SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 40
Running head: MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS
1
MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS 3
Memo
To: Prof. Thomas Smith
From: Student- Jane Doe
Reference: Health Care Policy
Date: March 18, 2018
Subject: Massachusetts’ Healthcare Reform Act
Massachusetts’ Healthcare Reform Act
Rationale
Massachusetts State is among the states that have made a
number of attempts aimed at reforming the state's healthcare
system to make access to quality healthcare available for its
residents. Recently in 2006, Massachusetts passed the
Healthcare Reform Act, which was later, signed into law by
former Governor Mitt Romney (Van der Wees et al., 2013). The
rationale for this healthcare reform was to provide near-
universal health insurance coverage for Massachusetts’
residents.
Adoption of the Reform
The Massachusetts Healthcare Reform Act was passed by the
State legislators after years of negotiation between Mitt
Romney and the legislators with a compromise reached in 2006
resulting in the enactment of the reform that was effectively
signed into law by Romney on 12 April 206. The reform has
made several changes to its healthcare system in a move aimed
at achieving a near-universal healthcare coverage for the
residents of the state. The first change was made to the state's
Medicaid program that was broadened by providing a
MassHealth waiver, extending health insurance coverage to
children in low-income families with up to 300% of the federal
poverty level (FPL) (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012).
Massachusetts created what is called Commonwealth Care,
which provides the residents of the state with access to
subsidized health insurance for eligible individuals with
earnings below 300% of FPL. Under this new healthcare reform,
individuals with income below 150% of FPL also have the
option of selecting a plan without a monthly premium and low-
cost sharing. However, eligible individuals with earnings falling
between 150-300% PL are subsidized by the state using a
sliding scale.
The Massachusetts Healthcare Reform Act also saw the state
expand its Insurance Partnership Program by providing
incentives and subsidies to the employers to give and workers to
enroll in the state's employer-sponsored insurance. In this
respect, Massachusetts State subsidized insurance costs for the
workers in the state who would otherwise be eligible for
programs subsidized by the government. However, small
businesses are only eligible for up to $1,000 in support per
qualified worker who falls below the 300% FPL (Van der Wees
et al., 2013). Under the program, the state government pays the
portion of qualified workers' premiums that is equal to what the
employees would be expected to pay if employees were on a
subsidized plan. Additionally, under this new healthcare reform,
any employer in the state who fails to provide health insurance
to its workers is expected to pay what is called a ‘fair share'
assessment to the government of up to $295 per worker every
year (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012).
The reform also created what is called the Commonwealth
Health Insurance Connector whose primary aim is to link those
without access to employer-sponsored insurance and companies
with 50 or fewer employees that provide insurance coverage for
its workers. According to this health reform, small businesses
with 50 of fewer employees have the option of buying insurance
coverage on their own or via the Connector (Rapoza, 2012).
Funding Structure
Although Romney and the state legislators agreed on most of
the components of the bill, agreeing on how this healthcare
reform would be financed was a major issue as it was clear that
financing the reform would result in an increase in healthcare
cost. However, following a compromise that was reached, the
state legislators agreed that the reform would be financed by
individuals, employers and the government. First, the
Massachusetts Healthcare Reform is funded by the existing
$320 million obtained in hospital assessments and covered
levies (Van der Wees et al., 2013). Second, the Massachusetts
state legislators agreed that the health reform would also be
financed through by federal safety-net payments of $610 million
as well as federal matching payments on the MassHealth
expansion. Additionally, part of the money to be used in
financing the health care reform is to come from rate increases
projected at $299 million. Further, $295 fair assessment for
employers per employee and the Free Rider Surcharge also
generates revenue used to finance the ambitious health care
reform in Massachusetts (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012).
Impacts
The impacts of this Massachusetts Healthcare Reform Act have
been so profound. The first major achievement of this
healthcare reform is that it has increased access to affordable
coverage to residents of Massachusetts. Because the law
requires all residents of Massachusetts to have a health
insurance or pay a fine, the law had seen more that 99% of the
residents of the state now get health insurance coverage up from
90% before this healthcare reform was introduced. According to
Rapoza (2012), prior to 2006, more than 24% of low-income
residents of Massachusetts had no health insurance. However,
by 2012, only 8% of low-income adults in the state were still
without healthcare coverage. Overall, about 650,000
Massachusetts residents who lacked health insurance are now
covered.
Another significant achievement of the Massachusetts health
insurance is that it has increased insurance status of higher
income persons for the self-employed who did not qualify for
MassHealth. According to Urban Institute, the population of
higher income earners who were without health insurance before
2006 has dropped from 5% then to below 1% three years after
the reform (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012).
The only notable shortcoming of this healthcare reform is the
cost burden associated with its implementation. The health cost
in the state has risen to a historic high following the
introduction of this healthcare reform was introduced. By 2007,
just one year after the reform, Massachusetts healthcare
expenditure accounted for about 15.2% of its GDP, which is
higher than the nation's average of 13.7% as a whole (Kaiser
Family Foundation, 2012).
References
Kaiser Family Foundation. (2012). Massachusetts health care
reform: Six years later. Retrieved from
https://kaiserfamilyfoundation.files.wordpress.com/2013/01/831
1.pdf
Rapoza, K. (2012, Jan. 20). If ObamaCare is so bad, how does
RomneyCare survive? Forbes p. 1
https://www.forbes.com/sites/kenrapoza/2012/01/20/romney-
care-massachusetts-healthcare-reform/#3d6701195b00
Van der Wees, P. J., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Ayanian, J. Z. (2013).
Improvements in health status after Massachusetts health care
reform. The Milbank Quarterly, 91(4), 663–689.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29 (2005), 436–445.
Blackwell Publishing. Printed in the USA.
Copyright C! 2005 Division 35, American Psychological
Association. 0361-6843/05
ATTITUDES TOWARD TRADITIONAL
AND NONTRADITIONAL PARENTS
Victoria L. Brescoll and Eric Luis Uhlmann
Yale University
Three studies investigated attitudes toward traditional parents
(stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers) and nontra-
ditional parents (stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers)
among adult men and women. Using a between-subjects
design, Study 1 found that nontraditional parents were liked
significantly less than traditional parents. Participants
also believed that stay-at-home fathers were not regarded highly
by others. Study 2 replicated these results using a
within-subjects design, suggesting that participants felt little
compunction about expressing negative attitudes toward
nontraditional parents. Study 3 further found that employed
mothers were less disliked when described as working out
of financial necessity rather than for personal fulfillment. Both
male and female participants reported negative evalu-
ations of employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers,
suggesting that prescriptive gender role stereotypes represent a
consensual ideology shared by men and women.
For the past three decades, conservatives have argued that
feminism has caused society to devalue women’s tradi-
tional roles, such as homemaking and caring for children
(Robertson, 2000; Schlafly, 2003). Recently, some have even
asserted that society has stigmatized stay-at-home mothers
because they are not pursuing careers outside the home.
“Stay-at-home moms are used to the silent snubs they re-
ceive from mothers who decide to pursue careers—as if
they were nothing but pre-feminist breeders who don’t
lead worthwhile lives” (Miller & Ponnuru, 2001). According
to this perspective, society’s stigmatization of stay-at-home
mothers has discouraged women from staying home to raise
their children and, more generally, has contributed to the
devaluing of the traditional American family.
Psychological theories of gender take a starkly different
position by hypothesizing that people respond negatively
to men and women who do not conform to traditional
gender roles (Deaux & Major, 1987; Eagly & Karau,
2002; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Prentice & Carranza, 2002;
Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999; Russo, 1976;
Silverstein, 1996). This is because gender stereotypes do
Victoria L. Brescoll and Eric Luis Uhlmann, Department of Psy-
chology, Yale University.
Both authors were supported by graduate research fellowships
from the National Science Foundation while this research was
conducted.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Victoria
Brescoll, Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2
Hillhouse
Ave., New Haven, CT 06520. E-mail: [email protected]
not just describe how men and women behave, but also
prescribe gender appropriate behavior. For example, when
women lead in a masculine manner (e.g., authoritatively)
they are judged more harshly than men who lead in the
same way (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992). Likewise,
women who self-promote and behave agentically are liked
less than women who behave in stereotypically feminine
ways, and agentic women suffer a “backlash effect” as a
result (Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999).
Mothers who are employed full-time outside the home
and fathers who stay home to care for children occupy non-
traditional gender roles. Contemporary psychological the-
ories predict that they would be stigmatized, as are agen-
tic, “masculine” women (Eagly et al., 1992; Rudman, 1998;
Rudman & Glick, 1999), because these nontraditional par-
ents are violating prescriptive gender stereotypes.
Previous research has examined people’s beliefs about
and perceptions of mothers but has not directly addressed
whether certain types of parents, such as stay-at-home
mothers and fathers, are disliked or stigmatized (Bridges &
Etaugh, 1995; Bridges & Orza, 1993; Etaugh & Poertner,
1992; Etaugh & Nekolny, 1990; Etaugh & Folger, 1998;
Etaugh & Moss, 2001; Etaugh & Petroski, 1985). This re-
search has varied women’s marital status (divorced vs. sin-
gle vs. married), work status (full-time vs. part-time; con-
tinuously employed vs. interrupted employment), prestige
of job (moderate vs. low), and parental status (children
vs. no children) and has revealed a number of interest-
ing findings. For example, married women are seen as
better adjusted and more nurturant than divorced women
(Etaugh & Nekolny, 1990; Etaugh & Poertner, 1992, 1991)
436
Attitudes Toward Parents 437
and mothers are seen as more nurturant but less compe-
tent than nonmothers (Etaugh & Poertner, 1991, 1992).
Furthermore, continuously employed mothers are evalu-
ated more negatively than mothers who interrupted their
employment to care for their children because people view
continuously employed mothers as less committed to their
maternal role (Bridges & Etaugh, 1995). These findings
suggest that people’s evaluations of mothers depend on their
perceived level of commitment to their children.
The present research builds upon and extends this previ-
ous work in six major ways. First, past studies have focused
primarily on documenting people’s stereotypes and beliefs
about parents, rather than their attitudes toward them. This
emphasis on cognition at the expense of affect is unfortu-
nate given that attitudes predict discriminatory judgments
and behavior more effectively than do stereotypes (Fiske,
1998; Stangor, Sullivan, & Ford, 1991; for a meta-analytic
review, see Dovidio, Brigham, Johnson, & Gaertner, 1996).
Accordingly, the present studies explored people’s affective
reactions to mothers and fathers who occupy traditional and
nontraditional roles. We expected that individuals who vio-
late prescriptive gender stereotypes (i.e., employed moth-
ers and stay-at-home fathers) would elicit negative affective
reactions.
Second, we examined reactions to stay-at-home and em-
ployed fathers. Prior research was primarily concerned with
how responses to mothers vary based on their employment
status (see Bridges, Etaugh, & Barnes-Farrell, 2002, for
an exception). To some extent, this emphasis is reason-
able because there are far more employed mothers than
there are stay-at-home fathers (U.S. Census Bureau, 2002).
However, the number of fathers who stay at home to care
for the children while their wife works outside the home
is steadily increasing, rising 18% since 1994 (U.S. Cen-
sus Bureau, 2002). Moreover, personal and social preju-
dices against stay-at-home fathers are directly relevant to
women’s options because they may make fathers unwilling
to assume a homemaker role while their wife works outside
the home. Indeed, the stigma against stay-at-home fathers
may contribute to some fathers’ unwillingness to stay home
full-time with their children out of fear that they may en-
counter problems when trying to re-enter the workplace
(Duindam, 1999; “Stay-at-home dads,” 2003).
Third, we assessed people’s beliefs about society’s re-
action to nontraditional parents. Perceived cultural norms
have been shown to predict behavior above and beyond
personal attitudes (Ajzen, 1996), moderate the expression
of personally endorsed attitudes (Sechrist & Stangor, 2001),
and strongly influence automatic judgments and behaviors
(Correll, Park, Judd, & Wittenbrink, 2002; Devine, 1989;
Livingston, 2002). To the extent that it is perceived as cul-
turally normative to lack respect for stay-at-home fathers
and/or employed mothers, sexist individuals should be more
likely to express their attitudes, nonsexist individuals should
be more likely to “go along” with sexist social norms, and au-
tomatic “gut” responses are likely to be gender biased. Thus,
it is important to know not only people’s personal reactions
to nontraditional parents but also their beliefs about how
most other people regard such individuals.
Fourth, we investigated the extent to which people feel
a sense of compunction about expressing negative atti-
tudes toward nontraditional parents. Previous work has
shown that people feel strong internal and external pres-
sures not to express racial prejudice or endorse racial stereo-
types (Devine, Monteith, Zuwerink, & Elliot, 1991; Dunton
& Fazio, 1997; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Monteith,
Ashburn-Nardo, Voils, & Czopp, 2002; Plant & Devine,
1998). In fact, discrimination based on race occurs mainly
under ambiguous circumstances when racial biases are eas-
iest to rationalize and justify (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986;
Hodson, Dovidio, & Gaertner, 2002). However, there is rea-
son to believe that such pressures are considerably weaker
when it comes to gender role stereotypes. Because many
men are dependent on women for child rearing and sex-
ual companionship, stereotypes regarding women’s behav-
ior are often more prescriptive than stereotypes of racial
groups (Fiske & Stevens, 1993; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001).
Notably, one recent study found that while people antic-
ipate feeling guilty at having judged a Black person in a
stereotypical manner, they react with amusement at having
stereotyped a woman (Czopp & Monteith, 2003). Thus, in
Study 2 we employed a within-subjects design, presenting
participants with side-by-side descriptions of employed and
stay-at-home mothers and fathers and asking them to pro-
vide their attitudes toward each target. We were particularly
interested in whether participants would express negative
attitudes toward nontraditional parents when such a bias
would be blatant and obvious.
Fifth, we examined the effects of motivations for work-
ing outside the home on attitudes toward both mothers and
fathers (Study 3). Extensive work in the field of moral judg-
ment indicates that people receive less blame for socially
undesirable acts when the behavior is externally compelled
(Weiner, 1995, 1996). Because women who work out of
financial necessity are violating prescriptive gender stereo-
types for situational (i.e., external) reasons, we hypothesized
that they would provoke less negative reactions than women
who seek employment for reasons of personal fulfillment. In
contrast, motivation should have little impact on attitudes
toward employed fathers because, whatever their reason
for employment, they are fulfilling their traditional role.
Although earlier work has shown that mothers who work
outside the home out of financial necessity are perceived
as more communal than employed mothers whose motive
is personal fulfillment (Bridges & Orza, 1992), the present
research is the first to look at attitudes and to use fathers as
well as mothers as targets.
Finally, the present research fills an important gap in the
literature because we used a racially diverse adult sample
(average age = 38 years) rather than college students. Our
participants thus had considerable experience with parent-
ing and working. Moreover, because prejudice varies greatly
across different cohorts (Judd, Park, Ryan, Brauer, & Kraus,
1995; Sears, 1986), adult samples may be necessary to obtain
438 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN
a complete picture of attitudes toward parents (cf. Bridges
et al., 2002). Although a few other studies have examined
adult women’s attitudes toward parents, to our knowledge
the present studies are the first to assess those of adult men.
In summary, three studies examined adult men’s and
women’s affective reactions to traditional parents (em-
ployed fathers and stay-at-home mothers) and nontra-
ditional parents (employed mothers and stay-at-home
fathers). Also investigated were: perceptions of other peo-
ple’s responses to nontraditional parents (Study 1), the ex-
tent to which people feel a sense of compunction about
reporting negative attitudes toward stay-at-home fathers
and employed mothers (Study 2), and the effects of moth-
ers’ and fathers’ motivations for working (Study 3). Taken
together, these studies were designed to investigate prej-
udices against nontraditional parents and explore some of
the potential parameters of such biases.
While in part an effort to build upon and extend prior
work on prescriptive gender stereotyping and perceptions
of parents, the present studies also make important novel
contributions. First, the present work is the first to empir-
ically investigate attitudes toward stay-at-home fathers, a
stigmatized category that has received no attention in the
stereotyping and prejudice literature. Second, the present
research points to a double-standard regarding women’s and
men’s reasons for working. Specifically, in Study 3, we inves-
tigate whether mothers are evaluated more negatively for
choosing to work out of personal fulfillment than fathers
who do the same.
STUDY 1
Study 1 presented participants with a description of either
an employed mother, an employed father, a stay-at-home
mother, or a stay-at-home father. Each paragraph described
either a man’s or a woman’s decision to either stay home to
care for his or her children or to work outside the home. The
target individual was described as being married with two
children who recently had another baby. Participants an-
swered a series of questions about these individuals, includ-
ing an assessment of their affective reaction to the person.
We hypothesized an interaction between gender of target
and decision to work versus stay at home. That is, those who
violated traditional gender roles (i.e., stay-at-home fathers
and employed mothers) were expected to be more nega-
tively evaluated than those who conformed to traditional
gender roles (i.e., stay-at-home mothers and employed
fathers).
In addition to assessing participants’ personal attitudes
toward the target persons, we were interested in measuring
participants’ beliefs about other people’s reactions to em-
ployed and stay-at-home mothers and fathers. Perceived so-
cial norms are powerful influences on judgment and behav-
ior (Ajzen, 1996; Correll et al., 2002; Devine, 1989; Sechrist
& Stangor, 2001). For example, a man who believes that
stay-at-home fathers are not respected may be reluctant
to assume a homemaker role, even though his personal at-
titudes toward staying at home while his wife works are
positive.
In fact, there are reasons to expect stay-at-home fathers
to be held in lower social regard than employed mothers.
Among the most powerful prescriptive stereotypes directed
at men are those that emphasize avoiding “effeminate” be-
haviors (e.g., playing with dolls for young boys, failing to
defend one’s honor for adult men; Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle,
& Schwarz, 1996; Muller & Goldberg, 1980). Stay-at-home
fathers, by adopting a traditionally feminine role, may thus
incur a steep drop in perceived social regard. In contrast,
while an employed woman might be disliked for violating
prescriptive stereotypes, her adoption of the high-status,
traditionally male role of breadwinner may win her some of
the social respect and regard associated with that role (for
a discussion of the distinction between perceived warmth
and perceived competence, see Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu,
2002).
Method
Participants
Seventy-three adults (44 males and 29 females) between the
ages of 17 and 79 years (M = 31.33, SD = 16.64) were re-
cruited from a public park in Connecticut. Seventy-seven
percent of the sample was European American. The re-
maining 23% were African American, Asian, and Hispanic.
Participants were largely middle class (the median income
level was $40,000 per year). Participants were given a lot-
tery ticket, a drink, or paid $1.00 in exchange for participa-
tion in the study. Data from two participants were excluded
because they were not sufficiently fluent in English to com-
plete the survey.
Procedure and Measures
Participants were randomly assigned to read one of four
paragraph-long descriptions. Each paragraph described ei-
ther a man’s or a woman’s decision to either stay home
to care for his or her children (including an infant) or to
work outside the home. A situation in which parents have
an infant to care for was chosen as particularly relevant
to prescriptive stereotypes regarding maternal care of chil-
dren (Russo, 1976). We used two names, John and Jennifer,
which were pretested for comparability. The four para-
graphs were identical in every way except for the decision
made and whether it was a man or a woman making the
decision, yielding a 2 (stay home vs. work outside home) "
2 (male target vs. female target) design. With this design, it
was possible to compare participants’ reactions to stay-at-
home mothers, employed mothers, stay-at-home fathers,
and employed fathers in a between-subjects manner.
Participants answered seven questions assessing their at-
titudes and beliefs about the person described in the para-
graph. These seven items fell into two groups: personal
Attitudes Toward Parents 439
affective evaluations of the target and beliefs about others’
opinions of the target.
Affective evaluations. Five items assessed participants’
affective evaluations of the target. Two items pertained to
whether participants thought the target was a good par-
ent (“John [Jennifer] is a good parent”) and whether the
target was contributing equally to the family’s well-being
(“John [Jennifer] is contributing equally to the family’s well-
being”). Two items assessed beliefs that were especially rel-
evant to employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers. In
particular, it is frequently suggested that employed mothers
are more selfish than stay-at-home mothers (Russo, 1976).
Therefore we included an item assessing this particular be-
lief (“John [Jennifer] is selfish”). Another item focused on
participants’ attitudes toward the target’s decision (“John’s
[Jennifer’s] decision to work was a good one”). These four
items were assessed with 9-point Likert-type scales (1 =
completely disagree, 9 = completely agree). The final item
in this set consisted of a feeling thermometer for the target
person (“On a scale from 0–100, how warmly or coldly do
you feel toward this person? [0 = extremely cold, 50 = neu-
tral, 100 = extremely warm]). Feeling thermometers have
been widely used as a measure of affective evaluation (e.g.,
Eagly, Mladinic, & Otto, 1991; Haddock & Zanna, 1994).
Others’ opinions. Using 9-point Likert-type scales
(1 = completely disagree, 9 = completely agree), two items
were included to assess participants’ perceptions of whether
these roles are stigmatized differently by gender (“John
[Jennifer] is the type of person that others see as success-
ful” and “John’s [Jennifer’s] coworkers will respect his [her]
decision to stay at home with his [her] children”). We refer
to these items throughout as measures of perceived social
regard.
Last, participants completed demographic information,
including gender, race/ethnicity, and age. Participants were
then thanked and debriefed.
Results
We performed a factor analysis on the items so as to de-
scribe the variables more parsimoniously. Visual inspection
of the scree plot and varimax factor analysis revealed two
distinct factors. All of the items had factor loadings over .41
and were therefore retained for further analysis. The first
factor contained the Affective Evaluations items while the
second factor contained the Others’ Opinions items. The
two subscales had moderate internal reliability. Cronbach’s
alpha was .70 for the Affective Evaluation items and .46 for
the two Others’ Opinions items. Taken together, these two
factors accounted for 55% of the total variance. The reason
the Others’ Opinions alpha was low was at least partially
because there were only two items in this measure. It is
common for measures with few items to have reliabilities
in this range (e.g., Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Moreover, the
relatively low reliability of this measure, while certainly not
desirable, does provide a conservative test of our hypoth-
esis that scores would differ significantly by condition. At
the same time, it should be acknowledged that the relia-
bility of this measure is below what is generally considered
psychometrically acceptable, potentially qualifying some of
the present findings.
Affective Evaluations
The five affective evaluation items were standardized and
summed to form a single index. We predicted that partic-
ipants would hold more negative attitudes toward nontra-
ditional parents (i.e., employed mothers and stay-at-home
fathers) than traditional parents (i.e., employed fathers and
stay-at-home mothers). This prediction was tested with a
2 (target gender) " 2 (target role: stay-at-home vs. em-
ployed outside the home) " 2 (participant gender) analysis
of variance (ANOVA) on the Affective Evaluations index.
There were no significant effects involving the gender of
participants so all the data were collapsed across this vari-
able. As predicted, we did not find main effects for tar-
get gender or target role. However, and also as predicted,
results revealed the expected target gender " target role
interaction, F(1, 68) = 4.37, p < .05. Specifically, the non-
traditional parents (employed mother M = #.27, SD = .64;
stay-at-home father M = #.08, SD = .58) were evaluated
more negatively than traditional parents (employed father
M = .09, SD = .77; stay-at-home mother M = .21, SD =
.61). Notably, by using the terms nontraditional versus tra-
ditional parents, we are describing not a main effect, but the
interaction between target role and target gender because
the overlap between these two independent variables cre-
ates the categories nontraditional and traditional parents.
Individual Item Analysis
We also examined each item individually to further explore
our hypotheses. The item that measured pure affect, the
feeling thermometer, revealed the same expected pattern of
results as the overall affective evaluations index. Specifically,
the interaction between target gender and target role was
significant, F(1, 68) = 11.36, p < .01. No main effects were
found for target gender or target role. Participants felt less
warmly toward employed mothers (M = 64.01, SD = 17.30)
and stay-at-home fathers (M = 68.74, SD = 18.11) than
employed fathers (M = 75.28, SD = 19.20) and stay-at-
home mothers (M = 84.11, SD = 11.45).
A planned contrast on the item “Jennifer [John] is a good
parent,” suggested that participants believed that the stay-
at-home father was a worse parent (M = 6.63, SD = 2.00)
than the stay-at-home mother (M = 7.47, SD = 1.84), em-
ployed mother (M = 7.44, SD = 1.55), or the employed
father (M = 7.68, SD = 1.60), t(69) = 1.91, p = .06. As pre-
dicted, a planned contrast also suggested that participants
viewed the employed mother as more selfish (M = 6.50,
SD = 2.42) than the stay-at-home mother (M = 7.48, SD =
1.86), employed father (M = 7.32, SD = 2.03), or
440 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN
stay-at-home father (M = 7.89, SD = 1.66), t(69) = 1.88,
p = .06. However, both of these effects were only marginally
significant. We also predicted that for the item, “John’s
[Jennifer’s] decision to work was a good one,” partici-
pants would view the nontraditional parents’ decision as
worse than the traditional parents’ decision. A 2 " 2
(employed vs. stay-at-home " mother vs. father) ANOVA
supported this hypothesis. The interaction between target
gender and target role was significant, F(1, 69) = 1.64,
p < .05, meaning that the mothers’ decision to work (M =
7.11, SD = 1.67) was seen as worse than the fathers’ deci-
sion to work (M = 7.89, SD = 1.35). Again, no main effects
for target gender or target role were found. Finally, partici-
pants did not rate the target parents significantly differently
on the item, “John [Jennifer] is contributing equally to the
family’s well-being” (p > .05 for all).
Others’ opinions. We standardized the two items as-
sessing others’ opinions and summed them to form a sin-
gle index of perceived social regard. The overall one-way
ANOVA was significant, F(3, 69) = 9.22, p < .001. More
important, the planned contrasts were significant, t(69) =
5.03, p < .001. As expected, participants perceived less so-
cial regard for stay-at-home fathers (M = 4.42, SD = 1.79)
than employed fathers (M = 6.26, SD = 1.31), stay-at-home
mothers (M = 6.03, SD = 1.48), or employed mothers
(M = 6.87, SD = 1.16).
Discussion
Study 1 supported our hypotheses that people hold more
negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents than to-
ward traditional parents. Contradicting claims that modern
society stigmatizes stay-at-home mothers (Robertson, 2000;
Schlafly, 2003), but supporting theories of prescriptive gen-
der stereotyping (e.g., Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Prentice
& Carranza, 2002; Rudman & Glick, 1999), stay-at-home
mothers and employed fathers were evaluated more pos-
itively than stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers.
Presumably because they violate stereotypic prescriptions
for how men and women are supposed to behave and the
roles they ought to fill, nontraditional parents were disliked
relative to traditional parents.
An item analysis provided some potential insights into
people’s perceptions of employed mothers, stay-at-home
fathers, employed fathers, and stay-at-home mothers. Par-
ticipants tended to perceive the stay-at-home father as the
worst parent. Although speculative, it seems possible that
participants saw fathers as lacking the skills to be the pri-
mary caretaker for young children. Also, employed mothers
were seen as more selfish than stay-at-home mothers, em-
ployed fathers, and stay-at-home fathers. Because women’s
prescribed role is to care for children, abandoning this role
may be perceived as an especially selfish act. In contrast, it
seems possible that stay-at-home fathers are seen as rela-
tively unselfish because they are adopting a low-status, stig-
matized role for the sake of the family. So while employed
mothers and stay-at-home fathers were both perceived as
having made a bad decision, somewhat different impres-
sions may underlie this belief (i.e., perceived selfishness
on the part of employed mothers, perceived lack of ability
on the part of stay-at-home fathers). Of course this inter-
pretation is speculative and additional work is required to
clarify the specific cognitions that underlie responses to
nontraditional mothers and fathers.
Interestingly, perceived social regard was lowest for stay-
at-home fathers. Despite evaluating employed mothers
negatively, participants felt that other people would re-
spect employed mothers and perceive them as successful—
perhaps because by assuming the traditionally male “bread-
winner” role they gain some of the social status associated
with that role. This finding is consistent with the distinction
of Fiske et al. (2002) between the perceived warmth and
competence of social targets. Apparently, employed moth-
ers are disliked but respected, whereas stay-at-home fathers
are neither liked nor respected. Fathers appear to be aware
of this stigmatization of stay-at-home fathers because they
report that one of the major reasons they do not take pater-
nity leave is due to the stigma that it will carry (Duindam,
1999). This reluctance on the part of fathers to assume a
homemaker role (even temporarily) may limit mothers’ em-
ployment opportunities and serve as an important barrier
to gender equality both in the home and in the workplace.
An alternative explanation for the present results is that
the statistical infrequency of stay-at-home fathers may ex-
plain why people react to them negatively. Indeed, research
on the mere exposure effect shows that increased familiarity
with a stimulus can increase liking of the stimulus (Zajonc,
1980). Although we certainly do not rule out the possibility
that a lack of familiarity makes some contribution to atti-
tudes toward stay-at-home fathers, this is not a satisfying
explanation for the present results. While employed moth-
ers are far more statistically frequent than stay-at-home fa-
thers, they were not better liked. Stay-at-home fathers were
only rated more negatively than employed mothers when
it came to perceptions of other people’s beliefs. There is
no evidence that familiarity with a stimulus has a greater
influence on perceptions of other people’s attitudes toward
the stimulus than it does on one’s own attitudes. Theories of
prescriptive stereotyping provide a much better account of
the present data than an explanation based on the statistical
frequency of the groups in question.
STUDY 2
Study 1 leaves open the question of whether participants
feel any sense of compunction about expressing negative
attitudes toward nontraditional parents. Previous work has
documented that White people often feel guilty and self-
critical when they have stereotypical reactions to Black
people (Devine et al., 1991; Monteith et al., 2002). Racial
discrimination is rare when such a bias is obvious and,
Attitudes Toward Parents 441
in general, under circumstances that promote socially
desirable responding (Evans, Garcia, Garcia, & Baron,
2003; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Hodson et al., 2002).
However, additional research suggests that people are much
less concerned about discriminating based on gender than
on race (Czopp & Monteith, 2003). In Study 2, we employed
a within-subjects design to determine whether participants
would continue to report negative reactions to stay-at-home
fathers and employed mothers, relative to employed fathers
and stay-at-home mothers, when their evaluations were
assessed at the same time. Under such circumstances, a
bias based on the gender of the employed or stay-at-home
parent would be blatantly obvious. Therefore, any preju-
dice against nontraditional parents that participants express
must occur with relatively little compunction.
Method
Participants
Seventy-nine adults (46 males and 33 females) between the
ages of 17 and 53 years (M = 30.11, SD = 12.62) were re-
cruited from a public park in Connecticut. Participants were
given a lottery ticket, a drink, or paid $1.00 in exchange for
participation in the study. Sixty-five participants were
European American and the remaining 15 participants were
African American, Asian, and Hispanic. None of the partic-
ipants under the age of 30 had children whereas 65% of
the participants over 30 years of age reported having one or
more children. Additionally, 91.14% of the participants re-
ported that they were currently working outside the home,
ranging from 8 hours per week to 65 hours per week (M =
38.22, SD = 13.29).
Procedure and Measures
Participants read four descriptions of parents: an employed
father, an employed mother, a stay-at-home father, and
a stay-at-home mother. These materials were identical to
those used in Study 1 and were presented in a random or-
der. In Study 1, the feeling thermometer rating correlated
.89 with the entire affective evaluation index.
Participants then completed demographic information
(including gender, race/ethnicity, and age) and were then
thanked and debriefed.
Results and Discussion
We expected that even using a within-subjects design, in
which participants evaluated stay-at-home and employed
mothers and fathers at the same time, more positive affect
would be reported toward traditional than nontraditional
parents. These predictions were tested with a mixed model,
within- and between-subjects (with gender of participant as
the between-subjects factor) ANOVA on the feeling ther-
mometer ratings.
There were no significant effects involving the gender of
participants, so all the data were collapsed across participant
gender. However, as predicted, the within-subjects ANOVA
was significant, F(3, 231) = 10.60, p < .01. Within-subjects
contrasts revealed that participants reported significantly
more positive attitudes toward traditional than nontradi-
tional parents. Specifically, attitudes toward stay-at-home
mothers (M = 79.00, SD = 19.10) were significantly more
positive than attitudes toward stay-at-home fathers (M =
65.40, SD = 24.75), F(1, 77) = 19.93, p < .001, and em-
ployed mothers (M = 71.55, SD = 23.96), F(1, 77) = 5.22,
p < .05. Likewise, attitudes toward employed fathers (M =
84.82, SD = 19.01) were significantly more positive than at-
titudes toward employed mothers (M = 71.55, SD = 23.96),
F(1, 77) = 22.30, p < .001, and stay-at-home fathers (M =
65.40, SD = 24.75), F(1, 77) = 27.93, p < .001. As predicted,
attitudes toward the two traditional parents (stay-at-home
mothers and employed fathers) were not significantly differ-
ent from each other nor were attitudes toward the two non-
traditional parents. Notably, participants reported nearly
the same pattern of attitudes in this within-subjects design
as in the between-subjects design in Study 1. Although di-
rect statistical comparisons cannot be made across studies,
assessing attitudes toward parents using a within-subjects
design clearly did not eliminate participants’ self-reported
dislike for nontraditional parents.
STUDY 3
Studies 1 and 2 demonstrated that people generally hold
more positive attitudes toward traditional than nontradi-
tional parents. Study 2 further indicated that people will re-
port negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents even
under conditions that enhance social desirability concerns.
When participants evaluated stay-at-home and employed
mothers and fathers side-by-side, in a within-subjects de-
sign, they continued to report more negative attitudes to-
ward employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers. This
complements research suggesting that people feel little
compunction about stereotyping based on gender (Czopp
& Monteith, 2003).
Study 3 built on Studies 1 and 2 by investigating whether
attitudes toward employed parents would vary based on
the reasons that parents provide for working outside the
home. In Study 3, the target parent was said to work outside
the home either for personal fulfillment or out of financial
necessity. Bridges and Orza (1992) examined reactions to
employed and unemployed mothers while varying their em-
ployment motive. They found that participants perceived an
employed mother who worked out of personal fulfillment
as less communal than the employed mother who worked
out of financial need. We extend this work by looking at at-
titudes rather than trait attributions and further examining
the effects of motivation on reactions to fathers.
Because women who work out of financial necessity are
violating prescriptive gender stereotypes for reasons be-
yond their control, we hypothesized that they would pro-
voke less negative reactions than women who work outside
442 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN
the home for reasons of personal fulfillment. Research on
moral judgment indicates that actors are blamed less for
engaging in socially undesirable behaviors when the act is
externally compelled or otherwise outside of their control
(Weiner, 1995, 1996). Women who work for personal ful-
fillment may be perceived as willfully neglecting gender
prescriptions such as the “Motherhood Mandate,” which
demands that women always be available to their children
(Russo, 1976). They should therefore receive more moral
censure than women who work outside of the home be-
cause their family’s financial circumstances leave them lit-
tle option. However, motivation should have little impact
on evaluations of employed fathers because, whatever their
reason for employment, they are fulfilling their traditional,
expected role.
Method
Participants
One hundred twelve adults (51 males and 61 females) be-
tween the ages of 18 and 75 years (M = 34.50, SD = 14.74)
were recruited from a public park in Connecticut. Eighty
percent of the sample was European American. The re-
maining 20% were African American, Asian, and Hispanic.
Participants were given a lottery ticket, a drink, or paid $1.00
in exchange for participation in the study.
Procedure and Measures
Participants were randomly assigned to read one of six
paragraph-long descriptions. Each paragraph described ei-
ther a father’s or a mother’s decision to work outside the
home and one of two motives (personal fulfillment or fi-
nancial necessity) for why the target parent made that deci-
sion. A third, control condition did not describe the parent’s
motivation for his or her decision. The six paragraphs were
identical in every way except for the gender of the parent
and the motive for working outside the home, yielding a
2 (participant gender) " 2 (target gender) " 3 (motive:
financial, personal fulfillment, none mentioned) design.
Participants answered seven questions assessing their at-
titudes and beliefs about the parent. Five of these seven
items were identical to the affective evaluation items used
in Study 1 and two additional items assessed participants’
beliefs about the level of dedication the target parent pos-
sessed (“John is a dedicated father”) and perceptions of
the warmth of the target parent (“John is a warm person”),
using 9-point Likert-type scales (1 = completely disagree,
9 = completely agree). We added these two items to bet-
ter assess affective reactions to the targets. As predicted, a
factor analysis with a varimax rotation revealed one distinct
factor tapping affective evaluations for these seven items
(Cronbach’s alpha = .58).
Last, participants completed demographic information,
including gender, race/ethnicity, and age. Participants were
then thanked and debriefed.
Results and Discussion
The seven affective evaluation items were standardized and
summed to form a single index. We predicted that partici-
pants would hold the most negative attitudes toward moth-
ers who worked outside the home for personal fulfillment,
compared to mothers who worked outside the home out
of financial necessity and fathers who worked outside the
home regardless of motive. We also predicted that partici-
pants would hold equally positive attitudes toward fathers
who work outside the home for personal fulfillment or fi-
nancial necessity. In other words, for fathers, the reason
given for working outside the home should not have an im-
pact on participants’ attitudes.
These predictions were tested with a 2 " 2 " 3 ANOVA
and planned contrasts on the Affective Evaluations index.
Consistent with Studies 1 and 2, there were no signifi-
cant effects involving the gender of participants, so all the
data were collapsed across participant gender. Results were
consistent with our hypotheses, F(5, 102) = 3.34, p < .01.
Specifically, planned contrasts revealed that participants re-
ported the most negative attitudes toward mothers who
worked outside the home for personal fulfillment, com-
pared to all other types of employed parents. Simple effects
analyses revealed that fathers were evaluated the same re-
gardless of the reason stated for their working outside the
home.
Examining the feeling thermometer item separately
from the other Affective Evaluation items revealed the same
pattern of results (see Table 1). Specifically, participants felt
most coldly toward women who worked outside the home
for personal fulfillment compared to all other types of em-
ployed parents, t(103) = 3.52, p < .01. The evaluation of
employed fathers was not impacted by the stated reason
for their working outside the home, while the evaluation of
employed mothers was affected by the stated reasons for
their working outside the home, F(2, 53) = 3.67, p < .05.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
All three studies reveal that people report more negative at-
titudes toward nontraditional parents (i.e., employed moth-
ers and stay-at-home fathers) than toward traditional par-
ents (i.e., stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers).
This finding is consistent with other research showing that
Table 1
Mean Feeling Thermometer Ratings of Employed
Mothers and Fathers by Employment Motive (Study 3)
Gender of Target
Reason for Employment Father Mother
Financial need 69.21 (18.65) 66.06 (12.74)
Personal fulfillment 66.50 (23.16) 47.84 (25.20)
No reason given (control) 63.24 (15.30) 56.58 (20.35)
Attitudes Toward Parents 443
people dislike those who violate prescriptive stereotypes
(e.g., Rudman & Glick, 1999) and prior studies of the
trait attributions made about traditional and nontraditional
parents (Bridges & Etaugh, 1995; Bridges & Orza, 1993;
Etaugh & Folger, 1998; Etaugh & Moss, 2001; Etaugh
& Nekolny, 1990; Etaugh & Petroski, 1985; Etaugh &
Poertner, 1992). However, the present research is the first
to empirically document prejudice against stay-at-home fa-
thers, a stigmatized category that has received insufficient
attention in the literature.
Participants’ beliefs about other people’s reactions to
stay-at-home and employed mothers and fathers were fur-
ther examined in Study 1, revealing that perceived social
regard was lowest for stay-at-home fathers. The perceived
social stigmatization of male homemakers may represent a
major barrier to mothers’ opportunities if it makes fathers
reluctant to stay at home with the children while their wife
works outside the home. Notably, perceived social regard
for employed mothers was just as high as for traditional
parents. It may be that by assuming the traditionally male
breadwinner role, employed women accrue some of the so-
cial respect and regard associated with that role. Thus, em-
ployed women may be simultaneously disliked and socially
respected. This highlights the distinction between percep-
tions of competence and warmth drawn by previous re-
searchers (Fiske et al., 2002; Rudman, 1998; Rudman &
Glick, 1999).
Participants apparently felt little compunction about ex-
pressing negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents.
People generally experience guilt and self-criticism about
their negative feelings toward racial minorities (Devine
et al., 1991). Other work indicates that people are most
likely to discriminate based on race under ambiguous cir-
cumstances, when their prejudices are easy to rationalize
and justify (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Hodson et al., 2002).
However, even in Study 2’s within-subjects design, where
gender-biased judgments were blatantly obvious, partici-
pants continued to evaluate nontraditional parents more
negatively than traditional parents. These results are con-
sistent with those of Czopp and Monteith (2003), who found
that people feel guilty at the thought of stereotyping a Black
person, but amused at the thought of stereotyping a woman.
As Fiske and Stevens (1993) note, gender stereotypes may
be more strongly prescriptive, and therefore normative,
than stereotypes of other groups. As a consequence, people
may not fear social censure for expressing gender stereo-
types to the same extent that they do for racial stereotypes.
The effects of motivations for working on attitudes to-
ward employed mothers and fathers were investigated in
Study 3. Participants reported more negative attitudes to-
ward mothers who worked out of personal fulfillment than
toward mothers who worked out of financial necessity or
mothers who did not mention a reason for employment.
However, motivation for working outside the home did not
affect people’s attitudes toward employed fathers. This find-
ing suggests that mothers are subjected to an unfair double
standard in that they are required to have a socially accept-
able reason for working outside the home while fathers are
not. If employed mothers are thought to work outside the
home for personal fulfillment, they may be perceived as
failing to fulfill the role of the selfless mother, thus causing
people to dislike them (Russo, 1976).
Remarkably, no gender differences in attitudes toward
traditional and nontraditional parents were observed. One
might expect that female participants would feel more pos-
itively toward employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers,
given that women should be less likely to endorse and act
on prescriptive stereotypes that are detrimental to their
own life opportunities. However, these results are consis-
tent with theories in which consensual ideologies, adopted
by both dominant and subordinate group members, pro-
mote social inequality (Jost & Banaji, 1994; Glick & Fiske,
1996, 2001; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). For example, Jost
and Banaji’s (1994) system justification theory proposes
that due to implicit socialization by the dominant culture,
members of low status groups adopt stereotypes and be-
lief systems that perpetuate their low social position. For
example, many African Americans endorse the Protestant
Work Ethic, which implies that laziness is the primary cause
of poverty (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Similarly, women of-
ten endorse ideologies such as benevolent sexism, which
holds that “good” women who fulfill their traditional roles
as mothers and wives should be “put on a pedestal” (Glick &
Fiske, 1996, 2001). Indeed, many women likely enjoy the
special status traditional gender roles accord the “weaker
sex.” However, they may do so at the cost of limiting their
personal options and those of women as a whole.
It is equally important to note that men’s life choices
are also limited by restrictive gender roles and prescriptive
gender stereotypes. Some men may want to care for their
children full-time rather than working outside the home,
but the stigma attached to being a stay-at-home father may
prevent them from doing so. Prescriptive gender stereo-
types and the stigma attached to violations of them limit and
restrict both men’s and women’s opportunities and lives.
Previous research that has examined the consequences
of prescriptive stereotype violations generally has not fo-
cused on whether men who violate prescriptive gender
stereotypes experience similar “backlash” effects as women
who violate gender norms. The media, however, has re-
cently paid a great deal of attention to the stigma that stay-at-
home fathers face. For example, a recent article in the Wall
Street Journal (“Stay-at-home dads,” 2003) reported that
employers view stay-at-home fathers either with disdain or
confusion. Sometimes employers even “wonder whether
‘stay-at-home dad’ is a cover for ‘couldn’t find work.’” (“Stay-
at-home dads,” 2003). Anecdotal reports have even sur-
faced of parents not allowing their children to socialize with
the children of stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers
(“Your career,” 2001). To our knowledge, the present studies
are the first to document this stigmatization of stay-at-home
fathers.
444 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN
One limitation of the present research is worth noting.
The present studies examined attitudes toward parents with
an infant. This scenario was selected as particularly relevant
to prescriptive gender stereotypes regarding care of chil-
dren. However, only future research can reveal whether
the present findings generalize to parents whose children
are older.
Future research should further seek to understand the
reasons why people dislike nontraditional parents and, more
generally, why people dislike those who violate prescrip-
tive gender stereotypes. Perhaps reactions to nontraditional
parents are negative because people generally dislike indi-
viduals who violate social norms (Cialdini & Trost, 1998).
However, this only raises the issue of how and why such
norms developed in the first place. Prescriptive stereotypes
may serve the system-justifying function of keeping women
“in their place,” such that they do not act in ways that dis-
rupt the social order (Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Jost &
Banaji, 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). That is, people may
dislike employed mothers because they (consciously or un-
consciously) perceive these women as trying to increase
their power and status, thereby rejecting a subordinate role.
Initial submission: December 30, 2003
Initial acceptance: December 14, 2004
Final acceptance: April 5, 2005
REFERENCES
Ajzen, I. (1996). The directive influence of attitudes on
behavior.
In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of
action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp.
385–403). New York: Guilford.
Bridges, J. S., & Etaugh, C. (1995). College students’
perceptions
of mothers: Effects of maternal employment-childrearing
pattern and motive for employment. Sex Roles, 32, 735–751.
Bridges, J. S., Etaugh, C., & Barnes-Farrell, J. (2002). Trait
judg-
ments of stay-at-home and employed parents: A function of
social role and/or shifting standards? Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 26, 140–150.
Bridges, J. S., & Orza, A. M. (1992). The effects of employment
role and motive for employment on the perceptions of
mothers. Sex Roles, 27, 331–343.
Bridges, J. S., & Orza, A. M. (1993). Effects of maternal
employ-
ment childrearing pattern on college students’ perceptions
of a mother and her child. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
17, 103–117.
Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social
norms, conformity, and compliance. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T.
Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psy-
chology (4th ed., pp. 151–192). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., & Schwarz, N. (1996).
Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An
“experimental ethnography.” Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 70, 945–960.
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The
police officer’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate
potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 83, 1314–1329.
Czopp, A. M., & Monteith, M. J. (2003). Confronting prejudice
(literally): Reactions to confrontations of racial and gender
bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 532–
544.
Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An
interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological
Review, 94, 369–389.
Devine, P. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic
and
controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 56, 5–18.
Devine, P. G., Monteith, M. J., Zuwerink, J. R., & Elliot, A. J.
(1991). Prejudice with and without compunction. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 817–830.
Dovidio, J. F., Brigham, J. C., Johnson, B. T., & Gaertner, S. L.
(1996). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination: An-
other look. In N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone
(Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping (pp. 276–319). New
York: Guilford.
Duindam, V. (1999). Men in the household: Caring fathers. In
L.
McKie, S. Bowlby, & S. Gregory (Eds.), Gender, power and
the household. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference
measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316–326.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of
prej-
udice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109,
573–598.
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992).
Gender
and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 111, 3–22.
Eagly, A. H., Mladinic, A., & Otto, S. (1991). Are women
evaluated
more favorably than men? An analysis of attitudes, beliefs,
and emotions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 203–
216.
Etaugh, C., & Folger, D. (1998). Perceptions of parents whose
work and parenting behaviors deviate from role expecta-
tions. Sex Roles, 39, 215–223.
Etaugh, C., & Moss, C. (2001). Attitudes of employed women
toward parents who choose full-time or part-time em-
ployment following their child’s birth. Sex Roles, 44, 611–
619.
Etaugh, C., & Nekolny, K. (1990). Effects of employment status
and marital status on perceptions of mothers. Sex Roles, 23,
273–280.
Etaugh, C., & Petroski, B. (1985). Perceptions of women:
Effects
of employment status and marital status. Sex Roles, 12, 329–
339.
Etaugh, C., & Poertner, P. (1991). Effects of occupational
prestige,
employment status, and marital status on perceptions of
mothers. Sex Roles, 24, 345–353.
Etaugh, C., & Poertner, P. (1992). Perceptions of women: Influ-
ence of performance: Marital, and parental variables. Sex
Roles, 26, 311–321.
Evans, D. C., Garcia, D. G., Garcia, D. M., & Baron, R. S.
(2003).
In the privacy of their own homes: Using the internet to as-
sess racial bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
29, 273–284.
Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination.
In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The
handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 357–411). New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Attitudes Toward Parents 445
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A
model of
(often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth
respectively follow from perceived status and competition.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Fiske, S. T., & Stevens, L. E. (1993). What’s so special about
sex?
Gender stereotyping and discrimination. In S. Oskamp &
M. Costanza (Eds.), Gender issues in contemporary society
(Vol. 6., pp. 173–196). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of
racism. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice,
discrimination, and racism (pp. 61–89). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism
Inventory:
Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile
and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for
gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
Haddock, G., & Zanna, G. (1994). Preferring “housewives” to
“feminists”: Categorization and the favorability of attitudes
toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 25–52.
Hodson, G., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Processes
in
racial discrimination: Differential weighting of conflicting
information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
460–471.
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in
system-justification and the production of false conscious-
ness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1–27.
Judd, C. M., Park, B., Ryan, C. S., Brauer, M., & Kraus, S.
(1995).
Stereotypes and ethnocentrism: Diverging interethnic per-
ceptions of African American and White American youth.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 460–481.
Livingston, R. W. (2002). The role of perceived negativity in
the
moderation of African Americans’ implicit and explicit racial
attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38,
405–413.
Miller, J. J., & Ponnuru, R. (2001). A modest proposal for Jane
Swift. National Review Online. Retrieved Sept. 3, 2005 from
http://www.nationalreview.com/daily/nr052101.shtml
Monteith, M. J., Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Czopp, A.
M.
(2002). Putting the brakes on prejudice: On the develop-
ment and operation of cues for control. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 83, 1029–1050.
Muller, R., & Goldberg, S. (1980). Why William doesn’t want a
doll: Preschoolers’ expectations of adult behavior toward
girls and boys. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 259–269.
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external moti-
vation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 75, 811–832.
Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men
should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have
to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psy-
chology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281.
Robertson, B. (2000). There’s no place like home: How
business,
government, and our obsession with work has driven par-
ents from home. New York: Spence.
Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for
women:
The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression
management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74, 629–645.
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management
and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to
women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1004–
1010.
Russo, N. (1976). The motherhood mandate. Journal of Social
Issues, 32, 143–153.
Schlafly, P. (2003). Feminist fantasies: Essays on feminism in
the
media, the workplace, the home, and the military. New York:
Spence.
Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory:
Influ-
ences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of
human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 51, 515–530.
Sechrist, G. B., & Stangor, C. (2001). Perceived consensus in-
fluences intergroup behavior and stereotype accessibility.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 645–654.
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An
intergroup
theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Silverstein, L. B. (1996). Fathering is a feminist issue.
Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 20, 3–37.
Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A., & Ford, T. E. (1991). Affective and
cognitive determinants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9,
359–380.
Stay-at-home dads fight stigma. (2003, September 9). Wall
Street
Journal, p. B1.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). Children’s living arrangements and
characteristics: March 2002. Current population reports,
P20-547. Washington, DC: Author.
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility. New York:
Guilford.
Weiner, B. (1996). Searching for order in social motivation.
Psy-
chological Inquiry, 7, 199–216.
Your career matters: Have husband, will travel. (2001, February
13). Wall Street Journal, p. B1.
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no
inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

More Related Content

Similar to Running head MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS1MASSACHUSE.docx

Compare Contrast Example
Compare Contrast ExampleCompare Contrast Example
Compare Contrast Examplecadavis78
 
The Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docx
The Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docxThe Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docx
The Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
StudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docx
StudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docxStudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docx
StudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docxflorriezhamphrey3065
 
Senior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final Submission
Senior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final SubmissionSenior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final Submission
Senior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final SubmissionJesse Berwanger
 
Running Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docx
Running Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docxRunning Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docx
Running Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docxtodd521
 
Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper
Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper
Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper Nicholas Niesen
 
575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01
575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01
575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01Jon Nadler
 
Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...
Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...
Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...Paige Catizone
 
Case Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power point
Case Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power pointCase Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power point
Case Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power pointKaryssa Costagliola
 
U.S. Healthcare
U.S. HealthcareU.S. Healthcare
U.S. Healthcareparikh32
 

Similar to Running head MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS1MASSACHUSE.docx (17)

Compare Contrast Example
Compare Contrast ExampleCompare Contrast Example
Compare Contrast Example
 
Essay Health Care Reform
Essay Health Care ReformEssay Health Care Reform
Essay Health Care Reform
 
The Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docx
The Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docxThe Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docx
The Theme of Love in Sulaby The Theme Of Love In Sula The Th.docx
 
StudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docx
StudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docxStudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docx
StudentProfessor English 102March 6, 2016Toulmin Argu.docx
 
Health Care Reform Essay
Health Care Reform EssayHealth Care Reform Essay
Health Care Reform Essay
 
WGU VPT2 Task 2
WGU VPT2 Task 2WGU VPT2 Task 2
WGU VPT2 Task 2
 
Senior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final Submission
Senior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final SubmissionSenior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final Submission
Senior Seminar- Affordable Care Act Final Submission
 
Running Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docx
Running Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docxRunning Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docx
Running Head ROLE OF EXECUTIVE ORDER IN ACARole of Executiv.docx
 
Medicare solvency 2
Medicare solvency  2Medicare solvency  2
Medicare solvency 2
 
Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper
Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper
Single Payer Healthcare Research Paper
 
575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01
575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01
575c1cf5 7528-425e-b718-b2261305cc8b-150320130848-conversion-gate01
 
Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...
Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...
Case analysis of the affordable care act power point, hcs410, hcs organizatio...
 
Case Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power point
Case Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power pointCase Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power point
Case Analysis of the Affordable Care Act power point
 
EC499 Capstone Paper
EC499 Capstone PaperEC499 Capstone Paper
EC499 Capstone Paper
 
The On Health Care Reform
The On Health Care ReformThe On Health Care Reform
The On Health Care Reform
 
jsc160013
jsc160013jsc160013
jsc160013
 
U.S. Healthcare
U.S. HealthcareU.S. Healthcare
U.S. Healthcare
 

More from cowinhelen

Case Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docx
Case Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docxCase Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docx
Case Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docx
Case Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docxCase Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docx
Case Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docx
Case Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docxCase Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docx
Case Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docx
Case Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docxCase Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docx
Case Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study - Global Mobile Corporation Damn it, .docx
Case Study - Global Mobile Corporation      Damn it, .docxCase Study - Global Mobile Corporation      Damn it, .docx
Case Study - Global Mobile Corporation Damn it, .docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docx
Case Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docxCase Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docx
Case Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docxcowinhelen
 
CASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docx
CASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docxCASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docx
CASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docx
Case Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docxCase Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docx
Case Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docxcowinhelen
 
CASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docx
CASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docxCASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docx
CASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docx
Case Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docxCase Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docx
Case Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docx
Case Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docxCase Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docx
Case Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docx
Case Study  – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docxCase Study  – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docx
Case Study – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study   THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docx
Case Study    THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docxCase Study    THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docx
Case Study   THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docxcowinhelen
 
CASE STUDY Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docx
CASE STUDY  Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docxCASE STUDY  Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docx
CASE STUDY Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docx
Case Study  Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docxCase Study  Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docx
Case Study Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docxcowinhelen
 
case studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docx
case studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docxcase studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docx
case studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docx
Case Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docxCase Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docx
Case Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docxcowinhelen
 
Case Study Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docx
Case Study  Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docxCase Study  Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docx
Case Study Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docxcowinhelen
 
Case Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docx
Case Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docxCase Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docx
Case Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docxcowinhelen
 
Case Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docx
Case Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docxCase Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docx
Case Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docxcowinhelen
 

More from cowinhelen (20)

Case Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docx
Case Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docxCase Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docx
Case Study 1 Applying Theory to PracticeSocial scientists hav.docx
 
Case Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docx
Case Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docxCase Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docx
Case Study - Option 3 BarbaraBarbara is a 22 year old woman who h.docx
 
Case Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docx
Case Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docxCase Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docx
Case Study - Cyberterrorism—A New RealityWhen hackers claiming .docx
 
Case Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docx
Case Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docxCase Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docx
Case Study - APA paper with min 4 page content Review the Blai.docx
 
Case Study - Global Mobile Corporation Damn it, .docx
Case Study - Global Mobile Corporation      Damn it, .docxCase Study - Global Mobile Corporation      Damn it, .docx
Case Study - Global Mobile Corporation Damn it, .docx
 
Case Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docx
Case Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docxCase Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docx
Case Study #3Apple Suppliers & Labor PracticesWith its h.docx
 
CASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docx
CASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docxCASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docx
CASE STUDY (Individual) Scotland  In terms of its physical l.docx
 
Case Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docx
Case Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docxCase Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docx
Case Study #2 T.D. enjoys caring for the children and young peop.docx
 
CASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docx
CASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docxCASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docx
CASE STUDY #2 Chief Complaint I have pain in my belly”.docx
 
Case Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docx
Case Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docxCase Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docx
Case Study #1Jennifer is a 29-year-old administrative assistan.docx
 
Case Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docx
Case Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docxCase Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docx
Case Study # 2 –Danny’s Unhappy DutyEmployee ProfilesCaro.docx
 
Case Study – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docx
Case Study  – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docxCase Study  – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docx
Case Study – Multicultural ParadeRead the Case below, and answe.docx
 
Case Study   THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docx
Case Study    THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docxCase Study    THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docx
Case Study   THE INVISIBLE SPONSOR1BackgroundSome execut.docx
 
CASE STUDY Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docx
CASE STUDY  Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docxCASE STUDY  Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docx
CASE STUDY Experiential training encourages changes in work beha.docx
 
Case Study Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docx
Case Study  Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docxCase Study  Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docx
Case Study Hereditary AngioedemaAll responses must be in your .docx
 
case studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docx
case studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docxcase studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docx
case studieson Gentrification and Displacement in the Sa.docx
 
Case Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docx
Case Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docxCase Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docx
Case Studt on KFC Introduction1) Identify the type of .docx
 
Case Study Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docx
Case Study  Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docxCase Study  Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docx
Case Study Crocs Revolutionizing an Industry’s Supply Chain .docx
 
Case Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docx
Case Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docxCase Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docx
Case Studies Student must complete 5 case studies as instructed.docx
 
Case Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docx
Case Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docxCase Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docx
Case Studies in Telehealth AdoptionThe mission of The Comm.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersChitralekhaTherkar
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 

Running head MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS1MASSACHUSE.docx

  • 1. Running head: MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS 1 MASSACHUSETTS’ HEALTHCARE REFORMS 3 Memo To: Prof. Thomas Smith From: Student- Jane Doe Reference: Health Care Policy Date: March 18, 2018 Subject: Massachusetts’ Healthcare Reform Act Massachusetts’ Healthcare Reform Act Rationale Massachusetts State is among the states that have made a number of attempts aimed at reforming the state's healthcare system to make access to quality healthcare available for its residents. Recently in 2006, Massachusetts passed the Healthcare Reform Act, which was later, signed into law by former Governor Mitt Romney (Van der Wees et al., 2013). The rationale for this healthcare reform was to provide near- universal health insurance coverage for Massachusetts’ residents. Adoption of the Reform The Massachusetts Healthcare Reform Act was passed by the State legislators after years of negotiation between Mitt Romney and the legislators with a compromise reached in 2006 resulting in the enactment of the reform that was effectively signed into law by Romney on 12 April 206. The reform has made several changes to its healthcare system in a move aimed at achieving a near-universal healthcare coverage for the residents of the state. The first change was made to the state's Medicaid program that was broadened by providing a MassHealth waiver, extending health insurance coverage to
  • 2. children in low-income families with up to 300% of the federal poverty level (FPL) (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012). Massachusetts created what is called Commonwealth Care, which provides the residents of the state with access to subsidized health insurance for eligible individuals with earnings below 300% of FPL. Under this new healthcare reform, individuals with income below 150% of FPL also have the option of selecting a plan without a monthly premium and low- cost sharing. However, eligible individuals with earnings falling between 150-300% PL are subsidized by the state using a sliding scale. The Massachusetts Healthcare Reform Act also saw the state expand its Insurance Partnership Program by providing incentives and subsidies to the employers to give and workers to enroll in the state's employer-sponsored insurance. In this respect, Massachusetts State subsidized insurance costs for the workers in the state who would otherwise be eligible for programs subsidized by the government. However, small businesses are only eligible for up to $1,000 in support per qualified worker who falls below the 300% FPL (Van der Wees et al., 2013). Under the program, the state government pays the portion of qualified workers' premiums that is equal to what the employees would be expected to pay if employees were on a subsidized plan. Additionally, under this new healthcare reform, any employer in the state who fails to provide health insurance to its workers is expected to pay what is called a ‘fair share' assessment to the government of up to $295 per worker every year (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012). The reform also created what is called the Commonwealth Health Insurance Connector whose primary aim is to link those without access to employer-sponsored insurance and companies with 50 or fewer employees that provide insurance coverage for its workers. According to this health reform, small businesses with 50 of fewer employees have the option of buying insurance coverage on their own or via the Connector (Rapoza, 2012).
  • 3. Funding Structure Although Romney and the state legislators agreed on most of the components of the bill, agreeing on how this healthcare reform would be financed was a major issue as it was clear that financing the reform would result in an increase in healthcare cost. However, following a compromise that was reached, the state legislators agreed that the reform would be financed by individuals, employers and the government. First, the Massachusetts Healthcare Reform is funded by the existing $320 million obtained in hospital assessments and covered levies (Van der Wees et al., 2013). Second, the Massachusetts state legislators agreed that the health reform would also be financed through by federal safety-net payments of $610 million as well as federal matching payments on the MassHealth expansion. Additionally, part of the money to be used in financing the health care reform is to come from rate increases projected at $299 million. Further, $295 fair assessment for employers per employee and the Free Rider Surcharge also generates revenue used to finance the ambitious health care reform in Massachusetts (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012). Impacts The impacts of this Massachusetts Healthcare Reform Act have been so profound. The first major achievement of this healthcare reform is that it has increased access to affordable coverage to residents of Massachusetts. Because the law requires all residents of Massachusetts to have a health insurance or pay a fine, the law had seen more that 99% of the residents of the state now get health insurance coverage up from 90% before this healthcare reform was introduced. According to Rapoza (2012), prior to 2006, more than 24% of low-income residents of Massachusetts had no health insurance. However, by 2012, only 8% of low-income adults in the state were still without healthcare coverage. Overall, about 650,000 Massachusetts residents who lacked health insurance are now covered. Another significant achievement of the Massachusetts health
  • 4. insurance is that it has increased insurance status of higher income persons for the self-employed who did not qualify for MassHealth. According to Urban Institute, the population of higher income earners who were without health insurance before 2006 has dropped from 5% then to below 1% three years after the reform (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012). The only notable shortcoming of this healthcare reform is the cost burden associated with its implementation. The health cost in the state has risen to a historic high following the introduction of this healthcare reform was introduced. By 2007, just one year after the reform, Massachusetts healthcare expenditure accounted for about 15.2% of its GDP, which is higher than the nation's average of 13.7% as a whole (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2012). References Kaiser Family Foundation. (2012). Massachusetts health care reform: Six years later. Retrieved from https://kaiserfamilyfoundation.files.wordpress.com/2013/01/831 1.pdf Rapoza, K. (2012, Jan. 20). If ObamaCare is so bad, how does RomneyCare survive? Forbes p. 1 https://www.forbes.com/sites/kenrapoza/2012/01/20/romney- care-massachusetts-healthcare-reform/#3d6701195b00 Van der Wees, P. J., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Ayanian, J. Z. (2013). Improvements in health status after Massachusetts health care reform. The Milbank Quarterly, 91(4), 663–689. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29 (2005), 436–445. Blackwell Publishing. Printed in the USA. Copyright C! 2005 Division 35, American Psychological Association. 0361-6843/05
  • 5. ATTITUDES TOWARD TRADITIONAL AND NONTRADITIONAL PARENTS Victoria L. Brescoll and Eric Luis Uhlmann Yale University Three studies investigated attitudes toward traditional parents (stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers) and nontra- ditional parents (stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers) among adult men and women. Using a between-subjects design, Study 1 found that nontraditional parents were liked significantly less than traditional parents. Participants also believed that stay-at-home fathers were not regarded highly by others. Study 2 replicated these results using a within-subjects design, suggesting that participants felt little compunction about expressing negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents. Study 3 further found that employed mothers were less disliked when described as working out of financial necessity rather than for personal fulfillment. Both male and female participants reported negative evalu- ations of employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers, suggesting that prescriptive gender role stereotypes represent a consensual ideology shared by men and women. For the past three decades, conservatives have argued that feminism has caused society to devalue women’s tradi- tional roles, such as homemaking and caring for children (Robertson, 2000; Schlafly, 2003). Recently, some have even asserted that society has stigmatized stay-at-home mothers because they are not pursuing careers outside the home. “Stay-at-home moms are used to the silent snubs they re- ceive from mothers who decide to pursue careers—as if they were nothing but pre-feminist breeders who don’t lead worthwhile lives” (Miller & Ponnuru, 2001). According to this perspective, society’s stigmatization of stay-at-home mothers has discouraged women from staying home to raise
  • 6. their children and, more generally, has contributed to the devaluing of the traditional American family. Psychological theories of gender take a starkly different position by hypothesizing that people respond negatively to men and women who do not conform to traditional gender roles (Deaux & Major, 1987; Eagly & Karau, 2002; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Prentice & Carranza, 2002; Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999; Russo, 1976; Silverstein, 1996). This is because gender stereotypes do Victoria L. Brescoll and Eric Luis Uhlmann, Department of Psy- chology, Yale University. Both authors were supported by graduate research fellowships from the National Science Foundation while this research was conducted. Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Victoria Brescoll, Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2 Hillhouse Ave., New Haven, CT 06520. E-mail: [email protected] not just describe how men and women behave, but also prescribe gender appropriate behavior. For example, when women lead in a masculine manner (e.g., authoritatively) they are judged more harshly than men who lead in the same way (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992). Likewise, women who self-promote and behave agentically are liked less than women who behave in stereotypically feminine ways, and agentic women suffer a “backlash effect” as a result (Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999). Mothers who are employed full-time outside the home and fathers who stay home to care for children occupy non- traditional gender roles. Contemporary psychological the- ories predict that they would be stigmatized, as are agen-
  • 7. tic, “masculine” women (Eagly et al., 1992; Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999), because these nontraditional par- ents are violating prescriptive gender stereotypes. Previous research has examined people’s beliefs about and perceptions of mothers but has not directly addressed whether certain types of parents, such as stay-at-home mothers and fathers, are disliked or stigmatized (Bridges & Etaugh, 1995; Bridges & Orza, 1993; Etaugh & Poertner, 1992; Etaugh & Nekolny, 1990; Etaugh & Folger, 1998; Etaugh & Moss, 2001; Etaugh & Petroski, 1985). This re- search has varied women’s marital status (divorced vs. sin- gle vs. married), work status (full-time vs. part-time; con- tinuously employed vs. interrupted employment), prestige of job (moderate vs. low), and parental status (children vs. no children) and has revealed a number of interest- ing findings. For example, married women are seen as better adjusted and more nurturant than divorced women (Etaugh & Nekolny, 1990; Etaugh & Poertner, 1992, 1991) 436 Attitudes Toward Parents 437 and mothers are seen as more nurturant but less compe- tent than nonmothers (Etaugh & Poertner, 1991, 1992). Furthermore, continuously employed mothers are evalu- ated more negatively than mothers who interrupted their employment to care for their children because people view continuously employed mothers as less committed to their maternal role (Bridges & Etaugh, 1995). These findings suggest that people’s evaluations of mothers depend on their perceived level of commitment to their children.
  • 8. The present research builds upon and extends this previ- ous work in six major ways. First, past studies have focused primarily on documenting people’s stereotypes and beliefs about parents, rather than their attitudes toward them. This emphasis on cognition at the expense of affect is unfortu- nate given that attitudes predict discriminatory judgments and behavior more effectively than do stereotypes (Fiske, 1998; Stangor, Sullivan, & Ford, 1991; for a meta-analytic review, see Dovidio, Brigham, Johnson, & Gaertner, 1996). Accordingly, the present studies explored people’s affective reactions to mothers and fathers who occupy traditional and nontraditional roles. We expected that individuals who vio- late prescriptive gender stereotypes (i.e., employed moth- ers and stay-at-home fathers) would elicit negative affective reactions. Second, we examined reactions to stay-at-home and em- ployed fathers. Prior research was primarily concerned with how responses to mothers vary based on their employment status (see Bridges, Etaugh, & Barnes-Farrell, 2002, for an exception). To some extent, this emphasis is reason- able because there are far more employed mothers than there are stay-at-home fathers (U.S. Census Bureau, 2002). However, the number of fathers who stay at home to care for the children while their wife works outside the home is steadily increasing, rising 18% since 1994 (U.S. Cen- sus Bureau, 2002). Moreover, personal and social preju- dices against stay-at-home fathers are directly relevant to women’s options because they may make fathers unwilling to assume a homemaker role while their wife works outside the home. Indeed, the stigma against stay-at-home fathers may contribute to some fathers’ unwillingness to stay home full-time with their children out of fear that they may en- counter problems when trying to re-enter the workplace (Duindam, 1999; “Stay-at-home dads,” 2003).
  • 9. Third, we assessed people’s beliefs about society’s re- action to nontraditional parents. Perceived cultural norms have been shown to predict behavior above and beyond personal attitudes (Ajzen, 1996), moderate the expression of personally endorsed attitudes (Sechrist & Stangor, 2001), and strongly influence automatic judgments and behaviors (Correll, Park, Judd, & Wittenbrink, 2002; Devine, 1989; Livingston, 2002). To the extent that it is perceived as cul- turally normative to lack respect for stay-at-home fathers and/or employed mothers, sexist individuals should be more likely to express their attitudes, nonsexist individuals should be more likely to “go along” with sexist social norms, and au- tomatic “gut” responses are likely to be gender biased. Thus, it is important to know not only people’s personal reactions to nontraditional parents but also their beliefs about how most other people regard such individuals. Fourth, we investigated the extent to which people feel a sense of compunction about expressing negative atti- tudes toward nontraditional parents. Previous work has shown that people feel strong internal and external pres- sures not to express racial prejudice or endorse racial stereo- types (Devine, Monteith, Zuwerink, & Elliot, 1991; Dunton & Fazio, 1997; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Monteith, Ashburn-Nardo, Voils, & Czopp, 2002; Plant & Devine, 1998). In fact, discrimination based on race occurs mainly under ambiguous circumstances when racial biases are eas- iest to rationalize and justify (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Hodson, Dovidio, & Gaertner, 2002). However, there is rea- son to believe that such pressures are considerably weaker when it comes to gender role stereotypes. Because many men are dependent on women for child rearing and sex- ual companionship, stereotypes regarding women’s behav- ior are often more prescriptive than stereotypes of racial groups (Fiske & Stevens, 1993; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001).
  • 10. Notably, one recent study found that while people antic- ipate feeling guilty at having judged a Black person in a stereotypical manner, they react with amusement at having stereotyped a woman (Czopp & Monteith, 2003). Thus, in Study 2 we employed a within-subjects design, presenting participants with side-by-side descriptions of employed and stay-at-home mothers and fathers and asking them to pro- vide their attitudes toward each target. We were particularly interested in whether participants would express negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents when such a bias would be blatant and obvious. Fifth, we examined the effects of motivations for work- ing outside the home on attitudes toward both mothers and fathers (Study 3). Extensive work in the field of moral judg- ment indicates that people receive less blame for socially undesirable acts when the behavior is externally compelled (Weiner, 1995, 1996). Because women who work out of financial necessity are violating prescriptive gender stereo- types for situational (i.e., external) reasons, we hypothesized that they would provoke less negative reactions than women who seek employment for reasons of personal fulfillment. In contrast, motivation should have little impact on attitudes toward employed fathers because, whatever their reason for employment, they are fulfilling their traditional role. Although earlier work has shown that mothers who work outside the home out of financial necessity are perceived as more communal than employed mothers whose motive is personal fulfillment (Bridges & Orza, 1992), the present research is the first to look at attitudes and to use fathers as well as mothers as targets. Finally, the present research fills an important gap in the literature because we used a racially diverse adult sample (average age = 38 years) rather than college students. Our participants thus had considerable experience with parent-
  • 11. ing and working. Moreover, because prejudice varies greatly across different cohorts (Judd, Park, Ryan, Brauer, & Kraus, 1995; Sears, 1986), adult samples may be necessary to obtain 438 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN a complete picture of attitudes toward parents (cf. Bridges et al., 2002). Although a few other studies have examined adult women’s attitudes toward parents, to our knowledge the present studies are the first to assess those of adult men. In summary, three studies examined adult men’s and women’s affective reactions to traditional parents (em- ployed fathers and stay-at-home mothers) and nontra- ditional parents (employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers). Also investigated were: perceptions of other peo- ple’s responses to nontraditional parents (Study 1), the ex- tent to which people feel a sense of compunction about reporting negative attitudes toward stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers (Study 2), and the effects of moth- ers’ and fathers’ motivations for working (Study 3). Taken together, these studies were designed to investigate prej- udices against nontraditional parents and explore some of the potential parameters of such biases. While in part an effort to build upon and extend prior work on prescriptive gender stereotyping and perceptions of parents, the present studies also make important novel contributions. First, the present work is the first to empir- ically investigate attitudes toward stay-at-home fathers, a stigmatized category that has received no attention in the stereotyping and prejudice literature. Second, the present research points to a double-standard regarding women’s and men’s reasons for working. Specifically, in Study 3, we inves-
  • 12. tigate whether mothers are evaluated more negatively for choosing to work out of personal fulfillment than fathers who do the same. STUDY 1 Study 1 presented participants with a description of either an employed mother, an employed father, a stay-at-home mother, or a stay-at-home father. Each paragraph described either a man’s or a woman’s decision to either stay home to care for his or her children or to work outside the home. The target individual was described as being married with two children who recently had another baby. Participants an- swered a series of questions about these individuals, includ- ing an assessment of their affective reaction to the person. We hypothesized an interaction between gender of target and decision to work versus stay at home. That is, those who violated traditional gender roles (i.e., stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers) were expected to be more nega- tively evaluated than those who conformed to traditional gender roles (i.e., stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers). In addition to assessing participants’ personal attitudes toward the target persons, we were interested in measuring participants’ beliefs about other people’s reactions to em- ployed and stay-at-home mothers and fathers. Perceived so- cial norms are powerful influences on judgment and behav- ior (Ajzen, 1996; Correll et al., 2002; Devine, 1989; Sechrist & Stangor, 2001). For example, a man who believes that stay-at-home fathers are not respected may be reluctant to assume a homemaker role, even though his personal at- titudes toward staying at home while his wife works are positive.
  • 13. In fact, there are reasons to expect stay-at-home fathers to be held in lower social regard than employed mothers. Among the most powerful prescriptive stereotypes directed at men are those that emphasize avoiding “effeminate” be- haviors (e.g., playing with dolls for young boys, failing to defend one’s honor for adult men; Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996; Muller & Goldberg, 1980). Stay-at-home fathers, by adopting a traditionally feminine role, may thus incur a steep drop in perceived social regard. In contrast, while an employed woman might be disliked for violating prescriptive stereotypes, her adoption of the high-status, traditionally male role of breadwinner may win her some of the social respect and regard associated with that role (for a discussion of the distinction between perceived warmth and perceived competence, see Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002). Method Participants Seventy-three adults (44 males and 29 females) between the ages of 17 and 79 years (M = 31.33, SD = 16.64) were re- cruited from a public park in Connecticut. Seventy-seven percent of the sample was European American. The re- maining 23% were African American, Asian, and Hispanic. Participants were largely middle class (the median income level was $40,000 per year). Participants were given a lot- tery ticket, a drink, or paid $1.00 in exchange for participa- tion in the study. Data from two participants were excluded because they were not sufficiently fluent in English to com- plete the survey. Procedure and Measures Participants were randomly assigned to read one of four
  • 14. paragraph-long descriptions. Each paragraph described ei- ther a man’s or a woman’s decision to either stay home to care for his or her children (including an infant) or to work outside the home. A situation in which parents have an infant to care for was chosen as particularly relevant to prescriptive stereotypes regarding maternal care of chil- dren (Russo, 1976). We used two names, John and Jennifer, which were pretested for comparability. The four para- graphs were identical in every way except for the decision made and whether it was a man or a woman making the decision, yielding a 2 (stay home vs. work outside home) " 2 (male target vs. female target) design. With this design, it was possible to compare participants’ reactions to stay-at- home mothers, employed mothers, stay-at-home fathers, and employed fathers in a between-subjects manner. Participants answered seven questions assessing their at- titudes and beliefs about the person described in the para- graph. These seven items fell into two groups: personal Attitudes Toward Parents 439 affective evaluations of the target and beliefs about others’ opinions of the target. Affective evaluations. Five items assessed participants’ affective evaluations of the target. Two items pertained to whether participants thought the target was a good par- ent (“John [Jennifer] is a good parent”) and whether the target was contributing equally to the family’s well-being (“John [Jennifer] is contributing equally to the family’s well- being”). Two items assessed beliefs that were especially rel- evant to employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers. In particular, it is frequently suggested that employed mothers
  • 15. are more selfish than stay-at-home mothers (Russo, 1976). Therefore we included an item assessing this particular be- lief (“John [Jennifer] is selfish”). Another item focused on participants’ attitudes toward the target’s decision (“John’s [Jennifer’s] decision to work was a good one”). These four items were assessed with 9-point Likert-type scales (1 = completely disagree, 9 = completely agree). The final item in this set consisted of a feeling thermometer for the target person (“On a scale from 0–100, how warmly or coldly do you feel toward this person? [0 = extremely cold, 50 = neu- tral, 100 = extremely warm]). Feeling thermometers have been widely used as a measure of affective evaluation (e.g., Eagly, Mladinic, & Otto, 1991; Haddock & Zanna, 1994). Others’ opinions. Using 9-point Likert-type scales (1 = completely disagree, 9 = completely agree), two items were included to assess participants’ perceptions of whether these roles are stigmatized differently by gender (“John [Jennifer] is the type of person that others see as success- ful” and “John’s [Jennifer’s] coworkers will respect his [her] decision to stay at home with his [her] children”). We refer to these items throughout as measures of perceived social regard. Last, participants completed demographic information, including gender, race/ethnicity, and age. Participants were then thanked and debriefed. Results We performed a factor analysis on the items so as to de- scribe the variables more parsimoniously. Visual inspection of the scree plot and varimax factor analysis revealed two distinct factors. All of the items had factor loadings over .41 and were therefore retained for further analysis. The first factor contained the Affective Evaluations items while the
  • 16. second factor contained the Others’ Opinions items. The two subscales had moderate internal reliability. Cronbach’s alpha was .70 for the Affective Evaluation items and .46 for the two Others’ Opinions items. Taken together, these two factors accounted for 55% of the total variance. The reason the Others’ Opinions alpha was low was at least partially because there were only two items in this measure. It is common for measures with few items to have reliabilities in this range (e.g., Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Moreover, the relatively low reliability of this measure, while certainly not desirable, does provide a conservative test of our hypoth- esis that scores would differ significantly by condition. At the same time, it should be acknowledged that the relia- bility of this measure is below what is generally considered psychometrically acceptable, potentially qualifying some of the present findings. Affective Evaluations The five affective evaluation items were standardized and summed to form a single index. We predicted that partic- ipants would hold more negative attitudes toward nontra- ditional parents (i.e., employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers) than traditional parents (i.e., employed fathers and stay-at-home mothers). This prediction was tested with a 2 (target gender) " 2 (target role: stay-at-home vs. em- ployed outside the home) " 2 (participant gender) analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the Affective Evaluations index. There were no significant effects involving the gender of participants so all the data were collapsed across this vari- able. As predicted, we did not find main effects for tar- get gender or target role. However, and also as predicted, results revealed the expected target gender " target role interaction, F(1, 68) = 4.37, p < .05. Specifically, the non- traditional parents (employed mother M = #.27, SD = .64;
  • 17. stay-at-home father M = #.08, SD = .58) were evaluated more negatively than traditional parents (employed father M = .09, SD = .77; stay-at-home mother M = .21, SD = .61). Notably, by using the terms nontraditional versus tra- ditional parents, we are describing not a main effect, but the interaction between target role and target gender because the overlap between these two independent variables cre- ates the categories nontraditional and traditional parents. Individual Item Analysis We also examined each item individually to further explore our hypotheses. The item that measured pure affect, the feeling thermometer, revealed the same expected pattern of results as the overall affective evaluations index. Specifically, the interaction between target gender and target role was significant, F(1, 68) = 11.36, p < .01. No main effects were found for target gender or target role. Participants felt less warmly toward employed mothers (M = 64.01, SD = 17.30) and stay-at-home fathers (M = 68.74, SD = 18.11) than employed fathers (M = 75.28, SD = 19.20) and stay-at- home mothers (M = 84.11, SD = 11.45). A planned contrast on the item “Jennifer [John] is a good parent,” suggested that participants believed that the stay- at-home father was a worse parent (M = 6.63, SD = 2.00) than the stay-at-home mother (M = 7.47, SD = 1.84), em- ployed mother (M = 7.44, SD = 1.55), or the employed father (M = 7.68, SD = 1.60), t(69) = 1.91, p = .06. As pre- dicted, a planned contrast also suggested that participants viewed the employed mother as more selfish (M = 6.50, SD = 2.42) than the stay-at-home mother (M = 7.48, SD = 1.86), employed father (M = 7.32, SD = 2.03), or
  • 18. 440 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN stay-at-home father (M = 7.89, SD = 1.66), t(69) = 1.88, p = .06. However, both of these effects were only marginally significant. We also predicted that for the item, “John’s [Jennifer’s] decision to work was a good one,” partici- pants would view the nontraditional parents’ decision as worse than the traditional parents’ decision. A 2 " 2 (employed vs. stay-at-home " mother vs. father) ANOVA supported this hypothesis. The interaction between target gender and target role was significant, F(1, 69) = 1.64, p < .05, meaning that the mothers’ decision to work (M = 7.11, SD = 1.67) was seen as worse than the fathers’ deci- sion to work (M = 7.89, SD = 1.35). Again, no main effects for target gender or target role were found. Finally, partici- pants did not rate the target parents significantly differently on the item, “John [Jennifer] is contributing equally to the family’s well-being” (p > .05 for all). Others’ opinions. We standardized the two items as- sessing others’ opinions and summed them to form a sin- gle index of perceived social regard. The overall one-way ANOVA was significant, F(3, 69) = 9.22, p < .001. More important, the planned contrasts were significant, t(69) = 5.03, p < .001. As expected, participants perceived less so- cial regard for stay-at-home fathers (M = 4.42, SD = 1.79) than employed fathers (M = 6.26, SD = 1.31), stay-at-home mothers (M = 6.03, SD = 1.48), or employed mothers (M = 6.87, SD = 1.16). Discussion Study 1 supported our hypotheses that people hold more negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents than to- ward traditional parents. Contradicting claims that modern society stigmatizes stay-at-home mothers (Robertson, 2000;
  • 19. Schlafly, 2003), but supporting theories of prescriptive gen- der stereotyping (e.g., Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Prentice & Carranza, 2002; Rudman & Glick, 1999), stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers were evaluated more pos- itively than stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers. Presumably because they violate stereotypic prescriptions for how men and women are supposed to behave and the roles they ought to fill, nontraditional parents were disliked relative to traditional parents. An item analysis provided some potential insights into people’s perceptions of employed mothers, stay-at-home fathers, employed fathers, and stay-at-home mothers. Par- ticipants tended to perceive the stay-at-home father as the worst parent. Although speculative, it seems possible that participants saw fathers as lacking the skills to be the pri- mary caretaker for young children. Also, employed mothers were seen as more selfish than stay-at-home mothers, em- ployed fathers, and stay-at-home fathers. Because women’s prescribed role is to care for children, abandoning this role may be perceived as an especially selfish act. In contrast, it seems possible that stay-at-home fathers are seen as rela- tively unselfish because they are adopting a low-status, stig- matized role for the sake of the family. So while employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers were both perceived as having made a bad decision, somewhat different impres- sions may underlie this belief (i.e., perceived selfishness on the part of employed mothers, perceived lack of ability on the part of stay-at-home fathers). Of course this inter- pretation is speculative and additional work is required to clarify the specific cognitions that underlie responses to nontraditional mothers and fathers. Interestingly, perceived social regard was lowest for stay- at-home fathers. Despite evaluating employed mothers
  • 20. negatively, participants felt that other people would re- spect employed mothers and perceive them as successful— perhaps because by assuming the traditionally male “bread- winner” role they gain some of the social status associated with that role. This finding is consistent with the distinction of Fiske et al. (2002) between the perceived warmth and competence of social targets. Apparently, employed moth- ers are disliked but respected, whereas stay-at-home fathers are neither liked nor respected. Fathers appear to be aware of this stigmatization of stay-at-home fathers because they report that one of the major reasons they do not take pater- nity leave is due to the stigma that it will carry (Duindam, 1999). This reluctance on the part of fathers to assume a homemaker role (even temporarily) may limit mothers’ em- ployment opportunities and serve as an important barrier to gender equality both in the home and in the workplace. An alternative explanation for the present results is that the statistical infrequency of stay-at-home fathers may ex- plain why people react to them negatively. Indeed, research on the mere exposure effect shows that increased familiarity with a stimulus can increase liking of the stimulus (Zajonc, 1980). Although we certainly do not rule out the possibility that a lack of familiarity makes some contribution to atti- tudes toward stay-at-home fathers, this is not a satisfying explanation for the present results. While employed moth- ers are far more statistically frequent than stay-at-home fa- thers, they were not better liked. Stay-at-home fathers were only rated more negatively than employed mothers when it came to perceptions of other people’s beliefs. There is no evidence that familiarity with a stimulus has a greater influence on perceptions of other people’s attitudes toward the stimulus than it does on one’s own attitudes. Theories of prescriptive stereotyping provide a much better account of the present data than an explanation based on the statistical frequency of the groups in question.
  • 21. STUDY 2 Study 1 leaves open the question of whether participants feel any sense of compunction about expressing negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents. Previous work has documented that White people often feel guilty and self- critical when they have stereotypical reactions to Black people (Devine et al., 1991; Monteith et al., 2002). Racial discrimination is rare when such a bias is obvious and, Attitudes Toward Parents 441 in general, under circumstances that promote socially desirable responding (Evans, Garcia, Garcia, & Baron, 2003; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Hodson et al., 2002). However, additional research suggests that people are much less concerned about discriminating based on gender than on race (Czopp & Monteith, 2003). In Study 2, we employed a within-subjects design to determine whether participants would continue to report negative reactions to stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers, relative to employed fathers and stay-at-home mothers, when their evaluations were assessed at the same time. Under such circumstances, a bias based on the gender of the employed or stay-at-home parent would be blatantly obvious. Therefore, any preju- dice against nontraditional parents that participants express must occur with relatively little compunction. Method Participants Seventy-nine adults (46 males and 33 females) between the
  • 22. ages of 17 and 53 years (M = 30.11, SD = 12.62) were re- cruited from a public park in Connecticut. Participants were given a lottery ticket, a drink, or paid $1.00 in exchange for participation in the study. Sixty-five participants were European American and the remaining 15 participants were African American, Asian, and Hispanic. None of the partic- ipants under the age of 30 had children whereas 65% of the participants over 30 years of age reported having one or more children. Additionally, 91.14% of the participants re- ported that they were currently working outside the home, ranging from 8 hours per week to 65 hours per week (M = 38.22, SD = 13.29). Procedure and Measures Participants read four descriptions of parents: an employed father, an employed mother, a stay-at-home father, and a stay-at-home mother. These materials were identical to those used in Study 1 and were presented in a random or- der. In Study 1, the feeling thermometer rating correlated .89 with the entire affective evaluation index. Participants then completed demographic information (including gender, race/ethnicity, and age) and were then thanked and debriefed. Results and Discussion We expected that even using a within-subjects design, in which participants evaluated stay-at-home and employed mothers and fathers at the same time, more positive affect would be reported toward traditional than nontraditional parents. These predictions were tested with a mixed model, within- and between-subjects (with gender of participant as the between-subjects factor) ANOVA on the feeling ther- mometer ratings.
  • 23. There were no significant effects involving the gender of participants, so all the data were collapsed across participant gender. However, as predicted, the within-subjects ANOVA was significant, F(3, 231) = 10.60, p < .01. Within-subjects contrasts revealed that participants reported significantly more positive attitudes toward traditional than nontradi- tional parents. Specifically, attitudes toward stay-at-home mothers (M = 79.00, SD = 19.10) were significantly more positive than attitudes toward stay-at-home fathers (M = 65.40, SD = 24.75), F(1, 77) = 19.93, p < .001, and em- ployed mothers (M = 71.55, SD = 23.96), F(1, 77) = 5.22, p < .05. Likewise, attitudes toward employed fathers (M = 84.82, SD = 19.01) were significantly more positive than at- titudes toward employed mothers (M = 71.55, SD = 23.96), F(1, 77) = 22.30, p < .001, and stay-at-home fathers (M = 65.40, SD = 24.75), F(1, 77) = 27.93, p < .001. As predicted, attitudes toward the two traditional parents (stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers) were not significantly differ- ent from each other nor were attitudes toward the two non- traditional parents. Notably, participants reported nearly the same pattern of attitudes in this within-subjects design as in the between-subjects design in Study 1. Although di- rect statistical comparisons cannot be made across studies, assessing attitudes toward parents using a within-subjects design clearly did not eliminate participants’ self-reported dislike for nontraditional parents. STUDY 3 Studies 1 and 2 demonstrated that people generally hold more positive attitudes toward traditional than nontradi- tional parents. Study 2 further indicated that people will re- port negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents even under conditions that enhance social desirability concerns.
  • 24. When participants evaluated stay-at-home and employed mothers and fathers side-by-side, in a within-subjects de- sign, they continued to report more negative attitudes to- ward employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers. This complements research suggesting that people feel little compunction about stereotyping based on gender (Czopp & Monteith, 2003). Study 3 built on Studies 1 and 2 by investigating whether attitudes toward employed parents would vary based on the reasons that parents provide for working outside the home. In Study 3, the target parent was said to work outside the home either for personal fulfillment or out of financial necessity. Bridges and Orza (1992) examined reactions to employed and unemployed mothers while varying their em- ployment motive. They found that participants perceived an employed mother who worked out of personal fulfillment as less communal than the employed mother who worked out of financial need. We extend this work by looking at at- titudes rather than trait attributions and further examining the effects of motivation on reactions to fathers. Because women who work out of financial necessity are violating prescriptive gender stereotypes for reasons be- yond their control, we hypothesized that they would pro- voke less negative reactions than women who work outside 442 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN the home for reasons of personal fulfillment. Research on moral judgment indicates that actors are blamed less for engaging in socially undesirable behaviors when the act is externally compelled or otherwise outside of their control (Weiner, 1995, 1996). Women who work for personal ful-
  • 25. fillment may be perceived as willfully neglecting gender prescriptions such as the “Motherhood Mandate,” which demands that women always be available to their children (Russo, 1976). They should therefore receive more moral censure than women who work outside of the home be- cause their family’s financial circumstances leave them lit- tle option. However, motivation should have little impact on evaluations of employed fathers because, whatever their reason for employment, they are fulfilling their traditional, expected role. Method Participants One hundred twelve adults (51 males and 61 females) be- tween the ages of 18 and 75 years (M = 34.50, SD = 14.74) were recruited from a public park in Connecticut. Eighty percent of the sample was European American. The re- maining 20% were African American, Asian, and Hispanic. Participants were given a lottery ticket, a drink, or paid $1.00 in exchange for participation in the study. Procedure and Measures Participants were randomly assigned to read one of six paragraph-long descriptions. Each paragraph described ei- ther a father’s or a mother’s decision to work outside the home and one of two motives (personal fulfillment or fi- nancial necessity) for why the target parent made that deci- sion. A third, control condition did not describe the parent’s motivation for his or her decision. The six paragraphs were identical in every way except for the gender of the parent and the motive for working outside the home, yielding a 2 (participant gender) " 2 (target gender) " 3 (motive: financial, personal fulfillment, none mentioned) design.
  • 26. Participants answered seven questions assessing their at- titudes and beliefs about the parent. Five of these seven items were identical to the affective evaluation items used in Study 1 and two additional items assessed participants’ beliefs about the level of dedication the target parent pos- sessed (“John is a dedicated father”) and perceptions of the warmth of the target parent (“John is a warm person”), using 9-point Likert-type scales (1 = completely disagree, 9 = completely agree). We added these two items to bet- ter assess affective reactions to the targets. As predicted, a factor analysis with a varimax rotation revealed one distinct factor tapping affective evaluations for these seven items (Cronbach’s alpha = .58). Last, participants completed demographic information, including gender, race/ethnicity, and age. Participants were then thanked and debriefed. Results and Discussion The seven affective evaluation items were standardized and summed to form a single index. We predicted that partici- pants would hold the most negative attitudes toward moth- ers who worked outside the home for personal fulfillment, compared to mothers who worked outside the home out of financial necessity and fathers who worked outside the home regardless of motive. We also predicted that partici- pants would hold equally positive attitudes toward fathers who work outside the home for personal fulfillment or fi- nancial necessity. In other words, for fathers, the reason given for working outside the home should not have an im- pact on participants’ attitudes. These predictions were tested with a 2 " 2 " 3 ANOVA and planned contrasts on the Affective Evaluations index.
  • 27. Consistent with Studies 1 and 2, there were no signifi- cant effects involving the gender of participants, so all the data were collapsed across participant gender. Results were consistent with our hypotheses, F(5, 102) = 3.34, p < .01. Specifically, planned contrasts revealed that participants re- ported the most negative attitudes toward mothers who worked outside the home for personal fulfillment, com- pared to all other types of employed parents. Simple effects analyses revealed that fathers were evaluated the same re- gardless of the reason stated for their working outside the home. Examining the feeling thermometer item separately from the other Affective Evaluation items revealed the same pattern of results (see Table 1). Specifically, participants felt most coldly toward women who worked outside the home for personal fulfillment compared to all other types of em- ployed parents, t(103) = 3.52, p < .01. The evaluation of employed fathers was not impacted by the stated reason for their working outside the home, while the evaluation of employed mothers was affected by the stated reasons for their working outside the home, F(2, 53) = 3.67, p < .05. GENERAL DISCUSSION All three studies reveal that people report more negative at- titudes toward nontraditional parents (i.e., employed moth- ers and stay-at-home fathers) than toward traditional par- ents (i.e., stay-at-home mothers and employed fathers). This finding is consistent with other research showing that Table 1 Mean Feeling Thermometer Ratings of Employed Mothers and Fathers by Employment Motive (Study 3)
  • 28. Gender of Target Reason for Employment Father Mother Financial need 69.21 (18.65) 66.06 (12.74) Personal fulfillment 66.50 (23.16) 47.84 (25.20) No reason given (control) 63.24 (15.30) 56.58 (20.35) Attitudes Toward Parents 443 people dislike those who violate prescriptive stereotypes (e.g., Rudman & Glick, 1999) and prior studies of the trait attributions made about traditional and nontraditional parents (Bridges & Etaugh, 1995; Bridges & Orza, 1993; Etaugh & Folger, 1998; Etaugh & Moss, 2001; Etaugh & Nekolny, 1990; Etaugh & Petroski, 1985; Etaugh & Poertner, 1992). However, the present research is the first to empirically document prejudice against stay-at-home fa- thers, a stigmatized category that has received insufficient attention in the literature. Participants’ beliefs about other people’s reactions to stay-at-home and employed mothers and fathers were fur- ther examined in Study 1, revealing that perceived social regard was lowest for stay-at-home fathers. The perceived social stigmatization of male homemakers may represent a major barrier to mothers’ opportunities if it makes fathers reluctant to stay at home with the children while their wife works outside the home. Notably, perceived social regard for employed mothers was just as high as for traditional parents. It may be that by assuming the traditionally male breadwinner role, employed women accrue some of the so- cial respect and regard associated with that role. Thus, em- ployed women may be simultaneously disliked and socially
  • 29. respected. This highlights the distinction between percep- tions of competence and warmth drawn by previous re- searchers (Fiske et al., 2002; Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999). Participants apparently felt little compunction about ex- pressing negative attitudes toward nontraditional parents. People generally experience guilt and self-criticism about their negative feelings toward racial minorities (Devine et al., 1991). Other work indicates that people are most likely to discriminate based on race under ambiguous cir- cumstances, when their prejudices are easy to rationalize and justify (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Hodson et al., 2002). However, even in Study 2’s within-subjects design, where gender-biased judgments were blatantly obvious, partici- pants continued to evaluate nontraditional parents more negatively than traditional parents. These results are con- sistent with those of Czopp and Monteith (2003), who found that people feel guilty at the thought of stereotyping a Black person, but amused at the thought of stereotyping a woman. As Fiske and Stevens (1993) note, gender stereotypes may be more strongly prescriptive, and therefore normative, than stereotypes of other groups. As a consequence, people may not fear social censure for expressing gender stereo- types to the same extent that they do for racial stereotypes. The effects of motivations for working on attitudes to- ward employed mothers and fathers were investigated in Study 3. Participants reported more negative attitudes to- ward mothers who worked out of personal fulfillment than toward mothers who worked out of financial necessity or mothers who did not mention a reason for employment. However, motivation for working outside the home did not affect people’s attitudes toward employed fathers. This find- ing suggests that mothers are subjected to an unfair double
  • 30. standard in that they are required to have a socially accept- able reason for working outside the home while fathers are not. If employed mothers are thought to work outside the home for personal fulfillment, they may be perceived as failing to fulfill the role of the selfless mother, thus causing people to dislike them (Russo, 1976). Remarkably, no gender differences in attitudes toward traditional and nontraditional parents were observed. One might expect that female participants would feel more pos- itively toward employed mothers and stay-at-home fathers, given that women should be less likely to endorse and act on prescriptive stereotypes that are detrimental to their own life opportunities. However, these results are consis- tent with theories in which consensual ideologies, adopted by both dominant and subordinate group members, pro- mote social inequality (Jost & Banaji, 1994; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). For example, Jost and Banaji’s (1994) system justification theory proposes that due to implicit socialization by the dominant culture, members of low status groups adopt stereotypes and be- lief systems that perpetuate their low social position. For example, many African Americans endorse the Protestant Work Ethic, which implies that laziness is the primary cause of poverty (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Similarly, women of- ten endorse ideologies such as benevolent sexism, which holds that “good” women who fulfill their traditional roles as mothers and wives should be “put on a pedestal” (Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001). Indeed, many women likely enjoy the special status traditional gender roles accord the “weaker sex.” However, they may do so at the cost of limiting their personal options and those of women as a whole. It is equally important to note that men’s life choices are also limited by restrictive gender roles and prescriptive gender stereotypes. Some men may want to care for their
  • 31. children full-time rather than working outside the home, but the stigma attached to being a stay-at-home father may prevent them from doing so. Prescriptive gender stereo- types and the stigma attached to violations of them limit and restrict both men’s and women’s opportunities and lives. Previous research that has examined the consequences of prescriptive stereotype violations generally has not fo- cused on whether men who violate prescriptive gender stereotypes experience similar “backlash” effects as women who violate gender norms. The media, however, has re- cently paid a great deal of attention to the stigma that stay-at- home fathers face. For example, a recent article in the Wall Street Journal (“Stay-at-home dads,” 2003) reported that employers view stay-at-home fathers either with disdain or confusion. Sometimes employers even “wonder whether ‘stay-at-home dad’ is a cover for ‘couldn’t find work.’” (“Stay- at-home dads,” 2003). Anecdotal reports have even sur- faced of parents not allowing their children to socialize with the children of stay-at-home fathers and employed mothers (“Your career,” 2001). To our knowledge, the present studies are the first to document this stigmatization of stay-at-home fathers. 444 BRESCOLL AND UHLMANN One limitation of the present research is worth noting. The present studies examined attitudes toward parents with an infant. This scenario was selected as particularly relevant to prescriptive gender stereotypes regarding care of chil- dren. However, only future research can reveal whether the present findings generalize to parents whose children are older.
  • 32. Future research should further seek to understand the reasons why people dislike nontraditional parents and, more generally, why people dislike those who violate prescrip- tive gender stereotypes. Perhaps reactions to nontraditional parents are negative because people generally dislike indi- viduals who violate social norms (Cialdini & Trost, 1998). However, this only raises the issue of how and why such norms developed in the first place. Prescriptive stereotypes may serve the system-justifying function of keeping women “in their place,” such that they do not act in ways that dis- rupt the social order (Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Jost & Banaji, 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). That is, people may dislike employed mothers because they (consciously or un- consciously) perceive these women as trying to increase their power and status, thereby rejecting a subordinate role. Initial submission: December 30, 2003 Initial acceptance: December 14, 2004 Final acceptance: April 5, 2005 REFERENCES Ajzen, I. (1996). The directive influence of attitudes on behavior. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 385–403). New York: Guilford. Bridges, J. S., & Etaugh, C. (1995). College students’ perceptions of mothers: Effects of maternal employment-childrearing pattern and motive for employment. Sex Roles, 32, 735–751. Bridges, J. S., Etaugh, C., & Barnes-Farrell, J. (2002). Trait judg- ments of stay-at-home and employed parents: A function of
  • 33. social role and/or shifting standards? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 140–150. Bridges, J. S., & Orza, A. M. (1992). The effects of employment role and motive for employment on the perceptions of mothers. Sex Roles, 27, 331–343. Bridges, J. S., & Orza, A. M. (1993). Effects of maternal employ- ment childrearing pattern on college students’ perceptions of a mother and her child. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 103–117. Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity, and compliance. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psy- chology (4th ed., pp. 151–192). New York: McGraw-Hill. Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An “experimental ethnography.” Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 70, 945–960. Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1314–1329. Czopp, A. M., & Monteith, M. J. (2003). Confronting prejudice (literally): Reactions to confrontations of racial and gender bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 532– 544. Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.
  • 34. Devine, P. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18. Devine, P. G., Monteith, M. J., Zuwerink, J. R., & Elliot, A. J. (1991). Prejudice with and without compunction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 817–830. Dovidio, J. F., Brigham, J. C., Johnson, B. T., & Gaertner, S. L. (1996). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination: An- other look. In N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping (pp. 276–319). New York: Guilford. Duindam, V. (1999). Men in the household: Caring fathers. In L. McKie, S. Bowlby, & S. Gregory (Eds.), Gender, power and the household. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316–326. Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prej- udice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3–22. Eagly, A. H., Mladinic, A., & Otto, S. (1991). Are women
  • 35. evaluated more favorably than men? An analysis of attitudes, beliefs, and emotions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 203– 216. Etaugh, C., & Folger, D. (1998). Perceptions of parents whose work and parenting behaviors deviate from role expecta- tions. Sex Roles, 39, 215–223. Etaugh, C., & Moss, C. (2001). Attitudes of employed women toward parents who choose full-time or part-time em- ployment following their child’s birth. Sex Roles, 44, 611– 619. Etaugh, C., & Nekolny, K. (1990). Effects of employment status and marital status on perceptions of mothers. Sex Roles, 23, 273–280. Etaugh, C., & Petroski, B. (1985). Perceptions of women: Effects of employment status and marital status. Sex Roles, 12, 329– 339. Etaugh, C., & Poertner, P. (1991). Effects of occupational prestige, employment status, and marital status on perceptions of mothers. Sex Roles, 24, 345–353. Etaugh, C., & Poertner, P. (1992). Perceptions of women: Influ- ence of performance: Marital, and parental variables. Sex Roles, 26, 311–321. Evans, D. C., Garcia, D. G., Garcia, D. M., & Baron, R. S. (2003). In the privacy of their own homes: Using the internet to as- sess racial bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
  • 36. 29, 273–284. Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 357–411). New York: McGraw-Hill. Attitudes Toward Parents 445 Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902. Fiske, S. T., & Stevens, L. E. (1993). What’s so special about sex? Gender stereotyping and discrimination. In S. Oskamp & M. Costanza (Eds.), Gender issues in contemporary society (Vol. 6., pp. 173–196). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 61–89). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
  • 37. Haddock, G., & Zanna, G. (1994). Preferring “housewives” to “feminists”: Categorization and the favorability of attitudes toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 25–52. Hodson, G., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Processes in racial discrimination: Differential weighting of conflicting information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 460–471. Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false conscious- ness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1–27. Judd, C. M., Park, B., Ryan, C. S., Brauer, M., & Kraus, S. (1995). Stereotypes and ethnocentrism: Diverging interethnic per- ceptions of African American and White American youth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 460–481. Livingston, R. W. (2002). The role of perceived negativity in the moderation of African Americans’ implicit and explicit racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 405–413. Miller, J. J., & Ponnuru, R. (2001). A modest proposal for Jane Swift. National Review Online. Retrieved Sept. 3, 2005 from http://www.nationalreview.com/daily/nr052101.shtml Monteith, M. J., Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Czopp, A. M. (2002). Putting the brakes on prejudice: On the develop- ment and operation of cues for control. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 83, 1029–1050.
  • 38. Muller, R., & Goldberg, S. (1980). Why William doesn’t want a doll: Preschoolers’ expectations of adult behavior toward girls and boys. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 259–269. Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external moti- vation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 811–832. Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psy- chology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281. Robertson, B. (2000). There’s no place like home: How business, government, and our obsession with work has driven par- ents from home. New York: Spence. Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645. Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1004– 1010. Russo, N. (1976). The motherhood mandate. Journal of Social Issues, 32, 143–153. Schlafly, P. (2003). Feminist fantasies: Essays on feminism in the
  • 39. media, the workplace, the home, and the military. New York: Spence. Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influ- ences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 51, 515–530. Sechrist, G. B., & Stangor, C. (2001). Perceived consensus in- fluences intergroup behavior and stereotype accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 645–654. Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cam- bridge University Press. Silverstein, L. B. (1996). Fathering is a feminist issue. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 3–37. Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A., & Ford, T. E. (1991). Affective and cognitive determinants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9, 359–380. Stay-at-home dads fight stigma. (2003, September 9). Wall Street Journal, p. B1. U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). Children’s living arrangements and characteristics: March 2002. Current population reports, P20-547. Washington, DC: Author. Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility. New York: Guilford.
  • 40. Weiner, B. (1996). Searching for order in social motivation. Psy- chological Inquiry, 7, 199–216. Your career matters: Have husband, will travel. (2001, February 13). Wall Street Journal, p. B1. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.