SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 123
Investment Analysis
2/18/2020
Letter of Intent/Memorandum of Understanding Elements
1. Local economic & social impact
· Sports organizations generate revenues from enthusiastic and
supporting fans. It is crucial for a sports organization to
establish local support before constructing a sports
facility/stadium. In return, a well-designed stadium will benefit
the city-county by boosting its local economy and increasing the
quality of life for its local citizens.
· Economic Impact: Increases the city’s media exposure, attracts
more tourists to the city and thereby increases the city’s tax
revenue. It also benefits local businesses by attracting more
customers on the event day
· Social Impact: Gives fans a sense of belonging, generates
pride
2. Stadium Site / Location
· Site selection is an important consideration for a new stadium
project. Organizations must evaluate their unique market and
establish its site requirements.
· Site acquisition: Organization and city must agree on a proper
site acquisition plan. A privately financed project such as the
Oakland Athletics’ Coliseum, purchases its stadium site from
the county. Whereas a publicly financed project such as the
Broward County Civic Arena may have a designated stadium
site recommended by the city-county (Florida Panthers LOI 12)
3. Stadium Design
· New stadiums should be designed to satisfy their unique
market needs. The design should also support the main tenant’s
business operation while accommodating other tenants
· Seating: appropriate seating number to maximize ticket
revenue. Broward County Civic Arena has 21,000 seats to
accommodate baseball fans (Florida Panthers LOI 3) while the
Eagles designed near 70,000 seats for football games (Eagles
Lease Terms 1)
· Parking: if stadium located away from the city, parking spaces
need to be designed
· Other: amenities, restroom, social areas, shop, and restaurants
also need to be considered
4. Stadium Construction and Completion date
· The sports organization and city must have a mutual
agreement on the roles and responsibilities in the stadium
construction process. City county often contract with a third-
party company to serve as “construction manager” to pursuant
to the CM agreement and manage the construction process
(Atlanta Braves Cobb County MOU 5)
· Completion deadline: it is crucial for all parties to finish the
construction process prior to the deadline/ beginning of the
season. Any delay will cause significant revenue loss
5. Project Funding
· It is fundamental for an organization to develop a funding plan
for the new stadium. The project funding plan should include
the estimated cost of construction, as well as parties’
contribution agreement. For instance, the city of Philadelphia
agreed to cover the new Eagle’s stadium’s site acquisition and
construction cost as well as a part of the costs of demolition of
existing buildings. While the Eagles are responsible for the rest
of the demolition, plus the basic stadium elements costs and
environmental remediation cost (Eagles Lease Terms 3)
6. Ownership
· Most publicly funded stadiums such as the Broward County
Civic Arena are owned by the city-county, while other privily
funded stadiums are owned by the team/organization. Different
ownership structure provides different opportunities and threat
· Media Rights / Naming rights ownership: both being a large
portion of the revenue. Stadium and tenants must establish
media and stadium naming rights agreement
· Intellectual Property ownership: stadium and tenants must also
agree on an Intellectual Property License Agreement, which
manages tenant’s rights to use the stadium’s intellectual
properties in their marketing program. For instance, the Georgia
World Congress Center Authority entered into an IP license
agreement, which granted the Atlanta Falcons the right to use
Mercedes Benz Stadium’s trademarks and trade names in their
marketing programs. The agreement also restricts GWCCA from
using the Falcons image on any other GWCCA campus
buildings (Atlanta New Stadium Project 23)
7. License Agreement
· Depending on the ownership structure, sports organizations
might be required to acquire the license to operate in their
stadium. For instance, the Florida Panthers pays the county five
percent of its ticket sales from general seating, in order to play
in the publicly funded stadium (Florida Panthers LOI 5)
· Scheduling of event: tenants must communicate their event
dates with the stadium in advance so there is no concurring
event on the same date. Booking procedures should be
established as well as a priority booking policy for the main
tenant (Atlanta New Stadium Project 10)
8. Operation Management
· Both the organization and the stadium must reach an
agreement on the responsibilities of the facility’s daily
operation. Organizations may use a third-party Management
company to facilitate stadium operations. All parties should
establish a management agreement, and the management
company must fulfill all tenant’s operational goals and
objectives
· O&M, R&R responsibilities: Organizations and facilities must
have a mutual agreement on the responsibilities of the facility’s
future operational maintenance and repair/ replacement. For
example, the Georgia World Congress Center Authority
cooperates with the Falcons with its stadium maintenance and
covers all landscaping maintenance costs. However, each year,
the GWCCA requires the Falcons to submit its maintenance and
capital improvement plan before the next season (Atlanta New
Stadium Project 19)
9. Financial Terms
· Having an operating expense budget is important because
organizations must minimize expenses in order to maximize
profit. Organizations must efficiently allocate their budget
between employee salary, field maintenance, equipment
purchases, administrative services and taxes. (Florida Panthers
A-2) Some operating expenses may be covered by the stadium.
For instance, the GWCCA will provide the Atlanta Falcons an
annual budget for traffic allocation services (Atlanta New
Stadium Project 14)
· Organizations also need to consider how the operating revenue
is being distributed. Such as ticket revenue, merchandise sales,
food and beverage sales. Teams might need to pay a certain
percentage of their revenue to the county issued revenue bonds.
10. Development strategy Agreement
· In order to maximize a sporting facility’s usage, it is
important that a stadium and its local county, to share the same
commitment for a long-term growth strategy. Teams such as the
Atlanta Falcons might be required to enter into a Non-
Relocation Agreement with its stadium. In such cases, the teams
are restricted to relocate, unless a material breach has been
made by the stadium operator (Atlanta New Stadium Project 9)
Citation
Florida Panthers and Broward County, FL for Broward County
Civic Arena 1996.
Atlanta New Stadium Project 2012.
Eagles Lease Terms 2000.
“8 Things to Consider When Building a New Sports Complex.”
Earth Networks, 8 Nov. 2019, www.earthnetworks.com/blog/8-
things-consider-building-a-new-sports-complex/.
1
3
Title of PaperStudent NameCourse/Number
Due Date
Faculty Name
Detailed expectations for a 100-299 Level Course(remove this
line)Title of Paper
Triple click your mouse anywhere in this paragraph to replace
this text with your introduction. Often the most important
paragraph in the entire essay, the introduction grabs the reader's
attention—sometimes a difficult task for academic writing.
When writing an introduction, some approaches are best
avoided. Avoid starting sentences with “The purpose of this
essay is . . .” or “In this essay I will . . .” or any similar flat
announcement of your intention or topic. Read more: Center for
Writing Excellence>Tutorials and Guides>Essay
Development>Guidelines for Writing Academic Essays. Level
One Heading
Replace the level one heading with the words for your heading.
The heading must be in bold font. Headings help your audience
track the sub-topics discussed in the body of the essay or report.
Begin a new heading for each sub-topic.
Be sure to indent the first line of each paragraph between five
and seven spaces by pressing the Tab key one time on the
keyboard. In addition, remember to double space the entire
paper using the double space functionality in Word. This
template is already formatted for double spacing. Read more:
Center for Writing Excellence>Tutorials and Guides>Software
Tutorials and Guides>Formatting Tutorial for APA.
In addition, keep in mind an academic essay should contain at
least five paragraphs, which includes the introduction
(introductory paragraph), the body (which is generally at least 3
paragraphs), and the conclusion (generally one paragraph). Most
well-developed paragraphs contain at least 3-5 sentences, one of
which is the topic sentence. Limit each body paragraph to one
main idea.
Conclusion
The closing paragraph is designed to bring the reader to your
way of thinking if you are writing a persuasive essay, to
understand relationships if you are writing a
comparison/contrast essay, or simply to value the information
you provide in an informational essay. The closing paragraph
summarizes the key points from the supporting paragraphs
without introducing any new information.
References
This is a hanging indent. To keep the hanging indent format,
triple click your mouse on this line of text and replace the
information with your reference entry. You can use the
Reference and Citation Examples (Center for Writing
Excellence>Tutorials and Guides>Reference and Citation
Examples) to help format your source information into a
reference entry.
The reference page always begins on the top of the next page
after the conclusion.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, Vol. 2, No. 2,
1999
Corporal Punishment by American Parents: National Data
on Prevalence, Chronicity, Severity, and Duration, in
Relation to Child and Family Characteristics
Murray A. Straus1,2 and Julie H. Stewart1
We present data on corporal punishment (CP) by a nationally
representative sample of 991
American parents interviewed in 1995. Six types of CP were
examined: slaps on the hand
or leg, spanking on the buttocks, pinching, shaking, hitting on
the buttocks with a belt or
paddle, and slapping in the face. The overall prevalence rate
(the percentage of parents
using any of these types of CP during the previous year) was
35% for infants and reached
a peak of 94% at ages 3 and 4. Despite rapid decline after age 5,
just over half of American
parents hit children at age 12, a third at age 14, and 13% at age
17. Analysis of chronicity
found that parents who hit teenage children did so an average of
about six times during the
year. Severity, as measured by hitting the child with a belt or
paddle, was greatest for children
age 5-12 (28% of such children). CP was more prevalent among
African American and low
socioeconomic status parents, in the South, for boys, and by
mothers. The pervasiveness of
CP reported in this article, and the harmful side effects of CP
shown by recent longitudinal
research, indicates a need for psychology and sociology
textbooks to reverse the current
tendency to almost ignore CP and instead treat it as a major
aspect of the socialization
experience of American children; and for developmental
psychologists to be cognizant of
the likelihood that parents are using CP far more often than
even advocates of CP recommend,
and to inform parents about the risks involved.
INTRODUCTION
Evidence indicating that almost all American
parents use spanking and other legal forms of corpo-
ral punishment (CP) on toddlers has been available
for many years. Sears, Maccoby, and Levin (1957),
for example, found that 99% of the children they
studied experienced CP at least occasionally. Straus
(1983) found a 95% rate for toddlers, and Bryan and
Freed (1982) found that 95% of a sample of commu-
nity college students had experienced CP. Numerous
other studies (e.g., Giles-Sims, Straus, & Sugarman,
1Family Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire,
Durham, New Hampshire, 03824.
2Address all correspondence to Murray A. Straus, Family
Research
Laboratory, University of New Hampshire, Durham, New Hamp-
shire 03824; e-mail: [email protected]
1995; Goodenough, 1931/1975; Holden, Coleman, &
Schmidt, 1995; Straus, 1994a; Wauchope & Straus,
1990) also show extremely high rates of CP. CP there-
fore appears to be a near universal aspect of the
socialization experience of American children, al-
though to widely varying degrees in individual cases.
There is also increasing evidence that, even when
done by loving and supportive parents, CP is associ-
ated with an increased risk of unwanted later effects
such as aggression and depression as an adult (Straus,
1994a; Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1997;
Thompson, In press).
Despite the high prevalence rates and the evi-
dence suggesting harmful side effects, there is also
evidence that this pervasive aspect of the socialization
of American children is either not perceived or ig-
nored. One indication is the extremely limited cover-
age of CP revealed by a content analysis of 10 widely
55
1096-4037/99/0600.0055$16.00/0 © 1999 Plenum Publishing
Corporation
KEY WORDS: Punishment; physical; corporal; spanking;
parent; age; infant; gender; SES; ethnic; region.
56 Straus and Stewart
used textbooks in child development published be-
tween 1985 and 1989 (Straus, 1994a). For this article,
the analysis was repeated for books published be-
tween 1992 and 1996. The results in Table I shows
little change from the 1980s to the 1990s. The omis-
sion of information on CP in child development text-
books is unfortunate because discipline problems are
frequently the focus of clinical child psychology and,
as indicated above, CP is a widely used disciplinary
technique. Consequently, the background under-
standing the nature and extent of CP provided by
the research reported in this article may be helpful
in clinical practice.
Among the books for the period 1992-1996, only
a third had entries in the index for corporal punish-
ment, spanking, physical punishment, or anything on
CP under subheads such as discipline or punishment.
Although page by page scanning the text located
material on corporal punishment in 9 of the 11 books,
these 9 books devoted an average of only three tenths
of a page to CP.
Because, as noted earlier, data on CP has been
available for many years, the absence of information
on CP in child development textbooks cannot be
attributed to lack of developmentally relevant data.
Straus (1994a) has suggested methodological and so-
ciocultural factors to explain the neglect of a nearly
universal aspect of the socialization of American chil -
dren. Whatever the reason, current information is
needed because the last few years have been a period
in which public endorsement of CP declined sharply
(Straus & Mathur, 1996). There has also been a recent
surge of professional debate about the potential
harmful effects of CP, as evidenced by special issues
of Pediatrics in 1996; Psychological Inquiry, Archives
of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, and Aggression
and Violent Behavior in 1997; and by the 1998 policy
statement on CP by the American Academy of Pedi-
atrics.
Deep value commitments underlie the debate on
the appropriateness of CP (Friedman & Schonberg,
1996; Straus, 1994a). Readers of this article therefore
need to be aware that our position differs from the
standard assumptions of American culture—that CP
is sometimes necessary, and if done in moderation
by loving parents, is harmless. This tenet of American
culture is shared by at least two thirds of Americans
(Straus & Mathur, 1996), including the majority of
pediatricians (White, 1993) and clinical psychologists
(Anderson & Anderson, 1976). Our view, however,
is that parents should never use CP because a growing
body of research evidence shows that other disciplin-
ary strategies are just as effective and do not involve
the risk of escalation into physical abuse (Straus,
1994a) or of subsequent psychological problems for
children and adults (Straus, 1994b; Straus & Paschall,
1998; Straus et al., 1997).
Regardless of whether one accepts or rejects use
of CP, we suggest that clinical child psychology can
benefit from a background understanding of the ex-
tent to which parents use this mode of discipline with
children of different ages. Consequently, the purpose
of this article is to make available information on the
use of CP by a nationally representative sample of
American parents in 1995 for each year of life from
birth through 17, and other age categories. It also
provides information on the prevalence of six specific
acts of CP that vary in severity, on the chronicity of
CP (how often CP was used), and on the duration
of CP (the number of years until CP ceases). In addi-
tion, we examine the extent to which CP varies by
seven other characteristics of the children and the
families.
The information on the extent to which CP dif-
fers according to these characteristics of children and
families can help to understand the social structural
determinants of CP and can also have practical value
because, as indicated above, there is a growing pro-
fessional interest in helping parents shift from CP to
nonviolent forms of discipline. Such programs need
to be guided by empirical data on the extent to which
CP is prevalent at different ages and in different
sectors of the population. For example, African
American parents, and low education parents of all
ethnic groups, are more likely to approve of and use
CP (Alvy, 1987; Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, &
Pettit, 1996; Giles-Sims et al., 1995). Consequently,
parent education intended to reduce CP may need
to be structured to reflect the culture of these groups.
Table I. Treatment of Corporal Punishment in Child
Development
Textbooks in 1980s and 1990s
CP in Index (%)
Range of pages on CP
Mean no. of pages on CP
% of pages on CP
Advised avoiding CP if
possible (%)
Advised to never use CP (%)
Year of publication
1985-1989
(n = 10)
20
1 sentence
to 4
pages
0.4
0.0 to 0.3
10
0
1992-1996
(n = 11)
36
1 sentence
to 0.8
pages
0.3
0.0 to 0.08
27
9
Corporal Punishment 57
If, in recent years, fathers have taken on additional
child care responsibilities, the additional effort
needed to include fathers in parent education pro-
grams may be even more important than previously.
Because this article is intended primarily to ex-
plore and describe rather than to test formal hypothe-
ses, each topic in the Results section is introduced by
a brief description of findings from previous research.
METHOD
Sample
The data were obtained from a survey by the
Gallup Organization (1995). The survey was con-
ducted by telephone in August and September 1995.
The telephone numbers were selected by a random-
digit stratified probability design. A random proce-
dure was used to provide representation of both listed
and unlisted numbers. These methods are designed
to produce, with proper weighting, and unbiased
probability sample of telephone households in the
continental United States, which includes 94% of all
households. Among households that met the eligibil-
ity criterion (one or more children under 18 living
there), the participation rate was 81%. A total of 991
interviews were completed. In two-parent house-
holds, because of budget limitations, one parent was
randomly selected for the interview. In multichild
households, one child was randomly identified, and a
parent of that child interviewed, and all data reported
pertain to the selected child. See Gallup Organization
(1995) for more detailed sampling information. A
telephone sample obviously excludes those without
telephones, which tends to be low socioeconomic sta-
tus (SES) persons. However, face-to-face interview
surveys also have great difficulty locating and en-
listing the participation of such persons, and there is
considerable evidence that properly conducted tele-
phone surveys obtain a higher participation rate and
are usually more representative than face-to-face in-
terview surveys (Bermack, 1989; Groves et al., 1988;
Wells, Burnam, Leake, & Robbins, 1988).
Sample Characteristics and Weighting
The division between boys and girls in this sam-
ple was almost equal (49% girls). The ethnic compo-
sition was 81% Euro-American, 12% African
American, and 7% other ethnic groups (Hispanic
Americans, Native Americans, and Asian Ameri-
cans). The mean age of the parents interviewed was
36.8 years. More mothers than fathers were inter-
viewed (66% of the sample), partly because the sam-
ple included single parents who are predominantly
mothers. Fifty-two percent of the parents were mar-
ried, 15% remarried, 20% divorced, 8% never mar-
ried, and 4% were cohabiting. The referent children
ranged in age from infants to age 17, with a mean
age of 8.4 years. Households with college-educated
parents were overrepresented (34 vs. 23% in the Cen-
sus) and those with less than a high school education
were underrepresented (8 vs. 14% in the Census). In
all analyses, data were weighted to make the data
reflect U.S. Census statistics with regard to the gender
of the respondent, child's age, ethnic group, region
of the country, and parent's education. This involved
an unproven assumption that the replies of nonsur-
veyed members of underrepresented groups would
be similar to those who were surveyed.
Definition and Measurement of
Corporal Punishment
The definition of CP which guided this research
is "the use of physical force with the intention of
causing a child to experience pain, but not injury, for
the purpose of correction or control of the child's
behavior" (Straus, 1994a, p. 4). This corresponds in
practice to the legal definition of CP in all states of
the U.S., which is in the form of an exemption from
the crime of assault for parents who use physical
force for purposes of correction and control (see
Straus, 1994a, for a discussion and illustrative
statutes).
The Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scales
(CTSPC; Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore, &
Runyan, 1998) was used to obtain the data on CP.
This version of the CTS includes six items that fall
within the range of legal CP. It asks parents how
often each of these acts occurred in the previous 12
months. The response categories were never, once,
twice, 3-5 times, 6-10 times, 11-20 times, and more
than 20 times.
Prevalence of CP. The overall prevalence statis-
tics in this article are based on whether the parent
reported one or more instances of one or more of
the following acts in the previous 12 months. Thus,
multiple acts could have occurred in a particular CP
episode. (See Straus et al., 1998, for the exact ques-
tion wording.)
58 Straus and Stewart
Spanked on the bottom with your bare hand
Slapped on the hand, arm, or leg
Pinched
Shook (for children age 3 and over only)
Hit on the bottom with something like a belt, hair-
brush, a stick or some other hard object
Slapped on the face or head or ears
Chronicity. For the parents who used CP, it is
also important to know how often they engaged in
that disciplinary tactic. A chronicity measure was
therefore computed for each parent who reported
using CP by summing the frequency of each of the
six acts in the CP scale. The chronicity of the specific
acts are also reported. See Straus (1998) and Straus
et al. (1998) for an explanation of the need to measure
prevalence and chronicity separately.
Severity. Severity of CP can be measured in a
number of ways (Straus, 1998). The severity of CP
was based on a judgment concerning the risk of injury
and degree of normative acceptance made by a five-
person team that included a pediatrician specializing
in child abuse, a clinical psychologist, two sociologists
specializing in research on the family, and a political
scientist specializing in public opinion research
(Straus et al., 1998). Three acts were judged to carry
a higher risk and be less widely accepted, and were
classified as severe CP (slap on face or head, hit with
belt or hard object, pinched) and two as less severe
(spanked and slapped on the hand or leg). Shaking
was considered separately because of the extreme
danger if done to infants.
Duration. Because this is a cross-sectional study,
an estimate of the median duration can only be in-
ferred from the age-specific prevalence statistics. If
for example, the percentage hit at ages 11, 12, 13,
and 14 were 61, 52, 45, and 32%, respectively, we
would infer that the median duration is about 12
years. This inference depends on the assumption that
parents who used CP at age 12 were doing so in
previous years.
One-Year Reporting. A short reporting period,
such the previous week, results in a much higher
estimate of chronicity, but a low prevalence rate be-
cause not every parent who uses CP did so in the
previous week. This is particularly the case with older
children. Conversely, a 1-year reporting period re-
sults in a higher prevalence rate, but the difficulty of
recalling the number of instances produces an under-
estimate of chronicity. To deal with this problem, the
questions need to be repeated for the previous week
and previous year (Straus, 1998). However, budget
limitations restricted this study to the previous year
as the reporting period. Consequently, the chronicity
data in this article must be regarded as minimum esti-
mates.
Measures of Independent Variables
Age of Child. The analyses of age differences in
CP used 1-year age intervals (from under 1 through
17). The analysis of the interaction of age with the
other independent variables grouped the children
into the following six categories in order to have
enough cases per cell to be statistically reliable: In-
fants (0-1), toddlers (age 2-4), early primary school
(age 5-8), later primary school (age 9-12), and teens
(age 13-17).
Age of Parent. Three age categories were used:
18-29, 30-39, and 40 and over.
Single Parent. This variable indicates whether
the child was living with only one parent, versus all
other living arrangements.
Socioeconomic Status. SES was measured by a
factor score obtained by a principle components anal -
ysis of the respondent's education and income. The
factor score was then coded into quintiles in order
to have enough cases per cell for the ANOVA.
Ethnic Group. Ethnic group was included in the
analysis with three categories. Euro-Americans were
coded as 1, African Americans as 2, and all other
ethnic groups as 3 because there were not sufficient
cases to make finer differentiations.
Gender of Parent and Child. Girls were coded
as 1 and boys as 0. Mothers were coded as 1 and
fathers as 0.
Region. The Northeast was coded as 1, the Mid-
west as 2, the South as 3, and the West as 4.
Data Analysis
Consistent with the emphasis on describing the
prevalence of CP rather than testing a theoretical
model, ANOVA was used as the main mode of analy-
sis because it provides the observed mean CP for
children of each age and each category of the other
independent variables, and for the interactions of
these variables. The ANOVAs were computed using
the "regression approach" option in SPSS/PC,
namely, "All effects are assessed simultaneously,
with each effect adjusted for all other effects in the
model" (Norusis, 1992, p. 257). Thus, the test for
each independent variable controls for the other in-
Corporal Punishment 59
dependent variables, and the means were adjusted
to control for the other independent variables.
The N for the child age trend analyses of preva-
lence is 991. The analysis of the chronicity of CP are
based on the 609 cases where CP was used at least
once. For the ANOVA relating differences in charac-
teristics of the family and child to the prevalence of
CP, the analyses of prevalence are based on 978 cases.
The ANOVA for chronicity is based on 602 cases.
The number of cases for each category of the in-
dependent variables are given in Table II. The
ANOVAs were restricted to main effects and two-
way interactions because higher order interactions
would have resulted in empty cells and singular vari-
ance-covariance matrices.
recently become a focus of attention. Previous re-
search indicates that CP increases from infancy to
age 2, stays about the same for ages 3 through 5, and
decreases steadily from age 5 through 17 (Bachman,
1967; Straus, 1991,1994a; Wauchope & Straus, 1990),
and that pattern was expected to hold for this sample.
Age and Prevalence of Corporal Punishment
In Figure 1 the dashed line is for the observed
means and the solid line plots the moving average.
(The moving average controls for random fluctua-
tions by combining the mean of the observed mean
for each age and the age just below and just above.)
Figure 1 shows that just over a third of infants (chil-
dren under 1 year of age) were hit by their parents.
The rate then rises to a peak at ages 4 and 5, whenRESULTS
Age of Child and Corporal Punishment
The age of the child is uniquely important for
understanding the place of CP in the socialization of
American children. This is because of the tremendous
variation by age and, as indicated earlier, because
the age at which CP is considered appropriate has
Fig. 1. Prevalence of corporal punishment by child's age.
Table II. Number of Cases in Categories Used for ANOVAs
Characteristic and category
Age of child
0-1
2-4
5-8
9-12
13-17
Gender of child
Male
Female
Gender of respondent
Male
Female
Age of respondent
18-29
30-39
40+
SES quintiles
1 Low
2
3
4
5 High
Ethnic group
White
Black
Other
Single parent
Yes
No
Region
NE
Midwest
South
West
No. of cases
Table III
92
190
230
228
246
503
483
333
653
224
438
323
203
139
228
226
189
817
125
44
324
662
190
252
328
215
Table IV
43
166
191
131
70
320
282
181
420
171
285
146
148
81
145
126
101
480
97
25
207
395
101
159
223
119
60 Straus and Stewart
94% of parents said that, during the previous 12
months, they had used one or more of the types of
CP included in the CP scale. From there on, the rate
declined steadily to age 17. However, it is important
to note that as late as age 13, over 40% of parents
used CP as a disciplinary technique.
Age and Chronicity of Corporal Punishment
Age Differences and Severity of
Corporal Punishment
Table III gives the percentage of parents who
used ordinary CP and more severe types of CP, and
also two other disciplinary tactics. It uses age catego-
ries rather than 1-year age intervals because, as ex-
plained in the Method section, single-year categories
produced too many empty cells for the more rarely
occurring types of CP.
Comparing parts A and B of Table III reveals
a developmental pattern. The milder forms of CP in
Part A (spanking and slapping) were used most often
with toddlers (ages 2-4). The more severe and less
culturally approved forms of CP (see Method section)
in Part B of Table III were most prevalent in middle
childhood (ages 5-12 in Table III). Teenagers had
the lowest prevalence rates, but in our opinion were
still remarkably high.
Hitting a child on the bottom with objects such as
a belt, hairbrush, or stick was, presumably, extremely
common as recently as the 1940s. Even as recently
as the 1970s, two thirds of a random sample of the
population of Texas believed that hitting a child with
such objects is acceptable (Teske & Parker, 1983).
Table III indicates that actual use of belts and paddles
has also not disappeared. More than one in four
American parents reported having used such objects
on a child age 5 to 12 in 1995.
The least frequent forms of CP were slapping
on the face, head, or ears and pinching. Except for
infants, from 3 to 8% of children of all age groups
experienced these two forms of CP.
Part C of Table III includes two important
disciplinary tactics that do not fit neatly into the
categories "minor" and "severe" CP. The first of
these, shaking a child, can cause brain injury or
death to children under 2. So it is an indicator of
child abuse rather than CP for children that young.
Although 4.3% of this national sample reported
shaking a child that was under 2, none of the cases
of shaking were for infants. On the one hand, this
makes the statistic less ominous. On the other hand,
it makes the rate for the 12- to 23-month-old
children 10.3%. Looking across the Shaking row of
Table III, about 1 of 10 parents of children of all
ages except infants and teens used shaking as a
mode of CP.
The Threatened to Spank row of Table III de-
scribes the prevalence of a tactic that, strictly speak-
ing is a form of verbal aggression, not an act of CP
as previously defined. It was therefore not one of the
Although Figure 1 indicates how many children
were hit at each age, it does not indicate how often
parents hit the children in this study. This information
is given in Figure 2. It refers to those parents who
used CP at least once during the previous year and
gives the mean number of times they reported using
CP. Figure 2 shows that CP was most chronic by
parents of 2-year-old children. They reported using
CP an average of 18 times during the previous year.
The chronicity of CP declined from there on, to a
mean of six times per year by parents of 14-, 15-, and
16-year-old children.
Fig. 2. Chronicity of corporal punishment by child's age.
Corporal Punishment 61
Table III. Percentage of Parents using Specific Acts of Corporal
Punishment by Age of Child
Type of corporal punishment
A. Ordinary
Spank on bottom with hand
Slap on hand, arm or leg
B. Severe
Hit on bottom with object
Slap on face, head or ears
Pinch
C. Other
Shake
Threatened to spank
Age of child
0-1
31.8
36.4
2.5
0.5
2.8
4.3
22.8
2-4
72.1
63.3
18.1
4.8
2.8
12.8
65.5
5-8
71.2
47.5
28.4
6.9
7.9
10.8
70.7
9-12
43.1
26.9
28.5
2.9
5.1
11.4
55.6
13-17
14.0
16.2
15.6
5.8
2.3
5.7
38.9
F
64.6
28.5
11.3
2.6
2.8
3.0
26.8
P<
.01
.01
.01
.02
.03
.02
.01
items in the CP scale used to obtain the prevalence
rates in Figures 1 to 3. It is included in Table III,
however, because it is a form of verbal aggression
that often leads to the actual use of CP and because
it is so frequent right up through the teen years.
Other Child and Parent Characteristics Associated
with CP
The analyses in this section are based on
ANOVAs using eight independent variables, as de-
scribed in the Method section. The tests of signifi-
cance for each of the independent variables therefore
controlled for the level of all the other variables in
the analysis. The means for each variable in this sec-
tion have been adjusted for the level of the other
variables and therefore describes the net effect of the
variable after controlling for the other independent
variables. The numbers for each category of the vari-
ables are given in Table II.
Age of Parent
Studies of three nationally representative sam-
ples of parents found that the younger the parent,
the greater the prevalence of CP (Day, Peterson, &
McCracken, 1998; Giles-Sims et al., 1995; Straus,
1994a). For the current study, although there is a
tendency for younger parents to be more likely to
use CP, the second row of Table IV shows that the
differences were not significant. However, row 2 of
Table V shows a significant relation of parents' age
to the chronicity of CP. Younger parents (18-29)
used CP an average of 17.1 times during the previous
12 months in comparison to 12.6 times for those
30-39 and 9.1 times for parents over 40. This differ-
ence is not the result of younger parents having
younger children (who, on average, are hit more fre-
quently) because the analysis controlled for the age
of the child.
Socioeconomic Status
Although some studies have found that the
higher the SES, the less use of CP (Giles-Sims et
al., 1995), most have found no important difference
(Erlanger, 1974a, 1974b). The present study found a
significant main effect for SES (Table IV, row 3).
Figure 3 shows that the percentage of parents using
CP was highest among parents in the lowest quintile
of the SES scale, and decreased with increasing SES.
The solid line is for the observed percentage, and the
dotted line gives the percentage after controlling for
the other seven independent variables (age and gen-
der of the parent and the child, the ethnicity of the
child, single parents, and region). Comparing the two
lines shows that controlling for these variables low -
ered the relation of SES to CP, but only slightly.
We also found significant interactions of SES
with age of parent and age of child. The interaction
of SES with age of the parent mainly occurred be-
cause for younger parents the percentage who used
CP was high regardless of SES. Thus the main effect
finding, that the higher the SES, the lower the per-
centage using CP, applies only to the two older age
groups (ages 30 to 39 and 40+).
Ethnic Group
Previous research on ethnic differences in CP is
also contradictory. For example, Straus (1994a)
found no important difference between African
62 Straus and Stewart
Table IV. Analysis of Variance of Prevalence of Corporal
Punishment (N = 978)
Source of variation
Main effects
Age of child
Age of parent
SES
Ethnicity of child
Gender of child
Gender of parent
Single parent
Region
Two-way interactions
Gender of child X age of child
Gender of child X Gender of parent
Gender of child x Age of parent
Gender of child X SES
Gender of child x Ethnicity of child
Gender of child x Single parent
Gender of child x Region
Age of child X Gender of parent
Age of child x Age of Parent
Age of child x SES
Age of child x Ethnicity of Child
Age of child X Single parent
Age of child x Region
Gender of parent x age of parent
Gender of parent X SES
Gender of parent x ethnicity of child
Gender of parent x single parent
Gender of parent X Region
Age of parent x SES
Age of parent X Ethnicity of child
Age of parent x Single parent
Age of parent x Region
SES X Ethnicity of child
SES X Single parent
SES x Region
Ethnicity of child X single parent
Ethnicity of child X region
Single parent X region
df
4
2
4
2
1
1
1
3
4
1
2
4
2
1
3
4
8
16
8
4
12
2
4
2
1
3
8
4
2
6
8
4
12
2
6
3
Mean square
102762.392
1598.307
5157.449
6434.534
11947.483
6539.097
5132.157
10701.384
2792.791
166.825
1741.532
2614.281
2965.120
388.886
253.075
7003.780
770.383
3830.571
2262.647
2929.269
3919.154
2994.410
3222.397
3137.298
1268.802
648.837
3361.851
599.639
1098.502
2870.937
1111.666
2882.174
2232.964
1851.213
2414.525
2404.452
f
63.407
0.986
3.182
3.970
7.372
4.035
3.167
6.603
1.723
0.103
1.075
1.613
1.830
0.240
0.156
4.322
0.475
2.364
1.396
1.807
2.418
1.848
1.988
1.936
0.783
0.400
2.074
0.370
0.678
1.771
0.686
1.778
1.378
1.142
1.490
1.484
P<
.001
.373
.013
.019
.007
.045
.076
.001
.143
.748
.342
.169
.161
.624
.926
.002
.874
.002
.194
.125
.004
.158
.094
.145
.377
.753
.036
.830
.508
.102
.704
.131
.171
.320
.179
.218
Americans and Euro-Americans, but Day et al.
(1998) found more CP by African American parents,
but only for children age 5 to 11 years; and among
younger children, only for unmarried mothers. On
the other hand, Deater-Deckard et al. (1996) and
Giles-Sims et al. (1995) found more CP by African
American parents.
Row 4 of Table IV shows that for this sample there
was a significant difference between ethnic groups,
even after controlling for seven other variables, includ-
ing SES. Seventy percent of African American parents
reported using CP during the previous year, …
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Associations between childhood experiences
of parental corporal punishment and
neglectful parenting and undergraduate
students’ endorsement of corporal
punishment as an acceptable parenting
strategy
Naomi KitanoID
1,2,3*, Kouichi YoshimasuID
4*, Beverley Anne Yamamoto3,
Yasuhide Nakamura
5
1 Research Center for Community Medicine, Wakayama
Medical University, Wakayama, Japan,
2 Department of Public Health, School of Medicine, Wakayama
Medical University, Wakayama, Japan,
3 Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Suita,
Japan, 4 Department of Hygiene, School of
Medicine, Wakayama Medical University, Wakayama, Japan, 5
Faculty of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Konan
Women’s University, Kobe, Japan
* [email protected] (NK); [email protected] (KY)
Abstract
This study evaluated the effects of childhood experiences of
parental corporal punishment
(CP) and neglectful parenting (NP) on Japanese university
students’ endorsement of paren-
tal CP (EPP) to discipline children, in relation to subjective
happiness (SH). A total of 536
undergraduate students who showed no physical symptoms
completed anonymous paper-
based questionnaires addressing demographic characteristics,
undergraduate classes, and
recent health conditions on SF-8 (PCS, MCS). It was found that
the proportions of partici-
pants who experienced pervasive CP and NP were larger in men
than in women (36.5% vs.
19.4% for CP; 22.1% vs. 9.7% for NP). Multiple regression
analyses (n = 346) revealed that
the CP score was associated with positive EPP (β = 0.310, p <
0.001). Further, students
whose major was nursery education reported significantly lower
level of EPP; however, nei-
ther SH nor good recent health conditions significantly reduced
EPP. The NP score was
inversely associated with the SH score (β = -0.253, p < 0.001)
(n = 346). In conclusion, child-
hood experiences of parental CP may affect adolescents’ views
related to their own parent-
ing. Further investigation using internationally comparable
methodologies, especially in
prospective cohort studies, is warranted, not only in Japan but
also in other Asian countries.
Introduction
The prevention of child maltreatment is one of the most urgent
public health issues worldwide
[1,2]. Recent neurobiological, morphological, genetic, and
epigenetic findings have offered
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 1 / 16
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Kitano N, Yoshimasu K, Yamamoto BA,
Nakamura Y (2018) Associations between
childhood experiences of parental corporal
punishment and neglectful parenting and
undergraduate students’ endorsement of corporal
punishment as an acceptable parenting strategy.
PLoS ONE 13(10): e0206243. https://doi.org/
10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
Editor: Neha John-Henderson, Montana State
University, UNITED STATES
Received: June 25, 2018
Accepted: October 9, 2018
Published: October 26, 2018
Copyright: © 2018 Kitano et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: There are ethical
restrictions on making data publicly available.
Since the data set of the current study contains
potentially sensitive student information, data
availability is under strict control of the Research
Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of
Human Sciences, Osaka University (email: jinka-
[email protected]).
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6103-4907
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5638-183X
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
experimental evidence to support the relationship between child
maltreatment and increased
susceptibility to health impairments later in life [3,4].
Furthermore, many longitudinal cohort
studies have suggested that child maltreatment and adverse
familial environments have nega-
tive effects on long-term health [5–8]. Epidemiological studies
have consistently shown a high
prevalence of mental health problems among children and
adolescents in community popula-
tions [9]. The heterogeneity in individual responses to such
experiences of maltreatment and
emotional abuse is an important predictor of poor mental health
throughout life [10,11], and
this might lead to the perpetuation of child abuse or neglect
between generations. Such inter-
generational transmission of maltreatment has been reported
based on several theoretical
models in Western countries [12–14]. The moderating effects of
cultural factors on the link
between parenting and child outcomes have been inconsistent
[15–17]. Across cultural groups,
more frequent use of corporal punishment is associated with
greater externalizing behaviors in
children [18]. However, previous multicultural studies did not
include Japan, where parenting
styles and discipline tactics are somewhat different compared
with even other East Asian coun-
tries [19], suggesting that Japan has set comparatively high
value on ‘loyalty’ while Korea has
set higher value on ‘piety for their parents’. In Japan, a
dramatic increase in the number of
reported cases of child maltreatment [20] has raised concerns
about not only the health and
welfare of the children most at risk but also the long-term
impacts of corporal punishment
(CP) [21] and neglectful parenting (NP). Therefore, we sought
to identify young people who
experienced childhood parenting problems in a population of
university students without
physical symptoms to explore the effects of such experiences on
young adulthood.
Hence, this study assessed the frequencies of reported parental
CP and/or NP and their
effects on young adults, focusing on their endorsement of CP as
an acceptable parenting strat-
egy and self-reported well-being, such as subjective happiness
(SH). We also examined the
confounding effects of SH and subjective mental and physical
health conditions on the associa-
tion between experiences of CP and/or NP and endorsement of
CP. Those with poor SH or
with bad mental health and experiences of CP and/or NP may
endorse CP. However, consider-
ing the traditional background of home discipline, even those
with good SH might strongly
endorse CP.
We also hypothesized that students who experienced childhood
CP and/or NP would
report lower levels of subjective well-being regardless of their
most recent physical/mental
health status. A previous study has suggested that adolescents’
exposure to maternal negative
affective behavior is associated with adolescents’ subjective
daily well-being [22]. Happiness is
a composite of life satisfaction, coping resources, and positive
emotions [23]. Positive emotions
are a powerful source of growth and change, predicting both
individuals’ judgments about life
and their skills for living well, including recovery from illness
[24,25].
Additionally, studying nursery education may be a possible
factor for the prevention of
maltreatment, which is inversely associated with endorsing CP.
Children exposed to school-
based programs show improvements in protective behaviors and
knowledge as well as increase
disclosure of abuse [26]. A nursery educational course includes
a variety of social sciences
such as psychology, psychiatry, and welfare, including
prevention or coping with child
maltreatment.
As such, the main purpose of this study was to examine the
association between childhood
experiences of parental CP and/or NP and endorsement of CP in
relation to current subjective
well-being in apparently healthy Japanese university students.
Next, to explore whether the
endorsement of CP can be changed by students’ appropriate
recognition, we also examined
whether university students who study nursery education are
less likely to endorse CP as a
suitable parenting practice.
Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 2 / 16
Funding: This research was supported by an Osaka
University, Graduate School of Human Sciences
‘Support Program for Improving Graduate School
Education’ grant: ‘Promoting the Use of Human
Science Data in General and Professional
Education’. The funder had no role in study design,
data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
Material and methods
The present study was an exploratory, cross-sectional
investigation targeting undergraduate
students at five universities in Western Japan. It was conducted
from September 2007 to July
2008. The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the
Research Ethics Committee of the
Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University.
Completing and returning the anony-
mous questionnaires indicated participants’ tacit informed
consent.
Study participants
Participants were recruited from first- and second-year
undergraduate classes in nursery edu-
cation, health nursing, social welfare, domestic science,
nutrition, and psychosociology. Eligi-
ble students were those studying in the Kansai area who could
be contacted by the first author
(NK), who was the principal investigator, in cooperation with
co-investigators at multiple uni-
versities. A total of 692 students from five universities were
recruited. Valid responses were
received from 536 respondents (104 men and 432 women, valid
response rate: 77.5%). None of
the study participants had any experience in caring for children.
Definitions
In the present study, the definition of CP reflected the
definitions by the American Association
of Pediatrics [27] and Straus and Donnelly [28]. We defined CP
as the use of physical force,
including spanking and hitting, for correcting or controlling a
child’s behavior.
We employed the definition of NP behaviors developed by
Straus and Kantor [29]. NP
behaviors are those that constitute a failure to act in ways
presumed by a society’s culture to be
necessary to meet a child’s developmental needs and that are
the caregiver’s responsibility to
provide.
Measurements
We constructed a five-part questionnaire, drawing on existing
instruments for use in the general
population. Our self-reported anonymous questionnaire (paper-
based) covered demographic
characteristics, experiences of CP and NP, recent physical and
mental health conditions, endorse-
ment of parental use of CP, and subjective well-being. Since no
official Japanese version existed
for a pre-existing instrument, we conducted a translation using
cross-cultural translation method-
ology and validation based on Acquadro et al. [30], and we
checked the reliability of these transla-
tions by confirmatory factor analyses and calculation of
Cronbach’s α coefficients.
Demographic data
The first part of the questionnaire collected basic demographic
data (i.e., gender, age, number of
siblings, birth-order, mother’s and father’s ages, and years of
education completed by parents) and
basic information about the participant’s academic affiliation
and major (i.e., university classes).
Experience of parental corporal punishment
To assess participants’ experience of parental CP, two items
were translated into Japanese and
administered: one asked whether respondents had been hit or
spanked “a lot” before the age of
12, and the other asked whether they were hit “a lot” during
their teenage years [31]. These
items were chosen because they have been used internationally
[31], facilitating comparisons
between findings.
Any response to these two items other than “strongly disagree”
was highly suggestive of the
respondent having experienced CP because of the inclusion of
the word “a lot” [31]; even if
Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 3 / 16
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
they disagreed with “a lot,” they might agree with “a little.”
Thus, for either question, responses
of “strongly agree,” “agree,” and “disagree” were considered
suggestive of CP, and responses of
“agree” and “strongly agree” were considered to indicate
pervasive CP.
To form the predictor measure in this study, responses to the
two items concerning CP
(strongly agree = 4, agree = 3, disagree = 2, and strongly
disagree = 1) were summed to pro-
duce the score representing the experience of CP (CP score).
Experience of neglectful parenting
To assess participants’ experience of NP, the eight-item revised
version of the Multidimen-
sional Neglectful Behavior Scale Adult Recall Short Form
(revised MNBS-AS) was employed
[32]: two items concerning physical neglect (e.g., my parents
did not keep me clean), two
items concerning cognitive neglect (e.g., my parents did not
help me to do my best), two items
concerning emotional neglect (e.g., my parents did not comfort
me when I was upset), and
two items concerning supervisory neglect (e.g., my parents did
not care if I did things like
shoplifting). The revised MNBS-AS was developed as a brief
instrument for use in surveys tar-
geting general populations [29,32,33].
As a predictor measure in the study, responses to the eight items
of the revised MNBS-AS
were rated (strongly agree = 4, agree = 3, disagree = 2, and
strongly disagree = 1) and summed
to produce the score representing the experience of NP (NP
score). To distinguish between
single and multiple experiences of NP, we followed Straus and
Savage [33] by presenting sepa-
rate data about the percentage of students who reported three or
more experiences of NP,
which was evidence of “pervasive NP.”
Recent health condition
This part of the questionnaire concerned respondents’ self-
reported physical and mental health
condition over the past month. The official Japanese version of
the 8-item SF-8 Health Survey
was administered (e.g., During the past four weeks, how much
did physical health problems
limit your physical activities [such as walking or climbing
stairs]? and During the past four
weeks, how much have you been bothered by emotional
problems [such as feeling anxious,
depressed, or irritable]?) [34]. Two indicators, the physical
condition score (PCS) and mental
condition score (MCS), were calculated as indicators of
respondents’ recent health condition.
Subjective happiness
To capture each participant’s subjective well-being, we
administered the official Japanese ver-
sion of the Subjective Happiness Scale, a global SH seven-point
Likert scale consisting of four
items (e.g., Compared with most of my peers, I consider myself:
less happy (1–7) more happy;
Some people are generally very happy. They enjoy life
regardless of what is going on, getting
the most out of everything. To what extent does this
characterization describe you?) [35,36]. In
the present study, the SH score—the mean score of the four
items, assessed on the seven-point
Likert scale—was used as an outcome measure of subjective
well-being. The degree of SH rises
as the SH score increases.
Endorsement of parental use of physical punishment on children
as a form
of discipline
The Adult Adolescent Parenting Inventory-2 (AAPI-2) was
developed to assess the parenting
and child-rearing attitudes of adults and adolescents, regardless
of experience in raising their
own children [37]. We obtained permission to translate this
instrument into Japanese, and its
Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 4 / 16
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
translated contents were confirmed using back-translation. In
the present study, our Japanese
version of AAPI-2 Form B was administered. We modified the
original five-point Likert scale
to a four-point scale (strongly disagree, disagree, agree, and
strongly agree) in this study to
avoid Japanese people’s tendency to choose the “uncertain”
response [38].
Following the 2005 edition of the AAPI Online Development
Handbook [37], we performed
a confirmatory factor analysis for each subscale of AAPI-2
Form B in our sample. We identi-
fied one factor (Cronbach’s α = 0.769) consisting of six items
regarding the endorsement of
the use of physical punishment (e.g., “Spanking children when
they misbehave teaches them
how to behave”). We called this factor the “Endorsement of
Physical Punishment as a Form of
Discipline Indicator” (EPP). The response to each EPP item was
rated as follows: strongly
agree = 4, agree = 3, disagree = 2, and strongly disagree = 1.
Adding the response ratings from
the six EPP items produces the EPP score, which was used as an
outcome measure in this
study. The degree of endorsement of physical punishment rises
as the EPP score increases.
Statistical methods
First, descriptive analyses were performed to examine
respondents’ demographic characteristics
and the frequency distributions of responses to items concerning
experience of CP and NP. To
examine the differences between the two groups of responses, t-
tests and chi-square tests or
Fisher’s exact test were conducted for parametric and non-
parametric variables, respectively.
Next, using data of 346 participants (56 men and 290 women),
we conducted multiple
regression analyses to detect the independent contributions of
the predictors of CP and NP
scores to the EPP score as outcome variable, adjusting for
participants’ basic demographic fac-
tors, undergraduate class, recent health condition on SF-8 (PCS,
MCS), and with/without the
SH score. As preparation for the multivariate analysis, bivariate
correlation analyses were per-
formed to examine the associations among the independent
variables, using list-wise case dele-
tion. Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were also used as
diagnostic for multicollinearity.
Another multiple regression analysis was conducted using the
SH score as an outcome variable
to examine the independent effects of CP and NP scores on the
SH score.
For all statistical analyses, SPSS version 24.0 for Windows
(IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA)
were used. A two-tailed P-value of less than 0.05 was required
for statistical significance.
Results
As shown in Table 1, there were no statistically significant
gender differences in the sociode-
mographic characteristics or present status of the 536
respondents, except for the variable “has
studied health or welfare at university.”
Experience of parental corporal punishment
Table 2 shows the frequency distribution of the number and
percentage of responses to the
two items concerning experience of parental CP. Responses of
“strongly agree,” “agree,” or
“disagree” on either or both of the items were considered
suggestive of CP, whereas responses
of “agree” or “strongly agree” were considered to indicate
pervasive CP.
Overall, 59.5% of students (72.1% of men and 56.5% of women)
provided responses sugges-
tive of experience of CP, indicating that they were spanked or
hit before the age of 12 by their
parents. Regarding their teenage years, 46.8% of students
(61.5% of men and 43.3% of women)
gave responses suggestive of experience of parental CP.
The percentage of respondents who gave answers suggestive of
experience of pervasive CP
was 22.8%. There was a significant gender difference, with
36.5% of men and 19.4% of women
(chi-square = 13.932, df = 1, P < 0.001) agreeing or strongly
agreeing with either or both items
Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 5 / 16
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
concerning experience of CP. There was a strong association
between experience of pervasive
CP before the age of 12 and during the teenage years (chi -
square = 146.504, df = 1, P < 0.001).
Experiences of neglectful parenting behaviors
Table 2 presents the frequency distribution of responses to each
of the eight items used to
assess experience of NP. Emotional and cognitive forms of NP
were reported more frequently
than were physical and supervisory forms.
The percentage of respondents who gave answers suggestive of
experience of pervasive NP
was 12.2%. There was a significant gender difference, with
22.1% of men compared with 9.7%
of women (chi-square = 12.081, df = 1, P = 0.001) reporting
experience of pervasive NP.
Gender differences in predictor and outcome measures
The mean ± SD of the CP score among 346 respondents (56 men
and 290 women) was
3.5 ± 1.6 (range: 2–8, median: 3, skewness: 0.886, kurtosis:
0.196); as shown in Table 1, there
Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants (n = 536).
Male (n = 104)
Mean (SD) / n (%)
Female (n = 432)
Mean (SD) / n (%)
P-value�
Socio-demographic variables
Age 19.4 (0.71) 19.5 (0.96) 0.693
Number of siblings
†
2.5 (0.75) 2.5 (0.73) 0.785
Birth-order
‡
1.7 (0.79) 1.8 (0.79) 0.670
Mother’s age at birth
§
28.1 (4.1) 28.1 (3.9) 0.970
Age difference between mother and father (years)
k
-2.9 (4.2) -2.7 (3.5) 0.607
Mother’s years of education
¶
13.2 (1.6) 13.3 (1.6) 0.746
Father’s years of education
#
13.8 (2.1) 14.0 (2.0) 0.357
Undergraduate classes
Nursery education 21 (20.2%) 127 (29.4%) 0.059
Health nursing 11 (10.6%) 123 (28.5%) <0.001
Social welfare 51 (49.0%) 49 (11.3%) <0.001
Domestic science 0 62 (14.4%) <0.001
Nutrition 15 (14.4%) 39 (9.0%) 0.101
Psychosociology 6 (5.8%) 32 (7.4%) 0.674
Recent health condition on SF-8
Physical condition score (PCS) 48.9 (7.4) 51.3 (6.4) 0.014
Mental condition score (MCS) 45.9 (8.7) 43.4 (7.3) 0.025
Predictive and outcome measures��
Score representing experience of corporal punishment (CP
score) 4.3 (1.6) 3.4 (1.5) <0.001
Score representing experience of neglectful parenting (NP
score) 15.1 (2.8) 12.8 (3.3) <0.001
Subjective happiness (SH) score 4.6 (1.1) 4.7 (0.94) 0.836
Endorsement of parental use of physical punishment on children
(EPP) score 13.1 (3.4) 12.7 (2.7) 0.413
�Continuous variable: t-test; Categorical variable: chi-square
test or Fisher’s exact test
†n = 530 (104 males and 426 females)
‡n = 521 (103 males and 418 females)
§n = 462 (74 males and 388 females)
kn = 437 (70 males and 367 females)
¶n = 476 (87 males and 389 females)
#n = 461 (85 males and 376 females)
��n = 346 (56 males and 290 females).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t001
Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 6 / 16
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
was a significant gender difference in the mean CP score (P <
0.001).The mean ± SD of the
NP score among them was 13.1 ± 3.4 (range: 8–24, median: 13,
skewness: 0.427, kurtosis: −-
0.215); as shown in Table 1, there was a significant gender
difference in mean NP scores
(P < 0.001). The mean ± SD of the SH score among them was
4.6 ± 0.97 (range: 2–7, median:
4.5, skewness: 0.083, kurtosis: −0.151); as shown in Table 1,
there was no significant gender dif-
ference in mean SH scores (P = 0.836).The mean ± SD of the
EPP score among 346 respon-
dents (56 men and 290 women) was 12.8 ± 2.8 (range: 6–21,
median: 13, skewness: −0.028;
kurtosis: 0.269); as shown in Table 1, there was no significant
gender difference in mean EPP
scores (P = 0.413).
Associations between predictor and outcome variables
As shown in Table 3, a few variables were significantly
correlated with EPP or SH scores, but
no multicollinearity was detected among the study variables by
VIFs. Hence, we included
some non-significant variables into the regression models based
on the previous evidence. As
shown in Table 4, by multiple regression analysis, the CP score
had a significantly positive
effect on the EPP score, whereas the NP score had no
significant effect on the EPP score. The
robustness of the standardized partial regression coefficient of
the CP score was demonstrated
by controlling for respondents’ basic demographic factors, the
NP score, undergraduate clas-
ses, and PCS and MCS (adjusted R
2
= 0.135). Subsequently, after the inclusion of the SH score
in the model, the results did not change (ΔR2 = 0.001) and the
CP score had a significantly pos-
itive effect on the EPP score (β = 0.310, P < 0.001). Another
independent variable with
Table 2. Frequency distribution of responses to each question
on experience of parental corporal punishment and neglectful
parenting in childhood and adoles-
cence (n = 536: 104 men and 432 women).
Corporal punishment item Strongly disagree
n (%)
Disagree
n (%)
Agree
n (%)
Strongly agree
n (%)
When I was less than 12 years old, I was spanked or hit a lot by
my mother or father. Men 29 (27.9) 40 (38.5) 29 (27.9) 6 (5.8)
Women 188 (43.5) 165 (38.2) 57 (13.2) 22 (5.1)
When I was a teenager, I was hit a lot by my mother or father.
Men 40 (38.5) 46 (44.2) 14 (13.5) 4 (3.8)
Women 245 (56.7) 151 (35.0) 28 (6.5) 8 (1.9)
Revised MNBS-AS item (sub-category of neglect) Strongly
disagree Disagree Agree Strongly agree
My parents helped me when I had trouble understanding
something.
1
Men 2 (1.9) 18 (17.3) 63 (60.6) 21 (20.2)
(cognitively) Women 10 (2.3) 61 (14.1) 247 (57.2) 114 (26.4)
My parents did not comfort me when I was upset. Men 15 (14.4)
56 (53.8) 27 (26.0) 6 (5.8)
(emotionally) Women 121 (28.0) 211 (48.8) 80 (18.5) 20 (4.6)
My parents helped me when I had problems.
1
Men 3 (2.9) 29 (27.9) 52 (50.0) 20 (19.2)
(emotionally) Women 7 (1.6) 73 (16.9) 230 (53.2) 122 (28.2)
My parents did not care if I did things like shoplifting. Men 60
(57.7) 30 (28.8) 11 (10.6) 3 (2.9)
(supervisory) Women 320 (74.1) 104 (24.1) 7 (1.6) 1 (0.2)
My parents did not help me to do my best. Men 24 (23.1) 56
(53.8) 17 (16.3) 7 (6.7)
(cognitively) Women 220 (50.9) 188 (43.5) 20 (4.6) 4 (0.9)
My parents gave me enough clothes to keep me warm.
1
Men 2 (1.9) 8 (7.7) 52 (50.0) 42 (40.4)
(physically) Women 8 (1.9) 16 (3.7) 147 (34.0) 261 (60.4)
My parents did not keep me clean. Men 46 (44.2) 47 (45.2) 7
(6.7) 4 (3.8)
(physically) Women 297 (68.8) 120 (27.8) 9 (2.1) 6 (1.4)
My parents did not care if I got into trouble in school. Men 51
(49.0) 42 (40.4) 10 (9.6) 1 (1.0)
(supervisory) Women 284 (65.7) 131 (30.3) 11 (2.5) 6 (1.4)
1: reverse-scored item
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t002
Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
October 26, 2018 7 / 16
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t002
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
T
a
b
le
3
.
P
e
a
rs
o
n
’s
c
o
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
c
o
e
ff
ic
ie
n
ts
b
y
b
iv
a
ri
a
te
c
o
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
a
n
a
ly
se
s
a
m
o
n
g
st
u
d
y
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
(l
is
t-
w
is
e
,
n
=
3
4
6
).
①
②
③
④
⑤
⑥
⑦
⑧
⑨
⑩
⑪
⑫
⑬
⑭
⑮
⑯
①
1
②
0
.3
2
0
�
�
�
1
③
0
.1
2
2
�
�
0
.2
4
6
�
�
�
1
④
-0
.0
4
4
-0
.2
1
0
�
�
�
-0
.2
5
4
�
�
�
1
⑤
0
.0
1
8
-0
.0
7
8
�
0
.0
3
6
0
.0
2
1
1
⑥
-0
.0
1
5
-0
.0
5
6
0
.0
3
3
0
.0
4
2
-0
.0
5
2
1
⑦
-0
.0
4
8
-0
.0
7
0
�
0
.0
1
9
0
.0
3
9
0
.0
5
6
0
.5
1
2
�
�
�
1
⑧
0
.0
2
2
-0
.0
4
8
0
.0
4
2
0
.0
3
2
0
.1
0
2
�
�
0
.1
5
3
�
�
�
0
.5
0
0
�
�
�
1
⑨
-0
.0
6
8
0
.0
8
3
�
-0
.0
4
3
-0
.0
0
6
-0
.0
2
0
-0
.0
1
5
0
.0
1
9
0
.2
4
2
�
�
�
1
⑩
-0
.0
5
7
-0
.1
2
8
�
�
�
-0
.0
8
9
�
�
0
.0
2
1
0
.0
8
5
�
-0
.0
Parental Corporal Punishment Predicts Behavior Problems
in Early Childhood
Matthew K. Mulvaney and Carolyn J. Mebert
University of New Hampshire
Using data from the National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development Study of
Early Child Care and Youth Development (Research Triangle
Institute, 2002), this study
examined the impact of corporal punishment (CP) on children’s
behavior problems. Longi-
tudinal analyses were specified that controlled for covarying
contextual and parenting
variables and that partialed child effects. The results indicate
that parental CP uniquely
contributes to negative behavioral adjustment in children at
both 36 months and at 1st grade,
with the effects at the earlier age more pronounced in children
with difficult temperaments.
Parents and mental health professionals who work to modify
children’s negative behavior
should be aware of the unique impact that CP likely plays in
triggering and maintaining
children’s behavior problems. Broad-based family policies that
reduce the use of this
parenting behavior would potentially increase children’s mental
health and decrease the
incidence of children’s behavior problems.
Keywords: corporal punishment, physical punishment, parental
discipline, behavior
problems
Corporal punishment (CP) is defined as “the use of phys-
ical force with the intention of causing a child pain, but not
injury, for the purposes of correction or control of the
child’s behavior” (Straus, 2001, p. 4). More than 90% of
children and approximately 50% of adolescents during their
adolescent years have experienced parental CP at least once
(Straus & Stewart, 1999). This form of discipline is admin-
istered frequently—an average of three times per week
during the toddler years (Straus, 2001)—and it is used more
often with male, African American, and poor children
(Straus & Stewart, 1999). Thus, for many children, CP
represents an important component of their socialization
experiences within the family. However, because of limita-
tions in most of the relevant research, relatively few defin-
itive conclusions can be drawn about the effects of CP on
child adjustment.
Most available research indicates that there are few, if
any, positive developmental outcomes associated with CP
beyond immediate compliance with a parent’s directive
(Gershoff, 2002). In fact, a growing body of research sug-
gests there may be unintended negative consequences, in-
cluding increasing children’s aggressive behavior and their
likelihood of becoming delinquent and contributing to
poorer psychological and cognitive functioning (e.g., Ger-
shoff, 2002; Smith & Brooks-Gunn, 1997). The strongest
link is between CP and externalizing behavior problems,
especially aggression (e.g., Strassberg, Dodge, Pettit, &
Bates, 1994; Straus & Kantor, 1994). Internalizing prob-
lems, such as depression and lower self-esteem, have also
been linked to CP (e.g., Turner & Finkelhor, 1996). Al-
though there are some contrary findings (e.g., Simons, John-
son, & Conger, 1994), most available evidence indicates
that CP represents a risk factor for the development of
negative behavioral, psychological, and cognitive character -
istics.
Despite the many studies linking parental CP to negative
developmental outcomes, there is continuing debate among
social scientists and policymakers regarding the interpreta-
tion of these studies and the overall conclusions that can be
drawn about the unique, specific impact of CP (e.g., Baum-
rind, 1996; Baumrind, Larzelere, & Cowan, 2002). A major
factor underlying the controversy is that there are aspects of
the research that limit interpretations, including the nonex-
perimental, correlational methodology that must be em-
ployed to examine outcomes. As is the case with most
research concerning the effects of parental socialization
practices, third variables and child effects may explain the
reported associations between CP and child outcomes.
Several parenting variables covary with the use of CP and
therefore generally are not controlled in examinations of the
relation between CP and child outcomes. Although CP is
used within all parenting-style groups described by Baum-
rind (1973), its use varies reliably between groups. Many
authoritative parents use CP, but authoritarian parents use it
much more frequently; permissive parents use it less fre-
Matthew K. Mulvaney and Carolyn J. Mebert, Department of
Psychology, University of New Hampshire.
We thank Murray Straus, Toni Bisconti, and Glenda Kaufman-
Kantor for their helpful contributions to this research.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Matthew K. Mulvaney, who is now at the Department of
Psychology,
State University of New York College at Brockport, 350 New
Cam-
pus Drive, Brockport, NY 14420. E-mail: [email protected]
Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American
Psychological Association
2007, Vol. 21, No. 3, 389 –397 0893-3200/07/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.389
389
quently but more harshly (Parke & Collmer, 1975). The link
between CP and negative developmental outcomes may
thus be reflecting the influence of the broader parenting
style rather than CP specifically. Supporting this notion,
Simons et al. (1994) found that CP significantly predicted
aggressiveness and delinquency but that the effects became
nonsignificant after controlling for parental involvement.
Similarly, Larzelere, Kleinn, Schumm, and Alibrando
(1989) found that the amount of CP received in adolescence
negatively predicted self-esteem but that the association
became nonsignificant after controlling for the amount of
positive communication in the parent– child relationship.
However, not all research supports the notion that the ef-
fects of CP are epiphenomenal to broader parenting styles.
For instance, Buehler and Gerard (2002) reported that CP
influenced global psychological functioning, even after con-
trolling for parental involvement. The issue of whether CP
influences development beyond the parenting context in
which it is used is far from resolved, but it is evident that
models examining the influence of CP must simultaneously
consider the parenting style in which it occurs.
An additional problem with the reliance on correlational
methods is that it is difficult to identify the direction of the
effects (e.g., Baumrind, 1997). Most studies assume a
parent-to-child effect, although it is clear that within-child
characteristics play an important role in evoking different
kinds of parenting behaviors (Lengua & Kovacs, 2005;
Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Distinguishing the direction of
effects between physical discipline and child outcomes is
imperative. An effective way to deal with this issue is to
employ developmental designs in which the outcome be-
havior of interest is statistically controlled at the initial time
point (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Born-
stein, 2000). That is, the behavior of interest, along with the
measure of parental behavior, is measured at the initial
assessment. The prediction equation then controls for the
initial level of the outcome variable so that the actual
influence of parental behavior can be determined. Recent
studies of CP have incorporated such designs and have
provided more convincing evidence for a parent-to-child
effect in antisocial and other problem behavior (e.g., Lengua
& Kovacs, 2005; Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1997).
A final issue is how best to conceptualize the impact of
CP on children’s development. Given the array of outcomes
associated with CP, a simple social learning model does not
provide a sufficient explanation. Rather, as Turner and
Finkelhor (1996) have argued, the effect of CP may best be
understood from a stress-process framework. Receiving
physical discipline from a parent is likely to be stressful for
a child, and children who are continuously exposed to
stressors in their environment (and perceive them as stress-
ful) show increased internalizing and externalizing behav-
iors (e.g., Cicchetti & Toth, 1991; Grant et al., 2003).
Repetti, Taylor, and Seeman (2002) suggested that ongoing
stress within the family environment will have long-term
effects on mental health via changes in the biological self-
regulatory systems of the child. Repetti et al. also describe,
beyond physiological changes that increase susceptibility to
mental health disorders, a transactional process in which
family stressors lead to additional developmental problems,
including decreased emotion processing and poorer peer
relationships. These, in turn, contribute to poorer mental
health. The effects may also be cumulative, with CP that
extends throughout childhood being the most detrimental.
Contextual variables may contribute to the stress re-
sponse of children to their parents’ discipline and moderate
the impact of that discipline. Supporting this notion is the
work on parenting style mentioned above, as well as re-
search on ethnic group differences in the effects of CP.
Specifically, CP has been found to be associated with ag-
gression in European American children but not in African
American children, even though CP is used with greater
frequency by African American parents (Deater-Deckard &
Dodge, 1997; Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997). This may reflect
both a greater degree of cultural acceptance of CP within
African American communities, including the view that it is
a legitimate parental behavior, and differing interpretations
of such punishment by African American and European
American children. However, following a review of the
relevant studies, Horn, Joseph, and Cheng (2004) cautioned
that additional research, particularly longitudinal studies
controlling for potentially confounding variables (e.g., so-
cioeconomic status, child effects), is necessary before con-
clusions can be drawn about ethnic or racial differences in
the effects of CP.
In the present study, the effects of parental CP on tod-
dlers’ and young children’s behavior problems were exam-
ined in the large, longitudinal, and diverse data set of the
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development
(NICHD SECCYD; Research Triangle Institute, 2002). The
primary focus of the analyses was to clarify the existence
and direction of influence of parental behavior on develop-
mental outcomes. It was hypothesized that CP would be
associated with both broadband categories of problem be-
haviors (externalizing and internalizing behaviors) at 36
months and first grade. The developmental analyses that
were performed included controlling for outcome variables
at an initial time point, which allowed for stronger claims to
be made about the direction of effects (Collins et al., 2000).
Because of the myriad parenting and contextual variables
assessed, it was possible to control for a substantial number
of factors implicated in the relation between CP and nega-
tive child outcomes (e.g., parenting style, income, maternal
depression, and ethnicity). A test of the moderating effect of
intrapersonal and contextual variables on the impact of CP
was also included. It was hypothesized that child tempera-
ment and African American ethnicity would moderate the
influence of CP on these developmental outcomes.
Method
Sample
Data collection for the NICHD SECCYD (Research Tri-
angle Institute, 2002), a multisite longitudinal study de-
signed to examine the effects of child care on children’s
development, began in 1991 and continues presently. The
390 MULVANEY AND MEBERT
sample originally consisted of 705 male (51.7%) and 659
(48.3%) female children. There were 1,014 non-Hispanic
Caucasian (74.34%), 176 African American (12.9%), 83
Hispanic (6.09%), and 93 otherwise classified (6.82%) chil-
dren. The mean age of the mothers at their child’s birth was
28.11 years (SD ! 5.63). Twenty-four percent of the fam-
ilies were classified as living in poverty, as indicated by an
income-to-needs ratio of less than 1. For more detailed
recruitment procedures of the NICHD SECCYD, see the
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2001).
At the first-grade assessment, 1,028 mothers (75.37% of
the original sample) completed the outcome measure of
interest in the present study (the Child Behavior Checklist
[CBCL]; Achenbach, 1991). As is the case with most lon-
gitudinal studies, participant attrition occurred in a nonran-
dom fashion. Independent-sample t tests, with status in first
grade as the between-subjects factor (still participating or
not), were computed to assess group differences in maternal
education and average income. Families that completed the
first-grade outcome measure had a higher average income-
to-needs ratio (M ! 3.55, SD ! 2.68) than did families that
did not (M ! 2.67, SD ! 2.92), t(1353) ! 5.05, p " .001,
d ! .27. Mothers in families participating at the first-grade
level had more years of education (M ! 14.46, SD ! 2.45)
than did those who were not (M ! 13.56, SD ! 2.57),
t(1361) ! 5.76, p " .001, d ! .31. Ethnicity was also
associated with subject attrition, #2(5, N ! 1,364) ! 12.69,
p " .05, $ ! .10. There were more Caucasian families than
African American families in the first-grade sample relative
to the initial sample.
Measures
CP. The CP variable was derived from the Home Obser-
vation for the Measurement of the Environment (HOME;
Caldwell & Bradley, 1984), administered when the children
were 15, 36, and 54 months of age. HOME assesses the
overall quality of the family environment by both interview -
ing the mother and observing the family in a naturalistic
setting during an extensive observation process. Two items
from HOME were used: (a) an interview with the mother, to
determine whether the children had been spanked more than
once in the previous week, and (b) the test administrators’
observation of whether the children were spanked in their
presence. Scores could range from 0 to 2, depending on
whether the answer to neither, one, or both of the items was
a yes. Because this composite variable included both self-
report and observations of the parenting behavior, the va-
lidity of the measure should be better than either alone,
although a two-item measure is not ideal with respect to
reliability. Despite the potentially decreased reliability, re -
searchers have been able to employ this measure, or very
similar measures, to investigate the impact of CP on devel -
opmental outcomes (McLoyd & Smith, 2002; Smith &
Brooks-Gunn, 1997).
Behavior problems. The CBCL (Achenbach, 1991,
1992) was used as an index of children’s behavior problems.
The CBCL for Ages 2–3 (CBCL/2–3; Achenbach, 1992)
was administered at 36 months, and the CBCL for Ages
4 –18 (CBCL/4 –18; Achenbach, 1991) was administered
when the children were in first grade. Mothers completed
the scale in their homes at 36 months and in the lab during
the first-grade assessment. The broadband Externalizing and
Internalizing scales were used at both ages. Although these
scales assess conceptually similar constructs across the two
ages, the overlap in the actual items is modest, reflecting
age-related differences in behavioral characteristics. Stan-
dardized T scores were used in the following analyses:
Demographic characteristics and temperament. At the
1-month assessment, the mothers reported on their chil-
dren’s ethnicity and sex. When the children were 6 months
of age, the mothers completed the Activity, Adaptability,
Approach, Mood, and Intensity subscales of the Infant Tem-
perament Questionnaire—Revised (Carey & McDevitt,
1978). The scores of the nonmissing items for all subscales
were combined into a single variable, called difficult child
temperament, by averaging across the items. Higher scores
represent a more “difficult” temperament. The measure has
been found to have good test–retest reliability (Carey &
McDevitt, 1978). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .81.
Maternal depression. When the children were 1, 6, 15,
24, 36, and 54 months of age, as well as during the first-
grade assessment, the mothers completed the Center for
Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Rad-
loff, 1977), a well-known measure with good psychometric
properties. Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was equal to or
greater than .88 for each assessment.
Income-to-needs. Income-to-needs information was ob-
tained when children were 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months
old, as well as when they were in first grade, via interviews
with the parents. At each age, the income of the total
household from all sources was divided by the cutoff point
for poverty for that particular year, as based on the number
of people in the household, with guidelines established by
the U.S. Bureau of the Census (2004).
Maternal sensitivity. Maternal sensitivity was assessed
when children were 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months old, as well
as when they were in first grade, with a semistructured
observational procedure. This procedure, specifically de-
signed for the NICHD SECCYD by D. L. Vandell and M. T.
Owen, consists of observing a mother and her child playing
with age-appropriate toys. For detailed information about
the development and administration of the scale, see
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999). The 6-
and 15-month measures were assessed in the family’s home.
The 24-, 36-, and 54-month and first-grade measures were
assessed in a laboratory. For the 6-, 15-, and 24-month
assessments, a composite measure of sensitivity was created
by summing global ratings of sensitivity to nondistress,
intrusiveness (reverse scored), and positive regard. For mea -
sures at 36 and 54 months, as well as in first grade, the
sensitivity composite was constructed by summing support-
ive presence, hostility (reverse-scored), and respect for au-
tonomy. All interactions were videotaped and coded at a
central location. At all ages, approximately 20% of the
interactions were coded by a second coder and yielded the
following interrater reliability coefficients: .87, .83, .84 .84,
.79, and .84 for the 6-, 15-, 24-, 36-, and 54-month and
391CORPORAL PUNISHMENT AND BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS
first-grade composite measures, respectively. Higher scores
indicate more sensitive parenting. Scores on this measure
are significantly related to attachment, suggesting moderate
construct validity (NICHD Early Child Care Research Net-
work, 1997).
Results
Data Analysis Plan
To isolate the unique and directional influence of parental
CP on developmental outcomes in infancy and early child-
hood, we specified two sets of hierarchical multiple-
regression models. The first set assessed the impact of CP in
infancy and toddlerhood by examining the associations be-
tween the 15-month CP variable and the 36-month Internal-
izing and Externalizing scores. The second set of analyses
examined the associations between internalizing and exter-
nalizing behavior problems in first grade and a composite
CP variable constructed from the 36- and 54-month assess-
ments. Because of the temporal precedence of the CP vari -
able in these analyses, its unique contribution to the predic-
tion of the subsequently measured behaviors would
strengthen a causal argument for the directional influence of
CP on developmental outcomes. All analyses were carried
out with the actual sample and with the sample weighted to
account for the relation between ethnicity and attrition.
These analyses yielded virtually identical results, so onl y
the unweighted versions are presented.
For each model, the child’s gender, ethnicity, and tem-
perament and the aggregate variables maternal sensitivity,
maternal depression, and family income-to-needs were in-
cluded as control variables. The wide array of control vari-
ables included in these analyses should decrease the likeli -
hood of omitted-variables bias. The maternal depression,
family income, and maternal sensitivity variables were ob-
tained over the course of earlier assessments and col -
lapsed into a single measure for each analysis by aver-
aging across all the assessments. The internal consistency
for each of these composite measures was acceptable
(Cronbach’s % & .69).
CP and Behavior Problems at 36 Months
The correlation matrix and descriptive statistics for all
variables in the first set of analyses are in Table 1. The CP
variable was modestly associated with both of the outcome
variables (36-month externalizing and internalizing behav-
iors). The matrix also indicated substantial covariation be-
tween the CP, control, and outcome variables. Maternal
depression had the strongest association with the variables
of interest, likely reflecting the fact that many of the child
variables were measured through maternal report. Thus,
controlling for depression is important for all analyses, not
only because of the influence of maternal mental health on
children’s development but also because mothers’ percep-
tions of children’s behavior are influenced by their own
mental health (e.g., Fergusson, Lynskey, Horwood, 1993;
Mebert, 1991).
For the regression analyses, the control variables were
entered in the first step. In the second step, the CP variable
was entered. A product of the standardized CP and difficult
child temperament variables, which assessed the moderat-
ing role of difficult child temperament, was also entered in
the second step. The results of the analyses predicting both
externalizing and internalizing behaviors at 36 months are
in Table 2. This table contains the standardized and unstand-
ardized regression coefficients, the standard errors, and the
partial correlations for each model. For internalizing behav-
iors, the first step was significant, F(6, 1131) ! 49.38, p "
.001, adjusted R2 ! .20. Maternal depression and sensitivity
and difficult child temperament were unique predictors. At
the second step, CP, but not the interaction term, contributed
significantly to the proportion of variance accounted for,
F(2, 1129) ! 3.62, p " .05, 'R2 ! .005. All variables that
had been associated with internalizing in the first step re-
mained significant in the second step.
In the analysis of externalizing behaviors, the first step
was also significant, F(6, 1131) ! 39.83, p " .001, adjusted
R2 ! .17. As shown in Table 2, both maternal depression
and difficult child temperament were associated with in-
creased externalizing behavior. In the second step, the CP
variable and the interaction term were found to add signif-
Table 1
Descriptives and Correlation Matrix for Variables Used in the
36-Month Analysis (N ! 1,138)
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. 36-month externalizing behaviors 51.07 8.49 — .70*** (.04
.07** .24*** .38*** (.18*** (.20*** .20***
2. 36-month internalizing behaviors 51.16 9.50 — .04 .10***
.26*** .41*** (.18*** (.23*** .16***
3. Gender (female) 1.49 0.50 — .00 .04 .02 .04 .10*** (.11***
4. Ethnicity (African American) 0.11 0.32 — .17*** .16***
(.27*** (.40*** .11***
5. Difficult child temperament (0.01 0.99 — .23*** (.14***
(.18*** .08**
6. Average maternal depression 9.43 6.40 — (.29*** (.33***
.19***
7. Average income-to-needs 3.47 2.74 — .44*** (.25***
8. Average maternal sensitivity 0.02 0.74 — (.34***
9. Corporal punishment (0.01 1.00 —
Note. The average maternal depression, income-to-needs, and
maternal sensitivity variables are aggregates of all assessments
up to and
including the 36-month assessment. The difficult child
temperament, average maternal sensitivity, and corporal
punishment variables are
standardized (Z scores).
** p " .01. *** p " .001.
392 MULVANEY AND MEBERT
icantly to the prediction equation, F(2, 1129) ! 8.87, p "
.001, 'R2 ! .01. Figure 1 displays the interaction. In this
figure, the externalizing scores at 36 months were regressed
onto the 15-month CP raw scores separately for children
with “easy” and “difficult” temperaments, as designated by
a median split. CP was more strongly associated with ex-
ternalizing behavior problems for children with more diffi -
cult temperaments.
CP and Behavior Problems in First Grade
Correlations and descriptive statistics for the variables
used in the first-grade analyses are in Table 3. CP was
associated with all control and outcome variables used in
these analyses. The 36-month broadband factors were
strongly correlated with the same broadband factors at first
grade.
The first-grade regression models employed residualized
change analysis in which the outcome variable of interest
was statistically controlled at an earlier time (36 months).
That is, mothers’ 36-month ratings of internalizing (or ex-
ternalizing) behaviors were included as a control variable in
predicting internalizing (or externalizing) behavior at first
grade. Although this method of analysis is correlational and
constrained by limitations in causal interpretation, it can
provide strong evidence of a parent-to-child effect (e.g.,
Collins et al., 2000). The other control variables that were
entered in the first step were the child’s gender, ethnicity,
and temperament and the aggregate variables maternal sen-
sitivity, maternal depression, and family income-to-needs.
In the second step, CP and an interaction variable represent-
ing the moderating impact of African American ethnicity on
CP was tested. The CP variable used in these analyses was
a standardized average of the 36- and 54-month measures.
Two interaction variables were constructed and tested as
moderators. The first interaction variable was constructed
by multiplying the standardized 6-month difficult child tem-
perament variable by the aggregate CP variable. The second
interaction variable was constructed by multiplying the Af-
rican American status variable by the aggregate CP variable.
Only control variables contributed to the prediction of
internalizing behaviors at first grade. Neither CP nor the
moderator terms were significantly associated with change
in internalizing behaviors.
CP was, however, associated with increased externalizing
behaviors from 36 months to first grade. These results are
presented in Table 4. The first step was significant, F(7,
971) ! 77.68, p " .001, adjusted R2 ! .35. Not surpris-
ingly, externalizing behavior at 36 months was strongly
associated with externalizing behavior in first grade. The
child’s gender predicted change in externalizing behavior,
as did maternal depression and family income. In the second
step, CP, but not the interaction terms, contributed signifi -
cantly to the variance accounted for, F(3, 968) ! 2.93, p "
.05, 'R2 ! .006. The control variables that had been sig-
nificant in the first step remained significant with the inclu-
sion of CP and the interaction variables. We also respecified
this model to test an additional moderating hypothesis. A
term representing the interaction between the 36-month
externalizing behavior and the CP variables was entered in
the second step. It was not significantly associated with
first-grade externalizing behavior.
Discussion
The primary goal of this study was to address the con-
cerns of researchers (e.g., Baumrind, 1996; Baumrind et al.
2002) who have argued that normative CP does not have
detrimental effects on children’s adjustment. Although this
study does not address all of the concerns of these research-
Table 2
Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting 36-
Month Behavior Problems
Variable
Externalizing behaviors Internalizing behaviors
B SE ) Sr B SE ) Sr
Step 1
Gender (female) (0.71 0.46 (.04 0.61 0.51 .03
Ethnicity (African American) (1.19 0.81 (.04 (0.85 0.88 (.03
Difficult child temperament 1.36 0.24 .16*** 1.56 0.26 .17***
Average material depression 0.41 0.04 .31*** 0.51 0.04 .34***
Average income-to-needs (0.16 0.10 (.05 (0.09 0.10 (.03
Average maternal sensitivity (0.68 0.38 (.06 (1.12 0.41 (.09**
Step 2
Gender (female) (0.57 0.46 (.03 (.03 0.70 0.51 .04 .04
Ethnicity (African American) (1.09 0.80 (.04 (.04 (0.79 0.88
(.03 (.02
Difficult child temperament 1.39 0.24 .16*** .16 1.56 0.26
.17*** .16
Average maternal depression 0.40 0.04 .30*** .27 0.50 0.04
.34*** .30
Average income-to-needs (0.13 0.10 (.04 (.04 (0.08 0.11 (.02
(.02
Average maternal sensitivity (0.28 0.39 (.03 (.02 (0.85 0.43
(.07* (.05
Corporal punishment 0.93 0.25 .11*** .10 0.59 0.27 .06* .06
Corporal Punishment * Difficult Child Temperament 0.47 0.24
.06* .05 0.43 0.26 .04 .04
Note. The difficult child temperament and corporal punishment
variables are standardized. The gender and ethnicity variables
are
dichotomous variables. The average maternal depression,
income-to-needs and maternal sensitivity variables are
aggregated from all
assessments prior to and inclusive of the 36-month assessment.
* p " .05. ** p " .01. *** p " .001.
393CORPORAL PUNISHMENT AND BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS
ers and certainly does not conclusively establish a causal
link between CP and child adjustment, the results extend
and complement the growing body of literature suggesting
that there is a unique negative impact of CP on children’s
behavior problems. CP was associated with increased inter -
nalizing behaviors during toddlerhood and with increased
externalizing behavior problems both in toddlerhood and at
first grade. Although this research was correlational, several
features of the analyses strengthen a causal argument re-
garding the impact of CP on behavioral outcomes. First, the
presumed causal variable (i.e., CP) was assessed temporally
prior to the outcome …
10.1177/0192513X03258313 ARTICLEJOURNAL OF FAMILY
ISSUES / September 2004Turner, Muller / LONG-TERM
EFFECTS OF CORPORAL PUNISHMENT
Long-Term Effects of Child Corporal
Punishment on Depressive Symptoms
in Young Adults
Potential Moderators and Mediators
HEATHER A. TURNER
PAUL A. MULLER
University of New Hampshire
Based on a sample of 649 students from 3 New England
colleges, this study examined the
long-term effects of childhood corporal punishment on
symptoms of depression and consid-
ered factors that may moderate or mediate the association.
Similar to national studies, ap-
proximately 40% of the sample reported experiencing some
level of corporal punishment
when they were 13 years old. Findings indicated that level of
corporal punishment is posi-
tively related to depressive symptoms, independent of any
history of abuse and the frequency
of other forms of punishment. Although parental monitoring and
perceived norms regarding
corporal punishment had no direct or moderating effects, level
of parental anger during cor-
poral punishment was the strongest predictor of depression. The
association between “angry
corporal punishment” and symptoms of depression in young
adulthood is partially mediated
by mastery and self-esteem. Implications of these finding are
discussed.
Keywords: corporal punishment; depression; parental anger;
self-concept
It is widely accepted, and there is much empirical evidence, that
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und
Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und

More Related Content

Similar to Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und

Assignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docx
Assignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docxAssignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docx
Assignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docxbraycarissa250
 
The Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants Suite
The Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants SuiteThe Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants Suite
The Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants SuiteDavid Driesbach, PMP
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startHaashimm
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startshyaminfopvtltd
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startvernonlopezz
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startmybrands2
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startCaedmoncadoc
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startanglecharles28
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startanglecharles28
 
396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx
396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx
396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docxgilbertkpeters11344
 
United Airlines Case Study Assignment Help
United Airlines Case Study Assignment HelpUnited Airlines Case Study Assignment Help
United Airlines Case Study Assignment HelpMark Jack
 
Accounting principles 1e
Accounting principles 1eAccounting principles 1e
Accounting principles 1eJose Cintron
 
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-startUop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-startaagnaa
 
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-startUop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-starti88057782
 
BUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docx
BUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docxBUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docx
BUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Sentry Financial Corporation
Sentry Financial CorporationSentry Financial Corporation
Sentry Financial CorporationJohn Bonnemort
 
Lcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issues
Lcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issuesLcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issues
Lcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issueserbinhp
 

Similar to Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und (20)

Assignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docx
Assignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docxAssignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docx
Assignment 1Positioning Statement and MottoUse the pro.docx
 
The Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants Suite
The Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants SuiteThe Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants Suite
The Tables You Need to Know in the PeopleSoft Grants Suite
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment startUop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
Uop fin 571 week 6 assignment start
 
Mobi business-plan-template-
Mobi business-plan-template-Mobi business-plan-template-
Mobi business-plan-template-
 
Accounting principles 1E
Accounting principles 1EAccounting principles 1E
Accounting principles 1E
 
396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx
396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx
396Chapter Plant Assets, NaturalResources, andInta.docx
 
United Airlines Case Study Assignment Help
United Airlines Case Study Assignment HelpUnited Airlines Case Study Assignment Help
United Airlines Case Study Assignment Help
 
Accounting principles 1e
Accounting principles 1eAccounting principles 1e
Accounting principles 1e
 
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-startUop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
 
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-startUop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
Uop fin-571-week-6-assignment-start
 
BUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docx
BUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docxBUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docx
BUSM1227 International Business Entry Modes (II)Busine.docx
 
Sentry Financial Corporation
Sentry Financial CorporationSentry Financial Corporation
Sentry Financial Corporation
 
Lcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issues
Lcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issuesLcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issues
Lcfpd fort sheridan rfp consideration and issues
 
Privatization -reimer
Privatization -reimerPrivatization -reimer
Privatization -reimer
 

More from mariuse18nolet

IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docxIRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docxmariuse18nolet
 
Ironwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docx
Ironwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docxIronwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docx
Ironwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docxmariuse18nolet
 
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docxIRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docxmariuse18nolet
 
Iranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docx
Iranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docxIranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docx
Iranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docxmariuse18nolet
 
IRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook E.docx
IRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook  E.docxIRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook  E.docx
IRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook E.docxmariuse18nolet
 
IQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docx
IQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docxIQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docx
IQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docxmariuse18nolet
 
iPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docx
iPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docxiPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docx
iPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docxmariuse18nolet
 
Iranian Journal of Military Medicine Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docx
Iranian Journal of Military Medicine  Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docxIranian Journal of Military Medicine  Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docx
Iranian Journal of Military Medicine Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docxmariuse18nolet
 
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docxIoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docxmariuse18nolet
 
IP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docx
IP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docxIP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docx
IP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docxmariuse18nolet
 
IranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docx
IranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docxIranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docx
IranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docxmariuse18nolet
 
ipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docx
ipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docxipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docx
ipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docxmariuse18nolet
 
In Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docx
In Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docxIn Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docx
In Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docxmariuse18nolet
 
Investor’s Business Daily – Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docx
Investor’s Business Daily –  Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docxInvestor’s Business Daily –  Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docx
Investor’s Business Daily – Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docxmariuse18nolet
 
Invitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docx
Invitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docxInvitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docx
Invitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docxmariuse18nolet
 
Invitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docx
Invitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docxInvitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docx
Invitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docxmariuse18nolet
 
IOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docx
IOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docxIOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docx
IOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docxmariuse18nolet
 
INVITATION TO Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docx
INVITATION TO  Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docxINVITATION TO  Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docx
INVITATION TO Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docxmariuse18nolet
 
Investment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docx
Investment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docxInvestment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docx
Investment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docxmariuse18nolet
 
Investment BAFI 1042 Kevin Dorr 3195598 GOODMAN .docx
Investment BAFI 1042  Kevin Dorr 3195598  GOODMAN .docxInvestment BAFI 1042  Kevin Dorr 3195598  GOODMAN .docx
Investment BAFI 1042 Kevin Dorr 3195598 GOODMAN .docxmariuse18nolet
 

More from mariuse18nolet (20)

IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docxIRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeFall 2014Proje.docx
 
Ironwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docx
Ironwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docxIronwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docx
Ironwood Company manufactures cast-iron barbeque cookware. During .docx
 
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docxIRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docx
IRM 3305 Risk Management Theory and PracticeGroup Project.docx
 
Iranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docx
Iranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docxIranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docx
Iranian Women and GenderRelations in Los AngelesNAYEREH .docx
 
IRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook E.docx
IRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook  E.docxIRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook  E.docx
IRB HANDBOOK IRB A-Z Handbook E.docx
 
IQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docx
IQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docxIQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docx
IQuiz # II-Emerson QuizGeneral For Emerson, truth (or.docx
 
iPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docx
iPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docxiPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docx
iPython 2For Beginners OnlyVersion 1.0Matthew .docx
 
Iranian Journal of Military Medicine Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docx
Iranian Journal of Military Medicine  Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docxIranian Journal of Military Medicine  Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docx
Iranian Journal of Military Medicine Spring 2011, Volume 13, .docx
 
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docxIoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
 
IP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docx
IP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docxIP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docx
IP Subnet Design Project- ONLY QUALITY ASSIGNMENTS AND 0 PLAG.docx
 
IranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docx
IranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docxIranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docx
IranAyatollahTheocracyTwelver ShiismVilayat-e Faghih (jur.docx
 
ipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docx
ipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docxipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docx
ipopulation monitoring in radiation emergencies a gui.docx
 
In Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docx
In Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docxIn Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docx
In Innovation as Usual How to Help Your People Bring Great Ideas .docx
 
Investor’s Business Daily – Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docx
Investor’s Business Daily –  Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docxInvestor’s Business Daily –  Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docx
Investor’s Business Daily – Investors.comBloomberg Business – Blo.docx
 
Invitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docx
Invitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docxInvitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docx
Invitation to Public Speaking, Fifth EditionChapter 8 Introdu.docx
 
Invitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docx
Invitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docxInvitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docx
Invitation to the Life SpanRead chapters 13 and 14.Objectives.docx
 
IOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docx
IOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docxIOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docx
IOBOARD Week 2 Lab BPage 2 of 4Name _________________ Gr.docx
 
INVITATION TO Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docx
INVITATION TO  Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docxINVITATION TO  Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docx
INVITATION TO Computer Science 1 1 Chapter 17 Making .docx
 
Investment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docx
Investment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docxInvestment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docx
Investment Analysis & Portfolio Management AD 717 OLHomework E.docx
 
Investment BAFI 1042 Kevin Dorr 3195598 GOODMAN .docx
Investment BAFI 1042  Kevin Dorr 3195598  GOODMAN .docxInvestment BAFI 1042  Kevin Dorr 3195598  GOODMAN .docx
Investment BAFI 1042 Kevin Dorr 3195598 GOODMAN .docx
 

Recently uploaded

APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfSanaAli374401
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Shubhangi Sonawane
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.MateoGardella
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingTeacherCyreneCayanan
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxnegromaestrong
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.christianmathematics
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docxPoojaSen20
 

Recently uploaded (20)

APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 

Investment Analysis2182020Letter of IntentMemorandum of Und

  • 1. Investment Analysis 2/18/2020 Letter of Intent/Memorandum of Understanding Elements 1. Local economic & social impact · Sports organizations generate revenues from enthusiastic and supporting fans. It is crucial for a sports organization to establish local support before constructing a sports facility/stadium. In return, a well-designed stadium will benefit the city-county by boosting its local economy and increasing the quality of life for its local citizens. · Economic Impact: Increases the city’s media exposure, attracts more tourists to the city and thereby increases the city’s tax revenue. It also benefits local businesses by attracting more customers on the event day · Social Impact: Gives fans a sense of belonging, generates pride 2. Stadium Site / Location · Site selection is an important consideration for a new stadium project. Organizations must evaluate their unique market and establish its site requirements. · Site acquisition: Organization and city must agree on a proper site acquisition plan. A privately financed project such as the Oakland Athletics’ Coliseum, purchases its stadium site from the county. Whereas a publicly financed project such as the Broward County Civic Arena may have a designated stadium site recommended by the city-county (Florida Panthers LOI 12) 3. Stadium Design · New stadiums should be designed to satisfy their unique market needs. The design should also support the main tenant’s business operation while accommodating other tenants · Seating: appropriate seating number to maximize ticket revenue. Broward County Civic Arena has 21,000 seats to accommodate baseball fans (Florida Panthers LOI 3) while the Eagles designed near 70,000 seats for football games (Eagles
  • 2. Lease Terms 1) · Parking: if stadium located away from the city, parking spaces need to be designed · Other: amenities, restroom, social areas, shop, and restaurants also need to be considered 4. Stadium Construction and Completion date · The sports organization and city must have a mutual agreement on the roles and responsibilities in the stadium construction process. City county often contract with a third- party company to serve as “construction manager” to pursuant to the CM agreement and manage the construction process (Atlanta Braves Cobb County MOU 5) · Completion deadline: it is crucial for all parties to finish the construction process prior to the deadline/ beginning of the season. Any delay will cause significant revenue loss 5. Project Funding · It is fundamental for an organization to develop a funding plan for the new stadium. The project funding plan should include the estimated cost of construction, as well as parties’ contribution agreement. For instance, the city of Philadelphia agreed to cover the new Eagle’s stadium’s site acquisition and construction cost as well as a part of the costs of demolition of existing buildings. While the Eagles are responsible for the rest of the demolition, plus the basic stadium elements costs and environmental remediation cost (Eagles Lease Terms 3) 6. Ownership · Most publicly funded stadiums such as the Broward County Civic Arena are owned by the city-county, while other privily funded stadiums are owned by the team/organization. Different ownership structure provides different opportunities and threat · Media Rights / Naming rights ownership: both being a large portion of the revenue. Stadium and tenants must establish media and stadium naming rights agreement · Intellectual Property ownership: stadium and tenants must also agree on an Intellectual Property License Agreement, which manages tenant’s rights to use the stadium’s intellectual
  • 3. properties in their marketing program. For instance, the Georgia World Congress Center Authority entered into an IP license agreement, which granted the Atlanta Falcons the right to use Mercedes Benz Stadium’s trademarks and trade names in their marketing programs. The agreement also restricts GWCCA from using the Falcons image on any other GWCCA campus buildings (Atlanta New Stadium Project 23) 7. License Agreement · Depending on the ownership structure, sports organizations might be required to acquire the license to operate in their stadium. For instance, the Florida Panthers pays the county five percent of its ticket sales from general seating, in order to play in the publicly funded stadium (Florida Panthers LOI 5) · Scheduling of event: tenants must communicate their event dates with the stadium in advance so there is no concurring event on the same date. Booking procedures should be established as well as a priority booking policy for the main tenant (Atlanta New Stadium Project 10) 8. Operation Management · Both the organization and the stadium must reach an agreement on the responsibilities of the facility’s daily operation. Organizations may use a third-party Management company to facilitate stadium operations. All parties should establish a management agreement, and the management company must fulfill all tenant’s operational goals and objectives · O&M, R&R responsibilities: Organizations and facilities must have a mutual agreement on the responsibilities of the facility’s future operational maintenance and repair/ replacement. For example, the Georgia World Congress Center Authority cooperates with the Falcons with its stadium maintenance and covers all landscaping maintenance costs. However, each year, the GWCCA requires the Falcons to submit its maintenance and capital improvement plan before the next season (Atlanta New Stadium Project 19) 9. Financial Terms
  • 4. · Having an operating expense budget is important because organizations must minimize expenses in order to maximize profit. Organizations must efficiently allocate their budget between employee salary, field maintenance, equipment purchases, administrative services and taxes. (Florida Panthers A-2) Some operating expenses may be covered by the stadium. For instance, the GWCCA will provide the Atlanta Falcons an annual budget for traffic allocation services (Atlanta New Stadium Project 14) · Organizations also need to consider how the operating revenue is being distributed. Such as ticket revenue, merchandise sales, food and beverage sales. Teams might need to pay a certain percentage of their revenue to the county issued revenue bonds. 10. Development strategy Agreement · In order to maximize a sporting facility’s usage, it is important that a stadium and its local county, to share the same commitment for a long-term growth strategy. Teams such as the Atlanta Falcons might be required to enter into a Non- Relocation Agreement with its stadium. In such cases, the teams are restricted to relocate, unless a material breach has been made by the stadium operator (Atlanta New Stadium Project 9)
  • 5. Citation Florida Panthers and Broward County, FL for Broward County Civic Arena 1996. Atlanta New Stadium Project 2012. Eagles Lease Terms 2000. “8 Things to Consider When Building a New Sports Complex.” Earth Networks, 8 Nov. 2019, www.earthnetworks.com/blog/8- things-consider-building-a-new-sports-complex/. 1 3 Title of PaperStudent NameCourse/Number Due Date Faculty Name Detailed expectations for a 100-299 Level Course(remove this line)Title of Paper Triple click your mouse anywhere in this paragraph to replace this text with your introduction. Often the most important paragraph in the entire essay, the introduction grabs the reader's attention—sometimes a difficult task for academic writing. When writing an introduction, some approaches are best avoided. Avoid starting sentences with “The purpose of this essay is . . .” or “In this essay I will . . .” or any similar flat announcement of your intention or topic. Read more: Center for Writing Excellence>Tutorials and Guides>Essay Development>Guidelines for Writing Academic Essays. Level One Heading
  • 6. Replace the level one heading with the words for your heading. The heading must be in bold font. Headings help your audience track the sub-topics discussed in the body of the essay or report. Begin a new heading for each sub-topic. Be sure to indent the first line of each paragraph between five and seven spaces by pressing the Tab key one time on the keyboard. In addition, remember to double space the entire paper using the double space functionality in Word. This template is already formatted for double spacing. Read more: Center for Writing Excellence>Tutorials and Guides>Software Tutorials and Guides>Formatting Tutorial for APA. In addition, keep in mind an academic essay should contain at least five paragraphs, which includes the introduction (introductory paragraph), the body (which is generally at least 3 paragraphs), and the conclusion (generally one paragraph). Most well-developed paragraphs contain at least 3-5 sentences, one of which is the topic sentence. Limit each body paragraph to one main idea. Conclusion The closing paragraph is designed to bring the reader to your way of thinking if you are writing a persuasive essay, to understand relationships if you are writing a comparison/contrast essay, or simply to value the information you provide in an informational essay. The closing paragraph summarizes the key points from the supporting paragraphs without introducing any new information. References This is a hanging indent. To keep the hanging indent format, triple click your mouse on this line of text and replace the information with your reference entry. You can use the Reference and Citation Examples (Center for Writing Excellence>Tutorials and Guides>Reference and Citation Examples) to help format your source information into a reference entry.
  • 7. The reference page always begins on the top of the next page after the conclusion. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, Vol. 2, No. 2, 1999 Corporal Punishment by American Parents: National Data on Prevalence, Chronicity, Severity, and Duration, in Relation to Child and Family Characteristics Murray A. Straus1,2 and Julie H. Stewart1 We present data on corporal punishment (CP) by a nationally representative sample of 991 American parents interviewed in 1995. Six types of CP were examined: slaps on the hand or leg, spanking on the buttocks, pinching, shaking, hitting on the buttocks with a belt or paddle, and slapping in the face. The overall prevalence rate (the percentage of parents using any of these types of CP during the previous year) was 35% for infants and reached a peak of 94% at ages 3 and 4. Despite rapid decline after age 5, just over half of American parents hit children at age 12, a third at age 14, and 13% at age 17. Analysis of chronicity found that parents who hit teenage children did so an average of about six times during the year. Severity, as measured by hitting the child with a belt or paddle, was greatest for children age 5-12 (28% of such children). CP was more prevalent among African American and low socioeconomic status parents, in the South, for boys, and by mothers. The pervasiveness of
  • 8. CP reported in this article, and the harmful side effects of CP shown by recent longitudinal research, indicates a need for psychology and sociology textbooks to reverse the current tendency to almost ignore CP and instead treat it as a major aspect of the socialization experience of American children; and for developmental psychologists to be cognizant of the likelihood that parents are using CP far more often than even advocates of CP recommend, and to inform parents about the risks involved. INTRODUCTION Evidence indicating that almost all American parents use spanking and other legal forms of corpo- ral punishment (CP) on toddlers has been available for many years. Sears, Maccoby, and Levin (1957), for example, found that 99% of the children they studied experienced CP at least occasionally. Straus (1983) found a 95% rate for toddlers, and Bryan and Freed (1982) found that 95% of a sample of commu- nity college students had experienced CP. Numerous other studies (e.g., Giles-Sims, Straus, & Sugarman, 1Family Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire, Durham, New Hampshire, 03824. 2Address all correspondence to Murray A. Straus, Family Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire, Durham, New Hamp- shire 03824; e-mail: [email protected] 1995; Goodenough, 1931/1975; Holden, Coleman, & Schmidt, 1995; Straus, 1994a; Wauchope & Straus, 1990) also show extremely high rates of CP. CP there- fore appears to be a near universal aspect of the
  • 9. socialization experience of American children, al- though to widely varying degrees in individual cases. There is also increasing evidence that, even when done by loving and supportive parents, CP is associ- ated with an increased risk of unwanted later effects such as aggression and depression as an adult (Straus, 1994a; Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1997; Thompson, In press). Despite the high prevalence rates and the evi- dence suggesting harmful side effects, there is also evidence that this pervasive aspect of the socialization of American children is either not perceived or ig- nored. One indication is the extremely limited cover- age of CP revealed by a content analysis of 10 widely 55 1096-4037/99/0600.0055$16.00/0 © 1999 Plenum Publishing Corporation KEY WORDS: Punishment; physical; corporal; spanking; parent; age; infant; gender; SES; ethnic; region. 56 Straus and Stewart used textbooks in child development published be- tween 1985 and 1989 (Straus, 1994a). For this article, the analysis was repeated for books published be- tween 1992 and 1996. The results in Table I shows little change from the 1980s to the 1990s. The omis- sion of information on CP in child development text- books is unfortunate because discipline problems are frequently the focus of clinical child psychology and, as indicated above, CP is a widely used disciplinary
  • 10. technique. Consequently, the background under- standing the nature and extent of CP provided by the research reported in this article may be helpful in clinical practice. Among the books for the period 1992-1996, only a third had entries in the index for corporal punish- ment, spanking, physical punishment, or anything on CP under subheads such as discipline or punishment. Although page by page scanning the text located material on corporal punishment in 9 of the 11 books, these 9 books devoted an average of only three tenths of a page to CP. Because, as noted earlier, data on CP has been available for many years, the absence of information on CP in child development textbooks cannot be attributed to lack of developmentally relevant data. Straus (1994a) has suggested methodological and so- ciocultural factors to explain the neglect of a nearly universal aspect of the socialization of American chil - dren. Whatever the reason, current information is needed because the last few years have been a period in which public endorsement of CP declined sharply (Straus & Mathur, 1996). There has also been a recent surge of professional debate about the potential harmful effects of CP, as evidenced by special issues of Pediatrics in 1996; Psychological Inquiry, Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, and Aggression and Violent Behavior in 1997; and by the 1998 policy statement on CP by the American Academy of Pedi- atrics. Deep value commitments underlie the debate on the appropriateness of CP (Friedman & Schonberg,
  • 11. 1996; Straus, 1994a). Readers of this article therefore need to be aware that our position differs from the standard assumptions of American culture—that CP is sometimes necessary, and if done in moderation by loving parents, is harmless. This tenet of American culture is shared by at least two thirds of Americans (Straus & Mathur, 1996), including the majority of pediatricians (White, 1993) and clinical psychologists (Anderson & Anderson, 1976). Our view, however, is that parents should never use CP because a growing body of research evidence shows that other disciplin- ary strategies are just as effective and do not involve the risk of escalation into physical abuse (Straus, 1994a) or of subsequent psychological problems for children and adults (Straus, 1994b; Straus & Paschall, 1998; Straus et al., 1997). Regardless of whether one accepts or rejects use of CP, we suggest that clinical child psychology can benefit from a background understanding of the ex- tent to which parents use this mode of discipline with children of different ages. Consequently, the purpose of this article is to make available information on the use of CP by a nationally representative sample of American parents in 1995 for each year of life from birth through 17, and other age categories. It also provides information on the prevalence of six specific acts of CP that vary in severity, on the chronicity of CP (how often CP was used), and on the duration of CP (the number of years until CP ceases). In addi- tion, we examine the extent to which CP varies by seven other characteristics of the children and the families. The information on the extent to which CP dif- fers according to these characteristics of children and
  • 12. families can help to understand the social structural determinants of CP and can also have practical value because, as indicated above, there is a growing pro- fessional interest in helping parents shift from CP to nonviolent forms of discipline. Such programs need to be guided by empirical data on the extent to which CP is prevalent at different ages and in different sectors of the population. For example, African American parents, and low education parents of all ethnic groups, are more likely to approve of and use CP (Alvy, 1987; Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1996; Giles-Sims et al., 1995). Consequently, parent education intended to reduce CP may need to be structured to reflect the culture of these groups. Table I. Treatment of Corporal Punishment in Child Development Textbooks in 1980s and 1990s CP in Index (%) Range of pages on CP Mean no. of pages on CP % of pages on CP Advised avoiding CP if possible (%) Advised to never use CP (%) Year of publication 1985-1989 (n = 10) 20 1 sentence
  • 13. to 4 pages 0.4 0.0 to 0.3 10 0 1992-1996 (n = 11) 36 1 sentence to 0.8 pages 0.3 0.0 to 0.08 27 9 Corporal Punishment 57 If, in recent years, fathers have taken on additional child care responsibilities, the additional effort needed to include fathers in parent education pro- grams may be even more important than previously. Because this article is intended primarily to ex- plore and describe rather than to test formal hypothe-
  • 14. ses, each topic in the Results section is introduced by a brief description of findings from previous research. METHOD Sample The data were obtained from a survey by the Gallup Organization (1995). The survey was con- ducted by telephone in August and September 1995. The telephone numbers were selected by a random- digit stratified probability design. A random proce- dure was used to provide representation of both listed and unlisted numbers. These methods are designed to produce, with proper weighting, and unbiased probability sample of telephone households in the continental United States, which includes 94% of all households. Among households that met the eligibil- ity criterion (one or more children under 18 living there), the participation rate was 81%. A total of 991 interviews were completed. In two-parent house- holds, because of budget limitations, one parent was randomly selected for the interview. In multichild households, one child was randomly identified, and a parent of that child interviewed, and all data reported pertain to the selected child. See Gallup Organization (1995) for more detailed sampling information. A telephone sample obviously excludes those without telephones, which tends to be low socioeconomic sta- tus (SES) persons. However, face-to-face interview surveys also have great difficulty locating and en- listing the participation of such persons, and there is considerable evidence that properly conducted tele- phone surveys obtain a higher participation rate and are usually more representative than face-to-face in- terview surveys (Bermack, 1989; Groves et al., 1988;
  • 15. Wells, Burnam, Leake, & Robbins, 1988). Sample Characteristics and Weighting The division between boys and girls in this sam- ple was almost equal (49% girls). The ethnic compo- sition was 81% Euro-American, 12% African American, and 7% other ethnic groups (Hispanic Americans, Native Americans, and Asian Ameri- cans). The mean age of the parents interviewed was 36.8 years. More mothers than fathers were inter- viewed (66% of the sample), partly because the sam- ple included single parents who are predominantly mothers. Fifty-two percent of the parents were mar- ried, 15% remarried, 20% divorced, 8% never mar- ried, and 4% were cohabiting. The referent children ranged in age from infants to age 17, with a mean age of 8.4 years. Households with college-educated parents were overrepresented (34 vs. 23% in the Cen- sus) and those with less than a high school education were underrepresented (8 vs. 14% in the Census). In all analyses, data were weighted to make the data reflect U.S. Census statistics with regard to the gender of the respondent, child's age, ethnic group, region of the country, and parent's education. This involved an unproven assumption that the replies of nonsur- veyed members of underrepresented groups would be similar to those who were surveyed. Definition and Measurement of Corporal Punishment The definition of CP which guided this research is "the use of physical force with the intention of causing a child to experience pain, but not injury, for
  • 16. the purpose of correction or control of the child's behavior" (Straus, 1994a, p. 4). This corresponds in practice to the legal definition of CP in all states of the U.S., which is in the form of an exemption from the crime of assault for parents who use physical force for purposes of correction and control (see Straus, 1994a, for a discussion and illustrative statutes). The Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scales (CTSPC; Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore, & Runyan, 1998) was used to obtain the data on CP. This version of the CTS includes six items that fall within the range of legal CP. It asks parents how often each of these acts occurred in the previous 12 months. The response categories were never, once, twice, 3-5 times, 6-10 times, 11-20 times, and more than 20 times. Prevalence of CP. The overall prevalence statis- tics in this article are based on whether the parent reported one or more instances of one or more of the following acts in the previous 12 months. Thus, multiple acts could have occurred in a particular CP episode. (See Straus et al., 1998, for the exact ques- tion wording.) 58 Straus and Stewart Spanked on the bottom with your bare hand Slapped on the hand, arm, or leg Pinched Shook (for children age 3 and over only) Hit on the bottom with something like a belt, hair-
  • 17. brush, a stick or some other hard object Slapped on the face or head or ears Chronicity. For the parents who used CP, it is also important to know how often they engaged in that disciplinary tactic. A chronicity measure was therefore computed for each parent who reported using CP by summing the frequency of each of the six acts in the CP scale. The chronicity of the specific acts are also reported. See Straus (1998) and Straus et al. (1998) for an explanation of the need to measure prevalence and chronicity separately. Severity. Severity of CP can be measured in a number of ways (Straus, 1998). The severity of CP was based on a judgment concerning the risk of injury and degree of normative acceptance made by a five- person team that included a pediatrician specializing in child abuse, a clinical psychologist, two sociologists specializing in research on the family, and a political scientist specializing in public opinion research (Straus et al., 1998). Three acts were judged to carry a higher risk and be less widely accepted, and were classified as severe CP (slap on face or head, hit with belt or hard object, pinched) and two as less severe (spanked and slapped on the hand or leg). Shaking was considered separately because of the extreme danger if done to infants. Duration. Because this is a cross-sectional study, an estimate of the median duration can only be in- ferred from the age-specific prevalence statistics. If for example, the percentage hit at ages 11, 12, 13, and 14 were 61, 52, 45, and 32%, respectively, we would infer that the median duration is about 12 years. This inference depends on the assumption that
  • 18. parents who used CP at age 12 were doing so in previous years. One-Year Reporting. A short reporting period, such the previous week, results in a much higher estimate of chronicity, but a low prevalence rate be- cause not every parent who uses CP did so in the previous week. This is particularly the case with older children. Conversely, a 1-year reporting period re- sults in a higher prevalence rate, but the difficulty of recalling the number of instances produces an under- estimate of chronicity. To deal with this problem, the questions need to be repeated for the previous week and previous year (Straus, 1998). However, budget limitations restricted this study to the previous year as the reporting period. Consequently, the chronicity data in this article must be regarded as minimum esti- mates. Measures of Independent Variables Age of Child. The analyses of age differences in CP used 1-year age intervals (from under 1 through 17). The analysis of the interaction of age with the other independent variables grouped the children into the following six categories in order to have enough cases per cell to be statistically reliable: In- fants (0-1), toddlers (age 2-4), early primary school (age 5-8), later primary school (age 9-12), and teens (age 13-17). Age of Parent. Three age categories were used: 18-29, 30-39, and 40 and over. Single Parent. This variable indicates whether
  • 19. the child was living with only one parent, versus all other living arrangements. Socioeconomic Status. SES was measured by a factor score obtained by a principle components anal - ysis of the respondent's education and income. The factor score was then coded into quintiles in order to have enough cases per cell for the ANOVA. Ethnic Group. Ethnic group was included in the analysis with three categories. Euro-Americans were coded as 1, African Americans as 2, and all other ethnic groups as 3 because there were not sufficient cases to make finer differentiations. Gender of Parent and Child. Girls were coded as 1 and boys as 0. Mothers were coded as 1 and fathers as 0. Region. The Northeast was coded as 1, the Mid- west as 2, the South as 3, and the West as 4. Data Analysis Consistent with the emphasis on describing the prevalence of CP rather than testing a theoretical model, ANOVA was used as the main mode of analy- sis because it provides the observed mean CP for children of each age and each category of the other independent variables, and for the interactions of these variables. The ANOVAs were computed using the "regression approach" option in SPSS/PC, namely, "All effects are assessed simultaneously, with each effect adjusted for all other effects in the model" (Norusis, 1992, p. 257). Thus, the test for each independent variable controls for the other in-
  • 20. Corporal Punishment 59 dependent variables, and the means were adjusted to control for the other independent variables. The N for the child age trend analyses of preva- lence is 991. The analysis of the chronicity of CP are based on the 609 cases where CP was used at least once. For the ANOVA relating differences in charac- teristics of the family and child to the prevalence of CP, the analyses of prevalence are based on 978 cases. The ANOVA for chronicity is based on 602 cases. The number of cases for each category of the in- dependent variables are given in Table II. The ANOVAs were restricted to main effects and two- way interactions because higher order interactions would have resulted in empty cells and singular vari- ance-covariance matrices. recently become a focus of attention. Previous re- search indicates that CP increases from infancy to age 2, stays about the same for ages 3 through 5, and decreases steadily from age 5 through 17 (Bachman, 1967; Straus, 1991,1994a; Wauchope & Straus, 1990), and that pattern was expected to hold for this sample. Age and Prevalence of Corporal Punishment In Figure 1 the dashed line is for the observed means and the solid line plots the moving average. (The moving average controls for random fluctua- tions by combining the mean of the observed mean for each age and the age just below and just above.)
  • 21. Figure 1 shows that just over a third of infants (chil- dren under 1 year of age) were hit by their parents. The rate then rises to a peak at ages 4 and 5, whenRESULTS Age of Child and Corporal Punishment The age of the child is uniquely important for understanding the place of CP in the socialization of American children. This is because of the tremendous variation by age and, as indicated earlier, because the age at which CP is considered appropriate has Fig. 1. Prevalence of corporal punishment by child's age. Table II. Number of Cases in Categories Used for ANOVAs Characteristic and category Age of child 0-1 2-4 5-8 9-12 13-17 Gender of child Male Female Gender of respondent Male Female Age of respondent 18-29 30-39
  • 22. 40+ SES quintiles 1 Low 2 3 4 5 High Ethnic group White Black Other Single parent Yes No Region NE Midwest South West No. of cases Table III 92 190 230 228 246 503 483
  • 24. 181 420 171 285 146 148 81 145 126 101 480 97 25 207 395 101 159 223 119 60 Straus and Stewart 94% of parents said that, during the previous 12 months, they had used one or more of the types of CP included in the CP scale. From there on, the rate declined steadily to age 17. However, it is important to note that as late as age 13, over 40% of parents
  • 25. used CP as a disciplinary technique. Age and Chronicity of Corporal Punishment Age Differences and Severity of Corporal Punishment Table III gives the percentage of parents who used ordinary CP and more severe types of CP, and also two other disciplinary tactics. It uses age catego- ries rather than 1-year age intervals because, as ex- plained in the Method section, single-year categories produced too many empty cells for the more rarely occurring types of CP. Comparing parts A and B of Table III reveals a developmental pattern. The milder forms of CP in Part A (spanking and slapping) were used most often with toddlers (ages 2-4). The more severe and less culturally approved forms of CP (see Method section) in Part B of Table III were most prevalent in middle childhood (ages 5-12 in Table III). Teenagers had the lowest prevalence rates, but in our opinion were still remarkably high. Hitting a child on the bottom with objects such as a belt, hairbrush, or stick was, presumably, extremely common as recently as the 1940s. Even as recently as the 1970s, two thirds of a random sample of the population of Texas believed that hitting a child with such objects is acceptable (Teske & Parker, 1983). Table III indicates that actual use of belts and paddles has also not disappeared. More than one in four American parents reported having used such objects on a child age 5 to 12 in 1995.
  • 26. The least frequent forms of CP were slapping on the face, head, or ears and pinching. Except for infants, from 3 to 8% of children of all age groups experienced these two forms of CP. Part C of Table III includes two important disciplinary tactics that do not fit neatly into the categories "minor" and "severe" CP. The first of these, shaking a child, can cause brain injury or death to children under 2. So it is an indicator of child abuse rather than CP for children that young. Although 4.3% of this national sample reported shaking a child that was under 2, none of the cases of shaking were for infants. On the one hand, this makes the statistic less ominous. On the other hand, it makes the rate for the 12- to 23-month-old children 10.3%. Looking across the Shaking row of Table III, about 1 of 10 parents of children of all ages except infants and teens used shaking as a mode of CP. The Threatened to Spank row of Table III de- scribes the prevalence of a tactic that, strictly speak- ing is a form of verbal aggression, not an act of CP as previously defined. It was therefore not one of the Although Figure 1 indicates how many children were hit at each age, it does not indicate how often parents hit the children in this study. This information is given in Figure 2. It refers to those parents who used CP at least once during the previous year and gives the mean number of times they reported using CP. Figure 2 shows that CP was most chronic by parents of 2-year-old children. They reported using CP an average of 18 times during the previous year. The chronicity of CP declined from there on, to a
  • 27. mean of six times per year by parents of 14-, 15-, and 16-year-old children. Fig. 2. Chronicity of corporal punishment by child's age. Corporal Punishment 61 Table III. Percentage of Parents using Specific Acts of Corporal Punishment by Age of Child Type of corporal punishment A. Ordinary Spank on bottom with hand Slap on hand, arm or leg B. Severe Hit on bottom with object Slap on face, head or ears Pinch C. Other Shake Threatened to spank Age of child 0-1 31.8 36.4 2.5 0.5
  • 30. .02 .03 .02 .01 items in the CP scale used to obtain the prevalence rates in Figures 1 to 3. It is included in Table III, however, because it is a form of verbal aggression that often leads to the actual use of CP and because it is so frequent right up through the teen years. Other Child and Parent Characteristics Associated with CP The analyses in this section are based on ANOVAs using eight independent variables, as de- scribed in the Method section. The tests of signifi- cance for each of the independent variables therefore controlled for the level of all the other variables in the analysis. The means for each variable in this sec- tion have been adjusted for the level of the other variables and therefore describes the net effect of the variable after controlling for the other independent variables. The numbers for each category of the vari- ables are given in Table II. Age of Parent Studies of three nationally representative sam- ples of parents found that the younger the parent, the greater the prevalence of CP (Day, Peterson, & McCracken, 1998; Giles-Sims et al., 1995; Straus,
  • 31. 1994a). For the current study, although there is a tendency for younger parents to be more likely to use CP, the second row of Table IV shows that the differences were not significant. However, row 2 of Table V shows a significant relation of parents' age to the chronicity of CP. Younger parents (18-29) used CP an average of 17.1 times during the previous 12 months in comparison to 12.6 times for those 30-39 and 9.1 times for parents over 40. This differ- ence is not the result of younger parents having younger children (who, on average, are hit more fre- quently) because the analysis controlled for the age of the child. Socioeconomic Status Although some studies have found that the higher the SES, the less use of CP (Giles-Sims et al., 1995), most have found no important difference (Erlanger, 1974a, 1974b). The present study found a significant main effect for SES (Table IV, row 3). Figure 3 shows that the percentage of parents using CP was highest among parents in the lowest quintile of the SES scale, and decreased with increasing SES. The solid line is for the observed percentage, and the dotted line gives the percentage after controlling for the other seven independent variables (age and gen- der of the parent and the child, the ethnicity of the child, single parents, and region). Comparing the two lines shows that controlling for these variables low - ered the relation of SES to CP, but only slightly. We also found significant interactions of SES with age of parent and age of child. The interaction of SES with age of the parent mainly occurred be-
  • 32. cause for younger parents the percentage who used CP was high regardless of SES. Thus the main effect finding, that the higher the SES, the lower the per- centage using CP, applies only to the two older age groups (ages 30 to 39 and 40+). Ethnic Group Previous research on ethnic differences in CP is also contradictory. For example, Straus (1994a) found no important difference between African 62 Straus and Stewart Table IV. Analysis of Variance of Prevalence of Corporal Punishment (N = 978) Source of variation Main effects Age of child Age of parent SES Ethnicity of child Gender of child Gender of parent Single parent Region Two-way interactions Gender of child X age of child Gender of child X Gender of parent Gender of child x Age of parent Gender of child X SES
  • 33. Gender of child x Ethnicity of child Gender of child x Single parent Gender of child x Region Age of child X Gender of parent Age of child x Age of Parent Age of child x SES Age of child x Ethnicity of Child Age of child X Single parent Age of child x Region Gender of parent x age of parent Gender of parent X SES Gender of parent x ethnicity of child Gender of parent x single parent Gender of parent X Region Age of parent x SES Age of parent X Ethnicity of child Age of parent x Single parent Age of parent x Region SES X Ethnicity of child SES X Single parent SES x Region Ethnicity of child X single parent Ethnicity of child X region Single parent X region df 4 2 4 2 1 1 1 3
  • 39. .704 .131 .171 .320 .179 .218 Americans and Euro-Americans, but Day et al. (1998) found more CP by African American parents, but only for children age 5 to 11 years; and among younger children, only for unmarried mothers. On the other hand, Deater-Deckard et al. (1996) and Giles-Sims et al. (1995) found more CP by African American parents. Row 4 of Table IV shows that for this sample there was a significant difference between ethnic groups, even after controlling for seven other variables, includ- ing SES. Seventy percent of African American parents reported using CP during the previous year, … RESEARCH ARTICLE Associations between childhood experiences of parental corporal punishment and neglectful parenting and undergraduate
  • 40. students’ endorsement of corporal punishment as an acceptable parenting strategy Naomi KitanoID 1,2,3*, Kouichi YoshimasuID 4*, Beverley Anne Yamamoto3, Yasuhide Nakamura 5 1 Research Center for Community Medicine, Wakayama Medical University, Wakayama, Japan, 2 Department of Public Health, School of Medicine, Wakayama Medical University, Wakayama, Japan, 3 Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Suita, Japan, 4 Department of Hygiene, School of Medicine, Wakayama Medical University, Wakayama, Japan, 5 Faculty of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Konan Women’s University, Kobe, Japan * [email protected] (NK); [email protected] (KY) Abstract This study evaluated the effects of childhood experiences of parental corporal punishment
  • 41. (CP) and neglectful parenting (NP) on Japanese university students’ endorsement of paren- tal CP (EPP) to discipline children, in relation to subjective happiness (SH). A total of 536 undergraduate students who showed no physical symptoms completed anonymous paper- based questionnaires addressing demographic characteristics, undergraduate classes, and recent health conditions on SF-8 (PCS, MCS). It was found that the proportions of partici- pants who experienced pervasive CP and NP were larger in men than in women (36.5% vs. 19.4% for CP; 22.1% vs. 9.7% for NP). Multiple regression analyses (n = 346) revealed that the CP score was associated with positive EPP (β = 0.310, p < 0.001). Further, students whose major was nursery education reported significantly lower level of EPP; however, nei- ther SH nor good recent health conditions significantly reduced EPP. The NP score was inversely associated with the SH score (β = -0.253, p < 0.001) (n = 346). In conclusion, child- hood experiences of parental CP may affect adolescents’ views related to their own parent- ing. Further investigation using internationally comparable methodologies, especially in
  • 42. prospective cohort studies, is warranted, not only in Japan but also in other Asian countries. Introduction The prevention of child maltreatment is one of the most urgent public health issues worldwide [1,2]. Recent neurobiological, morphological, genetic, and epigenetic findings have offered PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 1 / 16 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 OPEN ACCESS Citation: Kitano N, Yoshimasu K, Yamamoto BA, Nakamura Y (2018) Associations between childhood experiences of parental corporal punishment and neglectful parenting and undergraduate students’ endorsement of corporal
  • 43. punishment as an acceptable parenting strategy. PLoS ONE 13(10): e0206243. https://doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 Editor: Neha John-Henderson, Montana State University, UNITED STATES Received: June 25, 2018 Accepted: October 9, 2018 Published: October 26, 2018 Copyright: © 2018 Kitano et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Data Availability Statement: There are ethical restrictions on making data publicly available. Since the data set of the current study contains potentially sensitive student information, data
  • 44. availability is under strict control of the Research Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University (email: jinka- [email protected]). http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6103-4907 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5638-183X https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0206243&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-26 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ mailto:[email protected] mailto:[email protected] experimental evidence to support the relationship between child maltreatment and increased susceptibility to health impairments later in life [3,4]. Furthermore, many longitudinal cohort
  • 45. studies have suggested that child maltreatment and adverse familial environments have nega- tive effects on long-term health [5–8]. Epidemiological studies have consistently shown a high prevalence of mental health problems among children and adolescents in community popula- tions [9]. The heterogeneity in individual responses to such experiences of maltreatment and emotional abuse is an important predictor of poor mental health throughout life [10,11], and this might lead to the perpetuation of child abuse or neglect between generations. Such inter- generational transmission of maltreatment has been reported based on several theoretical models in Western countries [12–14]. The moderating effects of cultural factors on the link between parenting and child outcomes have been inconsistent [15–17]. Across cultural groups, more frequent use of corporal punishment is associated with greater externalizing behaviors in children [18]. However, previous multicultural studies did not include Japan, where parenting styles and discipline tactics are somewhat different compared with even other East Asian coun-
  • 46. tries [19], suggesting that Japan has set comparatively high value on ‘loyalty’ while Korea has set higher value on ‘piety for their parents’. In Japan, a dramatic increase in the number of reported cases of child maltreatment [20] has raised concerns about not only the health and welfare of the children most at risk but also the long-term impacts of corporal punishment (CP) [21] and neglectful parenting (NP). Therefore, we sought to identify young people who experienced childhood parenting problems in a population of university students without physical symptoms to explore the effects of such experiences on young adulthood. Hence, this study assessed the frequencies of reported parental CP and/or NP and their effects on young adults, focusing on their endorsement of CP as an acceptable parenting strat- egy and self-reported well-being, such as subjective happiness (SH). We also examined the confounding effects of SH and subjective mental and physical health conditions on the associa- tion between experiences of CP and/or NP and endorsement of CP. Those with poor SH or
  • 47. with bad mental health and experiences of CP and/or NP may endorse CP. However, consider- ing the traditional background of home discipline, even those with good SH might strongly endorse CP. We also hypothesized that students who experienced childhood CP and/or NP would report lower levels of subjective well-being regardless of their most recent physical/mental health status. A previous study has suggested that adolescents’ exposure to maternal negative affective behavior is associated with adolescents’ subjective daily well-being [22]. Happiness is a composite of life satisfaction, coping resources, and positive emotions [23]. Positive emotions are a powerful source of growth and change, predicting both individuals’ judgments about life and their skills for living well, including recovery from illness [24,25]. Additionally, studying nursery education may be a possible factor for the prevention of maltreatment, which is inversely associated with endorsing CP. Children exposed to school- based programs show improvements in protective behaviors and
  • 48. knowledge as well as increase disclosure of abuse [26]. A nursery educational course includes a variety of social sciences such as psychology, psychiatry, and welfare, including prevention or coping with child maltreatment. As such, the main purpose of this study was to examine the association between childhood experiences of parental CP and/or NP and endorsement of CP in relation to current subjective well-being in apparently healthy Japanese university students. Next, to explore whether the endorsement of CP can be changed by students’ appropriate recognition, we also examined whether university students who study nursery education are less likely to endorse CP as a suitable parenting practice. Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 2 / 16 Funding: This research was supported by an Osaka University, Graduate School of Human Sciences
  • 49. ‘Support Program for Improving Graduate School Education’ grant: ‘Promoting the Use of Human Science Data in General and Professional Education’. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 Material and methods The present study was an exploratory, cross-sectional investigation targeting undergraduate students at five universities in Western Japan. It was conducted from September 2007 to July 2008. The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University. Completing and returning the anony- mous questionnaires indicated participants’ tacit informed consent.
  • 50. Study participants Participants were recruited from first- and second-year undergraduate classes in nursery edu- cation, health nursing, social welfare, domestic science, nutrition, and psychosociology. Eligi- ble students were those studying in the Kansai area who could be contacted by the first author (NK), who was the principal investigator, in cooperation with co-investigators at multiple uni- versities. A total of 692 students from five universities were recruited. Valid responses were received from 536 respondents (104 men and 432 women, valid response rate: 77.5%). None of the study participants had any experience in caring for children. Definitions In the present study, the definition of CP reflected the definitions by the American Association of Pediatrics [27] and Straus and Donnelly [28]. We defined CP as the use of physical force, including spanking and hitting, for correcting or controlling a child’s behavior. We employed the definition of NP behaviors developed by Straus and Kantor [29]. NP
  • 51. behaviors are those that constitute a failure to act in ways presumed by a society’s culture to be necessary to meet a child’s developmental needs and that are the caregiver’s responsibility to provide. Measurements We constructed a five-part questionnaire, drawing on existing instruments for use in the general population. Our self-reported anonymous questionnaire (paper- based) covered demographic characteristics, experiences of CP and NP, recent physical and mental health conditions, endorse- ment of parental use of CP, and subjective well-being. Since no official Japanese version existed for a pre-existing instrument, we conducted a translation using cross-cultural translation method- ology and validation based on Acquadro et al. [30], and we checked the reliability of these transla- tions by confirmatory factor analyses and calculation of Cronbach’s α coefficients. Demographic data The first part of the questionnaire collected basic demographic data (i.e., gender, age, number of
  • 52. siblings, birth-order, mother’s and father’s ages, and years of education completed by parents) and basic information about the participant’s academic affiliation and major (i.e., university classes). Experience of parental corporal punishment To assess participants’ experience of parental CP, two items were translated into Japanese and administered: one asked whether respondents had been hit or spanked “a lot” before the age of 12, and the other asked whether they were hit “a lot” during their teenage years [31]. These items were chosen because they have been used internationally [31], facilitating comparisons between findings. Any response to these two items other than “strongly disagree” was highly suggestive of the respondent having experienced CP because of the inclusion of the word “a lot” [31]; even if Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 3 / 16 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243
  • 53. they disagreed with “a lot,” they might agree with “a little.” Thus, for either question, responses of “strongly agree,” “agree,” and “disagree” were considered suggestive of CP, and responses of “agree” and “strongly agree” were considered to indicate pervasive CP. To form the predictor measure in this study, responses to the two items concerning CP (strongly agree = 4, agree = 3, disagree = 2, and strongly disagree = 1) were summed to pro- duce the score representing the experience of CP (CP score). Experience of neglectful parenting To assess participants’ experience of NP, the eight-item revised version of the Multidimen- sional Neglectful Behavior Scale Adult Recall Short Form (revised MNBS-AS) was employed [32]: two items concerning physical neglect (e.g., my parents did not keep me clean), two items concerning cognitive neglect (e.g., my parents did not help me to do my best), two items concerning emotional neglect (e.g., my parents did not comfort me when I was upset), and two items concerning supervisory neglect (e.g., my parents did not care if I did things like
  • 54. shoplifting). The revised MNBS-AS was developed as a brief instrument for use in surveys tar- geting general populations [29,32,33]. As a predictor measure in the study, responses to the eight items of the revised MNBS-AS were rated (strongly agree = 4, agree = 3, disagree = 2, and strongly disagree = 1) and summed to produce the score representing the experience of NP (NP score). To distinguish between single and multiple experiences of NP, we followed Straus and Savage [33] by presenting sepa- rate data about the percentage of students who reported three or more experiences of NP, which was evidence of “pervasive NP.” Recent health condition This part of the questionnaire concerned respondents’ self- reported physical and mental health condition over the past month. The official Japanese version of the 8-item SF-8 Health Survey was administered (e.g., During the past four weeks, how much did physical health problems limit your physical activities [such as walking or climbing stairs]? and During the past four
  • 55. weeks, how much have you been bothered by emotional problems [such as feeling anxious, depressed, or irritable]?) [34]. Two indicators, the physical condition score (PCS) and mental condition score (MCS), were calculated as indicators of respondents’ recent health condition. Subjective happiness To capture each participant’s subjective well-being, we administered the official Japanese ver- sion of the Subjective Happiness Scale, a global SH seven-point Likert scale consisting of four items (e.g., Compared with most of my peers, I consider myself: less happy (1–7) more happy; Some people are generally very happy. They enjoy life regardless of what is going on, getting the most out of everything. To what extent does this characterization describe you?) [35,36]. In the present study, the SH score—the mean score of the four items, assessed on the seven-point Likert scale—was used as an outcome measure of subjective well-being. The degree of SH rises as the SH score increases. Endorsement of parental use of physical punishment on children
  • 56. as a form of discipline The Adult Adolescent Parenting Inventory-2 (AAPI-2) was developed to assess the parenting and child-rearing attitudes of adults and adolescents, regardless of experience in raising their own children [37]. We obtained permission to translate this instrument into Japanese, and its Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 4 / 16 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 translated contents were confirmed using back-translation. In the present study, our Japanese version of AAPI-2 Form B was administered. We modified the original five-point Likert scale to a four-point scale (strongly disagree, disagree, agree, and strongly agree) in this study to avoid Japanese people’s tendency to choose the “uncertain” response [38]. Following the 2005 edition of the AAPI Online Development Handbook [37], we performed a confirmatory factor analysis for each subscale of AAPI-2
  • 57. Form B in our sample. We identi- fied one factor (Cronbach’s α = 0.769) consisting of six items regarding the endorsement of the use of physical punishment (e.g., “Spanking children when they misbehave teaches them how to behave”). We called this factor the “Endorsement of Physical Punishment as a Form of Discipline Indicator” (EPP). The response to each EPP item was rated as follows: strongly agree = 4, agree = 3, disagree = 2, and strongly disagree = 1. Adding the response ratings from the six EPP items produces the EPP score, which was used as an outcome measure in this study. The degree of endorsement of physical punishment rises as the EPP score increases. Statistical methods First, descriptive analyses were performed to examine respondents’ demographic characteristics and the frequency distributions of responses to items concerning experience of CP and NP. To examine the differences between the two groups of responses, t- tests and chi-square tests or Fisher’s exact test were conducted for parametric and non- parametric variables, respectively. Next, using data of 346 participants (56 men and 290 women),
  • 58. we conducted multiple regression analyses to detect the independent contributions of the predictors of CP and NP scores to the EPP score as outcome variable, adjusting for participants’ basic demographic fac- tors, undergraduate class, recent health condition on SF-8 (PCS, MCS), and with/without the SH score. As preparation for the multivariate analysis, bivariate correlation analyses were per- formed to examine the associations among the independent variables, using list-wise case dele- tion. Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were also used as diagnostic for multicollinearity. Another multiple regression analysis was conducted using the SH score as an outcome variable to examine the independent effects of CP and NP scores on the SH score. For all statistical analyses, SPSS version 24.0 for Windows (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) were used. A two-tailed P-value of less than 0.05 was required for statistical significance. Results As shown in Table 1, there were no statistically significant gender differences in the sociode-
  • 59. mographic characteristics or present status of the 536 respondents, except for the variable “has studied health or welfare at university.” Experience of parental corporal punishment Table 2 shows the frequency distribution of the number and percentage of responses to the two items concerning experience of parental CP. Responses of “strongly agree,” “agree,” or “disagree” on either or both of the items were considered suggestive of CP, whereas responses of “agree” or “strongly agree” were considered to indicate pervasive CP. Overall, 59.5% of students (72.1% of men and 56.5% of women) provided responses sugges- tive of experience of CP, indicating that they were spanked or hit before the age of 12 by their parents. Regarding their teenage years, 46.8% of students (61.5% of men and 43.3% of women) gave responses suggestive of experience of parental CP. The percentage of respondents who gave answers suggestive of experience of pervasive CP was 22.8%. There was a significant gender difference, with 36.5% of men and 19.4% of women
  • 60. (chi-square = 13.932, df = 1, P < 0.001) agreeing or strongly agreeing with either or both items Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 5 / 16 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 concerning experience of CP. There was a strong association between experience of pervasive CP before the age of 12 and during the teenage years (chi - square = 146.504, df = 1, P < 0.001). Experiences of neglectful parenting behaviors Table 2 presents the frequency distribution of responses to each of the eight items used to assess experience of NP. Emotional and cognitive forms of NP were reported more frequently than were physical and supervisory forms. The percentage of respondents who gave answers suggestive of experience of pervasive NP was 12.2%. There was a significant gender difference, with 22.1% of men compared with 9.7% of women (chi-square = 12.081, df = 1, P = 0.001) reporting experience of pervasive NP.
  • 61. Gender differences in predictor and outcome measures The mean ± SD of the CP score among 346 respondents (56 men and 290 women) was 3.5 ± 1.6 (range: 2–8, median: 3, skewness: 0.886, kurtosis: 0.196); as shown in Table 1, there Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants (n = 536). Male (n = 104) Mean (SD) / n (%) Female (n = 432) Mean (SD) / n (%) P-value� Socio-demographic variables Age 19.4 (0.71) 19.5 (0.96) 0.693 Number of siblings † 2.5 (0.75) 2.5 (0.73) 0.785 Birth-order ‡ 1.7 (0.79) 1.8 (0.79) 0.670 Mother’s age at birth §
  • 62. 28.1 (4.1) 28.1 (3.9) 0.970 Age difference between mother and father (years) k -2.9 (4.2) -2.7 (3.5) 0.607 Mother’s years of education ¶ 13.2 (1.6) 13.3 (1.6) 0.746 Father’s years of education # 13.8 (2.1) 14.0 (2.0) 0.357 Undergraduate classes Nursery education 21 (20.2%) 127 (29.4%) 0.059 Health nursing 11 (10.6%) 123 (28.5%) <0.001 Social welfare 51 (49.0%) 49 (11.3%) <0.001 Domestic science 0 62 (14.4%) <0.001 Nutrition 15 (14.4%) 39 (9.0%) 0.101 Psychosociology 6 (5.8%) 32 (7.4%) 0.674 Recent health condition on SF-8 Physical condition score (PCS) 48.9 (7.4) 51.3 (6.4) 0.014
  • 63. Mental condition score (MCS) 45.9 (8.7) 43.4 (7.3) 0.025 Predictive and outcome measures�� Score representing experience of corporal punishment (CP score) 4.3 (1.6) 3.4 (1.5) <0.001 Score representing experience of neglectful parenting (NP score) 15.1 (2.8) 12.8 (3.3) <0.001 Subjective happiness (SH) score 4.6 (1.1) 4.7 (0.94) 0.836 Endorsement of parental use of physical punishment on children (EPP) score 13.1 (3.4) 12.7 (2.7) 0.413 �Continuous variable: t-test; Categorical variable: chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test †n = 530 (104 males and 426 females) ‡n = 521 (103 males and 418 females) §n = 462 (74 males and 388 females) kn = 437 (70 males and 367 females) ¶n = 476 (87 males and 389 females) #n = 461 (85 males and 376 females) ��n = 346 (56 males and 290 females). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t001 Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment
  • 64. PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 6 / 16 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t001 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 was a significant gender difference in the mean CP score (P < 0.001).The mean ± SD of the NP score among them was 13.1 ± 3.4 (range: 8–24, median: 13, skewness: 0.427, kurtosis: −- 0.215); as shown in Table 1, there was a significant gender difference in mean NP scores (P < 0.001). The mean ± SD of the SH score among them was 4.6 ± 0.97 (range: 2–7, median: 4.5, skewness: 0.083, kurtosis: −0.151); as shown in Table 1, there was no significant gender dif- ference in mean SH scores (P = 0.836).The mean ± SD of the EPP score among 346 respon- dents (56 men and 290 women) was 12.8 ± 2.8 (range: 6–21, median: 13, skewness: −0.028; kurtosis: 0.269); as shown in Table 1, there was no significant gender difference in mean EPP scores (P = 0.413). Associations between predictor and outcome variables As shown in Table 3, a few variables were significantly correlated with EPP or SH scores, but no multicollinearity was detected among the study variables by VIFs. Hence, we included some non-significant variables into the regression models based
  • 65. on the previous evidence. As shown in Table 4, by multiple regression analysis, the CP score had a significantly positive effect on the EPP score, whereas the NP score had no significant effect on the EPP score. The robustness of the standardized partial regression coefficient of the CP score was demonstrated by controlling for respondents’ basic demographic factors, the NP score, undergraduate clas- ses, and PCS and MCS (adjusted R 2 = 0.135). Subsequently, after the inclusion of the SH score in the model, the results did not change (ΔR2 = 0.001) and the CP score had a significantly pos- itive effect on the EPP score (β = 0.310, P < 0.001). Another independent variable with Table 2. Frequency distribution of responses to each question on experience of parental corporal punishment and neglectful parenting in childhood and adoles- cence (n = 536: 104 men and 432 women). Corporal punishment item Strongly disagree n (%) Disagree
  • 66. n (%) Agree n (%) Strongly agree n (%) When I was less than 12 years old, I was spanked or hit a lot by my mother or father. Men 29 (27.9) 40 (38.5) 29 (27.9) 6 (5.8) Women 188 (43.5) 165 (38.2) 57 (13.2) 22 (5.1) When I was a teenager, I was hit a lot by my mother or father. Men 40 (38.5) 46 (44.2) 14 (13.5) 4 (3.8) Women 245 (56.7) 151 (35.0) 28 (6.5) 8 (1.9) Revised MNBS-AS item (sub-category of neglect) Strongly disagree Disagree Agree Strongly agree My parents helped me when I had trouble understanding something. 1 Men 2 (1.9) 18 (17.3) 63 (60.6) 21 (20.2) (cognitively) Women 10 (2.3) 61 (14.1) 247 (57.2) 114 (26.4) My parents did not comfort me when I was upset. Men 15 (14.4) 56 (53.8) 27 (26.0) 6 (5.8) (emotionally) Women 121 (28.0) 211 (48.8) 80 (18.5) 20 (4.6)
  • 67. My parents helped me when I had problems. 1 Men 3 (2.9) 29 (27.9) 52 (50.0) 20 (19.2) (emotionally) Women 7 (1.6) 73 (16.9) 230 (53.2) 122 (28.2) My parents did not care if I did things like shoplifting. Men 60 (57.7) 30 (28.8) 11 (10.6) 3 (2.9) (supervisory) Women 320 (74.1) 104 (24.1) 7 (1.6) 1 (0.2) My parents did not help me to do my best. Men 24 (23.1) 56 (53.8) 17 (16.3) 7 (6.7) (cognitively) Women 220 (50.9) 188 (43.5) 20 (4.6) 4 (0.9) My parents gave me enough clothes to keep me warm. 1 Men 2 (1.9) 8 (7.7) 52 (50.0) 42 (40.4) (physically) Women 8 (1.9) 16 (3.7) 147 (34.0) 261 (60.4) My parents did not keep me clean. Men 46 (44.2) 47 (45.2) 7 (6.7) 4 (3.8) (physically) Women 297 (68.8) 120 (27.8) 9 (2.1) 6 (1.4) My parents did not care if I got into trouble in school. Men 51 (49.0) 42 (40.4) 10 (9.6) 1 (1.0) (supervisory) Women 284 (65.7) 131 (30.3) 11 (2.5) 6 (1.4) 1: reverse-scored item
  • 68. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t002 Child abuse and endorsement of corporal punishment PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 October 26, 2018 7 / 16 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243.t002 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206243 T a b le 3 . P e a rs o n ’s c o rr e la
  • 81. 8 5 � -0 .0 Parental Corporal Punishment Predicts Behavior Problems in Early Childhood Matthew K. Mulvaney and Carolyn J. Mebert University of New Hampshire Using data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (Research Triangle Institute, 2002), this study examined the impact of corporal punishment (CP) on children’s behavior problems. Longi- tudinal analyses were specified that controlled for covarying contextual and parenting variables and that partialed child effects. The results indicate that parental CP uniquely contributes to negative behavioral adjustment in children at both 36 months and at 1st grade, with the effects at the earlier age more pronounced in children with difficult temperaments. Parents and mental health professionals who work to modify children’s negative behavior should be aware of the unique impact that CP likely plays in triggering and maintaining children’s behavior problems. Broad-based family policies that reduce the use of this
  • 82. parenting behavior would potentially increase children’s mental health and decrease the incidence of children’s behavior problems. Keywords: corporal punishment, physical punishment, parental discipline, behavior problems Corporal punishment (CP) is defined as “the use of phys- ical force with the intention of causing a child pain, but not injury, for the purposes of correction or control of the child’s behavior” (Straus, 2001, p. 4). More than 90% of children and approximately 50% of adolescents during their adolescent years have experienced parental CP at least once (Straus & Stewart, 1999). This form of discipline is admin- istered frequently—an average of three times per week during the toddler years (Straus, 2001)—and it is used more often with male, African American, and poor children (Straus & Stewart, 1999). Thus, for many children, CP represents an important component of their socialization experiences within the family. However, because of limita- tions in most of the relevant research, relatively few defin- itive conclusions can be drawn about the effects of CP on child adjustment. Most available research indicates that there are few, if any, positive developmental outcomes associated with CP beyond immediate compliance with a parent’s directive (Gershoff, 2002). In fact, a growing body of research sug- gests there may be unintended negative consequences, in- cluding increasing children’s aggressive behavior and their likelihood of becoming delinquent and contributing to poorer psychological and cognitive functioning (e.g., Ger- shoff, 2002; Smith & Brooks-Gunn, 1997). The strongest link is between CP and externalizing behavior problems,
  • 83. especially aggression (e.g., Strassberg, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1994; Straus & Kantor, 1994). Internalizing prob- lems, such as depression and lower self-esteem, have also been linked to CP (e.g., Turner & Finkelhor, 1996). Al- though there are some contrary findings (e.g., Simons, John- son, & Conger, 1994), most available evidence indicates that CP represents a risk factor for the development of negative behavioral, psychological, and cognitive character - istics. Despite the many studies linking parental CP to negative developmental outcomes, there is continuing debate among social scientists and policymakers regarding the interpreta- tion of these studies and the overall conclusions that can be drawn about the unique, specific impact of CP (e.g., Baum- rind, 1996; Baumrind, Larzelere, & Cowan, 2002). A major factor underlying the controversy is that there are aspects of the research that limit interpretations, including the nonex- perimental, correlational methodology that must be em- ployed to examine outcomes. As is the case with most research concerning the effects of parental socialization practices, third variables and child effects may explain the reported associations between CP and child outcomes. Several parenting variables covary with the use of CP and therefore generally are not controlled in examinations of the relation between CP and child outcomes. Although CP is used within all parenting-style groups described by Baum- rind (1973), its use varies reliably between groups. Many authoritative parents use CP, but authoritarian parents use it much more frequently; permissive parents use it less fre- Matthew K. Mulvaney and Carolyn J. Mebert, Department of Psychology, University of New Hampshire. We thank Murray Straus, Toni Bisconti, and Glenda Kaufman-
  • 84. Kantor for their helpful contributions to this research. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Matthew K. Mulvaney, who is now at the Department of Psychology, State University of New York College at Brockport, 350 New Cam- pus Drive, Brockport, NY 14420. E-mail: [email protected] Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 21, No. 3, 389 –397 0893-3200/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.389 389 quently but more harshly (Parke & Collmer, 1975). The link between CP and negative developmental outcomes may thus be reflecting the influence of the broader parenting style rather than CP specifically. Supporting this notion, Simons et al. (1994) found that CP significantly predicted aggressiveness and delinquency but that the effects became nonsignificant after controlling for parental involvement. Similarly, Larzelere, Kleinn, Schumm, and Alibrando (1989) found that the amount of CP received in adolescence negatively predicted self-esteem but that the association became nonsignificant after controlling for the amount of positive communication in the parent– child relationship. However, not all research supports the notion that the ef- fects of CP are epiphenomenal to broader parenting styles. For instance, Buehler and Gerard (2002) reported that CP influenced global psychological functioning, even after con- trolling for parental involvement. The issue of whether CP influences development beyond the parenting context in which it is used is far from resolved, but it is evident that
  • 85. models examining the influence of CP must simultaneously consider the parenting style in which it occurs. An additional problem with the reliance on correlational methods is that it is difficult to identify the direction of the effects (e.g., Baumrind, 1997). Most studies assume a parent-to-child effect, although it is clear that within-child characteristics play an important role in evoking different kinds of parenting behaviors (Lengua & Kovacs, 2005; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Distinguishing the direction of effects between physical discipline and child outcomes is imperative. An effective way to deal with this issue is to employ developmental designs in which the outcome be- havior of interest is statistically controlled at the initial time point (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Born- stein, 2000). That is, the behavior of interest, along with the measure of parental behavior, is measured at the initial assessment. The prediction equation then controls for the initial level of the outcome variable so that the actual influence of parental behavior can be determined. Recent studies of CP have incorporated such designs and have provided more convincing evidence for a parent-to-child effect in antisocial and other problem behavior (e.g., Lengua & Kovacs, 2005; Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1997). A final issue is how best to conceptualize the impact of CP on children’s development. Given the array of outcomes associated with CP, a simple social learning model does not provide a sufficient explanation. Rather, as Turner and Finkelhor (1996) have argued, the effect of CP may best be understood from a stress-process framework. Receiving physical discipline from a parent is likely to be stressful for a child, and children who are continuously exposed to stressors in their environment (and perceive them as stress- ful) show increased internalizing and externalizing behav- iors (e.g., Cicchetti & Toth, 1991; Grant et al., 2003).
  • 86. Repetti, Taylor, and Seeman (2002) suggested that ongoing stress within the family environment will have long-term effects on mental health via changes in the biological self- regulatory systems of the child. Repetti et al. also describe, beyond physiological changes that increase susceptibility to mental health disorders, a transactional process in which family stressors lead to additional developmental problems, including decreased emotion processing and poorer peer relationships. These, in turn, contribute to poorer mental health. The effects may also be cumulative, with CP that extends throughout childhood being the most detrimental. Contextual variables may contribute to the stress re- sponse of children to their parents’ discipline and moderate the impact of that discipline. Supporting this notion is the work on parenting style mentioned above, as well as re- search on ethnic group differences in the effects of CP. Specifically, CP has been found to be associated with ag- gression in European American children but not in African American children, even though CP is used with greater frequency by African American parents (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997). This may reflect both a greater degree of cultural acceptance of CP within African American communities, including the view that it is a legitimate parental behavior, and differing interpretations of such punishment by African American and European American children. However, following a review of the relevant studies, Horn, Joseph, and Cheng (2004) cautioned that additional research, particularly longitudinal studies controlling for potentially confounding variables (e.g., so- cioeconomic status, child effects), is necessary before con- clusions can be drawn about ethnic or racial differences in the effects of CP. In the present study, the effects of parental CP on tod-
  • 87. dlers’ and young children’s behavior problems were exam- ined in the large, longitudinal, and diverse data set of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD; Research Triangle Institute, 2002). The primary focus of the analyses was to clarify the existence and direction of influence of parental behavior on develop- mental outcomes. It was hypothesized that CP would be associated with both broadband categories of problem be- haviors (externalizing and internalizing behaviors) at 36 months and first grade. The developmental analyses that were performed included controlling for outcome variables at an initial time point, which allowed for stronger claims to be made about the direction of effects (Collins et al., 2000). Because of the myriad parenting and contextual variables assessed, it was possible to control for a substantial number of factors implicated in the relation between CP and nega- tive child outcomes (e.g., parenting style, income, maternal depression, and ethnicity). A test of the moderating effect of intrapersonal and contextual variables on the impact of CP was also included. It was hypothesized that child tempera- ment and African American ethnicity would moderate the influence of CP on these developmental outcomes. Method Sample Data collection for the NICHD SECCYD (Research Tri- angle Institute, 2002), a multisite longitudinal study de- signed to examine the effects of child care on children’s development, began in 1991 and continues presently. The 390 MULVANEY AND MEBERT
  • 88. sample originally consisted of 705 male (51.7%) and 659 (48.3%) female children. There were 1,014 non-Hispanic Caucasian (74.34%), 176 African American (12.9%), 83 Hispanic (6.09%), and 93 otherwise classified (6.82%) chil- dren. The mean age of the mothers at their child’s birth was 28.11 years (SD ! 5.63). Twenty-four percent of the fam- ilies were classified as living in poverty, as indicated by an income-to-needs ratio of less than 1. For more detailed recruitment procedures of the NICHD SECCYD, see the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2001). At the first-grade assessment, 1,028 mothers (75.37% of the original sample) completed the outcome measure of interest in the present study (the Child Behavior Checklist [CBCL]; Achenbach, 1991). As is the case with most lon- gitudinal studies, participant attrition occurred in a nonran- dom fashion. Independent-sample t tests, with status in first grade as the between-subjects factor (still participating or not), were computed to assess group differences in maternal education and average income. Families that completed the first-grade outcome measure had a higher average income- to-needs ratio (M ! 3.55, SD ! 2.68) than did families that did not (M ! 2.67, SD ! 2.92), t(1353) ! 5.05, p " .001, d ! .27. Mothers in families participating at the first-grade level had more years of education (M ! 14.46, SD ! 2.45) than did those who were not (M ! 13.56, SD ! 2.57), t(1361) ! 5.76, p " .001, d ! .31. Ethnicity was also associated with subject attrition, #2(5, N ! 1,364) ! 12.69, p " .05, $ ! .10. There were more Caucasian families than African American families in the first-grade sample relative to the initial sample. Measures CP. The CP variable was derived from the Home Obser-
  • 89. vation for the Measurement of the Environment (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984), administered when the children were 15, 36, and 54 months of age. HOME assesses the overall quality of the family environment by both interview - ing the mother and observing the family in a naturalistic setting during an extensive observation process. Two items from HOME were used: (a) an interview with the mother, to determine whether the children had been spanked more than once in the previous week, and (b) the test administrators’ observation of whether the children were spanked in their presence. Scores could range from 0 to 2, depending on whether the answer to neither, one, or both of the items was a yes. Because this composite variable included both self- report and observations of the parenting behavior, the va- lidity of the measure should be better than either alone, although a two-item measure is not ideal with respect to reliability. Despite the potentially decreased reliability, re - searchers have been able to employ this measure, or very similar measures, to investigate the impact of CP on devel - opmental outcomes (McLoyd & Smith, 2002; Smith & Brooks-Gunn, 1997). Behavior problems. The CBCL (Achenbach, 1991, 1992) was used as an index of children’s behavior problems. The CBCL for Ages 2–3 (CBCL/2–3; Achenbach, 1992) was administered at 36 months, and the CBCL for Ages 4 –18 (CBCL/4 –18; Achenbach, 1991) was administered when the children were in first grade. Mothers completed the scale in their homes at 36 months and in the lab during the first-grade assessment. The broadband Externalizing and Internalizing scales were used at both ages. Although these scales assess conceptually similar constructs across the two ages, the overlap in the actual items is modest, reflecting age-related differences in behavioral characteristics. Stan- dardized T scores were used in the following analyses:
  • 90. Demographic characteristics and temperament. At the 1-month assessment, the mothers reported on their chil- dren’s ethnicity and sex. When the children were 6 months of age, the mothers completed the Activity, Adaptability, Approach, Mood, and Intensity subscales of the Infant Tem- perament Questionnaire—Revised (Carey & McDevitt, 1978). The scores of the nonmissing items for all subscales were combined into a single variable, called difficult child temperament, by averaging across the items. Higher scores represent a more “difficult” temperament. The measure has been found to have good test–retest reliability (Carey & McDevitt, 1978). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .81. Maternal depression. When the children were 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months of age, as well as during the first- grade assessment, the mothers completed the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Rad- loff, 1977), a well-known measure with good psychometric properties. Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was equal to or greater than .88 for each assessment. Income-to-needs. Income-to-needs information was ob- tained when children were 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months old, as well as when they were in first grade, via interviews with the parents. At each age, the income of the total household from all sources was divided by the cutoff point for poverty for that particular year, as based on the number of people in the household, with guidelines established by the U.S. Bureau of the Census (2004). Maternal sensitivity. Maternal sensitivity was assessed when children were 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months old, as well as when they were in first grade, with a semistructured observational procedure. This procedure, specifically de- signed for the NICHD SECCYD by D. L. Vandell and M. T.
  • 91. Owen, consists of observing a mother and her child playing with age-appropriate toys. For detailed information about the development and administration of the scale, see NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999). The 6- and 15-month measures were assessed in the family’s home. The 24-, 36-, and 54-month and first-grade measures were assessed in a laboratory. For the 6-, 15-, and 24-month assessments, a composite measure of sensitivity was created by summing global ratings of sensitivity to nondistress, intrusiveness (reverse scored), and positive regard. For mea - sures at 36 and 54 months, as well as in first grade, the sensitivity composite was constructed by summing support- ive presence, hostility (reverse-scored), and respect for au- tonomy. All interactions were videotaped and coded at a central location. At all ages, approximately 20% of the interactions were coded by a second coder and yielded the following interrater reliability coefficients: .87, .83, .84 .84, .79, and .84 for the 6-, 15-, 24-, 36-, and 54-month and 391CORPORAL PUNISHMENT AND BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS first-grade composite measures, respectively. Higher scores indicate more sensitive parenting. Scores on this measure are significantly related to attachment, suggesting moderate construct validity (NICHD Early Child Care Research Net- work, 1997). Results Data Analysis Plan To isolate the unique and directional influence of parental CP on developmental outcomes in infancy and early child- hood, we specified two sets of hierarchical multiple-
  • 92. regression models. The first set assessed the impact of CP in infancy and toddlerhood by examining the associations be- tween the 15-month CP variable and the 36-month Internal- izing and Externalizing scores. The second set of analyses examined the associations between internalizing and exter- nalizing behavior problems in first grade and a composite CP variable constructed from the 36- and 54-month assess- ments. Because of the temporal precedence of the CP vari - able in these analyses, its unique contribution to the predic- tion of the subsequently measured behaviors would strengthen a causal argument for the directional influence of CP on developmental outcomes. All analyses were carried out with the actual sample and with the sample weighted to account for the relation between ethnicity and attrition. These analyses yielded virtually identical results, so onl y the unweighted versions are presented. For each model, the child’s gender, ethnicity, and tem- perament and the aggregate variables maternal sensitivity, maternal depression, and family income-to-needs were in- cluded as control variables. The wide array of control vari- ables included in these analyses should decrease the likeli - hood of omitted-variables bias. The maternal depression, family income, and maternal sensitivity variables were ob- tained over the course of earlier assessments and col - lapsed into a single measure for each analysis by aver- aging across all the assessments. The internal consistency for each of these composite measures was acceptable (Cronbach’s % & .69). CP and Behavior Problems at 36 Months The correlation matrix and descriptive statistics for all variables in the first set of analyses are in Table 1. The CP variable was modestly associated with both of the outcome variables (36-month externalizing and internalizing behav-
  • 93. iors). The matrix also indicated substantial covariation be- tween the CP, control, and outcome variables. Maternal depression had the strongest association with the variables of interest, likely reflecting the fact that many of the child variables were measured through maternal report. Thus, controlling for depression is important for all analyses, not only because of the influence of maternal mental health on children’s development but also because mothers’ percep- tions of children’s behavior are influenced by their own mental health (e.g., Fergusson, Lynskey, Horwood, 1993; Mebert, 1991). For the regression analyses, the control variables were entered in the first step. In the second step, the CP variable was entered. A product of the standardized CP and difficult child temperament variables, which assessed the moderat- ing role of difficult child temperament, was also entered in the second step. The results of the analyses predicting both externalizing and internalizing behaviors at 36 months are in Table 2. This table contains the standardized and unstand- ardized regression coefficients, the standard errors, and the partial correlations for each model. For internalizing behav- iors, the first step was significant, F(6, 1131) ! 49.38, p " .001, adjusted R2 ! .20. Maternal depression and sensitivity and difficult child temperament were unique predictors. At the second step, CP, but not the interaction term, contributed significantly to the proportion of variance accounted for, F(2, 1129) ! 3.62, p " .05, 'R2 ! .005. All variables that had been associated with internalizing in the first step re- mained significant in the second step. In the analysis of externalizing behaviors, the first step was also significant, F(6, 1131) ! 39.83, p " .001, adjusted R2 ! .17. As shown in Table 2, both maternal depression and difficult child temperament were associated with in- creased externalizing behavior. In the second step, the CP
  • 94. variable and the interaction term were found to add signif- Table 1 Descriptives and Correlation Matrix for Variables Used in the 36-Month Analysis (N ! 1,138) Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1. 36-month externalizing behaviors 51.07 8.49 — .70*** (.04 .07** .24*** .38*** (.18*** (.20*** .20*** 2. 36-month internalizing behaviors 51.16 9.50 — .04 .10*** .26*** .41*** (.18*** (.23*** .16*** 3. Gender (female) 1.49 0.50 — .00 .04 .02 .04 .10*** (.11*** 4. Ethnicity (African American) 0.11 0.32 — .17*** .16*** (.27*** (.40*** .11*** 5. Difficult child temperament (0.01 0.99 — .23*** (.14*** (.18*** .08** 6. Average maternal depression 9.43 6.40 — (.29*** (.33*** .19*** 7. Average income-to-needs 3.47 2.74 — .44*** (.25*** 8. Average maternal sensitivity 0.02 0.74 — (.34*** 9. Corporal punishment (0.01 1.00 — Note. The average maternal depression, income-to-needs, and maternal sensitivity variables are aggregates of all assessments up to and including the 36-month assessment. The difficult child temperament, average maternal sensitivity, and corporal punishment variables are standardized (Z scores). ** p " .01. *** p " .001. 392 MULVANEY AND MEBERT
  • 95. icantly to the prediction equation, F(2, 1129) ! 8.87, p " .001, 'R2 ! .01. Figure 1 displays the interaction. In this figure, the externalizing scores at 36 months were regressed onto the 15-month CP raw scores separately for children with “easy” and “difficult” temperaments, as designated by a median split. CP was more strongly associated with ex- ternalizing behavior problems for children with more diffi - cult temperaments. CP and Behavior Problems in First Grade Correlations and descriptive statistics for the variables used in the first-grade analyses are in Table 3. CP was associated with all control and outcome variables used in these analyses. The 36-month broadband factors were strongly correlated with the same broadband factors at first grade. The first-grade regression models employed residualized change analysis in which the outcome variable of interest was statistically controlled at an earlier time (36 months). That is, mothers’ 36-month ratings of internalizing (or ex- ternalizing) behaviors were included as a control variable in predicting internalizing (or externalizing) behavior at first grade. Although this method of analysis is correlational and constrained by limitations in causal interpretation, it can provide strong evidence of a parent-to-child effect (e.g., Collins et al., 2000). The other control variables that were entered in the first step were the child’s gender, ethnicity, and temperament and the aggregate variables maternal sen- sitivity, maternal depression, and family income-to-needs. In the second step, CP and an interaction variable represent- ing the moderating impact of African American ethnicity on CP was tested. The CP variable used in these analyses was a standardized average of the 36- and 54-month measures. Two interaction variables were constructed and tested as
  • 96. moderators. The first interaction variable was constructed by multiplying the standardized 6-month difficult child tem- perament variable by the aggregate CP variable. The second interaction variable was constructed by multiplying the Af- rican American status variable by the aggregate CP variable. Only control variables contributed to the prediction of internalizing behaviors at first grade. Neither CP nor the moderator terms were significantly associated with change in internalizing behaviors. CP was, however, associated with increased externalizing behaviors from 36 months to first grade. These results are presented in Table 4. The first step was significant, F(7, 971) ! 77.68, p " .001, adjusted R2 ! .35. Not surpris- ingly, externalizing behavior at 36 months was strongly associated with externalizing behavior in first grade. The child’s gender predicted change in externalizing behavior, as did maternal depression and family income. In the second step, CP, but not the interaction terms, contributed signifi - cantly to the variance accounted for, F(3, 968) ! 2.93, p " .05, 'R2 ! .006. The control variables that had been sig- nificant in the first step remained significant with the inclu- sion of CP and the interaction variables. We also respecified this model to test an additional moderating hypothesis. A term representing the interaction between the 36-month externalizing behavior and the CP variables was entered in the second step. It was not significantly associated with first-grade externalizing behavior. Discussion The primary goal of this study was to address the con- cerns of researchers (e.g., Baumrind, 1996; Baumrind et al. 2002) who have argued that normative CP does not have
  • 97. detrimental effects on children’s adjustment. Although this study does not address all of the concerns of these research- Table 2 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting 36- Month Behavior Problems Variable Externalizing behaviors Internalizing behaviors B SE ) Sr B SE ) Sr Step 1 Gender (female) (0.71 0.46 (.04 0.61 0.51 .03 Ethnicity (African American) (1.19 0.81 (.04 (0.85 0.88 (.03 Difficult child temperament 1.36 0.24 .16*** 1.56 0.26 .17*** Average material depression 0.41 0.04 .31*** 0.51 0.04 .34*** Average income-to-needs (0.16 0.10 (.05 (0.09 0.10 (.03 Average maternal sensitivity (0.68 0.38 (.06 (1.12 0.41 (.09** Step 2 Gender (female) (0.57 0.46 (.03 (.03 0.70 0.51 .04 .04 Ethnicity (African American) (1.09 0.80 (.04 (.04 (0.79 0.88 (.03 (.02 Difficult child temperament 1.39 0.24 .16*** .16 1.56 0.26 .17*** .16 Average maternal depression 0.40 0.04 .30*** .27 0.50 0.04 .34*** .30 Average income-to-needs (0.13 0.10 (.04 (.04 (0.08 0.11 (.02 (.02 Average maternal sensitivity (0.28 0.39 (.03 (.02 (0.85 0.43 (.07* (.05 Corporal punishment 0.93 0.25 .11*** .10 0.59 0.27 .06* .06 Corporal Punishment * Difficult Child Temperament 0.47 0.24 .06* .05 0.43 0.26 .04 .04
  • 98. Note. The difficult child temperament and corporal punishment variables are standardized. The gender and ethnicity variables are dichotomous variables. The average maternal depression, income-to-needs and maternal sensitivity variables are aggregated from all assessments prior to and inclusive of the 36-month assessment. * p " .05. ** p " .01. *** p " .001. 393CORPORAL PUNISHMENT AND BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS ers and certainly does not conclusively establish a causal link between CP and child adjustment, the results extend and complement the growing body of literature suggesting that there is a unique negative impact of CP on children’s behavior problems. CP was associated with increased inter - nalizing behaviors during toddlerhood and with increased externalizing behavior problems both in toddlerhood and at first grade. Although this research was correlational, several features of the analyses strengthen a causal argument re- garding the impact of CP on behavioral outcomes. First, the presumed causal variable (i.e., CP) was assessed temporally prior to the outcome … 10.1177/0192513X03258313 ARTICLEJOURNAL OF FAMILY ISSUES / September 2004Turner, Muller / LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF CORPORAL PUNISHMENT Long-Term Effects of Child Corporal Punishment on Depressive Symptoms
  • 99. in Young Adults Potential Moderators and Mediators HEATHER A. TURNER PAUL A. MULLER University of New Hampshire Based on a sample of 649 students from 3 New England colleges, this study examined the long-term effects of childhood corporal punishment on symptoms of depression and consid- ered factors that may moderate or mediate the association. Similar to national studies, ap- proximately 40% of the sample reported experiencing some level of corporal punishment when they were 13 years old. Findings indicated that level of corporal punishment is posi- tively related to depressive symptoms, independent of any history of abuse and the frequency of other forms of punishment. Although parental monitoring and perceived norms regarding corporal punishment had no direct or moderating effects, level of parental anger during cor- poral punishment was the strongest predictor of depression. The association between “angry corporal punishment” and symptoms of depression in young adulthood is partially mediated by mastery and self-esteem. Implications of these finding are discussed. Keywords: corporal punishment; depression; parental anger; self-concept It is widely accepted, and there is much empirical evidence, that