SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 44
ENG-106 Rubric: Cause and Effect Argument
Criteria
% Value
1: Unsatisfactory
2: Less Than Satisfactory
3: Satisfactory
4: Good
5: Excellent
% Scaling
0%
65%
75%
85%
100%
Content & Ideas – 40%
Cause and Effect Content and Ideas Should:
Include an effective title.
Present a thesis that centers specifically on a problem of cause
and effect.
Use cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning to support
the cause and effect argument established in the thesis.
Cite evidence that supports the cause and effect chains and
inductive reasoning.
40%
Does not have title, and has missing or indiscernible thesis
statement and minimal evidence to support main ideas.
Argument includes elements of cause/effect, but the argument
does not center on causes or consequences. The student does not
use strategies that support cause and effect arguments.
Title may not suggest subject and does not spark interest. Thesis
statement and/or the controlling idea are not clearly stated.
Argument includes elements of cause/effect, but the argument
does not center on causes or consequences and/or the writer
does not use strategies that support cause and effect arguments
very well. Ideas are underdeveloped and clichéd. They do not
support the thesis. Evidence from outside sources can be
irrelevant.
Title suggests subject but does not spark interest. Thesis
statement identifies the main point the author is trying to make.
Most of content relates to thesis statement, but lacks sufficient
support through appropriate strategies. Argument may not
center specifically on cause/effect or use strategies that support
cause and effect arguments effectively. Cited evidence
sometimes does not justify ideas.
Title suggests subject but does not necessarily spark interest.
Thesis statement clearly identifies the main point the author is
trying to make. Argument centers specifically on cause/effect
and uses strategies that support cause and effect arguments
(cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning). Most of the
content supports thesis, and cited evidence usually justifies
ideas.
Title suggests subject and sparks interest. With a clear,
controlling idea, thesis statement effectively identifies the main
point the student is trying to make. Content supports thesis
well. Argument centers specifically on cause/effect and
effectively uses strategies that support cause and effect
arguments (cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning).
Specific, cited evidence justifies ideas and enriches the essay.
Organization – 12%
Organization
12%
No apparent organization present. Ineffective introduction does
not invite readers or explain the subject. The reader cannot find
the thesis statement. Underdeveloped paragraphs lack focus and
topic sentences. No conclusion present.
No apparent organization present. Introduction explains subject,
but does not engage readers. Thesis is difficult to find.
Underdeveloped paragraphs lack focus and topic sentences.
Weak conclusion offered.
Organization is clear, but with minor errors. Introduction
explains subject, but does not adequately engage readers. Thesis
may be misplaced. Paragraphs are not developed around topic
sentences, and may not always advance essay's ideas.
Conclusion summarizes but does not conclude.
Organization aids readers in understanding content. Introduction
explains subject, but may not engage readers. Thesis statement
is placed appropriately, according to the genre of the writing set
forth in the assignment description in the syllabus. Well-ordered
paragraphs are developed around topic sentences, and advance
essay's ideas. Conclusion may be more of a summary.
Logically organized to lead readers to understanding content.
Introduction explains subject and engages readers. Thesis
statement is placed appropriately, according to the genre set
forth in the assignment description in the syllabus. Well-ordered
paragraphs are developed around topic sentences, and advance
essay's ideas. Conclusion provides strong, satisfying ending, not
a mere summary of the essay.
Format – 16%
Paper Format
16%
Layout: Essay lacks more than THREE of the following:
double-spaced, 12 pt, Times New Roman font, 1inch margins,
heading (with name, course, date, and instructor), assignment
title, and page numbers using appropriate header function. Not
all information, paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are
cited on the page they appear; little or no in-text citations
and/or entries on reference page used; major documentation
oversights noted; major format errors and omissions noted;
inappropriate number of required sources used.
Layout: Essay lacks THREE of the following: double-spaced, 12
pt. Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name,
course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers
using appropriate header function. Not all information,
paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the
page they appear; missing more than one citation and/or
reference entry; significant documentation oversights noted;
significant format errors or omissions noted; inappropriate
number of required sources used.
Layout: Essay lacks TWO of the following: double-spaced, 12
pt. Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name,
course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers
using appropriate header function. Not all information,
paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the
page they appear; missing one in-text citation and/or reference
entry; minor documentation oversights noted; minor formatting
errors or omissions noted; appropriate number of required
sources are used.
Essay lacks ONE of the following: double-spaced, 12 pt. Times
New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course,
date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers using
appropriate header function. All information, paraphrases,
quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the page that they
appear and are listed on the references page (GCU format);
some minor errors or omissions in format noted; appropriate
number of required sources is used.
Layout: Essay is double-spaced with 12 pt. Times New Roman
font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course, date, and
instructor), assignment title, and page numbers using
appropriate header function. All information, paraphrases,
quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited in parenthetical GCU
format; all sources are listed on the references page (GCU
format); all citations and reference entries are complete and in
alphabetical order; appropriate number of required sources is
used.
Language & Style – 16%
Language & Style
16%
Voice & tone are inappropriate and ineffective in creating
appropriate mood. Inappropriate word choice used. Sentence
structure includes ungrammatical structures and no variety.
Writing is wordy.
Voice & tone are inappropriate and ineffective in creating
appropriate mood. Word choice fails in use of appropriate,
precise language and strong verbs. Includes too many to be
verbs. No attempt to vary sentence structure noted. Writing is
wordy.
Voice & tone usually do not characterize ideas appropriately or
effectively create appropriate mood. Word choice includes
nonstandard outdated usage, too many to be verbs, is not
precise, and is occasionally incorrect. Some slang or jargon
exists in the paper. Inadequate variety in sentence structure
noted. Writing is wordy.
Voice & tone usually characterize ideas effectively create
appropriate mood. Word choice usually includes current
standard usage, active verbs, concrete nouns, and precise words.
Some slang or jargon exists in the paper. Some variety of
sentence structures strengthens the ideas, creates vitality, and
avoids choppiness in the writing. Writing is mostly concisely
written.
Voice & tone characterize ideas and effectively create
appropriate mood. Word choice includes current standard usage,
active verbs, concrete nouns, and precise words. Sentence
structures strengthen the ideas, create vitality, and avoid
choppiness in the writing. Writing is concise.
Grammar & Mechanics – 16%
Grammar & Mechanics
16%
Demonstrates no control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation
conventions.
Many errors, such as:
Apostrophe use
Capitalization
Commas misplaced or missing
Parallelism
Faulty point of view shifts
Pronoun agreement
Quotation errors
Semicolons misused
Run-ons & fragments
Spelling errors
Subject-verb agreement
Tense shifts
Demonstrates minimal control of grammar, spelling, &
punctuation conventions. Several errors, such as:
Apostrophe use
Capitalization
Commas misplaced or missing
Parallelism
Faulty point of view shifts
Pronoun agreement
Quotation errors
Semicolons misused
Run-ons & fragments
Spelling errors
Subject-verb agreement
Tense shifts
Demonstrates reasonable control of grammar, spelling, &
punctuation conventions.
Some errors, such as:
Apostrophe use
Capitalization
Commas misplaced or missing
Parallelism
Faulty point of view shifts
Pronoun agreement
Quotation errors
Semicolons misused
Run-ons & fragments
Spelling errors
Subject-verb agreement
Tense shifts
Demonstrates high control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation
conventions. Few errors, such as:
Apostrophe use
Capitalization
Commas misplaced or missing
Parallelism
Faulty point of view shifts
Pronoun agreement
Quotation errors
Semicolons misused
Run-ons & fragments
Spelling errors
Subject-verb agreement
Tense shifts
Demonstrates outstanding control of grammar, spelling, &
punctuation conventions.
No errors, such as:
Apostrophe use
Capitalization
Commas misplaced or missing
Parallelism
Faulty point of view shifts
Pronoun agreement
Quotation errors
Semicolons misused
Run-ons & fragments
Spelling errors
Subject-verb agreement
Tense shifts
PAGE
© 2015. Grand Canyon University. All Rights Reserved.
Introduction
Every author, I suppose, has in mind a setting in which readers
of his or her work could benefit
from having read it. Mine is the proverbial office watercooler,
where opinions are shared and
gossip is exchanged. I hope to enrich the vocabulary that people
use when they talk about the
judgments and choices of others, the company’s new policies, or
a colleague’s investment
decisions. Why be concerned with gossip? Because it is much
easier, as well as far more
enjoyable, to identify and label the mistakes of others than to
recognize our own. Questioning what
we believe and want is difficult at the best of times, and
especially difficult when we most need to
do it, but we can benefit from the informed opinions of others.
Many of us spontaneously anticipate
how friends and colleagues will evaluate our choices; the
quality and content of these anticipated
judgments therefore matters. The expectation of intelligent
gossip is a powerful motive for serious
self-criticism, more powerful than New Year resolutions to
improve one’s decision making at
work and at home.
To be a good diagnostician, a physician needs to acquire a large
set of labels for diseases, each
of which binds an idea of the illness and its symptoms, possible
antecedents and causes, possible
developments and consequences, and possible interventions to
cure or mitigate the illness.
Learning medicine consists in part of learning the language of
medicine. A deeper understanding of
judgments and choices also requires a richer vocabulary than is
available in everyday language.
The hope for informed gossip is that there are distinctive
patterns in the errors people make.
Systematic errors are known as biases, and they recur
predictably in particular circumstances.
When the handsome and confident speaker bounds onto the
stage, for example, you can anticipate
that the audience will judge his comments more favorably than
he deserves. The availability of a
diagnostic label for this bias—the halo effect—makes it easier
to anticipate, recognize, and
understand.
When you are asked what you are thinking about, you can
normally answer. You believe you
know what goes on in your mind, which often consists of one
conscious thought leading in an
orderly way to another. But that is not the only way the mind
works, nor indeed is that the typical
way. Most impressions and thoughts arise in your conscious
experience without your knowing how
they got there. You cannot tracryd>e how you came to the belief
that there is a lamp on the desk in
front of you, or how you detected a hint of irritation in your
spouse’s voice on the telephone, or
how you managed to avoid a threat on the road before you
became consciously aware of it. The
mental work that produces impressions, intuitions, and many
decisions goes on in silence in our
mind.
Much of the discussion in this book is about biases of intuition.
However, the focus on error
does not denigrate human intelligence, any more than the
attention to diseases in medical texts
denies good health. Most of us are healthy most of the time, and
most of our judgments and actions
are appropriate most of the time. As we navigate our lives, we
normally allow ourselves to be
guided by impressions and feelings, and the confidence we have
in our intuitive beliefs and
preferences is usually justified. But not always. We are often
confident even when we are wrong,
and an objective observer is more likely to detect our errors
than we are.
So this is my aim for watercooler conversations: improve the
ability to identify and understand
errors of judgment and choice, in others and eventually in
ourselves, by providing a richer
and more precise language to discuss them. In at least some
cases, an accurate diagnosis may
suggest an intervention to limit the damage that bad judgments
and choices often cause.
7
Two Systems
The Characters of the Story
To observe your mind in automatic mode, glance at the image
below.
Figure 1
Your experience as you look at the woman’s face seamlessly
combines what we normally call
seeing and intuitive thinking. As surely and quickly as you saw
that the young woman’s hair is
dark, you knew she is angry. Furthermore, what you saw
extended into the future. You sensed that
this woman is about to say some very unkind words, probably in
a loud and strident voice. A
premonition of what she was going to do next came to mind
automatically and effortlessly. You did
not intend to assess her mood or to anticipate what she might
do, and your reaction to the picture
did not have the feel of something you did. It just happened to
you. It was an instance of fast
thinking.
Now look at the following problem:
17 × 24
You knew immediately that this is a multiplication problem, and
probably knew that you could
solve it, with paper and pencil, if not without. You also had
some vague intuitive knowledge of the
range of possible results. You would be quick to recognize that
both 12,609 and 123 are
implausible. Without spending some time on the problem,
however, you would not be certain that
the answer is not 568. A precise solution did not come to mind,
and you felt that you could choose
whether or not to engage in the computation. If you have not
done so yet, you should attempt the
multiplication problem now, completing at least part of it.
You experienced slow thinking as you proceeded through a
sequence of steps. You first
retrieved from memory the cognitive program for multiplication
that you learned in school, then
you implemented it. Carrying out the computation was a strain.
You felt the burden of holding much
material in memory, as you needed to keep track of where you
were and of where you were going,
while holding on to the intermediate result. The process was
mental work: deliberate, effortful,
and orderly—a prototype of slow thinking. The computation
was not only an event in your mind;
your body was also involved. Your muscles tensed up, your
blood pressure rose, and your heart
17
rate increased. Someone looking closely at your eyes while you
tackled this problem would have
seen your pupils dilate. Your pupils contracted back to normal
size as soon as you ended your
work—when you found the answer (which is 408, by the way)
or when you gave up.
Two Systems
Psychologists have been intensely interested for several decades
in the two modagee fi Pn="cees
of thinking evoked by the picture of the angry woman and by
the multiplication problem, and have
offered many labels for them. I adopt terms originally proposed
by the psychologists Keith
Stanovich and Richard West, and will refer to two systems in
the mind, System 1 and System 2.
System 1 operates automatically and quickly, with little or no
effort and no sense of voluntary
control.
System 2 allocates attention to the effortful mental activities
that demand it, including
complex computations. The operations of System 2 are often
associated with the subjective
experience of agency, choice, and concentration.
The labels of System 1 and System 2 are widely used in
psychology, but I go further than most in
this book, which you can read as a psychodrama with two
characters.
When we think of ourselves, we identify with System 2, the
conscious, reasoning self that has
beliefs, makes choices, and decides what to think about and
what to do. Although System 2
believes itself to be where the action is, the automatic System 1
is the hero of the book. I describe
System 1 as effortlessly originating impressions and feelings
that are the main sources of the
explicit beliefs and deliberate choices of System 2. The
automatic operations of System 1 generate
surprisingly complex patterns of ideas, but only the slower
System 2 can construct thoughts in an
orderly series of steps. I also describe circumstances in which
System 2 takes over, overruling the
freewheeling impulses and associations of System 1. You will
be invited to think of the two
systems as agents with their individual abilities, limitations, and
functions.
In rough order of complexity, here are some examples of the
automatic activities that are
attributed to System 1:
Detect that one object is more distant than another.
Orient to the source of a sudden sound.
Complete the phrase “bread and…”
Make a “disgust face” when shown a horrible picture.
Detect hostility in a voice.
Answer to 2 + 2 = ?
Read words on large billboards.
Drive a car on an empty road.
Find a strong move in chess (if you are a chess master).
Understand simple sentences.
Recognize that a “meek and tidy soul with a passion for detail”
resembles an occupational
18
stereotype.
All these mental events belong with the angry woman—they
occur automatically and require little
or no effort. The capabilities of System 1 include innate skills
that we share with other animals.
We are born prepared to perceive the world around us,
recognize objects, orient attention, avoid
losses, and fear spiders. Other mental activities become fast and
automatic through prolonged
practice. System 1 has learned associations between ideas (the
capital of France?); it has also
learned skills such as reading and understanding nuances of
social situations. Some skills, such as
finding strong chess moves, are acquired only by specialized
experts. Others are widely shared.
Detecting the similarity of a personality sketch to an
occupatiohein occupatnal stereotype requires
broad knowledge of the language and the culture, which most of
us possess. The knowledge is
stored in memory and accessed without intention and without
effort.
Several of the mental actions in the list are completely
involuntary. You cannot refrain from
understanding simple sentences in your own language or from
orienting to a loud unexpected
sound, nor can you prevent yourself from knowing that 2 + 2 =
4 or from thinking of Paris when the
capital of France is mentioned. Other activities, such as
chewing, are susceptible to voluntary
control but normally run on automatic pilot. The control of
attention is shared by the two systems.
Orienting to a loud sound is normally an involuntary operation
of System 1, which immediately
mobilizes the voluntary attention of System 2. You may be able
to resist turning toward the source
of a loud and offensive comment at a crowded party, but even if
your head does not move, your
attention is initially directed to it, at least for a while. However,
attention can be moved away from
an unwanted focus, primarily by focusing intently on another
target.
The highly diverse operations of System 2 have one feature in
common: they require attention
and are disrupted when attention is drawn away. Here are some
examples:
Brace for the starter gun in a race.
Focus attention on the clowns in the circus.
Focus on the voice of a particular person in a crowded and
noisy room.
Look for a woman with white hair.
Search memory to identify a surprising sound.
Maintain a faster walking speed than is natural for you.
Monitor the appropriateness of your behavior in a social
situation.
Count the occurrences of the letter a in a page of text.
Tell someone your phone number.
Park in a narrow space (for most people except garage
attendants).
Compare two washing machines for overall value.
Fill out a tax form.
Check the validity of a complex logical argument.
In all these situations you must pay attention, and you will
perform less well, or not at all, if you
are not ready or if your attention is directed inappropriately.
System 2 has some ability to change
the way System 1 works, by programming the normally
automatic functions of attention and
memory. When waiting for a relative at a busy train station, for
example, you can set yourself at
19
will to look for a white-haired woman or a bearded man, and
thereby increase the likelihood of
detecting your relative from a distance. You can set your
memory to search for capital cities that
start with N or for French existentialist novels. And when you
rent a car at London’s Heathrow
Airport, the attendant will probably remind you that “we drive
on the left side of the road over
here.” In all these cases, you are asked to do something that
does not come naturally, and you will
find that the consistent maintenance of a set requires continuous
exertion of at least some effort.
The often-used phrase “pay attention” is apt: you dispose of a
limited budget of attention that
you can allocate to activities, and if you try to i>Cyou try tgo
beyond your budget, you will fail. It
is the mark of effortful activities that they interfere with each
other, which is why it is difficult or
impossible to conduct several at once. You could not compute
the product of 17 × 24 while making
a left turn into dense traffic, and you certainly should not try.
You can do several things at once, but
only if they are easy and undemanding. You are probably safe
carrying on a conversation with a
passenger while driving on an empty highway, and many parents
have discovered, perhaps with
some guilt, that they can read a story to a child while thinking
of something else.
Everyone has some awareness of the limited capacity of
attention, and our social behavior
makes allowances for these limitations. When the driver of a car
is overtaking a truck on a narrow
road, for example, adult passengers quite sensibly stop talking.
They know that distracting the
driver is not a good idea, and they also suspect that he is
temporarily deaf and will not hear what
they say.
Intense focusing on a task can make people effectively blind,
even to stimuli that normally attract
attention. The most dramatic demonstration was offered by
Christopher Chabris and Daniel
Simons in their book The Invisible Gorilla. They constructed a
short film of two teams passing
basketballs, one team wearing white shirts, the other wearing
black. The viewers of the film are
instructed to count the number of passes made by the white
team, ignoring the black players. This
task is difficult and completely absorbing. Halfway through the
video, a woman wearing a gorilla
suit appears, crosses the court, thumps her chest, and moves on.
The gorilla is in view for 9
seconds. Many thousands of people have seen the video, and
about half of them do not notice
anything unusual. It is the counting task—and especially the
instruction to ignore one of the teams
—that causes the blindness. No one who watches the video
without that task would miss the
gorilla. Seeing and orienting are automatic functions of System
1, but they depend on the allocation
of some attention to the relevant stimulus. The authors note that
the most remarkable observation of
their study is that people find its results very surprising. Indeed,
the viewers who fail to see the
gorilla are initially sure that it was not there—they cannot
imagine missing such a striking event.
The gorilla study illustrates two important facts about our
minds: we can be blind to the obvious,
and we are also blind to our blindness.
Plot Synopsis
The interaction of the two systems is a recurrent theme of the
book, and a brief synopsis of the plot
is in order. In the story I will tell, Systems 1 and 2 are both
active whenever we are awake. System
1 runs automatically and System 2 is normally in a comfortable
low-effort mode, in which only a
fraction of its capacity is engaged. System 1 continuously
generates suggestions for System 2:
impressions, intuitions, intentions, and feelings. If endorsed by
System 2, impressions and
intuitions turn into beliefs, and impulses turn into voluntary
actions. When all goes smoothly, which
is most of the time, System 2 adopts the suggestions of System
1 with little or no modification. You
generally believe your impressions and act on your desires, and
that is fine—usually.
When System 1 runs into difficulty, it calls on System 2 to
support more detailed and specific
20
processing that may solve the problem of the moment. System 2
is mobilized when a question
arises for which System 1 does not offer an answer, as probably
happened to you when you
encountered the multiplication problem 17 × 24. You can also
feel a surge of conscious attention
whenever you are surprised. System 2 is activ">< 2 is actated
when an event is detected that
violates the model of the world that System 1 maintains. In that
world, lamps do not jump, cats do
not bark, and gorillas do not cross basketball courts. The gorilla
experiment demonstrates that
some attention is needed for the surprising stimulus to be
detected. Surprise then activates and
orients your attention: you will stare, and you will search your
memory for a story that makes sense
of the surprising event. System 2 is also credited with the
continuous monitoring of your own
behavior—the control that keeps you polite when you are angry,
and alert when you are driving at
night. System 2 is mobilized to increased effort when it detects
an error about to be made.
Remember a time when you almost blurted out an offensive
remark and note how hard you worked
to restore control. In summary, most of what you (your System
2) think and do originates in your
System 1, but System 2 takes over when things get difficult, and
it normally has the last word.
The division of labor between System 1 and System 2 is highly
efficient: it minimizes effort and
optimizes performance. The arrangement works well most of the
time because System 1 is
generally very good at what it does: its models of familiar
situations are accurate, its short-term
predictions are usually accurate as well, and its initial reactions
to challenges are swift and
generally appropriate. System 1 has biases, however, systematic
errors that it is prone to make in
specified circumstances. As we shall see, it sometimes answers
easier questions than the one it
was asked, and it has little understanding of logic and statistics.
One further limitation of System 1
is that it cannot be turned off. If you are shown a word on the
screen in a language you know, you
will read it—unless your attention is totally focused elsewhere.
Conflict
Figure 2 is a variant of a classic experiment that produces a
conflict between the two systems. You
should try the exercise before reading on.
Figure 2
21
You were almost certainly successful in saying the correct
words in both tasks, and you surely
discovered that some parts of each task were much easier than
others. When you identified upper-
and lowercase, the left-hand column was easy and the right-
hand column caused you to slow down
and perhaps to stammer or stumble. When you named the
position of words, the left-hand column
was difficult and the right-hand column was much easier.
These tasks engage System 2, because saying “upper/lower” or
“right/left” is not what you
routinely do when looking down a column of words. One of the
things you did to set yourself for
the task was to program your memory so that the relevant words
(upper and lower for the first
task) were “on the tip of your tongue.” The prioritizing of the
chosen words is effective and the
mild temptation to read other words was fairly easy to resist
when you went through the first
column. But the second column was different, because it
contained words for which you were set,
and you could not ignore them. You were mostly able to
respond correctly, but overcoming the
competing response was a strain, and it slowed you down. You
experienced a conflict between a
task that you intended to carry out and an automatic response
that interfered with it.
Conflict between an automatic reaction and an intention to
conWhetion to ctrol it is common in
our lives. We are all familiar with the experience of trying not
to stare at the oddly dressed couple
at the neighboring table in a restaurant. We also know what it is
like to force our attention on a
boring book, when we constantly find ourselves returning to the
point at which the reading lost its
meaning. Where winters are hard, many drivers have memories
of their car skidding out of control
on the ice and of the struggle to follow well-rehearsed
instructions that negate what they would
naturally do: “Steer into the skid, and whatever you do, do not
touch the brakes!” And every human
being has had the experience of not telling someone to go to
hell. One of the tasks of System 2 is to
overcome the impulses of System 1. In other words, System 2 is
in charge of self-control.
Illusions
To appreciate the autonomy of System 1, as well as the
distinction between impressions and
beliefs, take a good look at figure 3.
This picture is unremarkable: two horizontal lines of different
lengths, with fins appended,
pointing in different directions. The bottom line is obviously
longer than the one above it. That is
what we all see, and we naturally believe what we see. If you
have already encountered this
image, however, you recognize it as the famous Müller-Lyer
illusion. As you can easily confirm by
measuring them with a ruler, the horizontal lines are in fact
identical in length.
Figure 3
22
Now that you have measured the lines, you—your System 2, the
conscious being you call “I”—
have a new belief: you know that the lines are equally long. If
asked about their length, you will
say what you know. But you still see the bottom line as longer.
You have chosen to believe the
measurement, but you cannot prevent System 1 from doing its
thing; you cannot decide to see the
lines as equal, although you know they are. To resist the
illusion, there is only one thing you can
do: you must learn to mistrust your impressions of the length of
lines when fins are attached to
them. To implement that rule, you must be able to recognize the
illusory pattern and recall what you
know about it. If you can do this, you will never again be fooled
by the Müller-Lyer illusion. But
you will still see one line as longer than the other.
Not all illusions are visual. There are illusions of thought,
which we call cognitive illusions.
As a graduate student, I attended some courses on the art and
science of psychotherapy. During one
of these lectures, our teacher imparted a morsel of clinical
wisdom. This is what he told us: “You
will from time to time meet a patient who shares a disturbing
tale of multiple mistakes in his
previous treatment. He has been seen by several clinicians, and
all failed him. The patient can
lucidly describe how his therapists misunderstood him, but he
has quickly perceived that you are
different. You share the same feeling, are convinced that you
understand him, and will be able to
help.” At this point my teacher raised his voice as he said, “Do
not even think of taking on this
patient! Throw him out of the office! He is most likely a
psychopath and you will not be able to
help him.”
Many years later I learned that the teacher had warned us
against psychopathic charm, and the
leading authority in the strn y in the udy of psychopathy
confirmed that the teacher’s advice was
sound. The analogy to the Müller-Lyer illusion is close. What
we were being taught was not how
to feel about that patient. Our teacher took it for granted that
the sympathy we would feel for the
patient would not be under our control; it would arise from
System 1. Furthermore, we were not
being taught to be generally suspicious of our feelings about
patients. We were told that a strong
attraction to a patient with a repeated history of failed treatment
is a danger sign—like the fins on
the parallel lines. It is an illusion—a cognitive illusion—and I
(System 2) was taught how to
recognize it and advised not to believe it or act on it.
The question that is most often asked about cognitive illusions
is whether they can be overcome.
The message of these examples is not encouraging. Because
System 1 operates automatically and
cannot be turned off at will, errors of intuitive thought are often
difficult to prevent. Biases cannot
always be avoided, because System 2 may have no clue to the
error. Even when cues to likely
errors are available, errors can be prevented only by the
enhanced monitoring and effortful activity
of System 2. As a way to live your life, however, continuous
vigilance is not necessarily good, and
it is certainly impractical. Constantly questioning our own
thinking would be impossibly tedious,
and System 2 is much too slow and inefficient to serve as a
substitute for System 1 in making
routine decisions. The best we can do is a compromise: learn to
recognize situations in which
mistakes are likely and try harder to avoid significant mistakes
when the stakes are high. The
premise of this book is that it is easier to recognize other
people’s mistakes than our own.
Useful Fictions
You have been invited to think of the two systems as agents
within the mind, with their individual
personalities, abilities, and limitations. I will often use
sentences in which the systems are the
subjects, such as, “System 2 calculates products.”
The use of such language is considered a sin in the professional
circles in which I travel,
because it seems to explain the thoughts and actions of a person
by the thoughts and actions of little
23
people inside the person’s head. Grammatically the sentence
about System 2 is similar to “The
butler steals the petty cash.” My colleagues would point out that
the butler’s action actually
explains the disappearance of the cash, and they rightly
question whether the sentence about
System 2 explains how products are calculated. My answer is
that the brief active sentence that
attributes calculation to System 2 is intended as a description,
not an explanation. It is meaningful
only because of what you already know about System 2. It is
shorthand for the following: “Mental
arithmetic is a voluntary activity that requires effort, should not
be performed while making a left
turn, and is associated with dilated pupils and an accelerated
heart rate.”
Similarly, the statement that “highway driving under routine
conditions is left to System 1”
means that steering the car around a bend is automatic and
almost effortless. It also implies that an
experienced driver can drive on an empty highway while
conducting a conversation. Finally,
“System 2 prevented James from reacting foolishly to the
insult” means that James would have
been more aggressive in his response if his capacity for
effortful control had been disrupted (for
example, if he had been drunk).
System 1 and System 2 are so central to the story I tell in this
book that I must make it absolutely
clear that they are217at they a fictitious characters. Systems 1
and 2 are not systems in the standard
sense of entities with interacting aspects or parts. And there is
no one part of the brain that either
of the systems would call home. You may well ask: What is the
point of introducing fictitious
characters with ugly names into a serious book? The answer is
that the characters are useful
because of some quirks of our minds, yours and mine. A
sentence is understood more easily if it
describes what an agent (System 2) does than if it describes
what something is, what properties it
has. In other words, “System 2” is a better subject for a
sentence than “mental arithmetic.” The
mind—especially System 1—appears to have a special aptitude
for the construction and
interpretation of stories about active agents, who have
personalities, habits, and abilities. You
quickly formed a bad opinion of the thieving butler, you expect
more bad behavior from him, and
you will remember him for a while. This is also my hope for the
language of systems.
Why call them System 1 and System 2 rather than the more
descriptive “automatic system” and
“effortful system”? The reason is simple: “Automatic system”
takes longer to say than “System 1”
and therefore takes more space in your working memory. This
matters, because anything that
occupies your working memory reduces your ability to think.
You should treat “System 1” and
“System 2” as nicknames, like Bob and Joe, identifying
characters that you will get to know over
the course of this book. The fictitious systems make it easier for
me to think about judgment and
choice, and will make it easier for you to understand what I say.
Speaking of System 1 and System 2
“He had an impression, but some of his impressions are
illusions.”
“This was a pure System 1 response. She reacted to the threat
before she recognized it.”
“This is your System 1 talking. Slow down and let your System
2 take control.”
24
Stripped down to its essentials, business is about one thing:
making decisions. We’re always deciding something - from the
small and the daily ( which emails to answer, what meetings to
have ) to the macro and the strategic ( what product to launch
and when ) to the intensely personal ( what job to take, whom
to hire, whom to marry ). But what does it take to make a
“good” decision? Is it about going by facts and percentages - or
about following your gut instinct? Does time produce better
decisions, or does pressure make you decide not only faster but
also more wisely? And finally, is better decision making some-
thing that you can learn? We asked 11 decisive leaders, inside
and outside of business, to answer these and other, related
questions. Does their advice help? You decide.
Ed Koch
Partner
Robinson Silverman Pearce Aronsohn & Berman LLP
New York, New York
To make good decisions, you need confidence in your judg-
ment. We all make bad decisions, but the important thing is
not to worry too much about them. Otherwise, you’ll never do
a thing. I was mayor of New York City for 12 years. I had a $28
billion budget. Each decision I made affected seven and a half
million people. The stakes were high. I had to show confidence
- in particular, confidence in my decision-making ability -
because a lot of people had put their trust in me. The worst
decision I ever made was to run for governor of New York State
in 1982. I did it on a lark, and it was stupid. Thank god the peo-
ple of New York had enough sense to understand that I didn’t
have my heart in that race - and to vote against me. They did
the right thing, and I was happier because of it.
As a judge, I have to decide on the credibility of two people
standing before me. I don’t know who is telling the truth, but I
know that one of them is lying. That’s when making decisions
gets sticky. Sure, the law helps me make such decisions. But
there have been many times when I’ve had a hell of a time
deciding. Even so, I never base my courtroom decisions on gut
instinct. I go by the evidence - or the lack of evidence - and by
the applicable law.
Ed Koch was mayor of New York City from 1978 to 1989.
Today he writes a weekly column for the New York Daily
News,
has his own radio talk show, and is the judge on the television
program “The People’s Court.”
Pamela Lopker
Chairman and President
QAD Inc.
Carpinteria, California
I make bad decisions all the time. But I’ve been successful
because I’ve developed a process for identifying and changing
those decisions quickly. I approach every decision with an eye
to the long-term outcome. That’s a hard method to adopt in a
fast-paced business environment. But it’s the only way to create
sustainable value on either a professional or a personal level.
The quick-fix method that I see so many companies rely on is
just that - a quick fix. There’s a big difference between invest-
ing heavily in marketing the product that you have today - with
no sense of what it takes to survive tomorrow - and investing
heavily in R&D today so that you can have tomorrow’s product.
Even in my personal life, I looked at decisions from the per-
spective of what’s going to be better for me over the long term.
When I was in college, I needed to decide on a career path.
While most of my peers were concerned with following their
passion, with doing what they “loved” to do, I was looking for a
career that would support me financially over the long run.
Sometimes making important decisions is just a matter of ask-
ing some basic questions: What are my skills? Where is a cer-
tain industry going? What should I be doing now to get to
where I want to be later? Because I was always strong in math
and in analytical thinking, I figured that I would do statistical
or actuarial work. Then I realized that statistics wasn’t a boom-
ing industry. Computer science seemed like a better long-term
bet, so I changed my major.
As a leader, I try not to make decisions for others. Sure, being a
dictator is often the fastest way to get things done. But it’s not a
process that allows an organization to sustain growth. I want the
people in my organization to learn the lessons that come with
making decisions: that everything is a compromise, that noth-
ing is ever completely logical, but that you can deal with things
through a logical decision-making process.
Pamela Lopker has been called America’s “richest self-made
1© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC • Visit:
http://www.fastcompany.com/learning
FC :L E A R N I N G
Decisions, Decisions
Unit of One
BY ANNA MUOIO
First appeared: FC18, p.93
woman.” QAD, a $172 million enterprise resource-planning
software company, is one of the 30 largest public companies to
be led by a female CEO. Its clients include Avon Products,
PepsiCo, Ford, and Johnson & Johnson.
W. Brian Arthur
Citibank Professor
Santa Fe Institute
Santa Fe, New Mexico
I received my PhD in operations research, which is a highly sci-
entific, mathematical way of strategizing and of making deci-
sions. I once thought that I could make any decision, whether
professional or personal, by using decision trees, game theory,
and optimization. Over time, I’ve changed my mind. For the
day-to-day work of running a business - scheduling a fleet of oil
tankers, choosing where to open a new factory - scientific deci-
sion theory works pretty well. But for just about every other
kind of decision, it doesn’t work at all.
For the big decisions in life, you need to reach a deeper region
of consciousness. Making decisions then becomes not so much
about “deciding” as about letting an inner wisdom emerge.
We’ve been bamboozled into believing that cognition is ration-
al - that our mind is a gigantic computer, or a blackboard on
which we can reach a decision by calculating pluses and
minuses. Recent research on cognition shows that our minds
rarely make strictly logical deductions. Instead, we rely on pat-
terns - and on feelings associated with those patterns.
So for those big decisions - Should I marry this person? Should
I follow that career? Should I sell my company? When should
we go public? - let patterns develop in your mind. Let clues and
evidence emerge from your environment. This approach to
decision making requires time, patience, and another key
ingredient: courage. It takes courage to listen to your inner wis-
dom. But once you hear that wisdom, making a decision
becomes fairly easy.
W. Brian Arthur’s work on the economics of increasing returns
and their role in technology-based industries has won him
international recognition in the scientific and business com-
munities. He is a pioneer of the new science of complexity and
the author of The Economy as an Evolving Complex System II
( Addison-Wesley, 1997 ).
Chung-Jen Tan
Senior Manager, Application Systems
IBM’s Thomas J. Watson Research Center
Yorktown Heights, New York
I’ve learned a lot about decision making by watching how a
machine plays chess. In 1996, Deep Blue played Garry
Kasparov, the world chess champion. Deep Blue won the first
game but lost the match. That experience taught us what we
needed to do to be ready for the rematch in 1997.
A human uses a combination of knowledge, strategy, and intu-
ition to make chess decisions. A machine relies on brute com-
putational power and on an ability to examine a tremendous
amount of data. On average, the human mind can manage
three or four positions per second. Deep Blue can evaluate 200
million alternatives per second. So the machine has a huge
advantage. But Deep Blue also had weaknesses, and they cen-
tered on its inability to be flexible. Our challenge was to equip
Deep Blue to make decisions like a human grandmaster.
We built in several programming tools that allowed us to adjust
some of the decision parameters dynamically and to change
strategies in between games. Joel Benjamin, the U.S. chess
champion, played chess with Deep Blue for nine months - test-
ing and tunneling the system - and we programmed some of his
knowledge into the computer.
An interesting thing happened when Deep Blue met Kasparov
for the rematch. While we had refined Deep Blue so that it
could make decisions and play more like a human, Kasparov
had refined his strategy so that he could play better against
standard computer-chess programs. In game six, Deep Blue
surprised Kasparov by sacrificing a knight to gain strategic
advantage. Kasparov, who hadn’t planned for such a decision,
realized how inflexible his own strategy had become. We
showed that a machine can reach a level of play comparable
with that of a grandmaster .
Chung-Jen Tan manages IBM’s Deep Blue computer-chess
project. Since 1984, Tan has worked on the architecture devel-
opment and machine design of highly parallel, scalable sys-
tems for solving large, complex problems.
Deborah Triant
CEO and President
Check Point Software Technologies Inc.
Redwood City, California
Making and implementing decisions boils down to a key ingre-
dient: listening.
I often wonder why schools emphasize debating. Why not have
listening classes as well? Debating is easy; listening with an
open mind is not. The worst thing that you as a leader can do
in the decision-making process is to voice your opinion before
anyone else can. No matter how open and honest your people
are, stating your opinion first will short-change the discussion
process and taint what you hear later. I’ve learned this the hard
way.
FC :L E A R N I N G
2© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC
There are two classes of decisions. The first is the easy kind.
For
instance, how do you choose which features to include in the
next release of your product? It’s a pretty straightforward
process: Survey your customers, identify the information you
need, and formulate a decision by using rational, objective
measurements. The second class is the tough kind. Fo r
instance: Should you hire this person? Should you go after this
key market? Should you switch jobs? Should you have a kid?
These are bet-your-life decisions for which there’s no obvious
right or wrong answer - and no way of gathering objective
infor-
mation.
To tackle decisions of either class, I first kick into a talkative
mode. I ask everybody I know to give me an opinion. I get dif-
ferent perspectives - and I listen to them. I also listen to that
thing called intuition. Some people warn against that
approach. But there’s a kind of processing that happens in a far
deeper place than logical processing does. Intuition offers a
way to integrate and synthesize, to weigh and balance informa-
tion. If I have to make a big decision, I listen to what others
think. But ultimately, I listen to my intuition. I postpone a deci-
sion until I wake up one morning and know where my gut is
going.
Deborah Triant was president and CEO of Sitka Corp., a sub-
sidiary of Sun Microsystems, before she joined Adobe Systems
as president of marketing in 1993. In 1995, she left Adobe to
head Check Point, a network-security and traffic-management
software company.
Roger Rainbow
VP, Global Business Environment
Shell International Ltd.
London, England
Nothing can paralyze a decision-making process more than
uncertainty can. The big decisions that have failed at Shell did-
n’t fail because of our operations or because of project man-
agement; they failed because we misunderstood the external
world. That’s why, when we’re on the verge of a big decision,
we do scenario planning. A scenario is valuable because it’s a
plausible, coherent story that we can use to articulate why we
want to do something and which issues to factor into the deci-
sion-making process.
Everyone has two things in mind when approaching a big deci-
sion. The first thing is a scenario - a view of how the world will
turn out. The second thing is an objective or a strategy. A good
decision-making process marries those two things.
Decisions fall into three categories. With the first category, you
know a lot. Take a plant that’s been operating for 20 years. You
know what makes it succeed or fail, and the decisions you take
regarding it tend to be fairly straightforward. They may be hard
to implement, but they’re simple to make.
With the second category, you know something but not every-
thing. This is where scenario planning helps. For instance, if
we’re trying to decide whether to invest in an oil field in
Russia,
we talk about which factors are uncertain and why. What has
to happen in order for this investment to succeed? What could
go wrong? What would the consequences be? Are any of those
consequences absolutely catastrophic? If so, how can we guard
against them?
With the third category, you know virtually nothing, and you’re
starting from scratch. This is where intuition comes into play -
and where you make truly creative decisions: You’re deciding
on things that haven’t been done before.
Each category of decisions requires different skills and process-
es. Understand which kind of decision you’re facing, and use
scenarios to narrow the range of uncertainty.
Roger Rainbow ( [email protected] ) joined Shell
in 1970. His team provides analyses of the business environ-
ment by preparing long-term scenarios that consider econom-
ic, social, technological, and environmental issues, as well as
the long-term supply and demand of energy sources.
Chris Newell
Executive Director, the Lotus Institute
Lotus Development Corp.
Cambridge, Massachusetts
The crux of making good decisions isn’t doing things right - it’s
making sure that you focus on the right things. As citizens of
the Information Age, we’re overwhelmed with data, reports,
facts - much of it noise that we need to filter out before we can
figure out what’s relevant to a decision. So I’ve learned to hone
my discrimination skills. But if you’re exploring something that
has no precedent - an idea, a product, a service - you can’t rely
solely on data to guide you. You have to rely on people too.
I’ve
developed a personal advisory council that I consult before
tackling a big decision, a group of people who are anchored by
similar values - though not necessarily by similar perspectives.
Sometimes making the “wrong” decision is the right thing to
do. Last year, I decided to overrun my budget - to continue
investing in LearningSpace, a product that Lotus was unsure
about investing in. I knew it was the right thing to do, even
though I couldn’t get the company to agree. When news of my
decision surfaced on the radar of some key people, I got ham-
mered. At times, I even doubted my own decision. A year later,
LearningSpace has become one of Lotus’s most important
3© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC
FC :L E A R N I N G
strategic products.
I made one mistake: I didn’t communicate all of the implica-
tions of going over budget. Otherwise, I wouldn’t change a
thing about what I did.
Chris Newell founded the Lotus Institute in 1994. Its mission
is to explore ways in which groupware technology intersects
with cultural issues. It has developed two applications:
TeamRoom, which supports virtual teaming; and
L e a r ningSpace, which supports collaborative, di s t r i b u t i
v e
learning.
Buz Mertes
VP, Loss Management and Policy Servicing
GE Capital Mortgage Insurance Corp.
Raleigh, North Carolina
By transforming the art of decision making into a science, you
can save time, money, and frustration. My team handles tough
decisions every day. It has to decide whether we can “cure” a
loan for someone who has stopped making payments on it or
whether we have to recommend foreclosure because of real
financial trouble.
For a long time, we couldn’t figure out what allowed some of
our reps to make better decisions than others did. We pulled six
of our best reps off their jobs and analyzed their decision-mak-
ing processes for 10 months. We found that these reps had rad-
ically different styles. Our challenge was to figure out the criti-
cal processes that drove their decisions and then to make sure
that those decisions were driven by factual, up-to-date informa-
tion. We had a goal: to become more consistent in our decision
making and to improve our customers’ perception of us. We
ended up creating a software program, called Loss Mitigation
Optimizer, to aid us in that process.
Turning decision making into a science saved us time and
money. Before we implemented this technology, we were cur-
ing fewer than 30% of our cases. Now we cure more than 50%
of them. The decision-making process itself takes 30% to 50%
less time per deal. Our savings jumped dramatically - by about
$8,000 per case. In the project’s first 18 months, these addi-
tional savings came to $15 million in net income. Some peo-
ple think that decisions of this type can’t be driven by data. Our
results show otherwise.
Buz Mertes joined GE in 1984. Since 1996, Mertes has spon-
sored a series of projects that have increased the company’s net
income by more than $20 million.
Jerry Seeman
Senior Director of Officiating
National Football League
New York, New York
As a referee, I found that the pressure was the same whether I
was officiating a preseason game or a Super Bowl. Being on the
field is like being in a fishbowl: Everyone - players, coaches,
fans, the media - is waiting for your decision. A successful call
depends on three things: You must be in position, you must
have a deep knowledge of the game, and you must have intense
concentration. Remember, no matter what the reaction to your
decision may be, you answer to only one thing: your con-
science. Above all, when making a decision, you have to keep
your cool.
One of the biggest errors that I see officials fall into is making
calls too quickly. Each decision has two phases: You read and
analyze the play, and then you make the call. But when things
happen in a split second, it can be tempting to throw a penalty
flag before you know what happened. That’s why I tell all of my
officials to work in “cruise control.” The fans may go crazy, the
players and coaches may get excited, but there should be seven
people on the field who work every game the same way from
beginning to end - and who exude a quiet confidence.
Once you make a decision, you’d better be able to communi-
cate it. I remember having to make a call on a bizarre play in
a playoff game between Houston and Cleveland. After an
attempted onside kick, the kicking team illegally touched the
ball, which then went out of bounds. The rules dictate that the
kicking team should kick again. That team again tried an
onside kick, and again it illegally touched the ball - but this
time, it recovered the ball. I called that the receiving team
would gain control of the ball on the spot where it was illegal-
ly touched. This was a complicated play: The NFL probably
hasn’t seen another of its kind in the past 15 years. At first, the
fans didn’t understand my decision. But I explained to them
what the proper ruling was, and they accepted it. I was confi-
dent about my knowledge of the game. And I didn’t lose my
cool.
Jerry Seeman, one of the most celebrated referees in football
history, became an NFL line judge in 1975. He was appointed
referee in 1979. His responsibilities now cover the league’s
supervisory staff, as well as regional game observers, collegiate
scouts, and all game officials.
Max Bazerman
J. Jay Gerber Distinguished Professor of Dispute Resolution
and Organizations
Kellogg Graduate School of Management
Evanston, Illinois
When making a decision, don’t listen to your intuition.
Intuition will lead you astray; it’s drastically overrated. The
desire to follow intuition reflects the mythology of people who
4© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC
FC :L E A R N I N G
don’t want to think rationally and systematically. They tell sto-
ries about how their intuition guided them through a decision,
but they don’t understand how or why. Often, when you hear
about intuition, what you’re really hearing is a justification of
luck. Intuition might be fine for the small decisions in life - like
what kind of ice cream to buy. But when you get to the biggies,
you need a more systematic thought process.
One of the biggest challenges I face is teaching people how to
change their decision-making behavior. Everyone makes mis-
takes and bad decisions. I try to show people that their mistakes
are predictable.
Here’s an interesting thought experiment: You’ve got to decide
between two things - two executives to hire for a key position,
two acquisition targets, two houses that you’re thinking about
buying.
You go through a systematic process of identifying all of the
fac-
tors and weighing them for each alternative. You do a little
arithmetic, and your analysis says to pick A over B. But your
intuition says to pick B over A. So what do you do?
Most people will go on their intuition - which raises the ques-
tion, Why did you do all that work in the first place? One of
these two options is wrong. So postpone your decision until you
can determine why your intuition is out of sync with the sys-
tematic analysis. That’s the purpose of systematic analysis: to
inform your intuition, to make you consider all of the options,
and to help you make a wise decision.
Max Bazerman ( [email protected] ) studies decision mak-
ing, negotiation, and environmental issues. He is the author of
Judgment in Managerial Decision Making ( John Wiley &
Sons, 1986 ) and Why Smart People Make Dumb Money
Moves ( forthcoming from Wiley, April 1999 ). Bazerman is a
visiting professor of business admi nistration at Harvard
Business School.
Howard Raiffa
Professor Emeritus
Harvard Business School
Boston, Massachusetts
One of the biggest pitfalls in decision making is not creating
enough alternatives. You can make a decision in order to solve
a problem - or you can make a decision in order to exploit an
opportunity. To make this point, I use the case study of a busi-
nessman named Bill. His dilemma: to sell or not to sell his
business. Bill and his partner had struggled for years to build a
soundproofing business in Brooklyn, and finally the company
was doing well. But Bill was bored; at age 57, he wanted a
change. The solution, he thought, was to sell out to his partner,
move to California, and start a new business. He framed his
problem around how much he should sell his part of the busi-
ness for - $400,000 ( a price that his partner could afford ) or
something higher.
At my suggestion, Bill went through a rigorous process of
reframing his decision. He realized that what he wanted was to
spend more time outdoors, to be challenged intellectually, and
to minimize stress. In the end, Bill decided to move to
California and to establish a branch of the soundproofing
business there. That way, he could change his life and also be
challenged - but without taking on an enormous amount of
risk. The business did well: Eight years later, he sold out to his
partner for $1.7 million.
Howard Raiffa is a pioneer in the development of decision
analysis, negotiation analysis, and game theory. He is coauthor,
with John S. Hammond and Ralph L. Keeney, of Smart
Choices: A Practical Guide to Making Better Decisions (
Harvard Business School Press, 1998 ).
5© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC
FC :L E A R N I N G

More Related Content

Similar to ENG-106 Rubric Cause and Effect Argument Criteria Value1 .docx

Graduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docx
Graduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docxGraduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docx
Graduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docx
shericehewat
 
Evaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docx
Evaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docxEvaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docx
Evaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docx
pauline234567
 
Strategic Management Course The report should includ.docx
Strategic Management Course   The report should includ.docxStrategic Management Course   The report should includ.docx
Strategic Management Course The report should includ.docx
mckellarhastings
 
Research Paper Rubric Component 100 75 50 25 0
Research Paper Rubric    Component 100 75 50 25 0 Research Paper Rubric    Component 100 75 50 25 0
Research Paper Rubric Component 100 75 50 25 0
myrljjcpoarch
 
Top of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docx
Top of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docxTop of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docx
Top of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docx
juliennehar
 
WarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docx
WarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docxWarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docx
WarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docx
melbruce90096
 
Collaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docx
Collaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docxCollaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docx
Collaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docx
mccormicknadine86
 
Two popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docx
Two popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docxTwo popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docx
Two popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docx
willcoxjanay
 
Graduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docx
Graduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docxGraduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docx
Graduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docx
shericehewat
 
PSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docx
PSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docxPSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docx
PSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docx
amrit47
 
Top of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docx
Top of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docxTop of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docx
Top of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docx
edwardmarivel
 
1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx
1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx
1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx
jeremylockett77
 
Rubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docx
Rubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docxRubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docx
Rubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docx
SUBHI7
 
Top of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docx
Top of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docxTop of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docx
Top of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docx
turveycharlyn
 
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docxCompare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
janthony65
 
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docxCompare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
templestewart19
 
Healthcare Informatics Applications.pdf
Healthcare Informatics Applications.pdfHealthcare Informatics Applications.pdf
Healthcare Informatics Applications.pdf
bkbk37
 
1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx
1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx
1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx
RAJU852744
 
Benchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docx
Benchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docxBenchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docx
Benchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docx
lascellesjaimie
 

Similar to ENG-106 Rubric Cause and Effect Argument Criteria Value1 .docx (19)

Graduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docx
Graduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docxGraduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docx
Graduate Writing RubricPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEXE.docx
 
Evaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docx
Evaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docxEvaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docx
Evaluate your improvements made and weaknesses that still persist .docx
 
Strategic Management Course The report should includ.docx
Strategic Management Course   The report should includ.docxStrategic Management Course   The report should includ.docx
Strategic Management Course The report should includ.docx
 
Research Paper Rubric Component 100 75 50 25 0
Research Paper Rubric    Component 100 75 50 25 0 Research Paper Rubric    Component 100 75 50 25 0
Research Paper Rubric Component 100 75 50 25 0
 
Top of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docx
Top of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docxTop of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docx
Top of FormBottom of FormPersuasive Essay Peer Review Wor.docx
 
WarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docx
WarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docxWarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docx
WarningYou do not have permissions to perform this activityA.docx
 
Collaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docx
Collaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docxCollaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docx
Collaborate Summary RubricCollaborate Summary RubricCriteria0 Poin.docx
 
Two popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docx
Two popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docxTwo popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docx
Two popular methods of financial statement analysis are horizontal.docx
 
Graduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docx
Graduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docxGraduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docx
Graduate Writing RubricAPUS AssignmentRubricGraduate WritingEX.docx
 
PSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docx
PSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docxPSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docx
PSYC 221Article Critique Grading RubricStudent ________.docx
 
Top of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docx
Top of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docxTop of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docx
Top of FormCoyne and Messina Articles, Part 3 Spearman Coefficie.docx
 
1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx
1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx
1. From the breadth and depth of the economic downturn, it was cle.docx
 
Rubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docx
Rubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docxRubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docx
Rubric For The Evaluation Of Modern Challenges” PaperCRITERIA.docx
 
Top of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docx
Top of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docxTop of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docx
Top of Formreviewing the Literature and Theory 1Unsatisfac.docx
 
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docxCompare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
 
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docxCompare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
Compare and contrast the Church Shooting that occurred on June 1.docx
 
Healthcare Informatics Applications.pdf
Healthcare Informatics Applications.pdfHealthcare Informatics Applications.pdf
Healthcare Informatics Applications.pdf
 
1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx
1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx
1009W MLC Prerequisite Expository Essay NAME ASSIGNMENT.docx
 
Benchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docx
Benchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docxBenchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docx
Benchmark - Evidence-Based Practice Project—Paper on Diabetes .docx
 

More from gidmanmary

Using Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docx
Using Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docxUsing Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docx
Using Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docx
gidmanmary
 
Using APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docx
Using APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docxUsing APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docx
Using APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docx
gidmanmary
 
Use the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docx
Use the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docxUse the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docx
Use the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docx
gidmanmary
 
Use the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docx
Use the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docxUse the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docx
Use the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docx
gidmanmary
 
use the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docx
use the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docxuse the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docx
use the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docx
gidmanmary
 
Use the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docx
Use the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docxUse the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docx
Use the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docx
gidmanmary
 
Use the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docx
Use the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docxUse the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docx
Use the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docx
gidmanmary
 

More from gidmanmary (20)

Using C#, write a program to find the nearest common parent of any t.docx
Using C#, write a program to find the nearest common parent of any t.docxUsing C#, write a program to find the nearest common parent of any t.docx
Using C#, write a program to find the nearest common parent of any t.docx
 
Using autoethnography in a qualitative research,consider a resid.docx
Using autoethnography in a qualitative research,consider a resid.docxUsing autoethnography in a qualitative research,consider a resid.docx
Using autoethnography in a qualitative research,consider a resid.docx
 
Using Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docx
Using Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docxUsing Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docx
Using Earned Value to Determine StatusJennifer turned in her statu.docx
 
Using at least your textbook and a minimum of three additional sourc.docx
Using at least your textbook and a minimum of three additional sourc.docxUsing at least your textbook and a minimum of three additional sourc.docx
Using at least your textbook and a minimum of three additional sourc.docx
 
Using APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docx
Using APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docxUsing APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docx
Using APA style format, write a 4-5 page paper describing the provis.docx
 
Use the organization you selected in Week One and the Security Ass.docx
Use the organization you selected in Week One and the Security Ass.docxUse the organization you selected in Week One and the Security Ass.docx
Use the organization you selected in Week One and the Security Ass.docx
 
Using DuPont analysis is a quick and relatively easy way to assess t.docx
Using DuPont analysis is a quick and relatively easy way to assess t.docxUsing DuPont analysis is a quick and relatively easy way to assess t.docx
Using DuPont analysis is a quick and relatively easy way to assess t.docx
 
Using APA style, prepare written research paper of 4 double spaced p.docx
Using APA style, prepare written research paper of 4 double spaced p.docxUsing APA style, prepare written research paper of 4 double spaced p.docx
Using APA style, prepare written research paper of 4 double spaced p.docx
 
Use the organization selected for your Managing Change Paper Part .docx
Use the organization selected for your Managing Change Paper Part .docxUse the organization selected for your Managing Change Paper Part .docx
Use the organization selected for your Managing Change Paper Part .docx
 
Use your knowledge to develop the circumstances and details involved.docx
Use your knowledge to develop the circumstances and details involved.docxUse your knowledge to develop the circumstances and details involved.docx
Use your knowledge to develop the circumstances and details involved.docx
 
Use the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docx
Use the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docxUse the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docx
Use the percentage method to compute the federal income taxes to w.docx
 
Use the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docx
Use the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docxUse the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docx
Use the textbook andor online sources to locate and capture three w.docx
 
Use the Web to conduct research on User Domain Security Policy and A.docx
Use the Web to conduct research on User Domain Security Policy and A.docxUse the Web to conduct research on User Domain Security Policy and A.docx
Use the Web to conduct research on User Domain Security Policy and A.docx
 
Use this Article to create the powerpoint slidesBrown, S. L., No.docx
Use this Article to create the powerpoint slidesBrown, S. L., No.docxUse this Article to create the powerpoint slidesBrown, S. L., No.docx
Use this Article to create the powerpoint slidesBrown, S. L., No.docx
 
use the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docx
use the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docxuse the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docx
use the work sheet format made available  but only to give brief, bu.docx
 
Use these three sources to compose an annotated bibliography on Data.docx
Use these three sources to compose an annotated bibliography on Data.docxUse these three sources to compose an annotated bibliography on Data.docx
Use these three sources to compose an annotated bibliography on Data.docx
 
Use this outline to crete a four page essay. Guideline included and .docx
Use this outline to crete a four page essay. Guideline included and .docxUse this outline to crete a four page essay. Guideline included and .docx
Use this outline to crete a four page essay. Guideline included and .docx
 
Use the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docx
Use the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docxUse the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docx
Use the link provided to complete a self-assessment. After completin.docx
 
Use the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docx
Use the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docxUse the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docx
Use the SPSS software and the data set (2004)GSS.SAV (attached).docx
 
Use the Internet to research functional systems that would contain a.docx
Use the Internet to research functional systems that would contain a.docxUse the Internet to research functional systems that would contain a.docx
Use the Internet to research functional systems that would contain a.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseSpellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
AnaAcapella
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptxGoogle Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseSpellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxHow to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 

ENG-106 Rubric Cause and Effect Argument Criteria Value1 .docx

  • 1. ENG-106 Rubric: Cause and Effect Argument Criteria % Value 1: Unsatisfactory 2: Less Than Satisfactory 3: Satisfactory 4: Good 5: Excellent % Scaling 0% 65% 75% 85% 100% Content & Ideas – 40% Cause and Effect Content and Ideas Should: Include an effective title. Present a thesis that centers specifically on a problem of cause and effect. Use cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning to support the cause and effect argument established in the thesis. Cite evidence that supports the cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning. 40% Does not have title, and has missing or indiscernible thesis statement and minimal evidence to support main ideas. Argument includes elements of cause/effect, but the argument does not center on causes or consequences. The student does not use strategies that support cause and effect arguments. Title may not suggest subject and does not spark interest. Thesis statement and/or the controlling idea are not clearly stated. Argument includes elements of cause/effect, but the argument
  • 2. does not center on causes or consequences and/or the writer does not use strategies that support cause and effect arguments very well. Ideas are underdeveloped and clichéd. They do not support the thesis. Evidence from outside sources can be irrelevant. Title suggests subject but does not spark interest. Thesis statement identifies the main point the author is trying to make. Most of content relates to thesis statement, but lacks sufficient support through appropriate strategies. Argument may not center specifically on cause/effect or use strategies that support cause and effect arguments effectively. Cited evidence sometimes does not justify ideas. Title suggests subject but does not necessarily spark interest. Thesis statement clearly identifies the main point the author is trying to make. Argument centers specifically on cause/effect and uses strategies that support cause and effect arguments (cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning). Most of the content supports thesis, and cited evidence usually justifies ideas. Title suggests subject and sparks interest. With a clear, controlling idea, thesis statement effectively identifies the main point the student is trying to make. Content supports thesis well. Argument centers specifically on cause/effect and effectively uses strategies that support cause and effect arguments (cause and effect chains and inductive reasoning). Specific, cited evidence justifies ideas and enriches the essay. Organization – 12% Organization 12% No apparent organization present. Ineffective introduction does not invite readers or explain the subject. The reader cannot find the thesis statement. Underdeveloped paragraphs lack focus and topic sentences. No conclusion present. No apparent organization present. Introduction explains subject, but does not engage readers. Thesis is difficult to find.
  • 3. Underdeveloped paragraphs lack focus and topic sentences. Weak conclusion offered. Organization is clear, but with minor errors. Introduction explains subject, but does not adequately engage readers. Thesis may be misplaced. Paragraphs are not developed around topic sentences, and may not always advance essay's ideas. Conclusion summarizes but does not conclude. Organization aids readers in understanding content. Introduction explains subject, but may not engage readers. Thesis statement is placed appropriately, according to the genre of the writing set forth in the assignment description in the syllabus. Well-ordered paragraphs are developed around topic sentences, and advance essay's ideas. Conclusion may be more of a summary. Logically organized to lead readers to understanding content. Introduction explains subject and engages readers. Thesis statement is placed appropriately, according to the genre set forth in the assignment description in the syllabus. Well-ordered paragraphs are developed around topic sentences, and advance essay's ideas. Conclusion provides strong, satisfying ending, not a mere summary of the essay. Format – 16% Paper Format 16% Layout: Essay lacks more than THREE of the following: double-spaced, 12 pt, Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers using appropriate header function. Not all information, paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the page they appear; little or no in-text citations and/or entries on reference page used; major documentation oversights noted; major format errors and omissions noted; inappropriate number of required sources used. Layout: Essay lacks THREE of the following: double-spaced, 12 pt. Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers
  • 4. using appropriate header function. Not all information, paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the page they appear; missing more than one citation and/or reference entry; significant documentation oversights noted; significant format errors or omissions noted; inappropriate number of required sources used. Layout: Essay lacks TWO of the following: double-spaced, 12 pt. Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers using appropriate header function. Not all information, paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the page they appear; missing one in-text citation and/or reference entry; minor documentation oversights noted; minor formatting errors or omissions noted; appropriate number of required sources are used. Essay lacks ONE of the following: double-spaced, 12 pt. Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers using appropriate header function. All information, paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited on the page that they appear and are listed on the references page (GCU format); some minor errors or omissions in format noted; appropriate number of required sources is used. Layout: Essay is double-spaced with 12 pt. Times New Roman font, 1inch margins, heading (with name, course, date, and instructor), assignment title, and page numbers using appropriate header function. All information, paraphrases, quotations, and borrowed ideas are cited in parenthetical GCU format; all sources are listed on the references page (GCU format); all citations and reference entries are complete and in alphabetical order; appropriate number of required sources is used.
  • 5. Language & Style – 16% Language & Style 16% Voice & tone are inappropriate and ineffective in creating appropriate mood. Inappropriate word choice used. Sentence structure includes ungrammatical structures and no variety. Writing is wordy. Voice & tone are inappropriate and ineffective in creating appropriate mood. Word choice fails in use of appropriate, precise language and strong verbs. Includes too many to be verbs. No attempt to vary sentence structure noted. Writing is wordy. Voice & tone usually do not characterize ideas appropriately or effectively create appropriate mood. Word choice includes nonstandard outdated usage, too many to be verbs, is not precise, and is occasionally incorrect. Some slang or jargon exists in the paper. Inadequate variety in sentence structure noted. Writing is wordy. Voice & tone usually characterize ideas effectively create appropriate mood. Word choice usually includes current standard usage, active verbs, concrete nouns, and precise words. Some slang or jargon exists in the paper. Some variety of sentence structures strengthens the ideas, creates vitality, and avoids choppiness in the writing. Writing is mostly concisely written. Voice & tone characterize ideas and effectively create appropriate mood. Word choice includes current standard usage, active verbs, concrete nouns, and precise words. Sentence structures strengthen the ideas, create vitality, and avoid choppiness in the writing. Writing is concise. Grammar & Mechanics – 16%
  • 6. Grammar & Mechanics 16% Demonstrates no control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation conventions. Many errors, such as: Apostrophe use Capitalization Commas misplaced or missing Parallelism Faulty point of view shifts Pronoun agreement Quotation errors Semicolons misused Run-ons & fragments Spelling errors Subject-verb agreement Tense shifts Demonstrates minimal control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation conventions. Several errors, such as: Apostrophe use Capitalization Commas misplaced or missing Parallelism Faulty point of view shifts Pronoun agreement Quotation errors
  • 7. Semicolons misused Run-ons & fragments Spelling errors Subject-verb agreement Tense shifts Demonstrates reasonable control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation conventions. Some errors, such as: Apostrophe use Capitalization Commas misplaced or missing Parallelism Faulty point of view shifts Pronoun agreement Quotation errors Semicolons misused Run-ons & fragments Spelling errors Subject-verb agreement Tense shifts
  • 8. Demonstrates high control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation conventions. Few errors, such as: Apostrophe use Capitalization Commas misplaced or missing Parallelism Faulty point of view shifts Pronoun agreement Quotation errors Semicolons misused Run-ons & fragments Spelling errors Subject-verb agreement Tense shifts Demonstrates outstanding control of grammar, spelling, & punctuation conventions. No errors, such as: Apostrophe use Capitalization Commas misplaced or missing
  • 9. Parallelism Faulty point of view shifts Pronoun agreement Quotation errors Semicolons misused Run-ons & fragments Spelling errors Subject-verb agreement Tense shifts PAGE © 2015. Grand Canyon University. All Rights Reserved. Introduction Every author, I suppose, has in mind a setting in which readers of his or her work could benefit from having read it. Mine is the proverbial office watercooler, where opinions are shared and gossip is exchanged. I hope to enrich the vocabulary that people use when they talk about the judgments and choices of others, the company’s new policies, or
  • 10. a colleague’s investment decisions. Why be concerned with gossip? Because it is much easier, as well as far more enjoyable, to identify and label the mistakes of others than to recognize our own. Questioning what we believe and want is difficult at the best of times, and especially difficult when we most need to do it, but we can benefit from the informed opinions of others. Many of us spontaneously anticipate how friends and colleagues will evaluate our choices; the quality and content of these anticipated judgments therefore matters. The expectation of intelligent gossip is a powerful motive for serious self-criticism, more powerful than New Year resolutions to improve one’s decision making at work and at home. To be a good diagnostician, a physician needs to acquire a large set of labels for diseases, each of which binds an idea of the illness and its symptoms, possible antecedents and causes, possible developments and consequences, and possible interventions to cure or mitigate the illness. Learning medicine consists in part of learning the language of medicine. A deeper understanding of judgments and choices also requires a richer vocabulary than is available in everyday language. The hope for informed gossip is that there are distinctive patterns in the errors people make. Systematic errors are known as biases, and they recur predictably in particular circumstances. When the handsome and confident speaker bounds onto the stage, for example, you can anticipate that the audience will judge his comments more favorably than he deserves. The availability of a diagnostic label for this bias—the halo effect—makes it easier
  • 11. to anticipate, recognize, and understand. When you are asked what you are thinking about, you can normally answer. You believe you know what goes on in your mind, which often consists of one conscious thought leading in an orderly way to another. But that is not the only way the mind works, nor indeed is that the typical way. Most impressions and thoughts arise in your conscious experience without your knowing how they got there. You cannot tracryd>e how you came to the belief that there is a lamp on the desk in front of you, or how you detected a hint of irritation in your spouse’s voice on the telephone, or how you managed to avoid a threat on the road before you became consciously aware of it. The mental work that produces impressions, intuitions, and many decisions goes on in silence in our mind. Much of the discussion in this book is about biases of intuition. However, the focus on error does not denigrate human intelligence, any more than the attention to diseases in medical texts denies good health. Most of us are healthy most of the time, and most of our judgments and actions are appropriate most of the time. As we navigate our lives, we normally allow ourselves to be guided by impressions and feelings, and the confidence we have in our intuitive beliefs and preferences is usually justified. But not always. We are often confident even when we are wrong, and an objective observer is more likely to detect our errors than we are.
  • 12. So this is my aim for watercooler conversations: improve the ability to identify and understand errors of judgment and choice, in others and eventually in ourselves, by providing a richer and more precise language to discuss them. In at least some cases, an accurate diagnosis may suggest an intervention to limit the damage that bad judgments and choices often cause. 7 Two Systems The Characters of the Story To observe your mind in automatic mode, glance at the image below. Figure 1 Your experience as you look at the woman’s face seamlessly combines what we normally call seeing and intuitive thinking. As surely and quickly as you saw that the young woman’s hair is dark, you knew she is angry. Furthermore, what you saw extended into the future. You sensed that this woman is about to say some very unkind words, probably in a loud and strident voice. A premonition of what she was going to do next came to mind automatically and effortlessly. You did not intend to assess her mood or to anticipate what she might do, and your reaction to the picture did not have the feel of something you did. It just happened to you. It was an instance of fast thinking.
  • 13. Now look at the following problem: 17 × 24 You knew immediately that this is a multiplication problem, and probably knew that you could solve it, with paper and pencil, if not without. You also had some vague intuitive knowledge of the range of possible results. You would be quick to recognize that both 12,609 and 123 are implausible. Without spending some time on the problem, however, you would not be certain that the answer is not 568. A precise solution did not come to mind, and you felt that you could choose whether or not to engage in the computation. If you have not done so yet, you should attempt the multiplication problem now, completing at least part of it. You experienced slow thinking as you proceeded through a sequence of steps. You first retrieved from memory the cognitive program for multiplication that you learned in school, then you implemented it. Carrying out the computation was a strain. You felt the burden of holding much material in memory, as you needed to keep track of where you were and of where you were going, while holding on to the intermediate result. The process was mental work: deliberate, effortful, and orderly—a prototype of slow thinking. The computation was not only an event in your mind; your body was also involved. Your muscles tensed up, your blood pressure rose, and your heart 17
  • 14. rate increased. Someone looking closely at your eyes while you tackled this problem would have seen your pupils dilate. Your pupils contracted back to normal size as soon as you ended your work—when you found the answer (which is 408, by the way) or when you gave up. Two Systems Psychologists have been intensely interested for several decades in the two modagee fi Pn="cees of thinking evoked by the picture of the angry woman and by the multiplication problem, and have offered many labels for them. I adopt terms originally proposed by the psychologists Keith Stanovich and Richard West, and will refer to two systems in the mind, System 1 and System 2. System 1 operates automatically and quickly, with little or no effort and no sense of voluntary control. System 2 allocates attention to the effortful mental activities that demand it, including complex computations. The operations of System 2 are often associated with the subjective experience of agency, choice, and concentration. The labels of System 1 and System 2 are widely used in psychology, but I go further than most in this book, which you can read as a psychodrama with two characters. When we think of ourselves, we identify with System 2, the conscious, reasoning self that has
  • 15. beliefs, makes choices, and decides what to think about and what to do. Although System 2 believes itself to be where the action is, the automatic System 1 is the hero of the book. I describe System 1 as effortlessly originating impressions and feelings that are the main sources of the explicit beliefs and deliberate choices of System 2. The automatic operations of System 1 generate surprisingly complex patterns of ideas, but only the slower System 2 can construct thoughts in an orderly series of steps. I also describe circumstances in which System 2 takes over, overruling the freewheeling impulses and associations of System 1. You will be invited to think of the two systems as agents with their individual abilities, limitations, and functions. In rough order of complexity, here are some examples of the automatic activities that are attributed to System 1: Detect that one object is more distant than another. Orient to the source of a sudden sound. Complete the phrase “bread and…” Make a “disgust face” when shown a horrible picture. Detect hostility in a voice. Answer to 2 + 2 = ? Read words on large billboards. Drive a car on an empty road. Find a strong move in chess (if you are a chess master). Understand simple sentences. Recognize that a “meek and tidy soul with a passion for detail” resembles an occupational 18
  • 16. stereotype. All these mental events belong with the angry woman—they occur automatically and require little or no effort. The capabilities of System 1 include innate skills that we share with other animals. We are born prepared to perceive the world around us, recognize objects, orient attention, avoid losses, and fear spiders. Other mental activities become fast and automatic through prolonged practice. System 1 has learned associations between ideas (the capital of France?); it has also learned skills such as reading and understanding nuances of social situations. Some skills, such as finding strong chess moves, are acquired only by specialized experts. Others are widely shared. Detecting the similarity of a personality sketch to an occupatiohein occupatnal stereotype requires broad knowledge of the language and the culture, which most of us possess. The knowledge is stored in memory and accessed without intention and without effort. Several of the mental actions in the list are completely involuntary. You cannot refrain from understanding simple sentences in your own language or from orienting to a loud unexpected sound, nor can you prevent yourself from knowing that 2 + 2 = 4 or from thinking of Paris when the capital of France is mentioned. Other activities, such as chewing, are susceptible to voluntary control but normally run on automatic pilot. The control of attention is shared by the two systems.
  • 17. Orienting to a loud sound is normally an involuntary operation of System 1, which immediately mobilizes the voluntary attention of System 2. You may be able to resist turning toward the source of a loud and offensive comment at a crowded party, but even if your head does not move, your attention is initially directed to it, at least for a while. However, attention can be moved away from an unwanted focus, primarily by focusing intently on another target. The highly diverse operations of System 2 have one feature in common: they require attention and are disrupted when attention is drawn away. Here are some examples: Brace for the starter gun in a race. Focus attention on the clowns in the circus. Focus on the voice of a particular person in a crowded and noisy room. Look for a woman with white hair. Search memory to identify a surprising sound. Maintain a faster walking speed than is natural for you. Monitor the appropriateness of your behavior in a social situation. Count the occurrences of the letter a in a page of text. Tell someone your phone number. Park in a narrow space (for most people except garage attendants). Compare two washing machines for overall value. Fill out a tax form. Check the validity of a complex logical argument.
  • 18. In all these situations you must pay attention, and you will perform less well, or not at all, if you are not ready or if your attention is directed inappropriately. System 2 has some ability to change the way System 1 works, by programming the normally automatic functions of attention and memory. When waiting for a relative at a busy train station, for example, you can set yourself at 19 will to look for a white-haired woman or a bearded man, and thereby increase the likelihood of detecting your relative from a distance. You can set your memory to search for capital cities that start with N or for French existentialist novels. And when you rent a car at London’s Heathrow Airport, the attendant will probably remind you that “we drive on the left side of the road over here.” In all these cases, you are asked to do something that does not come naturally, and you will find that the consistent maintenance of a set requires continuous exertion of at least some effort. The often-used phrase “pay attention” is apt: you dispose of a limited budget of attention that you can allocate to activities, and if you try to i>Cyou try tgo beyond your budget, you will fail. It is the mark of effortful activities that they interfere with each other, which is why it is difficult or impossible to conduct several at once. You could not compute the product of 17 × 24 while making a left turn into dense traffic, and you certainly should not try. You can do several things at once, but
  • 19. only if they are easy and undemanding. You are probably safe carrying on a conversation with a passenger while driving on an empty highway, and many parents have discovered, perhaps with some guilt, that they can read a story to a child while thinking of something else. Everyone has some awareness of the limited capacity of attention, and our social behavior makes allowances for these limitations. When the driver of a car is overtaking a truck on a narrow road, for example, adult passengers quite sensibly stop talking. They know that distracting the driver is not a good idea, and they also suspect that he is temporarily deaf and will not hear what they say. Intense focusing on a task can make people effectively blind, even to stimuli that normally attract attention. The most dramatic demonstration was offered by Christopher Chabris and Daniel Simons in their book The Invisible Gorilla. They constructed a short film of two teams passing basketballs, one team wearing white shirts, the other wearing black. The viewers of the film are instructed to count the number of passes made by the white team, ignoring the black players. This task is difficult and completely absorbing. Halfway through the video, a woman wearing a gorilla suit appears, crosses the court, thumps her chest, and moves on. The gorilla is in view for 9 seconds. Many thousands of people have seen the video, and about half of them do not notice anything unusual. It is the counting task—and especially the instruction to ignore one of the teams —that causes the blindness. No one who watches the video
  • 20. without that task would miss the gorilla. Seeing and orienting are automatic functions of System 1, but they depend on the allocation of some attention to the relevant stimulus. The authors note that the most remarkable observation of their study is that people find its results very surprising. Indeed, the viewers who fail to see the gorilla are initially sure that it was not there—they cannot imagine missing such a striking event. The gorilla study illustrates two important facts about our minds: we can be blind to the obvious, and we are also blind to our blindness. Plot Synopsis The interaction of the two systems is a recurrent theme of the book, and a brief synopsis of the plot is in order. In the story I will tell, Systems 1 and 2 are both active whenever we are awake. System 1 runs automatically and System 2 is normally in a comfortable low-effort mode, in which only a fraction of its capacity is engaged. System 1 continuously generates suggestions for System 2: impressions, intuitions, intentions, and feelings. If endorsed by System 2, impressions and intuitions turn into beliefs, and impulses turn into voluntary actions. When all goes smoothly, which is most of the time, System 2 adopts the suggestions of System 1 with little or no modification. You generally believe your impressions and act on your desires, and that is fine—usually. When System 1 runs into difficulty, it calls on System 2 to support more detailed and specific 20
  • 21. processing that may solve the problem of the moment. System 2 is mobilized when a question arises for which System 1 does not offer an answer, as probably happened to you when you encountered the multiplication problem 17 × 24. You can also feel a surge of conscious attention whenever you are surprised. System 2 is activ">< 2 is actated when an event is detected that violates the model of the world that System 1 maintains. In that world, lamps do not jump, cats do not bark, and gorillas do not cross basketball courts. The gorilla experiment demonstrates that some attention is needed for the surprising stimulus to be detected. Surprise then activates and orients your attention: you will stare, and you will search your memory for a story that makes sense of the surprising event. System 2 is also credited with the continuous monitoring of your own behavior—the control that keeps you polite when you are angry, and alert when you are driving at night. System 2 is mobilized to increased effort when it detects an error about to be made. Remember a time when you almost blurted out an offensive remark and note how hard you worked to restore control. In summary, most of what you (your System 2) think and do originates in your System 1, but System 2 takes over when things get difficult, and it normally has the last word. The division of labor between System 1 and System 2 is highly efficient: it minimizes effort and optimizes performance. The arrangement works well most of the time because System 1 is
  • 22. generally very good at what it does: its models of familiar situations are accurate, its short-term predictions are usually accurate as well, and its initial reactions to challenges are swift and generally appropriate. System 1 has biases, however, systematic errors that it is prone to make in specified circumstances. As we shall see, it sometimes answers easier questions than the one it was asked, and it has little understanding of logic and statistics. One further limitation of System 1 is that it cannot be turned off. If you are shown a word on the screen in a language you know, you will read it—unless your attention is totally focused elsewhere. Conflict Figure 2 is a variant of a classic experiment that produces a conflict between the two systems. You should try the exercise before reading on. Figure 2 21 You were almost certainly successful in saying the correct words in both tasks, and you surely discovered that some parts of each task were much easier than others. When you identified upper- and lowercase, the left-hand column was easy and the right- hand column caused you to slow down and perhaps to stammer or stumble. When you named the position of words, the left-hand column
  • 23. was difficult and the right-hand column was much easier. These tasks engage System 2, because saying “upper/lower” or “right/left” is not what you routinely do when looking down a column of words. One of the things you did to set yourself for the task was to program your memory so that the relevant words (upper and lower for the first task) were “on the tip of your tongue.” The prioritizing of the chosen words is effective and the mild temptation to read other words was fairly easy to resist when you went through the first column. But the second column was different, because it contained words for which you were set, and you could not ignore them. You were mostly able to respond correctly, but overcoming the competing response was a strain, and it slowed you down. You experienced a conflict between a task that you intended to carry out and an automatic response that interfered with it. Conflict between an automatic reaction and an intention to conWhetion to ctrol it is common in our lives. We are all familiar with the experience of trying not to stare at the oddly dressed couple at the neighboring table in a restaurant. We also know what it is like to force our attention on a boring book, when we constantly find ourselves returning to the point at which the reading lost its meaning. Where winters are hard, many drivers have memories of their car skidding out of control on the ice and of the struggle to follow well-rehearsed instructions that negate what they would naturally do: “Steer into the skid, and whatever you do, do not touch the brakes!” And every human being has had the experience of not telling someone to go to
  • 24. hell. One of the tasks of System 2 is to overcome the impulses of System 1. In other words, System 2 is in charge of self-control. Illusions To appreciate the autonomy of System 1, as well as the distinction between impressions and beliefs, take a good look at figure 3. This picture is unremarkable: two horizontal lines of different lengths, with fins appended, pointing in different directions. The bottom line is obviously longer than the one above it. That is what we all see, and we naturally believe what we see. If you have already encountered this image, however, you recognize it as the famous Müller-Lyer illusion. As you can easily confirm by measuring them with a ruler, the horizontal lines are in fact identical in length. Figure 3 22 Now that you have measured the lines, you—your System 2, the conscious being you call “I”— have a new belief: you know that the lines are equally long. If asked about their length, you will say what you know. But you still see the bottom line as longer. You have chosen to believe the measurement, but you cannot prevent System 1 from doing its thing; you cannot decide to see the lines as equal, although you know they are. To resist the
  • 25. illusion, there is only one thing you can do: you must learn to mistrust your impressions of the length of lines when fins are attached to them. To implement that rule, you must be able to recognize the illusory pattern and recall what you know about it. If you can do this, you will never again be fooled by the Müller-Lyer illusion. But you will still see one line as longer than the other. Not all illusions are visual. There are illusions of thought, which we call cognitive illusions. As a graduate student, I attended some courses on the art and science of psychotherapy. During one of these lectures, our teacher imparted a morsel of clinical wisdom. This is what he told us: “You will from time to time meet a patient who shares a disturbing tale of multiple mistakes in his previous treatment. He has been seen by several clinicians, and all failed him. The patient can lucidly describe how his therapists misunderstood him, but he has quickly perceived that you are different. You share the same feeling, are convinced that you understand him, and will be able to help.” At this point my teacher raised his voice as he said, “Do not even think of taking on this patient! Throw him out of the office! He is most likely a psychopath and you will not be able to help him.” Many years later I learned that the teacher had warned us against psychopathic charm, and the leading authority in the strn y in the udy of psychopathy confirmed that the teacher’s advice was sound. The analogy to the Müller-Lyer illusion is close. What we were being taught was not how to feel about that patient. Our teacher took it for granted that
  • 26. the sympathy we would feel for the patient would not be under our control; it would arise from System 1. Furthermore, we were not being taught to be generally suspicious of our feelings about patients. We were told that a strong attraction to a patient with a repeated history of failed treatment is a danger sign—like the fins on the parallel lines. It is an illusion—a cognitive illusion—and I (System 2) was taught how to recognize it and advised not to believe it or act on it. The question that is most often asked about cognitive illusions is whether they can be overcome. The message of these examples is not encouraging. Because System 1 operates automatically and cannot be turned off at will, errors of intuitive thought are often difficult to prevent. Biases cannot always be avoided, because System 2 may have no clue to the error. Even when cues to likely errors are available, errors can be prevented only by the enhanced monitoring and effortful activity of System 2. As a way to live your life, however, continuous vigilance is not necessarily good, and it is certainly impractical. Constantly questioning our own thinking would be impossibly tedious, and System 2 is much too slow and inefficient to serve as a substitute for System 1 in making routine decisions. The best we can do is a compromise: learn to recognize situations in which mistakes are likely and try harder to avoid significant mistakes when the stakes are high. The premise of this book is that it is easier to recognize other people’s mistakes than our own. Useful Fictions
  • 27. You have been invited to think of the two systems as agents within the mind, with their individual personalities, abilities, and limitations. I will often use sentences in which the systems are the subjects, such as, “System 2 calculates products.” The use of such language is considered a sin in the professional circles in which I travel, because it seems to explain the thoughts and actions of a person by the thoughts and actions of little 23 people inside the person’s head. Grammatically the sentence about System 2 is similar to “The butler steals the petty cash.” My colleagues would point out that the butler’s action actually explains the disappearance of the cash, and they rightly question whether the sentence about System 2 explains how products are calculated. My answer is that the brief active sentence that attributes calculation to System 2 is intended as a description, not an explanation. It is meaningful only because of what you already know about System 2. It is shorthand for the following: “Mental arithmetic is a voluntary activity that requires effort, should not be performed while making a left turn, and is associated with dilated pupils and an accelerated heart rate.” Similarly, the statement that “highway driving under routine conditions is left to System 1” means that steering the car around a bend is automatic and almost effortless. It also implies that an
  • 28. experienced driver can drive on an empty highway while conducting a conversation. Finally, “System 2 prevented James from reacting foolishly to the insult” means that James would have been more aggressive in his response if his capacity for effortful control had been disrupted (for example, if he had been drunk). System 1 and System 2 are so central to the story I tell in this book that I must make it absolutely clear that they are217at they a fictitious characters. Systems 1 and 2 are not systems in the standard sense of entities with interacting aspects or parts. And there is no one part of the brain that either of the systems would call home. You may well ask: What is the point of introducing fictitious characters with ugly names into a serious book? The answer is that the characters are useful because of some quirks of our minds, yours and mine. A sentence is understood more easily if it describes what an agent (System 2) does than if it describes what something is, what properties it has. In other words, “System 2” is a better subject for a sentence than “mental arithmetic.” The mind—especially System 1—appears to have a special aptitude for the construction and interpretation of stories about active agents, who have personalities, habits, and abilities. You quickly formed a bad opinion of the thieving butler, you expect more bad behavior from him, and you will remember him for a while. This is also my hope for the language of systems. Why call them System 1 and System 2 rather than the more descriptive “automatic system” and “effortful system”? The reason is simple: “Automatic system”
  • 29. takes longer to say than “System 1” and therefore takes more space in your working memory. This matters, because anything that occupies your working memory reduces your ability to think. You should treat “System 1” and “System 2” as nicknames, like Bob and Joe, identifying characters that you will get to know over the course of this book. The fictitious systems make it easier for me to think about judgment and choice, and will make it easier for you to understand what I say. Speaking of System 1 and System 2 “He had an impression, but some of his impressions are illusions.” “This was a pure System 1 response. She reacted to the threat before she recognized it.” “This is your System 1 talking. Slow down and let your System 2 take control.” 24 Stripped down to its essentials, business is about one thing: making decisions. We’re always deciding something - from the small and the daily ( which emails to answer, what meetings to have ) to the macro and the strategic ( what product to launch and when ) to the intensely personal ( what job to take, whom to hire, whom to marry ). But what does it take to make a “good” decision? Is it about going by facts and percentages - or about following your gut instinct? Does time produce better
  • 30. decisions, or does pressure make you decide not only faster but also more wisely? And finally, is better decision making some- thing that you can learn? We asked 11 decisive leaders, inside and outside of business, to answer these and other, related questions. Does their advice help? You decide. Ed Koch Partner Robinson Silverman Pearce Aronsohn & Berman LLP New York, New York To make good decisions, you need confidence in your judg- ment. We all make bad decisions, but the important thing is not to worry too much about them. Otherwise, you’ll never do a thing. I was mayor of New York City for 12 years. I had a $28 billion budget. Each decision I made affected seven and a half million people. The stakes were high. I had to show confidence - in particular, confidence in my decision-making ability - because a lot of people had put their trust in me. The worst decision I ever made was to run for governor of New York State in 1982. I did it on a lark, and it was stupid. Thank god the peo- ple of New York had enough sense to understand that I didn’t have my heart in that race - and to vote against me. They did the right thing, and I was happier because of it. As a judge, I have to decide on the credibility of two people standing before me. I don’t know who is telling the truth, but I know that one of them is lying. That’s when making decisions gets sticky. Sure, the law helps me make such decisions. But there have been many times when I’ve had a hell of a time deciding. Even so, I never base my courtroom decisions on gut instinct. I go by the evidence - or the lack of evidence - and by the applicable law. Ed Koch was mayor of New York City from 1978 to 1989. Today he writes a weekly column for the New York Daily
  • 31. News, has his own radio talk show, and is the judge on the television program “The People’s Court.” Pamela Lopker Chairman and President QAD Inc. Carpinteria, California I make bad decisions all the time. But I’ve been successful because I’ve developed a process for identifying and changing those decisions quickly. I approach every decision with an eye to the long-term outcome. That’s a hard method to adopt in a fast-paced business environment. But it’s the only way to create sustainable value on either a professional or a personal level. The quick-fix method that I see so many companies rely on is just that - a quick fix. There’s a big difference between invest- ing heavily in marketing the product that you have today - with no sense of what it takes to survive tomorrow - and investing heavily in R&D today so that you can have tomorrow’s product. Even in my personal life, I looked at decisions from the per- spective of what’s going to be better for me over the long term. When I was in college, I needed to decide on a career path. While most of my peers were concerned with following their passion, with doing what they “loved” to do, I was looking for a career that would support me financially over the long run. Sometimes making important decisions is just a matter of ask- ing some basic questions: What are my skills? Where is a cer- tain industry going? What should I be doing now to get to where I want to be later? Because I was always strong in math and in analytical thinking, I figured that I would do statistical or actuarial work. Then I realized that statistics wasn’t a boom- ing industry. Computer science seemed like a better long-term bet, so I changed my major.
  • 32. As a leader, I try not to make decisions for others. Sure, being a dictator is often the fastest way to get things done. But it’s not a process that allows an organization to sustain growth. I want the people in my organization to learn the lessons that come with making decisions: that everything is a compromise, that noth- ing is ever completely logical, but that you can deal with things through a logical decision-making process. Pamela Lopker has been called America’s “richest self-made 1© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC • Visit: http://www.fastcompany.com/learning FC :L E A R N I N G Decisions, Decisions Unit of One BY ANNA MUOIO First appeared: FC18, p.93 woman.” QAD, a $172 million enterprise resource-planning software company, is one of the 30 largest public companies to be led by a female CEO. Its clients include Avon Products, PepsiCo, Ford, and Johnson & Johnson. W. Brian Arthur Citibank Professor Santa Fe Institute Santa Fe, New Mexico I received my PhD in operations research, which is a highly sci- entific, mathematical way of strategizing and of making deci-
  • 33. sions. I once thought that I could make any decision, whether professional or personal, by using decision trees, game theory, and optimization. Over time, I’ve changed my mind. For the day-to-day work of running a business - scheduling a fleet of oil tankers, choosing where to open a new factory - scientific deci- sion theory works pretty well. But for just about every other kind of decision, it doesn’t work at all. For the big decisions in life, you need to reach a deeper region of consciousness. Making decisions then becomes not so much about “deciding” as about letting an inner wisdom emerge. We’ve been bamboozled into believing that cognition is ration- al - that our mind is a gigantic computer, or a blackboard on which we can reach a decision by calculating pluses and minuses. Recent research on cognition shows that our minds rarely make strictly logical deductions. Instead, we rely on pat- terns - and on feelings associated with those patterns. So for those big decisions - Should I marry this person? Should I follow that career? Should I sell my company? When should we go public? - let patterns develop in your mind. Let clues and evidence emerge from your environment. This approach to decision making requires time, patience, and another key ingredient: courage. It takes courage to listen to your inner wis- dom. But once you hear that wisdom, making a decision becomes fairly easy. W. Brian Arthur’s work on the economics of increasing returns and their role in technology-based industries has won him international recognition in the scientific and business com- munities. He is a pioneer of the new science of complexity and the author of The Economy as an Evolving Complex System II ( Addison-Wesley, 1997 ). Chung-Jen Tan Senior Manager, Application Systems
  • 34. IBM’s Thomas J. Watson Research Center Yorktown Heights, New York I’ve learned a lot about decision making by watching how a machine plays chess. In 1996, Deep Blue played Garry Kasparov, the world chess champion. Deep Blue won the first game but lost the match. That experience taught us what we needed to do to be ready for the rematch in 1997. A human uses a combination of knowledge, strategy, and intu- ition to make chess decisions. A machine relies on brute com- putational power and on an ability to examine a tremendous amount of data. On average, the human mind can manage three or four positions per second. Deep Blue can evaluate 200 million alternatives per second. So the machine has a huge advantage. But Deep Blue also had weaknesses, and they cen- tered on its inability to be flexible. Our challenge was to equip Deep Blue to make decisions like a human grandmaster. We built in several programming tools that allowed us to adjust some of the decision parameters dynamically and to change strategies in between games. Joel Benjamin, the U.S. chess champion, played chess with Deep Blue for nine months - test- ing and tunneling the system - and we programmed some of his knowledge into the computer. An interesting thing happened when Deep Blue met Kasparov for the rematch. While we had refined Deep Blue so that it could make decisions and play more like a human, Kasparov had refined his strategy so that he could play better against standard computer-chess programs. In game six, Deep Blue surprised Kasparov by sacrificing a knight to gain strategic advantage. Kasparov, who hadn’t planned for such a decision, realized how inflexible his own strategy had become. We showed that a machine can reach a level of play comparable
  • 35. with that of a grandmaster . Chung-Jen Tan manages IBM’s Deep Blue computer-chess project. Since 1984, Tan has worked on the architecture devel- opment and machine design of highly parallel, scalable sys- tems for solving large, complex problems. Deborah Triant CEO and President Check Point Software Technologies Inc. Redwood City, California Making and implementing decisions boils down to a key ingre- dient: listening. I often wonder why schools emphasize debating. Why not have listening classes as well? Debating is easy; listening with an open mind is not. The worst thing that you as a leader can do in the decision-making process is to voice your opinion before anyone else can. No matter how open and honest your people are, stating your opinion first will short-change the discussion process and taint what you hear later. I’ve learned this the hard way. FC :L E A R N I N G 2© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC There are two classes of decisions. The first is the easy kind. For instance, how do you choose which features to include in the next release of your product? It’s a pretty straightforward process: Survey your customers, identify the information you need, and formulate a decision by using rational, objective
  • 36. measurements. The second class is the tough kind. Fo r instance: Should you hire this person? Should you go after this key market? Should you switch jobs? Should you have a kid? These are bet-your-life decisions for which there’s no obvious right or wrong answer - and no way of gathering objective infor- mation. To tackle decisions of either class, I first kick into a talkative mode. I ask everybody I know to give me an opinion. I get dif- ferent perspectives - and I listen to them. I also listen to that thing called intuition. Some people warn against that approach. But there’s a kind of processing that happens in a far deeper place than logical processing does. Intuition offers a way to integrate and synthesize, to weigh and balance informa- tion. If I have to make a big decision, I listen to what others think. But ultimately, I listen to my intuition. I postpone a deci- sion until I wake up one morning and know where my gut is going. Deborah Triant was president and CEO of Sitka Corp., a sub- sidiary of Sun Microsystems, before she joined Adobe Systems as president of marketing in 1993. In 1995, she left Adobe to head Check Point, a network-security and traffic-management software company. Roger Rainbow VP, Global Business Environment Shell International Ltd. London, England Nothing can paralyze a decision-making process more than uncertainty can. The big decisions that have failed at Shell did- n’t fail because of our operations or because of project man- agement; they failed because we misunderstood the external world. That’s why, when we’re on the verge of a big decision,
  • 37. we do scenario planning. A scenario is valuable because it’s a plausible, coherent story that we can use to articulate why we want to do something and which issues to factor into the deci- sion-making process. Everyone has two things in mind when approaching a big deci- sion. The first thing is a scenario - a view of how the world will turn out. The second thing is an objective or a strategy. A good decision-making process marries those two things. Decisions fall into three categories. With the first category, you know a lot. Take a plant that’s been operating for 20 years. You know what makes it succeed or fail, and the decisions you take regarding it tend to be fairly straightforward. They may be hard to implement, but they’re simple to make. With the second category, you know something but not every- thing. This is where scenario planning helps. For instance, if we’re trying to decide whether to invest in an oil field in Russia, we talk about which factors are uncertain and why. What has to happen in order for this investment to succeed? What could go wrong? What would the consequences be? Are any of those consequences absolutely catastrophic? If so, how can we guard against them? With the third category, you know virtually nothing, and you’re starting from scratch. This is where intuition comes into play - and where you make truly creative decisions: You’re deciding on things that haven’t been done before. Each category of decisions requires different skills and process- es. Understand which kind of decision you’re facing, and use scenarios to narrow the range of uncertainty.
  • 38. Roger Rainbow ( [email protected] ) joined Shell in 1970. His team provides analyses of the business environ- ment by preparing long-term scenarios that consider econom- ic, social, technological, and environmental issues, as well as the long-term supply and demand of energy sources. Chris Newell Executive Director, the Lotus Institute Lotus Development Corp. Cambridge, Massachusetts The crux of making good decisions isn’t doing things right - it’s making sure that you focus on the right things. As citizens of the Information Age, we’re overwhelmed with data, reports, facts - much of it noise that we need to filter out before we can figure out what’s relevant to a decision. So I’ve learned to hone my discrimination skills. But if you’re exploring something that has no precedent - an idea, a product, a service - you can’t rely solely on data to guide you. You have to rely on people too. I’ve developed a personal advisory council that I consult before tackling a big decision, a group of people who are anchored by similar values - though not necessarily by similar perspectives. Sometimes making the “wrong” decision is the right thing to do. Last year, I decided to overrun my budget - to continue investing in LearningSpace, a product that Lotus was unsure about investing in. I knew it was the right thing to do, even though I couldn’t get the company to agree. When news of my decision surfaced on the radar of some key people, I got ham- mered. At times, I even doubted my own decision. A year later, LearningSpace has become one of Lotus’s most important 3© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC FC :L E A R N I N G
  • 39. strategic products. I made one mistake: I didn’t communicate all of the implica- tions of going over budget. Otherwise, I wouldn’t change a thing about what I did. Chris Newell founded the Lotus Institute in 1994. Its mission is to explore ways in which groupware technology intersects with cultural issues. It has developed two applications: TeamRoom, which supports virtual teaming; and L e a r ningSpace, which supports collaborative, di s t r i b u t i v e learning. Buz Mertes VP, Loss Management and Policy Servicing GE Capital Mortgage Insurance Corp. Raleigh, North Carolina By transforming the art of decision making into a science, you can save time, money, and frustration. My team handles tough decisions every day. It has to decide whether we can “cure” a loan for someone who has stopped making payments on it or whether we have to recommend foreclosure because of real financial trouble. For a long time, we couldn’t figure out what allowed some of our reps to make better decisions than others did. We pulled six of our best reps off their jobs and analyzed their decision-mak- ing processes for 10 months. We found that these reps had rad- ically different styles. Our challenge was to figure out the criti- cal processes that drove their decisions and then to make sure that those decisions were driven by factual, up-to-date informa-
  • 40. tion. We had a goal: to become more consistent in our decision making and to improve our customers’ perception of us. We ended up creating a software program, called Loss Mitigation Optimizer, to aid us in that process. Turning decision making into a science saved us time and money. Before we implemented this technology, we were cur- ing fewer than 30% of our cases. Now we cure more than 50% of them. The decision-making process itself takes 30% to 50% less time per deal. Our savings jumped dramatically - by about $8,000 per case. In the project’s first 18 months, these addi- tional savings came to $15 million in net income. Some peo- ple think that decisions of this type can’t be driven by data. Our results show otherwise. Buz Mertes joined GE in 1984. Since 1996, Mertes has spon- sored a series of projects that have increased the company’s net income by more than $20 million. Jerry Seeman Senior Director of Officiating National Football League New York, New York As a referee, I found that the pressure was the same whether I was officiating a preseason game or a Super Bowl. Being on the field is like being in a fishbowl: Everyone - players, coaches, fans, the media - is waiting for your decision. A successful call depends on three things: You must be in position, you must have a deep knowledge of the game, and you must have intense concentration. Remember, no matter what the reaction to your decision may be, you answer to only one thing: your con- science. Above all, when making a decision, you have to keep your cool.
  • 41. One of the biggest errors that I see officials fall into is making calls too quickly. Each decision has two phases: You read and analyze the play, and then you make the call. But when things happen in a split second, it can be tempting to throw a penalty flag before you know what happened. That’s why I tell all of my officials to work in “cruise control.” The fans may go crazy, the players and coaches may get excited, but there should be seven people on the field who work every game the same way from beginning to end - and who exude a quiet confidence. Once you make a decision, you’d better be able to communi- cate it. I remember having to make a call on a bizarre play in a playoff game between Houston and Cleveland. After an attempted onside kick, the kicking team illegally touched the ball, which then went out of bounds. The rules dictate that the kicking team should kick again. That team again tried an onside kick, and again it illegally touched the ball - but this time, it recovered the ball. I called that the receiving team would gain control of the ball on the spot where it was illegal- ly touched. This was a complicated play: The NFL probably hasn’t seen another of its kind in the past 15 years. At first, the fans didn’t understand my decision. But I explained to them what the proper ruling was, and they accepted it. I was confi- dent about my knowledge of the game. And I didn’t lose my cool. Jerry Seeman, one of the most celebrated referees in football history, became an NFL line judge in 1975. He was appointed referee in 1979. His responsibilities now cover the league’s supervisory staff, as well as regional game observers, collegiate scouts, and all game officials. Max Bazerman J. Jay Gerber Distinguished Professor of Dispute Resolution and Organizations Kellogg Graduate School of Management
  • 42. Evanston, Illinois When making a decision, don’t listen to your intuition. Intuition will lead you astray; it’s drastically overrated. The desire to follow intuition reflects the mythology of people who 4© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC FC :L E A R N I N G don’t want to think rationally and systematically. They tell sto- ries about how their intuition guided them through a decision, but they don’t understand how or why. Often, when you hear about intuition, what you’re really hearing is a justification of luck. Intuition might be fine for the small decisions in life - like what kind of ice cream to buy. But when you get to the biggies, you need a more systematic thought process. One of the biggest challenges I face is teaching people how to change their decision-making behavior. Everyone makes mis- takes and bad decisions. I try to show people that their mistakes are predictable. Here’s an interesting thought experiment: You’ve got to decide between two things - two executives to hire for a key position, two acquisition targets, two houses that you’re thinking about buying. You go through a systematic process of identifying all of the fac- tors and weighing them for each alternative. You do a little arithmetic, and your analysis says to pick A over B. But your intuition says to pick B over A. So what do you do?
  • 43. Most people will go on their intuition - which raises the ques- tion, Why did you do all that work in the first place? One of these two options is wrong. So postpone your decision until you can determine why your intuition is out of sync with the sys- tematic analysis. That’s the purpose of systematic analysis: to inform your intuition, to make you consider all of the options, and to help you make a wise decision. Max Bazerman ( [email protected] ) studies decision mak- ing, negotiation, and environmental issues. He is the author of Judgment in Managerial Decision Making ( John Wiley & Sons, 1986 ) and Why Smart People Make Dumb Money Moves ( forthcoming from Wiley, April 1999 ). Bazerman is a visiting professor of business admi nistration at Harvard Business School. Howard Raiffa Professor Emeritus Harvard Business School Boston, Massachusetts One of the biggest pitfalls in decision making is not creating enough alternatives. You can make a decision in order to solve a problem - or you can make a decision in order to exploit an opportunity. To make this point, I use the case study of a busi- nessman named Bill. His dilemma: to sell or not to sell his business. Bill and his partner had struggled for years to build a soundproofing business in Brooklyn, and finally the company was doing well. But Bill was bored; at age 57, he wanted a change. The solution, he thought, was to sell out to his partner, move to California, and start a new business. He framed his problem around how much he should sell his part of the busi- ness for - $400,000 ( a price that his partner could afford ) or something higher.
  • 44. At my suggestion, Bill went through a rigorous process of reframing his decision. He realized that what he wanted was to spend more time outdoors, to be challenged intellectually, and to minimize stress. In the end, Bill decided to move to California and to establish a branch of the soundproofing business there. That way, he could change his life and also be challenged - but without taking on an enormous amount of risk. The business did well: Eight years later, he sold out to his partner for $1.7 million. Howard Raiffa is a pioneer in the development of decision analysis, negotiation analysis, and game theory. He is coauthor, with John S. Hammond and Ralph L. Keeney, of Smart Choices: A Practical Guide to Making Better Decisions ( Harvard Business School Press, 1998 ). 5© All rights reserved. Fast Company, LLC FC :L E A R N I N G