SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 217
Implicit Theories of Relationships: Orientations Toward
Evaluation and Cultivation
C. Raymond Knee, Heather Patrick, and Cynthia Lonsbary
Department of Psychology
University of Houston
Implicit theories of relationships (ITRs) influence goals,
motivations, attributions,
and behavior in romantic relationships. We developed a model
of ITRs that draws
from social cognition, motivation, and achievement literatures,
and derived concep-
tual parallels and hypotheses with regard to relationships. It is
proposed that ITRs re-
flect the belief component of a larger system of motivations and
goals that can influ-
ence the degree to which people are oriented toward the
evaluation and cultivation of
relationships. Research on ITRs is reviewed with regard to how
they moderate
well-documented associations between relationship perceptions
and outcomes. Dif-
ferences between ITRs and implicit theories in other domains
are also discussed.
Individuals have different beliefs about what makes
for a good relationship. These beliefs or implicit theo-
ries determine, in part, one’s goals and motivations in
relationships (Knee, 1998; Knee, Nanayakkara, Vietor,
Neighbors, & Patrick, 2001), much as implicit theories
in other domains have been shown to determine goals
and motivations in those contexts (see Dweck, Chiu, &
Hong, 1995, for review). Implicit theories of relation-
ships (ITRs), as defined here, are characterized by a
belief in romantic destiny and a belief in relationship
growth. Destiny belief is defined as the belief that po-
tential relationship partners are either compatible or
they are not. Growth belief is defined as the belief that
relationship challenges can be overcome. Theo-
retically, those who believe more (relative to less)
strongly in destiny attempt to determine the compati-
bility of their partner and the viability of the relation-
ship based on minimal information. They place a high
value on determining whether a relationship is meant
to be, and tend to diagnose the potential of the relation-
ship based on specific events. Those who believe more
(relative to less) strongly in growth are primarily inter-
ested in developing the relationship, and believe that
relationships grow not despite obstacles but rather be-
cause of them. Destiny belief is linked to attempts to
diagnose the status and potential success of the rela-
tionship, and growth belief is linked to attempts to
maintain the relationship.
Implicit Theories of Attributes
Implicit theories were first studied with regard to
traits such as personality, intelligence, and morality
(Dweck, 1996; Dweck et al., 1995; Dweck, Hong, &
Chiu, 1993). Research on ITRs grew out of Dweck and
colleagues’ research on implicit theories in these other
domains. Research has shown that implicit theories are
relatively domain-specific such that the particular the-
ories within a given domain (e.g., intelligence) most
strongly predict behavior within that domain (e.g., giv-
ing up on a challenging test). Within Dweck’s theoreti-
cal framework, a belief that certain traits are fixed and
unchangeable is called an entity theory, and a belief
that these traits are flexible and can be changed with ef-
fort is called an incremental theory (Dweck & Leggett,
1988). Implicit theories have been conceptualized as
schematic knowledge structures that involve specific
beliefs about the stability of an attribute and those con-
ditions that are likely to promote change (Ross, 1989).
Historically, these beliefs have been examined in the
achievement and social judgment literatures yielding a
number of provocative findings.
Most of the work on implicit theories has centered
on implicit theories of intelligence. This body of re-
search has suggested that an entity belief about intelli -
gence fosters concern about ability. These concerns are
then manifested in dispositional inferences about abil -
ity following feedback, even in the face of limited or
contradictory information. For example, those who
hold an entity theory of intelligence seem to be particu-
larly sensitive to early indications of potential failure,
thus leading them to give up on challenging achieve-
ment tasks (Dweck, 1996; Dweck et al., 1995; Dweck
et al., 1993; Hong, Chiu, & Dweck, 1995). In this do-
main, an entity theory (e.g., “You have a certain
41
Personality and Social Psychology Review
2003, Vol. 7, No. 1, 41–55
Copyright © 2003 by
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
This research was supported in part by grant 1R03MH61805
from the National Institute of Mental Health.
We thank the Interpersonal Relations and Motivation Research
Group at the University of Houston for help at various stages of
this ar-
ticle. Linda K. Acitelli provided helpful comments on an earlier
draft.
Requests for reprints should be sent to C. Raymond Knee, De-
partment of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston,
TX 77204–5341. E-mail: [email protected]
amount of intelligence and you can’t do much to
change it”) is linked to robust, global dispositional in-
ferences about intelligence whereas an incremental
theory predicts fewer dispositional and more provi-
sional inferences (Dweck, 1991; Dweck et al., 1993).
In addition, an entity belief about personality has been
associated with making global trait inferences from
brief samples of behavior, perceiving behavior as sta-
ble, and showing an increased likelihood to blame and
punish others for undesirable behaviors (Erdley &
Dweck, 1993). With regard to relationships, implicit
theories of personality have been shown to moderate
the correlation between individuals’ views of their ro-
mantic partner and how satisfied they are in the rela-
tionship (Ruvolo & Rotondo, 1998). View of partner
was defined as rating the partner on 24 personal char-
acteristics. Results showed that the correlation be-
tween having a favorable view of one’s partner and be-
ing satisfied in the relationship was weaker with an
entity theory of personality.
From Attributes to Relationships
Implicit theories in other areas have been shown to
be domain specific, and judgments in one domain are
best predicted by implicit theories of attributes in the
same domain (Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997; Dweck
et al., 1993). Thus, Dweck and her colleagues have
developed separate scales to identify people’s implicit
theories about intelligence, personality, and moral
character (Dweck et al., 1995). Implicit theories set
up different goals and orient individuals to focus on
different factors for explaining performance. An en-
tity theory is associated with performance goals and a
focus on gaining favorable judgments and avoiding
negative ones. Individuals holding an entity belief
about intelligence focus more on fixed abilities rather
than malleable aspects of the trait, leading them to
explain negative performance more in terms of lack
of ability rather than lack of effort. Thus, these indi -
viduals seem to be more prone to helpless responses
in the face of failure (Diener & Dweck, 1980; Goetz
& Dweck, 1980). Individuals with an incremental
theory tend to be more mastery-oriented when faced
with failure and tend to search for ways to improve
ability and performance through increased effort and
engaging in remedial actions such as help-seeking
(Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999). An incre-
mental theory of intelligence orients an individual to-
ward learning goals and increasing ability. Thus, im-
plicit theories set up a general frame of reference
from which to evaluate performance, abilities, and
traits. ITRs grew out of Dweck and colleagues’ work
on implicit theories in these other domains. Thus, im-
plicit theories in the relationships domain are thought
to reflect similar orientations toward challenging rela-
tionship events and failures as implicit theories in
other domains (Knee et al., 2001).
With regard to relationships, those who believe
more strongly in romantic destiny tend to be especially
sensitive to signs that indicate that the relationship is
not meant to be. Indeed, Knee (1998) found that rela-
tionship survival was more strongly linked to initial
satisfaction for those who believed more strongly in
destiny. When those who believed more strongly in
destiny initially felt more satisfied, their relationships
lasted particularly long, whereas when they initially
felt less satisfied, their relationships ended quickly. A
stronger belief in destiny is associated with a more
judgmental approach to relationships and a focus on
fixed aspects of the relationship or the partner. Thus,
when problems arise, they are more likely to view the
problem as a sign that the relationship is not meant to
be. Indeed, destiny belief has already been associated
with disengaging from the relationship when there was
a problem (Knee, 1998).
Those who more strongly endorse a growth belief
about relationships tend to believe that successful rela-
tionships are developed by conquering obstacles and
growing closer. Knee (1998) found that belief in
growth was associated with fewer one-night stands
during the first month of college and with dating a par-
ticular person for a longer period of time. Moreover,
belief in growth was associated with attempts to main-
tain the relationship when there was a problem through
a variety of coping strategies.
Measurement of ITRs
ITRs were originally measured with eight items ad-
ministered on a 7-point Likert-type scale (Knee, 1998).
Four items measured destiny belief and four items
measured growth belief. For example, a destiny item is
the following: “Potential partners are either compatible
or they are not.” A growth item is the following: “Chal-
lenges and obstacles in a relationship can make love
even stronger.” Factor analyses in several samples re-
vealed that destiny and growth beliefs were independ-
ent. When items reflecting each belief were averaged,
destiny and growth were uncorrelated (r = –.01). Later
confirmatory factor analyses using a 15-item measure
also revealed a clear two-factor structure in which des-
tiny and growth beliefs were independent. This
two-factor model fit the data better than a single-factor
model (Knee et al., 2001). Finally, the most recent
22-item version of the ITR contains 11 destiny and 11
growth items and is stable across a variety of diverse
samples. Table 1 provides all 22 items along with
item-total correlations. Internal reliabilities for destiny
and growth were .82 and .74, respectively, in a sample
of 400 participants. Scores on destiny and growth are
uncorrelated (rs typically range from –.01 to –.07, de-
42
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
pending on the sample). Confirmatory factor analysis
of the 22-item ITR specifying a single factor solution
in which all destiny and growth items were forced to
load on a single dimension, did not fit the data well, as
before, χ2 (209, N = 436) = 1229.47, p < .001, Good-
ness of Fit Index (GFI) = .72, Root Mean Square Error
of Approximation (RMSEA) = .11. Instead, as we have
shown elsewhere, specifying independent destiny and
growth factors provides a better fit, χ2 (209, N = 436) =
707.29, p < .001, GFI = .86, RMSEA = .07. Further, al-
lowing the factors to correlate does not significantly
improve the fit, χ2 (208, N = 436) = 705.12, p < .001,
GFI = .86, RMSEA = .07.
Destiny belief is correlated positively with the be-
lief that partners cannot change themselves or their re-
lationship (Partners Cannot Change; Eidelson & Ep-
stein, 1982), as well as a pragmatic shopping-list
approach to love (Pragma; Hendrick & Hendrick,
1986). Growth belief is correlated negatively with the
belief that partners cannot change and positively with a
gradual, friendship-based approach to love (Storge;
Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986). With regard to the Big
Five dimensions of personality, destiny belief is associ-
ated with slightly less openness, and slightly more
extraversion and neuroticism. Growth belief is associ -
ated with more conscientiousness and somewhat more
agreeableness (Knee, 1998). ITRs are associated with
Sprecher and Metts’s (1989) romantic beliefs
subscales such that destiny is associated with belief
that there is only one potential partner, idealizing ro-
mance, and belief in love at first sight, whereas growth
is associated with the belief that love will find a way.
With regard to Druen’s (1996) partner selection strate-
gies, destiny belief is associated with searching for an
ideal partner and believing that love conquers all, and
growth belief is associated with working on relation-
ships. ITRs are also associated with Rusbult’s
exit-voice-loyalty-neglect typology of responses to
dissatisfaction in relationships (see Rusbult,
Zembrodt, & Gunn, 1982). Specifically, growth belief
is correlated with voicing concerns about the relation-
ship, and being loyal to the relationship; destiny belief
is correlated with neglecting the relationship. With re-
gard to attachment dimensions, growth belief is mod-
estly correlated with attachment security, but other-
wise ITRs are not significantly correlated with the
attachment dimensions of security, ambivalence, or
avoidance. Finally, because destiny belief reflects the
notion that a relationship’s potential can be diagnosed,
it is modestly associated with personal need for struc-
ture (Neuberg & Newsom, 1993) which captures the
desire to seek certainty and know what one can expect
from situations.
Further, ITRs are moderately correlated with im-
plicit theories in other domains, although these other
domains tap primarily the destiny component. For ex-
ample, destiny belief is moderately correlated with
having stronger entity theories of personality, morality,
and intelligence. Growth belief is only slightly and
nonsignificantly correlated with implicit theories in
43
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
TABLE 1. Items on the 22-Item Implicit Theories of
Relationships Scale Along With Item-Total Correlations
Destiny Belief Items
.45–1. Potential relationship partners are either compatible or
they are not.
.45–3. A successful relationship is mostly a matter of finding a
compatible partner right from the start.
.48–5. Potential relationship partners are either destined to get
along or they are not.
.54–7. Relationships that do not start off well inevitably fail.
.36–9. If a potential relationship is not meant to be, it wil l
become apparent very soon.
.59–11. The success of a potential relationship is destined from
the very beginning.
.61–13. To last, a relationship must seem right from the start.
.56–15. A relationship that does not get off to a perfect start
will never work.
.55–17. Struggles at the beginning of a relationship are a sure
sign that the relationship will fail.
.34–19. Unsuccessful relationships were never meant to be.
.53–21. Early troubles in a relationship signify a poor match
between partners.
Growth Belief Items
.41–2. The ideal relationship develops gradually over time.
.49–4. A successful relationship evolves through hard work and
resolution of incompatibilities.
.39–6. A successful relationship is mostly a matter of learning
to resolve conflicts with a partner.
.42–8. Challenges and obstacles in a relationship can make love
even stronger.
.42–10. Problems in a relationship can bring partners closer
together.
.37–12. Relationships often fail because people do not try hard
enough.
.26–14. With enough effort, almost any relationship can work.
.48–16. It takes a lot of time and effort to cultivate a good
relationship.
.29–18. Without conflict from time to time, relationships cannot
improve.
.41–20. Arguments often enable a relationship to improve.
.35–22. Successful relationships require regular maintenance.
Note: Destiny and growth items are typically alternated when
administered. Destiny items are averaged to yield a destiny
score. Growth items are
averaged to yield a growth score. The resulting scores are
independent (r = –.01).
these other domains (rs < .09). Destiny and growth be-
liefs are typically not significantly correlated with sex,
age, whether one is currently in a relationship, length
of relationship, number of previous intimate relation-
ships, current relationship satisfaction, social desir-
ability, or self-esteem.
The Independence of Destiny and
Growth Beliefs
One important difference between implicit theories
in other domains and ITRs is that the former are tradi-
tionally assessed as opposing ends of a single contin-
uum whereas the latter have been found repeatedly to
comprise independent dimensions (Franiuk, Cohen, &
Pomerantz, 2001; Knee, 1998; Knee et al., 2001). This
difference is as much a conceptual issue as it is an issue
of measurement. In the domain of relationships, it is
somewhat desirable to believe both that potential rela-
tionships can be diagnosed and that relationships re-
quire maintenance. Thus, it is conceivable to hold both
beliefs simultaneously.
In contrast, in other domains, attempts to measure
entity and incremental beliefs as independent dimen-
sions have been inconclusive because it is more so-
cially desirable to endorse an incremental belief (e.g.,
that one can improve one’s intelligence; Dweck et al.,
1995). In Dweck and colleagues’ framework, endorse-
ment of an incremental theory is reflected by rejection
of an entity theory. Several studies have indicated that
even for those who endorse an entity belief, there is a
strong tendency to endorse items indicative of an incre-
mental theory, suggesting that incremental items are
highly compelling and more socially desirable (see
Dweck et al., 1995, for review). Thus, items depicting
an incremental theory are not used in most of those
studies. In contrast, ITRs are characterized by inde-
pendent destiny and growth beliefs such that individu-
als can endorse both or neither, as well as the typical
extreme combinations (higher on one and lower on the
other). This affords a more complex theoretical frame-
work in which the dimensions of relationship mainte-
nance (growth) and relationship diagnosis (destiny)
can be examined jointly, and seems to better capture
lay people’s beliefs about relationships. It is fairly
common for participants to believe that, “Fate brings
potential partners together, but once the relationship
blooms, they must work hard to maintain it.” The inde-
pendence of destiny and growth beliefs also suggests
that, in the domain of relationships, a growth belief is
not necessarily more socially desirable than a destiny
belief, because participants vary in their endorsement
of each dimension. This may be partly due to the fact
that both destiny and growth beliefs have a positive
connotation, and both beliefs are positively correlated
with traditional scales of romanticism.
Interestingly, the independence of destiny and
growth beliefs suggests that each dimension can con-
tribute uniquely to predicting and explaining relation-
ship perceptions, cognitions, and behaviors. However,
it also suggests that particular combinations, or inter-
active effects, of destiny and growth may be especially
relevant. For example, those with the combination of a
higher growth belief and a lower destiny belief (culti-
vation orientation) believe that relationships evolve
through development, confrontation, and efforts to
maintain and improve the relationship, and are less fo-
cused on diagnosing the potential of relationships.
When cultivating, one’s goal becomes the development
and maintenance of the relationship without diagnos-
ing or inferring grand meaning from otherwise minor
incompatibilities. Those with the combination of a
higher destiny belief and a lower growth belief (evalua-
tion orientation) believe that relationships can be easily
diagnosed and evaluated, but that they cannot be con-
siderably improved. When evaluating, one’s goal be-
comes the diagnosis of one’s partner and relationship
in an effort to determine whether the relationship
would seem to have immediate promise, without at-
tempting to improve the relationship.
In data analytic terms, this implies that although
destiny and growth beliefs can be meaningful on their
own (as main effects), they can also interact with each
other in predicting or moderating relationship phe-
nomena. Indeed, we often find the interaction of des-
tiny and growth beliefs to be particularly interesting.
We focus on the interactive extremes of destiny and
growth beliefs in terms of evaluation and cultivation
orientations for two reasons. First, these orientations
reflect the clearest conceptual extremes and afford the
most useful hypotheses. Second, research thus far has
shown that the interactive effects of ITRs are often
most clear when the effect of one belief is particularly
strong (or weak) depending on the level of the other be-
lief. For example, cultivation orientation tends to
buffer negative consequences and evaluation orienta-
tion tends to exacerbate them (Knee et al., 2001; Knee,
Patrick, Vietor, & Neighbors, 2002).
ITRs as Moderators of
Perceptions and Outcomes
Although ITRs can have direct effects on inferences,
they can also moderate the consequences of those infer-
ences. For example, perceiving limitations in one’s part-
ner or the relationship can have different consequences
depending on one’s ITRs. Because ITRs, in part, deter-
mine how meaning will be ascribed to relationship-rele-
vant events and partner qualities, the same events or
qualities can take on different meaning with corre-
spondingly different relationship implications. For ex-
ample, considerable research has shown that idealistic
44
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
views of one’s partner are associated with increased re-
lationship satisfaction (Murray, Holmes, & Griffin,
1996). However, this tendency to feel more satisfied
when one views one’s partner more favorably than one’s
partner views himself or herself may depend on one’s
ITRs. Indeed, in two studies, Knee et al. (2001) found
that ITRs moderate the relation between wanting more
in one’s partner and feeling satisfied in the relationship.
Study 1 defined “wanting more” in terms of perceiving a
discrepancy between what one wants in an ideal partner
and what one believes he or she has in a current partner.
Further, this discrepancy was measured in both direct
and less-direct ways. Across several indexes of discrep-
ancy from an ideal, wanting more in one’s partner was
consistently and strongly related to feeling less happy
with the relationship. However, this relation was moder-
ated by destiny and growth beliefs such that “wanting
more” was less strongly linked to one’s satisfaction
when one had a cultivation orientation (higher growth
and lower destiny beliefs). With a cultivation orienta-
tion, people were able to acknowledge their partner’s
less positive attributes and still remain relatively satis-
fied. In this way, these individuals did not view their
partner’s imperfections as fatal flaws in the relationship.
Study 2 tested whether ITRs moderate the projected
illusions hypothesis (Murray et al., 1996). It was found
that viewing one’s partner more favorably than one’s
partner views himself or herself was generally linked
to feeling more satisfied with the relationship. This re-
lation was moderated by ITRs such that the link be-
tween viewing one’s partner favorably and feeling sat-
isfied was weaker among those who believed more
strongly in growth, without regard to destiny belief.
The moderating effects of ITRs are not limited to
perceptions of one’s partner. For example, it is plausi -
ble that conflict in relationships could either bring part-
ners closer or force them apart, again depending on
one’s ITRs. When one holds a belief that conflict is a
healthy part of relationships and can bring partners
closer by resolving it, then differences and disagree-
ments can take on different meaning than when one be-
lieves that conflict is a sign of insurmountable prob-
lems in the relationship. This notion of increased
commitment as a function of relationship adversity is
not new (see Lydon, 1999). However, for some part-
ners, the very presence of differences may be enough
to strain the relationship, whereas others view it as an
opportunity to learn more about their partner and de-
velop the relationship. A recent series of studies was
designed to test this hypothesis (Knee et al., 2002). In
Study 1, 128 individuals in romantic relationships kept
event-contingent diaries, recording every disagree-
ment they and their partner had over a 10-day period.
“Disagreement” was broadly defined as “anytime it be-
comes evident to you that you and your partner dis-
agree on an opinion, perspective, idea, goal, etc.” Rela-
tionship quality was assessed after each disagreement
as well. Multilevel analyses showed that, consistent
with the hypothesis, the association between having
longer conflicts and reporting reduced relationship
quality afterward was generally strong, except among
those who were both higher in growth and lower in des-
tiny. Among these individuals who were oriented to-
ward cultivation, there was virtually no relation be-
tween the length of disagreements and reduced
relationship quality on a daily basis.
In Study 2, 75 dating couples discussed problems in
their relationship, with commitment measured before
and after the discussion. Multilevel analyses examined
changes in commitment. As hypothesized, an orienta-
tion toward cultivation was associated with less de-
crease in commitment after discussing problems with
one’s partner. Further, because those with stronger
growth beliefs are particularly motivated to improve
the relationship when it is lacking, the relation between
growth belief and commitment was stronger when one
had a less favorable view of one’s partner. Thus, an ori -
entation toward cultivation seems to buffer the negative
consequences of adverse relationship experiences.
Taken together, it appears that an orientation toward
cultivation, and growth belief in particular, can buffer
the negative impact of arguments, discrepancies, and
differences of opinion—events that normally are asso-
ciated with a decline in satisfaction and commitment.
The moderating effects of ITRs found thus far may
reflect both cognitive and motivational mechanisms.
The cognitive mechanism stems from the fact that a
cultivation orientation is characterized by a belief that
problems can be overcome and that the relationship’s
potential is not readily diagnosed. Perhaps the part-
ner’s negative qualities recede into the background, or
perhaps partners come to authentically understand and
appreciate each other’s faults and weaknesses. The
motivational mechanism underlying cultivation may
allow one to feel less threatened by conflict and dis-
crepant qualities between oneself and one’s partner,
given that cultivation involves viewing such attributes
as latent and emergent rather than immediately evi-
dent. They may be motivated to maintain and improve
the relationship, and acknowledging limitations and
weaknesses would seem to be a key asset in this pro-
cess. ITRs thus reflect both cognitive and motivational
processes that can influence one’s relationship-rele-
vant perceptions, emotions, and behavior in meaning-
ful ways.
From Orientations to
Behavioral Outcomes
Levy, Plaks, and Dweck (1999) viewed implicit
theories broadly as two modes of social thought. One
mode is organized around static traits, leading one to
seek trait information, view traits as causes of behav-
45
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
ior, draw trait-centered inferences, and categorize
people by traits. The other mode is organized around
more dynamic psychological mediators, leading one
to consider people’s goals, needs, and mental states.
We view evaluation and cultivation orientations as
different modes of thought as well. One mode is based
on a set of beliefs that relationships are relatively
static, rigid, and diagnosable with little opportunity
for improvement. The other mode is based on the be-
liefs that relationships are dynamic and can be culti-
vated and developed, and their potential not readily
diagnosed. These orientations come to influence rela-
tionship judgments, attributions, and inferences in
much the same manner as they do in other domains.
Research on implicit theories in other domains has
shown that implicit theories can influence attributions,
emotions, and behavior at both trait and state levels
(see Dweck et al., 1995, for review). In particular, im-
plicit theories of intelligence were recently linked to
attributions and coping strategies in three studies
(Hong et al., 1999). This research revealed that implicit
theories can be measured overtly but can also be in-
duced situationally to influence attributions about
one’s performance and willingness to seek and accept
help. Whether measured or induced, those who be-
lieved that intelligence could be improved made stron-
ger attributions to effort and were more likely to take
remedial action when performance was unsatisfactory
(Hong et al., 1999). We propose that a similar process
occurs with evaluation and cultivation orientations in
which both overtly measured beliefs as well as induced
orientations toward relationships can influence attribu-
tions for relationship events and willingness to seek
help in response to relationship problems. Thus, al-
though ITRs have been shown to be relatively stable in-
dividual differences over time, they are also somew hat
open to experience, as are most relationship knowledge
structures and schema. For example, Knee (1998)
found that the test–retest reliability of destiny belief
was lower whereas that of growth was nonsignificantly
higher when one had recently experienced a traumatic
relationship event. We believe that evaluation and
cultivation orientations, of which ITRs are perhaps a
more stable component, can be induced momentarily
much as Hong and colleagues (1999) demonstrated in
the domain of intelligence.
Figure 1 illustrates how ITRs can be viewed as
part of a larger framework that distinguishes between
cultivation and evaluation orientations toward rela-
tionships. ITRs are but one component of these orien-
46
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
Figure 1. Conceptual model of how evaluation and cultivation
orientations may influence attributions and behavioral
outcomes.
tations. Based on Dweck and colleagues’ (1995) re-
search along with our own research on relationships,
we view a cultivation orientation as containi ng three
components: (a) a stable belief component comprised
of higher growth and lower destiny beliefs, (b) a mo-
tivational component that reflects a desire to develop
the relationship for its own sake, and (c) a goal com-
ponent in which one intends to master relationship
problems, interpret feedback informatively and
nondefensively, and become interdependent with the
partner. An evaluation orientation contains the fol-
lowing three components: (a) a stable belief compo-
nent comprised of higher destiny and lower growth
beliefs, (b) a motivational component that reflects an
outcome-driven desire to evaluate and diagnose the
relationship’s potential, and (c) a goal component in
which one intends to judge the partner and determine
the viability of the relationship. Each of these compo-
nents has been established in the social cognition and
motivation literatures, but not necessarily with regard
to relationships.
An important outcome of evaluation and cultiva-
tion orientations includes the emotional reactions en-
gendered by viewing conflict as something that can
or cannot be overcome. Indeed, Knee et al. (2001)
found that ITRs predicted affective reactions on
learning that one’s partner views the relationship dif-
ferently. First, being higher in growth belief (without
regard to destiny belief) predicted greater happiness
and less depression after discussing discrepant views
of the relationship. Thus, the value of believing that
relationship limitations can be overcome should not
be understated. Indeed, the benefits of growth belief
became even more impressive when considered in
combination with destiny belief. Specifically, those
oriented toward cultivation felt increased positivity
whereas those oriented toward evaluation felt in-
creased hostility after discussing discrepant views of
the relationship with their partner. When oriented to-
ward cultivation, limitations and weaknesses become
opportunities for closeness and understanding with-
out diagnosis of the relationship’s potential. When
oriented toward evaluation, even small differences
can take on strong meaning, possibly leading to
stronger, more judgmental inferences about one’s
partner in combination with feelings that the differ-
ences are insurmountable. Indeed, using a slightly
different method of measuring ITRs, Franiuk et al.
(2001) recently found that feeling one’s specific part-
ner was ideal predicted relationship satisfaction and
longevity to a greater extent for those with stronger
destiny beliefs.
Motivation
The motivation behind a cultivation orientation is
thought to be largely intrinsic and improve-
ment-driven, with an emphasis on mastery and flexi-
bility in reacting to and dealing with relationship
challenges. Similar motives have been examined in
the achievement and motivation literatures, albeit
with regard to different outcomes. For example, Deci
and Ryan (1985, 1991) have elaborated on the nature
and process of growth motivation from the perspec-
tive of self-determination theory. This other literature
has shown how an orientation toward growth and
mastery can positively influence academic achieve-
ment, mental and physical health, as well as interper-
sonal behavior (e.g., Hodgins, Koestner, & Duncan,
1996; Koestner & Zuckerman, 1994; Williams, Grow,
Freedman, Ryan, & Deci, 1996). A motivation orien-
tation toward growth and improvement has also been
examined with regard to the self (Deci & Ryan, 1987,
1991). In particular, such studies have linked an ori-
entation toward growth and improvement with fewer
self-enhancement strategies including the self-serving
bias, self-handicapping, and defensive coping (Knee
& Zuckerman, 1996, 1998).
Recently, Knee, Patrick, Vietor, Nanayakkara, and
Neighbors (2002) examined self-determination as
growth motivation in relationships. Growth motiva-
tion was defined as an orientation toward improve-
ment, choicefulness, and authenticity with regard to
oneself and others and was based on Deci and Ryan’s
conceptualization of self-determination (Deci &
Ryan, 1985). As self-determination theory would pre-
dict, an orientation toward growth and improvement
was associated with (a) less tendency to view an ideal
partner as a function of one’s view of self, (b) more
active and integrative coping strategies, (c) less denial
and fewer avoidance strategies, and (d) decreases in
negative emotion, along with more positive interac-
tion behaviors when discussing contrary relationship
perceptions with one’s partner. These findings are
consistent with previous research regarding self-de-
termination and interpersonal outcomes (Blais,
Sabourin, Boucher, & Vallerand, 1990; Hodgins et
al., 1996). Moreover, these results suggested the im-
portant ways in which a general motivation toward
growth in relationships is beneficial by allowing peo-
ple to approach threats as challenges, adversity as op-
portunity, and conflict as the potential for new appre-
ciation and understanding.
Recent data indicate that ITRs are substantially
and meaningfully correlated with motivation orienta-
tions. For example, growth belief is associated with
an orientation toward autonomy and feeling intrinsi-
cally motivated to be in the relationship, and destiny
belief is associated with an orientation toward feeling
controlled and being extrinsically motivated to be in
the relationship (Patrick & Knee, 2002). Extrinsic re-
lationship motivation involves being in the relation-
ship for reasons other than the spontaneous feelings
and pleasures of the relationship (e.g., rewards, ap-
47
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
proval from others). Because destiny belief centers on
diagnosing a relationship’s potential, it is especially
important for those higher in destiny belief to associ -
ate their relationship with positive experiences. These
positive experiences and other relationship rewards
confirm that the relationship is viable and meant to
be. In addition, extrinsic relationship motivation in-
cludes feedback and approval from other people (e.g.,
family and friends). This feedback provides addi-
tional diagnostic information about the potential of
the relationship, and thus is important to those higher
in destiny belief.
Along these lines, a recent study suggests that
ITRs moderate the link between relationship motiva-
tion and relationship quality (Patrick & Knee, 2002).
Specifically, we found that being higher in growth be-
lief (without regard to destiny belief) buffers the neg-
ative consequences of having extrinsic relationship
motivation. When people believe that relationships
can be improved, feeling obligated to the relationship
does not seem so bad. When one feels stuck in the re-
lationship, discussing problems with one’s partner
maintains commitment if one believes the relation-
ship can be improved. In addition, being higher in
destiny belief exacerbates the negative consequences
of having extrinsic relationship motivation. Spe-
cifically, when one is extrinsically motivated to be in
the relationship, a stronger destiny belief (without re-
gard to growth belief) is linked to feeling less com-
mitted after discussing relationship problems.
Another way in which motivation affects relation-
ships is in openness to experiences within the rela-
tionship. One might predict that cultivation would be
associated with acknowledging challenges to the rela-
tionship and attempting to deal with them openly and
directly, rather than denying that they exist or giving
up and abandoning the relationship altogether
(Hodgins & Knee, in press; Knee et al., 2001). It is
important to note that the motivation behind evalua-
tion and cultivation orientations may not be all that
different. In both cases, people will want to learn
more about the nature of the relationship. It is the un-
derlying goals that may best distinguish between
evaluation and cultivation orientations. When ori-
ented toward evaluation, one attempts to diagnose the
future potential of the relationship, whereas when ori-
ented toward cultivation, one attempts to improve the
relationship. It could be argued that by improving the
relationship, one is improving and expanding the self,
and thus the motivation behind a cultivation orienta-
tion may reflect a desire for self-expansion as well
(Aron & Aron, 1997). Indeed, ITRs have been corre-
lated with inclusion of other in the self. Specifically,
growth belief (and not destiny belief) is associated
with greater self-expansion when operationalized as
Aron and colleagues’ (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992)
Inclusion of Other in the Self scale.
Attributions
In Dweck and colleagues’ early work, attributions
played a key role in how implicit theories were thought
to influence coping with challenges (Dweck & Leggett,
1988). That earlier work presumed that implicit theories
set up goals which in turn set up attributions and re-
sponses. However, more recent research has suggested
that implicit theories are more consistently predictive of
attributions and responses than are the corresponding
goal orientations (Hong et al., 1999). We think that eval -
uation and cultivation orientations may influence attri-
butions in relationships in a manner similar to implicit
theories in other domains. Specifically, those who are
oriented to evaluate relationships should make stronger
attributions to stable causes, and weaker attributions to
effort and controllability for negative relationship
events. This pattern of attributions follows from an eval-
uation orientation for two reasons. First, the higher des-
tiny belief component predisposes people to diagnose
relationship potential, and stable attributions may facili -
tate such diagnoses. Second, the lower growth belief
component of evaluation orientation involves not be-
lieving that things can be changed, which inherently in-
volves attributions about effort and control over the
course of the relationship.
Those with a cultivation orientation should make
stronger attributions to effort and controllability, and
weaker attributions to stability as causes for negative
relationship events. First, the higher growth belief
component deals with believing that relationships can
be improved, lending itself to effort and control attribu-
tions. Second, the lower destiny belief component
deals with not believing that the relationship’s poten-
tial can be readily diagnosed, and thus suggests a lack
of stability in the course of the relationship.
Dispositional Inferences
Chiu, Hong, and Dweck (1997) examined the rela-
tion between implicit theories of personality and lay
dispositionism. Lay dispositionism refers to people’s
tendency to use traits as the basic unit of analysis in so-
cial perception. In Study 1, results suggested that those
with an entity theory tended to expect trait-relevant be-
havior to be consistent across time and situations.
Those with an incremental theory did not predict the
same relative likelihood of a person displaying the
same kind of behavior in different situations. In Study
2, participants with an entity belief about personality
were more likely to predict the target’s future behavior
in a particular situation based on information about the
target person’s traits. Study 3 examined the likelihood
of those with an entity theory confidently inferring
traits from a particular behavior. Findings from this
study further suggested that, for those who hold an en-
tity theory of personality, dispositional traits mediate
48
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
behaviors, leading to a belief that behaviors are consis-
tent across situations.
With regard to ITRs and inferences in relationships,
we would expect that an orientation toward evaluation
would promote stronger trait inferences about one’s ro-
mantic partner and the relationship, using relatively
limited samples of behavior. For example, when ori-
ented toward evaluation, one may come to view brief
samples of behavior as strongly indicative of the fu-
ture, and thus come to believe that a relationship is de-
sirable and promising or undesirable and hopeless. An
orientation toward cultivation would promote less ex-
treme inferences about the partner and relationship, or
at least more positive expectations about working
through potential problems. For example, if Jane were
oriented toward evaluation, she may be more sensitive
to negative aspects of the partner and relationship, and
interpret the occasional argument as an indication that
the relationship will always be hindered by disagree-
ments that cannot be resolved. However, if Jane were
oriented toward cultivation, she may view the same se-
ries of occasional arguments as opportunities to learn
about each other and become closer without inferring
grand conclusions about the relationship’s potential.
Thus, the perceived meaning and consequences of the
same event can be rather different depending on one’s
orientation, beliefs, and motivation at the time that it
occurs. Indeed, Dweck and colleagues have shown that
the belief that attributes are fixed in a particular domain
predicts global dispositional inferences for self and
other, even in the face of limited evidence. In addition,
the belief in fixed attributes leads to an over-reliance on
dispositional information in making judgments and de-
cisions. The belief that attributes are flexible predicts
inferences that are more specific, conditional, and pro-
visional (Dweck et al., 1993).
Reactions to Feedback
Implicit theories in the achievement literature have
traditionally been linked with reactions to negative feed-
back on tasks related to the domain of the particular the-
ory. For example, people who believe in the fixed nature
of intelligence tend to give up and abandon difficult
math problems following negative feedback, presum-
ably because their static view of ability leads to stable at-
tributions for performance (Dweck et al., 1995). It
would seem that romantic relationships, too, tend to be
replete with feedback, often of the unsolicited variety.
Although the nature of feedback in everyday romantic
relationships is not as clear and unambiguous as manip-
ulated feedback in experimental studies on intelligence,
evaluative information regarding the partner’s perfor-
mance (broadly defined), or the relationship in general,
is quite common. Feedback in the proposed model of re-
lationships could be broadly conceived as any
evaluative information or cue that may be interpreted as
implying something about the ability of one’s partner or
the relationship to continue satisfactorily.
Probably the most common example of feedback
within the relationship is when one partner raises con-
cerns about the relationship, ranging from relatively
minor issues (“We don’t do enough fun things to-
gether”) to potentially more serious matters (“You
don’t understand me”). If oriented toward evaluation,
feedback about the relationship can take on stronger
meaning, with more serious implications about the fu-
ture success of the relationship, in part because they do
not believe that such difficulties can be overcome. Re-
gardless of how serious the issue may appear on the
surface, an orientation toward cultivation would pro-
mote less stable inferences about the relationship, and
an improvement-driven perspective, because of the be-
liefs that problems can be resolved and a relationship’s
potential cannot be easily diagnosed. So, when one’s
partner claims “You don’t understand me,” one who is
evaluation-oriented may infer that understanding will
never be possible, and that this diagnosis is not condu-
cive to romantic bliss. One who is cultivation-oriented,
however, may infer that now is the time to better at-
tempt to understand one’s partner, and that with
enough effort and time, understanding will come.
Feedback about the relationship can also originate
from one’s friends and relatives. Others may provide
potentially evaluative information such as, “You two
seem perfect together.” When oriented toward evalua-
tion, this feedback can take on strong meaning along
with inferences about the future stability of the rela-
tionship. When oriented toward cultivation, this same
feedback may seem relatively meaningless as people
are not thought to be “matched for each other” to begin
with, and because relationships are thought to develop
and change along the way such that today’s mismatch
may be tomorrow’s synchrony. It is important to note
that it is not the nature of the feedback per se that is rel -
evant to how ITRs influence inferences, reactions, and
behavior, but rather it is the meaning that is assigned to
the feedback as a function of the ITRs and orientations.
The same feedback can take on rather different mean-
ing, suggesting different relationship implications, de-
pending on what one believes about relationships and
how one is oriented.
Reactions to Negative
Partner Behavior
Implicit theories of morality have also been shown to
predict the extent to which people seek out and utilize
character information when making decisions about
one’s guilt or innocence (Gervey, Chiu, Hong, &
Dweck, 1999). Specifically, in three studies, when de-
fendant respectability was manipulated, information
about the defendant’s appearance, clothing, and life-
style affected entity theorists’ character inferences and
49
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
verdicts more than those of incremental theorists. Fur-
ther, when given the opportunity to request additional
information about the defendant that they thought
would be relevant to the verdict, entity theorists were
more likely to seek dispositional information. Compar-
atively, incremental theorists did not consider informa-
tion such as the style of clothing, or character of the de-
fendant in making their judgments (Gervey et al., 1999).
In another series of studies, entity theorists were found
to believe more strongly in duty-based behavior than
rights-based behavior, and thus tend to view violations
of existing codes of conduct and expectations as less ac-
ceptable, and are less tolerant of deviance (Chiu,
Dweck, Tong, & Fu, 1997). This same research found
that entity theorists of morality are also more likely to
impose punishment or issue directives to transgressors.
More generally, when people are asked how they would
deal with another person’s disreputable actions or trans-
gressions, entity theorists tend to propose punishment
and retaliation whereas incremental theorists tend to
propose education and reform (Dweck, 1996). Individ-
uals who believe in fixed traits tend to more readily infer
global traits from limited, concrete behaviors compared
to those who believe that traits can change.
Negative behavior is all too common in many ro-
mantic relationships. Based on the proposed frame-
work, evaluation and cultivation orientations may in-
fluence how one interprets and assigns meaning to
such events. Specifically, an evaluation orientation
may be associated with stronger character inferences
from relatively limited samples of behavior. Thus, a
partner’s “one-night stand” may take on far greater
meaning, with the assumption that the behavior re-
flects something fundamental about the nature of the
partner and one’s relationship. Having higher destiny
and lower growth beliefs could make even a single
event seem indicative of a deeper truth and a repeat-
able pattern. Consequently, the evaluation-oriented
partner may make strong and stable negative attribu-
tions to the partner and the relationship, bringing to
mind “once a cheater, always a cheater.” This could
be a rather different inference compared to a partner
who was oriented toward cultivation. Cultivation ori-
entation could lead one to seek mediators of the be-
havior, with less emphasis on judgment and more em-
phasis on remediation and improvement. For
example, in the case of a single instance of infidelity,
someone with a cultivation orientation may seek out
information about the circumstances that may have
facilitated unfaithful behavior (e.g., decline in close-
ness). Moreover, a cultivative orientation may moti-
vate the thwarted partner to examine weaknesses in
the relationship that could be addressed to prevent in-
fidelity in the future. A cultivation orientation may
make one more motivated to understand the condi-
tions that surrounded the negative behavior, and seek
a supportive, less judgmental method of remediation.
In a preliminary study of the role of ITRs in re-
sponses to romantic infidelity, 149 participants were
randomly assigned to read one of four scenarios about
infidelity which varied according to relationship
prognosis (i.e., good or poor) and transgressor of infi -
delity (i.e., self or partner; Patrick, Knee, & Lonsbary,
2001). After reading the scenario, participants an-
swered two open-ended items designed to capture their
anticipated responses to such a situation. Results re-
vealed that what people believe about relationships has
a strong effect on how they respond to even hypotheti-
cal relationship threats. Those who were oriented to-
ward evaluation and in the good prognosis condition
were more likely to blame their partner, regardless of
the transgressor of infidelity. In addition, an orientation
toward evaluation was associated with less blame of a
problem in the relationship, even when participants
had been told “you aren’t sure if [this partner] is the
right one for you.” Taken together, these findings seem
to suggest that an evaluation orientation may some-
times involve evaluating the partner rather than the re-
lationship, and also that the meaning of such sin-
gle-event instances can take on relatively different
meaning depending on one’s ITRs.
Seeking Remediation
Implicit theories of intelligence have been linked to
remedial action following unsatisfactory performance
(Hong et al., 1999). Specifically, incremental theorists
were more willing than entity theorists to take a reme-
dial course that could potentially improve their perfor -
mance following negative feedback. ITRs and their
corresponding goal orientations may analogously in-
fluence one’s willingness to seek help when faced with
relationship difficulties. Seeking help may seem rela-
tively pointless when one is oriented toward evaluation
because one interprets relationship difficulties as evi-
dence that this is not the right partner, and believes that
such difficulties cannot be overcome. On the other
hand, seeking help may seem particularly useful when
oriented toward cultivation because one believes that
the future of the relationship is not predetermined and
that relationship challenges can be overcome. Thus, re-
lationship counseling, whether through professional
services or friends and family, may seem more poten-
tially fruitful when operating according to certain be-
liefs and their corresponding goal orientations. Again,
it is not the actual utility of counseling or remedial help
that is important here, but rather the way in which one’s
beliefs and orientations guide expectations and infer-
ences about the likelihood of improvement in the face
of problems. In this way, seeking help for a dying rela-
tionship may not always yield improved results, but re-
gardless, one should be more likely to invite assistance
when oriented toward cultivation.
50
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
It is not clear whether a cultivation orientation
would lead one to predominantly infer that one can
tackle relationship problems oneself or through the
help of others, but in extreme cases, seeking help from
others may seem most useful to those with a cultivation
orientation. An evaluation orientation, rather than
leading to the seeking of help, may lead to the seeking
of additional evaluation from others. Specifically,
feedback may seem particularly important when ori-
ented toward evaluation, not because it can guide one
on how to improve and develop the relationship, but
because it can sharpen one’s evaluation of the future
potential of the relationship by making the diagnosis of
problems (and hence the relationship’s future) that
much more evident.
Developmental Origins
ITRs probably have a variety of origins, some of
which have been described elsewhere. For example,
ITRs may develop vicariously from watching parents
engage in their own relationships (Bandura, Ross, &
Ross, 1963). If a child never saw his or her parents
quarreling, then the child may feel that conflict is not
a usual occurrence in a relationship, and therefore,
when it does occur it may signal that something is
very wrong. On the other hand, a child who sees his
or her parents disagree and come to some resolution
may come to feel less threatened by conflict because
they have seen first hand that it is resolvable. Some-
times, however, parents fight and then divorce. What
effect might this have on the developing relationship
theories of the child? One can envision at least three
possible outcomes. First, the child may develop a fa-
talistic view of relationships such that, no matter
what one does, relationships are bound to bring you
sadness and pain. This may be especially the case if
the parents fought often and parted badly. A second
outcome may result from parents who try to salvage a
deteriorating relationship out of genuine care for each
other. However, despite their efforts there are certain
irreconcilable differences. If these parents part well
and have successful remarriages, the child may de-
cide that relationships can be successful, but only if
the partners are well matched to begin with. A third
possible outcome may result from watching parents
fight and divorce if the child develops a reactance be-
lief. The child may see the difficulties and pain that
the parents experienced, and become determined that
he or she will not experience the same. This child
may energetically seek to find “good matches” and
then work to maintain or improve them.
Research on implicit theories in other domains has
suggested additional origins. Benenson and Dweck
(1986) found that trait explanations for behaviors
emerged in the social domain earlier than in the aca-
demic domain. Their reasoning was that children
experience the social domain before the academic do-
main. This rationale fits well with the notion that im-
plicit theories of intelligence can evolve from rela-
tionships with one’s parents and other caregivers. For
example, Jane is playing with her friend Susie, who is
acting very unpleasantly, and Jane runs to her mother
crying about how Susie was treating her. There are at
least two reactions that Jane’s mother could offer.
One that might reinforce stable, trait-based explana-
tions for behavior could involve the following: “Susie
is a bad girl for behaving that way; maybe you
shouldn’t play with her.” In contrast, one that might
reinforce unstable, situation-based explanations for
behavior could involve the following: “Why don’t
you ask Susie why she acted that way? Maybe she
was just having a bad day.” The first response might
indicate to Jane that people’s actions are a reflection
of their underlying personalities and that they are
likely to remain the same over time. Therefore, Jane
may come to infer that if a person behaves unpleas-
antly, it reflects something fundamental and un-
changeable such that future interactions would also
be unpleasant. Consequently, Jane may steer clear of
forming relationships with people who seem unpleas-
ant, even from a single occasion. In contrast, the
mother’s second response might give Jane the impres-
sion that people’s behavior can be influenced by a
particular situation and that they do not necessarily
act that way all the time. Therefore, a relationship
with someone could be rewarding if one takes the
time to learn why the person was behaving that way.
A similar process may be at work with regard to ITRs.
Now imagine that Jane is dating Tom. Tom calls to can-
cel a date at the last minute. Jane’s mother could respond
in a way that reinforces the evaluative notion that “He is-
n’t good enough for you” or “You can do better than
him.” She could also respond in a way that reinforces the
cultivative notion that “I’m sure if you ask Tom to ex-
plain why he cancelled it, the two of you can get past
this.” The modeling of evaluative and cultivative orien-
tations is almost certainly not limited to parents, but
rather carries over from friends, siblings, role models,
and other sources of feedback and modeling that are im-
portant to children and adolescents.
Also, Dweck (1998) suggested that the origins of
mastery and helpless responses could result, in part,
from parental responses to failures. Specifically, chil -
dren with a mastery response (and its corresponding in-
cremental belief) may have had experiences with par-
ents who encouraged them to try again after failure,
perhaps offering strategies for doing so and praising the
efforts they made. Children with a helpless response
(and its corresponding entity belief) may have had expe-
riences with parents who only praised success and were
critical of failed attempts, applying the criticism not just
to the attempt itself, but to the child as well. Indeed, in six
51
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
studies, Mueller and Dweck (1998) showed that praise
for success had more negative consequences for stu-
dents’ achievement motivation than praise for effort.
Children who were praised for intelligence cared more
about performance goals relative to learning goals com-
pared to children who were praised for effort. These
children also displayed less task persistence, less task
enjoyment, more attributions to low ability, and worse
task performance than children who were praised for ef-
fort. Further, it seems likely that praise for good perfor-
mance may lead to viewing intelligence as a fixed trait.
Specifically, children who were praised for intelligence
described it as a fixed trait more than children who were
praised for hard work, who believed intelligence to be
subject to improvement.
In romantic relationships, failure may involve get-
ting turned down when asking for a date, or getting
into an argument with one’s significant other. The pa-
rental (and peer) response to these and other relation-
ship “milestones” may set the stage for the notion
that relationships are either destined to work or they
are not, and that with enough effort a relationship
with almost anyone can be nurtured. Parents’ reaction
on bringing home one’s first date may be one such
example. In some cases, parents might proceed to im-
mediately evaluate their child’s romantic interest, at-
tempting to diagnose the character of the person and
the long-term potential for the incipient relationship.
In other cases, parents might afford support and en-
couragement of almost any new date, and virtually
unconditional support of the evolving relationship
(despite what the cat dragged in). These reactions
may become internalized by the child across repeated
salient episodes such that evaluation or cultivation
goals become typical of how they view and interact
with potential romantic partners. Thus, they may
come to gradually diagnose and evaluate potential
partners, attempting to proceed as they feel their par-
ents and friends (and others whose opinions they
value) would. Alternatively, if parents and peers have
largely modeled cultivation goals and unconditionally
supportive behavior with regard to relationship part-
ners, the child may gradually become less concerned
by what some people would consider grave weak-
nesses or deficits in potential dates.
Revision of ITRs Based on Experience
Like many other knowledge structures about rela-
tionships (e.g., working models, relational schemas,
and scripts), ITRs are probably susceptible to revision
as a function of salient relationship experiences. For
example, research on working models of attachment
has shown that involvement in satisfying relationships
at one point in time is associated with increased secu-
rity at a later time (Hammond & Fletcher, 1991). Simi-
larly, relationship breakups have been shown to be as-
sociated with change from secure to insecure working
models, and avoidant models are susceptible to becom-
ing less avoidant on formation of a new relationship
(Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994). Changes in relationship
knowledge structures have been found to be particu-
larly likely when significant events in the social envi -
ronment disconfirm existing expectations (Feeney,
1999; Feeney, Noller, & Callan, 1994). In a similar
manner, ITRs are also thought to both guide the inter-
pretation of relational experiences as well as be some-
what sensitive to particularly salient relational events.
In this way, one’s belief in destiny could be strength-
ened through involvement in a relationship in which
everything seems wonderful from the beginning and a
positive future seems evident, or through involvement
in a relationship that never seemed right from the be-
ginning and indeed ended up dissolving. Conversely,
one’s destiny belief could be weakened if one inaccu-
rately diagnoses a relationship as ideal and yet it fails.
One’s growth belief could analogously be strengthened
through times in which problems were discussed and
resolved and the relationship benefited. Growth belief
could be weakened through times when no amount of
effort seemed to help. As mentioned earlier, in support
of the notion that ITRs are somewhat sensitive to ex-
treme or salient relationship experiences, Knee (1998)
found lower test–retest correlations for ITRs when par-
ticipants had reported a relationship event as their sin-
gle most stressful experience over a semester.
It is also possible that ITRs can change somewhat
within the same relationship. For example, someone
who initially has a lower growth belief may become in-
volved in a relationship in which they are able to wit-
ness the benefits of confronting and resolving issues,
thus coming to believe more strongly in growth. In ad-
dition, someone who is initially lower in destiny belief
may come to believe that their partner is truly “the one”
for them, particularly if they remain very satisfied with
the relationship. Indeed, one’s partner’s beliefs and be-
haviors in the relationship may be a major factor in the
extent to which ITRs evolve in the context of any par-
ticular relationship. Moreover, it is not yet clear how
destiny belief functions in longer-term committed rela-
tionships. Although the destiny items seem to be espe-
cially relevant to the early stages of relationships, it
seems likely that the diagnostic process continues to be
active, particularly in the presence of unexpected rela-
tionship events, both positive and negative. However,
these processes remain to be documented empirically.
Domain Specificity
Implicit theories in other areas have been shown to
be domain specific, and judgments in one domain are
best predicted by implicit theories of attributes in the
52
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
same domain (Chiu et al., 1997; Dweck et al., 1993).
Thus, although there is considerable conceptual over-
lap between ITRs and implicit theories of other attrib-
utes, we would expect that ITRs tend to better predict
relationship phenomena. A key difference between im-
plicit theories of attributes and ITRs is that the latter
are explicitly relational whereas the former are largely
individual. Beliefs about relationships may indeed in-
fluence (and be influenced by) the relation between
partners, including how they view each other and what
they come to infer about the relationship as a function
of relevant events. In contrast, beliefs about individual
attributes probably more often influence one’s own
goals, attributions, and behaviors. It may be premature
to conclude that beliefs about relationships tend to be
more complex than beliefs about individual attributes.
However, the thoughts, emotions, and behaviors of
people in close relationships tend to be interdependent,
such that they mutually influence each other. In addi-
tion, previous research has demonstrated that people
develop rather elaborate schema about both partners
and relationships (Fletcher & Thomas, 1996; Murray
& Holmes, 1993). This additional complexity of rela-
tionships may require a more vast assessment of the
particular beliefs, goals, and attributions that are rele-
vant to specific behaviors, and during a specific inter-
action sequence.
We believe that there may be an additional level of
domain-specificity within the realm of relationships.
Whereas people may have a global set of beliefs about
what makes for good relationships, there may also be
particular aspects of relationships that they believe are
especially fixed or malleable. For example, Jane may
believe that, generally, successful relationships are nur-
tured and that they develop through the process of dis-
cussing differences and resolving controversy. How-
ever, she may also believe that if there is no initial
chemistry or physical attraction between partners, the
relationship is not meant to be. Thus, if she feels physi -
cally attracted to her partner, Jane may believe that all
other problems in the relationship can be managed and
dealt with. Indeed, previous research has shown that in-
dividuals view certain factors as crucial to relationship
success including trust, respect, sex, and love (Fletcher
& Kininmonth, 1992). Because individuals tend to
value particular characteristics in an ideal mate
(Fletcher, Simpson, Thomas, & Giles, 1999), they may
also have implicit beliefs about the fixedness or mallea-
bility of these traits. The importance of particular char -
acteristics has not yet been examined with regard to
ITRs. People may come to value something because it
comes easily (e.g., you either have a good sex life or you
do not). People may also come to value those things that
they have had to work the hardest to attain (e.g., although
my partner and I didn’t hit it off sexually, we have
worked to improve that aspect and we are now closer).
More important, there may be an interaction between
importance ratings of a characteristic and ITRs about
the same characteristic in predicting reactions to nega-
tive relationship events (such as disagreements) that
concern that dimension. For example, if Jane believes
that trust in romantic relationships is very important and
she also believes that trust is relatively fixed (e.g., part-
ners either trust each other or they do not), then Jane may
be particularly troubled by a disagreement with her part-
ner that concerns or implies a lack of trust. If Jane be-
lieves that trust is important but also malleable (e.g.,
when trust has been violated, it can be restored with ef-
fort), then a disagreement about trust may not be as trou-
blesome. Thus, a more specific measure of ITRs may
provide a more sophisticated model that takes into ac-
count specific aspects of relationships.
It is also possible that ITRs of specific relationship
aspects would mediate the association between general
ITRs and various relationship phenomena. The litera-
ture on attitudes has shown that more specific attitudes
are better predictors of specific behaviors (Ajzen &
Fishbein, 1980). Research on social cognition in close
relationships has revealed a similar pattern of findings,
albeit with regard to different variables. For example,
Fletcher and Fitness (1990) identified ways in which re-
lationship cognitions could serve as both proximal and
distal variables in predicting various relationship out-
comes. In their study, distal cognition involved the
thoughts and feelings that partners experienced during
an interaction with each other. Proximal cognition in-
volved the words used in the interactions that reflected
thoughts and beliefs (and not emotions). Fletcher and
Fitness (1990) found that couples who had more posi-
tive assessments of the quality of their relationship (dis-
tal) produced more positive cognitions (proximal) dur-
ing interactions with each other. ITRs may operate in a
similar manner such that more global ITRs are related to
ITRs about specific aspects of relationships, which in
turn predict or moderate relationship phenomena.
Conclusion
ITRs can be a useful framework for conceptualizing
how particular beliefs and orientations can influence
peoples’ goals, inferences, attributions, emotions, and
ultimately, behaviors in romantic relationships. In a re-
view of research areas and topics on relationships that
need further exploration and development, Aron and
Aron (1995) cited (a) an examination of deep, passion-
ate relational experiences; (b) the development of a the-
ory that links cognitive, motivational, and emotional el-
ements to behaviors and outcomes; and (c) a theoretical
framework that examines not only how individual phe-
nomena influence relational experiences, but how rela-
tional experiences can influence the individual.
Research on ITRs has begun to address passionate
relational experiences including feelings of love, ideal-
53
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
ization, anger, and coping with infidelity. The ITRs’
framework also begins to integrate cognition, motiva-
tion, and emotional responses within the relationship.
Finally, as mentioned earlier, ITRs may also be subject
to situational influences. Thus, ITRs may be somewhat
malleable and may change as a function of salient rela-
tionship experiences. Although in its incipient stages,
the ITRs’ framework affords a unique perspective on
how particular social cognitions and motivations may
operate together to influence how people approach,
perceive, and negotiate their romantic lives. Some em-
pirical support already exists, but far more remains to
be done.
References
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and
pre-
dicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Three suggestions for increased
em-
phasis in the study of personal relationships. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 12, 559–562.
Aron, A., & Aron, E. (1997). Self-expansion motivation and
includ-
ing other in the self. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal re-
lationships (2nd ed., pp. 251–270). New York: Wiley.
Aron, A., Aron, E., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in
the
self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612.
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-
medi-
ated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy-
chology, 66, 3–11.
Benenson, J. F., & Dweck, C. S. (1986). The development of
trait ex-
planations and self-evaluations in the academic and social do-
mains. Child Development, 57, 1179–1187.
Blais, M. R., Sabourin, S., Boucher, C., & Vallerand, R. J.
(1990).
Toward a motivational model of couple happiness. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1021–1031.
Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Tong, Y., & Fu, H. (1997). Implicit
theories
and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 923–940.
Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism
and
implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 73, 19–30.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-de-
termination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy
and the
control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol -
ogy, 53, 1024–1037.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to
self:
Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation, 1990 (pp. 237–288). Lincoln: Uni-
versity of Nebraska Press.
Diener, E. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). An analysis of learned
help-
lessness: II. The processing of success. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 39, 940–952.
Druen, P. B. (1996). Cumulative relationship experience and
partner
selection strategies: Associations and implications for relation-
ship quality. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Louisville, Kentucky.
Dweck, C. S. (1991). Self-theories and goals: Their role in
motiva-
tion, personality, and development. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Ne-
braska symposium on motivation (pp. 199–235). Lincoln: Uni-
versity of Nebraska Press.
Dweck, C. S. (1996). Implicit theories as organizers of goals
and be-
havior. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychol -
ogy of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
(pp. 69–90). New York: Guilford.
Dweck, C. S. (1998). The development of early self-
conceptions:
Their relevance for motivational processes. In J. Heckhausen
& C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across
the lifespan (pp. 257–280). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories
and
their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two per -
spectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.
Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories:
Indi-
vidual differences in the likelihood and meaning of
dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bul -
letin, 19, 644–656.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social–cognitive
approach to
motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 25, 109–116.
Eidelson, R. J., & Epstein, N. (1982). Cognition and
relationship
maladjustment: Development of a measure of dysfunctional re-
lationship beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol -
ogy, 50, 715–720.
Erdley, C. A., & Dweck, C. S. (1993). Children’s implicit
personality
theories as predictors of their social judgments. Child Develop-
ment, 64, 863–878.
Feeney, J. A. (1999). Adult romantic attachment and couple
relation-
ships. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach-
ment (pp. 355–377). New York: Guilford.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1994). Attachment
style,
communication, and satisfaction in the early years of marriage.
In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal
relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment processes in adulthood. (pp.
269–308). London: Jessica Kingsley.
Fletcher, G. J. O., & Fitness, J. (1990). Occurrent social
cognition in
close relationship interaction: The role of proximal and distal
vari-
ables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 464–
474.
Fletcher, G. J. O., & Kininmonth, L. (1992). Measuring
relationship
beliefs: An individual differences measure. Journal of Research
in Personality, 26, 371–397.
Fletcher, G. J. O., Simpson, J. A., Thomas, G., & Giles, L.
(1999).
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 72–89.
Fletcher, G. J. O., & Thomas, G. (1996). Close relationship lay
theo-
ries: Their structure and function. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fit-
ness (Eds.), Knowledge structures and interaction in close rela-
tionships: A social psychological approach (pp. 3–24).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Franiuk, R., Cohen, D., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2001). Implicit
theories
of relationships: Implications for relationship satisfaction and
longevity. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Gervey, B. M., Chiu, Y., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1999).
Differen-
tial use of person information in decisions about guilt versus in-
nocence: The role of implicit theories. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 25, 17–27.
Goetz, T. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). Learned helplessness in
social
situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39,
246–255.
Hammond, J. R., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1991). Attachment styles
and
relationship satisfaction in the development of close relation-
ships. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 20, 56–62.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. (1986). A theory and method of
love.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 392–402.
Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and
con-
scious experience. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Hand-
book of self-determination research (pp. 87–100). New York:
University of Rochester Press.
Hodgins, H. S., Koestner, R., & Duncan, N. (1996). On the
compati-
bility of autonomy and relatedness. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 22, 227–237.
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., & Dweck, C. S. (1995). Implicit theories of
intel-
ligence: Reconsidering the role of confidence in achievement
54
KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
motivation. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-es-
teem. New York: Plenum.
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W.
(1999).
Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system
approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
588–599.
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and
close
relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Rela-
tionships, 1, 123–142.
Knee, C. R. (1998). Implicit theories of relationships:
Assessment
and prediction of romantic relationship initiation, coping, and
longevity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
360–370.
Knee, C. R., Nanayakkara, A., Vietor, N. A., Neighbors, C., &
Pat-
rick, H. (2001). Implicit theories of relationships: Who cares if
romantic partners are less than ideal? Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 27, 808–819.
Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., Vietor, N. A., Nanayakkara, A., &
Neigh-
bors, C. (2002). Self-determination as growth motivation in ro-
mantic relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
tin, 28, 609–619
Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., Vietor, N. A., & Neighbors, C. (2002).
Im-
plicit theories of relationships as moderators of the link be -
tween conflict and relationship quality. Manuscript submitted
for publication.
Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1996). Causality orientations
and the
disappearance of the self-serving bias. Journal of Research in
Personality, 30, 76–87.
Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1998). A nondefensive
personality:
Autonomy and control as moderators of defensive coping and
self-handicapping. Journal of Research in Personality, 32,
115–130.
Koestner, R. & Zuckerman, M. (1994). Causality orientations,
fail-
ure, and achievement. Journal of Personality, 62, 321–346.
Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Modes of
social
thought: Implicit theories and social understanding. In S.
Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psy-
chology (pp. 179–202). New York: Guilford.
Lydon, J. (1999). Commitment and adversity: A reciprocal
relation.
In J. M. Adams & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Handbook on interper-
sonal commitment and relationship stability. (pp. 193–203).
New York: Plenum.
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence
can
undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52.
Murray, S. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1993). Seeing virtues in faults:
Negativity and the transformation of interpersonal narratives in
close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol -
ogy, 65, 707–722.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The
benefits of
positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfac -
tion in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70, 79–98.
Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for
struc-
ture: Individual differences in the desire for simple struc-
ture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
113–131.
Patrick, H., & Knee, C. R. (2002). Implicit theories of
relationships
and motivation: Buffering the negative consequences of extrin-
sic relationship motivation. Manuscript in preparation.
Patrick, H., Knee, C. R., & Lonsbary, C. (2001). Implicit
theories of
relationships and responses to romantic infidelity. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construct
of per-
sonal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357.
Rusbult, C. E., Zembrodt, I. M., & Gunn, L. K. (1982). Exit,
voice,
loyalty, and neglect: Responses to dissatisfaction in romantic
involvements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
43, 1230–1242.
Ruvolo, A. P., & Rotondo, J. L. (1998). Diamonds in the rough:
Im-
plicit personality theories and views of the partner and self.
Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 750–58.
Sprecher, S., & Metts, S. (1989). Development of the ‘Romantic
Be-
liefs Scale’ and examination of the effects of gender and gen-
der-role orientation. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 6, 387–411.
Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z. R., Ryan, R. M., &
Deci,
E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss and
weight-loss maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70, 115–126.
55
IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
What Do You Do When Things Go Right? The Intrapersonal and
Interpersonal Benefits of Sharing Positive Events
Shelly L. Gable
University of California, Los Angeles
Harry T. Reis
University of Rochester
Emily A. Impett
University of California, Los Angeles
Evan R. Asher
University of Rochester
Four studies examined the intrapersonal and interpersonal
consequences of seeking out others when good
things happen (i.e., capitalization). Two studies showed that
communicating personal positive events
with others was associated with increased daily positive affect
and well-being, above and beyond the
impact of the positive event itself and other daily events.
Moreover, when others were perceived to
respond actively and constructively (and not passively or
destructively) to capitalization attempts, the
benefits were further enhanced. Two studies found that close
relationships in which one’s partner
typically responds to capitalization attempts enthusiastically
were associated with higher relationship
well-being (e.g., intimacy, daily marital satisfaction). The
results are discussed in terms of the theoretical
and empirical importance of understanding how people “cope”
with positive events, cultivate positive
emotions, and enhance social bonds.
Good, the more communicated, more abundant grows.
—John Milton, Paradise Lost, Book V
The puzzle of well-being has many pieces. One piece that has
been the focus of much research is how people maintain or
restore
their well-being in the face of negative events or stressors. Re-
search has often asked, “What can people do when things go
wrong?” and useful answers to this question have come from
studies on appraisals (e.g., Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter,
Delongis, & Gruen, 1986; Lazarus, 1991), coping (e.g., Bolger,
1990; Carver & Scheier, 1994), and rumination (e.g., Nolen-
Hoeksema, 1996, 1998). These and other studies have demon-
strated that people routinely turn to others for support in times
of
stress, be it in the face of everyday stressors (e.g., Harlow &
Cantor, 1995) or major life events (e.g., Bolger & Eckenrode,
1991), and that the availability of social support has clear
benefits
for the support-seeker’s health and well-being (e.g., Sarason,
Sara-
son, & Gurung, 1997; Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser,
1996).
Furthermore, people commonl y cite the possibility of receiving
social support, if and when needed, as one of the major benefits
of
close relationships (e.g., Cunningham & Barbee, 2000). Without
doubt, the processes involved in utilizing social relations to
cope
with negative events are central to understanding intrapersonal
and
interpersonal well-being. Nevertheless, this article suggests that
another, complementary piece of the puzzle has been largely
overlooked: the intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of
seeking out others when good things happen.
Although the social sharing of good news has received
relatively
little attention, research on responses to good fortune has not
been
entirely lacking. For example, in two daily experience studies,
Langston (1994) found that when people shared the news of a
positive event with others or celebrated the event in some way,
they experienced greater positive affect, beyond increases
associ-
ated with the valence of the positive event itself. He called this
capitalization, a term that we adopt to refer to the process of
informing another person about the occurrence of a personal
positive event and thereby deriving additional benefit from it.
Langston’s findings complement research by Bryant (1989),
who
found that individual differences in the self-reported ability to
savor positive events were correlated with subjective well -
being.
These studies also complement research by Tesser and his col -
leagues on the extended self-evaluation maintenance model
(e.g.,
Beach & Tesser, 1995; Tesser, 2000), which demonstrates that
reflection processes—that is, the ability to share in a partner’s
success— contribute to emotional well-being and relationship
sat-
isfaction. The present research seeks to extend these studies by
investigating the intrapersonal and interpersonal outcomes
associ-
ated with capitalizing on positive events. Paralleling existing
re-
search on stressors and social support, we examined both the
process of seeking a response to one’s good fortune and the
impact
of perceiving the provision of a positive response.
Shelly L. Gable and Emily A. Impett, Department of
Psychology,
University of California, Los Angeles; Harry T. Reis and Evan
R. Asher,
Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology,
University of
Rochester.
Study 2 was supported by a Barth–Crapsey award from the
University
of Rochester, Study 3 was collected as part of Shelly L. Gable’s
doctoral
dissertation (Gable, 2000, Study 2), and Study 4 was supported
by a grant
from the Templeton Foundation/Positive Psycholo gy Network
awarded to
Shelly L. Gable.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Shelly L.
Gable, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los
Angeles,
4560 Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563. E-
mail:
[email protected]
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2004 by
the American Psychological Association
2004, Vol. 87, No. 2, 228 –245 0022-3514/04/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.228
228
Positive Events and Well-Being
Traditionally, researchers interested in health and well-being
have examined the psychological and physical consequences of
stressors and other negative events (e.g., Cohen & Wills, 1985;
Hobfoll, 1998; Taylor, 1991). Recent theorizing has suggested
that
processes linked to positive events may have independent and
important associations with well-being and health (e.g., Reis &
Gable, 2003; Ryff & Singer, 1998). For example, Lewinsohn
and
Graf (1973) found that everyday pleasant events were associated
with decreases in depressive symptoms (Zautra, Schultz, &
Reich,
2000). More recently, Nezlek and Gable (2001) found that the
occurrence of minor positive events was associated with
increases
in daily self-esteem and perceived control and decreases in
daily
depressive cognitions, above and beyond the effect of negative
events.
That positive events may have independent effects on well -
being, over and above the impact of negative events, is
consistent
with the extensive research demonstrating the independence of
appetitive and aversive processes (Gable & Reis, 2001; Higgins,
1997). This independence has been most clearly established in
studies of emotion, in which the processes regulating positive
and
negative emotions are functionally independent (Cacioppo &
Gardner, 1999). For example, Gable, Reis, and Elliot (2000)
showed that everyday positive events were associated with in-
creased positive affect but not with changes in negative affect.
Other studies showing that positive, pleasant, or desirable
events
are differentially related to positive but not negative affect
include
L. A. Clark and Watson (1988) and David, Green, Martin, and
Suls
(1997). If positive events are uniquely and distinguishably
associ-
ated with positive emotions, then it becomes important to inves-
tigate how people “cope”1 with positive events, inasmuch as
these
processes may differ from the processes involved in coping with
negative events (which include social support). In fact, positive
emotions appear to have important consequences for health and
well-being over and above negative emotions, as recent studies
have shown (Salovey, Rothman, Detweiler, & Steward, 2000).
For
example, Pettit, Kline, Gencoz, and Gencoz (2001) found that
positive affectivity but not negative affectivity predicted self-
reported health. Danner, Snowdon, and Friesen (2001) found
that
positive emotional expression in autobiographical statements in-
versely predicted mortality 6 decades later. Similarly, Harker
and
Keltner (2001) showed that the expression of positive emotion
in
college yearbook photos predicted well-being 30 years later.
In a recent and influential article, Fredrickson (1998) suggested
that positive emotions have received insufficient attention
because
they do not fit existing models of emotion and emotion
regulation.
To fill this gap, Fredrickson (1998, 2001) has proposed a
“broaden-and-build” theory of positive emotions. This theory
pos-
its that the function of positive emotions is to broaden an
individ-
ual’s thought-action repertoire (that is, to facilitate a broader
scope
of cognition, attention, and action) and to build resources (that
is,
to enhance the individual’s physical, intellectual, and social re -
sources). Preliminary evidence supports this model. For
example,
Fredrickson and Joiner (2002) found that broad-minded coping
(i.e., flexible cognitive responses to stressors) was associated
with
increased positive emotions and an upward spiral toward
increased
well-being. Empirical evidence for the broaden-and-build model
of
positive emotions has predominantly focused on the broadening
component of the theory (e.g., Fredrickson & Branigan, 2002;
Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). The capitalization process, we
sug-
gest, is central to the process of using positive emotions to
build
resources.
Capitalization Builds Personal and Social Resources
We propose that capitalization—telling others about positive
events in one’s life—is likely to generate additional positive
affect,
over and above positive affect associated with the event itself.
There are several possible mechanisms for such an effect. First,
sharing a positive event with others requires retelling the event,
which creates an opportunity for reliving and reexperiencing the
event. Furthermore, the communicative act may involve
rehearsal
and elaboration, both of which seem likely to prolong and
enhance
the experience by increasing its salience and accessibility in
mem-
ory. If so, positive events that are communicated to others
should
be remembered better than positive events not communicated.
Another potential mechanism is more interpersonal in nature.
Sharing events with others may build social resources by
fostering
positive social interactions, which are reinforcing in and of
them-
selves (Gable & Reis, 2001), and thereby strengthening
relation-
ships. Furthermore, sharing good news initiates an interaction
that
may allow individuals to perceive that others are pleased for
them,
a process likely to boost self-esteem (e.g., Beach & Tesser,
1995;
Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988) and more generally to facilitate
positive reflected appraisals—perceiving oneself positively in
the
eyes of the other (Baumeister, 1998; Leary & Baumeister, 2000;
Shrauger & Schoeneman, 1979). This latter effect requires, of
course, that the listener’s response be experienced as positive —
that is, as recognizing and validating the good news (Reis &
Patrick, 1996). More destructive responses (for example,
pointing
out the downside of a positive event or minimizing its
importance)
may well dampen or even reverse the positive affect produced
by
one’s good fortune. Thus, we propose that the personal and
social
resources to be gained from capitalization depend in an
important
way on the ability to perceive the other’s response as positive.
This
process provides one central mechanism, we suggest, for the
upward spiral of positive affect and well-being described by
Fredrickson (1998).
Capitalization and Responsiveness to the Self
As proposed above, capitalization may depend significantly on
the ability to perceive the partner’s response as supporting
one’s
good fortune. People are unlikely to share their good news if
they
anticipate rejection, defensiveness, or an otherwise
unappreciative
response. Supportive responses suggest that an interaction
partner
is pleased for one, and this may be diagnostic not only of the
partner’s positive evaluation of the event itself but also of the
relationship. One reason for this is that an enthusiastic response
may indicate that the partner is “basking in reflected glory”
(Cial-
dini et al., 1976)—feeling that the good fortune rubs off to some
1 Although the use of the word cope has traditionally referred to
how
people deal with negative events, we extend this meaning to
responses to
positive events.
229CAPITALIZING ON POSITIVE EVENTS
extent on themselves, a process that at least nominally imputes
a
relationship between self and partner. This tendency is likely to
be
enhanced the closer the relationship (Beach & Tesser, 1995).
Aron,
Aron, Tudor, and Nelson (1991) defined closeness as “including
the other in the self,” a process that implies that a partner’s
good
fortune may psychologically be experienced as one’s own in a
close relationship (over and above whatever material conse-
quences a partner’s good fortune may have for the self). Thus, a
partner’s response of genuine pleasure to one’s capitalization
at-
tempts may indicate a sense of connection. Research on
empathy
and emotional contagion, although not usually focused on
positive
events, points to a similar conclusion (Hatfield, Cacioppo, &
Rapson, 1994).
Similarly, a constructive response may engender processes cen-
tral to the intimacy process. As Reis and Shaver (1988) defined
that process, disclosure of self-relevant material fosters
intimacy
when the partner’s response is experienced as understanding,
val-
idating, and caring. The partner’s response is critical to this
pro-
cess because it indicates that he or she is aware of central
aspects
of the self (which include goal attainment, positive affects, and
the
personally relevant activities that foster them) and is willing
and
able to be empathic and supportive. In fact, as Reis, Clark, and
Holmes (2004) have recently suggested, “perceived responsive-
ness to the self” is a central idea behind numerous self-relevant
interpersonal processes, such as attachmen t theory (Mikulincer
&
Shaver, 2003), rejection sensitivity (Downey, Freitas,
Michaelis, &
Khouri, 1998), the construction of a sense of felt security
(Murray
& Holmes, 1993), mutual cyclical growth (Rusbult, Olsen,
Davis,
& Hannon, 2001), and communal caring (M. S. Clark, Fitness,
&
Brissette, 2001). Typically, however, perceived responsiveness
is
conceptualized and investigated in terms of responses either to
negative events or to nominally private information whose
public
disclosure might leave one vulnerable. We propose that the con-
cept of responsiveness may also apply to personal positive
events
by revealing whether or not a partner is pleased for one, an act
that
incorporates evaluations of the event, its self-relevance, and the
relationship.
Overview of the Research and Hypotheses
The present research was designed to examine the intrapersonal
and interpersonal correlates of sharing positive events with
others
(i.e., capitalization). We first sought to establish that the
sharing of
good news has beneficial intrapersonal effects. Study 1 was a
daily
experience study in which participants reported on the sharing
(or
not) of positive events and their daily affect and well -being. On
the
basis of previous research and Fredrickson’s (1998) broaden-
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu
Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu

More Related Content

Similar to Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu

What Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docx
What Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docxWhat Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docx
What Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docxphilipnelson29183
 
Attachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human DevelopmentAttachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human DevelopmentLacey Desper
 
Com 262 research paper
Com 262 research paperCom 262 research paper
Com 262 research paperstonehel
 
Personality and Career Choice
Personality and Career ChoicePersonality and Career Choice
Personality and Career ChoiceMargarit Davtian
 
Psychological contract
Psychological contractPsychological contract
Psychological contractJaehyeon Nam
 
2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf
2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf
2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdfsrinivasschandru
 
The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...
The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...
The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...inventionjournals
 
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2
Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2
Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2Haley Ho
 
Gender differences
Gender differencesGender differences
Gender differencesSamerYaqoob
 
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docxTheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docxssusera34210
 
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docxTheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docxchristalgrieg
 
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for socialLittle research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for socialMerrileeDelvalle969
 
WHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docx
WHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docxWHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docx
WHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docxharold7fisher61282
 
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAPRule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAPSarah Kenehan
 
Dyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic Relationships
Dyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic RelationshipsDyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic Relationships
Dyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic RelationshipsAJHSSR Journal
 
Topics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic RelationshipsTopics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic RelationshipsBrittany Weber
 

Similar to Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu (20)

What Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docx
What Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docxWhat Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docx
What Makes A Hero The Impact of Integrity onAdmiration an.docx
 
Attachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human DevelopmentAttachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human Development
 
Com 262 research paper
Com 262 research paperCom 262 research paper
Com 262 research paper
 
Personality and Career Choice
Personality and Career ChoicePersonality and Career Choice
Personality and Career Choice
 
Psychological contract
Psychological contractPsychological contract
Psychological contract
 
2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf
2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf
2020 - Smillie - Personality and moral judgement.pdf
 
Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...
Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...
Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...
 
The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...
The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...
The Mediating Role of Group Cohesion in the Relationship between Interpersona...
 
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
 
Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2
Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2
Relationships AQA Paper 3 PSYCHOLOGY A2
 
Gender differences
Gender differencesGender differences
Gender differences
 
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docxTheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
 
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docxTheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
TheIncubatorAttribution theory in the organizational.docx
 
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for socialLittle research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
 
Relationship
RelationshipRelationship
Relationship
 
WHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docx
WHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docxWHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docx
WHO’S WITH ME FALSE CONSENSUS, BROKERAGE, ANDETHICAL DECISI.docx
 
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAPRule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
 
Department of Psychology at Northwestern University
Department of Psychology at Northwestern UniversityDepartment of Psychology at Northwestern University
Department of Psychology at Northwestern University
 
Dyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic Relationships
Dyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic RelationshipsDyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic Relationships
Dyadic Coping and Attachment Dimensions in Young Adult Romantic Relationships
 
Topics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic RelationshipsTopics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
 

More from MalikPinckney86

Find a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docx
Find a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docxFind a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docx
Find a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Find an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docx
Find an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docxFind an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docx
Find an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Find a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docx
Find a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docxFind a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docx
Find a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docxMalikPinckney86
 
Find a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docx
Find a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docxFind a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docx
Find a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Find a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docx
Find a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docxFind a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docx
Find a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Find a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docx
Find a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docxFind a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docx
Find a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docx
Financial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docxFinancial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docx
Financial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Find a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docx
Find a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docxFind a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docx
Find a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docx
Financing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docxFinancing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docx
Financing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docx
Financing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docxFinancing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docx
Financing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docx
Financial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docxFinancial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docx
Financial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docx
Financial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docxFinancial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docx
Financial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docx
Financial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docxFinancial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docx
Financial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docx
Financial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docxFinancial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docx
Financial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docx
Financial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docxFinancial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docx
Financial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Final Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docx
Final Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docxFinal Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docx
Final Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Final Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docx
Final Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docxFinal Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docx
Final Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Financial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docx
Financial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docxFinancial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docx
Financial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docxMalikPinckney86
 
Final ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docx
Final ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docxFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docx
Final ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docxMalikPinckney86
 
Final ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docx
Final ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docxFinal ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docx
Final ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docxMalikPinckney86
 

More from MalikPinckney86 (20)

Find a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docx
Find a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docxFind a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docx
Find a recent merger or acquisition that has been announced in the.docx
 
Find an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docx
Find an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docxFind an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docx
Find an example of a document that misuses graphics. This can be a d.docx
 
Find a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docx
Find a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docxFind a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docx
Find a scholarly research study from the Ashford University Library .docx
 
Find a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docx
Find a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docxFind a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docx
Find a work of visual art, architecture, or literature from either A.docx
 
Find a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docx
Find a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docxFind a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docx
Find a real-life” example of one of the following institutions. Exa.docx
 
Find a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docx
Find a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docxFind a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docx
Find a listing of expenses by diagnosis or by procedure. The source .docx
 
Financial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docx
Financial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docxFinancial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docx
Financial Reporting Problem  and spreedsheet exercise.This is an.docx
 
Find a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docx
Find a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docxFind a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docx
Find a Cybersecurity-related current event that happned THIS WEEK, a.docx
 
Financing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docx
Financing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docxFinancing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docx
Financing Health Care in a Time of Insurance Restructuring Pleas.docx
 
Financing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docx
Financing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docxFinancing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docx
Financing International Trade Please respond to the followingCom.docx
 
Financial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docx
Financial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docxFinancial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docx
Financial Statement Analysis and DisclosuresDiscuss the import.docx
 
Financial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docx
Financial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docxFinancial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docx
Financial Ratios what are the limitations of financial ratios  .docx
 
Financial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docx
Financial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docxFinancial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docx
Financial mangers make decisions today that will affect the firm i.docx
 
Financial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docx
Financial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docxFinancial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docx
Financial Laws and RegulationsComplete an APA formatted 2 page pap.docx
 
Financial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docx
Financial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docxFinancial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docx
Financial Management DiscussionWhen reviewing the financial st.docx
 
Final Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docx
Final Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docxFinal Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docx
Final Written Art Project (500 words) carefully and creatively wri.docx
 
Final Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docx
Final Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docxFinal Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docx
Final Research Paper Research the responsibility of a critical t.docx
 
Financial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docx
Financial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docxFinancial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docx
Financial management homeworkUnit III Financial Planning, .docx
 
Final ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docx
Final ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docxFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docx
Final ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrate an understanding.docx
 
Final ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docx
Final ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docxFinal ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docx
Final ProjectImagine that you work for a health department and hav.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingTeacherCyreneCayanan
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajanpragatimahajan3
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...PsychoTech Services
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 

Implicit Theories of Relationships Orientations TowardEvalu

  • 1. Implicit Theories of Relationships: Orientations Toward Evaluation and Cultivation C. Raymond Knee, Heather Patrick, and Cynthia Lonsbary Department of Psychology University of Houston Implicit theories of relationships (ITRs) influence goals, motivations, attributions, and behavior in romantic relationships. We developed a model of ITRs that draws from social cognition, motivation, and achievement literatures, and derived concep- tual parallels and hypotheses with regard to relationships. It is proposed that ITRs re- flect the belief component of a larger system of motivations and goals that can influ- ence the degree to which people are oriented toward the evaluation and cultivation of relationships. Research on ITRs is reviewed with regard to how they moderate well-documented associations between relationship perceptions and outcomes. Dif- ferences between ITRs and implicit theories in other domains are also discussed. Individuals have different beliefs about what makes for a good relationship. These beliefs or implicit theo- ries determine, in part, one’s goals and motivations in relationships (Knee, 1998; Knee, Nanayakkara, Vietor, Neighbors, & Patrick, 2001), much as implicit theories
  • 2. in other domains have been shown to determine goals and motivations in those contexts (see Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995, for review). Implicit theories of relation- ships (ITRs), as defined here, are characterized by a belief in romantic destiny and a belief in relationship growth. Destiny belief is defined as the belief that po- tential relationship partners are either compatible or they are not. Growth belief is defined as the belief that relationship challenges can be overcome. Theo- retically, those who believe more (relative to less) strongly in destiny attempt to determine the compati- bility of their partner and the viability of the relation- ship based on minimal information. They place a high value on determining whether a relationship is meant to be, and tend to diagnose the potential of the relation- ship based on specific events. Those who believe more (relative to less) strongly in growth are primarily inter- ested in developing the relationship, and believe that relationships grow not despite obstacles but rather be- cause of them. Destiny belief is linked to attempts to diagnose the status and potential success of the rela- tionship, and growth belief is linked to attempts to maintain the relationship. Implicit Theories of Attributes Implicit theories were first studied with regard to traits such as personality, intelligence, and morality (Dweck, 1996; Dweck et al., 1995; Dweck, Hong, & Chiu, 1993). Research on ITRs grew out of Dweck and colleagues’ research on implicit theories in these other domains. Research has shown that implicit theories are relatively domain-specific such that the particular the- ories within a given domain (e.g., intelligence) most strongly predict behavior within that domain (e.g., giv- ing up on a challenging test). Within Dweck’s theoreti-
  • 3. cal framework, a belief that certain traits are fixed and unchangeable is called an entity theory, and a belief that these traits are flexible and can be changed with ef- fort is called an incremental theory (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Implicit theories have been conceptualized as schematic knowledge structures that involve specific beliefs about the stability of an attribute and those con- ditions that are likely to promote change (Ross, 1989). Historically, these beliefs have been examined in the achievement and social judgment literatures yielding a number of provocative findings. Most of the work on implicit theories has centered on implicit theories of intelligence. This body of re- search has suggested that an entity belief about intelli - gence fosters concern about ability. These concerns are then manifested in dispositional inferences about abil - ity following feedback, even in the face of limited or contradictory information. For example, those who hold an entity theory of intelligence seem to be particu- larly sensitive to early indications of potential failure, thus leading them to give up on challenging achieve- ment tasks (Dweck, 1996; Dweck et al., 1995; Dweck et al., 1993; Hong, Chiu, & Dweck, 1995). In this do- main, an entity theory (e.g., “You have a certain 41 Personality and Social Psychology Review 2003, Vol. 7, No. 1, 41–55 Copyright © 2003 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. This research was supported in part by grant 1R03MH61805 from the National Institute of Mental Health.
  • 4. We thank the Interpersonal Relations and Motivation Research Group at the University of Houston for help at various stages of this ar- ticle. Linda K. Acitelli provided helpful comments on an earlier draft. Requests for reprints should be sent to C. Raymond Knee, De- partment of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204–5341. E-mail: [email protected] amount of intelligence and you can’t do much to change it”) is linked to robust, global dispositional in- ferences about intelligence whereas an incremental theory predicts fewer dispositional and more provi- sional inferences (Dweck, 1991; Dweck et al., 1993). In addition, an entity belief about personality has been associated with making global trait inferences from brief samples of behavior, perceiving behavior as sta- ble, and showing an increased likelihood to blame and punish others for undesirable behaviors (Erdley & Dweck, 1993). With regard to relationships, implicit theories of personality have been shown to moderate the correlation between individuals’ views of their ro- mantic partner and how satisfied they are in the rela- tionship (Ruvolo & Rotondo, 1998). View of partner was defined as rating the partner on 24 personal char- acteristics. Results showed that the correlation be- tween having a favorable view of one’s partner and be- ing satisfied in the relationship was weaker with an entity theory of personality. From Attributes to Relationships
  • 5. Implicit theories in other areas have been shown to be domain specific, and judgments in one domain are best predicted by implicit theories of attributes in the same domain (Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997; Dweck et al., 1993). Thus, Dweck and her colleagues have developed separate scales to identify people’s implicit theories about intelligence, personality, and moral character (Dweck et al., 1995). Implicit theories set up different goals and orient individuals to focus on different factors for explaining performance. An en- tity theory is associated with performance goals and a focus on gaining favorable judgments and avoiding negative ones. Individuals holding an entity belief about intelligence focus more on fixed abilities rather than malleable aspects of the trait, leading them to explain negative performance more in terms of lack of ability rather than lack of effort. Thus, these indi - viduals seem to be more prone to helpless responses in the face of failure (Diener & Dweck, 1980; Goetz & Dweck, 1980). Individuals with an incremental theory tend to be more mastery-oriented when faced with failure and tend to search for ways to improve ability and performance through increased effort and engaging in remedial actions such as help-seeking (Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999). An incre- mental theory of intelligence orients an individual to- ward learning goals and increasing ability. Thus, im- plicit theories set up a general frame of reference from which to evaluate performance, abilities, and traits. ITRs grew out of Dweck and colleagues’ work on implicit theories in these other domains. Thus, im- plicit theories in the relationships domain are thought to reflect similar orientations toward challenging rela- tionship events and failures as implicit theories in other domains (Knee et al., 2001).
  • 6. With regard to relationships, those who believe more strongly in romantic destiny tend to be especially sensitive to signs that indicate that the relationship is not meant to be. Indeed, Knee (1998) found that rela- tionship survival was more strongly linked to initial satisfaction for those who believed more strongly in destiny. When those who believed more strongly in destiny initially felt more satisfied, their relationships lasted particularly long, whereas when they initially felt less satisfied, their relationships ended quickly. A stronger belief in destiny is associated with a more judgmental approach to relationships and a focus on fixed aspects of the relationship or the partner. Thus, when problems arise, they are more likely to view the problem as a sign that the relationship is not meant to be. Indeed, destiny belief has already been associated with disengaging from the relationship when there was a problem (Knee, 1998). Those who more strongly endorse a growth belief about relationships tend to believe that successful rela- tionships are developed by conquering obstacles and growing closer. Knee (1998) found that belief in growth was associated with fewer one-night stands during the first month of college and with dating a par- ticular person for a longer period of time. Moreover, belief in growth was associated with attempts to main- tain the relationship when there was a problem through a variety of coping strategies. Measurement of ITRs ITRs were originally measured with eight items ad- ministered on a 7-point Likert-type scale (Knee, 1998). Four items measured destiny belief and four items
  • 7. measured growth belief. For example, a destiny item is the following: “Potential partners are either compatible or they are not.” A growth item is the following: “Chal- lenges and obstacles in a relationship can make love even stronger.” Factor analyses in several samples re- vealed that destiny and growth beliefs were independ- ent. When items reflecting each belief were averaged, destiny and growth were uncorrelated (r = –.01). Later confirmatory factor analyses using a 15-item measure also revealed a clear two-factor structure in which des- tiny and growth beliefs were independent. This two-factor model fit the data better than a single-factor model (Knee et al., 2001). Finally, the most recent 22-item version of the ITR contains 11 destiny and 11 growth items and is stable across a variety of diverse samples. Table 1 provides all 22 items along with item-total correlations. Internal reliabilities for destiny and growth were .82 and .74, respectively, in a sample of 400 participants. Scores on destiny and growth are uncorrelated (rs typically range from –.01 to –.07, de- 42 KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY pending on the sample). Confirmatory factor analysis of the 22-item ITR specifying a single factor solution in which all destiny and growth items were forced to load on a single dimension, did not fit the data well, as before, χ2 (209, N = 436) = 1229.47, p < .001, Good- ness of Fit Index (GFI) = .72, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) = .11. Instead, as we have shown elsewhere, specifying independent destiny and growth factors provides a better fit, χ2 (209, N = 436) =
  • 8. 707.29, p < .001, GFI = .86, RMSEA = .07. Further, al- lowing the factors to correlate does not significantly improve the fit, χ2 (208, N = 436) = 705.12, p < .001, GFI = .86, RMSEA = .07. Destiny belief is correlated positively with the be- lief that partners cannot change themselves or their re- lationship (Partners Cannot Change; Eidelson & Ep- stein, 1982), as well as a pragmatic shopping-list approach to love (Pragma; Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986). Growth belief is correlated negatively with the belief that partners cannot change and positively with a gradual, friendship-based approach to love (Storge; Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986). With regard to the Big Five dimensions of personality, destiny belief is associ- ated with slightly less openness, and slightly more extraversion and neuroticism. Growth belief is associ - ated with more conscientiousness and somewhat more agreeableness (Knee, 1998). ITRs are associated with Sprecher and Metts’s (1989) romantic beliefs subscales such that destiny is associated with belief that there is only one potential partner, idealizing ro- mance, and belief in love at first sight, whereas growth is associated with the belief that love will find a way. With regard to Druen’s (1996) partner selection strate- gies, destiny belief is associated with searching for an ideal partner and believing that love conquers all, and growth belief is associated with working on relation- ships. ITRs are also associated with Rusbult’s exit-voice-loyalty-neglect typology of responses to dissatisfaction in relationships (see Rusbult, Zembrodt, & Gunn, 1982). Specifically, growth belief is correlated with voicing concerns about the relation- ship, and being loyal to the relationship; destiny belief is correlated with neglecting the relationship. With re-
  • 9. gard to attachment dimensions, growth belief is mod- estly correlated with attachment security, but other- wise ITRs are not significantly correlated with the attachment dimensions of security, ambivalence, or avoidance. Finally, because destiny belief reflects the notion that a relationship’s potential can be diagnosed, it is modestly associated with personal need for struc- ture (Neuberg & Newsom, 1993) which captures the desire to seek certainty and know what one can expect from situations. Further, ITRs are moderately correlated with im- plicit theories in other domains, although these other domains tap primarily the destiny component. For ex- ample, destiny belief is moderately correlated with having stronger entity theories of personality, morality, and intelligence. Growth belief is only slightly and nonsignificantly correlated with implicit theories in 43 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS TABLE 1. Items on the 22-Item Implicit Theories of Relationships Scale Along With Item-Total Correlations Destiny Belief Items .45–1. Potential relationship partners are either compatible or they are not. .45–3. A successful relationship is mostly a matter of finding a compatible partner right from the start. .48–5. Potential relationship partners are either destined to get along or they are not.
  • 10. .54–7. Relationships that do not start off well inevitably fail. .36–9. If a potential relationship is not meant to be, it wil l become apparent very soon. .59–11. The success of a potential relationship is destined from the very beginning. .61–13. To last, a relationship must seem right from the start. .56–15. A relationship that does not get off to a perfect start will never work. .55–17. Struggles at the beginning of a relationship are a sure sign that the relationship will fail. .34–19. Unsuccessful relationships were never meant to be. .53–21. Early troubles in a relationship signify a poor match between partners. Growth Belief Items .41–2. The ideal relationship develops gradually over time. .49–4. A successful relationship evolves through hard work and resolution of incompatibilities. .39–6. A successful relationship is mostly a matter of learning to resolve conflicts with a partner. .42–8. Challenges and obstacles in a relationship can make love even stronger. .42–10. Problems in a relationship can bring partners closer together.
  • 11. .37–12. Relationships often fail because people do not try hard enough. .26–14. With enough effort, almost any relationship can work. .48–16. It takes a lot of time and effort to cultivate a good relationship. .29–18. Without conflict from time to time, relationships cannot improve. .41–20. Arguments often enable a relationship to improve. .35–22. Successful relationships require regular maintenance. Note: Destiny and growth items are typically alternated when administered. Destiny items are averaged to yield a destiny score. Growth items are averaged to yield a growth score. The resulting scores are independent (r = –.01). these other domains (rs < .09). Destiny and growth be- liefs are typically not significantly correlated with sex, age, whether one is currently in a relationship, length of relationship, number of previous intimate relation- ships, current relationship satisfaction, social desir- ability, or self-esteem. The Independence of Destiny and Growth Beliefs One important difference between implicit theories in other domains and ITRs is that the former are tradi-
  • 12. tionally assessed as opposing ends of a single contin- uum whereas the latter have been found repeatedly to comprise independent dimensions (Franiuk, Cohen, & Pomerantz, 2001; Knee, 1998; Knee et al., 2001). This difference is as much a conceptual issue as it is an issue of measurement. In the domain of relationships, it is somewhat desirable to believe both that potential rela- tionships can be diagnosed and that relationships re- quire maintenance. Thus, it is conceivable to hold both beliefs simultaneously. In contrast, in other domains, attempts to measure entity and incremental beliefs as independent dimen- sions have been inconclusive because it is more so- cially desirable to endorse an incremental belief (e.g., that one can improve one’s intelligence; Dweck et al., 1995). In Dweck and colleagues’ framework, endorse- ment of an incremental theory is reflected by rejection of an entity theory. Several studies have indicated that even for those who endorse an entity belief, there is a strong tendency to endorse items indicative of an incre- mental theory, suggesting that incremental items are highly compelling and more socially desirable (see Dweck et al., 1995, for review). Thus, items depicting an incremental theory are not used in most of those studies. In contrast, ITRs are characterized by inde- pendent destiny and growth beliefs such that individu- als can endorse both or neither, as well as the typical extreme combinations (higher on one and lower on the other). This affords a more complex theoretical frame- work in which the dimensions of relationship mainte- nance (growth) and relationship diagnosis (destiny) can be examined jointly, and seems to better capture lay people’s beliefs about relationships. It is fairly common for participants to believe that, “Fate brings potential partners together, but once the relationship
  • 13. blooms, they must work hard to maintain it.” The inde- pendence of destiny and growth beliefs also suggests that, in the domain of relationships, a growth belief is not necessarily more socially desirable than a destiny belief, because participants vary in their endorsement of each dimension. This may be partly due to the fact that both destiny and growth beliefs have a positive connotation, and both beliefs are positively correlated with traditional scales of romanticism. Interestingly, the independence of destiny and growth beliefs suggests that each dimension can con- tribute uniquely to predicting and explaining relation- ship perceptions, cognitions, and behaviors. However, it also suggests that particular combinations, or inter- active effects, of destiny and growth may be especially relevant. For example, those with the combination of a higher growth belief and a lower destiny belief (culti- vation orientation) believe that relationships evolve through development, confrontation, and efforts to maintain and improve the relationship, and are less fo- cused on diagnosing the potential of relationships. When cultivating, one’s goal becomes the development and maintenance of the relationship without diagnos- ing or inferring grand meaning from otherwise minor incompatibilities. Those with the combination of a higher destiny belief and a lower growth belief (evalua- tion orientation) believe that relationships can be easily diagnosed and evaluated, but that they cannot be con- siderably improved. When evaluating, one’s goal be- comes the diagnosis of one’s partner and relationship in an effort to determine whether the relationship would seem to have immediate promise, without at- tempting to improve the relationship. In data analytic terms, this implies that although
  • 14. destiny and growth beliefs can be meaningful on their own (as main effects), they can also interact with each other in predicting or moderating relationship phe- nomena. Indeed, we often find the interaction of des- tiny and growth beliefs to be particularly interesting. We focus on the interactive extremes of destiny and growth beliefs in terms of evaluation and cultivation orientations for two reasons. First, these orientations reflect the clearest conceptual extremes and afford the most useful hypotheses. Second, research thus far has shown that the interactive effects of ITRs are often most clear when the effect of one belief is particularly strong (or weak) depending on the level of the other be- lief. For example, cultivation orientation tends to buffer negative consequences and evaluation orienta- tion tends to exacerbate them (Knee et al., 2001; Knee, Patrick, Vietor, & Neighbors, 2002). ITRs as Moderators of Perceptions and Outcomes Although ITRs can have direct effects on inferences, they can also moderate the consequences of those infer- ences. For example, perceiving limitations in one’s part- ner or the relationship can have different consequences depending on one’s ITRs. Because ITRs, in part, deter- mine how meaning will be ascribed to relationship-rele- vant events and partner qualities, the same events or qualities can take on different meaning with corre- spondingly different relationship implications. For ex- ample, considerable research has shown that idealistic 44 KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY
  • 15. views of one’s partner are associated with increased re- lationship satisfaction (Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 1996). However, this tendency to feel more satisfied when one views one’s partner more favorably than one’s partner views himself or herself may depend on one’s ITRs. Indeed, in two studies, Knee et al. (2001) found that ITRs moderate the relation between wanting more in one’s partner and feeling satisfied in the relationship. Study 1 defined “wanting more” in terms of perceiving a discrepancy between what one wants in an ideal partner and what one believes he or she has in a current partner. Further, this discrepancy was measured in both direct and less-direct ways. Across several indexes of discrep- ancy from an ideal, wanting more in one’s partner was consistently and strongly related to feeling less happy with the relationship. However, this relation was moder- ated by destiny and growth beliefs such that “wanting more” was less strongly linked to one’s satisfaction when one had a cultivation orientation (higher growth and lower destiny beliefs). With a cultivation orienta- tion, people were able to acknowledge their partner’s less positive attributes and still remain relatively satis- fied. In this way, these individuals did not view their partner’s imperfections as fatal flaws in the relationship. Study 2 tested whether ITRs moderate the projected illusions hypothesis (Murray et al., 1996). It was found that viewing one’s partner more favorably than one’s partner views himself or herself was generally linked to feeling more satisfied with the relationship. This re- lation was moderated by ITRs such that the link be- tween viewing one’s partner favorably and feeling sat- isfied was weaker among those who believed more strongly in growth, without regard to destiny belief.
  • 16. The moderating effects of ITRs are not limited to perceptions of one’s partner. For example, it is plausi - ble that conflict in relationships could either bring part- ners closer or force them apart, again depending on one’s ITRs. When one holds a belief that conflict is a healthy part of relationships and can bring partners closer by resolving it, then differences and disagree- ments can take on different meaning than when one be- lieves that conflict is a sign of insurmountable prob- lems in the relationship. This notion of increased commitment as a function of relationship adversity is not new (see Lydon, 1999). However, for some part- ners, the very presence of differences may be enough to strain the relationship, whereas others view it as an opportunity to learn more about their partner and de- velop the relationship. A recent series of studies was designed to test this hypothesis (Knee et al., 2002). In Study 1, 128 individuals in romantic relationships kept event-contingent diaries, recording every disagree- ment they and their partner had over a 10-day period. “Disagreement” was broadly defined as “anytime it be- comes evident to you that you and your partner dis- agree on an opinion, perspective, idea, goal, etc.” Rela- tionship quality was assessed after each disagreement as well. Multilevel analyses showed that, consistent with the hypothesis, the association between having longer conflicts and reporting reduced relationship quality afterward was generally strong, except among those who were both higher in growth and lower in des- tiny. Among these individuals who were oriented to- ward cultivation, there was virtually no relation be- tween the length of disagreements and reduced relationship quality on a daily basis.
  • 17. In Study 2, 75 dating couples discussed problems in their relationship, with commitment measured before and after the discussion. Multilevel analyses examined changes in commitment. As hypothesized, an orienta- tion toward cultivation was associated with less de- crease in commitment after discussing problems with one’s partner. Further, because those with stronger growth beliefs are particularly motivated to improve the relationship when it is lacking, the relation between growth belief and commitment was stronger when one had a less favorable view of one’s partner. Thus, an ori - entation toward cultivation seems to buffer the negative consequences of adverse relationship experiences. Taken together, it appears that an orientation toward cultivation, and growth belief in particular, can buffer the negative impact of arguments, discrepancies, and differences of opinion—events that normally are asso- ciated with a decline in satisfaction and commitment. The moderating effects of ITRs found thus far may reflect both cognitive and motivational mechanisms. The cognitive mechanism stems from the fact that a cultivation orientation is characterized by a belief that problems can be overcome and that the relationship’s potential is not readily diagnosed. Perhaps the part- ner’s negative qualities recede into the background, or perhaps partners come to authentically understand and appreciate each other’s faults and weaknesses. The motivational mechanism underlying cultivation may allow one to feel less threatened by conflict and dis- crepant qualities between oneself and one’s partner, given that cultivation involves viewing such attributes as latent and emergent rather than immediately evi- dent. They may be motivated to maintain and improve the relationship, and acknowledging limitations and weaknesses would seem to be a key asset in this pro-
  • 18. cess. ITRs thus reflect both cognitive and motivational processes that can influence one’s relationship-rele- vant perceptions, emotions, and behavior in meaning- ful ways. From Orientations to Behavioral Outcomes Levy, Plaks, and Dweck (1999) viewed implicit theories broadly as two modes of social thought. One mode is organized around static traits, leading one to seek trait information, view traits as causes of behav- 45 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS ior, draw trait-centered inferences, and categorize people by traits. The other mode is organized around more dynamic psychological mediators, leading one to consider people’s goals, needs, and mental states. We view evaluation and cultivation orientations as different modes of thought as well. One mode is based on a set of beliefs that relationships are relatively static, rigid, and diagnosable with little opportunity for improvement. The other mode is based on the be- liefs that relationships are dynamic and can be culti- vated and developed, and their potential not readily diagnosed. These orientations come to influence rela- tionship judgments, attributions, and inferences in much the same manner as they do in other domains. Research on implicit theories in other domains has shown that implicit theories can influence attributions,
  • 19. emotions, and behavior at both trait and state levels (see Dweck et al., 1995, for review). In particular, im- plicit theories of intelligence were recently linked to attributions and coping strategies in three studies (Hong et al., 1999). This research revealed that implicit theories can be measured overtly but can also be in- duced situationally to influence attributions about one’s performance and willingness to seek and accept help. Whether measured or induced, those who be- lieved that intelligence could be improved made stron- ger attributions to effort and were more likely to take remedial action when performance was unsatisfactory (Hong et al., 1999). We propose that a similar process occurs with evaluation and cultivation orientations in which both overtly measured beliefs as well as induced orientations toward relationships can influence attribu- tions for relationship events and willingness to seek help in response to relationship problems. Thus, al- though ITRs have been shown to be relatively stable in- dividual differences over time, they are also somew hat open to experience, as are most relationship knowledge structures and schema. For example, Knee (1998) found that the test–retest reliability of destiny belief was lower whereas that of growth was nonsignificantly higher when one had recently experienced a traumatic relationship event. We believe that evaluation and cultivation orientations, of which ITRs are perhaps a more stable component, can be induced momentarily much as Hong and colleagues (1999) demonstrated in the domain of intelligence. Figure 1 illustrates how ITRs can be viewed as part of a larger framework that distinguishes between cultivation and evaluation orientations toward rela- tionships. ITRs are but one component of these orien-
  • 20. 46 KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY Figure 1. Conceptual model of how evaluation and cultivation orientations may influence attributions and behavioral outcomes. tations. Based on Dweck and colleagues’ (1995) re- search along with our own research on relationships, we view a cultivation orientation as containi ng three components: (a) a stable belief component comprised of higher growth and lower destiny beliefs, (b) a mo- tivational component that reflects a desire to develop the relationship for its own sake, and (c) a goal com- ponent in which one intends to master relationship problems, interpret feedback informatively and nondefensively, and become interdependent with the partner. An evaluation orientation contains the fol- lowing three components: (a) a stable belief compo- nent comprised of higher destiny and lower growth beliefs, (b) a motivational component that reflects an outcome-driven desire to evaluate and diagnose the relationship’s potential, and (c) a goal component in which one intends to judge the partner and determine the viability of the relationship. Each of these compo- nents has been established in the social cognition and motivation literatures, but not necessarily with regard to relationships. An important outcome of evaluation and cultiva- tion orientations includes the emotional reactions en- gendered by viewing conflict as something that can
  • 21. or cannot be overcome. Indeed, Knee et al. (2001) found that ITRs predicted affective reactions on learning that one’s partner views the relationship dif- ferently. First, being higher in growth belief (without regard to destiny belief) predicted greater happiness and less depression after discussing discrepant views of the relationship. Thus, the value of believing that relationship limitations can be overcome should not be understated. Indeed, the benefits of growth belief became even more impressive when considered in combination with destiny belief. Specifically, those oriented toward cultivation felt increased positivity whereas those oriented toward evaluation felt in- creased hostility after discussing discrepant views of the relationship with their partner. When oriented to- ward cultivation, limitations and weaknesses become opportunities for closeness and understanding with- out diagnosis of the relationship’s potential. When oriented toward evaluation, even small differences can take on strong meaning, possibly leading to stronger, more judgmental inferences about one’s partner in combination with feelings that the differ- ences are insurmountable. Indeed, using a slightly different method of measuring ITRs, Franiuk et al. (2001) recently found that feeling one’s specific part- ner was ideal predicted relationship satisfaction and longevity to a greater extent for those with stronger destiny beliefs. Motivation The motivation behind a cultivation orientation is thought to be largely intrinsic and improve- ment-driven, with an emphasis on mastery and flexi- bility in reacting to and dealing with relationship
  • 22. challenges. Similar motives have been examined in the achievement and motivation literatures, albeit with regard to different outcomes. For example, Deci and Ryan (1985, 1991) have elaborated on the nature and process of growth motivation from the perspec- tive of self-determination theory. This other literature has shown how an orientation toward growth and mastery can positively influence academic achieve- ment, mental and physical health, as well as interper- sonal behavior (e.g., Hodgins, Koestner, & Duncan, 1996; Koestner & Zuckerman, 1994; Williams, Grow, Freedman, Ryan, & Deci, 1996). A motivation orien- tation toward growth and improvement has also been examined with regard to the self (Deci & Ryan, 1987, 1991). In particular, such studies have linked an ori- entation toward growth and improvement with fewer self-enhancement strategies including the self-serving bias, self-handicapping, and defensive coping (Knee & Zuckerman, 1996, 1998). Recently, Knee, Patrick, Vietor, Nanayakkara, and Neighbors (2002) examined self-determination as growth motivation in relationships. Growth motiva- tion was defined as an orientation toward improve- ment, choicefulness, and authenticity with regard to oneself and others and was based on Deci and Ryan’s conceptualization of self-determination (Deci & Ryan, 1985). As self-determination theory would pre- dict, an orientation toward growth and improvement was associated with (a) less tendency to view an ideal partner as a function of one’s view of self, (b) more active and integrative coping strategies, (c) less denial and fewer avoidance strategies, and (d) decreases in negative emotion, along with more positive interac- tion behaviors when discussing contrary relationship perceptions with one’s partner. These findings are
  • 23. consistent with previous research regarding self-de- termination and interpersonal outcomes (Blais, Sabourin, Boucher, & Vallerand, 1990; Hodgins et al., 1996). Moreover, these results suggested the im- portant ways in which a general motivation toward growth in relationships is beneficial by allowing peo- ple to approach threats as challenges, adversity as op- portunity, and conflict as the potential for new appre- ciation and understanding. Recent data indicate that ITRs are substantially and meaningfully correlated with motivation orienta- tions. For example, growth belief is associated with an orientation toward autonomy and feeling intrinsi- cally motivated to be in the relationship, and destiny belief is associated with an orientation toward feeling controlled and being extrinsically motivated to be in the relationship (Patrick & Knee, 2002). Extrinsic re- lationship motivation involves being in the relation- ship for reasons other than the spontaneous feelings and pleasures of the relationship (e.g., rewards, ap- 47 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS proval from others). Because destiny belief centers on diagnosing a relationship’s potential, it is especially important for those higher in destiny belief to associ - ate their relationship with positive experiences. These positive experiences and other relationship rewards confirm that the relationship is viable and meant to be. In addition, extrinsic relationship motivation in- cludes feedback and approval from other people (e.g.,
  • 24. family and friends). This feedback provides addi- tional diagnostic information about the potential of the relationship, and thus is important to those higher in destiny belief. Along these lines, a recent study suggests that ITRs moderate the link between relationship motiva- tion and relationship quality (Patrick & Knee, 2002). Specifically, we found that being higher in growth be- lief (without regard to destiny belief) buffers the neg- ative consequences of having extrinsic relationship motivation. When people believe that relationships can be improved, feeling obligated to the relationship does not seem so bad. When one feels stuck in the re- lationship, discussing problems with one’s partner maintains commitment if one believes the relation- ship can be improved. In addition, being higher in destiny belief exacerbates the negative consequences of having extrinsic relationship motivation. Spe- cifically, when one is extrinsically motivated to be in the relationship, a stronger destiny belief (without re- gard to growth belief) is linked to feeling less com- mitted after discussing relationship problems. Another way in which motivation affects relation- ships is in openness to experiences within the rela- tionship. One might predict that cultivation would be associated with acknowledging challenges to the rela- tionship and attempting to deal with them openly and directly, rather than denying that they exist or giving up and abandoning the relationship altogether (Hodgins & Knee, in press; Knee et al., 2001). It is important to note that the motivation behind evalua- tion and cultivation orientations may not be all that different. In both cases, people will want to learn more about the nature of the relationship. It is the un-
  • 25. derlying goals that may best distinguish between evaluation and cultivation orientations. When ori- ented toward evaluation, one attempts to diagnose the future potential of the relationship, whereas when ori- ented toward cultivation, one attempts to improve the relationship. It could be argued that by improving the relationship, one is improving and expanding the self, and thus the motivation behind a cultivation orienta- tion may reflect a desire for self-expansion as well (Aron & Aron, 1997). Indeed, ITRs have been corre- lated with inclusion of other in the self. Specifically, growth belief (and not destiny belief) is associated with greater self-expansion when operationalized as Aron and colleagues’ (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992) Inclusion of Other in the Self scale. Attributions In Dweck and colleagues’ early work, attributions played a key role in how implicit theories were thought to influence coping with challenges (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). That earlier work presumed that implicit theories set up goals which in turn set up attributions and re- sponses. However, more recent research has suggested that implicit theories are more consistently predictive of attributions and responses than are the corresponding goal orientations (Hong et al., 1999). We think that eval - uation and cultivation orientations may influence attri- butions in relationships in a manner similar to implicit theories in other domains. Specifically, those who are oriented to evaluate relationships should make stronger attributions to stable causes, and weaker attributions to effort and controllability for negative relationship events. This pattern of attributions follows from an eval- uation orientation for two reasons. First, the higher des- tiny belief component predisposes people to diagnose
  • 26. relationship potential, and stable attributions may facili - tate such diagnoses. Second, the lower growth belief component of evaluation orientation involves not be- lieving that things can be changed, which inherently in- volves attributions about effort and control over the course of the relationship. Those with a cultivation orientation should make stronger attributions to effort and controllability, and weaker attributions to stability as causes for negative relationship events. First, the higher growth belief component deals with believing that relationships can be improved, lending itself to effort and control attribu- tions. Second, the lower destiny belief component deals with not believing that the relationship’s poten- tial can be readily diagnosed, and thus suggests a lack of stability in the course of the relationship. Dispositional Inferences Chiu, Hong, and Dweck (1997) examined the rela- tion between implicit theories of personality and lay dispositionism. Lay dispositionism refers to people’s tendency to use traits as the basic unit of analysis in so- cial perception. In Study 1, results suggested that those with an entity theory tended to expect trait-relevant be- havior to be consistent across time and situations. Those with an incremental theory did not predict the same relative likelihood of a person displaying the same kind of behavior in different situations. In Study 2, participants with an entity belief about personality were more likely to predict the target’s future behavior in a particular situation based on information about the target person’s traits. Study 3 examined the likelihood of those with an entity theory confidently inferring traits from a particular behavior. Findings from this
  • 27. study further suggested that, for those who hold an en- tity theory of personality, dispositional traits mediate 48 KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY behaviors, leading to a belief that behaviors are consis- tent across situations. With regard to ITRs and inferences in relationships, we would expect that an orientation toward evaluation would promote stronger trait inferences about one’s ro- mantic partner and the relationship, using relatively limited samples of behavior. For example, when ori- ented toward evaluation, one may come to view brief samples of behavior as strongly indicative of the fu- ture, and thus come to believe that a relationship is de- sirable and promising or undesirable and hopeless. An orientation toward cultivation would promote less ex- treme inferences about the partner and relationship, or at least more positive expectations about working through potential problems. For example, if Jane were oriented toward evaluation, she may be more sensitive to negative aspects of the partner and relationship, and interpret the occasional argument as an indication that the relationship will always be hindered by disagree- ments that cannot be resolved. However, if Jane were oriented toward cultivation, she may view the same se- ries of occasional arguments as opportunities to learn about each other and become closer without inferring grand conclusions about the relationship’s potential. Thus, the perceived meaning and consequences of the same event can be rather different depending on one’s
  • 28. orientation, beliefs, and motivation at the time that it occurs. Indeed, Dweck and colleagues have shown that the belief that attributes are fixed in a particular domain predicts global dispositional inferences for self and other, even in the face of limited evidence. In addition, the belief in fixed attributes leads to an over-reliance on dispositional information in making judgments and de- cisions. The belief that attributes are flexible predicts inferences that are more specific, conditional, and pro- visional (Dweck et al., 1993). Reactions to Feedback Implicit theories in the achievement literature have traditionally been linked with reactions to negative feed- back on tasks related to the domain of the particular the- ory. For example, people who believe in the fixed nature of intelligence tend to give up and abandon difficult math problems following negative feedback, presum- ably because their static view of ability leads to stable at- tributions for performance (Dweck et al., 1995). It would seem that romantic relationships, too, tend to be replete with feedback, often of the unsolicited variety. Although the nature of feedback in everyday romantic relationships is not as clear and unambiguous as manip- ulated feedback in experimental studies on intelligence, evaluative information regarding the partner’s perfor- mance (broadly defined), or the relationship in general, is quite common. Feedback in the proposed model of re- lationships could be broadly conceived as any evaluative information or cue that may be interpreted as implying something about the ability of one’s partner or the relationship to continue satisfactorily. Probably the most common example of feedback
  • 29. within the relationship is when one partner raises con- cerns about the relationship, ranging from relatively minor issues (“We don’t do enough fun things to- gether”) to potentially more serious matters (“You don’t understand me”). If oriented toward evaluation, feedback about the relationship can take on stronger meaning, with more serious implications about the fu- ture success of the relationship, in part because they do not believe that such difficulties can be overcome. Re- gardless of how serious the issue may appear on the surface, an orientation toward cultivation would pro- mote less stable inferences about the relationship, and an improvement-driven perspective, because of the be- liefs that problems can be resolved and a relationship’s potential cannot be easily diagnosed. So, when one’s partner claims “You don’t understand me,” one who is evaluation-oriented may infer that understanding will never be possible, and that this diagnosis is not condu- cive to romantic bliss. One who is cultivation-oriented, however, may infer that now is the time to better at- tempt to understand one’s partner, and that with enough effort and time, understanding will come. Feedback about the relationship can also originate from one’s friends and relatives. Others may provide potentially evaluative information such as, “You two seem perfect together.” When oriented toward evalua- tion, this feedback can take on strong meaning along with inferences about the future stability of the rela- tionship. When oriented toward cultivation, this same feedback may seem relatively meaningless as people are not thought to be “matched for each other” to begin with, and because relationships are thought to develop and change along the way such that today’s mismatch may be tomorrow’s synchrony. It is important to note that it is not the nature of the feedback per se that is rel -
  • 30. evant to how ITRs influence inferences, reactions, and behavior, but rather it is the meaning that is assigned to the feedback as a function of the ITRs and orientations. The same feedback can take on rather different mean- ing, suggesting different relationship implications, de- pending on what one believes about relationships and how one is oriented. Reactions to Negative Partner Behavior Implicit theories of morality have also been shown to predict the extent to which people seek out and utilize character information when making decisions about one’s guilt or innocence (Gervey, Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1999). Specifically, in three studies, when de- fendant respectability was manipulated, information about the defendant’s appearance, clothing, and life- style affected entity theorists’ character inferences and 49 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS verdicts more than those of incremental theorists. Fur- ther, when given the opportunity to request additional information about the defendant that they thought would be relevant to the verdict, entity theorists were more likely to seek dispositional information. Compar- atively, incremental theorists did not consider informa- tion such as the style of clothing, or character of the de- fendant in making their judgments (Gervey et al., 1999). In another series of studies, entity theorists were found to believe more strongly in duty-based behavior than
  • 31. rights-based behavior, and thus tend to view violations of existing codes of conduct and expectations as less ac- ceptable, and are less tolerant of deviance (Chiu, Dweck, Tong, & Fu, 1997). This same research found that entity theorists of morality are also more likely to impose punishment or issue directives to transgressors. More generally, when people are asked how they would deal with another person’s disreputable actions or trans- gressions, entity theorists tend to propose punishment and retaliation whereas incremental theorists tend to propose education and reform (Dweck, 1996). Individ- uals who believe in fixed traits tend to more readily infer global traits from limited, concrete behaviors compared to those who believe that traits can change. Negative behavior is all too common in many ro- mantic relationships. Based on the proposed frame- work, evaluation and cultivation orientations may in- fluence how one interprets and assigns meaning to such events. Specifically, an evaluation orientation may be associated with stronger character inferences from relatively limited samples of behavior. Thus, a partner’s “one-night stand” may take on far greater meaning, with the assumption that the behavior re- flects something fundamental about the nature of the partner and one’s relationship. Having higher destiny and lower growth beliefs could make even a single event seem indicative of a deeper truth and a repeat- able pattern. Consequently, the evaluation-oriented partner may make strong and stable negative attribu- tions to the partner and the relationship, bringing to mind “once a cheater, always a cheater.” This could be a rather different inference compared to a partner who was oriented toward cultivation. Cultivation ori- entation could lead one to seek mediators of the be- havior, with less emphasis on judgment and more em-
  • 32. phasis on remediation and improvement. For example, in the case of a single instance of infidelity, someone with a cultivation orientation may seek out information about the circumstances that may have facilitated unfaithful behavior (e.g., decline in close- ness). Moreover, a cultivative orientation may moti- vate the thwarted partner to examine weaknesses in the relationship that could be addressed to prevent in- fidelity in the future. A cultivation orientation may make one more motivated to understand the condi- tions that surrounded the negative behavior, and seek a supportive, less judgmental method of remediation. In a preliminary study of the role of ITRs in re- sponses to romantic infidelity, 149 participants were randomly assigned to read one of four scenarios about infidelity which varied according to relationship prognosis (i.e., good or poor) and transgressor of infi - delity (i.e., self or partner; Patrick, Knee, & Lonsbary, 2001). After reading the scenario, participants an- swered two open-ended items designed to capture their anticipated responses to such a situation. Results re- vealed that what people believe about relationships has a strong effect on how they respond to even hypotheti- cal relationship threats. Those who were oriented to- ward evaluation and in the good prognosis condition were more likely to blame their partner, regardless of the transgressor of infidelity. In addition, an orientation toward evaluation was associated with less blame of a problem in the relationship, even when participants had been told “you aren’t sure if [this partner] is the right one for you.” Taken together, these findings seem to suggest that an evaluation orientation may some- times involve evaluating the partner rather than the re- lationship, and also that the meaning of such sin- gle-event instances can take on relatively different
  • 33. meaning depending on one’s ITRs. Seeking Remediation Implicit theories of intelligence have been linked to remedial action following unsatisfactory performance (Hong et al., 1999). Specifically, incremental theorists were more willing than entity theorists to take a reme- dial course that could potentially improve their perfor - mance following negative feedback. ITRs and their corresponding goal orientations may analogously in- fluence one’s willingness to seek help when faced with relationship difficulties. Seeking help may seem rela- tively pointless when one is oriented toward evaluation because one interprets relationship difficulties as evi- dence that this is not the right partner, and believes that such difficulties cannot be overcome. On the other hand, seeking help may seem particularly useful when oriented toward cultivation because one believes that the future of the relationship is not predetermined and that relationship challenges can be overcome. Thus, re- lationship counseling, whether through professional services or friends and family, may seem more poten- tially fruitful when operating according to certain be- liefs and their corresponding goal orientations. Again, it is not the actual utility of counseling or remedial help that is important here, but rather the way in which one’s beliefs and orientations guide expectations and infer- ences about the likelihood of improvement in the face of problems. In this way, seeking help for a dying rela- tionship may not always yield improved results, but re- gardless, one should be more likely to invite assistance when oriented toward cultivation. 50
  • 34. KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY It is not clear whether a cultivation orientation would lead one to predominantly infer that one can tackle relationship problems oneself or through the help of others, but in extreme cases, seeking help from others may seem most useful to those with a cultivation orientation. An evaluation orientation, rather than leading to the seeking of help, may lead to the seeking of additional evaluation from others. Specifically, feedback may seem particularly important when ori- ented toward evaluation, not because it can guide one on how to improve and develop the relationship, but because it can sharpen one’s evaluation of the future potential of the relationship by making the diagnosis of problems (and hence the relationship’s future) that much more evident. Developmental Origins ITRs probably have a variety of origins, some of which have been described elsewhere. For example, ITRs may develop vicariously from watching parents engage in their own relationships (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963). If a child never saw his or her parents quarreling, then the child may feel that conflict is not a usual occurrence in a relationship, and therefore, when it does occur it may signal that something is very wrong. On the other hand, a child who sees his or her parents disagree and come to some resolution may come to feel less threatened by conflict because they have seen first hand that it is resolvable. Some- times, however, parents fight and then divorce. What effect might this have on the developing relationship
  • 35. theories of the child? One can envision at least three possible outcomes. First, the child may develop a fa- talistic view of relationships such that, no matter what one does, relationships are bound to bring you sadness and pain. This may be especially the case if the parents fought often and parted badly. A second outcome may result from parents who try to salvage a deteriorating relationship out of genuine care for each other. However, despite their efforts there are certain irreconcilable differences. If these parents part well and have successful remarriages, the child may de- cide that relationships can be successful, but only if the partners are well matched to begin with. A third possible outcome may result from watching parents fight and divorce if the child develops a reactance be- lief. The child may see the difficulties and pain that the parents experienced, and become determined that he or she will not experience the same. This child may energetically seek to find “good matches” and then work to maintain or improve them. Research on implicit theories in other domains has suggested additional origins. Benenson and Dweck (1986) found that trait explanations for behaviors emerged in the social domain earlier than in the aca- demic domain. Their reasoning was that children experience the social domain before the academic do- main. This rationale fits well with the notion that im- plicit theories of intelligence can evolve from rela- tionships with one’s parents and other caregivers. For example, Jane is playing with her friend Susie, who is acting very unpleasantly, and Jane runs to her mother crying about how Susie was treating her. There are at least two reactions that Jane’s mother could offer. One that might reinforce stable, trait-based explana-
  • 36. tions for behavior could involve the following: “Susie is a bad girl for behaving that way; maybe you shouldn’t play with her.” In contrast, one that might reinforce unstable, situation-based explanations for behavior could involve the following: “Why don’t you ask Susie why she acted that way? Maybe she was just having a bad day.” The first response might indicate to Jane that people’s actions are a reflection of their underlying personalities and that they are likely to remain the same over time. Therefore, Jane may come to infer that if a person behaves unpleas- antly, it reflects something fundamental and un- changeable such that future interactions would also be unpleasant. Consequently, Jane may steer clear of forming relationships with people who seem unpleas- ant, even from a single occasion. In contrast, the mother’s second response might give Jane the impres- sion that people’s behavior can be influenced by a particular situation and that they do not necessarily act that way all the time. Therefore, a relationship with someone could be rewarding if one takes the time to learn why the person was behaving that way. A similar process may be at work with regard to ITRs. Now imagine that Jane is dating Tom. Tom calls to can- cel a date at the last minute. Jane’s mother could respond in a way that reinforces the evaluative notion that “He is- n’t good enough for you” or “You can do better than him.” She could also respond in a way that reinforces the cultivative notion that “I’m sure if you ask Tom to ex- plain why he cancelled it, the two of you can get past this.” The modeling of evaluative and cultivative orien- tations is almost certainly not limited to parents, but rather carries over from friends, siblings, role models, and other sources of feedback and modeling that are im- portant to children and adolescents.
  • 37. Also, Dweck (1998) suggested that the origins of mastery and helpless responses could result, in part, from parental responses to failures. Specifically, chil - dren with a mastery response (and its corresponding in- cremental belief) may have had experiences with par- ents who encouraged them to try again after failure, perhaps offering strategies for doing so and praising the efforts they made. Children with a helpless response (and its corresponding entity belief) may have had expe- riences with parents who only praised success and were critical of failed attempts, applying the criticism not just to the attempt itself, but to the child as well. Indeed, in six 51 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS studies, Mueller and Dweck (1998) showed that praise for success had more negative consequences for stu- dents’ achievement motivation than praise for effort. Children who were praised for intelligence cared more about performance goals relative to learning goals com- pared to children who were praised for effort. These children also displayed less task persistence, less task enjoyment, more attributions to low ability, and worse task performance than children who were praised for ef- fort. Further, it seems likely that praise for good perfor- mance may lead to viewing intelligence as a fixed trait. Specifically, children who were praised for intelligence described it as a fixed trait more than children who were praised for hard work, who believed intelligence to be subject to improvement.
  • 38. In romantic relationships, failure may involve get- ting turned down when asking for a date, or getting into an argument with one’s significant other. The pa- rental (and peer) response to these and other relation- ship “milestones” may set the stage for the notion that relationships are either destined to work or they are not, and that with enough effort a relationship with almost anyone can be nurtured. Parents’ reaction on bringing home one’s first date may be one such example. In some cases, parents might proceed to im- mediately evaluate their child’s romantic interest, at- tempting to diagnose the character of the person and the long-term potential for the incipient relationship. In other cases, parents might afford support and en- couragement of almost any new date, and virtually unconditional support of the evolving relationship (despite what the cat dragged in). These reactions may become internalized by the child across repeated salient episodes such that evaluation or cultivation goals become typical of how they view and interact with potential romantic partners. Thus, they may come to gradually diagnose and evaluate potential partners, attempting to proceed as they feel their par- ents and friends (and others whose opinions they value) would. Alternatively, if parents and peers have largely modeled cultivation goals and unconditionally supportive behavior with regard to relationship part- ners, the child may gradually become less concerned by what some people would consider grave weak- nesses or deficits in potential dates. Revision of ITRs Based on Experience Like many other knowledge structures about rela- tionships (e.g., working models, relational schemas, and scripts), ITRs are probably susceptible to revision
  • 39. as a function of salient relationship experiences. For example, research on working models of attachment has shown that involvement in satisfying relationships at one point in time is associated with increased secu- rity at a later time (Hammond & Fletcher, 1991). Simi- larly, relationship breakups have been shown to be as- sociated with change from secure to insecure working models, and avoidant models are susceptible to becom- ing less avoidant on formation of a new relationship (Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994). Changes in relationship knowledge structures have been found to be particu- larly likely when significant events in the social envi - ronment disconfirm existing expectations (Feeney, 1999; Feeney, Noller, & Callan, 1994). In a similar manner, ITRs are also thought to both guide the inter- pretation of relational experiences as well as be some- what sensitive to particularly salient relational events. In this way, one’s belief in destiny could be strength- ened through involvement in a relationship in which everything seems wonderful from the beginning and a positive future seems evident, or through involvement in a relationship that never seemed right from the be- ginning and indeed ended up dissolving. Conversely, one’s destiny belief could be weakened if one inaccu- rately diagnoses a relationship as ideal and yet it fails. One’s growth belief could analogously be strengthened through times in which problems were discussed and resolved and the relationship benefited. Growth belief could be weakened through times when no amount of effort seemed to help. As mentioned earlier, in support of the notion that ITRs are somewhat sensitive to ex- treme or salient relationship experiences, Knee (1998) found lower test–retest correlations for ITRs when par- ticipants had reported a relationship event as their sin- gle most stressful experience over a semester.
  • 40. It is also possible that ITRs can change somewhat within the same relationship. For example, someone who initially has a lower growth belief may become in- volved in a relationship in which they are able to wit- ness the benefits of confronting and resolving issues, thus coming to believe more strongly in growth. In ad- dition, someone who is initially lower in destiny belief may come to believe that their partner is truly “the one” for them, particularly if they remain very satisfied with the relationship. Indeed, one’s partner’s beliefs and be- haviors in the relationship may be a major factor in the extent to which ITRs evolve in the context of any par- ticular relationship. Moreover, it is not yet clear how destiny belief functions in longer-term committed rela- tionships. Although the destiny items seem to be espe- cially relevant to the early stages of relationships, it seems likely that the diagnostic process continues to be active, particularly in the presence of unexpected rela- tionship events, both positive and negative. However, these processes remain to be documented empirically. Domain Specificity Implicit theories in other areas have been shown to be domain specific, and judgments in one domain are best predicted by implicit theories of attributes in the 52 KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY same domain (Chiu et al., 1997; Dweck et al., 1993). Thus, although there is considerable conceptual over-
  • 41. lap between ITRs and implicit theories of other attrib- utes, we would expect that ITRs tend to better predict relationship phenomena. A key difference between im- plicit theories of attributes and ITRs is that the latter are explicitly relational whereas the former are largely individual. Beliefs about relationships may indeed in- fluence (and be influenced by) the relation between partners, including how they view each other and what they come to infer about the relationship as a function of relevant events. In contrast, beliefs about individual attributes probably more often influence one’s own goals, attributions, and behaviors. It may be premature to conclude that beliefs about relationships tend to be more complex than beliefs about individual attributes. However, the thoughts, emotions, and behaviors of people in close relationships tend to be interdependent, such that they mutually influence each other. In addi- tion, previous research has demonstrated that people develop rather elaborate schema about both partners and relationships (Fletcher & Thomas, 1996; Murray & Holmes, 1993). This additional complexity of rela- tionships may require a more vast assessment of the particular beliefs, goals, and attributions that are rele- vant to specific behaviors, and during a specific inter- action sequence. We believe that there may be an additional level of domain-specificity within the realm of relationships. Whereas people may have a global set of beliefs about what makes for good relationships, there may also be particular aspects of relationships that they believe are especially fixed or malleable. For example, Jane may believe that, generally, successful relationships are nur- tured and that they develop through the process of dis- cussing differences and resolving controversy. How- ever, she may also believe that if there is no initial
  • 42. chemistry or physical attraction between partners, the relationship is not meant to be. Thus, if she feels physi - cally attracted to her partner, Jane may believe that all other problems in the relationship can be managed and dealt with. Indeed, previous research has shown that in- dividuals view certain factors as crucial to relationship success including trust, respect, sex, and love (Fletcher & Kininmonth, 1992). Because individuals tend to value particular characteristics in an ideal mate (Fletcher, Simpson, Thomas, & Giles, 1999), they may also have implicit beliefs about the fixedness or mallea- bility of these traits. The importance of particular char - acteristics has not yet been examined with regard to ITRs. People may come to value something because it comes easily (e.g., you either have a good sex life or you do not). People may also come to value those things that they have had to work the hardest to attain (e.g., although my partner and I didn’t hit it off sexually, we have worked to improve that aspect and we are now closer). More important, there may be an interaction between importance ratings of a characteristic and ITRs about the same characteristic in predicting reactions to nega- tive relationship events (such as disagreements) that concern that dimension. For example, if Jane believes that trust in romantic relationships is very important and she also believes that trust is relatively fixed (e.g., part- ners either trust each other or they do not), then Jane may be particularly troubled by a disagreement with her part- ner that concerns or implies a lack of trust. If Jane be- lieves that trust is important but also malleable (e.g., when trust has been violated, it can be restored with ef- fort), then a disagreement about trust may not be as trou- blesome. Thus, a more specific measure of ITRs may provide a more sophisticated model that takes into ac- count specific aspects of relationships.
  • 43. It is also possible that ITRs of specific relationship aspects would mediate the association between general ITRs and various relationship phenomena. The litera- ture on attitudes has shown that more specific attitudes are better predictors of specific behaviors (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). Research on social cognition in close relationships has revealed a similar pattern of findings, albeit with regard to different variables. For example, Fletcher and Fitness (1990) identified ways in which re- lationship cognitions could serve as both proximal and distal variables in predicting various relationship out- comes. In their study, distal cognition involved the thoughts and feelings that partners experienced during an interaction with each other. Proximal cognition in- volved the words used in the interactions that reflected thoughts and beliefs (and not emotions). Fletcher and Fitness (1990) found that couples who had more posi- tive assessments of the quality of their relationship (dis- tal) produced more positive cognitions (proximal) dur- ing interactions with each other. ITRs may operate in a similar manner such that more global ITRs are related to ITRs about specific aspects of relationships, which in turn predict or moderate relationship phenomena. Conclusion ITRs can be a useful framework for conceptualizing how particular beliefs and orientations can influence peoples’ goals, inferences, attributions, emotions, and ultimately, behaviors in romantic relationships. In a re- view of research areas and topics on relationships that need further exploration and development, Aron and Aron (1995) cited (a) an examination of deep, passion- ate relational experiences; (b) the development of a the- ory that links cognitive, motivational, and emotional el-
  • 44. ements to behaviors and outcomes; and (c) a theoretical framework that examines not only how individual phe- nomena influence relational experiences, but how rela- tional experiences can influence the individual. Research on ITRs has begun to address passionate relational experiences including feelings of love, ideal- 53 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS ization, anger, and coping with infidelity. The ITRs’ framework also begins to integrate cognition, motiva- tion, and emotional responses within the relationship. Finally, as mentioned earlier, ITRs may also be subject to situational influences. Thus, ITRs may be somewhat malleable and may change as a function of salient rela- tionship experiences. Although in its incipient stages, the ITRs’ framework affords a unique perspective on how particular social cognitions and motivations may operate together to influence how people approach, perceive, and negotiate their romantic lives. Some em- pirical support already exists, but far more remains to be done. References Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and pre- dicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Three suggestions for increased em-
  • 45. phasis in the study of personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12, 559–562. Aron, A., & Aron, E. (1997). Self-expansion motivation and includ- ing other in the self. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal re- lationships (2nd ed., pp. 251–270). New York: Wiley. Aron, A., Aron, E., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612. Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film- medi- ated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- chology, 66, 3–11. Benenson, J. F., & Dweck, C. S. (1986). The development of trait ex- planations and self-evaluations in the academic and social do- mains. Child Development, 57, 1179–1187. Blais, M. R., Sabourin, S., Boucher, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (1990). Toward a motivational model of couple happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1021–1031. Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Tong, Y., & Fu, H. (1997). Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 923–940. Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and So-
  • 46. cial Psychology, 73, 19–30. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-de- termination in human behavior. New York: Plenum. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol - ogy, 53, 1024–1037. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1990 (pp. 237–288). Lincoln: Uni- versity of Nebraska Press. Diener, E. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). An analysis of learned help- lessness: II. The processing of success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 940–952. Druen, P. B. (1996). Cumulative relationship experience and partner selection strategies: Associations and implications for relation- ship quality. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Louisville, Kentucky. Dweck, C. S. (1991). Self-theories and goals: Their role in motiva- tion, personality, and development. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Ne- braska symposium on motivation (pp. 199–235). Lincoln: Uni- versity of Nebraska Press. Dweck, C. S. (1996). Implicit theories as organizers of goals and be-
  • 47. havior. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychol - ogy of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 69–90). New York: Guilford. Dweck, C. S. (1998). The development of early self- conceptions: Their relevance for motivational processes. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the lifespan (pp. 257–280). New York: Cambridge University Press. Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two per - spectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285. Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories: Indi- vidual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bul - letin, 19, 644–656. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social–cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 25, 109–116. Eidelson, R. J., & Epstein, N. (1982). Cognition and relationship maladjustment: Development of a measure of dysfunctional re- lationship beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol - ogy, 50, 715–720. Erdley, C. A., & Dweck, C. S. (1993). Children’s implicit personality theories as predictors of their social judgments. Child Develop-
  • 48. ment, 64, 863–878. Feeney, J. A. (1999). Adult romantic attachment and couple relation- ships. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach- ment (pp. 355–377). New York: Guilford. Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1994). Attachment style, communication, and satisfaction in the early years of marriage. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment processes in adulthood. (pp. 269–308). London: Jessica Kingsley. Fletcher, G. J. O., & Fitness, J. (1990). Occurrent social cognition in close relationship interaction: The role of proximal and distal vari- ables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 464– 474. Fletcher, G. J. O., & Kininmonth, L. (1992). Measuring relationship beliefs: An individual differences measure. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 371–397. Fletcher, G. J. O., Simpson, J. A., Thomas, G., & Giles, L. (1999). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 72–89. Fletcher, G. J. O., & Thomas, G. (1996). Close relationship lay theo- ries: Their structure and function. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fit- ness (Eds.), Knowledge structures and interaction in close rela- tionships: A social psychological approach (pp. 3–24). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • 49. Franiuk, R., Cohen, D., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2001). Implicit theories of relationships: Implications for relationship satisfaction and longevity. Manuscript submitted for publication. Gervey, B. M., Chiu, Y., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Differen- tial use of person information in decisions about guilt versus in- nocence: The role of implicit theories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 17–27. Goetz, T. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). Learned helplessness in social situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 246–255. Hammond, J. R., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1991). Attachment styles and relationship satisfaction in the development of close relation- ships. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 20, 56–62. Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. (1986). A theory and method of love. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 392–402. Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and con- scious experience. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Hand- book of self-determination research (pp. 87–100). New York: University of Rochester Press. Hodgins, H. S., Koestner, R., & Duncan, N. (1996). On the compati- bility of autonomy and relatedness. Personality and Social Psy- chology Bulletin, 22, 227–237.
  • 50. Hong, Y., Chiu, C., & Dweck, C. S. (1995). Implicit theories of intel- ligence: Reconsidering the role of confidence in achievement 54 KNEE, PATRICK, & LONSBARY motivation. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-es- teem. New York: Plenum. Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588–599. Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Rela- tionships, 1, 123–142. Knee, C. R. (1998). Implicit theories of relationships: Assessment and prediction of romantic relationship initiation, coping, and longevity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 360–370. Knee, C. R., Nanayakkara, A., Vietor, N. A., Neighbors, C., & Pat- rick, H. (2001). Implicit theories of relationships: Who cares if romantic partners are less than ideal? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 808–819.
  • 51. Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., Vietor, N. A., Nanayakkara, A., & Neigh- bors, C. (2002). Self-determination as growth motivation in ro- mantic relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- tin, 28, 609–619 Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., Vietor, N. A., & Neighbors, C. (2002). Im- plicit theories of relationships as moderators of the link be - tween conflict and relationship quality. Manuscript submitted for publication. Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1996). Causality orientations and the disappearance of the self-serving bias. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 76–87. Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1998). A nondefensive personality: Autonomy and control as moderators of defensive coping and self-handicapping. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 115–130. Koestner, R. & Zuckerman, M. (1994). Causality orientations, fail- ure, and achievement. Journal of Personality, 62, 321–346. Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Modes of social thought: Implicit theories and social understanding. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psy- chology (pp. 179–202). New York: Guilford. Lydon, J. (1999). Commitment and adversity: A reciprocal relation.
  • 52. In J. M. Adams & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Handbook on interper- sonal commitment and relationship stability. (pp. 193–203). New York: Plenum. Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52. Murray, S. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1993). Seeing virtues in faults: Negativity and the transformation of interpersonal narratives in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol - ogy, 65, 707–722. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfac - tion in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 79–98. Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for struc- ture: Individual differences in the desire for simple struc- ture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113–131. Patrick, H., & Knee, C. R. (2002). Implicit theories of relationships and motivation: Buffering the negative consequences of extrin- sic relationship motivation. Manuscript in preparation. Patrick, H., Knee, C. R., & Lonsbary, C. (2001). Implicit theories of relationships and responses to romantic infidelity. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • 53. Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construct of per- sonal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357. Rusbult, C. E., Zembrodt, I. M., & Gunn, L. K. (1982). Exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect: Responses to dissatisfaction in romantic involvements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1230–1242. Ruvolo, A. P., & Rotondo, J. L. (1998). Diamonds in the rough: Im- plicit personality theories and views of the partner and self. Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 750–58. Sprecher, S., & Metts, S. (1989). Development of the ‘Romantic Be- liefs Scale’ and examination of the effects of gender and gen- der-role orientation. Journal of Social and Personal Relation- ships, 6, 387–411. Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z. R., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss and weight-loss maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 115–126. 55 IMPLICIT THEORIES OF RELATIONSHIPS
  • 54. What Do You Do When Things Go Right? The Intrapersonal and Interpersonal Benefits of Sharing Positive Events Shelly L. Gable University of California, Los Angeles Harry T. Reis University of Rochester Emily A. Impett University of California, Los Angeles Evan R. Asher University of Rochester Four studies examined the intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of seeking out others when good things happen (i.e., capitalization). Two studies showed that communicating personal positive events with others was associated with increased daily positive affect and well-being, above and beyond the impact of the positive event itself and other daily events. Moreover, when others were perceived to respond actively and constructively (and not passively or destructively) to capitalization attempts, the benefits were further enhanced. Two studies found that close relationships in which one’s partner typically responds to capitalization attempts enthusiastically were associated with higher relationship well-being (e.g., intimacy, daily marital satisfaction). The results are discussed in terms of the theoretical and empirical importance of understanding how people “cope” with positive events, cultivate positive emotions, and enhance social bonds.
  • 55. Good, the more communicated, more abundant grows. —John Milton, Paradise Lost, Book V The puzzle of well-being has many pieces. One piece that has been the focus of much research is how people maintain or restore their well-being in the face of negative events or stressors. Re- search has often asked, “What can people do when things go wrong?” and useful answers to this question have come from studies on appraisals (e.g., Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter, Delongis, & Gruen, 1986; Lazarus, 1991), coping (e.g., Bolger, 1990; Carver & Scheier, 1994), and rumination (e.g., Nolen- Hoeksema, 1996, 1998). These and other studies have demon- strated that people routinely turn to others for support in times of stress, be it in the face of everyday stressors (e.g., Harlow & Cantor, 1995) or major life events (e.g., Bolger & Eckenrode, 1991), and that the availability of social support has clear benefits for the support-seeker’s health and well-being (e.g., Sarason, Sara- son, & Gurung, 1997; Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996). Furthermore, people commonl y cite the possibility of receiving social support, if and when needed, as one of the major benefits of close relationships (e.g., Cunningham & Barbee, 2000). Without doubt, the processes involved in utilizing social relations to cope with negative events are central to understanding intrapersonal and interpersonal well-being. Nevertheless, this article suggests that another, complementary piece of the puzzle has been largely overlooked: the intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of
  • 56. seeking out others when good things happen. Although the social sharing of good news has received relatively little attention, research on responses to good fortune has not been entirely lacking. For example, in two daily experience studies, Langston (1994) found that when people shared the news of a positive event with others or celebrated the event in some way, they experienced greater positive affect, beyond increases associ- ated with the valence of the positive event itself. He called this capitalization, a term that we adopt to refer to the process of informing another person about the occurrence of a personal positive event and thereby deriving additional benefit from it. Langston’s findings complement research by Bryant (1989), who found that individual differences in the self-reported ability to savor positive events were correlated with subjective well - being. These studies also complement research by Tesser and his col - leagues on the extended self-evaluation maintenance model (e.g., Beach & Tesser, 1995; Tesser, 2000), which demonstrates that reflection processes—that is, the ability to share in a partner’s success— contribute to emotional well-being and relationship sat- isfaction. The present research seeks to extend these studies by investigating the intrapersonal and interpersonal outcomes associ- ated with capitalizing on positive events. Paralleling existing re- search on stressors and social support, we examined both the process of seeking a response to one’s good fortune and the impact of perceiving the provision of a positive response.
  • 57. Shelly L. Gable and Emily A. Impett, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; Harry T. Reis and Evan R. Asher, Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester. Study 2 was supported by a Barth–Crapsey award from the University of Rochester, Study 3 was collected as part of Shelly L. Gable’s doctoral dissertation (Gable, 2000, Study 2), and Study 4 was supported by a grant from the Templeton Foundation/Positive Psycholo gy Network awarded to Shelly L. Gable. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shelly L. Gable, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, 4560 Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563. E- mail: [email protected] Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 87, No. 2, 228 –245 0022-3514/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.228 228 Positive Events and Well-Being
  • 58. Traditionally, researchers interested in health and well-being have examined the psychological and physical consequences of stressors and other negative events (e.g., Cohen & Wills, 1985; Hobfoll, 1998; Taylor, 1991). Recent theorizing has suggested that processes linked to positive events may have independent and important associations with well-being and health (e.g., Reis & Gable, 2003; Ryff & Singer, 1998). For example, Lewinsohn and Graf (1973) found that everyday pleasant events were associated with decreases in depressive symptoms (Zautra, Schultz, & Reich, 2000). More recently, Nezlek and Gable (2001) found that the occurrence of minor positive events was associated with increases in daily self-esteem and perceived control and decreases in daily depressive cognitions, above and beyond the effect of negative events. That positive events may have independent effects on well - being, over and above the impact of negative events, is consistent with the extensive research demonstrating the independence of appetitive and aversive processes (Gable & Reis, 2001; Higgins, 1997). This independence has been most clearly established in studies of emotion, in which the processes regulating positive and negative emotions are functionally independent (Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999). For example, Gable, Reis, and Elliot (2000) showed that everyday positive events were associated with in- creased positive affect but not with changes in negative affect. Other studies showing that positive, pleasant, or desirable events are differentially related to positive but not negative affect
  • 59. include L. A. Clark and Watson (1988) and David, Green, Martin, and Suls (1997). If positive events are uniquely and distinguishably associ- ated with positive emotions, then it becomes important to inves- tigate how people “cope”1 with positive events, inasmuch as these processes may differ from the processes involved in coping with negative events (which include social support). In fact, positive emotions appear to have important consequences for health and well-being over and above negative emotions, as recent studies have shown (Salovey, Rothman, Detweiler, & Steward, 2000). For example, Pettit, Kline, Gencoz, and Gencoz (2001) found that positive affectivity but not negative affectivity predicted self- reported health. Danner, Snowdon, and Friesen (2001) found that positive emotional expression in autobiographical statements in- versely predicted mortality 6 decades later. Similarly, Harker and Keltner (2001) showed that the expression of positive emotion in college yearbook photos predicted well-being 30 years later. In a recent and influential article, Fredrickson (1998) suggested that positive emotions have received insufficient attention because they do not fit existing models of emotion and emotion regulation. To fill this gap, Fredrickson (1998, 2001) has proposed a “broaden-and-build” theory of positive emotions. This theory pos- its that the function of positive emotions is to broaden an individ- ual’s thought-action repertoire (that is, to facilitate a broader
  • 60. scope of cognition, attention, and action) and to build resources (that is, to enhance the individual’s physical, intellectual, and social re - sources). Preliminary evidence supports this model. For example, Fredrickson and Joiner (2002) found that broad-minded coping (i.e., flexible cognitive responses to stressors) was associated with increased positive emotions and an upward spiral toward increased well-being. Empirical evidence for the broaden-and-build model of positive emotions has predominantly focused on the broadening component of the theory (e.g., Fredrickson & Branigan, 2002; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). The capitalization process, we sug- gest, is central to the process of using positive emotions to build resources. Capitalization Builds Personal and Social Resources We propose that capitalization—telling others about positive events in one’s life—is likely to generate additional positive affect, over and above positive affect associated with the event itself. There are several possible mechanisms for such an effect. First, sharing a positive event with others requires retelling the event, which creates an opportunity for reliving and reexperiencing the event. Furthermore, the communicative act may involve rehearsal and elaboration, both of which seem likely to prolong and enhance the experience by increasing its salience and accessibility in
  • 61. mem- ory. If so, positive events that are communicated to others should be remembered better than positive events not communicated. Another potential mechanism is more interpersonal in nature. Sharing events with others may build social resources by fostering positive social interactions, which are reinforcing in and of them- selves (Gable & Reis, 2001), and thereby strengthening relation- ships. Furthermore, sharing good news initiates an interaction that may allow individuals to perceive that others are pleased for them, a process likely to boost self-esteem (e.g., Beach & Tesser, 1995; Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988) and more generally to facilitate positive reflected appraisals—perceiving oneself positively in the eyes of the other (Baumeister, 1998; Leary & Baumeister, 2000; Shrauger & Schoeneman, 1979). This latter effect requires, of course, that the listener’s response be experienced as positive — that is, as recognizing and validating the good news (Reis & Patrick, 1996). More destructive responses (for example, pointing out the downside of a positive event or minimizing its importance) may well dampen or even reverse the positive affect produced by one’s good fortune. Thus, we propose that the personal and social resources to be gained from capitalization depend in an important way on the ability to perceive the other’s response as positive.
  • 62. This process provides one central mechanism, we suggest, for the upward spiral of positive affect and well-being described by Fredrickson (1998). Capitalization and Responsiveness to the Self As proposed above, capitalization may depend significantly on the ability to perceive the partner’s response as supporting one’s good fortune. People are unlikely to share their good news if they anticipate rejection, defensiveness, or an otherwise unappreciative response. Supportive responses suggest that an interaction partner is pleased for one, and this may be diagnostic not only of the partner’s positive evaluation of the event itself but also of the relationship. One reason for this is that an enthusiastic response may indicate that the partner is “basking in reflected glory” (Cial- dini et al., 1976)—feeling that the good fortune rubs off to some 1 Although the use of the word cope has traditionally referred to how people deal with negative events, we extend this meaning to responses to positive events. 229CAPITALIZING ON POSITIVE EVENTS extent on themselves, a process that at least nominally imputes a relationship between self and partner. This tendency is likely to
  • 63. be enhanced the closer the relationship (Beach & Tesser, 1995). Aron, Aron, Tudor, and Nelson (1991) defined closeness as “including the other in the self,” a process that implies that a partner’s good fortune may psychologically be experienced as one’s own in a close relationship (over and above whatever material conse- quences a partner’s good fortune may have for the self). Thus, a partner’s response of genuine pleasure to one’s capitalization at- tempts may indicate a sense of connection. Research on empathy and emotional contagion, although not usually focused on positive events, points to a similar conclusion (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). Similarly, a constructive response may engender processes cen- tral to the intimacy process. As Reis and Shaver (1988) defined that process, disclosure of self-relevant material fosters intimacy when the partner’s response is experienced as understanding, val- idating, and caring. The partner’s response is critical to this pro- cess because it indicates that he or she is aware of central aspects of the self (which include goal attainment, positive affects, and the personally relevant activities that foster them) and is willing and able to be empathic and supportive. In fact, as Reis, Clark, and Holmes (2004) have recently suggested, “perceived responsive- ness to the self” is a central idea behind numerous self-relevant interpersonal processes, such as attachmen t theory (Mikulincer
  • 64. & Shaver, 2003), rejection sensitivity (Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998), the construction of a sense of felt security (Murray & Holmes, 1993), mutual cyclical growth (Rusbult, Olsen, Davis, & Hannon, 2001), and communal caring (M. S. Clark, Fitness, & Brissette, 2001). Typically, however, perceived responsiveness is conceptualized and investigated in terms of responses either to negative events or to nominally private information whose public disclosure might leave one vulnerable. We propose that the con- cept of responsiveness may also apply to personal positive events by revealing whether or not a partner is pleased for one, an act that incorporates evaluations of the event, its self-relevance, and the relationship. Overview of the Research and Hypotheses The present research was designed to examine the intrapersonal and interpersonal correlates of sharing positive events with others (i.e., capitalization). We first sought to establish that the sharing of good news has beneficial intrapersonal effects. Study 1 was a daily experience study in which participants reported on the sharing (or not) of positive events and their daily affect and well -being. On the basis of previous research and Fredrickson’s (1998) broaden-