SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Would you be happier if you moved more? Physical activity
focusing illusion
Lukasz D. Kaczmarek1
& Maciej Behnke1
& Jolanta Enko1
& Agnieszka Hoffman1
& Marcin Kiciński1
& Jakub Paruszewski1
&
Michał Szabat1
& Stephan U. Dombrowski2
# The Author(s) 2019
Abstract
Research shows that individuals who are more physically active also report greater happiness. However, subjective well-being is
prone to cognitive biases. For instance, people overrate the influence of single factors (e.g., money) on their happiness; a
phenomenon termed the focusing illusion. In this study, we examined whether the relationship between physical activity and
subjective well-being is stronger when individuals focus on physical activity explicitly compared to individuals with no specific
focus. We experimentally manipulated the physical activity focus by varying the order of scales administration. Participants (N =
200) completed questionnaires that measured physical activity and subjective well-being placed in separate envelopes and
provided in a random order. We found that individuals with higher levels of vigorous physical activity were more satisfied with
life regardless of the order of scale presentation (no focusing effect). However, we found evidence of a possible focusing illusion
for moderate-intensity physical activity. Individuals with higher levels of moderate-intensity physical activity reported higher
subjective well-being when they were asked about physical activity first but not when they reported their well-being unaware of
the upcoming physical activity questions. Thus, subjective well-being judgments can be biased by a prior focus on moderate-
intensity physical activity. The order of scale administration when assessing subjective well-being should be carefully considered.
Keywords Physical activity . Satisfaction with life . Positive emotions . Negative emotions . Focusing illusion . Happiness
Introduction
Individuals tend to overrate the influence of any single factor
on their well-being; a phenomenon termed ‘focusing illusion’
(Schkade and Kahneman 1998). Evidence shows that people
can be biased in their estimations of the extent to which their
well-being is determined by income (Aknin et al. 2009;
Kahneman et al. 2006), romantic relationships (Schwarz
et al. 1991), health (Smith et al. 2006), or physical appearance
(Kaczmarek et al. 2016). This bias may impact the decisions
that people make to increase their well-being. For instance,
individuals may pursue activities which fail to maximize their
well-being, such as choosing a highly paid job over a self-
concordant job (Al-Zoubi 2012). However, not all domains
of life are prone to the focusing illusion (Oishi et al. 2003)
and some factors induce partial focusing illusion, i.e., the in-
dividuals overrate the importance of otherwise significant fac-
tors (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). As focusing illusions can poten-
tially have an impact on people’s lives, it is vital to identify
which factors trigger focusing illusion.
In this study, we examined the focusing effects of physical
activity ratings on subjective well-being assessments. Physical
activity has often been presented as a precursor to a happy life
(e.g., Brown et al. 2015; Hogan et al. 2015). Yet, little is
known to what extent physically active people report higher
well-being which might be biased by focusing effects.
Regardless of whether physical activity promotes well-being
or not, people who are more physically active are healthier
(Kyu et al. 2016) and live longer (Arem et al. 2015).
However, identifying whether a focusing illusion regarding
physical activity exists may be important. Individuals who
prioritize increases in well-being might pursue more effective
happiness-boosting cognitive activities (Bolier et al. 2013).
* Lukasz D. Kaczmarek
Lkacz@amu.edu.pl
1
Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickiewicz University, 89
Szamarzewskiego Street, PL-60-568 Poznan, Poland
2
University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, Canada
Current Psychology
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00315-9
Due to the focusing illusion, individuals who initiate physical
activity to enhance their subjective well-being rather than fit-
ness can be less likely to pursue other potentially more effec-
tive happiness-boosting interventions.
Moreover, some people may have unrealistic expectations
about the extent to which physical activity increases their
well-being and give up once they notice that these expecta-
tions are not met. New more nuanced insights on the relation-
ship between physical activity and well-being are imperative
to inform individual lifestyle decisions and interventions as
well as to support public health policies (e.g., World Health
Organization 2010). Additionally, some approaches to the
study of focusing illusion are also critical from the methodo-
logical standpoint (Schwarz 1999; Smith et al. 2006). The
focusing illusion can, for instance, explain some questionnaire
order effects (Schwarz et al. 1991) thereby informing our un-
derstanding of cognitive aspects of survey design.
Focusing Illusion
Brief and broad evaluations of well-being (e.g., overall satis-
faction with life or the number of emotions experienced dur-
ing the last days) depend on information that comes to mind
when individuals think about their life (Schwarz and Strack
2016). The speed and ease of these evaluations can come at
the cost of their accuracy. When individuals focus their atten-
tion on a specific life domain, they may overestimate its im-
portance other relevant information might be missed to form
an accurate judgment (Schwarz and Strack 2016; Oishi et al.
2003). From a happiness perspective, human information pro-
cessing produces can lead to overestimated ratings of the im-
portance of any single factor on well-being (Schkade and
Kahneman 1998). The focusing illusion is an instance of bi-
ased judgment because an entire object (such as one’s life) is
evaluated with attention focused on a specific subset of this
category, e.g., physical appearance (Kaczmarek et al. 2016).
Focusing effects have been evidenced using a variety of
methodologies. For instance, in one study people were asked
to report how much time they spent in a bad mood as well as
how much time people in different circumstances, such as
with high or low income, spent in a bad mood (Kahneman
et al. 2006). Participants overestimated bad mood for individ-
uals in unfavorable life circumstances compared to real-life
reports. Similarly, in another study participants reported their
income, their life satisfaction and predicted satisfaction of oth-
er individuals covering a wide range of income (Aknin et al.
2009). The results suggested that wealthier individuals tended
to overestimate dissatisfaction with life among less wealthy
individuals compared to the actual self-reports provided by
these individuals. In a well-being survey among patients di-
agnosed with Parkinson’s disease, Smith et al. (2006) in-
formed half of the participants that the survey was conducted
by a medical center and its topic was related to Parkinson’s
disease. The other group was asked the same questions but
was informed that they participated in a general population
survey conducted by the university. The patients that explic-
itly focused on their health showed a higher correlation be-
tween health and well-being measures compared to the group
with no explicit focus on health. Furthermore, when both
groups received health-related questions first, the correlation
between health and well-being was high regardless of the
study context presented in the introduction. In a similar study,
the order of scales measuring body satisfaction and life satis-
faction was manipulated (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). In the fo-
cusing group that reported their body satisfaction first, there
was a higher correlation between body satisfaction and life
satisfaction than in the group that reported their life satisfac-
tion first; unaware of the upcoming body satisfaction ques-
tions. This suggests that a momentary focus on the body can
influence subsequent judgments of well-being.
Subjective Well-Being and Physical Activity
There are several approaches to the conceptualization and the
measurement of well-being (Kaczmarek 2018). Of these ap-
proaches, subjective well-being has been among the most
popular due to its versatility and ability to cover a wide range
of experience and evaluations in different contexts (Diener
et al. 2018). The subjective well-being theory is particularly
relevant to the study of the focusing illusion because it covers
the evaluations of the quality of a person’s life from that per-
son’s perspective. Subjective well-being consists of three
components: positive emotions, negative emotions, and satis-
faction with life (Diener 2000). Emotions are short-lived psy-
chological-physiological phenomena that facilitate adaptation
to changing environmental demands (Levenson 2003).
Positive emotions are associated with pleasure whereas nega-
tive emotions are associated with displeasure or pain (Gable
and Harmon-Jones 2010). Furthermore, positive emotions
pull individuals toward specific objects, actions, and ideas,
whereas negative emotions push away from others. Positive
and negative emotions are distinct at a functional level with
negative emotions facilitating efficient use of psycho-social
coping resources and positive emotions helping to build new
resources (Fredrickson 2001). A happy individual has an
abundance of positive emotions and little negative emotions.
Emotions, in turn, influence global evaluations of life and
determine whether individuals endorse their life (Kim-Prieto
et al. 2005). These global evaluations form satisfaction with
life which is a cognitive, judgmental process of life as a whole
according to individual criteria of what is desirable.
The link between physical activity and various facets of
well-being such as satisfaction with life or positive emotions
has been established in the literature. Physical activity has
been related to life satisfaction and positive emotions among
employees (Edmunds et al. 2013), satisfaction with life among
Curr Psychol
adolescents (Valois et al. 2004), or personal well-being in
cyclists (Lovretic et al. 2013). Individuals who participate in
sports also report higher life satisfaction compared to individ-
uals who prefer sedentary leisure activities predictive (Brown
et al. 2015). Physical activity has also been related to positive
affect (Mata et al. 2012). The relationship between physical
activity and well-being is bi-directional as indicated by studies
where baseline psychological well-being (a compound of
pleasure and positive functioning) predicted future long-term
physical activity outcomes (Kim et al. 2016).
Some findings regarding physical activity and well-being
have been inconsistent. Maher et al. (2012) found that al-
though satisfaction with life was influenced by daily varia-
tions in physical activity, there was no relationship between
satisfaction with life and trait-like physical activity. Physical
activity was also unrelated to satisfaction with life among
student nurses (Hawker 2012). There are also inconsistent
results regarding physical activity and negative affect with
no effect in some studies (e.g., Mata et al. 2012), whereas
others find significant (e.g., Petruzzello et al. 1991), or mixed
results (e.g., Hogan et al. 2015). No research has examined
whether focusing effects exist in the domain of physical activ-
ity which may contribute towards explaining the inconsistent
evidence on the link between physical activity and various
facets of well-being.
The Present Study
Building upon prior work on the focusing illusion (Kaczmarek
et al. 2016; Schkade and Kahneman 1998; Smith et al. 2006)
we hypothesized that the relationship between physical activity
and subjective well-being would be stronger for individuals
explicitly focused on physical activity compared to individuals
with no specific focus. Specifically, we hypothesized that when
physical activity is measured before well-being, the correlation
between physical activity and well-being would be stronger
compared to the reverse order. Thus, the order of the adminis-
tration of scales measuring both constructs would moderate the
physical activity and well-being link.
Method
Participants
Participants were 200 undergraduate students aged 18 to
45 years (M = 23.55, SD = 5.00). There was a similar number
of women (n = 104; 52.0%) and men (n = 93; 46.5%) in the
sample. Three participants who did not report their gender were
excluded from the analyses as gender was one of the predictors
in the model. Recruitment included researchers inviting stu-
dents before classes to participate in a psychology study. The
experimental group and control group included 98 and 102
participants, respectively. Based on prior research on focusing
illusion testing similar models (Kaczmarek et al. 2016), we
expected small effect sizes for the focusing effects. Given that
outcome variables in the model were planned to regress upon
eight predictors (including the interaction terms), power analy-
ses with G*Power 3.1.9.2 suggested that a total sample size of
159 would be the minimum required to detect effects of f2
=
0.10 with an α = .05 and power of .80. Missing data (0.2%)
were determined to be random via Little’s (1988) test, χ2
(95) =
91.88, p = .57, and were imputed using Expectation-
Maximization algorithm in SPSS 21. The study was conducted
by the guidelines of the Institutional Ethics Committee.
Participation in the study was voluntary, and each participant
signed informed consent. Participants remained anonymous
and received no incentives for taking part in this study.
Measures
We measured subjective well-being using a three-factor ap-
proach that accounted for positive emotions, negative emo-
tions, and satisfaction with life (Kim-Prieto et al. 2005).
Life satisfaction was measured with the 5-item Satisfaction
with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al. 1985). The SWLS in-
cludes items such as ‘The conditions of my life are excellent.’
The answering scale ranges from 1 ‘strongly disagree’ to 7
‘strongly agree.’ The scale was internally consistent with
Cronbach’s α = .85.
Emotions were measured with The Scale of Positive and
Negative Experience (Diener et al. 2010). This scale measures
positive feelings (e.g., happy) and negative feelings (e.g., sad)
with six items for each subscale. Participants reported how
often they had been experiencing each of these feelings over
the last 7 days using a scale from 1 ‘very rarely or never’ to 5
‘very often or always’ (Cronbach’s α = .87 for positive feel-
ings and α = .85 for negative feelings).
Physical activity was measured with the short version of
the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ,
Craig et al. 2003). This questionnaire asks four groups of
items regarding vigorous physical activity, moderate physical
activity, walking, and sitting during the last 7 days at work, as
part of house and yard work, to get from place to place, and for
recreation, exercise, or sport. Vigorous activity is defined as
taking hard physical effort that makes someone breathe much
harder than normal such as heavy lifting, digging, aerobics, or
fast bicycling for at least 10 min at a time. Moderate physical
activity is defined as a physical effort that makes someone
breathe somewhat harder than normal such as carrying light
loads, bicycling at a regular pace for at least 10 min, etc., and
does not include walking. For each level of physical activity
(e.g., vigorous) participants report how many days they did
this type of activities, and how much time they usually spend
doing this type of activity on one of those days. Participants
also reported if they were not able to provide a reliable
Curr Psychol
estimate of this type of activity. The index of physical activity
in each intensity is calculated as the product of the number of
days and the average daily time. This product is further
weighted by estimated metabolic equivalent energy expendi-
ture (×8 for vigorous activity and × 4 for moderate activity).
Items regarding vigorous and moderate physical activity were
used in the analyses. Items regarding walking and sitting were
not examined as 25.5% participants reported that they were
not able to estimate their walking and sitting time compared to
only 3% and 4.5% for moderate and vigorous physical activity
respectively. The self-report IPAQ has excellent test-retest re-
liability and correlates with objective measures of physical
activity via accelerometry (Craig et al. 2003). Log transforma-
tion of each physical activity metric was used in the analyses
to adjust the positive skew of the initial data.
Procedure
We placed questionnaires for physical activity, and well-being
in separate envelopes marked A or B. For a random half of the
participants, we placed the well-being questionnaires into the
envelope A and the physical activity questionnaire in the en-
velope B (no focus group). For the other half of participants,
we placed the physical activity questionnaire into envelope A,
and the well-being questionnaires into envelope B (focusing
group). We asked participants to complete the questionnaire
placed in the envelope A first, place it back into the envelope,
open envelope B, and complete the other questionnaire.
Analytical Strategy
We performed multiple regression analyses using SPSS 23.0
(Armonk, NY) and PROCESS 2.16 macro (Hayes 2013) with
two physical activity outcomes, gender, scale order, and their
interactions as predictors and three well-being components as
outcome variables. In testing this hypothesis, we controlled for
gender as physical activity differs between women and men.
Gender (0 = man, 1 = woman) and scale order (0 = well-being
first, 1 = physical activity first) were entered as binary predic-
tors. We used R2
coefficient to calculate the amount of variance
in outcome variables explained by the predictors.
Results
We presented descriptive statistics and correlations between
study variables in Table 1. On average, participants were neither
satisfied nor dissatisfied with their lives, experienced positive
emotions often, and negative emotions rarely. Correlations
showed that individuals who reported their well-being first
(no focus condition), reported higher levels of positive affect
and higher estimates of vigorous physical activity compared to
those who reported their physical activity first.
Individuals with higher levels of vigorous physical activity
reported higher levels of satisfaction with life as indicated by a
main effect of vigorous activity on satisfaction with life
(Table 2). Furthermore, a significant interaction term (moder-
ate physical activity × scale order) revealed that moderate
physical activity had a stronger relationship with life satisfac-
tion when individuals were asked about physical activity first
(Fig. 1). The interaction of moderate physical activity with the
scale order showed a significant relationship between moder-
ate physical activity and satisfaction with life when physical
activity was reported first, b = .28, SE b = .12, p < .05 for men
and b = .34, SE b = .13, p < .01 for women, and a non-
significant relationship when participants were asked about
their life first, b = −.09, SE b = .14, p > .05 for men and b =
−.03, SE b = .14, p > .05 for women. This model explained
11.1% of the variance in satisfaction with life, R2
= .111, F
(8, 177) = 2.77, p < .01. The interaction representing the fo-
cusing illusion explained additional 3.2% of the variance,
ΔR2
= .032, F (1, 177) = 6.33, p < .05.
We found a similar focusing effect regarding moderate
physical activity and positive emotions. There was a signifi-
cant interaction between scale order and moderate physical
activity in respect to positive emotions. This interaction com-
prised a significant relationship between moderate physical
activity and positive emotions when physical activity was re-
ported first, b = .40, SE b = .12, p < .01 for men and b = .44,
SE b = .13, p < .01 for women, and a non-significant relation-
ship when participants were asked about their life first,
b = .05, SE b = .14, p > .05 for men and b = .09, SE b = .12,
p > .05 for women. This model explained 16.3% of the vari-
ance in positive emotions, R2
= .163, F (8, 177) = 4.31,
p < .001. The interaction representing focusing illusion ex-
plained additional 2.6% of the variance, ΔR2
= .026, F (1,
177) = 5.59, p < .05.
Finally, we found no focusing effects for physical activity
regarding negative emotions. We also did not observe any
significant gender differences.
Discussion
In the present study, we examined whether the link between
physical activity and subjective well-being is affected by a
focusing illusion. We tested if a temporal evaluative focus
(experimentally induced) on physical activity influenced judg-
ments regarding three components of subjective well-being.
We found that the positive relationship between vigorous
physical activity and satisfaction with life was significant
and showed no evidence of any focusing effect. However,
we found evidence of a focusing effect for moderate-
intensity physical activity regarding satisfaction with life and
positive but not negative affect.
Curr Psychol
Individuals who self-reported higher levels of vigorous
physical activity were more likely to report higher levels of
life satisfaction regardless of the scale order administration.
We found that the psychological effects of vigorous activity
were present even when individuals did not focus their atten-
tion explicitly on this sphere of life. Moreover, there were no
additional benefits or losses for subjective well-being when
individuals were encouraged to focus on their past intense
physical output. This observation indicates an unbiased rela-
tionship between vigorous physical activity and satisfaction
with life that is free from focusing illusions. A partial focusing
illusion occurs when an explicit focus enhances a weaker, yet
significant, relationship between a factor and well-being
(Kaczmarek et al. 2016). This finding means that individuals
who are physically active in the range of high intensities are
also more satisfied with their life. It corroborates previous
research regarding life-enhancing outcomes of physical activ-
ity (e.g., Edmunds et al. 2013). Yet, we found no relationship
between vigorous physical activity and positive or negative
affect. The absence of a relationship of physical activity with
negative affect is also congruent with some previous studies
(Mata et al. 2012). However, no relationship between physical
activity and positive affect was not in line with our hypothe-
ses. A possible explanation for this finding is that satisfaction
with life is a more reliable overarching construct that forms
global judgments based on cognitive as well as affective in-
formation (Kim-Prieto et al. 2005). The unique effects of high-
intensity physical activity may result from endorphins secre-
tion that is higher for high-intensity activities such as running
or weight lifting (McGowan et al. 1991).
Table 1 Descriptive statistics and
correlations among study
variables
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Vigorous PA
2. Moderate PA .29**
3. Satisfaction with life .17*
.14
4. Positive affect .11 .21**
.68**
5. Negative affect .03 −.03 −.40**
−.56**
6. Age −.05 .13 −.01 −.02 −.21**
7. Gender −.01 .05 −.05 −.14 .23**
−.19**
8. Scale order −.21**
−.08 −.05 .17*
−.12 −.12
M 1433.50 614.43 4.06 3.62 2.34 23.55
SD 2714.46 1010.24 1.13 0.79 0.80 5.00
MLn 5.03 4.85 – – – –
SDLn 2.64 2.16 – – – –
Scale order coded as 0 = well-being first (no focusing), 1 = physical activity first (focusing). Gender coded as 0 =
man, 1 = women. PA Physical activity. Ln Log transformed data
*
p < .05. **
p < .01
Table 2 Physical activity,
subjective well-being, and
focusing effects
Life satisfaction Positive emotions Negative emotions
β SE β β SE β β SE β
Vigorous PA .41**
.13 .15 .13 .04 .14
Moderate PA −.09 .14 .05 .14 .06 .14
Scale order .46 .32 .25 .32 −.14 .33
Gender .22 .31 −.03 .31 .53 .32
Vigorous PA × Scale order −.09 .06 .04 .06 −.02 .06
Vigorous PA × Gender −.08 .06 −.05 .06 −.01 .06
Moderate PA × Scale order .37*
.15 .35*
.15 −.19 .15
Moderate PA × Gender .06 .15 .05 .15 −.07 .15
Scale order coded as 0 = well-being first (no focusing), 1 = physical activity first (focusing). Gender coded as 0 =
man, 1 = women. PA Physical activity
*
p < .05. **
p < .01
Curr Psychol
We found that moderate-intensity physical activity
produced the focusing effects for satisfaction with life
and positive emotions. Men and women who self-
reported greater moderate physical activity were more
likely to also reported greater satisfaction with life and
positive emotions only when they were asked about
physical activity first. In contrast, there was no relation-
ship between moderate physical activity and satisfaction
with life or positive emotions, when subjective partici-
pants reported their well-being before reporting their
physical activity. This phenomenon suggests a full fo-
cusing illusion that suggests that people are likely to
overrate the role of moderate physical activity in their
well-being estimates. These findings add to the literature
on the prevalence of biases in well-being judgments
(Schkade and Kahneman 1998; Smith et al. 2006).
Finally, we observed that the scale order influenced
reported levels of vigorous physical activity and positive
emotions. Individuals who reported their well-being first
had higher levels of positive affect and higher estimates
of vigorous physical activity compared to those who
reported their physical activity first. This effect suggests
that reflecting on physical activity decreased positive
affect; a variable reported later. Our ad hoc hypothesis
would be that this difference might result from partici-
pants low on physical activity who might dampen their
enthusiasm after reflecting their physical inactivity.
Moreover, participants who reported their well-being
first reported more physical activity. One ad hoc hy-
pothesis explaining this effect would be that well-being
primed less critical or less accurate behavioral judg-
ments. In summary, these scale order effects serve as a
new example which is worthwhile to control for within
future surveys to minimize the bias in subjective esti-
mates of affective experience and behavior (Schwarz
1999).
This study has several potential practical implications.
First, vigorous and moderate physical activity is of primary
interest to the public health agencies due to its well-
documented impact on health (World Health Organization
2010). The current study provides new evidence that vigorous
and moderate activity are related differently to psychological
Life
satisfaction
Positive
emotions
Negative
emotions
Fig. 1 Focusing effects regarding
the relationship between
moderate-intensity physical ac-
tivity (Y) and subjective
well-being in men (left)
and women (right)
Curr Psychol
outcomes. These two physical activity intensities were weakly
correlated suggesting that individuals who report vigorous
exercises are only slightly more likely to report moderate ex-
ercises and vice versa. Based on this behavioral distinction,
we found that vigorous physical activity had a stable link to
satisfaction with life, whereas the link between moderate
physical activity and satisfaction with life was prone to the
focusing illusion. This differential effect suggests that com-
pound measures of physical activity are likely to omit more
nuanced psychological phenomena that are present when re-
searchers study these intensities as separate predictors within
one model. The evidence presented in this study suggests that
individuals who report higher levels of vigorous physical ac-
tivity are happier than their less vigorous peers. Health inter-
ventionists can capitalize on this finding as a persuasive argu-
ment to facilitate positive attitudes towards intense physical
training (Michie et al. 2011). Second, we found that the effects
of moderate-intensity physical activity on well-being were
prone to the focusing illusion. Thus, individuals who engage
in physical activity as a subjective well-being regulation meth-
od should be aware that moderate-intensity activity might not
be sufficient to obtain the expected effect for well-being. This
finding is meaningful, because in the recommendations for
physical health, high- and moderate-intensities are exchange-
able (World Health Organization 2010). These recommenda-
tions suggest that longer periods of moderate-intensity physi-
cal activity are the equivalent for shorter periods of high-
intensity physical activity. We found that this is not the case
for well-being, where high- and moderate-intensity physical
activity is not exchangeable. Vigorous physical activity pro-
vides a superior contribution to well-being after controlling
for moderate-intensity physical activity. However, adverse
psychological effects are likely to occur in the case of
overtraining (Pearce 2002). Furthermore, high-intensity
trainers are at the risk of addiction to exercises (or exercise
dependence syndrome) that occurs when exercise becomes an
obligation, is excessive, and interferes with work and family
responsibilities (Hausenblas et al. 2017). This problem affects
c.a. 0.3% to 0.5% of the general population (Mónok et al.
2012). Finally, our findings are a caveat for researchers who
aim to minimize measurement bias. Despite extensive litera-
ture which indicates that the order of scales presentation mat-
ters (Schwarz 1999), it is still not a standard procedure to
present questionnaires in a counterbalanced order across par-
ticipants. Studies such as this one, where the order of scales
was controlled and entered into the multiple regression equa-
tions, provide suggestive evidence that order effects are likely
to occur and researchers should employ solutions (e.g.,
counterbalancing) to minimize the order effects.
This study has several limitations. First, we accounted for
subjective well-being, yet other approaches to well-being
(e.g., eudaimonic) might yield different results (Kaczmarek
2018). Theorists have argued that satisfaction and positive
experience result from hedonia as well as eudaimonia
(Waterman 1990). Yet, a question remains open how physical
activity is related to eudaimonic pursuits, that is the fulfillment
of one’s best potentials and enhanced personal expressiveness
(Waterman et al. 2010) and whether focusing illusion is likely
to bias eudaimonic well-being judgments. Second, the mean
values for well-being and physical activity in this study sug-
gest that the participants were neither unhappy nor very happy
and were physically active. Further studies might investigate if
the same pattern of results holds for clinical groups, such as
depressed or physically passive groups where more effective
interventions are needed (e.g., Mata et al. 2012). Third, other
methods of testing focusing illusion are feasible. Further stud-
ies might capitalize on other methods (such as longitudinal
designs or intra-individuals comparisons) to provide a more
robust picture of the focusing effects in physical activity.
This study has several strengths. We used an experi-
mental approach that provided novel causal evidence for
the focusing effects regarding physical activity.
Furthermore, we studied vigorous and moderate activity
as separate constructs within the same statistical model
revealing how these two aspects of physical activity
contribute to well-being independently producing stable
(vigorous intensity) as well as illusory (moderate inten-
sity) effects. Our findings have the potential to produce
stimulate further research into the order effects in well-
being and physical activity self-report instruments.
In conclusion, our research has revealed that vigorous
activity may be necessary to obtain stable well-being
benefits. Yet, a focus on moderate physical activity
may be sufficient to elicit a transient increase in satis-
faction with life and positive emotions. These findings
are relevant to researchers and interventionists within
the field of well-being and health domains as well as
lay people who are seeking new scientific evidence to
inform their life-enhancing decisions.
Author’s Contribution L.K., A.H., M.K, J.P., M.S. study design; A.H.,
M.K, J.P., M.S. data collection; L.K., M.B., J.E., S.D. manuscript writ-
ing;; L.K., M.B. statistical analysis; L.K. project supervision.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author
states that there is no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human
participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institu-
tional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants in-
cluded in the study.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study.
Curr Psychol
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use,
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appro-
priate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
References
Aknin, L. B., Norton, M. I., & Dunn, E. W. (2009). From wealth to well-
being? Money matters, but less than people think. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 4, 523–527.
Al-Zoubi, M. T. (2012). The shape of the relationship between salary and
job satisfaction: A field study. Far East Journal of Psychology and
Business, 7, 1–12.
Arem, H., Moore, S. C., Patel, A., Hartge, P., De Gonzalez, A. B.,
Visvanathan, K., et al. (2015). Leisure time physical activity and
mortality: A detailed pooled analysis of the dose-response relation-
ship. JAMA Internal Medicine, 175, 959–967.
Bolier, L., Haverman, M., Westerhof, G. J., Riper, H., Smit, F., &
Bohlmeijer, E. (2013). Positive psychology interventions: A
meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies. BMC Public
Health, 13, 119.
Brown, J. L., MacDonald, R., & Mitchell, R. (2015). Are people who
participate in cultural activities more satisfied with life? Social
Indicators Research, 122, 135–146.
Craig, C. L., Marshall, A. J., Sjöström, M., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L.,
Ainsworth, B. E., Pratt, M., Ekelund, U., Yngve, A., Sallis, J. F., &
Pekka, O. J. A. (2003). International physical activity questionnaire:
12-country reliability and validity. Medicine & Science in Sports &
Exercise, 35, 1381–1395.
Diener, E. D. (2000). Subjective well-being. The science of happiness and
a proposal for a nation. American Psychologist, 55, 34–43.
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49,
71–75.
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., Oishi, S., &
Biswas-Diener, R. (2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to
assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social
Indicators Research, 97, 143–156.
Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open ques-
tions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra: Psychology,
4(1), 15.
Edmunds, S., Stephenson, D., & Clow, A. (2013). The effects of a phys-
ical activity intervention on employees in small and medium enter-
prises: A mixed methods study. Work, 46, 39–49.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psy-
chology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions.
American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimen-
sional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention,
memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognition and
Emotions, 24(2), 322–327.
Hausenblas, H. A., Schreiber, K., & Smoliga, J. M. (2017). Addiction to
exercise. BMJ, 357, j1745. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j1745
Hawker, C. L. (2012). Physical activity and mental well-being in student
nurses. Nurse Education Today, 32, 325–331.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi-
tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford
Press.
Hogan, C. L., Catalino, L. I., Mata, J., & Fredrickson, B. (2015). Beyond
emotional benefits: Physical activity and sedentary behavior affect
psychosocial resources through emotions. Psychology & Health, 30,
354–369.
Kaczmarek, L. D. (2018). Happiness. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K.
Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual
differences. New York: Springer.
Kaczmarek, L. D., Enko, J., Awdziejczyk, M., Hoffmann, N.,
Białobrzeska, N., Mielniczuk, P., & Dombrowski, S. U. (2016).
Would you be happier if you looked better? A focusing illusion.
Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 357–365.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A.
(2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illu-
sion. Science, 312, 1908–1910.
Kim, E. S., Kubzansky, L. D., Soo, J., & Boehm, J. K. (2016).
Maintaining healthy behavior: A prospective study of psy-
chological well-being and physical activity. Annals of
Behavioral Medicine, 1–11.
Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., Tamir, M., Scollon, C., & Diener, M. (2005).
Integrating the diverse definitions of happiness: A time-sequential
framework of subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies,
6, 261–300.
Kyu, H. H., Bachman, V. F., Alexander, L. T., Mumford, J. E., Afshin, A.,
Estep, K., et al. (2016). Physical activity and risk of breast cancer,
colon cancer, diabetes, ischemic heart disease, and ischemic stroke
events: Systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis for the
global burden of disease study 2013. British Medical Journal, 354,
i3857.
Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: The autonomic archi-
tecture of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
1000, 348–366.
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for mul-
tivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American
Statistical Association, 83, 1198–1202.
Lovretic, V., Benjak, T., & Vuletic, G. (2013). Subjective
wellbeing of cyclists and physically inactive subjects.
Kinesiology, 45, 101–106.
Maher, J.P., Doerksen, S.E., Elavsky, S., Hyde, A.L., Pincus,A.L.,
Ram, N., & Conroy, D.E. (2012). A daily analysis of phys-
ical activity and satisfaction with life in emerging adults,
Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 32,
647–656.
Mata, J., Thompson, R. J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., &
Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Walk on the bright side: Physical activity and
affect in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 121, 297–308.
McGowan, R. W., Pierce, E. F., & Jordan, D. (1991). Mood alterations
with a single bout of physical activity. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
72, 1203–1209.
Michie, S., Ashford, S., Sniehotta, F. F., Dombrowski, S. U.,
Bishop, A., & French, D. P. (2011). A refined taxonomy of
behaviour change techniques to help people change their
physical activity and healthy eating behaviours: The CALO-
RE taxonomy. Psychology & Health, 26, 1479–1498.
Mónok, K., Berczik, K., Urbán, R., Szabo, A., Griffiths, M. D.,
Farkas, J., et al. (2012). Psychometric properties and concur-
rent validity of two exercise addiction measures: A
population-wide study. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,
13(6), 739–746.
Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., & Colcombe, S. J. (2003). The contextual and
systematic nature of life satisfaction judgments. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 232–247.
Pearce, P. Z. (2002). A practical approach to the overtraining syndrome.
Current Sports Medicine Reports, 1(3), 179–183.
Petruzzello, S. J., Landers, D. M., Hatfield, B. D., Kubitz, K. A.,
& Salazar, W. (1991). A meta-analysis on the anxiety-
reducing effects of acute and chronic exercise. Sports
Medicine, 11, 143–182.
Curr Psychol
Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make
people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction.
Psychological Science, 9, 340–346.
Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers.
American Psychologist, 54, 93–105.
Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (2016). Beyond ‘what’ comes to mind:
Experiential and conversational determinants of information use.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 12, 89–93.
Schwarz, N., Strack, F., & Mai, H. P. (1991). Assimilation and
contrast effects in part-whole question sequences: A con-
versational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55,
3–23.
Smith, D. M., Schwarz, N., Roberts, T. R., & Ubel, P. A. (2006). Why are
you calling me? How study introductions change response patterns.
Quality of Life Research, 15, 621–630.
Valois, R. F., Zullig, K. J., Huebner, E. S., & Drane, J. W.
(2004). Physical activity behaviors and perceived life satis-
faction among public high school adolescents. Journal of
School Health, 74, 59–65.
Waterman, A. S. (1990). The relevance of Aristotle’s conception of
eudaimonia for the psychological study of happiness. Theoretical
& Philosophical Psychology, 10, 39–44.
Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., Ravert, R. D.,
Williams, M. K., Bede Agocha, V., Yeong Kim, S., & Brent
Donnellan, M. (2010). The questionnaire for Eudaimonic well-be-
ing: Psychometric properties, demographic comparisons, and evi-
dence of validity. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 41–61.
World Health Organization. (2010). Global recommendations on physical
activity for health. Geneva: WHO.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris-
dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Curr Psychol

More Related Content

What's hot

International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1
Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1
Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1Christina Sanderson
 
Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...
Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...
Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...ellen1066
 
Cognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeing
Cognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeingCognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeing
Cognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeingInna Ramadania
 
CHRONIC PAIN in adolescents final
CHRONIC PAIN in adolescents finalCHRONIC PAIN in adolescents final
CHRONIC PAIN in adolescents finaljuliann trumpower
 
LI.575.Reflection.Robert.Toynbee
LI.575.Reflection.Robert.ToynbeeLI.575.Reflection.Robert.Toynbee
LI.575.Reflection.Robert.ToynbeeRobert Toynbee
 
Me, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya Matthews
Me, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya MatthewsMe, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya Matthews
Me, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya MatthewsPsychological Society of Ireland
 
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...Victor Sojo Monzon
 
APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016
APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016
APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016Shahar Bar Yehuda
 
PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673
PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673
PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673Philip Guillet
 
Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...
Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...
Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...iosrjce
 
Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535Brianna Bradley
 
The effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiver
The effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiverThe effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiver
The effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregivercandice_Rasheed
 

What's hot (20)

SocietyInfluencingHealth(1)
SocietyInfluencingHealth(1)SocietyInfluencingHealth(1)
SocietyInfluencingHealth(1)
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1
Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1
Christina Sanderson Thesis Draft Final-1
 
Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...
Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...
Evolution of the biopsychosocial model: prospects and challenges for health p...
 
Cognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeing
Cognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeingCognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeing
Cognitive, affective and contextual predictors of subjective wellbeing
 
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & GynecologyInternational Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
 
CHRONIC PAIN in adolescents final
CHRONIC PAIN in adolescents finalCHRONIC PAIN in adolescents final
CHRONIC PAIN in adolescents final
 
Gledhill et al (2016)
Gledhill et al (2016)Gledhill et al (2016)
Gledhill et al (2016)
 
LI.575.Reflection.Robert.Toynbee
LI.575.Reflection.Robert.ToynbeeLI.575.Reflection.Robert.Toynbee
LI.575.Reflection.Robert.Toynbee
 
Me, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya Matthews
Me, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya MatthewsMe, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya Matthews
Me, Myself and Trauma: But What Does it Mean For My Health? by Soraya Matthews
 
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
 
APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016
APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016
APS conference Poster-Session-X May 2016
 
PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673
PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673
PhilipGuillet.Critique1.PC673
 
Dissertation PDF
Dissertation PDFDissertation PDF
Dissertation PDF
 
Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...
Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...
Assessment of Stress Levels among Undergraduates in Nigeria: Implication for ...
 
Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535
 
Sspc conference
Sspc conferenceSspc conference
Sspc conference
 
Final Paper--FHS420
Final Paper--FHS420Final Paper--FHS420
Final Paper--FHS420
 
Health psych
Health psychHealth psych
Health psych
 
The effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiver
The effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiverThe effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiver
The effects of alzheimer’s disease on the caregiver
 

Similar to Would you be happier if you moved more? Physical activity focusing illusion

Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy
Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy
Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy Marlene Garzona
 
Persistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docx
Persistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docxPersistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docx
Persistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docxkarlhennesey
 
Social Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docx
Social Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docxSocial Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docx
Social Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
Activity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docx
Activity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docxActivity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docx
Activity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docxdaniahendric
 
L4594103.pdf
L4594103.pdfL4594103.pdf
L4594103.pdfaijbm
 
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEMNovacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEMLouise Wilce
 
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docxTemperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docxmanningchassidy
 
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docxTemperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docxbradburgess22840
 
Raul Quiroz - Research Paper Business Communications
Raul Quiroz - Research Paper Business CommunicationsRaul Quiroz - Research Paper Business Communications
Raul Quiroz - Research Paper Business CommunicationsRaul Quiroz
 
Sociology and psychology in public health
Sociology and psychology in public healthSociology and psychology in public health
Sociology and psychology in public healthUniversity of Khartoum
 
Running head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY .docx
Running head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY               .docxRunning head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY               .docx
Running head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY .docxcharisellington63520
 
Technical And Business Of Entrepreneurship
Technical And Business Of EntrepreneurshipTechnical And Business Of Entrepreneurship
Technical And Business Of EntrepreneurshipDiane Allen
 
JMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docx
JMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docxJMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docx
JMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docxchristiandean12115
 
Lecture 1.27.09
Lecture 1.27.09Lecture 1.27.09
Lecture 1.27.09stbubany
 

Similar to Would you be happier if you moved more? Physical activity focusing illusion (20)

Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy
Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy
Anxiety Exercise Self Efficacy
 
Persistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docx
Persistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docxPersistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docx
Persistent link httpssearch-proquest-com.library.capella.edu.docx
 
Social Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docx
Social Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docxSocial Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docx
Social Relationships, Leisure Activity, and Health in Older Ad.docx
 
Activity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docx
Activity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docxActivity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docx
Activity Week 2 SWOT PowerPointDue Week 2 and worth 200 points.docx
 
L4594103.pdf
L4594103.pdfL4594103.pdf
L4594103.pdf
 
Article Summary.pdf
Article Summary.pdfArticle Summary.pdf
Article Summary.pdf
 
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEMNovacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
 
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docxTemperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
 
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docxTemperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
Temperament, Childhood Illness Burden, and Illness Behavior in.docx
 
Raul Quiroz - Research Paper Business Communications
Raul Quiroz - Research Paper Business CommunicationsRaul Quiroz - Research Paper Business Communications
Raul Quiroz - Research Paper Business Communications
 
Sociology and psychology in public health
Sociology and psychology in public healthSociology and psychology in public health
Sociology and psychology in public health
 
B036409016
B036409016B036409016
B036409016
 
Running head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY .docx
Running head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY               .docxRunning head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY               .docx
Running head PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SELF-EFFICACY .docx
 
jpm12168_2
jpm12168_2jpm12168_2
jpm12168_2
 
Open Hearts Build Lives
Open Hearts Build LivesOpen Hearts Build Lives
Open Hearts Build Lives
 
Health & Happiness: Hand in Hand (October 2012)
Health & Happiness: Hand in Hand (October 2012)Health & Happiness: Hand in Hand (October 2012)
Health & Happiness: Hand in Hand (October 2012)
 
MPH Internship Proposal
MPH Internship ProposalMPH Internship Proposal
MPH Internship Proposal
 
Technical And Business Of Entrepreneurship
Technical And Business Of EntrepreneurshipTechnical And Business Of Entrepreneurship
Technical And Business Of Entrepreneurship
 
JMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docx
JMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docxJMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docx
JMPT PATIENT EDUCATION PAGEHEALTH AND WELLNESSHealth is .docx
 
Lecture 1.27.09
Lecture 1.27.09Lecture 1.27.09
Lecture 1.27.09
 

More from Maciej Behnke

Towards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports Minitrack
Towards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports MinitrackTowards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports Minitrack
Towards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports MinitrackMaciej Behnke
 
Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...
Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...
Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...Maciej Behnke
 
Ethical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with Wearables
Ethical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with WearablesEthical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with Wearables
Ethical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with WearablesMaciej Behnke
 
Esports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than Athletes
Esports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than AthletesEsports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than Athletes
Esports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than AthletesMaciej Behnke
 
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic Review
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic ReviewThe Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic Review
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic ReviewMaciej Behnke
 
The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...
The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...
The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...Maciej Behnke
 
Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...
Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...
Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...Maciej Behnke
 
The Role of Emotions in Esports Performance
The Role of Emotions in Esports PerformanceThe Role of Emotions in Esports Performance
The Role of Emotions in Esports PerformanceMaciej Behnke
 
Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...
Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...
Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...Maciej Behnke
 
Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...
Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...
Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...Maciej Behnke
 
Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...
Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...
Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...Maciej Behnke
 
Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...
Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...
Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...Maciej Behnke
 
A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...
A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...
A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...Maciej Behnke
 
Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...
Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...
Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...Maciej Behnke
 
I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...
I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...
I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...Maciej Behnke
 
How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...
How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...
How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...Maciej Behnke
 
The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...
The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...
The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...Maciej Behnke
 
Quo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwa
Quo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwaQuo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwa
Quo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwaMaciej Behnke
 
Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje
 Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje
Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencjeMaciej Behnke
 
Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...
Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...
Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...Maciej Behnke
 

More from Maciej Behnke (20)

Towards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports Minitrack
Towards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports MinitrackTowards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports Minitrack
Towards a Future Esports Research: Introduction to Esports Minitrack
 
Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...
Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...
Native and non-native language contexts differently modulate mood-driven elec...
 
Ethical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with Wearables
Ethical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with WearablesEthical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with Wearables
Ethical Considerations and Checklist for Affective Research with Wearables
 
Esports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than Athletes
Esports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than AthletesEsports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than Athletes
Esports Players Are Less Extroverted and Conscientious than Athletes
 
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic Review
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic ReviewThe Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic Review
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic Review
 
The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...
The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...
The Cold Start Problem and Per-Group Personalization in Real-Life Emotion Rec...
 
Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...
Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...
Psychometric Properties of the Persian Version of the Sport Mental Training Q...
 
The Role of Emotions in Esports Performance
The Role of Emotions in Esports PerformanceThe Role of Emotions in Esports Performance
The Role of Emotions in Esports Performance
 
Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...
Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...
Psychophysiology of positive and negative emotions, dataset of 1157 cases and...
 
Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...
Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...
Positive Emotions Boost Enthusiastic Responsiveness to Capitalization Attempt...
 
Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...
Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...
Evil Joy Is Hard to Share: Negative Affect Attenuates Interpersonal Capitaliz...
 
Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...
Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...
Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic R...
 
A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...
A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...
A system for collecting emotionally annotated physiological signals in daily ...
 
Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...
Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...
Blunted cardiovascular reactivity may serve as an index of psychological task...
 
I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...
I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...
I am afraid, so I buy it! The effects of anxiety on consumer assimilation and...
 
How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...
How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...
How seasons, weather, and part of day influence baseline affective valence in...
 
The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...
The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...
The Origin of the non-governmental sector in Russia during the presidencies o...
 
Quo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwa
Quo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwaQuo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwa
Quo vadis Eurazjo? W poszukiwaniu nowych dróg partnerstwa
 
Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje
 Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje
Pucz sierpniowy jako próba zachowania jedności ZSRR i jego konsekwencje
 
Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...
Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...
Possibilities for cooperation between the non-governmental, non-commercial se...
 

Recently uploaded

Isotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on Io
Isotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on IoIsotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on Io
Isotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on IoSérgio Sacani
 
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)Areesha Ahmad
 
Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...
Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...
Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...jana861314
 
Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)
Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)
Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)PraveenaKalaiselvan1
 
SOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptx
SOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptxSOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptx
SOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptxkessiyaTpeter
 
Botany 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Botany 4th semester series (krishna).pdfBotany 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Botany 4th semester series (krishna).pdfSumit Kumar yadav
 
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questionsBotany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questionsSumit Kumar yadav
 
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral AnalysisRaman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral AnalysisDiwakar Mishra
 
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroidsHubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroidsSérgio Sacani
 
Nightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43b
Nightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43bNightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43b
Nightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43bSérgio Sacani
 
Orientation, design and principles of polyhouse
Orientation, design and principles of polyhouseOrientation, design and principles of polyhouse
Orientation, design and principles of polyhousejana861314
 
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)Areesha Ahmad
 
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPirithiRaju
 
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptxGreen chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptxRajatChauhan518211
 
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOSTDisentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOSTSérgio Sacani
 
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdfPirithiRaju
 
Biopesticide (2).pptx .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...
Biopesticide (2).pptx  .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...Biopesticide (2).pptx  .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...
Biopesticide (2).pptx .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...RohitNehra6
 
GFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptx
GFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptxGFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptx
GFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptxAleenaTreesaSaji
 
TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...
TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...
TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...ssifa0344
 
Hire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls AgencyHire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls AgencySheetal Arora
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Isotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on Io
Isotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on IoIsotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on Io
Isotopic evidence of long-lived volcanism on Io
 
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
 
Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...
Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...
Traditional Agroforestry System in India- Shifting Cultivation, Taungya, Home...
 
Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)
Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)
Recombinant DNA technology (Immunological screening)
 
SOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptx
SOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptxSOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptx
SOLUBLE PATTERN RECOGNITION RECEPTORS.pptx
 
Botany 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Botany 4th semester series (krishna).pdfBotany 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Botany 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
 
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questionsBotany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
 
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral AnalysisRaman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
 
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroidsHubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
 
Nightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43b
Nightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43bNightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43b
Nightside clouds and disequilibrium chemistry on the hot Jupiter WASP-43b
 
Orientation, design and principles of polyhouse
Orientation, design and principles of polyhouseOrientation, design and principles of polyhouse
Orientation, design and principles of polyhouse
 
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
 
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
 
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptxGreen chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptx
 
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOSTDisentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
 
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
 
Biopesticide (2).pptx .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...
Biopesticide (2).pptx  .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...Biopesticide (2).pptx  .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...
Biopesticide (2).pptx .This slides helps to know the different types of biop...
 
GFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptx
GFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptxGFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptx
GFP in rDNA Technology (Biotechnology).pptx
 
TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...
TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...
TEST BANK For Radiologic Science for Technologists, 12th Edition by Stewart C...
 
Hire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls AgencyHire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 9907093804 Hooghly Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
 

Would you be happier if you moved more? Physical activity focusing illusion

  • 1. Would you be happier if you moved more? Physical activity focusing illusion Lukasz D. Kaczmarek1 & Maciej Behnke1 & Jolanta Enko1 & Agnieszka Hoffman1 & Marcin Kiciński1 & Jakub Paruszewski1 & Michał Szabat1 & Stephan U. Dombrowski2 # The Author(s) 2019 Abstract Research shows that individuals who are more physically active also report greater happiness. However, subjective well-being is prone to cognitive biases. For instance, people overrate the influence of single factors (e.g., money) on their happiness; a phenomenon termed the focusing illusion. In this study, we examined whether the relationship between physical activity and subjective well-being is stronger when individuals focus on physical activity explicitly compared to individuals with no specific focus. We experimentally manipulated the physical activity focus by varying the order of scales administration. Participants (N = 200) completed questionnaires that measured physical activity and subjective well-being placed in separate envelopes and provided in a random order. We found that individuals with higher levels of vigorous physical activity were more satisfied with life regardless of the order of scale presentation (no focusing effect). However, we found evidence of a possible focusing illusion for moderate-intensity physical activity. Individuals with higher levels of moderate-intensity physical activity reported higher subjective well-being when they were asked about physical activity first but not when they reported their well-being unaware of the upcoming physical activity questions. Thus, subjective well-being judgments can be biased by a prior focus on moderate- intensity physical activity. The order of scale administration when assessing subjective well-being should be carefully considered. Keywords Physical activity . Satisfaction with life . Positive emotions . Negative emotions . Focusing illusion . Happiness Introduction Individuals tend to overrate the influence of any single factor on their well-being; a phenomenon termed ‘focusing illusion’ (Schkade and Kahneman 1998). Evidence shows that people can be biased in their estimations of the extent to which their well-being is determined by income (Aknin et al. 2009; Kahneman et al. 2006), romantic relationships (Schwarz et al. 1991), health (Smith et al. 2006), or physical appearance (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). This bias may impact the decisions that people make to increase their well-being. For instance, individuals may pursue activities which fail to maximize their well-being, such as choosing a highly paid job over a self- concordant job (Al-Zoubi 2012). However, not all domains of life are prone to the focusing illusion (Oishi et al. 2003) and some factors induce partial focusing illusion, i.e., the in- dividuals overrate the importance of otherwise significant fac- tors (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). As focusing illusions can poten- tially have an impact on people’s lives, it is vital to identify which factors trigger focusing illusion. In this study, we examined the focusing effects of physical activity ratings on subjective well-being assessments. Physical activity has often been presented as a precursor to a happy life (e.g., Brown et al. 2015; Hogan et al. 2015). Yet, little is known to what extent physically active people report higher well-being which might be biased by focusing effects. Regardless of whether physical activity promotes well-being or not, people who are more physically active are healthier (Kyu et al. 2016) and live longer (Arem et al. 2015). However, identifying whether a focusing illusion regarding physical activity exists may be important. Individuals who prioritize increases in well-being might pursue more effective happiness-boosting cognitive activities (Bolier et al. 2013). * Lukasz D. Kaczmarek Lkacz@amu.edu.pl 1 Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickiewicz University, 89 Szamarzewskiego Street, PL-60-568 Poznan, Poland 2 University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, Canada Current Psychology https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00315-9
  • 2. Due to the focusing illusion, individuals who initiate physical activity to enhance their subjective well-being rather than fit- ness can be less likely to pursue other potentially more effec- tive happiness-boosting interventions. Moreover, some people may have unrealistic expectations about the extent to which physical activity increases their well-being and give up once they notice that these expecta- tions are not met. New more nuanced insights on the relation- ship between physical activity and well-being are imperative to inform individual lifestyle decisions and interventions as well as to support public health policies (e.g., World Health Organization 2010). Additionally, some approaches to the study of focusing illusion are also critical from the methodo- logical standpoint (Schwarz 1999; Smith et al. 2006). The focusing illusion can, for instance, explain some questionnaire order effects (Schwarz et al. 1991) thereby informing our un- derstanding of cognitive aspects of survey design. Focusing Illusion Brief and broad evaluations of well-being (e.g., overall satis- faction with life or the number of emotions experienced dur- ing the last days) depend on information that comes to mind when individuals think about their life (Schwarz and Strack 2016). The speed and ease of these evaluations can come at the cost of their accuracy. When individuals focus their atten- tion on a specific life domain, they may overestimate its im- portance other relevant information might be missed to form an accurate judgment (Schwarz and Strack 2016; Oishi et al. 2003). From a happiness perspective, human information pro- cessing produces can lead to overestimated ratings of the im- portance of any single factor on well-being (Schkade and Kahneman 1998). The focusing illusion is an instance of bi- ased judgment because an entire object (such as one’s life) is evaluated with attention focused on a specific subset of this category, e.g., physical appearance (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). Focusing effects have been evidenced using a variety of methodologies. For instance, in one study people were asked to report how much time they spent in a bad mood as well as how much time people in different circumstances, such as with high or low income, spent in a bad mood (Kahneman et al. 2006). Participants overestimated bad mood for individ- uals in unfavorable life circumstances compared to real-life reports. Similarly, in another study participants reported their income, their life satisfaction and predicted satisfaction of oth- er individuals covering a wide range of income (Aknin et al. 2009). The results suggested that wealthier individuals tended to overestimate dissatisfaction with life among less wealthy individuals compared to the actual self-reports provided by these individuals. In a well-being survey among patients di- agnosed with Parkinson’s disease, Smith et al. (2006) in- formed half of the participants that the survey was conducted by a medical center and its topic was related to Parkinson’s disease. The other group was asked the same questions but was informed that they participated in a general population survey conducted by the university. The patients that explic- itly focused on their health showed a higher correlation be- tween health and well-being measures compared to the group with no explicit focus on health. Furthermore, when both groups received health-related questions first, the correlation between health and well-being was high regardless of the study context presented in the introduction. In a similar study, the order of scales measuring body satisfaction and life satis- faction was manipulated (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). In the fo- cusing group that reported their body satisfaction first, there was a higher correlation between body satisfaction and life satisfaction than in the group that reported their life satisfac- tion first; unaware of the upcoming body satisfaction ques- tions. This suggests that a momentary focus on the body can influence subsequent judgments of well-being. Subjective Well-Being and Physical Activity There are several approaches to the conceptualization and the measurement of well-being (Kaczmarek 2018). Of these ap- proaches, subjective well-being has been among the most popular due to its versatility and ability to cover a wide range of experience and evaluations in different contexts (Diener et al. 2018). The subjective well-being theory is particularly relevant to the study of the focusing illusion because it covers the evaluations of the quality of a person’s life from that per- son’s perspective. Subjective well-being consists of three components: positive emotions, negative emotions, and satis- faction with life (Diener 2000). Emotions are short-lived psy- chological-physiological phenomena that facilitate adaptation to changing environmental demands (Levenson 2003). Positive emotions are associated with pleasure whereas nega- tive emotions are associated with displeasure or pain (Gable and Harmon-Jones 2010). Furthermore, positive emotions pull individuals toward specific objects, actions, and ideas, whereas negative emotions push away from others. Positive and negative emotions are distinct at a functional level with negative emotions facilitating efficient use of psycho-social coping resources and positive emotions helping to build new resources (Fredrickson 2001). A happy individual has an abundance of positive emotions and little negative emotions. Emotions, in turn, influence global evaluations of life and determine whether individuals endorse their life (Kim-Prieto et al. 2005). These global evaluations form satisfaction with life which is a cognitive, judgmental process of life as a whole according to individual criteria of what is desirable. The link between physical activity and various facets of well-being such as satisfaction with life or positive emotions has been established in the literature. Physical activity has been related to life satisfaction and positive emotions among employees (Edmunds et al. 2013), satisfaction with life among Curr Psychol
  • 3. adolescents (Valois et al. 2004), or personal well-being in cyclists (Lovretic et al. 2013). Individuals who participate in sports also report higher life satisfaction compared to individ- uals who prefer sedentary leisure activities predictive (Brown et al. 2015). Physical activity has also been related to positive affect (Mata et al. 2012). The relationship between physical activity and well-being is bi-directional as indicated by studies where baseline psychological well-being (a compound of pleasure and positive functioning) predicted future long-term physical activity outcomes (Kim et al. 2016). Some findings regarding physical activity and well-being have been inconsistent. Maher et al. (2012) found that al- though satisfaction with life was influenced by daily varia- tions in physical activity, there was no relationship between satisfaction with life and trait-like physical activity. Physical activity was also unrelated to satisfaction with life among student nurses (Hawker 2012). There are also inconsistent results regarding physical activity and negative affect with no effect in some studies (e.g., Mata et al. 2012), whereas others find significant (e.g., Petruzzello et al. 1991), or mixed results (e.g., Hogan et al. 2015). No research has examined whether focusing effects exist in the domain of physical activ- ity which may contribute towards explaining the inconsistent evidence on the link between physical activity and various facets of well-being. The Present Study Building upon prior work on the focusing illusion (Kaczmarek et al. 2016; Schkade and Kahneman 1998; Smith et al. 2006) we hypothesized that the relationship between physical activity and subjective well-being would be stronger for individuals explicitly focused on physical activity compared to individuals with no specific focus. Specifically, we hypothesized that when physical activity is measured before well-being, the correlation between physical activity and well-being would be stronger compared to the reverse order. Thus, the order of the adminis- tration of scales measuring both constructs would moderate the physical activity and well-being link. Method Participants Participants were 200 undergraduate students aged 18 to 45 years (M = 23.55, SD = 5.00). There was a similar number of women (n = 104; 52.0%) and men (n = 93; 46.5%) in the sample. Three participants who did not report their gender were excluded from the analyses as gender was one of the predictors in the model. Recruitment included researchers inviting stu- dents before classes to participate in a psychology study. The experimental group and control group included 98 and 102 participants, respectively. Based on prior research on focusing illusion testing similar models (Kaczmarek et al. 2016), we expected small effect sizes for the focusing effects. Given that outcome variables in the model were planned to regress upon eight predictors (including the interaction terms), power analy- ses with G*Power 3.1.9.2 suggested that a total sample size of 159 would be the minimum required to detect effects of f2 = 0.10 with an α = .05 and power of .80. Missing data (0.2%) were determined to be random via Little’s (1988) test, χ2 (95) = 91.88, p = .57, and were imputed using Expectation- Maximization algorithm in SPSS 21. The study was conducted by the guidelines of the Institutional Ethics Committee. Participation in the study was voluntary, and each participant signed informed consent. Participants remained anonymous and received no incentives for taking part in this study. Measures We measured subjective well-being using a three-factor ap- proach that accounted for positive emotions, negative emo- tions, and satisfaction with life (Kim-Prieto et al. 2005). Life satisfaction was measured with the 5-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al. 1985). The SWLS in- cludes items such as ‘The conditions of my life are excellent.’ The answering scale ranges from 1 ‘strongly disagree’ to 7 ‘strongly agree.’ The scale was internally consistent with Cronbach’s α = .85. Emotions were measured with The Scale of Positive and Negative Experience (Diener et al. 2010). This scale measures positive feelings (e.g., happy) and negative feelings (e.g., sad) with six items for each subscale. Participants reported how often they had been experiencing each of these feelings over the last 7 days using a scale from 1 ‘very rarely or never’ to 5 ‘very often or always’ (Cronbach’s α = .87 for positive feel- ings and α = .85 for negative feelings). Physical activity was measured with the short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ, Craig et al. 2003). This questionnaire asks four groups of items regarding vigorous physical activity, moderate physical activity, walking, and sitting during the last 7 days at work, as part of house and yard work, to get from place to place, and for recreation, exercise, or sport. Vigorous activity is defined as taking hard physical effort that makes someone breathe much harder than normal such as heavy lifting, digging, aerobics, or fast bicycling for at least 10 min at a time. Moderate physical activity is defined as a physical effort that makes someone breathe somewhat harder than normal such as carrying light loads, bicycling at a regular pace for at least 10 min, etc., and does not include walking. For each level of physical activity (e.g., vigorous) participants report how many days they did this type of activities, and how much time they usually spend doing this type of activity on one of those days. Participants also reported if they were not able to provide a reliable Curr Psychol
  • 4. estimate of this type of activity. The index of physical activity in each intensity is calculated as the product of the number of days and the average daily time. This product is further weighted by estimated metabolic equivalent energy expendi- ture (×8 for vigorous activity and × 4 for moderate activity). Items regarding vigorous and moderate physical activity were used in the analyses. Items regarding walking and sitting were not examined as 25.5% participants reported that they were not able to estimate their walking and sitting time compared to only 3% and 4.5% for moderate and vigorous physical activity respectively. The self-report IPAQ has excellent test-retest re- liability and correlates with objective measures of physical activity via accelerometry (Craig et al. 2003). Log transforma- tion of each physical activity metric was used in the analyses to adjust the positive skew of the initial data. Procedure We placed questionnaires for physical activity, and well-being in separate envelopes marked A or B. For a random half of the participants, we placed the well-being questionnaires into the envelope A and the physical activity questionnaire in the en- velope B (no focus group). For the other half of participants, we placed the physical activity questionnaire into envelope A, and the well-being questionnaires into envelope B (focusing group). We asked participants to complete the questionnaire placed in the envelope A first, place it back into the envelope, open envelope B, and complete the other questionnaire. Analytical Strategy We performed multiple regression analyses using SPSS 23.0 (Armonk, NY) and PROCESS 2.16 macro (Hayes 2013) with two physical activity outcomes, gender, scale order, and their interactions as predictors and three well-being components as outcome variables. In testing this hypothesis, we controlled for gender as physical activity differs between women and men. Gender (0 = man, 1 = woman) and scale order (0 = well-being first, 1 = physical activity first) were entered as binary predic- tors. We used R2 coefficient to calculate the amount of variance in outcome variables explained by the predictors. Results We presented descriptive statistics and correlations between study variables in Table 1. On average, participants were neither satisfied nor dissatisfied with their lives, experienced positive emotions often, and negative emotions rarely. Correlations showed that individuals who reported their well-being first (no focus condition), reported higher levels of positive affect and higher estimates of vigorous physical activity compared to those who reported their physical activity first. Individuals with higher levels of vigorous physical activity reported higher levels of satisfaction with life as indicated by a main effect of vigorous activity on satisfaction with life (Table 2). Furthermore, a significant interaction term (moder- ate physical activity × scale order) revealed that moderate physical activity had a stronger relationship with life satisfac- tion when individuals were asked about physical activity first (Fig. 1). The interaction of moderate physical activity with the scale order showed a significant relationship between moder- ate physical activity and satisfaction with life when physical activity was reported first, b = .28, SE b = .12, p < .05 for men and b = .34, SE b = .13, p < .01 for women, and a non- significant relationship when participants were asked about their life first, b = −.09, SE b = .14, p > .05 for men and b = −.03, SE b = .14, p > .05 for women. This model explained 11.1% of the variance in satisfaction with life, R2 = .111, F (8, 177) = 2.77, p < .01. The interaction representing the fo- cusing illusion explained additional 3.2% of the variance, ΔR2 = .032, F (1, 177) = 6.33, p < .05. We found a similar focusing effect regarding moderate physical activity and positive emotions. There was a signifi- cant interaction between scale order and moderate physical activity in respect to positive emotions. This interaction com- prised a significant relationship between moderate physical activity and positive emotions when physical activity was re- ported first, b = .40, SE b = .12, p < .01 for men and b = .44, SE b = .13, p < .01 for women, and a non-significant relation- ship when participants were asked about their life first, b = .05, SE b = .14, p > .05 for men and b = .09, SE b = .12, p > .05 for women. This model explained 16.3% of the vari- ance in positive emotions, R2 = .163, F (8, 177) = 4.31, p < .001. The interaction representing focusing illusion ex- plained additional 2.6% of the variance, ΔR2 = .026, F (1, 177) = 5.59, p < .05. Finally, we found no focusing effects for physical activity regarding negative emotions. We also did not observe any significant gender differences. Discussion In the present study, we examined whether the link between physical activity and subjective well-being is affected by a focusing illusion. We tested if a temporal evaluative focus (experimentally induced) on physical activity influenced judg- ments regarding three components of subjective well-being. We found that the positive relationship between vigorous physical activity and satisfaction with life was significant and showed no evidence of any focusing effect. However, we found evidence of a focusing effect for moderate- intensity physical activity regarding satisfaction with life and positive but not negative affect. Curr Psychol
  • 5. Individuals who self-reported higher levels of vigorous physical activity were more likely to report higher levels of life satisfaction regardless of the scale order administration. We found that the psychological effects of vigorous activity were present even when individuals did not focus their atten- tion explicitly on this sphere of life. Moreover, there were no additional benefits or losses for subjective well-being when individuals were encouraged to focus on their past intense physical output. This observation indicates an unbiased rela- tionship between vigorous physical activity and satisfaction with life that is free from focusing illusions. A partial focusing illusion occurs when an explicit focus enhances a weaker, yet significant, relationship between a factor and well-being (Kaczmarek et al. 2016). This finding means that individuals who are physically active in the range of high intensities are also more satisfied with their life. It corroborates previous research regarding life-enhancing outcomes of physical activ- ity (e.g., Edmunds et al. 2013). Yet, we found no relationship between vigorous physical activity and positive or negative affect. The absence of a relationship of physical activity with negative affect is also congruent with some previous studies (Mata et al. 2012). However, no relationship between physical activity and positive affect was not in line with our hypothe- ses. A possible explanation for this finding is that satisfaction with life is a more reliable overarching construct that forms global judgments based on cognitive as well as affective in- formation (Kim-Prieto et al. 2005). The unique effects of high- intensity physical activity may result from endorphins secre- tion that is higher for high-intensity activities such as running or weight lifting (McGowan et al. 1991). Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 1. Vigorous PA 2. Moderate PA .29** 3. Satisfaction with life .17* .14 4. Positive affect .11 .21** .68** 5. Negative affect .03 −.03 −.40** −.56** 6. Age −.05 .13 −.01 −.02 −.21** 7. Gender −.01 .05 −.05 −.14 .23** −.19** 8. Scale order −.21** −.08 −.05 .17* −.12 −.12 M 1433.50 614.43 4.06 3.62 2.34 23.55 SD 2714.46 1010.24 1.13 0.79 0.80 5.00 MLn 5.03 4.85 – – – – SDLn 2.64 2.16 – – – – Scale order coded as 0 = well-being first (no focusing), 1 = physical activity first (focusing). Gender coded as 0 = man, 1 = women. PA Physical activity. Ln Log transformed data * p < .05. ** p < .01 Table 2 Physical activity, subjective well-being, and focusing effects Life satisfaction Positive emotions Negative emotions β SE β β SE β β SE β Vigorous PA .41** .13 .15 .13 .04 .14 Moderate PA −.09 .14 .05 .14 .06 .14 Scale order .46 .32 .25 .32 −.14 .33 Gender .22 .31 −.03 .31 .53 .32 Vigorous PA × Scale order −.09 .06 .04 .06 −.02 .06 Vigorous PA × Gender −.08 .06 −.05 .06 −.01 .06 Moderate PA × Scale order .37* .15 .35* .15 −.19 .15 Moderate PA × Gender .06 .15 .05 .15 −.07 .15 Scale order coded as 0 = well-being first (no focusing), 1 = physical activity first (focusing). Gender coded as 0 = man, 1 = women. PA Physical activity * p < .05. ** p < .01 Curr Psychol
  • 6. We found that moderate-intensity physical activity produced the focusing effects for satisfaction with life and positive emotions. Men and women who self- reported greater moderate physical activity were more likely to also reported greater satisfaction with life and positive emotions only when they were asked about physical activity first. In contrast, there was no relation- ship between moderate physical activity and satisfaction with life or positive emotions, when subjective partici- pants reported their well-being before reporting their physical activity. This phenomenon suggests a full fo- cusing illusion that suggests that people are likely to overrate the role of moderate physical activity in their well-being estimates. These findings add to the literature on the prevalence of biases in well-being judgments (Schkade and Kahneman 1998; Smith et al. 2006). Finally, we observed that the scale order influenced reported levels of vigorous physical activity and positive emotions. Individuals who reported their well-being first had higher levels of positive affect and higher estimates of vigorous physical activity compared to those who reported their physical activity first. This effect suggests that reflecting on physical activity decreased positive affect; a variable reported later. Our ad hoc hypothesis would be that this difference might result from partici- pants low on physical activity who might dampen their enthusiasm after reflecting their physical inactivity. Moreover, participants who reported their well-being first reported more physical activity. One ad hoc hy- pothesis explaining this effect would be that well-being primed less critical or less accurate behavioral judg- ments. In summary, these scale order effects serve as a new example which is worthwhile to control for within future surveys to minimize the bias in subjective esti- mates of affective experience and behavior (Schwarz 1999). This study has several potential practical implications. First, vigorous and moderate physical activity is of primary interest to the public health agencies due to its well- documented impact on health (World Health Organization 2010). The current study provides new evidence that vigorous and moderate activity are related differently to psychological Life satisfaction Positive emotions Negative emotions Fig. 1 Focusing effects regarding the relationship between moderate-intensity physical ac- tivity (Y) and subjective well-being in men (left) and women (right) Curr Psychol
  • 7. outcomes. These two physical activity intensities were weakly correlated suggesting that individuals who report vigorous exercises are only slightly more likely to report moderate ex- ercises and vice versa. Based on this behavioral distinction, we found that vigorous physical activity had a stable link to satisfaction with life, whereas the link between moderate physical activity and satisfaction with life was prone to the focusing illusion. This differential effect suggests that com- pound measures of physical activity are likely to omit more nuanced psychological phenomena that are present when re- searchers study these intensities as separate predictors within one model. The evidence presented in this study suggests that individuals who report higher levels of vigorous physical ac- tivity are happier than their less vigorous peers. Health inter- ventionists can capitalize on this finding as a persuasive argu- ment to facilitate positive attitudes towards intense physical training (Michie et al. 2011). Second, we found that the effects of moderate-intensity physical activity on well-being were prone to the focusing illusion. Thus, individuals who engage in physical activity as a subjective well-being regulation meth- od should be aware that moderate-intensity activity might not be sufficient to obtain the expected effect for well-being. This finding is meaningful, because in the recommendations for physical health, high- and moderate-intensities are exchange- able (World Health Organization 2010). These recommenda- tions suggest that longer periods of moderate-intensity physi- cal activity are the equivalent for shorter periods of high- intensity physical activity. We found that this is not the case for well-being, where high- and moderate-intensity physical activity is not exchangeable. Vigorous physical activity pro- vides a superior contribution to well-being after controlling for moderate-intensity physical activity. However, adverse psychological effects are likely to occur in the case of overtraining (Pearce 2002). Furthermore, high-intensity trainers are at the risk of addiction to exercises (or exercise dependence syndrome) that occurs when exercise becomes an obligation, is excessive, and interferes with work and family responsibilities (Hausenblas et al. 2017). This problem affects c.a. 0.3% to 0.5% of the general population (Mónok et al. 2012). Finally, our findings are a caveat for researchers who aim to minimize measurement bias. Despite extensive litera- ture which indicates that the order of scales presentation mat- ters (Schwarz 1999), it is still not a standard procedure to present questionnaires in a counterbalanced order across par- ticipants. Studies such as this one, where the order of scales was controlled and entered into the multiple regression equa- tions, provide suggestive evidence that order effects are likely to occur and researchers should employ solutions (e.g., counterbalancing) to minimize the order effects. This study has several limitations. First, we accounted for subjective well-being, yet other approaches to well-being (e.g., eudaimonic) might yield different results (Kaczmarek 2018). Theorists have argued that satisfaction and positive experience result from hedonia as well as eudaimonia (Waterman 1990). Yet, a question remains open how physical activity is related to eudaimonic pursuits, that is the fulfillment of one’s best potentials and enhanced personal expressiveness (Waterman et al. 2010) and whether focusing illusion is likely to bias eudaimonic well-being judgments. Second, the mean values for well-being and physical activity in this study sug- gest that the participants were neither unhappy nor very happy and were physically active. Further studies might investigate if the same pattern of results holds for clinical groups, such as depressed or physically passive groups where more effective interventions are needed (e.g., Mata et al. 2012). Third, other methods of testing focusing illusion are feasible. Further stud- ies might capitalize on other methods (such as longitudinal designs or intra-individuals comparisons) to provide a more robust picture of the focusing effects in physical activity. This study has several strengths. We used an experi- mental approach that provided novel causal evidence for the focusing effects regarding physical activity. Furthermore, we studied vigorous and moderate activity as separate constructs within the same statistical model revealing how these two aspects of physical activity contribute to well-being independently producing stable (vigorous intensity) as well as illusory (moderate inten- sity) effects. Our findings have the potential to produce stimulate further research into the order effects in well- being and physical activity self-report instruments. In conclusion, our research has revealed that vigorous activity may be necessary to obtain stable well-being benefits. Yet, a focus on moderate physical activity may be sufficient to elicit a transient increase in satis- faction with life and positive emotions. These findings are relevant to researchers and interventionists within the field of well-being and health domains as well as lay people who are seeking new scientific evidence to inform their life-enhancing decisions. Author’s Contribution L.K., A.H., M.K, J.P., M.S. study design; A.H., M.K, J.P., M.S. data collection; L.K., M.B., J.E., S.D. manuscript writ- ing;; L.K., M.B. statistical analysis; L.K. project supervision. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of Interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest. Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institu- tional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants in- cluded in the study. Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Curr Psychol
  • 8. Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appro- priate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. References Aknin, L. B., Norton, M. I., & Dunn, E. W. (2009). From wealth to well- being? Money matters, but less than people think. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 523–527. Al-Zoubi, M. T. (2012). The shape of the relationship between salary and job satisfaction: A field study. Far East Journal of Psychology and Business, 7, 1–12. Arem, H., Moore, S. C., Patel, A., Hartge, P., De Gonzalez, A. B., Visvanathan, K., et al. (2015). Leisure time physical activity and mortality: A detailed pooled analysis of the dose-response relation- ship. JAMA Internal Medicine, 175, 959–967. Bolier, L., Haverman, M., Westerhof, G. J., Riper, H., Smit, F., & Bohlmeijer, E. (2013). Positive psychology interventions: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies. BMC Public Health, 13, 119. Brown, J. L., MacDonald, R., & Mitchell, R. (2015). Are people who participate in cultural activities more satisfied with life? Social Indicators Research, 122, 135–146. Craig, C. L., Marshall, A. J., Sjöström, M., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L., Ainsworth, B. E., Pratt, M., Ekelund, U., Yngve, A., Sallis, J. F., & Pekka, O. J. A. (2003). International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 35, 1381–1395. Diener, E. D. (2000). Subjective well-being. The science of happiness and a proposal for a nation. American Psychologist, 55, 34–43. Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97, 143–156. Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open ques- tions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 15. Edmunds, S., Stephenson, D., & Clow, A. (2013). The effects of a phys- ical activity intervention on employees in small and medium enter- prises: A mixed methods study. Work, 46, 39–49. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psy- chology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226. Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimen- sional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognition and Emotions, 24(2), 322–327. Hausenblas, H. A., Schreiber, K., & Smoliga, J. M. (2017). Addiction to exercise. BMJ, 357, j1745. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j1745 Hawker, C. L. (2012). Physical activity and mental well-being in student nurses. Nurse Education Today, 32, 325–331. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi- tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press. Hogan, C. L., Catalino, L. I., Mata, J., & Fredrickson, B. (2015). Beyond emotional benefits: Physical activity and sedentary behavior affect psychosocial resources through emotions. Psychology & Health, 30, 354–369. Kaczmarek, L. D. (2018). Happiness. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. New York: Springer. Kaczmarek, L. D., Enko, J., Awdziejczyk, M., Hoffmann, N., Białobrzeska, N., Mielniczuk, P., & Dombrowski, S. U. (2016). Would you be happier if you looked better? A focusing illusion. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 357–365. Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illu- sion. Science, 312, 1908–1910. Kim, E. S., Kubzansky, L. D., Soo, J., & Boehm, J. K. (2016). Maintaining healthy behavior: A prospective study of psy- chological well-being and physical activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 1–11. Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., Tamir, M., Scollon, C., & Diener, M. (2005). Integrating the diverse definitions of happiness: A time-sequential framework of subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 261–300. Kyu, H. H., Bachman, V. F., Alexander, L. T., Mumford, J. E., Afshin, A., Estep, K., et al. (2016). Physical activity and risk of breast cancer, colon cancer, diabetes, ischemic heart disease, and ischemic stroke events: Systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis for the global burden of disease study 2013. British Medical Journal, 354, i3857. Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: The autonomic archi- tecture of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000, 348–366. Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for mul- tivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83, 1198–1202. Lovretic, V., Benjak, T., & Vuletic, G. (2013). Subjective wellbeing of cyclists and physically inactive subjects. Kinesiology, 45, 101–106. Maher, J.P., Doerksen, S.E., Elavsky, S., Hyde, A.L., Pincus,A.L., Ram, N., & Conroy, D.E. (2012). A daily analysis of phys- ical activity and satisfaction with life in emerging adults, Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 32, 647–656. Mata, J., Thompson, R. J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Walk on the bright side: Physical activity and affect in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 297–308. McGowan, R. W., Pierce, E. F., & Jordan, D. (1991). Mood alterations with a single bout of physical activity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1203–1209. Michie, S., Ashford, S., Sniehotta, F. F., Dombrowski, S. U., Bishop, A., & French, D. P. (2011). A refined taxonomy of behaviour change techniques to help people change their physical activity and healthy eating behaviours: The CALO- RE taxonomy. Psychology & Health, 26, 1479–1498. Mónok, K., Berczik, K., Urbán, R., Szabo, A., Griffiths, M. D., Farkas, J., et al. (2012). Psychometric properties and concur- rent validity of two exercise addiction measures: A population-wide study. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(6), 739–746. Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., & Colcombe, S. J. (2003). The contextual and systematic nature of life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 232–247. Pearce, P. Z. (2002). A practical approach to the overtraining syndrome. Current Sports Medicine Reports, 1(3), 179–183. Petruzzello, S. J., Landers, D. M., Hatfield, B. D., Kubitz, K. A., & Salazar, W. (1991). A meta-analysis on the anxiety- reducing effects of acute and chronic exercise. Sports Medicine, 11, 143–182. Curr Psychol
  • 9. Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 9, 340–346. Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers. American Psychologist, 54, 93–105. Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (2016). Beyond ‘what’ comes to mind: Experiential and conversational determinants of information use. Current Opinion in Psychology, 12, 89–93. Schwarz, N., Strack, F., & Mai, H. P. (1991). Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole question sequences: A con- versational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 3–23. Smith, D. M., Schwarz, N., Roberts, T. R., & Ubel, P. A. (2006). Why are you calling me? How study introductions change response patterns. Quality of Life Research, 15, 621–630. Valois, R. F., Zullig, K. J., Huebner, E. S., & Drane, J. W. (2004). Physical activity behaviors and perceived life satis- faction among public high school adolescents. Journal of School Health, 74, 59–65. Waterman, A. S. (1990). The relevance of Aristotle’s conception of eudaimonia for the psychological study of happiness. Theoretical & Philosophical Psychology, 10, 39–44. Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., Ravert, R. D., Williams, M. K., Bede Agocha, V., Yeong Kim, S., & Brent Donnellan, M. (2010). The questionnaire for Eudaimonic well-be- ing: Psychometric properties, demographic comparisons, and evi- dence of validity. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 41–61. World Health Organization. (2010). Global recommendations on physical activity for health. Geneva: WHO. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris- dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Curr Psychol