SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 140
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999Salas et al. / TEAM
BUILDING
THE EFFECT OF TEAM BUILDING
ON PERFORMANCE
An Integration
EDUARDO SALAS
Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division
DREW ROZELL
BRIAN MULLEN
Syracuse University
JAMES E. DRISKELL
Florida Maxima Corporation
In this article, meta-analytic integration of research examining
the effects of team building
on performance is reported. Overall, there was no significant
effect of team building on per-
formance. However, the effects of team building varied as a
function of the type of operation-
alization of performance: On objective measures of
performance, there was a nonsignificant
tendency for team building to decrease performance, whereas on
subjective measures of per-
formance, there was a significant, albeit small, tendency for
team building to increase per-
formance. Examination of the specific components of team
building revealed that interven-
tions emphasizing role clarification were more likely to increase
performance, whereas
interventions that emphasized goal setting, problem solving, or
interpersonal relations were
no more likely to render an increase or decrease in performance.
Finally, the effects of team
building decreased as a function of the size of the team. The
discussion considers implica-
tions of these effects of team building on performance.
Harry, you weren’t kidding when you said this Florida trip
would be
no vacation! Building a group like us into a high-performing
team
takes really hard work! But I wouldn’t have missed it for
worlds!
—Jack, a participant in a team-building intervention
(Shandler & Egan, 1996)
Team building, also called team development or group develop-
ment, is an extremely popular and common intervention. In fact,
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH, Vol. 30 No. 3, June 1999 309-
329
© 1999 Sage Publications, Inc.
309
310 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
Porras and Berg (1978) observed that team building was one of
the
most frequently used organization development interventions.
Enthusiastic reports can be found for the application of team
build-
ing to such diverse settings as hotel management organizations
(e.g., Beckhard, 1966), microwave cooking manufacturers
(George, 1977), metropolitan rapid transit authorities (Golem-
biewski & Kiepper, 1976), medical rehabilitation teams
(Halstead
et al., 1986), and nursing teams (Robinson-Kurpius & Keim,
1994).
A recent special issue of a scholarly journal was devoted to
applica-
tions of team building to sports teams (Hardy & Crace, 1997).
Also,
as conveyed in the epigram above, team-building interventions
are
evidently believed to lead to a substantive increase in team per-
formance. Shandler and Egan (1996) claim that by applying
princi-
ples of team building, “any group can transform itself . . . into
a
high-performing team” (p. x).
In spite of this popularity of the concept of team building, how-
ever, several reviewers (e.g., Buller, 1986; Woodman &
Sherwood,
1980) have observed that there is no conclusive evidence that
team
building renders an increase in team performance. Druckman
and
Bjork (1994) noted that the enthusiasm for these approaches
among
practitioners “is not matched by strong empirical support for
their
effect on team performance” (p. 125). Similar to this, Smither,
Houston, and McIntire (1996) concluded that “Research
findings
on the effectiveness of team building provide a complex mix of
results that make drawing firm conclusions difficult” (p. 324).
More than 20 years after Beer (1976) attempted to formalize the
notion of team building, some of the most fundamental
questions
about the effects of team building remain: Does team building
enhance performance? Why? Under what conditions? This
article
reports the results of a meta-analytic integration of previous
research examining the effects of team building on performance.
Several issues need to be addressed in examining the effect
of team building on performance. First, the significance and
AUTHORS’NOTE: The views expressed in this article are those
of the authors and do not re-
flect the official position of their organizations. Correspondence
concerning this article
should be addressed to Eduardo Salas, Naval Air Warfare
Center Training Systems Division,
Code 4961, 12350 Research Parkway, Orlando, FL 32826-3275.
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 311
magnitude—indeed, the very existence—of an effect of team
build-
ing on performance needs to be established. Second, differences
between objective and subjective indices of performance have
been
delineated in other domains, but there is no a priori
determination
of the extent to which team building would differentially affect
these two different ways of operationalizing performance.
Third,
the degree to which a given team-building intervention engages
dif-
ferent components of team-building might influence the
effective-
ness of the team-building intervention. Fourth, the effect of
team
size needs to be specified, particularly in light of recent
findings in
cognate areas indicating that group size exerts a considerable
effect
on other group phenomena. Finally, the effect of the duration of
team-building interventions on their effectiveness is of
consider-
able practical significance. Each of these considerations is
addressed in turn.
THE EXISTENCE OF A TEAM-BUILDING EFFECT
Team building has at its core the central notion that enlisting
the
participation of a group in planning and implementing change
will
be more effective than simply imposing change on the group
from
outside. The process by which the members of the team become
able to effectively participate in the targeted change requires
that
the team acquire new skills and perceptions. The various defini-
tions of team building that have been proposed in the research
lit-
erature resonate to these elements of participation and
acquisition
of new skills and perceptions. For example, Buller (1986)
defined
team building as a planned intervention facilitated by a third-
party
consultant that develops the problem-solving capacity and
solves
major problems of an intact work group. Woodman and
Sherwood
(1980) proposed that team building was designed to enhance
organ-
izational effectiveness by improving team operation through
devel-
oping problem-solving procedures and skills and increasing role
clarity (cf. Beer, 1976, 1980; DeMeuse & Liebowitz, 1981;
Dyer,
1977).
The effects of team building are often described in extremely
sanguinary terms. For example, the epigram at the beginning of
this
312 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
report is extracted from a primer on team building, entitled
VROOM! Turbo-Charged Team Building (Shandler & Egan,
1996).
Therein, the president of a high-tech research and development
firm uses a team-building intervention to rescue his foundering
company, ensure his promotion in the corporate hierarchy, and
even
save his failing marriage. It would seem that any intervention
capa-
ble of such impressive effects would have generated an
imposing
body of evidence supporting its effectiveness.
Unfortunately, a clear summary of the effects of team building
on performance apparently cannot be formulated from narrative
readings of previous research. Indeed, several narrative
literature
reviews have been conducted in this domain within the past two
decades, and the judgments rendered by these previous review
efforts have been inconclusive. Woodman and Sherwood (1980)
reviewed 30 papers and concluded that there were generally
posi-
tive results for most of the studies but that there was no clear-
cut
evidence to suggest that team building can improve
performance.
DeMeuse and Liebowitz (1981) reviewed 36 previous reports
and
similarly concluded that there were generally positive
descriptions
of the team-building interventions but that there was a dearth of
evi-
dence for beneficial effects of team building on performance.
Bul-
ler (1986) reviewed 9 studies, Sundstrom, DeMeuse, and Futrell
(1990) reviewed 13 studies, and Tannenbaum, Beard, and Salas
(1992) reviewed 17 studies, and (like each of the previous
reviews)
each of these narrative reviews concluded that team building
was
described in encouraging ways by most of the studies reviewed,
but
there was a general lack of definitive, compelling evidence for
the
beneficial effects of team building on performance.
It is interesting to consider the nature of the body of studies
con-
sidered in many previous narrative reviews of the effects of
team
building. First, there was a remarkable lack of convergence
among
these reviews regarding which studies to include in a review of
team-building interventions. Of the 69 reports examined in at
least
one of these narrative reviews, on average only 4 studies (or
5.8%)
were included by both of any two reviews. The point is that
more
than 90% of the literature summarized in each of these narrative
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 313
reviews of team building was referred to in none of the other
narra-
tive reviews. This stunning lack of convergence on precisely
what
constitutes an includable study of team building is in part a
reflec-
tion of the complexity of the notion of team building (to be
eluci-
dated further).
It is useful to consider a few other intriguing facets of the
studies
considered in previous narrative reviews of the effects of team
building. The most common type of report examined in these
previ-
ous reviews was the case study (20 out of 69 studies or 29%),
describing the application of a team-building intervention in
some
organizational setting with no empirical examination of the
effec-
tiveness of the intervention (e.g., Beckhard, 1966; George,
1977).
These case studies, although interesting and illuminating in an
anecdotal way, can hardly be said to provide definitive,
conclusive
evidence in support of team building. The second most common
type of report examined in these previous reviews reported the
effects of team building on some affective, nonperformance out-
come measure (15 out of 69 studies or 22%). This tendency for
stud-
ies of team-building interventions to focus on affective
responses to
the intervention while excluding genuine performance results of
the intervention was recognized in some of the previous
narrative
reviews (notably Buller, 1986; DeMeuse & Liebowitz, 1981;
Woodman & Sherwood, 1980). Only 6 of the 69 studies
considered
in the previous narrative reviews provided a precise statistical
test
of the effects of a team-building intervention on performance.
In sum, we cannot determine whether there is a beneficial effect
of team building on performance based on previous narrative
perusals of this research domain. Part of the problem lies in the
ambiguity of what precisely is team building and what studies
should be included in an effort to integrate the effect of team
build-
ing on performance. Part of the problem is that the team-
building
literature includes a variety of case studies, nonempirical
reports,
and tests of various types of outcome measures. Therefore, the
first
goal of the present effort was to establish the empirical
significance
and the strength of the effects of team building on performance.
314 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
COMPONENTS OF TEAM BUILDING
Beer (1976, 1980), Dyer (1977), and Buller (1986) have dis-
cussed four current models of team building: Goal setting,
interper-
sonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification. Goal
set-
ting emphasizes the setting of objectives and the development
of
individual and team goals. Team members exposed to a goal-
setting team-building intervention are supposed to become
involved in action planning to identify ways to achieve those
goals.
Interpersonal relations emphasizes an increase in team work
skills,
such as mutual supportiveness, communication, and sharing of
feelings. Team members exposed to an interpersonal relations
team-building intervention are supposed to develop trust in one
another and confidence in the team. Problem solving
emphasizes
the identification of major problems in the team. Team members
exposed to a problem-solving team-building intervention are
sup-
posed to become involved in action planning for the solution of
those problems and for implementing and evaluating those solu-
tions. Role clarification emphasizes increased communication
among team members regarding their respective roles within the
team. Team members exposed to a role-clarification team-
building
intervention are supposed to achieve better understanding of
their
and others’ respective roles and duties within the team.
These models might be best thought of as components of any
given team-building intervention. That is, any team-building
inter-
vention might engage any or all of these various components in
varying degrees. For example, Wegenast’s (1983) team-building
intervention in child protective service workers involved a rela-
tively high emphasis on problem solving, but relatively low
empha-
sis on interpersonal relations. Alternatively, Paul and Gross’s
(1981) team-building intervention in the communications and
elec-
trical division of city government involved a relatively high
empha-
sis on role clarification but relatively low emphasis on goal
setting.
This diversity in team-building interventions represents one of
the major challenges to previous efforts to make sense of the re-
search literature on team building. In the present effort, we
viewed
this diversity among team-building interventions as a unique
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 315
opportunity to gauge the relative impact of the four components
of
team building delineated above (goal setting, interpersonal rela-
tions, problem solving, and role clarification). To date, there is
no
evidence regarding which of these components of team building
are
more critical to the putative effects of team building on
perform-
ance. Therefore, another important goal of the present effort is
to
determine the relative contributions of these components of
team
building to the team building–performance effect.
CONTRIBUTIONS OF OPERATIONALIZATION OF
PERFORMANCE
Two broad sets of operationalizations of performance typify
research in the area of team performance. On one hand,
objective
indicators of performance include direct measures of countable
behaviors, like the number of units sold or the tons of ore
mined. On
the other hand, subjective indicators of performance include
ratings
of the performance or effectiveness of the team. Two separate
meta-analyses have reported relatively modest correlations
between objective and subjective measures of performance
(Bom-
mer, Johnson, Rich, Podsakoff, & MacKenzie, 1995, r = .389;
Heneman, 1986, r = .27). This led Heneman (1986) to conclude
that
objective and subjective measures of performance are not
substitut-
able, and that “when reviews of the literature are conducted,
results
should be grouped by the type of performance criteria” (p. 820).
There is no a priori expectation for the effect of team building
on
performance to be greater for one or the other of these two
opera-
tionalizations. However, the relatively modest correlations
previ-
ously observed between objective and subjective measures of
per-
formance led us to examine the effect of team building on
performance separately for each type of performance measure.
CONTRIBUTIONS OF TEAM SIZE
The size of social groups has been shown to affect the
magnitude
of several other group phenomena, including the participation-
leadership effect (Mullen, Salas, & Driskell, 1989), the ingroup-
316 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
bias effect (Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992), the cohesiveness-
performance effect (Mullen & Copper, 1994), the groupthink
effect
(Mullen, Anthony, Salas, & Driskell, 1994), and the effect of
stress
inoculation training on performance (Saunders, Driskell,
Johnston,
& Salas, 1996) (for a discussion, see Mullen, 1991). Larger
groups
tend to encourage deindividuation among group members (e.g.,
Mullen, 1987). Moreover, it is well established that as group
size
increases, members’liking for the group (e.g., Indik, 1965) and
per-
formance (Mullen, 1987) tend to decrease. In larger groups,
group
members would be expected to be less engaged in the group,
less
open to the team-building intervention, and less likely to
perform
with any concern for excellence. Thus, we would expect that the
effect of team building on performance would be weaker in
larger
teams and stronger in smaller teams.
In spite of the consistent tendency for the size of the group to
affect several similar group phenomena, there has been no
consid-
eration to date of whether team size moderates the effect of
team
building on performance. Therefore, another important goal of
the
present effort was to determine the extent to which team size
affects
the impact of team building on performance.
CONTRIBUTIONS OF DURATION OF
THE TEAM-BUILDING INTERVENTION
The duration of team-building interventions varies considerably
in the extant literature, from 2 days (Smith, 1976) to 8 months
(Bul-
ler & Bell, 1986). Common sense might suggest that
interventions
of longer duration might exert greater effects: If a little of the
inter-
vention works, then a lot of the intervention might work even
more.
However, interventions of longer duration have paradoxically
been
found to render weaker effects in such diverse areas as mental
prac-
tice effects (Driskell, Copper, & Moran, 1994), self-help
treatment
groups (Gould & Clum, 1993), and group-work interventions
with
depressed older persons (Gorey & Cryns, 1991). However, the
nature of the relationship between the duration of team building
and the effect of team building on performance remains unclear.
Therefore, another goal of the present effort is to determine the
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 317
effect of the duration of the team-building intervention on the
team
building–performance effect.
A META-ANALYTIC INTEGRATION
In an effort to provide a clearer picture of the effects of team
building on performance, a meta-analytic integration (Mullen,
1989; Mullen & Rosenthal, 1985; Rosenthal, 1991) was
conducted.
The goals of this effort were (a) to provide a precise summary
of the
significance, the strength, and the direction of the effects of
team
building on performance; (b) to examine different magnitudes of
this effect of team building on performance as a function of the
operationalization of performance; (c) to examine the possible
moderation of the effect of team building on performance by the
four components of team building (goal setting, interpersonal
rela-
tions, problem solving, and role clarification); (d) to determine
whether the effect of team building on performance would
decrease
as a function of team size; and (e) to scrutinize the effects of
the
duration of the team-building intervention on the effects of team
building.
PROCEDURE
Using all of the standard literature search techniques, an
exhaus-
tive search was conducted for studies testing the effect of team-
building interventions on performance. First, on-line computer
searches were conducted, using the keywords team(s), group(s),
or
crew(s) and building, development, performance, productivity,
or
effects. These computer searches were supplemented by
ancestry
approach and descendency approach searches, correspondence
with researchers active in this domain (the invisible college),
and
manually examining the past 30 years of social psychology,
applied
psychology, and management journals (see Mullen, 1989, for a
dis-
cussion of literature search techniques). Second, we reviewed
each
of the team building studies included in the previous narrative
318 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
reviews by Woodman and Sherwood (1980), DeMeuse and Lie-
bowitz (1981), Buller (1986), Sundstrom, DeMeuse, and Futrell
(1990), and Tannenbaum, Beard, and Salas (1992). Any studies
that were available as of March 1998 were eligible for inclusion
in
this integration.
Studies were included if they met the following criteria: Team
members in the studies had to be adolescents or adults not
sampled
from abnormal populations. Studies had to report (or allow the
reconstruction of) a precise test of the effect of a team-building
intervention either on an objective measure of performance
(e.g.,
productivity data) or a subjective measure of performance (e.g.,
rat-
ing of team effectiveness). Case studies were not included (e.g.,
“Teamwork Through Conflict,” 1971), nor were T-group (e.g.,
Argy-
ris, 1962) or managerial grid (e.g., Blake & Mouton, 1966)
inter-
ventions. The effect of these criteria for inclusion was to focus
on
the effects of team building on performance in studies that were
optimally homogeneous in methodological terms. Hypothesis
tests
were coded as having a positive direction of effect if team
building
increased performance, and they were coded as having a
negative
direction of effect if team building decreased performance.
These selection criteria rendered a total of 11 studies (Buller &
Bell, 1986; Eden, 1986; Friedlander, 1967; Howard, 1979; Kim-
berly & Nielsen, 1975; Paul & Gross, 1981; Smith, 1976;
Wakeley &
Shaw, 1965; Wegenast, 1983; Wexler, 1990; Woodman, 1978).
These 11 articles yielded 16 separate tests of the effects of team
building on performance, representing the responses of 2,806
team
members in 307 teams.1 Attesting to the thoroughness of the
litera-
ture search, the included reports spanned a period of more than
30
years, and 4 of the 11 reports (36%) were unpublished
dissertations.
In addition to the requisite statistical information, each
hypothe-
sis test was coded for seven predictors. The nature of the
operation-
alization of performance (objective vs. subjective), the size of
the
team, and the duration of the team-building intervention were
all
directly coded from each report by two judges with perfect
reliabil-
ity. In addition, each team-building intervention procedure was
also scored for the extent to which that intervention would
engage
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 319
each of the components of team building. To be specific, two
judges
independently rated each team-building intervention on a scale
from 0 (low) to 100 (high) for goal setting, interpersonal
relations,
problem solving, and role clarification (as defined above).
Acceptably high levels of interjudge reliability were obtained
for
each of these components: Goal setting (interjudge reliability r
=
.693, Spearman-Brown Effective Reliability R = .819), interper-
sonal relations (r = .822, R = .902), problem solving (r = .945,
R =
.972), and role clarification (r = .580, R = .734). The extent to
which
each intervention engaged each component of team building was
set equal to the average of the judges’ ratings for each
component.
The hypothesis tests included in this meta-analysis, along with
the
corresponding statistical information and predictors for each
hypothesis test, are presented in Table 1.
RESULTS
General effect. Overall, there was a nonsignificant, z = 0.132, p
=
.45, negligible, ZFisher = 0.007, r = .007, effect of team-
building inter-
ventions on performance. Thus, the weight of available evidence
fails to substantiate the effectiveness of team-building interven-
tions to increase performance.2
Objective versus subjective measures of performance. For the k
= 8 hypothesis tests that used objective measures of
performance,
the effect of team building on performance was a
nonsignificant, z =
–0.511, p = .70, negative, ZFisher = –0.040, r = –.040. For the k
= 8
hypothesis tests that used subjective measures of performance,
there was a significant, z = 2.589, p = .004, albeit still weak,
ZFisher =
0.140, r = .139, positive effect of team building on performance.
These two effects were significantly different from one another,
z =
3.398, p = .0003. Thus, although subjective reports indicate that
team building appears to increase performance, objective
indica-
tors of performance reveal no genuine effect of team building
on
performance.
TA
B
LE
1
:
S
tu
di
es
In
cl
ud
ed
in
th
e
M
et
a-
A
na
ly
si
s
D
u
ra
tio
n
S
tu
d
y
S
ta
tis
tic
r
N
D
O
E
b
M
e
a
su
re
S
iz
e
(i
n
d
a
ys
)
G
S
IR
P
S
R
C
B
ul
le
r
an
d
B
el
l (
19
86
)
t(
16
)
=
0
.3
99
–.
01
0
6/
18
–
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
3
24
0
83
.5
32
.5
82
.5
10
.0
t(
16
)
=
1
.5
84
.3
68
4/
8
+
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
2
24
0
87
.5
5.
0
10
.0
20
.0
t(
16
)
=
0
.0
05
–.
00
1
6/
18
–
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
3
24
0
25
.0
32
.5
90
.0
10
.0
E
de
n
(1
98
6)
t(
29
)
=
1
.2
52
–.
22
6
7/
22
0
–
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
31
.5
3
40
.0
77
.5
29
.0
35
.0
F
rie
dl
an
de
r
(1
96
7)
F
(1
, 8
8)
=
3
.9
8
.2
08
12
/9
1
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
7.
6
5
22
.5
70
.0
90
.0
20
.0
H
ow
ar
d
(1
97
9)
r(
17
)
=
.0
40
.0
40
2/
19
+
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
9
14
30
.0
90
.0
40
.0
10
.0
K
im
be
rle
y
an
d
N
ie
ls
en
(
19
75
) t(
43
)
=
0
.7
1
–.
10
8
45
/9
00
–
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
20
2
25
.0
70
.0
80
.0
15
.0
t(
43
)
=
0
.3
8
–.
05
8
45
/9
00
–
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
20
2
25
.0
70
.0
80
.0
15
.0
P
au
l a
nd
G
ro
ss
(
19
81
)
F
(1
, 3
5)
=
3
6.
35
.7
14
37
/9
0
+
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
2.
5
3
25
.0
30
.0
70
.0
75
.0
S
m
ith
(
19
76
)
t(
35
)
=
6
.5
52
.7
42
12
/3
6
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
3
2
60
.0
70
.0
65
.0
80
.0
W
ak
el
ey
a
nd
S
ha
w
(
19
65
)
t(
5)
=
8
.4
28
.9
67
1/
6
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
7
14
20
.0
60
.0
60
.0
85
.0
W
eg
en
as
t (
19
83
)
F
(1
, 1
0)
=
3
.5
4
.5
11
2/
12
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
6
42
27
.5
20
.0
75
.0
60
.0
W
ex
le
r
(1
99
0)
t(
80
)
=
2
.0
68
.2
25
22
/1
32
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
6
90
22
.5
32
.5
75
.0
20
.0
t(
80
)
=
2
.0
04
.2
19
18
/1
08
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
6
90
15
.0
90
.0
75
.0
20
.0
W
oo
dm
an
(
19
78
)
t(
64
)
=
1
.1
10
–.
13
7
44
/1
24
–
O
bj
ec
tiv
e
3
14
57
.5
27
.5
60
.0
45
.0
t(
64
)
=
1
.6
99
.2
08
44
/1
24
+
S
ub
je
ct
iv
e
3
14
57
.5
27
.5
60
.0
45
.0
N
O
T
E
: D
O
E
=
D
ire
ct
io
n
of
E
ffe
ct
, G
S
=
G
oa
l S
et
tin
g,
IR
=
In
te
rp
er
so
na
l R
el
at
io
ns
, P
S
=
P
ro
bl
em
S
ol
vi
ng
, R
C
=
R
ol
e
C
la
rif
ic
at
io
n.
a.
T
he
fi
rs
t n
um
be
r
re
pr
es
en
ts
th
e
nu
m
be
r
of
te
am
s;
th
e
se
co
nd
n
um
be
r
re
pr
es
en
ts
th
e
nu
m
be
r
of
te
am
m
em
be
rs
.
b.
T
he
p
os
iti
ve
s
ig
n
in
di
ca
te
s
th
at
te
am
b
ui
ld
in
g
in
cr
ea
se
d
pe
rf
or
m
an
ce
; t
he
n
eg
at
iv
e
si
gn
in
di
ca
te
s
th
at
te
am
b
ui
ld
in
g
de
cr
ea
se
d
pe
rf
or
m
an
ce
.
320
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 321
Contributions of components of team building. The mean judged
level of each component of team building was used to examine
the
contributions of the various components of team building. The
team building–performance effect did not vary as a function of
the
level of goal setting engaged by the team-building intervention,
(r =
–.164, z = 1.165, p = .12). In other words, the extent to which a
given
team-building intervention engaged the component of goal
setting
neither increased or decreased the magnitude of the effect of
team
building on performance. Similar to this, the extent to which a
given
team-building intervention engaged the component of interper-
sonal relations (r = –.056, z = 0.485, p = .3138) or problem
solving
(r = –.049, z = 0.342, p = .37) neither increased or decreased the
magnitude of the effect of team building on performance. The
only
component of team building to significantly predict the effect of
team building on performance was role clarification (r = .759, z
=
5.533, p < .001). In other words, the more a given team-building
intervention engaged the component of role clarification, the
stronger the magnitude of the effect of team building on
performance.
It should be noted that there is no moderation of these patterns
for components of team building by the operationalization of
per-
formance. That is, for both objective and subjective indices
respec-
tively, there was no effect of goal setting (r = –.060, z = 0.182,
p = .43
and r = –.110, z = 0.972, p = .17), interpersonal (r = –.382, z =
1.258,
p = .10 and r = –.035, z = 0.325, p = .37), or problem solving
(r = –.305, z = 0.780, p = .22 and r = .088, z = 1.298, p = .10).
How-
ever, for both objective and subjective indices respectively,
there
was a significant effect of role clarification (r = .712, z = 4.565,
p < .001 and r = .752, z = 4.594, p < .001).
Contributions of team size. Overall, the effects of team building
on performance decreased as a function of the size of the team,
r =
–344, z = 5.965, p < .001. This inverse effect of team size on
the
effect of team building held for both objective measures of per-
formance (r = –.400, z = 2.383, p = .009) and subjective
measures of
performance (r = –.407, z = 7.796, p < .001). Thus, whatever
322 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
positive effect team building may have is most likely to emerge
in
small groups.
Contributions of duration of the team-building intervention.
Over-
all, there was a marginal tendency for the effects of team
building
on performance to decrease as a function of duration of the
team-
building intervention, r = –.197, z = 1.452, p = .07. This inverse
effect of duration on the effect of team building did not obtain
for
objective measures of performance (r = –.056, z = 0.194, p =
.42),
but did obtain for subjective measures of performance (r = –
.208,
z = 2.562, p = .005). Thus, the negligible effects of team
building on
objective indices of performance are not influenced by the
duration
of the team-building intervention, but the appearance of benefits
from team building on more subjective measures seems to
dissipate
for more lengthy team-building interventions.
DISCUSSION
One of the most important results to emerge from the present
analyses is the documentation of the overall nonsignificant and
negligible effect of team building on performance. These results
suggest that the enthusiastic testimonials on behalf of team-
build-
ing interventions should be interpreted with caution. The overall
magnitude of the effect of team building on performance of r =
.007
indicates that approximately .005% of the variability in a team’s
performance might be accounted for by knowing whether the
team
had gone through a team-building intervention. In other words,
approximately 99% of the variability in a team’s performance is
attributable to factors other than whether the team had gone
through a team-building intervention. Relative to some rather
bold
proclamations regarding the beneficial effects of team building
interventions (e.g., Shandler & Egan, 1996), future summaries
of
the effects of team building on performance should be a bit
more
modest.
When the data were disaggregated into objective performance
indicators versus subjective performance indicators, it was
shown
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 323
that objective performance indicators rendered functionally no
effect of team building on performance, whereas subjective per-
formance indicators rendered a significant, albeit still weak,
effect
of team building on performance. This difference between
results
for objective performance indicators and subjective performance
indicators is not surprising, given the modest convergence
previ-
ously observed between these different operationalizations
(Bom-
mer et al., 1995; Heneman, 1986). The analyses for these two
dif-
ferent performance indicators are even more sobering than the
overall lack of beneficial effects of team building: Not only is
there
an overall lack of beneficial effects of team building, but there
is not
even much of a measurement-specific benefit of team-building
interventions of which to speak.
The examination of the contributions of the four components of
team building yielded a very intriguing result: The team
building-
performance effect did not vary as a function of the level of
goal set-
ting, interpersonal relations, or problem solving engaged by the
team-building intervention. It was only role clarification that
seemed to make a genuine contribution to the effect of team
build-
ing on performance, and this pattern emerged for both of the
opera-
tionalizations of performance. This significant effect for role
clari-
fication is entirely consistent with evidence from other quarters
regarding the importance of role clarification and role
ambiguity in
team performance. For example, Abramis’s (1994) recent meta-
analysis reported that the general weight of evidence indicates
that
role ambiguity exerts a negative impact on performance. The
pres-
ent results suggest that future research on team building and
per-
formance should further examine the positive effects of the role
clarification component of team building.
The results for team size on the effects of team building are
con-
sistent with patterns documented in other group phenomena
(e.g.,
Mullen et al., 1989; Mullen et al., 1992; Mullen et al., 1994;
Mullen &
Copper, 1994; Mullen, 1991). For years, researchers studying
group and team performance have searched for the magic
numbers
that could summarize the effects of the group on the individual.
It is
interesting to note that Old (1946) long ago proposed that the
opti-
mum size for a group was 0.7 persons. This tongue-in-cheek
324 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
proposal, although somewhat limiting in terms of implementing
group training, keeps with the results of the present analyses:
As the
size of the team increases, the effectiveness of team-building
inter-
ventions decreases. Optimal benefit from a team-building
interven-
tion seems most likely to be obtained with relatively small
teams.
The results for the effects of duration of the team-building
inter-
vention are more complex. Although there was a marginal ten-
dency overall for longer team-building interventions to render
weaker effects, this pattern was actually due only to the
influence of
duration on the effects of team building on subjective indicators
of
performance. Thus, although shorter duration team-building
inter-
ventions might be of some benefit to subjective performance
mea-
sures, there does not seem to be any difference between shorter
and
longer team-building interventions on objective performance
measures.
Of course, there are limitations to our analysis and to the impli-
cations that can be drawn from it. As noted earlier, the term
team
building has been defined somewhat broadly in the literature to
include various types of group interventions that are
conceptually
quite dissimilar. As Lipsey and Wilson (1993) noted in a recent
review of the general efficacy of psychological treatments, there
are often gray areas as to what constitutes a certain
intervention.
Our approach was to cast a very narrow net, adopting a precise
approach to defining team building and to including studies in
the
analysis. This decision resulted in a small, tightly-focused
database
of studies, yet one that we feel is highly representative of this
research domain. Second, this analysis allowed us to examine
sev-
eral theoretically and practically interesting moderators of the
effect of team building on performance, such as team size and
dura-
tion of training. There are other potentially informative modera-
tors, such as the type or task (cf. Tannenbaum et al., 1992), that
the
literature did not allow us to examine. Further primary level
research is needed to examine other conditions under which
team
building may be more effective. Finally, there are
methodological
limitations of this analysis. There were no studies in this
database
that reported reliability coefficients for the performance
measures
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 325
used, so adjustments for attenuation were not attempted. The
fail-
ure of the source information to report reliabilities for
performance
measures is not unique to this particular meta-analysis, but it
repre-
sents a common limitation of the research literature.
In summary, the present results indicate that overall there is no
significant effect of team building on performance. Moreover,
what
little benefits team building might exert on performance are
likely
to be seen in interventions that emphasize role clarification in
smaller groups (and perhaps, in interventions that are more
brief).
Future research might be directed toward further examinations
of
the relative contributions of the four components of team
building
and to scrutinizing the differential effects of team building on
dif-
ferent indicators of performance. Finally, we note that in
examining
team building almost 20 years ago, Scherer (1979) posed the
ques-
tion, “How can something that feels so good not be
worthwhile?”
(p. 335). Subsequent reviews of team-building research
cautioned
that evidence of an effect of team building on performance was
“inconclusive” (Buller, 1986, p. 147), “unsubstantiated” (Wood-
man & Sherwood, 1980, p. 166), “equivocal” (Tannenbaum et
al.,
1992, p. 146), and “mixed” (Sundstrom et al., 1990, p. 128).
The
present analysis of the effect of team building on performance
pro-
vides an empirical basis for the caution voiced by these
reviewers.
NOTES
1. The included studies reported varying numbers of hypothesis
tests, ranging from one
per study (e.g., Eden, 1986) to three per study (e.g., Buller &
Bell, 1986). In the meta-
analysis reported, each hypothesis test was treated as an
independent observation. This
assumption of independence is patently false. For example, each
of the three hypothesis tests
included in Buller and Bell (1986) was derived from the same
subject population at the same
time. However, without making this assumption of
nonindependence, one would be forced to
select the best hypothesis test from a study such as Buller and
Bell or to pool the results from
the reported hypothesis tests into a single test. In the present
context, these alternatives seem
even more arbitrary and capricious than the present assumption
of independence. The effects
of this assumption of independence are examined later.
2. As indicated in Note 1, the assumption that each of the 16
hypothesis tests represented
an independent observation is false. However, it can be seen
that such an assumption does not
seem to render a distorted summary of this research domain.
Consider the results of a
326 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
supplementary meta-analysis of wholly independent effects, in
which multiple hypothesis
tests obtained from a single study were combined into a single
test. This heavy-handed solu-
tion precludes the examination of the effects of any of the
moderators considered above, but
it does eliminate the problem of nonindependence. This
produced 11 distinct, wholly inde-
pendent hypothesis tests, one from each includable study. The
results of this supplemental
meta-analysis revealed the same patterns reported previously:
There was a nonsignificant,
z = 0.440, p = .33, negligible, ZFisher = 0.032, r = .032, effect
of team-building interventions
on performance. These results indicate that the degree of
distortion engendered by the
assumption of independence of the original 16 hypothesis tests
is (at worst) tolerable.
REFERENCES
Abramis, D. J. (1994). Work role ambiguity, job satisfaction,
and job performance: Meta-
analyses and review. Psychological Reports, 75, 1411-1433.
Argyris, C. (1962). Interpersonal competence and organizational
behavior. Homewood, IL:
Irwin.
Beckhard, R. (1966). An organization improvement program in
a decentralized organiza-
tion. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 2, 3-25.
Beer, M. (1976). The technology of organization development.
In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.),
Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 937-
994). Chicago: Rand
McNally.
Beer, M. (1980). Organization change and development: A
systems view. Glenview, IL:
Scott, Foresman.
Bigelow, R. C. (1971). Changing classroom interaction through
organization development.
In R. A. Schmuck & M. B. Miles (Eds.), Organization
development in schools. Palo Alto,
CA: National Press.
Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. (1966). Some effects of
managerial grid training on union and
management attitudes towards supervision. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 2,
387-400.
Bommer, W. H., Johnson, J. L., Rich, G. A., Podsakoff, P. M.,
& MacKenzie, S. B. (1995).
On the interchangeability of objective and subjective measures
of employee perform-
ance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 48, 587-605.
Buller, P. F. (1986). The team building–task performance
relation: Some conceptual and
methodological refinements. Group and Organization Studies,
11, 147-168.
Buller, P. F., & Bell, C. H. (1986). Effects of team building and
goal setting on productivity:
A field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 305-
328.
DeMeuse, K. P., & Liebowitz, S. J. (1981). An empirical
analysis of team building research.
Group and Organization Studies, 6, 357-378.
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C. M., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental
practice enhance perform-
ance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481-492.
Druckman, D., & Bjork, R. A. (Eds.). (1994). Learning,
remembering, believing: Enhancing
human performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Dyer, W. G. (1977). Team building: Issues and alternatives.
Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.
Eden, D. (1986). Team development: Quasi-experimental
confirmation among combat com-
panies. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 133-146.
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 327
Friedlander, F. (1967). The impact of organizational training
laboratories upon effectiveness
of ongoing work groups. Personnel Psychology, 20, 289-307.
George, W. W. (1977). Task teams for rapid growth. Harvard
Business Review, 55, 71-80.
Golembiewski, R. T., & Kiepper, A. (1976). MARTA: Toward
an effective open giant. Public
Administration Review, 36, 46-60.
Gorey, K. M., & Cryns, A. G. (1991). Group work as
interventive modality with the older
depressed client: A meta-analytic review. Journal of
Gerontological Social Work, 16,
137-157.
Gould, R. A., & Clum, G. A. (1993). A meta-analysis of self-
help treatment approaches.
Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 169-186.
Halstead, L. S., Rintala, D. H., Kanellos, M., Griffin, B.,
Higgins, L., Rheinecker, N., White-
side, W., & Healy, J. E. (1986). The innovative rehabilitation
team: An experiment in
team building. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation, 67, 357-361.
Hardy, C. J., & Crace, R. K. (1997). Foundations of team
building: Introduction to the team
building primer. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 9, 1-10.
Heneman, R. L. (1986). The relationship between supervisory
ratings and results-oriented
measures of performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel
Psychology, 39, 811-826.
Howard, J. (1979). Organizational team building in church-
related teams. Unpublished dis-
sertation, California School of Professional Psychology, San
Diego.
Indik, B. P. (1965). Operational size and member participation:
Some empirical tests of alter-
native explanations. Human Relations, 18, 339-350.
Kimberly, J. R., & Nielsen, W. R. (1975). Organization
development and change in organiza-
tional performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 20, 191-
206.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of
psychological, educational, and
behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis.
American Psychologist, 48,
1181-1209.
Mullen, B. (1987). Self-attention theory. In B. Mullen & G. R.
Goethals (Eds.), Theories of
group behavior (pp. 125-146). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC Meta-analysis. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mullen, B. (1991). Group composition, salience, and cognitive
representations: The phe-
nomenology of being in a group. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 27,
297-323.
Mullen, B., Anthony, T., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1994).
Group cohesiveness and quality
of decision making: An integration of tests of the groupthink
hypothesis. Small Group
Research, 25, 189-204.
Mullen, B., Brown, R. J., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a
function of salience, rele-
vance, and status: An integration. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 22, 103-122.
Mullen, B., & Copper, C. (1994). The relation between group
cohesiveness and perform-
ance: An integration. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 210-227.
Mullen, B., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). BASIC Meta-analysis:
Procedures and programs. Hills-
dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mullen, B., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1989). Salience,
motivation, and artifact as contribu-
tions to the relation between participation rate and leadership.
Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 25, 545-559.
Old, B. S. (1946). On the mathematics of committees, boards,
and panels. Scientific Monthly,
63, 75-78.
328 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999
Paul, C. F., & Gross, A. C. (1981). Increasing productivity and
morale in a municipality:
Effects of organizational development. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 17,
59-78.
Porras, J. I., & Berg, P. O. (1978). The impact of organization
development. Academy of
Management Review, 3, 249-266.
Robinson-Kurpius, S. E., & Keim, J. (1994). Team building for
nurses experiencing burnout
and poor morale. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 19,
155-161.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social
research. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Saunders, T., Driskell, J. E., Johnston, J., & Salas, E. (1996).
The effect of stress inoculation
training on anxiety and performance. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 1,
170-186.
Scherer, J. J. (1979). Can team building increase productivity?
or How can something that
feels so good not be worthwhile? Group and Organizational
Studies, 4, 335-351.
Shandler, M., & Egan, M. (1996). VROOM! Turbo-charged
team building. New York:
American Management Association.
Smith, P. E. (1976). Management modeling training to improve
morale and customer satis-
faction. Personnel Psychology, 29, 351-359.
Smither, R. D., Houston, J. M., & McIntire, S. A. (1996).
Organizational development:
Strategies for changing environments. New York:
HarperCollins.
Sundstrom, E., DeMeuse, K. P., & Futrell, D. (1990). Work
teams: Applications and effec-
tiveness. American Psychologist, 45, 120-133.
Tannenbaum, S. I., Beard, R. L., & Salas, E. (1992). Team
building and its influence on team
effectiveness: An examination of conceptual and empirical
developments. In K. Kelley
(Ed.), Issues, theory, and research in industrial/organizational
psychology. Amsterdam:
Elsevier.
Teamwork through conflict. (1971). Business Week, 2168, pp.
44-50.
Wakeley, J. H., & Shaw, M. F. (1965). Management training:
An integrated approach. Train-
ing Directors Journal, 19, 2-13.
Wegenast, D. P. (1983). Team building: An application of
leadership skills to case manage-
ment in child protective services. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, City University of
New York, New York City.
Wexler, K. J. (1990). Team building: A multidimensional
analysis. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Hofstra University, Hempstead, New York.
Woodman, R. W. (1978). Effects of team development
intervention: A field experiment.
Ph.D. dissertation, Purdue University.
Woodman, R. W., & Sherwood, J. J. (1980). The role of team
development in organizational
effectiveness: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 88,
166-186.
Eduardo Salas, Ph.D., is a senior research psychologist at the
Naval Air Warfare
Center Training Systems Division. His research interests
include team training,
training effectiveness, and team performance.
Drew Rozell, Ph.D., is a personal and business coach with
Evolution Coaching in
Syracuse, New York. His interests include team building,
management training, and
executive success.
Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 329
James E. Driskell, Ph.D., is president of Florida Maxima
Corporation and an
adjunct professor of psychology at Rollins College, Winter
Park, FL. His research
interests include status processes, group dynamics, and
performance under stress.
Brian Mullen, Ph.D., is a professor in the Department of
Psychology at Syracuse
University. His research interests include social cognition
perspectives on (inter)
group phenomena and meta-analysis.
Structure BookmarksArtifactTHE EFFECT OF TEAM
BUILDING ON PERFORMANCE An Integration THE EFFECT
OF TEAM BUILDING ON PERFORMANCE An Integration
EDUARDO SALAS Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems
Division DREW ROZELL BRIAN MULLEN Syracuse
University JAMES E. DRISKELL Florida Maxima Corporation
In this article, meta-analytic integration of research examining
the effects of team building on performance is reported.
Overall, there was no significant effect of team building on
performance. However, the effects of team building varied as a
function of the type of operationalization of performance: On
objective measures of performance, there was a nonsignificant
tendency for team building to decrease performance, whereas on
subjective measures of performance, there was a significant,
albeit small, tend------Harry, you weren’t kidding when you
said this Florida trip would be no vacation! Building a group
like us into a high-performing team takes really hard work! But
I wouldn’t have missed it for worlds! —Jack, a participant in a
team-building intervention (Shandler & Egan, 1996) Team
building, also called team development or group development,
is an extremely popular and common intervention. In fact, -
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH, Vol. 30 No. 3, June 1999 309-
329 © 1999 Sage Publications, Inc. Porras and Berg (1978)
observed that team building was one of the most frequently used
organization development interventions. Enthusiastic reports
can be found for the application of team building to such
diverse settings as hotel management organizations (e.g.,
Beckhard, 1966), microwave cooking manufacturers (George,
1977), metropolitan rapid transit authorities (Golembiewski &
Kiepper, 1976), medical rehabilitation teams (Halstead et al.,
1986), and nursing teams (Robinson-Kurpius & Keim, 1994). A
recen-----In spite of this popularity of the concept of team
building, however, several reviewers (e.g., Buller, 1986;
Woodman & Sherwood, 1980) have observed that there is no
conclusive evidence that team building renders an increase in
team performance. Druckman and Bjork (1994) noted that the
enthusiasm for these approaches among practitioners “is not
matched by strong empirical support for their effect on team
performance” (p. 125). Similar to this, Smither, Houston, and
McIntire (1996) concluded that “Research f-Several issues need
to be addressed in examining the effect of team building on
performance. First, the significance and AUTHORS’NOTE: The
views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do
not reflect the official position of their organizations.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Eduardo Salas, Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems
Division, Code 4961, 12350 Research Parkway, Orlando, FL
32826-3275. -magnitude—indeed, the very existence—of an
effect of team building on performance needs to be established.
Second, differences between objective and subjective indices of
performance have been delineated in other domains, but there is
no a priori determination of the extent to which team building
would differentially affect these two different ways of
operationalizing performance. Third, the degree to which a
given team-building intervention engages different components
of team-building might influence the ----THE EXISTENCE OF
A TEAM-BUILDING EFFECT THE EXISTENCE OF A TEAM-
BUILDING EFFECT Team building has at its core the central
notion that enlisting the participation of a group in planning and
implementing change will be more effective than simply
imposing change on the group from outside. The process by
which the members of the team become able to effectively
participate in the targeted change requires that the team acquire
new skills and perceptions. The various definitions of team
building that have been proposed in the research literature
resonate to these elements of participation and----The effects of
team building are often described in extremely sanguinary
terms. For example, the epigram at the beginning of this The
effects of team building are often described in extremely
sanguinary terms. For example, the epigram at the beginning of
this report is extracted from a primer on team building, entitled
VROOM! Turbo-Charged Team Building (Shandler & Egan,
1996). Therein, the president of a high-tech research and
development firm uses a team-building intervention to rescue
his foundering company, ensure his promotion in the corporate
hierarchy, and even save his failing marriage. It would seem
that any intervention capable of such impressive effects would
have generated an imposing body of evidence supporting its
effectiveness. -Unfortunately, a clear summary of the effects of
team building on performance apparently cannot be formulated
from narrative readings of previous research. Indeed, several
narrative literature reviews have been conducted in this domain
within the past two decades, and the judgments rendered by
these previous review efforts have been inconclusive. Woodman
and Sherwood (1980) reviewed 30 papers and concluded that
there were generally positive results for most of the studies but
that there was no clear-cut evi---It is interesting to consider the
nature of the body of studies considered in many previous
narrative reviews of the effects of team building. First, there
was a remarkable lack of convergence among these reviews
regarding which studies to include in a review of team-building
interventions. Of the 69 reports examined in at least one of
these narrative reviews, on average only 4 studies (or 5.8%)
were included by both of any two reviews. The point is that
more than 90% of the literature summarized in each ofIt is
interesting to consider the nature of the body of studies
considered in many previous narrative reviews of the effects of
team building. First, there was a remarkable lack of
convergence among these reviews regarding which studies to
include in a review of team-building interventions. Of the 69
reports examined in at least one of these narrative reviews, on
average only 4 studies (or 5.8%) were included by both of any
two reviews. The point is that more than 90% of the literature
summarized in each of-reviews of team building was referred to
in none of the other narrative reviews. This stunning lack of
convergence on precisely what constitutes an includable study
of team building is in part a reflection of the complexity of the
notion of team building (to be elucidated further). ---It is useful
to consider a few other intriguing facets of the studies
considered in previous narrative reviews of the effects of team
building. The most common type of report examined in these
previous reviews was the case study (20 out of 69 studies or
29%), describing the application of a team-building intervention
in some organizational setting with no empirical examination of
the effectiveness of the intervention (e.g., Beckhard, 1966;
George, 1977). These case studies, although interesting and
illumina----In sum, we cannot determine whether there is a
beneficial effect of team building on performance based on
previous narrative perusals of this research domain. Part of the
problem lies in the ambiguity of what precisely is team building
and what studies should be included in an effort to integrate the
effect of team building on performance. Part of the problem is
that the team-building literature includes a variety of case
studies, nonempirical reports, and tests of various types of
outcome measures. Therefo-COMPONENTS OF TEAM
BUILDING COMPONENTS OF TEAM BUILDING Beer (1976,
1980), Dyer (1977), and Buller (1986) have discussed four
current models of team building: Goal setting, interpersonal
relations, problem solving, and role clarification. Goal setting
emphasizes the setting of objectives and the development of
individual and team goals. Team members exposed to a goal-
setting team-building intervention are supposed to become
involved in action planning to identify ways to achieve those
goals. Interpersonal relations emphasizes an increase in team
work skills, suc-----These models might be best thought of as
components of any given team-building intervention. That is,
any team-building intervention might engage any or all of these
various components in varying degrees. For example,
Wegenast’s (1983) team-building intervention in child
protective service workers involved a relatively high emphasis
on problem solving, but relatively low emphasis on
interpersonal relations. Alternatively, Paul and Gross’s (1981)
team-building intervention in the communications and electrica-
----This diversity in team-building interventions represents one
of the major challenges to previous efforts to make sense of the
research literature on team building. In the present effort, we
viewed this diversity among team-building interventions as a
unique This diversity in team-building interventions represents
one of the major challenges to previous efforts to make sense of
the research literature on team building. In the present effort,
we viewed this diversity among team-building interventions as a
unique -opportunity to gauge the relative impact of the four
components of team building delineated above (goal setting,
interpersonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification).
To date, there is no evidence regarding which of these
components of team building are more critical to the putative
effects of team building on performance. Therefore, another
important goal of the present effort is to determine the relative
contributions of these components of team building to the team
building–performance effec--CONTRIBUTIONS OF
OPERATIONALIZATION OF PERFORMANCE
CONTRIBUTIONS OF OPERATIONALIZATION OF
PERFORMANCE Two broad sets of operationalizations of
performance typify research in the area of team performance.
On one hand, objective indicators of performance include direct
measures of countable behaviors, like the number of units sold
or the tons of ore mined. On the other hand, subjective
indicators of performance include ratings of the performance or
effectiveness of the team. Two separate meta-analyses have
reported relatively modest correlations between objective and
subjective measures of performance (Bommer-----
CONTRIBUTIONS OF TEAM SIZE CONTRIBUTIONS OF
TEAM SIZE The size of social groups has been shown to affect
the magnitude of several other group phenomena, including the
participation-leadership effect (Mullen, Salas, & Driskell,
1989), the ingroupThe size of social groups has been shown to
affect the magnitude of several other group phenomena,
including the participation-leadership effect (Mullen, Salas, &
Driskell, 1989), the ingroup-bias effect (Mullen, Brown, &
Smith, 1992), the cohesiveness-performance effect (Mullen &
Copper, 1994), the groupthink effect (Mullen, Anthony, Salas,
& Driskell, 1994), and the effect of stress inoculation training
on performance (Saunders, Driskell, Johnston, & Salas, 1996)
(for a discussion, see Mullen, 1991). Larger groups tend to
encourage deindividuation among group members (e.g., Mullen,
1987). Moreover, it is well established that as group size
increases, members’liking for the group (e.g., Indik, -In spite of
the consistent tendency for the size of the group to affect
several similar group phenomena, there has been no
consideration to date of whether team size moderates the effect
of team building on performance. Therefore, another important
goal of the present effort was to determine the extent to which
team size affects the impact of team building on performance. -
CONTRIBUTIONS OF DURATION OF THE TEAM-
BUILDING INTERVENTION CONTRIBUTIONS OF
DURATION OF THE TEAM-BUILDING INTERVENTION The
duration of team-building interventions varies considerably in
the extant literature, from 2 days (Smith, 1976) to 8 months
(Buller & Bell, 1986). Common sense might suggest that
interventions of longer duration might exert greater effects: If a
little of the intervention works, then a lot of the intervention
might work even more. However, interventions of longer
duration have paradoxically been found to render weaker effects
in such diverse areas as mental practice effects (Driskell,
Copper, & Moran, 1The duration of team-building interventions
varies considerably in the extant literature, from 2 days (Smith,
1976) to 8 months (Buller & Bell, 1986). Common sense might
suggest that interventions of longer duration might exert greater
effects: If a little of the intervention works, then a lot of the
intervention might work even more. However, interventions of
longer duration have paradoxically been found to render weaker
effects in such diverse areas as mental practice effects
(Driskell, Copper, & Moran, 1---effect of the duration of the
team-building intervention on the team building–performance
effect. A META-ANALYTIC INTEGRATION A META-
ANALYTIC INTEGRATION In an effort to provide a clearer
picture of the effects of team building on performance, a meta-
analytic integration (Mullen, 1989; Mullen & Rosenthal, 1985;
Rosenthal, 1991) was conducted. The goals of this effort were
(a) to provide a precise summary of the significance, the
strength, and the direction of the effects of team building on
performance; (b) to examine different magnitudes of this effect
of team building on performance as a function of the
operationalization of performance; (c) to examine the-
PROCEDURE PROCEDURE Using all of the standard literature
search techniques, an exhaustive search was conducted for
studies testing the effect of team-building interventions on
performance. First, on-line computer searches were conducted,
using the keywords team(s), group(s), or crew(s) and building,
development, performance, productivity, or effects. These
computer searches were supplemented by ancestry approach and
descendency approach searches, correspondence with
researchers active in this domain (the invisible college),
anUsing all of the standard literature search techniques, an
exhaustive search was conducted for studies testing the effect of
team-building interventions on performance. First, on-line
computer searches were conducted, using the keywords team(s),
group(s), or crew(s) and building, development, performance,
productivity, or effects. These computer searches were
supplemented by ancestry approach and descendency approach
searches, correspondence with researchers active in this domain
(the invisible college), an--reviews by Woodman and Sherwood
(1980), DeMeuse and Liebowitz (1981), Buller (1986),
Sundstrom, DeMeuse, and Futrell (1990), and Tannenbaum,
Beard, and Salas (1992). Any studies that were available as of
March 1998 were eligible for inclusion in this integration. -
Studies were included if they met the following criteria: Team
members in the studies had to be adolescents or adults not
sampled from abnormal populations. Studies had to report (or
allow the reconstruction of) a precise test of the effect of a
team-building intervention either on an objective measure of
performance (e.g., productivity data) or a subjective measure of
performance (e.g., rating of team effectiveness). Case studies
were not included (e.g., “Teamwork Through Conflict,” 1971),
nor were T-group---These selection criteria rendered a total of
11 studies (Buller & Bell, 1986; Eden, 1986; Friedlander, 1967;
Howard, 1979; Kimberly & Nielsen, 1975; Paul & Gross, 1981;
Smith, 1976; Wakeley & Shaw, 1965; Wegenast, 1983; Wexler,
1990; Woodman, 1978). These 11 articles yielded 16 separate
tests of the effects of team building on performance,
representing the responses of 2,806 team members in 307
teams.Attesting to the thoroughness of the literature search, the
included reports spanned a period of more than 3-1 -In addition
to the requisite statistical information, each hypothesis test was
coded for seven predictors. The nature of the operationalization
of performance (objective vs. subjective), the size of the team,
and the duration of the team-building intervention were all
directly coded from each report by two judges with perfect
reliability. In addition, each team-building intervention
procedure was also scored for the extent to which that
intervention would engage In addition to the requisite statistical
information, each hypothesis test was coded for seven
predictors. The nature of the operationalization of performance
(objective vs. subjective), the size of the team, and the duration
of the team-building intervention were all directly coded from
each report by two judges with perfect reliability. In addition,
each team-building intervention procedure was also scored for
the extent to which that intervention would engage ---each of
the components of team building. To be specific, two judges
independently rated each team-building intervention on a scale
from 0 (low) to 100 (high) for goal setting, interpersonal
relations, problem solving, and role clarification (as defined
above). Acceptably high levels of interjudge reliability were
obtained for each of these components: Goal setting (interjudge
reliability r = .693, Spearman-Brown Effective Reliability R =
.819), interpersonal relations (r = .822, R = .902), problem
solving (-RESULTS RESULTS General effect. Overall, there
was a nonsignificant, z = 0.132, p = .45, negligible, ZFisher =
0.007, r = .007, effect of team-building interventions on
performance. Thus, the weight of available evidence fails to
substantiate the effectiveness of team-building interventions to
increase performance.--2 Objective versus subjective measures
of performance. For the k = 8 hypothesis tests that used
objective measures of performance, the effect of team building
on performance was a nonsignificant, z = –0.511, p = .70,
negative, ZFisher = –0.040, r = –.040. For the k =8 hypothesis
tests that used subjective measures of performance, there was a
significant, z = 2.589, p = .004, albeit still weak, ZFisher =
0.140, r = .139, positive effect of team building on performance.
These two effects were significantly diff-TABLE 1: Studies
Included in the Meta-Analysis Duration Study Statistic rN
DOEMeasure Size (in days) GS IR PS RC b Buller and Bell
(1986) t(16) = 0.399 –.010 6/18 – Objective 3 240 83.5 32.5
82.5 10.0 t(16) = 1.584 .368 4/8 + Objective 2 240 87.5 5.0 10.0
20.0 t(16) = 0.005 –.001 6/18 – Objective 3 240 25.0 32.5 90.0
10.0 Eden (1986) t(29) = 1.252 –.226 7/220 – Subjective 31.5 3
40.0 77.5 29.0 35.0 Friedlander (1967) F(1, 88) = 3.98 .208
12/91 + Subjective 7.6 5 22.5 70.0 90.0 20.0 Howard (1979)
r(17) = .040 .040 2/19 + Objective 9 14 30.0 90.0 40.0 10.0
Kimberley and Nielsen (1975) t(43) = 0.71 –.108 45/900 –
ObjectiveNOTE: DOE = Direction of Effect, GS = Goal Setting,
IR = Interpersonal Relations, PS = Problem Solving, RC = Role
Clarification. a. The first number represents the number of
teams; the second number represents the number of team
members. b. The positive sign indicates that team building
increased performance; the negative sign indicates that team
building decreased performance. Contributions of components
of team building. The mean judged level of each component of
team building was used to examine the contributions of the
various components of team building. The team building–
performance effect did not vary as a function of the level of
goal setting engaged by the team-building intervention, (r = –
.164, z = 1.165, p = .12). In other words, the extent to which a
given team-building intervention engaged the component of
goal setting neither increased or decreased the magnitude of t-It
should be noted that there is no moderation of these patterns for
components of team building by the operationalization of
performance. That is, for both objective and subjective indices
respectively, there was no effect of goal setting (r = –.060, z =
0.182, p = .43 and r = –.110, z = 0.972, p = .17), interpersonal
(r = –.382, z = 1.258, p = .10 and r = –.035, z = 0.325, p = .37),
or problem solving (r = –.305, z = 0.780, p = .22 and r = .088, z
= 1.298, p = .10). However, for both objective and subject---
Contributions of team size. Overall, the effects of team building
on performance decreased as a function of the size of the team,
r = –344, z = 5.965, p < .001. This inverse effect of team size
on the effect of team building held for both objective measures
of performance (r = –.400, z = 2.383, p = .009) and subjective
measures of performance (r = –.407, z = 7.796, p < .001). Thus,
whatever Contributions of team size. Overall, the effects of
team building on performance decreased as a function of the
size of the team, r = –344, z = 5.965, p < .001. This inverse
effect of team size on the effect of team building held for both
objective measures of performance (r = –.400, z = 2.383, p =
.009) and subjective measures of performance (r = –.407, z =
7.796, p < .001). Thus, whatever -positive effect team building
may have is most likely to emerge in small groups.
Contributions of duration of the team-building
intervention.Overall, there was a marginal tendency for the
effects of team building on performance to decrease as a
function of duration of the team-building intervention, r = –
.197, z = 1.452, p = .07. This inverse effect of duration on the
effect of team building did not obtain for objective measures of
performance (r = –.056, z = 0.194, p = .42), but did obtain for
subjective measures of performance (r = –.208, z = 2.562, p =
.005). Thus, the negligible eff-DISCUSSION DISCUSSION One
of the most important results to emerge from the present
analyses is the documentation of the overall nonsignificant and
negligible effect of team building on performance. These results
suggest that the enthusiastic testimonials on behalf of team-
building interventions should be interpreted with caution. The
overall magnitude of the effect of team building on performance
of r = .007 indicates that approximately .005% of the variability
in a team’s performance might be accounted for by knowing
whether th-When the data were disaggregated into objective
performance indicators versus subjective performance
indicators, it was shown When the data were disaggregated into
objective performance indicators versus subjective performance
indicators, it was shown that objective performance indicators
rendered functionally no effect of team building on
performance, whereas subjective performance indicators
rendered a significant, albeit still weak, effect of team building
on performance. This difference between results for objective
performance indicators and subjective performance indicators is
not surprising, given the modest convergence previously
observed between these different operationalizations (Bommer
et al., 1995; Heneman, 1986). The analyses for these two ----
The examination of the contributions of the four components of
team building yielded a very intriguing result: The team
building-performance effect did not vary as a function of the
level of goal setting, interpersonal relations, or problem solving
engaged by the team-building intervention. It was only role
clarification that seemed to make a genuine contribution to the
effect of team building on performance, and this pattern
emerged for both of the operationalizations of performance.
This significant effec------The results for team size on the
effects of team building are consistent with patterns
documented in other group phenomena (e.g., Mullen et al.,
1989; Mullen et al., 1992; Mullen et al., 1994; Mullen &
Copper, 1994; Mullen, 1991). For years, researchers studying
group and team performance have searched for the magic
numbers that could summarize the effects of the group on the
individual. It is interesting to note that Old (1946) long ago
proposed that the optimum size for a group was 0.7 persons.
This tonguThe results for team size on the effects of team
building are consistent with patterns documented in other group
phenomena (e.g., Mullen et al., 1989; Mullen et al., 1992;
Mullen et al., 1994; Mullen & Copper, 1994; Mullen, 1991). For
years, researchers studying group and team performance have
searched for the magic numbers that could summarize the
effects of the group on the individual. It is interesting to note
that Old (1946) long ago proposed that the optimum size for a
group was 0.7 persons. This tongu--proposal, although
somewhat limiting in terms of implementing group training,
keeps with the results of the present analyses: As the size of the
team increases, the effectiveness of team-building interventions
decreases. Optimal benefit from a team-building intervention
seems most likely to be obtained with relatively small teams. --
The results for the effects of duration of the team-building
intervention are more complex. Although there was a marginal
tendency overall for longer team-building interventions to
render weaker effects, this pattern was actually due only to the
influence of duration on the effects of team building on
subjective indicators of performance. Thus, although shorter
duration team-building interventions might be of some benefit
to subjective performance measures, there does not seem to be
any difference between s----Of course, there are limitations to
our analysis and to the implications that can be drawn from it.
As noted earlier, the term team building has been defined
somewhat broadly in the literature to include various types of
group interventions that are conceptually quite dissimilar. As
Lipsey and Wilson (1993) noted in a recent review of the
general efficacy of psychological treatments, there are often
gray areas as to what constitutes a certain intervention. Our
approach was to cast a very narrow net, adoptinOf course, there
are limitations to our analysis and to the implications that can
be drawn from it. As noted earlier, the term team building has
been defined somewhat broadly in the literature to include
various types of group interventions that are conceptually quite
dissimilar. As Lipsey and Wilson (1993) noted in a recent
review of the general efficacy of psychological treatments, there
are often gray areas as to what constitutes a certain
intervention. Our approach was to cast a very narrow net,
adoptin----used, so adjustments for attenuation were not
attempted. The failure of the source information to report
reliabilities for performance measures is not unique to this
particular meta-analysis, but it represents a common limitation
of the research literature. --In summary, the present results
indicate that overall there is no significant effect of team
building on performance. Moreover, what little benefits team
building might exert on performance are likely to be seen in
interventions that emphasize role clarification in smaller groups
(and perhaps, in interventions that are more brief). Future
research might be directed toward further examinations of the
relative contributions of the four components of team building
and to scrutinizing the differential effects o--(p. 335).
Subsequent reviews of team-building research cautioned that
evidence of an effect of team building on performance was
“inconclusive” (Buller, 1986, p. 147), “unsubstantiated”
(Woodman & Sherwood, 1980, p. 166), “equivocal”
(Tannenbaum et al., 1992, p. 146), and “mixed” (Sundstrom et
al., 1990, p. 128). The present analysis of the effect of team
building on performance provides an empirical basis for the
caution voiced by these reviewers. --NOTES NOTES 1. 1. 1.
The included studies reported varying numbers of hypothesis
tests, ranging from one per study (e.g., Eden, 1986) to three per
study (e.g., Buller & Bell, 1986). In the meta-analysis reported,
each hypothesis test was treated as an independent observation.
This assumption of independence is patently false. For example,
each of the three hypothesis tests included in Buller and Bell
(1986) was derived from the same subject population at the
same time. However, without making this assumption of
nonindependence,2. 2. As indicated in Note 1, the assumption
that each of the 16 hypothesis tests represented an independent
observation is false. However, it can be seen that such an
assumption does not seem to render a distorted summary of this
research domain. Consider the results of a supplementary meta-
analysis of wholly independent effects, in which multiple
hypothesis tests obtained from a single study were combined
into a single test. This heavy-handed solution precludes the
examination of the effects of any of the moderators considered
above, but it does eliminate the problem of nonindependence.
This produced 11 distinct, wholly independent hypothesis tests,
one from each includable study. The results of this
supplemental meta-analysis revealed the same patterns reported
previously:--REFERENCES REFERENCES Abramis, D. J.
(1994). Work role ambiguity, job satisfaction, and job
performance: Meta-analyses and review. Psychological Reports,
75, 1411-1433. Argyris, C. (1962). Interpersonal competence
and organizational behavior. Homewood, IL: Irwin. Beckhard,
R. (1966). An organization improvement program in a
decentralized organization. Journal of Applied Behavioral
Science, 2, 3-25. -Beer, M. (1976). The technology of
organization development. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook
of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 937-994).
Chicago: Rand McNally. Beer, M. (1980). Organization change
and development: A systems view. Glenview, IL: Scott,
Foresman. Bigelow, R. C. (1971). Changing classroom
interaction through organization development. In R. A.
Schmuck & M. B. Miles (Eds.), Organization development in
schools. Palo Alto, CA: National Press. Blake, R. R., &
Mouton, J. S. (1966). Some effects of managerial grid training
on union and management attitudes towards supervision. Journal
of Applied Behavioral Science, 2, 387-400. Bommer, W. H.,
Johnson, J. L., Rich, G. A., Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S.
B. (1995). On the interchangeability of objective and subjective
measures of employee performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel
Psychology, 48, 587-605. -Buller, P. F. (1986). The team
building–task performance relation: Some conceptual and
methodological refinements. Group and Organization Studies,
11, 147-168. Buller, P. F., & Bell, C. H. (1986). Effects of team
building and goal setting on productivity: A field experiment.
Academy of Management Journal, 29, 305-328. DeMeuse, K. P.,
& Liebowitz, S. J. (1981). An empirical analysis of team
building research. Group and Organization Studies, 6, 357-378.
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C. M., & Moran, A. (1994). Does-human
performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Dyer,
W. G. (1977). Team building: Issues and alternatives. Reading,
MA: Addison Wesley. Eden, D. (1986). Team development:
Quasi-experimental confirmation among combat com-panies.
Group and Organization Studies, 11, 133-146. Friedlander, F.
(1967). The impact of organizational training laboratories upon
effectiveness of ongoing work groups. Personnel Psychology,
20, 289-307. George, W. W. (1977). Task teams for rapid
growth. Harvard Business Review, 55, 71-80. Golembiewski, R.
T., & Kiepper, A. (1976). MARTA: Toward an effective open
giant. Public Administration Review, 36, 46-60. Gorey, K. M.,
& Cryns, A. G. (1991). Group work as interventive modality
with the older depressed client: A meta-analytic review. Journal
of Gerontological Social Work, 16, 137-157. Gould, R. A., &
Clum, G. A. (1993). A meta-analysis of self-help treatment
approaches. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 169-186.
Halstead, L. S., Rintala, D. H., Kanellos, M., Griffin, B.,
Higgins, L., Rheinecker, N., White-side, W., & Healy, J. E.
(1986). The innovative rehabilitation team: An experiment in
team building. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation, 67, 357-361. Hardy, C. J., & Crace, R. K.
(1997). Foundations of team building: Introduction to the team
building primer. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 9, 1-10.
Heneman, R. L. (1986). The relationship between supervisory
ratings and results-oriented measures of performance: A meta-
analysis. Personnel Psychology, 39, 811-826. Howard, J.
(1979). Organizational team building in church-related teams.
Unpublished dissertation, California School of Professional
Psychology, San Diego. -Indik, B. P. (1965). Operational size
and member participation: Some empirical tests of alternative
explanations. Human Relations, 18, 339-350. -Kimberly, J. R.,
& Nielsen, W. R. (1975). Organization development and change
in organizational performance. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 20, 191-206. -Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B.
(1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and
behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis.
American Psychologist, 48, 1181-1209. Mullen, B. (1987). Self-
attention theory. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.),
Theories of group behavior (pp. 125-146). New York: Springer-
Verlag. Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC Meta-analysis.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Mullen, B. (1991). Group
composition, salience, and cognitive representations: The
phenomenology of being in a group. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 27, 297-323. -Mullen, B., Anthony, T.,
Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1994). Group cohesiveness and
quality of decision making: An integration of tests of the
groupthink hypothesis. Small Group Research, 25, 189-204.
Mullen, B., Brown, R. J., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a
function of salience, relevance, and status: An integration.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 103-122. -Mullen,
B., & Copper, C. (1994). The relation between group
cohesiveness and performance: An integration. Psychological
Bulletin, 115, 210-227. -Mullen, B., & Rosenthal, R. (1985).
BASIC Meta-analysis: Procedures and programs. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum. -Mullen, B., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E.
(1989). Salience, motivation, and artifact as contributions to the
relation between participation rate and leadership. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 545-559. -Old, B. S.
(1946). On the mathematics of committees, boards, and panels.
Scientific Monthly, 63, 75-78. Paul, C. F., & Gross, A. C.
(1981). Increasing productivity and morale in a municipality:
Effects of organizational development. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 17, 59-78. Porras, J. I., & Berg, P. O.
(1978). The impact of organization development. Academy of
Management Review, 3, 249-266. Robinson-Kurpius, S. E., &
Keim, J. (1994). Team building for nurses experiencing burnout
and poor morale. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 19,
155-161. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for
social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Saunders, T.,
Driskell, J. E., Johnston, J., & Salas, E. (1996). The effect of
stress inoculation training on anxiety and performance. Journal
of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 170-186. Scherer, J. J.
(1979). Can team building increase productivity? or How can
something that feels so good not be worthwhile? Group and
Organizational Studies, 4, 335-351. Shandler, M., & Egan, M.
(1996). VROOM! Turbo-charged team building. New York:
American Management Association. Smith, P. E. (1976).
Management modeling training to improve morale and customer
satisfaction. Personnel Psychology, 29, 351-359. -Smither, R.
D., Houston, J. M., & McIntire, S. A. (1996). Organizational
development: Strategies for changing environments. New York:
HarperCollins. Sundstrom, E., DeMeuse, K. P., & Futrell, D.
(1990). Work teams: Applications and effectiveness. American
Psychologist, 45, 120-133. -Tannenbaum, S. I., Beard, R. L., &
Salas, E. (1992). Team building and its influence on team
effectiveness: An examination of conceptual and empirical
developments. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Issues, theory, and research
in industrial/organizational psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Teamwork through conflict. (1971). Business Week, 2168, pp.
44-50. Wakeley, J. H., & Shaw, M. F. (1965). Management
training: An integrated approach. Training Directors Journal,
19, 2-13. -Wegenast, D. P. (1983). Team building: An
application of leadership skills to case management in child
protective services. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City
University of New York, New York City. -Wexler, K. J. (1990).
Team building: A multidimensional analysis. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Hofstra University, Hempstead, New
York. Woodman, R. W. (1978). Effects of team development
intervention: A field experiment. Ph.D. dissertation, Purdue
University. Woodman, R. W., & Sherwood, J. J. (1980). The
role of team development in organizational effectiveness: A
critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 166-186. Eduardo
Salas, Ph.D., is a senior research psychologist at the Naval Air
Warfare Center Training Systems Division. His research
interests include team training, training effectiveness, and team
performance. Drew Rozell, Ph.D., is a personal and business
coach with Evolution Coaching in Syracuse, New York. His
interests include team building, management training, and
executive success. James E. Driskell, Ph.D., is president of
Florida Maxima Corporation and an adjunct professor of
psychology at Rollins College, Winter Park, FL. His research
interests include status processes, group dynamics, and
performance under stress. Brian Mullen, Ph.D., is a professor in
the Department of Psychology at Syracuse University. His
research interests include social cognition perspectives on
(inter) group phenomena and meta-analysis.
European Journal of Personality, Eur. J. Pers. 25: 31–42 (2011)
Published online 4 April 2010 (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI:
10.1002/per.769
Personality and the Prediction of Team Performance
THOMAS A. O’NEILL* and NATALIE J. ALLEN
Department of Psychology, The University of Western Ontario,
London, Ontario, Canada
Abstract: Although much is known about personality and
individuals’ job performance, only a few studies have
considered the effects of team-level personality on team
performance. Existing research examining the effects of
personality on team performance has found that, of the Big Five
factors of personality, Conscientiousness is often the most
important predictor. Accordingly, we investigated the criterion
validity of lower-level Conscientiousness traits to
determine whether any one trait is particularly predictive of
team performance. In addition to Conscientiousness, we
examined the criterion validity of the other Big Five personality
factors. We found that Conscientiousness and its facets
predicted team performance. Agreeableness, Extraversion and
Neuroticism were not predictive of team performance,
whereas Openness had a modest negative relation with team
performance. Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Key words: team performance; team composition; personality;
Big Five; narrow traits; personality facets
INTRODUCTION
The composition of a work team is defined by the individual
characteristics of its members. One implicit rationale
underlying the research on team composition is that
individual characteristics of team members (i.e. their
personalities, demographic characteristics, attitudes and so
on) serve as inputs that indirectly influence team perform-
ance through group processes (e.g. collaboration) and
emergent states (e.g. team cohesion). In other words, the
characteristics of team members affect the way in which a
team operates and its subsequent performance.
Personality, as a class of team composition variables, is
the focus of the present study. Over the past several years,
research on personality has received considerable attention
in the teams literature (e.g. Bell, 2007; Humphrey,
Hollenbeck, Meyer, & Ilgen, 2007; Peeters, van Tuijl, Rutte,
& Reymen, 2006). Interest in this topic continues for at least
two reasons. First, there is an intuitive appeal to the argument
that personality will influence team-related variables.
Conceptually, personality should be related to (a) team
knowledge, skills and abilities, (b) processes and emergent
states and (c) general dimensions of teamwork (e.g.
collaboration, supportive behaviour, team trust). Most of
these variables appear to be natural outgrowths of personality
and, therefore, one would expect personality to be a valid
predictor in many cases (see Halfhill, Sundstrom, Lahner,
Calderone, & Nielsen, 2005; Kichuk & Wiesner, 1998).
A second reason that personality continues to be
investigated in team settings is that it is a consistent and
important predictor of individuals’ job performance (e.g.
Barrick & Mount, 1991; Tett, Jackson, Rothstein, & Reddon,
*Correspondence to: Thomas A. O’Neill, Department of
Psychology, Social
Science Centre, The University of Western Ontario, London,
Ontario N6A
5C2, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]
Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
1999). Extending these findings to the team level is needed as
organizations are increasingly turning to teamwork in an
effort to stay competitive in the global marketplace (Allen &
West, 2005; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006). Thus, research on
personality and team performance is an ongoing priority.
In this study, we collected personality data from members
of project design teams, operationalized those data at the
group level (e.g. using the group mean on each trait), and
correlated the resulting team-level personality scores with
team performance. Our purpose in this research was
threefold. First, we examined the extent to which any
content-relevant personality facets of Conscientiousness
could demonstrate superior prediction of team performance
relative to a broad Conscientiousness composite. An
investigation of this type is needed given that Conscien-
tiousness has been shown to be one of the most consistent Big
Five predictors of job performance and team performance,
but the criterion validity of its facets have rarely been
examined at the team level (but see LePine, 2003). Second,
we investigated whether any personality factors besides
Conscientiousness could be valid predictors of team
performance in the present context. Specifically, we assessed
the criterion validity of the other Big Five factors:
Agreeableneness, Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness.
Third, when considered as a team-level construct, personality
has historically been operationalized in several ways. In this
study we provide new evidence regarding the criterion
validity of the four most common team-level personality
operationalizations.
TEAM-LEVEL PERSONALITY
Typically, the operationalization of personality variables at
the team level is accomplished by aggregating individual-
level personality scores using one of four group-level
Received 29 July 2009
Revised 16 February 2010, Accepted 16 February 2010
mailto:[email protected]
https://wileyonlinelibrary.com
32 T. A. O’Neill and N. J. Allen
indices: Mean, variance, minimum and maximum scores
(see Barrick, Stewart, Neubert, & Mount, 1998; Halfhill
et al., 2005; Williams & Allen, 2008). The particular
operationalization is usually chosen through a consideration
of the personality variable, the nature of the task, and how the
two are expected to interact (e.g. Allen & West, 2005; Hecht
& Allen, 1999; LePine, Hollenbeck, Ilgen, & Hedlund,
1997).
The mean approach involves computing the arithmetic
average of each team member’s score on the personality
variable. This approach is appropriate when the trait is
theorized to work additively—that is, when it is suspected
that the more (or less) team members possess the trait, the
better the team will perform. The variance approach indexes
the dispersion, or heterogeneity, of the trait across team
members. This operationalization is used when the
researcher believes a greater (or lesser) amount of variation
in the trait will be related to the criterion. Finally, sometimes
it is appropriate to consider only the team member with the
highest, or lowest, score on a trait (referred to as the
maximum or minimum approach, respectively), and refer to
that value as the team-level score. As an analogy, on an
assembly line, the number of units produced will often depend
on the slowest working team member, and, accordingly, the
minimum score on a trait such as Achievement could be most
predictive of team performance. Conversely, on a creativity
task, the team member with the highest score on a trait such as
Innovation could be most responsible for the level of team
performance achieved (because a novel idea has only to
come from one team member). Theorizing about the most
appropriate operationalization for team personality is critical
as these may substantially affect the magnitude of person-
ality’s criterion validity (Moynihan & Peterson, 2004;
Williams & Allen, 2008).
In the most recent and comprehensive meta-analysis
examining relations between team-level personality and
performance, Bell (2007) found that, overall, team-level
personality does predict team performance. The findings for
lab studies were generally weak, likely because team
performance measurement in those studies tended to be
too coarse to detect small variations in behaviour related to
expressions of personality. Field studies in Bell’s meta-
analysis, however, demonstrated the strongest and most
consistent findings for Conscientiousness. Teams with high
means, high team member maximum and minimum scores
and low variance had the greatest performance levels
(Emotional Stability was coded in the socially desirable
direction). Other Big Five factors were predictive of team
performance, but not with the same magnitude and
consistency across operationalizations.
Given that Conscientiousness was the most consist-
ently predictive trait of team performance in Bell’s (2007)
meta-analysis, it is reasonable to consider that facets of
Conscientiousness might even be more predictive (see
Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina, 2006). For example,
the factor of Conscientiousness encompasses several
more specific facets of personality, such as Industrious-
ness, Order, Self-Control, Responsibility, Traditionalism
and Virtue (see Roberts, Chernyshenko, Stark, & Gold-
berg, 2005). Arguably, some of these lower-level
personality variables belonging to the same higher-level
personality factor may correlate differently, in magnitude
or direction, from the others in the prediction of a
criterion (see Ashton, 1998; Ashton, Jackson, Paunonen,
Helmes, & Rothstein, 1995; Hough, 1992; LePine, 2003;
Paunonen, 1998, 2003). Reflecting on Bell’s meta-
analytic findings, as well as the literature demonstrating
the validity of narrow traits, we suggest that, in order to
maximize the predictive power of Conscientiousness as it
relates to team performance, criterion-relevant facets
ought to be considered.
The fact that personality variables other than Conscien-
tiousness (e.g. Agreeableness) were predictive of team
performance in Bell’s meta-analysis suggests that they, too,
may be relevant in the present study. As we will argue later,
our criterion, project team performance, could be associated
with certain team-level operationalizations of Agreeable-
ness, Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness. Finally, the
method of operationalizing team personality (e.g. mean,
minimum) that will be most predictive of team performance
must also be considered in maximizing criterion validity. In
the section that follows we develop our predictions regarding
the operationalization that is, in the context of our study,
most theoretically appropriate for each personality factor and
facet included in this study.
THE PRESENT STUDY
Our sample consisted of concept design teams, composed of
engineering students, who worked interdependently for 6.5
months. The teams were engaged in an intensive, complex
engineering design task. The team members had shared
outcomes of significant value, and coordinated most work
dynamically and reciprocally (rather than through pooled or
sequential processes). These were classic ‘project teams’ as
they were created for a specific purpose and time frame, after
which they would disband (see Chiocchio & Essiembre,
2009). Knowledge of these contextual details was important
in generating predictions, outlined below.
The Big Five
In the present research we assessed the Big Five factors of
personality. In order to optimize their prediction of team
performance, we judged it most appropriate to operationalize
the Big Five factors, at the team-level, as follows:
Conscientiousness (mean), Agreeableness (mean), Neuroti-
cism (mean), Extraversion (variance) and Openness (maxi-
mum). Important theoretical rationales underlie the choice of
team personality operationalizations. Beginning with Con-
scientiousness, we contend that this factor captures a class of
attributes that manifest themselves as valuable resources,
such as achievement-striving, organization, planning and
task focus. The team may draw upon resources of this type to
accomplish its work (see LePine et al., 1997; Stewart, 2003).
An additive team-level conceptualization, using the mean
approach, is most appropriate in the present research because
Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Pers. 25: 31–
42 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/per
Personality and team performance 33
the more team members are conscientious, the better the
team should perform (see also Barrick et al., 1998).
Similarly, Agreeableness represents a factor of personality
that can be expected to foster effective team interactions
because members are trusting, altruistic and cooperative.
Such teams could perform well because of their smooth
conflict resolution, and inclination towards open communi-
cation and information seeking (Peeters et al., 2006). We also
see Agreeableness as accumulating additively, as the more
members are characterized as agreeable, the more they
should have positive interactions, and in turn, create a higher
performing team. Regarding the personality factor Neuroti-
cism, most previous studies have found important relations
with team performance for the mean only (see Bell, 2007).
As Neuman, Wagner and Christiansen (1999) pointed out,
teams that are higher on Neuroticism will have difficulty
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx
SMALL GROUP RESEARCH  June 1999Salas et al.  TEAM BUILDING.docx

More Related Content

Similar to SMALL GROUP RESEARCH June 1999Salas et al. TEAM BUILDING.docx

Journal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docx
Journal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docxJournal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docx
Journal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docxdonnajames55
 
Academic Article Review.pdf
Academic Article Review.pdfAcademic Article Review.pdf
Academic Article Review.pdfstirlingvwriters
 
Salas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performance
Salas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performanceSalas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performance
Salas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performanceIgnacio Fernández
 
team type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docx
team type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docxteam type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docx
team type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docxssuserf9c51d
 
Prior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docx
Prior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docxPrior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docx
Prior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docxstilliegeorgiana
 
Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...
Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...
Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...Mohammad Qasim Bin Ayaz
 
Effect of Team Work in employees performance
Effect of Team Work in employees performanceEffect of Team Work in employees performance
Effect of Team Work in employees performanceMohammad Qasim Bin Ayaz
 
Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...
Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...
Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...Eng. José Luis Peralta Barbano
 
· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx
· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx
· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docxLynellBull52
 
A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor
A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor
A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor Kristian Suhartadi
 
TOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOC
TOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOCTOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOC
TOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOCKamal Birdi
 
Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13
Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13
Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13NailaKhalida
 
Assignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docxAssignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docxwilliejgrant41084
 
Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013
Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013
Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013Gustavo Decap Agüero
 
SAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docx
SAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docxSAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docx
SAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docxrtodd599
 
A roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organization
A roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organizationA roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organization
A roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organizationLeandro Silvério
 

Similar to SMALL GROUP RESEARCH June 1999Salas et al. TEAM BUILDING.docx (20)

Journal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docx
Journal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docxJournal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docx
Journal of Asia Business Studies SPRING 200832 Journal of Asia.docx
 
Academic Article Review.pdf
Academic Article Review.pdfAcademic Article Review.pdf
Academic Article Review.pdf
 
Salas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performance
Salas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performanceSalas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performance
Salas et al (2008) Teams, teamwork and team performance
 
team type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docx
team type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docxteam type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docx
team type Determine the type of team (types of teams) that .docx
 
Proposal for a thesis
Proposal for a thesisProposal for a thesis
Proposal for a thesis
 
Prior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docx
Prior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docxPrior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docx
Prior to beginning work on this discussion, read Chapter 14 of Nor.docx
 
4.rajeev verma 30 41
4.rajeev verma 30 414.rajeev verma 30 41
4.rajeev verma 30 41
 
Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...
Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...
Effect of Team Work in employees performance (The Empirical Assessment of ban...
 
Effect of Team Work in employees performance
Effect of Team Work in employees performanceEffect of Team Work in employees performance
Effect of Team Work in employees performance
 
Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...
Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...
Organization Learning Disorders Conceptual Model: Conceptual Model and Interv...
 
Thesis-final-RGZ
Thesis-final-RGZThesis-final-RGZ
Thesis-final-RGZ
 
· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx
· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx
· Gagnon, M. A., Jansen, K. J., & Michael, J. H. (2008). Employee .docx
 
A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor
A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor
A comparison of a competitive and non competitive outdoor
 
TOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOC
TOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOCTOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOC
TOTADO paper and templates Birdi (2011).DOC
 
Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13
Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13
Naila kf 4520210097 tgs is 13
 
Assignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docxAssignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormPurpose of AssignmentAll p.docx
 
Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013
Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013
Hamann, schiemann, bellora &amp; guenther 2013
 
SAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docx
SAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docxSAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docx
SAGE OpenJuly-September 2013 1 –10© The Author(s) 2013D.docx
 
A roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organization
A roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organizationA roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organization
A roadmap for a leanness company to emerge as a true lean organization
 
J0361075082
J0361075082J0361075082
J0361075082
 

More from mckellarhastings

Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docxConduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docxmckellarhastings
 
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docxConduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docxmckellarhastings
 
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docxConduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docxmckellarhastings
 
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docxAt this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docxmckellarhastings
 
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docxAt the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docxmckellarhastings
 
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docxAt many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docxAt least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docxAt least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docxAt least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docxAt least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docxmckellarhastings
 
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docxAt its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docxmckellarhastings
 
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docxassumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docxmckellarhastings
 
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docxAt age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docxmckellarhastings
 
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docxAt each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docxAssume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docxAssume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docxAssume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docxAssume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docxAssume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docxAssume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docxmckellarhastings
 

More from mckellarhastings (20)

Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docxConduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
 
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docxConduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
 
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docxConduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
 
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docxAt this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
 
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docxAt the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
 
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docxAt many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
 
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docxAt least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
 
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docxAt least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
 
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docxAt least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docx
 
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docxAt least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
 
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docxAt its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
 
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docxassumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
 
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docxAt age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
 
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docxAt each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
 
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docxAssume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
 
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docxAssume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
 
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docxAssume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
 
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docxAssume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
 
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docxAssume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
 
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docxAssume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfNirmal Dwivedi
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptxMaritesTamaniVerdade
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.christianmathematics
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxAmanpreet Kaur
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Association for Project Management
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseSpellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseAnaAcapella
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxnegromaestrong
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.MaryamAhmad92
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...pradhanghanshyam7136
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptxAsian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseSpellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 

SMALL GROUP RESEARCH June 1999Salas et al. TEAM BUILDING.docx

  • 1. SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING THE EFFECT OF TEAM BUILDING ON PERFORMANCE An Integration EDUARDO SALAS Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division DREW ROZELL BRIAN MULLEN Syracuse University JAMES E. DRISKELL Florida Maxima Corporation In this article, meta-analytic integration of research examining the effects of team building on performance is reported. Overall, there was no significant effect of team building on per- formance. However, the effects of team building varied as a function of the type of operation- alization of performance: On objective measures of performance, there was a nonsignificant tendency for team building to decrease performance, whereas on subjective measures of per- formance, there was a significant, albeit small, tendency for team building to increase per- formance. Examination of the specific components of team
  • 2. building revealed that interven- tions emphasizing role clarification were more likely to increase performance, whereas interventions that emphasized goal setting, problem solving, or interpersonal relations were no more likely to render an increase or decrease in performance. Finally, the effects of team building decreased as a function of the size of the team. The discussion considers implica- tions of these effects of team building on performance. Harry, you weren’t kidding when you said this Florida trip would be no vacation! Building a group like us into a high-performing team takes really hard work! But I wouldn’t have missed it for worlds! —Jack, a participant in a team-building intervention (Shandler & Egan, 1996) Team building, also called team development or group develop- ment, is an extremely popular and common intervention. In fact, SMALL GROUP RESEARCH, Vol. 30 No. 3, June 1999 309- 329 © 1999 Sage Publications, Inc. 309 310 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 Porras and Berg (1978) observed that team building was one of the
  • 3. most frequently used organization development interventions. Enthusiastic reports can be found for the application of team build- ing to such diverse settings as hotel management organizations (e.g., Beckhard, 1966), microwave cooking manufacturers (George, 1977), metropolitan rapid transit authorities (Golem- biewski & Kiepper, 1976), medical rehabilitation teams (Halstead et al., 1986), and nursing teams (Robinson-Kurpius & Keim, 1994). A recent special issue of a scholarly journal was devoted to applica- tions of team building to sports teams (Hardy & Crace, 1997). Also, as conveyed in the epigram above, team-building interventions are evidently believed to lead to a substantive increase in team per- formance. Shandler and Egan (1996) claim that by applying princi- ples of team building, “any group can transform itself . . . into a high-performing team” (p. x). In spite of this popularity of the concept of team building, how- ever, several reviewers (e.g., Buller, 1986; Woodman & Sherwood, 1980) have observed that there is no conclusive evidence that team building renders an increase in team performance. Druckman and Bjork (1994) noted that the enthusiasm for these approaches among practitioners “is not matched by strong empirical support for their effect on team performance” (p. 125). Similar to this, Smither, Houston, and McIntire (1996) concluded that “Research
  • 4. findings on the effectiveness of team building provide a complex mix of results that make drawing firm conclusions difficult” (p. 324). More than 20 years after Beer (1976) attempted to formalize the notion of team building, some of the most fundamental questions about the effects of team building remain: Does team building enhance performance? Why? Under what conditions? This article reports the results of a meta-analytic integration of previous research examining the effects of team building on performance. Several issues need to be addressed in examining the effect of team building on performance. First, the significance and AUTHORS’NOTE: The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not re- flect the official position of their organizations. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eduardo Salas, Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division, Code 4961, 12350 Research Parkway, Orlando, FL 32826-3275. Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 311 magnitude—indeed, the very existence—of an effect of team build- ing on performance needs to be established. Second, differences between objective and subjective indices of performance have been delineated in other domains, but there is no a priori determination of the extent to which team building would differentially affect these two different ways of operationalizing performance.
  • 5. Third, the degree to which a given team-building intervention engages dif- ferent components of team-building might influence the effective- ness of the team-building intervention. Fourth, the effect of team size needs to be specified, particularly in light of recent findings in cognate areas indicating that group size exerts a considerable effect on other group phenomena. Finally, the effect of the duration of team-building interventions on their effectiveness is of consider- able practical significance. Each of these considerations is addressed in turn. THE EXISTENCE OF A TEAM-BUILDING EFFECT Team building has at its core the central notion that enlisting the participation of a group in planning and implementing change will be more effective than simply imposing change on the group from outside. The process by which the members of the team become able to effectively participate in the targeted change requires that the team acquire new skills and perceptions. The various defini- tions of team building that have been proposed in the research lit- erature resonate to these elements of participation and acquisition of new skills and perceptions. For example, Buller (1986) defined team building as a planned intervention facilitated by a third-
  • 6. party consultant that develops the problem-solving capacity and solves major problems of an intact work group. Woodman and Sherwood (1980) proposed that team building was designed to enhance organ- izational effectiveness by improving team operation through devel- oping problem-solving procedures and skills and increasing role clarity (cf. Beer, 1976, 1980; DeMeuse & Liebowitz, 1981; Dyer, 1977). The effects of team building are often described in extremely sanguinary terms. For example, the epigram at the beginning of this 312 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 report is extracted from a primer on team building, entitled VROOM! Turbo-Charged Team Building (Shandler & Egan, 1996). Therein, the president of a high-tech research and development firm uses a team-building intervention to rescue his foundering company, ensure his promotion in the corporate hierarchy, and even save his failing marriage. It would seem that any intervention capa- ble of such impressive effects would have generated an imposing body of evidence supporting its effectiveness. Unfortunately, a clear summary of the effects of team building
  • 7. on performance apparently cannot be formulated from narrative readings of previous research. Indeed, several narrative literature reviews have been conducted in this domain within the past two decades, and the judgments rendered by these previous review efforts have been inconclusive. Woodman and Sherwood (1980) reviewed 30 papers and concluded that there were generally posi- tive results for most of the studies but that there was no clear- cut evidence to suggest that team building can improve performance. DeMeuse and Liebowitz (1981) reviewed 36 previous reports and similarly concluded that there were generally positive descriptions of the team-building interventions but that there was a dearth of evi- dence for beneficial effects of team building on performance. Bul- ler (1986) reviewed 9 studies, Sundstrom, DeMeuse, and Futrell (1990) reviewed 13 studies, and Tannenbaum, Beard, and Salas (1992) reviewed 17 studies, and (like each of the previous reviews) each of these narrative reviews concluded that team building was described in encouraging ways by most of the studies reviewed, but there was a general lack of definitive, compelling evidence for the beneficial effects of team building on performance. It is interesting to consider the nature of the body of studies con- sidered in many previous narrative reviews of the effects of team
  • 8. building. First, there was a remarkable lack of convergence among these reviews regarding which studies to include in a review of team-building interventions. Of the 69 reports examined in at least one of these narrative reviews, on average only 4 studies (or 5.8%) were included by both of any two reviews. The point is that more than 90% of the literature summarized in each of these narrative Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 313 reviews of team building was referred to in none of the other narra- tive reviews. This stunning lack of convergence on precisely what constitutes an includable study of team building is in part a reflec- tion of the complexity of the notion of team building (to be eluci- dated further). It is useful to consider a few other intriguing facets of the studies considered in previous narrative reviews of the effects of team building. The most common type of report examined in these previ- ous reviews was the case study (20 out of 69 studies or 29%), describing the application of a team-building intervention in some organizational setting with no empirical examination of the effec- tiveness of the intervention (e.g., Beckhard, 1966; George,
  • 9. 1977). These case studies, although interesting and illuminating in an anecdotal way, can hardly be said to provide definitive, conclusive evidence in support of team building. The second most common type of report examined in these previous reviews reported the effects of team building on some affective, nonperformance out- come measure (15 out of 69 studies or 22%). This tendency for stud- ies of team-building interventions to focus on affective responses to the intervention while excluding genuine performance results of the intervention was recognized in some of the previous narrative reviews (notably Buller, 1986; DeMeuse & Liebowitz, 1981; Woodman & Sherwood, 1980). Only 6 of the 69 studies considered in the previous narrative reviews provided a precise statistical test of the effects of a team-building intervention on performance. In sum, we cannot determine whether there is a beneficial effect of team building on performance based on previous narrative perusals of this research domain. Part of the problem lies in the ambiguity of what precisely is team building and what studies should be included in an effort to integrate the effect of team build- ing on performance. Part of the problem is that the team- building literature includes a variety of case studies, nonempirical reports, and tests of various types of outcome measures. Therefore, the first goal of the present effort was to establish the empirical significance and the strength of the effects of team building on performance.
  • 10. 314 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 COMPONENTS OF TEAM BUILDING Beer (1976, 1980), Dyer (1977), and Buller (1986) have dis- cussed four current models of team building: Goal setting, interper- sonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification. Goal set- ting emphasizes the setting of objectives and the development of individual and team goals. Team members exposed to a goal- setting team-building intervention are supposed to become involved in action planning to identify ways to achieve those goals. Interpersonal relations emphasizes an increase in team work skills, such as mutual supportiveness, communication, and sharing of feelings. Team members exposed to an interpersonal relations team-building intervention are supposed to develop trust in one another and confidence in the team. Problem solving emphasizes the identification of major problems in the team. Team members exposed to a problem-solving team-building intervention are sup- posed to become involved in action planning for the solution of those problems and for implementing and evaluating those solu- tions. Role clarification emphasizes increased communication among team members regarding their respective roles within the team. Team members exposed to a role-clarification team- building intervention are supposed to achieve better understanding of their
  • 11. and others’ respective roles and duties within the team. These models might be best thought of as components of any given team-building intervention. That is, any team-building inter- vention might engage any or all of these various components in varying degrees. For example, Wegenast’s (1983) team-building intervention in child protective service workers involved a rela- tively high emphasis on problem solving, but relatively low empha- sis on interpersonal relations. Alternatively, Paul and Gross’s (1981) team-building intervention in the communications and elec- trical division of city government involved a relatively high empha- sis on role clarification but relatively low emphasis on goal setting. This diversity in team-building interventions represents one of the major challenges to previous efforts to make sense of the re- search literature on team building. In the present effort, we viewed this diversity among team-building interventions as a unique Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 315 opportunity to gauge the relative impact of the four components of team building delineated above (goal setting, interpersonal rela- tions, problem solving, and role clarification). To date, there is no evidence regarding which of these components of team building are more critical to the putative effects of team building on
  • 12. perform- ance. Therefore, another important goal of the present effort is to determine the relative contributions of these components of team building to the team building–performance effect. CONTRIBUTIONS OF OPERATIONALIZATION OF PERFORMANCE Two broad sets of operationalizations of performance typify research in the area of team performance. On one hand, objective indicators of performance include direct measures of countable behaviors, like the number of units sold or the tons of ore mined. On the other hand, subjective indicators of performance include ratings of the performance or effectiveness of the team. Two separate meta-analyses have reported relatively modest correlations between objective and subjective measures of performance (Bom- mer, Johnson, Rich, Podsakoff, & MacKenzie, 1995, r = .389; Heneman, 1986, r = .27). This led Heneman (1986) to conclude that objective and subjective measures of performance are not substitut- able, and that “when reviews of the literature are conducted, results should be grouped by the type of performance criteria” (p. 820). There is no a priori expectation for the effect of team building on performance to be greater for one or the other of these two opera- tionalizations. However, the relatively modest correlations previ-
  • 13. ously observed between objective and subjective measures of per- formance led us to examine the effect of team building on performance separately for each type of performance measure. CONTRIBUTIONS OF TEAM SIZE The size of social groups has been shown to affect the magnitude of several other group phenomena, including the participation- leadership effect (Mullen, Salas, & Driskell, 1989), the ingroup- 316 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 bias effect (Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992), the cohesiveness- performance effect (Mullen & Copper, 1994), the groupthink effect (Mullen, Anthony, Salas, & Driskell, 1994), and the effect of stress inoculation training on performance (Saunders, Driskell, Johnston, & Salas, 1996) (for a discussion, see Mullen, 1991). Larger groups tend to encourage deindividuation among group members (e.g., Mullen, 1987). Moreover, it is well established that as group size increases, members’liking for the group (e.g., Indik, 1965) and per- formance (Mullen, 1987) tend to decrease. In larger groups, group members would be expected to be less engaged in the group, less open to the team-building intervention, and less likely to perform
  • 14. with any concern for excellence. Thus, we would expect that the effect of team building on performance would be weaker in larger teams and stronger in smaller teams. In spite of the consistent tendency for the size of the group to affect several similar group phenomena, there has been no consid- eration to date of whether team size moderates the effect of team building on performance. Therefore, another important goal of the present effort was to determine the extent to which team size affects the impact of team building on performance. CONTRIBUTIONS OF DURATION OF THE TEAM-BUILDING INTERVENTION The duration of team-building interventions varies considerably in the extant literature, from 2 days (Smith, 1976) to 8 months (Bul- ler & Bell, 1986). Common sense might suggest that interventions of longer duration might exert greater effects: If a little of the inter- vention works, then a lot of the intervention might work even more. However, interventions of longer duration have paradoxically been found to render weaker effects in such diverse areas as mental prac- tice effects (Driskell, Copper, & Moran, 1994), self-help treatment groups (Gould & Clum, 1993), and group-work interventions with
  • 15. depressed older persons (Gorey & Cryns, 1991). However, the nature of the relationship between the duration of team building and the effect of team building on performance remains unclear. Therefore, another goal of the present effort is to determine the Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 317 effect of the duration of the team-building intervention on the team building–performance effect. A META-ANALYTIC INTEGRATION In an effort to provide a clearer picture of the effects of team building on performance, a meta-analytic integration (Mullen, 1989; Mullen & Rosenthal, 1985; Rosenthal, 1991) was conducted. The goals of this effort were (a) to provide a precise summary of the significance, the strength, and the direction of the effects of team building on performance; (b) to examine different magnitudes of this effect of team building on performance as a function of the operationalization of performance; (c) to examine the possible moderation of the effect of team building on performance by the four components of team building (goal setting, interpersonal rela- tions, problem solving, and role clarification); (d) to determine whether the effect of team building on performance would decrease as a function of team size; and (e) to scrutinize the effects of the duration of the team-building intervention on the effects of team building.
  • 16. PROCEDURE Using all of the standard literature search techniques, an exhaus- tive search was conducted for studies testing the effect of team- building interventions on performance. First, on-line computer searches were conducted, using the keywords team(s), group(s), or crew(s) and building, development, performance, productivity, or effects. These computer searches were supplemented by ancestry approach and descendency approach searches, correspondence with researchers active in this domain (the invisible college), and manually examining the past 30 years of social psychology, applied psychology, and management journals (see Mullen, 1989, for a dis- cussion of literature search techniques). Second, we reviewed each of the team building studies included in the previous narrative 318 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 reviews by Woodman and Sherwood (1980), DeMeuse and Lie- bowitz (1981), Buller (1986), Sundstrom, DeMeuse, and Futrell (1990), and Tannenbaum, Beard, and Salas (1992). Any studies that were available as of March 1998 were eligible for inclusion in this integration. Studies were included if they met the following criteria: Team
  • 17. members in the studies had to be adolescents or adults not sampled from abnormal populations. Studies had to report (or allow the reconstruction of) a precise test of the effect of a team-building intervention either on an objective measure of performance (e.g., productivity data) or a subjective measure of performance (e.g., rat- ing of team effectiveness). Case studies were not included (e.g., “Teamwork Through Conflict,” 1971), nor were T-group (e.g., Argy- ris, 1962) or managerial grid (e.g., Blake & Mouton, 1966) inter- ventions. The effect of these criteria for inclusion was to focus on the effects of team building on performance in studies that were optimally homogeneous in methodological terms. Hypothesis tests were coded as having a positive direction of effect if team building increased performance, and they were coded as having a negative direction of effect if team building decreased performance. These selection criteria rendered a total of 11 studies (Buller & Bell, 1986; Eden, 1986; Friedlander, 1967; Howard, 1979; Kim- berly & Nielsen, 1975; Paul & Gross, 1981; Smith, 1976; Wakeley & Shaw, 1965; Wegenast, 1983; Wexler, 1990; Woodman, 1978). These 11 articles yielded 16 separate tests of the effects of team building on performance, representing the responses of 2,806 team members in 307 teams.1 Attesting to the thoroughness of the litera- ture search, the included reports spanned a period of more than 30
  • 18. years, and 4 of the 11 reports (36%) were unpublished dissertations. In addition to the requisite statistical information, each hypothe- sis test was coded for seven predictors. The nature of the operation- alization of performance (objective vs. subjective), the size of the team, and the duration of the team-building intervention were all directly coded from each report by two judges with perfect reliabil- ity. In addition, each team-building intervention procedure was also scored for the extent to which that intervention would engage Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 319 each of the components of team building. To be specific, two judges independently rated each team-building intervention on a scale from 0 (low) to 100 (high) for goal setting, interpersonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification (as defined above). Acceptably high levels of interjudge reliability were obtained for each of these components: Goal setting (interjudge reliability r = .693, Spearman-Brown Effective Reliability R = .819), interper- sonal relations (r = .822, R = .902), problem solving (r = .945, R = .972), and role clarification (r = .580, R = .734). The extent to which
  • 19. each intervention engaged each component of team building was set equal to the average of the judges’ ratings for each component. The hypothesis tests included in this meta-analysis, along with the corresponding statistical information and predictors for each hypothesis test, are presented in Table 1. RESULTS General effect. Overall, there was a nonsignificant, z = 0.132, p = .45, negligible, ZFisher = 0.007, r = .007, effect of team- building inter- ventions on performance. Thus, the weight of available evidence fails to substantiate the effectiveness of team-building interven- tions to increase performance.2 Objective versus subjective measures of performance. For the k = 8 hypothesis tests that used objective measures of performance, the effect of team building on performance was a nonsignificant, z = –0.511, p = .70, negative, ZFisher = –0.040, r = –.040. For the k = 8 hypothesis tests that used subjective measures of performance, there was a significant, z = 2.589, p = .004, albeit still weak, ZFisher = 0.140, r = .139, positive effect of team building on performance. These two effects were significantly different from one another, z = 3.398, p = .0003. Thus, although subjective reports indicate that team building appears to increase performance, objective indica- tors of performance reveal no genuine effect of team building on
  • 59. se d pe rf or m an ce . 320 Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 321 Contributions of components of team building. The mean judged level of each component of team building was used to examine the contributions of the various components of team building. The team building–performance effect did not vary as a function of the level of goal setting engaged by the team-building intervention, (r = –.164, z = 1.165, p = .12). In other words, the extent to which a given team-building intervention engaged the component of goal setting neither increased or decreased the magnitude of the effect of team building on performance. Similar to this, the extent to which a given
  • 60. team-building intervention engaged the component of interper- sonal relations (r = –.056, z = 0.485, p = .3138) or problem solving (r = –.049, z = 0.342, p = .37) neither increased or decreased the magnitude of the effect of team building on performance. The only component of team building to significantly predict the effect of team building on performance was role clarification (r = .759, z = 5.533, p < .001). In other words, the more a given team-building intervention engaged the component of role clarification, the stronger the magnitude of the effect of team building on performance. It should be noted that there is no moderation of these patterns for components of team building by the operationalization of per- formance. That is, for both objective and subjective indices respec- tively, there was no effect of goal setting (r = –.060, z = 0.182, p = .43 and r = –.110, z = 0.972, p = .17), interpersonal (r = –.382, z = 1.258, p = .10 and r = –.035, z = 0.325, p = .37), or problem solving (r = –.305, z = 0.780, p = .22 and r = .088, z = 1.298, p = .10). How- ever, for both objective and subjective indices respectively, there was a significant effect of role clarification (r = .712, z = 4.565, p < .001 and r = .752, z = 4.594, p < .001). Contributions of team size. Overall, the effects of team building on performance decreased as a function of the size of the team, r = –344, z = 5.965, p < .001. This inverse effect of team size on the
  • 61. effect of team building held for both objective measures of per- formance (r = –.400, z = 2.383, p = .009) and subjective measures of performance (r = –.407, z = 7.796, p < .001). Thus, whatever 322 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 positive effect team building may have is most likely to emerge in small groups. Contributions of duration of the team-building intervention. Over- all, there was a marginal tendency for the effects of team building on performance to decrease as a function of duration of the team- building intervention, r = –.197, z = 1.452, p = .07. This inverse effect of duration on the effect of team building did not obtain for objective measures of performance (r = –.056, z = 0.194, p = .42), but did obtain for subjective measures of performance (r = – .208, z = 2.562, p = .005). Thus, the negligible effects of team building on objective indices of performance are not influenced by the duration of the team-building intervention, but the appearance of benefits from team building on more subjective measures seems to dissipate for more lengthy team-building interventions. DISCUSSION
  • 62. One of the most important results to emerge from the present analyses is the documentation of the overall nonsignificant and negligible effect of team building on performance. These results suggest that the enthusiastic testimonials on behalf of team- build- ing interventions should be interpreted with caution. The overall magnitude of the effect of team building on performance of r = .007 indicates that approximately .005% of the variability in a team’s performance might be accounted for by knowing whether the team had gone through a team-building intervention. In other words, approximately 99% of the variability in a team’s performance is attributable to factors other than whether the team had gone through a team-building intervention. Relative to some rather bold proclamations regarding the beneficial effects of team building interventions (e.g., Shandler & Egan, 1996), future summaries of the effects of team building on performance should be a bit more modest. When the data were disaggregated into objective performance indicators versus subjective performance indicators, it was shown Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 323 that objective performance indicators rendered functionally no effect of team building on performance, whereas subjective per- formance indicators rendered a significant, albeit still weak, effect
  • 63. of team building on performance. This difference between results for objective performance indicators and subjective performance indicators is not surprising, given the modest convergence previ- ously observed between these different operationalizations (Bom- mer et al., 1995; Heneman, 1986). The analyses for these two dif- ferent performance indicators are even more sobering than the overall lack of beneficial effects of team building: Not only is there an overall lack of beneficial effects of team building, but there is not even much of a measurement-specific benefit of team-building interventions of which to speak. The examination of the contributions of the four components of team building yielded a very intriguing result: The team building- performance effect did not vary as a function of the level of goal set- ting, interpersonal relations, or problem solving engaged by the team-building intervention. It was only role clarification that seemed to make a genuine contribution to the effect of team build- ing on performance, and this pattern emerged for both of the opera- tionalizations of performance. This significant effect for role clari- fication is entirely consistent with evidence from other quarters regarding the importance of role clarification and role ambiguity in team performance. For example, Abramis’s (1994) recent meta- analysis reported that the general weight of evidence indicates that
  • 64. role ambiguity exerts a negative impact on performance. The pres- ent results suggest that future research on team building and per- formance should further examine the positive effects of the role clarification component of team building. The results for team size on the effects of team building are con- sistent with patterns documented in other group phenomena (e.g., Mullen et al., 1989; Mullen et al., 1992; Mullen et al., 1994; Mullen & Copper, 1994; Mullen, 1991). For years, researchers studying group and team performance have searched for the magic numbers that could summarize the effects of the group on the individual. It is interesting to note that Old (1946) long ago proposed that the opti- mum size for a group was 0.7 persons. This tongue-in-cheek 324 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 proposal, although somewhat limiting in terms of implementing group training, keeps with the results of the present analyses: As the size of the team increases, the effectiveness of team-building inter- ventions decreases. Optimal benefit from a team-building interven- tion seems most likely to be obtained with relatively small teams.
  • 65. The results for the effects of duration of the team-building inter- vention are more complex. Although there was a marginal ten- dency overall for longer team-building interventions to render weaker effects, this pattern was actually due only to the influence of duration on the effects of team building on subjective indicators of performance. Thus, although shorter duration team-building inter- ventions might be of some benefit to subjective performance mea- sures, there does not seem to be any difference between shorter and longer team-building interventions on objective performance measures. Of course, there are limitations to our analysis and to the impli- cations that can be drawn from it. As noted earlier, the term team building has been defined somewhat broadly in the literature to include various types of group interventions that are conceptually quite dissimilar. As Lipsey and Wilson (1993) noted in a recent review of the general efficacy of psychological treatments, there are often gray areas as to what constitutes a certain intervention. Our approach was to cast a very narrow net, adopting a precise approach to defining team building and to including studies in the analysis. This decision resulted in a small, tightly-focused database of studies, yet one that we feel is highly representative of this research domain. Second, this analysis allowed us to examine sev- eral theoretically and practically interesting moderators of the
  • 66. effect of team building on performance, such as team size and dura- tion of training. There are other potentially informative modera- tors, such as the type or task (cf. Tannenbaum et al., 1992), that the literature did not allow us to examine. Further primary level research is needed to examine other conditions under which team building may be more effective. Finally, there are methodological limitations of this analysis. There were no studies in this database that reported reliability coefficients for the performance measures Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 325 used, so adjustments for attenuation were not attempted. The fail- ure of the source information to report reliabilities for performance measures is not unique to this particular meta-analysis, but it repre- sents a common limitation of the research literature. In summary, the present results indicate that overall there is no significant effect of team building on performance. Moreover, what little benefits team building might exert on performance are likely to be seen in interventions that emphasize role clarification in smaller groups (and perhaps, in interventions that are more brief). Future research might be directed toward further examinations
  • 67. of the relative contributions of the four components of team building and to scrutinizing the differential effects of team building on dif- ferent indicators of performance. Finally, we note that in examining team building almost 20 years ago, Scherer (1979) posed the ques- tion, “How can something that feels so good not be worthwhile?” (p. 335). Subsequent reviews of team-building research cautioned that evidence of an effect of team building on performance was “inconclusive” (Buller, 1986, p. 147), “unsubstantiated” (Wood- man & Sherwood, 1980, p. 166), “equivocal” (Tannenbaum et al., 1992, p. 146), and “mixed” (Sundstrom et al., 1990, p. 128). The present analysis of the effect of team building on performance pro- vides an empirical basis for the caution voiced by these reviewers. NOTES 1. The included studies reported varying numbers of hypothesis tests, ranging from one per study (e.g., Eden, 1986) to three per study (e.g., Buller & Bell, 1986). In the meta- analysis reported, each hypothesis test was treated as an independent observation. This assumption of independence is patently false. For example, each of the three hypothesis tests included in Buller and Bell (1986) was derived from the same subject population at the same
  • 68. time. However, without making this assumption of nonindependence, one would be forced to select the best hypothesis test from a study such as Buller and Bell or to pool the results from the reported hypothesis tests into a single test. In the present context, these alternatives seem even more arbitrary and capricious than the present assumption of independence. The effects of this assumption of independence are examined later. 2. As indicated in Note 1, the assumption that each of the 16 hypothesis tests represented an independent observation is false. However, it can be seen that such an assumption does not seem to render a distorted summary of this research domain. Consider the results of a 326 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 supplementary meta-analysis of wholly independent effects, in which multiple hypothesis tests obtained from a single study were combined into a single test. This heavy-handed solu- tion precludes the examination of the effects of any of the moderators considered above, but it does eliminate the problem of nonindependence. This produced 11 distinct, wholly inde- pendent hypothesis tests, one from each includable study. The results of this supplemental meta-analysis revealed the same patterns reported previously: There was a nonsignificant, z = 0.440, p = .33, negligible, ZFisher = 0.032, r = .032, effect of team-building interventions on performance. These results indicate that the degree of
  • 69. distortion engendered by the assumption of independence of the original 16 hypothesis tests is (at worst) tolerable. REFERENCES Abramis, D. J. (1994). Work role ambiguity, job satisfaction, and job performance: Meta- analyses and review. Psychological Reports, 75, 1411-1433. Argyris, C. (1962). Interpersonal competence and organizational behavior. Homewood, IL: Irwin. Beckhard, R. (1966). An organization improvement program in a decentralized organiza- tion. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 2, 3-25. Beer, M. (1976). The technology of organization development. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 937- 994). Chicago: Rand McNally. Beer, M. (1980). Organization change and development: A systems view. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Bigelow, R. C. (1971). Changing classroom interaction through organization development. In R. A. Schmuck & M. B. Miles (Eds.), Organization development in schools. Palo Alto, CA: National Press. Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. (1966). Some effects of managerial grid training on union and
  • 70. management attitudes towards supervision. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 2, 387-400. Bommer, W. H., Johnson, J. L., Rich, G. A., Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1995). On the interchangeability of objective and subjective measures of employee perform- ance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 48, 587-605. Buller, P. F. (1986). The team building–task performance relation: Some conceptual and methodological refinements. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 147-168. Buller, P. F., & Bell, C. H. (1986). Effects of team building and goal setting on productivity: A field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 305- 328. DeMeuse, K. P., & Liebowitz, S. J. (1981). An empirical analysis of team building research. Group and Organization Studies, 6, 357-378. Driskell, J. E., Copper, C. M., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance perform- ance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481-492. Druckman, D., & Bjork, R. A. (Eds.). (1994). Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Dyer, W. G. (1977). Team building: Issues and alternatives. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley. Eden, D. (1986). Team development: Quasi-experimental confirmation among combat com-
  • 71. panies. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 133-146. Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 327 Friedlander, F. (1967). The impact of organizational training laboratories upon effectiveness of ongoing work groups. Personnel Psychology, 20, 289-307. George, W. W. (1977). Task teams for rapid growth. Harvard Business Review, 55, 71-80. Golembiewski, R. T., & Kiepper, A. (1976). MARTA: Toward an effective open giant. Public Administration Review, 36, 46-60. Gorey, K. M., & Cryns, A. G. (1991). Group work as interventive modality with the older depressed client: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 16, 137-157. Gould, R. A., & Clum, G. A. (1993). A meta-analysis of self- help treatment approaches. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 169-186. Halstead, L. S., Rintala, D. H., Kanellos, M., Griffin, B., Higgins, L., Rheinecker, N., White- side, W., & Healy, J. E. (1986). The innovative rehabilitation team: An experiment in team building. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 67, 357-361. Hardy, C. J., & Crace, R. K. (1997). Foundations of team
  • 72. building: Introduction to the team building primer. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 9, 1-10. Heneman, R. L. (1986). The relationship between supervisory ratings and results-oriented measures of performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 39, 811-826. Howard, J. (1979). Organizational team building in church- related teams. Unpublished dis- sertation, California School of Professional Psychology, San Diego. Indik, B. P. (1965). Operational size and member participation: Some empirical tests of alter- native explanations. Human Relations, 18, 339-350. Kimberly, J. R., & Nielsen, W. R. (1975). Organization development and change in organiza- tional performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 20, 191- 206. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48, 1181-1209. Mullen, B. (1987). Self-attention theory. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 125-146). New York: Springer-Verlag. Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC Meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Mullen, B. (1991). Group composition, salience, and cognitive representations: The phe-
  • 73. nomenology of being in a group. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 297-323. Mullen, B., Anthony, T., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1994). Group cohesiveness and quality of decision making: An integration of tests of the groupthink hypothesis. Small Group Research, 25, 189-204. Mullen, B., Brown, R. J., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a function of salience, rele- vance, and status: An integration. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 103-122. Mullen, B., & Copper, C. (1994). The relation between group cohesiveness and perform- ance: An integration. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 210-227. Mullen, B., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). BASIC Meta-analysis: Procedures and programs. Hills- dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Mullen, B., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1989). Salience, motivation, and artifact as contribu- tions to the relation between participation rate and leadership. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 545-559. Old, B. S. (1946). On the mathematics of committees, boards, and panels. Scientific Monthly, 63, 75-78.
  • 74. 328 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / June 1999 Paul, C. F., & Gross, A. C. (1981). Increasing productivity and morale in a municipality: Effects of organizational development. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 17, 59-78. Porras, J. I., & Berg, P. O. (1978). The impact of organization development. Academy of Management Review, 3, 249-266. Robinson-Kurpius, S. E., & Keim, J. (1994). Team building for nurses experiencing burnout and poor morale. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 19, 155-161. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Saunders, T., Driskell, J. E., Johnston, J., & Salas, E. (1996). The effect of stress inoculation training on anxiety and performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 170-186. Scherer, J. J. (1979). Can team building increase productivity? or How can something that feels so good not be worthwhile? Group and Organizational Studies, 4, 335-351. Shandler, M., & Egan, M. (1996). VROOM! Turbo-charged team building. New York: American Management Association.
  • 75. Smith, P. E. (1976). Management modeling training to improve morale and customer satis- faction. Personnel Psychology, 29, 351-359. Smither, R. D., Houston, J. M., & McIntire, S. A. (1996). Organizational development: Strategies for changing environments. New York: HarperCollins. Sundstrom, E., DeMeuse, K. P., & Futrell, D. (1990). Work teams: Applications and effec- tiveness. American Psychologist, 45, 120-133. Tannenbaum, S. I., Beard, R. L., & Salas, E. (1992). Team building and its influence on team effectiveness: An examination of conceptual and empirical developments. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Issues, theory, and research in industrial/organizational psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Teamwork through conflict. (1971). Business Week, 2168, pp. 44-50. Wakeley, J. H., & Shaw, M. F. (1965). Management training: An integrated approach. Train- ing Directors Journal, 19, 2-13. Wegenast, D. P. (1983). Team building: An application of leadership skills to case manage- ment in child protective services. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City University of New York, New York City. Wexler, K. J. (1990). Team building: A multidimensional analysis. Unpublished doctoral
  • 76. dissertation, Hofstra University, Hempstead, New York. Woodman, R. W. (1978). Effects of team development intervention: A field experiment. Ph.D. dissertation, Purdue University. Woodman, R. W., & Sherwood, J. J. (1980). The role of team development in organizational effectiveness: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 166-186. Eduardo Salas, Ph.D., is a senior research psychologist at the Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division. His research interests include team training, training effectiveness, and team performance. Drew Rozell, Ph.D., is a personal and business coach with Evolution Coaching in Syracuse, New York. His interests include team building, management training, and executive success. Salas et al. / TEAM BUILDING 329 James E. Driskell, Ph.D., is president of Florida Maxima Corporation and an adjunct professor of psychology at Rollins College, Winter Park, FL. His research interests include status processes, group dynamics, and performance under stress. Brian Mullen, Ph.D., is a professor in the Department of Psychology at Syracuse
  • 77. University. His research interests include social cognition perspectives on (inter) group phenomena and meta-analysis. Structure BookmarksArtifactTHE EFFECT OF TEAM BUILDING ON PERFORMANCE An Integration THE EFFECT OF TEAM BUILDING ON PERFORMANCE An Integration EDUARDO SALAS Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division DREW ROZELL BRIAN MULLEN Syracuse University JAMES E. DRISKELL Florida Maxima Corporation In this article, meta-analytic integration of research examining the effects of team building on performance is reported. Overall, there was no significant effect of team building on performance. However, the effects of team building varied as a function of the type of operationalization of performance: On objective measures of performance, there was a nonsignificant tendency for team building to decrease performance, whereas on subjective measures of performance, there was a significant, albeit small, tend------Harry, you weren’t kidding when you said this Florida trip would be no vacation! Building a group like us into a high-performing team takes really hard work! But I wouldn’t have missed it for worlds! —Jack, a participant in a team-building intervention (Shandler & Egan, 1996) Team building, also called team development or group development, is an extremely popular and common intervention. In fact, - SMALL GROUP RESEARCH, Vol. 30 No. 3, June 1999 309- 329 © 1999 Sage Publications, Inc. Porras and Berg (1978) observed that team building was one of the most frequently used organization development interventions. Enthusiastic reports can be found for the application of team building to such diverse settings as hotel management organizations (e.g., Beckhard, 1966), microwave cooking manufacturers (George, 1977), metropolitan rapid transit authorities (Golembiewski & Kiepper, 1976), medical rehabilitation teams (Halstead et al., 1986), and nursing teams (Robinson-Kurpius & Keim, 1994). A recen-----In spite of this popularity of the concept of team building, however, several reviewers (e.g., Buller, 1986;
  • 78. Woodman & Sherwood, 1980) have observed that there is no conclusive evidence that team building renders an increase in team performance. Druckman and Bjork (1994) noted that the enthusiasm for these approaches among practitioners “is not matched by strong empirical support for their effect on team performance” (p. 125). Similar to this, Smither, Houston, and McIntire (1996) concluded that “Research f-Several issues need to be addressed in examining the effect of team building on performance. First, the significance and AUTHORS’NOTE: The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not reflect the official position of their organizations. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eduardo Salas, Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division, Code 4961, 12350 Research Parkway, Orlando, FL 32826-3275. -magnitude—indeed, the very existence—of an effect of team building on performance needs to be established. Second, differences between objective and subjective indices of performance have been delineated in other domains, but there is no a priori determination of the extent to which team building would differentially affect these two different ways of operationalizing performance. Third, the degree to which a given team-building intervention engages different components of team-building might influence the ----THE EXISTENCE OF A TEAM-BUILDING EFFECT THE EXISTENCE OF A TEAM- BUILDING EFFECT Team building has at its core the central notion that enlisting the participation of a group in planning and implementing change will be more effective than simply imposing change on the group from outside. The process by which the members of the team become able to effectively participate in the targeted change requires that the team acquire new skills and perceptions. The various definitions of team building that have been proposed in the research literature resonate to these elements of participation and----The effects of team building are often described in extremely sanguinary terms. For example, the epigram at the beginning of this The effects of team building are often described in extremely
  • 79. sanguinary terms. For example, the epigram at the beginning of this report is extracted from a primer on team building, entitled VROOM! Turbo-Charged Team Building (Shandler & Egan, 1996). Therein, the president of a high-tech research and development firm uses a team-building intervention to rescue his foundering company, ensure his promotion in the corporate hierarchy, and even save his failing marriage. It would seem that any intervention capable of such impressive effects would have generated an imposing body of evidence supporting its effectiveness. -Unfortunately, a clear summary of the effects of team building on performance apparently cannot be formulated from narrative readings of previous research. Indeed, several narrative literature reviews have been conducted in this domain within the past two decades, and the judgments rendered by these previous review efforts have been inconclusive. Woodman and Sherwood (1980) reviewed 30 papers and concluded that there were generally positive results for most of the studies but that there was no clear-cut evi---It is interesting to consider the nature of the body of studies considered in many previous narrative reviews of the effects of team building. First, there was a remarkable lack of convergence among these reviews regarding which studies to include in a review of team-building interventions. Of the 69 reports examined in at least one of these narrative reviews, on average only 4 studies (or 5.8%) were included by both of any two reviews. The point is that more than 90% of the literature summarized in each ofIt is interesting to consider the nature of the body of studies considered in many previous narrative reviews of the effects of team building. First, there was a remarkable lack of convergence among these reviews regarding which studies to include in a review of team-building interventions. Of the 69 reports examined in at least one of these narrative reviews, on average only 4 studies (or 5.8%) were included by both of any two reviews. The point is that more than 90% of the literature summarized in each of-reviews of team building was referred to in none of the other narrative reviews. This stunning lack of
  • 80. convergence on precisely what constitutes an includable study of team building is in part a reflection of the complexity of the notion of team building (to be elucidated further). ---It is useful to consider a few other intriguing facets of the studies considered in previous narrative reviews of the effects of team building. The most common type of report examined in these previous reviews was the case study (20 out of 69 studies or 29%), describing the application of a team-building intervention in some organizational setting with no empirical examination of the effectiveness of the intervention (e.g., Beckhard, 1966; George, 1977). These case studies, although interesting and illumina----In sum, we cannot determine whether there is a beneficial effect of team building on performance based on previous narrative perusals of this research domain. Part of the problem lies in the ambiguity of what precisely is team building and what studies should be included in an effort to integrate the effect of team building on performance. Part of the problem is that the team-building literature includes a variety of case studies, nonempirical reports, and tests of various types of outcome measures. Therefo-COMPONENTS OF TEAM BUILDING COMPONENTS OF TEAM BUILDING Beer (1976, 1980), Dyer (1977), and Buller (1986) have discussed four current models of team building: Goal setting, interpersonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification. Goal setting emphasizes the setting of objectives and the development of individual and team goals. Team members exposed to a goal- setting team-building intervention are supposed to become involved in action planning to identify ways to achieve those goals. Interpersonal relations emphasizes an increase in team work skills, suc-----These models might be best thought of as components of any given team-building intervention. That is, any team-building intervention might engage any or all of these various components in varying degrees. For example, Wegenast’s (1983) team-building intervention in child protective service workers involved a relatively high emphasis on problem solving, but relatively low emphasis on
  • 81. interpersonal relations. Alternatively, Paul and Gross’s (1981) team-building intervention in the communications and electrica- ----This diversity in team-building interventions represents one of the major challenges to previous efforts to make sense of the research literature on team building. In the present effort, we viewed this diversity among team-building interventions as a unique This diversity in team-building interventions represents one of the major challenges to previous efforts to make sense of the research literature on team building. In the present effort, we viewed this diversity among team-building interventions as a unique -opportunity to gauge the relative impact of the four components of team building delineated above (goal setting, interpersonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification). To date, there is no evidence regarding which of these components of team building are more critical to the putative effects of team building on performance. Therefore, another important goal of the present effort is to determine the relative contributions of these components of team building to the team building–performance effec--CONTRIBUTIONS OF OPERATIONALIZATION OF PERFORMANCE CONTRIBUTIONS OF OPERATIONALIZATION OF PERFORMANCE Two broad sets of operationalizations of performance typify research in the area of team performance. On one hand, objective indicators of performance include direct measures of countable behaviors, like the number of units sold or the tons of ore mined. On the other hand, subjective indicators of performance include ratings of the performance or effectiveness of the team. Two separate meta-analyses have reported relatively modest correlations between objective and subjective measures of performance (Bommer----- CONTRIBUTIONS OF TEAM SIZE CONTRIBUTIONS OF TEAM SIZE The size of social groups has been shown to affect the magnitude of several other group phenomena, including the participation-leadership effect (Mullen, Salas, & Driskell, 1989), the ingroupThe size of social groups has been shown to affect the magnitude of several other group phenomena,
  • 82. including the participation-leadership effect (Mullen, Salas, & Driskell, 1989), the ingroup-bias effect (Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992), the cohesiveness-performance effect (Mullen & Copper, 1994), the groupthink effect (Mullen, Anthony, Salas, & Driskell, 1994), and the effect of stress inoculation training on performance (Saunders, Driskell, Johnston, & Salas, 1996) (for a discussion, see Mullen, 1991). Larger groups tend to encourage deindividuation among group members (e.g., Mullen, 1987). Moreover, it is well established that as group size increases, members’liking for the group (e.g., Indik, -In spite of the consistent tendency for the size of the group to affect several similar group phenomena, there has been no consideration to date of whether team size moderates the effect of team building on performance. Therefore, another important goal of the present effort was to determine the extent to which team size affects the impact of team building on performance. - CONTRIBUTIONS OF DURATION OF THE TEAM- BUILDING INTERVENTION CONTRIBUTIONS OF DURATION OF THE TEAM-BUILDING INTERVENTION The duration of team-building interventions varies considerably in the extant literature, from 2 days (Smith, 1976) to 8 months (Buller & Bell, 1986). Common sense might suggest that interventions of longer duration might exert greater effects: If a little of the intervention works, then a lot of the intervention might work even more. However, interventions of longer duration have paradoxically been found to render weaker effects in such diverse areas as mental practice effects (Driskell, Copper, & Moran, 1The duration of team-building interventions varies considerably in the extant literature, from 2 days (Smith, 1976) to 8 months (Buller & Bell, 1986). Common sense might suggest that interventions of longer duration might exert greater effects: If a little of the intervention works, then a lot of the intervention might work even more. However, interventions of longer duration have paradoxically been found to render weaker effects in such diverse areas as mental practice effects (Driskell, Copper, & Moran, 1---effect of the duration of the
  • 83. team-building intervention on the team building–performance effect. A META-ANALYTIC INTEGRATION A META- ANALYTIC INTEGRATION In an effort to provide a clearer picture of the effects of team building on performance, a meta- analytic integration (Mullen, 1989; Mullen & Rosenthal, 1985; Rosenthal, 1991) was conducted. The goals of this effort were (a) to provide a precise summary of the significance, the strength, and the direction of the effects of team building on performance; (b) to examine different magnitudes of this effect of team building on performance as a function of the operationalization of performance; (c) to examine the- PROCEDURE PROCEDURE Using all of the standard literature search techniques, an exhaustive search was conducted for studies testing the effect of team-building interventions on performance. First, on-line computer searches were conducted, using the keywords team(s), group(s), or crew(s) and building, development, performance, productivity, or effects. These computer searches were supplemented by ancestry approach and descendency approach searches, correspondence with researchers active in this domain (the invisible college), anUsing all of the standard literature search techniques, an exhaustive search was conducted for studies testing the effect of team-building interventions on performance. First, on-line computer searches were conducted, using the keywords team(s), group(s), or crew(s) and building, development, performance, productivity, or effects. These computer searches were supplemented by ancestry approach and descendency approach searches, correspondence with researchers active in this domain (the invisible college), an--reviews by Woodman and Sherwood (1980), DeMeuse and Liebowitz (1981), Buller (1986), Sundstrom, DeMeuse, and Futrell (1990), and Tannenbaum, Beard, and Salas (1992). Any studies that were available as of March 1998 were eligible for inclusion in this integration. - Studies were included if they met the following criteria: Team members in the studies had to be adolescents or adults not sampled from abnormal populations. Studies had to report (or
  • 84. allow the reconstruction of) a precise test of the effect of a team-building intervention either on an objective measure of performance (e.g., productivity data) or a subjective measure of performance (e.g., rating of team effectiveness). Case studies were not included (e.g., “Teamwork Through Conflict,” 1971), nor were T-group---These selection criteria rendered a total of 11 studies (Buller & Bell, 1986; Eden, 1986; Friedlander, 1967; Howard, 1979; Kimberly & Nielsen, 1975; Paul & Gross, 1981; Smith, 1976; Wakeley & Shaw, 1965; Wegenast, 1983; Wexler, 1990; Woodman, 1978). These 11 articles yielded 16 separate tests of the effects of team building on performance, representing the responses of 2,806 team members in 307 teams.Attesting to the thoroughness of the literature search, the included reports spanned a period of more than 3-1 -In addition to the requisite statistical information, each hypothesis test was coded for seven predictors. The nature of the operationalization of performance (objective vs. subjective), the size of the team, and the duration of the team-building intervention were all directly coded from each report by two judges with perfect reliability. In addition, each team-building intervention procedure was also scored for the extent to which that intervention would engage In addition to the requisite statistical information, each hypothesis test was coded for seven predictors. The nature of the operationalization of performance (objective vs. subjective), the size of the team, and the duration of the team-building intervention were all directly coded from each report by two judges with perfect reliability. In addition, each team-building intervention procedure was also scored for the extent to which that intervention would engage ---each of the components of team building. To be specific, two judges independently rated each team-building intervention on a scale from 0 (low) to 100 (high) for goal setting, interpersonal relations, problem solving, and role clarification (as defined above). Acceptably high levels of interjudge reliability were obtained for each of these components: Goal setting (interjudge reliability r = .693, Spearman-Brown Effective Reliability R =
  • 85. .819), interpersonal relations (r = .822, R = .902), problem solving (-RESULTS RESULTS General effect. Overall, there was a nonsignificant, z = 0.132, p = .45, negligible, ZFisher = 0.007, r = .007, effect of team-building interventions on performance. Thus, the weight of available evidence fails to substantiate the effectiveness of team-building interventions to increase performance.--2 Objective versus subjective measures of performance. For the k = 8 hypothesis tests that used objective measures of performance, the effect of team building on performance was a nonsignificant, z = –0.511, p = .70, negative, ZFisher = –0.040, r = –.040. For the k =8 hypothesis tests that used subjective measures of performance, there was a significant, z = 2.589, p = .004, albeit still weak, ZFisher = 0.140, r = .139, positive effect of team building on performance. These two effects were significantly diff-TABLE 1: Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis Duration Study Statistic rN DOEMeasure Size (in days) GS IR PS RC b Buller and Bell (1986) t(16) = 0.399 –.010 6/18 – Objective 3 240 83.5 32.5 82.5 10.0 t(16) = 1.584 .368 4/8 + Objective 2 240 87.5 5.0 10.0 20.0 t(16) = 0.005 –.001 6/18 – Objective 3 240 25.0 32.5 90.0 10.0 Eden (1986) t(29) = 1.252 –.226 7/220 – Subjective 31.5 3 40.0 77.5 29.0 35.0 Friedlander (1967) F(1, 88) = 3.98 .208 12/91 + Subjective 7.6 5 22.5 70.0 90.0 20.0 Howard (1979) r(17) = .040 .040 2/19 + Objective 9 14 30.0 90.0 40.0 10.0 Kimberley and Nielsen (1975) t(43) = 0.71 –.108 45/900 – ObjectiveNOTE: DOE = Direction of Effect, GS = Goal Setting, IR = Interpersonal Relations, PS = Problem Solving, RC = Role Clarification. a. The first number represents the number of teams; the second number represents the number of team members. b. The positive sign indicates that team building increased performance; the negative sign indicates that team building decreased performance. Contributions of components of team building. The mean judged level of each component of team building was used to examine the contributions of the various components of team building. The team building– performance effect did not vary as a function of the level of
  • 86. goal setting engaged by the team-building intervention, (r = – .164, z = 1.165, p = .12). In other words, the extent to which a given team-building intervention engaged the component of goal setting neither increased or decreased the magnitude of t-It should be noted that there is no moderation of these patterns for components of team building by the operationalization of performance. That is, for both objective and subjective indices respectively, there was no effect of goal setting (r = –.060, z = 0.182, p = .43 and r = –.110, z = 0.972, p = .17), interpersonal (r = –.382, z = 1.258, p = .10 and r = –.035, z = 0.325, p = .37), or problem solving (r = –.305, z = 0.780, p = .22 and r = .088, z = 1.298, p = .10). However, for both objective and subject--- Contributions of team size. Overall, the effects of team building on performance decreased as a function of the size of the team, r = –344, z = 5.965, p < .001. This inverse effect of team size on the effect of team building held for both objective measures of performance (r = –.400, z = 2.383, p = .009) and subjective measures of performance (r = –.407, z = 7.796, p < .001). Thus, whatever Contributions of team size. Overall, the effects of team building on performance decreased as a function of the size of the team, r = –344, z = 5.965, p < .001. This inverse effect of team size on the effect of team building held for both objective measures of performance (r = –.400, z = 2.383, p = .009) and subjective measures of performance (r = –.407, z = 7.796, p < .001). Thus, whatever -positive effect team building may have is most likely to emerge in small groups. Contributions of duration of the team-building intervention.Overall, there was a marginal tendency for the effects of team building on performance to decrease as a function of duration of the team-building intervention, r = – .197, z = 1.452, p = .07. This inverse effect of duration on the effect of team building did not obtain for objective measures of performance (r = –.056, z = 0.194, p = .42), but did obtain for subjective measures of performance (r = –.208, z = 2.562, p = .005). Thus, the negligible eff-DISCUSSION DISCUSSION One of the most important results to emerge from the present
  • 87. analyses is the documentation of the overall nonsignificant and negligible effect of team building on performance. These results suggest that the enthusiastic testimonials on behalf of team- building interventions should be interpreted with caution. The overall magnitude of the effect of team building on performance of r = .007 indicates that approximately .005% of the variability in a team’s performance might be accounted for by knowing whether th-When the data were disaggregated into objective performance indicators versus subjective performance indicators, it was shown When the data were disaggregated into objective performance indicators versus subjective performance indicators, it was shown that objective performance indicators rendered functionally no effect of team building on performance, whereas subjective performance indicators rendered a significant, albeit still weak, effect of team building on performance. This difference between results for objective performance indicators and subjective performance indicators is not surprising, given the modest convergence previously observed between these different operationalizations (Bommer et al., 1995; Heneman, 1986). The analyses for these two ---- The examination of the contributions of the four components of team building yielded a very intriguing result: The team building-performance effect did not vary as a function of the level of goal setting, interpersonal relations, or problem solving engaged by the team-building intervention. It was only role clarification that seemed to make a genuine contribution to the effect of team building on performance, and this pattern emerged for both of the operationalizations of performance. This significant effec------The results for team size on the effects of team building are consistent with patterns documented in other group phenomena (e.g., Mullen et al., 1989; Mullen et al., 1992; Mullen et al., 1994; Mullen & Copper, 1994; Mullen, 1991). For years, researchers studying group and team performance have searched for the magic numbers that could summarize the effects of the group on the individual. It is interesting to note that Old (1946) long ago
  • 88. proposed that the optimum size for a group was 0.7 persons. This tonguThe results for team size on the effects of team building are consistent with patterns documented in other group phenomena (e.g., Mullen et al., 1989; Mullen et al., 1992; Mullen et al., 1994; Mullen & Copper, 1994; Mullen, 1991). For years, researchers studying group and team performance have searched for the magic numbers that could summarize the effects of the group on the individual. It is interesting to note that Old (1946) long ago proposed that the optimum size for a group was 0.7 persons. This tongu--proposal, although somewhat limiting in terms of implementing group training, keeps with the results of the present analyses: As the size of the team increases, the effectiveness of team-building interventions decreases. Optimal benefit from a team-building intervention seems most likely to be obtained with relatively small teams. -- The results for the effects of duration of the team-building intervention are more complex. Although there was a marginal tendency overall for longer team-building interventions to render weaker effects, this pattern was actually due only to the influence of duration on the effects of team building on subjective indicators of performance. Thus, although shorter duration team-building interventions might be of some benefit to subjective performance measures, there does not seem to be any difference between s----Of course, there are limitations to our analysis and to the implications that can be drawn from it. As noted earlier, the term team building has been defined somewhat broadly in the literature to include various types of group interventions that are conceptually quite dissimilar. As Lipsey and Wilson (1993) noted in a recent review of the general efficacy of psychological treatments, there are often gray areas as to what constitutes a certain intervention. Our approach was to cast a very narrow net, adoptinOf course, there are limitations to our analysis and to the implications that can be drawn from it. As noted earlier, the term team building has been defined somewhat broadly in the literature to include various types of group interventions that are conceptually quite
  • 89. dissimilar. As Lipsey and Wilson (1993) noted in a recent review of the general efficacy of psychological treatments, there are often gray areas as to what constitutes a certain intervention. Our approach was to cast a very narrow net, adoptin----used, so adjustments for attenuation were not attempted. The failure of the source information to report reliabilities for performance measures is not unique to this particular meta-analysis, but it represents a common limitation of the research literature. --In summary, the present results indicate that overall there is no significant effect of team building on performance. Moreover, what little benefits team building might exert on performance are likely to be seen in interventions that emphasize role clarification in smaller groups (and perhaps, in interventions that are more brief). Future research might be directed toward further examinations of the relative contributions of the four components of team building and to scrutinizing the differential effects o--(p. 335). Subsequent reviews of team-building research cautioned that evidence of an effect of team building on performance was “inconclusive” (Buller, 1986, p. 147), “unsubstantiated” (Woodman & Sherwood, 1980, p. 166), “equivocal” (Tannenbaum et al., 1992, p. 146), and “mixed” (Sundstrom et al., 1990, p. 128). The present analysis of the effect of team building on performance provides an empirical basis for the caution voiced by these reviewers. --NOTES NOTES 1. 1. 1. The included studies reported varying numbers of hypothesis tests, ranging from one per study (e.g., Eden, 1986) to three per study (e.g., Buller & Bell, 1986). In the meta-analysis reported, each hypothesis test was treated as an independent observation. This assumption of independence is patently false. For example, each of the three hypothesis tests included in Buller and Bell (1986) was derived from the same subject population at the same time. However, without making this assumption of nonindependence,2. 2. As indicated in Note 1, the assumption that each of the 16 hypothesis tests represented an independent observation is false. However, it can be seen that such an
  • 90. assumption does not seem to render a distorted summary of this research domain. Consider the results of a supplementary meta- analysis of wholly independent effects, in which multiple hypothesis tests obtained from a single study were combined into a single test. This heavy-handed solution precludes the examination of the effects of any of the moderators considered above, but it does eliminate the problem of nonindependence. This produced 11 distinct, wholly independent hypothesis tests, one from each includable study. The results of this supplemental meta-analysis revealed the same patterns reported previously:--REFERENCES REFERENCES Abramis, D. J. (1994). Work role ambiguity, job satisfaction, and job performance: Meta-analyses and review. Psychological Reports, 75, 1411-1433. Argyris, C. (1962). Interpersonal competence and organizational behavior. Homewood, IL: Irwin. Beckhard, R. (1966). An organization improvement program in a decentralized organization. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 2, 3-25. -Beer, M. (1976). The technology of organization development. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 937-994). Chicago: Rand McNally. Beer, M. (1980). Organization change and development: A systems view. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Bigelow, R. C. (1971). Changing classroom interaction through organization development. In R. A. Schmuck & M. B. Miles (Eds.), Organization development in schools. Palo Alto, CA: National Press. Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. (1966). Some effects of managerial grid training on union and management attitudes towards supervision. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 2, 387-400. Bommer, W. H., Johnson, J. L., Rich, G. A., Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1995). On the interchangeability of objective and subjective measures of employee performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 48, 587-605. -Buller, P. F. (1986). The team building–task performance relation: Some conceptual and methodological refinements. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 147-168. Buller, P. F., & Bell, C. H. (1986). Effects of team
  • 91. building and goal setting on productivity: A field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 305-328. DeMeuse, K. P., & Liebowitz, S. J. (1981). An empirical analysis of team building research. Group and Organization Studies, 6, 357-378. Driskell, J. E., Copper, C. M., & Moran, A. (1994). Does-human performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Dyer, W. G. (1977). Team building: Issues and alternatives. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley. Eden, D. (1986). Team development: Quasi-experimental confirmation among combat com-panies. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 133-146. Friedlander, F. (1967). The impact of organizational training laboratories upon effectiveness of ongoing work groups. Personnel Psychology, 20, 289-307. George, W. W. (1977). Task teams for rapid growth. Harvard Business Review, 55, 71-80. Golembiewski, R. T., & Kiepper, A. (1976). MARTA: Toward an effective open giant. Public Administration Review, 36, 46-60. Gorey, K. M., & Cryns, A. G. (1991). Group work as interventive modality with the older depressed client: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 16, 137-157. Gould, R. A., & Clum, G. A. (1993). A meta-analysis of self-help treatment approaches. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 169-186. Halstead, L. S., Rintala, D. H., Kanellos, M., Griffin, B., Higgins, L., Rheinecker, N., White-side, W., & Healy, J. E. (1986). The innovative rehabilitation team: An experiment in team building. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 67, 357-361. Hardy, C. J., & Crace, R. K. (1997). Foundations of team building: Introduction to the team building primer. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 9, 1-10. Heneman, R. L. (1986). The relationship between supervisory ratings and results-oriented measures of performance: A meta- analysis. Personnel Psychology, 39, 811-826. Howard, J. (1979). Organizational team building in church-related teams. Unpublished dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, San Diego. -Indik, B. P. (1965). Operational size and member participation: Some empirical tests of alternative explanations. Human Relations, 18, 339-350. -Kimberly, J. R.,
  • 92. & Nielsen, W. R. (1975). Organization development and change in organizational performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 20, 191-206. -Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48, 1181-1209. Mullen, B. (1987). Self- attention theory. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 125-146). New York: Springer- Verlag. Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC Meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Mullen, B. (1991). Group composition, salience, and cognitive representations: The phenomenology of being in a group. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 297-323. -Mullen, B., Anthony, T., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1994). Group cohesiveness and quality of decision making: An integration of tests of the groupthink hypothesis. Small Group Research, 25, 189-204. Mullen, B., Brown, R. J., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a function of salience, relevance, and status: An integration. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 103-122. -Mullen, B., & Copper, C. (1994). The relation between group cohesiveness and performance: An integration. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 210-227. -Mullen, B., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). BASIC Meta-analysis: Procedures and programs. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. -Mullen, B., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1989). Salience, motivation, and artifact as contributions to the relation between participation rate and leadership. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 545-559. -Old, B. S. (1946). On the mathematics of committees, boards, and panels. Scientific Monthly, 63, 75-78. Paul, C. F., & Gross, A. C. (1981). Increasing productivity and morale in a municipality: Effects of organizational development. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 17, 59-78. Porras, J. I., & Berg, P. O. (1978). The impact of organization development. Academy of Management Review, 3, 249-266. Robinson-Kurpius, S. E., & Keim, J. (1994). Team building for nurses experiencing burnout and poor morale. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 19,
  • 93. 155-161. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Saunders, T., Driskell, J. E., Johnston, J., & Salas, E. (1996). The effect of stress inoculation training on anxiety and performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 170-186. Scherer, J. J. (1979). Can team building increase productivity? or How can something that feels so good not be worthwhile? Group and Organizational Studies, 4, 335-351. Shandler, M., & Egan, M. (1996). VROOM! Turbo-charged team building. New York: American Management Association. Smith, P. E. (1976). Management modeling training to improve morale and customer satisfaction. Personnel Psychology, 29, 351-359. -Smither, R. D., Houston, J. M., & McIntire, S. A. (1996). Organizational development: Strategies for changing environments. New York: HarperCollins. Sundstrom, E., DeMeuse, K. P., & Futrell, D. (1990). Work teams: Applications and effectiveness. American Psychologist, 45, 120-133. -Tannenbaum, S. I., Beard, R. L., & Salas, E. (1992). Team building and its influence on team effectiveness: An examination of conceptual and empirical developments. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Issues, theory, and research in industrial/organizational psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Teamwork through conflict. (1971). Business Week, 2168, pp. 44-50. Wakeley, J. H., & Shaw, M. F. (1965). Management training: An integrated approach. Training Directors Journal, 19, 2-13. -Wegenast, D. P. (1983). Team building: An application of leadership skills to case management in child protective services. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City University of New York, New York City. -Wexler, K. J. (1990). Team building: A multidimensional analysis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Hofstra University, Hempstead, New York. Woodman, R. W. (1978). Effects of team development intervention: A field experiment. Ph.D. dissertation, Purdue University. Woodman, R. W., & Sherwood, J. J. (1980). The role of team development in organizational effectiveness: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 166-186. Eduardo Salas, Ph.D., is a senior research psychologist at the Naval Air
  • 94. Warfare Center Training Systems Division. His research interests include team training, training effectiveness, and team performance. Drew Rozell, Ph.D., is a personal and business coach with Evolution Coaching in Syracuse, New York. His interests include team building, management training, and executive success. James E. Driskell, Ph.D., is president of Florida Maxima Corporation and an adjunct professor of psychology at Rollins College, Winter Park, FL. His research interests include status processes, group dynamics, and performance under stress. Brian Mullen, Ph.D., is a professor in the Department of Psychology at Syracuse University. His research interests include social cognition perspectives on (inter) group phenomena and meta-analysis. European Journal of Personality, Eur. J. Pers. 25: 31–42 (2011) Published online 4 April 2010 (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.769 Personality and the Prediction of Team Performance THOMAS A. O’NEILL* and NATALIE J. ALLEN Department of Psychology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada Abstract: Although much is known about personality and individuals’ job performance, only a few studies have considered the effects of team-level personality on team performance. Existing research examining the effects of personality on team performance has found that, of the Big Five factors of personality, Conscientiousness is often the most important predictor. Accordingly, we investigated the criterion validity of lower-level Conscientiousness traits to
  • 95. determine whether any one trait is particularly predictive of team performance. In addition to Conscientiousness, we examined the criterion validity of the other Big Five personality factors. We found that Conscientiousness and its facets predicted team performance. Agreeableness, Extraversion and Neuroticism were not predictive of team performance, whereas Openness had a modest negative relation with team performance. Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Key words: team performance; team composition; personality; Big Five; narrow traits; personality facets INTRODUCTION The composition of a work team is defined by the individual characteristics of its members. One implicit rationale underlying the research on team composition is that individual characteristics of team members (i.e. their personalities, demographic characteristics, attitudes and so on) serve as inputs that indirectly influence team perform- ance through group processes (e.g. collaboration) and emergent states (e.g. team cohesion). In other words, the characteristics of team members affect the way in which a team operates and its subsequent performance. Personality, as a class of team composition variables, is the focus of the present study. Over the past several years, research on personality has received considerable attention in the teams literature (e.g. Bell, 2007; Humphrey, Hollenbeck, Meyer, & Ilgen, 2007; Peeters, van Tuijl, Rutte, & Reymen, 2006). Interest in this topic continues for at least two reasons. First, there is an intuitive appeal to the argument that personality will influence team-related variables. Conceptually, personality should be related to (a) team knowledge, skills and abilities, (b) processes and emergent
  • 96. states and (c) general dimensions of teamwork (e.g. collaboration, supportive behaviour, team trust). Most of these variables appear to be natural outgrowths of personality and, therefore, one would expect personality to be a valid predictor in many cases (see Halfhill, Sundstrom, Lahner, Calderone, & Nielsen, 2005; Kichuk & Wiesner, 1998). A second reason that personality continues to be investigated in team settings is that it is a consistent and important predictor of individuals’ job performance (e.g. Barrick & Mount, 1991; Tett, Jackson, Rothstein, & Reddon, *Correspondence to: Thomas A. O’Neill, Department of Psychology, Social Science Centre, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada. E-mail: [email protected] Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 1999). Extending these findings to the team level is needed as organizations are increasingly turning to teamwork in an effort to stay competitive in the global marketplace (Allen & West, 2005; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006). Thus, research on personality and team performance is an ongoing priority. In this study, we collected personality data from members of project design teams, operationalized those data at the group level (e.g. using the group mean on each trait), and correlated the resulting team-level personality scores with team performance. Our purpose in this research was threefold. First, we examined the extent to which any content-relevant personality facets of Conscientiousness could demonstrate superior prediction of team performance relative to a broad Conscientiousness composite. An investigation of this type is needed given that Conscien- tiousness has been shown to be one of the most consistent Big
  • 97. Five predictors of job performance and team performance, but the criterion validity of its facets have rarely been examined at the team level (but see LePine, 2003). Second, we investigated whether any personality factors besides Conscientiousness could be valid predictors of team performance in the present context. Specifically, we assessed the criterion validity of the other Big Five factors: Agreeableneness, Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness. Third, when considered as a team-level construct, personality has historically been operationalized in several ways. In this study we provide new evidence regarding the criterion validity of the four most common team-level personality operationalizations. TEAM-LEVEL PERSONALITY Typically, the operationalization of personality variables at the team level is accomplished by aggregating individual- level personality scores using one of four group-level Received 29 July 2009 Revised 16 February 2010, Accepted 16 February 2010 mailto:[email protected] https://wileyonlinelibrary.com 32 T. A. O’Neill and N. J. Allen indices: Mean, variance, minimum and maximum scores (see Barrick, Stewart, Neubert, & Mount, 1998; Halfhill et al., 2005; Williams & Allen, 2008). The particular operationalization is usually chosen through a consideration of the personality variable, the nature of the task, and how the two are expected to interact (e.g. Allen & West, 2005; Hecht & Allen, 1999; LePine, Hollenbeck, Ilgen, & Hedlund,
  • 98. 1997). The mean approach involves computing the arithmetic average of each team member’s score on the personality variable. This approach is appropriate when the trait is theorized to work additively—that is, when it is suspected that the more (or less) team members possess the trait, the better the team will perform. The variance approach indexes the dispersion, or heterogeneity, of the trait across team members. This operationalization is used when the researcher believes a greater (or lesser) amount of variation in the trait will be related to the criterion. Finally, sometimes it is appropriate to consider only the team member with the highest, or lowest, score on a trait (referred to as the maximum or minimum approach, respectively), and refer to that value as the team-level score. As an analogy, on an assembly line, the number of units produced will often depend on the slowest working team member, and, accordingly, the minimum score on a trait such as Achievement could be most predictive of team performance. Conversely, on a creativity task, the team member with the highest score on a trait such as Innovation could be most responsible for the level of team performance achieved (because a novel idea has only to come from one team member). Theorizing about the most appropriate operationalization for team personality is critical as these may substantially affect the magnitude of person- ality’s criterion validity (Moynihan & Peterson, 2004; Williams & Allen, 2008). In the most recent and comprehensive meta-analysis examining relations between team-level personality and performance, Bell (2007) found that, overall, team-level personality does predict team performance. The findings for lab studies were generally weak, likely because team performance measurement in those studies tended to be too coarse to detect small variations in behaviour related to
  • 99. expressions of personality. Field studies in Bell’s meta- analysis, however, demonstrated the strongest and most consistent findings for Conscientiousness. Teams with high means, high team member maximum and minimum scores and low variance had the greatest performance levels (Emotional Stability was coded in the socially desirable direction). Other Big Five factors were predictive of team performance, but not with the same magnitude and consistency across operationalizations. Given that Conscientiousness was the most consist- ently predictive trait of team performance in Bell’s (2007) meta-analysis, it is reasonable to consider that facets of Conscientiousness might even be more predictive (see Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina, 2006). For example, the factor of Conscientiousness encompasses several more specific facets of personality, such as Industrious- ness, Order, Self-Control, Responsibility, Traditionalism and Virtue (see Roberts, Chernyshenko, Stark, & Gold- berg, 2005). Arguably, some of these lower-level personality variables belonging to the same higher-level personality factor may correlate differently, in magnitude or direction, from the others in the prediction of a criterion (see Ashton, 1998; Ashton, Jackson, Paunonen, Helmes, & Rothstein, 1995; Hough, 1992; LePine, 2003; Paunonen, 1998, 2003). Reflecting on Bell’s meta- analytic findings, as well as the literature demonstrating the validity of narrow traits, we suggest that, in order to maximize the predictive power of Conscientiousness as it relates to team performance, criterion-relevant facets ought to be considered. The fact that personality variables other than Conscien-
  • 100. tiousness (e.g. Agreeableness) were predictive of team performance in Bell’s meta-analysis suggests that they, too, may be relevant in the present study. As we will argue later, our criterion, project team performance, could be associated with certain team-level operationalizations of Agreeable- ness, Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness. Finally, the method of operationalizing team personality (e.g. mean, minimum) that will be most predictive of team performance must also be considered in maximizing criterion validity. In the section that follows we develop our predictions regarding the operationalization that is, in the context of our study, most theoretically appropriate for each personality factor and facet included in this study. THE PRESENT STUDY Our sample consisted of concept design teams, composed of engineering students, who worked interdependently for 6.5 months. The teams were engaged in an intensive, complex engineering design task. The team members had shared outcomes of significant value, and coordinated most work dynamically and reciprocally (rather than through pooled or sequential processes). These were classic ‘project teams’ as they were created for a specific purpose and time frame, after which they would disband (see Chiocchio & Essiembre, 2009). Knowledge of these contextual details was important in generating predictions, outlined below. The Big Five In the present research we assessed the Big Five factors of personality. In order to optimize their prediction of team performance, we judged it most appropriate to operationalize the Big Five factors, at the team-level, as follows: Conscientiousness (mean), Agreeableness (mean), Neuroti- cism (mean), Extraversion (variance) and Openness (maxi-
  • 101. mum). Important theoretical rationales underlie the choice of team personality operationalizations. Beginning with Con- scientiousness, we contend that this factor captures a class of attributes that manifest themselves as valuable resources, such as achievement-striving, organization, planning and task focus. The team may draw upon resources of this type to accomplish its work (see LePine et al., 1997; Stewart, 2003). An additive team-level conceptualization, using the mean approach, is most appropriate in the present research because Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Pers. 25: 31– 42 (2011) DOI: 10.1002/per Personality and team performance 33 the more team members are conscientious, the better the team should perform (see also Barrick et al., 1998). Similarly, Agreeableness represents a factor of personality that can be expected to foster effective team interactions because members are trusting, altruistic and cooperative. Such teams could perform well because of their smooth conflict resolution, and inclination towards open communi- cation and information seeking (Peeters et al., 2006). We also see Agreeableness as accumulating additively, as the more members are characterized as agreeable, the more they should have positive interactions, and in turn, create a higher performing team. Regarding the personality factor Neuroti- cism, most previous studies have found important relations with team performance for the mean only (see Bell, 2007). As Neuman, Wagner and Christiansen (1999) pointed out, teams that are higher on Neuroticism will have difficulty