SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 133
F ive years ago, several executives at McKinsey &
Company,America’s largest and most prestigious management-
consulting
firm, launched what they called the War for Talent. Thousands
of
questionnaires were sent to managers across the country.
Eighteen
companies were singled out for special attention, and the
consultants
spent up to three days at each firm, interviewing everyone from
the
C.E.O. down to the human-resources staff. McKinsey wanted to
document how the top-performing companies in America
differed
from other firms in the way they handle matters like hiring and
promotion. But, as the consultants sifted through the piles of
reports
and questionnaires and interview transcripts, they grew
convinced
that the difference between winners and losers was more
profound
than they had realized. “We looked at one another and suddenly
the
light bulb blinked on,” the three consultants who headed the
project—Ed Michaels, Helen Handfield-Jones, and Beth
Axelrod
—write in their new book, also called “The War for Talent.”
The
very best companies, they concluded, had leaders who were
obsessed
with the talent issue. They recruited ceaselessly, finding and
hiring as
many top performers as possible. They singled out and
segregated
DEPT. OF HUMAN RESOURCES JULY 22, 2002 ISSUE
THE TALENT MYTH
Are smart people overrated?
By Malcolm Gladwell
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
1 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
HarryJoo
Sticky Note
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
their stars, rewarding them disproportionately, and pushing
them
into ever more senior positions. “Bet on the natural athletes, the
ones
with the strongest intrinsic skills,” the authors approvingly
quote one
senior General Electric executive as saying. “Don’t be afraid to
promote stars without specifically relevant experience,
seemingly over
their heads.” Success in the modern economy, according to
Michaels,
Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod, requires “the talent mind-set”:
the
“deep-seated belief that having better talent at all levels is how
you
outperform your competitors.”
This “talent mind-set” is the new orthodoxy of American
management. It is the intellectual justification for why such a
high
premium is placed on degrees from first-tier business schools,
and
why the compensation packages for top executives have become
so
lavish. In the modern corporation, the system is considered only
as
strong as its stars, and, in the past few years, this message has
been
preached by consultants and management gurus all over the
world.
None, however, have spread the word quite so ardently as
McKinsey,
and, of all its clients, one firm took the talent mind-set closest
to
heart. It was a company where McKinsey conducted twenty
separate
projects, where McKinsey’s billings topped ten million dollars a
year,
where a McKinsey director regularly attended board meetings,
and
where the C.E.O. himself was a former McKinsey partner. The
company, of course, was Enron.
The Enron scandal is now almost a year old. The reputations of
Jeffrey Skilling and Kenneth Lay, the company’s two top
executives,
have been destroyed. Arthur Andersen, Enron’s auditor, has
been
driven out of business, and now investigators have turned their
attention to Enron’s investment bankers. The one Enron partner
that
has escaped largely unscathed is McKinsey, which is odd, given
that
it essentially created the blueprint for the Enron culture. Enron
was
the ultimate “talent” company. When Skilling started the
corporate
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
2 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
A
division known as Enron Capital and Trade, in 1990, he
“decided to
bring in a steady stream of the very best college and M.B.A.
graduates he could find to stock the company with talent,”
Michaels,
Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod tell us. During the nineties, Enron
was bringing in two hundred and fifty newly minted M.B.A.s a
year.
“We had these things called Super Saturdays,” one former
Enron
manager recalls. “I’d interview some of these guys who were
fresh out
of Harvard, and these kids could blow me out of the water. They
knew things I’d never heard of.” Once at Enron, the top
performers
were rewarded inordinately, and promoted without regard for
seniority or experience. Enron was a star system. “The only
thing
that differentiates Enron from our competitors is our people, our
talent,” Lay, Enron’s former chairman and C.E.O., told the
McKinsey consultants when they came to the company’s
headquarters, in Houston. Or, as another senior Enron executive
put
it to Richard Foster, a McKinsey partner who celebrated Enron
in his
2001 book, “Creative Destruction,” “We hire very smart people
and
we pay them more than they think they are worth.”
The management of Enron, in other words, did exactly what the
consultants at McKinsey said that companies ought to do in
order to
succeed in the modern economy. It hired and rewarded the very
best
and the very brightest—and it is now in bankruptcy. The
reasons for
its collapse are complex, needless to say. But what if Enron
failed not
in spite of its talent mind-set but because of it? What if smart
people
are overrated?
t the heart of the McKinsey vision is a process that the War for
Talent advocates refer to as “differentiation and affirmation.”
Employers, they argue, need to sit down once or twice a year
and
hold a “candid, probing, no-holds-barred debate about each
individual,” sorting employees into A, B, and C groups. The A’s
must
be challenged and disproportionately rewarded. The B’s need to
be
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
3 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
encouraged and affirmed. The C’s need to shape up or be
shipped
out. Enron followed this advice almost to the letter, setting up
internal Performance Review Committees. The members got
together twice a year, and graded each person in their section on
ten
separate criteria, using a scale of one to five. The process was
called
“rank and yank.” Those graded at the top of their unit received
bonuses two-thirds higher than those in the next thirty per cent;
those who ranked at the bottom received no bonuses and no
extra
stock options—and in some cases were pushed out.
How should that ranking be done? Unfortunately, the McKinsey
consultants spend very little time discussing the matter. One
possibility is simply to hire and reward the smartest people. But
the
link between, say, I.Q. and job performance is distinctly
underwhelming. On a scale where 0.1 or below means virtually
no
correlation and 0.7 or above implies a strong correlation (your
height,
for example, has a 0.7 correlation with your parents’ height),
the
correlation between I.Q. and occupational success is between
0.2 and
0.3. “What I.Q. doesn’t pick up is effectiveness at common-
sense
sorts of things, especially working with people,” Richard
Wagner, a
psychologist at Florida State University, says. “In terms of how
we
evaluate schooling, everything is about working by yourself. If
you
work with someone else, it’s called cheating. Once you get out
in the
real world, everything you do involves working with other
people.”
Wagner and Robert Sternberg, a psychologist at Yale
University, have
developed tests of this practical component, which they call
“tacit
knowledge.” Tacit knowledge involves things like knowing how
to
manage yourself and others, and how to navigate complicated
social
situations. Here is a question from one of their tests:
You have just been promoted to head of an important
department in
your organization. The previous head has been transferred to an
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
4 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
equivalent position in a less important department. Your
understanding
of the reason for the move is that the performance of the
department
as a whole has been mediocre. There have not been any glaring
deficiencies, just a perception of the department as so-so rather
than
very good. Your charge is to shape up the department. Results
are
expected quickly. Rate the quality of the following strategies
for
succeeding at your new position.
a) Always delegate to the most junior person who can be trusted
with
the task.
b) Give your superiors frequent progress reports.
c) Announce a major reorganization of the department that
includes
getting rid of whomever you believe to be “dead wood.”
d) Concentrate more on your people than on the tasks to be
done.
e) Make people feel completely responsible for their work.
Wagner finds that how well people do on a test like this predicts
how
well they will do in the workplace: good managers pick (b) and
(e);
bad managers tend to pick (c). Yet there’s no clear connection
between such tacit knowledge and other forms of knowledge and
experience. The process of assessing ability in the workplace is
a lot
messier than it appears.
An employer really wants to assess not potential but
performance. Yet
that’s just as tricky. In “The War for Talent,” the authors talk
about
how the Royal Air Force used the A, B, and C ranking system
for its
pilots during the Battle of Britain. But ranking fighter pilots—
for
whom there are a limited and relatively objective set of
performance
criteria (enemy kills, for example, and the ability to get their
formations safely home)—is a lot easier than assessing how the
manager of a new unit is doing at, say, marketing or business
development. And whom do you ask to rate the manager’s
performance? Studies show that there is very little correlation
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
5 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
between how someone’s peers rate him and how his boss rates
him.
The only rigorous way to assess performance, according to
human-
resources specialists, is to use criteria that are as specific as
possible.
Managers are supposed to take detailed notes on their
employees
throughout the year, in order to remove subjective personal
reactions
from the process of assessment. You can grade someone’s
performance only if you know their performance. And, in the
freewheeling culture of Enron, this was all but impossible.
People
deemed “talented” were constantly being pushed into new jobs
and
given new challenges. Annual turnover from promotions was
close to
twenty per cent. Lynda Clemmons, the so-called “weather babe”
who
started Enron’s weather derivatives business, jumped, in seven
quick
years, from trader to associate to manager to director and,
finally, to
head of her own business unit. How do you evaluate someone’s
performance in a system where no one is in a job long enough to
allow such evaluation?
The answer is that you end up doing performance evaluations
that
aren’t based on performance. Among the many glowing books
about
Enron written before its fall was the best-seller “Leading the
Revolution,” by the management consultant Gary Hamel, which
tells
the story of Lou Pai, who launched Enron’s power-trading
business.
Pai’s group began with a disaster: it lost tens of millions of
dollars
trying to sell electricity to residential consumers in newly
deregulated
markets. The problem, Hamel explains, is that the markets
weren’t
truly deregulated: “The states that were opening their markets to
competition were still setting rules designed to give their
traditional
utilities big advantages.” It doesn’t seem to have occurred to
anyone
that Pai ought to have looked into those rules more carefully
before
risking millions of dollars. He was promptly given the chance to
build the commercial electricity-outsourcing business, where he
ran
up several more years of heavy losses before cashing out of
Enron last
year with two hundred and seventy million dollars. Because Pai
had
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
6 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
W
“talent,” he was given new opportunities, and when he failed at
those
new opportunities he was given still more opportunities . . .
because
he had “talent.” “At Enron, failure—even of the type that ends
up on
the front page of the Wall Street Journal—doesn’t necessarily
sink a
career,” Hamel writes, as if that were a good thing. Presumably,
companies that want to encourage risk-taking must be willing to
tolerate mistakes. Yet if talent is defined as something separate
from
an employee’s actual performance, what use is it, exactly?
hat the War for Talent amounts to is an argument for
indulging A employees, for fawning over them. “You need to
do everything you can to keep them engaged and satisfied—
even
delighted,” Michaels, Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod write.
“Find out
what they would most like to be doing, and shape their career
and
responsibilities in that direction. Solve any issues that might be
pushing them out the door, such as a boss that frustrates them or
travel demands that burden them.” No company was better at
this
than Enron. In one oft-told story, Louise Kitchin, a twenty-
nine-year-old gas trader in Europe, became convinced that the
company ought to develop an online-trading business. She told
her
boss, and she began working in her spare time on the project,
until
she had two hundred and fifty people throughout Enron helping
her.
After six months, Skilling was finally informed. “I was never
asked
for any capital,” Skilling said later. “I was never asked for any
people.
They had already purchased the servers. They had already
started
ripping apart the building. They had started legal reviews in
twenty-two countries by the time I heard about it.” It was,
Skilling
went on approvingly, “exactly the kind of behavior that will
continue
to drive this company forward.”
Kitchin’s qualification for running EnronOnline, it should be
pointed
out, was not that she was good at it. It was that she wanted to
do it,
and Enron was a place where stars did whatever they wanted.
“Fluid
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
7 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
movement is absolutely necessary in our company. And the type
of
people we hire enforces that,” Skilling told the team from
McKinsey.
“Not only does this system help the excitement level for each
manager, it shapes Enron’s business in the direction that its
managers
find most exciting.” Here is Skilling again: “If lots of
[employees] are
flocking to a new business unit, that’s a good sign that the
opportunity is a good one. . . . If a business unit can’t attract
people
very easily, that’s a good sign that it’s a business Enron
shouldn’t be
in.” You might expect a C.E.O. to say that if a business unit
can’t
attract customers very easily that’s a good sign it’s a business
the
company shouldn’t be in. A company’s business is supposed to
be
shaped in the direction that its managers find most prof itable.
But at
Enron the needs of the customers and the shareholders were
secondary to the needs of its stars.
A dozen years ago, the psychologists Robert Hogan, Robert
Raskin,
and Dan Fazzini wrote a brilliant essay called “The Dark Side
of
Charisma.” It argued that flawed managers fall into three types.
One
is the High Likability Floater, who rises effortlessly in an
organization because he never takes any difficult decisions or
makes
any enemies. Another is the Homme de Ressentiment, who
seethes
below the surface and plots against his enemies. The most
interesting
of the three is the Narcissist, whose energy and self-confidence
and
charm lead him inexorably up the corporate ladder. Narcissists
are
terrible managers. They resist accepting suggestions, thinking it
will
make them appear weak, and they don’t believe that others have
anything useful to tell them. “Narcissists are biased to take
more
credit for success than is legitimate,” Hogan and his co-authors
write, and “biased to avoid acknowledging responsibility for
their
failures and shortcomings for the same reasons that they claim
more
success than is their due.” Moreover:
Narcissists typically make judgments with greater confidence
than
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
8 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
other people . . . and, because their judgments are rendered with
such
conviction, other people tend to believe them and the narcissists
become disproportionately more influential in group situations.
Finally,
because of their self-confidence and strong need for
recognition,
narcissists tend to “self-nominate”; consequently, when a
leadership
gap appears in a group or organization, the narcissists rush to
fill it.
Tyco Corporation and WorldCom were the Greedy
Corporations:
they were purely interested in short-term financial gain. Enron
was
the Narcissistic Corporation—a company that took more credit
for
success than was legitimate, that did not acknowledge
responsibility
for its failures, that shrewdly sold the rest of us on its genius,
and that
substituted self-nomination for disciplined management. At one
point in “Leading the Revolution,” Hamel tracks down a senior
Enron executive, and what he breathlessly recounts—the
braggadocio, the self-satisfaction—could be an epitaph for the
talent
mind-set:
“You cannot control the atoms within a nuclear fusion
reaction,” said
Ken Rice when he was head of Enron Capital and Trade
Resources
(ECT), America’s largest marketer of natural gas and largest
buyer and
seller of electricity. Adorned in a black T-shirt, blue jeans, and
cowboy
boots, Rice drew a box on an office whiteboard that pictured his
business unit as a nuclear reactor. Little circles in the box
represented
its “contract originators,” the gunslingers charged with doing
deals and
creating new businesses. Attached to each circle was an arrow.
In Rice’s
diagram the arrows were pointing in all different directions.
“We allow
people to go in whichever direction that they want to go.”
The distinction between the Greedy Corporation and the
Narcissistic
Corporation matters, because the way we conceive our
attainments
helps determine how we behave. Carol Dweck, a psychologist at
Columbia University, has found that people generally hold one
of
two fairly firm beliefs about their intelligence: they consider it
either
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
9 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
a fixed trait or something that is malleable and can be
developed over
time. Five years ago, Dweck did a study at the University of
Hong
Kong, where all classes are conducted in English. She and her
colleagues approached a large group of social-sciences students,
told
them their English-proficiency scores, and asked them if they
wanted
to take a course to improve their language skills. One would
expect
all those who scored poorly to sign up for the remedial course.
The
University of Hong Kong is a demanding institution, and it is
hard
to do well in the social sciences without strong English skills.
Curiously, however, only the ones who believed in malleable
intelligence expressed interest in the class. The students who
believed
that their intelligence was a fixed trait were so concerned about
appearing to be deficient that they preferred to stay home.
“Students
who hold a fixed view of their intelligence care so much about
looking smart that they act dumb,” Dweck writes, “for what
could be
dumber than giving up a chance to learn something that is
essential
for your own success?”
In a similar experiment, Dweck gave a class of preadolescent
students
a test filled with challenging problems. After they were
finished, one
group was praised for its effort and another group was praised
for its
intelligence. Those praised for their intelligence were reluctant
to
tackle difficult tasks, and their performance on subsequent tests
soon
began to suffer. Then Dweck asked the children to write a letter
to
students at another school, describing their experience in the
study.
She discovered something remarkable: forty per cent of those
students who were praised for their intelligence lied about how
they
had scored on the test, adjusting their grade upward. They
weren’t
naturally deceptive people, and they weren’t any less intelligent
or
self-confident than anyone else. They simply did what people do
when they are immersed in an environment that celebrates them
solely for their innate “talent.” They begin to define themselves
by
that description, and when times get tough and that self-image
is
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
10 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
T
threatened they have difficulty with the consequences. They
will not
take the remedial course. They will not stand up to investors
and the
public and admit that they were wrong. They’d sooner lie.
he broader failing of McKinsey and its acolytes at Enron is
their assumption that an organization’s intelligence is simply a
function of the intelligence of its employees. They believe in
stars,
because they don’t believe in systems. In a way, that’s
understandable,
because our lives are so obviously enriched by individual
brilliance.
Groups don’t write great novels, and a committee didn’t come
up
with the theory of relativity. But companies work by different
rules.
They don’t just create; they execute and compete and coördinate
the
efforts of many different people, and the organizations that are
most
successful at that task are the ones where the system is the star.
There is a wonderful example of this in the story of the so-
called
Eastern Pearl Harbor, of the Second World War. During the first
nine months of 1942, the United States Navy suffered a
catastrophe.
German U-boats, operating just off the Atlantic coast and in the
Caribbean, were sinking our merchant ships almost at will. U-
boat
captains marvelled at their good fortune. “Before this sea of
light,
against this footlight glare of a carefree new world were passing
the
silhouettes of ships recognizable in every detail and sharp as the
outlines in a sales catalogue,” one U-boat commander wrote.
“All we
had to do was press the button.”
What made this such a puzzle is that, on the other side of the
Atlantic, the British had much less trouble defending their ships
against U-boat attacks. The British, furthermore, eagerly passed
on
to the Americans everything they knew about sonar and depth-
charge throwers and the construction of destroyers. And still the
Germans managed to paralyze America’s coastal zones.
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
11 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
You can imagine what the consultants at McKinsey would have
concluded: they would have said that the Navy did not have a
talent
mind-set, that President Roosevelt needed to recruit and
promote
top performers into key positions in the Atlantic command. In
fact,
he had already done that. At the beginning of the war, he had
pushed
out the solid and unspectacular Admiral Harold R. Stark as
Chief of
Naval Operations and replaced him with the legendary Ernest
Joseph
King. “He was a supreme realist with the arrogance of genius,”
Ladislas Farago writes in “The Tenth Fleet,” a history of the
Navy’s
U-boat battles in the Second World War. “He had unbounded
faith
in himself, in his vast knowledge of naval matters and in the
soundness of his ideas. Unlike Stark, who tolerated
incompetence all
around him, King had no patience with fools.”
The Navy had plenty of talent at the top, in other words. What it
didn’t have was the right kind of organization. As Eliot A.
Cohen, a
scholar of military strategy at Johns Hopkins, writes in his
brilliant
book “Military Misfortunes in the Atlantic”:
To wage the antisubmarine war well, analysts had to bring
together
fragments of information, direction-finding fixes, visual
sightings,
decrypts, and the “flaming datum” of a U-boat attack—for use
by a
commander to coordinate the efforts of warships, aircraft, and
convoy
commanders. Such synthesis had to occur in near “real time”—
within
hours, even minutes in some cases.
The British excelled at the task because they had a centralized
operational system. The controllers moved the British ships
around
the Atlantic like chess pieces, in order to outsmart U-boat “wolf
packs.” By contrast, Admiral King believed strongly in a
decentralized management structure: he held that managers
should
never tell their subordinates ” ‘how’ as well as what to ‘do.’ “
In
today’s jargon, we would say he was a believer in “loose-tight”
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
12 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
T
management, of the kind celebrated by the McKinsey
consultants
Thomas J. Peters and Robert H. Waterman in their 1982 best-
seller,
“In Search of Excellence.” But “loose-tight” doesn’t help you
find
U-boats. Throughout most of 1942, the Navy kept trying to act
smart by relying on technical know-how, and stubbornly refused
to
take operational lessons from the British. The Navy also lacked
the
organizational structure necessary to apply the technical
knowledge it
did have to the field. Only when the Navy set up the Tenth
Fleet—a
single unit to coördinate all anti-submarine warfare in the
Atlantic—did the situation change. In the year and a half before
the
Tenth Fleet was formed, in May of 1943, the Navy sank thirty-
six
U-boats. In the six months afterward, it sank seventy-five. “The
creation of the Tenth Fleet did not bring more talented
individuals
into the field of ASW”—anti-submarine warfare—”than had
previous organizations,” Cohen writes. “What Tenth Fleet did
allow,
by virtue of its organization and mandate, was for these
individuals to
become far more effective than previously.” The talent myth
assumes
that people make organizations smart. More often than not, it’s
the
other way around.
here is ample evidence of this principle among America’s most
successful companies. Southwest Airlines hires very few
M.B.A.s, pays its managers modestly, and gives raises
according to
seniority, not “rank and yank.” Yet it is by far the most
successful of
all United States airlines, because it has created a vastly more
efficient
organization than its competitors have. At Southwest, the time it
takes to get a plane that has just landed ready for takeoff—a key
index of productivity—is, on average, twenty minutes, and
requires a
ground crew of four, and two people at the gate. (At United
Airlines,
by contrast, turnaround time is closer to thirty-five minutes, and
requires a ground crew of twelve and three agents at the gate.)
In the case of the giant retailer Wal-Mart, one of the most
critical
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
13 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
A
periods in its history came in 1976, when Sam Walton
“unretired,”
pushing out his handpicked successor, Ron Mayer. Mayer was
just
over forty. He was ambitious. He was charismatic. He was, in
the
words of one Walton biographer, “the boy-genius financial
officer.”
But Walton was convinced that Mayer was, as people at
McKinsey
would say, “differentiating and affirming” in the corporate
suite, in
defiance of Wal-Mart’s inclusive culture. Mayer left, and Wal-
Mart
survived. After all, Wal-Mart is an organization, not an all-star
team.
Walton brought in David Glass, late of the Army and Southern
Missouri State University, as C.E.O.; the company is now
ranked
No. 1 on the Fortune 500 list.
Procter & Gamble doesn’t have a star system, either. How could
it?
Would the top M.B.A. graduates of Harvard and Stanford move
to
Cincinnati to work on detergent when they could make three
times
as much reinventing the world in Houston? Procter & Gamble
isn’t
glamorous. Its C.E.O. is a lifer—a former Navy officer who
began
his corporate career as an assistant brand manager for Joy
dishwashing liquid—and, if Procter & Gamble’s best played
Enron’s
best at Trivial Pursuit, no doubt the team from Houston would
win
handily. But Procter & Gamble has dominated the consumer-
products field for close to a century, because it has a carefully
conceived managerial system, and a rigorous marketing
methodology
that has allowed it to win battles for brands like Crest and Tide
decade after decade. In Procter & Gamble’s Navy, Admiral
Stark
would have stayed. But a cross-divisional management
committee
would have set the Tenth Fleet in place before the war ever
started.
mong the most damning facts about Enron, in the end, was
something its managers were proudest of. They had what, in
McKinsey terminology, is called an “open market” for hiring. In
the
open-market system—McKinsey’s assault on the very idea of a
fixed
organization—anyone could apply for any job that he or she
wanted,
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
14 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
and no manager was allowed to hold anyone back. Poaching was
encouraged. When an Enron executive named Kevin Hannon
started
the company’s global broadband unit, he launched what he
called
Project Quick Hire. A hundred top performers from around the
company were invited to the Houston Hyatt to hear Hannon give
his
pitch. Recruiting booths were set up outside the meeting room.
“Hannon had his fifty top performers for the broadband unit by
the
end of the week,” Michaels, Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod
write,
“and his peers had fifty holes to fill.” Nobody, not even the
consultants who were paid to think about the Enron culture,
seemed
worried that those fifty holes might disrupt the functioning of
the
affected departments, that stability in a firm’s existing
businesses
might be a good thing, that the self-fulfillment of Enron’s star
employees might possibly be in conflict with the best interests
of the
firm as a whole.
These are the sort of concerns that management consultants
ought to
raise. But Enron’s management consultant was McKinsey, and
McKinsey was as much a prisoner of the talent myth as its
clients
were. In 1998, Enron hired ten Wharton M.B.A.s; that same
year,
McKinsey hired forty. In 1999, Enron hired twelve from
Wharton;
McKinsey hired sixty-one. The consultants at McKinsey were
preaching at Enron what they believed about themselves. “When
we
would hire them, it wouldn’t just be for a week,” one former
Enron
manager recalls, of the brilliant young men and women from
McKinsey who wandered the hallways at the company’s
headquarters. “It would be for two to four months. They were
always
around.” They were there looking for people who had the talent
to
think outside the box. It never occurred to them that, if
everyone had
to think outside the box, maybe it was the box that needed
fixing. ♦
Malcolm Gladwell has been a staff writer for the The New
Yorker
since 1996.
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
15 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
MORE: ENRON CORPORATION AXELROD
THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent-
myth
16 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM
OUTLIERS
68
CHAPTER THREE
The Trouble with
Geniuses, Part 1
"KNOWLEDGE OF A BOY'S IQ IS OF
LITTLE HELP IF yc)u ARE FACED WITH
A FORMFUL OF CLEVER BOYS."
1.
In the fifth episode of the 2008 season, the American tele-
vision quiz show I vs. loo had as its special guest a man
named Christopher Langan.
The television show l vs. loo is one of many that
sprang up in the wake of the phenomenal success of Who
Wants to Be a Millionaire. It features a permanent gallery
of one hundred ordinary people who serve as what is called
the "mob." Each week they match wits with a special
invited guest. At stake is a million dollars. The guest has
to be smart enough to answer more questions correctly
than his or her one hundred adversaries-and by that
standard, few have ever seemed as superbly qualified as
Christopher Langan.
"Tonight the mob takes on their fiercest competition
yet," the voice-over began. "Meet Chris Langan, who many
OUTLIERS
call the smartest man in America." The camera did a slow
pan of a stocky, muscular man in his fifties. "The aver-
age person has an IQ of one hundred," the voice-over
continued. "Einstein one fifty. Chris has an IQ of one
ninety-five. He's currently wrapping his big brain around
a theory of the universe. But will his king-size cranium
be enough to take down the mob for one million dollars?
Find out right now on One versus One Hundred."
Out strode Langan onto the stage amid wild applause.
"You don't think you need to have a high intellect to
do well on One versus One Hundred, do you?" the show's
host, Bob Sager, asked him. Sager looked at Langan oddly,
as if he were some kind of laboratory specimen.
"Actually, I think it could be a hindrance," Langan
replied. He had a deep, certain voice. "To have a high
IQ, you tend to specialize, think deep thoughts. You
avoid trivia. But now that I see these people" -he glanced
at the mob, the amusement in his eyes betraying just how
ridiculous he found the proceedings- "I think I'll do
okay."
Over the past decade, Chris Langan has achieved a
strange kind of fame. He has become the public face of
genius in American life, a celebrity outlier. He gets invited
on news shows and profiled in magazines, and he has been
the subject of a documentary by the filmmaker Errol Mor-
ris, all because of a brain that appears to defy description.
The television news show 20/20 once hired a neuro-
psychologist to give Langan an IQ test, and Langan's score
was literally off the charts-too high to be accurately
measured. Another time, Langan took an IQ test specially
designed for people too smart for ordinary IQ tests. He
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART I
got all the questions right except one.'' He was speaking
at six months of age. When he was three, he would listen
to the radio on Sundays as the announcer read the comics
aloud, and he would follow along on his own until he had
taught himself to read. At five, he began questioning his
grandfather about the existence of God-and remembers
being disappointed in the answers he got.
In school, Langan could walk into a test in a foreign-
language class, not having studied at all,. and if there
were two or three minutes before the instructor arrived,
he could skim through the textbook and ace the test. In .
his early teenage years, while working as a farmhand, he
started to read widely in the area of theoretical physics. At
sixteen, he made his way through Bertrand Russell and
Alfred North Whitehead's famously abstruse masterpiece
Principia Mathematica. He got a perfect score ·on his SAT,
even though he fell asleep at one point during the test.
"He did math for an hour," his brother Mark says of
Langan's summer routine in high school. "Then he did
French for an hour. Then he studied Russian. Then he
would read philosophy. He did that religiously, every day."
Another of his brothers, Jeff, says, "You know, when
Christopher was fourteen or fifteen, he would draw things
just as a joke, and it would be like a photograph. When he
was fifteen, he could match Jimi Hendrix lick for lick on
a guitar. Boom. Boom. Boom. Half the time, Christopher
didn't attend school at all. He would just show up for tests
:.<The super IQ test was created by Ronald K. Hoeflin, who is
himself
someone with an unusually high IQ. Here's a sample question,
from
the verbal analogies section. "Teeth is to Hen as Nest is to ?" If
you
want to know the answer, I'm afraid I have no idea.
71
OUTLIERS
and there was nothing they could do about it. To us, it was
hilarious. He could brief a semester's worth of textbooks
in two days, and take care of whatever he had to take care
of, and then get back to whatever he was doing in the first
place."'' ·
On the set of 1 vs. 100, Langan was poised and confi-
dent. His voice was deep. His eyes were small and fiercely
bright. He did not circle about topics, searching for the
right phrase, or double back to restate a previous sentence.
* To get a sense of what Chris Langan must hive been like
growing
up, consider the following description of a child named "L,"
who had
an IQ in the same 200 range as Langan's. It's from a study by
Leta
Stetter Hollingworth, who was one of the first psychologists to
study
exceptionally gifted children. As the description makes obvious,
an
IQ of 200 is really, really high: "Young L's erudition was
astonishing.
His passion for scholarly accuracy and thoroughness set a high
stand-
ard for accomplishment. He was relatively large, robust and
impres-
sive, and was fondly dubbed 'Professor.' His attitudes and
abilities
were appreciated by both pupils and teachers. He was often
allowed
to lecture (for as long as an hour) on some special topic, such as
the
history of timepieces, ancient theories of engine construction,
math-
ematics, and history. He constructed out of odds and ends
(typewriter
ribbon spools, for example) a homemade clock of the pendular
type to
illustrate some of the principles of chronometry, and this clock
was
set up before the class during the enrichment unit on 'Time and
Time
Keeping' to demonstrate some of the principles of chronometry.
His
notebooks were marvels of scholarly exposition.
"Being discontented with what he considered the inadequate
treatment of land travel in a class unit on 'Transportation,' he
agreed
that time was too limited to do justice to everything. But he
insisted
that 'at least they should have covered ancient theory.' As an
extra and
voluntary project, 'he brought in elaborate drawings and
accounts
of the ancient theories of engines, locomotives etc.' ... He was
at that
time ro years of age."
72
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART I
For that matter, he did not say um, or ah, or use any form
of conversational mitigation: his sentences came marching
out, one after another, polished and crisp, like soldiers on
a parade ground. Every question Saget threw at him, he
tossed aside, as if it were a triviality. When his winnings
reached $150,000, he appeared to make a mental calcula-
tion that the risks of losing everything were at that point
greater than the potential benefits of staying in. Abruptly,
he stopped. "I'll take the cash," he said. He shook Saget's
hand firmly and was finished- exiting on top as, we like
to think, geniuses invariably do.
73
OUTLIERS
CHAPTER FOUR
The Trouble with
Geniuses, Part 2
''AFTER PROTRACTED NEGOTIATIONS,
IT WAS AGREED THAT ROBERT WOULD BE
PUT ON PROBATION."
1.
Chris Langan's mother was from San Francisco and was
estranged from her family. She had four sons, each with
a different father. Chris was the eldest. His father disap-
peared b_efore Chris was born; he was said to have died in
Mexico. His mother's second husband was murdered. Her
third committed suicide. Her fourth was a failed journal-
ist named Jack Langan.
"To this day I haven't met anybody who was as poor
when they were kids as our family was," Chris Langan
says. "We didn't have a pair of matched socks. Our shoes
had holes in them. Our pants had holes in them. We only
had one set of clothes. I remember my brothers and I going
into the bathroom and using the bathtub to wash our only
set of clothes and we were bare-assed naked when we were
doing that because we didn't have anything to wear.''
Jack Langan would go on drinking sprees and disappear.
9 I
OUTLIERS
He would lock the kitchen cabinets so the boys couldn't get
to the food. He used a bullwhip to keep the boys in line.
He would get jobs and then lose them, moving the family
on to the next town. One summer the family lived on an
Indian reservation in a teepee, subsisting on government-
surplus peanut butter and cornmeal. For a time, they lived
in Virginia City, Nevada. "There was only one law offi-
cer in town, and when the Hell's Angels came to town, he
would crouch down in the back of his office," Mark Langan
remembers. "There was a bar there, I'll always remember. It
was called the Bucket of Blood Saloon."
When the boys were in grade school, the family moved
to Bozeman, Montana. One of Chris's brothers spent time
in a foster home. Another was sent to reform school.
"I don't think the school ever understood just how
gifted Christopher was," his brother Jeff says. "He sure as
hell didn't play it up. This was Bozeman. It wasn't like it is
today. It was a small hick town when we were growing up.
We weren't treated well there. They'd just decided that my
family was a bunch of deadbeats." To stick up for himself
and his brothers, Chris started to lift weights. One.day,
when Chris was fourteen, Jack Langan got rough with the
boys, as he sometimes did, and Chris knocked him out
cold. Jack left, never to return. Upon graduation from
high school, Chris was offered two full scholarships, one
to Reed College in Oregon and the other to the Univer-
sity of Chicago. He chose Reed.
"It was a huge mistake," Chris recalls. "I had a real
case of culture shock. I was a crew-cut kid who had been
working as a ranch hand in the summers in Montana,
and there I was, with a whole bunch of long-haired city
92
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
kids, most of them from New York. And these kids had
a whole different style than I was used to. I couldn't get
a word in edgewise at class. They were very inquisitive.
Asking questions all the time. I was crammed into a dorm
room. There were four of us, and the other three guys had
a whole different other lifestyle. They were smoking pot.
They would bring their girlfriends into the room. I had
never smoked pot before. So basically I took to hiding in
the library."
He continued: "Then I lost that scholarship .... My
mother was supposed to fill out a parents' financial state-
ment for the renewal of that scholarship. She neglected to
do so. She was confused by the requirements or whatever.
At some point, it came to my attention that my scholar-
ship had not been renewed. So I went to the office to ask
why, and they told me, Well, no one sent us the financial
statement, and we allocated all the scholarship money and
it's all gone, so I'm afraid that you don't have a scholar-
ship here ,anymore. That was the style of the place. They
simply didn't care. They didn't give a shit about their stu-
dents. There was no counseling, no mentoring, nothing."
Chris left Reed before the final set of exams, leaving
him with a row of Fs on his transcript. In the first semes-
ter, he had earned As. He went back to Bozeman and
worked in construction and as a forest services firefighter
for a year and a half. Then he enrolled at Montana State
University.
"I was taking math and philosophy classes," he recalled.
"And then in the winter quarter, I was living thirteen miles
out of town, out on Beach Hill Road, and the transmis-
sion fell out of my car. My brothers had used it when I was
93
OUTLIERS
gone that summer. They were working for the railroad
and had driven it on the railroad tracks. I didn't have the
money to repair it. So I went to my adviser and the dean in
sequence and said, I have a problem. The transmission fell
out of my car, and you have me in a seven-thirty a.m. and
eight-thirty a.m. class. If you could please just transfer me
to the afternoon sections of these classes, I would appreci-
ate it because of this car problem. There was a neighbor
who was a rancher who was going to take me in at eleven
o'clock. My adviser was this cowboy-looking guy with a
handlebar mustache, dressed in a tweed jacket. He said,
'Well, son, after looking at your transcript at Reed Col-
lege, I see that you have yet to learn that everyone has to
make sacrifices to get an education. Request denied.' So
then I went to the dean. Same treatment.''
His voice grew tight. He was describing things that
had happened more than thirty years ago, but the mem-
ory still made him angry. "At that point I realized, here
I was, knocking myself out to make the money to make
my way back to school, and it's the middle of the Montana
winter. I am willing to hitchhike into town every day, do
whatever I had to do, just to get into school and back, and
they are unwilling to do anything for me. So bananas.
And that was the point I decided I could do without the
higher-education system. Even if I couldn't do without it,
it was sufficiently repugnant to me that I wouldn't do it
anymore. So I dropped out of college, simple as that."
Chris Langan's experiences at Reed and Montana State
represented a turning point in his life. As a child, he had
dreamt of becoming an academic. He should have gotten a
PhD; universities are institutions structured, in large part,
94
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
for people with his kind of deep intellectual interests and
curiosity. "Once he got into the university environment, I
thought he would prosper, I really did," his brother Mark
says. "I thought he would somehow find a niche. It made
absolutely no sense to me when he left that."
Without a degree, Langan floundered. He worked in
construction. One frigid winter he worked on a clam boat
on Long Island. He took factory jobs and minor civil ser-
vice positions and eventually became a bouncer in a bar
on Long Island, which was his principal occupation for
much of his adult years. Through it all, he continued to
read deeply in philosophy, mathematics, and physics as he
worked on a sprawling treatise he calls the "CTMU" -the
"Cognitive Theoretic Model of the Universe.'' But without
academic credentials, he despairs of ever getting published
in a scholarly journal.
"I am a guy who has a year and a half of college," he
says, with a shrug. "And at some point this will come to
the attention of the editor, as he is going to take the paper
and send ,it off to the referees, and these referees are going
to try and look me up, and they are not going to find me.
And they are going to say, This guy has a year and a half
of college. How can he know what he's talking about?"
I tis a heartbreaking story. At one point I asked Langan-
hypothetically-whether he would take a job at Harvard
University were it offered to him. "Well, that's a difficult
question," he replied. "Obviously, as a full professor at
Harvard I would count. My ideas would have weight and
I could use my position, my affiliation at Harvard, to pro-
mote my ideas. An institution like that is a great source of
intellectual energy, and if I were at a place like that, I could
9 5
OUTLIERS
absorb the vibration in the air." It was suddenly clear how
lonely his life has been. Here he was, a man with an insa-
tiable appetite for learning, forced for most of his adult life
to live in intellectual isolation. "I even noticed that kind of
intellectual energy in the year and a half I was in college,"
he said, almost wistfully. "Ideas are in the air constantly.
It's such a stimulating place to be.
"On the other hand," he went on, "Harvard is basically
a glorified corporation, operating with a profit incentive.
That's what makes it tick. It has an endowment in the bil-
lions of dollars. The people running it are not necessarily
searching for truth and knowledge. They want to be big
shots, and when you accept a paycheck from these people,
it is going to come down to what you want to do and what
you feel is right versus what the man says you can do to
receive another paycheck. When you're there, they got a
thumb right on you. They are out to make sure you don't
step out of line."
2.
What does the story of Chris Langan tell us? His explana-
tions, as heartbreaking as they are, are also a little strange.
His mother forgets to sign his financial aid form and-just
like that-no scholarship. He tries to move from a morn-
ing to an afternoon class, something students do every
day, and gets stopped cold. And why were Langan's teach-
ers at Reed and Montana State so indifferent to his plight?
Teachers typically delight in minds as brilliant as his.
Langan talks about dealing with Reed and Montana State
as if they were some kind of vast and unyielding govern-
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
ment bureaucracy. But colleges, particularly small liberal
arts colleges like Reed, tend not to be rigid bureaucracies.
Making allowances in the name of helping someone stay
in school is what professors do all the time.
Even in his discussion of Harvard, it's as if Langan has
no conception of the culture and particulars of the institu-
tion he's talking about. When you accept a paycheck from
these people, it is going to come down to what you want
to do and what you feel is right versus what .the man says
you can do to receive another paycheck. What? One of the
main reasons college professors accept a lower paycheck
than they could get in private industry is that university
life gives them the freedom to do what they want to do and
what they feel is right. Langan has Harvard backwards.
When Langan told me his life story, I couldn't help
thin_king of the life of Robert Oppenheimer, the physicist
who famously headed the American effort to develop the
nuclear bomb during World War II. Oppenheimer, by all
accounts, was a child with a mind very much like Chris
Langan''s. His parents considered him a genius. One of
his teachers recalled that "he received every new idea as
perfectly beautiful." He was doing lab experiments by
the third grade and studying physics and chemistry by
the fifth grade. When he was nine, he once told one of his
cousins, "Ask me a question in Latin and I will answer
you in Greek."
Oppenheimer went to Harvard and then on to Cam-
bridge University to pursue a doctorate in physics. There,
Oppenheimer, who struggled with depression his entire
life, grew despondent. His gift was for theoretical physics,
and his tutor, a man named Patrick Blackett (who would
97
OUTLIERS
win a Nobel Prize in 1948), was forcing him to attend to
the minutiae of experimental physics, which he hated. He
grew more and more emotionally unstable, and then, in
an act so strange that to this day no one has properly made
sense of it, Oppenheimer took some chemicals from the
laboratory and tried to poison his tutor.
Blackett, luckily, found out that something was amiss.
The university was informed. Oppenheimer was called on
the carpet. And what happened next is every bit as unbe-
lievable as the crime itself. Here is how the incident is
described in American Prometheus, Kai Bird and Martin
Sherwin's biography of Oppenheimer: "After protracted
negotiations, it was agreed that Robert would be put on pro-
bation and have regular sessions with a prominent Harley
Street psychiatrist in London."
On probation?
Here we have two very brilliant young students, each of
whom runs into a problem that imperils his college career.
Langan's mother has missed a deadline for his financial aid.
Oppenheimer has tried to poison his tutor. To continue
on, they are required to plead their cases to authority. And
what happens? Langan gets his scholarship taken away, and
Oppenheimer gets sent to a psychiatrist. Oppenheimer and
Langan might both be geniuses, but in other ways, they
could not be more different.
The story of Oppenheimer's appointment to be scien-
tific director of the Manhattan Project twenty years later is
perhaps an even better example of this difference. The gen-
eral in charge of the Manhattan Project was Leslie Groves,
and he scoured the country, trying to find the right person
to lead the atomic-bomb effort. Oppenheimer, by rights,
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
was a long shot. He was just thirty-eight, and junior to
many of the people whom he would have to manage. He
was a theorist, and this was a job that called for experi-
menters and engineers. His political affiliations were dodgy:
he had all kinds of friends who were Communists. Perhaps
more striking, he had never had any administrative experi-
ence. "He was a very impractical fellow," one of Oppen-
heimer's friends later said. "He walked about with scuffed
shoes and a funny hat, and, more important, he didn't
know anything about equipment." As one Berkeley s.cien-
tist put it, more succinctly: "He couldn't run a hamburger
stand."
Oh, and by the way, in graduate school he tried to kill
his tutor. This was the resume of the man who was trying
out for what might be said to be-without exaggeration-
one.of the most important jobs of the twentieth century.
And what happened? The same thing that happened
twenty years earlier at Cambridge: he got the rest of the
world to see things his way.
He~e are Bird and Sherwin again: "Oppenheimer under-
stood that Groves guarded the entrance to the Manhattan
Project, and he therefore turned on all his charm and bril-
liance. It was an irresistible pedormance." Groves was smit-
ten. "'He's a genius,' Groves later told a reporter. 'A real
genius.'" Groves was an engineer by training with a gradu-
ate degree from MIT, and Oppenheimer's great insight was
to appeal to that side of Groves. Bird and Sherwin go on:
"Oppenheimer was the first scientist Groves had met on
his tour [of potential candidates] who grasped that build-
ing an atomic bomb required finding practical solutions
to a variety of cross-disciplinary problems .... [Groves]
99
OUTLIERS
found himself nodding in agreement when Oppenheimer
pitched the notion of a central laboratory devoted to this
purpose, where, as he later testified, 'we could begin to come
to grips with chemical, metallurgical, engineering and ord-
nance problems that had so far received no consideration.'"
Would Oppenheimer have lost his scholarship at Reed?
Would he have been unable to convince his professors to
move his classes to the afternoon? Of course not. And
that's not because he was smarter than Chris Langan. It's
because he possessed the kind of savvy that allowed him
to get what he wanted from the world.
"They required that everyone take introductory cal-
culus," Langan said of his brief stay at Montana State.
"And I happened to get a guy who taught it in a very dry,
very trivial way. I didn't understand why he was teach-
ing it this way. So I asked him questions. I actually had to
chase him down to his office. I asked him, 'Why are you
teaching this way? Why do you consider this practice to
be relevant to calculus?' And this guy, this tall, lanky guy,
always had sweat stains under his arms, he turned and
looked at me and said, 'You know, there is something you
should probably get straight. Some people just don't have
the intellectual firepower to be mathematicians.'"
There they are, the professor and the prodigy, and what
the prodigy clearly wants is to be engaged, at long last,
with a mind that loves mathematics as much as he does.
But he fails. In fact-and this is the most heartbreaking
part of all-he manages to have an entire conversation
with his calculus professor without ever communicating
the one fact most likely to appeal to a calculus professor.
IOO
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
The professor never realizes that Chris Langan is good at
calculus.
3.
The particular skill that allows you to talk your way out
of a murder rap, or convince your professor to move you
from the morning to the afternoon section, is what the psy-
chologist Robert Sternberg calls "practical intelligence.''
To Sternberg, practical intelligence includes things like
"knowing what to say to whom, knowing when to say it,
and knowing how to say it for maximum effect." It is pro-
cedural: it is about knowing how to do something with-
out necessarily knowing why you know it or being able to
explain it. It's practical in nature: that is, it's not knowledge
for its own sake. It's knowledge that helps you read situa-
tions correctly and get what you want. And, critically, it is
a kind of intelligence separate from the sort of analytical
ability measured by IQ. To use the technical term, general
intelligence and practical intelligence are "orthogonal": the
presence of one doesn't imply the presence of the other.
You can have lots of analytical intelligence and very little
practical intelligence, or lots of practical intelligence and
not much analytical intelligence, or-as in the lucky case
of someone like Robert Oppenheimer-you can have lots
of both.
So where does something like practical intelligence
come from? We know where analytical intelligence comes
from. It's something, at least in part, that's in your genes.
Chris Langan started talking at six months. He taught
IOI
OUTLIERS
himself to read at three years of age. He was born smart.
IQ is a measure, to some degree, of innate ability.''· But
social savvy is knowledge. It's a set of skills that have to
be learned. It has to come from somewhere, and the place
where we seem to get these kinds of attitudes and skills is
from our families.
Perhaps the best explanation we have of this process
comes from the sociologist Annette Lareau, who a few years
ago conducted a fascinating study of a group of third grad-
ers. She picked both blacks and whites and children from
both wealthy homes and poor homes, zeroing in, ultimately,
on twelve families. Lareau and her team visited each family
at least twenty times, for hours at a stretch. She and her assis-
tants told their subjects to treat them like "the family dog,"
and they followed them to church and to soccer games and
to doctor's appointments, with a tape recorder in one hand
and a notebook in the other.
You might expect that if you spent such an extended
period in twelve different households, what you would
gather is twelve different ideas about how to raise children:
there would be the strict parents and the lax parents and
the hyperinvolved parents and the mellow parents and on
and on. What Lareau found, however, is something much
different. There were only two parenting "philosophies,"
and they divided almost perfectly along class lines. The
wealthier parents raised their kids one way, and the poorer
parents raised their kids another way.
The wealthier parents were heavily involved in their
children's free time, shuttling them from one activity to
''Most estimates put the heritability of IQ at roughly 50 percent.
102
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
the next, quizzing them about their teachers and coaches
and teammates. One of the well-off children Lareau fol-
lowed played on a baseball team, two soccer teams, a swim
team, and a basketball team in the summer, as well as play-
ing in an orchestra and taking piano lessons.
That kind of intensive scheduling was almost entirely
absent from the lives of the poor children. Play for them
wasn't soccer practice twice a week. It was making up games
outside with their siblings and other kids in the neighbor-
hood. What a child did was considered by his or her par-
ents as something separate from the adult world and not
particularly consequential. One girl from a working-class
family-Katie Brindle-sang in a choir after school. But
she signed up for it herself and walked to choir practice on
her own. Lareau writes:
What Mrs. Brindle doesn't do that is routine for middle-
class mothers is view her daughter's interest in singing as
a signal to look for other ways to help her develop that
interest into a formal talent. Similarly Mrs. Brindle does
not discuss Katie's interest in drama or express regret
that she cannot afford to cultivate her daughter's talent.
Instead she frames Katie's skills and interests as character
traits-singing and acting are part of what makes Katie
"Katie." She sees the shows her daughter puts on as "cute"
and as a way for Katie to "get attention."
The middle-class parents talked things through with
their children, reasoning with them. They didn't just issue
commands. They expected their children to talk back
to them, to negotiate, to question adults in positions of
authority. If their children were doing poorly at school, the
IO)
OUTLIERS
wealthier parents challenged their teachers. They inter-
vened on behalf of their kids. One child Lareau follows
just misses qualifying for a gifted program. Her mother
arranges for her to be retested privately, petitions the
school, and gets her daughter admitted. The poor parents,
by contrast, are intimidated by authority. They react pas-
sively and stay in the background. Lareau writes of one
low-income parent:
At a parent-teacher conference, for example, Ms. McAl-
lister (who is a high school graduate) seems subdued. The
gregarious and outgoing nature she displays at home is
hidden in this setting. She sits hunched over in the chair
and she keeps her jacket zipped up. She is very quiet.
When the teacher reports that Harold has not been turn-
ing in his homework, Ms. McAllister clearly is flabber-
gasted, but all she says is, "He did it at home." She does
not follow up with the teacher or attempt to intervene
on Harold's behalf. In her view, it is up to the teachers to
manage her son's education. That is their job, not hers.
Lareau calls the middle-class parenting style "concerted
cultivation." It's an attempt to actively "foster and assess a
child's talents, opinions and skills." Poor parents tend to fol-
low, by contrast, a strategy of "accomplishment of natural
growth." They see as their responsibility to care for their
children but to let them grow and develop on their own.
Lareau stresses that one style isn't morally better than
the other. The poorer children were, to her mind, often
better behaved, less whiny, more creative in making use
of their own time, and had a well-developed sense of inde-
pendence. But in practical terms, concerted cultivation
104
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
has enormous advantages. The heavily scheduled middle-
class child is exposed to a constantly shifting set of expe-
riences. She learns teamwork and how to cope in highly
structured settings. She is taught how to interact comfort-
ably with adults, and to speak up when she needs to. In
Lareau's words, the middle-class children learn a sense of
''entitlement."
That word, of course, has negative connotations these
days. But Lareau means it in the best sense of the term:
"They acted as though they had a right to pursue their own
individual preferences and to actively manage interactions
in institutional settings. They appeared comfortable in
those settings; they were open to sharing information and
asking for attention .... It was common practice among
middle-class children to shift interactions to suit their
prefere]lces." They knew the rules. "Even in fourth grade,
middle-class children appeared to be acting on their own
behalf to gain advantages. They made special requests of
teachers apd doctors to adjust procedures to accommo-
date their desires."
By contrast, the working-class and poor children
were characterized by "an emerging sense of distance, dis-
trust, and constraint." They didn't know how to get their
way, or how to "customize" -using Lareau's wonderful
term -whatever environment they were in, for their best
purposes.
In one telling scene, Lareau describes a visit to the doc-
tor by Alex Williams, a nine-year-old boy, and his mother,
Christina. The Williamses are wealthy professionals.
"Alex, you should be thinking of questions you might
want to ask the doctor," Christina says in the car on the
105
OUTLIERS
way to the doctor's office. "You can ask him anything you
want. Don't be shy. You can ask anything."
Alex thinks for a minute, then says, "I have some
bumps under my arms from my deodorant." Christina:
"Really? You mean from your new deodorant?" Alex:
"Yes." Christina: "Well, you should ask the doctor."
Alex's mother, Lareau writes, "is teaching that he has
the right to speak up" -that even though he's going to be
in a room with an older person and authority figure, it's
perfectly all right for him to assert himself. They meet
the doctor, a genial man in his early forties. He tells Alex
that he is in the ninety-fifth percentile in height. Alex then
interrupts:
ALEX: I'm in the what?
DocTOR: It means that you're taller than more than
ninety-five out of a hundred young men when they're,
uh, ten years old.
ALEX: I'm not ten.
DocTOR: Well, they graphed you at ten. You're-nine
years and ten months. They-they usually take the
closest year to that graph.
Look at how easily Alex interrupts the doctor- "I'm
not ten." That's entitlement: his mother permits that casual
incivility because she wants him to learn to assert himself
with people in positions of authority.
THE DocToR TURNS TO ALEX: Well, now the most
important question. Do you have any questions you
want to ask me befor~ I do your physical?
I 0 6
THE TROUBLE VITH GENIUSES, PART 2
ALEX: Um ... only one. I've been getting some bumps
on my arms, right around here (indicates underarm).
DOCTOR: Underneath?
ALEx:Yeah.
DocTOR: Okay. I'll have to take a look at those when I
come in closer to do the checkup. And I'll see what
they are and what I can do. Do they hurt or itch?
ALEX: No, they're just there.
DocTOR: Okay, I'll take a look at those bumps for you.
This kind of interaction simply doesn't happen with
lower-class children, Lareau says. They would be quiet
and submissive, with eyes turned away. Alex takes charge
of the moment. "In remembering to raise the question he
prepared in advance, he gains the doctor's full attention
and foc.µses it on an issue of his choosing," Lareau writes.
In so doing, he successfully shifts the balance of power
away from the adults and toward himself. The transi-
tion goes smoothly. Alex is used to being treated with
respect. He is seen as special and as a person worthy of
adult attention and interest. These are key characteris-
tics of the strategy of concerted cultivation. Alex is not
showing off during his checkup. He is behaving much
as he does with his parents- he reasons, negotiates, and
jokes with equal ease.
It is important to understand where the particular
mastery of that moment comes from. It's not genetic. Alex
Williams didn't inherit the skills to interact with author-
ity figures from his parents and grandparents the way he
inherited the color of his eyes. Nor is it racial: it's not a
107
OUTLIERS
practice specific to either black or white people. As it turns
out, Alex Williams is black and Katie Brindle is white. It's
a cultural advantage. Alex has those skills because over the
course of his young life, his mother and father-in the
manner of educated families-have painstakingly taught
them to him, nudging and prodding and encouraging and
showing him the rules of the game, right down to that lit-
tle rehearsal in the car on the way to the doctor's office.
When we talk about the advantages of class, Lareau
argues, this is in large part what we mean. Alex Williams
is better off than Katie Brindle because he's wealthier and
because he goes to a better school, but also because- and
perhaps this is even more critical-the sense of entitle-
ment that he has been taught is an attitude perfectly suited
to succeeding in the modern world.
4.
This is the advantage that Oppenheimer had and that Chris
Langan lacked. Oppenheimer was raised in one of the
wealthiest neighborhoods in Manhattan, the son of an art-
ist and a successful garment manufacturer. His childhood
was the embodiment of concerted cultivation. On weekends,
the Oppenheimers would go driving in the countryside in
a chauffeur-driven Packard. Summers he would be taken to
Europe to see his grandfather. He attended the Ethical Cul-
ture School on Central Park West, perhaps the most pro-
gressive school in the nation, where, his biographers write,
students were "infused with the notion that they were being
groomed to reform the world." When his math teacher real-
ized he was bored, she sent him off to do independent work.
ro8
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES,. PART 2
As a child, Oppenheimer was passionate about rock
collecting. At the age of twelve, he began corresponding
with local geologists about rock formations he had seen in
Central Park, and he so impressed them that they invited
him to give a lecture before the New York Mineralogical
Club. As Sherwin and Bird write, Oppenheimer's par-
ents responded to their son's hobby in an almost textbook
example of concerted cultivation:
Dreading the thought of having to talk to an audience
of adults, Robert begged his father to explain that they
had invited a twelve-year-old. Greatly amused, Julius
encouraged his son to accept this honor. On the des-
ignated evening, Robert showed up at the club with his
parents, who proudly introduced their son as J. Robert
Oppenheimer. The startled audience of geologists and
amateur rock collectors burst out laughing when he
stepped up to the podium: a wooden box had to be found
for him to stand on so that the audience could see more
than ,the shock of his wiry black hair sticking up above
the lectern. Shy and awkward, Robert nevertheless read
his prepared remarks and was given a hearty round of
applause.
Is it any wonder Oppenheimer handled the challenges
of his life so brilliantly? If you are someone whose father
has made his way up in the business world, then you've
seen, firsthand, what it means to negotiate your way out of
a tight spot. If you're someone who was sent to the Ethical
Culture School, then you aren't going to be intimidated
by a row of Cambridge dons arrayed in judgment against
you. If you studied physics at Harvard, then you know
OUTLIERS
how to talk to an army general who did engineering just
down the road at MIT.
Chris Langan, by contrast, had only the bleakness of
Bozeman, and a home dominated by an angry, drunken
stepfather. "[Jack] Langan did this to all of us," said Mark.
"We all have a true resentment of authority." That was
the lesson Langan learned from his childhood: distrust
authority and be independent. He never had a parent teach
him on the way to the doctor how to speak up for himself,
or how to reason and negotiate with those in positions of
authority. He didn't learn entitlement. He learned con-
straint. It may seem like a small thing, but it was a crip-
pling handicap in navigating the world beyond Bozeman.
"I couldn't get any financial aid either," Mark went on.
"We just had zero knowledge, less than zero knowledge,
of the process. How to apply. The forms. Checkbooks. It
was not our environment."
"If Christopher had been born into a wealthy family,
if he was the son of a doctor who was well connected in
some major market, I guarantee you he would have been
one of those guys you read about, knocking back PhDs
at seventeen," his brother Jeff says. "It's the culture you
find yourself in that determines that. The issue with Chris
is that he was always too bored to actually sit there and
listen to his teachers. If someone had recognized his intel-
ligence and if he was from a family where there was some
kind of value on education, they would have made sure he
wasn't bored."
!IO
THE TROUBLE VITii GENIUSES, PART 2
-
I I I
OUTLIERS
I I 2
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
6.
Today, Chris Langan lives in rural Missouri on a horse
farm. He moved there a few years ago, after he got mar-
ried. He is in his fifties but looks many years younger. He
has the build of a linebacker, thick through the chest, with
enormous biceps. His hair is combed straight back from
his forehead. He has a neat, graying moustache and avia-
tor-style glasses. If you look into his eyes, you can see the
intelligence burning behind them.
"A typical day is, I get up and make coffee. I go in and
sit in front of the computer and begin working on what-
ever I ;was working on the night before," he told me not
long ago. "I found if I go to bed with a question on my
mind, all I have to do is concentrate on the question before
I go to sleep and I virtually always have the answer in the
morning./Sometimes I realize what the answer is because
I dreamt the answer and I can remember it. Other times
I just feel the answer, and I start typing and the answer
h " emerges onto t e page.
He had just been reading the work of the linguist
Noam Chomsky. There were piles of books in his study.
He ordered books from the library all the time. "I always
feel that the closer you get to the original sources, the bet-
ter off you are," he said.
Langan seemed content. He had farm animals to take
care of, and books to read, and a wife he loved. It was a
much better life than being a bouncer.
I I 3
OUTLIERS
"I don't think there is anyone smarter than me out
there," he went on. "I have never met anybody like me or
never seen even an indication that there is somebody who
actually has better powers of comprehension.Never seen it
and I don't think I am going to. I could-my mind is open
to the possibility. If anyone should challenge me- 'Oh, I
think that I am smarter than you are' - I think I could
have them."
What he said sounded boastful, but it wasn't really. It
was the opposite-a touch defensive. He'd been working for
decades now on a project of enormous sophistication-but
almost none of what he had done had ever been published
much less read by the physicists and philosophers and
mathematicians who might be able to judge its value. Here
he was, a man with a one-in-a-million mind, and he had
yet to have any impact on the world. He wasn't holding
forth at academic conferences. He wasn't leading a gradu-
ate seminar at some prestigious university. He was living
on a slightly tumbledown horse farm in northern Mis-
souri, sitting on the back porch in jeans and a cutoff T-
shirt. He knew how it looked: it was the great paradox of
Chris Langan's genius.
"I have not pursued mainstream publishers as hard as I
should have," he conceded. "Going around, querying pub-
lishers, trying to find an agent. I haven't done it, and I am
not interested in doing it."
It was an admission of defeat. Every experience he had
had outside of his own mind had ended in frustration. He
knew he needed to do a better job of navigating the world,
but he didn't know how. He couldn't even talk to his cal-
culus teacher, for goodness' sake. These were things that
I I 4
THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2
others, with lesser minds, could master easily. But that's
because those others had had help along the way, and
Chris Langan never had. It wasn't an excuse. It was a fact.
He'd had to make his way alone, and no one-not rock
stars, not professional athletes, not software billionaires,
and not even geniuses- ever makes it alone.
I I 5
O n the day of the bigfootball game between
the University of Missouri
Tigers and the Cowboys of Oklahoma State, a football scout
named
Dan Shonka sat in his hotel, in Columbia, Missouri, with a
portable
DVD player. Shonka has worked for three National Football
League
teams. Before that, he was a football coach, and before that he
played
linebacker—although, he says, “that was three knee operations
and a
hundred pounds ago.” Every year, he evaluates somewhere
between
eight hundred and twelve hundred players around the country,
helping professional teams decide whom to choose in the
college
draft, which means that over the last thirty years he has
probably
seen as many football games as anyone else in America. In his
DVD
player was his homework for the evening’s big game—an edited
video
of the Tigers’ previous contest, against the University of
Nebraska
Cornhuskers.
Shonka methodically made his way through the video, stopping
and
re-winding whenever he saw something that caught his eye. He
liked
ANNALS OF EDUCATION DECEMBER 15, 2008 ISSUE
MOST LIKELY TO SUCCEED
How do we hire when we can’t tell who’s right for the job?
By Malcolm Gladwell
Effective teachers have a gift for noticing—what one
researcher calls “withitness.”
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
1 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
HarryJoo
Sticky Note
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-succeed-malcolm-gladwell
Jeremy Maclin and Chase Coffman, two of the Mizzou
receivers. He
loved William Moore, the team’s bruising strong safety. But,
most of
all, he was interested in the Tigers’ quarterback and star, a
stocky,
strong-armed senior named Chase Daniel.
“I like to see that the quarterback can hit a receiver in stride, so
he
doesn’t have to slow for the ball,” Shonka began. He had a
stack of
evaluation forms next to him and, as he watched the game, he
was
charting and grading every throw that Daniel made. “Then
judgment. Hey, if it’s not there, throw it away and play another
day.
Will he stand in there and take a hit, with a guy breathing down
his
face? Will he be able to step right in there, throw, and still take
that
hit? Does the guy throw better when he’s in the pocket, or does
he
throw equally well when he’s on the move? You want a great
competitor. Durability. Can they hold up, their strength,
toughness?
Can they make big plays? Can they lead a team down the field
and
score late in the game? Can they see the field? When your
team’s way
ahead, that’s fine. But when you’re getting your ass kicked I
want to
see what you’re going to do.”
He pointed to his screen. Daniel had thrown a dart, and, just as
he
did, a defensive player had hit him squarely. “See how he
popped
up?” Shonka said. “He stood right there and threw the ball in
the
face of that rush. This kid has got a lot of courage.” Daniel was
six
feet tall and weighed two hundred and twenty-five pounds: thick
through the chest and trunk. He carried himself with a self-
assurance
that bordered on cockiness. He threw quickly and in rhythm. He
nimbly evaded defenders. He made short throws with touch and
longer throws with accuracy. By the game’s end, he had
completed an
astonishing seventy-eight per cent of his passes, and handed
Nebraska its worst home defeat in fifty-three years. “He can zip
it,”
Shonka said. “He can really gun, when he has to.” Shonka had
seen
all the promising college quarterbacks, charted and graded their
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
2 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
throws, and to his mind Daniel was special: “He might be one of
the
best college quarterbacks in the country.”
But then Shonka began to talk about when he was on the staff of
the
Philadelphia Eagles, in 1999. Five quarterbacks were taken in
the
first round of the college draft that year, and each looked as
promising as Chase Daniel did now. But only one of them,
Donovan
McNabb, ended up fulfilling that promise. Of the rest, one
descended into mediocrity after a decent start. Two were
complete
busts, and the last was so awful that after failing out of the
N.F.L. he
ended up failing out of the Canadian Football League as well.
The year before, the same thing happened with Ryan Leaf, who
was
the Chase Daniel of 1998. The San Diego Chargers made him
the
second player taken over all in the draft, and gave him an
eleven-
million-dollar signing bonus. Leaf turned out to be terrible. In
2002,
it was Joey Harrington’s turn. Harrington was a golden boy out
of the
University of Oregon, and the third player taken in the draft.
Shonka
still can’t get over what happened to him.
“I tell you, I saw Joey live,” he said. “This guy threw lasers, he
could
throw under tight spots, he had the arm strength, he had the
size, he
had the intelligence.” Shonka got as misty as a two-hundred-
and-eighty-pound ex-linebacker in a black tracksuit can get.
“He’s a
concert pianist, you know? I really—I mean, I really—liked
Joey.”
And yet Harrington’s career consisted of a failed stint with the
Detroit Lions and a slide into obscurity. Shonka looked back at
the
screen, where the young man he felt might be the best
quarterback in
the country was marching his team up and down the field. “How
will
that ability translate to the National Football League?” He
shook his
head slowly. “Shoot.”
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
3 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
O
This is the quarterback problem. There are certain jobs where
almost
nothing you can learn about candidates before they start
predicts how
they’ll do once they’re hired. So how do we know whom to
choose in
cases like that? In recent years, a number of fields have begun
to
wrestle with this problem, but none with such profound social
consequences as the profession of teaching.
ne of the most important tools in contemporary educational
research is “value added” analysis. It uses standardized test
scores to look at how much the academic performance of
students in
a given teacher’s classroom changes between the beginning and
the
end of the school year. Suppose that Mrs. Brown and Mr. Smith
both teach a classroom of third graders who score at the fiftieth
percentile on math and reading tests on the first day of school,
in
September. When the students are retested, in June, Mrs.
Brown’s
class scores at the seventieth percentile, while Mr. Smith’s
students
have fallen to the fortieth percentile. That change in the
students’
rankings, value-added theory says, is a meaningful indicator of
how
much more effective Mrs. Brown is as a teacher than Mr. Smith.
It’s only a crude measure, of course. A teacher is not solely
responsible for how much is learned in a classroom, and not
everything of value that a teacher imparts to his or her students
can
be captured on a standardized test. Nonetheless, if you follow
Brown
and Smith for three or four years, their effect on their students’
test
scores starts to become predictable: with enough data, it is
possible to
identify who the very good teachers are and who the very poor
teachers are. What’s more—and this is the finding that has
galvanized the educational world—the difference between good
teachers and poor teachers turns out to be vast.
Eric Hanushek, an economist at Stanford, estimates that the
students of a very bad teacher will learn, on average, half a
year’s
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
4 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
K
worth of material in one school year. The students in the class
of a
very good teacher will learn a year and a half ’s worth of
material.
That difference amounts to a year’s worth of learning in a single
year.
Teacher effects dwarf school effects: your child is actually
better off in
a “bad” school with an excellent teacher than in an excellent
school
with a bad teacher. Teacher effects are also much stronger than
class-size effects. You’d have to cut the average class almost in
half to
get the same boost that you’d get if you switched from an
average
teacher to a teacher in the eighty-fifth percentile. And
remember
that a good teacher costs as much as an average one, whereas
halving
class size would require that you build twice as many
classrooms and
hire twice as many teachers.
Hanushek recently did a back-of-the-envelope calculation about
what even a rudimentary focus on teacher quality could mean
for the
United States. If you rank the countries of the world in terms of
the
academic performance of their schoolchildren, the U.S. is just
below
average, half a standard deviation below a clump of relatively
high-performing countries like Canada and Belgium. According
to
Hanushek, the U.S. could close that gap simply by replacing the
bottom six per cent to ten per cent of public-school teachers
with
teachers of average quality. After years of worrying about
issues like
school funding levels, class size, and curriculum design, many
reformers have come to the conclusion that nothing matters
more
than finding people with the potential to be great teachers. But
there’s a hitch: no one knows what a person with the potential
to be a
great teacher looks like. The school system has a quarterback
problem.
ickoff time for Missouri’s game against Oklahoma State was
seven o’clock. It was a perfect evening for football: cloudless
skies and a light fall breeze. For hours, fans had been tailgating
in the
parking lots around the stadium. Cars lined the roads leading to
the
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
5 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
university, many with fuzzy yellow-and-black Tiger tails
hanging
from their trunks. It was one of Mizzou’s biggest games in
years. The
Tigers were undefeated, and had a chance to become the No. 1
college football team in the country. Shonka made his way
through
the milling crowds and took a seat in the press box. Below him,
the
players on the field looked like pieces on a chessboard.
The Tigers held the ball first. Chase Daniel stood a good seven
yards
behind his offensive line. He had five receivers, two to his left
and
three to his right, spaced from one side of the field to the other.
His
linemen were widely spaced as well. In play after play, Daniel
caught
the snap from his center, planted his feet, and threw the ball in
quick
seven- and eight-yard diagonal passes to one of his five
receivers.
The style of offense that the Tigers run is called the “spread,”
and
most of the top quarterbacks in college football—the players
who
will be drafted into the pros—are spread quarterbacks. By
spacing
out the offensive linemen and wide receivers, the system makes
it
easy for the quarterback to figure out the intentions of the
opposing
defense before the ball is snapped: he can look up and down the
line,
“read” the defense, and decide where to throw the ball before
anyone
has moved a muscle. Daniel had been playing in the spread
since
high school; he was its master. “Look how quickly he gets the
ball
out,” Shonka said. “You can hardly go a thousand and one, a
thousand and two, and it’s out of his hand. He knows right
where
he’s going. When everyone is spread out like that, the defense
can’t
disguise its coverage. Chase knows right away what they are
going to
do. The system simplifies the quarterback’s decisions.”
But for Shonka this didn’t help matters. It had always been hard
to
predict how a college quarterback would fare in the pros. The
professional game was, simply, faster and more complicated.
With
the advent of the spread, though, the correspondence between
the
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
6 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
“ You got anything that doesn’t have green tea in
it?”
two levels of play had broken down almost entirely. N.F.L.
teams
don’t run the spread. They can’t. The defenders in the pros are
so
much faster than their college counterparts that they would
shoot
through those big gaps in the offensive line and flatten the
quarterback. In the N.F.L., the offensive line is bunched closely
together. Daniel wouldn’t have five receivers. Most of the time,
he’d
have just three or four. He wouldn’t have the luxury of standing
seven
yards behind the center, planting his feet, and knowing instantly
where to throw. He’d have to crouch right behind the center,
take the
snap directly, and run backward before planting his feet to
throw.
The onrushing defenders wouldn’t be seven yards away. They
would
be all around him, from the start. The defense would no longer
have
to show its hand, because the field would not be so spread out.
It
could now disguise its intentions. Daniel wouldn’t be able to
read the
defense before the snap was taken. He’d have to read it in the
seconds
after the play began.
“In the spread, you see a lot of
guys wide open,” Shonka said.
“But when a guy like Chase goes
to the N.F.L. he’s never going to see his receivers that open—
only in
some rare case, like someone slips or there’s a bust in the
coverage.
When that ball’s leaving your hands in the pros, if you don’t use
your
eyes to move the defender a little bit, they’ll break on the ball
and
intercept it. The athletic ability that they’re playing against in
the
league is unbelievable.”
As Shonka talked, Daniel was moving his team down the field.
But
he was almost always throwing those quick, diagonal passes. In
the
N.F.L., he would have to do much more than that—he would
have
to throw long, vertical passes over the top of the defense. Could
he
make that kind of throw? Shonka didn’t know. There was also
the
matter of his height. Six feet was fine in a spread system, where
the
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
7 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
big gaps in the offensive line gave Daniel plenty of opportunity
to
throw the ball and see downfield. But in the N.F.L. there
wouldn’t be
gaps, and the linemen rushing at him would be six-five, not six-
one.
“I wonder,” Shonka went on. “Can he see? Can he be productive
in a
new kind of offense? How will he handle that? I’d like to see
him set
up quickly from center. I’d like to see his ability to read
coverages that
are not in the spread. I’d like to see him in the pocket. I’d like
to see
him move his feet. I’d like to see him do a deep dig, or deep
comeback. You know, like a throw twenty to twenty-five yards
down
the field.”
It was clear that Shonka didn’t feel the same hesitancy in
evaluating
the other Mizzou stars—the safety Moore, the receivers Maclin
and
Coffman. The game that they would play in the pros would also
be
different from the game they were playing in college, but the
difference was merely one of degree. They had succeeded at
Missouri
because they were strong and fast and skilled, and these traits
translate in kind to professional football.
A college quarterback joining the N.F.L., by contrast, has to
learn to
play an entirely new game. Shonka began to talk about Tim
Couch,
the quarterback taken first in that legendary draft of 1999.
Couch set
every record imaginable in his years at the University of
Kentucky.
“They used to put five garbage cans on the field,” Shonka
recalled,
shaking his head, “and Couch would stand there and throw and
just
drop the ball into every one.” But Couch was a flop in the pros.
It
wasn’t that professional quarterbacks didn’t need to be accurate.
It
was that the kind of accuracy required to do the job well could
be
measured only in a real N.F.L. game.
Similarly, all quarterbacks drafted into the pros are required to
take
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
8 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
an I.Q. test—the Wonderlic Personnel Test. The theory behind
the
test is that the pro game is so much more cognitively demanding
than the college game that high intelligence should be a good
predictor of success. But when the economists David Berri and
Rob
Simmons analyzed the scores—which are routinely leaked to the
press—they found that Wonderlic scores are all but useless as
predictors. Of the five quarterbacks taken in round one of the
1999
draft, Donovan McNabb, the only one of the five with a shot at
the
Hall of Fame, had the lowest Wonderlic score. And who else
had
I.Q. scores in the same range as McNabb? Dan Marino and
Terry
Bradshaw, two of the greatest quarterbacks ever to play the
game.
We’re used to dealing with prediction problems by going back
and
looking for better predictors. We now realize that being a good
doctor requires the ability to communicate, listen, and
empathize—and so there is increasing pressure on medical
schools to
pay attention to interpersonal skills as well as to test scores. We
can
have better physicians if we’re just smarter about how we
choose
medical-school students. But no one is saying that Dan Shonka
is
somehow missing some key ingredient in his analysis; that if he
were
only more perceptive he could predict Chase Daniel’s career
trajectory. The problem with picking quarterbacks is that Chase
Daniel’s performance can’t be predicted. The job he’s being
groomed
for is so particular and specialized that there is no way to know
who
will succeed at it and who won’t. In fact, Berri and Simmons
found
no connection between where a quarterback was taken in the
draft—that is, how highly he was rated on the basis of his
college
performance—and how well he played in the pros.
The entire time that Chase Daniel was on the field against
Oklahoma State, his backup, Chase Patton, stood on the
sidelines,
watching. Patton didn’t play a single down. In his four years at
Missouri, up to that point, he had thrown a total of twenty-six
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
9 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
P
passes. And yet there were people in Shonka’s world who
thought
that Patton would end up as a better professional quarterback
than
Daniel. The week of the Oklahoma State game, the national
sports
magazine ESPN even put the two players on its cover, with the
title
“chase daniel might win the heisman”—referring to the trophy
given
to college football’s best player. “his backup could win the
super bowl.”
Why did everyone like Patton so much? It wasn’t clear. Maybe
he
looked good in practice. Maybe it was because this season in
the
N.F.L. a quarterback who had also never started in a single
college
game is playing superbly for the New England Patriots. It
sounds
absurd to put an athlete on the cover of a magazine for no
particular
reason. But perhaps that’s just the quarterback problem taken to
an
extreme. If college performance doesn’t tell us anything, why
shouldn’t we value someone who hasn’t had the chance to play
as
highly as someone who plays as well as anyone in the land?
icture a young preschool teacher, sitting on a classroom floor
surrounded by seven children. She is holding an alphabet book,
and working through the letters with the children, one by one: “
‘A’ is
for apple. . . . ‘C’ is for cow.” The session was taped, and the
videotape is being watched by a group of experts, who are
charting
and grading each of the teacher’s moves.
After thirty seconds, the leader of the group—Bob Pianta, the
dean
of the University of Virginia’s Curry School of Education—
stops the
tape. He points to two little girls on the right side of the circle.
They
are unusually active, leaning into the circle and reaching out to
touch
the book.
“What I’m struck by is how lively the affect is in this room,”
Pianta
said. “One of the things the teacher is doing is creating a
holding
space for that. And what distinguishes her from other teachers is
that
she flexibly allows the kids to move and point to the book.
She’s not
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
10 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
rigidly forcing the kids to sit back.”
Pianta’s team has developed a system for evaluating various
competencies relating to student-teacher interaction. Among
them is
“regard for student perspective”; that is, a teacher’s knack for
allowing
students some flexibility in how they become engaged in the
classroom. Pianta stopped and rewound the tape twice, until
what
the teacher had managed to achieve became plain: the children
were
active, but somehow the class hadn’t become a free-for-all.
“A lesser teacher would have responded to the kids’ leaning
over as
misbehavior,” Pianta went on. “ ‘We can’t do this right now.
You need
to be sitting still.’ She would have turned this off.”
Bridget Hamre, one of Pianta’s colleagues, chimed in: “These
are
three- and four-year-olds. At this age, when kids show their
engagement it’s not like the way we show our engagement,
where we
look alert. They’re leaning forward and wriggling. That’s their
way of
doing it. And a good teacher doesn’t interpret that as bad
behavior.
You can see how hard it is to teach new teachers this idea,
because
the minute you teach them to have regard for the student’s
perspective, they think you have to give up control of the
classroom.”
The lesson continued. Pianta pointed out how the teacher
managed
to personalize the material. “ ‘C’ is for cow” turned into a short
discussion of which of the kids had ever visited a farm. “Almost
every
time a child says something, she responds to it, which is what
we
describe as teacher sensitivity,” Hamre said.
The teacher then asked the children if anyone’s name began
with that
letter. “Calvin,” a boy named Calvin says. The teacher nods,
and says,
“Calvin starts with ‘C.’ “ A little girl in the middle says, “Me!”
The
Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely-
to-suc...
11 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
HarryJoo
Highlight
teacher turns to her. “Your name’s Venisha. Letter ‘V.’
Venisha.”
It was a key moment. Of all the teacher elements analyzed by
the
Virginia group, feedback—a direct, personal response by a
teacher to
a specific statement by a student—seems to be most closely
linked to
academic success. Not only did the teacher catch the “Me!”
amid the
wiggling and tumult; she addressed it directly.
“Mind you, that’s not great feedback,” Hamre said. “High-
quality
feedback is where there is a back-and-forth exchange to get a
deeper
understanding.” The perfect way to handle that moment would
have
been for the teacher to pause and pull out Venisha’s name card,
point
to the letter “V,” show her how different it is from “C,” and
make the
class sound out both letters. But the teacher didn’t do that—
either
because it didn’t occur to her or because she was distracted by
the
wiggling of the girls to her right.
“On the other hand, she could have completely ignored the girl,
which happens a lot,” Hamre went on. “The other thing that
happens a lot is the teacher will just say, ‘You’re wrong.’ Yes-
no
feedback is probably the predominant kind of feedback, which
provides almost no information for the kid in terms of
learning.”
Pianta showed another tape, of a nearly identical situation: a
circle of
pre-schoolers around a teacher. The lesson was about how we
can tell
when someone is happy or sad. The teacher began by acting out
a
short conversation between two hand puppets, Henrietta and
Twiggle: Twiggle is sad until Henrietta shares some watermelon
with
him.
“The idea that the teacher is trying to get across is that you can
tell
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked
Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked

More Related Content

Similar to Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked

How to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant h
How to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant hHow to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant h
How to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant hPazSilviapm
 
Why annual performance reviews.docx
Why annual performance reviews.docxWhy annual performance reviews.docx
Why annual performance reviews.docxwrite5
 
Essay On Motivation Theory X
Essay On Motivation Theory XEssay On Motivation Theory X
Essay On Motivation Theory XVictoria Burke
 
Free Agent Nation Workshop Jazzy
Free Agent Nation Workshop JazzyFree Agent Nation Workshop Jazzy
Free Agent Nation Workshop JazzyCoachDavis
 
November 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part i
November 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part iNovember 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part i
November 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part iRonald Brown
 
Beyond "Hire To Retire"
Beyond "Hire To Retire"Beyond "Hire To Retire"
Beyond "Hire To Retire"Upgaged LLC
 
Performance Reviews Blue Paper
Performance Reviews Blue PaperPerformance Reviews Blue Paper
Performance Reviews Blue Paper4imprint
 
Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681
Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681
Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681Edrial Derajat
 
What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?
What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?
What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?Jim Tybur
 
[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...
[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...
[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...Appcast
 
Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)
Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)
Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)JUNAID RASHDI
 
Disruption Innovation Consulting
Disruption Innovation ConsultingDisruption Innovation Consulting
Disruption Innovation ConsultingCMassociates
 
Managing Knowledge Workers
Managing Knowledge WorkersManaging Knowledge Workers
Managing Knowledge WorkersOlivier Serrat
 
1 The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx
1  The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx1  The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx
1 The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docxhoney725342
 
How To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, R
How To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, RHow To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, R
How To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, RLupita Vickrey
 
What Employers Want
What Employers WantWhat Employers Want
What Employers WantS M
 
Career-ambitions-report_online
Career-ambitions-report_onlineCareer-ambitions-report_online
Career-ambitions-report_onlineZsolt
 
2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years
2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years
2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 yearsAhmed Al Bilal
 

Similar to Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked (20)

How to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant h
How to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant hHow to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant h
How to Be a C.E.O., From a Decade’s Worth of ThemAdam Bryant h
 
CEO Talent: America’s Scarcest Resource? 2017 CEO Talent Survey
CEO Talent: America’s Scarcest Resource? 2017 CEO Talent SurveyCEO Talent: America’s Scarcest Resource? 2017 CEO Talent Survey
CEO Talent: America’s Scarcest Resource? 2017 CEO Talent Survey
 
Why annual performance reviews.docx
Why annual performance reviews.docxWhy annual performance reviews.docx
Why annual performance reviews.docx
 
Essay On Motivation Theory X
Essay On Motivation Theory XEssay On Motivation Theory X
Essay On Motivation Theory X
 
Free Agent Nation Workshop Jazzy
Free Agent Nation Workshop JazzyFree Agent Nation Workshop Jazzy
Free Agent Nation Workshop Jazzy
 
November 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part i
November 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part iNovember 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part i
November 19 2020 -leadership thoughts and lessons final part i
 
Beyond "Hire To Retire"
Beyond "Hire To Retire"Beyond "Hire To Retire"
Beyond "Hire To Retire"
 
Performance Reviews Blue Paper
Performance Reviews Blue PaperPerformance Reviews Blue Paper
Performance Reviews Blue Paper
 
Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681
Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681
Talent management-and-the-older-worker-graeme-martin2681
 
What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?
What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?
What is the Difference Between Leadership and Management?
 
[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...
[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...
[Whitepaper] Talent Decisions that can Make or Break your Business - Lessons ...
 
Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)
Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)
Attracting and-retaining-the-right-talent-nov-2017 (1)
 
RIP- Management Consulting
RIP- Management ConsultingRIP- Management Consulting
RIP- Management Consulting
 
Disruption Innovation Consulting
Disruption Innovation ConsultingDisruption Innovation Consulting
Disruption Innovation Consulting
 
Managing Knowledge Workers
Managing Knowledge WorkersManaging Knowledge Workers
Managing Knowledge Workers
 
1 The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx
1  The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx1  The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx
1 The University of Manchester Alliance Mancheste.docx
 
How To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, R
How To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, RHow To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, R
How To Start A Good Essay Conclusion Writing Conclusions, R
 
What Employers Want
What Employers WantWhat Employers Want
What Employers Want
 
Career-ambitions-report_online
Career-ambitions-report_onlineCareer-ambitions-report_online
Career-ambitions-report_online
 
2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years
2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years
2014 q3 McKinsey quarterly - Management, the next 50 years
 

More from ssuser454af01

The following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docx
The following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docxThe following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docx
The following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docxssuser454af01
 
The following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docx
The following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docxThe following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docx
The following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docxssuser454af01
 
The following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docx
The following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docxThe following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docx
The following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docxssuser454af01
 
The following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docx
The following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docxThe following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docx
The following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docxssuser454af01
 
The following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docx
The following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docxThe following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docx
The following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docxssuser454af01
 
The following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docx
The following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docxThe following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docx
The following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docxssuser454af01
 
The following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docx
The following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docxThe following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docx
The following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docxssuser454af01
 
The Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docx
The Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docxThe Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docx
The Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docxssuser454af01
 
The first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docx
The first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docxThe first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docx
The first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docxssuser454af01
 
The first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docx
The first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docxThe first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docx
The first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docxssuser454af01
 
The first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docx
The first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docxThe first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docx
The first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docxssuser454af01
 
The final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docx
The final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docxThe final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docx
The final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docxssuser454af01
 
The first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docx
The first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docxThe first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docx
The first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docxssuser454af01
 
The first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docx
The first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docxThe first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docx
The first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docxssuser454af01
 
The first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docx
The first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docxThe first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docx
The first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docxssuser454af01
 
The final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docx
The final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docxThe final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docx
The final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docxssuser454af01
 
The finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docx
The finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docxThe finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docx
The finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docxssuser454af01
 
The Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docx
The Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docxThe Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docx
The Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docxssuser454af01
 
The Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docx
The Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docxThe Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docx
The Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docxssuser454af01
 
The Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docx
The Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docxThe Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docx
The Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docxssuser454af01
 

More from ssuser454af01 (20)

The following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docx
The following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docxThe following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docx
The following pairs of co-morbid disorders and  a write 700 words .docx
 
The following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docx
The following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docxThe following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docx
The following is an access verification technique, listing several f.docx
 
The following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docx
The following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docxThe following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docx
The following discussion board post has to have a response. Please r.docx
 
The following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docx
The following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docxThe following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docx
The following information has been taken from the ledger accounts of.docx
 
The following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docx
The following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docxThe following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docx
The following attach files are my History Homewrok and Lecture Power.docx
 
The following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docx
The following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docxThe following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docx
The following is adapted from the work of Paul Martin Lester.In .docx
 
The following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docx
The following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docxThe following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docx
The following article is related to deterring employee fraud within .docx
 
The Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docx
The Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docxThe Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docx
The Five stages of ChangeBy Thursday, June 25, 2015, respond to .docx
 
The first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docx
The first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docxThe first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docx
The first step in understanding the behaviors that are associated wi.docx
 
The first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docx
The first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docxThe first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docx
The first one is due Sep 24 at 1100AMthe French-born Mexican jo.docx
 
The first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docx
The first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docxThe first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docx
The first part is a direct quote, copied word for word. Includ.docx
 
The final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docx
The final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docxThe final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docx
The final research paper should be no less than 15 pages and in APA .docx
 
The first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docx
The first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docxThe first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docx
The first one Description Pick a physical activity. Somethi.docx
 
The first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docx
The first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docxThe first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docx
The first column suggests traditional familyschool relationships an.docx
 
The first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docx
The first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docxThe first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docx
The first president that I actually remembered was Jimmy Carter.  .docx
 
The final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docx
The final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docxThe final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docx
The final project for this course is the creation of a conceptual mo.docx
 
The finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docx
The finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docxThe finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docx
The finance department of a large corporation has evaluated a possib.docx
 
The Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docx
The Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docxThe Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docx
The Final Paper must have depth of scholarship, originality, theoret.docx
 
The Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docx
The Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docxThe Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docx
The Final exam primarily covers the areas of the hydrosphere, the bi.docx
 
The Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docx
The Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docxThe Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docx
The Final Paper must be 8 pages (not including title and reference p.docx
 

Recently uploaded

18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 

Recently uploaded (20)

18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 

Are smart people overrated? The talent myth debunked

  • 1. F ive years ago, several executives at McKinsey & Company,America’s largest and most prestigious management- consulting firm, launched what they called the War for Talent. Thousands of questionnaires were sent to managers across the country. Eighteen companies were singled out for special attention, and the consultants spent up to three days at each firm, interviewing everyone from the C.E.O. down to the human-resources staff. McKinsey wanted to document how the top-performing companies in America differed from other firms in the way they handle matters like hiring and promotion. But, as the consultants sifted through the piles of reports and questionnaires and interview transcripts, they grew convinced that the difference between winners and losers was more profound than they had realized. “We looked at one another and suddenly the light bulb blinked on,” the three consultants who headed the project—Ed Michaels, Helen Handfield-Jones, and Beth Axelrod —write in their new book, also called “The War for Talent.” The very best companies, they concluded, had leaders who were obsessed with the talent issue. They recruited ceaselessly, finding and hiring as
  • 2. many top performers as possible. They singled out and segregated DEPT. OF HUMAN RESOURCES JULY 22, 2002 ISSUE THE TALENT MYTH Are smart people overrated? By Malcolm Gladwell THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 1 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM HarryJoo Sticky Note http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth their stars, rewarding them disproportionately, and pushing them into ever more senior positions. “Bet on the natural athletes, the ones with the strongest intrinsic skills,” the authors approvingly quote one senior General Electric executive as saying. “Don’t be afraid to promote stars without specifically relevant experience, seemingly over their heads.” Success in the modern economy, according to Michaels, Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod, requires “the talent mind-set”: the
  • 3. “deep-seated belief that having better talent at all levels is how you outperform your competitors.” This “talent mind-set” is the new orthodoxy of American management. It is the intellectual justification for why such a high premium is placed on degrees from first-tier business schools, and why the compensation packages for top executives have become so lavish. In the modern corporation, the system is considered only as strong as its stars, and, in the past few years, this message has been preached by consultants and management gurus all over the world. None, however, have spread the word quite so ardently as McKinsey, and, of all its clients, one firm took the talent mind-set closest to heart. It was a company where McKinsey conducted twenty separate projects, where McKinsey’s billings topped ten million dollars a year, where a McKinsey director regularly attended board meetings, and where the C.E.O. himself was a former McKinsey partner. The company, of course, was Enron. The Enron scandal is now almost a year old. The reputations of Jeffrey Skilling and Kenneth Lay, the company’s two top executives, have been destroyed. Arthur Andersen, Enron’s auditor, has been driven out of business, and now investigators have turned their
  • 4. attention to Enron’s investment bankers. The one Enron partner that has escaped largely unscathed is McKinsey, which is odd, given that it essentially created the blueprint for the Enron culture. Enron was the ultimate “talent” company. When Skilling started the corporate THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 2 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM A division known as Enron Capital and Trade, in 1990, he “decided to bring in a steady stream of the very best college and M.B.A. graduates he could find to stock the company with talent,” Michaels, Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod tell us. During the nineties, Enron was bringing in two hundred and fifty newly minted M.B.A.s a year. “We had these things called Super Saturdays,” one former Enron manager recalls. “I’d interview some of these guys who were fresh out of Harvard, and these kids could blow me out of the water. They knew things I’d never heard of.” Once at Enron, the top performers were rewarded inordinately, and promoted without regard for seniority or experience. Enron was a star system. “The only
  • 5. thing that differentiates Enron from our competitors is our people, our talent,” Lay, Enron’s former chairman and C.E.O., told the McKinsey consultants when they came to the company’s headquarters, in Houston. Or, as another senior Enron executive put it to Richard Foster, a McKinsey partner who celebrated Enron in his 2001 book, “Creative Destruction,” “We hire very smart people and we pay them more than they think they are worth.” The management of Enron, in other words, did exactly what the consultants at McKinsey said that companies ought to do in order to succeed in the modern economy. It hired and rewarded the very best and the very brightest—and it is now in bankruptcy. The reasons for its collapse are complex, needless to say. But what if Enron failed not in spite of its talent mind-set but because of it? What if smart people are overrated? t the heart of the McKinsey vision is a process that the War for Talent advocates refer to as “differentiation and affirmation.” Employers, they argue, need to sit down once or twice a year and hold a “candid, probing, no-holds-barred debate about each individual,” sorting employees into A, B, and C groups. The A’s must be challenged and disproportionately rewarded. The B’s need to be
  • 6. THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 3 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM encouraged and affirmed. The C’s need to shape up or be shipped out. Enron followed this advice almost to the letter, setting up internal Performance Review Committees. The members got together twice a year, and graded each person in their section on ten separate criteria, using a scale of one to five. The process was called “rank and yank.” Those graded at the top of their unit received bonuses two-thirds higher than those in the next thirty per cent; those who ranked at the bottom received no bonuses and no extra stock options—and in some cases were pushed out. How should that ranking be done? Unfortunately, the McKinsey consultants spend very little time discussing the matter. One possibility is simply to hire and reward the smartest people. But the link between, say, I.Q. and job performance is distinctly underwhelming. On a scale where 0.1 or below means virtually no correlation and 0.7 or above implies a strong correlation (your height, for example, has a 0.7 correlation with your parents’ height), the correlation between I.Q. and occupational success is between 0.2 and 0.3. “What I.Q. doesn’t pick up is effectiveness at common-
  • 7. sense sorts of things, especially working with people,” Richard Wagner, a psychologist at Florida State University, says. “In terms of how we evaluate schooling, everything is about working by yourself. If you work with someone else, it’s called cheating. Once you get out in the real world, everything you do involves working with other people.” Wagner and Robert Sternberg, a psychologist at Yale University, have developed tests of this practical component, which they call “tacit knowledge.” Tacit knowledge involves things like knowing how to manage yourself and others, and how to navigate complicated social situations. Here is a question from one of their tests: You have just been promoted to head of an important department in your organization. The previous head has been transferred to an THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 4 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM equivalent position in a less important department. Your understanding
  • 8. of the reason for the move is that the performance of the department as a whole has been mediocre. There have not been any glaring deficiencies, just a perception of the department as so-so rather than very good. Your charge is to shape up the department. Results are expected quickly. Rate the quality of the following strategies for succeeding at your new position. a) Always delegate to the most junior person who can be trusted with the task. b) Give your superiors frequent progress reports. c) Announce a major reorganization of the department that includes getting rid of whomever you believe to be “dead wood.” d) Concentrate more on your people than on the tasks to be done. e) Make people feel completely responsible for their work. Wagner finds that how well people do on a test like this predicts how well they will do in the workplace: good managers pick (b) and (e); bad managers tend to pick (c). Yet there’s no clear connection between such tacit knowledge and other forms of knowledge and experience. The process of assessing ability in the workplace is a lot messier than it appears.
  • 9. An employer really wants to assess not potential but performance. Yet that’s just as tricky. In “The War for Talent,” the authors talk about how the Royal Air Force used the A, B, and C ranking system for its pilots during the Battle of Britain. But ranking fighter pilots— for whom there are a limited and relatively objective set of performance criteria (enemy kills, for example, and the ability to get their formations safely home)—is a lot easier than assessing how the manager of a new unit is doing at, say, marketing or business development. And whom do you ask to rate the manager’s performance? Studies show that there is very little correlation THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 5 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM between how someone’s peers rate him and how his boss rates him. The only rigorous way to assess performance, according to human- resources specialists, is to use criteria that are as specific as possible. Managers are supposed to take detailed notes on their employees throughout the year, in order to remove subjective personal reactions from the process of assessment. You can grade someone’s performance only if you know their performance. And, in the
  • 10. freewheeling culture of Enron, this was all but impossible. People deemed “talented” were constantly being pushed into new jobs and given new challenges. Annual turnover from promotions was close to twenty per cent. Lynda Clemmons, the so-called “weather babe” who started Enron’s weather derivatives business, jumped, in seven quick years, from trader to associate to manager to director and, finally, to head of her own business unit. How do you evaluate someone’s performance in a system where no one is in a job long enough to allow such evaluation? The answer is that you end up doing performance evaluations that aren’t based on performance. Among the many glowing books about Enron written before its fall was the best-seller “Leading the Revolution,” by the management consultant Gary Hamel, which tells the story of Lou Pai, who launched Enron’s power-trading business. Pai’s group began with a disaster: it lost tens of millions of dollars trying to sell electricity to residential consumers in newly deregulated markets. The problem, Hamel explains, is that the markets weren’t truly deregulated: “The states that were opening their markets to competition were still setting rules designed to give their traditional utilities big advantages.” It doesn’t seem to have occurred to anyone
  • 11. that Pai ought to have looked into those rules more carefully before risking millions of dollars. He was promptly given the chance to build the commercial electricity-outsourcing business, where he ran up several more years of heavy losses before cashing out of Enron last year with two hundred and seventy million dollars. Because Pai had THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 6 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM W “talent,” he was given new opportunities, and when he failed at those new opportunities he was given still more opportunities . . . because he had “talent.” “At Enron, failure—even of the type that ends up on the front page of the Wall Street Journal—doesn’t necessarily sink a career,” Hamel writes, as if that were a good thing. Presumably, companies that want to encourage risk-taking must be willing to tolerate mistakes. Yet if talent is defined as something separate from an employee’s actual performance, what use is it, exactly? hat the War for Talent amounts to is an argument for indulging A employees, for fawning over them. “You need to
  • 12. do everything you can to keep them engaged and satisfied— even delighted,” Michaels, Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod write. “Find out what they would most like to be doing, and shape their career and responsibilities in that direction. Solve any issues that might be pushing them out the door, such as a boss that frustrates them or travel demands that burden them.” No company was better at this than Enron. In one oft-told story, Louise Kitchin, a twenty- nine-year-old gas trader in Europe, became convinced that the company ought to develop an online-trading business. She told her boss, and she began working in her spare time on the project, until she had two hundred and fifty people throughout Enron helping her. After six months, Skilling was finally informed. “I was never asked for any capital,” Skilling said later. “I was never asked for any people. They had already purchased the servers. They had already started ripping apart the building. They had started legal reviews in twenty-two countries by the time I heard about it.” It was, Skilling went on approvingly, “exactly the kind of behavior that will continue to drive this company forward.” Kitchin’s qualification for running EnronOnline, it should be pointed out, was not that she was good at it. It was that she wanted to do it,
  • 13. and Enron was a place where stars did whatever they wanted. “Fluid THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 7 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM movement is absolutely necessary in our company. And the type of people we hire enforces that,” Skilling told the team from McKinsey. “Not only does this system help the excitement level for each manager, it shapes Enron’s business in the direction that its managers find most exciting.” Here is Skilling again: “If lots of [employees] are flocking to a new business unit, that’s a good sign that the opportunity is a good one. . . . If a business unit can’t attract people very easily, that’s a good sign that it’s a business Enron shouldn’t be in.” You might expect a C.E.O. to say that if a business unit can’t attract customers very easily that’s a good sign it’s a business the company shouldn’t be in. A company’s business is supposed to be shaped in the direction that its managers find most prof itable. But at Enron the needs of the customers and the shareholders were secondary to the needs of its stars.
  • 14. A dozen years ago, the psychologists Robert Hogan, Robert Raskin, and Dan Fazzini wrote a brilliant essay called “The Dark Side of Charisma.” It argued that flawed managers fall into three types. One is the High Likability Floater, who rises effortlessly in an organization because he never takes any difficult decisions or makes any enemies. Another is the Homme de Ressentiment, who seethes below the surface and plots against his enemies. The most interesting of the three is the Narcissist, whose energy and self-confidence and charm lead him inexorably up the corporate ladder. Narcissists are terrible managers. They resist accepting suggestions, thinking it will make them appear weak, and they don’t believe that others have anything useful to tell them. “Narcissists are biased to take more credit for success than is legitimate,” Hogan and his co-authors write, and “biased to avoid acknowledging responsibility for their failures and shortcomings for the same reasons that they claim more success than is their due.” Moreover: Narcissists typically make judgments with greater confidence than THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth
  • 15. 8 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM other people . . . and, because their judgments are rendered with such conviction, other people tend to believe them and the narcissists become disproportionately more influential in group situations. Finally, because of their self-confidence and strong need for recognition, narcissists tend to “self-nominate”; consequently, when a leadership gap appears in a group or organization, the narcissists rush to fill it. Tyco Corporation and WorldCom were the Greedy Corporations: they were purely interested in short-term financial gain. Enron was the Narcissistic Corporation—a company that took more credit for success than was legitimate, that did not acknowledge responsibility for its failures, that shrewdly sold the rest of us on its genius, and that substituted self-nomination for disciplined management. At one point in “Leading the Revolution,” Hamel tracks down a senior Enron executive, and what he breathlessly recounts—the braggadocio, the self-satisfaction—could be an epitaph for the talent mind-set: “You cannot control the atoms within a nuclear fusion reaction,” said Ken Rice when he was head of Enron Capital and Trade
  • 16. Resources (ECT), America’s largest marketer of natural gas and largest buyer and seller of electricity. Adorned in a black T-shirt, blue jeans, and cowboy boots, Rice drew a box on an office whiteboard that pictured his business unit as a nuclear reactor. Little circles in the box represented its “contract originators,” the gunslingers charged with doing deals and creating new businesses. Attached to each circle was an arrow. In Rice’s diagram the arrows were pointing in all different directions. “We allow people to go in whichever direction that they want to go.” The distinction between the Greedy Corporation and the Narcissistic Corporation matters, because the way we conceive our attainments helps determine how we behave. Carol Dweck, a psychologist at Columbia University, has found that people generally hold one of two fairly firm beliefs about their intelligence: they consider it either THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 9 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM a fixed trait or something that is malleable and can be developed over
  • 17. time. Five years ago, Dweck did a study at the University of Hong Kong, where all classes are conducted in English. She and her colleagues approached a large group of social-sciences students, told them their English-proficiency scores, and asked them if they wanted to take a course to improve their language skills. One would expect all those who scored poorly to sign up for the remedial course. The University of Hong Kong is a demanding institution, and it is hard to do well in the social sciences without strong English skills. Curiously, however, only the ones who believed in malleable intelligence expressed interest in the class. The students who believed that their intelligence was a fixed trait were so concerned about appearing to be deficient that they preferred to stay home. “Students who hold a fixed view of their intelligence care so much about looking smart that they act dumb,” Dweck writes, “for what could be dumber than giving up a chance to learn something that is essential for your own success?” In a similar experiment, Dweck gave a class of preadolescent students a test filled with challenging problems. After they were finished, one group was praised for its effort and another group was praised for its intelligence. Those praised for their intelligence were reluctant to tackle difficult tasks, and their performance on subsequent tests
  • 18. soon began to suffer. Then Dweck asked the children to write a letter to students at another school, describing their experience in the study. She discovered something remarkable: forty per cent of those students who were praised for their intelligence lied about how they had scored on the test, adjusting their grade upward. They weren’t naturally deceptive people, and they weren’t any less intelligent or self-confident than anyone else. They simply did what people do when they are immersed in an environment that celebrates them solely for their innate “talent.” They begin to define themselves by that description, and when times get tough and that self-image is THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 10 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM T threatened they have difficulty with the consequences. They will not take the remedial course. They will not stand up to investors and the public and admit that they were wrong. They’d sooner lie. he broader failing of McKinsey and its acolytes at Enron is
  • 19. their assumption that an organization’s intelligence is simply a function of the intelligence of its employees. They believe in stars, because they don’t believe in systems. In a way, that’s understandable, because our lives are so obviously enriched by individual brilliance. Groups don’t write great novels, and a committee didn’t come up with the theory of relativity. But companies work by different rules. They don’t just create; they execute and compete and coördinate the efforts of many different people, and the organizations that are most successful at that task are the ones where the system is the star. There is a wonderful example of this in the story of the so- called Eastern Pearl Harbor, of the Second World War. During the first nine months of 1942, the United States Navy suffered a catastrophe. German U-boats, operating just off the Atlantic coast and in the Caribbean, were sinking our merchant ships almost at will. U- boat captains marvelled at their good fortune. “Before this sea of light, against this footlight glare of a carefree new world were passing the silhouettes of ships recognizable in every detail and sharp as the outlines in a sales catalogue,” one U-boat commander wrote. “All we had to do was press the button.” What made this such a puzzle is that, on the other side of the
  • 20. Atlantic, the British had much less trouble defending their ships against U-boat attacks. The British, furthermore, eagerly passed on to the Americans everything they knew about sonar and depth- charge throwers and the construction of destroyers. And still the Germans managed to paralyze America’s coastal zones. THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 11 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM You can imagine what the consultants at McKinsey would have concluded: they would have said that the Navy did not have a talent mind-set, that President Roosevelt needed to recruit and promote top performers into key positions in the Atlantic command. In fact, he had already done that. At the beginning of the war, he had pushed out the solid and unspectacular Admiral Harold R. Stark as Chief of Naval Operations and replaced him with the legendary Ernest Joseph King. “He was a supreme realist with the arrogance of genius,” Ladislas Farago writes in “The Tenth Fleet,” a history of the Navy’s U-boat battles in the Second World War. “He had unbounded faith in himself, in his vast knowledge of naval matters and in the soundness of his ideas. Unlike Stark, who tolerated incompetence all
  • 21. around him, King had no patience with fools.” The Navy had plenty of talent at the top, in other words. What it didn’t have was the right kind of organization. As Eliot A. Cohen, a scholar of military strategy at Johns Hopkins, writes in his brilliant book “Military Misfortunes in the Atlantic”: To wage the antisubmarine war well, analysts had to bring together fragments of information, direction-finding fixes, visual sightings, decrypts, and the “flaming datum” of a U-boat attack—for use by a commander to coordinate the efforts of warships, aircraft, and convoy commanders. Such synthesis had to occur in near “real time”— within hours, even minutes in some cases. The British excelled at the task because they had a centralized operational system. The controllers moved the British ships around the Atlantic like chess pieces, in order to outsmart U-boat “wolf packs.” By contrast, Admiral King believed strongly in a decentralized management structure: he held that managers should never tell their subordinates ” ‘how’ as well as what to ‘do.’ “ In today’s jargon, we would say he was a believer in “loose-tight” THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth
  • 22. 12 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM T management, of the kind celebrated by the McKinsey consultants Thomas J. Peters and Robert H. Waterman in their 1982 best- seller, “In Search of Excellence.” But “loose-tight” doesn’t help you find U-boats. Throughout most of 1942, the Navy kept trying to act smart by relying on technical know-how, and stubbornly refused to take operational lessons from the British. The Navy also lacked the organizational structure necessary to apply the technical knowledge it did have to the field. Only when the Navy set up the Tenth Fleet—a single unit to coördinate all anti-submarine warfare in the Atlantic—did the situation change. In the year and a half before the Tenth Fleet was formed, in May of 1943, the Navy sank thirty- six U-boats. In the six months afterward, it sank seventy-five. “The creation of the Tenth Fleet did not bring more talented individuals into the field of ASW”—anti-submarine warfare—”than had previous organizations,” Cohen writes. “What Tenth Fleet did allow, by virtue of its organization and mandate, was for these individuals to become far more effective than previously.” The talent myth assumes
  • 23. that people make organizations smart. More often than not, it’s the other way around. here is ample evidence of this principle among America’s most successful companies. Southwest Airlines hires very few M.B.A.s, pays its managers modestly, and gives raises according to seniority, not “rank and yank.” Yet it is by far the most successful of all United States airlines, because it has created a vastly more efficient organization than its competitors have. At Southwest, the time it takes to get a plane that has just landed ready for takeoff—a key index of productivity—is, on average, twenty minutes, and requires a ground crew of four, and two people at the gate. (At United Airlines, by contrast, turnaround time is closer to thirty-five minutes, and requires a ground crew of twelve and three agents at the gate.) In the case of the giant retailer Wal-Mart, one of the most critical THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 13 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM A periods in its history came in 1976, when Sam Walton
  • 24. “unretired,” pushing out his handpicked successor, Ron Mayer. Mayer was just over forty. He was ambitious. He was charismatic. He was, in the words of one Walton biographer, “the boy-genius financial officer.” But Walton was convinced that Mayer was, as people at McKinsey would say, “differentiating and affirming” in the corporate suite, in defiance of Wal-Mart’s inclusive culture. Mayer left, and Wal- Mart survived. After all, Wal-Mart is an organization, not an all-star team. Walton brought in David Glass, late of the Army and Southern Missouri State University, as C.E.O.; the company is now ranked No. 1 on the Fortune 500 list. Procter & Gamble doesn’t have a star system, either. How could it? Would the top M.B.A. graduates of Harvard and Stanford move to Cincinnati to work on detergent when they could make three times as much reinventing the world in Houston? Procter & Gamble isn’t glamorous. Its C.E.O. is a lifer—a former Navy officer who began his corporate career as an assistant brand manager for Joy dishwashing liquid—and, if Procter & Gamble’s best played Enron’s best at Trivial Pursuit, no doubt the team from Houston would win handily. But Procter & Gamble has dominated the consumer-
  • 25. products field for close to a century, because it has a carefully conceived managerial system, and a rigorous marketing methodology that has allowed it to win battles for brands like Crest and Tide decade after decade. In Procter & Gamble’s Navy, Admiral Stark would have stayed. But a cross-divisional management committee would have set the Tenth Fleet in place before the war ever started. mong the most damning facts about Enron, in the end, was something its managers were proudest of. They had what, in McKinsey terminology, is called an “open market” for hiring. In the open-market system—McKinsey’s assault on the very idea of a fixed organization—anyone could apply for any job that he or she wanted, THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 14 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM and no manager was allowed to hold anyone back. Poaching was encouraged. When an Enron executive named Kevin Hannon started the company’s global broadband unit, he launched what he called Project Quick Hire. A hundred top performers from around the company were invited to the Houston Hyatt to hear Hannon give
  • 26. his pitch. Recruiting booths were set up outside the meeting room. “Hannon had his fifty top performers for the broadband unit by the end of the week,” Michaels, Handfield-Jones, and Axelrod write, “and his peers had fifty holes to fill.” Nobody, not even the consultants who were paid to think about the Enron culture, seemed worried that those fifty holes might disrupt the functioning of the affected departments, that stability in a firm’s existing businesses might be a good thing, that the self-fulfillment of Enron’s star employees might possibly be in conflict with the best interests of the firm as a whole. These are the sort of concerns that management consultants ought to raise. But Enron’s management consultant was McKinsey, and McKinsey was as much a prisoner of the talent myth as its clients were. In 1998, Enron hired ten Wharton M.B.A.s; that same year, McKinsey hired forty. In 1999, Enron hired twelve from Wharton; McKinsey hired sixty-one. The consultants at McKinsey were preaching at Enron what they believed about themselves. “When we would hire them, it wouldn’t just be for a week,” one former Enron manager recalls, of the brilliant young men and women from McKinsey who wandered the hallways at the company’s headquarters. “It would be for two to four months. They were always
  • 27. around.” They were there looking for people who had the talent to think outside the box. It never occurred to them that, if everyone had to think outside the box, maybe it was the box that needed fixing. ♦ Malcolm Gladwell has been a staff writer for the The New Yorker since 1996. THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 15 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM MORE: ENRON CORPORATION AXELROD THE TALENT MYTH - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2002/07/22/the-talent- myth 16 of 16 7/29/16, 12:00 AM OUTLIERS
  • 28. 68 CHAPTER THREE The Trouble with Geniuses, Part 1 "KNOWLEDGE OF A BOY'S IQ IS OF LITTLE HELP IF yc)u ARE FACED WITH A FORMFUL OF CLEVER BOYS." 1. In the fifth episode of the 2008 season, the American tele- vision quiz show I vs. loo had as its special guest a man named Christopher Langan. The television show l vs. loo is one of many that sprang up in the wake of the phenomenal success of Who Wants to Be a Millionaire. It features a permanent gallery of one hundred ordinary people who serve as what is called the "mob." Each week they match wits with a special invited guest. At stake is a million dollars. The guest has to be smart enough to answer more questions correctly than his or her one hundred adversaries-and by that standard, few have ever seemed as superbly qualified as Christopher Langan. "Tonight the mob takes on their fiercest competition yet," the voice-over began. "Meet Chris Langan, who many
  • 29. OUTLIERS call the smartest man in America." The camera did a slow pan of a stocky, muscular man in his fifties. "The aver- age person has an IQ of one hundred," the voice-over continued. "Einstein one fifty. Chris has an IQ of one ninety-five. He's currently wrapping his big brain around a theory of the universe. But will his king-size cranium be enough to take down the mob for one million dollars? Find out right now on One versus One Hundred." Out strode Langan onto the stage amid wild applause. "You don't think you need to have a high intellect to do well on One versus One Hundred, do you?" the show's host, Bob Sager, asked him. Sager looked at Langan oddly, as if he were some kind of laboratory specimen. "Actually, I think it could be a hindrance," Langan replied. He had a deep, certain voice. "To have a high IQ, you tend to specialize, think deep thoughts. You avoid trivia. But now that I see these people" -he glanced at the mob, the amusement in his eyes betraying just how ridiculous he found the proceedings- "I think I'll do okay." Over the past decade, Chris Langan has achieved a strange kind of fame. He has become the public face of genius in American life, a celebrity outlier. He gets invited on news shows and profiled in magazines, and he has been the subject of a documentary by the filmmaker Errol Mor- ris, all because of a brain that appears to defy description. The television news show 20/20 once hired a neuro-
  • 30. psychologist to give Langan an IQ test, and Langan's score was literally off the charts-too high to be accurately measured. Another time, Langan took an IQ test specially designed for people too smart for ordinary IQ tests. He THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART I got all the questions right except one.'' He was speaking at six months of age. When he was three, he would listen to the radio on Sundays as the announcer read the comics aloud, and he would follow along on his own until he had taught himself to read. At five, he began questioning his grandfather about the existence of God-and remembers being disappointed in the answers he got. In school, Langan could walk into a test in a foreign- language class, not having studied at all,. and if there were two or three minutes before the instructor arrived, he could skim through the textbook and ace the test. In . his early teenage years, while working as a farmhand, he started to read widely in the area of theoretical physics. At sixteen, he made his way through Bertrand Russell and Alfred North Whitehead's famously abstruse masterpiece Principia Mathematica. He got a perfect score ·on his SAT, even though he fell asleep at one point during the test. "He did math for an hour," his brother Mark says of Langan's summer routine in high school. "Then he did French for an hour. Then he studied Russian. Then he would read philosophy. He did that religiously, every day." Another of his brothers, Jeff, says, "You know, when Christopher was fourteen or fifteen, he would draw things just as a joke, and it would be like a photograph. When he was fifteen, he could match Jimi Hendrix lick for lick on a guitar. Boom. Boom. Boom. Half the time, Christopher
  • 31. didn't attend school at all. He would just show up for tests :.<The super IQ test was created by Ronald K. Hoeflin, who is himself someone with an unusually high IQ. Here's a sample question, from the verbal analogies section. "Teeth is to Hen as Nest is to ?" If you want to know the answer, I'm afraid I have no idea. 71 OUTLIERS and there was nothing they could do about it. To us, it was hilarious. He could brief a semester's worth of textbooks in two days, and take care of whatever he had to take care of, and then get back to whatever he was doing in the first place."'' · On the set of 1 vs. 100, Langan was poised and confi- dent. His voice was deep. His eyes were small and fiercely bright. He did not circle about topics, searching for the right phrase, or double back to restate a previous sentence. * To get a sense of what Chris Langan must hive been like growing up, consider the following description of a child named "L," who had an IQ in the same 200 range as Langan's. It's from a study by Leta Stetter Hollingworth, who was one of the first psychologists to study exceptionally gifted children. As the description makes obvious,
  • 32. an IQ of 200 is really, really high: "Young L's erudition was astonishing. His passion for scholarly accuracy and thoroughness set a high stand- ard for accomplishment. He was relatively large, robust and impres- sive, and was fondly dubbed 'Professor.' His attitudes and abilities were appreciated by both pupils and teachers. He was often allowed to lecture (for as long as an hour) on some special topic, such as the history of timepieces, ancient theories of engine construction, math- ematics, and history. He constructed out of odds and ends (typewriter ribbon spools, for example) a homemade clock of the pendular type to illustrate some of the principles of chronometry, and this clock was set up before the class during the enrichment unit on 'Time and Time Keeping' to demonstrate some of the principles of chronometry. His notebooks were marvels of scholarly exposition. "Being discontented with what he considered the inadequate treatment of land travel in a class unit on 'Transportation,' he agreed that time was too limited to do justice to everything. But he insisted that 'at least they should have covered ancient theory.' As an extra and voluntary project, 'he brought in elaborate drawings and accounts
  • 33. of the ancient theories of engines, locomotives etc.' ... He was at that time ro years of age." 72 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART I For that matter, he did not say um, or ah, or use any form of conversational mitigation: his sentences came marching out, one after another, polished and crisp, like soldiers on a parade ground. Every question Saget threw at him, he tossed aside, as if it were a triviality. When his winnings reached $150,000, he appeared to make a mental calcula- tion that the risks of losing everything were at that point greater than the potential benefits of staying in. Abruptly, he stopped. "I'll take the cash," he said. He shook Saget's hand firmly and was finished- exiting on top as, we like to think, geniuses invariably do. 73 OUTLIERS
  • 34. CHAPTER FOUR The Trouble with Geniuses, Part 2 ''AFTER PROTRACTED NEGOTIATIONS, IT WAS AGREED THAT ROBERT WOULD BE PUT ON PROBATION." 1. Chris Langan's mother was from San Francisco and was estranged from her family. She had four sons, each with a different father. Chris was the eldest. His father disap- peared b_efore Chris was born; he was said to have died in Mexico. His mother's second husband was murdered. Her third committed suicide. Her fourth was a failed journal- ist named Jack Langan. "To this day I haven't met anybody who was as poor when they were kids as our family was," Chris Langan says. "We didn't have a pair of matched socks. Our shoes had holes in them. Our pants had holes in them. We only had one set of clothes. I remember my brothers and I going into the bathroom and using the bathtub to wash our only set of clothes and we were bare-assed naked when we were doing that because we didn't have anything to wear.'' Jack Langan would go on drinking sprees and disappear.
  • 35. 9 I OUTLIERS He would lock the kitchen cabinets so the boys couldn't get to the food. He used a bullwhip to keep the boys in line. He would get jobs and then lose them, moving the family on to the next town. One summer the family lived on an Indian reservation in a teepee, subsisting on government- surplus peanut butter and cornmeal. For a time, they lived in Virginia City, Nevada. "There was only one law offi- cer in town, and when the Hell's Angels came to town, he would crouch down in the back of his office," Mark Langan remembers. "There was a bar there, I'll always remember. It was called the Bucket of Blood Saloon." When the boys were in grade school, the family moved to Bozeman, Montana. One of Chris's brothers spent time in a foster home. Another was sent to reform school. "I don't think the school ever understood just how gifted Christopher was," his brother Jeff says. "He sure as hell didn't play it up. This was Bozeman. It wasn't like it is today. It was a small hick town when we were growing up. We weren't treated well there. They'd just decided that my family was a bunch of deadbeats." To stick up for himself and his brothers, Chris started to lift weights. One.day, when Chris was fourteen, Jack Langan got rough with the boys, as he sometimes did, and Chris knocked him out cold. Jack left, never to return. Upon graduation from high school, Chris was offered two full scholarships, one to Reed College in Oregon and the other to the Univer- sity of Chicago. He chose Reed.
  • 36. "It was a huge mistake," Chris recalls. "I had a real case of culture shock. I was a crew-cut kid who had been working as a ranch hand in the summers in Montana, and there I was, with a whole bunch of long-haired city 92 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 kids, most of them from New York. And these kids had a whole different style than I was used to. I couldn't get a word in edgewise at class. They were very inquisitive. Asking questions all the time. I was crammed into a dorm room. There were four of us, and the other three guys had a whole different other lifestyle. They were smoking pot. They would bring their girlfriends into the room. I had never smoked pot before. So basically I took to hiding in the library." He continued: "Then I lost that scholarship .... My mother was supposed to fill out a parents' financial state- ment for the renewal of that scholarship. She neglected to do so. She was confused by the requirements or whatever. At some point, it came to my attention that my scholar- ship had not been renewed. So I went to the office to ask why, and they told me, Well, no one sent us the financial statement, and we allocated all the scholarship money and it's all gone, so I'm afraid that you don't have a scholar- ship here ,anymore. That was the style of the place. They simply didn't care. They didn't give a shit about their stu- dents. There was no counseling, no mentoring, nothing." Chris left Reed before the final set of exams, leaving him with a row of Fs on his transcript. In the first semes- ter, he had earned As. He went back to Bozeman and worked in construction and as a forest services firefighter
  • 37. for a year and a half. Then he enrolled at Montana State University. "I was taking math and philosophy classes," he recalled. "And then in the winter quarter, I was living thirteen miles out of town, out on Beach Hill Road, and the transmis- sion fell out of my car. My brothers had used it when I was 93 OUTLIERS gone that summer. They were working for the railroad and had driven it on the railroad tracks. I didn't have the money to repair it. So I went to my adviser and the dean in sequence and said, I have a problem. The transmission fell out of my car, and you have me in a seven-thirty a.m. and eight-thirty a.m. class. If you could please just transfer me to the afternoon sections of these classes, I would appreci- ate it because of this car problem. There was a neighbor who was a rancher who was going to take me in at eleven o'clock. My adviser was this cowboy-looking guy with a handlebar mustache, dressed in a tweed jacket. He said, 'Well, son, after looking at your transcript at Reed Col- lege, I see that you have yet to learn that everyone has to make sacrifices to get an education. Request denied.' So then I went to the dean. Same treatment.'' His voice grew tight. He was describing things that had happened more than thirty years ago, but the mem- ory still made him angry. "At that point I realized, here I was, knocking myself out to make the money to make my way back to school, and it's the middle of the Montana winter. I am willing to hitchhike into town every day, do
  • 38. whatever I had to do, just to get into school and back, and they are unwilling to do anything for me. So bananas. And that was the point I decided I could do without the higher-education system. Even if I couldn't do without it, it was sufficiently repugnant to me that I wouldn't do it anymore. So I dropped out of college, simple as that." Chris Langan's experiences at Reed and Montana State represented a turning point in his life. As a child, he had dreamt of becoming an academic. He should have gotten a PhD; universities are institutions structured, in large part, 94 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 for people with his kind of deep intellectual interests and curiosity. "Once he got into the university environment, I thought he would prosper, I really did," his brother Mark says. "I thought he would somehow find a niche. It made absolutely no sense to me when he left that." Without a degree, Langan floundered. He worked in construction. One frigid winter he worked on a clam boat on Long Island. He took factory jobs and minor civil ser- vice positions and eventually became a bouncer in a bar on Long Island, which was his principal occupation for much of his adult years. Through it all, he continued to read deeply in philosophy, mathematics, and physics as he worked on a sprawling treatise he calls the "CTMU" -the "Cognitive Theoretic Model of the Universe.'' But without academic credentials, he despairs of ever getting published in a scholarly journal. "I am a guy who has a year and a half of college," he
  • 39. says, with a shrug. "And at some point this will come to the attention of the editor, as he is going to take the paper and send ,it off to the referees, and these referees are going to try and look me up, and they are not going to find me. And they are going to say, This guy has a year and a half of college. How can he know what he's talking about?" I tis a heartbreaking story. At one point I asked Langan- hypothetically-whether he would take a job at Harvard University were it offered to him. "Well, that's a difficult question," he replied. "Obviously, as a full professor at Harvard I would count. My ideas would have weight and I could use my position, my affiliation at Harvard, to pro- mote my ideas. An institution like that is a great source of intellectual energy, and if I were at a place like that, I could 9 5 OUTLIERS absorb the vibration in the air." It was suddenly clear how lonely his life has been. Here he was, a man with an insa- tiable appetite for learning, forced for most of his adult life to live in intellectual isolation. "I even noticed that kind of intellectual energy in the year and a half I was in college," he said, almost wistfully. "Ideas are in the air constantly. It's such a stimulating place to be. "On the other hand," he went on, "Harvard is basically a glorified corporation, operating with a profit incentive. That's what makes it tick. It has an endowment in the bil- lions of dollars. The people running it are not necessarily searching for truth and knowledge. They want to be big shots, and when you accept a paycheck from these people,
  • 40. it is going to come down to what you want to do and what you feel is right versus what the man says you can do to receive another paycheck. When you're there, they got a thumb right on you. They are out to make sure you don't step out of line." 2. What does the story of Chris Langan tell us? His explana- tions, as heartbreaking as they are, are also a little strange. His mother forgets to sign his financial aid form and-just like that-no scholarship. He tries to move from a morn- ing to an afternoon class, something students do every day, and gets stopped cold. And why were Langan's teach- ers at Reed and Montana State so indifferent to his plight? Teachers typically delight in minds as brilliant as his. Langan talks about dealing with Reed and Montana State as if they were some kind of vast and unyielding govern- THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 ment bureaucracy. But colleges, particularly small liberal arts colleges like Reed, tend not to be rigid bureaucracies. Making allowances in the name of helping someone stay in school is what professors do all the time. Even in his discussion of Harvard, it's as if Langan has no conception of the culture and particulars of the institu- tion he's talking about. When you accept a paycheck from these people, it is going to come down to what you want to do and what you feel is right versus what .the man says you can do to receive another paycheck. What? One of the main reasons college professors accept a lower paycheck than they could get in private industry is that university life gives them the freedom to do what they want to do and what they feel is right. Langan has Harvard backwards.
  • 41. When Langan told me his life story, I couldn't help thin_king of the life of Robert Oppenheimer, the physicist who famously headed the American effort to develop the nuclear bomb during World War II. Oppenheimer, by all accounts, was a child with a mind very much like Chris Langan''s. His parents considered him a genius. One of his teachers recalled that "he received every new idea as perfectly beautiful." He was doing lab experiments by the third grade and studying physics and chemistry by the fifth grade. When he was nine, he once told one of his cousins, "Ask me a question in Latin and I will answer you in Greek." Oppenheimer went to Harvard and then on to Cam- bridge University to pursue a doctorate in physics. There, Oppenheimer, who struggled with depression his entire life, grew despondent. His gift was for theoretical physics, and his tutor, a man named Patrick Blackett (who would 97 OUTLIERS win a Nobel Prize in 1948), was forcing him to attend to the minutiae of experimental physics, which he hated. He grew more and more emotionally unstable, and then, in an act so strange that to this day no one has properly made sense of it, Oppenheimer took some chemicals from the laboratory and tried to poison his tutor. Blackett, luckily, found out that something was amiss. The university was informed. Oppenheimer was called on
  • 42. the carpet. And what happened next is every bit as unbe- lievable as the crime itself. Here is how the incident is described in American Prometheus, Kai Bird and Martin Sherwin's biography of Oppenheimer: "After protracted negotiations, it was agreed that Robert would be put on pro- bation and have regular sessions with a prominent Harley Street psychiatrist in London." On probation? Here we have two very brilliant young students, each of whom runs into a problem that imperils his college career. Langan's mother has missed a deadline for his financial aid. Oppenheimer has tried to poison his tutor. To continue on, they are required to plead their cases to authority. And what happens? Langan gets his scholarship taken away, and Oppenheimer gets sent to a psychiatrist. Oppenheimer and Langan might both be geniuses, but in other ways, they could not be more different. The story of Oppenheimer's appointment to be scien- tific director of the Manhattan Project twenty years later is perhaps an even better example of this difference. The gen- eral in charge of the Manhattan Project was Leslie Groves, and he scoured the country, trying to find the right person to lead the atomic-bomb effort. Oppenheimer, by rights, THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 was a long shot. He was just thirty-eight, and junior to many of the people whom he would have to manage. He was a theorist, and this was a job that called for experi- menters and engineers. His political affiliations were dodgy: he had all kinds of friends who were Communists. Perhaps more striking, he had never had any administrative experi- ence. "He was a very impractical fellow," one of Oppen-
  • 43. heimer's friends later said. "He walked about with scuffed shoes and a funny hat, and, more important, he didn't know anything about equipment." As one Berkeley s.cien- tist put it, more succinctly: "He couldn't run a hamburger stand." Oh, and by the way, in graduate school he tried to kill his tutor. This was the resume of the man who was trying out for what might be said to be-without exaggeration- one.of the most important jobs of the twentieth century. And what happened? The same thing that happened twenty years earlier at Cambridge: he got the rest of the world to see things his way. He~e are Bird and Sherwin again: "Oppenheimer under- stood that Groves guarded the entrance to the Manhattan Project, and he therefore turned on all his charm and bril- liance. It was an irresistible pedormance." Groves was smit- ten. "'He's a genius,' Groves later told a reporter. 'A real genius.'" Groves was an engineer by training with a gradu- ate degree from MIT, and Oppenheimer's great insight was to appeal to that side of Groves. Bird and Sherwin go on: "Oppenheimer was the first scientist Groves had met on his tour [of potential candidates] who grasped that build- ing an atomic bomb required finding practical solutions to a variety of cross-disciplinary problems .... [Groves] 99 OUTLIERS found himself nodding in agreement when Oppenheimer pitched the notion of a central laboratory devoted to this purpose, where, as he later testified, 'we could begin to come
  • 44. to grips with chemical, metallurgical, engineering and ord- nance problems that had so far received no consideration.'" Would Oppenheimer have lost his scholarship at Reed? Would he have been unable to convince his professors to move his classes to the afternoon? Of course not. And that's not because he was smarter than Chris Langan. It's because he possessed the kind of savvy that allowed him to get what he wanted from the world. "They required that everyone take introductory cal- culus," Langan said of his brief stay at Montana State. "And I happened to get a guy who taught it in a very dry, very trivial way. I didn't understand why he was teach- ing it this way. So I asked him questions. I actually had to chase him down to his office. I asked him, 'Why are you teaching this way? Why do you consider this practice to be relevant to calculus?' And this guy, this tall, lanky guy, always had sweat stains under his arms, he turned and looked at me and said, 'You know, there is something you should probably get straight. Some people just don't have the intellectual firepower to be mathematicians.'" There they are, the professor and the prodigy, and what the prodigy clearly wants is to be engaged, at long last, with a mind that loves mathematics as much as he does. But he fails. In fact-and this is the most heartbreaking part of all-he manages to have an entire conversation with his calculus professor without ever communicating the one fact most likely to appeal to a calculus professor. IOO THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 The professor never realizes that Chris Langan is good at
  • 45. calculus. 3. The particular skill that allows you to talk your way out of a murder rap, or convince your professor to move you from the morning to the afternoon section, is what the psy- chologist Robert Sternberg calls "practical intelligence.'' To Sternberg, practical intelligence includes things like "knowing what to say to whom, knowing when to say it, and knowing how to say it for maximum effect." It is pro- cedural: it is about knowing how to do something with- out necessarily knowing why you know it or being able to explain it. It's practical in nature: that is, it's not knowledge for its own sake. It's knowledge that helps you read situa- tions correctly and get what you want. And, critically, it is a kind of intelligence separate from the sort of analytical ability measured by IQ. To use the technical term, general intelligence and practical intelligence are "orthogonal": the presence of one doesn't imply the presence of the other. You can have lots of analytical intelligence and very little practical intelligence, or lots of practical intelligence and not much analytical intelligence, or-as in the lucky case of someone like Robert Oppenheimer-you can have lots of both. So where does something like practical intelligence come from? We know where analytical intelligence comes from. It's something, at least in part, that's in your genes. Chris Langan started talking at six months. He taught IOI
  • 46. OUTLIERS himself to read at three years of age. He was born smart. IQ is a measure, to some degree, of innate ability.''· But social savvy is knowledge. It's a set of skills that have to be learned. It has to come from somewhere, and the place where we seem to get these kinds of attitudes and skills is from our families. Perhaps the best explanation we have of this process comes from the sociologist Annette Lareau, who a few years ago conducted a fascinating study of a group of third grad- ers. She picked both blacks and whites and children from both wealthy homes and poor homes, zeroing in, ultimately, on twelve families. Lareau and her team visited each family at least twenty times, for hours at a stretch. She and her assis- tants told their subjects to treat them like "the family dog," and they followed them to church and to soccer games and to doctor's appointments, with a tape recorder in one hand and a notebook in the other. You might expect that if you spent such an extended period in twelve different households, what you would gather is twelve different ideas about how to raise children: there would be the strict parents and the lax parents and the hyperinvolved parents and the mellow parents and on and on. What Lareau found, however, is something much different. There were only two parenting "philosophies," and they divided almost perfectly along class lines. The wealthier parents raised their kids one way, and the poorer parents raised their kids another way. The wealthier parents were heavily involved in their children's free time, shuttling them from one activity to
  • 47. ''Most estimates put the heritability of IQ at roughly 50 percent. 102 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 the next, quizzing them about their teachers and coaches and teammates. One of the well-off children Lareau fol- lowed played on a baseball team, two soccer teams, a swim team, and a basketball team in the summer, as well as play- ing in an orchestra and taking piano lessons. That kind of intensive scheduling was almost entirely absent from the lives of the poor children. Play for them wasn't soccer practice twice a week. It was making up games outside with their siblings and other kids in the neighbor- hood. What a child did was considered by his or her par- ents as something separate from the adult world and not particularly consequential. One girl from a working-class family-Katie Brindle-sang in a choir after school. But she signed up for it herself and walked to choir practice on her own. Lareau writes: What Mrs. Brindle doesn't do that is routine for middle- class mothers is view her daughter's interest in singing as a signal to look for other ways to help her develop that interest into a formal talent. Similarly Mrs. Brindle does not discuss Katie's interest in drama or express regret that she cannot afford to cultivate her daughter's talent. Instead she frames Katie's skills and interests as character traits-singing and acting are part of what makes Katie "Katie." She sees the shows her daughter puts on as "cute" and as a way for Katie to "get attention." The middle-class parents talked things through with their children, reasoning with them. They didn't just issue
  • 48. commands. They expected their children to talk back to them, to negotiate, to question adults in positions of authority. If their children were doing poorly at school, the IO) OUTLIERS wealthier parents challenged their teachers. They inter- vened on behalf of their kids. One child Lareau follows just misses qualifying for a gifted program. Her mother arranges for her to be retested privately, petitions the school, and gets her daughter admitted. The poor parents, by contrast, are intimidated by authority. They react pas- sively and stay in the background. Lareau writes of one low-income parent: At a parent-teacher conference, for example, Ms. McAl- lister (who is a high school graduate) seems subdued. The gregarious and outgoing nature she displays at home is hidden in this setting. She sits hunched over in the chair and she keeps her jacket zipped up. She is very quiet. When the teacher reports that Harold has not been turn- ing in his homework, Ms. McAllister clearly is flabber- gasted, but all she says is, "He did it at home." She does not follow up with the teacher or attempt to intervene on Harold's behalf. In her view, it is up to the teachers to manage her son's education. That is their job, not hers. Lareau calls the middle-class parenting style "concerted cultivation." It's an attempt to actively "foster and assess a child's talents, opinions and skills." Poor parents tend to fol- low, by contrast, a strategy of "accomplishment of natural growth." They see as their responsibility to care for their
  • 49. children but to let them grow and develop on their own. Lareau stresses that one style isn't morally better than the other. The poorer children were, to her mind, often better behaved, less whiny, more creative in making use of their own time, and had a well-developed sense of inde- pendence. But in practical terms, concerted cultivation 104 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 has enormous advantages. The heavily scheduled middle- class child is exposed to a constantly shifting set of expe- riences. She learns teamwork and how to cope in highly structured settings. She is taught how to interact comfort- ably with adults, and to speak up when she needs to. In Lareau's words, the middle-class children learn a sense of ''entitlement." That word, of course, has negative connotations these days. But Lareau means it in the best sense of the term: "They acted as though they had a right to pursue their own individual preferences and to actively manage interactions in institutional settings. They appeared comfortable in those settings; they were open to sharing information and asking for attention .... It was common practice among middle-class children to shift interactions to suit their prefere]lces." They knew the rules. "Even in fourth grade, middle-class children appeared to be acting on their own behalf to gain advantages. They made special requests of teachers apd doctors to adjust procedures to accommo- date their desires." By contrast, the working-class and poor children were characterized by "an emerging sense of distance, dis-
  • 50. trust, and constraint." They didn't know how to get their way, or how to "customize" -using Lareau's wonderful term -whatever environment they were in, for their best purposes. In one telling scene, Lareau describes a visit to the doc- tor by Alex Williams, a nine-year-old boy, and his mother, Christina. The Williamses are wealthy professionals. "Alex, you should be thinking of questions you might want to ask the doctor," Christina says in the car on the 105 OUTLIERS way to the doctor's office. "You can ask him anything you want. Don't be shy. You can ask anything." Alex thinks for a minute, then says, "I have some bumps under my arms from my deodorant." Christina: "Really? You mean from your new deodorant?" Alex: "Yes." Christina: "Well, you should ask the doctor." Alex's mother, Lareau writes, "is teaching that he has the right to speak up" -that even though he's going to be in a room with an older person and authority figure, it's perfectly all right for him to assert himself. They meet the doctor, a genial man in his early forties. He tells Alex that he is in the ninety-fifth percentile in height. Alex then interrupts: ALEX: I'm in the what?
  • 51. DocTOR: It means that you're taller than more than ninety-five out of a hundred young men when they're, uh, ten years old. ALEX: I'm not ten. DocTOR: Well, they graphed you at ten. You're-nine years and ten months. They-they usually take the closest year to that graph. Look at how easily Alex interrupts the doctor- "I'm not ten." That's entitlement: his mother permits that casual incivility because she wants him to learn to assert himself with people in positions of authority. THE DocToR TURNS TO ALEX: Well, now the most important question. Do you have any questions you want to ask me befor~ I do your physical? I 0 6 THE TROUBLE VITH GENIUSES, PART 2 ALEX: Um ... only one. I've been getting some bumps on my arms, right around here (indicates underarm). DOCTOR: Underneath? ALEx:Yeah. DocTOR: Okay. I'll have to take a look at those when I come in closer to do the checkup. And I'll see what they are and what I can do. Do they hurt or itch? ALEX: No, they're just there.
  • 52. DocTOR: Okay, I'll take a look at those bumps for you. This kind of interaction simply doesn't happen with lower-class children, Lareau says. They would be quiet and submissive, with eyes turned away. Alex takes charge of the moment. "In remembering to raise the question he prepared in advance, he gains the doctor's full attention and foc.µses it on an issue of his choosing," Lareau writes. In so doing, he successfully shifts the balance of power away from the adults and toward himself. The transi- tion goes smoothly. Alex is used to being treated with respect. He is seen as special and as a person worthy of adult attention and interest. These are key characteris- tics of the strategy of concerted cultivation. Alex is not showing off during his checkup. He is behaving much as he does with his parents- he reasons, negotiates, and jokes with equal ease. It is important to understand where the particular mastery of that moment comes from. It's not genetic. Alex Williams didn't inherit the skills to interact with author- ity figures from his parents and grandparents the way he inherited the color of his eyes. Nor is it racial: it's not a 107 OUTLIERS practice specific to either black or white people. As it turns out, Alex Williams is black and Katie Brindle is white. It's a cultural advantage. Alex has those skills because over the course of his young life, his mother and father-in the
  • 53. manner of educated families-have painstakingly taught them to him, nudging and prodding and encouraging and showing him the rules of the game, right down to that lit- tle rehearsal in the car on the way to the doctor's office. When we talk about the advantages of class, Lareau argues, this is in large part what we mean. Alex Williams is better off than Katie Brindle because he's wealthier and because he goes to a better school, but also because- and perhaps this is even more critical-the sense of entitle- ment that he has been taught is an attitude perfectly suited to succeeding in the modern world. 4. This is the advantage that Oppenheimer had and that Chris Langan lacked. Oppenheimer was raised in one of the wealthiest neighborhoods in Manhattan, the son of an art- ist and a successful garment manufacturer. His childhood was the embodiment of concerted cultivation. On weekends, the Oppenheimers would go driving in the countryside in a chauffeur-driven Packard. Summers he would be taken to Europe to see his grandfather. He attended the Ethical Cul- ture School on Central Park West, perhaps the most pro- gressive school in the nation, where, his biographers write, students were "infused with the notion that they were being groomed to reform the world." When his math teacher real- ized he was bored, she sent him off to do independent work. ro8 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES,. PART 2 As a child, Oppenheimer was passionate about rock collecting. At the age of twelve, he began corresponding with local geologists about rock formations he had seen in
  • 54. Central Park, and he so impressed them that they invited him to give a lecture before the New York Mineralogical Club. As Sherwin and Bird write, Oppenheimer's par- ents responded to their son's hobby in an almost textbook example of concerted cultivation: Dreading the thought of having to talk to an audience of adults, Robert begged his father to explain that they had invited a twelve-year-old. Greatly amused, Julius encouraged his son to accept this honor. On the des- ignated evening, Robert showed up at the club with his parents, who proudly introduced their son as J. Robert Oppenheimer. The startled audience of geologists and amateur rock collectors burst out laughing when he stepped up to the podium: a wooden box had to be found for him to stand on so that the audience could see more than ,the shock of his wiry black hair sticking up above the lectern. Shy and awkward, Robert nevertheless read his prepared remarks and was given a hearty round of applause. Is it any wonder Oppenheimer handled the challenges of his life so brilliantly? If you are someone whose father has made his way up in the business world, then you've seen, firsthand, what it means to negotiate your way out of a tight spot. If you're someone who was sent to the Ethical Culture School, then you aren't going to be intimidated by a row of Cambridge dons arrayed in judgment against you. If you studied physics at Harvard, then you know OUTLIERS how to talk to an army general who did engineering just down the road at MIT.
  • 55. Chris Langan, by contrast, had only the bleakness of Bozeman, and a home dominated by an angry, drunken stepfather. "[Jack] Langan did this to all of us," said Mark. "We all have a true resentment of authority." That was the lesson Langan learned from his childhood: distrust authority and be independent. He never had a parent teach him on the way to the doctor how to speak up for himself, or how to reason and negotiate with those in positions of authority. He didn't learn entitlement. He learned con- straint. It may seem like a small thing, but it was a crip- pling handicap in navigating the world beyond Bozeman. "I couldn't get any financial aid either," Mark went on. "We just had zero knowledge, less than zero knowledge, of the process. How to apply. The forms. Checkbooks. It was not our environment." "If Christopher had been born into a wealthy family, if he was the son of a doctor who was well connected in some major market, I guarantee you he would have been one of those guys you read about, knocking back PhDs at seventeen," his brother Jeff says. "It's the culture you find yourself in that determines that. The issue with Chris is that he was always too bored to actually sit there and listen to his teachers. If someone had recognized his intel- ligence and if he was from a family where there was some kind of value on education, they would have made sure he wasn't bored." !IO THE TROUBLE VITii GENIUSES, PART 2
  • 56. - I I I OUTLIERS I I 2 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 6. Today, Chris Langan lives in rural Missouri on a horse farm. He moved there a few years ago, after he got mar- ried. He is in his fifties but looks many years younger. He has the build of a linebacker, thick through the chest, with enormous biceps. His hair is combed straight back from
  • 57. his forehead. He has a neat, graying moustache and avia- tor-style glasses. If you look into his eyes, you can see the intelligence burning behind them. "A typical day is, I get up and make coffee. I go in and sit in front of the computer and begin working on what- ever I ;was working on the night before," he told me not long ago. "I found if I go to bed with a question on my mind, all I have to do is concentrate on the question before I go to sleep and I virtually always have the answer in the morning./Sometimes I realize what the answer is because I dreamt the answer and I can remember it. Other times I just feel the answer, and I start typing and the answer h " emerges onto t e page. He had just been reading the work of the linguist Noam Chomsky. There were piles of books in his study. He ordered books from the library all the time. "I always feel that the closer you get to the original sources, the bet- ter off you are," he said. Langan seemed content. He had farm animals to take care of, and books to read, and a wife he loved. It was a much better life than being a bouncer. I I 3 OUTLIERS "I don't think there is anyone smarter than me out there," he went on. "I have never met anybody like me or never seen even an indication that there is somebody who actually has better powers of comprehension.Never seen it
  • 58. and I don't think I am going to. I could-my mind is open to the possibility. If anyone should challenge me- 'Oh, I think that I am smarter than you are' - I think I could have them." What he said sounded boastful, but it wasn't really. It was the opposite-a touch defensive. He'd been working for decades now on a project of enormous sophistication-but almost none of what he had done had ever been published much less read by the physicists and philosophers and mathematicians who might be able to judge its value. Here he was, a man with a one-in-a-million mind, and he had yet to have any impact on the world. He wasn't holding forth at academic conferences. He wasn't leading a gradu- ate seminar at some prestigious university. He was living on a slightly tumbledown horse farm in northern Mis- souri, sitting on the back porch in jeans and a cutoff T- shirt. He knew how it looked: it was the great paradox of Chris Langan's genius. "I have not pursued mainstream publishers as hard as I should have," he conceded. "Going around, querying pub- lishers, trying to find an agent. I haven't done it, and I am not interested in doing it." It was an admission of defeat. Every experience he had had outside of his own mind had ended in frustration. He knew he needed to do a better job of navigating the world, but he didn't know how. He couldn't even talk to his cal- culus teacher, for goodness' sake. These were things that I I 4 THE TROUBLE WITH GENIUSES, PART 2 others, with lesser minds, could master easily. But that's
  • 59. because those others had had help along the way, and Chris Langan never had. It wasn't an excuse. It was a fact. He'd had to make his way alone, and no one-not rock stars, not professional athletes, not software billionaires, and not even geniuses- ever makes it alone. I I 5 O n the day of the bigfootball game between the University of Missouri Tigers and the Cowboys of Oklahoma State, a football scout named Dan Shonka sat in his hotel, in Columbia, Missouri, with a portable DVD player. Shonka has worked for three National Football League teams. Before that, he was a football coach, and before that he played linebacker—although, he says, “that was three knee operations and a hundred pounds ago.” Every year, he evaluates somewhere between eight hundred and twelve hundred players around the country, helping professional teams decide whom to choose in the college draft, which means that over the last thirty years he has probably seen as many football games as anyone else in America. In his DVD player was his homework for the evening’s big game—an edited video of the Tigers’ previous contest, against the University of
  • 60. Nebraska Cornhuskers. Shonka methodically made his way through the video, stopping and re-winding whenever he saw something that caught his eye. He liked ANNALS OF EDUCATION DECEMBER 15, 2008 ISSUE MOST LIKELY TO SUCCEED How do we hire when we can’t tell who’s right for the job? By Malcolm Gladwell Effective teachers have a gift for noticing—what one researcher calls “withitness.” Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 1 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM HarryJoo Sticky Note http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-succeed-malcolm-gladwell Jeremy Maclin and Chase Coffman, two of the Mizzou receivers. He loved William Moore, the team’s bruising strong safety. But, most of all, he was interested in the Tigers’ quarterback and star, a
  • 61. stocky, strong-armed senior named Chase Daniel. “I like to see that the quarterback can hit a receiver in stride, so he doesn’t have to slow for the ball,” Shonka began. He had a stack of evaluation forms next to him and, as he watched the game, he was charting and grading every throw that Daniel made. “Then judgment. Hey, if it’s not there, throw it away and play another day. Will he stand in there and take a hit, with a guy breathing down his face? Will he be able to step right in there, throw, and still take that hit? Does the guy throw better when he’s in the pocket, or does he throw equally well when he’s on the move? You want a great competitor. Durability. Can they hold up, their strength, toughness? Can they make big plays? Can they lead a team down the field and score late in the game? Can they see the field? When your team’s way ahead, that’s fine. But when you’re getting your ass kicked I want to see what you’re going to do.” He pointed to his screen. Daniel had thrown a dart, and, just as he did, a defensive player had hit him squarely. “See how he popped up?” Shonka said. “He stood right there and threw the ball in the face of that rush. This kid has got a lot of courage.” Daniel was
  • 62. six feet tall and weighed two hundred and twenty-five pounds: thick through the chest and trunk. He carried himself with a self- assurance that bordered on cockiness. He threw quickly and in rhythm. He nimbly evaded defenders. He made short throws with touch and longer throws with accuracy. By the game’s end, he had completed an astonishing seventy-eight per cent of his passes, and handed Nebraska its worst home defeat in fifty-three years. “He can zip it,” Shonka said. “He can really gun, when he has to.” Shonka had seen all the promising college quarterbacks, charted and graded their Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 2 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM throws, and to his mind Daniel was special: “He might be one of the best college quarterbacks in the country.” But then Shonka began to talk about when he was on the staff of the Philadelphia Eagles, in 1999. Five quarterbacks were taken in the first round of the college draft that year, and each looked as promising as Chase Daniel did now. But only one of them, Donovan McNabb, ended up fulfilling that promise. Of the rest, one descended into mediocrity after a decent start. Two were
  • 63. complete busts, and the last was so awful that after failing out of the N.F.L. he ended up failing out of the Canadian Football League as well. The year before, the same thing happened with Ryan Leaf, who was the Chase Daniel of 1998. The San Diego Chargers made him the second player taken over all in the draft, and gave him an eleven- million-dollar signing bonus. Leaf turned out to be terrible. In 2002, it was Joey Harrington’s turn. Harrington was a golden boy out of the University of Oregon, and the third player taken in the draft. Shonka still can’t get over what happened to him. “I tell you, I saw Joey live,” he said. “This guy threw lasers, he could throw under tight spots, he had the arm strength, he had the size, he had the intelligence.” Shonka got as misty as a two-hundred- and-eighty-pound ex-linebacker in a black tracksuit can get. “He’s a concert pianist, you know? I really—I mean, I really—liked Joey.” And yet Harrington’s career consisted of a failed stint with the Detroit Lions and a slide into obscurity. Shonka looked back at the screen, where the young man he felt might be the best quarterback in the country was marching his team up and down the field. “How will that ability translate to the National Football League?” He
  • 64. shook his head slowly. “Shoot.” Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 3 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM O This is the quarterback problem. There are certain jobs where almost nothing you can learn about candidates before they start predicts how they’ll do once they’re hired. So how do we know whom to choose in cases like that? In recent years, a number of fields have begun to wrestle with this problem, but none with such profound social consequences as the profession of teaching. ne of the most important tools in contemporary educational research is “value added” analysis. It uses standardized test scores to look at how much the academic performance of students in a given teacher’s classroom changes between the beginning and the end of the school year. Suppose that Mrs. Brown and Mr. Smith both teach a classroom of third graders who score at the fiftieth percentile on math and reading tests on the first day of school, in September. When the students are retested, in June, Mrs.
  • 65. Brown’s class scores at the seventieth percentile, while Mr. Smith’s students have fallen to the fortieth percentile. That change in the students’ rankings, value-added theory says, is a meaningful indicator of how much more effective Mrs. Brown is as a teacher than Mr. Smith. It’s only a crude measure, of course. A teacher is not solely responsible for how much is learned in a classroom, and not everything of value that a teacher imparts to his or her students can be captured on a standardized test. Nonetheless, if you follow Brown and Smith for three or four years, their effect on their students’ test scores starts to become predictable: with enough data, it is possible to identify who the very good teachers are and who the very poor teachers are. What’s more—and this is the finding that has galvanized the educational world—the difference between good teachers and poor teachers turns out to be vast. Eric Hanushek, an economist at Stanford, estimates that the students of a very bad teacher will learn, on average, half a year’s Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 4 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
  • 66. K worth of material in one school year. The students in the class of a very good teacher will learn a year and a half ’s worth of material. That difference amounts to a year’s worth of learning in a single year. Teacher effects dwarf school effects: your child is actually better off in a “bad” school with an excellent teacher than in an excellent school with a bad teacher. Teacher effects are also much stronger than class-size effects. You’d have to cut the average class almost in half to get the same boost that you’d get if you switched from an average teacher to a teacher in the eighty-fifth percentile. And remember that a good teacher costs as much as an average one, whereas halving class size would require that you build twice as many classrooms and hire twice as many teachers. Hanushek recently did a back-of-the-envelope calculation about what even a rudimentary focus on teacher quality could mean for the United States. If you rank the countries of the world in terms of the academic performance of their schoolchildren, the U.S. is just below average, half a standard deviation below a clump of relatively high-performing countries like Canada and Belgium. According to Hanushek, the U.S. could close that gap simply by replacing the
  • 67. bottom six per cent to ten per cent of public-school teachers with teachers of average quality. After years of worrying about issues like school funding levels, class size, and curriculum design, many reformers have come to the conclusion that nothing matters more than finding people with the potential to be great teachers. But there’s a hitch: no one knows what a person with the potential to be a great teacher looks like. The school system has a quarterback problem. ickoff time for Missouri’s game against Oklahoma State was seven o’clock. It was a perfect evening for football: cloudless skies and a light fall breeze. For hours, fans had been tailgating in the parking lots around the stadium. Cars lined the roads leading to the Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 5 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM university, many with fuzzy yellow-and-black Tiger tails hanging from their trunks. It was one of Mizzou’s biggest games in years. The Tigers were undefeated, and had a chance to become the No. 1 college football team in the country. Shonka made his way through
  • 68. the milling crowds and took a seat in the press box. Below him, the players on the field looked like pieces on a chessboard. The Tigers held the ball first. Chase Daniel stood a good seven yards behind his offensive line. He had five receivers, two to his left and three to his right, spaced from one side of the field to the other. His linemen were widely spaced as well. In play after play, Daniel caught the snap from his center, planted his feet, and threw the ball in quick seven- and eight-yard diagonal passes to one of his five receivers. The style of offense that the Tigers run is called the “spread,” and most of the top quarterbacks in college football—the players who will be drafted into the pros—are spread quarterbacks. By spacing out the offensive linemen and wide receivers, the system makes it easy for the quarterback to figure out the intentions of the opposing defense before the ball is snapped: he can look up and down the line, “read” the defense, and decide where to throw the ball before anyone has moved a muscle. Daniel had been playing in the spread since high school; he was its master. “Look how quickly he gets the ball out,” Shonka said. “You can hardly go a thousand and one, a
  • 69. thousand and two, and it’s out of his hand. He knows right where he’s going. When everyone is spread out like that, the defense can’t disguise its coverage. Chase knows right away what they are going to do. The system simplifies the quarterback’s decisions.” But for Shonka this didn’t help matters. It had always been hard to predict how a college quarterback would fare in the pros. The professional game was, simply, faster and more complicated. With the advent of the spread, though, the correspondence between the Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 6 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM “ You got anything that doesn’t have green tea in it?” two levels of play had broken down almost entirely. N.F.L. teams don’t run the spread. They can’t. The defenders in the pros are so much faster than their college counterparts that they would shoot through those big gaps in the offensive line and flatten the quarterback. In the N.F.L., the offensive line is bunched closely together. Daniel wouldn’t have five receivers. Most of the time,
  • 70. he’d have just three or four. He wouldn’t have the luxury of standing seven yards behind the center, planting his feet, and knowing instantly where to throw. He’d have to crouch right behind the center, take the snap directly, and run backward before planting his feet to throw. The onrushing defenders wouldn’t be seven yards away. They would be all around him, from the start. The defense would no longer have to show its hand, because the field would not be so spread out. It could now disguise its intentions. Daniel wouldn’t be able to read the defense before the snap was taken. He’d have to read it in the seconds after the play began. “In the spread, you see a lot of guys wide open,” Shonka said. “But when a guy like Chase goes to the N.F.L. he’s never going to see his receivers that open— only in some rare case, like someone slips or there’s a bust in the coverage. When that ball’s leaving your hands in the pros, if you don’t use your eyes to move the defender a little bit, they’ll break on the ball and intercept it. The athletic ability that they’re playing against in the league is unbelievable.”
  • 71. As Shonka talked, Daniel was moving his team down the field. But he was almost always throwing those quick, diagonal passes. In the N.F.L., he would have to do much more than that—he would have to throw long, vertical passes over the top of the defense. Could he make that kind of throw? Shonka didn’t know. There was also the matter of his height. Six feet was fine in a spread system, where the Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 7 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM big gaps in the offensive line gave Daniel plenty of opportunity to throw the ball and see downfield. But in the N.F.L. there wouldn’t be gaps, and the linemen rushing at him would be six-five, not six- one. “I wonder,” Shonka went on. “Can he see? Can he be productive in a new kind of offense? How will he handle that? I’d like to see him set up quickly from center. I’d like to see his ability to read coverages that are not in the spread. I’d like to see him in the pocket. I’d like to see
  • 72. him move his feet. I’d like to see him do a deep dig, or deep comeback. You know, like a throw twenty to twenty-five yards down the field.” It was clear that Shonka didn’t feel the same hesitancy in evaluating the other Mizzou stars—the safety Moore, the receivers Maclin and Coffman. The game that they would play in the pros would also be different from the game they were playing in college, but the difference was merely one of degree. They had succeeded at Missouri because they were strong and fast and skilled, and these traits translate in kind to professional football. A college quarterback joining the N.F.L., by contrast, has to learn to play an entirely new game. Shonka began to talk about Tim Couch, the quarterback taken first in that legendary draft of 1999. Couch set every record imaginable in his years at the University of Kentucky. “They used to put five garbage cans on the field,” Shonka recalled, shaking his head, “and Couch would stand there and throw and just drop the ball into every one.” But Couch was a flop in the pros. It wasn’t that professional quarterbacks didn’t need to be accurate. It was that the kind of accuracy required to do the job well could be measured only in a real N.F.L. game.
  • 73. Similarly, all quarterbacks drafted into the pros are required to take Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 8 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM an I.Q. test—the Wonderlic Personnel Test. The theory behind the test is that the pro game is so much more cognitively demanding than the college game that high intelligence should be a good predictor of success. But when the economists David Berri and Rob Simmons analyzed the scores—which are routinely leaked to the press—they found that Wonderlic scores are all but useless as predictors. Of the five quarterbacks taken in round one of the 1999 draft, Donovan McNabb, the only one of the five with a shot at the Hall of Fame, had the lowest Wonderlic score. And who else had I.Q. scores in the same range as McNabb? Dan Marino and Terry Bradshaw, two of the greatest quarterbacks ever to play the game. We’re used to dealing with prediction problems by going back and looking for better predictors. We now realize that being a good doctor requires the ability to communicate, listen, and empathize—and so there is increasing pressure on medical
  • 74. schools to pay attention to interpersonal skills as well as to test scores. We can have better physicians if we’re just smarter about how we choose medical-school students. But no one is saying that Dan Shonka is somehow missing some key ingredient in his analysis; that if he were only more perceptive he could predict Chase Daniel’s career trajectory. The problem with picking quarterbacks is that Chase Daniel’s performance can’t be predicted. The job he’s being groomed for is so particular and specialized that there is no way to know who will succeed at it and who won’t. In fact, Berri and Simmons found no connection between where a quarterback was taken in the draft—that is, how highly he was rated on the basis of his college performance—and how well he played in the pros. The entire time that Chase Daniel was on the field against Oklahoma State, his backup, Chase Patton, stood on the sidelines, watching. Patton didn’t play a single down. In his four years at Missouri, up to that point, he had thrown a total of twenty-six Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 9 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM
  • 75. P passes. And yet there were people in Shonka’s world who thought that Patton would end up as a better professional quarterback than Daniel. The week of the Oklahoma State game, the national sports magazine ESPN even put the two players on its cover, with the title “chase daniel might win the heisman”—referring to the trophy given to college football’s best player. “his backup could win the super bowl.” Why did everyone like Patton so much? It wasn’t clear. Maybe he looked good in practice. Maybe it was because this season in the N.F.L. a quarterback who had also never started in a single college game is playing superbly for the New England Patriots. It sounds absurd to put an athlete on the cover of a magazine for no particular reason. But perhaps that’s just the quarterback problem taken to an extreme. If college performance doesn’t tell us anything, why shouldn’t we value someone who hasn’t had the chance to play as highly as someone who plays as well as anyone in the land? icture a young preschool teacher, sitting on a classroom floor surrounded by seven children. She is holding an alphabet book, and working through the letters with the children, one by one: “ ‘A’ is
  • 76. for apple. . . . ‘C’ is for cow.” The session was taped, and the videotape is being watched by a group of experts, who are charting and grading each of the teacher’s moves. After thirty seconds, the leader of the group—Bob Pianta, the dean of the University of Virginia’s Curry School of Education— stops the tape. He points to two little girls on the right side of the circle. They are unusually active, leaning into the circle and reaching out to touch the book. “What I’m struck by is how lively the affect is in this room,” Pianta said. “One of the things the teacher is doing is creating a holding space for that. And what distinguishes her from other teachers is that she flexibly allows the kids to move and point to the book. She’s not Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 10 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM rigidly forcing the kids to sit back.” Pianta’s team has developed a system for evaluating various competencies relating to student-teacher interaction. Among
  • 77. them is “regard for student perspective”; that is, a teacher’s knack for allowing students some flexibility in how they become engaged in the classroom. Pianta stopped and rewound the tape twice, until what the teacher had managed to achieve became plain: the children were active, but somehow the class hadn’t become a free-for-all. “A lesser teacher would have responded to the kids’ leaning over as misbehavior,” Pianta went on. “ ‘We can’t do this right now. You need to be sitting still.’ She would have turned this off.” Bridget Hamre, one of Pianta’s colleagues, chimed in: “These are three- and four-year-olds. At this age, when kids show their engagement it’s not like the way we show our engagement, where we look alert. They’re leaning forward and wriggling. That’s their way of doing it. And a good teacher doesn’t interpret that as bad behavior. You can see how hard it is to teach new teachers this idea, because the minute you teach them to have regard for the student’s perspective, they think you have to give up control of the classroom.” The lesson continued. Pianta pointed out how the teacher managed to personalize the material. “ ‘C’ is for cow” turned into a short discussion of which of the kids had ever visited a farm. “Almost every
  • 78. time a child says something, she responds to it, which is what we describe as teacher sensitivity,” Hamre said. The teacher then asked the children if anyone’s name began with that letter. “Calvin,” a boy named Calvin says. The teacher nods, and says, “Calvin starts with ‘C.’ “ A little girl in the middle says, “Me!” The Most Likely To Succeed - The New Yorker http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/12/15/most-likely- to-suc... 11 of 20 7/28/16, 11:59 PM HarryJoo Highlight teacher turns to her. “Your name’s Venisha. Letter ‘V.’ Venisha.” It was a key moment. Of all the teacher elements analyzed by the Virginia group, feedback—a direct, personal response by a teacher to a specific statement by a student—seems to be most closely linked to academic success. Not only did the teacher catch the “Me!” amid the wiggling and tumult; she addressed it directly. “Mind you, that’s not great feedback,” Hamre said. “High-
  • 79. quality feedback is where there is a back-and-forth exchange to get a deeper understanding.” The perfect way to handle that moment would have been for the teacher to pause and pull out Venisha’s name card, point to the letter “V,” show her how different it is from “C,” and make the class sound out both letters. But the teacher didn’t do that— either because it didn’t occur to her or because she was distracted by the wiggling of the girls to her right. “On the other hand, she could have completely ignored the girl, which happens a lot,” Hamre went on. “The other thing that happens a lot is the teacher will just say, ‘You’re wrong.’ Yes- no feedback is probably the predominant kind of feedback, which provides almost no information for the kid in terms of learning.” Pianta showed another tape, of a nearly identical situation: a circle of pre-schoolers around a teacher. The lesson was about how we can tell when someone is happy or sad. The teacher began by acting out a short conversation between two hand puppets, Henrietta and Twiggle: Twiggle is sad until Henrietta shares some watermelon with him. “The idea that the teacher is trying to get across is that you can tell