SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the
Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs
Taoufik Boulhrir*
Department of English Studies, Discourse, Creativity, and Society, USMBA, Saiss-Fes, Morocco
Corresponding author: Taoufik Boulhrir, E-mail: Taoufik.boulhrir@usmba.ac.ma
ABSTRACT
Twenty-first century education has undoubtedly witnessed changes of the definition of literacy to
cope with the economic, social, and intellectual trends. Technological advances, which include
skills of communication, creativity, critical thinking, and collaboration have become key in
education, especially when dealing with literacy and reading motivation. As motivation hinges
around two major theoretical approaches, intrinsic and extrinsic, numerous studies argue for
the first to be more sustainable in enhancing reading motivation. Accordingly, many research-
based interventional programs have emerged since the late nineties with increasing popularity
to offer answers to the dwindling rates in reading among youth. This article discusses traits of
21st
century education in light of trends and challenges as it probes the effectiveness of some
interventional programs that are meant, and argued for, to enhance literacy skills and reading
motivation.
Key words: Intrinsic, Extrinsic, Reading Motivation, Literacy Skills, Interventional Programs
ARTICLE INFO
Article history
Received: June 18, 2017
Accepted: July 30, 2017
Published: July 31, 2017
Volume: 5  Issue: 3
Conflicts of interest: None
Funding: None
INTRODUCTION
Educators as well as educationists have worked on reading
motivation for over three decades to find effective ways to
motivate students to read and find academic success. Re-
searchers have come up with different definitions of what
‘reading motivation’ really is. Despite relative differences
in defining reading motivation and what it consists of, there
is a general consensus about motivation being the will, the
desire, the urge, the intention and/or the decision to engage
(or not to engage) in a certain reading activity (Cambria and
Gurthie, 2010; Schiefele, 1999). Ulrich Schiefele, for in-
stance, simply defines it as the “wish or intention to engage
in a certain activity” (p. 259). Jill Janes, in her 2008 study on
families, motivation and reading, introduced motivation as a
“concept” (p. 8). This concept is one of the main drives be-
hind decisions related to reading. Individuals read different
genres of literature and they read for different reasons. These
reasons may vary from one person to another. Research has
categorized types of motivation into two major categories:
intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation.
Despite the consensus on the definition and the dichoto-
mous nature of motivation, there is an ongoing debate on the
relationship between reading competence/comprehension
and reading motivation. This debate extends into a further
discussion on whether the relationship is one of cause, ef-
fect, or both. It also includes a consideration of the signif-
icance (and ties) of comprehension and fluency, both being
acquired and required skills for reading. Major studies have
Published by Australian International Academic Centre PTY.LTD.
Copyright (c) the author(s). This is an open access article under CC BY license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
http://dx.doi.org/10.7575/aiac.ijels.v.5n.3p.57
been conducted to find evidence of the relation comprehen-
sion has with motivation, and to find links between fluency
and comprehension in correlation with reading motivation.
Theoretically, self-efficacy, self-determination and expec-
tancy value theories agree that developing reading compe-
tence in students positively impacts their intrinsic reading
motivation. Interventional reading programs are generally
keen on working on intrinsic motivation and seek effective-
ness in doing so.
This article is set forth to present a review of literature
pertaining to 21st
century education characteristics and theo-
retical approaches to reading motivation. Instructional class-
room practices constitute the practical aspect of this article
in that it exposes the characteristics of two internationally
recognized US based reading programs against their effec-
tiveness and flaws. It is an attempt to shed light upon theo-
retical approaches of reading motivation in light of demands
of 21st
century education and the effectiveness of two prac-
tical interventional programs that enhance intrinsic reading
motivation in K-6 classrooms based on research findings.
Specifically, characteristics of the CORI and AR programs
will be described in terms of their founding and support-
ing literature, then analyzed and critiqued in light of their
flaws highlighted by skeptic researchers. To do so, academic
work and research studies that have taken place since the
late 90’s were reviewed in this article in terms of their foci
on foundations and characteristics of 21st
century education,
reading motivation, and traits and criticism of the AR and
CORI approaches in catering for the intrinsic motivation of
International Journal of Education & Literacy Studies
ISSN: 2202-9478
www.ijels.aiac.org.au
58 IJELS 5(3):57-66
21st
century readers in elementary education. The reviewed
literature includes academic works which were primarily
selected based on their specific concerns and scopes being
directly linked to the following three main categories: the-
oretical background of 21st
century instructional practices
relevant to literacy and reading motivation, founding and
supporting research of AR and CORI, and criticism directed
at the validity of the findings and claims of supporting re-
search, respectively.
TWENTY-FIRST CENTURY EDUCATION AND
READING MOTIVATION
Reading Literacy Skills in the 21st
Century Education
Reading and literacy attitudes are defined by McKenna, Kear
and Ellsworth (2009) as acquired “predispositions to respond
in a consistently favorable or unfavorable manner”(p. 934)
with respect to aspects of reading. Modern education requires
individuals to possess a set of skills that are considered essen-
tial for modern economy and knowledge acquisition. For this
purpose, international assessments like Trends in Internation-
al Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) and Progress in
International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS) are adminis-
tered across the globe in participating countries to assess the
extent to which participants are capable of economic growth.
The more students possess critical thinking and problem
solving skills, the better they (are believed to) do on such
tests. Accordingly, King (2012) concludes, “high scores indi-
cate the capability of achieving an increase in gross domestic
product (GDP)” (p. 22). High scores imply that students are
not only able to decode “text”, regardless of its genre, but
they are also able to incorporate their text comprehension to
interpreting, solving, and relating to real life implications.
Not only is education meant to prepare individuals to be
literate for real life challenges, it is also meant to engage
them in learning environments where it “is often social, in-
teractive, and ongoing, as opposed to individualistic, isolat-
ed, competitive and static” (Guinier, 1998, p. 263). In order
to understand what the learner needs to learn so as to be an
effective reader in a modern world, key elements need to be
identified and applied in a classroom environment. Some of
these elements are critical thinking and goal setting, com-
munication, decision making/choice based on interest, and
access/availability of resources, to name a few.
An engaging motivating reading environment is char-
acterized by interactivity and openness to the world. That
is, such an environment must promote highly needed skills
such as:
1.	 Communication: which occurs both in class, in person
and at home with parents or relatives;
2.	 Creativity: which enables students to create and reflect
on their own understanding of materials being read/
learned;
3.	 Critical thinking: which happens before, during, and af-
ter reading activities such as questioning and predicting;
4.	 Collaboration: which involves students reading in
teams and analyzing what they read (Monroe, 2004.
pp. 82‑84).
The above-mentioned four Cs. skills (as they are com-
monly referred to) are believed to be critical to 21st
century
education, especially when reading and literacy skills are in-
volved. The concept of ‘literacy’, apparently, involves more
than just the text-coding and writing abilities. In this respect,
Jones and Flannigan (2015) affirm, “Prior to the year 2000,
the ubiquitous term, literate, defined one’s ability to read and
write. Its meaning delineated the educated from the uned-
ucated, as being illiterate proved an unthinkable dilemma”
(p. 3). What Monroe and Jones et al. attempt to point out is
the change in understanding of literacy with consideration
to world trends in education. Monroe adds that classrooms
where internet inquiry and online discussions take place are
capable of generating students with literacy skills. Not only
that, “such a classroom becomes a laboratory for social jus-
tice” (p. 21). It is not fair, in other words, to teach students
how to inquire, to communicate, to read and love to read
the same way it was done in the 20th
century – a century
during which the ability to decode a written/printed text, and
the ability to write were sufficient to be counted as literacy,
whereas the lack of this ability indicated a status of “illitera-
cy” (Prensky, 2001).
Since the late 90s and beginning of the 21st
century, it has
been evident that technology has become one of the most
significant phenomena in daily human lives. It has had a
tremendous impact on reading motivation in the classroom.
Research suggests that the use of technology has been taken
for granted in many households, and it has become one of
the most effective teaching/learning tools in the classrooms.
Teachers often use computer-assisted instruction (CAI) to
provide students with access to reading content in a motiva-
tional context (Murray and Rabiner, 2014).
Given the fact that the world has witnessed a tremendous
shift in technology use in economics and social communica-
tions, it is relevant to take into account the use of technolog-
ical advances in education, which takes place in a “Digital
Age” as described by Jerald (2009) and King (2012), among
others. A digital age, by necessity, requires reflection upon
digital literacy and what it might refer to. According to Mon-
roe (2004), digital literacy includes: (i) basic, scientific, and
technological literacies, (ii) visual and information literacies,
and (iii) multicultural literacy and global awareness. These
elements are essential when it comes to identifying reading
skills and motivation. A good education ought to consider
developing individuals who are not only able to read (de-
code text) and write but also able to comprehend and com-
municate. All this should be done without compromising the
pivotal role of reading with the intrinsic will to reflect on
the world and its trends. That is, being digitally literate does
not mean stepping away from reading for information and
leisure purposes.
The world nowadays involves different texts and au
courant ways to communicate and convey meaning (Lamb
and Larry 2011, pp. 56 – 63). There is a legitimate reason to
consider technology when thinking, planning, and discuss-
ing reading motivation. Several studies on the relationship
between reading motivation and technology have concluded
that students in general show a positive attitude and elevated
Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the
Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 59
engagement with long lasting stamina when reading is con-
ducted using some form of technology. This is, consequently,
related to combining the use of technologies with effective
strategies of literacy instruction that can help students de-
velop the skills and confidence they need to be successful
readers (Dalton and Hannafin, 1988, pp. 27 – 23). In another
study, Lamb et al. (2011) conclude that one third of the stu-
dents who participated in a study expressed their will to read
more books for their personal enjoyment if they were able to
use an e-reader or a computer. As recent research suggests,
there is compelling evidence that teachers, parents, educa-
tion authorities start, or continue, to consider technology as
an inevitable tool to enable students to gain independence in
their learning and adopt lifelong reading practices. Reading
practices are generally believed to be driven either by intrin-
sic or extrinsic motivational factors.
Intrinsic Motivation vs. Extrinsic Motivation
Intrinsic motivation is generally defined as the self-determina-
tion to do something or be engaged in a certain activity. “To be
motivated means to be moved to do something” as Ryan et al.
(2000) put it before they elaborate, “a person who feels no im-
petus or inspiration to act is thus characterized as unmotivat-
ed, whereas someone who is energized or activated toward an
end is considered motivated” (p. 54). Hence, intrinsic reading
motivation is the self-determination (hence self-determination
theory) of an individual to be engaged in reading for person-
al reasons and self-satisfaction. According to Guthrie et al.
(1996) motivated readers are “motivated” because they want
to read; they pick up books on their own and read. They do so
for the fun, challenge, enjoyment, entertainment, or the learn-
ing entailed, rather than external prods, rewards, pressures, or
rewards. These students do not necessary need to be assigned
to read for homework to do so, since they are driven by a per-
sonal will to read. Their engagement in reading may develop
as an outcome of the intrinsic value they attribute to reading.
In their study of reading motivation in 4th
graders, Durik et al.
(2006) found that the extent to which students would continue
to read, for the sake of reading, through their adolescence/high
school has a great deal to do with the value they place on read-
ing. This intrinsic motivation is also an outcome of several
factors that attribute to its development.
One of such factors is curiosity (Wang and Guthrie,
2004), which is argued to be one of the main drives of in-
trinsic motivation, an innate competence that is specific to
human behavior. When driven by curiosity, students tend to
read more frequently, and for longer periods of time. They
read because they simply want to know something or learn
about something; they decide on their own what to read,
when to read, and for how long they want/need to read. They
read to satisfy their curiosity. Allan Wigfield and John Guth-
rie (1997) relate curiosity to interest value, another aspect of
intrinsic motivation, in that curiosity leads to reading which,
in turn, leads to placing value on such activity as a source
of satisfaction, especially when readers become completely
involved that they lose track of time (Nell, 1988).
Although interest is related to curiosity, it is identified
by Schiefele (1999) as an intrinsic motivational factor in the
sense that reading occurs intrinsically due to the existence
of interest in reading. That is, students read more efficient-
ly when they are interested in reading, especially in certain
topics that relate to their lives. In this respect, Padak and Po-
tenza-Radis (2010) argue that for reading to be effective and
done intrinsically, “what students read […] must be connect-
ed to [their] interests and lives” (p. 1). Interest includes both
personal interest, which refers to emotional attachment to a
certain topical text, and situational interest which involves a
temporary state instigated by some features in the text, but
not necessarily all of it (Schiefele, 1999). An example of per-
sonal interest is reading literature on visual arts by a reader
whose hobby is painting. An example of situational interest
is reading about a topic/event that is currently all over the
news. Again, this implies how interest is directly related to
curiosity despite its potential influence on intrinsic reading
motivation.
Other factors of intrinsic motivation are reading effica-
cy and involvement (Janes, 2008). While reading efficacy is
directly related to reading comprehension as a motivator, in-
volvement “is the enjoyment of experiencing different texts”
(p. 10). The notion of being involved in reading is a state of
being immersed in the activity of reading for enjoyment or
leisure purposes. It is what researchers like Csikszentmihalyi
and Nakamura (2002) call “the flow experience”, an experi-
ence that takes the reader to a virtual world. This experience
is also referred to by Nell (1988) as an “intense and highly
energized state of concentrated attention” (p. 263). In other
words, when readers enjoy being taken to “different worlds”
during reading activities and they lose track of time, it be-
comes a source of satisfaction and entertainment which, in
turn, becomes a strong motivating factor of their desires to
read.
It is worth being noted, however, that these are only some
factors, among others, that work hand in hand with extrinsic
motivation, which research has argued to be another kind
of motivation that drives individuals toward reading. While
the theories behind intrinsic motivation argue that a student
is more likely to engage in a reading activity because it is
inherently enjoyable or desirable, some researchers have tra-
ditionally suggested that besides intrinsic motivation factors,
there are also extrinsic reasons that motivate students to read.
Extrinsic motivation is argued to vary in terms of its auton-
omy (Ryan et al., 2000). Thus, “it can either reflect external
control or true self- regulation” (p. 54). Ryan et al.’s study of
the distinction between types of motivation, in light of clas-
sical definitions and contemporary ones, introduce extrinsic
motivation as being any expected reward or gain other than
“curiosity and interest”, for instance. For them, this type of
motivation most basically means doing something because
of a separable outcome that is expected to be gained upon
fulfillment of such activity. In other words, students may en-
gage in reading for a variety of expected tangible and intan-
gible rewards (Chen and Wu, 2010), apart from the reading
being interesting, enjoyable, and/or entertaining in itself.
Studying extrinsic motivation in terms of the relationship
between home reading and literacy skills in school, research-
ers have concluded that extrinsic motives to read may vary
60 IJELS 5(3):57-66
from one social setting to another, and yet share some com-
mon ground. Families as well as school educators may con-
tribute in one way or another to the provision of such external
motives for students to read. A set of these separable rewards
are, but not limited to, the following examples: (i) grades,
(ii) social practice, (iii) competition, and (iv) compliance and
choices (Demos and Foshay, 2010; Durik, Vida, and Eccles,
2006; Sullivan, 2004; Wang and Gurthie, 2004).
i.	 Grades are an aspect that only teachers and school pro-
grams may implement to foster reading motivation in
students. Although closely associated with recognition
(Ryan and Deci, 2000), grades are believed to be strong
in engaging young students in reading and learning to
read. “A student could be motivated to learn a new set
of [reading] skills because he or she understands their
(grades’) potential utility or value or because learning
the skills will yield a good grade and the privileges a
good grade affords” (p. 55).
ii.	 “Social Practice” is quoted to be the perspective in
which reading must be viewed (Knoester, 2009, p. 677).
Adolescents, for instance, decide to read or not to read
depending on how this act is perceived in society. If
reading regarded favorably against other forms of en-
tertainment, per se, then it is more likely that they em-
brace reading as one of their activities (Baines, 2009).
Mucherah and Yoder (2008) explain that one of the
reasons why students avoid reading is the fact that it
has become less socially acceptable. They further argue
that not only peer interaction but also peer influence/
pressure strongly affect whether students choose to en-
gage in reading. Social practice can play a negative role
in reading motivation as much as it can play a positive
one. Moje, Overby, Tysvaer, and Morris (2008) assert
that the social aspect of reading is attached to the acqui-
sition of “social capital by providing information that
allows for maintenance of social networks” (p. 131). In
other words, adolescents often tend to read about topics
they generally interact about in their social networks. A
group of boys may engage, for instance, in reading about
their favorite sports to maintain their image among their
peers as being knowledgeable about all that matters in
that specific sport. Also, a group of girls may seem to
indulge in reading “gossip” magazines because they feel
socially pressured to know, and be able to engage in so-
cial daily conversations with peers (Winfree, 2013).
iii.	 Competition is another drive towards reading by means
of “the desire to outperform others in reading” McGe-
own et al. (2013, p. 314). Provided that a competitive
setting (peers competing in a classroom, or siblings in
the home) involves reading activities, students or sib-
lings may compete via reading more to receive recog-
nition (see 1 above) or grades in case the setting is a
classroom.
iv.	 Compliance and Choice are also extrinsic motivation-
al factors that may mainly be applied in classrooms to
have students read. Compliance is usually connected to
grades (see 2 above) and reading assignments prescribed
by teachers, school programs, school districts/ministries
or other education authorities. Where compliance is a
major requirement, choice remains one aspect of moti-
vation educators tend to mistakenly neglect. If students
are required to comply, educators need to supplement
their teaching practices with reading choices that corre-
spond to student’s needs. Research indicates that choice,
as an extrinsic motivator, has a tremendous impact on
students’ motivation to read (Reynolds and Symons,
2001). In their investigation of third graders reading
motivation in relation to the effects of choices provided
of reading materials, the researchers found, “self-deter-
mination, autonomy, and control over the learning situ-
ation are important aspects of choice” (p. 21).
Reading Competence and Reading Motivation
Behaviorists and social learning theorists stress the signif-
icance of “reinforces in shaping behavior” (Gambrell and
Codling, 1997, p. 19). Within such line of thought, Bandura
(1986), one of the most acknowledged social theorists, gave
credence to the role of competence (hence self-efficacy theo-
ry) in shaping and fostering motivation. Self-efficacy theory
can be defined as, “people’s judgment of their capabilities
to organize and execute courses of action required to attain
designated types of performances” (Bandura, 1986, p. 391).
Similarly, expectancy-value theory views motivation as a
result of one’s perception of whether the task can be done
successfully (Wigfield and Eccles, 2000). Hence, both theo-
ries stress the individuals’ perception in terms of capability
of performance which can be referred to as competence. In
support of self-efficacy theory and expectancy-value the-
ory, researchers have repeatedly admitted there is a strong
relationship between reading competence/comprehension
and reading motivation. This connection lies in the fact that
readers become more motivated when they are able to make
sense of what they read. They can related to it, question its
content, wonder about it, and interact with it and with others
about it. In this respect Gambrell and Marinak (2009) argue
that what motivates engaged readers to read is not the prac-
tice of reading or the act of reading itself, but the gain they
achieve from that interaction. It is the outcome of the expe-
rience that such readers gain during and after reading. Other
researchers agree and link this gain to interest in the sense
that it enhances comprehension, which, in turn, enhances
reading motivation. In other words, when students choose
and read topics they are interested in, their comprehension
tends to be higher and their motivation tends to increase ac-
cordingly (Guthrie et al., 2006). The relevance of a text to
students’ interests determines the degree to which they will
comprehend as well as the level of engagement they will ex-
perience. The more they practice reading of texts with con-
tent they are intrinsically motivated to read about, the more
they comprehend and connect with those texts.
Moreover, students who are more capable of reading are
reported to have more motivation to read compared to strug-
gling readers. Students who are capable of reading fluently,
and who enjoy reading, demonstrate strong comprehension
skills either regarding leisure reading or assigned reading.
On the other hand, poor readers who repeatedly experience
Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the
Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 61
failure, or struggle to understand during reading activities
seem to avoid reading and tend to engage in other activi-
ties that involve less reading (Davidson, 2008). Thanks to
their enjoyment and comprehension of text materials, the
established readers’ motivation grows, as well their reading
skills. The strugglers’ motivation, however, decreases over
time and consequently deprives them of the enjoyment of
reading, the experience of reading, and the reading skills ac-
quired in that process (Morgan and Fuchs, 2007).
Between students who are successful and motivated
readers and others who struggle and “do not like to read”,
there is a ‘grey’ area for students who are capable of reading
and yet choose not to. Tilley (2009) points out the fact that
the number of students who demonstrate an ‘aliterate’ atti-
tude does not dwindle over time; to the contrary, it increases.
Tilly further explains that these kinds of students, despite
their ability to read fluently, choose not to read, which en-
tails that reading is by no means made a lifelong habit by
these students. A habit that is believed to be a key element in
developing a large number of literacy skills such as compre-
hension, spelling, vocabulary, and grammar (Sanacore and
Palumbo, 2010). In their work on middle school students’
literacy skills across the curriculum, Sanacore and Palumbo
advocate for more opportunities for reading in an attempt
to reach out to teachers of all content areas to do their part
in engaging students in more reading practices for the sake
of comprehension development and reading motivation. Pro-
vision of more opportunities to read in the classroom and
as homework is capable of reversing ‘aliterate’ attitudes to
positive ones toward reading.
In a ten-year study on elementary and high school stu-
dents’ reading amounts and comprehension, Cunningham
and Stanovich (1997) found that there is a solid linkage be-
tween exposure to print and reading comprehension, which
is said to be developed through reading consistency. In their
longitudinal study, they concluded that reading comprehen-
sion – along with general knowledge and vocabulary – pos-
itively correlated with the amount of reading and frequency;
the more students read, and the more frequently they do so,
the better their comprehension of unfamiliar texts is. Simi-
lar works like Wang and Guthrie (2004), with focus on 4th
graders, support these results by claiming that the amount
of reading students were exposed to during the investigation
reflected correlatively on passage comprehension.
Claiming that there is a connection between reading mo-
tivation and reading comprehension is not only supported by
several research studies, but it is also backed by the Program
for International Students Assessment (PISA) which reports
that participating students, from participating countries, who
are motivated to read tend to engage in reading on a daily
basis (Risinger, 2013). Based on PISAresults of 2009 (as cit-
ed in Borgonovi, 2011), students who read for pleasure out-
perform students who were merely participating in reading
practices. Accordingly, the comprehension scores of the for-
mer were relatively higher than those of the latter. Francesca
Borgonovi goes on and accredits reading proficiency – in
addition to reading for pleasure, rather than reading practice
– to reading motivation and comprehension. She says:
Reading for enjoyment is associated with reading pro-
ficiency: PISA finds that a crucial difference between
students who perform well in the PISA reading as-
sessment and those who perform poorly lies in wheth-
er they read daily for enjoyment, rather than in how
much time they spend reading. (p. 2)
Logan and Johnson (2010) and Ülper (2011) support the
stronger role of reading for enjoyment over reading practice
and further assert that frequency and amount of reading is
not necessarily a contributing factor to improving reading
comprehension skills. Therefore, researchers advise teach-
ers not to expect significant improvement in reading liter-
acy skills and motivation just by assigning more reading
texts or provide more opportunities to read in the classroom
and/or outside. They suggest that teachers ought to focus
more on sustained reading approaches to reading, like con-
sidering students’ interests, sparking off their intrinsic moti-
vation, and scaffolding comprehension before, during, and
after reading activities.
In the process of investigating the relationship between
reading comprehension, fluency, and reading motivation,
researchers have identified major elements of reading fluen-
cy upon which reading comprehension can be examined in
light of its effect on reading motivation (Klauda and Guth-
rie, 2008). Some researchers have long considered speed and
accuracy as key elements of reading fluency (Fuchs, Fuchs,
Hosp and Jenkins, 2001). Others have added other compo-
nents such as pitch, emphasis, duration, pause and phrasing
to refer to appropriate expression as key in reading fluen-
cy. Distinctively, Wolf and Tami (2001) recognize fluency
in terms of not only its constituent parts, but also in terms
of the text units. In their study, they stated that researchers,
“attempt to define fluency in terms of either its component
parts or its various levels of reading sub skills – that is, letter,
letter pattern, word, sentence, and passage” (p. 218).
Several studies on the relationship of reading fluency and
comprehension in students in elementary to high school have
concluded that the more fluent readers are, the better their
comprehension of text is (Pinnell et al., 1995; Yovanoff et
al., 2005). Studies with a focus on the increase of fluency
in relation to comprehension have revealed a correlation in
terms of significant gains in both fluency and comprehen-
sion. This correlative association is recognized as relevant to
motivated/avid readers, new readers, struggling readers, as
well as ‘aliterate’ readers (Chard, Vaughn and Tyler, 2002;
Stahl and Kuhn, 2002). These findings, however, have been
challenged by claims that indicate a dissociation relationship
between fluency and comprehension, especially when accu-
racy and speed in reading individual words define reading
fluency skills. In a study that targeted third-graders, Wal-
czyk, Marsiglia, John, and Bryan (2004) found that reading
aloud a list of words in class revealed no correlation between
literal comprehension and accuracy in reading.
In this respect, many teachers of all content areas report
they encounter students who struggle with reading compre-
hension, which is argued to be directly related to reading
motivation and reading practice. Students also complain,
“they do not like to read or that they are not good at read-
62 IJELS 5(3):57-66
ing” (Risinger, 2013, p. 7). Students’ complaints imply that
their dislike of reading is a result of the lack of enjoyment
in reading or the lack of literacy skills required for engaged
reading. This also implies that these students do not often
read for leisure, or as a habit, because if they did, they could
have acquired a set of literacy skills, e.g. comprehension and
fluency that would enable them to read and enjoy reading.
Interventions for the sake of enhancing reading motivation
have always been initiated at the level of classrooms settings
two of which are discussed in this article.
THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ACCELERATED
READER AND CONCEPT ORIENTED READING
INSTRUCTION
Interventions to promote and enhance reading motivation
can be approached from different perspective including
exploiting aspects of technological advances and intrinsic
drives that are believed to be behind 21st
century readers.
Several programs have been introduced to classrooms in
order to encourage students to read and develop intrinsic
reading habits for the long run. The Accelerated Reader and
Concept Oriented Reading Instruction are examples of such
approaches which are discussed in terms of analysis of their
effectiveness claims.
Accelerated Reader (AR)
One of the most popular computerized reading programs
is Accelerated Reader, a product of Renaissance Learning
Company. It is an incentive program that claims and pro-
motes itself as “the answer” to reading motivation and
achievement problems (Davidson, 2008). By 2005, the
American born product garnered worldwide clientele with
offices in Canada, the United Kingdom, and Australia. On
their Parents’ Guide, Renaissance Learning (2015) introduc-
es the program as:
…a computer program that helps teachers and li-
brarians manage and monitor children’s independent
reading practice. Your child picks a book at his own
level and reads it at his own pace. When finished, your
child takes a short quiz on the computer. (Passing the
quiz is an indication that your child understood what
was read). AR gives children, teachers, and librarians
feedback based on the quiz results, which the teacher
then uses to help your child set goals and direct ongo-
ing reading practice. Children using AR choose their
own books to read, rather than having one assigned to
them. This makes reading a much more enjoyable ex-
perience as they can choose books that are interesting
to them. (p. 2)
It is a program that allows students to choose their own
books and finish them by taking quizzes that determine the
extent to which they comprehend the text. The company also
claims that their products develop life-long readers (Che-
noweth, 2001).
Supporters and promoters of AR highlight the fact that
the program is fitted with features that determine the read-
ing level of the child prior to starting the program. The
pre-tests (STAR) assure readers begin using the program at
their levels, which emphasizes intrinsic motivation associ-
ated with competence and self-efficacy approach to reading
motivation. When they start reading and taking tests, they
accumulate points to be redeemed later into rewards, which
supports extrinsic motivation in students. Each reading level
(associated with length too) has a certain number of points
assigned to it. As soon as they complete the quiz, readers
receive feedback in a form of a printout from the AR’s TOPS
(Three Opportunities to Praise Students), which works as an
extrinsic incentive to complete each book they start. Here
comes the implementation of extrinsic motivation to lure
students in reading to gain points. The program is also fitted
with an alert system where struggling students, identified by
the quizzes, are reported to their teachers for proper inter-
vention (Renaissance Learning, 2015, p. 8).
While they can be useful tools to motivate students to
read as well as to ameliorate their reading and comprehen-
sion skills, computer assisted programs, such as Accelerated
Reader, they have many disadvantages (Topping et al., 1999).
In their analytical study of the reading program, Topping and
colleagues (2003) underscore advantages and disadvantages
of the program. Some of the key advantages of the program
highlight the power of the program in developing indepen-
dent learning and assessment, empowering teachers’ prompt
and effective intervention, promoting literal comprehension,
and increasing motivation to read and achievement, which
is measured by offering of certification upon completion of
implementation standards. On the other hand, the program
falls short, according to the same authors, in catering for stu-
dents with special education needs and gifted/avid readers
who remain dependent throughout the process of reading
and unchallenged, respectively. The focus on literal compre-
hension (instead of a combination of literal and open-ended
questions) was listed among the disadvantages despite the
fact that it was previously described as an advantage. In ad-
dition to technical challenges and the requirement of cost-
ly training of teachers for effective implementation of the
program, the study underscores the fact that, in some cases,
competition to gain AR points becomes so intense that some
students read many easy books to outscore their classmates
(Topping et al., 2003).
Concept Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI)
Based on studies done on teaching strategies and reading
motivation, Concept Oriented Reading Instruction comes
as a curriculum based approach that amalgamates reading
instruction in the classroom and conceptual scientific knowl-
edge with support of student motivation to read, not only
fiction books but also information based literature. Accord-
ing to Guthrie (2015), it is based on the belief that reading
skills can be developed through a variety of strategies and
concepts taught together. More specifically, it is a teaching
method that defines reading engagement as, “the interplay
of motivation, conceptual knowledge, strategies, and social
interaction during literacy activities. […] engagement in
reading is crucial for the development of life-long literacy
learners”. It is also described by its founders as a program
Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the
Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 63
that “is designed to foster reading engagement and compre-
hension through the teaching of reading strategies, teaching
of scientific concepts and inquiry skills, and its explicit sup-
port of the development of student intrinsic motivation to
read”. This definition places comprehension and engagement
as essential properties for fostering reading as a lifelong skill
and practice. These longitudinal skills can be fostered by
implementing effective strategies in the classroom (National
Reading Panel, 2000).
It is worth to note that CORI adopts the recommenda-
tions of the American National Reading Panel Report (AN-
RPR) that came out in 2000 to affirm that there are seven
instructional strategies that “appear to have a firm scientific
basis for concluding that they improve comprehension in
normal readers” (pp. 4-42). The report listed the so-conclud-
ed-as effective strategies in an alphabetical order, rather than
significance or degree of effectiveness. The strategies are:
(i) comprehension student self-monitoring, (ii) cooperative
learning, (iii) graphic organizers, (iv) answering and gen-
erating questions, (v) summarizing, and (vi) using a com-
bination of strategies to achieve effective comprehension
and longstanding motivation, and is referred to as “multiple
strategy” method. (National Reading Panel, 2000, pp. 4-44).
In Addition to the above listed strategies, the CORI pro-
gram teachers, along with their students, base their work
on four phases: observe and personalize to create interest,
search and retrieve to learn about the topic, comprehend and
integrate learning autonomously and/or strategically, and
communicate to others which is basically sharing what one
has learned from the experience. The use of such phases in-
dicate that the program is founded on a competence theory
basis in that it fosters the ability to set goals, create interest,
search, comprehend, and communicate autonomously. Stu-
dents in such a program entertain some control over their
learning as their self-efficacy and self-determination helps
them engage and eventually be motivated to continue learn-
ing through reading.
Several research studies done on the effectiveness of the
CORI program suggest that it is an effective instructional
method that instills intrinsic reading motivation in students
in grades 3-9 (Guthrie and Davis, 2003). They, among oth-
ers, confirm the positive impact on students’ motivation to
read and learn. That said, like any other instructional pro-
gram, this have been subject to criticism.
PROBING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF
ACCELERATED READER AND CONCEPT
ORIENTED READING INSTRUCTIONS
Probing Topping’s description of Accelerated Reader’s pros
and cons points out features of the program that are some-
how contradictory or rather vague. For instance, if students
become motivated to read, why is it important to validate
reading through quizzes and certificates? To add, literal
comprehension is highlighted as an advantage, but also as
a disadvantage because it does not develop readers’ critical
thinking via open ended questions, which is a key 21st
cen-
tury skill. Completions to accumulate points seems to de-
feat the purpose of reading and motivation. Here, reading
becomes a way/means to gain rewards or recognition, not to
attain its enjoyment as its founders claim. That is, enjoyment
here is related to the gains rather than to reading itself.
Reviewing many research studies that speak in favor of
the effectiveness of the program are, strangely, either au-
thored by or co-authored by Keith Topping who is a member
of the Review Board. (Topping and Paul, 1999; Topping,
2006; Topping and Sanders, 2000; Topping, Samuel, and
Paul, 2007). Findings of these studies, according to WWC
Intervention Report (2016), were deemed as biased and “in-
eligible for review because [they do] not use an eligible de-
sign” (p. 10). Biggers (2001) goes further to question the
validity of many of the supporting research findings based
on the claim that they were done by the company’s own re-
searchers. More specifically, she identifies five of the 19 re-
search studies were authored by Topping.
Research on the effectiveness of the Accelerated Reader
is still characterized by bias, ambivalence, and ineligibility
of their designs (WWC Intervention Report, 2016), either in
favor or against the program. In “The (Lack of) Experimen-
tal Evidence Supporting the Use of Accelerated Reader”,
Krashen (2003) concludes that:
The results presented here strongly suggest that of
the four aspects of AR, access to books, time devoted
to reading, tests, and rewards, only the first two are
supported by research. There is considerable evidence
that providing access to books results in more read-
ing and better reading and considerable evidence that
providing time to read results in better reading. There
is suggestive evidence that incentives do not promote
additional reading in the long term. The AR research
literature does nothing to change these conclusions.
(p. 26)
In other words, the factors behind the success of this pro-
gram are yet to be investigated in further research. Stephen
Krashen advocates for a balanced program that allows stu-
dents to choose books based on their interests rather than
some other extrinsic reward. Also, providing time and access
to a variety of reading genres and text types for students to
choose from may have a bigger impact on motivation, than
the one extrinsic incentives may have.
Moving on to the Concept Oriented Reading Instruction,
it is worth to remember that the CORI program promotes
itself as an instructional program that is designed to im-
prove and to instill long-lasting intrinsic motivation to read
in young readers, the question to be asked is: does it work?
Despite the favorable conclusions many studies reported
about the program, What Works Clearinghouse (WWC), an
initiative of the Institute of Education Sciences (IES) at the
U.S. Department of Education, published a report in Au-
gust 2010 on the effectiveness of the CORI program. It is
worth mentioning that the WWC was established in 2002 to,
“promote informed education decision making by providing
educators, policymakers, researchers, and the public with a
central and trusted source of scientific evidence about ‘what
works’ in education” (IES, 2015). The report identified and
investigated 48 studies of the CORI program that were pub-
lished between 1989 and 2009, all of which were conclusive-
64 IJELS 5(3):57-66
ly in favor of the effectiveness of the program. The report
concludes that WWC is, “unable to draw any conclusions
based on research about the effectiveness and infectiveness”
(WWC, 2010, p. 1) of the CORI program. The reason is that
all the 48 studies cited in the report did not meet the WWC’s
“evidence standards”. For instance, some studies do not use
“a comparison group design or a single-case design” (WWC,
2010, p. 5). Moreover, a close look through the bibliography
of the report, by the researcher of this present study, reveals
that 29 of the 48 research studies were either authored or
co-authored by John Guthrie, the founder of the CORI pro-
gram. Although, it may be legitimate to claim that the found-
er of the program has simply promoted his own creation
though research he authored or co-authored; hence, further
research is still needed to investigate the effectiveness of
such approach.
CONCLUSION
In light of the demands century 21 has imposed on researchers,
educators, and policy makers to cope with the fast- changing
economics in a digital age, it has undoubtedly been necessary
to consider the implementation of teaching strategies that ef-
fectively develop and maintain literacy skills. There is a gen-
eral consensus about the necessity of redefining literacy skills
in ways that respond to the economic needs and requirements
in terms of digital literacy and implementation of the centu-
ry 21 skills in the classrooms (Jerald, 2009; McKenna et al.,
2009; King, 2012; and Jones et al., 2015).
To confront the prominent challenge of the dwindling
rates of reading which adversely affect literacy skills around
the world, many interventional reading motivation initiatives
have been advocated for by researchers claiming potential of
enhancing intrinsic reading motivation in the classroom (Re-
naissance Learning, 2015; Topping et al. 2000; and Guth-
rie 2015). Such research based programs assert promotion
of reading skills and motivation through implementation of
teaching methods and strategies that incorporate comput-
er-assisted reading programs (i.e. AR), and the use an amal-
gam of structural cognitive, social, and reading strategies
and motivation (i.e. CORI).
Although perusal of literature respective to such pro-
grams indicate prevalent supporting evidence for their effec-
tiveness, further scrutiny of study designs, research ethics,
and validity of the findings reveals significant flaws that
question the reliability of such programs in achieving in their
intended objectives. This article argues that research claims
in favor of the effectiveness of any interventional program
need to be examined against the validity, and the reliability
of their findings considering study methodologies and in-
terests founding such claims. Hence, students’ perspectives
and input need to be considered in approaching educational
problems. In addition, research based educational programs
need to be counter-examined by independent organizations
and researches to ensure independence, objectivity and sus-
tainability of interventional programs in an education pro-
vided as a service rather than a business.
For the sake of discussion, criticism can be directed
towards the validity of the claims of both programs in the
sense that both seem to heavily rely on research that is char-
acterized by potential bias with conclusions that are likely to
be tailored to promote the programs rather than serving the
interests of young readers. The case of Accelerated Reader
reveals that despite extensive published research that sup-
port well-grounded motivation theories, questions can still
be raised on the effectiveness of such program. The ‘corpo-
rate funded’ body of research backing the AR’s claims can
only serve the purpose of promoting the program for profit
rather than knowledge in the academic community. This also
applies to the CORI program in the sense that the majority
of research done on its effectiveness is done by its founders.
Numerous of such studies seem to lack many validity and
reliability characteristics which scientific research requires
for objectivity and unbiased judgments.
REFERENCES
Baines, L. (2009). Reading  Happiness. The Phi Delta
Kappan, 90(9), 686-688.
Biggers, D. (2001). The argument against Accelerated Read-
er. Journal of Adolescent  Adult 45 (1), 72-75.
Borgonovi, F. (2011, September). Do Students Today Read for
Pleasure? Retrieved May 8, 2014, from www.oecd.org.
Cambria, J.,  Guthrie, J. T. (2010). Motivating and Engag-
ing Students in Reading. The NERA Journal, Volume
46 (1).
Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S.,  Tyler, B.-J. (2002). A Synthe-
sis of Research on Effective Interventions for Building
Reading Fluency with Elementary Students with Learn-
ing Disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 35 (5),
386-406.
Chen, P.-H. a.-R. (2010). Rewards for Reading: Their Ef-
fects on Reading Motivation. Journal of Instructional
Pedagogies 3(1), 1-8.
Chenoweth, K. (2001). Keeping Score. School Library Jour-
nal 47(9), 48-51.
Csikszentmihalyi, M.,  Nakamura, J. (2002). The Concept
of Flow. Retrieved July 17, 2014, from http://eweaver.
myweb.usf.edu.
Cunningham, E. A.,  Stanovich, E. K. (1997, Novem-
ber). Early reading acquisition and its relation to read-
ing experience and ability 10 years later. Develop-
mental Psychology, 33(6), 934-945. doi:http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934
Dalton, D. W.,  Hannafin, M. J. (1988). The effects of com-
puter-assisted and traditional mastery methods on com-
putation accuracy and attitudes. Journal of Educational,
28(1), 27–33. Retrieved from www.Cited.org.
Davidson, G. A. (2008). Reading motivation and progress
through Accelerated Reader: An action research proj-
ect  (Master›s thesis). The College of St. Scholastica.
ProQuest.
Davidson, G. A. (2008). Reading Motivation and Progress
through Accelerated Reader: An Action Research Proj-
ect. The College of St. Scholastica. Ann Arbor: Pro-
Quest.
Demos, E. S.,  Foshay, J. D. (2010). Engaging the Disen-
gaged. Retrieved July 25, 2014, from www.Proquest.org.
Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the
Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 65
Durik, A. M., Mina, V.,  Eccles, J. S. (2006, May). Task
Values and Ability Beliefs as Predictors of High School
Literacy Choices: A developmental Analysis. Retrieved
June 5, 2014, from http://psycnet.apa.org/.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D.,  Hosp, M. K. (2001). Oral Reading
Fluency as an Indicator of Reading Competence: A The-
oretical, Empirical, and Historical Analysis. Scientific
Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239-56.
Gambrell, L.,  Marinak, B. (2009). Simple Practices to
Nurture the Motivation to Read. Retrieved September
22, 2015, from http://www.readingrockets.org.
Guinier, L. (1998). Lift Every Voice: Turning a Civil Rights
Setback into a New Vision of Social Justice. New York:
Simon  Schuster.
Guthrie, J. (2015). Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction.
Retrieved June 6, 2014, from http://www.cori.umd.edu/.
Guthrie, J. T.,  Davis, M. H. (2003). Motivating Struggling
Readers in Middle School through an Engagement Mod-
el of Classroom Practice. Reading  Writing Quarterly
19, 59-85.
Guthrie, J. T., Van Meter, P., McCann, A. D., Wigfield, A.,
Bennett, L., Poundstone, C. C...., Mitchell, A. M. (1996,
July). Growth of Literacy Engagement: Changes in Mo-
tivations and Strategies during Concept-Oriented Read-
ing Instruction. Retrieved June 16, 2014, from www.
Jstor.org.
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield,A., Humenick, N. M., Kathleen, P. C.,
Taboada, A.,  Barbosa, P. (2006, March). Influences of
Stimulating Tasks on Reading Motivation and Compre-
hension. Retrieved May 15, 2014, from http://www.cori.
umd.edu/.
Institute of Education Sciences. (n.d.). About the WWC. Re-
trieved July 17, 2015.
Janes, J. L. (2008). Families, Motivation, and Reading:
Pre-Adolescent Students and their Reading Motivation
and Family Reading Habits. doi:Order No. 1454596.
Jerald, C. D. (2009, July). Defining a 21st
Century Educa-
tion. Retrieved December 12, 2013, from www.center-
forpubliceducation.org.
Jones, B.,  Flannigan, S. (2008). Connecting the Digital
Dots: Literacy of the 21st
Century.  Teacher Librari-
an, 35(3), 13-16.
King, M. M. (2012, May). Twenty First Cnetury Teadhing
and Learning: Are Teachers Prepared? doi:3499020
Klauda, S. L.,  Guthrie, J. T. (2008, May). Relationships
of Three Components of Reading Fluency to Reading
Comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology,
100(2), 310-321.
Knoester, M. (2009, May). Inquiry Into Urban Adolescent
Independent Reading Habits: Can Gee’s Theory of Dis-
courses Provide Insight? Retrieved July 16, 2014, from
www.jstor.org/.
Krashen, S. (2003). The (Lack of) Experimental Evidence
Supporting the Use of Accelerated. Journal of Chil-
dren’s Literature, 29(2), 16-30.
Lamb, A.,  Larry, J. (2011, October). Nurturing a New
Breed of Reader: Five Real World Issues. Retrieved Oc-
tober 4, 2014, from www.scholarworks.iupui.edu.
Logan, S.,  Johnston, R. (2010, May). Investigating Gen-
der Differences in Reading. Educational Review, 62(2),
175-187.
March, J. (2001). How to Design Effective Blended Learn-
ing. Retrieved May 24, 2014, from www.academia.edu.
McGeown, S. P., Norgate, R.,  Warhurst, A. (2012). Ex-
ploring Intrinsic and Extrinsic Reading Motivation
among very Good and very Poor Readers. Educational
Research, 54(3), 309-322.
McKenna, M. C., Kear, D. J.,  Ellsworth, r. A. (1995, Oc-
tober). Children’s Attitudes toward Reading: A National
Survey. Retrieved October 13, 2013, from http://www.
jstor.org.
Moje, E. B., Overby, M., Tysvaer, N.,  Morris, K. (2008,
Spring). The Complex World of Adolescent Literacy:
Myths, Motivations, and Mysteries. Retrieved Agust 1,
2014, from http://www-personal.umich.edu/.
Monroe, B. J. (2004). Crossing the Digital Age: Race,
Writing, and Technology in the Classroom. New York:
Teachers College Press.
Moore, D. W., Prebble, S., Robertson, J., Waetford, R., 
Anderson, A. (2001). Self-recording With Goal Setting:
A self-management programme for the classroom. Edu-
cational Psychology, 21(3), 255-265.
Morgan, P.,  Fuchs, D. (2007). Is There a Bidirectional Re-
lationship between Children’s Reading Skills and Read-
ing Motivation?. Exceptional Children, 73(2), 165–183.
Mucherah, W.,  Yonder, A. (2008, June 5). Motivation for
Reading and Middle School Students’ Performance on
Standardized Testing in Reading. Retrieved May 14, 20,
from http://www.tandfonline.com/.
Murray, D. W.,  Rabiner, D. L. (2014, January 24). Teach-
er Use of Computer-Assisted Instruction for Young Inat-
tentive. Retrieved May 13, 2014, from http://files.eric.
ed.gov/.
Nell, V. (1988). Lost in a Book: The Psychology of Reading
for Pleasure. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Padak, N.,  Potenza-Radis, C. (2010). Motivating strug-
gling readers: Three keys to success. New England
Reading Association Journal, 45(2), 1-7.
Prensky, M. (2001, October). Digital Natives, Digital Im-
migrants. Retrieved May 13, 2014, from https://goo.gl/
ZBknrX.
Pinnell, G. S., Campbell, J., Pikulski, J., Kapinus, B., Wix-
son, K., Gough, P.,  Beatty, A. (1995). Listening to
Children Read Aloud. The National Center for Educa-
tion Statistics. Retrieved July 29, 2014, from http://nces.
ed.gov/pubs95/95728.pdf.
Renaissance Learning. (2015). A Parent’s Guide to Acceler-
ated Reader. Retrieved June 16, 2014, from http://www.
covenantknights.org/.
Reynolds, L. P.,  Symons, S. (2001, March). Motivational
Variables and Children’s Text Search. Retrieved July 21,
2014, from http://psycnet.apa.org.
Risinger, A. (2013). Teacher Reading Motivation Practices.
Retrieved May 16, 2014, from http://gradworks.umi.com/.
Ryan, R.,  Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and Extrinsic Mo-
tivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions. Re-
trieved Jaunary 24, 2014, from www.ideallibrary.com.
66 IJELS 5(3):57-66
Sanacore, J.,  Palumbo, A. (2010). Middle school students
need more opportunities to read across the curriculum. A
Journal of Educational Strategies, Issues and Ideas, 83,
180-185. doi:10.1080/00098650903583735
Schiefele, U. (1999). Interest and Learning From Text. Sci-
entific Studies of Reading, 3(3), 257-279.
Stahl, S.A., S. Kuhn, M.R. (2002). Making it sound like lan-
guage: Developing fluency. The Reading Teacher, 55,
582-584.
Sullivan, M. (2004). Why Johnny Will Not Read. School Li-
brary Journal, 50(8), 36-39.
The National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching Children to
Read: An Avidence-based Assessment. Eunice Kennedy
Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development.
Tilley, C. (2009). Reading motivation and engagement.
School Library Monthly, 26(4), 39-42.
Topping, K. J.,  Paul, T. D. (1999). Computer-Assissted
Assessment of Practice at Reading: A Large Scale Sur-
vey Using Accelerated Reader Data. Retrieved July 15,
2014, from http://ncset.uoregon.edu/.
Topping, K. J.,  Fisher, A. M. (2003). Computerised For-
mative Assessment of Reading Comprehension: field
trials in the UK. Journal of Research in Reading, 26(3),
267–279.
Topping, K. J. (2006). Accelerated Reader in specialist
schools. Scotland: Centre for Peer Learning, University
of Dundee.
Topping, K. J., Samuels, J.,  Paul, T. (2007). Computerized
assessment of independent reading: Effects of imple-
mentation quality on achievement gain. School Effec-
tiveness and School Improvement, 18(2), 191–208.
Topping, K. J.,  Sanders, W. L. (2000). Teacher effective-
ness and computer assessment of reading: Relating val-
ue added and learning information system data. School
Effectiveness and School Improvement, 11(3), 305–337.
Ülper, H. (2011). The Motivational Factors for Reading in
Terms of Students. Educational Sciences: Theory And
Practice,  11(2), 954-960. Retrieved from https://eric.
ed.gov/?id=EJ927385
Walczyk, J., Marsiglia, C., Johns, A.,  Bryan, K. (2004).
Children’s Compensations for Poorly Automated Read-
ing Skills. Discourse Processes, 37(1), 47-66. http://dx.
doi.org/DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp3701_3
Wang, J. H.-Y.,  Guthrie, J. (2004). Modeling the Effects
of Intrinsic Motivation, Amount of Reading, and Past
Reading Achievement on text Comprehension between
U.S. and Chinese Students. Retrieved July 2, 2014, from
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/.
What Works Clearinghouse. (2010). Concept-Oriented
Reading Instruction (CORI). US Department of Educa-
tion: Institute of Education Sciences.
What Works Clearinghouse. (2016). WWC Intervention Re-
port: Accelerated Reader. US Department of Education:
Institute of Education Sciences.
Wigfield, A.,  Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations Of Children’s
Motivation For Reading To The Amount And Breadth
Or Their Reading. Retrieved July 1, 2014, from http://
www.cori.umd.edu/.
Wigfield, A.,  Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy–Value Theory
of Achievement Motivation. Contemporary Education-
al Psychology, 25(1), 68-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/
ceps.1999.1015
Winfree, L. (2013, December). Reading Motivation and En-
gagement at a Rural Georgia High School. doi:Order
No. 3599489
Wolf, M.,  Tami, K.-C. (2001). Reading Fluency And
Its ­Intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading 5(3),
211–239
Yovanoff, P., Duesbery, L., Alonzo, J.,  Tindal, G. (2005,
September). Grade-Level Invariance of a Theoretical
Causal Structure Predicting Reading Comprehension
With Vocabulary and Oral Reading Fluency. Educa-
tional Measurement: Issues and Practice, 24: 4–12.
doi:10.1111/j.1745-3992.2005.00014.x

More Related Content

What's hot

Critical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching suresh canagarajah
Critical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching  suresh canagarajahCritical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching  suresh canagarajah
Critical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching suresh canagarajah
Amir Hamid Forough Ameri
 
Critical pedagogy powerpoint
Critical pedagogy powerpointCritical pedagogy powerpoint
Critical pedagogy powerpoint
LMR901
 
Literacy Challenges, Motivation and Education
Literacy Challenges, Motivation and EducationLiteracy Challenges, Motivation and Education
Literacy Challenges, Motivation and EducationAndrea Lagalisse
 
Harris, kevin smith, ann marie - identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...
Harris, kevin   smith, ann marie -  identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...Harris, kevin   smith, ann marie -  identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...
Harris, kevin smith, ann marie - identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...
William Kritsonis
 
Concept of literacy by tariq ghayyur
Concept of literacy by tariq ghayyurConcept of literacy by tariq ghayyur
Concept of literacy by tariq ghayyurTariq Ghayyur
 
Critical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedom
Critical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedomCritical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedom
Critical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedom
Alan Carbery
 
How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The Implici...
How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The  Implici...How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The  Implici...
How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The Implici...
Felix Antonio Gómez Hernández
 
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chiefwww.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
William Kritsonis
 
Edd conference proposal presentation 1
Edd conference proposal presentation 1Edd conference proposal presentation 1
Edd conference proposal presentation 1vdavis724
 
Topics In Critical Pedagogy
Topics In Critical PedagogyTopics In Critical Pedagogy
Topics In Critical Pedagogy
Glen Gatin
 
Knowless self directed 2
Knowless self directed 2Knowless self directed 2
Knowless self directed 2
syazalinah
 
Critical Pedagogy
Critical PedagogyCritical Pedagogy
Critical Pedagogy
guest490138
 
From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...
From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...
From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...
Alan Carbery
 
Inglés liberador
Inglés liberadorInglés liberador
"Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ...
"Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ..."Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ...
"Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ...
JMcGinniss
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
William Kritsonis
 
8P15 Week 2 MER
8P15 Week 2   MER8P15 Week 2   MER
8P15 Week 2 MER
KelleyMercuri1
 
Critical Pedagogy
Critical PedagogyCritical Pedagogy
Critical Pedagogy
CAITLAH
 

What's hot (20)

Critical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching suresh canagarajah
Critical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching  suresh canagarajahCritical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching  suresh canagarajah
Critical pedagogy in l2 learning and teaching suresh canagarajah
 
VayreDiss.3.28av
VayreDiss.3.28avVayreDiss.3.28av
VayreDiss.3.28av
 
Critical pedagogy powerpoint
Critical pedagogy powerpointCritical pedagogy powerpoint
Critical pedagogy powerpoint
 
Literacy Challenges, Motivation and Education
Literacy Challenges, Motivation and EducationLiteracy Challenges, Motivation and Education
Literacy Challenges, Motivation and Education
 
Harris, kevin smith, ann marie - identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...
Harris, kevin   smith, ann marie -  identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...Harris, kevin   smith, ann marie -  identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...
Harris, kevin smith, ann marie - identity and multiculturalism nfmij v14 n...
 
Teaching language in the postmodern classroom
Teaching language in the postmodern classroomTeaching language in the postmodern classroom
Teaching language in the postmodern classroom
 
Concept of literacy by tariq ghayyur
Concept of literacy by tariq ghayyurConcept of literacy by tariq ghayyur
Concept of literacy by tariq ghayyur
 
Critical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedom
Critical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedomCritical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedom
Critical pedagogy: education in the practice of freedom
 
How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The Implici...
How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The  Implici...How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The  Implici...
How Do Education Professionals Understand Creativity? A Study of The Implici...
 
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chiefwww.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
 
Edd conference proposal presentation 1
Edd conference proposal presentation 1Edd conference proposal presentation 1
Edd conference proposal presentation 1
 
Topics In Critical Pedagogy
Topics In Critical PedagogyTopics In Critical Pedagogy
Topics In Critical Pedagogy
 
Knowless self directed 2
Knowless self directed 2Knowless self directed 2
Knowless self directed 2
 
Critical Pedagogy
Critical PedagogyCritical Pedagogy
Critical Pedagogy
 
From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...
From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...
From traditional to critical: embracing critical pedagogy in instructional de...
 
Inglés liberador
Inglés liberadorInglés liberador
Inglés liberador
 
"Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ...
"Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ..."Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ...
"Everything we do is pedagogy": Critical Pedagogy, The Framework and Library ...
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
 
8P15 Week 2 MER
8P15 Week 2   MER8P15 Week 2   MER
8P15 Week 2 MER
 
Critical Pedagogy
Critical PedagogyCritical Pedagogy
Critical Pedagogy
 

Similar to Ej1153959

Learning (1)
Learning (1)Learning (1)
Learning (1)
Jonathan Dunnemann
 
National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...
National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal,  Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal,  Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...
National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...
William Kritsonis
 
Engaging Ideas for the L2 classroom
Engaging Ideas for the L2 classroomEngaging Ideas for the L2 classroom
Engaging Ideas for the L2 classroom
Brent Jones
 
Adult Learning An Overview
Adult Learning  An OverviewAdult Learning  An Overview
Adult Learning An Overview
Lindsey Sais
 
Jep 7
Jep 7Jep 7
Critical Literacy Guide
Critical Literacy GuideCritical Literacy Guide
Critical Literacy Guidemora-deyanira
 
Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...
Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...
Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...
Richter Thomas
 
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeriaCurriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeriaAlexander Decker
 
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeriaCurriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeriaAlexander Decker
 
The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...
The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...
The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...
International Journal of Science and Research (IJSR)
 
Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...
Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...
Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...
eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Global Issues in Comparative Education -Book review - by Fazal
Global Issues in Comparative Education -Book review -  by FazalGlobal Issues in Comparative Education -Book review -  by Fazal
Global Issues in Comparative Education -Book review - by Fazal
Fazal Hakeem
 
Wk7 assgngstewart-harman
Wk7 assgngstewart-harmanWk7 assgngstewart-harman
Wk7 assgngstewart-harman
Gina Stewart-Harman
 
Literate Environment Analysis Presentation
Literate Environment Analysis PresentationLiterate Environment Analysis Presentation
Literate Environment Analysis Presentation
lmckenzie37
 
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
William Kritsonis
 
69
6969
Text selection #1
Text selection #1Text selection #1
Text selection #1
Justin Cole
 
Literacy Environment ppt
Literacy Environment pptLiteracy Environment ppt
Literacy Environment pptAmparo Camacho
 

Similar to Ej1153959 (20)

Karl r.wirth learning to learn
Karl r.wirth   learning to learnKarl r.wirth   learning to learn
Karl r.wirth learning to learn
 
Learning (1)
Learning (1)Learning (1)
Learning (1)
 
National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...
National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal,  Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal,  Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...
National FORUM of Teacher Education Journal, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Ed...
 
Engaging Ideas for the L2 classroom
Engaging Ideas for the L2 classroomEngaging Ideas for the L2 classroom
Engaging Ideas for the L2 classroom
 
Adult Learning An Overview
Adult Learning  An OverviewAdult Learning  An Overview
Adult Learning An Overview
 
Jep 7
Jep 7Jep 7
Jep 7
 
Critical Literacy Guide
Critical Literacy GuideCritical Literacy Guide
Critical Literacy Guide
 
Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...
Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...
Culture, Gender and Technology Enhanced Learning (Richter & Zelenkauskaite, I...
 
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeriaCurriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
 
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeriaCurriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
Curriculum implementation in religious education in nigeria
 
The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...
The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...
The Role of Tonga Language and Culture Committee (TOLACCO), Roman Catholic an...
 
Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...
Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...
Connected libraries . Surveying the Current Landscape and Charting a Path to ...
 
Global Issues in Comparative Education -Book review - by Fazal
Global Issues in Comparative Education -Book review -  by FazalGlobal Issues in Comparative Education -Book review -  by Fazal
Global Issues in Comparative Education -Book review - by Fazal
 
Wk7 assgngstewart-harman
Wk7 assgngstewart-harmanWk7 assgngstewart-harman
Wk7 assgngstewart-harman
 
Literate Environment Analysis Presentation
Literate Environment Analysis PresentationLiterate Environment Analysis Presentation
Literate Environment Analysis Presentation
 
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
 
69
6969
69
 
The literate environment
The literate environmentThe literate environment
The literate environment
 
Text selection #1
Text selection #1Text selection #1
Text selection #1
 
Literacy Environment ppt
Literacy Environment pptLiteracy Environment ppt
Literacy Environment ppt
 

More from TinglyStig

Ej1112451
Ej1112451Ej1112451
Ej1112451
TinglyStig
 
Ej1080685
Ej1080685Ej1080685
Ej1080685
TinglyStig
 

More from TinglyStig (7)

Ej1112451
Ej1112451Ej1112451
Ej1112451
 
Ej1080685
Ej1080685Ej1080685
Ej1080685
 
05
0505
05
 
04
0404
04
 
03
0303
03
 
01
0101
01
 
02
0202
02
 

Recently uploaded

Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptxInstructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Jheel Barad
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
MIRIAMSALINAS13
 
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptxFrancesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
EduSkills OECD
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
DhatriParmar
 
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and ResearchDigital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Vikramjit Singh
 
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdfThe Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
kaushalkr1407
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
Levi Shapiro
 
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptxHonest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
timhan337
 
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdfWelcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
TechSoup
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Mohd Adib Abd Muin, Senior Lecturer at Universiti Utara Malaysia
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Atul Kumar Singh
 
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
MysoreMuleSoftMeetup
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
Celine George
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
Delapenabediema
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
Peter Windle
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Tamralipta Mahavidyalaya
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Balvir Singh
 
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideasThe geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
GeoBlogs
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Jisc
 
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptxSynthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Pavel ( NSTU)
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptxInstructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
 
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptxFrancesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
 
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and ResearchDigital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
 
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdfThe Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
 
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptxHonest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
 
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdfWelcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
 
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
 
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideasThe geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
 
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptxSynthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
 

Ej1153959

  • 1. Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs Taoufik Boulhrir* Department of English Studies, Discourse, Creativity, and Society, USMBA, Saiss-Fes, Morocco Corresponding author: Taoufik Boulhrir, E-mail: Taoufik.boulhrir@usmba.ac.ma ABSTRACT Twenty-first century education has undoubtedly witnessed changes of the definition of literacy to cope with the economic, social, and intellectual trends. Technological advances, which include skills of communication, creativity, critical thinking, and collaboration have become key in education, especially when dealing with literacy and reading motivation. As motivation hinges around two major theoretical approaches, intrinsic and extrinsic, numerous studies argue for the first to be more sustainable in enhancing reading motivation. Accordingly, many research- based interventional programs have emerged since the late nineties with increasing popularity to offer answers to the dwindling rates in reading among youth. This article discusses traits of 21st century education in light of trends and challenges as it probes the effectiveness of some interventional programs that are meant, and argued for, to enhance literacy skills and reading motivation. Key words: Intrinsic, Extrinsic, Reading Motivation, Literacy Skills, Interventional Programs ARTICLE INFO Article history Received: June 18, 2017 Accepted: July 30, 2017 Published: July 31, 2017 Volume: 5  Issue: 3 Conflicts of interest: None Funding: None INTRODUCTION Educators as well as educationists have worked on reading motivation for over three decades to find effective ways to motivate students to read and find academic success. Re- searchers have come up with different definitions of what ‘reading motivation’ really is. Despite relative differences in defining reading motivation and what it consists of, there is a general consensus about motivation being the will, the desire, the urge, the intention and/or the decision to engage (or not to engage) in a certain reading activity (Cambria and Gurthie, 2010; Schiefele, 1999). Ulrich Schiefele, for in- stance, simply defines it as the “wish or intention to engage in a certain activity” (p. 259). Jill Janes, in her 2008 study on families, motivation and reading, introduced motivation as a “concept” (p. 8). This concept is one of the main drives be- hind decisions related to reading. Individuals read different genres of literature and they read for different reasons. These reasons may vary from one person to another. Research has categorized types of motivation into two major categories: intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation. Despite the consensus on the definition and the dichoto- mous nature of motivation, there is an ongoing debate on the relationship between reading competence/comprehension and reading motivation. This debate extends into a further discussion on whether the relationship is one of cause, ef- fect, or both. It also includes a consideration of the signif- icance (and ties) of comprehension and fluency, both being acquired and required skills for reading. Major studies have Published by Australian International Academic Centre PTY.LTD. Copyright (c) the author(s). This is an open access article under CC BY license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) http://dx.doi.org/10.7575/aiac.ijels.v.5n.3p.57 been conducted to find evidence of the relation comprehen- sion has with motivation, and to find links between fluency and comprehension in correlation with reading motivation. Theoretically, self-efficacy, self-determination and expec- tancy value theories agree that developing reading compe- tence in students positively impacts their intrinsic reading motivation. Interventional reading programs are generally keen on working on intrinsic motivation and seek effective- ness in doing so. This article is set forth to present a review of literature pertaining to 21st century education characteristics and theo- retical approaches to reading motivation. Instructional class- room practices constitute the practical aspect of this article in that it exposes the characteristics of two internationally recognized US based reading programs against their effec- tiveness and flaws. It is an attempt to shed light upon theo- retical approaches of reading motivation in light of demands of 21st century education and the effectiveness of two prac- tical interventional programs that enhance intrinsic reading motivation in K-6 classrooms based on research findings. Specifically, characteristics of the CORI and AR programs will be described in terms of their founding and support- ing literature, then analyzed and critiqued in light of their flaws highlighted by skeptic researchers. To do so, academic work and research studies that have taken place since the late 90’s were reviewed in this article in terms of their foci on foundations and characteristics of 21st century education, reading motivation, and traits and criticism of the AR and CORI approaches in catering for the intrinsic motivation of International Journal of Education & Literacy Studies ISSN: 2202-9478 www.ijels.aiac.org.au
  • 2. 58 IJELS 5(3):57-66 21st century readers in elementary education. The reviewed literature includes academic works which were primarily selected based on their specific concerns and scopes being directly linked to the following three main categories: the- oretical background of 21st century instructional practices relevant to literacy and reading motivation, founding and supporting research of AR and CORI, and criticism directed at the validity of the findings and claims of supporting re- search, respectively. TWENTY-FIRST CENTURY EDUCATION AND READING MOTIVATION Reading Literacy Skills in the 21st Century Education Reading and literacy attitudes are defined by McKenna, Kear and Ellsworth (2009) as acquired “predispositions to respond in a consistently favorable or unfavorable manner”(p. 934) with respect to aspects of reading. Modern education requires individuals to possess a set of skills that are considered essen- tial for modern economy and knowledge acquisition. For this purpose, international assessments like Trends in Internation- al Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) and Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS) are adminis- tered across the globe in participating countries to assess the extent to which participants are capable of economic growth. The more students possess critical thinking and problem solving skills, the better they (are believed to) do on such tests. Accordingly, King (2012) concludes, “high scores indi- cate the capability of achieving an increase in gross domestic product (GDP)” (p. 22). High scores imply that students are not only able to decode “text”, regardless of its genre, but they are also able to incorporate their text comprehension to interpreting, solving, and relating to real life implications. Not only is education meant to prepare individuals to be literate for real life challenges, it is also meant to engage them in learning environments where it “is often social, in- teractive, and ongoing, as opposed to individualistic, isolat- ed, competitive and static” (Guinier, 1998, p. 263). In order to understand what the learner needs to learn so as to be an effective reader in a modern world, key elements need to be identified and applied in a classroom environment. Some of these elements are critical thinking and goal setting, com- munication, decision making/choice based on interest, and access/availability of resources, to name a few. An engaging motivating reading environment is char- acterized by interactivity and openness to the world. That is, such an environment must promote highly needed skills such as: 1. Communication: which occurs both in class, in person and at home with parents or relatives; 2. Creativity: which enables students to create and reflect on their own understanding of materials being read/ learned; 3. Critical thinking: which happens before, during, and af- ter reading activities such as questioning and predicting; 4. Collaboration: which involves students reading in teams and analyzing what they read (Monroe, 2004. pp. 82‑84). The above-mentioned four Cs. skills (as they are com- monly referred to) are believed to be critical to 21st century education, especially when reading and literacy skills are in- volved. The concept of ‘literacy’, apparently, involves more than just the text-coding and writing abilities. In this respect, Jones and Flannigan (2015) affirm, “Prior to the year 2000, the ubiquitous term, literate, defined one’s ability to read and write. Its meaning delineated the educated from the uned- ucated, as being illiterate proved an unthinkable dilemma” (p. 3). What Monroe and Jones et al. attempt to point out is the change in understanding of literacy with consideration to world trends in education. Monroe adds that classrooms where internet inquiry and online discussions take place are capable of generating students with literacy skills. Not only that, “such a classroom becomes a laboratory for social jus- tice” (p. 21). It is not fair, in other words, to teach students how to inquire, to communicate, to read and love to read the same way it was done in the 20th century – a century during which the ability to decode a written/printed text, and the ability to write were sufficient to be counted as literacy, whereas the lack of this ability indicated a status of “illitera- cy” (Prensky, 2001). Since the late 90s and beginning of the 21st century, it has been evident that technology has become one of the most significant phenomena in daily human lives. It has had a tremendous impact on reading motivation in the classroom. Research suggests that the use of technology has been taken for granted in many households, and it has become one of the most effective teaching/learning tools in the classrooms. Teachers often use computer-assisted instruction (CAI) to provide students with access to reading content in a motiva- tional context (Murray and Rabiner, 2014). Given the fact that the world has witnessed a tremendous shift in technology use in economics and social communica- tions, it is relevant to take into account the use of technolog- ical advances in education, which takes place in a “Digital Age” as described by Jerald (2009) and King (2012), among others. A digital age, by necessity, requires reflection upon digital literacy and what it might refer to. According to Mon- roe (2004), digital literacy includes: (i) basic, scientific, and technological literacies, (ii) visual and information literacies, and (iii) multicultural literacy and global awareness. These elements are essential when it comes to identifying reading skills and motivation. A good education ought to consider developing individuals who are not only able to read (de- code text) and write but also able to comprehend and com- municate. All this should be done without compromising the pivotal role of reading with the intrinsic will to reflect on the world and its trends. That is, being digitally literate does not mean stepping away from reading for information and leisure purposes. The world nowadays involves different texts and au courant ways to communicate and convey meaning (Lamb and Larry 2011, pp. 56 – 63). There is a legitimate reason to consider technology when thinking, planning, and discuss- ing reading motivation. Several studies on the relationship between reading motivation and technology have concluded that students in general show a positive attitude and elevated
  • 3. Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 59 engagement with long lasting stamina when reading is con- ducted using some form of technology. This is, consequently, related to combining the use of technologies with effective strategies of literacy instruction that can help students de- velop the skills and confidence they need to be successful readers (Dalton and Hannafin, 1988, pp. 27 – 23). In another study, Lamb et al. (2011) conclude that one third of the stu- dents who participated in a study expressed their will to read more books for their personal enjoyment if they were able to use an e-reader or a computer. As recent research suggests, there is compelling evidence that teachers, parents, educa- tion authorities start, or continue, to consider technology as an inevitable tool to enable students to gain independence in their learning and adopt lifelong reading practices. Reading practices are generally believed to be driven either by intrin- sic or extrinsic motivational factors. Intrinsic Motivation vs. Extrinsic Motivation Intrinsic motivation is generally defined as the self-determina- tion to do something or be engaged in a certain activity. “To be motivated means to be moved to do something” as Ryan et al. (2000) put it before they elaborate, “a person who feels no im- petus or inspiration to act is thus characterized as unmotivat- ed, whereas someone who is energized or activated toward an end is considered motivated” (p. 54). Hence, intrinsic reading motivation is the self-determination (hence self-determination theory) of an individual to be engaged in reading for person- al reasons and self-satisfaction. According to Guthrie et al. (1996) motivated readers are “motivated” because they want to read; they pick up books on their own and read. They do so for the fun, challenge, enjoyment, entertainment, or the learn- ing entailed, rather than external prods, rewards, pressures, or rewards. These students do not necessary need to be assigned to read for homework to do so, since they are driven by a per- sonal will to read. Their engagement in reading may develop as an outcome of the intrinsic value they attribute to reading. In their study of reading motivation in 4th graders, Durik et al. (2006) found that the extent to which students would continue to read, for the sake of reading, through their adolescence/high school has a great deal to do with the value they place on read- ing. This intrinsic motivation is also an outcome of several factors that attribute to its development. One of such factors is curiosity (Wang and Guthrie, 2004), which is argued to be one of the main drives of in- trinsic motivation, an innate competence that is specific to human behavior. When driven by curiosity, students tend to read more frequently, and for longer periods of time. They read because they simply want to know something or learn about something; they decide on their own what to read, when to read, and for how long they want/need to read. They read to satisfy their curiosity. Allan Wigfield and John Guth- rie (1997) relate curiosity to interest value, another aspect of intrinsic motivation, in that curiosity leads to reading which, in turn, leads to placing value on such activity as a source of satisfaction, especially when readers become completely involved that they lose track of time (Nell, 1988). Although interest is related to curiosity, it is identified by Schiefele (1999) as an intrinsic motivational factor in the sense that reading occurs intrinsically due to the existence of interest in reading. That is, students read more efficient- ly when they are interested in reading, especially in certain topics that relate to their lives. In this respect, Padak and Po- tenza-Radis (2010) argue that for reading to be effective and done intrinsically, “what students read […] must be connect- ed to [their] interests and lives” (p. 1). Interest includes both personal interest, which refers to emotional attachment to a certain topical text, and situational interest which involves a temporary state instigated by some features in the text, but not necessarily all of it (Schiefele, 1999). An example of per- sonal interest is reading literature on visual arts by a reader whose hobby is painting. An example of situational interest is reading about a topic/event that is currently all over the news. Again, this implies how interest is directly related to curiosity despite its potential influence on intrinsic reading motivation. Other factors of intrinsic motivation are reading effica- cy and involvement (Janes, 2008). While reading efficacy is directly related to reading comprehension as a motivator, in- volvement “is the enjoyment of experiencing different texts” (p. 10). The notion of being involved in reading is a state of being immersed in the activity of reading for enjoyment or leisure purposes. It is what researchers like Csikszentmihalyi and Nakamura (2002) call “the flow experience”, an experi- ence that takes the reader to a virtual world. This experience is also referred to by Nell (1988) as an “intense and highly energized state of concentrated attention” (p. 263). In other words, when readers enjoy being taken to “different worlds” during reading activities and they lose track of time, it be- comes a source of satisfaction and entertainment which, in turn, becomes a strong motivating factor of their desires to read. It is worth being noted, however, that these are only some factors, among others, that work hand in hand with extrinsic motivation, which research has argued to be another kind of motivation that drives individuals toward reading. While the theories behind intrinsic motivation argue that a student is more likely to engage in a reading activity because it is inherently enjoyable or desirable, some researchers have tra- ditionally suggested that besides intrinsic motivation factors, there are also extrinsic reasons that motivate students to read. Extrinsic motivation is argued to vary in terms of its auton- omy (Ryan et al., 2000). Thus, “it can either reflect external control or true self- regulation” (p. 54). Ryan et al.’s study of the distinction between types of motivation, in light of clas- sical definitions and contemporary ones, introduce extrinsic motivation as being any expected reward or gain other than “curiosity and interest”, for instance. For them, this type of motivation most basically means doing something because of a separable outcome that is expected to be gained upon fulfillment of such activity. In other words, students may en- gage in reading for a variety of expected tangible and intan- gible rewards (Chen and Wu, 2010), apart from the reading being interesting, enjoyable, and/or entertaining in itself. Studying extrinsic motivation in terms of the relationship between home reading and literacy skills in school, research- ers have concluded that extrinsic motives to read may vary
  • 4. 60 IJELS 5(3):57-66 from one social setting to another, and yet share some com- mon ground. Families as well as school educators may con- tribute in one way or another to the provision of such external motives for students to read. A set of these separable rewards are, but not limited to, the following examples: (i) grades, (ii) social practice, (iii) competition, and (iv) compliance and choices (Demos and Foshay, 2010; Durik, Vida, and Eccles, 2006; Sullivan, 2004; Wang and Gurthie, 2004). i. Grades are an aspect that only teachers and school pro- grams may implement to foster reading motivation in students. Although closely associated with recognition (Ryan and Deci, 2000), grades are believed to be strong in engaging young students in reading and learning to read. “A student could be motivated to learn a new set of [reading] skills because he or she understands their (grades’) potential utility or value or because learning the skills will yield a good grade and the privileges a good grade affords” (p. 55). ii. “Social Practice” is quoted to be the perspective in which reading must be viewed (Knoester, 2009, p. 677). Adolescents, for instance, decide to read or not to read depending on how this act is perceived in society. If reading regarded favorably against other forms of en- tertainment, per se, then it is more likely that they em- brace reading as one of their activities (Baines, 2009). Mucherah and Yoder (2008) explain that one of the reasons why students avoid reading is the fact that it has become less socially acceptable. They further argue that not only peer interaction but also peer influence/ pressure strongly affect whether students choose to en- gage in reading. Social practice can play a negative role in reading motivation as much as it can play a positive one. Moje, Overby, Tysvaer, and Morris (2008) assert that the social aspect of reading is attached to the acqui- sition of “social capital by providing information that allows for maintenance of social networks” (p. 131). In other words, adolescents often tend to read about topics they generally interact about in their social networks. A group of boys may engage, for instance, in reading about their favorite sports to maintain their image among their peers as being knowledgeable about all that matters in that specific sport. Also, a group of girls may seem to indulge in reading “gossip” magazines because they feel socially pressured to know, and be able to engage in so- cial daily conversations with peers (Winfree, 2013). iii. Competition is another drive towards reading by means of “the desire to outperform others in reading” McGe- own et al. (2013, p. 314). Provided that a competitive setting (peers competing in a classroom, or siblings in the home) involves reading activities, students or sib- lings may compete via reading more to receive recog- nition (see 1 above) or grades in case the setting is a classroom. iv. Compliance and Choice are also extrinsic motivation- al factors that may mainly be applied in classrooms to have students read. Compliance is usually connected to grades (see 2 above) and reading assignments prescribed by teachers, school programs, school districts/ministries or other education authorities. Where compliance is a major requirement, choice remains one aspect of moti- vation educators tend to mistakenly neglect. If students are required to comply, educators need to supplement their teaching practices with reading choices that corre- spond to student’s needs. Research indicates that choice, as an extrinsic motivator, has a tremendous impact on students’ motivation to read (Reynolds and Symons, 2001). In their investigation of third graders reading motivation in relation to the effects of choices provided of reading materials, the researchers found, “self-deter- mination, autonomy, and control over the learning situ- ation are important aspects of choice” (p. 21). Reading Competence and Reading Motivation Behaviorists and social learning theorists stress the signif- icance of “reinforces in shaping behavior” (Gambrell and Codling, 1997, p. 19). Within such line of thought, Bandura (1986), one of the most acknowledged social theorists, gave credence to the role of competence (hence self-efficacy theo- ry) in shaping and fostering motivation. Self-efficacy theory can be defined as, “people’s judgment of their capabilities to organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of performances” (Bandura, 1986, p. 391). Similarly, expectancy-value theory views motivation as a result of one’s perception of whether the task can be done successfully (Wigfield and Eccles, 2000). Hence, both theo- ries stress the individuals’ perception in terms of capability of performance which can be referred to as competence. In support of self-efficacy theory and expectancy-value the- ory, researchers have repeatedly admitted there is a strong relationship between reading competence/comprehension and reading motivation. This connection lies in the fact that readers become more motivated when they are able to make sense of what they read. They can related to it, question its content, wonder about it, and interact with it and with others about it. In this respect Gambrell and Marinak (2009) argue that what motivates engaged readers to read is not the prac- tice of reading or the act of reading itself, but the gain they achieve from that interaction. It is the outcome of the expe- rience that such readers gain during and after reading. Other researchers agree and link this gain to interest in the sense that it enhances comprehension, which, in turn, enhances reading motivation. In other words, when students choose and read topics they are interested in, their comprehension tends to be higher and their motivation tends to increase ac- cordingly (Guthrie et al., 2006). The relevance of a text to students’ interests determines the degree to which they will comprehend as well as the level of engagement they will ex- perience. The more they practice reading of texts with con- tent they are intrinsically motivated to read about, the more they comprehend and connect with those texts. Moreover, students who are more capable of reading are reported to have more motivation to read compared to strug- gling readers. Students who are capable of reading fluently, and who enjoy reading, demonstrate strong comprehension skills either regarding leisure reading or assigned reading. On the other hand, poor readers who repeatedly experience
  • 5. Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 61 failure, or struggle to understand during reading activities seem to avoid reading and tend to engage in other activi- ties that involve less reading (Davidson, 2008). Thanks to their enjoyment and comprehension of text materials, the established readers’ motivation grows, as well their reading skills. The strugglers’ motivation, however, decreases over time and consequently deprives them of the enjoyment of reading, the experience of reading, and the reading skills ac- quired in that process (Morgan and Fuchs, 2007). Between students who are successful and motivated readers and others who struggle and “do not like to read”, there is a ‘grey’ area for students who are capable of reading and yet choose not to. Tilley (2009) points out the fact that the number of students who demonstrate an ‘aliterate’ atti- tude does not dwindle over time; to the contrary, it increases. Tilly further explains that these kinds of students, despite their ability to read fluently, choose not to read, which en- tails that reading is by no means made a lifelong habit by these students. A habit that is believed to be a key element in developing a large number of literacy skills such as compre- hension, spelling, vocabulary, and grammar (Sanacore and Palumbo, 2010). In their work on middle school students’ literacy skills across the curriculum, Sanacore and Palumbo advocate for more opportunities for reading in an attempt to reach out to teachers of all content areas to do their part in engaging students in more reading practices for the sake of comprehension development and reading motivation. Pro- vision of more opportunities to read in the classroom and as homework is capable of reversing ‘aliterate’ attitudes to positive ones toward reading. In a ten-year study on elementary and high school stu- dents’ reading amounts and comprehension, Cunningham and Stanovich (1997) found that there is a solid linkage be- tween exposure to print and reading comprehension, which is said to be developed through reading consistency. In their longitudinal study, they concluded that reading comprehen- sion – along with general knowledge and vocabulary – pos- itively correlated with the amount of reading and frequency; the more students read, and the more frequently they do so, the better their comprehension of unfamiliar texts is. Simi- lar works like Wang and Guthrie (2004), with focus on 4th graders, support these results by claiming that the amount of reading students were exposed to during the investigation reflected correlatively on passage comprehension. Claiming that there is a connection between reading mo- tivation and reading comprehension is not only supported by several research studies, but it is also backed by the Program for International Students Assessment (PISA) which reports that participating students, from participating countries, who are motivated to read tend to engage in reading on a daily basis (Risinger, 2013). Based on PISAresults of 2009 (as cit- ed in Borgonovi, 2011), students who read for pleasure out- perform students who were merely participating in reading practices. Accordingly, the comprehension scores of the for- mer were relatively higher than those of the latter. Francesca Borgonovi goes on and accredits reading proficiency – in addition to reading for pleasure, rather than reading practice – to reading motivation and comprehension. She says: Reading for enjoyment is associated with reading pro- ficiency: PISA finds that a crucial difference between students who perform well in the PISA reading as- sessment and those who perform poorly lies in wheth- er they read daily for enjoyment, rather than in how much time they spend reading. (p. 2) Logan and Johnson (2010) and Ülper (2011) support the stronger role of reading for enjoyment over reading practice and further assert that frequency and amount of reading is not necessarily a contributing factor to improving reading comprehension skills. Therefore, researchers advise teach- ers not to expect significant improvement in reading liter- acy skills and motivation just by assigning more reading texts or provide more opportunities to read in the classroom and/or outside. They suggest that teachers ought to focus more on sustained reading approaches to reading, like con- sidering students’ interests, sparking off their intrinsic moti- vation, and scaffolding comprehension before, during, and after reading activities. In the process of investigating the relationship between reading comprehension, fluency, and reading motivation, researchers have identified major elements of reading fluen- cy upon which reading comprehension can be examined in light of its effect on reading motivation (Klauda and Guth- rie, 2008). Some researchers have long considered speed and accuracy as key elements of reading fluency (Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp and Jenkins, 2001). Others have added other compo- nents such as pitch, emphasis, duration, pause and phrasing to refer to appropriate expression as key in reading fluen- cy. Distinctively, Wolf and Tami (2001) recognize fluency in terms of not only its constituent parts, but also in terms of the text units. In their study, they stated that researchers, “attempt to define fluency in terms of either its component parts or its various levels of reading sub skills – that is, letter, letter pattern, word, sentence, and passage” (p. 218). Several studies on the relationship of reading fluency and comprehension in students in elementary to high school have concluded that the more fluent readers are, the better their comprehension of text is (Pinnell et al., 1995; Yovanoff et al., 2005). Studies with a focus on the increase of fluency in relation to comprehension have revealed a correlation in terms of significant gains in both fluency and comprehen- sion. This correlative association is recognized as relevant to motivated/avid readers, new readers, struggling readers, as well as ‘aliterate’ readers (Chard, Vaughn and Tyler, 2002; Stahl and Kuhn, 2002). These findings, however, have been challenged by claims that indicate a dissociation relationship between fluency and comprehension, especially when accu- racy and speed in reading individual words define reading fluency skills. In a study that targeted third-graders, Wal- czyk, Marsiglia, John, and Bryan (2004) found that reading aloud a list of words in class revealed no correlation between literal comprehension and accuracy in reading. In this respect, many teachers of all content areas report they encounter students who struggle with reading compre- hension, which is argued to be directly related to reading motivation and reading practice. Students also complain, “they do not like to read or that they are not good at read-
  • 6. 62 IJELS 5(3):57-66 ing” (Risinger, 2013, p. 7). Students’ complaints imply that their dislike of reading is a result of the lack of enjoyment in reading or the lack of literacy skills required for engaged reading. This also implies that these students do not often read for leisure, or as a habit, because if they did, they could have acquired a set of literacy skills, e.g. comprehension and fluency that would enable them to read and enjoy reading. Interventions for the sake of enhancing reading motivation have always been initiated at the level of classrooms settings two of which are discussed in this article. THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ACCELERATED READER AND CONCEPT ORIENTED READING INSTRUCTION Interventions to promote and enhance reading motivation can be approached from different perspective including exploiting aspects of technological advances and intrinsic drives that are believed to be behind 21st century readers. Several programs have been introduced to classrooms in order to encourage students to read and develop intrinsic reading habits for the long run. The Accelerated Reader and Concept Oriented Reading Instruction are examples of such approaches which are discussed in terms of analysis of their effectiveness claims. Accelerated Reader (AR) One of the most popular computerized reading programs is Accelerated Reader, a product of Renaissance Learning Company. It is an incentive program that claims and pro- motes itself as “the answer” to reading motivation and achievement problems (Davidson, 2008). By 2005, the American born product garnered worldwide clientele with offices in Canada, the United Kingdom, and Australia. On their Parents’ Guide, Renaissance Learning (2015) introduc- es the program as: …a computer program that helps teachers and li- brarians manage and monitor children’s independent reading practice. Your child picks a book at his own level and reads it at his own pace. When finished, your child takes a short quiz on the computer. (Passing the quiz is an indication that your child understood what was read). AR gives children, teachers, and librarians feedback based on the quiz results, which the teacher then uses to help your child set goals and direct ongo- ing reading practice. Children using AR choose their own books to read, rather than having one assigned to them. This makes reading a much more enjoyable ex- perience as they can choose books that are interesting to them. (p. 2) It is a program that allows students to choose their own books and finish them by taking quizzes that determine the extent to which they comprehend the text. The company also claims that their products develop life-long readers (Che- noweth, 2001). Supporters and promoters of AR highlight the fact that the program is fitted with features that determine the read- ing level of the child prior to starting the program. The pre-tests (STAR) assure readers begin using the program at their levels, which emphasizes intrinsic motivation associ- ated with competence and self-efficacy approach to reading motivation. When they start reading and taking tests, they accumulate points to be redeemed later into rewards, which supports extrinsic motivation in students. Each reading level (associated with length too) has a certain number of points assigned to it. As soon as they complete the quiz, readers receive feedback in a form of a printout from the AR’s TOPS (Three Opportunities to Praise Students), which works as an extrinsic incentive to complete each book they start. Here comes the implementation of extrinsic motivation to lure students in reading to gain points. The program is also fitted with an alert system where struggling students, identified by the quizzes, are reported to their teachers for proper inter- vention (Renaissance Learning, 2015, p. 8). While they can be useful tools to motivate students to read as well as to ameliorate their reading and comprehen- sion skills, computer assisted programs, such as Accelerated Reader, they have many disadvantages (Topping et al., 1999). In their analytical study of the reading program, Topping and colleagues (2003) underscore advantages and disadvantages of the program. Some of the key advantages of the program highlight the power of the program in developing indepen- dent learning and assessment, empowering teachers’ prompt and effective intervention, promoting literal comprehension, and increasing motivation to read and achievement, which is measured by offering of certification upon completion of implementation standards. On the other hand, the program falls short, according to the same authors, in catering for stu- dents with special education needs and gifted/avid readers who remain dependent throughout the process of reading and unchallenged, respectively. The focus on literal compre- hension (instead of a combination of literal and open-ended questions) was listed among the disadvantages despite the fact that it was previously described as an advantage. In ad- dition to technical challenges and the requirement of cost- ly training of teachers for effective implementation of the program, the study underscores the fact that, in some cases, competition to gain AR points becomes so intense that some students read many easy books to outscore their classmates (Topping et al., 2003). Concept Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI) Based on studies done on teaching strategies and reading motivation, Concept Oriented Reading Instruction comes as a curriculum based approach that amalgamates reading instruction in the classroom and conceptual scientific knowl- edge with support of student motivation to read, not only fiction books but also information based literature. Accord- ing to Guthrie (2015), it is based on the belief that reading skills can be developed through a variety of strategies and concepts taught together. More specifically, it is a teaching method that defines reading engagement as, “the interplay of motivation, conceptual knowledge, strategies, and social interaction during literacy activities. […] engagement in reading is crucial for the development of life-long literacy learners”. It is also described by its founders as a program
  • 7. Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 63 that “is designed to foster reading engagement and compre- hension through the teaching of reading strategies, teaching of scientific concepts and inquiry skills, and its explicit sup- port of the development of student intrinsic motivation to read”. This definition places comprehension and engagement as essential properties for fostering reading as a lifelong skill and practice. These longitudinal skills can be fostered by implementing effective strategies in the classroom (National Reading Panel, 2000). It is worth to note that CORI adopts the recommenda- tions of the American National Reading Panel Report (AN- RPR) that came out in 2000 to affirm that there are seven instructional strategies that “appear to have a firm scientific basis for concluding that they improve comprehension in normal readers” (pp. 4-42). The report listed the so-conclud- ed-as effective strategies in an alphabetical order, rather than significance or degree of effectiveness. The strategies are: (i) comprehension student self-monitoring, (ii) cooperative learning, (iii) graphic organizers, (iv) answering and gen- erating questions, (v) summarizing, and (vi) using a com- bination of strategies to achieve effective comprehension and longstanding motivation, and is referred to as “multiple strategy” method. (National Reading Panel, 2000, pp. 4-44). In Addition to the above listed strategies, the CORI pro- gram teachers, along with their students, base their work on four phases: observe and personalize to create interest, search and retrieve to learn about the topic, comprehend and integrate learning autonomously and/or strategically, and communicate to others which is basically sharing what one has learned from the experience. The use of such phases in- dicate that the program is founded on a competence theory basis in that it fosters the ability to set goals, create interest, search, comprehend, and communicate autonomously. Stu- dents in such a program entertain some control over their learning as their self-efficacy and self-determination helps them engage and eventually be motivated to continue learn- ing through reading. Several research studies done on the effectiveness of the CORI program suggest that it is an effective instructional method that instills intrinsic reading motivation in students in grades 3-9 (Guthrie and Davis, 2003). They, among oth- ers, confirm the positive impact on students’ motivation to read and learn. That said, like any other instructional pro- gram, this have been subject to criticism. PROBING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ACCELERATED READER AND CONCEPT ORIENTED READING INSTRUCTIONS Probing Topping’s description of Accelerated Reader’s pros and cons points out features of the program that are some- how contradictory or rather vague. For instance, if students become motivated to read, why is it important to validate reading through quizzes and certificates? To add, literal comprehension is highlighted as an advantage, but also as a disadvantage because it does not develop readers’ critical thinking via open ended questions, which is a key 21st cen- tury skill. Completions to accumulate points seems to de- feat the purpose of reading and motivation. Here, reading becomes a way/means to gain rewards or recognition, not to attain its enjoyment as its founders claim. That is, enjoyment here is related to the gains rather than to reading itself. Reviewing many research studies that speak in favor of the effectiveness of the program are, strangely, either au- thored by or co-authored by Keith Topping who is a member of the Review Board. (Topping and Paul, 1999; Topping, 2006; Topping and Sanders, 2000; Topping, Samuel, and Paul, 2007). Findings of these studies, according to WWC Intervention Report (2016), were deemed as biased and “in- eligible for review because [they do] not use an eligible de- sign” (p. 10). Biggers (2001) goes further to question the validity of many of the supporting research findings based on the claim that they were done by the company’s own re- searchers. More specifically, she identifies five of the 19 re- search studies were authored by Topping. Research on the effectiveness of the Accelerated Reader is still characterized by bias, ambivalence, and ineligibility of their designs (WWC Intervention Report, 2016), either in favor or against the program. In “The (Lack of) Experimen- tal Evidence Supporting the Use of Accelerated Reader”, Krashen (2003) concludes that: The results presented here strongly suggest that of the four aspects of AR, access to books, time devoted to reading, tests, and rewards, only the first two are supported by research. There is considerable evidence that providing access to books results in more read- ing and better reading and considerable evidence that providing time to read results in better reading. There is suggestive evidence that incentives do not promote additional reading in the long term. The AR research literature does nothing to change these conclusions. (p. 26) In other words, the factors behind the success of this pro- gram are yet to be investigated in further research. Stephen Krashen advocates for a balanced program that allows stu- dents to choose books based on their interests rather than some other extrinsic reward. Also, providing time and access to a variety of reading genres and text types for students to choose from may have a bigger impact on motivation, than the one extrinsic incentives may have. Moving on to the Concept Oriented Reading Instruction, it is worth to remember that the CORI program promotes itself as an instructional program that is designed to im- prove and to instill long-lasting intrinsic motivation to read in young readers, the question to be asked is: does it work? Despite the favorable conclusions many studies reported about the program, What Works Clearinghouse (WWC), an initiative of the Institute of Education Sciences (IES) at the U.S. Department of Education, published a report in Au- gust 2010 on the effectiveness of the CORI program. It is worth mentioning that the WWC was established in 2002 to, “promote informed education decision making by providing educators, policymakers, researchers, and the public with a central and trusted source of scientific evidence about ‘what works’ in education” (IES, 2015). The report identified and investigated 48 studies of the CORI program that were pub- lished between 1989 and 2009, all of which were conclusive-
  • 8. 64 IJELS 5(3):57-66 ly in favor of the effectiveness of the program. The report concludes that WWC is, “unable to draw any conclusions based on research about the effectiveness and infectiveness” (WWC, 2010, p. 1) of the CORI program. The reason is that all the 48 studies cited in the report did not meet the WWC’s “evidence standards”. For instance, some studies do not use “a comparison group design or a single-case design” (WWC, 2010, p. 5). Moreover, a close look through the bibliography of the report, by the researcher of this present study, reveals that 29 of the 48 research studies were either authored or co-authored by John Guthrie, the founder of the CORI pro- gram. Although, it may be legitimate to claim that the found- er of the program has simply promoted his own creation though research he authored or co-authored; hence, further research is still needed to investigate the effectiveness of such approach. CONCLUSION In light of the demands century 21 has imposed on researchers, educators, and policy makers to cope with the fast- changing economics in a digital age, it has undoubtedly been necessary to consider the implementation of teaching strategies that ef- fectively develop and maintain literacy skills. There is a gen- eral consensus about the necessity of redefining literacy skills in ways that respond to the economic needs and requirements in terms of digital literacy and implementation of the centu- ry 21 skills in the classrooms (Jerald, 2009; McKenna et al., 2009; King, 2012; and Jones et al., 2015). To confront the prominent challenge of the dwindling rates of reading which adversely affect literacy skills around the world, many interventional reading motivation initiatives have been advocated for by researchers claiming potential of enhancing intrinsic reading motivation in the classroom (Re- naissance Learning, 2015; Topping et al. 2000; and Guth- rie 2015). Such research based programs assert promotion of reading skills and motivation through implementation of teaching methods and strategies that incorporate comput- er-assisted reading programs (i.e. AR), and the use an amal- gam of structural cognitive, social, and reading strategies and motivation (i.e. CORI). Although perusal of literature respective to such pro- grams indicate prevalent supporting evidence for their effec- tiveness, further scrutiny of study designs, research ethics, and validity of the findings reveals significant flaws that question the reliability of such programs in achieving in their intended objectives. This article argues that research claims in favor of the effectiveness of any interventional program need to be examined against the validity, and the reliability of their findings considering study methodologies and in- terests founding such claims. Hence, students’ perspectives and input need to be considered in approaching educational problems. In addition, research based educational programs need to be counter-examined by independent organizations and researches to ensure independence, objectivity and sus- tainability of interventional programs in an education pro- vided as a service rather than a business. For the sake of discussion, criticism can be directed towards the validity of the claims of both programs in the sense that both seem to heavily rely on research that is char- acterized by potential bias with conclusions that are likely to be tailored to promote the programs rather than serving the interests of young readers. The case of Accelerated Reader reveals that despite extensive published research that sup- port well-grounded motivation theories, questions can still be raised on the effectiveness of such program. The ‘corpo- rate funded’ body of research backing the AR’s claims can only serve the purpose of promoting the program for profit rather than knowledge in the academic community. This also applies to the CORI program in the sense that the majority of research done on its effectiveness is done by its founders. Numerous of such studies seem to lack many validity and reliability characteristics which scientific research requires for objectivity and unbiased judgments. REFERENCES Baines, L. (2009). Reading Happiness. The Phi Delta Kappan, 90(9), 686-688. Biggers, D. (2001). The argument against Accelerated Read- er. Journal of Adolescent Adult 45 (1), 72-75. Borgonovi, F. (2011, September). Do Students Today Read for Pleasure? Retrieved May 8, 2014, from www.oecd.org. Cambria, J., Guthrie, J. T. (2010). Motivating and Engag- ing Students in Reading. The NERA Journal, Volume 46 (1). Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., Tyler, B.-J. (2002). A Synthe- sis of Research on Effective Interventions for Building Reading Fluency with Elementary Students with Learn- ing Disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 35 (5), 386-406. Chen, P.-H. a.-R. (2010). Rewards for Reading: Their Ef- fects on Reading Motivation. Journal of Instructional Pedagogies 3(1), 1-8. Chenoweth, K. (2001). Keeping Score. School Library Jour- nal 47(9), 48-51. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Nakamura, J. (2002). The Concept of Flow. Retrieved July 17, 2014, from http://eweaver. myweb.usf.edu. Cunningham, E. A., Stanovich, E. K. (1997, Novem- ber). Early reading acquisition and its relation to read- ing experience and ability 10 years later. Develop- mental Psychology, 33(6), 934-945. doi:http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934 Dalton, D. W., Hannafin, M. J. (1988). The effects of com- puter-assisted and traditional mastery methods on com- putation accuracy and attitudes. Journal of Educational, 28(1), 27–33. Retrieved from www.Cited.org. Davidson, G. A. (2008). Reading motivation and progress through Accelerated Reader: An action research proj- ect  (Master›s thesis). The College of St. Scholastica. ProQuest. Davidson, G. A. (2008). Reading Motivation and Progress through Accelerated Reader: An Action Research Proj- ect. The College of St. Scholastica. Ann Arbor: Pro- Quest. Demos, E. S., Foshay, J. D. (2010). Engaging the Disen- gaged. Retrieved July 25, 2014, from www.Proquest.org.
  • 9. Twenty-First Century Instructional Classroom Practices and Reading Motivation: Probing the Effectiveness of Interventional Reading Programs 65 Durik, A. M., Mina, V., Eccles, J. S. (2006, May). Task Values and Ability Beliefs as Predictors of High School Literacy Choices: A developmental Analysis. Retrieved June 5, 2014, from http://psycnet.apa.org/. Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K. (2001). Oral Reading Fluency as an Indicator of Reading Competence: A The- oretical, Empirical, and Historical Analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239-56. Gambrell, L., Marinak, B. (2009). Simple Practices to Nurture the Motivation to Read. Retrieved September 22, 2015, from http://www.readingrockets.org. Guinier, L. (1998). Lift Every Voice: Turning a Civil Rights Setback into a New Vision of Social Justice. New York: Simon Schuster. Guthrie, J. (2015). Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction. Retrieved June 6, 2014, from http://www.cori.umd.edu/. Guthrie, J. T., Davis, M. H. (2003). Motivating Struggling Readers in Middle School through an Engagement Mod- el of Classroom Practice. Reading Writing Quarterly 19, 59-85. Guthrie, J. T., Van Meter, P., McCann, A. D., Wigfield, A., Bennett, L., Poundstone, C. C...., Mitchell, A. M. (1996, July). Growth of Literacy Engagement: Changes in Mo- tivations and Strategies during Concept-Oriented Read- ing Instruction. Retrieved June 16, 2014, from www. Jstor.org. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield,A., Humenick, N. M., Kathleen, P. C., Taboada, A., Barbosa, P. (2006, March). Influences of Stimulating Tasks on Reading Motivation and Compre- hension. Retrieved May 15, 2014, from http://www.cori. umd.edu/. Institute of Education Sciences. (n.d.). About the WWC. Re- trieved July 17, 2015. Janes, J. L. (2008). Families, Motivation, and Reading: Pre-Adolescent Students and their Reading Motivation and Family Reading Habits. doi:Order No. 1454596. Jerald, C. D. (2009, July). Defining a 21st Century Educa- tion. Retrieved December 12, 2013, from www.center- forpubliceducation.org. Jones, B., Flannigan, S. (2008). Connecting the Digital Dots: Literacy of the 21st Century.  Teacher Librari- an, 35(3), 13-16. King, M. M. (2012, May). Twenty First Cnetury Teadhing and Learning: Are Teachers Prepared? doi:3499020 Klauda, S. L., Guthrie, J. T. (2008, May). Relationships of Three Components of Reading Fluency to Reading Comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 310-321. Knoester, M. (2009, May). Inquiry Into Urban Adolescent Independent Reading Habits: Can Gee’s Theory of Dis- courses Provide Insight? Retrieved July 16, 2014, from www.jstor.org/. Krashen, S. (2003). The (Lack of) Experimental Evidence Supporting the Use of Accelerated. Journal of Chil- dren’s Literature, 29(2), 16-30. Lamb, A., Larry, J. (2011, October). Nurturing a New Breed of Reader: Five Real World Issues. Retrieved Oc- tober 4, 2014, from www.scholarworks.iupui.edu. Logan, S., Johnston, R. (2010, May). Investigating Gen- der Differences in Reading. Educational Review, 62(2), 175-187. March, J. (2001). How to Design Effective Blended Learn- ing. Retrieved May 24, 2014, from www.academia.edu. McGeown, S. P., Norgate, R., Warhurst, A. (2012). Ex- ploring Intrinsic and Extrinsic Reading Motivation among very Good and very Poor Readers. Educational Research, 54(3), 309-322. McKenna, M. C., Kear, D. J., Ellsworth, r. A. (1995, Oc- tober). Children’s Attitudes toward Reading: A National Survey. Retrieved October 13, 2013, from http://www. jstor.org. Moje, E. B., Overby, M., Tysvaer, N., Morris, K. (2008, Spring). The Complex World of Adolescent Literacy: Myths, Motivations, and Mysteries. Retrieved Agust 1, 2014, from http://www-personal.umich.edu/. Monroe, B. J. (2004). Crossing the Digital Age: Race, Writing, and Technology in the Classroom. New York: Teachers College Press. Moore, D. W., Prebble, S., Robertson, J., Waetford, R., Anderson, A. (2001). Self-recording With Goal Setting: A self-management programme for the classroom. Edu- cational Psychology, 21(3), 255-265. Morgan, P., Fuchs, D. (2007). Is There a Bidirectional Re- lationship between Children’s Reading Skills and Read- ing Motivation?. Exceptional Children, 73(2), 165–183. Mucherah, W., Yonder, A. (2008, June 5). Motivation for Reading and Middle School Students’ Performance on Standardized Testing in Reading. Retrieved May 14, 20, from http://www.tandfonline.com/. Murray, D. W., Rabiner, D. L. (2014, January 24). Teach- er Use of Computer-Assisted Instruction for Young Inat- tentive. Retrieved May 13, 2014, from http://files.eric. ed.gov/. Nell, V. (1988). Lost in a Book: The Psychology of Reading for Pleasure. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Padak, N., Potenza-Radis, C. (2010). Motivating strug- gling readers: Three keys to success. New England Reading Association Journal, 45(2), 1-7. Prensky, M. (2001, October). Digital Natives, Digital Im- migrants. Retrieved May 13, 2014, from https://goo.gl/ ZBknrX. Pinnell, G. S., Campbell, J., Pikulski, J., Kapinus, B., Wix- son, K., Gough, P., Beatty, A. (1995). Listening to Children Read Aloud. The National Center for Educa- tion Statistics. Retrieved July 29, 2014, from http://nces. ed.gov/pubs95/95728.pdf. Renaissance Learning. (2015). A Parent’s Guide to Acceler- ated Reader. Retrieved June 16, 2014, from http://www. covenantknights.org/. Reynolds, L. P., Symons, S. (2001, March). Motivational Variables and Children’s Text Search. Retrieved July 21, 2014, from http://psycnet.apa.org. Risinger, A. (2013). Teacher Reading Motivation Practices. Retrieved May 16, 2014, from http://gradworks.umi.com/. Ryan, R., Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and Extrinsic Mo- tivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions. Re- trieved Jaunary 24, 2014, from www.ideallibrary.com.
  • 10. 66 IJELS 5(3):57-66 Sanacore, J., Palumbo, A. (2010). Middle school students need more opportunities to read across the curriculum. A Journal of Educational Strategies, Issues and Ideas, 83, 180-185. doi:10.1080/00098650903583735 Schiefele, U. (1999). Interest and Learning From Text. Sci- entific Studies of Reading, 3(3), 257-279. Stahl, S.A., S. Kuhn, M.R. (2002). Making it sound like lan- guage: Developing fluency. The Reading Teacher, 55, 582-584. Sullivan, M. (2004). Why Johnny Will Not Read. School Li- brary Journal, 50(8), 36-39. The National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching Children to Read: An Avidence-based Assessment. Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Tilley, C. (2009). Reading motivation and engagement. School Library Monthly, 26(4), 39-42. Topping, K. J., Paul, T. D. (1999). Computer-Assissted Assessment of Practice at Reading: A Large Scale Sur- vey Using Accelerated Reader Data. Retrieved July 15, 2014, from http://ncset.uoregon.edu/. Topping, K. J., Fisher, A. M. (2003). Computerised For- mative Assessment of Reading Comprehension: field trials in the UK. Journal of Research in Reading, 26(3), 267–279. Topping, K. J. (2006). Accelerated Reader in specialist schools. Scotland: Centre for Peer Learning, University of Dundee. Topping, K. J., Samuels, J., Paul, T. (2007). Computerized assessment of independent reading: Effects of imple- mentation quality on achievement gain. School Effec- tiveness and School Improvement, 18(2), 191–208. Topping, K. J., Sanders, W. L. (2000). Teacher effective- ness and computer assessment of reading: Relating val- ue added and learning information system data. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 11(3), 305–337. Ülper, H. (2011). The Motivational Factors for Reading in Terms of Students. Educational Sciences: Theory And Practice,  11(2), 954-960. Retrieved from https://eric. ed.gov/?id=EJ927385 Walczyk, J., Marsiglia, C., Johns, A., Bryan, K. (2004). Children’s Compensations for Poorly Automated Read- ing Skills. Discourse Processes, 37(1), 47-66. http://dx. doi.org/DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp3701_3 Wang, J. H.-Y., Guthrie, J. (2004). Modeling the Effects of Intrinsic Motivation, Amount of Reading, and Past Reading Achievement on text Comprehension between U.S. and Chinese Students. Retrieved July 2, 2014, from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/. What Works Clearinghouse. (2010). Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI). US Department of Educa- tion: Institute of Education Sciences. What Works Clearinghouse. (2016). WWC Intervention Re- port: Accelerated Reader. US Department of Education: Institute of Education Sciences. Wigfield, A., Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations Of Children’s Motivation For Reading To The Amount And Breadth Or Their Reading. Retrieved July 1, 2014, from http:// www.cori.umd.edu/. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy–Value Theory of Achievement Motivation. Contemporary Education- al Psychology, 25(1), 68-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ ceps.1999.1015 Winfree, L. (2013, December). Reading Motivation and En- gagement at a Rural Georgia High School. doi:Order No. 3599489 Wolf, M., Tami, K.-C. (2001). Reading Fluency And Its ­Intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading 5(3), 211–239 Yovanoff, P., Duesbery, L., Alonzo, J., Tindal, G. (2005, September). Grade-Level Invariance of a Theoretical Causal Structure Predicting Reading Comprehension With Vocabulary and Oral Reading Fluency. Educa- tional Measurement: Issues and Practice, 24: 4–12. doi:10.1111/j.1745-3992.2005.00014.x