SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 5
Download to read offline
, 20130518, published 11 September 201392013Biol. Lett.
Vivek Thuppil and Richard G. Coss
growls according to perceived danger
Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard
Supplementary data
ml
http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/suppl/2013/09/06/rsbl.2013.0518.DC1.ht
"Data Supplement"
References
http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/9/5/20130518.full.html#ref-list-1
This article cites 16 articles, 1 of which can be accessed free
Subject collections
(633 articles)behaviour
Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections
Email alerting service hereright-hand corner of the article or click
Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top
http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptionsgo to:Biol. Lett.To subscribe to
on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org
Research
Cite this article: Thuppil V, Coss RG. 2013
Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive
tiger and leopard growls according to
perceived danger. Biol Lett 9: 20130518.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0518
Received: 5 June 2013
Accepted: 16 August 2013
Subject Areas:
behaviour
Keywords:
antipredator behaviour, Asian elephant,
sound playbacks, growls
Author for correspondence:
Vivek Thuppil
e-mail: vthuppil@ucdavis.edu
Electronic supplementary material is available
at http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0518 or
via http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org.
Animal behaviour
Wild Asian elephants distinguish
aggressive tiger and leopard growls
according to perceived danger
Vivek Thuppil1 and Richard G. Coss1,2
1
Animal Behavior Graduate Group, and 2
Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
Prey species exhibit antipredator behaviours such as alertness, aggression and
flight, among others, in response to predators. The nature of this response is
variable, with animals reacting more strongly in situations of increased vulner-
ability. Our research described here is the first formal study to investigate
night-time antipredator behaviour in any species of elephants, Asian or
African. We examined the provocative effects of elephant-triggered tiger and
leopard growls while elephants attempted to crop-raid. Tigers opportunisti-
cally prey on elephant calves, whereas leopards pose no threat; therefore, we
predicted that the elephant response would be reflective of this difference.
Elephants reacted similarly cautiously to the simulated presence of felids of
both species by eventually moving away, but differed markedly in their
more immediate behavioural responses. Elephants retreated silently to tiger-
growl playbacks, whereas they responded with aggressive vocalizations,
such as trumpets and grunts, to leopard-growl playbacks. Elephants also lin-
gered in the area and displayed alert or investigative behaviours in response to
leopard growls when compared with tiger growls. We anticipate that the
methods outlined here will promote further study of elephant antipredator
behaviour in a naturalistic context, with applications for conservation efforts
as well.
1. Introduction
Antipredator behaviour refers to a suite of behaviours that prey species exhibit
in response to predators including alertness, aggression, flight and vocalizing,
among others [1–3]. This antipredator response is reflective of the degree of
predatory threat; animals respond more strongly to a more dangerous preda-
tory species or to situations of increased vulnerability [4]. While these
behaviours have been documented extensively in many mammals, they are
virtually unexplored in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus).
Comparatively, more antipredator behaviour research has been conducted on
African elephants (Loxodonta africana). McComb et al. [5] showed that, in African
elephants, female family groups hearing lion-roar playbacks were able to identify
situations that represented the greatest danger and those with older matriarchs
were particularly adept at recognizing the specific danger presented by male
(as opposed to female) lions. Other research on elephant risk assessment has
shown that African elephants retreat and alarm call in response to playbacks of
threatening African bees [6]. In contrast to these daytime playback studies, our
research is the first empirical investigation of night-time elephant antipredator
behaviour and the first to examine elephant differentiation of growls from two
sympatric felid species posing differential predatory threats.
We investigated whether wild elephants could discriminate the aggressive
growls of tigers and leopards. Tigers are known to opportunistically prey on ele-
phant calves, whereasthere is no mention, either in the literature or anecdotally, of
elephants being a part of leopards’ diets [7–9]. Elephants themselves produce
& 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.
on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
guttural growls while communicating with conspecifics [10,11]
and may possess a broad, low-frequency acoustical-assessment
ability useful for differentiating similar growls of other species.
Therefore, we predicted that elephants would discriminate the
acoustically distinct growls of tigers and leopards and would
display stronger antipredator behaviour to the more dangerous
tiger-growl playbacks.
2. Material and methods
From previous research, we had recordings of aggressive growls
of a single tiger and leopard at the Bannerghatta Zoological Park,
Bangalore, obtained using a Sennheiser ME 80 directional micro-
phone coupled to a Sony TC-D5 PROII analogue recorder. To
engender growling, both cats were agitated similarly when the
keeper entered their cages and banged a stick repeatedly. We
did not repeat this procedure with other individuals owing to
the potential danger involved. The natural pauses in growling
were used to split the growl playbacks into two exemplars
each, with durations of 9 and 17 s and 21 and 35 s for the leopard
and tiger, respectively. We digitized these growl recordings in
16-bit mode and 48 kHz sampling frequency for random play-
back by Blue Point Engineering 10-channel mp3 players. We
used AUDACITY software to generate spectrograms of the onset
of felid growls (figure 1).
We conducted field research near villages around Bandipur
Tiger Reserve and Wayanad Wildlife Sanctuary in southern
India. Playback growls were amplified by battery-powered
200 W PylePro speaker systems in water-proof enclosures posi-
tioned approximately 50 m ahead of where elephants triggered
the playbacks. Playback growls were produced at an intensity of
105 dB sound pressure level (SPL) at a distance of 1 m from the
source. At a distance of 50 m, elephants heard these growls at
intensities of 58–62 dB SPL. Elephants triggered these playbacks
by tripping a Takex PB-60 TK active infrared beam positioned at
a height of 1.85 m. Elephants encountered these playback systems
along paths leading to crop fields and we assume that these
elephants were similarly motivated to crop-raid.
We deployed this set-up simultaneously in three study sites
from August 2010 to February 2011 and recorded a total of 26
incidents of elephants activating leopard- and tiger-growl play-
backs. For the purposes of our analyses here, we have only
included distinct individuals in their first encounter of the play-
back system, without any repeated playbacks. This corresponds
to seven instances where elephants encountered tiger-growl
playbacks and eight instances where elephants encountered
leopard-growl playbacks.
We logged post-playback elephant movement paths on a
Garmin eTrex device by tracking their footprints on the morning
following a beam trip. From the point of beam trip, the difference
of the compass bearing of the elephant’s position at 25 m away and
the compass bearing of the speaker provided a movement angle
relative to the speaker. Elephant movements were categorized as
toward, lateral and away, corresponding to movement angle
ranges of 0–608, 60–1208 and 1208–1808, respectively. We used a
1-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA) test to determine whether
there was a significant difference in movement trajectory between
the two felid-growl categories. Upon discovering that the means
were nearly identical, we used the inverse F-ratio (with reversed
d.f.) statistic to determine whether the means were reliably similar.
Using high-definition infrared video cameras equipped
with microphones, we individually identified elephants using
15
10
5
0
15
10
5
0
15
10
5
kHzkHzkHz
0
0 0.5 1.0 1.5
0 0.5 1.0
0 0.5 1.0
seconds
1.4
1.5
(a)
(b)
(c)
Figure 1. Spectrograms of the onset of (a) tiger, (b) leopard and (c) elephant growls. The leopard growl exhibits a greater energy distribution at higher frequencies
than the tiger growl, yielding a raspier sound. The guttural pulse rate of the elephant growl approximates that of the leopard growl, a property that might facilitate
growl discrimination. (Online version in colour.)
rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgBiolLett9:20130518
2
on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
morphological attributes, such as ear or tusk shape, and herd
composition, if any. We aged elephants into adult/sub-adult or
juvenile categories based on height [12]. Our video cameras
also captured tigers and leopards at our study sites, ensuring
that these playbacks represented naturalistic threats.
Owing to the constraints of passively recording elephant be-
haviour at night, we restricted quantification of behaviour to
frequencies of occurrence instead of rates of behaviour reported
in daytime studies [6]. To compare the interactions of frequencies
of occurrence, we used multinomial log-linear analyses with
maximum-likelihood estimations. We observed both individual
elephants and herds of elephants encountering our playbacks
(table 1). In situations of herds encountering our playbacks, we
analysed the behaviour of the elephant leading the herd, usually
an adult female, whose response always characterized the overall
herd behaviour captured on video.
We used ANOVA to compare elephant reaction times, quanti-
fied in milliseconds (ms), from frame-by-frame mpeg4 video in
increments of 33 ms. Elephant vocalizations that were recorded on
video were categorized as trumpets and grunts (see electronic
supplementary material) based on previous literature showing
spectrograms and descriptions of dominant frequencies and dur-
ations [10,11]. Elephant grunts [13] have also been characterized
as ‘roars’ [11]. In some instances where a herd of elephants triggered
felid-growl playbacks, elephants also produced growls, which were
captured by our video cameras. Using AUDACITY software, we have
provided a spectrogram of an elephant growl for comparison
purposes (figure 1).
3. Results
Initial elephant responses to all playbacks were nearly instan-
taneous (mean reaction time ¼ 575 ms, range ¼ 393–820 ms,
n ¼ 11). Similarly, elephants vocalized shortly after growl-
playback onset (mean latency ¼ 4.3 s, range ¼ 2–10 s, n ¼ 7),
rendering natural differences in growl duration as unimportant
because elephants vocalized prior to growl termination.
We recorded seven instances where elephants encountered
tiger-growl playbacks and eight instances where elephants
encountered leopard-growl playbacks. Elephants exhibited
reliably similar movement angles away from both tiger- and
leopard-growl playbacks (1/F13,1 ¼ 378.39; p , 0.05). However,
a comparison of more immediate elephant responses provides
different insight into elephant antipredator behaviour.
Elephants vocalized in a reliably different manner to
playbacks of tiger and leopard growls (likelihood ratio
x2
1 ¼ 15:90; p , 0.001). They silently retreated in response to
tiger growls and vocalized in all but one instance to the
leopard growls (figure 2). Elephants were expected to linger
less, and thus display a lower likelihood of alert or investiga-
tive behaviours to growls posing a greater threat. Indeed, a
greater frequency of elephants displayed alert or investigative
behaviours (likelihood ratio x2
1 ¼ 4:43; p ¼ 0.035) to leopard-
growl playbacks than to tiger-growl playbacks (figure 2).
These behaviours included instances of elephants remain-
ing stationary, searching for acoustic and olfactory cues, as
well as elephants walking around and actively investigating
the area.
The dominant vocalizations in response to leopard growls
(figure 2), compared with the absence of vocalizing to tiger
growls, were trumpets (likelihood ratio x2
1 ¼ 8:51; p ¼ 0.004)
Table 1. Demographic breakdown of elephants that encountered our felid-growl playbacks. For herds, the leading elephant’s behavioural response was
considered for our analyses. All elephant encounters shown in this table are distinct individuals with no repeated playbacks.
no. instances
single
male
elephant
single
female
elephant
herd led
by male
elephant
herd led
by female
elephant
per cent of
herds that
contained
more than 1
adult
elephant
per cent of
herds that
contained
non-leading
adult males
per cent of
herds that
contained at
least one
juvenile
elephant
leopard-growl
playbacks
0 0 0 8 25 12.5 100
tiger-growl
playbacks
1 3 1 2 66.7 33.3 66.7
100
elephant vigilance and vocalizations
80
trumpeting
*
**
***
***
grunting
all vocalizations
alert/investigative behaviours
60
40
20
tiger growls
leopard tiger
123.88° 122.86°
movement away from playback origin
leopard growls
0
(%)
Figure 2. Elephant discrimination of felid-growl playbacks: *p  0.05;
**p  0.01; ***p  0.001. Frequency comparisons of elephants displaying
nearly immediate alert or investigative behaviours and vocalizing; delayed
elephant movement trajectories away from the felid-growl playbacks are
shown as means (solid lines) and standard deviations (dashed lines).
rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgBiolLett9:20130518
3
on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
and grunts (likelihood ratio x2
1 ¼ 11:193; p , 0.001). Both
trumpets and grunts are used by elephants in instances of
alarm, disturbance and interspecific aggression [11]. After
hearing an angry leopard maintaining its position rather
than fleeing, some elephants exhibited conflicted behaviour
as evidenced by their investigative circling, kicking dirt and
even kicking a battery box powering the playback-trigger
device. Such displays of redirected aggression are typical
following adversarial encounters [14].
4. Discussion
While both felid-growl playback categories eventually evoked
similarly cautious retreat away from the speaker systems, there
were interesting differences in the more immediate elephant
responses, indicating that elephants were able to discriminate
tiger and leopard growls and perceived differing levels of
threat from these two playback categories.
Elephants retreated stealthily from dangerous tigers,
yet announced their presence by vocalizing, particularly
through aggressive vocalizations, for instance trumpets, in
the majority of leopard-growl playback situations. Elephants
also did not linger in the area by displaying alert or investiga-
tive behaviours to tiger-growl playbacks. While there were
some demographic differences in exposure to the two playback
categories (table 1), we do not believe that this impacted our
results. Both herds and lone elephants encountering tiger-
growl playbacks responded identically by retreating quickly
and silently.
Our results are consistent with findings that African
elephants retreat significantly more quickly from clothes associ-
ated with dangerous Maasai hunters compared with ethnic
groups that present lower levels of perceived threat [15].
Elephants did display aggressive behaviours when dangerous
Maasai hunter clothes were displayed visually, but Maasai
olfactory cues were absent. Bates et al. [15] concluded that the
presence of clothes without accompanying olfactory cues indi-
cated that the humans were no longer present in the area.
Thus, a reduced level of threat perception allowed elephants
to react more aggressively, as we propose happened when our
elephants perceived lesser threat from leopard growls.
Research on elephant threat assessment can have other
applications as well. For example, fences incorporating hives
of dangerous African bees deterred crop-raiding attempting
elephants from breaking through them [16]. Similarly, we
implemented our research in a manner that mitigated night-
time crop-raiding [17]. Thus, we anticipate that future elephant
antipredator behaviour research will generate useful appli-
cations for elephant conservation in addition to providing
insights into how the world’s largest terrestrial animals
assess threats across their myriad habitats.
Acknowledgements. We thank A. Pittet, R. Johnson and S. Joshi for provid-
ing electronics assistance and R. Sukumar for hosting our research at his
field research station. We thank D. Thiwary for logistical support and
our assistants Arun, Madhan, Rajesh, Sudhakar and Sunil. We thank
the Karnataka and Kerala forest departments for research permissions.
Funding statement. Thisresearchwasfinanciallysupportedbythe U.S.Fish
and Wildlife Service Asian Elephant Conservation Fund, the Rufford
Small Grants Foundation and the University of California, Davis.
References
1. Sih A. 1992 Prey uncertainty and the balancing
of antipredator feeding needs. Am. Nat. 139,
1052–1069. (doi:10.1086/285372)
2. Coss RG, McCowan B, Ramakrishnan U. 2007 Threat-
related acoustical differences in alarm calls by wild
bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) elicited by
python and leopard models. Ethology 113,
352–367. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01336.x)
3. Fichtel C. 2007 Avoiding predators at night:
antipredator strategies in red-tailed sportive
lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus). Am. J. Primatol.
69, 611–624.
4. Owings DH, Coss RG, McKernon D, Rowe MP,
Arrowood PC. 2001 Snake-directed antipredator
behaviour of rock squirrels (Spermophilus
variegates): population differences and snake-
species discrimination. Behaviour 138, 575–595.
5. McComb K, Shannon G, Durant SM, Sayialel K,
Slotow R, Poole J, Moss C. 2011 Leadership in
elephants: the adaptive value of age. Proc. R. Soc. B
278, 3270–3276. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0168)
6. King LE, Soltis J, Douglas-Hamilton I, Savage A,
Vollrath F. 2010 Bee threat elicits alarm call
in African elephants. PLoS ONE 5, e10346. (doi:10.
1371/journal.pone.0010346)
7. Hayward MW, Henschel P, O’Brien J, Hofmeyr M,
Balme G, Kerley GIH. 2006 Prey preferences of the
leopard (Panthera pardus). J. Zool. 270, 298–313.
(doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00139.x)
8. Andheria AP, Karanth KU, Kumar NS. 2007 Diet and
prey profiles of three sympatric large carnivores
in Bandipur Tiger Reserve, India. J. Zool. 273,
169–175. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2007.00310.x)
9. Kumaraguru A, Saravanamuthu R, Brinda K, Asokan
S. 2011 Prey preference of large carnivores in
Anamalai Tiger Reserve, India. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 57,
627–637. (doi:10.1007/s10344-010-0473-y)
10. Berg JK. 1983 Vocalizations and associated
behaviors of the African elephant (Loxodonta
africana) in captivity. Z. Tierpsychol. 63, 63–79.
(doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1983.tb00741.x)
11. Nair S, Balakrishnan R, Seelamantula CS, Sukumar R.
2009 Vocalizations of wild Asian elephants (Elephas
maximus): structural classification and social context.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 126, 2768–2778. (doi:10.1121/
1.3224717)
12. Sukumar R. 1985 Ecology of the Asian elephant
Elephas maximus and its interaction with man in
south India. PhD dissertation, Indian Institute of
Science, Bangalore, India.
13. Stoeger-Horwath AS, Stoeger S, Schwammer HM,
Kratochvil H. 2007 Call repertoire of infant African
elephants: first insights into the early vocal
ontogeny. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 3922–3931.
(doi:10.1121/1.2722216)
14. Aureli F, van Schaik CP. 1991 Post-conflict
behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca
fiscularis). II. Coping with the uncertainty.
Ethology 89, 101–114. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.
1991.tb00297.x)
15. Bates LA, Sayialel KN, Njiraini NW, Moss CJ,
Poole JH, Byrne RW. 2007 Elephants classify
human ethnic groups by odor and garment
color. Curr. Biol. 17, 1938–1942. (doi:10.1016/j.
cub.2007.09.060)
16. King LE, Douglas-Hamilton I, Vollrath F. 2011
Beehive fences as effective deterrents for crop-
raiding elephants: field trials in northern Kenya.
Afr. J. Ecol. 49, 431–439. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2028.
2011.01275.x)
17. Thuppil V, Coss RG. 2012 Using threatening sounds
as a conservation tool: evolutionary bases for
managing human–elephant conflict in India. J. Int.
Wildl. Law Policy 15, 167–185. (doi:10.1080/
13880292.2012.678794)
rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgBiolLett9:20130518
4
on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from

More Related Content

Viewers also liked (19)

Scientific Poster of Bauer and Stanley
Scientific Poster of Bauer and StanleyScientific Poster of Bauer and Stanley
Scientific Poster of Bauer and Stanley
 
AgileCamp'11.Code review & Pair programming
AgileCamp'11.Code review & Pair programmingAgileCamp'11.Code review & Pair programming
AgileCamp'11.Code review & Pair programming
 
El plan de convivencia
El plan de convivenciaEl plan de convivencia
El plan de convivencia
 
My sportlife
My sportlifeMy sportlife
My sportlife
 
Stelo
SteloStelo
Stelo
 
easy payday loans
easy payday loanseasy payday loans
easy payday loans
 
Agencja Marketingu Relacyjnego
Agencja Marketingu RelacyjnegoAgencja Marketingu Relacyjnego
Agencja Marketingu Relacyjnego
 
MERCEDES BENZ
MERCEDES BENZMERCEDES BENZ
MERCEDES BENZ
 
Office 365
Office 365Office 365
Office 365
 
Informatica isaac
Informatica isaacInformatica isaac
Informatica isaac
 
Las fotos en a@maila i
Las fotos en a@maila iLas fotos en a@maila i
Las fotos en a@maila i
 
Elwood Digital & Marketing Services
Elwood Digital & Marketing ServicesElwood Digital & Marketing Services
Elwood Digital & Marketing Services
 
quick cash loans
quick cash loansquick cash loans
quick cash loans
 
1027實習
1027實習1027實習
1027實習
 
Memasukkan file dari slide share ke blog
Memasukkan file dari slide share ke blogMemasukkan file dari slide share ke blog
Memasukkan file dari slide share ke blog
 
Rajiv ranjan resume-us
Rajiv ranjan  resume-usRajiv ranjan  resume-us
Rajiv ranjan resume-us
 
Röntgen cihazları ve ortopantomografi
Röntgen cihazları ve ortopantomografiRöntgen cihazları ve ortopantomografi
Röntgen cihazları ve ortopantomografi
 
Latar belakang
Latar belakangLatar belakang
Latar belakang
 
Halloween
HalloweenHalloween
Halloween
 

Similar to Thuppil 2013 Biol. Let

Prehensile Tail Research Paper
Prehensile Tail Research PaperPrehensile Tail Research Paper
Prehensile Tail Research PaperMelanie Smith
 
Australian Endangered Animals
Australian Endangered AnimalsAustralian Endangered Animals
Australian Endangered Animalsswinburnegroup4
 
MAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docx
MAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docxMAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docx
MAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docxsmile790243
 
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docxECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docxbudabrooks46239
 
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docxECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docxtidwellveronique
 
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior AnalysisAnimal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior AnalysisAglazer1
 
J. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample ProposalJ. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample ProposalJohn Sequeira
 
In an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdf
In an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdfIn an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdf
In an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdfcontact32
 
Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)
Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)
Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)Lisa Campbell
 
Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...
Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...
Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...Premier Publishers
 
Science presentation wed
Science presentation wedScience presentation wed
Science presentation wedhayat
 
Anthropology Zoo Observations
Anthropology Zoo ObservationsAnthropology Zoo Observations
Anthropology Zoo ObservationsErika Nelson
 
Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...
Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...
Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...Journal of Research in Biology
 

Similar to Thuppil 2013 Biol. Let (20)

Prehensile Tail Research Paper
Prehensile Tail Research PaperPrehensile Tail Research Paper
Prehensile Tail Research Paper
 
Spot spotting
Spot spottingSpot spotting
Spot spotting
 
6th Grade Chapter 5
6th Grade  Chapter 56th Grade  Chapter 5
6th Grade Chapter 5
 
Tang_Sedgwick_Paper
Tang_Sedgwick_PaperTang_Sedgwick_Paper
Tang_Sedgwick_Paper
 
Australian Endangered Animals
Australian Endangered AnimalsAustralian Endangered Animals
Australian Endangered Animals
 
adaptation of elephant
adaptation of elephantadaptation of elephant
adaptation of elephant
 
MAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docx
MAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docxMAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docx
MAC 1105 2015-1 Test 2 Name _________________________________.docx
 
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docxECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
 
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docxECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
ECOL203403 – Ecology Populations to Ecosystems Assignment .docx
 
Arguments For Cloning Extinct Species
Arguments For Cloning Extinct SpeciesArguments For Cloning Extinct Species
Arguments For Cloning Extinct Species
 
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior AnalysisAnimal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
 
J. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample ProposalJ. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
 
In an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdf
In an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdfIn an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdf
In an analysis of hunting by African lions biologists filme.pdf
 
Platypus surfing
Platypus surfingPlatypus surfing
Platypus surfing
 
Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)
Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)
Stankowich & Campbell 2016 (2)
 
Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...
Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...
Morphological studies of Female Simulium damnosum s.l. in Akamkpa Local Gover...
 
Area of research
Area of research Area of research
Area of research
 
Science presentation wed
Science presentation wedScience presentation wed
Science presentation wed
 
Anthropology Zoo Observations
Anthropology Zoo ObservationsAnthropology Zoo Observations
Anthropology Zoo Observations
 
Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...
Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...
Snakes of the Bhopal district, Madhya Pradesh, India with special reference t...
 

Thuppil 2013 Biol. Let

  • 1. , 20130518, published 11 September 201392013Biol. Lett. Vivek Thuppil and Richard G. Coss growls according to perceived danger Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard Supplementary data ml http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/suppl/2013/09/06/rsbl.2013.0518.DC1.ht "Data Supplement" References http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/9/5/20130518.full.html#ref-list-1 This article cites 16 articles, 1 of which can be accessed free Subject collections (633 articles)behaviour Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections Email alerting service hereright-hand corner of the article or click Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptionsgo to:Biol. Lett.To subscribe to on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
  • 2. rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org Research Cite this article: Thuppil V, Coss RG. 2013 Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard growls according to perceived danger. Biol Lett 9: 20130518. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0518 Received: 5 June 2013 Accepted: 16 August 2013 Subject Areas: behaviour Keywords: antipredator behaviour, Asian elephant, sound playbacks, growls Author for correspondence: Vivek Thuppil e-mail: vthuppil@ucdavis.edu Electronic supplementary material is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0518 or via http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org. Animal behaviour Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard growls according to perceived danger Vivek Thuppil1 and Richard G. Coss1,2 1 Animal Behavior Graduate Group, and 2 Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA Prey species exhibit antipredator behaviours such as alertness, aggression and flight, among others, in response to predators. The nature of this response is variable, with animals reacting more strongly in situations of increased vulner- ability. Our research described here is the first formal study to investigate night-time antipredator behaviour in any species of elephants, Asian or African. We examined the provocative effects of elephant-triggered tiger and leopard growls while elephants attempted to crop-raid. Tigers opportunisti- cally prey on elephant calves, whereas leopards pose no threat; therefore, we predicted that the elephant response would be reflective of this difference. Elephants reacted similarly cautiously to the simulated presence of felids of both species by eventually moving away, but differed markedly in their more immediate behavioural responses. Elephants retreated silently to tiger- growl playbacks, whereas they responded with aggressive vocalizations, such as trumpets and grunts, to leopard-growl playbacks. Elephants also lin- gered in the area and displayed alert or investigative behaviours in response to leopard growls when compared with tiger growls. We anticipate that the methods outlined here will promote further study of elephant antipredator behaviour in a naturalistic context, with applications for conservation efforts as well. 1. Introduction Antipredator behaviour refers to a suite of behaviours that prey species exhibit in response to predators including alertness, aggression, flight and vocalizing, among others [1–3]. This antipredator response is reflective of the degree of predatory threat; animals respond more strongly to a more dangerous preda- tory species or to situations of increased vulnerability [4]. While these behaviours have been documented extensively in many mammals, they are virtually unexplored in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Comparatively, more antipredator behaviour research has been conducted on African elephants (Loxodonta africana). McComb et al. [5] showed that, in African elephants, female family groups hearing lion-roar playbacks were able to identify situations that represented the greatest danger and those with older matriarchs were particularly adept at recognizing the specific danger presented by male (as opposed to female) lions. Other research on elephant risk assessment has shown that African elephants retreat and alarm call in response to playbacks of threatening African bees [6]. In contrast to these daytime playback studies, our research is the first empirical investigation of night-time elephant antipredator behaviour and the first to examine elephant differentiation of growls from two sympatric felid species posing differential predatory threats. We investigated whether wild elephants could discriminate the aggressive growls of tigers and leopards. Tigers are known to opportunistically prey on ele- phant calves, whereasthere is no mention, either in the literature or anecdotally, of elephants being a part of leopards’ diets [7–9]. Elephants themselves produce & 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
  • 3. guttural growls while communicating with conspecifics [10,11] and may possess a broad, low-frequency acoustical-assessment ability useful for differentiating similar growls of other species. Therefore, we predicted that elephants would discriminate the acoustically distinct growls of tigers and leopards and would display stronger antipredator behaviour to the more dangerous tiger-growl playbacks. 2. Material and methods From previous research, we had recordings of aggressive growls of a single tiger and leopard at the Bannerghatta Zoological Park, Bangalore, obtained using a Sennheiser ME 80 directional micro- phone coupled to a Sony TC-D5 PROII analogue recorder. To engender growling, both cats were agitated similarly when the keeper entered their cages and banged a stick repeatedly. We did not repeat this procedure with other individuals owing to the potential danger involved. The natural pauses in growling were used to split the growl playbacks into two exemplars each, with durations of 9 and 17 s and 21 and 35 s for the leopard and tiger, respectively. We digitized these growl recordings in 16-bit mode and 48 kHz sampling frequency for random play- back by Blue Point Engineering 10-channel mp3 players. We used AUDACITY software to generate spectrograms of the onset of felid growls (figure 1). We conducted field research near villages around Bandipur Tiger Reserve and Wayanad Wildlife Sanctuary in southern India. Playback growls were amplified by battery-powered 200 W PylePro speaker systems in water-proof enclosures posi- tioned approximately 50 m ahead of where elephants triggered the playbacks. Playback growls were produced at an intensity of 105 dB sound pressure level (SPL) at a distance of 1 m from the source. At a distance of 50 m, elephants heard these growls at intensities of 58–62 dB SPL. Elephants triggered these playbacks by tripping a Takex PB-60 TK active infrared beam positioned at a height of 1.85 m. Elephants encountered these playback systems along paths leading to crop fields and we assume that these elephants were similarly motivated to crop-raid. We deployed this set-up simultaneously in three study sites from August 2010 to February 2011 and recorded a total of 26 incidents of elephants activating leopard- and tiger-growl play- backs. For the purposes of our analyses here, we have only included distinct individuals in their first encounter of the play- back system, without any repeated playbacks. This corresponds to seven instances where elephants encountered tiger-growl playbacks and eight instances where elephants encountered leopard-growl playbacks. We logged post-playback elephant movement paths on a Garmin eTrex device by tracking their footprints on the morning following a beam trip. From the point of beam trip, the difference of the compass bearing of the elephant’s position at 25 m away and the compass bearing of the speaker provided a movement angle relative to the speaker. Elephant movements were categorized as toward, lateral and away, corresponding to movement angle ranges of 0–608, 60–1208 and 1208–1808, respectively. We used a 1-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA) test to determine whether there was a significant difference in movement trajectory between the two felid-growl categories. Upon discovering that the means were nearly identical, we used the inverse F-ratio (with reversed d.f.) statistic to determine whether the means were reliably similar. Using high-definition infrared video cameras equipped with microphones, we individually identified elephants using 15 10 5 0 15 10 5 0 15 10 5 kHzkHzkHz 0 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 0.5 1.0 0 0.5 1.0 seconds 1.4 1.5 (a) (b) (c) Figure 1. Spectrograms of the onset of (a) tiger, (b) leopard and (c) elephant growls. The leopard growl exhibits a greater energy distribution at higher frequencies than the tiger growl, yielding a raspier sound. The guttural pulse rate of the elephant growl approximates that of the leopard growl, a property that might facilitate growl discrimination. (Online version in colour.) rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgBiolLett9:20130518 2 on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
  • 4. morphological attributes, such as ear or tusk shape, and herd composition, if any. We aged elephants into adult/sub-adult or juvenile categories based on height [12]. Our video cameras also captured tigers and leopards at our study sites, ensuring that these playbacks represented naturalistic threats. Owing to the constraints of passively recording elephant be- haviour at night, we restricted quantification of behaviour to frequencies of occurrence instead of rates of behaviour reported in daytime studies [6]. To compare the interactions of frequencies of occurrence, we used multinomial log-linear analyses with maximum-likelihood estimations. We observed both individual elephants and herds of elephants encountering our playbacks (table 1). In situations of herds encountering our playbacks, we analysed the behaviour of the elephant leading the herd, usually an adult female, whose response always characterized the overall herd behaviour captured on video. We used ANOVA to compare elephant reaction times, quanti- fied in milliseconds (ms), from frame-by-frame mpeg4 video in increments of 33 ms. Elephant vocalizations that were recorded on video were categorized as trumpets and grunts (see electronic supplementary material) based on previous literature showing spectrograms and descriptions of dominant frequencies and dur- ations [10,11]. Elephant grunts [13] have also been characterized as ‘roars’ [11]. In some instances where a herd of elephants triggered felid-growl playbacks, elephants also produced growls, which were captured by our video cameras. Using AUDACITY software, we have provided a spectrogram of an elephant growl for comparison purposes (figure 1). 3. Results Initial elephant responses to all playbacks were nearly instan- taneous (mean reaction time ¼ 575 ms, range ¼ 393–820 ms, n ¼ 11). Similarly, elephants vocalized shortly after growl- playback onset (mean latency ¼ 4.3 s, range ¼ 2–10 s, n ¼ 7), rendering natural differences in growl duration as unimportant because elephants vocalized prior to growl termination. We recorded seven instances where elephants encountered tiger-growl playbacks and eight instances where elephants encountered leopard-growl playbacks. Elephants exhibited reliably similar movement angles away from both tiger- and leopard-growl playbacks (1/F13,1 ¼ 378.39; p , 0.05). However, a comparison of more immediate elephant responses provides different insight into elephant antipredator behaviour. Elephants vocalized in a reliably different manner to playbacks of tiger and leopard growls (likelihood ratio x2 1 ¼ 15:90; p , 0.001). They silently retreated in response to tiger growls and vocalized in all but one instance to the leopard growls (figure 2). Elephants were expected to linger less, and thus display a lower likelihood of alert or investiga- tive behaviours to growls posing a greater threat. Indeed, a greater frequency of elephants displayed alert or investigative behaviours (likelihood ratio x2 1 ¼ 4:43; p ¼ 0.035) to leopard- growl playbacks than to tiger-growl playbacks (figure 2). These behaviours included instances of elephants remain- ing stationary, searching for acoustic and olfactory cues, as well as elephants walking around and actively investigating the area. The dominant vocalizations in response to leopard growls (figure 2), compared with the absence of vocalizing to tiger growls, were trumpets (likelihood ratio x2 1 ¼ 8:51; p ¼ 0.004) Table 1. Demographic breakdown of elephants that encountered our felid-growl playbacks. For herds, the leading elephant’s behavioural response was considered for our analyses. All elephant encounters shown in this table are distinct individuals with no repeated playbacks. no. instances single male elephant single female elephant herd led by male elephant herd led by female elephant per cent of herds that contained more than 1 adult elephant per cent of herds that contained non-leading adult males per cent of herds that contained at least one juvenile elephant leopard-growl playbacks 0 0 0 8 25 12.5 100 tiger-growl playbacks 1 3 1 2 66.7 33.3 66.7 100 elephant vigilance and vocalizations 80 trumpeting * ** *** *** grunting all vocalizations alert/investigative behaviours 60 40 20 tiger growls leopard tiger 123.88° 122.86° movement away from playback origin leopard growls 0 (%) Figure 2. Elephant discrimination of felid-growl playbacks: *p 0.05; **p 0.01; ***p 0.001. Frequency comparisons of elephants displaying nearly immediate alert or investigative behaviours and vocalizing; delayed elephant movement trajectories away from the felid-growl playbacks are shown as means (solid lines) and standard deviations (dashed lines). rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgBiolLett9:20130518 3 on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
  • 5. and grunts (likelihood ratio x2 1 ¼ 11:193; p , 0.001). Both trumpets and grunts are used by elephants in instances of alarm, disturbance and interspecific aggression [11]. After hearing an angry leopard maintaining its position rather than fleeing, some elephants exhibited conflicted behaviour as evidenced by their investigative circling, kicking dirt and even kicking a battery box powering the playback-trigger device. Such displays of redirected aggression are typical following adversarial encounters [14]. 4. Discussion While both felid-growl playback categories eventually evoked similarly cautious retreat away from the speaker systems, there were interesting differences in the more immediate elephant responses, indicating that elephants were able to discriminate tiger and leopard growls and perceived differing levels of threat from these two playback categories. Elephants retreated stealthily from dangerous tigers, yet announced their presence by vocalizing, particularly through aggressive vocalizations, for instance trumpets, in the majority of leopard-growl playback situations. Elephants also did not linger in the area by displaying alert or investiga- tive behaviours to tiger-growl playbacks. While there were some demographic differences in exposure to the two playback categories (table 1), we do not believe that this impacted our results. Both herds and lone elephants encountering tiger- growl playbacks responded identically by retreating quickly and silently. Our results are consistent with findings that African elephants retreat significantly more quickly from clothes associ- ated with dangerous Maasai hunters compared with ethnic groups that present lower levels of perceived threat [15]. Elephants did display aggressive behaviours when dangerous Maasai hunter clothes were displayed visually, but Maasai olfactory cues were absent. Bates et al. [15] concluded that the presence of clothes without accompanying olfactory cues indi- cated that the humans were no longer present in the area. Thus, a reduced level of threat perception allowed elephants to react more aggressively, as we propose happened when our elephants perceived lesser threat from leopard growls. Research on elephant threat assessment can have other applications as well. For example, fences incorporating hives of dangerous African bees deterred crop-raiding attempting elephants from breaking through them [16]. Similarly, we implemented our research in a manner that mitigated night- time crop-raiding [17]. Thus, we anticipate that future elephant antipredator behaviour research will generate useful appli- cations for elephant conservation in addition to providing insights into how the world’s largest terrestrial animals assess threats across their myriad habitats. Acknowledgements. We thank A. Pittet, R. Johnson and S. Joshi for provid- ing electronics assistance and R. Sukumar for hosting our research at his field research station. We thank D. Thiwary for logistical support and our assistants Arun, Madhan, Rajesh, Sudhakar and Sunil. We thank the Karnataka and Kerala forest departments for research permissions. Funding statement. Thisresearchwasfinanciallysupportedbythe U.S.Fish and Wildlife Service Asian Elephant Conservation Fund, the Rufford Small Grants Foundation and the University of California, Davis. References 1. Sih A. 1992 Prey uncertainty and the balancing of antipredator feeding needs. Am. Nat. 139, 1052–1069. (doi:10.1086/285372) 2. Coss RG, McCowan B, Ramakrishnan U. 2007 Threat- related acoustical differences in alarm calls by wild bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) elicited by python and leopard models. Ethology 113, 352–367. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01336.x) 3. Fichtel C. 2007 Avoiding predators at night: antipredator strategies in red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus). Am. J. Primatol. 69, 611–624. 4. Owings DH, Coss RG, McKernon D, Rowe MP, Arrowood PC. 2001 Snake-directed antipredator behaviour of rock squirrels (Spermophilus variegates): population differences and snake- species discrimination. Behaviour 138, 575–595. 5. McComb K, Shannon G, Durant SM, Sayialel K, Slotow R, Poole J, Moss C. 2011 Leadership in elephants: the adaptive value of age. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 3270–3276. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0168) 6. King LE, Soltis J, Douglas-Hamilton I, Savage A, Vollrath F. 2010 Bee threat elicits alarm call in African elephants. PLoS ONE 5, e10346. (doi:10. 1371/journal.pone.0010346) 7. Hayward MW, Henschel P, O’Brien J, Hofmeyr M, Balme G, Kerley GIH. 2006 Prey preferences of the leopard (Panthera pardus). J. Zool. 270, 298–313. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00139.x) 8. Andheria AP, Karanth KU, Kumar NS. 2007 Diet and prey profiles of three sympatric large carnivores in Bandipur Tiger Reserve, India. J. Zool. 273, 169–175. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2007.00310.x) 9. Kumaraguru A, Saravanamuthu R, Brinda K, Asokan S. 2011 Prey preference of large carnivores in Anamalai Tiger Reserve, India. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 57, 627–637. (doi:10.1007/s10344-010-0473-y) 10. Berg JK. 1983 Vocalizations and associated behaviors of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana) in captivity. Z. Tierpsychol. 63, 63–79. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1983.tb00741.x) 11. Nair S, Balakrishnan R, Seelamantula CS, Sukumar R. 2009 Vocalizations of wild Asian elephants (Elephas maximus): structural classification and social context. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 126, 2768–2778. (doi:10.1121/ 1.3224717) 12. Sukumar R. 1985 Ecology of the Asian elephant Elephas maximus and its interaction with man in south India. PhD dissertation, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore, India. 13. Stoeger-Horwath AS, Stoeger S, Schwammer HM, Kratochvil H. 2007 Call repertoire of infant African elephants: first insights into the early vocal ontogeny. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 3922–3931. (doi:10.1121/1.2722216) 14. Aureli F, van Schaik CP. 1991 Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fiscularis). II. Coping with the uncertainty. Ethology 89, 101–114. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310. 1991.tb00297.x) 15. Bates LA, Sayialel KN, Njiraini NW, Moss CJ, Poole JH, Byrne RW. 2007 Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Curr. Biol. 17, 1938–1942. (doi:10.1016/j. cub.2007.09.060) 16. King LE, Douglas-Hamilton I, Vollrath F. 2011 Beehive fences as effective deterrents for crop- raiding elephants: field trials in northern Kenya. Afr. J. Ecol. 49, 431–439. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2028. 2011.01275.x) 17. Thuppil V, Coss RG. 2012 Using threatening sounds as a conservation tool: evolutionary bases for managing human–elephant conflict in India. J. Int. Wildl. Law Policy 15, 167–185. (doi:10.1080/ 13880292.2012.678794) rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgBiolLett9:20130518 4 on September 11, 2013rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from