly two thirds of the respondents
agreed that toxic pollution is a cause of climate change and that the regulation of these prod-
ucts will help combat climate change. In a statement directly linking toxic waste and pollution
to a commonly understood effect of climate change, the melting of the ice caps, more than
half of the respondents expressed agreement. This mental model of climate change has been
noted, and called the “Pollution model,” in previous research (Bostrom et al., 1994; Kemp-
ton et al., 1995; Reynolds et al., 2010). The issue confusion seen with the ozone depletion
model and the pollution model of climate change in this study may be a case of lumping any
generally “good environmental practice” into the category of preventing climate change. In
1994, Bostrom et al. found that in the absence of sufficient information, people tend to agree
with all practices that they see as good for the environment to explain how to combat climate
change.
Statements of Climate Skepticism and Self-Reported Knowledge
Climate change skepticism promotes the mental model of natural occurrences, or cycles, as the
primary causes of climate change. Agreement with statements of this model can be seen in Table
3e. For the first of the two statements in this table, the majority of respondents indicated that
they neither agree nor disagree that natural elements beyond human control are bigger factors in
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
162 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
climate change than greenhouse gas emissions. In the second statement concerning water vapor
and volcanic eruptions, the response rates seen for “neither” and for “disagree” were almost
equal. These results may mean that there is some confusion among the respondents about the role
that natural occurrences play in climate change. For both statements, however, more respondents
disagreed than agreed with the statements that natural occurrences are more important factors in
climate change than human actions.
The final set of survey statements analyzed in this study, shown in Table 3f, are included to
illustrate respondents’ self-reported understanding of climate change. For statement 2.13, a slight
majority of respondents agreed that they do not feel they understand the scientific processes of
climate change. Larger majorities of respondents, however, disagreed with statements indicating
they had not learned about the greenhouse effect or that they did not know how they personally
affect climate change. These results show that a majority of respondents believe that they know the
correct ways in which to combat climate change. It has been shown, however, that self-reported
informedness does not correlate with objective measures of subject knowledge (Durant & Legge,
2005). Given the high number of respondents holding incorrect models of climate change as
indicated by responses to statements of issue confusion, this may be the case in this study.
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
In the overall population at the two universities represented in this study, the respondents show
some basic understanding of the scientific processes of climate change. The mean knowledge
score falls at 17.84 on a scale of 0–35. In terms of college “grading,” this is close to 50%
on a grading scale. These scores indicate that the answer to the first research question is that
students’ knowledge leaves much to be desired. Further research might look at respondent levels
of understanding, learning progressions of university students, and what levels of understanding
are necessary for correct and informed action, as suggested in Mohan, Chen, and Anderson’s
(2009) study of high school students.
The second research question asks how student knowledge and mental models compare to
the scientific model of climate change. In looking at specific statements made in the pilot study
interviews and the responses to corresponding questions from the survey, further detail in the
knowledge elements of respondents’ mental models is revealed. Three quarters of the respon-
dents were able to identify the burning of fossil fuels as a source of greenhouse gases and most
respondents knew that energy efficiency measures and renewable resources were potential solu-
tions. However, only half of the respondents understood that there is a high level of scientific
agreement about human impact on the global climate. Also, subtle changes in the wording of
similar questions about fossil fuels (i.e., calling them “carbon-based fuels”) may have influenced
responses. Overall, these results indicate that gaps exist in the knowledge elements of students’
mental models.
In addition to these gaps identified in respondent understanding of the scientific model, the
respondents also show a great deal of confusion between climate change and other environmental
issues. This confusion of issues is seen not only in respondents’ understanding of the causes
of climate change, but also in their understanding of the solutions and actions to be taken for
climate change. The areas of confusion, specifically in respondents’ ozone depletion model and
the pollution model, closely match those seen in previous studies of the public, dating back to
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 163
the early 1990s. These findings indicate that misconceptions in respondents’ mental models have
persisted and these misconceptions need attention to ensure that these young members of society
are prepared to tackle this serious issue effectively. One positive finding is that although the
role of natural causes has led to some confusion in respondent mental models, climate change
skepticism doesn’t seem to play nearly as large a role in the undergraduates’ mental models as has
the persistence of environmental issue confusion. Understanding the specific nature of the gaps
and misconceptions in the knowledge elements of mental models can better inform education,
both formal and informal, on this matter.
The third research question asks about the differences in student mental models between col-
lege majors and environmental group participants. Predictably, when looking at different groups
of respondents, those belonging to the related science majors are shown to have a higher level
of knowledge than those students not belonging to science majors. The finding that those re-
spondents belonging to environmental groups score higher on the Knowledge Score scale than
those not belonging to such groups could also have been predicted. What is more surprising,
however, is that environmental group membership is a greater indicator of climate change knowl-
edge (as defined by the Knowledge Score) than enrollment in a science major. This research
does not cover how these respondents reached this higher level of knowledge, or what causes
respondents in environmental groups to have a greater understanding. It is possible that these
findings could be attributed to the education and information respondents receive from envi-
ronmental groups, but we consider it unlikely that environmental groups are better sources of
information than science classes. Students in both a climate science-related major and an envi-
ronmental group have the highest mean knowledge scores. Another possible explanation for the
differences in scores could be that respondents’ greater understanding is a function of a higher
level of interest and concern for climate change, which is manifested in the respondents’ choice
of major and environmental group participation. These hypothesized relationships could also
explain why environmental group membership is a greater predictor of high Knowledge Score
than enrollment in a science major. Further study of the information covered in specific classes
and disseminated through environmental groups, as well as in students’ motives in choosing
college majors and belonging to an environmental group, would be needed to test this possible
explanation.
Further research is needed to sort through the possible explanations for the understanding
of science students and environmental group members. More important, the respondents who
likely need the most attention are those not belonging to either of these groups. The non-science,
non-environmental students represent the majority of the respondents (64%). The findings of this
research indicate that these respondents are either not receiving or not retaining the information
necessary to have an adequate level of understanding of the issue. If they are to contribute
meaningfully to climate change solutions as citizens, they will likely need to have a greater
understanding than shown in this study.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Dr. John Madsen for assistance in acquiring partial funding for this research from
the National Science Foundation grant for Research on Learning in Formal and Informal
Settings.
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
164 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE
NOTES
1. The work reported here draws on only the knowledge portion of the survey from Huxster 2013. Elsewhere
the survey is referred to as the “Survey of Student Understanding of Climate Change.”
2. For the purposes of reporting the results, statement responses for “Agree and Strongly Agree” and
“Disagree and Strongly Disagree” were condensed together in Table 3
3. Survey respondents were informed that the use of the word “climate change” was used in place of the term
“global warming” and represented the same phenomenon. In cases in which it was necessary to be more
specific, the phrase “current, rapid climate change” was used. The survey was framed to respondents as
an exploration of their opinions on environmental issues.
4. In reporting responses to individual survey statements, all students responding to that element are included
(even those who might not have completed the entire survey) to give the most complete analysis possible.
The n values in table 3 reflect the number of students responding to each survey item
REFERENCES
Agar, M. H. (1980). The professional stranger: An informal introduction to ethnography. New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Bernard, R. H. (2002). Research methods in anthropology: Qualitative and quantitative approaches, (3rd ed.). Lanham,
MD: Altamira Press.
Bly, B. M., & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.). (1999). Cognitive science. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischoff, B., & Read, D. (1994). What do people know about climate change? 1. Mental
models, 2. Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 959–982.
D’Andrade, R. (1992). Schemas and motivation. In R. D’Andrade & C. Strauss (Eds.), Human motives and cultural
models (pp. 22–44). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
D’Andrade, R. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Durant, R. F., & Legge Jr., J. S. (2005). Public opinion, risk perceptions, and genetically modified food regulatory policy.
European Union Politics, 6(1), 181–200.
Gentner, D., & Stevens, A. L. (2014). Mental models. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Holland, D., & Quinn, N. (1987). Culture and cognition. In N. Quinn & D. Holland (Eds.), Cultural models in language
and thought (pp. 3–40). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Huxster, Joanna K. (2013). Student understanding of climate change: Influences of college major and environmental
group membership on undergraduate knowledge and mental models (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest.
(Publication Number 3594926).
IPCC. (2013). Climate change 2013: The physical science basis summary of policy makers. Contribution of working
group I to the fourth assessment report of the intergovernmental panel on climate change. [Stocker, T., Dahe, Q., &
Gian-Kasper, P. (Coordinating Lead Authors)] Retrieved from www.ipcc.ch
Kempton, W. (1987). Two theories of home heat control. In N. Quinn & D. Holland (Eds.), Cultural models in language
and thought (pp. 222–242). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Kempton, W. (1991). Public understanding of global warming. Society and Natural Resources, 4, 331–345.
Kempton, W., Boster, J. S., & Hartley, J. A. (1995). Environmental values in American culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Klosterman, M. L., & Sadler, T. D. (2009). Multi-level assessment of scientific content knowledge gains associated with
socioscientific issues-based instruction. International Journal of Science Education, Research Report.
Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N., & Marlon, J. R. (2010). Americans’ knowledge of climate change. Yale University, New
Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication.
Liu, S. C., & Lin, H. (2015) Exploring undergraduate students’ mental models of the environment: Are they related to
environmental affect and behavior? Journal of Environmental Education, 46(1), 23–40.
Markowitz, E., Hodge, C., & Harp, G. (2014). Connecting on climate: A guide: A guide to effective climate change
communication. Columbia University, New York, NY: Center for Research on Environmental Decisions.
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
HUXSTER ET AL. 165
Mohan, L., Chen, J., & Anderson, W. A. (2009). Developing a multi-year learning progression for carbon cycling in
socio-ecological systems. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 675–698.
Mudditt, A. (1996). Cognitive science: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers.
Oreskes, N. (2004). Beyond the ivory tower: The scientific consensus on climate change. Science, 306(5702), 1686.
Reynolds, T. W., Bostrom, A., Read, D., & Morgan, M. G. (2010). Now what do people know about climate change?
Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis, 30(10), 1520–1538.
Rosenberg, S., Vedlitz, A., Cowman, D. F., & Zahran, S. (2010). Climate change: A profile of U.S. climate perspectives.
Climatic Change, 101, 311–329.
Shepardson, D. P., Choi, S., Niyogi, D., & Charusombat, U. (2011). Seventh grade students’ mental models of the
greenhouse effect. Environmental Education Research, 17(1) 1–17.
Zia, A., & Todd, A. M. (2010). Evaluating the effects of ideology on public understanding of climate change science:
How to improve communication across ideological divides? Public Understanding of Science, 19(6) 743–761.
Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015

More Related Content

What's hot

International Environmental Governance
International Environmental GovernanceInternational Environmental Governance
International Environmental GovernanceAmeliaBahr
 
Research Project - Anthony Winston - Final
Research Project - Anthony Winston - FinalResearch Project - Anthony Winston - Final
Research Project - Anthony Winston - FinalTony Winston
 
Nature and their life course
Nature and their life courseNature and their life course
Nature and their life course
Pjerin_Marku
 
Relationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green Behaviors
Relationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green BehaviorsRelationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green Behaviors
Relationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green Behaviors
Francesca Householder
 
Curricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by by Garima Tandon
Curricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by  by Garima TandonCurricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by  by Garima Tandon
Curricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by by Garima Tandon
garimatandon10
 
Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09
Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09
Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09
Louise Smyth
 
Nisbet aaas sept2016
Nisbet aaas sept2016Nisbet aaas sept2016
Nisbet aaas sept2016
Matthew Nisbet
 
Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...
Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...
Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...
Cassie Wandersee
 
Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010
Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010
Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010Mierza Miranti
 
Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...
Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...
Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...
Ecologistas en Accion
 
Speaker Selection
Speaker SelectionSpeaker Selection
International Journal of Music Education-2015-Rickert
International Journal of Music Education-2015-RickertInternational Journal of Music Education-2015-Rickert
International Journal of Music Education-2015-RickertDale Rickert
 
Humans & the environment 2012 syllabus
Humans & the environment 2012 syllabusHumans & the environment 2012 syllabus
Humans & the environment 2012 syllabusAlbert
 
internship environmental toxicology
 internship environmental toxicology  internship environmental toxicology
internship environmental toxicology
University of Richmond - Environmental Studies
 
Hartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference Poster
Hartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference PosterHartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference Poster
Hartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference Poster
Jenna Hartley
 
Heet & Parth research paper
Heet & Parth research paperHeet & Parth research paper
Heet & Parth research paper
Heet Patel
 
Canterbury paper
Canterbury paperCanterbury paper
Canterbury paperDavid Dixon
 

What's hot (20)

International Environmental Governance
International Environmental GovernanceInternational Environmental Governance
International Environmental Governance
 
Research Project - Anthony Winston - Final
Research Project - Anthony Winston - FinalResearch Project - Anthony Winston - Final
Research Project - Anthony Winston - Final
 
Nature and their life course
Nature and their life courseNature and their life course
Nature and their life course
 
Relationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green Behaviors
Relationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green BehaviorsRelationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green Behaviors
Relationships Between Perceived Risk, Responsibility, and Green Behaviors
 
Curricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by by Garima Tandon
Curricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by  by Garima TandonCurricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by  by Garima Tandon
Curricular initiiatives in india after ncf 2005 by by Garima Tandon
 
Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09
Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09
Annual nsf gk 12 report u toledo 4-7-09
 
Nisbet aaas sept2016
Nisbet aaas sept2016Nisbet aaas sept2016
Nisbet aaas sept2016
 
Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...
Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...
Masters Thesis Proposal: An Audience Focused Approach to Framing Climate Chan...
 
Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010
Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010
Incorporating the global warming issue into a ktsp paper-teflin 2010
 
Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...
Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...
Climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community (levandowsky...
 
Speaker Selection
Speaker SelectionSpeaker Selection
Speaker Selection
 
International Journal of Music Education-2015-Rickert
International Journal of Music Education-2015-RickertInternational Journal of Music Education-2015-Rickert
International Journal of Music Education-2015-Rickert
 
Humans & the environment 2012 syllabus
Humans & the environment 2012 syllabusHumans & the environment 2012 syllabus
Humans & the environment 2012 syllabus
 
Final SWRM Poster
Final SWRM PosterFinal SWRM Poster
Final SWRM Poster
 
internship environmental toxicology
 internship environmental toxicology  internship environmental toxicology
internship environmental toxicology
 
Hartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference Poster
Hartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference PosterHartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference Poster
Hartley, 2020 IASNR/ISSRM Conference Poster
 
Heet & Parth research paper
Heet & Parth research paperHeet & Parth research paper
Heet & Parth research paper
 
Dunlap Plan B
Dunlap Plan BDunlap Plan B
Dunlap Plan B
 
Canterbury paper
Canterbury paperCanterbury paper
Canterbury paper
 
UNC Water and Health Conference 2011: Heterogeneity in trial data: learning f...
UNC Water and Health Conference 2011: Heterogeneity in trial data: learning f...UNC Water and Health Conference 2011: Heterogeneity in trial data: learning f...
UNC Water and Health Conference 2011: Heterogeneity in trial data: learning f...
 

Viewers also liked

Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003
Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003
Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003
Mariafer Ortiz
 
Madison Listening Session Presentation
Madison Listening Session PresentationMadison Listening Session Presentation
Madison Listening Session PresentationTravis Halm
 
Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)
Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)
Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)
alvaro celis bedoya
 
Practica 3
Practica 3 Practica 3
Practica 3
Brenda Nicoramz
 
Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)
Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)
Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)Mr. Malcolm E. Freeman Jr.
 
Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017
Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017
Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017
Enjeuxformation
 
2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins
2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins
2014 BPI presentation_WigginsBrian Wiggins
 
2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins
2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins
2015 MSSJ presentation_WigginsBrian Wiggins
 
Hydraulic pumps(compelete )
Hydraulic pumps(compelete )Hydraulic pumps(compelete )
Hydraulic pumps(compelete )
Ahmed El-emam
 

Viewers also liked (13)

Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003
Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003
Guía de aprendizaje tec11 003
 
JUNE NEWSLETTER
JUNE NEWSLETTER JUNE NEWSLETTER
JUNE NEWSLETTER
 
resume2015
resume2015resume2015
resume2015
 
Madison Listening Session Presentation
Madison Listening Session PresentationMadison Listening Session Presentation
Madison Listening Session Presentation
 
Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)
Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)
Instalacion de windows 7 y ubuntu duos (1) (1)
 
Practica 3
Practica 3 Practica 3
Practica 3
 
Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)
Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)
Capstone Final Project (African-American males)(1)
 
African-American Males Presentation (1)
African-American Males Presentation (1)African-American Males Presentation (1)
African-American Males Presentation (1)
 
Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017
Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017
Catalogue entr’enjeux 2017
 
2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins
2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins
2014 BPI presentation_Wiggins
 
2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins
2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins
2015 MSSJ presentation_Wiggins
 
Hydraulic pumps(compelete )
Hydraulic pumps(compelete )Hydraulic pumps(compelete )
Hydraulic pumps(compelete )
 
abstract published
abstract publishedabstract published
abstract published
 

Similar to JEE Published - Knowledge Scores

A Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental Knowledge
A Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental KnowledgeA Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental Knowledge
A Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental Knowledge
Crystal Sanchez
 
61036666
6103666661036666
61036666
preeti7777
 
Uu114 report
Uu114 reportUu114 report
Uu114 report
Uaakeia Iotua
 
Tool Opinion SpaceTypical Actions Opinion mapping softwa.docx
Tool  Opinion SpaceTypical Actions  Opinion mapping softwa.docxTool  Opinion SpaceTypical Actions  Opinion mapping softwa.docx
Tool Opinion SpaceTypical Actions Opinion mapping softwa.docx
juliennehar
 
SJSU ENVS Poster Template copy
SJSU ENVS Poster Template copySJSU ENVS Poster Template copy
SJSU ENVS Poster Template copyCristal Galindo
 
Student 1 Student Professor English Da.docx
Student 1   Student  Professor   English  Da.docxStudent 1   Student  Professor   English  Da.docx
Student 1 Student Professor English Da.docx
florriezhamphrey3065
 
A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...
A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...
A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...
Sabrina Green
 
Running head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE .docx
Running head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE                               .docxRunning head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE                               .docx
Running head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE .docx
cowinhelen
 
Annotated Bibliography Definition Definitions annota.docx
Annotated Bibliography Definition Definitions  annota.docxAnnotated Bibliography Definition Definitions  annota.docx
Annotated Bibliography Definition Definitions annota.docx
justine1simpson78276
 
Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...
Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...
Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...Rafael Martins
 
Climatechange
ClimatechangeClimatechange
Climatechange
swirawan
 
Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)
Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)
Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)Courtney Bearns Tablante
 
Perceptions of students on environmental challenge issues
Perceptions of students on environmental challenge issuesPerceptions of students on environmental challenge issues
Perceptions of students on environmental challenge issues
Alexander Decker
 
Significant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publicationSignificant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publicationDonovon Ceaser
 
CCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdf
CCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdfCCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdf
CCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdf
RobinsonAlvarezTorre
 
A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...
A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...
A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...
Nat Rice
 
Whenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docx
Whenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docxWhenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docx
Whenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docx
jolleybendicty
 
Course DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docx
Course DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docxCourse DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docx
Course DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docx
faithxdunce63732
 

Similar to JEE Published - Knowledge Scores (20)

sus%2E2012%2E9939
sus%2E2012%2E9939sus%2E2012%2E9939
sus%2E2012%2E9939
 
A Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental Knowledge
A Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental KnowledgeA Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental Knowledge
A Model Of Twelfth-Grade Environmental Knowledge
 
61036666
6103666661036666
61036666
 
Uu114 report
Uu114 reportUu114 report
Uu114 report
 
Tool Opinion SpaceTypical Actions Opinion mapping softwa.docx
Tool  Opinion SpaceTypical Actions  Opinion mapping softwa.docxTool  Opinion SpaceTypical Actions  Opinion mapping softwa.docx
Tool Opinion SpaceTypical Actions Opinion mapping softwa.docx
 
SJSU ENVS Poster Template copy
SJSU ENVS Poster Template copySJSU ENVS Poster Template copy
SJSU ENVS Poster Template copy
 
Student 1 Student Professor English Da.docx
Student 1   Student  Professor   English  Da.docxStudent 1   Student  Professor   English  Da.docx
Student 1 Student Professor English Da.docx
 
A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...
A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...
A STUDY OF THE DECISION TO REMOVE CHILDREN FROM THEIR PARENTS BY CHILD WELFAR...
 
Running head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE .docx
Running head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE                               .docxRunning head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE                               .docx
Running head GLOBAL SOCIETAL ISSUE .docx
 
Annotated Bibliography Definition Definitions annota.docx
Annotated Bibliography Definition Definitions  annota.docxAnnotated Bibliography Definition Definitions  annota.docx
Annotated Bibliography Definition Definitions annota.docx
 
Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...
Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...
Assessing the Research on Human Dimensions of Global Environmental Change in ...
 
Reni jurnal bp
Reni jurnal bpReni jurnal bp
Reni jurnal bp
 
Climatechange
ClimatechangeClimatechange
Climatechange
 
Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)
Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)
Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation (2009)
 
Perceptions of students on environmental challenge issues
Perceptions of students on environmental challenge issuesPerceptions of students on environmental challenge issues
Perceptions of students on environmental challenge issues
 
Significant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publicationSignificant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publication
 
CCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdf
CCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdfCCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdf
CCI_Final_Web_Rev (4).pdf
 
A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...
A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...
A Social Identity Analysis Of Climate Change And Environmental Attitudes And ...
 
Whenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docx
Whenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docxWhenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docx
Whenever the people are well informed, they can be trusted with thei.docx
 
Course DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docx
Course DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docxCourse DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docx
Course DescriptionENVI110 is an introductory, interdisciplinary .docx
 

JEE Published - Knowledge Scores

  • 1. This article was downloaded by: [Joanna Huxster] On: 29 June 2015, At: 15:13 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Click for updates The Journal of Environmental Education Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjee20 Undergraduate Understanding of Climate Change: The Influences of College Major and Environmental Group Membership on Survey Knowledge Scores Joanna K. Huxster a , Ximena Uribe-Zarain b & Willett Kempton b a Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA b University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware, USA Published online: 29 Jun 2015. To cite this article: Joanna K. Huxster, Ximena Uribe-Zarain & Willett Kempton (2015) Undergraduate Understanding of Climate Change: The Influences of College Major and Environmental Group Membership on Survey Knowledge Scores, The Journal of Environmental Education, 46:3, 149-165, DOI: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1021661 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00958964.2015.1021661 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
  • 2. Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 3. THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION, 46(3), 149–165, 2015 Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0095-8964 / 1940-1892 online DOI: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1021661 Undergraduate Understanding of Climate Change: The Influences of College Major and Environmental Group Membership on Survey Knowledge Scores Joanna K. Huxster Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA Ximena Uribe-Zarain and Willett Kempton University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware, USA A survey covering the scientific and social aspects of climate change was administered to examine U.S. undergraduate student mental models, and compare knowledge between groups based on major and environmental group membership. A Knowledge Score (scale 0–35, mean score = 17.84) was generated for respondents at two, central East Coast, U.S. universities (n = 465). Elements of student mental models examined include environmental issue confusion, skepticism, and self-reported under- standing. This study finds that students frequently confuse climate change with other environmental issues, and that a substantial majority of students do not have an understanding of climate change that closely matches the scientific model. These misconceptions extend to their understanding of mitigation actions. Environmental group membership is shown to be a greater determinant of climate change knowledge than enrollment in a science major. Keywords climate change, mental models, public understanding of science, science literacy The scientific community has reached a high level of consensus about the occurrence of anthro- pogenic global climate change (IPCC, 2013; Oreskes, 2004; Rosenberg, Vedlitz, & Cowman, 2010). In the United States, outside of the scientific community, there continues to be consid- erable debate and confusion surrounding the topic. Studies of public understanding of climate change conducted in the 1990s by Kempton, Boster, & Hartley (1995) and Bostrom, Morgan, Fischoff, & Read (1994) found that members of the general public frequently held inaccurate mental models regarding the processes of climate change. Participants in these studies had of- ten gathered misinformation and combined multiple environmental problems into their mental models. Since the 1990s, climate change discussion and science have both advanced; however, the level of public understanding has not noticeably increased. A study by the Yale Project on Climate Change Communication (Leiserowitz, Smith, & Marlon, 2010) and a retest of Bostrom et al.’s 1994 study (Reynolds, Bostrom, Read, & Morgan, 2010), both revealed little change in the Correspondence should be sent to Joanna K. Huxster, Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA. E-mail: jkh49@drexel.edu Color versions of one or more figures in this article are available online at www.tandfonline.com/vjee. Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 4. 150 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE understanding of the United States public. The new findings again included persistent confusion between climate change and other environmental issues such as ozone depletion. This article examines the knowledge and mental models of an important indicator popula- tion, undergraduate students. They represent the future leaders and thinkers of the country, and, as voters, they are capable of contributing to political and social change in the present. Their understanding of this urgent, socioscientific issue is critical to the future of the global climate. Significant recent work, such as that of Markowitz, Hodge, & Harp (2014), has been done to increase climate change communication efforts. Understanding the mental models of respon- dents in higher education will help evaluate the efficacy of the current education and science communication efforts and identify ways in which they can be improved. This study samples students nearing the end of their undergraduate programs and evaluates the understanding they have accumulated and retained throughout their academic careers. Semi- structured interviews (n = 18) were conducted to develop and refine survey questions at one public, central East Coast, U.S. university. The resulting survey was administered to respondents from two public, central East Coast, U.S. universities (n = 853, completed to end of survey n = 465) and Knowledge Scores for these respondents were generated based on their responses. In this article, these Knowledge Scores, as well as the knowledge elements of participant mental models observed through interview and survey data, are analyzed. AMERICAN ENVIRONMENTAL VALUES AND SCIENTIFIC UNDERSTANDING In their book Environmental Values in American Culture, Kempton et al. (1995) conduct semi- structured interviews and surveys to understand American cultural models of the environment, and of specific issues, including climate change. Kempton et al. (1995) define a cultural model or mental model as a set of guidelines or an imaginary map each individual builds for his or herself about any particular subject through which he or she can run scenarios, analyze information, and reach conclusions (1995, p. 10). A further examination of the literature on mental models and their implications can be found in the next section of this literature review. In “Cultural Models of Weather and the Atmosphere” from Kempton et al. 1995, the authors closely examine their informants’ cultural models of climate change (or as it was more commonly called in 1995, the greenhouse effect). Overall, they find that the responses of their laypersons do not correspond to scientific models. The authors describe ways in which informants modeled climate change that are based upon models for previously existing concepts, all of which deviate from the scientific understanding. Some of these concepts include greenhouse gases as pollution, or the “pollution model,” and ozone depletion as the mechanism of climate change, or the “ozone depletion model” (Kempton, Boster, & Hartley, 1995). A 2010 study done by the Yale Project on Climate Change Communication reveals some similar results to the Kempton et al. studies (Leiserowitz et al., 2010). In the Yale study, 2,030 adult members of the American public were surveyed on their knowledge of climate change and were given grades (A–F) on their performance. A majority of those surveyed received Bs, Cs, and Ds on their knowledge, with the largest proportion scoring Cs and only 2% earning an A. The majority of the adults surveyed had heard of the “greenhouse effect” and could correctly identify carbon dioxide as a greenhouse gas, but few understood how great the increase in carbon Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 5. HUXSTER ET AL. 151 dioxide in the atmosphere has been since 1850. A majority were found to correctly understand that burning of fossil fuels contributes to climate change, but the study also finds that “majorities of Americans. .. incorrectly believe that the hole in the ozone layer, toxic wastes, aerosol spray cans, volcanic eruptions, the sun, and acid rain contribute to global warming” (2010, p. 11). The results of their understanding could be seen in the respondents’ listed solutions. These included both correct (reducing driving, switching to renewable energy sources) and incorrect (reducing toxic waste, banning aerosol spray cans) actions. The Yale study also documents the pervasiveness of climate skepticism in the American publics’ understanding of climate change. Many of those surveyed did not believe that scientists can predict future weather or that computer models are reliable. A third of those surveyed stated that they did not believe that humans cause climate change. More than half of the respondents said that they aren’t sure if climate change is happening, or that they do not believe it to be happening at all. Over one third said that there is much disagreement among scientists about whether climate change is happening. The respondents listed television, printed media, and the Internet as their main sources of information about climate change, and a majority stated that they needed more and better information on the subject (Leiserowitz et al., 2010). Overall, the literature reveals a lack of scientific understanding of the processes of climate change within the general public. The consequences of this lack of understanding tend to be misinformation on the possible effects and severity of the issue, and the adoption of ineffective mitigation actions, as well as possibly weakened support for governmental initiative on the issue. RESEARCH QUESTIONS 1. What levels of knowledge, as measured by a calculated Knowledge Score, do respondents at these two universities hold to explain climate change? 2. What knowledge elements of mental models are represented, and how do the respondents’ models compare to the scientific model of climate change? 3. How does the participants’ knowledge of climate change, as defined by the Knowl- edge Score, differ between respondents in climate-related science majors, respondents belonging to environmental groups, and respondents in neither category? MENTAL MODEL THEORY AND CLIMATE CHANGE Mental models are an important component of this study, as they are one of the tools by which respondents’ understanding will be described and measured. Mental models, also called cultural models or cognitive models, come from a subfield of anthropology known as cognitive anthro- pology. D’Andrade (1995, p. 151) defines cognitive models as a “small-scale model” of external reality and possible personal actions that people carry around in their heads. It “consists of an interrelated set of elements which fit together to represent something” and this model is used to reason or to calculate by mentally manipulating its parts to solve a problem. In cognitive science, mental models are commonly used to explain human reasoning. It is theorized, “people do not reason using abstract rules, but rather they construct and combine mental models and generate inferences consistent with those models” (Bly & Rumelhart, 1999, p. 156). People can “run” their mental models to come up with conclusions or solutions to a problem. Since mental models Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 6. 152 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE are constructions of human thought, they often include simplifying assumptions (Mudditt, 1996). Errors in conclusions, therefore, will likely not be random, but will instead correspond with gaps or errors in mental models. Studies show that mental models, complete with gaps or distortions, can be passed on, taught and transferred (Bly & Rumelhart, 1999). Mental models are not only seen as agents of reasoning, but also are linked to motivation and action. D’Andrade (1992, p. 23–24), in an essay from the book Human Motives and Cultural Models, describes mental models as having a “motivational force” because they describe the world, set forth both conscious and unconscious goals, and elicit desires. Holland and Quinn (1987, p. 9) specifically address how mental models motivate behavior, expressing that the basis of mental models’ directive force is in the authority and expertise with which they are invested and within the “intrinsic persuasiveness” the models hold for us. Indeed, the influence of mental models on the actions and perceptions of their holders can be seen in a variety of studies (Gentner & Stevens, 2014). The examples range from people’s interactions with their thermostat settings, which is influenced by mental models informants hold for how the thermostat works (Kempton, 1987) to middle school students’ understanding of the greenhouse effect (Shepardson, Choi, Niyogi, & Charusombat, 2011) to students’ models of their personal affect on the environment and the behaviors those mental models produce (Liu & Lin, 2015). This research will use respondent survey responses to reconstruct and examine the knowledge elements of participant mental models of climate change. Mental and cultural model theory has been used as the theoretical framework for numerous studies regarding public understanding of climate change. In Kempton (1991) and Kempton et al. (1995), the studies most influencing the methodology of the present research, semi-structured interviews and survey results were used to infer mental models of climate change held by members of the general public. The Kempton et al. (1995) study found that “laypersons” hold several overarching models to ex- plain climate change. These same models were found in research conducted by Bostrom et al. (1994) and in a retest by Reynolds et al. (2010) using a similar survey instrument. In 2010, Zia and Todd used mental and cultural model theory to examine and map the belief networks of members of the public as related to their ideological, religious, socioeconomic, and educational background. These and other studies set the methodological and conceptual precedent for the use of mental model theory to explain understanding of climate change in this study. This article analyzes the knowledge portion of a larger dissertation study (Huxster, 2013). DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY The methodology of this research contains elements of both ethnography (from cultural an- thropology) and survey research, like the work done by Kemptonand colleagues in their 1995 book. Kempton et al. (1995) conducted semi-structured interviews of members of the general public. The ideas, reasoning, and mental models expressed in these interviews were then used to construct survey statements to be administered to a larger population. This methodology, also described in Bernard (2002), allows the researcher to test the existence of the mental models seen in semi-structured interviews across a larger population. The mixed methods analysis of Kempton et al.’s (1995) data included the examination of informant quotations and survey re- sponses to infer the mental models of the sampled population regarding climate change. This Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 7. HUXSTER ET AL. 153 methodology is applied in the present study to illuminate the mental models of U.S. undergraduate students, assess their knowledge, and understanding of climate change, and answer the research questions. Prior to the development of the survey instrument, a pilot study was conducted consisting of semi-structured interviews of 18 university students. Semi-structured interviews include a pre-determined set of questions, but the interviewer is able to ask further questions and probe for more complete answers, depending on how the informants respond (e.g., Agar, 1980). The interviews conducted for this study were constructed of open-ended questions aimed at gathering information about students’ understanding, knowledge, and lines of reasoning regarding climate change. Transcripts of these semi-structured interviews were reviewed, and the mental models of these informants were inferred from the lines of reasoning displayed. This process involved the identification and systematic counting of keywords, statements, and ideas, and reconstruction of participant perceptions and models based on inferred logic. The comments and arguments of the informant mental models were then condensed into one-sentence statements on the survey.1 Representative statements from all of the displayed mental models were included to insure that even the models seen infrequently in the 18 interviews were represented and could be tested in the overall student population. Survey respondents were asked to indicate their agreement to the survey statements on a 5-point Likert scale.2 Using this methodology for survey creation results in some multi-part statement wording not used in simple opinion surveys. Since the statements on this survey draw from the interviews with the same population, the wording can be informal yet appropriate to the student interview subjects. Also, since we are attempting to elicit mental models rather than isolated beliefs, some survey statements include multipart ideas that attempt to capture the mental models and lines of reasoning displayed in the pilot study interviews. As noted by Bernard (2002) and Kempton et al. (1995), this survey item methodology is used for testing for lines of reasoning or multi-part mental models; the items are not intended to elicit individual components facts. The included survey questions can be seen in Table 1 and the full survey and interview instruments can be found in Huxster (2013). The informants in the semi-structured interviews held several different mental models, but their reasoning and understanding could be categorized into three overarching models: predom- inantly scientific understanding, environmental issue confusion, and natural causes and cycles. After the survey was administered, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were performed to provide evidence of instrument and construct validity, and it was found that the survey did measure the three main components of mental models observed in the pilot study. In the confir- matory factor analysis performed with varimax rotation, three factors were identified (Table 1). These three factors fit closely with the models seen in the semi-structured interviews. The first factor includes almost all of the scientifically correct statements about the mechanisms, causes, scientific consensus, and severity of climate change and has been labeled “Predominantly Sci- entific Understanding.” The items loading into the second factor are statements that attribute the causes or mechanisms of various environmental issues to climate change, but these causes are not actually significant to the issue. This factor is labeled “Issue Confusion.” The statements in the third factor all refer to skepticism about the severity and scientific certainty of climate change, as well as statements expressing a belief that natural causes are responsible for climate change. This factor has been labeled “Natural Causes and Skepticism.” One survey item was discarded after the factor analyses owing to a misprint in the Likert scale wording. A more detailed explanation Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 8. 154 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE TABLE 1 Factor Loadings of the Rotated Factors for Knowledge Variables Sci Und. Issue Conf Nat Cause and Skep 2.40 We need to change our transportation system, like creating more public transportation, in order to make a big impact on reducing climate change .710 2.37 Individuals can reduce climate change by not buying gas-guzzling cars and instead maybe owning a more fuel-efficient car or hybrid .687 2.43 The government could put more resources into developing alternative and renewable sources of energy in order to reduce climate change .672 2.42 I’ve heard that using efficient light bulbs, turning off electric appliances and insulating my house are all ways I can reduce my contribution to climate change .624 2.25 Even though it’s sometimes debated, I feel that it’s pretty well documented that humans have released too much carbon dioxide into the atmosphere .619 2.32 The government could tax carbon emissions or introduce a system of cap and trade in an effort to reduce climate change .616 2.30 The best way to deal with climate change would be to reduce or eliminate carbon-based fuel sources .611 2.23 Burning fossil fuels releases carbon dioxide into the atmosphere, which traps heat and causes climate change .600 2.31 Individuals can give political support to clean energy initiatives to help deal with climate change .571 2.39 Cutting down on waste and pollution will help stop the melting of the ice caps .512 .401 2.74 Even if the science isn’t clear to me, I think it’s better to act and be safe rather than sorry .508 2.27 I’ve heard that driving less and using bicycles and public transportation are ways to reduce climate change .507 2.44 Recycling is essential to reducing climate change because it keeps plastics out of landfills and reduces production .506 2.18 Climate change is caused by an increased amount of gases like carbon dioxide and methane in the atmosphere .502 2.75 It’s hard for scientists to be certain about anything, but I think the patterns and trends that they’re seeing in climate change have them pretty convinced .443 2.68 I think that it’s generally pretty well accepted that climate change is occurring .425 2.14 Gas and chemical emissions from our cars deplete the ozone and cause climate change .782 2.11 Climate change is happening because we are depleting the ozone layer, and that lets in more heat from the sun .778 2.16 Greenhouse gases make the hole in the ozone layer worse .724 2.15 Toxic pollution in the atmosphere causes climate change .710 2.21 Climate change happens because we release chemicals from aerosol cans into the atmosphere .681 2.36 I can help prevent climate change by not buying or using aerosol cans .625 2.22 Nuclear power plants create pollution that contributes to climate change .608 2.35 The government can reduce climate change by regulating toxic pollution .444 .561 (Continued on next page) Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 9. HUXSTER ET AL. 155 TABLE 1 Factor Loadings of the Rotated Factors for Knowledge Variables (Continued) Sci Und. Issue Conf Nat Cause and Skep 2.19 Climate change involves magnetic field and fluctuations in the sun, and other things beyond our control, and those things are bigger factors than greenhouse gas emissions .672 2.70 The climate has changed a lot in the past, so I think we might be making too big a deal about how it’s changing now .667 2.62 From my sources I’ve learned that too much emphasis is put on the human element of climate change, rather than the natural elements .654 2.9 I think that climate change concerns are exaggerated more than the science supports .637 2.69 I think that scientists are certain that the climate is changing, but not about who or what is responsible .616 2.20 Natural thing like volcanoes and water vapor have more of an impact on climate change than human actions .613 2.73 I don’t think that climate change is the most pressing environmental issue we face today, and I think that it gets too much press .612 2.10 Scientists are highly certain that humans are definitely the cause of current, rapid climate change .535 2.12 Climate change is caused by the greenhouse effect –.450 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. of the confirmatory factor analyses can be found in the supplementary material accompanying the online article. The survey was administered to a sample of junior and senior students at two public, central East Coast universities. These respondents were selected for the survey as they have spent three or four years studying their individual majors and therefore better represent the influence that a student’s major may have on their understanding of climate change. Systematic sampling was used to gather a representative sample of students and the survey was conducted online using Qualtrics survey software. Eight hundred fifty-three respondents from the two universities took the survey, with 465 respondents from the two universities completing the entire survey. For the comparison of climate-related science majors and non-science majors, a selection of majors was made including majors relating to environmental science and policy, chemistry, biology, and geology. In order to acquire sufficient response rates from majors of interest with fewer students, the geology majors and the Environmental Science and Policy Group were sampled 100%. For all other majors, proportional stratified sampling was used. Within the departmental subgroups, systematic sampling was used to select the final sample, with an interval of N = 4. In the statistical analysis of the survey results, those majors that were sampled at 100% with high response rates were weighted at .25 when used in conjunction with the other majors. Following the researchers’ sampling, the registrar’s office of each University performed the selection of students from the appropriate majors, and sent them e-mails asking them to take the online survey. The identities Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 10. 156 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE of the individual respondents sampled were not revealed to the researcher and no incentives were offered to the students for participation. To ensure higher numbers of participants in the selected majors of interest, respondents from capstone courses in those respective majors were also sampled based on identification of those students by their professors. Beyond the initial survey invitation and reminder emails sent to the entire sample, the researcher did not contact individual respondents. The survey’s Knowledge Variables, analyzed here, include both correct and incorrect state- ments about the mechanisms of climate change, as well as the actions that can be taken to prevent or mitigate climate change. In a socioscientific issue such as climate change, the so- cial components cannot be completely separated from the science (Klosterman & Sadler, 2009). The survey Knowledge Variables also involve those social components most closely associated with the scientific model and that can be coded as correct and incorrect in comparison with the scientific model. The social aspects of these Knowledge Variables are most clearly represented in statements dealing with scientific consensus and government action. A complete list of the Knowledge Variables is included in the factor analysis in Table 1. As part of the analysis of the Knowledge Variables, a “Knowledge Score” was generated for each respondent based on respondents’ agreement and disagreement with scientifically cor- rect and incorrect statements. The Knowledge Score was calculated from 35 survey statements. Students received 1 “point” for each statement matching the scientific model of climate change to which they agreed. Students also received 1 “point” for each statement not matching the scientific model to which they disagreed. This created a score on a scale of 0 to 35. Only the answers of the 465 participants who completed the survey in its entirety were included in the calculation of the Knowledge Score. The mean Knowledge Scores of science and non- science students were compared using a t-test, as were the mean knowledge scores of environ- mental group members and respondents not belonging to an environmental group. Two-Way Analysis of Variance was used to compare mean Knowledge Scores between four “Sets” of respondents: 1. Science major and Environmental group 2. Science major and Non-environmental group 3. Non-Science major and Environmental group 4. Non-science major and Non-environmental group. Thus, we can compare the influence of major and environmental group on climate change knowledge. In addition to the calculation and analysis of the Knowledge Scores, responses to indi- vidual survey items dealing with the three overarching types of mental models seen in the interviews are analyzed. The first set of statements examined are those similar to the scien- tific model, and are included as examples of respondents’ responses to individual parts of the model. Statements showing confusion between climate change and other environmental issues were included to illustrate ways in which respondents’ mental models differ from the scientific model. Incorrect statements attributing current, rapid climate change3 to natural causes were included in this analysis to show the role skepticism plays in respondents’ mental models. Fi- nally, survey statements asking participants to self-report their knowledge and understanding of climate change were included to examine how their self-assessment fits into their mental models. Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 11. HUXSTER ET AL. 157 TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics for Two Public, Central East Coast Universities Knowledge Scores N 465 Mean 17.84 Median 18.00 Mode 16.00 Std. Deviation 5.40 Variance 29.26 RESULTS AND ANALYSIS The creation of the Knowledge Score allowed for the evaluation of climate change knowledge of the respondents, and comparison of knowledge across specific groups of respondents. Incomplete surveys were excluded from this analysis, as their scores skewed the results. The mean Knowledge Score (scale 0–35) of the 465 respondents (both universities) was 17.84 with a standard deviation of 5.40. The highest score received was a 35, indicating all answers matched the scientific model, and the lowest was a 0, indicating that no answers matched the scientific model. The statistics for the Knowledge scores can be seen in Table 2 and the score distribution can be seen in Figure 1. Comparison of Knowledge Scores Between Groups Descriptive statistics were performed and compared between groups of respondents based on their science education and their environmental group membership. The first comparison was the science majors as compared with non-science majors. An independent samples t-test was run and FIGURE 1 Student respondent knowledge score distribution. Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 12. 158 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE FIGURE 2 Mean knowledge scores for four “core groups.” a significant difference was found between the means of the two groups (t(448) = 3.46, p < .05). The mean Knowledge Score of the science majors was significantly higher (m = 19.60, sd = 5.98) than the non-science majors (m = 17.43, sd = 5.19). Based on these results, it can be said that respondents in the selected science majors, unsurprisingly, have a greater understanding of climate change science than respondents who were not science majors. The next comparison was between participants who belong to an environmental group with those who do not. An independent samples t-test comparing the two found a significant difference between the means (t(456) = 4.44, p < .05). The mean Knowledge Score of environmental group respondents was significantly higher (m = 20.11, sd = 5.04) than that of the non-environmental group respondents (m = 17.36, sd = 5.35). In order to examine the effects of both environmental group membership and science major, a 2 (Environmental Group Membership) X 2(Science Major) between-subjects factorial ANOVA was calculated comparing the Knowledge Scores of the respondents. Environmental group mem- bership and science major were each shown to have significant main effects. The respondent Sets and their corresponding mean scores are as follows: 1. Science major and Environmental group (m = 21.38), 2. Science major and Non-environmental group (m = 18.36), 3. Non-Science major and Environmental group (m = 19.5), 4. Non-science major and Non-environmental group (m = 17.13). A significant difference was found between the groups F(3, 447) = 9.60, p < .05) and post hoc analysis shows that environmental group membership is a greater indicator of a high Knowledge Score than enrollment in a science major. The interaction between environmental group membership and science major was not significant (F(1, 447) = 6.36, p > .05). A graph of the mean scores (Figure 2) shows an obvious difference between the mean of Core Group Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 13. HUXSTER ET AL. 159 1, Science major and Environmental group, and that of Core Group 4, Non-science major and Non-environmental group. Specific Elements of Respondent Mental Models Specific statements from the survey were of particular interest in inferring elements of respon- dent’s mental models of climate change. The selection of these specific statements was based on the mental models observed in the responses of students in the pilot study interviews. During the interview process, students were able to describe correctly some aspects of climate change, but also showed significant issue confusion, in which they conflated climate change with other environmental issues such as ozone depletion. A few of the students interviewed expressed that natural causes play a significant role in recent climate change, while others indicated that they know little about how climate change occurs (Huxster, 2013). Representative statements from these lines of reasoning are presented here to test the prevalence of these specific aspects of student mental models across the student population. Separating and presenting these contradictory sets of statements also illustrates some of the interesting overlaps in student agreement seen between scientifically correct and scientifically incorrect statements. This serves to further explain and fill out the outline of their mental models of climate change. Statements Matching the Scientific Model First, responses to survey statements matching different aspects of the scientific model are examined to gain a clear picture of how respondents understand the causes of climate change and the actions that can be taken. Next, responses to statements of different models of climate change, specifically those that confuse climate change with other environmental issues and those attributing climate change to natural causes, are listed to investigate respondents’ subscription to those models. Finally, respondents’ responses to statements that self-identify confusion or ignorance about climate change are considered. Table 3a shows response percentages to survey items matching the scientific model of climate change.4 The responses to statement 2.10 show that just over half of the respondents understand the high level of scientific consensus that exists about climate change. Almost 30% of the respondents are unsure about the scientific agreement, and almost 20% do not agree that scientific agreement exists. This shows some lack of understanding of the scientific community’s views. A more positive indicator for respondents’ understanding of climate change is that 77% of the respondents agree with statement 2.23, that the burning of fossil fuels contributes to climate change. Complete comprehension of the scientific model of climate change also includes understanding of the mitigation actions that can be taken. Responses to survey statements matching the scientific model of actions to help prevent or lessen climate change are shown in Table 3b. The majority of the respondents have heard of energy efficiency measures they can enact in their own homes, and know that alternative and renewable energy sources are important parts to the solution to climate change. A smaller majority was able to identify that the reduction or elimination of carbon-based fuel sources is the best way to mitigate climate change. The difference in responses between 2.30 and 2.43 suggests that although respondents have retained the information that renewable Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 14. 160 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE TABLE 3 Responses to Survey Statements Grouped by Model Element 3a. Responses to Statements Matching the Scientific Model of Climate Change Item Agree Neither Disagree N 2.10 Scientists are highly certain that humans are definitely the cause of current, rapid climate change 53.2% 29.0% 17.8% 682 2.18 Climate change is caused by an increased amount of gases like carbon dioxide and methane in the atmosphere 75.1% 21.5% 3.4% 610 2.23 Burning fossil fuels releases carbon dioxide into the atmosphere, which traps heat and causes climate change 77.0% 19.6% 3.3% 566 3b. Responses to Statements Matching the Scientific Model of Mitigation Actions 2.30 The best way to deal with climate change would be to reduce or eliminate carbon-based fuel sources 59.2% 32.0% 8.8% 522 2.42 I’ve heard that using efficient light bulbs, turning off electric appliances and insulating my house are all ways I can reduce my contribution to climate change 91.7% 4.9% 3.4% 494 2.43 The government could put more resources into developing alternative and renewable sources of energy in order to reduce climate change 84.9% 10.8% 4.2% 489 3c. Responses to Statements Confusing Climate Change with Ozone Depletion 2.11 Climate change is happening because we are depleting the ozone layer, and that lets in more heat from the sun. 43.0% 26.9% 30.1% 672 2.14 Gas and chemical emissions from our cars deplete the ozone and cause climate change. 66.8% 17.4% 15.7% 648 2.16 Greenhouse gases make the hole in the ozone layer worse 54.8% 23.2% 21.9% 633 2.36 I can help prevent climate change by not buying or using aerosol cans 50.1% 28.7% 21.1% 505 3d. Responses to Statements Confusing Climate Change with Toxic Pollution 2.15 Toxic pollution in the atmosphere makes climate change worse 65.2% 24.0% 10.7% 641 2.35 The government can reduce climate change by regulating toxic pollution 65.7% 21.7% 12.7% 507 2.39 Cutting down on waste and pollution will help stop the melting of the ice caps 54.4% 31.8% 13.8% 500 3e. Responses to Statements attributing Climate Change to Natural Causes 2.19 Climate change involves magnetic field and fluctuations in the sun, and other things beyond our control, and those things are bigger factor than greenhouse gas emissions. 25.4% 47.1% 27.5% 594 2.20 Natural things like volcanoes and water vapor have more of an impact on climate change than human action. 16.3% 41.7% 42.0% 588 3f. Responses to Statements Expressing Confusion or Ignorance about Climate Change 2.13 I know that changes in the atmosphere cause climate change, but I don’t know very much about the scientific processes that make it happen 45.3% 18.2% 36.5% 658 2.24 No one has ever really explained to me how the greenhouse effect works, or if they have I can’t really remember 27.6% 11.6% 60.8% 562 Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 15. HUXSTER ET AL. 161 resources are an important solution, fewer respondents understand why these alternatives are important or why non-renewable energy sources exacerbate the problem. Statements Displaying Issue Confusion Despite the high number of responses indicating that fossil fuels play a role in climate change, responses to several statements dealing with the confusion of climate change with other envi- ronmental issues reinforce the mental models observed in the pilot study interviews. Some of the statements dealing with the confusion of climate change and ozone depletion are seen in Table 3c. The percentages of agreement with these statements indicate significant confusion in respondents’ mental models between climate change and ozone depletion. The responses were relatively split on statement 2.11, with a slight majority agreeing that depletion of the ozone layer causes climate change. However, more than half of the respondents incorrectly believed that greenhouse gases contribute to the hole in the ozone layer. Two thirds of the respondents have incorporated new information about the contribution of car emissions to climate change into their ozone depletion model of climate change, as can be seen in statement 2.14. The confusion of climate change and ozone depletion affects not only respondents’ understanding of causes of climate change, but also understanding of prevention measures. Half of the respondents mistak- enly believe that limiting their use of aerosol cans will help to prevent climate change. The issue confusion between climate change and ozone depletion as seen in this study is similar to issues encountered in previous studies of public understanding of climate change (Bostrom et al., 1994; Kempton et al., 1995). The confusion surprisingly has persisted over 20 years, despite public coverage and despite these respondents having taken science courses, presumably with lessons to the contrary, (Reynolds et al., 2010). Another environmental issue commonly confused with climate change in the pilot study and in previous literature is that of toxic pollution or waste. In Table 3d, statements illustrating this confusion are listed. For both statements 2.15 and 2.35, nearly two thirds of the respondents agreed that toxic pollution is a cause of climate change and that the regulation of these prod- ucts will help combat climate change. In a statement directly linking toxic waste and pollution to a commonly understood effect of climate change, the melting of the ice caps, more than half of the respondents expressed agreement. This mental model of climate change has been noted, and called the “Pollution model,” in previous research (Bostrom et al., 1994; Kemp- ton et al., 1995; Reynolds et al., 2010). The issue confusion seen with the ozone depletion model and the pollution model of climate change in this study may be a case of lumping any generally “good environmental practice” into the category of preventing climate change. In 1994, Bostrom et al. found that in the absence of sufficient information, people tend to agree with all practices that they see as good for the environment to explain how to combat climate change. Statements of Climate Skepticism and Self-Reported Knowledge Climate change skepticism promotes the mental model of natural occurrences, or cycles, as the primary causes of climate change. Agreement with statements of this model can be seen in Table 3e. For the first of the two statements in this table, the majority of respondents indicated that they neither agree nor disagree that natural elements beyond human control are bigger factors in Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 16. 162 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE climate change than greenhouse gas emissions. In the second statement concerning water vapor and volcanic eruptions, the response rates seen for “neither” and for “disagree” were almost equal. These results may mean that there is some confusion among the respondents about the role that natural occurrences play in climate change. For both statements, however, more respondents disagreed than agreed with the statements that natural occurrences are more important factors in climate change than human actions. The final set of survey statements analyzed in this study, shown in Table 3f, are included to illustrate respondents’ self-reported understanding of climate change. For statement 2.13, a slight majority of respondents agreed that they do not feel they understand the scientific processes of climate change. Larger majorities of respondents, however, disagreed with statements indicating they had not learned about the greenhouse effect or that they did not know how they personally affect climate change. These results show that a majority of respondents believe that they know the correct ways in which to combat climate change. It has been shown, however, that self-reported informedness does not correlate with objective measures of subject knowledge (Durant & Legge, 2005). Given the high number of respondents holding incorrect models of climate change as indicated by responses to statements of issue confusion, this may be the case in this study. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS In the overall population at the two universities represented in this study, the respondents show some basic understanding of the scientific processes of climate change. The mean knowledge score falls at 17.84 on a scale of 0–35. In terms of college “grading,” this is close to 50% on a grading scale. These scores indicate that the answer to the first research question is that students’ knowledge leaves much to be desired. Further research might look at respondent levels of understanding, learning progressions of university students, and what levels of understanding are necessary for correct and informed action, as suggested in Mohan, Chen, and Anderson’s (2009) study of high school students. The second research question asks how student knowledge and mental models compare to the scientific model of climate change. In looking at specific statements made in the pilot study interviews and the responses to corresponding questions from the survey, further detail in the knowledge elements of respondents’ mental models is revealed. Three quarters of the respon- dents were able to identify the burning of fossil fuels as a source of greenhouse gases and most respondents knew that energy efficiency measures and renewable resources were potential solu- tions. However, only half of the respondents understood that there is a high level of scientific agreement about human impact on the global climate. Also, subtle changes in the wording of similar questions about fossil fuels (i.e., calling them “carbon-based fuels”) may have influenced responses. Overall, these results indicate that gaps exist in the knowledge elements of students’ mental models. In addition to these gaps identified in respondent understanding of the scientific model, the respondents also show a great deal of confusion between climate change and other environmental issues. This confusion of issues is seen not only in respondents’ understanding of the causes of climate change, but also in their understanding of the solutions and actions to be taken for climate change. The areas of confusion, specifically in respondents’ ozone depletion model and the pollution model, closely match those seen in previous studies of the public, dating back to Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 17. HUXSTER ET AL. 163 the early 1990s. These findings indicate that misconceptions in respondents’ mental models have persisted and these misconceptions need attention to ensure that these young members of society are prepared to tackle this serious issue effectively. One positive finding is that although the role of natural causes has led to some confusion in respondent mental models, climate change skepticism doesn’t seem to play nearly as large a role in the undergraduates’ mental models as has the persistence of environmental issue confusion. Understanding the specific nature of the gaps and misconceptions in the knowledge elements of mental models can better inform education, both formal and informal, on this matter. The third research question asks about the differences in student mental models between col- lege majors and environmental group participants. Predictably, when looking at different groups of respondents, those belonging to the related science majors are shown to have a higher level of knowledge than those students not belonging to science majors. The finding that those re- spondents belonging to environmental groups score higher on the Knowledge Score scale than those not belonging to such groups could also have been predicted. What is more surprising, however, is that environmental group membership is a greater indicator of climate change knowl- edge (as defined by the Knowledge Score) than enrollment in a science major. This research does not cover how these respondents reached this higher level of knowledge, or what causes respondents in environmental groups to have a greater understanding. It is possible that these findings could be attributed to the education and information respondents receive from envi- ronmental groups, but we consider it unlikely that environmental groups are better sources of information than science classes. Students in both a climate science-related major and an envi- ronmental group have the highest mean knowledge scores. Another possible explanation for the differences in scores could be that respondents’ greater understanding is a function of a higher level of interest and concern for climate change, which is manifested in the respondents’ choice of major and environmental group participation. These hypothesized relationships could also explain why environmental group membership is a greater predictor of high Knowledge Score than enrollment in a science major. Further study of the information covered in specific classes and disseminated through environmental groups, as well as in students’ motives in choosing college majors and belonging to an environmental group, would be needed to test this possible explanation. Further research is needed to sort through the possible explanations for the understanding of science students and environmental group members. More important, the respondents who likely need the most attention are those not belonging to either of these groups. The non-science, non-environmental students represent the majority of the respondents (64%). The findings of this research indicate that these respondents are either not receiving or not retaining the information necessary to have an adequate level of understanding of the issue. If they are to contribute meaningfully to climate change solutions as citizens, they will likely need to have a greater understanding than shown in this study. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Dr. John Madsen for assistance in acquiring partial funding for this research from the National Science Foundation grant for Research on Learning in Formal and Informal Settings. Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 18. 164 STUDENT UNDERSTANDING OF CLIMATE CHANGE NOTES 1. The work reported here draws on only the knowledge portion of the survey from Huxster 2013. Elsewhere the survey is referred to as the “Survey of Student Understanding of Climate Change.” 2. For the purposes of reporting the results, statement responses for “Agree and Strongly Agree” and “Disagree and Strongly Disagree” were condensed together in Table 3 3. Survey respondents were informed that the use of the word “climate change” was used in place of the term “global warming” and represented the same phenomenon. In cases in which it was necessary to be more specific, the phrase “current, rapid climate change” was used. The survey was framed to respondents as an exploration of their opinions on environmental issues. 4. In reporting responses to individual survey statements, all students responding to that element are included (even those who might not have completed the entire survey) to give the most complete analysis possible. The n values in table 3 reflect the number of students responding to each survey item REFERENCES Agar, M. H. (1980). The professional stranger: An informal introduction to ethnography. New York, NY: Academic Press. Bernard, R. H. (2002). Research methods in anthropology: Qualitative and quantitative approaches, (3rd ed.). Lanham, MD: Altamira Press. Bly, B. M., & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.). (1999). Cognitive science. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischoff, B., & Read, D. (1994). What do people know about climate change? 1. Mental models, 2. Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 959–982. D’Andrade, R. (1992). Schemas and motivation. In R. D’Andrade & C. Strauss (Eds.), Human motives and cultural models (pp. 22–44). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. D’Andrade, R. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Durant, R. F., & Legge Jr., J. S. (2005). Public opinion, risk perceptions, and genetically modified food regulatory policy. European Union Politics, 6(1), 181–200. Gentner, D., & Stevens, A. L. (2014). Mental models. New York, NY: Psychology Press. Holland, D., & Quinn, N. (1987). Culture and cognition. In N. Quinn & D. Holland (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 3–40). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Huxster, Joanna K. (2013). Student understanding of climate change: Influences of college major and environmental group membership on undergraduate knowledge and mental models (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest. (Publication Number 3594926). IPCC. (2013). Climate change 2013: The physical science basis summary of policy makers. Contribution of working group I to the fourth assessment report of the intergovernmental panel on climate change. [Stocker, T., Dahe, Q., & Gian-Kasper, P. (Coordinating Lead Authors)] Retrieved from www.ipcc.ch Kempton, W. (1987). Two theories of home heat control. In N. Quinn & D. Holland (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 222–242). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Kempton, W. (1991). Public understanding of global warming. Society and Natural Resources, 4, 331–345. Kempton, W., Boster, J. S., & Hartley, J. A. (1995). Environmental values in American culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Klosterman, M. L., & Sadler, T. D. (2009). Multi-level assessment of scientific content knowledge gains associated with socioscientific issues-based instruction. International Journal of Science Education, Research Report. Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N., & Marlon, J. R. (2010). Americans’ knowledge of climate change. Yale University, New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication. Liu, S. C., & Lin, H. (2015) Exploring undergraduate students’ mental models of the environment: Are they related to environmental affect and behavior? Journal of Environmental Education, 46(1), 23–40. Markowitz, E., Hodge, C., & Harp, G. (2014). Connecting on climate: A guide: A guide to effective climate change communication. Columbia University, New York, NY: Center for Research on Environmental Decisions. Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015
  • 19. HUXSTER ET AL. 165 Mohan, L., Chen, J., & Anderson, W. A. (2009). Developing a multi-year learning progression for carbon cycling in socio-ecological systems. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 675–698. Mudditt, A. (1996). Cognitive science: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Oreskes, N. (2004). Beyond the ivory tower: The scientific consensus on climate change. Science, 306(5702), 1686. Reynolds, T. W., Bostrom, A., Read, D., & Morgan, M. G. (2010). Now what do people know about climate change? Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis, 30(10), 1520–1538. Rosenberg, S., Vedlitz, A., Cowman, D. F., & Zahran, S. (2010). Climate change: A profile of U.S. climate perspectives. Climatic Change, 101, 311–329. Shepardson, D. P., Choi, S., Niyogi, D., & Charusombat, U. (2011). Seventh grade students’ mental models of the greenhouse effect. Environmental Education Research, 17(1) 1–17. Zia, A., & Todd, A. M. (2010). Evaluating the effects of ideology on public understanding of climate change science: How to improve communication across ideological divides? Public Understanding of Science, 19(6) 743–761. Downloadedby[JoannaHuxster]at15:1329June2015