SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 10
Download to read offline
ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION 
Long-term Effects of Cognitive Training on 
Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older Adults 
Sherry L. Willis, PhD 
Sharon L. Tennstedt, PhD 
Michael Marsiske, PhD 
Karlene Ball, PhD 
Jeffrey Elias, PhD 
Kathy Mann Koepke, PhD 
John N. Morris, PhD 
George W. Rebok, PhD 
Frederick W. Unverzagt, PhD 
Anne M. Stoddard, ScD 
Elizabeth Wright, PhD 
for the ACTIVE Study Group 
DECLINE IN COGNITIVE ABILI-ties 
has been shown to lead 
to an increased risk of diffi-culty 
in performing instru-mental 
activities of daily living 
(IADL).1-5 However, whether inter-ventions 
to maintain or enhance cog-nitive 
abilities in older adults will pre-vent 
or delay these functional 
difficulties is unclear. Prior interven-tions 
with older adults have targeted 
those with cognitive deficits or func-tional 
disabilities and have focused on 
remediation rather than prevention.6,7 
Prior studies have shown that cogni-tive 
interventions can improve cogni-tive 
abilities in normal elders but have 
not included functional outcome 
measures and have been limited by 
small, homogeneous samples and lack 
of randomization.8-11 
For editorial comment see p 2852. 
Context Cognitive training has been shown to improve cognitive abilities in older adults 
but the effects of cognitive training on everyday function have not been demonstrated. 
Objective To determine the effects of cognitive training on daily function and du-rability 
Author Affiliations are listed at the end of this 
article. 
CorrespondingAuthor:Sherry L. Willis,PhD,Department 
ofHumanDevelopment and Family Studies, Pennsylva-nia 
State University, 135 E Nittany Ave, Suite 405, State 
College, PA 16801 (slw@psu.edu). 
of training on cognitive abilities. 
Design, Setting, and Participants Five-year follow-up of a randomized con-trolled 
single-blind trial with 4 treatment groups. A volunteer sample of 2832 persons 
(mean age, 73.6 years; 26% black), living independently in 6 US cities, was recruited 
from senior housing, community centers, and hospitals and clinics. The study was con-ducted 
between April 1998 and December 2004. Five-year follow-up was completed 
in 67% of the sample. 
Interventions Ten-session training for memory (verbal episodic memory), reason-ing 
(inductive reasoning), or speed of processing (visual search and identification); 4-ses-sion 
booster training at 11 and 35 months after training in a random sample of those 
who completed training. 
Main Outcome Measures Self-reported and performance-based measures of daily 
function and cognitive abilities. 
Results The reasoning group reported significantly less difficulty in the instrumental ac-tivities 
of daily living (IADL) than the control group (effect size, 0.29; 99% confidence 
interval [CI], 0.03-0.55). Neither speed of processing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% CI, 
−0.002 to 0.51) nor memory training (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 0.46) had a 
significant effect on IADL. The booster training for the speed of processing group, but 
not for the other 2 groups, showed a significant effect on the performance-based func-tional 
measure of everyday speed of processing (effect size, 0.30; 99% CI, 0.08-0.52). 
No booster effects were seen for any of the groups for everyday problem-solving or self-reported 
difficulty in IADL. Each intervention maintained effects on its specific targeted 
cognitive ability through 5 years (memory: effect size, 0.23 [99% CI, 0.11-0.35]; rea-soning: 
effect size, 0.26 [99% CI, 0.17-0.35]; speed of processing: effect size, 0.76 [99% 
CI, 0.62-0.90]). Booster training produced additional improvement with the reasoning 
intervention for reasoning performance (effect size, 0.28; 99% CI, 0.12-0.43) and the 
speed of processing intervention for speed of processing performance (effect size, 0.85; 
99% CI, 0.61-1.09). 
Conclusions Reasoning training resulted in less functional decline in self-reported 
IADL. Compared with the control group, cognitive training resulted in improved cog-nitive 
abilities specific to the abilities trained that continued 5 years after the initiation 
of the intervention. 
Trial Registration clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00298558 
JAMA. 2006;296:2805-2814 www.jama.com 
©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2805 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
The Advanced Cognitive Training for 
Independent and Vital Elderly (AC-TIVE) 
study is the first multicenter, ran-domized 
controlled trial to examine the 
long-term outcomes of cognitive inter-ventions 
on the daily functioning of 
older individuals living indepen-dently. 
12 Previously reported data from 
the ACTIVE study showed that each of 
3 cognitive interventions improved the 
cognitive ability it targeted and these 
improvements were maintained 
through the 2 years of follow-up.13 This 
article addresses the long-term effects 
of cognitive training on the mainte-nance 
of self-reported IADL. 
FIGURE 1 presents the conceptual 
model for the ACTIVE trial. Four 
hypotheses were derived from this 
model. First, cognitive training would 
affect the cognitive ability targeted by 
that training and these effects would be 
maintained over time. Second, main-tained 
improvements in cognitive abil-ity 
would have a positive transfer effect 
on everyday function. Based on prior 
research,14,15weexpected to first see this 
transfer of training effects to affect IADL 
functioning. Third, this training trans-fer 
from cognitive abilities toIADLfunc-tion 
would occur in all 3 training groups 
because each of the 3 cognitive abili-ties 
has been associated withIADLfunc-tioning. 
9,16 While the broadest transfer 
of training effects was expected to 
improve IADL functioning, we also 
expected to see other training-specific 
effects (ie, transfer of improvement in 
reasoning and memory to improve-ments 
in everyday problem solving or 
transfer of improvement in speed of pro-cessing 
to improvements in everyday 
speed of processing tasks). Finally, 
because of the functional indepen-dence 
of participants at baseline, it was 
hypothesized that observations of train-ing 
effects on IADL functioning would 
be delayed until the control group began 
to experience significant functional 
decline. This was observed at the 5-year 
follow-up and therefore the hypoth-esized 
delayed outcome of mainte-nance 
of IADL function could be tested. 
METHODS 
Participants 
The sample consisted of older adults liv-ing 
independently with good functional 
and cognitive status who were recruited 
from senior housing, community cen-ters, 
and hospitals and clinics in Bir-mingham, 
Ala; Detroit, Mich; Boston, 
Mass; Baltimore, Md; Indianapolis, Ind; 
and State College, Pa. Participants were 
enrolled in the study between March 
1998 and October 1999.12 Persons were 
excluded if they were younger than 65 
years; had substantial functional impair-ment 
(2ADLdisabilities) or cognitive 
decline (Mini-Mental State Examina-tion 
[MMSE] score 22)17; self-reported 
diagnosis of Alzheimer dis-ease; 
medical conditions associated with 
imminent functional decline or death; 
severe losses in vision, hearing, or com-municative 
ability that would interfere 
with study participation; recent cogni-tive 
training; or were unavailable dur-ing 
the study period. Because previous 
studies of cognitive training had been 
conducted in white older adults, recruit-mentof 
other race and ethnic groups was 
emphasized. Race was self-reported as 
white, black, Asian, Native Hawaiian/ 
Pacific Islander, American Indian/ 
Alaskan Native, or biracial. Participants 
indicated if they were Hispanic or Latino. 
Study Design 
ACTIVE was a randomized controlled, 
single-blind trial, using a4-groupdesign, 
including 3 treatment groups and a con-trol 
group (FIGURE2). Participants were 
randomized to a group by the data co-ordinating 
center using a computer ran-domization 
program. Assessments were 
conducted at baseline, following the in-tervention, 
and annually at 1, 2, 3, and 
5 years. Assessors were blinded to treat-ment 
assignment. Exposure to social 
contact was equated in the 3 interven-tion 
groups. The control group had no 
contact; a placebo social contact control 
wasnot included because prior interven-tion 
studies indicated no differences be-tween 
a social contact control and a “no 
contact” control in either cognitive or 
functional improvement.18,19 Prior 
ACTIVEresults13 showed that cognitive 
effectswerespecific toeachintervention. 
Thestudy protocol was approved by the 
institutionalreviewboardsat all sitesand 
the trial was monitored by a data and 
Figure 1. Conceptual Model of ACTIVE Trial 
RANDOMIZED COGNITIVE 
TRAINING INTERVENTION 
Reasoning Training 
Memory Training 
Speed Training 
COGNITIVE 
OUTCOMES 
Reasoning 
Memory 
Attentional Speed 
FUNCTIONAL 
OUTCOMES 
Everyday 
Problem Solving 
ADL and IADL 
Functioning 
Everyday Speed 
of Processing 
Individual Characteristics 
(Covariates) 
Cognitive Function/Disorder 
Health Conditions 
Physical Characteristics 
Sensory Function 
Sociodemographics 
ACTIVE indicates Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly; ADL, activities of daily liv-ing; 
IADL, instrumental activities of daily living. 
2806 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
safety monitoring board. Written in-formed 
consent was obtained from all 
participants. 
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
Interventions 
Each of the 3 training interventions was 
designed to narrowly target a specific 
cognitive ability—memory, reason-ing, 
or speed of processing—and in-cluded 
no overlap with the functional 
Figure 2. Flow of Individuals in ACTIVE Trial 
Year 2 Evaluation 
27 Partial Training 
1 Died 
36 Withdrew 
6 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
28 Not Assessed 
7 Received Partial Booster 
10 Died 
25 Withdrew 
3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
45 Not Assessed 
18 Excluded 
6 Died 
9 Withdrew 
3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
49 Not Assessed 
563 Assessed 
45 Partial Training 
5 Died 
37 Withdrew 
4 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
30 Not Assessed 
4 Received Partial Booster 
15 Died 
20 Withdrew 
2 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
56 Not Assessed 
17 Excluded 
3 Died 
11 Withdrew 
3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
50 Not Assessed 
555 Assessed 
35 Partial Training 
1 Died 
28 Withdrew 
1 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
29 Not Assessed 
4 Received Partial Booster 
11 Died 
14 Withdrew 
4 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
52 Not Assessed 
17 Excluded 
9 Died 
5 Withdrew 
3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
62 Not Assessed 
574 Assessed 
1 Died 
25 Withdrew 
8 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
31 Not Assessed 
9 Died 
24 Withdrew 
4 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
49 Not Assessed 
26 Excluded 
9 Died 
14 Withdrew 
3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
55 Not Assessed 
552 Assessed 
Year 1 Evaluation 
38 Excluded 
505 Assessed 
37 Excluded 
566 Assessed 
29 Excluded 
601 Assessed 
37 Excluded 
584 Assessed 
Immediate Posttest 
43 Excluded 
640 Assessed 
46 Excluded 
629 Assessed 
30 Excluded 
653 Assessed 
34 Excluded 
639 Assessed 
Booster Training 
372 Assigned to Receive Booster 
Training 
82 Did Not Receive Booster 
283 Completed Booster 
371 Assigned to Receive Booster 
Training 
66 Did Not Receive Booster 
301 Completed Booster 
370 Assigned to Receive Booster 
Training 
71 Did Not Receive Booster 
295 Completed Booster 
372 Assigned to Receive Booster 
Training 
118 Did Not Receive Booster 
4 Received Partial Booster 
250 Completed Booster 
371 Assigned to Receive Booster 
Training 
121 Did Not Receive Booster 
7 Received Partial Booster 
243 Completed Booster 
370 Assigned to Receive Booster 
Training 
133 Did Not Receive Booster 
7 Received Partial Booster 
230 Completed Booster 
Booster Training 
at Year 3 
2168 Excluded 
905 Ineligible 
1263 Refused 
Training 
711 Assigned to Receive Memory 
Training 
64 No Training 
620 Completed Training 
705 Assigned to Receive Reasoning 
Training 
33 No Training 
627 Completed Training 
712 Assigned to Receive Speed 
Training 
40 No Training 
637 Completed Training 
704 Assigned to Control 
5000 Individuals Assessed for Eligibility 
2832 Randomized Baseline 
32 Died 
54 Withdrew 
52 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
2 Family Refusal 
Total Attrition 
49 Died 
124 Withdrew 
64 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
2 Family Refusal 
41 Died 
49 Withdrew 
37 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
9 Family Refusal 
Total Attrition 
64 Died 
117 Withdrew 
46 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
9 Family Refusal 
46 Died 
54 Withdrew 
38 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
8 Family Refusal 
Total Attrition 
67 Died 
101 Withdrew 
46 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
8 Family Refusal 
46 Died 
66 Withdrew 
43 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
4 Family Refusal 
Total Attrition 
65 Died 
129 Withdrew 
58 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 
4 Family Refusal 
Years 3 and 5 Evaluation 
140 Excluded 
472 Assessed 
136 Excluded 
469 Assessed 
146 Excluded 
490 Assessed 
159 Excluded 
448 Assessed 
6 Protocol Violations 
699 Included in Analysis 
10 Protocol Violations 
702 Included in Analysis 
6 Protocol Violations 
698 Included in Analysis 
8 Protocol Violations 
703 Included in Analysis 
ACTIVE indicates Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly. 
*Site-level decision was made to withdraw individual from the study. 
©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2807 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
outcomes in this study.12,13 Memory 
training involved teaching mnemonic 
strategies (organization, visualiza-tion, 
association) for remembering ver-bal 
material (eg, word lists, texts).8,20 
Reasoning training involved teaching 
strategies for finding the pattern in a let-ter 
or word series (eg, a c e g i . . . ) and 
identifying the next item in the se-ries. 
9,21 Speed of processing training in-volved 
visual search and divided atten-tion 
(identifying an object on a 
computer screen at increasingly brief 
exposures followed by dividing atten-tion 
between 2 search tasks).16,22,23 Each 
training intervention was 10 sessions. 
Only 10% of the 60- to 75-minute train-ing 
sessions focused on applying these 
strategies to solving everyday prob-lems 
(eg, mnemonic strategies to re-member 
a grocery list; reasoning strat-egies 
to understand the pattern in a bus 
schedule). 
Booster training was conducted at 11 
and 35 months after the initial train-ing 
sessions and involved four 75- 
minute sessions. The goal of the booster 
sessions was to maintain the improve-ment 
in cognitive ability and the con-tent 
of these sessions was similar to the 
training sessions, again focusing on 
strategies related to the cognitive abili-ties 
not on functional outcomes. Par-ticipants 
who completed the initial 
training were eligible for booster train-ing. 
12,13 A subsample of eligible partici-pants 
was selected for booster training 
using a random number computer pro-gram. 
Given this selection contin-gency, 
participants who were selected 
and agreed to booster training were 
younger (P=.007) and had higher base-line 
cognitive function as evidenced by 
MMSE scores (P=.008) compared with 
participants who were eligible and not 
assigned to booster training and par-ticipants 
who were not eligible for 
booster training. Sixty percent of se-lected 
participants completed booster 
training at year 1 and year 3; 19% com-pleted 
year 1 booster only, 6% com-pleted 
year 3 booster only; and 15% did 
not complete any booster training. The 
last booster sessions were completed 2 
years prior to the 5-year follow-up. 
Outcome Measures 
Cognitive outcomes were used to as-sess 
cognitive training effects and func-tional 
outcomes were used to assess the 
impact of improved cognitive abilities 
on instrumental functioning (see Jobe 
et al12 for a detailed description). 
Cognitive outcomes assessed the ef-fects 
of each intervention on the cogni-tive 
ability trained. Memory training out-comes 
involved 3 measures of verbal 
memory ability: Hopkins Verbal Learn-ing 
Test, Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning 
Test, and the Rivermead Behavioral Para-graph 
Recall test.24-26 Reasoning train-ing 
outcomes involved 3 reasoning abil-ity 
measures: letter series, letter sets, and 
word series.27-29 Speed of processing train-ing 
outcomes involved 3 useful field of 
view subscales.30-32 
Functionaloutcomesassessedwhether 
the cognitive interventions had an effect 
on daily function. Everyday function-ing 
represented the participant’s self-ratings 
of difficulty(IADLdifficulty from 
theMinimumDataSet–HomeCare33 and 
ranged from “independent” to “total 
dependence” on a 6-point scale) in com-pleting 
cognitively demanding tasks 
involved in meal preparation, house-work, 
finances, health maintenance, tele-phone 
use, and shopping. Two perfor-mance- 
based categories of daily function 
were also assessed. Everyday problem 
solving34 assessed ability to reason and 
comprehend information in common 
everyday tasks (eg, identifying informa-tion 
in medication labels). Performance 
wasmeasured with printed materials (eg, 
yellow pages, using the Everyday Prob-lems 
Test34) and behavioral simulations 
(eg, making change, using the Observed 
Tasks of Daily Living35). These mea-sures 
were hypothesized to be most 
closely related to reasoning andmemory 
abilitiesdueto their taskdemands.Every-day 
speed of processing assessed partici-pants’ 
speed in interacting with real-world 
stimuli (eg, lookingupa telephone 
number, using the Timed IADL Test36), 
and the ability to react quickly to 1 of 4 
road signs (Complex Reaction Time 
Test22), which was hypothesized to be 
the most closely related to speed of 
processing. 
Because most outcomes were as-sessed 
by multiple measures, compos-ite 
scores were formed by data reduc-tion. 
Each measure was standardized to 
its baseline values and then an aver-age 
of the equally weighted standard-ized 
scores was calculated. These com-posite 
scores permitted inferences about 
training effects at the level of outcome 
rather than at the level of a single test. 
The composite scores were conse-quently 
more reliable than single mea-sures 
and also reduced the number of 
outcome analyses needed, reducing the 
overall type I error rate. 
Analysis 
To evaluate the effects of ACTIVE train-ing, 
a repeated-measures, mixed-effects 
model37 was used, including all 
assigned participants (for both initial 
training and booster training) consis-tent 
with the intention-to-treat prin-ciple. 
The dependent variables were the 
cognitive and functional outcomes at 
baseline and at 5-year follow-up. The 
scores for each test were transformed 
using the Blom transformation.38,39 This 
transformation standardized the com-ponents 
in each domain to have equal 
weight and reduced skewness in the 
measures. 
Independent variables were restricted 
to basic design features: fixed effects for 
treatment group, time, assignment to 
booster training, field site, and replicate 
within a site. Interaction terms were cho-sen 
for importance and interpretability: 
timetraining, representing the net 
effect of the trial; boostertraining, 
representing nonspecific effects of attend-ing 
booster training regardless of con-tent; 
timeboostertraining, repre-senting 
training-specific effects of each 
booster intervention;andreplicate within 
a site, representing variationbetweenand 
within field sites. Because there were no 
significant baseline differences in cog-nition 
and function across groups, these 
interaction terms capture the diver-gence 
across groups that can be attrib-uted 
to the training interventions. 
Because participantswhowere assigned 
to booster training were youngerandhad 
higher baseline cognitive function, the 
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
2808 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
model comparing booster and non-booster 
participants was then repeated 
controlling for baseline age and MMSE 
score.Themodels were fitted to the avail-able 
data, ignoring missing data.Todeter-mine 
if selective attrition influenced the 
trial results, missing data were imputed 
using multiple imputation proce-dures. 
40 The analysis was repeated and 
there were no differences in the results; 
therefore, the data presented do not 
include imputed values. All data analy-sis 
was conducted by statisticians at the 
data coordinating center using SAS sta-tistical 
software version 9.2 (SAS Insti-tute 
Inc, Cary, NC). 
Hypotheses were tested by compar-ing 
the net effect at year 5 in each treat-ment 
group with the net effect in the 
control group. The net effect of train-ing 
at year 5 was defined as mean im-provement 
from baseline to year 5 in 
the intervention group minus the mean 
improvement from baseline to year 5 
in the control group divided by the in-trasubject 
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
SD of the Blom-trans-formed 
score. Similarly, the effect of 
each cognitive ability–specific booster 
training was defined as mean improve-ment 
from baseline to year 5 in the 
booster-trained group minus the mean 
improvement from baseline to year 5 
in the nonbooster-trained group di-vided 
by the intrasubject SD of the 
score. Participants in the nonbooster 
group were those who received initial 
training but no booster training. Re-sults 
are expressed as effect sizes (ie, dif-ference 
in means divided by intra-subject 
SD) to allow direct comparison 
of different outcomes. 
Power calculations were based on the 
assumptions of 6 Bonferroni-cor-rected 
2-sided comparisons with an 
overall  error of .05 (.008 for each 
comparison), a correlation of 0.7 be-tween 
baseline and follow-up (based on 
pilot data), and completion rates of 80% 
at 2 years and 65% at 5 years. Follow-ing 
the methods of Cohen,41 the sample 
size of 2802 has 95% power to detect 
an effect size of 0.20. Based on the same 
assumptions, there is 90% power to de-tect 
booster training effects in the com-parison 
between the subgroup receiv-ing 
booster training in a training 
condition and the control group. In re-porting 
statistically significant train-ing 
effects, we used 99% confidence in-tervals 
(CIs; P=.008) to adjust for 
multiple comparisons. 
RESULTS 
Of 5000 individuals contacted for par-ticipation, 
2832 persons were eligible, 
905 (18.1%) were ineligible, and 1263 
(25.3%) refused to participate 
(Figure 2). Compared with those who 
refused, those who participated were 
less likely to be women (76% vs 79%), 
were younger (mean age, 74 vs 75 
years), more likely to be white (73% vs 
60%), married (36% vs 27%), and bet-ter 
educated (mean of 13.5 vs 12.3 
years). Their MMSE score was higher 
Table 1. Baseline Characteristics 
No. (%) of Participants (N = 2802)* 
Memory 
(n = 703) 
Reasoning 
(n = 699) 
Speed of Processing 
(n = 702) 
Control 
(n = 698) 
Age, mean (SD) [range], y 73.5 (6.0) [65-93] 73.5 (5.8) [65-91] 73.4 (5.8) [65-91] 74.1 (6.1) [65-94] 
Female sex 537 (76.4) 537 (76.8) 538 (76.6) 514 (73.6) 
Race 
White 524 (74.5) 504 (72.1) 523 (74.5) 503 (72.1) 
Black 176 (25.0) 190 (27.2) 175 (24.9) 187 (26.8) 
Other or unknown 3 (0.4) 5 (0.7) 4 (0.6) 8 (1.2) 
Years of education, mean (SD) [range] 13.6 (2.7) [5-20] 13.5 (2.7) [4-20] 13.7 (2.7) [5-20] 13.4 (2.7) [6-20] 
Married 257 (36.6) 249 (35.6) 242 (34.5) 259 (37.1) 
Mini-Mental State Examination score, mean (SD) [range] 27.3 (2.1) [23-30] 27.3 (2.0) [23-30] 27.4 (2.0) [23-30] 27.3 (2.0) [23-30] 
Short-Form 36 physical function score, mean (SD) [range] 69.1 (23.5) [5-100] 67.4 (24.1) [5-100] 69.7 (24.1) [0-100] 68.9 (24.6) [5-100] 
Alcohol consumption† 
Nondrinker 298 (43) 302 (43) 295 (42) 350 (51) 
Light drinker 341 (49) 347 (50) 362 (52) 313 (45) 
Heavy drinker 60 (8) 46 (7) 42 (6) 30 (4) 
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score, 
mean (SD) [range] 
5.1 (5.3) [0-36] 5.5 (5.3) [0-36] 5.2 (5.0) [0-36] 5.1 (4.9) [0-36] 
Disease history 
Hypertension 372 (53.1) 369 (53.2) 350 (50.1) 337 (48.8) 
Diabetes 95 (13.5) 99 (14.2) 87 (12.4) 77 (11) 
Transient ischemic attack or stroke 46 (6.6) 54 (7.8) 51 (7.3) 44 (6.3) 
Ischemic heart disease 108 (15.5) 117 (17) 94 (13.5) 102 (14.7) 
Congestive heart failure 30 (4.3) 44 (6.4) 27 (3.9) 37 (5.4) 
High cholesterol 309 (44.6) 316 (46.4) 305 (44.3) 296 (43.1) 
Myocardial infarction 79 (11.3) 78 (11.2) 76 (10.9) 76 (10.9) 
*Unless otherwise indicated. 
†Based on frequency of drinking alcohol and number of drinks on a typical day when drinking. 
©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2809 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
(mean 27.3 vs 26.8) and they were less 
likely to receive help with bathing 
(1% needed help vs 2%). For both 
groups, 1% or fewer needed help with 
dressing and personal hygiene. Fi-nally, 
those participating were less likely 
to have heart disease (11% vs 14%) and 
diabetes (13% vs 17%) but equally likely 
to have arthritis (57% vs 58%), stroke 
(0% in both groups), and cancer (6% 
in both groups). 
Thirty individuals were random-ized 
inappropriately in violation of the 
protocol and excluded from the analy-sis. 
Therefore, the analytic sample con-sists 
of 2802 randomized participants. 
Baseline characteristics for each of the 
4 study groups appear in TABLE 1. 
Eighty-nine percent of participants 
completed treatment (8/10 training 
sessions). Participants completing train- 
Table 2. Effect of Training on Cognitive Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5 
Intervention Groups 
Memory Reasoning Speed of Processing Control Group 
Memory (possible range: 0-132; n = 2790) 
Score at baseline, mean (SD) 81.0 (16.1) 80.7 (15.6) 80.9 (15.8) 79.4 (16.6) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 −1.0 −4.8 −5.3 −4.0 
Effect size (99% CI)* 0.23 (0.11 to 0.35) 0.05 (−0.07 to 0.17) 0.05 (−0.07 to 0.17) 
Reasoning (possible range: 0-75; n = 2802) 
Score at baseline, mean (SD) 25.9 (12.2) 25.2 (12.0) 25.6 (11.7) 24.5 (12.0) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 4.3 8.1 4.2 5.2 
Effect size (99% CI)* −0.01 (−0.10 to 0.08) 0.26 (0.17 to 0.35) 0.02 (−0.06 to 0.11) 
Speed of processing (possible range: 0-1500; n = 2802) 
Score at baseline, mean (SD) 899.0 (272.5) 904.0 (264.5) 906.8 (260.6) 920.1 (267.3) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 79.1 119.6 241.8 −96.1 
Effect size (99% CI)* −0.01 (−0.15 to 0.13) 0.15 (0.01 to 0.29) 0.76 (0.62 to 0.90) 
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval. 
*Effect size defined as training improvement from baseline to year 5 minus control improvement from baseline to year 5 divided by the intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite 
score. Positive effect sizes indicate improvement. 
Table 3. Effect of Booster Training on Cognitive Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5* 
Intervention Groups 
Memory Reasoning Speed of Processing 
Memory (possible range: 0-132; n = 2790) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 
Booster 81.7 (15.7) 82.1 (15.2) 80.4 (16.3) 
Nonbooster 80.2 (16.6) 79.1 (15.8) 81.5 (15.2) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 
Booster −0.1 −5.0 −5.1 
Nonbooster −2.2 −5.1 −5.5 
Effect size (99% CI)† 0.08 (−0.14 to 0.29) 0.14 (−0.07 to 0.36) 0.05 (−0.16 to 0.26) 
Reasoning (possible range: 0-75; n = 2802) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 
Booster 25.8 (11.8) 25.6 (12.3) 25.4 (11.7) 
Nonbooster 26.0 (12.6) 24.8 (11.8) 25.9 (11.8) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 
Booster 4.6 8.6 4.4 
Nonbooster 3.9 7.3 3.8 
Effect size (99% CI)† 0.09 (−0.06 to 0.24) 0.28 (0.12 to 0.43) 0.08 (−0.07 to 0.23) 
Speed of processing (possible range: 0-1500; n = 2802) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 
Booster 900.7 (278.5) 895.2 (275.4) 904.5 (258.5) 
Nonbooster 897.1 (266.1) 914.0 (251.8) 909.3 (263.1) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 
Booster 82.7 115.9 308.8 
Nonbooster 74.7 121.9 161.4 
Effect size (99% CI)† 0.01 (−0.23 to 0.25) 0.03 (−0.21 to 0.27) 0.85 (0.61 to 1.09) 
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval. 
*Adjusted for baseline age and Mini-Mental State Examination score. 
†Effect size defined as training improvement in the booster group minus improvement in the nonbooster group divided by intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite 
score. 
2810 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
Table 4. Effect of Training on Functional Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5 
ing were younger, had more educa-tion, 
and had higher baseline scores for 
the MMSE and cognitive function tests. 
Sixty-seven percent of the sample was 
retained 5 years after training despite 
the advanced age of the cohort. Selec-tivity 
of attrition was examined by mul-tiple 
logistic regression modeling, con-sidering 
sex, education, baseline age, 
MMSE score, and health status (num-ber 
of health conditions; Short-Form 
36) as well as intervention group and 
booster (vs nonbooster) status as pre-dictors 
of retention at 5 years. Partici-pants 
who were older, male, had less 
education and more health problems, 
and had lower cognitive function (in-dicated 
by lower baseline scores on the 
MMSE and the memory and reason-ing 
tests) were less likely to be re-tained 
at 5 years. Importantly, there 
were no significant interactions be-tween 
treatment group assignment and 
these covariates; that is, biases in re-tention 
are over the entire sample and 
not related to a particular treatment 
group, and therefore do not affect the 
between-group comparisons of inter-vention 
effects. 
Training Effects 
on Cognitive Abilities 
Each intervention produced immediate 
improvement in the cognitive ability 
trained13 that was retained across 5 years 
(TABLE 2). Similarly, when controlling 
Intervention Groups (N = 2802) 
Memory Reasoning Speed Control Group 
Figure 3. Training Effects on Everyday Function by Self-reported Instrumental Activities of 
Daily Living (IADL) Difficulty Scores 
0 
–0.2 
Reasoning 
Speed 
for baseline age and cognitive function, 
booster training for the reasoning and 
speed of processing groups produced sig-nificantly 
better performance (net of ini-tial 
training effect) on their targeted cog-nitive 
outcomes13 that remained 
significant at 5 years (TABLE 3). 
Training Effects 
on Daily Functioning 
Self-Reported IADL Difficulty. At year 
5, participants in all 3 intervention 
groups reported less difficulty com-pared 
with the control group in per-forming 
IADL (TABLE 4). However, this 
effect was significant only for the rea-soning 
group, which compared with the 
control group had an effect size of 0.29 
(99% CI, 0.03-0.55) for difficulty in per-forming 
IADL. Neither speed of pro-cessing 
training (effect size, 0.26; 99% 
CI, −0.002 to 0.51) nor memory train-ing 
(effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 
0.46) had a significant effect on IADL. 
FIGURE 3 presents the standardized 
(Blom-transformed) mean IADL diffi-culty 
scores. Group mean IADL diffi-culty 
ratings improved through the first 
2 years of the study (baseline through 
year 2). The decline in function for all 
groups is first evident between years 2 
and 3. From years 3 to 5, the decline is 
0.2 
–0.4 
–0.6 
–0.8 
Time 
Mean IADL Difficulty Score 
Memory 
Control 
Baseline 
(n = 2802) 
Year 1 
(n = 2325) 
Year 2 
(n = 2234) 
Year 3 
(n = 2101) 
Year 4 Year 5 
(n = 1877) 
The mean scores are Blom-transformed. Error bars indicate SE. The sample sizes for each time point represent 
the number of cases with complete data for the IADL difficulty score. 
IADL difficulty (possible range: 0-38) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 1.3 (2.3) 1.5 (2.5) 1.5 (2.5) 1.3 (2.4) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 −0.7 −0.4 −0.3 −1.2 
Effect size (99% CI)* 0.20 (−0.06 to 0.46) 0.29 (0.03 to 0.55) 0.26 (−0.002 to 0.51) 
Everyday problem solving (possible range: 0-56) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 36.5 (9.4) 36.4 (8.9) 36.4 (8.9) 35.6 (9.3) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 1.5 1.8 1.5 2.4 
Effect size (99% CI)* −0.15 (−0.28 to 0.02) −0.08 (−0.21 to 0.05) −0.05 (−0.18 to 0.07) 
Everyday speed of processing (possible range: −3 to 100)† 
Mean (SD) at baseline 4.1 (1.7) 4.1 (1.7) 4.0 (1.6) 4.2 (2.5) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 0.1 0.3 0.2 0.1 
Effect size (99% CI)* 0.04 (−0.09 to 0.17) 0.09 (−0.04 to 0.22) 0.08 (−0.05 to 0.21) 
Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living. 
*Effect size defined as training improvement from baseline to year 5 minus control improvement from baseline to year 5 divided by intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite 
score. Positive effect sizes indicate improvement. 
†One component of this composite score is a standardized z score with a potential range of − to . 
©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2811 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
dramatically accelerated for the con-trol 
group and to a lesser extent for the 
3 treatment groups. 
Performance-Based IADL Measures. 
Training had no general effect on the 
performance-based measures of every-day 
problem solving or everyday speed 
of processing (Table 4). However, af-ter 
controlling for baseline age and cog-nitive 
function, the effect size for those 
participants in the speed of processing 
training group assigned to receive ad-ditional 
booster training was 0.30 (99% 
CI, 0.08-0.52) better for performance 
on everyday speed of processing 
compared with participants in this 
group not assigned to booster training 
(TABLE 5). 
COMMENT 
The ACTIVE study is the first large-scale, 
randomized trial to show that cog-nitive 
training improves cognitive func-tion 
in well-functioning older adults 
and that this improvement lasts up to 
5 years from the beginning of the in-tervention. 
In addition, this is the first 
trial to provide limited evidence that im-provements 
in cognitive function can 
have a positive effect on daily func-tion. 
Participants who received cogni-tive 
training reported less difficulty with 
IADLs 5 years after training compared 
with those in the control group. Par-ticipants 
in the 3 training groups re-ported 
an IADL decline of at least 0.20 
SD less than the participants in the con-trol 
group. The effect size reached sta-tistical 
significance only for the reason-ing 
training group but the effect sizes 
seen for memory and speed of process-ing 
training were similar to that for rea-soning. 
We consider the relative com-parability 
of the training effects across 
the 3 interventions to support the clini-cal 
meaningfulness of these results. Self-report 
of functioning repeatedly has 
been shown to predict loss of indepen-dence, 
increased use of health ser-vices, 
and mortality.42 Participants in the 
intervention groups reported simi-larly 
lower declines in function com-pared 
with participants in the control 
group. 
The finding of less decline in self-reported 
everyday function is concep-tually 
complex. Training effects could 
reflect real benefits of intervention 
because each of the 3 trained cognitive 
abilities has been associated with every-day 
functioning.22,43,44 It was not pos-sible 
to blind participants to their treat-ment 
assignment (that is, participants 
knewwhether they were receiving train-ing). 
Thus, the effects of training on self-reported 
IADL function could also 
reflect personal beliefs and self-perception 
related to knowing whether 
one has received training. While self-report 
bias or overestimation of func-tional 
abilities has been reported in indi-viduals 
with cognitive impairment,45,46 
it is unlikely in this study because 
ACTIVE participants were screened for 
dementia and showed improved cog- 
Table 5. Effect of Booster Training on Functional Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5* 
Intervention Groups (N = 2802) 
Memory Reasoning Speed of Processing 
IADL difficulty (possible range: 0-38) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 
Booster 1.2 (2.1) 1.5 (2.7) 1.5 (2.4) 
Nonbooster 1.5 (2.4) 1.5 (2.3) 1.5 (2.6) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 
Booster −1.0 −0.2 −0.4 
Nonbooster −0.4 −0.6 −0.3 
Effect size (99% CI)† −0.09 (−0.49 to 0.31) 0.24 (−0.16 to 0.64) −0.29 (−0.68 to 0.11) 
Everyday problem solving (possible range: 0-56) 
Mean (SD) at baseline 
Booster 37.2 (9.1) 37.1 (8.9) 36.3 (8.9) 
Nonbooster 35.8 (9.8) 35.6 (8.9) 36.4 (8.9) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 
Booster 1.4 1.5 1.4 
Nonbooster 1.7 1.9 1.5 
Effect size (99% CI)† 0.04 (−0.20 to 0.28) 0.22 (−0.02 to 0.46) −0.02 (−0.25 to 0.21) 
Everyday speed of processing (possible range: −3 to 100)‡ 
Mean (SD) at baseline 
Booster 4.0 (1.6) 4.0 (1.6) 4.1 (1.8) 
Nonbooster 4.1 (1.9) 4.1 (1.8) 4.0 (1.5) 
Mean change from baseline to year 5 
Booster 0.2 0.3 0.3 
Nonbooster 0 0.3 0.1 
Effect size (99% CI)† −0.008 (−0.23 to 0.22) 0.08 (−0.14 to 0.31) 0.30 (0.08 to 0.52) 
Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living. 
*Adjusted for baseline age and Mini-Mental State Examination score. 
†Effect size defined as training improvement in the booster group minus improvement in the nonbooster group divided by intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite 
score. 
‡One component of this composite score is a standardized z score with a potential range of − to . 
2812 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
nitive function in their trained ability 
over the study period compared with 
the control group. These results sug-gest 
that for self-reported IADLs, a pla-cebo 
control group that would facili-tate 
blinding and additional analyses of 
the mechanisms of training-related 
improvement (eg, whether training-related 
improvements in self-efficacy 
mediated the self-rated functional 
improvements) will be important future 
directions for this kind of research. 
There is growing consensus that as-sessment 
of daily function requires both 
self-report and performance-based mea-sures. 
47 The ACTIVE study is one of the 
few large-scale intervention studies to 
include both self-report and perfor-mance- 
based outcome measures of 
function to assess the breadth of inter-vention 
effects. Consistent with the 
study’s conceptual model, we hypoth-esized 
that certain interventions would 
specifically affect 1 or more functional 
outcomes. This was observed for the 
performance-based functional mea-sures. 
When controlling for baseline age 
and cognitive function, booster train-ing 
for speed of processing showed a 
significant effect on the performance-based 
everyday speed of processing 
measure. This specific effect is consis-tent 
with prior research that indicated 
everyday speed of processing was more 
closely related to speed of process-ing9,13 
than to reasoning or memory. 
The fact that this effect occurred only 
in participants who received booster 
training, and not in those who re-ceived 
only initial training, may re-flect 
a need for larger doses of training 
before effects can be observed in this 
more cognitively demanding out-come. 
Across all outcomes, evidence for 
transfer of the effects of cognitive train-ing 
to function was modest and was not 
observed until the 5-year follow-up. 
There are 2 possible explanations for 
these delayed outcomes. First, prior re-search 
has suggested a temporal lag be-tween 
onset of cognitive decline and 
subsequent impact on daily func-tion, 
2-4 perhaps because of resulting dif-ficulty 
in adapting to emerging physi-cal 
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
limitations that affect tasks of daily 
living.48,49 However, if cognitive abil-ity 
is maintained or enhanced by train-ing, 
then this could result in a gradual 
emergence of adaptive, compensatory 
strategies for dealing with physical limi-tations. 
1 Second, delayed intervention 
effects on function may be attribut-able 
to the advantaged nature of the 
ACTIVE sample, and particularly the 
subset who received booster training. 
To select older adults most likely to 
benefit from cognitive training, per-sons 
with suspected cognitive and ADL 
decline were excluded from enroll-ment. 
This may have delayed the on-set 
of functional disabilities in the con-trol 
group until the 5-year follow-up. 
Only after the onset of decline in the 
control group could the positive train-ing 
effects on function be observed in 
the intervention groups. 
We are not aware of intervention 
trials with volunteer samples of older 
adults that include 5-year follow-up for 
direct comparison of retention rates. 
The 67% retention rate at 5 years is con-sistent 
with other longitudinal com-munity- 
based studies resembling 
ACTIVE in terms of sample age and eth-nicity 
and frequency and duration of 
study contact (eg, 70.6% in a 4-year 
study of blacks and whites in Chi-cago50; 
61.6% in a 5-year study of blacks 
in Indianapolis51; and 59.6% in a 7-year 
study of Hispanics, whites, and blacks 
in New York City52). However, be-cause 
these studies were not interven-tion 
trials, they did not have the same 
level of respondent burden as ACTIVE. 
Therefore, while the direct compara-bility 
of retention rates to other inter-vention 
trials cannot be made, the rates 
of these observational studies sug-gests 
that the retention rate in ACTIVE 
is quite acceptable given the burden-some 
nature of the study protocol. 
In conclusion, declines in cognitive 
abilities have been shown to lead to in-creased 
risk of functional disabilities 
that are primary risk factors for loss of 
independence. The 5-year results of the 
ACTIVE study provide limited evi-dence 
that cognitive interventions can 
reduce age-related decline in self-reported 
IADLs that are the precur-sors 
of dependence in basic ADLs as-sociated 
with increased use of hospital, 
outpatient, home health, and nursing 
home services,53,54 and health care ex-penditures. 
55 However, given the lag in 
the relationship between cognitive de-cline 
and functional deficits, the full ex-tent 
of the interventional effects on daily 
function would take longer than 5 years 
to observe in a population that was 
highly functioning at enrollment. We 
consider these results promising and 
support future research to examine if 
these and other cognitive interven-tions 
can prevent or delay functional 
disability in an aging population. 
Author Affiliations: Department of Human Develop-ment 
and Family Studies, Pennsylvania State Univer-sity, 
State College (Dr Willis); New England Research 
Institutes, Watertown, Mass (Drs Tennstedt, Stod-dard, 
and Wright); Institute on Aging and Depart-ment 
of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of 
Florida, Gainesville (Dr Marsiske); Department of Psy-chology, 
University of Alabama, Birmingham (Dr Ball); 
National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Md (Dr Elias); 
National Institute of Nursing Research, Bethesda, Md 
(Dr Koepke); Hebrew Senior Life, Boston, Mass (Dr 
Morris); Department of Mental Health, Johns Hop-kins 
University, Baltimore, Md (Dr Rebok); and De-partment 
of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of 
Medicine, Indianapolis (Dr Unverzagt). 
Author Contributions: Drs Wright and Stoddard had 
full access to all of the data in the study and take re-sponsibility 
for the integrity of the data and the ac-curacy 
of the data analysis. 
Study concept and design: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, 
Ball, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Wright. 
Acquisition of data: Willis, Marsiske, Ball, Morris, 
Rebok, Unverzagt. 
Analysis and interpretation of data: Willis, Tennstedt, 
Marsiske, Ball, Elias, Koepke, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, 
Stoddard, Wright. 
Drafting of the manuscript: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, 
Ball, Elias, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. 
Critical revision of the manuscript for important in-tellectual 
content: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, 
Koepke, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Stoddard, Wright. 
Statistical analysis: Marsiske, Morris, Stoddard, Wright. 
Obtained funding: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, 
Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. 
Administrative, technical, or material support: Willis, 
Marsiske, Ball, Elias, Koepke, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. 
Study supervision: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, 
Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Wright. 
Financial Disclosures: Dr Ball owns stock in Visual 
Awareness Inc, which owns the patent for the Useful 
Field of View testing and training software. No other 
authors reported disclosures. 
Funding/Support: ACTIVE is supported by grants from 
the National Institute on Aging and the National In-stitute 
of Nursing Research to Hebrew Senior Life (U01 
NR04507), Indiana University School of Medicine (U01 
NR04508), Johns Hopkins University (U01AG14260), 
New England Research Institutes (U01 AG14282), 
Pennsylvania State University (U01 AG14263), the Uni-versity 
of Alabama at Birmingham (U01 AG14289), 
and the University of Florida (U01AG14276). 
Role of the Sponsor: Representatives of the National 
Institute on Aging and the National Institute for Nurs-ing 
Research were directly involved in the design of 
©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2813 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
the study, interpretation of the data, and prepara-tion, 
review, and approval of the manuscript. These 
representatives also monitored the conduct of the 
study, collection, management, and analysis of the 
data. 
ACTIVE Study Investigators: Richard N. Jones, ScD, 
Adrienne L. Rosenberg,MS(Hebrew Senior Life); Kathy 
Johnson, PhD, Daniel F. Rexroth, PsyD, David M. 
Smith, MD, Elizabeth Way, BA, Fredric D. Wolinsky, 
PhD (Indiana University School of Medicine); Kay 
Cresci, PhD, RN, Joseph Gallo, MD, MPH, Laura Tal-bot, 
PhD, EdD, RN, CS ( Johns Hopkins University); 
Michael Doherty, MS, Patricia Forde, BS, Yan Xu, MS 
(New England Research Institutes, data coordinating 
center); Pamela Davis, MS, Scott Hofer, PhD, K. Warner 
Schaie, PhD (Pennsylvania State University); Jerri Ed-wards, 
PhD, Martha Frankel, Cynthia Owsley, PhD, 
Dan Roenker, PhD, David Vance, PhD, Virginia Wad-ley, 
PhD (University of Alabama at Birmingham); Man-fred 
K. Diehl, PhD, Ann L. Horgas, RN, PhD, FAAN, 
Peter A. Lichtenberg, PhD, ABPP (University of Florida/ 
Wayne State University). 
Acknowledgment: We thank Robin Barr, PhD, of the 
National Institute on Aging for his authorship of the re-quest 
for applications and his continuing support and 
helpful comments throughout the conduct of the project. 
Dr Barr was not compensated for this assistance. 
REFERENCES 
1. Kelly-Hayes M, Jette AM, Wolf PA, D’Agostino 
RB, Odell PM. Functional limitations and disability 
among elders in the Framingham Study. Am J Public 
Health. 1992;82:841-845. 
2. Stuck AE, Walthert JM, Nikolaus T, Bula CJ, Ho-hmann 
C, Beck JC. Risk factors for functional status 
decline in community-living elderly people: a system-atic 
literature review. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:445-469. 
3. Wolinsky FD, Callahan CM, Fitzgerald JF, Johnson 
RJ. Changes in functional status and the risks of sub-sequent 
nursing home placement and death. J Ger-ontol 
B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1993;48:S94-S101. 
4. Willis S. Societal Mechanisms for Maintaining Com-petence 
in Old Age. New York, NY: Springer Publish-ing; 
1997. 
5. Royall DR, Palmer R, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Normal 
rates of cognitive change in successful aging: the free-dom 
house study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11: 
899-909. 
6. Davis RN, Massman PJ, Doody RS. Cognitive in-tervention 
in Alzheimer disease: a randomized placebo-controlled 
study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2001; 
15:1-9. 
7. Cherry KE, Simmons-D’Gerolamo SS. Long-term 
effectiveness of spaced-retrieval memory training for 
older adults with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Exp 
Aging Res. 2005;31:261-289. 
8. Rebok GW, Balcerak LJ. Memory self-efficacy and 
performance differences in young and old adults: ef-fects 
of mnemonic training. Dev Psychol. 1989;25:714- 
721. 
9. Willis SL. Cognitive training and everyday 
competence. Annu Rev Gerontol Geriatr. 1987;7:159- 
188. 
10. O’Hara R, Brooks JO III, Friedman L, Schroder 
CM, Morgan KS, Kraemer HC. Long-term effects of 
mnemonic training in community-dwelling older adults 
[published online ahead of print June 14, 2006]. J Psy-chiatr 
Res. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.04.010. 
11. Derwinger A, Stigsdotter Neely A, MacDonald 
S, Backman L. Forgetting numbers in old age: strat-egy 
and learning speed matter. Gerontology. 2005;51: 
277-284. 
12. Jobe JB, Smith DM, Ball K, et al. ACTIVE: a cog-nitive 
intervention trial to promote independence in 
older adults. Control Clin Trials. 2001;22:453-479. 
13. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, et al. Effects of cog-nitive 
training interventions with older adults: a ran-domized 
controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271-2281. 
14. Lazaridis EN, Rudberg MA, Furner SE, Cassel CK. 
Do activities of daily living have a hierarchical struc-ture? 
an analysis using the longitudinal study of aging. 
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1994;49:M47-M51. 
15. Wolinsky F, Miller D. Disability concepts and mea-surement: 
contributions of the epidemiology of dis-ability 
to gerontological inquiry. In: Wilmoth J, Fer-raro 
K, eds. Gerontology: Perspectives and Issues. 3rd 
ed. New York, NY: Springer Publishing; 2006. 
16. Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK, Wadley VG, Ed-wards 
JD. Speed-of-processing and driving simulator 
training result in improved driving performance. Hum 
Factors. 2003;45:218-233. 
17. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini- 
Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cog-nitive 
state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 
1975;12:189-198. 
18. Clark F, Azen SP, Zemke R, et al. Occupational 
therapy for independent-living older adults: a ran-domized 
controlled trial. JAMA. 1997;278:1321-1326. 
19. Willis SL, Cornelius SW, Blow FC, Baltes PB. Train-ing 
research in aging: attentional processes. J Educ 
Psychol. 1983;75:257-270. 
20. Rasmusson D, Rebok G, Bylsma F, Brandt J. Ef-fects 
of three types of memory training in normal 
elderly. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 1999;6:56-66. 
21. Willis SL, Schaie KW. Training the elderly on the 
ability factors of spatial orientation and inductive 
reasoning. Psychol Aging. 1986;1:239-247. 
22. Ball K. Increased mobility and reducing acci-dents 
of older drivers. In: Schaie K, Pietrucha M, eds. 
Mobility and Transportation in the Elderly. Vol 5. New 
York, NY: Springer; 2000:213-250. 
23. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Myers RS, Roenker DL, 
Cissell GM, Ball KK. Transfer of a speed of processing 
intervention to near and far cognitive functions. 
Gerontology. 2002;48:329-340. 
24. Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: de-velopment 
of a new memory test with six equivalent 
forms. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5:125-142. 
25. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas 
d’encephalopathie tramatique. Arch Psychol. 1941;28: 
286-340. 
26. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Baddeley A. The River-mead 
Behavioral Memory Test. Reading, England and 
Gaylord, Mich: Thames Valley Test Co and National 
Rehabilitation Services; 1985. 
27. Gonda J, Schaie K. Schaie-Thurstone Mental Abili-ties 
Test: Word Series Test. Palo Alto, Calif: Consult-ing 
Psychologists Press; 1985. 
28. Thurstone L, Thurstone T. Examiner Manual for 
the SRA Primary Mental Abilities Test (Form 10-14). 
Chicago, Ill: Science Research Associates; 1949. 
29. Ekstrom R, French J, Harman H, Derman D. Kit 
of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Revised ed. 
Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service; 1976. 
30. Owsley C, Ball K, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni 
JR. Visual/cognitive correlates of vehicle accidents in 
older drivers. Psychol Aging. 1991;6:403-415. 
31. Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G Jr, et al. Visual 
processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle 
crash among older adults. JAMA. 1998;279:1083- 
1088. 
32. Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs 
DS. Age and visual search: expanding the useful field 
of view. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5:2210-2219. 
33. Teresi J, Lawton M, Holmes D, Ory M. Measure-ment 
in elderly chronic care populations. In: Morris J, 
Morris S, eds. ADL Assessment Measures for Use With 
Frail Elders. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co; 
1997. 
34. Willis S, Marsiske M. Manual for the Everyday 
Problems Test. University Park: Pennsylvania State Uni-versity; 
1993. 
35. Diehl M, Marsiske M, Horgas AL, Rosenberg A, 
Saczynski J, Willis SL. The Revised Observed Tasks of 
Daily Living: a performance-based assessment of ev-eryday 
problem solving in older adults. J Appl Gerontol. 
2005;24:211-230. 
36. Owsley C, Sloane M, McGwin G Jr, Ball K. Timed 
instrumental activities of daily living tasks: relation-ship 
to cognitive function and everyday performance 
assessments in older adults. Gerontology. 2002;48:254- 
265. 
37. Cnaan A, Laird NM, Slasor P. Using the general 
linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated 
measures and longitudinal data. Stat Med. 1997;16: 
2349-2380. 
38. Blom G. Statistical Estimates and Transformed 
Beta-Variables. New York, NY: Wiley; 1958. 
39. Lehmann E. Nonparametrics: Statistical Meth-ods 
Based on Ranks. San Francisco, Calif: Holden- 
Day; 1975. 
40. Schafer J. Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate 
Data. London, England: Chapman  Hall; 1997. 
41. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Be-havioral 
Sciences. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Er-lbaum 
Assoc; 1988. 
42. Schupf N, Tang MX, Albert SM, et al. Decline in 
cognitive and functional skills increases mortality 
risk in nondemented elderly. Neurology. 2005;65: 
1218-1226. 
43. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Well- and ill-defined mea-sures 
of everyday cognition: relationship to older adults’ 
intellectual ability and functional status. Psychol Aging. 
2002;17:101-115. 
44. Willis SL. Everyday cognitive competence in el-derly 
persons: conceptual issues and empirical findings. 
Gerontologist. 1996;36:595-601. 
45. Wadley VG, Harrell LE, Marson DC. Self- and in-formant 
report of financial abilities in patients with Alz-heimer’s 
disease: reliable and valid? J Am Geriatr Soc. 
2003;51:1621-1626. 
46. Koltai DC, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Schmechel DE. In-fluence 
of anosognosia on treatment outcome among 
dementia patients. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2001;11: 
455-475. 
47. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Everyday cognition: age 
and intellectual ability correlates. Psychol Aging. 1999; 
14:627-644. 
48. Wolinsky FD, Callahan CM, Fitzgerald JF, Johnson 
RJ. The risk of nursing home placement and subse-quent 
death among older adults. J Gerontol. 1992;47: 
S173-S182. 
49. Boyle PA, Paul RH, Moser DJ, Cohen RA. Execu-tive 
impairments predict functional declines in vascu-lar 
dementia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18:75-82. 
50. Evans DA, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, et al. Inci-dence 
of Alzheimer disease in a biracial urban com-munity: 
relation to apolipoprotein E allele status. Arch 
Neurol. 2003;60:185-189. 
51. Hendrie HC, Ogunniyi A, Hall KS, et al. Inci-dence 
of dementia and Alzheimer disease in 2 com-munities: 
Yoruba residing in Ibadan, Nigeria, and Af-rican 
Americans residing in Indianapolis, Indiana. JAMA. 
2001;285:739-747. 
52. Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, et al. Incidence 
of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and 
Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology. 2001; 
56:49-56. 
53. Fried TR, Bradley EH, Williams CS, Tinetti ME. Func-tional 
disability and health care expenditures for older 
persons. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:2602-2607. 
54. Plehn K, Marcopulos BA, McLain CA. The rela-tionship 
between neuropsychological test perfor-mance, 
social functioning, and instrumental activities 
of daily living in a sample of rural older adults. Clin 
Neuropsychol. 2004;18:101-113. 
55. Kane R, Kane R, Ladd R. The Heart of Long- 
Term Care. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 
1998. 
COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 
2814 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. 
Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006

More Related Content

What's hot

Memory for Intentions Screening Test
Memory for Intentions Screening TestMemory for Intentions Screening Test
Memory for Intentions Screening TestColin MacKichan
 
Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409
Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409
Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409Cassio Meira Jr.
 
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 NiedzweckiThe Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 NiedzweckiChristian Niedzwecki
 
Interventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBI
Interventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBIInterventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBI
Interventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBILoki Stormbringer
 
Senior Thesis Presentation Final
Senior Thesis Presentation FinalSenior Thesis Presentation Final
Senior Thesis Presentation Finalrobrhenderson
 
In-Service- Fieldwork II - CRT
In-Service- Fieldwork II - CRTIn-Service- Fieldwork II - CRT
In-Service- Fieldwork II - CRTJenna Bisignano
 
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- Research
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- ResearchNeurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- Research
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- Researchda5884
 
Bayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components Analysis
Bayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components AnalysisBayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components Analysis
Bayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components AnalysisKevin Cummins
 
Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy management
Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy managementDevelopmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy management
Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy managementShruthi Puthran
 
Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...
Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...
Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...Health Evidence™
 
Therese jackson
Therese jacksonTherese jackson
Therese jacksonCOT SSNP
 
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...iosrjce
 
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsey
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral PalseyNeurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsey
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palseyda5884
 
Cognitive Rehabilitation in MS
Cognitive Rehabilitation in MSCognitive Rehabilitation in MS
Cognitive Rehabilitation in MSErdil Arsoy
 
Artigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia
Artigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophreniaArtigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia
Artigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophreniaJeane Araujo
 
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA ReviewPCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA ReviewScott Miller
 

What's hot (19)

Memory for Intentions Screening Test
Memory for Intentions Screening TestMemory for Intentions Screening Test
Memory for Intentions Screening Test
 
Nora Eilert
Nora Eilert Nora Eilert
Nora Eilert
 
Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409
Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409
Bokums et al_2012_psych_v3_n5_406_409
 
E03403019026
E03403019026E03403019026
E03403019026
 
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 NiedzweckiThe Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
 
Interventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBI
Interventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBIInterventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBI
Interventions to Improve Cognitive Functioning After TBI
 
Senior Thesis Presentation Final
Senior Thesis Presentation FinalSenior Thesis Presentation Final
Senior Thesis Presentation Final
 
In-Service- Fieldwork II - CRT
In-Service- Fieldwork II - CRTIn-Service- Fieldwork II - CRT
In-Service- Fieldwork II - CRT
 
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- Research
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- ResearchNeurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- Research
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsy- Research
 
Bayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components Analysis
Bayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components AnalysisBayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components Analysis
Bayesian Model for Multivatiate Functional Principle Components Analysis
 
Scientifi c Journal of Depression & Anxiety
Scientifi c Journal of Depression & AnxietyScientifi c Journal of Depression & Anxiety
Scientifi c Journal of Depression & Anxiety
 
Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy management
Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy managementDevelopmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy management
Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) - Physiotherapy management
 
Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...
Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...
Psychological depression prevention programs for 5-10 year olds: What’s the e...
 
Therese jackson
Therese jacksonTherese jackson
Therese jackson
 
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
 
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsey
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral PalseyNeurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsey
Neurodevelopmental Treatment and Cerebral Palsey
 
Cognitive Rehabilitation in MS
Cognitive Rehabilitation in MSCognitive Rehabilitation in MS
Cognitive Rehabilitation in MS
 
Artigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia
Artigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophreniaArtigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia
Artigo esquizofrenia a meta analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia
 
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA ReviewPCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
PCOMS ICCE SAMHSA Review
 

Similar to Long term effects of cognitive training

Running Head EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docx
Running Head  EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docxRunning Head  EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docx
Running Head EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docxSUBHI7
 
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebralesMeditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebralesRAUL TAYA PEREZ
 
Salud U4 P2
Salud U4 P2Salud U4 P2
Salud U4 P2c.meza
 
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docxRunning  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docxInternational Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docxnormanibarber20063
 
public health _03302015 resized
public health _03302015 resizedpublic health _03302015 resized
public health _03302015 resizedZakkoyya Lewis
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity Giuseppe Fattori
 
EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...
EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...
EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...Shabana2428
 
Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...
Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...
Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...Sports Journal
 
Technology for restitution
Technology for restitution Technology for restitution
Technology for restitution Derek Jones
 
Linkedin version of systematic review
Linkedin version of systematic reviewLinkedin version of systematic review
Linkedin version of systematic reviewJunQuanNg
 
Journal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptx
Journal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptxJournal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptx
Journal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptxSaran A K
 
Cognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and Teens
Cognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and TeensCognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and Teens
Cognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and TeensMark Dreher PhD
 
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docxArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docxfredharris32
 
Bauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_Print
Bauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_PrintBauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_Print
Bauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_PrintSarah Bauermeister PhD
 
Cognitive rehabilitation
Cognitive rehabilitationCognitive rehabilitation
Cognitive rehabilitationDikerJoshi
 
Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015
Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015
Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015Claudia Hilton
 

Similar to Long term effects of cognitive training (20)

Running Head EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docx
Running Head  EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docxRunning Head  EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docx
Running Head EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON COGNITION 2EFFECTS O.docx
 
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebralesMeditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
 
Salud U4 P2
Salud U4 P2Salud U4 P2
Salud U4 P2
 
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docxRunning  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
 
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docxInternational Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
 
public health _03302015 resized
public health _03302015 resizedpublic health _03302015 resized
public health _03302015 resized
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
 
EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...
EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...
EVALUATION OF PHYSICAL INDEPENDENCE OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES AND ITS COR...
 
Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...
Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...
Effects physical exercises and minor games on speed in mild mentally challeng...
 
Technology for restitution
Technology for restitution Technology for restitution
Technology for restitution
 
Linkedin version of systematic review
Linkedin version of systematic reviewLinkedin version of systematic review
Linkedin version of systematic review
 
Experimental study on alzhimer
Experimental study on alzhimerExperimental study on alzhimer
Experimental study on alzhimer
 
Journal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptx
Journal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptxJournal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptx
Journal Club Presentation - AKL03 Depression.pptx
 
ayres1972.pdf
ayres1972.pdfayres1972.pdf
ayres1972.pdf
 
Cognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and Teens
Cognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and TeensCognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and Teens
Cognitive Benefits of Exercise for Children and Teens
 
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docxArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
ArticlehejHealth Education Journal70(1) 9–20© Th.docx
 
Bauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_Print
Bauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_PrintBauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_Print
Bauermeister and Bunce GHQ CAC2014_FINAL_Print
 
Cognitive rehabilitation
Cognitive rehabilitationCognitive rehabilitation
Cognitive rehabilitation
 
Heart
HeartHeart
Heart
 
Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015
Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015
Hilton.Exergaming to improve physical and mental fitness in ASD.2015
 

More from MiriamTrapagaO (20)

Caso 1 tema 3
Caso 1 tema 3Caso 1 tema 3
Caso 1 tema 3
 
Caso 4 tema 3
Caso 4 tema 3Caso 4 tema 3
Caso 4 tema 3
 
Caso 3 tema 3
Caso 3 tema 3Caso 3 tema 3
Caso 3 tema 3
 
Caso 2 tema 3
Caso 2 tema 3Caso 2 tema 3
Caso 2 tema 3
 
Caso 4 tema 2
Caso 4 tema 2Caso 4 tema 2
Caso 4 tema 2
 
Caso 3 tema 2
Caso 3 tema 2Caso 3 tema 2
Caso 3 tema 2
 
Caso 2 tema 2
Caso 2 tema 2Caso 2 tema 2
Caso 2 tema 2
 
Caso 1 tema 2
Caso 1 tema 2Caso 1 tema 2
Caso 1 tema 2
 
Caso 4 tema 1
Caso 4 tema 1Caso 4 tema 1
Caso 4 tema 1
 
Caso 3 tema 1
Caso 3 tema 1Caso 3 tema 1
Caso 3 tema 1
 
Caso 2 tema 1
Caso 2 tema 1Caso 2 tema 1
Caso 2 tema 1
 
Caso 1 tema 1
Caso 1 tema 1Caso 1 tema 1
Caso 1 tema 1
 
Fichas 2 basicas
Fichas 2 basicasFichas 2 basicas
Fichas 2 basicas
 
Fichas 1 lenguaje
Fichas 1 lenguajeFichas 1 lenguaje
Fichas 1 lenguaje
 
Fichas 4 atención
Fichas 4 atenciónFichas 4 atención
Fichas 4 atención
 
Fichas 3 memoria
Fichas 3 memoriaFichas 3 memoria
Fichas 3 memoria
 
Ficha 6 percepcion
Ficha 6 percepcionFicha 6 percepcion
Ficha 6 percepcion
 
Ficha 5 habilidades constructivas
Ficha 5 habilidades constructivasFicha 5 habilidades constructivas
Ficha 5 habilidades constructivas
 
Principios de rn
Principios de rnPrincipios de rn
Principios de rn
 
Propuesta mejoramiento cognitivo del am
Propuesta mejoramiento cognitivo del amPropuesta mejoramiento cognitivo del am
Propuesta mejoramiento cognitivo del am
 

Recently uploaded

Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...astropune
 
Call Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...Genuine Call Girls
 
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...Call Girls in Nagpur High Profile
 
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...astropune
 
O898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
O898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In AhmedabadO898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
O898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In AhmedabadGenuine Call Girls
 
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...Dipal Arora
 
The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...
The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...
The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...chandars293
 
All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...
All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...
All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...Arohi Goyal
 
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...tanya dube
 
Call Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Call Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...hotbabesbook
 
Call Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Call Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Call Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Call Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...
Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...
Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...parulsinha
 
Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Dipal Arora
 
(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...
(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...
(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...Taniya Sharma
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
 
Call Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Gwalior Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
 
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
 
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
 
O898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
O898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In AhmedabadO898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
O898O367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
 
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
 
The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...
The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...
The Most Attractive Hyderabad Call Girls Kothapet 𖠋 9332606886 𖠋 Will You Mis...
 
All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...
All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...
All Time Service Available Call Girls Marine Drive 📳 9820252231 For 18+ VIP C...
 
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
 
Call Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Bangalore Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Call Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Agra Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai City Center Call Girls 👉👉 7427069034⭐⭐ 100% Genuine E...
 
Call Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Nagpur Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Call Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Ooty Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Call Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Jabalpur Just Call 8250077686 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Call Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Faridabad Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...
Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...
Premium Call Girls In Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP SEEMA Call Girl in Jaipur Ra...
 
Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Guwahati ) Call Girls Guwahati ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
 
(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...
(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...
(👑VVIP ISHAAN ) Russian Call Girls Service Navi Mumbai🖕9920874524🖕Independent...
 

Long term effects of cognitive training

  • 1. ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Long-term Effects of Cognitive Training on Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older Adults Sherry L. Willis, PhD Sharon L. Tennstedt, PhD Michael Marsiske, PhD Karlene Ball, PhD Jeffrey Elias, PhD Kathy Mann Koepke, PhD John N. Morris, PhD George W. Rebok, PhD Frederick W. Unverzagt, PhD Anne M. Stoddard, ScD Elizabeth Wright, PhD for the ACTIVE Study Group DECLINE IN COGNITIVE ABILI-ties has been shown to lead to an increased risk of diffi-culty in performing instru-mental activities of daily living (IADL).1-5 However, whether inter-ventions to maintain or enhance cog-nitive abilities in older adults will pre-vent or delay these functional difficulties is unclear. Prior interven-tions with older adults have targeted those with cognitive deficits or func-tional disabilities and have focused on remediation rather than prevention.6,7 Prior studies have shown that cogni-tive interventions can improve cogni-tive abilities in normal elders but have not included functional outcome measures and have been limited by small, homogeneous samples and lack of randomization.8-11 For editorial comment see p 2852. Context Cognitive training has been shown to improve cognitive abilities in older adults but the effects of cognitive training on everyday function have not been demonstrated. Objective To determine the effects of cognitive training on daily function and du-rability Author Affiliations are listed at the end of this article. CorrespondingAuthor:Sherry L. Willis,PhD,Department ofHumanDevelopment and Family Studies, Pennsylva-nia State University, 135 E Nittany Ave, Suite 405, State College, PA 16801 (slw@psu.edu). of training on cognitive abilities. Design, Setting, and Participants Five-year follow-up of a randomized con-trolled single-blind trial with 4 treatment groups. A volunteer sample of 2832 persons (mean age, 73.6 years; 26% black), living independently in 6 US cities, was recruited from senior housing, community centers, and hospitals and clinics. The study was con-ducted between April 1998 and December 2004. Five-year follow-up was completed in 67% of the sample. Interventions Ten-session training for memory (verbal episodic memory), reason-ing (inductive reasoning), or speed of processing (visual search and identification); 4-ses-sion booster training at 11 and 35 months after training in a random sample of those who completed training. Main Outcome Measures Self-reported and performance-based measures of daily function and cognitive abilities. Results The reasoning group reported significantly less difficulty in the instrumental ac-tivities of daily living (IADL) than the control group (effect size, 0.29; 99% confidence interval [CI], 0.03-0.55). Neither speed of processing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% CI, −0.002 to 0.51) nor memory training (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 0.46) had a significant effect on IADL. The booster training for the speed of processing group, but not for the other 2 groups, showed a significant effect on the performance-based func-tional measure of everyday speed of processing (effect size, 0.30; 99% CI, 0.08-0.52). No booster effects were seen for any of the groups for everyday problem-solving or self-reported difficulty in IADL. Each intervention maintained effects on its specific targeted cognitive ability through 5 years (memory: effect size, 0.23 [99% CI, 0.11-0.35]; rea-soning: effect size, 0.26 [99% CI, 0.17-0.35]; speed of processing: effect size, 0.76 [99% CI, 0.62-0.90]). Booster training produced additional improvement with the reasoning intervention for reasoning performance (effect size, 0.28; 99% CI, 0.12-0.43) and the speed of processing intervention for speed of processing performance (effect size, 0.85; 99% CI, 0.61-1.09). Conclusions Reasoning training resulted in less functional decline in self-reported IADL. Compared with the control group, cognitive training resulted in improved cog-nitive abilities specific to the abilities trained that continued 5 years after the initiation of the intervention. Trial Registration clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00298558 JAMA. 2006;296:2805-2814 www.jama.com ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2805 Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 2. COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES The Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (AC-TIVE) study is the first multicenter, ran-domized controlled trial to examine the long-term outcomes of cognitive inter-ventions on the daily functioning of older individuals living indepen-dently. 12 Previously reported data from the ACTIVE study showed that each of 3 cognitive interventions improved the cognitive ability it targeted and these improvements were maintained through the 2 years of follow-up.13 This article addresses the long-term effects of cognitive training on the mainte-nance of self-reported IADL. FIGURE 1 presents the conceptual model for the ACTIVE trial. Four hypotheses were derived from this model. First, cognitive training would affect the cognitive ability targeted by that training and these effects would be maintained over time. Second, main-tained improvements in cognitive abil-ity would have a positive transfer effect on everyday function. Based on prior research,14,15weexpected to first see this transfer of training effects to affect IADL functioning. Third, this training trans-fer from cognitive abilities toIADLfunc-tion would occur in all 3 training groups because each of the 3 cognitive abili-ties has been associated withIADLfunc-tioning. 9,16 While the broadest transfer of training effects was expected to improve IADL functioning, we also expected to see other training-specific effects (ie, transfer of improvement in reasoning and memory to improve-ments in everyday problem solving or transfer of improvement in speed of pro-cessing to improvements in everyday speed of processing tasks). Finally, because of the functional indepen-dence of participants at baseline, it was hypothesized that observations of train-ing effects on IADL functioning would be delayed until the control group began to experience significant functional decline. This was observed at the 5-year follow-up and therefore the hypoth-esized delayed outcome of mainte-nance of IADL function could be tested. METHODS Participants The sample consisted of older adults liv-ing independently with good functional and cognitive status who were recruited from senior housing, community cen-ters, and hospitals and clinics in Bir-mingham, Ala; Detroit, Mich; Boston, Mass; Baltimore, Md; Indianapolis, Ind; and State College, Pa. Participants were enrolled in the study between March 1998 and October 1999.12 Persons were excluded if they were younger than 65 years; had substantial functional impair-ment (2ADLdisabilities) or cognitive decline (Mini-Mental State Examina-tion [MMSE] score 22)17; self-reported diagnosis of Alzheimer dis-ease; medical conditions associated with imminent functional decline or death; severe losses in vision, hearing, or com-municative ability that would interfere with study participation; recent cogni-tive training; or were unavailable dur-ing the study period. Because previous studies of cognitive training had been conducted in white older adults, recruit-mentof other race and ethnic groups was emphasized. Race was self-reported as white, black, Asian, Native Hawaiian/ Pacific Islander, American Indian/ Alaskan Native, or biracial. Participants indicated if they were Hispanic or Latino. Study Design ACTIVE was a randomized controlled, single-blind trial, using a4-groupdesign, including 3 treatment groups and a con-trol group (FIGURE2). Participants were randomized to a group by the data co-ordinating center using a computer ran-domization program. Assessments were conducted at baseline, following the in-tervention, and annually at 1, 2, 3, and 5 years. Assessors were blinded to treat-ment assignment. Exposure to social contact was equated in the 3 interven-tion groups. The control group had no contact; a placebo social contact control wasnot included because prior interven-tion studies indicated no differences be-tween a social contact control and a “no contact” control in either cognitive or functional improvement.18,19 Prior ACTIVEresults13 showed that cognitive effectswerespecific toeachintervention. Thestudy protocol was approved by the institutionalreviewboardsat all sitesand the trial was monitored by a data and Figure 1. Conceptual Model of ACTIVE Trial RANDOMIZED COGNITIVE TRAINING INTERVENTION Reasoning Training Memory Training Speed Training COGNITIVE OUTCOMES Reasoning Memory Attentional Speed FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES Everyday Problem Solving ADL and IADL Functioning Everyday Speed of Processing Individual Characteristics (Covariates) Cognitive Function/Disorder Health Conditions Physical Characteristics Sensory Function Sociodemographics ACTIVE indicates Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly; ADL, activities of daily liv-ing; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living. 2806 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 3. safety monitoring board. Written in-formed consent was obtained from all participants. COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES Interventions Each of the 3 training interventions was designed to narrowly target a specific cognitive ability—memory, reason-ing, or speed of processing—and in-cluded no overlap with the functional Figure 2. Flow of Individuals in ACTIVE Trial Year 2 Evaluation 27 Partial Training 1 Died 36 Withdrew 6 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 28 Not Assessed 7 Received Partial Booster 10 Died 25 Withdrew 3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 45 Not Assessed 18 Excluded 6 Died 9 Withdrew 3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 49 Not Assessed 563 Assessed 45 Partial Training 5 Died 37 Withdrew 4 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 30 Not Assessed 4 Received Partial Booster 15 Died 20 Withdrew 2 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 56 Not Assessed 17 Excluded 3 Died 11 Withdrew 3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 50 Not Assessed 555 Assessed 35 Partial Training 1 Died 28 Withdrew 1 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 29 Not Assessed 4 Received Partial Booster 11 Died 14 Withdrew 4 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 52 Not Assessed 17 Excluded 9 Died 5 Withdrew 3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 62 Not Assessed 574 Assessed 1 Died 25 Withdrew 8 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 31 Not Assessed 9 Died 24 Withdrew 4 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 49 Not Assessed 26 Excluded 9 Died 14 Withdrew 3 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 55 Not Assessed 552 Assessed Year 1 Evaluation 38 Excluded 505 Assessed 37 Excluded 566 Assessed 29 Excluded 601 Assessed 37 Excluded 584 Assessed Immediate Posttest 43 Excluded 640 Assessed 46 Excluded 629 Assessed 30 Excluded 653 Assessed 34 Excluded 639 Assessed Booster Training 372 Assigned to Receive Booster Training 82 Did Not Receive Booster 283 Completed Booster 371 Assigned to Receive Booster Training 66 Did Not Receive Booster 301 Completed Booster 370 Assigned to Receive Booster Training 71 Did Not Receive Booster 295 Completed Booster 372 Assigned to Receive Booster Training 118 Did Not Receive Booster 4 Received Partial Booster 250 Completed Booster 371 Assigned to Receive Booster Training 121 Did Not Receive Booster 7 Received Partial Booster 243 Completed Booster 370 Assigned to Receive Booster Training 133 Did Not Receive Booster 7 Received Partial Booster 230 Completed Booster Booster Training at Year 3 2168 Excluded 905 Ineligible 1263 Refused Training 711 Assigned to Receive Memory Training 64 No Training 620 Completed Training 705 Assigned to Receive Reasoning Training 33 No Training 627 Completed Training 712 Assigned to Receive Speed Training 40 No Training 637 Completed Training 704 Assigned to Control 5000 Individuals Assessed for Eligibility 2832 Randomized Baseline 32 Died 54 Withdrew 52 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 2 Family Refusal Total Attrition 49 Died 124 Withdrew 64 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 2 Family Refusal 41 Died 49 Withdrew 37 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 9 Family Refusal Total Attrition 64 Died 117 Withdrew 46 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 9 Family Refusal 46 Died 54 Withdrew 38 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 8 Family Refusal Total Attrition 67 Died 101 Withdrew 46 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 8 Family Refusal 46 Died 66 Withdrew 43 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 4 Family Refusal Total Attrition 65 Died 129 Withdrew 58 Administrative Withdrawal∗ 4 Family Refusal Years 3 and 5 Evaluation 140 Excluded 472 Assessed 136 Excluded 469 Assessed 146 Excluded 490 Assessed 159 Excluded 448 Assessed 6 Protocol Violations 699 Included in Analysis 10 Protocol Violations 702 Included in Analysis 6 Protocol Violations 698 Included in Analysis 8 Protocol Violations 703 Included in Analysis ACTIVE indicates Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly. *Site-level decision was made to withdraw individual from the study. ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2807 Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 4. outcomes in this study.12,13 Memory training involved teaching mnemonic strategies (organization, visualiza-tion, association) for remembering ver-bal material (eg, word lists, texts).8,20 Reasoning training involved teaching strategies for finding the pattern in a let-ter or word series (eg, a c e g i . . . ) and identifying the next item in the se-ries. 9,21 Speed of processing training in-volved visual search and divided atten-tion (identifying an object on a computer screen at increasingly brief exposures followed by dividing atten-tion between 2 search tasks).16,22,23 Each training intervention was 10 sessions. Only 10% of the 60- to 75-minute train-ing sessions focused on applying these strategies to solving everyday prob-lems (eg, mnemonic strategies to re-member a grocery list; reasoning strat-egies to understand the pattern in a bus schedule). Booster training was conducted at 11 and 35 months after the initial train-ing sessions and involved four 75- minute sessions. The goal of the booster sessions was to maintain the improve-ment in cognitive ability and the con-tent of these sessions was similar to the training sessions, again focusing on strategies related to the cognitive abili-ties not on functional outcomes. Par-ticipants who completed the initial training were eligible for booster train-ing. 12,13 A subsample of eligible partici-pants was selected for booster training using a random number computer pro-gram. Given this selection contin-gency, participants who were selected and agreed to booster training were younger (P=.007) and had higher base-line cognitive function as evidenced by MMSE scores (P=.008) compared with participants who were eligible and not assigned to booster training and par-ticipants who were not eligible for booster training. Sixty percent of se-lected participants completed booster training at year 1 and year 3; 19% com-pleted year 1 booster only, 6% com-pleted year 3 booster only; and 15% did not complete any booster training. The last booster sessions were completed 2 years prior to the 5-year follow-up. Outcome Measures Cognitive outcomes were used to as-sess cognitive training effects and func-tional outcomes were used to assess the impact of improved cognitive abilities on instrumental functioning (see Jobe et al12 for a detailed description). Cognitive outcomes assessed the ef-fects of each intervention on the cogni-tive ability trained. Memory training out-comes involved 3 measures of verbal memory ability: Hopkins Verbal Learn-ing Test, Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test, and the Rivermead Behavioral Para-graph Recall test.24-26 Reasoning train-ing outcomes involved 3 reasoning abil-ity measures: letter series, letter sets, and word series.27-29 Speed of processing train-ing outcomes involved 3 useful field of view subscales.30-32 Functionaloutcomesassessedwhether the cognitive interventions had an effect on daily function. Everyday function-ing represented the participant’s self-ratings of difficulty(IADLdifficulty from theMinimumDataSet–HomeCare33 and ranged from “independent” to “total dependence” on a 6-point scale) in com-pleting cognitively demanding tasks involved in meal preparation, house-work, finances, health maintenance, tele-phone use, and shopping. Two perfor-mance- based categories of daily function were also assessed. Everyday problem solving34 assessed ability to reason and comprehend information in common everyday tasks (eg, identifying informa-tion in medication labels). Performance wasmeasured with printed materials (eg, yellow pages, using the Everyday Prob-lems Test34) and behavioral simulations (eg, making change, using the Observed Tasks of Daily Living35). These mea-sures were hypothesized to be most closely related to reasoning andmemory abilitiesdueto their taskdemands.Every-day speed of processing assessed partici-pants’ speed in interacting with real-world stimuli (eg, lookingupa telephone number, using the Timed IADL Test36), and the ability to react quickly to 1 of 4 road signs (Complex Reaction Time Test22), which was hypothesized to be the most closely related to speed of processing. Because most outcomes were as-sessed by multiple measures, compos-ite scores were formed by data reduc-tion. Each measure was standardized to its baseline values and then an aver-age of the equally weighted standard-ized scores was calculated. These com-posite scores permitted inferences about training effects at the level of outcome rather than at the level of a single test. The composite scores were conse-quently more reliable than single mea-sures and also reduced the number of outcome analyses needed, reducing the overall type I error rate. Analysis To evaluate the effects of ACTIVE train-ing, a repeated-measures, mixed-effects model37 was used, including all assigned participants (for both initial training and booster training) consis-tent with the intention-to-treat prin-ciple. The dependent variables were the cognitive and functional outcomes at baseline and at 5-year follow-up. The scores for each test were transformed using the Blom transformation.38,39 This transformation standardized the com-ponents in each domain to have equal weight and reduced skewness in the measures. Independent variables were restricted to basic design features: fixed effects for treatment group, time, assignment to booster training, field site, and replicate within a site. Interaction terms were cho-sen for importance and interpretability: timetraining, representing the net effect of the trial; boostertraining, representing nonspecific effects of attend-ing booster training regardless of con-tent; timeboostertraining, repre-senting training-specific effects of each booster intervention;andreplicate within a site, representing variationbetweenand within field sites. Because there were no significant baseline differences in cog-nition and function across groups, these interaction terms capture the diver-gence across groups that can be attrib-uted to the training interventions. Because participantswhowere assigned to booster training were youngerandhad higher baseline cognitive function, the COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 2808 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 5. model comparing booster and non-booster participants was then repeated controlling for baseline age and MMSE score.Themodels were fitted to the avail-able data, ignoring missing data.Todeter-mine if selective attrition influenced the trial results, missing data were imputed using multiple imputation proce-dures. 40 The analysis was repeated and there were no differences in the results; therefore, the data presented do not include imputed values. All data analy-sis was conducted by statisticians at the data coordinating center using SAS sta-tistical software version 9.2 (SAS Insti-tute Inc, Cary, NC). Hypotheses were tested by compar-ing the net effect at year 5 in each treat-ment group with the net effect in the control group. The net effect of train-ing at year 5 was defined as mean im-provement from baseline to year 5 in the intervention group minus the mean improvement from baseline to year 5 in the control group divided by the in-trasubject COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES SD of the Blom-trans-formed score. Similarly, the effect of each cognitive ability–specific booster training was defined as mean improve-ment from baseline to year 5 in the booster-trained group minus the mean improvement from baseline to year 5 in the nonbooster-trained group di-vided by the intrasubject SD of the score. Participants in the nonbooster group were those who received initial training but no booster training. Re-sults are expressed as effect sizes (ie, dif-ference in means divided by intra-subject SD) to allow direct comparison of different outcomes. Power calculations were based on the assumptions of 6 Bonferroni-cor-rected 2-sided comparisons with an overall error of .05 (.008 for each comparison), a correlation of 0.7 be-tween baseline and follow-up (based on pilot data), and completion rates of 80% at 2 years and 65% at 5 years. Follow-ing the methods of Cohen,41 the sample size of 2802 has 95% power to detect an effect size of 0.20. Based on the same assumptions, there is 90% power to de-tect booster training effects in the com-parison between the subgroup receiv-ing booster training in a training condition and the control group. In re-porting statistically significant train-ing effects, we used 99% confidence in-tervals (CIs; P=.008) to adjust for multiple comparisons. RESULTS Of 5000 individuals contacted for par-ticipation, 2832 persons were eligible, 905 (18.1%) were ineligible, and 1263 (25.3%) refused to participate (Figure 2). Compared with those who refused, those who participated were less likely to be women (76% vs 79%), were younger (mean age, 74 vs 75 years), more likely to be white (73% vs 60%), married (36% vs 27%), and bet-ter educated (mean of 13.5 vs 12.3 years). Their MMSE score was higher Table 1. Baseline Characteristics No. (%) of Participants (N = 2802)* Memory (n = 703) Reasoning (n = 699) Speed of Processing (n = 702) Control (n = 698) Age, mean (SD) [range], y 73.5 (6.0) [65-93] 73.5 (5.8) [65-91] 73.4 (5.8) [65-91] 74.1 (6.1) [65-94] Female sex 537 (76.4) 537 (76.8) 538 (76.6) 514 (73.6) Race White 524 (74.5) 504 (72.1) 523 (74.5) 503 (72.1) Black 176 (25.0) 190 (27.2) 175 (24.9) 187 (26.8) Other or unknown 3 (0.4) 5 (0.7) 4 (0.6) 8 (1.2) Years of education, mean (SD) [range] 13.6 (2.7) [5-20] 13.5 (2.7) [4-20] 13.7 (2.7) [5-20] 13.4 (2.7) [6-20] Married 257 (36.6) 249 (35.6) 242 (34.5) 259 (37.1) Mini-Mental State Examination score, mean (SD) [range] 27.3 (2.1) [23-30] 27.3 (2.0) [23-30] 27.4 (2.0) [23-30] 27.3 (2.0) [23-30] Short-Form 36 physical function score, mean (SD) [range] 69.1 (23.5) [5-100] 67.4 (24.1) [5-100] 69.7 (24.1) [0-100] 68.9 (24.6) [5-100] Alcohol consumption† Nondrinker 298 (43) 302 (43) 295 (42) 350 (51) Light drinker 341 (49) 347 (50) 362 (52) 313 (45) Heavy drinker 60 (8) 46 (7) 42 (6) 30 (4) Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score, mean (SD) [range] 5.1 (5.3) [0-36] 5.5 (5.3) [0-36] 5.2 (5.0) [0-36] 5.1 (4.9) [0-36] Disease history Hypertension 372 (53.1) 369 (53.2) 350 (50.1) 337 (48.8) Diabetes 95 (13.5) 99 (14.2) 87 (12.4) 77 (11) Transient ischemic attack or stroke 46 (6.6) 54 (7.8) 51 (7.3) 44 (6.3) Ischemic heart disease 108 (15.5) 117 (17) 94 (13.5) 102 (14.7) Congestive heart failure 30 (4.3) 44 (6.4) 27 (3.9) 37 (5.4) High cholesterol 309 (44.6) 316 (46.4) 305 (44.3) 296 (43.1) Myocardial infarction 79 (11.3) 78 (11.2) 76 (10.9) 76 (10.9) *Unless otherwise indicated. †Based on frequency of drinking alcohol and number of drinks on a typical day when drinking. ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2809 Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 6. COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES (mean 27.3 vs 26.8) and they were less likely to receive help with bathing (1% needed help vs 2%). For both groups, 1% or fewer needed help with dressing and personal hygiene. Fi-nally, those participating were less likely to have heart disease (11% vs 14%) and diabetes (13% vs 17%) but equally likely to have arthritis (57% vs 58%), stroke (0% in both groups), and cancer (6% in both groups). Thirty individuals were random-ized inappropriately in violation of the protocol and excluded from the analy-sis. Therefore, the analytic sample con-sists of 2802 randomized participants. Baseline characteristics for each of the 4 study groups appear in TABLE 1. Eighty-nine percent of participants completed treatment (8/10 training sessions). Participants completing train- Table 2. Effect of Training on Cognitive Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5 Intervention Groups Memory Reasoning Speed of Processing Control Group Memory (possible range: 0-132; n = 2790) Score at baseline, mean (SD) 81.0 (16.1) 80.7 (15.6) 80.9 (15.8) 79.4 (16.6) Mean change from baseline to year 5 −1.0 −4.8 −5.3 −4.0 Effect size (99% CI)* 0.23 (0.11 to 0.35) 0.05 (−0.07 to 0.17) 0.05 (−0.07 to 0.17) Reasoning (possible range: 0-75; n = 2802) Score at baseline, mean (SD) 25.9 (12.2) 25.2 (12.0) 25.6 (11.7) 24.5 (12.0) Mean change from baseline to year 5 4.3 8.1 4.2 5.2 Effect size (99% CI)* −0.01 (−0.10 to 0.08) 0.26 (0.17 to 0.35) 0.02 (−0.06 to 0.11) Speed of processing (possible range: 0-1500; n = 2802) Score at baseline, mean (SD) 899.0 (272.5) 904.0 (264.5) 906.8 (260.6) 920.1 (267.3) Mean change from baseline to year 5 79.1 119.6 241.8 −96.1 Effect size (99% CI)* −0.01 (−0.15 to 0.13) 0.15 (0.01 to 0.29) 0.76 (0.62 to 0.90) Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval. *Effect size defined as training improvement from baseline to year 5 minus control improvement from baseline to year 5 divided by the intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite score. Positive effect sizes indicate improvement. Table 3. Effect of Booster Training on Cognitive Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5* Intervention Groups Memory Reasoning Speed of Processing Memory (possible range: 0-132; n = 2790) Mean (SD) at baseline Booster 81.7 (15.7) 82.1 (15.2) 80.4 (16.3) Nonbooster 80.2 (16.6) 79.1 (15.8) 81.5 (15.2) Mean change from baseline to year 5 Booster −0.1 −5.0 −5.1 Nonbooster −2.2 −5.1 −5.5 Effect size (99% CI)† 0.08 (−0.14 to 0.29) 0.14 (−0.07 to 0.36) 0.05 (−0.16 to 0.26) Reasoning (possible range: 0-75; n = 2802) Mean (SD) at baseline Booster 25.8 (11.8) 25.6 (12.3) 25.4 (11.7) Nonbooster 26.0 (12.6) 24.8 (11.8) 25.9 (11.8) Mean change from baseline to year 5 Booster 4.6 8.6 4.4 Nonbooster 3.9 7.3 3.8 Effect size (99% CI)† 0.09 (−0.06 to 0.24) 0.28 (0.12 to 0.43) 0.08 (−0.07 to 0.23) Speed of processing (possible range: 0-1500; n = 2802) Mean (SD) at baseline Booster 900.7 (278.5) 895.2 (275.4) 904.5 (258.5) Nonbooster 897.1 (266.1) 914.0 (251.8) 909.3 (263.1) Mean change from baseline to year 5 Booster 82.7 115.9 308.8 Nonbooster 74.7 121.9 161.4 Effect size (99% CI)† 0.01 (−0.23 to 0.25) 0.03 (−0.21 to 0.27) 0.85 (0.61 to 1.09) Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval. *Adjusted for baseline age and Mini-Mental State Examination score. †Effect size defined as training improvement in the booster group minus improvement in the nonbooster group divided by intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite score. 2810 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 7. COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES Table 4. Effect of Training on Functional Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5 ing were younger, had more educa-tion, and had higher baseline scores for the MMSE and cognitive function tests. Sixty-seven percent of the sample was retained 5 years after training despite the advanced age of the cohort. Selec-tivity of attrition was examined by mul-tiple logistic regression modeling, con-sidering sex, education, baseline age, MMSE score, and health status (num-ber of health conditions; Short-Form 36) as well as intervention group and booster (vs nonbooster) status as pre-dictors of retention at 5 years. Partici-pants who were older, male, had less education and more health problems, and had lower cognitive function (in-dicated by lower baseline scores on the MMSE and the memory and reason-ing tests) were less likely to be re-tained at 5 years. Importantly, there were no significant interactions be-tween treatment group assignment and these covariates; that is, biases in re-tention are over the entire sample and not related to a particular treatment group, and therefore do not affect the between-group comparisons of inter-vention effects. Training Effects on Cognitive Abilities Each intervention produced immediate improvement in the cognitive ability trained13 that was retained across 5 years (TABLE 2). Similarly, when controlling Intervention Groups (N = 2802) Memory Reasoning Speed Control Group Figure 3. Training Effects on Everyday Function by Self-reported Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) Difficulty Scores 0 –0.2 Reasoning Speed for baseline age and cognitive function, booster training for the reasoning and speed of processing groups produced sig-nificantly better performance (net of ini-tial training effect) on their targeted cog-nitive outcomes13 that remained significant at 5 years (TABLE 3). Training Effects on Daily Functioning Self-Reported IADL Difficulty. At year 5, participants in all 3 intervention groups reported less difficulty com-pared with the control group in per-forming IADL (TABLE 4). However, this effect was significant only for the rea-soning group, which compared with the control group had an effect size of 0.29 (99% CI, 0.03-0.55) for difficulty in per-forming IADL. Neither speed of pro-cessing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% CI, −0.002 to 0.51) nor memory train-ing (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 0.46) had a significant effect on IADL. FIGURE 3 presents the standardized (Blom-transformed) mean IADL diffi-culty scores. Group mean IADL diffi-culty ratings improved through the first 2 years of the study (baseline through year 2). The decline in function for all groups is first evident between years 2 and 3. From years 3 to 5, the decline is 0.2 –0.4 –0.6 –0.8 Time Mean IADL Difficulty Score Memory Control Baseline (n = 2802) Year 1 (n = 2325) Year 2 (n = 2234) Year 3 (n = 2101) Year 4 Year 5 (n = 1877) The mean scores are Blom-transformed. Error bars indicate SE. The sample sizes for each time point represent the number of cases with complete data for the IADL difficulty score. IADL difficulty (possible range: 0-38) Mean (SD) at baseline 1.3 (2.3) 1.5 (2.5) 1.5 (2.5) 1.3 (2.4) Mean change from baseline to year 5 −0.7 −0.4 −0.3 −1.2 Effect size (99% CI)* 0.20 (−0.06 to 0.46) 0.29 (0.03 to 0.55) 0.26 (−0.002 to 0.51) Everyday problem solving (possible range: 0-56) Mean (SD) at baseline 36.5 (9.4) 36.4 (8.9) 36.4 (8.9) 35.6 (9.3) Mean change from baseline to year 5 1.5 1.8 1.5 2.4 Effect size (99% CI)* −0.15 (−0.28 to 0.02) −0.08 (−0.21 to 0.05) −0.05 (−0.18 to 0.07) Everyday speed of processing (possible range: −3 to 100)† Mean (SD) at baseline 4.1 (1.7) 4.1 (1.7) 4.0 (1.6) 4.2 (2.5) Mean change from baseline to year 5 0.1 0.3 0.2 0.1 Effect size (99% CI)* 0.04 (−0.09 to 0.17) 0.09 (−0.04 to 0.22) 0.08 (−0.05 to 0.21) Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living. *Effect size defined as training improvement from baseline to year 5 minus control improvement from baseline to year 5 divided by intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite score. Positive effect sizes indicate improvement. †One component of this composite score is a standardized z score with a potential range of − to . ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2811 Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 8. COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES dramatically accelerated for the con-trol group and to a lesser extent for the 3 treatment groups. Performance-Based IADL Measures. Training had no general effect on the performance-based measures of every-day problem solving or everyday speed of processing (Table 4). However, af-ter controlling for baseline age and cog-nitive function, the effect size for those participants in the speed of processing training group assigned to receive ad-ditional booster training was 0.30 (99% CI, 0.08-0.52) better for performance on everyday speed of processing compared with participants in this group not assigned to booster training (TABLE 5). COMMENT The ACTIVE study is the first large-scale, randomized trial to show that cog-nitive training improves cognitive func-tion in well-functioning older adults and that this improvement lasts up to 5 years from the beginning of the in-tervention. In addition, this is the first trial to provide limited evidence that im-provements in cognitive function can have a positive effect on daily func-tion. Participants who received cogni-tive training reported less difficulty with IADLs 5 years after training compared with those in the control group. Par-ticipants in the 3 training groups re-ported an IADL decline of at least 0.20 SD less than the participants in the con-trol group. The effect size reached sta-tistical significance only for the reason-ing training group but the effect sizes seen for memory and speed of process-ing training were similar to that for rea-soning. We consider the relative com-parability of the training effects across the 3 interventions to support the clini-cal meaningfulness of these results. Self-report of functioning repeatedly has been shown to predict loss of indepen-dence, increased use of health ser-vices, and mortality.42 Participants in the intervention groups reported simi-larly lower declines in function com-pared with participants in the control group. The finding of less decline in self-reported everyday function is concep-tually complex. Training effects could reflect real benefits of intervention because each of the 3 trained cognitive abilities has been associated with every-day functioning.22,43,44 It was not pos-sible to blind participants to their treat-ment assignment (that is, participants knewwhether they were receiving train-ing). Thus, the effects of training on self-reported IADL function could also reflect personal beliefs and self-perception related to knowing whether one has received training. While self-report bias or overestimation of func-tional abilities has been reported in indi-viduals with cognitive impairment,45,46 it is unlikely in this study because ACTIVE participants were screened for dementia and showed improved cog- Table 5. Effect of Booster Training on Functional Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5* Intervention Groups (N = 2802) Memory Reasoning Speed of Processing IADL difficulty (possible range: 0-38) Mean (SD) at baseline Booster 1.2 (2.1) 1.5 (2.7) 1.5 (2.4) Nonbooster 1.5 (2.4) 1.5 (2.3) 1.5 (2.6) Mean change from baseline to year 5 Booster −1.0 −0.2 −0.4 Nonbooster −0.4 −0.6 −0.3 Effect size (99% CI)† −0.09 (−0.49 to 0.31) 0.24 (−0.16 to 0.64) −0.29 (−0.68 to 0.11) Everyday problem solving (possible range: 0-56) Mean (SD) at baseline Booster 37.2 (9.1) 37.1 (8.9) 36.3 (8.9) Nonbooster 35.8 (9.8) 35.6 (8.9) 36.4 (8.9) Mean change from baseline to year 5 Booster 1.4 1.5 1.4 Nonbooster 1.7 1.9 1.5 Effect size (99% CI)† 0.04 (−0.20 to 0.28) 0.22 (−0.02 to 0.46) −0.02 (−0.25 to 0.21) Everyday speed of processing (possible range: −3 to 100)‡ Mean (SD) at baseline Booster 4.0 (1.6) 4.0 (1.6) 4.1 (1.8) Nonbooster 4.1 (1.9) 4.1 (1.8) 4.0 (1.5) Mean change from baseline to year 5 Booster 0.2 0.3 0.3 Nonbooster 0 0.3 0.1 Effect size (99% CI)† −0.008 (−0.23 to 0.22) 0.08 (−0.14 to 0.31) 0.30 (0.08 to 0.52) Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living. *Adjusted for baseline age and Mini-Mental State Examination score. †Effect size defined as training improvement in the booster group minus improvement in the nonbooster group divided by intrasubject SD of the Blom-transformed composite score. ‡One component of this composite score is a standardized z score with a potential range of − to . 2812 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 9. nitive function in their trained ability over the study period compared with the control group. These results sug-gest that for self-reported IADLs, a pla-cebo control group that would facili-tate blinding and additional analyses of the mechanisms of training-related improvement (eg, whether training-related improvements in self-efficacy mediated the self-rated functional improvements) will be important future directions for this kind of research. There is growing consensus that as-sessment of daily function requires both self-report and performance-based mea-sures. 47 The ACTIVE study is one of the few large-scale intervention studies to include both self-report and perfor-mance- based outcome measures of function to assess the breadth of inter-vention effects. Consistent with the study’s conceptual model, we hypoth-esized that certain interventions would specifically affect 1 or more functional outcomes. This was observed for the performance-based functional mea-sures. When controlling for baseline age and cognitive function, booster train-ing for speed of processing showed a significant effect on the performance-based everyday speed of processing measure. This specific effect is consis-tent with prior research that indicated everyday speed of processing was more closely related to speed of process-ing9,13 than to reasoning or memory. The fact that this effect occurred only in participants who received booster training, and not in those who re-ceived only initial training, may re-flect a need for larger doses of training before effects can be observed in this more cognitively demanding out-come. Across all outcomes, evidence for transfer of the effects of cognitive train-ing to function was modest and was not observed until the 5-year follow-up. There are 2 possible explanations for these delayed outcomes. First, prior re-search has suggested a temporal lag be-tween onset of cognitive decline and subsequent impact on daily func-tion, 2-4 perhaps because of resulting dif-ficulty in adapting to emerging physi-cal COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES limitations that affect tasks of daily living.48,49 However, if cognitive abil-ity is maintained or enhanced by train-ing, then this could result in a gradual emergence of adaptive, compensatory strategies for dealing with physical limi-tations. 1 Second, delayed intervention effects on function may be attribut-able to the advantaged nature of the ACTIVE sample, and particularly the subset who received booster training. To select older adults most likely to benefit from cognitive training, per-sons with suspected cognitive and ADL decline were excluded from enroll-ment. This may have delayed the on-set of functional disabilities in the con-trol group until the 5-year follow-up. Only after the onset of decline in the control group could the positive train-ing effects on function be observed in the intervention groups. We are not aware of intervention trials with volunteer samples of older adults that include 5-year follow-up for direct comparison of retention rates. The 67% retention rate at 5 years is con-sistent with other longitudinal com-munity- based studies resembling ACTIVE in terms of sample age and eth-nicity and frequency and duration of study contact (eg, 70.6% in a 4-year study of blacks and whites in Chi-cago50; 61.6% in a 5-year study of blacks in Indianapolis51; and 59.6% in a 7-year study of Hispanics, whites, and blacks in New York City52). However, be-cause these studies were not interven-tion trials, they did not have the same level of respondent burden as ACTIVE. Therefore, while the direct compara-bility of retention rates to other inter-vention trials cannot be made, the rates of these observational studies sug-gests that the retention rate in ACTIVE is quite acceptable given the burden-some nature of the study protocol. In conclusion, declines in cognitive abilities have been shown to lead to in-creased risk of functional disabilities that are primary risk factors for loss of independence. The 5-year results of the ACTIVE study provide limited evi-dence that cognitive interventions can reduce age-related decline in self-reported IADLs that are the precur-sors of dependence in basic ADLs as-sociated with increased use of hospital, outpatient, home health, and nursing home services,53,54 and health care ex-penditures. 55 However, given the lag in the relationship between cognitive de-cline and functional deficits, the full ex-tent of the interventional effects on daily function would take longer than 5 years to observe in a population that was highly functioning at enrollment. We consider these results promising and support future research to examine if these and other cognitive interven-tions can prevent or delay functional disability in an aging population. Author Affiliations: Department of Human Develop-ment and Family Studies, Pennsylvania State Univer-sity, State College (Dr Willis); New England Research Institutes, Watertown, Mass (Drs Tennstedt, Stod-dard, and Wright); Institute on Aging and Depart-ment of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville (Dr Marsiske); Department of Psy-chology, University of Alabama, Birmingham (Dr Ball); National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Md (Dr Elias); National Institute of Nursing Research, Bethesda, Md (Dr Koepke); Hebrew Senior Life, Boston, Mass (Dr Morris); Department of Mental Health, Johns Hop-kins University, Baltimore, Md (Dr Rebok); and De-partment of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis (Dr Unverzagt). Author Contributions: Drs Wright and Stoddard had full access to all of the data in the study and take re-sponsibility for the integrity of the data and the ac-curacy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Wright. Acquisition of data: Willis, Marsiske, Ball, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. Analysis and interpretation of data: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Elias, Koepke, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Stoddard, Wright. Drafting of the manuscript: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Elias, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. Critical revision of the manuscript for important in-tellectual content: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Koepke, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Stoddard, Wright. Statistical analysis: Marsiske, Morris, Stoddard, Wright. Obtained funding: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. Administrative, technical, or material support: Willis, Marsiske, Ball, Elias, Koepke, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt. Study supervision: Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Morris, Rebok, Unverzagt, Wright. Financial Disclosures: Dr Ball owns stock in Visual Awareness Inc, which owns the patent for the Useful Field of View testing and training software. No other authors reported disclosures. Funding/Support: ACTIVE is supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging and the National In-stitute of Nursing Research to Hebrew Senior Life (U01 NR04507), Indiana University School of Medicine (U01 NR04508), Johns Hopkins University (U01AG14260), New England Research Institutes (U01 AG14282), Pennsylvania State University (U01 AG14263), the Uni-versity of Alabama at Birmingham (U01 AG14289), and the University of Florida (U01AG14276). Role of the Sponsor: Representatives of the National Institute on Aging and the National Institute for Nurs-ing Research were directly involved in the design of ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 2813 Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006
  • 10. the study, interpretation of the data, and prepara-tion, review, and approval of the manuscript. These representatives also monitored the conduct of the study, collection, management, and analysis of the data. ACTIVE Study Investigators: Richard N. Jones, ScD, Adrienne L. Rosenberg,MS(Hebrew Senior Life); Kathy Johnson, PhD, Daniel F. Rexroth, PsyD, David M. Smith, MD, Elizabeth Way, BA, Fredric D. Wolinsky, PhD (Indiana University School of Medicine); Kay Cresci, PhD, RN, Joseph Gallo, MD, MPH, Laura Tal-bot, PhD, EdD, RN, CS ( Johns Hopkins University); Michael Doherty, MS, Patricia Forde, BS, Yan Xu, MS (New England Research Institutes, data coordinating center); Pamela Davis, MS, Scott Hofer, PhD, K. Warner Schaie, PhD (Pennsylvania State University); Jerri Ed-wards, PhD, Martha Frankel, Cynthia Owsley, PhD, Dan Roenker, PhD, David Vance, PhD, Virginia Wad-ley, PhD (University of Alabama at Birmingham); Man-fred K. Diehl, PhD, Ann L. Horgas, RN, PhD, FAAN, Peter A. Lichtenberg, PhD, ABPP (University of Florida/ Wayne State University). Acknowledgment: We thank Robin Barr, PhD, of the National Institute on Aging for his authorship of the re-quest for applications and his continuing support and helpful comments throughout the conduct of the project. Dr Barr was not compensated for this assistance. REFERENCES 1. Kelly-Hayes M, Jette AM, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB, Odell PM. Functional limitations and disability among elders in the Framingham Study. Am J Public Health. 1992;82:841-845. 2. Stuck AE, Walthert JM, Nikolaus T, Bula CJ, Ho-hmann C, Beck JC. Risk factors for functional status decline in community-living elderly people: a system-atic literature review. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:445-469. 3. Wolinsky FD, Callahan CM, Fitzgerald JF, Johnson RJ. Changes in functional status and the risks of sub-sequent nursing home placement and death. J Ger-ontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1993;48:S94-S101. 4. Willis S. Societal Mechanisms for Maintaining Com-petence in Old Age. New York, NY: Springer Publish-ing; 1997. 5. Royall DR, Palmer R, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: the free-dom house study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11: 899-909. 6. Davis RN, Massman PJ, Doody RS. Cognitive in-tervention in Alzheimer disease: a randomized placebo-controlled study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2001; 15:1-9. 7. Cherry KE, Simmons-D’Gerolamo SS. Long-term effectiveness of spaced-retrieval memory training for older adults with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Exp Aging Res. 2005;31:261-289. 8. Rebok GW, Balcerak LJ. Memory self-efficacy and performance differences in young and old adults: ef-fects of mnemonic training. Dev Psychol. 1989;25:714- 721. 9. Willis SL. Cognitive training and everyday competence. Annu Rev Gerontol Geriatr. 1987;7:159- 188. 10. O’Hara R, Brooks JO III, Friedman L, Schroder CM, Morgan KS, Kraemer HC. Long-term effects of mnemonic training in community-dwelling older adults [published online ahead of print June 14, 2006]. J Psy-chiatr Res. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.04.010. 11. Derwinger A, Stigsdotter Neely A, MacDonald S, Backman L. Forgetting numbers in old age: strat-egy and learning speed matter. Gerontology. 2005;51: 277-284. 12. Jobe JB, Smith DM, Ball K, et al. ACTIVE: a cog-nitive intervention trial to promote independence in older adults. Control Clin Trials. 2001;22:453-479. 13. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, et al. Effects of cog-nitive training interventions with older adults: a ran-domized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271-2281. 14. Lazaridis EN, Rudberg MA, Furner SE, Cassel CK. Do activities of daily living have a hierarchical struc-ture? an analysis using the longitudinal study of aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1994;49:M47-M51. 15. Wolinsky F, Miller D. Disability concepts and mea-surement: contributions of the epidemiology of dis-ability to gerontological inquiry. In: Wilmoth J, Fer-raro K, eds. Gerontology: Perspectives and Issues. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Springer Publishing; 2006. 16. Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK, Wadley VG, Ed-wards JD. Speed-of-processing and driving simulator training result in improved driving performance. Hum Factors. 2003;45:218-233. 17. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini- Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cog-nitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189-198. 18. Clark F, Azen SP, Zemke R, et al. Occupational therapy for independent-living older adults: a ran-domized controlled trial. JAMA. 1997;278:1321-1326. 19. Willis SL, Cornelius SW, Blow FC, Baltes PB. Train-ing research in aging: attentional processes. J Educ Psychol. 1983;75:257-270. 20. Rasmusson D, Rebok G, Bylsma F, Brandt J. Ef-fects of three types of memory training in normal elderly. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 1999;6:56-66. 21. Willis SL, Schaie KW. Training the elderly on the ability factors of spatial orientation and inductive reasoning. Psychol Aging. 1986;1:239-247. 22. Ball K. Increased mobility and reducing acci-dents of older drivers. In: Schaie K, Pietrucha M, eds. Mobility and Transportation in the Elderly. Vol 5. New York, NY: Springer; 2000:213-250. 23. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Myers RS, Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK. Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology. 2002;48:329-340. 24. Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: de-velopment of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5:125-142. 25. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathie tramatique. Arch Psychol. 1941;28: 286-340. 26. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Baddeley A. The River-mead Behavioral Memory Test. Reading, England and Gaylord, Mich: Thames Valley Test Co and National Rehabilitation Services; 1985. 27. Gonda J, Schaie K. Schaie-Thurstone Mental Abili-ties Test: Word Series Test. Palo Alto, Calif: Consult-ing Psychologists Press; 1985. 28. Thurstone L, Thurstone T. Examiner Manual for the SRA Primary Mental Abilities Test (Form 10-14). Chicago, Ill: Science Research Associates; 1949. 29. Ekstrom R, French J, Harman H, Derman D. Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Revised ed. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service; 1976. 30. Owsley C, Ball K, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni JR. Visual/cognitive correlates of vehicle accidents in older drivers. Psychol Aging. 1991;6:403-415. 31. Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G Jr, et al. Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. JAMA. 1998;279:1083- 1088. 32. Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs DS. Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5:2210-2219. 33. Teresi J, Lawton M, Holmes D, Ory M. Measure-ment in elderly chronic care populations. In: Morris J, Morris S, eds. ADL Assessment Measures for Use With Frail Elders. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co; 1997. 34. Willis S, Marsiske M. Manual for the Everyday Problems Test. University Park: Pennsylvania State Uni-versity; 1993. 35. Diehl M, Marsiske M, Horgas AL, Rosenberg A, Saczynski J, Willis SL. The Revised Observed Tasks of Daily Living: a performance-based assessment of ev-eryday problem solving in older adults. J Appl Gerontol. 2005;24:211-230. 36. Owsley C, Sloane M, McGwin G Jr, Ball K. Timed instrumental activities of daily living tasks: relation-ship to cognitive function and everyday performance assessments in older adults. Gerontology. 2002;48:254- 265. 37. Cnaan A, Laird NM, Slasor P. Using the general linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal data. Stat Med. 1997;16: 2349-2380. 38. Blom G. Statistical Estimates and Transformed Beta-Variables. New York, NY: Wiley; 1958. 39. Lehmann E. Nonparametrics: Statistical Meth-ods Based on Ranks. San Francisco, Calif: Holden- Day; 1975. 40. Schafer J. Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data. London, England: Chapman Hall; 1997. 41. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Be-havioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Er-lbaum Assoc; 1988. 42. Schupf N, Tang MX, Albert SM, et al. Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderly. Neurology. 2005;65: 1218-1226. 43. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Well- and ill-defined mea-sures of everyday cognition: relationship to older adults’ intellectual ability and functional status. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:101-115. 44. Willis SL. Everyday cognitive competence in el-derly persons: conceptual issues and empirical findings. Gerontologist. 1996;36:595-601. 45. Wadley VG, Harrell LE, Marson DC. Self- and in-formant report of financial abilities in patients with Alz-heimer’s disease: reliable and valid? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1621-1626. 46. Koltai DC, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Schmechel DE. In-fluence of anosognosia on treatment outcome among dementia patients. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2001;11: 455-475. 47. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Everyday cognition: age and intellectual ability correlates. Psychol Aging. 1999; 14:627-644. 48. Wolinsky FD, Callahan CM, Fitzgerald JF, Johnson RJ. The risk of nursing home placement and subse-quent death among older adults. J Gerontol. 1992;47: S173-S182. 49. Boyle PA, Paul RH, Moser DJ, Cohen RA. Execu-tive impairments predict functional declines in vascu-lar dementia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18:75-82. 50. Evans DA, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, et al. Inci-dence of Alzheimer disease in a biracial urban com-munity: relation to apolipoprotein E allele status. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:185-189. 51. Hendrie HC, Ogunniyi A, Hall KS, et al. Inci-dence of dementia and Alzheimer disease in 2 com-munities: Yoruba residing in Ibadan, Nigeria, and Af-rican Americans residing in Indianapolis, Indiana. JAMA. 2001;285:739-747. 52. Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, et al. Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology. 2001; 56:49-56. 53. Fried TR, Bradley EH, Williams CS, Tinetti ME. Func-tional disability and health care expenditures for older persons. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:2602-2607. 54. Plehn K, Marcopulos BA, McLain CA. The rela-tionship between neuropsychological test perfor-mance, social functioning, and instrumental activities of daily living in a sample of rural older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18:101-113. 55. Kane R, Kane R, Ladd R. The Heart of Long- Term Care. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998. COGNITIVE TRAINING ON EVERDAY FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES 2814 JAMA, December 20, 2006—Vol 296, No. 23 (Reprinted) ©2006 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. Downloaded from www.jama.com on December 20, 2006