SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 162
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY
BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. ASSIGNMENTS WITH
SIMILARITY RATINGS GREATER THAN 35% WILL NOT BE
GRADED.
In your own words and GIVE EXAMPLES. Use business
English. Check your spelling and grammar. Each answer should
be about one-half page
Chapter One
1. Discuss (with examples) what is an I/O and what is a
resource-based business model.
Type answer here:
Chapter Two:
2. What ae the differences between the general environment and
the industry environment? Why are these differences important?
Type answer here:
Chapter Three
3. What is Value Chain Analysis? How does it help companies
earn above-average returns?
Type answer here:
Chapter Four
4. Explain Cost Leadership and Differentiation strategies. Be
sure to name a real-life company that practices each strategy
Type answer here.
Chapter Five
5. What factors contribute to the likelihood of a response to a
competitive action?
Type answer here:
Chapter Six:
6. What is corporate-level strategy? what is business-level
strategy?
Type answer here.
Chapter Eight:
8. List and explain with examples the five entry modes firms
may consider as paths to enter the international markets?
Type answer here
Chapter Nine:
9. What is a strategic alliance? What are the three major types
of strategic alliances?
Type answer here
Chapters 10
10-. Discuss what is meant by “agency relationship”? What are
some actions that firms can take to align the interests of
managerial agents with those of the firm’s shareholders?
Type answer here.
Chapters 11
11-. Compare and contrast strategic and financial controls?
Type answer here.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalC
ode=cphm20
Download by: [University of Newcastle, Australia] Date: 28
November 2017, At: 15:34
Psychology, Health & Medicine
ISSN: 1354-8506 (Print) 1465-3966 (Online) Journal homepage:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cphm20
Feasibility and effectiveness of psychosocial
resilience training: A pilot study of the READY
program
Nicola W. Burton , Ken I. Pakenham & Wendy J. Brown
To cite this article: Nicola W. Burton , Ken I. Pakenham &
Wendy J. Brown (2010) Feasibility and
effectiveness of psychosocial resilience training: A pilot study
of the READY program, Psychology,
Health & Medicine, 15:3, 266-277, DOI:
10.1080/13548501003758710
To link to this article:
https://doi.org/10.1080/13548501003758710
Published online: 17 May 2010.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 1217
View related articles
Citing articles: 49 View citing articles
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalC
ode=cphm20
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cphm20
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.108
0/13548501003758710
https://doi.org/10.1080/13548501003758710
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCo
de=cphm20&show=instructions
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCo
de=cphm20&show=instructions
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/135485010037587
10
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/135485010037587
10
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/135485010037
58710#tabModule
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/135485010037
58710#tabModule
Feasibility and effectiveness of psychosocial resilience training:
A pilot
study of the READY program
Nicola W. Burton
a
*, Ken I. Pakenham
b
and Wendy J. Brown
a
a
School of Human Movement Studies, The University of
Queensland, Blair Drive, St Lucia,
Brisbane 4072, Australia;
b
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Blair
Drive,
St Lucia, Brisbane 4072, Australia
(Received 11 September 2009; final version received 4 February
2010)
Despite many studies on the characteristics associated with
resilience, there is
little research on interventions to promote resilience in adults.
The aims of this
study were to gather preliminary information regarding the
feasibility of
implementing a group psychosocial resilience training program
(REsilience and
Activity for every DaY, READY) in a workplace setting, and to
assess if program
would potentially promote well-being. The program targets five
protective factors
identified from empirical evidence: Positive emotions, cognitive
flexibility, social
support, life meaning, and active coping. Resilience
enhancement strategies reflect
core acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) processes and
cognitive
behavior therapy strategies. Sessions involve psychoeducation,
discussions,
experiential exercises, and home assignments. Sixteen
participants completed
11 6 two h group sessions over 13 weeks. Baseline and post-
intervention
assessment included self-administered questionnaires,
pedometer step counts, and
physical and hematological measures. Data were analyzed using
standardized
mean differences and paired t-tests. There was a significant
improvement between
baseline and post intervention scores on measures of mastery (p
¼ 0.001), positive
emotions (p ¼ 0.002), personal growth (p ¼ 0.004), mindfulness
(p ¼ 0.004),
acceptance (p ¼ 0.012), stress (p ¼ 0.013), self acceptance (p ¼
0.016), valued
living (p ¼ 0.022), autonomy (p ¼ 0.032) and total cholesterol
(p ¼ 0.025).
Participants rated the program and materials very highly. These
results indicate
that the READY program is feasible to implement as a group
training program in
a workplace setting to promote psychosocial well-being.
Keywords: resilience; well-being; mental health; health
promotion; ACT;
depression; stress management
Background
Resilience is the capacity of people to effectively cope with,
adjust, or recover from
stress or adversity. When faced with adversity, people with low
resilience are at risk
of depression, stress, anxiety and interpersonal difficulties, and
may adopt health
compromising behaviors and experience somatic complaints and
poor physical
health. Prolonged stress and poor psychosocial functioning may
negatively impact
on physical health for example, via biological mechanisms such
as hypertension and
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Psychology, Health & Medicine
Vol. 15, No. 3, May 2010, 266–277
ISSN 1354-8506 print/ISSN 1465-3966 online
� 2010 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/13548501003758710
http://www.informaworld.com
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
blood pressure reactivity to stress, atherogenic lipid profiles,
pro-inflammatory
cytokines (e.g. C reactive protein), and the development of
metabolic syndrome
(Rozanski, Blumenthal, & Kaplan, 1999; Strike & Steptoe,
2004).
Most studies of resilience have focused on young people or
individuals
experiencing specific adverse circumstances, and have explored
the personal
characteristics associated with effective coping (Richardson &
Waite, 2002). Only
a few studies have focused on adults and investigated the
effectiveness of
resilience training. A worksite trial of the ‘‘Personal Resilience
and Resilient
Relationships’’ program, with 5 6 7 h modules implemented
weekly over five
weeks, demonstrated significantly higher levels of self esteem,
locus of control,
purpose in life, and interpersonal relations among program
participants compared
with a control group (Waite & Richardson, 2004). A modified
version of this
program, with 10 6 90 min modules implemented twice weekly
over five weeks
for people with diabetes, found no significant difference
between program
participants and a usual care group on the same psychological
measures or
glycosylated hemoglobin or waist circumference (Bradshaw et
al., 2007).
Participants of a worksite trial of the ‘‘Resilience
Reintegration’’ program,
implemented in approximately 14 days during six months for
employees with
illnesses attributed to work stress, demonstrated higher levels of
effective coping
(including seeking social support) and lower levels of
depression at post-
intervention compared with baseline (Steensma, Den Heijer, &
Stallen, 2006).
These latter two programs involved a tertiary approach to
psychosocial well-
being, as they targeted individuals with developed conditions so
as to minimize
suffering associated with poor health. Only the first program
adopted a primary
approach of promoting resilience in otherwise healthy adults.
We have developed a psychosocial resilience training program
(READY:
REsilience and Activity for every DaY) to promote resilience
and psychosocial
well-being in adults. The program is designed as a primary or
secondary level of
intervention, and targets adults at risk of stress or stress
induced depressive
symptoms, but otherwise generally healthy. The aim of this
article is to describe a
pilot study of the program that was conducted to obtain
information on the
feasibility of implementing the program as group-based training
in a workplace
setting. The study also examined the potential effectiveness of
the program to
promote subjective well-being, and reduce symptoms associated
with depression and
stress.
Method
The study protocol was approved by The University of
Queensland Medical
Research Ethics Committee (2007000303).
Study design
This study was a single group pre–post trial with outcome
measures assessed the
week immediately prior to and after the 13 week intervention
period.
Setting
The program was conducted with employees of a university in a
capital city.
Psychology, Health & Medicine 267
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
Participants
An invitation to participate in the program, without cost, was
circulated in a weekly
email communication to administrative staff at The University
of Queensland
(Australia). Interested respondents contacted the project staff by
return email, and
were then contacted by telephone by a research assistant who
provided preliminary
study information. Respondents were considered ineligible if
they reported receiving
current psychiatric/psychological treatment (pharmacological or
therapy-based).
Those who provided preliminary consent to participate were
then asked to attend a
group-based assessment session at the University. No incentives
were offered for
participation.
The intervention
The READY program targets five key resilience protective
factors that were identified
from empirical literature: (1) positive emotions; (2) cognitive
flexibility (e.g. accep-
tance), (3) life meaning, (4) social support, and (5) active
coping strategies (including
physical activity) (Southwick, Vythilingam, & Charney, 2005).
The intervention approach is based on Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy
(ACT), which is an empirically based third generation Cognitive
Behavioral Therapy
that uses acceptance and mindfulness strategies, and
commitment and behavior
change strategies to produce psychological flexibility and
resilience through six core
processes: Acceptance, cognitive defusion (changing our
relationship with thoughts),
being present (mindfulness), self-as-context, values and
committed action (Hayes,
Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). The program also
includes cognitive
behavioral therapy processes of skills training for relaxation
and building social
support. One program module promotes participation in
purposive and incidental
physical activity. Physical activity has previously been
identified as a potential
coping resource (Southwick et al., 2005) that can provide
enduring resilience to stress
(Salmon, 2001), enhance well-being (Penedo & Dahn, 2005;
Scully, Kremer, Meade,
Graham, & Dudgeon, 1998; Stathopoulou, Powers, Berry, Smits,
& Otto, 2006), and
protect against incident depression symptoms (Brown, Ford,
Burton, Marshall, &
Dobson, 2005; Strawbridge, Deleger, Roberts, & Kaplan, 2002;
van Gool et al.,
2007).
The program has 11 modules, and sessions include
psychoeducation, discussion,
experiential exercises, and structured learning and practice
activities. Session topics
include an introduction to the READY resilience model,
physical activity,
mindfulness, defusion (two modules including self-as-context),
acceptance, life
values, social connectedness, relaxation and pleasant activities,
and activating and
trouble shooting strategies (two modules).
Participants receive a detailed workbook that includes an audio
compact disc
(with guided exercises), written notes, sections for critical
reflection, and
structured learning activities to complete. The reflection and
learning activities
comprise the READY Personal Plan, which is a personalized
resource to help
participants apply the generalized information to their specific
context and
individual style.
Eleven sessions each of two h duration were run weekly over 13
weeks (with 1
week off in weeks three and 10 because of public holidays) at
the university. Sessions
268 N.W. Burton et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
were held during the week after work hours (five–seven pm),
and were led by two of
the authors (N.B., K.P.) who are clinical and health
psychologists.
Measures
Group assessment sessions were conducted at the university, in
the week before and
the week after the program, by three independent research
assistants (one each for
questionnaire, physical, and hematological measures) with the
lead investigator
(N.B.) in attendance.
Feasibility and acceptability
Participant attendance records were kept for each session by the
interventionists,
and reasons for non-attendance were recorded. The post
intervention ques-
tionnaire included items to obtain participant feedback on the
program and
materials.
Psychosocial well-being
A battery of self completed questionnaires included:
(1) Ryff’s Scales of Psychological Well-Being, including
subscales of autonomy,
environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relations,
purpose in life,
and self-acceptance (Ryff, 1989). This measure is widely used,
has well
established reliability and validity (Ryff & Singer, 2003), and
has previously
been shown to be sensitive to detecting treatment effects (Fava,
Rafanelli,
Grandi, Conti, & Belluardo, 1998).
(2) The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale
(CES-D) (Radloff,
1977). This is one of the most commonly used self report
questionnaires on
depression for a general (vs. clinical) population, and has
established
reliability and validity (Radloff, 1977).
(3) The Short Version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale
(DASS-21) with
subscales of depression (DASS_Dep), stress (DASS_Stress),
and anxiety
(DASS_Anx), (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). Each subscale has
been shown
to have high internal consistency and has yielded meaningful
discriminations
in a variety of settings in both Australian clinical and
community samples
(Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995).
(4) The positive affect scale from The Positive and Negative
Affect Schedule
(PANAS-X), which is widely used with well established
psychometric
properties (Watson & Clark, 1999).
(5) The action consistency items from The Valued Living
Questionnaire
(Wilson & Groom, 2002). Psychometric data on this measure is
not yet
available.
(6) The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale, which has been
shown to be
reliable, valid, and sensitive to change (Brown & Ryan, 2003).
(7) The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire II (AAQII), which
has
acceptable levels of factorial validity, criterion validity, and
reliability (Hayes
et al., 2006).
Psychology, Health & Medicine 269
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
(8) The MOS Social Support Survey (Sherbourne & Stewart,
1991), which has
acceptable levels of reliability and validity (Hays, Sherbourne,
& Mazel,
1995).
Physical activity
Physical activity was measured via self-report using items
adapted from the Active
Australia surveys that ask about the total time spent during the
previous week in
walking (Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW),
2003). These items
have acceptable levels of reliability and validity (Brown,
Bauman, Chey, Trost, &
Mummery, 2004; Brown, Burton, Marshall, & Miller, 2008;
Brown, Trost, Bauman,
Mummery, & Owen, 2004). Total time spent in activity was
calculated by summing
the time spent in minutes of activity across these three
categories, after weighting
minutes in vigorous activity by two to allow for the greater
intensity [i.e. (minutes
walking) þ (minutes moderate activity) þ (minutes vigorous
activity 62)]. Physical
activity was also assessed using pedometer step counts.
Participants were asked to
wear a pedometer for seven consecutive days and record the
total number of steps
taken each day. The pedometer and the step log were returned at
the first group
training session, and by mail after the program completion. Data
was used to derive
average daily step counts.
Physical and hematological measures
Physical measures included height and weight (used to derive
body mass index
[BMI]) and blood pressure (BP_Sys and BP_Dias).
Hematological data involved a
fasting blood sample to measure blood glucose, total
cholesterol, C-reactive protein
(CRP), and cortisol.
Sociodemographic measures
Questionnaire data were used to assess age, gender, country of
birth, household
composition, educational qualifications, employment status,
occupation, ability to
manage on income received, overall health status, caffeine and
alcohol consumption,
and tobacco cigarette consumption.
Analyses
Descriptive statistics were used to analyze attendance records,
responses to
questionnaire items about the program, and sociodemographic
data. Outcome
measure data were analyzed on an as per protocol basis.
Standardized
mean differences (with Hedges adjustment for a small sample
size) and 95%
confidence intervals were used to examine the relative size of
the intervention
effect across the different measures. For the following
variables, scores were
interpreted as positive intervention effects when the post-
intervention values were
lower than baseline: DASS_Stress, DASS_Dep (depression),
DASS_Anx (anxiety),
CES-D (depression), BP_Dias and BP_Sys (blood pressure),
cholesterol, CRP (C-
reactive protein), and BMI. Cohen’s standards (large [0.8],
medium [0.5], and
small [0.2]) were used to interpret the magnitude of intervention
effects (Cohen,
1988).
270 N.W. Burton et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
Paired t-tests were used to determine the significance of
differences between
baseline and post intervention scores. As this was a feasibility
study with a small
sample, an a of 0.05 was used initially instead of a more
conservative value. However
given the number of planned comparisons, results were also
examined using a
sequential Bonferroni adjustment of a levels.
Results
Participants
The sociodemographic characteristics of participants are
presented in Table 1. The
age of participants ranged from 24 to 50 years, with a mean of
36.5 (SD 8.6). The
self-reported time spent in physical activity ranged from 45 to
1200 min/week, with a
median of 205 minutes/week. Average daily steps ranged from
4956 to 14,249, with a
mean of 9801 (SD ¼ 2784).
Feasibility
Thirty-five people responded to the study invitation and 18
people consented to
participate. Two participants did not complete the program; one
moved interstate,
and the other discontinued because of a change in personal
circumstances.
The average proportion of sessions attended by participants was
81%, with three
participants attending all 11 sessions. During the program, 37%
(n ¼ 6) missed one
or two sessions, and 44% (n ¼ 7) missed three or four sessions.
The most common
reasons given for missing sessions were time conflicts with
work meetings and
planned recreation leave.
Participant feedback on the program was very positive. On a
five-point Likert
scale, the mean rating for the program overall was 4.67 (where
5 was excellent and 4
was very good), and the mean rating of personal helpfulness
was 4.44 (where 5 was a
lot and 4 was moderately so). On a four-point Likert scale
(where 4 was very helpful
and 3 was moderately helpful), the mean rating for the
workbook was 3.87, and the
mean rating for the READY Personal Plan was 3.5. The
majority of participants
agreed with the weekly frequency (75%) and the two hour
session duration (87%). In
terms of the overall program length, 56% agreed that it was
good, and 31% thought
it was too short.
Effect on psychosocial, physical activity, physical, and
hematological measures
Figure 1 shows the standardized mean differences and 95%
confidence intervals for
each measure. There were large favorable intervention effects
on measures of
acceptance, environmental mastery, positive emotions,
mindfulness and personal
growth; moderate effects on measures of stress, self acceptance,
valued living,
autonomy, and depression; and a small effect on the total
cholesterol measure.
The mean scores at baseline and post-intervention are shown in
Table 2. Paired
t-tests indicated a significant difference (p 5 0.05) between
baseline and post
intervention scores on measures of mastery (t[15] ¼ 4.234, p ¼
0.001), positive
emotions (t[15] ¼ 3.696, p ¼ 0.002), personal growth (t[15] ¼
3.357, p ¼ 0.004),
mindfulness (t[15] ¼ 3.362, p ¼ 0.004), acceptance (t[15] ¼
2.847, p ¼ 0.012), stress
(t[15] ¼ 72.807, p ¼ 0.013), self acceptance (t[15] ¼ 2.720, p ¼
0.016), valued living
(t[15] ¼ 2.557, p ¼ 0.022), autonomy (t[15] ¼ 2.369, p ¼
0.032), and total
Psychology, Health & Medicine 271
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
cholesterol (t[15] ¼ 72.483, p ¼ 0.025). The difference on a
measure of depression
(CES-D) had borderline significance (t[15] ¼ 72.063, p ¼
0.057). Using the
sequential Bonferroni adjustment of a levels, only mastery
remained significant.
Discussion
These results provide promising preliminary support for the
READY program as an
intervention that can be feasibly implemented as group-based
training in a
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of study sample.
Study sample % (n)
Gender
Men 85 (15)
Women 17 (3)
Age group (years)
20–30 33 (6)
31–40 33 (6)
41–50 33 (6)
Country of birth
Australia 61 (11)
UK/USA 17 (3)
Other 22 (4)
Highest educational qualification completed
School only 11 (2)
Certificate/diploma 11 (2)
University degree 78 (14)
Household composition
Living alone 11 (2)
Single and living with others 17 (3)
Couple, no children 50 (9)
Couple with children 22 (4)
Employment status
Full time 83 (15)
Part time/casual 17 (3)
Occupational group
Manager or Senior Administrator 28 (5)
Professional or Associate Professional 33 (6)
Clerical/Other 39 (7)
Ability to manage on income received
Easy 33 (6)
Not too bad/Difficult sometimes 61 (11)
Impossible 6 (1)
General health status
Excellent/very good 17 (3)
Good 33 (6)
Fair/Poor 50 (9)
Body mass index
518.5–525 67 (12)
425 33 (6)
272 N.W. Burton et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
workplace setting to improve psychosocial well-being. Our
three-month implemen-
tation period was twice as long as both the original and the
modified Personal
Resilience and Resilient Relationships program (Bradshaw et
al., 2007; Waite &
Richardson, 2004), but half the length of the Resilience and
Reintegration program,
although the majority of that was conducted in the first seven
weeks (Steensma et al.,
2006). READY involved 22 h of contact time, which was
slightly longer than the
Personal Resilience and Resilient Relationships Program
(Bradshaw et al., 2007) but
much shorter than the other two training programs that required
40 h (Waite &
Richardson, 2004) and (approximately) 14 days (Steensma et
al., 2006).
Written feedback indicated that the READY participants liked
the weekly
frequency of sessions, as this kept training issues ‘‘on the
agenda’’ while also
allowing time for reflection and skills practice. Although the 11
6 2 h sessions were
seen as a significant time commitment, and less than 20% of
participants attended all
sessions, a shorter implementation period or shorter sessions
was not favored by the
majority of participants. There was some support for having a
longer implementa-
tion period to allow for more breaks during the program, e.g.
the occasional two
week break from weekly sessions, so as to consolidate learning
and prevent fatigue.
Consideration of this for future applications of the program
would however, need to
be in consultation with the employer organization; in this trial
the implementation
period was within one term in the university calendar.
Participants rated the program highly, and saw it as personally
helpful and
enjoyable. The participant workbook and READY Personal Plan
were well received
Figure 1. Standardized mean difference and 95% confidence
intervals for study measures.
Psychology, Health & Medicine 273
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
and seen as a useful resource both during and after the program.
Specific suggestions
to improve the program were to have more time in each session
to review homework
and activities from the previous week, more small group work,
and more time to
review the overall program and the READY Personal Plan.
There were large and significantly favorable intervention effects
on measures of
acceptance (acknowledging both positive and negative
experiences), environmental
mastery (effective and competent use of opportunities and
external activities), positive
emotions (e.g. interest, enthusiasm, determination), mindfulness
(conscious attention
and awareness), and personal growth (self improvement,
openness to new experiences).
There were also moderate and significant favorable intervention
effects on
measures of stress, self acceptance (positive self attitude),
valued living (actions
consistent with life priorities and desires), and autonomy (self-
determination, self
regulation). As the READY program targeted positive emotions,
problem solving,
and life purpose as key protective factors, and included session
modules on
mindfulness and acceptance (targeting the protective factor of
cognitive flexibility),
improvements in these areas were particularly pleasing.
Although there was a
moderate favorable effect on depression, this did not reach
statistical significance.
This may reflect low baseline levels; the mean score for both
the DASS_Depression
Table 2. Mean differences in measures between baseline and
post-intervention.
Measure
Mean
difference (SD)
95% confidence
interval p
Questionnaire measures
Ryff_Autonomy 3.06 (5.17) 0.31–5.82 0.03
Ryff_Environmental mastery 6.25 (5.91) 3.10–9.40 0.001
Ryff_Personal growth 4.37 (5.21) 1.60–7.15 0.004
Ryff_Positive relations 1.06 (6.29) 72.29–4.41 0.509
Ryff_Life purpose 3.87 (9.82) 71.36–9.11 0.135
Ryff_Self acceptance 3.94 (5.79) 0.85–7.02 0.016
CES-D (Depression)
a 75.25 (10.18) 710.67–0.17 0.057
DASS_Depression
a 73.75 (9.46) 78.79–1.29 0.134
DASS_Stress
a 75.75 (8.19) 710.12 to 1.38 0.013
DASS_Anxiety
a
0.50 (7.17) 73.32–4.32 0.784
PANAS_positive affect 6.44 (6.97) 2.72–10.15 0.002
Valued living questionnaire 7.19 (11.24) 1.20–13.18 0.022
Mindful attention awareness scale 7.12 (8.48) 2.61–11.64 0.004
Acceptance and action questionnaire II (AAQII) 6.81 (9.57)
1.71–11.91 0.012
MOS social support survey 0.87 (2.03) 70.21 to 1.96 0.105
Physical activity
Minutes/week 15.33 (289) 7144–175 0.840
Average steps/day 223.85 (2870) 71356–1823 0.757
Physical measures
Body mass index
a
0.09 (1.11) 70.49–0.68 0.74
Blood pressure_systolic
a 72.87 (7.62) 76.94–1.19 0.152
Blood pressure_diastolic
a 70.87 (6.25) 74.20–2.45 0.584
Hematological measures
Fasting blood glucose
a 70.74 (0.42) 70.30–0.15 0.489
Total cholesterol
a 70.26 (0.41) 70.48 to 0.03 0.025
C-Reactive protein
a 70.07 (0.83) 70.52–0.37 0.730
Cortisol
a
0.500 (27.69) 714.26–15.26 0.943
a
Negative mean difference implies favorable change.
274 N.W. Burton et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
subscale (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) and the CES-D (Radloff,
1977) were below
‘‘clinical’’ levels.
Because of the different measures used, our results are not
directly comparable
with other resilience training studies. There is however,
conceptual similarity
between our improvements on measures of valued living,
autonomy, and self
acceptance, and the higher levels of life purpose, locus of
control, and self esteem
reported by Waite and Richardson (2004). Consistent with
Steensma et al., (2006)
our study demonstrated improvements in depression, although
as previously stated,
this had only borderline significance.
Unlike other studies (Steensma et al., 2006; Waite &
Richardson, 2004), we did
not find significant improvements on measures of interpersonal
relations. We used
the MOS Social Support Survey (Sherbourne & Stewart, 1991),
which assesses the
frequency of the availability of sources of support (e.g.
someone whose advice you
really want, someone to show you love and affection), which
may not have been
sufficiently sensitive to change. Resilience was conceptualized
as a primarily intra-
personal construct to be developed by participants, while this
measure assesses
supportive behaviours provided by inter-personal networks,
which was not under the
direct influence of program participants. Other studies used
measures that assessed
seeking social support as a coping strategy (Steensma et al.,
2006) and the frequency
of specified interpersonal experiences (Waite & Richardson,
2004).
Consistent with the findings of Bradshaw et al., (2007), we did
not find significant
changes in the physical or haematological measures, or in self-
reported physical
activity. A lack of significant change in the hematological
measures may be because
these were largely within the accepted healthy ranges at
baseline. We did however, have
a small but significant change in total cholesterol. No
significant changes in physical
activity may have been because of high baseline levels; the
median of 205 min/week is
higher than the 5 6 30 min/week of moderate activity or 3 6 20
min/week of
vigorous activity specified in national recommendations
(Haskell et al., 2007). It should
be noted however, that physical activity was conceptualized as
only one aspect of
resilience, and already active participants could still benefit
from other program
modules. Furthermore, participants may have qualitatively
changed their experience of
physical activity participation in response to the other program
modules, such as doing
physical activity with mindfulness. This could have had a
synergistic effect on other
outcome measures, such as for example positive emotions.
Limitations
As this study was a small-scale feasibility study, and not a
controlled trial, we are
unable to make conclusions about the efficacy of the program.
There were however,
significant pre–post improvements in specific indicators of well
being and
psychosocial functioning. Without a follow-up assessment, we
are unable to
comment on the sustainability of these improvements. As the
construct of resilience
is such that it implies a protective effect against future
adversity, the longevity of any
improvements will be important to assess in future research.
The short implementa-
tion period may have been insufficient to see changes in the
physiological measures
such as BMI. The study relied heavily on self-report data, which
are vulnerable to
social desirability bias and measurement error. However, we
used questionnaires
that have previously been demonstrated to have acceptable
levels of sensitivity to
change and reliability, and complemented the physical activity
questionnaire data
Psychology, Health & Medicine 275
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
with objective step count (pedometer) data. As the study sample
was recruited from
staff at a university in a capital city, and included a majority of
women and people
with a university education, further work is needed to examine
the acceptability and
effectiveness of the program with a more heterogenous sample.
Conclusions
These results suggest that it is feasible to implement the
READY program as a group
training program in a worksite setting to promote psychosocial
functioning and well-
being. Participant feedback indicated that no major changes
were required to the
session duration, session frequency or program materials, but
that a longer
implementation period and more time for review activities could
be considered.
Sessions could also be restructured to allow for more small
group work, and more
review of previous sessions and homework activities. An
alternative measure of social
support, that focuses on the behaviours of the respondent (e.g.
seeking support) rather
than others (e.g. support provision) may be more useful. Future
work will examine the
efficacy of the program in a controlled trial, the sustainability
of any improvements,
and the mechanisms of change. This will facilitate our
conceptual understanding of
resilience, and our practical understanding of how to positively
influence it.
Acknowledgements
Nicola Burton is a Heart Foundation Research Fellow (PH 08B
3904), and is also supported
by a (Australian) National Health and Medical Research Council
Capacity Building Grant
(ID 252977) and Program Grant (ID 310200). This study was
funded by a Program Grant
from the (Australian) National Health and Medical Research
Council (ID 301200). The
authors thank the participants for their ongoing enthusiasm and
support for the program, and
Sarah Walters for research support.
References
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW). (2003). The
Active Australia survey: A
guide and manual for implementation, analysis and reporting.
Canberra: AIHW.
Bradshaw, B., Richardson, G., Kumpfer, K., Carlson, J.,
Stanchfield, J., Overall, J., et al.
(2007). Determining the efficacy of a resiliency training
approach in adults with type 2
diabetes. The Diabetes Educator, 33(4), 650–659.
Brown, K., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present:
Mindfulness and its role in
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.
Brown, W., Trost, S.G., Bauman, A., Mummery, W.K., & Owen,
N. (2004). Test–retest
reliability of four physical activity measures used in population
surveys. Journal of Science
and Medicine in Sport, 28(2), 128–134.
Brown, W.J., Bauman, A., Chey, T., Trost, S.G., & Mummery,
W.K. (2004). Comparison of
surveys used to measure physical activity. Australian and New
Zealand Journal of Public
Health, 28(2), 128–134.
Brown, W.J., Burton, N.W., Marshall, A.L., & Miller, Y.D.
(2008). Reliability and validity of
a modified self-administered version of the Active Australia
physical activity survey in a
sample of mid-age women. Australian and New Zealand Journal
of Public Health, 32(6),
535–541.
Brown, W.J., Ford, J., Burton, N.W., Marshall, A.L., & Dobson,
A. (2005). Prospective study
of physical activity and depressive symptoms in mid age
women. American Journal of
Preventive Medicine, 29(4), 265–272.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Fava, G., Rafanelli, C., Grandi, S., Conti, S., & Belluardo, P.
(1998). Prevention of recurrent
depression with cognitive behavioral therapy. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 55, 816–820.
276 N.W. Burton et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
Haskell, W.L., Lee, I.-M., Pate, R., Powell, K., Blair, S.,
Franklin, B., et al. (2007). Physical
activity and public health: Updated recommendation for adults
from the American
College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart
Association. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 39(8), 1423–1434.
Hayes, S., Luoma, J.B., Bond, F.W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J.
(2006). Acceptance and
commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes.
Behaviour Research and Therapy,
44, 1–25.
Hays, R., Sherbourne, C., & Mazel, R. (1995). User’s manual
for medical outcomes study
(MOS) core measures of health-related quality of life: RAND
Corporation, MR-162-RC.
Santa Monica, CA.
Lovibond, S., & Lovibond, P. (1995). Manual for the
Depression Anxiety Stress Scales
(DASS) (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation of Australia.
Penedo, F.J., & Dahn, J.R. (2005). Exercise and well-being: A
review of mental and physical
health benefits associated with physical activity. Current
Opinions in Psychiatry, 18(2),
189–193.
Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression
scale for research in the
general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1,
385–401.
Richardson, G., & Waite, P. (2002). Mental health promotion
through resilience and resiliency
education. International Journal of Emergency Mental Health,
4(1), 65–75.
Rozanski, A., Blumenthal, J., & Kaplan, J. (1999). Impact of
psychosocial factors on the
pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for
therapy. Circulation, 99,
2192–2217.
Ryff, C. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations
on the meaning of
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.
Ryff, C., & Singer, B. (2003). Flourishing under fire. In C.
Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.),
Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp.
15–36). Washington: American
Psychological Association.
Salmon, P. (2001). Effects of physical exercise on anxiety,
depression, and sensitivity to stress:
A unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(1), 33–61.
Scully, D., Kremer, J., Meade, M., Graham, R.C., & Dudgeon,
K. (1998). Physical exercise
and psychological well being: A critical review. British Journal
of Sports Medicine, 32(2),
111–120.
Sherbourne, C., & Stewart, A. (1991). The MOS social support
survey. Social Science and
Medicine, 32(6), 705–714.
Southwick, S., Vythilingam, M., & Charney, D. (2005). The
psychobiology of depression and
resilience to stress: Implications for prevention and treatment.
Annual Review of Clinical
Psychology, 1, 255–291.
Stathopoulou, G., Powers, M., Berry, A., Smits, J., & Otto, M.
(2006). Exercise interventions
for mental health: A quantitative and qualitative review.
Clinical Psychology-Science and
Practice, 13(2), 179–193.
Steensma, H., Den Heijer, M., & Stallen, V. (2006). Research
note: Effects of resilience
training on the reduction of stress and depression among Dutch
workers. International
Quarterly of Community Health Education, 27(2), 145–159.
Strawbridge, W., Deleger, S., Roberts, R., & Kaplan, G. (2002).
Physical activity reduces the
risk of subsequent depression for older adults. American Journal
of Epidemiology, 156(4),
327–334.
Strike, P., & Steptoe, A. (2004). Psychosocial factors in the
development of coronary artery
disease. Progress in Cardiovascular Diseases, 46(4), 337–347.
van Gool, C., Kempen, G., Bosma, H., van Boxtel, M., Jolles,
J., & Van Eijk, J. (2007).
Associations between lifestyle and depressed mood:
Longitudinal results from the
Maastricht Aging Study. American Journal of Public Health,
97(5), 887–815.
Waite, P., & Richardson, G. (2004). Determining the efficacy of
resilience training in the
worksite. Journal of Allied Health, 33(3), 178–183.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. (1999). PANAS-X manual for positive
and negative affect schedule-
expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa.
Wilson, K., & Groom, J. (2002). The valued living
questionnaire. University of Mississippi.
(Available from K.G. Wilson, Department of Psychology, 205
Peabody Building,
University of Mississippi, US 38677).
Psychology, Health & Medicine 277
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[
U
ni
ve
rs
it
y
of
N
ew
ca
st
le
, A
us
tr
al
ia
]
at
1
5:
34
2
8
N
ov
em
be
r
20
17
Promoting Resilience in Children
and Youth
Preventive Interventions and Their Interface
with Neuroscience
MARK T. GREENBERG
Prevention Research Center, Pennsylvania State University,
University Park,
Pennsylvania, USA
ABSTRACT: Preventive interventions focus on reducing risk
and promot-
ing protective factors in the child as well as their cultural
ecologies (family,
classroom, school, peer groups, neighborhood, etc). By
improving com-
petencies in both the child and their contexts many of these
interventions
promote resilience. Although there are now a substantial
number of pre-
ventive interventions that reduce problem behaviors and build
competen-
cies across childhood and adolescence, there has been little
integration
with recent findings in neuropsychology and neuroscience. This
article
focuses on the integration of prevention research and
neuroscience in the
context of interventions that promote resilience by improving
the exec-
utive functions (EF); inhibitory control, planning, and problem
solving
skills, emotional regulation, and attentional capacities of
children and
youth. Illustrations are drawn from recent randomized
controlled trials
of the Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies (PATHS)
curriculum.
The discussion focuses on the next steps in transdisciplinary
research in
prevention and social neuroscience.
KEYWORDS: prevention; intervention; neuroscience; children;
youth;
frontal lobe
PROMOTING RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN AND YOUTH:
PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS AND THEIR INTERFACE
WITH NEUROSCIENCE
As a psychologist interested in creating change in communities
and influenc-
ing public policy, I am often asked questions about
developmental processes
and trajectories in children. For example, “How can we help
children resist
Address for correspondence: Dr. Mark T. Greenberg, Prevention
Research Center, Henderson Build-
ing, South Room 109, Penn State University, University Park,
PA 16802. Voice: 814-863-0112; fax:
814-865-2530.
e-mail: [email protected]
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1094: 139–150 (2006). C© 2006 New
York Academy of Sciences.
doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.013
139
140 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
negative influences when they live in high-risk neighborhoods?”
“How can we
improve children’s academic outcomes when their families have
experienced
an intergenerational history of school failure?” “How can we
help children
control their emotions when their peers are bringing out the
worst in them
through teasing and taunting?” All of these questions imply the
notion that
many children are exposed to high-risk situations and the
implication is that
unless something is done, the probability of a poor outcome for
some chil-
dren is relatively high. Such situations are ripe for efforts to
attempt to prevent
difficulties that may spiral into longer-term poor outcomes
across childhood,
adolescence, and even adulthood. Beginning with the War on
Poverty and the
emergence of Head Start in the 1960s, preventive interventions
have become
central to our nation’s public policy. As a result of the
increased rate of both
adolescent delinquency and drug use seen in the ensuing
decades, the devel-
opment of both preventive intervention programs and
social/legislative policy
initiatives has dramatically increased. The role of
developmental science in
public policy making has never been greater or more influential.
Improving the public’s health, especially for those at greatest
risk, is a com-
plex problem that involves interventions at the level of
economic and social
policy as well as the ability to strengthen the skills of
educators, parents, and
youth themselves. Macrolevel interventions focused on
changing community-
level ecologies, attitudes, and behavior have ranged from large
experiments in
economic policy (e.g., earned income tax credits, TANF),
changes in housing
patterns,1 social legislation ranging from raising the age at
which youth can
drive to building community partnerships to reduce youth
problem behaviors
and build positive youth development at the community level.2
At a more mi-
crolevel, attempts to improve the culture, attitudes, and
relations in families,
peer groups, and schools have focused on building
communication skills and
values that promote positive developmental outcomes. Finally,
there has been
recognition that some attributes inside the individual, including
skills, cogni-
tions, and behaviors, may be malleable in response to
preventive efforts. The
enormous scope of activities undertaken is a testimony to our
implicit under-
standing that child and youth outcomes are multidetermined and
that various
levels of influence impact developmental trajectories. Although
at times these
initiatives seem (or are) both random and chaotic, at another
level they clearly
reflect the combination of our growing basic knowledge of both
the ecology of
human development,3 the advances made in developmental
psychopathology,4
and the emergent development of the field of prevention
science.5
The study of resilience emerges out of the large research
endeavor of public
health epidemiology and the study of risk and protective factors
and how they
impact development. A number of guiding principles have
emerged both from
epidemiology and developmental psychopathology.6,7 First, it
is unlikely that
there is a single “cause” of many of the preventable outcomes
(e.g., mental
disorders, substance abuse, school failure, delinquency); even in
the case of
disorders in which a biochemical or genetic mechanism has
been discovered,
GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 141
the expression of the disorder is influenced by other biological
or environ-
mental events.8,9 Second, there are multiple pathways both to
and from risk
and problem outcomes; for example, there are different
combinations of risk
factors that might lead to the same disorder. Third, no single
cause may be
either necessary or sufficient10 and the effect of a risk factor
will depend on
its timing and relation to other risk factors. Fourth, many risk
factors are not
disorder-specific, but instead relate to a variety of outcomes.
Finally, risk fac-
tors may vary in influence with host factors, such as gender,
ethnicity, and
culture.
Resiliency is commonly defined as positive or protective
processes that re-
duce maladaptive outcomes under conditions of risk. That is,
they are protective
factors that may be especially important under conditions of
risk. Although
much less is known about protective factors and their
operation,7,11 at least
three broad types of protective factors have been identified.
These include
characteristics of the individual (e.g., temperamental qualities
and intelligence/
cognitive ability), the quality of the child’s relationships, and
broader ecological
factors, such as quality schools, safe neighborhoods, and
regulatory activities.
An essential question related to adversity and resilience is the
individual’s
development of an effective set of responses to stress. Central
components of
the stress response include the initial appraisal of the event and
its emotional
meaning, the ability to sufficiently regulate one’s emotions and
arousal to
initiate problem solving and gather more information, the fuller
cognitive-
affective interpretation of the event, and one’s behavioral
response. Masten12
has noted that among the most important resiliency factors are
these very
cognitive and emotion regulation skills.
Prospective longitudinal designs are critical to understanding
the role of
resiliency as they can identify (a) which risk factors are
predictive of different
developmental stages of a problem, (b) the dynamic relation
between risk and
protective factors in different developmental periods, and (c)
what factors are
most likely to “protect” or buffer persons under risk conditions
from negative
outcomes. In addition, randomized trials of preventive
interventions can test
these theories by examining how behavior changes are
mediated.
THE INTERFACE OF PREVENTION, DEVELOPMENTAL
PSYCHOPATHOLOGY, AND NEUROSCIENCE
Although developmental psychopathology, prevention, and
neuroscience
have developed in isolation, integrated research on protective
factors and re-
silience has the potential to answer central questions regarding
plasticity and
the role of environmental and genetic process. While the
primary goal of
prevention science is to change behavior, behavior can be
broadly defined
as action, emotion, and cognition. Further, biological substrates
underlie all
of these processes and may serve as moderators, mediators, or
outcomes of
142 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
TABLE 1. Levels and measures of the biological substrate
I. Neural processes II. Autonomic nervous system
1. Structural aspects 1. Parasympathetic activity
A. Neuronal development and connections A. Cardiac vagal
tone
B. Localization of action
2. Functional aspects 2. Sympathetic nervous system
A. Neurochemical systems (dopamine, A. Resting heart rate
noradrenaline, serotonin, brain-
derived neurotrophic factor)
3. Neurocognitive function 3. Neuroendocrine function
A. Neuropsychological testing A. HPA axis–Glucocorticoids
4. Immunological function
A. T cells/antibody titers to vaccines
preventive interventions.13 A broad vision of the integration of
prevention and
neuroscience would examine how a variety of biological
processes play a role
in a deeper understanding of the processes and effects of
preventive interven-
tions. TABLE 1 provides a list of some of the biological
substrates that would
be of interest at the levels of both the brain and autonomic
nervous system.
A central task for the next decade is to understand in much
greater detail
the relations between the multiple levels of the biological
substrate and these
resilience processes involved in cognitive processes and
emotional regulation.
With transdisciplinary collaboration involving neuroscientists
and the use of
multilevel models of measurement driven by the theory/logic
model, prevention
research has the potential to make a major contribution to
understanding the
developing interplay of biology and behavior.
Many preventive interventions focus on supporting improved
emotion reg-
ulation and problem-solving skills in which executive functions
(EF) and the
actions of the prefrontal lobes play a central role. EF generally
refers to the psy-
chological processes that are involved in the conscious control
of thought. Ex-
amples of processes include inhibition, future time orientation,
consequential
thinking, and the planning, initiation, and regulation of goal-
directed behav-
ior.14 Substantial data indicate that EF skills as assessed by
neuropsychological
tasks are related to childhood maladaptation.13,15 However,
there has been lit-
tle evidence in childhood between the performance of EF tasks
(inhibitory
control, working memory, planning) and neuroanatomical
localization of ac-
tivity in areas of the frontal lobe.16,17 Due to the
methodological requirements
for valid Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)
assessments with
young children, few data are available before the age of 10
years, although
recent work using high-density Event Related Potential (ERP)
assessments
are particularly promising.18 A series of methodological and
conceptual chal-
lenges still have to be solved in order to fully assess the
specific brain local-
ization of neurocognitive and affective skills in children.19
Further, there is a
need to broaden methods to understand how childhood cognitive
and affective
GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 143
processing (especially under conditions of stress) are related to
other biologi-
cal processes, including action in the autonomic nervous
systems that include
correlates of the hypo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis,20,21
immunological func-
tion, the parasympathetic system (vagal tone), as well as
functional analysis of
brain action (neurotransmitter release).
Pioneering work with children has already begun to show the
potential yield
of this vision in which interventions use measurement models
and theories
based on our rapidly developing knowledge of neuroscience.
Research has
indicated that there is correspondence between improved
reading skills and
changes in brain activity in reading-deficient children.22 An
intervention to im-
prove the outcomes of children in the foster care system has
indicated changes
in both behavior and children’s salivary cortisol.23 Computer-
based training for
children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
has indicated
changes in EF and behavior.16 Meditation training in adults has
been shown
to alter both frontal brain activity (hemispheric laterality) and
immunological
response.24 Further, a number of studies has shown the
moderating role of
biological variables, including how EF moderates the effect of a
brief inter-
vention on high-risk teens25 and how the hypoactivity of
anterior cingulate
cortex predicts poor response to treatment for depression.26
Although neuroanatomical findings on cognitive and emotion
regulation
skills in childhood are sparse, there is a burgeoning literature
on adults re-
garding brain localization of EF that can judiciously guide
theory and action
with children. The field of social–cognitive neuroscience
(studies with lesion
patients, patients with psychopathologies, and normally
developing adults) has
clearly implicated the orbital/dorsolateral/limbic circuit in the
processing of
emotional stimuli and the cognitive control and regulation of
behavior.27,28
Findings indicate a clear role for the anterior cingulate in the
processing of
emotions, executive attention processes, and working
memory.29–31 The role of
the dorsolaternal prefrontal area has been shown in cognitive
control and inhibi-
tion of emotional arousal.32,33 Further, the orbital frontal area
has been related
to emotion processing and regulation.34 Although research has
attempted to
completely localize processes in single neuroanatomical areas,
it is clear that
there is strong and rapid connectivity between these areas
during decision
making and there are sometimes contradictory findings between
studies of
specific loci.35 As most of this work is less than a decade old,
conclusions re-
garding specific loci may be premature. Further, noradrenaline,
serotonin, and
dopamine are projected to all these areas and thus energize
action across sys-
tems. Double dissociation36 and lesion studies as well as
intervention trials24
will play substantial roles in further differentiation.
THE DEVELOPMENT OF EF
Although infancy and toddlerhood provide a basis for critical
aspects of later
coping,21,37 much of the child’s more complex cognitive
processes, coping,
144 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
and regulation skills arise with neurocognitive maturation in the
frontal lobes.
This maturation proceeds from the preschool years through late
adolescence.
Although numerous linguistic and cognitive processes are
developing, the de-
velopment of EF appears crucial to healthy development and
deficiencies in
EF have been related to numerous poor outcomes. These
outcomes involve
cognitive processes related to effective emotional regulation
and behavioral
performance, including aggression, delinquency, depression,
and disorders of
attention.13,38
INTERVENTION AND EF: ILLUSTRATIONS
FROM THE PROMOTING ALTERNATIVE THINKING
STRATEGIES (PATHS) CURRICULUM
During the past few decades our research group has been
involved in the
development, implementation, evaluation, and refinement of a
social and emo-
tional learning curriculum based on neuroscientific principles
that focus on
promoting emotional awareness and effective cognitive control.
The PATHS
curriculum is a universal school-based prevention curriculum
aimed at re-
ducing aggression and behavior problems by promoting the
development of
social–emotional competence in children during the preschool
and elementary
school years.39 PATHS is based on the affective-behavioral-
cognitive-dynamic
(ABCD) model of development.40 The ABCD model focuses on
how cogni-
tion, affect, language, and behavior become integrated in the
developing child.
A fundamental concept is that as youth mature, emotional
development pre-
cedes most forms of cognitive development. That is, young
children experience
emotions and react to them long before they can verbalize their
experiences.
Early in life, emotional development is an important precursor
to other ways of
thinking and must be integrated with cognitive and linguistic
abilities, which
are much slower to develop. Then, during the elementary years,
further devel-
opmental integration occurs among affect, behavior, and
cognition/language
through maturation of the prefrontal circuit. These processes of
brain matu-
ration are important in achieving socially competent action and
healthy peer
relations.
The PATHS curriculum places special attention on
neurocognitive models
of development.41 Of significant importance are the concepts of
vertical con-
trol and verbal processing of action (e.g., horizontal control).
Vertical control
refers to the process of higher-order cognitive processes
exerting control over
lower-level limbic impulses vı̀s-a-vı̀s the development of
frontal cognitive con-
trol.14 PATHS attempts to consciously teach children the
processes of vertical
control by providing opportunities to practice conscious
strategies for self-
control. This is achieved via instruction with curriculum lessons
and a variety
of cognitive/behavioral techniques that are developmentally
appropriate from
the ages of 4 to 11 years. One central example is the use of a
control signals
poster that teaches children the steps for problem solving in
social contexts.
GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 145
The curriculum also has an intentional and intensive focus on
helping children
to verbally identify and label feelings in order to manage them.
This is achieved
through curriculum lessons and the integration of “feeling
faces” that children
use throughout the day to identify their feelings and those of
others.
A series of outcome trials have indicated that effective
implementation of
the PATHS curriculum leads to decreases in externalizing and
internalizing
problems by both teacher and self-report and to increases in
social and emo-
tional competence.42–45 However, as with many preventive
interventions, there
has been little investigation of how such change is mediated.
Although some
aspect of this mediation may be due to changes external to the
child (improved
classroom environment, warmer teacher–student relations), we
believe that
the curriculum promotes more effective inhibitory control,
emotion regula-
tion, and planning skills. The curriculum logic model is based
on the idea that
the intervention will lead children (1) to become less impulsive
and more plan-
ful in their social interactions, and (2) to recruit language to
regulate behavior
and communicate effectively with others.
We recently tested this mediation model in a randomized
controlled study
of 318 second-and third-grade children.46 Schools were
randomized to receive
the PATHS intervention or to control status. Intervention
teachers received
both a 3-day initial training workshop as well as ongoing
weekly coaching in
curriculum implementation. The PATHS lessons were taught
approximately
three times per week, with each lesson lasting 20–30 min. In
addition, teachers
used techniques to generalize PATHS skills with the goal of
supporting stu-
dents to apply the PATHS skills in the “hot” naturally occurring
contexts of
their school day. These situations of high emotional arousal
usually occurred
during conflictual interactions with peers, with their teachers,
or when feel-
ing academic frustration. Students were assessed at pretest,
posttest (7 months
later), and follow-up (1 year after the curriculum ended).
Outcome findings examined teachers’ ratings of both
internalizing and exter-
nalizing behavioral problems using the Child Behavioral
Checklist (CBCL47).
EF were assessed by two well-known measures validated to
activate anterior
cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.48 Inhibitory
control was assessed
with the Stroop Test and verbal fluency was assessed using the
Verbal Fluency
Subtest of the McCarthy Scales of Children Abilities. To test a
mediational
model, it was first necessary to demonstrate that the
intervention affected
both behavior and EF. Results indicated that there were
significant differences
at posttest showing greater improvements in both inhibitory
control and ver-
bal fluency in the intervention children. At the 1-year follow-
up, intervention
children also were rated by teachers as lower in externalizing
and internaliz-
ing problems. Further, posttest changes in both inhibitory
control and verbal
fluency were significantly related to teacher ratings of behavior
problems at
follow-up.
The specific mediational hypothesis we tested was that EF
would me-
diate the relationship between prevention/control group
assignment and
146 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
teacher-reported externalizing and internalizing behavior
problems. The find-
ings indicated that improvements in inhibitory control at
posttest signifi-
cantly mediated the relation between experimental condition
and both teacher-
reported externalizing and internalizing behavior at 1-year
follow-up. In
addition, improvements in verbal fluency significantly mediated
the relation
between experimental condition and teacher-reported
internalizing behavior.
However, improvements in verbal fluency showed only a trend
toward explain-
ing change in teacher-reported externalizing behavior.
These findings provide empirical support for the conceptual
theory of action
that underlies the PATHS curriculum model. That is, child
neurocognitive
functioning plays a key role in children’s social and emotional
adaptation and
changes in EF directly relate to reductions in behavioral
problems. However,
a broader view and greater incorporation of the biological
substrate into our
understanding of the processes would begin to assess less
peripheral systems
of mediation than only the use of neuropsychological tests.
Although our own work with the PATHS intervention is very
preliminary, I
use it as a case example of how we might develop
transdisciplinary connec-
tions between prevention scientists and neuroscientists. A clear
logic model
of the intervention might hypothesize that such behavioral
changes would
lead to greater activation in the anterior cingulate and
dosolateral prefrontal
areas. Although such assessments could not be readily
accomplished using
fMRI at these younger ages, EEG–ERP assessments might be
used. One
might also hypothesize that such an intervention might impact
the child’s
stress reactivity (HPA axis) or their parasympathetic activity
under moder-
ately stressful testing conditions. Finally, if such an
intervention impacted
both frontal activity and stress-reactivity, one might
hypothesize that over
time it might impact overall bodily health as assessed by
immunological
function.
The point here is that effective preventive models that have
more fully ar-
ticulated logic models of action should begin to ask “deeper”
questions about
the neuroscientific underpinnings of either change processes or
obstacles to
intervention impact. That is, how might measures of the
biological substrate
serve as mediators, moderators, and outcomes?
Of course, there are some important caveats at this stage in the
scientific
enterprise. First, children may recruit different brain regions
than do adults to
accomplish the same task and extrapolated theories from
research on adults
should be used with caution.49 Further, even when fMRI can be
used with
older children, there are substantial conceptual and
methodological challenges
in interpreting such findings.19 Finally, although some aspects
of the effects of
preventive interventions may be better understood by taking a
neuroscientific
perspective, much of the action of some prevention models
occurs primarily
through changes in the environment (quality of the classroom or
community)
or in the context of social interactions that may not be well-
captured by current
models of neuroscience.
GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 147
These findings and others in neuroscience point to the
importance of con-
sidering social–emotional development as best understood
within broader the-
ories that take into account how children’s experiences and
relationships affect
their brain organization, structuralization, and development.38
As such, there
is a need for an extensive research agenda in which there is a
transdisciplinary
collaboration among prevention scientists, developmental
psychopathologists,
and neuroscientists. However, this will require not only clearer
logic models
of change and possibly more potent preventive interventions,
but substantial
advances in basic research in childhood neuroscience including
improvements
in both measurement and conceptualization. Through such work,
carefully de-
veloped studies should take us past the “black box” outcome to
more fully
understand the cognitive and neural mediators and moderators
of change.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental
Health (NIMH)
grant R01MH42131. Dr. Greenberg is one of the developers of
the PATHS cur-
riculum program and has a publishing agreement with
Channing–Bete Pub-
lishers.
REFERENCES
1. LEVENTHAL, T., R.C. FAUTH & J. BROOKS-GUNN. 2005.
Neighborhood poverty and
public policy: a 5-year follow-up of children’s educational
outcomes in the New
York City moving to opportunity demonstration. Dev.
Psychopath. 43: 933–952.
2. HAWKINS, J.D. & R.F. CATALANO, JR. 1992.
Communities that Care: Action for
Drug Abuse Prevention. Jossey-Bass. San Francisco.
3. BRONFENBRENNER, U. 1977. Toward an experimental
ecology of human develop-
ment. Am. Psych. 32: 513–531.
4. CICCHETTI, D. & D. TUCKER. 1994. Development and
self-regulatory structures of
the mind. Dev. Psychopath. 6: 533–549.
5. COIE, J.D., N.F. WATT, S.G. WEST, et al. 1993. The
science of prevention: a con-
ceptual framework and some directions for a national research
program. Am.
Psych. 48: 1013–1022.
6. CICCHETTI, D. & D.J. COHEN. 1995. Developmental
Psychopathology: Vol 2. Risk,
Disorder, and Adaptation. Wiley. New York.
7. MASTEN, A.S. 2001. Ordinary magic: resilience processes in
development. Am.
Psych. 56: 227–238.
8. CASPI, A., T.E. MOFF ITT, M. CANNON, et al. 2005.
Moderation of the effect of
adolescent onset cannibus use on adult psychosis by a
functional polymorphism
in the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase gene. Bio. Psych. 57:
1117–1127.
9. RUTTER, M. Implications of resilience concepts for
scientific understanding. Ann.
N. Y. Acad. Sci. This volume.
10. GREENBERG, M.T., M.L. SPELTZ & M. DEKLYEN. 1993.
The role of attachment
in the early development of disruptive behavior problems. Dev.
Psychopath. 5:
191–213.
148 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
11. LUTHAR, S.S. 1993. Annotation: methodological and
conceptual issues on research
on childhood resilience. Pediatric Annals 20: 501–506.
12. MASTEN, A.S. Competence and resilience in development.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
This volume.
13. RIGGS, N.R. & M.T. GREENBERG. 2004. The role of
neuro-cognitive models in
prevention research. In The Science, Treatment, and Prevention
of Antiso-
cial Behaviors (Vol. 2). D. Fishbein, Ed.: Civic Research
Institute. Kingston,
NJ.
14. LURIA, A.R. 1966. Higher Cortical Functions in Man. Basic
Books. New York.
15. RAINE, A. 2002. Annotation: the role of prefrontal deficits,
low autonomic arousal,
and early health factors in the development of antisocial and
aggressive behavior
in children. J. Child Psych. Psy. 43: 417–434.
16. KLINGBERG, T., E. FERNELL, P.J. OLESEN, et al. 2005.
Computerized training of
working memory in children with ADHD: a randomized,
controlled trial. J. Am.
Acad. Child Adol. Psy. 44: 177–186.
17. CASEY, B.J., N. TOTTENHAM, C. LISTEN, et al. 2005.
Imaging the developing brain:
What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in
Cog. Sci. 9:
104–110.
18. LEWIS, M. Behavioral differences in aggressive children
linked with neural mech-
anisms of emotional regulation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. This
volume.
19. PETERSEN, B.S. 2003. Conceptual, methodological, and
statistical challenges in
brain imaging studies of developmentally based
psychopathologies. Dev. Psy-
chopath. 15: 811–832.
20. BLAIR, C. 2002. School readiness: integrating cognition
and emotion in a neurobi-
ological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school
entry. Am. Psych.
57: 111–127.
21. GUNNAR, M.R. & D.M. VAZQUEZ. 2001. Low cortisol
and a flattening of ex-
pected daytime rhythm: potential indices of risk in human
development. Dev.
Psychopath. 13: 515–538.
22. SHAYWITZ, B.A., S.E. SHAYWITZ, B.A. BLACHMAN, et
al. 2004. Development of left
ocipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a
phonologically-
based intervention. Bio. Psy. 55: 926–933.
23. FISHER, P., M.R. GUNNAR, P. CHAMBERLAIN, et al.
2000. Preventive intervention
for maltreated preschool children: impact on children’s
behavior, neuroendocrine
activity, and foster parent functioning. J. Am. Acad. Child
Adol. Psy. 39: 1356–
1364.
24. DAVIDSON, R.J., J. KABAT-ZINN, J. SCHUMACHER, et
al. 2003. Alterations in brain
and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation.
Psycho. Med. 65:
564–570.
25. FISHBEIN, D., C. HYDE, D. ELDRETH, et al. 2006.
Neurocognitive skills moder-
ate urban male adolescent responses to preventive intervention
materials. Drug
Alcohol Depend. 82: 47–60.
26. PIZZGALLI, D.A., J.B. NITSCHKE, T.R. OAKES, et al.
2001. Anterior cingulate activity
as a predictor of degree of treatment response in major
depression: evidence from
brain electrical tomography analysis. Am. J. Psy. 158: 405–415.
27. BECHARA, A. 2005. Decision making, impulse control, and
loss of willpower to
resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 8:
1458–1463.
28. OCHSNER, K.N. 2004. Current directions in social
cognitive neuroscience. Cur. Op.
Neurobiol. 14: 254–258.
GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 149
29. BUSH, G., P. LUU & M.I. POSNER. 2000. Cognitive and
emotional influences in the
anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cog. Sci. 4: 215–222.
30. DREVETS, W.C. & M.E. RAICHLE. 1998. Reciprocal
suppression of regional cerebral
blood flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes:
implications for
interactions between emotion and cognition. Cog. Emo. 12:
353–385.
31. STERZER, P., C. STADLER, A. KREBBS, et al. 2005.
Abnormal neural responses to
emotional visual stimuli in adolescents with conduct disorder.
Biol. Psy. 57:
7–15.
32. OCHSNER, K.N., S.A. BUNGE, J.J. GROSS, et al. 2002.
Rethinking feelings: an
fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cog.
Neurosci. 14: 1215–
1229.
33. PHAN, K.L., D.A. FITZGERALD, P.J. NATHAN, et al.
2005. Neural substrates for vol-
untary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic
resonance imaging
study. Bio. Psy. 57: 210–219.
34. BEER, L.S., E.A. HEERY, D. KLETNER, et al. 2003. The
regulatory function of self-
conscious emotion: insights from patients with oribitofrontal
damage. J. Pers.
Soc. Psych. 85: 594–604.
35. FARRAH, M.J. & L.K. FELLOWS. 2005. Is anterior
cingulate cortex necessary for
cognitive control. Brain 205: 788–799.
36. CHAMBERLAIN, S.R., U. MULLER, A.D. BLACKWELL,
et al. 2006. Neurochemical
modulation of responses inhibition and probabilistic learning in
humans. Science
311: 861–863.
37. GREENBERG, M.T. 1999. Attachment and psychopathology
in childhood. In Hand-
book of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical
Applications. J. Cassidy &
P.R. Shaver, Eds.: 469–496. Guilford. New York.
38. FISHBEIN, D. 2001. The importance of neurobiological
research to the prevention
of psychopathology. Prev. Sci. 1: 89–106.
39. KUSCHÉ, C.A. & M.T. GREENBERG. 1994. The PATHS
(Promoting Alternative
Thinking Strategies) Curriculum. Channing-Bete. South
Deerfield, MA.
40. GREENBERG, M.T. & C.A. KUSCHÉ. 1993. Promoting
Social and Emotional Devel-
opment in Deaf Children: The PATHS Project. University of
Washington Press.
Seattle.
41. KUSCHÉ, C.A. & M.T. GREENBERG. 2006. Brain
development and social emotional
learning: an introduction for educators. In The Educator’s Guide
to Emotional
Intelligence and Academic Achievement. M. Elias & H. Arnold,
Eds.: Corwin
Press. Thousand Oaks, CA.
42. CONDUCT PROBLEMS PREVENTION RESEARCH
GROUP. 1999. Initial impact of the
Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems: II. Classroom
effects. J Cons.
Clin. Psych. 67: 648–657.
43. GREENBERG, M.T. & C.A. KUSCHÉ. 2002. Promoting
Alternative Thinking Strate-
gies. Blueprint for Violence Prevention (Book 10), Institute of
Behavioral Sci-
ences, University of Colorado. CO
44. KAM, C., M.T. GREENBERG & C.A. KUSCHÉ. 2004.
Sustained effects of the PATHS
curriculum on the social and psychological adjustment of
children in special
education. J. Emot. Behav. Disorders 12: 66–78.
45. RIGGS, N.R., M.T. GREENBERG, C.A. KUSCHÉ, et al.
2006. The mediational role
of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-
emotional prevention
program in elementary school students: effects of the PATHS
Curriculum. Prev.
Sci. 7: 91–102.
150 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
46. RIGGS, N.R., C. BLAIR & M.T. GREENBERG. 2003.
Concurrent and 2-year longitu-
dinal relations between executive function and the behavior of
1st and 2nd grade
children. Child Neuropsych. 9: 267–276.
47. ACHENBACH, T.M. 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior
Checklist and 1991 Profile.
Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont. Burlington,
VT.
48. RAVNKILDE, B., P. VIDEBECH, R. ROSENBERG, et al.
2002. Putative tests of frontal
lobe function: a PET-study of brain activation during Stroop’s
Test and verbal
fluency. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24: 534–547.
49. BUNGE, S.A., N.M. DUDUKOVIC, M.E. THOMANSON, et
al. 2002. Immature frontal
lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence
from fMRI. Neuron
33: 1–20.
Enhancing Resilience in Children: A Proactive Approach
Mary Karapetian Alvord and Judy Johnson Grados
Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC
Many clinical practitioners today are interested in helping
children be more resilient. The authors briefly
review the literature and identify protective factors that are
related to or foster resilience in children. After
discussing individual and family intervention strategies
currently in use, the authors present a practical,
proactive, resilience-based model that clinicians may use in a
group intervention setting. The model
entails interactive identification of protective factors with
children, free play and behavioral rehearsal,
training in relaxation and self-control techniques, practice in
generalizing skills acquired, and active
parent involvement. Implications of this group intervention
model are discussed.
Keywords: resilience, children, social skills, groups, cognitive–
behavioral therapy
Vala is a 16-year-old Russian girl who first came to our practice
at age 6. Neglected as a child, separated from her younger
sister,
and taken to an orphanage following her mother’s suicide, she
was
adopted by an American family and came to this country. The
tribulations she experienced were extreme. However, because of
her intelligence, easygoing temperament, and other personal
strengths, as well as loving support of family and friends and a
positive school environment, she is functioning well today and
appears to be successful and happy.
Many children encounter fewer and less severe traumatic expe-
riences than did Vala. Yet they do experience the inevitable
stresses and adversities in life that may challenge their healthy
development and successful functioning. In the past three
decades,
a group of children have been identified in the research who
appear
to have fared well despite exposure to severe adversity. These
children, who have been referred to as stress-resistant, invulner-
able, and, more recently, resilient, were found to possess certain
strengths and to have benefited from protective influences that
helped them to overcome adverse conditions and to thrive. As
practitioners, we must understand what environmental factors
place children at risk and what protective factors may be
fostered
to enhance and strengthen resilience in children.
In this article we define resilience and provide an overview of
the literature and recent advances that are guiding work in the
field
today. We then define and discuss protective factors linked with
the resilience process. Finally, we offer clinical implications for
individual, family, and group therapy with children.
Definitions of Resilience
The term resilience has been defined in many ways. Masten,
Best, and Garmezy’s (1990) definition of resilience as “the
process
of, capacity for, or outcome of successful adaptation despite
chal-
lenging or threatening circumstances” (p. 426) is one of the
more
familiar and widely accepted in the field. Many definitions of
resilience require specification of an identified risk or challenge
to
which an individual is subjected, followed by some defined
mea-
sure of positive outcome. However, controversy remains
regarding
what constitutes resilient behavior and how to best measure suc-
cessful adaptation to adversity. Some have suggested that a
resil-
ient person must show positive outcomes across several aspects
of
his life over periods of time (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997).
Further,
resilience is not a one-dimensional, dichotomous attribute that
persons either have or do not have (Reivich & Shatté, 2003).
Rather, resilience implies the possession of multiple skills, in
varying degrees, that help individuals to cope.
For the purpose of this article, we define resilience broadly as
those skills, attributes, and abilities that enable individuals to
adapt
to hardships, difficulties, and challenges. Although some
attributes
are biologically determined, we believe resilience skills can be
strengthened as well as learned.
Early Studies in Resilience
Early clinical case descriptions spawned an interest in determin-
ing why some children manage to cope with adversity whereas
others succumb. One such case of a 14-year-old Swiss girl was
described by Bleuler (1984; as cited in Anthony, 1987a).
“Vreni,”
in the absence of her mother (who was hospitalized with mental
illness), raised her siblings, cared for her alcoholic and
physically
compromised father, and later reported having a happy marriage
and contented life. Anthony’s description of “invulnerable”
chil-
dren (Anthony, 1987b) and Murphy and Moriarty’s “good
copers”
MARY KARAPETIAN ALVORD received her PhD from the
University of
Maryland in 1977. She is currently the director of the Group
Therapy
Center at Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC, an independent
therapy
practice in the Washington, DC, metropolitan area. Her interests
are
focused on resilience and social skill development of children
and adoles-
cents.
JUDY JOHNSON GRADOS received her PsyD from Indiana
State University in
1995. She currently practices with Alvord, Baker & Associates,
LLC. Her
research interests include social skills training for children and
resilience in
children and adolescents.
WE GRATEFULLY ACKNOWLEDGE Dr. W. Gregory Alvord,
Dr. Patricia K. S.
Baker, and Ms. Anne McGrath for their support and editorial
comments.
We also thank Mrs. Maria Manolatos for assistance in the
preparation of
the manuscript.
CORRESPONDENCE CONCERNING THIS ARTICLE should
be addressed to Mary
Karapetian Alvord, Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC, 11161
New Hamp-
shire Avenue, Suite 307, Silver Spring, MD 20904. E-mail:
[email protected]
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice Copyright 2005
by the American Psychological Association
2005, Vol. 36, No. 3, 238 –245 0735-7028/05/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/0735-7028.36.3.238
238
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
(Murphy & Moriarty, 1976) sparked early interest as well.
Rutter’s
Isle of Wight studies (Rutter, Tizard, Yule, Graham, &
Whitmore,
1976), large-scale longitudinal undertakings such as Project
Com-
petence (Garmezy, Masten, & Tellegen, 1984; Masten, 2000),
and
Werner’s now four-decade-long study of high-risk infants born
into poverty on the Hawaiian island of Kauai (Werner, 1993;
Werner & Smith, 2001) have helped to lay the groundwork for
what we know today.
Recent Advances
The emerging research in the area of resilience encompasses
many areas. These include investigations of children from
various
cultures (Grotberg, 1995; Hart, Hofmann, Edelstein, & Keller,
1997), studies of the influences of biological mechanisms
(Curtis
& Cicchetti, 2003; Rutter, 2002), new findings in recovering to
normal functioning in varying stages of development, research
on
the impact of internal challenges, and theories on the processes
that
promote resilience under nonrisk conditions. We consider the
latter
three areas below.
Evidence that children can recover and develop normally comes
from a recent study of Romanian children who experienced
severe
deprivation in orphanages and were later adopted into nurturing
homes in the United Kingdom. At the time of adoption, most of
these children showed substantial gross physical and cognitive
lags
in development. Later, when assessed at age 4, many of these
adoptees showed significant “catch-up,” both physically and
cog-
nitively (Rutter & The English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA)
Study Team, 1998; Rutter, Pickles, Murray, & Eaves, 2001).
Follow-up in Werner and Smith’s (2001) Kauai study revealed
that
even those individuals who were troubled as adolescents were
able
to change the course of their lives in dramatic ways by making
wise choices and taking advantage of opportunities, for
example,
by continuing their education, learning new skills, joining the
military, relocating to break ties with deviant peers, and
choosing
healthy life partners.
Major longitudinal studies have followed the outcome of indi-
viduals with learning disabilities and attention-
deficit/hyperactiv-
ity disorder (ADHD) to determine those factors that contribute
to
their resilience (Gerber, Ginsburg, & Reiff, 1990, and Spekman,
Goldberg, & Herman, 1992, as cited in Katz, 1997; Werner &
Smith, 2001). Studies reveal that resilient learning-disabled
youth
search for personal control over their lives, possess a strong
desire
to succeed, set goals, demonstrate high levels of persistence,
and
are willing to seek out and accept support. Resilient learning-
disabled young adults report better ability to identify their suc-
cesses and unique strengths, are more likely to report turning
points in their lives as motivations to overcome their
challenges,
and show a stronger self-determination (Miller, 2002).
Hechtman
(1991; as cited in Katz, 1997), in a long-term prospective
follow-up of young adults diagnosed with ADHD as children,
found that the presence of an influential person in their lives
who
believed in them (e.g., parent, teacher, coach) was most signifi-
cant. Effort is being put forth to better understand the risk and
resilience processes in this population of youth (Murray, 2003).
Masten (2001) challenged the notion that resilient children
possess some rare and special qualities. She suggested that
resil-
ience stems from a healthy operation of basic human
adaptational
systems. If systems are intact, children should develop
appropri-
ately even if challenged. However, if children’s basic
adaptational
systems are impaired, prior to or following challenge, the risk
for
problems in development is increased.
Protective Factors
Protective factors are “influences that modify, ameliorate, or
alter a person’s response to some environmental hazard that pre-
disposes to a maladaptive outcome” (Rutter, 1985, p. 600). Pro-
tective factors arise from within the child, from the family or
extended family, and from the community (Werner, 1995). A
child’s intelligence, success at making friends, and ability to
regulate his behavior are examples of internal strengths that
pro-
mote resilience. Examples of external influences that enhance
resilience are competent parents, friendships, support networks,
and effective schools.
Protective factors that help children successfully adapt and cope
with life’s challenges must be viewed in the context of their
individual cultures and developmental stages. The International
Resilience Project (Grotberg, 1995) showed, for example, that
faith operates as a stronger protective factor in some cultures
than
in others. In addition, children’s developmental and cognitive
levels affect their ability to use various protective factors, as do
internal and biological vulnerabilities such as ADHD and
learning
disabilities.
While researchers forge ahead to examine the complex interplay
between risk factors, protective factors, and prevention and
inter-
vention strategies, practitioners need to know what factors may
be
strengthened in children to further promote positive appropriate
responses. Below we discuss six protective factors that appear
to
buffer against risk factors. These factors have been categorized
in
accordance with the accumulating resilience literature and our
experience in clinical practice. These six categories are not
mutu-
ally exclusive. Many of the components described in one factor
are
related to components in other factors. For example, a child
who
can self-regulate is more apt to make friends and connect with
others. A child who experiences academic success is likely to
have
higher self-esteem. The presence of several factors seems to en-
hance performance in multiple arenas.
Proactive Orientation
Proactive orientation, that is, taking initiative in one’s own life
and believing in one’s own effectiveness, has been identified as
a
primary characteristic defining resilience in the literature. Such
terms as self-efficacy and self-esteem (Rutter, 1985), positive
fu-
ture expectations (Wyman, Cowen, Work, & Kerley, 1993),
good
coping (Murphy & Moriarty, 1976), primary and secondary con-
trol coping (Thurber & Weisz, 1997), personal control (Walsh,
1998), problem solving (Werner, 1995), initiative (Wolin &
Wolin,
1993), optimistic thinking (Seligman, 1995), and internal
motiva-
tion (Masten, 2001) have been identified as protective factors of
resilience in studies across heterogeneous populations and envi-
ronments. These terms mean that resilient individuals have a
realistic, positive sense of self. They regard themselves as
survi-
vors (Wolin & Wolin, 1993). They feel that they can have an
impact on their environment or situation, rather than just be
pas-
sive observers. They are hopeful about the future. They are con-
fident in their ability to surmount obstacles (Werner, 1993),
make
use of resources and opportunities around them, and view hard-
ships as “learning experiences” (Werner & Smith, 2001).
Resilient
239SPECIAL SECTION: ENHANCING RESILIENCE IN
CHILDREN
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
individuals take positive action in their lives, such as seeking
mentors, pursuing educational opportunities, participating in ex-
tracurricular activities, and choosing supportive mates (Werner,
1993; Werner & Smith, 2001). Those who possess a high degree
of
“perceived self-efficacy” are more likely to interpret successes
as
an indication of their capabilities (Bandura, Pastorelli, Bar-
baranelli, & Caprara, 1999). Seligman (2002) pointed out that
when people think adverse events are permanent and pervasive
for
long periods of time, they assume feelings of helplessness and
hopelessness. In contrast, he noted, when they think that
negative
things are temporary, this attitude encourages resilience.
Teaching children to help others is an effective way to promote
responsibility, empathy, and self-esteem (Brooks, 1994; Werner,
1993). Giving of oneself in an effort to ease the plight of others,
such as contributing time and effort at a soup kitchen, nursing
home, hospital, and so forth, fosters resilience. Similarly,
“required
helpfulness” (Rachman, 1979) refers to the process that occurs
when an individual is striving to overcome adversity and during
the course of this pursuit is required to perform actions to help
others in their personal times of need.
Self-Regulation
One of the most fundamental protective factors is success in
developing self-regulation or self-control. Masten and
Coatsworth
(1998) define self-regulation as gaining control over attention,
emotions, and behavior. If a child is able to modulate her
emotions
and behavior and can self-soothe or calm herself, she will most
likely elicit positive attention from others (including parents)
and
will have healthy social relationships. She will more likely be
independent and will be more able to put things in perspective.
Easygoing temperament and good self-regulation have been
iden-
tified as protective factors in resilience (Buckner, Mezzacappa,
&
Beardslee, 2003; Eisenberg et al., 1997, 2003; Werner, 1993).
Additionally, impulse control and delay of gratification are part
of
self-control. The ability to self-regulate also seems to be at the
core
of good interpersonal relationships and peer relationships
(Rubin,
Coplan, Fox, & Calkins, 1995), rule compliance (Feldman &
Klein, 2003), reduced risk of depression and anxiety, and a host
of
other areas fundamental to successful adaptation and
functioning.
Common sense would dictate that positive emotionality should
result in positive outcomes. Although this is true, recent
research
indicates that perhaps it is not positive or negative emotion per
se
that is the critical variable in adaptation, but the ability to
regulate
the emotion. In a longitudinal study of 5-year-olds, Rydell,
Berlin,
and Bohlin (2003) found that low regulation of positive
emotions
and exuberance was correlated with externalizing problem
behav-
iors and low levels of prosocial behavior, whereas high
regulation
of positive emotions and exuberance was associated with high
levels of prosocial behavior.
Proactive Parenting
Children with at least one warm, loving parent or surrogate
caregiver (grandparent, foster parent) who provides firm limits
and
boundaries (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998) are more likely to be
resilient. They tend to be more compliant with their parents
(Feld-
man & Klein, 2003) and have better peer relationships
(Contreras,
Kerns, Weimer, Gentzler, & Tomich, 2000). A significant longi-
tudinal study that began in 1959 has identified the authoritative
parenting style as associated with “optimal competence” in chil-
dren and adolescents (Baumrind, 1989). Authoritative parents
are
characterized as “responsive” and “demanding” (Baumrind,
1991).
Responsive parents are warm, loving, and supportive and
provide
a cognitively stimulating environment. They are also demanding
in
that they apply rational, firm, and consistent, but not
overbearing,
control on their children and place high behavioral expectations
on
them (Baumrind, 1991). Eisenberg et al. (2003) found that
mater-
nal expression of positive emotion is related to children’s social
competence and adjustment. Correspondingly, Rubin, Burgess,
Dwyer, and Hastings (2003) found that dysregulated toddlers
who
experienced high levels of maternal negativity had a greater
like-
lihood of externalizing problem behaviors 2 years later than
tod-
dlers whose mothers showed low to average levels of negativity.
Connections and Attachments
The desire to belong and to form attachments with family and
friends is considered a fundamental human need (Baumeister &
Leary, 1995). Multiple positive health and adjustment effects
have
been associated with a sense of belonging and attachments. It is
also through supportive relationships that self-esteem and self-
efficacy are promoted (Werner, 1993). Having social
competence
and having positive connections with peers, family, and
prosocial
adults are significantly related to children’s ability to adapt to
life
stressors (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). Resilient children also
elicit positive attention from others (Werner, 1993).
For children, the development of friendships and the ability to
get along with peers individually and in groups is paramount.
Friendships provide support systems that can foster emotional,
social, and educational adjustment (Rubin, 2002). Being part of
at
least one best friendship may also improve children’s
adjustment
(Hartup & Stevens, 1997). Positive peer relationships have been
shown to protect children during times of family crisis.
Acceptance
by a group lowers the risk of externalizing behavior problems
(Criss, Pettit, Bates, Dodge, & Lapp, 2002). A child who has
friends and is well liked is less likely to be bullied or otherwise
victimized (Pellegrini, Bartini, & Brooks, 1999; Rubin,
Bukowski,
& Parker, 1998).
Although possessing a strong social support network renders
children less vulnerable to stress, depression, and externalizing
problems, the availability of social supports cannot be taken for
granted. “Social support is not a self-forming entity waiting
around
to buffer harried people against stressors” (Bandura et al., 1999,
p.
259). An active process is involved, and children, like adults,
need
to create and maintain supportive relationships. Connections
affect
how one treats and responds to others and how they, in turn,
respond, thus forming a reciprocal relationship. It is easier for
people to respond with more positive feedback and affection to
an
easygoing child who is sensitive to others and shows good self-
control than to a child who is impulsive, who overreacts to
events,
and whose emotions are not well regulated. Children who share,
who are compliant with social rules, and who are positive with
peers are more likely to sustain their relationships.
School Achievement and Involvement, IQ, and Special
Talents
Schools give young people a chance to excel academically and
socially. Educational aspirations (Tiet et al., 1998) and active
240 ALVORD AND GRADOS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
engagement in academics (Morrison, Robertson, Laurie, &
Kelly,
2002) have been associated with resilience in challenged youth.
Although the reasons for this are not yet clear, a number of
factors
are likely to contribute. Encouragement from teachers fosters
resilience through connections, as noted above. Further,
involve-
ment in extracurricular activities such as art, music, drama,
special
interest clubs, and sporting activities gives youth opportunities
to
participate in prosocial groups and achieve recognition for their
efforts. A positive orientation toward school and school
activities
has been shown to protect against antisocial behavior (Jessor,
Van
Den Bos, Vanderryn, Costa, & Turbin, 1995).
Cognitive ability has been found to be associated with resilience
in children (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996). It also appears to
exert
a strong influence on other factors that contribute to resilience.
For
example, strong cognitive skills may allow youth to excel in
school, as well as to make the most of educational opportunities
and cultural experiences. Additionally, a commitment to school
helps to counter the risk of violent behaviors (Department of
Health and Human Services, 2001).
Fostering competence in children is central to resilient outcome.
Brooks and Goldstein (2001) believe each child possesses at
least
one small “island of competence,” or area that has the potential
to
be a source of pride or achievement. Fostering resilience in
youth
requires that parents highlight children’s areas of competence to
help them experience a sense of accomplishment. They suggest
involving children in daily activities that allow them to feel
they
are contributing to the world. Examples include assisting other
children, acting as a school patrol, or helping an older neighbor.
Community
Community factors, including the availability of supportive
relationships outside of the family (Masten, 2001; Werner,
1995),
are also well documented as having a protective influence on
children. Resilient youth form relationships with positive role
models and elders outside of their own family (Wolin & Wolin,
1993). Youths often join clubs, teams, and other groups and
frequently find mentors such as coaches, teachers, scout leaders,
and other prosocial adults in their communities. Effective
schools
identify the needs of their students and address those needs with
services, as well as by consideration of class size and curricula.
Important elements of an effective community are environments
and social structures that promote resilience. Early prevention
and
intervention programs, safety in neighborhoods, support
services,
recreational facilities and programs, accessibility to adequate
health services, and economic opportunities for families have
all
been identified as protective factors (Thomlison, 1997).
Religious
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx
YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx

More Related Content

Similar to YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx

The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity Giuseppe Fattori
 
Social Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docx
Social Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docxSocial Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docx
Social Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docxpbilly1
 
Sharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported Employment
Sharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported EmploymentSharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported Employment
Sharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported EmploymentCentre for Mental Health
 
The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...
The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...
The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...Bagus Utomo
 
71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx
71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx
71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docxblondellchancy
 
Bachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed unitsBachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed unitsShira Rotem
 
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA ReviewPCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA ReviewScott Miller
 
Assignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docx
Assignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docxAssignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docx
Assignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docxjesuslightbody
 
Evidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docx
Evidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docxEvidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docx
Evidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docxturveycharlyn
 
HEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I Part IV JOURNAL Instruct.docx
HEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I    Part IV    JOURNAL    Instruct.docxHEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I    Part IV    JOURNAL    Instruct.docx
HEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I Part IV JOURNAL Instruct.docxpooleavelina
 
Holistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School Teachers
Holistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School TeachersHolistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School Teachers
Holistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School TeachersIanDielParagoso
 
Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?
Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?
Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?Health Evidence™
 
Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013
Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013
Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013Richard Bogue
 
Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3
Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3
Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3honey690131
 
Running head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT .docx
Running head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT                       .docxRunning head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT                       .docx
Running head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT .docxjeanettehully
 
BUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docx
BUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docxBUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docx
BUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docxRAHUL126667
 
A Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees Health
A Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees  HealthA Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees  Health
A Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees HealthValerie Felton
 
Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016
Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016
Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016Daniel Bowen
 
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docxArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docxfredharris32
 

Similar to YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx (20)

211712443.pdf
211712443.pdf211712443.pdf
211712443.pdf
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
 
Social Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docx
Social Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docxSocial Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docx
Social Cognitive TheoryThis theory was introduced by Albert Band.docx
 
Sharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported Employment
Sharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported EmploymentSharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported Employment
Sharing what works - Evidence-Based Supported Employment
 
The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...
The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...
The Urgent need to establish an Indonesian NIMH to shape the future of resear...
 
71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx
71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx
71719, 1124 PMEvidence–Based Health Evaluation and Applicat.docx
 
Bachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed unitsBachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed units
 
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA ReviewPCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
 
Assignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docx
Assignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docxAssignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docx
Assignment WK 9Assessing a Healthcare ProgramPolicy Evaluation.docx
 
Evidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docx
Evidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docxEvidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docx
Evidence Translation and ChangeWeek 7What are the common.docx
 
HEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I Part IV JOURNAL Instruct.docx
HEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I    Part IV    JOURNAL    Instruct.docxHEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I    Part IV    JOURNAL    Instruct.docx
HEALTH CARE MANAGEMENTUNIT I Part IV JOURNAL Instruct.docx
 
Holistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School Teachers
Holistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School TeachersHolistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School Teachers
Holistic Health and Wellness of Elementary School Teachers
 
Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?
Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?
Intersectoral Action & the Social Determinants of Health: What's the Evidence?
 
Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013
Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013
Slockett and bogue natl physician well being conference april 5 2013
 
Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3
Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3
Article critique assignment course drugs and health (hlth 3
 
Running head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT .docx
Running head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT                       .docxRunning head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT                       .docx
Running head IMPROVEMENT PLAN TOOL KIT .docx
 
BUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docx
BUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docxBUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docx
BUSI 230Project 1 InstructionsBased on Larson & Farber sectio.docx
 
A Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees Health
A Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees  HealthA Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees  Health
A Corporate Wellness Program And Nursing Home Employees Health
 
Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016
Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016
Bowen PhD Final Seminar 2016
 
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docxArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
 

More from odiliagilby

Per the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docx
Per the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docxPer the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docx
Per the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docxodiliagilby
 
Pennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docx
Pennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docxPennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docx
Pennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docxodiliagilby
 
Penetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docx
Penetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docxPenetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docx
Penetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docxodiliagilby
 
Perform an analysis of the social demographic, technological, econ.docx
Perform an analysis of the social  demographic, technological, econ.docxPerform an analysis of the social  demographic, technological, econ.docx
Perform an analysis of the social demographic, technological, econ.docxodiliagilby
 
Perform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docx
Perform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docxPerform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docx
Perform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docxodiliagilby
 
People in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docx
People in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docxPeople in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docx
People in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docxodiliagilby
 
Pease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docx
Pease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docxPease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docx
Pease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docxodiliagilby
 
Peer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docx
Peer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docxPeer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docx
Peer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docxodiliagilby
 
Perception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docx
Perception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docxPerception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docx
Perception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docxodiliagilby
 
Performance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docx
Performance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docxPerformance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docx
Performance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docxodiliagilby
 
People. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docx
People. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docxPeople. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docx
People. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docxodiliagilby
 
Perceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docx
Perceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docxPerceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docx
Perceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docxodiliagilby
 
People are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docx
People are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docxPeople are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docx
People are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docxodiliagilby
 
Perception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docx
Perception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docxPerception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docx
Perception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docxodiliagilby
 
Pennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docx
Pennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docxPennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docx
Pennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docxodiliagilby
 
PBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docx
PBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docxPBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docx
PBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docxodiliagilby
 
Part1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docx
Part1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docxPart1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docx
Part1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docxodiliagilby
 
Paul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docx
Paul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docxPaul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docx
Paul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docxodiliagilby
 
Past and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docx
Past and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docxPast and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docx
Past and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docxodiliagilby
 
Partisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docx
Partisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docxPartisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docx
Partisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docxodiliagilby
 

More from odiliagilby (20)

Per the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docx
Per the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docxPer the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docx
Per the text, computers are playing an increasingly important role i.docx
 
Pennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docx
Pennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docxPennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docx
Pennsylvania was the leader in sentencing and correctional reform .docx
 
Penetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docx
Penetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docxPenetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docx
Penetration testing is a simulated cyberattack against a computer or.docx
 
Perform an analysis of the social demographic, technological, econ.docx
Perform an analysis of the social  demographic, technological, econ.docxPerform an analysis of the social  demographic, technological, econ.docx
Perform an analysis of the social demographic, technological, econ.docx
 
Perform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docx
Perform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docxPerform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docx
Perform research and discuss whether text messaging is cheaper or mo.docx
 
People in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docx
People in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docxPeople in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docx
People in developed nations are fond of warning people in developing.docx
 
Pease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docx
Pease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docxPease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docx
Pease read and incorporate the following articles from the EBSCO h.docx
 
Peer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docx
Peer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docxPeer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docx
Peer Review Journal Paper Overview of assignment due 17 April 2014 I.docx
 
Perception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docx
Perception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docxPerception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docx
Perception is all EXCEPT [removed] [removed]Structuring and orga.docx
 
Performance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docx
Performance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docxPerformance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docx
Performance Based Factors and Measures for Quality AssessmentWri.docx
 
People. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docx
People. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docxPeople. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docx
People. I need some help with this assignment that needs to be done .docx
 
Perceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docx
Perceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docxPerceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docx
Perceptions and Causes of Psychopathology PaperPrepare a 1,0.docx
 
People are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docx
People are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docxPeople are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docx
People are attracted to occupations that complement their personalit.docx
 
Perception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docx
Perception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docxPerception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docx
Perception of Pleasure and Pain Presentation3 slides- An explanati.docx
 
Pennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docx
Pennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docxPennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docx
Pennsylvania v. MarkMark Davis has been charged with Driving W.docx
 
PBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docx
PBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docxPBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docx
PBAD201-1501A-02 Public AdministrationTask NamePhase 3 Individu.docx
 
Part1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docx
Part1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docxPart1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docx
Part1 Q1. Classify each of the following as-      (i)qual.docx
 
Paul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docx
Paul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docxPaul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docx
Paul’s Letter to the EphesiansThe First Letter of PeterThe Fir.docx
 
Past and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docx
Past and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docxPast and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docx
Past and FuturePlease respond to the followingImagine back .docx
 
Partisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docx
Partisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docxPartisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docx
Partisan considerations have increasingly influenced the selection.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,Virag Sontakke
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
 

YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. .docx

  • 1. YOUR WORK WILL BE AUTOMATICALLY CHECKED BY BLACKBOARD SAFEASSIGN. ASSIGNMENTS WITH SIMILARITY RATINGS GREATER THAN 35% WILL NOT BE GRADED. In your own words and GIVE EXAMPLES. Use business English. Check your spelling and grammar. Each answer should be about one-half page Chapter One 1. Discuss (with examples) what is an I/O and what is a resource-based business model. Type answer here: Chapter Two: 2. What ae the differences between the general environment and the industry environment? Why are these differences important? Type answer here: Chapter Three 3. What is Value Chain Analysis? How does it help companies earn above-average returns? Type answer here: Chapter Four 4. Explain Cost Leadership and Differentiation strategies. Be sure to name a real-life company that practices each strategy Type answer here. Chapter Five 5. What factors contribute to the likelihood of a response to a competitive action?
  • 2. Type answer here: Chapter Six: 6. What is corporate-level strategy? what is business-level strategy? Type answer here. Chapter Eight: 8. List and explain with examples the five entry modes firms may consider as paths to enter the international markets? Type answer here Chapter Nine: 9. What is a strategic alliance? What are the three major types of strategic alliances? Type answer here Chapters 10 10-. Discuss what is meant by “agency relationship”? What are some actions that firms can take to align the interests of managerial agents with those of the firm’s shareholders? Type answer here. Chapters 11 11-. Compare and contrast strategic and financial controls? Type answer here.
  • 3. Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalC ode=cphm20 Download by: [University of Newcastle, Australia] Date: 28 November 2017, At: 15:34 Psychology, Health & Medicine ISSN: 1354-8506 (Print) 1465-3966 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cphm20 Feasibility and effectiveness of psychosocial resilience training: A pilot study of the READY program Nicola W. Burton , Ken I. Pakenham & Wendy J. Brown To cite this article: Nicola W. Burton , Ken I. Pakenham & Wendy J. Brown (2010) Feasibility and effectiveness of psychosocial resilience training: A pilot study of the READY program, Psychology, Health & Medicine, 15:3, 266-277, DOI: 10.1080/13548501003758710 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/13548501003758710 Published online: 17 May 2010. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 1217
  • 4. View related articles Citing articles: 49 View citing articles http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalC ode=cphm20 http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cphm20 http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.108 0/13548501003758710 https://doi.org/10.1080/13548501003758710 http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCo de=cphm20&show=instructions http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCo de=cphm20&show=instructions http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/135485010037587 10 http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/135485010037587 10 http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/135485010037 58710#tabModule http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/135485010037 58710#tabModule Feasibility and effectiveness of psychosocial resilience training: A pilot study of the READY program Nicola W. Burton a *, Ken I. Pakenham b and Wendy J. Brown
  • 5. a a School of Human Movement Studies, The University of Queensland, Blair Drive, St Lucia, Brisbane 4072, Australia; b School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Blair Drive, St Lucia, Brisbane 4072, Australia (Received 11 September 2009; final version received 4 February 2010) Despite many studies on the characteristics associated with resilience, there is little research on interventions to promote resilience in adults. The aims of this study were to gather preliminary information regarding the feasibility of implementing a group psychosocial resilience training program (REsilience and Activity for every DaY, READY) in a workplace setting, and to assess if program would potentially promote well-being. The program targets five protective factors identified from empirical evidence: Positive emotions, cognitive flexibility, social support, life meaning, and active coping. Resilience enhancement strategies reflect core acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) processes and cognitive behavior therapy strategies. Sessions involve psychoeducation, discussions,
  • 6. experiential exercises, and home assignments. Sixteen participants completed 11 6 two h group sessions over 13 weeks. Baseline and post- intervention assessment included self-administered questionnaires, pedometer step counts, and physical and hematological measures. Data were analyzed using standardized mean differences and paired t-tests. There was a significant improvement between baseline and post intervention scores on measures of mastery (p ¼ 0.001), positive emotions (p ¼ 0.002), personal growth (p ¼ 0.004), mindfulness (p ¼ 0.004), acceptance (p ¼ 0.012), stress (p ¼ 0.013), self acceptance (p ¼ 0.016), valued living (p ¼ 0.022), autonomy (p ¼ 0.032) and total cholesterol (p ¼ 0.025). Participants rated the program and materials very highly. These results indicate that the READY program is feasible to implement as a group training program in a workplace setting to promote psychosocial well-being. Keywords: resilience; well-being; mental health; health promotion; ACT; depression; stress management Background Resilience is the capacity of people to effectively cope with, adjust, or recover from stress or adversity. When faced with adversity, people with low resilience are at risk of depression, stress, anxiety and interpersonal difficulties, and may adopt health
  • 7. compromising behaviors and experience somatic complaints and poor physical health. Prolonged stress and poor psychosocial functioning may negatively impact on physical health for example, via biological mechanisms such as hypertension and *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] Psychology, Health & Medicine Vol. 15, No. 3, May 2010, 266–277 ISSN 1354-8506 print/ISSN 1465-3966 online � 2010 Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/13548501003758710 http://www.informaworld.com D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs
  • 9. r 20 17 blood pressure reactivity to stress, atherogenic lipid profiles, pro-inflammatory cytokines (e.g. C reactive protein), and the development of metabolic syndrome (Rozanski, Blumenthal, & Kaplan, 1999; Strike & Steptoe, 2004). Most studies of resilience have focused on young people or individuals experiencing specific adverse circumstances, and have explored the personal characteristics associated with effective coping (Richardson & Waite, 2002). Only a few studies have focused on adults and investigated the effectiveness of resilience training. A worksite trial of the ‘‘Personal Resilience and Resilient Relationships’’ program, with 5 6 7 h modules implemented weekly over five weeks, demonstrated significantly higher levels of self esteem, locus of control, purpose in life, and interpersonal relations among program participants compared with a control group (Waite & Richardson, 2004). A modified version of this program, with 10 6 90 min modules implemented twice weekly over five weeks
  • 10. for people with diabetes, found no significant difference between program participants and a usual care group on the same psychological measures or glycosylated hemoglobin or waist circumference (Bradshaw et al., 2007). Participants of a worksite trial of the ‘‘Resilience Reintegration’’ program, implemented in approximately 14 days during six months for employees with illnesses attributed to work stress, demonstrated higher levels of effective coping (including seeking social support) and lower levels of depression at post- intervention compared with baseline (Steensma, Den Heijer, & Stallen, 2006). These latter two programs involved a tertiary approach to psychosocial well- being, as they targeted individuals with developed conditions so as to minimize suffering associated with poor health. Only the first program adopted a primary approach of promoting resilience in otherwise healthy adults. We have developed a psychosocial resilience training program (READY: REsilience and Activity for every DaY) to promote resilience and psychosocial well-being in adults. The program is designed as a primary or secondary level of intervention, and targets adults at risk of stress or stress induced depressive symptoms, but otherwise generally healthy. The aim of this article is to describe a pilot study of the program that was conducted to obtain information on the
  • 11. feasibility of implementing the program as group-based training in a workplace setting. The study also examined the potential effectiveness of the program to promote subjective well-being, and reduce symptoms associated with depression and stress. Method The study protocol was approved by The University of Queensland Medical Research Ethics Committee (2007000303). Study design This study was a single group pre–post trial with outcome measures assessed the week immediately prior to and after the 13 week intervention period. Setting The program was conducted with employees of a university in a capital city. Psychology, Health & Medicine 267 D ow nl oa de d
  • 13. 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 Participants An invitation to participate in the program, without cost, was circulated in a weekly email communication to administrative staff at The University of Queensland (Australia). Interested respondents contacted the project staff by return email, and were then contacted by telephone by a research assistant who provided preliminary study information. Respondents were considered ineligible if they reported receiving current psychiatric/psychological treatment (pharmacological or therapy-based). Those who provided preliminary consent to participate were then asked to attend a group-based assessment session at the University. No incentives were offered for
  • 14. participation. The intervention The READY program targets five key resilience protective factors that were identified from empirical literature: (1) positive emotions; (2) cognitive flexibility (e.g. accep- tance), (3) life meaning, (4) social support, and (5) active coping strategies (including physical activity) (Southwick, Vythilingam, & Charney, 2005). The intervention approach is based on Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT), which is an empirically based third generation Cognitive Behavioral Therapy that uses acceptance and mindfulness strategies, and commitment and behavior change strategies to produce psychological flexibility and resilience through six core processes: Acceptance, cognitive defusion (changing our relationship with thoughts), being present (mindfulness), self-as-context, values and committed action (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). The program also includes cognitive behavioral therapy processes of skills training for relaxation and building social support. One program module promotes participation in purposive and incidental physical activity. Physical activity has previously been identified as a potential coping resource (Southwick et al., 2005) that can provide enduring resilience to stress (Salmon, 2001), enhance well-being (Penedo & Dahn, 2005; Scully, Kremer, Meade,
  • 15. Graham, & Dudgeon, 1998; Stathopoulou, Powers, Berry, Smits, & Otto, 2006), and protect against incident depression symptoms (Brown, Ford, Burton, Marshall, & Dobson, 2005; Strawbridge, Deleger, Roberts, & Kaplan, 2002; van Gool et al., 2007). The program has 11 modules, and sessions include psychoeducation, discussion, experiential exercises, and structured learning and practice activities. Session topics include an introduction to the READY resilience model, physical activity, mindfulness, defusion (two modules including self-as-context), acceptance, life values, social connectedness, relaxation and pleasant activities, and activating and trouble shooting strategies (two modules). Participants receive a detailed workbook that includes an audio compact disc (with guided exercises), written notes, sections for critical reflection, and structured learning activities to complete. The reflection and learning activities comprise the READY Personal Plan, which is a personalized resource to help participants apply the generalized information to their specific context and individual style. Eleven sessions each of two h duration were run weekly over 13 weeks (with 1 week off in weeks three and 10 because of public holidays) at the university. Sessions
  • 16. 268 N.W. Burton et al. D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca st le , A us
  • 17. tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 were held during the week after work hours (five–seven pm), and were led by two of the authors (N.B., K.P.) who are clinical and health psychologists. Measures
  • 18. Group assessment sessions were conducted at the university, in the week before and the week after the program, by three independent research assistants (one each for questionnaire, physical, and hematological measures) with the lead investigator (N.B.) in attendance. Feasibility and acceptability Participant attendance records were kept for each session by the interventionists, and reasons for non-attendance were recorded. The post intervention ques- tionnaire included items to obtain participant feedback on the program and materials. Psychosocial well-being A battery of self completed questionnaires included: (1) Ryff’s Scales of Psychological Well-Being, including subscales of autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relations, purpose in life, and self-acceptance (Ryff, 1989). This measure is widely used, has well established reliability and validity (Ryff & Singer, 2003), and has previously been shown to be sensitive to detecting treatment effects (Fava, Rafanelli, Grandi, Conti, & Belluardo, 1998). (2) The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff,
  • 19. 1977). This is one of the most commonly used self report questionnaires on depression for a general (vs. clinical) population, and has established reliability and validity (Radloff, 1977). (3) The Short Version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21) with subscales of depression (DASS_Dep), stress (DASS_Stress), and anxiety (DASS_Anx), (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). Each subscale has been shown to have high internal consistency and has yielded meaningful discriminations in a variety of settings in both Australian clinical and community samples (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). (4) The positive affect scale from The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS-X), which is widely used with well established psychometric properties (Watson & Clark, 1999). (5) The action consistency items from The Valued Living Questionnaire (Wilson & Groom, 2002). Psychometric data on this measure is not yet available. (6) The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale, which has been shown to be reliable, valid, and sensitive to change (Brown & Ryan, 2003). (7) The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire II (AAQII), which has
  • 20. acceptable levels of factorial validity, criterion validity, and reliability (Hayes et al., 2006). Psychology, Health & Medicine 269 D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca st le
  • 21. , A us tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 (8) The MOS Social Support Survey (Sherbourne & Stewart, 1991), which has acceptable levels of reliability and validity (Hays, Sherbourne, & Mazel,
  • 22. 1995). Physical activity Physical activity was measured via self-report using items adapted from the Active Australia surveys that ask about the total time spent during the previous week in walking (Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW), 2003). These items have acceptable levels of reliability and validity (Brown, Bauman, Chey, Trost, & Mummery, 2004; Brown, Burton, Marshall, & Miller, 2008; Brown, Trost, Bauman, Mummery, & Owen, 2004). Total time spent in activity was calculated by summing the time spent in minutes of activity across these three categories, after weighting minutes in vigorous activity by two to allow for the greater intensity [i.e. (minutes walking) þ (minutes moderate activity) þ (minutes vigorous activity 62)]. Physical activity was also assessed using pedometer step counts. Participants were asked to wear a pedometer for seven consecutive days and record the total number of steps taken each day. The pedometer and the step log were returned at the first group training session, and by mail after the program completion. Data was used to derive average daily step counts. Physical and hematological measures Physical measures included height and weight (used to derive body mass index
  • 23. [BMI]) and blood pressure (BP_Sys and BP_Dias). Hematological data involved a fasting blood sample to measure blood glucose, total cholesterol, C-reactive protein (CRP), and cortisol. Sociodemographic measures Questionnaire data were used to assess age, gender, country of birth, household composition, educational qualifications, employment status, occupation, ability to manage on income received, overall health status, caffeine and alcohol consumption, and tobacco cigarette consumption. Analyses Descriptive statistics were used to analyze attendance records, responses to questionnaire items about the program, and sociodemographic data. Outcome measure data were analyzed on an as per protocol basis. Standardized mean differences (with Hedges adjustment for a small sample size) and 95% confidence intervals were used to examine the relative size of the intervention effect across the different measures. For the following variables, scores were interpreted as positive intervention effects when the post- intervention values were lower than baseline: DASS_Stress, DASS_Dep (depression), DASS_Anx (anxiety), CES-D (depression), BP_Dias and BP_Sys (blood pressure), cholesterol, CRP (C-
  • 24. reactive protein), and BMI. Cohen’s standards (large [0.8], medium [0.5], and small [0.2]) were used to interpret the magnitude of intervention effects (Cohen, 1988). 270 N.W. Burton et al. D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca st
  • 25. le , A us tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 Paired t-tests were used to determine the significance of differences between
  • 26. baseline and post intervention scores. As this was a feasibility study with a small sample, an a of 0.05 was used initially instead of a more conservative value. However given the number of planned comparisons, results were also examined using a sequential Bonferroni adjustment of a levels. Results Participants The sociodemographic characteristics of participants are presented in Table 1. The age of participants ranged from 24 to 50 years, with a mean of 36.5 (SD 8.6). The self-reported time spent in physical activity ranged from 45 to 1200 min/week, with a median of 205 minutes/week. Average daily steps ranged from 4956 to 14,249, with a mean of 9801 (SD ¼ 2784). Feasibility Thirty-five people responded to the study invitation and 18 people consented to participate. Two participants did not complete the program; one moved interstate, and the other discontinued because of a change in personal circumstances. The average proportion of sessions attended by participants was 81%, with three participants attending all 11 sessions. During the program, 37% (n ¼ 6) missed one or two sessions, and 44% (n ¼ 7) missed three or four sessions.
  • 27. The most common reasons given for missing sessions were time conflicts with work meetings and planned recreation leave. Participant feedback on the program was very positive. On a five-point Likert scale, the mean rating for the program overall was 4.67 (where 5 was excellent and 4 was very good), and the mean rating of personal helpfulness was 4.44 (where 5 was a lot and 4 was moderately so). On a four-point Likert scale (where 4 was very helpful and 3 was moderately helpful), the mean rating for the workbook was 3.87, and the mean rating for the READY Personal Plan was 3.5. The majority of participants agreed with the weekly frequency (75%) and the two hour session duration (87%). In terms of the overall program length, 56% agreed that it was good, and 31% thought it was too short. Effect on psychosocial, physical activity, physical, and hematological measures Figure 1 shows the standardized mean differences and 95% confidence intervals for each measure. There were large favorable intervention effects on measures of acceptance, environmental mastery, positive emotions, mindfulness and personal growth; moderate effects on measures of stress, self acceptance, valued living, autonomy, and depression; and a small effect on the total cholesterol measure.
  • 28. The mean scores at baseline and post-intervention are shown in Table 2. Paired t-tests indicated a significant difference (p 5 0.05) between baseline and post intervention scores on measures of mastery (t[15] ¼ 4.234, p ¼ 0.001), positive emotions (t[15] ¼ 3.696, p ¼ 0.002), personal growth (t[15] ¼ 3.357, p ¼ 0.004), mindfulness (t[15] ¼ 3.362, p ¼ 0.004), acceptance (t[15] ¼ 2.847, p ¼ 0.012), stress (t[15] ¼ 72.807, p ¼ 0.013), self acceptance (t[15] ¼ 2.720, p ¼ 0.016), valued living (t[15] ¼ 2.557, p ¼ 0.022), autonomy (t[15] ¼ 2.369, p ¼ 0.032), and total Psychology, Health & Medicine 271 D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs
  • 30. r 20 17 cholesterol (t[15] ¼ 72.483, p ¼ 0.025). The difference on a measure of depression (CES-D) had borderline significance (t[15] ¼ 72.063, p ¼ 0.057). Using the sequential Bonferroni adjustment of a levels, only mastery remained significant. Discussion These results provide promising preliminary support for the READY program as an intervention that can be feasibly implemented as group-based training in a Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of study sample. Study sample % (n) Gender Men 85 (15) Women 17 (3) Age group (years) 20–30 33 (6) 31–40 33 (6) 41–50 33 (6) Country of birth
  • 31. Australia 61 (11) UK/USA 17 (3) Other 22 (4) Highest educational qualification completed School only 11 (2) Certificate/diploma 11 (2) University degree 78 (14) Household composition Living alone 11 (2) Single and living with others 17 (3) Couple, no children 50 (9) Couple with children 22 (4) Employment status Full time 83 (15) Part time/casual 17 (3) Occupational group Manager or Senior Administrator 28 (5) Professional or Associate Professional 33 (6) Clerical/Other 39 (7) Ability to manage on income received Easy 33 (6) Not too bad/Difficult sometimes 61 (11) Impossible 6 (1) General health status Excellent/very good 17 (3) Good 33 (6) Fair/Poor 50 (9) Body mass index 518.5–525 67 (12)
  • 32. 425 33 (6) 272 N.W. Burton et al. D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca st le , A us
  • 33. tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 workplace setting to improve psychosocial well-being. Our three-month implemen- tation period was twice as long as both the original and the modified Personal Resilience and Resilient Relationships program (Bradshaw et al., 2007; Waite &
  • 34. Richardson, 2004), but half the length of the Resilience and Reintegration program, although the majority of that was conducted in the first seven weeks (Steensma et al., 2006). READY involved 22 h of contact time, which was slightly longer than the Personal Resilience and Resilient Relationships Program (Bradshaw et al., 2007) but much shorter than the other two training programs that required 40 h (Waite & Richardson, 2004) and (approximately) 14 days (Steensma et al., 2006). Written feedback indicated that the READY participants liked the weekly frequency of sessions, as this kept training issues ‘‘on the agenda’’ while also allowing time for reflection and skills practice. Although the 11 6 2 h sessions were seen as a significant time commitment, and less than 20% of participants attended all sessions, a shorter implementation period or shorter sessions was not favored by the majority of participants. There was some support for having a longer implementa- tion period to allow for more breaks during the program, e.g. the occasional two week break from weekly sessions, so as to consolidate learning and prevent fatigue. Consideration of this for future applications of the program would however, need to be in consultation with the employer organization; in this trial the implementation period was within one term in the university calendar. Participants rated the program highly, and saw it as personally
  • 35. helpful and enjoyable. The participant workbook and READY Personal Plan were well received Figure 1. Standardized mean difference and 95% confidence intervals for study measures. Psychology, Health & Medicine 273 D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca
  • 36. st le , A us tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 and seen as a useful resource both during and after the program.
  • 37. Specific suggestions to improve the program were to have more time in each session to review homework and activities from the previous week, more small group work, and more time to review the overall program and the READY Personal Plan. There were large and significantly favorable intervention effects on measures of acceptance (acknowledging both positive and negative experiences), environmental mastery (effective and competent use of opportunities and external activities), positive emotions (e.g. interest, enthusiasm, determination), mindfulness (conscious attention and awareness), and personal growth (self improvement, openness to new experiences). There were also moderate and significant favorable intervention effects on measures of stress, self acceptance (positive self attitude), valued living (actions consistent with life priorities and desires), and autonomy (self- determination, self regulation). As the READY program targeted positive emotions, problem solving, and life purpose as key protective factors, and included session modules on mindfulness and acceptance (targeting the protective factor of cognitive flexibility), improvements in these areas were particularly pleasing. Although there was a moderate favorable effect on depression, this did not reach statistical significance. This may reflect low baseline levels; the mean score for both the DASS_Depression
  • 38. Table 2. Mean differences in measures between baseline and post-intervention. Measure Mean difference (SD) 95% confidence interval p Questionnaire measures Ryff_Autonomy 3.06 (5.17) 0.31–5.82 0.03 Ryff_Environmental mastery 6.25 (5.91) 3.10–9.40 0.001 Ryff_Personal growth 4.37 (5.21) 1.60–7.15 0.004 Ryff_Positive relations 1.06 (6.29) 72.29–4.41 0.509 Ryff_Life purpose 3.87 (9.82) 71.36–9.11 0.135 Ryff_Self acceptance 3.94 (5.79) 0.85–7.02 0.016 CES-D (Depression) a 75.25 (10.18) 710.67–0.17 0.057 DASS_Depression a 73.75 (9.46) 78.79–1.29 0.134 DASS_Stress a 75.75 (8.19) 710.12 to 1.38 0.013 DASS_Anxiety a 0.50 (7.17) 73.32–4.32 0.784 PANAS_positive affect 6.44 (6.97) 2.72–10.15 0.002 Valued living questionnaire 7.19 (11.24) 1.20–13.18 0.022 Mindful attention awareness scale 7.12 (8.48) 2.61–11.64 0.004
  • 39. Acceptance and action questionnaire II (AAQII) 6.81 (9.57) 1.71–11.91 0.012 MOS social support survey 0.87 (2.03) 70.21 to 1.96 0.105 Physical activity Minutes/week 15.33 (289) 7144–175 0.840 Average steps/day 223.85 (2870) 71356–1823 0.757 Physical measures Body mass index a 0.09 (1.11) 70.49–0.68 0.74 Blood pressure_systolic a 72.87 (7.62) 76.94–1.19 0.152 Blood pressure_diastolic a 70.87 (6.25) 74.20–2.45 0.584 Hematological measures Fasting blood glucose a 70.74 (0.42) 70.30–0.15 0.489 Total cholesterol a 70.26 (0.41) 70.48 to 0.03 0.025 C-Reactive protein a 70.07 (0.83) 70.52–0.37 0.730 Cortisol a 0.500 (27.69) 714.26–15.26 0.943 a
  • 40. Negative mean difference implies favorable change. 274 N.W. Burton et al. D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca st le , A us
  • 41. tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 subscale (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) and the CES-D (Radloff, 1977) were below ‘‘clinical’’ levels. Because of the different measures used, our results are not directly comparable
  • 42. with other resilience training studies. There is however, conceptual similarity between our improvements on measures of valued living, autonomy, and self acceptance, and the higher levels of life purpose, locus of control, and self esteem reported by Waite and Richardson (2004). Consistent with Steensma et al., (2006) our study demonstrated improvements in depression, although as previously stated, this had only borderline significance. Unlike other studies (Steensma et al., 2006; Waite & Richardson, 2004), we did not find significant improvements on measures of interpersonal relations. We used the MOS Social Support Survey (Sherbourne & Stewart, 1991), which assesses the frequency of the availability of sources of support (e.g. someone whose advice you really want, someone to show you love and affection), which may not have been sufficiently sensitive to change. Resilience was conceptualized as a primarily intra- personal construct to be developed by participants, while this measure assesses supportive behaviours provided by inter-personal networks, which was not under the direct influence of program participants. Other studies used measures that assessed seeking social support as a coping strategy (Steensma et al., 2006) and the frequency of specified interpersonal experiences (Waite & Richardson, 2004). Consistent with the findings of Bradshaw et al., (2007), we did
  • 43. not find significant changes in the physical or haematological measures, or in self- reported physical activity. A lack of significant change in the hematological measures may be because these were largely within the accepted healthy ranges at baseline. We did however, have a small but significant change in total cholesterol. No significant changes in physical activity may have been because of high baseline levels; the median of 205 min/week is higher than the 5 6 30 min/week of moderate activity or 3 6 20 min/week of vigorous activity specified in national recommendations (Haskell et al., 2007). It should be noted however, that physical activity was conceptualized as only one aspect of resilience, and already active participants could still benefit from other program modules. Furthermore, participants may have qualitatively changed their experience of physical activity participation in response to the other program modules, such as doing physical activity with mindfulness. This could have had a synergistic effect on other outcome measures, such as for example positive emotions. Limitations As this study was a small-scale feasibility study, and not a controlled trial, we are unable to make conclusions about the efficacy of the program. There were however, significant pre–post improvements in specific indicators of well being and psychosocial functioning. Without a follow-up assessment, we
  • 44. are unable to comment on the sustainability of these improvements. As the construct of resilience is such that it implies a protective effect against future adversity, the longevity of any improvements will be important to assess in future research. The short implementa- tion period may have been insufficient to see changes in the physiological measures such as BMI. The study relied heavily on self-report data, which are vulnerable to social desirability bias and measurement error. However, we used questionnaires that have previously been demonstrated to have acceptable levels of sensitivity to change and reliability, and complemented the physical activity questionnaire data Psychology, Health & Medicine 275 D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve
  • 46. be r 20 17 with objective step count (pedometer) data. As the study sample was recruited from staff at a university in a capital city, and included a majority of women and people with a university education, further work is needed to examine the acceptability and effectiveness of the program with a more heterogenous sample. Conclusions These results suggest that it is feasible to implement the READY program as a group training program in a worksite setting to promote psychosocial functioning and well- being. Participant feedback indicated that no major changes were required to the session duration, session frequency or program materials, but that a longer implementation period and more time for review activities could be considered. Sessions could also be restructured to allow for more small group work, and more review of previous sessions and homework activities. An alternative measure of social support, that focuses on the behaviours of the respondent (e.g. seeking support) rather
  • 47. than others (e.g. support provision) may be more useful. Future work will examine the efficacy of the program in a controlled trial, the sustainability of any improvements, and the mechanisms of change. This will facilitate our conceptual understanding of resilience, and our practical understanding of how to positively influence it. Acknowledgements Nicola Burton is a Heart Foundation Research Fellow (PH 08B 3904), and is also supported by a (Australian) National Health and Medical Research Council Capacity Building Grant (ID 252977) and Program Grant (ID 310200). This study was funded by a Program Grant from the (Australian) National Health and Medical Research Council (ID 301200). The authors thank the participants for their ongoing enthusiasm and support for the program, and Sarah Walters for research support. References Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW). (2003). The Active Australia survey: A guide and manual for implementation, analysis and reporting. Canberra: AIHW. Bradshaw, B., Richardson, G., Kumpfer, K., Carlson, J., Stanchfield, J., Overall, J., et al. (2007). Determining the efficacy of a resiliency training approach in adults with type 2 diabetes. The Diabetes Educator, 33(4), 650–659.
  • 48. Brown, K., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. Brown, W., Trost, S.G., Bauman, A., Mummery, W.K., & Owen, N. (2004). Test–retest reliability of four physical activity measures used in population surveys. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 28(2), 128–134. Brown, W.J., Bauman, A., Chey, T., Trost, S.G., & Mummery, W.K. (2004). Comparison of surveys used to measure physical activity. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 28(2), 128–134. Brown, W.J., Burton, N.W., Marshall, A.L., & Miller, Y.D. (2008). Reliability and validity of a modified self-administered version of the Active Australia physical activity survey in a sample of mid-age women. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 32(6), 535–541. Brown, W.J., Ford, J., Burton, N.W., Marshall, A.L., & Dobson, A. (2005). Prospective study of physical activity and depressive symptoms in mid age women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 29(4), 265–272. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Fava, G., Rafanelli, C., Grandi, S., Conti, S., & Belluardo, P.
  • 49. (1998). Prevention of recurrent depression with cognitive behavioral therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 816–820. 276 N.W. Burton et al. D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca st le
  • 50. , A us tr al ia ] at 1 5: 34 2 8 N ov em be r 20 17 Haskell, W.L., Lee, I.-M., Pate, R., Powell, K., Blair, S., Franklin, B., et al. (2007). Physical activity and public health: Updated recommendation for adults from the American
  • 51. College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 39(8), 1423–1434. Hayes, S., Luoma, J.B., Bond, F.W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25. Hays, R., Sherbourne, C., & Mazel, R. (1995). User’s manual for medical outcomes study (MOS) core measures of health-related quality of life: RAND Corporation, MR-162-RC. Santa Monica, CA. Lovibond, S., & Lovibond, P. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation of Australia. Penedo, F.J., & Dahn, J.R. (2005). Exercise and well-being: A review of mental and physical health benefits associated with physical activity. Current Opinions in Psychiatry, 18(2), 189–193. Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. Richardson, G., & Waite, P. (2002). Mental health promotion through resilience and resiliency education. International Journal of Emergency Mental Health, 4(1), 65–75.
  • 52. Rozanski, A., Blumenthal, J., & Kaplan, J. (1999). Impact of psychosocial factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation, 99, 2192–2217. Ryff, C. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. Ryff, C., & Singer, B. (2003). Flourishing under fire. In C. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 15–36). Washington: American Psychological Association. Salmon, P. (2001). Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: A unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(1), 33–61. Scully, D., Kremer, J., Meade, M., Graham, R.C., & Dudgeon, K. (1998). Physical exercise and psychological well being: A critical review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 32(2), 111–120. Sherbourne, C., & Stewart, A. (1991). The MOS social support survey. Social Science and Medicine, 32(6), 705–714. Southwick, S., Vythilingam, M., & Charney, D. (2005). The psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress: Implications for prevention and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 255–291.
  • 53. Stathopoulou, G., Powers, M., Berry, A., Smits, J., & Otto, M. (2006). Exercise interventions for mental health: A quantitative and qualitative review. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice, 13(2), 179–193. Steensma, H., Den Heijer, M., & Stallen, V. (2006). Research note: Effects of resilience training on the reduction of stress and depression among Dutch workers. International Quarterly of Community Health Education, 27(2), 145–159. Strawbridge, W., Deleger, S., Roberts, R., & Kaplan, G. (2002). Physical activity reduces the risk of subsequent depression for older adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156(4), 327–334. Strike, P., & Steptoe, A. (2004). Psychosocial factors in the development of coronary artery disease. Progress in Cardiovascular Diseases, 46(4), 337–347. van Gool, C., Kempen, G., Bosma, H., van Boxtel, M., Jolles, J., & Van Eijk, J. (2007). Associations between lifestyle and depressed mood: Longitudinal results from the Maastricht Aging Study. American Journal of Public Health, 97(5), 887–815. Waite, P., & Richardson, G. (2004). Determining the efficacy of resilience training in the worksite. Journal of Allied Health, 33(3), 178–183. Watson, D., & Clark, L. (1999). PANAS-X manual for positive and negative affect schedule-
  • 54. expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa. Wilson, K., & Groom, J. (2002). The valued living questionnaire. University of Mississippi. (Available from K.G. Wilson, Department of Psychology, 205 Peabody Building, University of Mississippi, US 38677). Psychology, Health & Medicine 277 D ow nl oa de d by [ U ni ve rs it y of N ew ca
  • 56. Promoting Resilience in Children and Youth Preventive Interventions and Their Interface with Neuroscience MARK T. GREENBERG Prevention Research Center, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, USA ABSTRACT: Preventive interventions focus on reducing risk and promot- ing protective factors in the child as well as their cultural ecologies (family, classroom, school, peer groups, neighborhood, etc). By improving com- petencies in both the child and their contexts many of these interventions promote resilience. Although there are now a substantial number of pre- ventive interventions that reduce problem behaviors and build competen- cies across childhood and adolescence, there has been little integration with recent findings in neuropsychology and neuroscience. This article focuses on the integration of prevention research and neuroscience in the context of interventions that promote resilience by improving the exec- utive functions (EF); inhibitory control, planning, and problem solving skills, emotional regulation, and attentional capacities of
  • 57. children and youth. Illustrations are drawn from recent randomized controlled trials of the Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies (PATHS) curriculum. The discussion focuses on the next steps in transdisciplinary research in prevention and social neuroscience. KEYWORDS: prevention; intervention; neuroscience; children; youth; frontal lobe PROMOTING RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN AND YOUTH: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS AND THEIR INTERFACE WITH NEUROSCIENCE As a psychologist interested in creating change in communities and influenc- ing public policy, I am often asked questions about developmental processes and trajectories in children. For example, “How can we help children resist Address for correspondence: Dr. Mark T. Greenberg, Prevention Research Center, Henderson Build- ing, South Room 109, Penn State University, University Park, PA 16802. Voice: 814-863-0112; fax: 814-865-2530. e-mail: [email protected] Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1094: 139–150 (2006). C© 2006 New York Academy of Sciences. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.013
  • 58. 139 140 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES negative influences when they live in high-risk neighborhoods?” “How can we improve children’s academic outcomes when their families have experienced an intergenerational history of school failure?” “How can we help children control their emotions when their peers are bringing out the worst in them through teasing and taunting?” All of these questions imply the notion that many children are exposed to high-risk situations and the implication is that unless something is done, the probability of a poor outcome for some chil- dren is relatively high. Such situations are ripe for efforts to attempt to prevent difficulties that may spiral into longer-term poor outcomes across childhood, adolescence, and even adulthood. Beginning with the War on Poverty and the emergence of Head Start in the 1960s, preventive interventions have become central to our nation’s public policy. As a result of the increased rate of both adolescent delinquency and drug use seen in the ensuing decades, the devel- opment of both preventive intervention programs and social/legislative policy initiatives has dramatically increased. The role of developmental science in
  • 59. public policy making has never been greater or more influential. Improving the public’s health, especially for those at greatest risk, is a com- plex problem that involves interventions at the level of economic and social policy as well as the ability to strengthen the skills of educators, parents, and youth themselves. Macrolevel interventions focused on changing community- level ecologies, attitudes, and behavior have ranged from large experiments in economic policy (e.g., earned income tax credits, TANF), changes in housing patterns,1 social legislation ranging from raising the age at which youth can drive to building community partnerships to reduce youth problem behaviors and build positive youth development at the community level.2 At a more mi- crolevel, attempts to improve the culture, attitudes, and relations in families, peer groups, and schools have focused on building communication skills and values that promote positive developmental outcomes. Finally, there has been recognition that some attributes inside the individual, including skills, cogni- tions, and behaviors, may be malleable in response to preventive efforts. The enormous scope of activities undertaken is a testimony to our implicit under- standing that child and youth outcomes are multidetermined and that various levels of influence impact developmental trajectories. Although at times these
  • 60. initiatives seem (or are) both random and chaotic, at another level they clearly reflect the combination of our growing basic knowledge of both the ecology of human development,3 the advances made in developmental psychopathology,4 and the emergent development of the field of prevention science.5 The study of resilience emerges out of the large research endeavor of public health epidemiology and the study of risk and protective factors and how they impact development. A number of guiding principles have emerged both from epidemiology and developmental psychopathology.6,7 First, it is unlikely that there is a single “cause” of many of the preventable outcomes (e.g., mental disorders, substance abuse, school failure, delinquency); even in the case of disorders in which a biochemical or genetic mechanism has been discovered, GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 141 the expression of the disorder is influenced by other biological or environ- mental events.8,9 Second, there are multiple pathways both to and from risk and problem outcomes; for example, there are different combinations of risk factors that might lead to the same disorder. Third, no single
  • 61. cause may be either necessary or sufficient10 and the effect of a risk factor will depend on its timing and relation to other risk factors. Fourth, many risk factors are not disorder-specific, but instead relate to a variety of outcomes. Finally, risk fac- tors may vary in influence with host factors, such as gender, ethnicity, and culture. Resiliency is commonly defined as positive or protective processes that re- duce maladaptive outcomes under conditions of risk. That is, they are protective factors that may be especially important under conditions of risk. Although much less is known about protective factors and their operation,7,11 at least three broad types of protective factors have been identified. These include characteristics of the individual (e.g., temperamental qualities and intelligence/ cognitive ability), the quality of the child’s relationships, and broader ecological factors, such as quality schools, safe neighborhoods, and regulatory activities. An essential question related to adversity and resilience is the individual’s development of an effective set of responses to stress. Central components of the stress response include the initial appraisal of the event and its emotional meaning, the ability to sufficiently regulate one’s emotions and arousal to
  • 62. initiate problem solving and gather more information, the fuller cognitive- affective interpretation of the event, and one’s behavioral response. Masten12 has noted that among the most important resiliency factors are these very cognitive and emotion regulation skills. Prospective longitudinal designs are critical to understanding the role of resiliency as they can identify (a) which risk factors are predictive of different developmental stages of a problem, (b) the dynamic relation between risk and protective factors in different developmental periods, and (c) what factors are most likely to “protect” or buffer persons under risk conditions from negative outcomes. In addition, randomized trials of preventive interventions can test these theories by examining how behavior changes are mediated. THE INTERFACE OF PREVENTION, DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY, AND NEUROSCIENCE Although developmental psychopathology, prevention, and neuroscience have developed in isolation, integrated research on protective factors and re- silience has the potential to answer central questions regarding plasticity and the role of environmental and genetic process. While the primary goal of prevention science is to change behavior, behavior can be
  • 63. broadly defined as action, emotion, and cognition. Further, biological substrates underlie all of these processes and may serve as moderators, mediators, or outcomes of 142 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES TABLE 1. Levels and measures of the biological substrate I. Neural processes II. Autonomic nervous system 1. Structural aspects 1. Parasympathetic activity A. Neuronal development and connections A. Cardiac vagal tone B. Localization of action 2. Functional aspects 2. Sympathetic nervous system A. Neurochemical systems (dopamine, A. Resting heart rate noradrenaline, serotonin, brain- derived neurotrophic factor) 3. Neurocognitive function 3. Neuroendocrine function A. Neuropsychological testing A. HPA axis–Glucocorticoids 4. Immunological function A. T cells/antibody titers to vaccines preventive interventions.13 A broad vision of the integration of prevention and neuroscience would examine how a variety of biological processes play a role in a deeper understanding of the processes and effects of
  • 64. preventive interven- tions. TABLE 1 provides a list of some of the biological substrates that would be of interest at the levels of both the brain and autonomic nervous system. A central task for the next decade is to understand in much greater detail the relations between the multiple levels of the biological substrate and these resilience processes involved in cognitive processes and emotional regulation. With transdisciplinary collaboration involving neuroscientists and the use of multilevel models of measurement driven by the theory/logic model, prevention research has the potential to make a major contribution to understanding the developing interplay of biology and behavior. Many preventive interventions focus on supporting improved emotion reg- ulation and problem-solving skills in which executive functions (EF) and the actions of the prefrontal lobes play a central role. EF generally refers to the psy- chological processes that are involved in the conscious control of thought. Ex- amples of processes include inhibition, future time orientation, consequential thinking, and the planning, initiation, and regulation of goal- directed behav- ior.14 Substantial data indicate that EF skills as assessed by neuropsychological tasks are related to childhood maladaptation.13,15 However, there has been lit-
  • 65. tle evidence in childhood between the performance of EF tasks (inhibitory control, working memory, planning) and neuroanatomical localization of ac- tivity in areas of the frontal lobe.16,17 Due to the methodological requirements for valid Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) assessments with young children, few data are available before the age of 10 years, although recent work using high-density Event Related Potential (ERP) assessments are particularly promising.18 A series of methodological and conceptual chal- lenges still have to be solved in order to fully assess the specific brain local- ization of neurocognitive and affective skills in children.19 Further, there is a need to broaden methods to understand how childhood cognitive and affective GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 143 processing (especially under conditions of stress) are related to other biologi- cal processes, including action in the autonomic nervous systems that include correlates of the hypo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis,20,21 immunological func- tion, the parasympathetic system (vagal tone), as well as functional analysis of brain action (neurotransmitter release). Pioneering work with children has already begun to show the
  • 66. potential yield of this vision in which interventions use measurement models and theories based on our rapidly developing knowledge of neuroscience. Research has indicated that there is correspondence between improved reading skills and changes in brain activity in reading-deficient children.22 An intervention to im- prove the outcomes of children in the foster care system has indicated changes in both behavior and children’s salivary cortisol.23 Computer- based training for children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) has indicated changes in EF and behavior.16 Meditation training in adults has been shown to alter both frontal brain activity (hemispheric laterality) and immunological response.24 Further, a number of studies has shown the moderating role of biological variables, including how EF moderates the effect of a brief inter- vention on high-risk teens25 and how the hypoactivity of anterior cingulate cortex predicts poor response to treatment for depression.26 Although neuroanatomical findings on cognitive and emotion regulation skills in childhood are sparse, there is a burgeoning literature on adults re- garding brain localization of EF that can judiciously guide theory and action with children. The field of social–cognitive neuroscience (studies with lesion patients, patients with psychopathologies, and normally
  • 67. developing adults) has clearly implicated the orbital/dorsolateral/limbic circuit in the processing of emotional stimuli and the cognitive control and regulation of behavior.27,28 Findings indicate a clear role for the anterior cingulate in the processing of emotions, executive attention processes, and working memory.29–31 The role of the dorsolaternal prefrontal area has been shown in cognitive control and inhibi- tion of emotional arousal.32,33 Further, the orbital frontal area has been related to emotion processing and regulation.34 Although research has attempted to completely localize processes in single neuroanatomical areas, it is clear that there is strong and rapid connectivity between these areas during decision making and there are sometimes contradictory findings between studies of specific loci.35 As most of this work is less than a decade old, conclusions re- garding specific loci may be premature. Further, noradrenaline, serotonin, and dopamine are projected to all these areas and thus energize action across sys- tems. Double dissociation36 and lesion studies as well as intervention trials24 will play substantial roles in further differentiation. THE DEVELOPMENT OF EF Although infancy and toddlerhood provide a basis for critical
  • 68. aspects of later coping,21,37 much of the child’s more complex cognitive processes, coping, 144 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES and regulation skills arise with neurocognitive maturation in the frontal lobes. This maturation proceeds from the preschool years through late adolescence. Although numerous linguistic and cognitive processes are developing, the de- velopment of EF appears crucial to healthy development and deficiencies in EF have been related to numerous poor outcomes. These outcomes involve cognitive processes related to effective emotional regulation and behavioral performance, including aggression, delinquency, depression, and disorders of attention.13,38 INTERVENTION AND EF: ILLUSTRATIONS FROM THE PROMOTING ALTERNATIVE THINKING STRATEGIES (PATHS) CURRICULUM During the past few decades our research group has been involved in the development, implementation, evaluation, and refinement of a social and emo- tional learning curriculum based on neuroscientific principles that focus on promoting emotional awareness and effective cognitive control.
  • 69. The PATHS curriculum is a universal school-based prevention curriculum aimed at re- ducing aggression and behavior problems by promoting the development of social–emotional competence in children during the preschool and elementary school years.39 PATHS is based on the affective-behavioral- cognitive-dynamic (ABCD) model of development.40 The ABCD model focuses on how cogni- tion, affect, language, and behavior become integrated in the developing child. A fundamental concept is that as youth mature, emotional development pre- cedes most forms of cognitive development. That is, young children experience emotions and react to them long before they can verbalize their experiences. Early in life, emotional development is an important precursor to other ways of thinking and must be integrated with cognitive and linguistic abilities, which are much slower to develop. Then, during the elementary years, further devel- opmental integration occurs among affect, behavior, and cognition/language through maturation of the prefrontal circuit. These processes of brain matu- ration are important in achieving socially competent action and healthy peer relations. The PATHS curriculum places special attention on neurocognitive models of development.41 Of significant importance are the concepts of
  • 70. vertical con- trol and verbal processing of action (e.g., horizontal control). Vertical control refers to the process of higher-order cognitive processes exerting control over lower-level limbic impulses vı̀s-a-vı̀s the development of frontal cognitive con- trol.14 PATHS attempts to consciously teach children the processes of vertical control by providing opportunities to practice conscious strategies for self- control. This is achieved via instruction with curriculum lessons and a variety of cognitive/behavioral techniques that are developmentally appropriate from the ages of 4 to 11 years. One central example is the use of a control signals poster that teaches children the steps for problem solving in social contexts. GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 145 The curriculum also has an intentional and intensive focus on helping children to verbally identify and label feelings in order to manage them. This is achieved through curriculum lessons and the integration of “feeling faces” that children use throughout the day to identify their feelings and those of others. A series of outcome trials have indicated that effective implementation of the PATHS curriculum leads to decreases in externalizing and
  • 71. internalizing problems by both teacher and self-report and to increases in social and emo- tional competence.42–45 However, as with many preventive interventions, there has been little investigation of how such change is mediated. Although some aspect of this mediation may be due to changes external to the child (improved classroom environment, warmer teacher–student relations), we believe that the curriculum promotes more effective inhibitory control, emotion regula- tion, and planning skills. The curriculum logic model is based on the idea that the intervention will lead children (1) to become less impulsive and more plan- ful in their social interactions, and (2) to recruit language to regulate behavior and communicate effectively with others. We recently tested this mediation model in a randomized controlled study of 318 second-and third-grade children.46 Schools were randomized to receive the PATHS intervention or to control status. Intervention teachers received both a 3-day initial training workshop as well as ongoing weekly coaching in curriculum implementation. The PATHS lessons were taught approximately three times per week, with each lesson lasting 20–30 min. In addition, teachers used techniques to generalize PATHS skills with the goal of supporting stu- dents to apply the PATHS skills in the “hot” naturally occurring
  • 72. contexts of their school day. These situations of high emotional arousal usually occurred during conflictual interactions with peers, with their teachers, or when feel- ing academic frustration. Students were assessed at pretest, posttest (7 months later), and follow-up (1 year after the curriculum ended). Outcome findings examined teachers’ ratings of both internalizing and exter- nalizing behavioral problems using the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL47). EF were assessed by two well-known measures validated to activate anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.48 Inhibitory control was assessed with the Stroop Test and verbal fluency was assessed using the Verbal Fluency Subtest of the McCarthy Scales of Children Abilities. To test a mediational model, it was first necessary to demonstrate that the intervention affected both behavior and EF. Results indicated that there were significant differences at posttest showing greater improvements in both inhibitory control and ver- bal fluency in the intervention children. At the 1-year follow- up, intervention children also were rated by teachers as lower in externalizing and internaliz- ing problems. Further, posttest changes in both inhibitory control and verbal fluency were significantly related to teacher ratings of behavior problems at follow-up.
  • 73. The specific mediational hypothesis we tested was that EF would me- diate the relationship between prevention/control group assignment and 146 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES teacher-reported externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. The find- ings indicated that improvements in inhibitory control at posttest signifi- cantly mediated the relation between experimental condition and both teacher- reported externalizing and internalizing behavior at 1-year follow-up. In addition, improvements in verbal fluency significantly mediated the relation between experimental condition and teacher-reported internalizing behavior. However, improvements in verbal fluency showed only a trend toward explain- ing change in teacher-reported externalizing behavior. These findings provide empirical support for the conceptual theory of action that underlies the PATHS curriculum model. That is, child neurocognitive functioning plays a key role in children’s social and emotional adaptation and changes in EF directly relate to reductions in behavioral problems. However, a broader view and greater incorporation of the biological substrate into our
  • 74. understanding of the processes would begin to assess less peripheral systems of mediation than only the use of neuropsychological tests. Although our own work with the PATHS intervention is very preliminary, I use it as a case example of how we might develop transdisciplinary connec- tions between prevention scientists and neuroscientists. A clear logic model of the intervention might hypothesize that such behavioral changes would lead to greater activation in the anterior cingulate and dosolateral prefrontal areas. Although such assessments could not be readily accomplished using fMRI at these younger ages, EEG–ERP assessments might be used. One might also hypothesize that such an intervention might impact the child’s stress reactivity (HPA axis) or their parasympathetic activity under moder- ately stressful testing conditions. Finally, if such an intervention impacted both frontal activity and stress-reactivity, one might hypothesize that over time it might impact overall bodily health as assessed by immunological function. The point here is that effective preventive models that have more fully ar- ticulated logic models of action should begin to ask “deeper” questions about the neuroscientific underpinnings of either change processes or obstacles to
  • 75. intervention impact. That is, how might measures of the biological substrate serve as mediators, moderators, and outcomes? Of course, there are some important caveats at this stage in the scientific enterprise. First, children may recruit different brain regions than do adults to accomplish the same task and extrapolated theories from research on adults should be used with caution.49 Further, even when fMRI can be used with older children, there are substantial conceptual and methodological challenges in interpreting such findings.19 Finally, although some aspects of the effects of preventive interventions may be better understood by taking a neuroscientific perspective, much of the action of some prevention models occurs primarily through changes in the environment (quality of the classroom or community) or in the context of social interactions that may not be well- captured by current models of neuroscience. GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 147 These findings and others in neuroscience point to the importance of con- sidering social–emotional development as best understood within broader the- ories that take into account how children’s experiences and relationships affect
  • 76. their brain organization, structuralization, and development.38 As such, there is a need for an extensive research agenda in which there is a transdisciplinary collaboration among prevention scientists, developmental psychopathologists, and neuroscientists. However, this will require not only clearer logic models of change and possibly more potent preventive interventions, but substantial advances in basic research in childhood neuroscience including improvements in both measurement and conceptualization. Through such work, carefully de- veloped studies should take us past the “black box” outcome to more fully understand the cognitive and neural mediators and moderators of change. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grant R01MH42131. Dr. Greenberg is one of the developers of the PATHS cur- riculum program and has a publishing agreement with Channing–Bete Pub- lishers. REFERENCES 1. LEVENTHAL, T., R.C. FAUTH & J. BROOKS-GUNN. 2005. Neighborhood poverty and public policy: a 5-year follow-up of children’s educational outcomes in the New York City moving to opportunity demonstration. Dev.
  • 77. Psychopath. 43: 933–952. 2. HAWKINS, J.D. & R.F. CATALANO, JR. 1992. Communities that Care: Action for Drug Abuse Prevention. Jossey-Bass. San Francisco. 3. BRONFENBRENNER, U. 1977. Toward an experimental ecology of human develop- ment. Am. Psych. 32: 513–531. 4. CICCHETTI, D. & D. TUCKER. 1994. Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Dev. Psychopath. 6: 533–549. 5. COIE, J.D., N.F. WATT, S.G. WEST, et al. 1993. The science of prevention: a con- ceptual framework and some directions for a national research program. Am. Psych. 48: 1013–1022. 6. CICCHETTI, D. & D.J. COHEN. 1995. Developmental Psychopathology: Vol 2. Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation. Wiley. New York. 7. MASTEN, A.S. 2001. Ordinary magic: resilience processes in development. Am. Psych. 56: 227–238. 8. CASPI, A., T.E. MOFF ITT, M. CANNON, et al. 2005. Moderation of the effect of adolescent onset cannibus use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase gene. Bio. Psych. 57: 1117–1127. 9. RUTTER, M. Implications of resilience concepts for
  • 78. scientific understanding. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. This volume. 10. GREENBERG, M.T., M.L. SPELTZ & M. DEKLYEN. 1993. The role of attachment in the early development of disruptive behavior problems. Dev. Psychopath. 5: 191–213. 148 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES 11. LUTHAR, S.S. 1993. Annotation: methodological and conceptual issues on research on childhood resilience. Pediatric Annals 20: 501–506. 12. MASTEN, A.S. Competence and resilience in development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. This volume. 13. RIGGS, N.R. & M.T. GREENBERG. 2004. The role of neuro-cognitive models in prevention research. In The Science, Treatment, and Prevention of Antiso- cial Behaviors (Vol. 2). D. Fishbein, Ed.: Civic Research Institute. Kingston, NJ. 14. LURIA, A.R. 1966. Higher Cortical Functions in Man. Basic Books. New York. 15. RAINE, A. 2002. Annotation: the role of prefrontal deficits, low autonomic arousal, and early health factors in the development of antisocial and aggressive behavior
  • 79. in children. J. Child Psych. Psy. 43: 417–434. 16. KLINGBERG, T., E. FERNELL, P.J. OLESEN, et al. 2005. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adol. Psy. 44: 177–186. 17. CASEY, B.J., N. TOTTENHAM, C. LISTEN, et al. 2005. Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cog. Sci. 9: 104–110. 18. LEWIS, M. Behavioral differences in aggressive children linked with neural mech- anisms of emotional regulation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. This volume. 19. PETERSEN, B.S. 2003. Conceptual, methodological, and statistical challenges in brain imaging studies of developmentally based psychopathologies. Dev. Psy- chopath. 15: 811–832. 20. BLAIR, C. 2002. School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobi- ological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. Am. Psych. 57: 111–127. 21. GUNNAR, M.R. & D.M. VAZQUEZ. 2001. Low cortisol and a flattening of ex- pected daytime rhythm: potential indices of risk in human development. Dev. Psychopath. 13: 515–538.
  • 80. 22. SHAYWITZ, B.A., S.E. SHAYWITZ, B.A. BLACHMAN, et al. 2004. Development of left ocipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention. Bio. Psy. 55: 926–933. 23. FISHER, P., M.R. GUNNAR, P. CHAMBERLAIN, et al. 2000. Preventive intervention for maltreated preschool children: impact on children’s behavior, neuroendocrine activity, and foster parent functioning. J. Am. Acad. Child Adol. Psy. 39: 1356– 1364. 24. DAVIDSON, R.J., J. KABAT-ZINN, J. SCHUMACHER, et al. 2003. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psycho. Med. 65: 564–570. 25. FISHBEIN, D., C. HYDE, D. ELDRETH, et al. 2006. Neurocognitive skills moder- ate urban male adolescent responses to preventive intervention materials. Drug Alcohol Depend. 82: 47–60. 26. PIZZGALLI, D.A., J.B. NITSCHKE, T.R. OAKES, et al. 2001. Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor of degree of treatment response in major depression: evidence from brain electrical tomography analysis. Am. J. Psy. 158: 405–415. 27. BECHARA, A. 2005. Decision making, impulse control, and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 8:
  • 81. 1458–1463. 28. OCHSNER, K.N. 2004. Current directions in social cognitive neuroscience. Cur. Op. Neurobiol. 14: 254–258. GREENBERG: PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS 149 29. BUSH, G., P. LUU & M.I. POSNER. 2000. Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cog. Sci. 4: 215–222. 30. DREVETS, W.C. & M.E. RAICHLE. 1998. Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cog. Emo. 12: 353–385. 31. STERZER, P., C. STADLER, A. KREBBS, et al. 2005. Abnormal neural responses to emotional visual stimuli in adolescents with conduct disorder. Biol. Psy. 57: 7–15. 32. OCHSNER, K.N., S.A. BUNGE, J.J. GROSS, et al. 2002. Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cog. Neurosci. 14: 1215– 1229. 33. PHAN, K.L., D.A. FITZGERALD, P.J. NATHAN, et al. 2005. Neural substrates for vol- untary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic
  • 82. resonance imaging study. Bio. Psy. 57: 210–219. 34. BEER, L.S., E.A. HEERY, D. KLETNER, et al. 2003. The regulatory function of self- conscious emotion: insights from patients with oribitofrontal damage. J. Pers. Soc. Psych. 85: 594–604. 35. FARRAH, M.J. & L.K. FELLOWS. 2005. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control. Brain 205: 788–799. 36. CHAMBERLAIN, S.R., U. MULLER, A.D. BLACKWELL, et al. 2006. Neurochemical modulation of responses inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science 311: 861–863. 37. GREENBERG, M.T. 1999. Attachment and psychopathology in childhood. In Hand- book of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver, Eds.: 469–496. Guilford. New York. 38. FISHBEIN, D. 2001. The importance of neurobiological research to the prevention of psychopathology. Prev. Sci. 1: 89–106. 39. KUSCHÉ, C.A. & M.T. GREENBERG. 1994. The PATHS (Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies) Curriculum. Channing-Bete. South Deerfield, MA. 40. GREENBERG, M.T. & C.A. KUSCHÉ. 1993. Promoting Social and Emotional Devel-
  • 83. opment in Deaf Children: The PATHS Project. University of Washington Press. Seattle. 41. KUSCHÉ, C.A. & M.T. GREENBERG. 2006. Brain development and social emotional learning: an introduction for educators. In The Educator’s Guide to Emotional Intelligence and Academic Achievement. M. Elias & H. Arnold, Eds.: Corwin Press. Thousand Oaks, CA. 42. CONDUCT PROBLEMS PREVENTION RESEARCH GROUP. 1999. Initial impact of the Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems: II. Classroom effects. J Cons. Clin. Psych. 67: 648–657. 43. GREENBERG, M.T. & C.A. KUSCHÉ. 2002. Promoting Alternative Thinking Strate- gies. Blueprint for Violence Prevention (Book 10), Institute of Behavioral Sci- ences, University of Colorado. CO 44. KAM, C., M.T. GREENBERG & C.A. KUSCHÉ. 2004. Sustained effects of the PATHS curriculum on the social and psychological adjustment of children in special education. J. Emot. Behav. Disorders 12: 66–78. 45. RIGGS, N.R., M.T. GREENBERG, C.A. KUSCHÉ, et al. 2006. The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social- emotional prevention program in elementary school students: effects of the PATHS Curriculum. Prev.
  • 84. Sci. 7: 91–102. 150 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES 46. RIGGS, N.R., C. BLAIR & M.T. GREENBERG. 2003. Concurrent and 2-year longitu- dinal relations between executive function and the behavior of 1st and 2nd grade children. Child Neuropsych. 9: 267–276. 47. ACHENBACH, T.M. 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and 1991 Profile. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont. Burlington, VT. 48. RAVNKILDE, B., P. VIDEBECH, R. ROSENBERG, et al. 2002. Putative tests of frontal lobe function: a PET-study of brain activation during Stroop’s Test and verbal fluency. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24: 534–547. 49. BUNGE, S.A., N.M. DUDUKOVIC, M.E. THOMANSON, et al. 2002. Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence from fMRI. Neuron 33: 1–20. Enhancing Resilience in Children: A Proactive Approach Mary Karapetian Alvord and Judy Johnson Grados Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC
  • 85. Many clinical practitioners today are interested in helping children be more resilient. The authors briefly review the literature and identify protective factors that are related to or foster resilience in children. After discussing individual and family intervention strategies currently in use, the authors present a practical, proactive, resilience-based model that clinicians may use in a group intervention setting. The model entails interactive identification of protective factors with children, free play and behavioral rehearsal, training in relaxation and self-control techniques, practice in generalizing skills acquired, and active parent involvement. Implications of this group intervention model are discussed. Keywords: resilience, children, social skills, groups, cognitive– behavioral therapy Vala is a 16-year-old Russian girl who first came to our practice at age 6. Neglected as a child, separated from her younger sister, and taken to an orphanage following her mother’s suicide, she was adopted by an American family and came to this country. The tribulations she experienced were extreme. However, because of her intelligence, easygoing temperament, and other personal strengths, as well as loving support of family and friends and a positive school environment, she is functioning well today and appears to be successful and happy. Many children encounter fewer and less severe traumatic expe- riences than did Vala. Yet they do experience the inevitable stresses and adversities in life that may challenge their healthy development and successful functioning. In the past three decades,
  • 86. a group of children have been identified in the research who appear to have fared well despite exposure to severe adversity. These children, who have been referred to as stress-resistant, invulner- able, and, more recently, resilient, were found to possess certain strengths and to have benefited from protective influences that helped them to overcome adverse conditions and to thrive. As practitioners, we must understand what environmental factors place children at risk and what protective factors may be fostered to enhance and strengthen resilience in children. In this article we define resilience and provide an overview of the literature and recent advances that are guiding work in the field today. We then define and discuss protective factors linked with the resilience process. Finally, we offer clinical implications for individual, family, and group therapy with children. Definitions of Resilience The term resilience has been defined in many ways. Masten, Best, and Garmezy’s (1990) definition of resilience as “the process of, capacity for, or outcome of successful adaptation despite chal- lenging or threatening circumstances” (p. 426) is one of the more familiar and widely accepted in the field. Many definitions of resilience require specification of an identified risk or challenge to which an individual is subjected, followed by some defined mea- sure of positive outcome. However, controversy remains regarding what constitutes resilient behavior and how to best measure suc-
  • 87. cessful adaptation to adversity. Some have suggested that a resil- ient person must show positive outcomes across several aspects of his life over periods of time (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997). Further, resilience is not a one-dimensional, dichotomous attribute that persons either have or do not have (Reivich & Shatté, 2003). Rather, resilience implies the possession of multiple skills, in varying degrees, that help individuals to cope. For the purpose of this article, we define resilience broadly as those skills, attributes, and abilities that enable individuals to adapt to hardships, difficulties, and challenges. Although some attributes are biologically determined, we believe resilience skills can be strengthened as well as learned. Early Studies in Resilience Early clinical case descriptions spawned an interest in determin- ing why some children manage to cope with adversity whereas others succumb. One such case of a 14-year-old Swiss girl was described by Bleuler (1984; as cited in Anthony, 1987a). “Vreni,” in the absence of her mother (who was hospitalized with mental illness), raised her siblings, cared for her alcoholic and physically compromised father, and later reported having a happy marriage and contented life. Anthony’s description of “invulnerable” chil- dren (Anthony, 1987b) and Murphy and Moriarty’s “good copers” MARY KARAPETIAN ALVORD received her PhD from the
  • 88. University of Maryland in 1977. She is currently the director of the Group Therapy Center at Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC, an independent therapy practice in the Washington, DC, metropolitan area. Her interests are focused on resilience and social skill development of children and adoles- cents. JUDY JOHNSON GRADOS received her PsyD from Indiana State University in 1995. She currently practices with Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC. Her research interests include social skills training for children and resilience in children and adolescents. WE GRATEFULLY ACKNOWLEDGE Dr. W. Gregory Alvord, Dr. Patricia K. S. Baker, and Ms. Anne McGrath for their support and editorial comments. We also thank Mrs. Maria Manolatos for assistance in the preparation of the manuscript. CORRESPONDENCE CONCERNING THIS ARTICLE should be addressed to Mary Karapetian Alvord, Alvord, Baker & Associates, LLC, 11161 New Hamp- shire Avenue, Suite 307, Silver Spring, MD 20904. E-mail: [email protected] Professional Psychology: Research and Practice Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 36, No. 3, 238 –245 0735-7028/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7028.36.3.238 238
  • 92. na l u se o f t he in di vi du al u se r a nd is n ot to b e di ss em
  • 93. in at ed b ro ad ly . (Murphy & Moriarty, 1976) sparked early interest as well. Rutter’s Isle of Wight studies (Rutter, Tizard, Yule, Graham, & Whitmore, 1976), large-scale longitudinal undertakings such as Project Com- petence (Garmezy, Masten, & Tellegen, 1984; Masten, 2000), and Werner’s now four-decade-long study of high-risk infants born into poverty on the Hawaiian island of Kauai (Werner, 1993; Werner & Smith, 2001) have helped to lay the groundwork for what we know today. Recent Advances The emerging research in the area of resilience encompasses many areas. These include investigations of children from various cultures (Grotberg, 1995; Hart, Hofmann, Edelstein, & Keller, 1997), studies of the influences of biological mechanisms (Curtis
  • 94. & Cicchetti, 2003; Rutter, 2002), new findings in recovering to normal functioning in varying stages of development, research on the impact of internal challenges, and theories on the processes that promote resilience under nonrisk conditions. We consider the latter three areas below. Evidence that children can recover and develop normally comes from a recent study of Romanian children who experienced severe deprivation in orphanages and were later adopted into nurturing homes in the United Kingdom. At the time of adoption, most of these children showed substantial gross physical and cognitive lags in development. Later, when assessed at age 4, many of these adoptees showed significant “catch-up,” both physically and cog- nitively (Rutter & The English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team, 1998; Rutter, Pickles, Murray, & Eaves, 2001). Follow-up in Werner and Smith’s (2001) Kauai study revealed that even those individuals who were troubled as adolescents were able to change the course of their lives in dramatic ways by making wise choices and taking advantage of opportunities, for example, by continuing their education, learning new skills, joining the military, relocating to break ties with deviant peers, and choosing healthy life partners. Major longitudinal studies have followed the outcome of indi- viduals with learning disabilities and attention- deficit/hyperactiv-
  • 95. ity disorder (ADHD) to determine those factors that contribute to their resilience (Gerber, Ginsburg, & Reiff, 1990, and Spekman, Goldberg, & Herman, 1992, as cited in Katz, 1997; Werner & Smith, 2001). Studies reveal that resilient learning-disabled youth search for personal control over their lives, possess a strong desire to succeed, set goals, demonstrate high levels of persistence, and are willing to seek out and accept support. Resilient learning- disabled young adults report better ability to identify their suc- cesses and unique strengths, are more likely to report turning points in their lives as motivations to overcome their challenges, and show a stronger self-determination (Miller, 2002). Hechtman (1991; as cited in Katz, 1997), in a long-term prospective follow-up of young adults diagnosed with ADHD as children, found that the presence of an influential person in their lives who believed in them (e.g., parent, teacher, coach) was most signifi- cant. Effort is being put forth to better understand the risk and resilience processes in this population of youth (Murray, 2003). Masten (2001) challenged the notion that resilient children possess some rare and special qualities. She suggested that resil- ience stems from a healthy operation of basic human adaptational systems. If systems are intact, children should develop appropri- ately even if challenged. However, if children’s basic adaptational systems are impaired, prior to or following challenge, the risk
  • 96. for problems in development is increased. Protective Factors Protective factors are “influences that modify, ameliorate, or alter a person’s response to some environmental hazard that pre- disposes to a maladaptive outcome” (Rutter, 1985, p. 600). Pro- tective factors arise from within the child, from the family or extended family, and from the community (Werner, 1995). A child’s intelligence, success at making friends, and ability to regulate his behavior are examples of internal strengths that pro- mote resilience. Examples of external influences that enhance resilience are competent parents, friendships, support networks, and effective schools. Protective factors that help children successfully adapt and cope with life’s challenges must be viewed in the context of their individual cultures and developmental stages. The International Resilience Project (Grotberg, 1995) showed, for example, that faith operates as a stronger protective factor in some cultures than in others. In addition, children’s developmental and cognitive levels affect their ability to use various protective factors, as do internal and biological vulnerabilities such as ADHD and learning disabilities. While researchers forge ahead to examine the complex interplay between risk factors, protective factors, and prevention and inter- vention strategies, practitioners need to know what factors may be strengthened in children to further promote positive appropriate responses. Below we discuss six protective factors that appear
  • 97. to buffer against risk factors. These factors have been categorized in accordance with the accumulating resilience literature and our experience in clinical practice. These six categories are not mutu- ally exclusive. Many of the components described in one factor are related to components in other factors. For example, a child who can self-regulate is more apt to make friends and connect with others. A child who experiences academic success is likely to have higher self-esteem. The presence of several factors seems to en- hance performance in multiple arenas. Proactive Orientation Proactive orientation, that is, taking initiative in one’s own life and believing in one’s own effectiveness, has been identified as a primary characteristic defining resilience in the literature. Such terms as self-efficacy and self-esteem (Rutter, 1985), positive fu- ture expectations (Wyman, Cowen, Work, & Kerley, 1993), good coping (Murphy & Moriarty, 1976), primary and secondary con- trol coping (Thurber & Weisz, 1997), personal control (Walsh, 1998), problem solving (Werner, 1995), initiative (Wolin & Wolin, 1993), optimistic thinking (Seligman, 1995), and internal motiva- tion (Masten, 2001) have been identified as protective factors of resilience in studies across heterogeneous populations and envi- ronments. These terms mean that resilient individuals have a realistic, positive sense of self. They regard themselves as
  • 98. survi- vors (Wolin & Wolin, 1993). They feel that they can have an impact on their environment or situation, rather than just be pas- sive observers. They are hopeful about the future. They are con- fident in their ability to surmount obstacles (Werner, 1993), make use of resources and opportunities around them, and view hard- ships as “learning experiences” (Werner & Smith, 2001). Resilient 239SPECIAL SECTION: ENHANCING RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN T hi s do cu m en t i s co py ri gh te d
  • 101. ol el y fo r t he p er so na l u se o f t he in di vi du al u se r a nd
  • 102. is n ot to b e di ss em in at ed b ro ad ly . individuals take positive action in their lives, such as seeking mentors, pursuing educational opportunities, participating in ex- tracurricular activities, and choosing supportive mates (Werner, 1993; Werner & Smith, 2001). Those who possess a high degree of “perceived self-efficacy” are more likely to interpret successes as an indication of their capabilities (Bandura, Pastorelli, Bar-
  • 103. baranelli, & Caprara, 1999). Seligman (2002) pointed out that when people think adverse events are permanent and pervasive for long periods of time, they assume feelings of helplessness and hopelessness. In contrast, he noted, when they think that negative things are temporary, this attitude encourages resilience. Teaching children to help others is an effective way to promote responsibility, empathy, and self-esteem (Brooks, 1994; Werner, 1993). Giving of oneself in an effort to ease the plight of others, such as contributing time and effort at a soup kitchen, nursing home, hospital, and so forth, fosters resilience. Similarly, “required helpfulness” (Rachman, 1979) refers to the process that occurs when an individual is striving to overcome adversity and during the course of this pursuit is required to perform actions to help others in their personal times of need. Self-Regulation One of the most fundamental protective factors is success in developing self-regulation or self-control. Masten and Coatsworth (1998) define self-regulation as gaining control over attention, emotions, and behavior. If a child is able to modulate her emotions and behavior and can self-soothe or calm herself, she will most likely elicit positive attention from others (including parents) and will have healthy social relationships. She will more likely be independent and will be more able to put things in perspective. Easygoing temperament and good self-regulation have been iden- tified as protective factors in resilience (Buckner, Mezzacappa, &
  • 104. Beardslee, 2003; Eisenberg et al., 1997, 2003; Werner, 1993). Additionally, impulse control and delay of gratification are part of self-control. The ability to self-regulate also seems to be at the core of good interpersonal relationships and peer relationships (Rubin, Coplan, Fox, & Calkins, 1995), rule compliance (Feldman & Klein, 2003), reduced risk of depression and anxiety, and a host of other areas fundamental to successful adaptation and functioning. Common sense would dictate that positive emotionality should result in positive outcomes. Although this is true, recent research indicates that perhaps it is not positive or negative emotion per se that is the critical variable in adaptation, but the ability to regulate the emotion. In a longitudinal study of 5-year-olds, Rydell, Berlin, and Bohlin (2003) found that low regulation of positive emotions and exuberance was correlated with externalizing problem behav- iors and low levels of prosocial behavior, whereas high regulation of positive emotions and exuberance was associated with high levels of prosocial behavior. Proactive Parenting Children with at least one warm, loving parent or surrogate caregiver (grandparent, foster parent) who provides firm limits and
  • 105. boundaries (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998) are more likely to be resilient. They tend to be more compliant with their parents (Feld- man & Klein, 2003) and have better peer relationships (Contreras, Kerns, Weimer, Gentzler, & Tomich, 2000). A significant longi- tudinal study that began in 1959 has identified the authoritative parenting style as associated with “optimal competence” in chil- dren and adolescents (Baumrind, 1989). Authoritative parents are characterized as “responsive” and “demanding” (Baumrind, 1991). Responsive parents are warm, loving, and supportive and provide a cognitively stimulating environment. They are also demanding in that they apply rational, firm, and consistent, but not overbearing, control on their children and place high behavioral expectations on them (Baumrind, 1991). Eisenberg et al. (2003) found that mater- nal expression of positive emotion is related to children’s social competence and adjustment. Correspondingly, Rubin, Burgess, Dwyer, and Hastings (2003) found that dysregulated toddlers who experienced high levels of maternal negativity had a greater like- lihood of externalizing problem behaviors 2 years later than tod- dlers whose mothers showed low to average levels of negativity. Connections and Attachments The desire to belong and to form attachments with family and
  • 106. friends is considered a fundamental human need (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Multiple positive health and adjustment effects have been associated with a sense of belonging and attachments. It is also through supportive relationships that self-esteem and self- efficacy are promoted (Werner, 1993). Having social competence and having positive connections with peers, family, and prosocial adults are significantly related to children’s ability to adapt to life stressors (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). Resilient children also elicit positive attention from others (Werner, 1993). For children, the development of friendships and the ability to get along with peers individually and in groups is paramount. Friendships provide support systems that can foster emotional, social, and educational adjustment (Rubin, 2002). Being part of at least one best friendship may also improve children’s adjustment (Hartup & Stevens, 1997). Positive peer relationships have been shown to protect children during times of family crisis. Acceptance by a group lowers the risk of externalizing behavior problems (Criss, Pettit, Bates, Dodge, & Lapp, 2002). A child who has friends and is well liked is less likely to be bullied or otherwise victimized (Pellegrini, Bartini, & Brooks, 1999; Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 1998). Although possessing a strong social support network renders children less vulnerable to stress, depression, and externalizing problems, the availability of social supports cannot be taken for granted. “Social support is not a self-forming entity waiting around
  • 107. to buffer harried people against stressors” (Bandura et al., 1999, p. 259). An active process is involved, and children, like adults, need to create and maintain supportive relationships. Connections affect how one treats and responds to others and how they, in turn, respond, thus forming a reciprocal relationship. It is easier for people to respond with more positive feedback and affection to an easygoing child who is sensitive to others and shows good self- control than to a child who is impulsive, who overreacts to events, and whose emotions are not well regulated. Children who share, who are compliant with social rules, and who are positive with peers are more likely to sustain their relationships. School Achievement and Involvement, IQ, and Special Talents Schools give young people a chance to excel academically and socially. Educational aspirations (Tiet et al., 1998) and active 240 ALVORD AND GRADOS T hi s do cu m en t i
  • 112. engagement in academics (Morrison, Robertson, Laurie, & Kelly, 2002) have been associated with resilience in challenged youth. Although the reasons for this are not yet clear, a number of factors are likely to contribute. Encouragement from teachers fosters resilience through connections, as noted above. Further, involve- ment in extracurricular activities such as art, music, drama, special interest clubs, and sporting activities gives youth opportunities to participate in prosocial groups and achieve recognition for their efforts. A positive orientation toward school and school activities has been shown to protect against antisocial behavior (Jessor, Van Den Bos, Vanderryn, Costa, & Turbin, 1995). Cognitive ability has been found to be associated with resilience in children (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996). It also appears to exert a strong influence on other factors that contribute to resilience. For example, strong cognitive skills may allow youth to excel in school, as well as to make the most of educational opportunities and cultural experiences. Additionally, a commitment to school helps to counter the risk of violent behaviors (Department of Health and Human Services, 2001). Fostering competence in children is central to resilient outcome. Brooks and Goldstein (2001) believe each child possesses at least
  • 113. one small “island of competence,” or area that has the potential to be a source of pride or achievement. Fostering resilience in youth requires that parents highlight children’s areas of competence to help them experience a sense of accomplishment. They suggest involving children in daily activities that allow them to feel they are contributing to the world. Examples include assisting other children, acting as a school patrol, or helping an older neighbor. Community Community factors, including the availability of supportive relationships outside of the family (Masten, 2001; Werner, 1995), are also well documented as having a protective influence on children. Resilient youth form relationships with positive role models and elders outside of their own family (Wolin & Wolin, 1993). Youths often join clubs, teams, and other groups and frequently find mentors such as coaches, teachers, scout leaders, and other prosocial adults in their communities. Effective schools identify the needs of their students and address those needs with services, as well as by consideration of class size and curricula. Important elements of an effective community are environments and social structures that promote resilience. Early prevention and intervention programs, safety in neighborhoods, support services, recreational facilities and programs, accessibility to adequate health services, and economic opportunities for families have all been identified as protective factors (Thomlison, 1997). Religious