SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 17
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
The Impact of Olfactory Stimulation on the Behavior and Hormone Levels of
the African Lions at the Detroit Zoo
Introduction
African lions (Panthera leo) are found in the open woodlands of Sub-Saharan Africa with a
habitat covering a wide range of mixed areas of thick bush, scrub, and grass. An obligatory
carnivore, their diet consists of medium to large ungulates (i.e. buffalo, zebra, wildebeest,
waterbuck); small rodents and birds; young rhinos, elephants, hippos; and giraffes (McDade
2003). Lions typically are nocturnal hunters, with peak activity levels between 21:30-22:30h and
2:00-4:00h (Wilson et al. 2009). Activity levels also increase in the early morning and late
afternoon during the cooler hours of the day. To allow for proper digestion and to avoid the heat
of the day, lions spend 20-21 hours resting per day (Estes 1991). Resting periods may be
interrupted by brief bouts of activity when engaging in social behaviors or hunting (Wilson et.al.
2009). After stalking their prey to a close range, they make a short dash to catch prey and kill by
suffocation, clamping their jaws on the prey’s windpipe or muzzle (McDade 2003). Both male
and females work together to catch prey, often alternating who catches prey based on prey size,
large and small, respectively. Lions, social animals that live in prides of four to six individuals
on average, keep territories anywhere from eight to 200m2
(McDade 2003). During the dry
season when water sources are scarce, lions may need to travel further distances to find food,
often inducing the need to scavenge.
In captivity, zoos are faced with the challenge of inducing the hunting and scavenging behaviors
instinctive to a lion, often failing to satisfy the need to utilize these natural behaviors. One study
stated that since a natural environment only requires big cats to spend a portion of their daily life
stalking and killing prey, the “single greatest period of intensive activity and energy expenditure
is during the hunt for food” (Baker 1997). Wild conspecifics often spend three to four hours a
day hunting and scavenging for food where they may be forced to travel long distances and
utilize their senses to aid in their search. As animals in captivity no longer have the need to
provide their own nourishment, there subsequently is an absence of food-finding behaviors and
sensory stimulation. While this provides more “free” time, not filling this time with another
stimulating activity or behavior could impact the welfare of the animal. The absence of these
species-specific behaviors removes the necessity to use sensory input and also poses the potential
to replace this time with inactivity or stereotypic behaviors. Mason stated that behavioral
inflexibility may indicate an “inadequate ability to suppress natural activities that a captive
environment makes impossible” (Mason 2010). This could be detrimental to captive species
welfare, as it has the potential to cause chronic frustration and may induce coping methods such
as stereotypic behavior or an increased level of inactivity.
The inability to express species-specific behaviors such as hunting, scavenging, and using
sensory cues to locate food could result in a prolonged negative behavioral and physiological
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
response in captive lions. An animal motivated to perform a behavior pattern in a situation, but
unable to do so, becomes frustrated (Mason 1991). A repetitive situation that produces chronic
frustration may lead to a negative physiological or behavioral response that can be deleterious for
the animal (Mason 1991). The presence of prolonged frustration in any animal presents an
opportunity to develop a coping strategy. A stereotypy is one behavioral response in which an
animal can cope with prolonged frustration (Mason 1991). The most commonly seen stereotypy
in big cats is pacing (Mason 2010, Breton 2014); however, upon initial observation of the lions at
the Detroit Zoo, pacing seldom is seen. The lack of pacing in the lions suggests that the inability
to perform natural behaviors either does not cause a negative behavioral response or that the
lions are substituting that time with a different behavior. Initial observations of these lions
indicate a high level of inactivity; suggesting that inactivity, or a lack of behavior, may be their
form of substitution. However, it is important to note that since the peak activity levels of their
wild conspecifics occur in the late night and early morning hours, it is possible that the lions at
the Detroit Zoo are active during times when we are not observing them.
Both stereotypic behavior and inactivity are potential ways for an animal to substitute for
species-specific behaviors they may be less able to perform. However, while stereotypies allow
the animal to perform some type of behavior, inactivity is a lack of behavior. Therefore,
inactivity may not be as effective of a substitute as a stereotypic behavior. Researchers have
found that in some species, animals that display stereotypic patterns have lower glucocorticoid
levels than animals who do not display these patterns (Kuhar 2008, Duncan 1970, Barehem
1972, Wiepkema 1984, 1987, Cronin 1985, Kennes et al. 1988, Cabib et al. 1985, Sheperdson
2004). Duncan (1970) found that stereotypic pacing in domestic fowl correlated with a decrease
in corticosteroid levels, and adrenal weight showed to be negatively correlated with head-
flicking in this species (Barehem 1972). A study by Shepherdson and colleagues (2004) found
that polar bears performing stereotypic behaviors had lower peak cortisol levels and greater
cortisol level variation than polar bears that did not perform stereotypies. Wiepkema (1984,
1987) determined that older sows that perform stereotypies have no hormonal response, and veal
calves showed a significant negative relationship between the severity of abomasal lesions and
stereotypic tongue-playing. Conversely, in some species, the absence of stereotypic behaviors
correlates with increased stress hormone levels. Young, tethered sows that perform no
stereotypic behavior, versus their older conspecifics, have a chronic rise in corticosteroids
(Cronin 1985). A study with voles looked at the effects that lowering cage ceilings had on their
stereotypic jumping behavior. The lower ceilings removed the ability to perform this behavior
and corticosteroid levels subsequently increased (Kennes et al. 1988) Responding to these
changes, some voles developed a new type of stereotypy, which returned corticosteroid to
normal levels (Kennes et al. 1988). Similarly, a study on mice by Cabib et al. (1985) found that
stereotypies “induced by stimulant drugs” are depressed by the introduction of stressors,
indicating a lack of behavior when exposed to a source of stress. While stereotypies often are
construed as negative reactions to an environment, these studies suggest that for some
individuals, stereotypic behaviors do provide some relief to prolonged frustration versus no
behavior at all (inactivity).
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
While inactivity is a perceived problem in captive felids, there also is often a lack of stimuli
necessary to challenge a lion to employ all senses, and according to Powell (1995) “enrichment
should take advantage of as many senses as possible”. As hunters and scavengers, lions are
innately predisposed to utilize their senses to search for viable food options. As primarily
nocturnal hunters, a lion’s primary sense is an acute color vision that reacts quickly to darkness
(McDade 2003). However, while being highly equipped visually, all senses are pertinent to a
lion’s success to locate food. Olfaction is important in the life of a lion; using scent marking as
an indicator of an animal’s sex, how recently that animal was present, and its physiological state
(Schuett and Frase 2001).
Many studies have focused on olfactory enrichment in captive felids to reduce inactivity and
stereotypies (Baker et al. 1997, Lewis 1992, Schuett et al. 2001, Noonan 1999, Pearson 2002). A
study by Baker (1997) at the Houston Zoological Gardens looked at the effect prey and
adversary feces had on lion behavior. They found that the prey species Cape hartebeest
(Alcelaphas buselaphus caama), Dorcas gazelle (Gazella dorcas), Grant’s zebra (Equus
burchelli bohmi), greater zebra (Tragelaphus strepsiceros), nyala antelope (Tragelaphus angasi),
and white–beared gnu (Connochaetus taurinus albojubatus) samples led to a significant increase
in olfaction, territorial, and social behaviors between lions, including “play behavior, rubbing,
and scent marking” (Baker 1997). These elevated behavior levels continued for the following 24
hours, whereas investigative behavior, a behavior used in the search for prey, was observed
during sample presentation and for 48 hours following the removal of the samples. Other studies
have found that zebra feces elicits higher activity levels and behaviors such as scratching,
tracking, sniffing, rolling, and social behavior versus no scent presentation that showed very low
activity levels and no social behavior (Schuett and Frase 2001, Noonan 1999). The introduction
of scents (such as cat nip, peppermint oil, rosemary, all-spice, cinnamon, and chili powder) to
large felids have all shown to increase activity levels and behaviors such as rolling, rubbing,
solitary play, and allo-grooming (Lewis 1992, Schuett et al. 2001, Pearson 2002).
The relationship between cortisol, activity levels, and olfactory enrichment has been poorly
studied. Rafacz and colleagues (2014) looked at the hormonal and behavioral response to odor
cues as enrichment in captive African wild dogs (AWD: Lycaon pictus), one dominant and one
subordinate individual. They presented three odor cues: a competitor (lion), a natural prey
(Grant’s gazelle), and an unnatural prey (cattle). Both AWDs showed a significant increase in
activity with the presentation of the gazelle cue; and while not significant, activity levels
increased during and post presentations of the lion and cattle cues. The dominant AWD
demonstrated significantly lower fecal glucocorticoid metabolite (FGM) after the lion cue than
before, while the subordinate AWD demonstrated higher FGM after the gazelle cue than before.
While the relationship between FGM and activity level was inconsistent, researchers attribute
these results to the interaction of the different social ranks.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
No published study has examined the relationship between activity levels, enrichment, and
cortisol levels in captive felids. While both the African wild dog and African lion are carnivores
and pack species, canids have a significantly more structured social organization that is
“phylogenetically more ancient and thus more highly developed” and requires greater
cooperation in hunting (Rudnai 1973). As lions have a more relaxed social structure and group
cooperation is less vital during a hunt (Rudnai 1973), presenting olfactory cues to lions may not
produce the same relationship between hormone and activity level as it did in the African wild
dogs. Additionally, the three odor cues in the AWD study were presented only once in one
location behind a chain-linked fence and therefore, were not accessible by the dogs. Presenting
each olfactory cue in an accessible location may mitigate any negative effects produced by social
structure.
It is possible that inactivity in the Detroit Zoo lions is the result of the lack of necessity to
perform species-specific behaviors, which subsequently could cause physiological side effects.
The objectives of this study are to a) determine the fecal cortisol levels in the absence of a
stimulus b) understand how presenting olfactory stimuli affects behavior and cortisol levels c)
compare the effectivenessof two novel scents: that of a prey species and the oil of a plant that
would not be encountered in a lion’s natural habitat but has been shown to increase activity
levels in other captive felid studies (Schuett et al. 2001, Pearson 1999). It is expected that the
presentation of both scent cues will increase active behaviors and decrease inactive behaviors,
which subsequently will decrease fecal cortisol levels. It is important to note that since the peak
activity levels of their wild conspecifics occur in the late night and early morning hours, it is
possible that the lions at the Detroit Zoo are active during times when we are not observing them.
If this is the case, the results may not show a significant difference in activity level before and
after enrichment presentation. Nonetheless, the expectation is that the difference still would be
evident in fecal cortisol levels.
Methods
The Detroit Zoo is home to 1.2 African lions (Panthera leo): Simba, Bikira, and Erin. Their
habitat consists of a 7500ft2
outdoor enclosure (on exhibit) and a smaller indoor enclosure.
Features of the outdoor enclosure includes several trees, a grassy area with a small pool and flat
rocks which can be heated, and a large rock wall in the rear of the enclosure that has alcoves and
flat areas. The indoor enclosure is made up of 10 individual stalls with concrete floors, mesh
barriers, an overhead chute system that allows Simba and Erin access to one another, several
rotating toys, elevated platforms within each stall with ramp access to some, movable fire hose
beds, and scratch boards. Due to old age and poor health, Bikira is housed separately from her
two conspecifics, but still has auditory, visual, and olfactory access to them when all three lions
are indoors. Simba and Erin are locked out on exhibit during the day, being let out in the
morning at approximately 10:00hr and brought in any time after 17:00hr. Observations will be
conducted when the lions are on exhibit, therefore, only Simba and Erin will be subjects of this
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
project. All further reference to the lions will exclude Bikira as she is not a part of this study.
The lions are fed in the evening when they are brought in for the night. Animals are fed a
standard feline diet with the addition of a carcass on occasion. Training sessions are provided in
the morning, before the cats are let out, anywhere from one to six times a week, depending on
the cat. Part of the lions’ diet is used as reinforcers during training sessions. The lions are trained
to perform behaviors pertinent to their husbandry and medical care: tail presentation for blood
draw and blood pressure, mouth open for mouth checks, as well as up, down, and side. The
keepers of the Detroit Zoo lions do not use an enrichment schedule, rather providing enrichment
at a random frequency. Types of enrichment used span from an array of various toys, beddings,
and scents. The lions have received various scent enrichment over their time at the Detroit Zoo: a
variety of spices and herbs, various perfumes, and also fecal samples from the Southern white
rhinoceros (Ceratotherium simum simum) at the Detroit Zoo (L. Butler, personal
communication).
Fecal samples will be collected during the three weeks prior to experimental data collection.
There will be no change in the current enrichment schedule or husbandry activities, with the
exception that no scent enrichment will be given outside of study conditions three weeks prior to
or during data collection. Behavior, exhibit location (see Figure 1), proximity to conspecific,
weather, visitor number and noise level (collected using the NoiseWatch application on an
iPhone), and substrate used for inactive behaviors will be observed and recorded for 30 minute
intervals. The introduction of olfactory enrichment will begin following three weeks of fecal
collections. The two scents will be presented separately over a six week period. Zebra will be
presented the first six weeks followed by a one week break. Peppermint oil will be presented for
three weeks followed by a two week break, and presented again for another three weeks (see
Table 1). Each scent will be given in the morning of the second day of observation each week
and taken away at the end of that second day. As the lions’ rotation schedule is often
inconsistent, it may not be possible to take out the scents at the end of the second day. In such
cases, scents will be taken out as soon as possible and keepers will record appropriately.
Enrichment items will be presented in the morning after the lions have entered the outside
enclosure. Zebra feces and a peppermint oil-saturated piece of cardboard will be presented in
separate paper bags on their corresponding days. Keepers will stand on the roof overlooking the
outdoor enclosure and toss each scent bag in the same area. Each scent will have an equal
number of presentations (see table 1). Observations will occur on the day before presentation, the
day of presentation, and the day following presentation. A four-day break will follow in which
no observations will be taken and no new scent presented. The purpose of the break is to reduce
the potential continued impact of the previous week’s enrichment on the results of any given
week.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Location will be recorded by these guidelines (all directions will be oriented from an observer’s
view point facing the enclosure): Location 1 is from the left most point of the enclosure to the
right most point of the pool. Location 2 is from the right most point of the pool to the left side of
the large tree trunk towards the middle of the enclosure. Location 3 is from the left point of the
tree trunk to the right most point of the two stalagmites. Location 4 is from the right most point
of the right stalagmite to the right most point of the enclosure (see Figure 1 for visual
representation).
Week Scent Given
-31
None
-2 None
-1 None
12
Zebra
2 Zebra
3 Zebra
4 Zebra
5 Zebra
6 Zebra
7 Break
8 Peppermint
9 Peppermint
10 Peppermint
11 Break
12 Break
13 Peppermint
14 Peppermint
15 Peppermint
Table 1. A proposed timeline for the presentation of both olfactory enrichments.
1. A negative number indicates a week that data is not collected.
2. A positive number indicates a week that data is collected.
Data will be collected on the pair of lions using an instantaneous and an all-occurrence sampling
method. Instantaneous sampling will occur at one-minute intervals for activity budget behaviors
(see table 3) and all-occurrence sampling will be recorded for the all-occurrence behaviors (see
table 4) that occur. Each observation session will be 30 minutes in length. Three observation
sessions will occur during three time intervals, respectively (see Table 2) throughout the day.
Three days of observations (Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday) will be conducted each week
for the duration of 12 weeks.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Session Time Interval
1 10:00-12:00
2 12:00-14:00
3 14:00-16:00
Table 2. Proposed data collecting time intervals for a given day.
Fecal samples for each lion will be collected as often as possible (allowing at least 24 hours
between each sample) starting the day of scent presentation and continuing for the following four
days, with a total of five potential collections each week per lion. For the three weeks prior to
data collection, fecal samples will be collected following the same schedule as the data collection
period. Shown by Fuller et al. (2011), it takes approximately one day for a lion to complete a
cycle of digestion. Therefore, the proposed fecal collection schedule is chosen to capture the
complete realm of digestion, including before the expected effects of scent presentation and
several days following. Individual samples will be marked with blue and green food coloring to
distinguish between animals. This will allow for a potential total of 15 pre-study samples and 60
study samples for a total of 75 samples per lion. The proposed fecal collection schedule is
preferred; however, as the lions’ rotation schedule is often inconsistent, five samples per week
may not be possible. In this case, as many samples as possible will be collected. An average level
in the pre-study period will be compared to average levels for each scent in the study period.
Keeper staff will be responsible for collecting samples. Samples will be stored in a Ziploc bag
marked with the lion name, date, and time of collection. After collection, samples will be stored
immediately in a -20 degrees C freezer until they are sent to the St. Louis Endocrinology lab for
analysis.
To account for extraneous husbandry variables, the keepers will complete a form each week in
which they will record enrichment given, training sessions, scent time introduction and removal,
any diet additions, time the lions are let out in the morning, nights when the lions remained
outside, and any additional factors of note.
Data Analysis
Activity budgets will be calculated by dividing the instances of each behavior by the total
number of scans. Activity budgets will be compared between baseline and enrichment conditions
(both overall and individual) using G-tests. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite levels (ng/g) will be
compared across conditions using mixed effect linear models with lion ID as a random effect
and/or non-parametric statistics (Friedman’s two way ANOVA and Wilcoxon signed rank tests).
Expected Results
It is expected that the presentation of both scent cues will correlate with an increase in active
behaviors and a decrease in inactive behaviors. Expectations are such that activity levels will be
higher on the day of and following presentation than the day before presentation. Fecal cortisol
levels will be higher during the pre-study collection than during study collection. Decreased fecal
cortisol levels will be seen starting approximately 24 hours after scent presentation each week.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Ethogram for African Lion
Instantaneous Behaviors
Behavior
Category
Behavior Definition
Inactivity2
Rest Cat has all four limbs and body on ground1
, eyes can be open or
closed, head up or down
Sit Cat has haunches and front paws on ground, chest and forearms are
upright
Stand Cat has all four paws on ground
Locomotion
Walk/Run Cat moves from one location to another with an apparent goal
Jump/Climb Cat moves from a lower point to a higher point, or vice versa, in a
single motion
Ingest
Eat Cat lowers mouth toward food (mouth is within 6in of food), uses
paw(s) to move food to mouth, inserts food into mouth, bites food
with mouth, licks food with tongue, includes jaw movement after
food insertion
Drink Cat lowers mouth toward water source or laps up water into mouth
with tongue
Other
Excretion Cat urinates or defecates, includes vomiting
Self-groom Cat licks, scratches, or bites self with the intention of cleaning
Stretch Cat extends part(s) of its body to full length
Not visible Cat is not visible to observer
Table 3. Ethogram for instantaneous behaviors.
1. Ground is defined as any substrate an animal has the opportunity to be on in its enclosure:
grass, rock, water, or tree limb.
2. Inactive behaviors are only recorded in absence of other behaviors; if animal is
performing an inactive behavior with another behavior, the other behavior takes
precedence.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
All-Occurrence Behaviors
Behavior
Category
Behavior Definition
Stereotypic Pace
Cat walks with no apparent goal; walking becomes pacing
after cat completes three rotations of the walking pattern
Olfaction
Smell/Sniff Cat inhales scent from an object, area, or the air
Flehmen
Cat opens its mouth, upper lip curled back, teeth are
exposed, face is scrunched, eyes are squinted or closed,
tongue may or may not be out; position must be held for
more than two seconds
Rub3
Cat pushes head and/or body against an object
Roll3 Cat lowers body to the ground and rotates from side to side
while lying down on back
Social
Allo-groom
Cat licks or scratches conspecific with the intention of
cleaning
Play Cat swats, pounces, “hugs”, or jumps on conspecific
Chase
Cat runs in pursuit of the other who is simultaneously
running and withdrawing
Agonistic
Dominance/Threat
(Strut)
Cat stands tall, shows side, walks stiffly, tail erect over back
Defense Display
Cat has flat ears, barred teeth, marrow eyes, head may or
may not be turned, can be crouched or lying on back, may or
may not be vocalizing
Aggression Cat slaps, grapples, or bites conspecific in a hostile manner
Object
Manipulation
Paw Manipulation Cat moves non-food object between front paws
Scratch
Cat has front claws protracted and moves claws against an
object or substrate, sweeping paw(s) towards body
Carry
Cat picks up non-food object with mouth and moves it from
one location to another
Lick Cat drags tongue across non-food object; does not include
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
grooming
Bite
Cat puts mouth around a non-food object and exerts force;
may be a form of play
Chew Cat grinds a non-food object with mouth
Other
Investigate
Cat moves head toward an object or area to gain more
information
Vocalize Cat creates any noise with its mouth(e.g. roar, hiss)
Mark
Cat sprays a vertical object (e.g. on a rock) from posterior,
tail raised or rubs paws on ground
Stalk
Cat walks slowly with legs bent slightly, eyes focused on a
specific item or conspecific
Table 4. Ethogram for all-occurrence behaviors.
3. This behavior is included in the olfaction category due to the findings of previous studies
that the introduction of novel scents produces this behavior in captive felids. (Lewis
1992, Schuett et al. 2001, Pearson 2002).
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Figure 1. Map of the outdoor lion
enclosure representing the four
possible locations.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
CZAW Lion Study-Keeper Form
*This sheet pertains to Simba and Erin only.
*Please fill out by the end of the week
*Once completed, please return to Jourdan Ringenberg,
Stephanie Allard, or Anna Murray. Thank you!! 
Please answer #1-4 for Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday.
1. Check if enrichment was given: T___ W___ Th___
2. Type of enrichment (describe below):
Day Type
Tues
Wed
Thurs
3. Training sessions (circle which lion): Tues S E Wed S E Thurs S E
4. Time let out (keep blank if they were already outside): Tues ______ Wed______ Thurs______
5. Wednesday: Scent enrichment given at ________ AM.
6. Scent enrichment was removed (please indicate day of week and time of day):
__________________________________________________________________
Please describe the following conditions that happened this week for #7-10.
7. Diet additions, type and what day(s) (carcass given, etc.):
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
________________________________________________________________________________
_
8. Simba and Erin stayed outside on which nights:
________________________________________________________________________________
_
9. Stereotypic behavior witnessed, type and what day(s) (pacing, etc.):
________________________________________________________________________________
_
10. Any unusual events that happened or other additional comments:
________________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________________
__
References
Baker, W.K., et al. (1997). Enriching the pride: scents that make sense. Worley and Hare,
editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 6(1): 1-3.
Bareham, J.R. (1972). Effects of cages and semi-intensive deep litter pens on the behavior,
adrenal responses and production in two strains of laying hens. Br. Vet. J. 128: 153-163.
Breton, G., Barrot, S. (2014). Influence of enclosure size on the distances covered and paced by
captive tigers (Panthera tigris). Applied Animal Behavior Science. 154: 66-75.
Cabib, S., Puglisis-Allegra, S., Oliverio, A. (1985). A genetic analysis of sterotypy in the mouse.
Behavioral-neural Biology. 44: 239-248.
Cronin, G.M. (1985). The development and significance of abnormal stereotyped behaviours in
tethered sows [Ph.D thesis].The Netherlands: Agricultural University of Wageningen.
Duncan, I.J.H. (1970). Frustration in the fowl. B.M. Freeman and R.G. Gordon, editors. Aspects
of Poultry Behavior. Edinburgh: British Poultry Science. p 15-31.
Estes, R.D. (1991). The behavior guide to African mammals: including hoofed mammals,
carnivores, primates. Berkeley, CA: The University of California Press. P 369-377
Fuller, G., Margulis, S.W., Santymire, R. (2011). The Effectiveness of Indigestible Markers for
Identifying Individual Animal Feces and Their Prevalence of Use in North American
Zoos. Zoo Biology. 30 (4): 379-398.
Kelling A.S. et.al. (2012). Lion, Ungulate, and Visitor Reactions to Playbacks of Lion Roars at
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Zoo Atlanta. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 15: 313-328.
Kuhar, C.W., Bettinger T.L., Laudenslager, M.L. (2008). Using cortisol to assess well-being:
limitations and daydreams. Bettinger and Bielitzki, editors. The Well-being of Animals in
Zoo and Aquarium-Sponsored Research: Putting Best Practices Forward. Greenbelt,
MD: Scientists Center for Animal Welfare. (2): 53-67.
Lewis, C. (1992). Cat nips. In: Hare and Woreley, editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego:
The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 1 (2): 1-2.
Mason, G.J. (1991). Stereotypies: a critical review. Animal Behaviour. Roper, editor. London:
Academic Press. 41 (6). 1017-
Mason, G.J. ed. (2010). Species differences in responses to captivity: stress, welfare and the
comparative method. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 25 (12): 713-721.
McDade, M.C. (2003). Grzimek’s Animal Life Encyclopedia. Farmington Hills, MI: Gale. 2
(14): 379-381.
McPhee, M. E. (2002). Intact carcasses as enrichment for large felids: effects on on- and off-
exhibit behaviors.
Metter, V. J. E., Harriger, M. D. & Bolen, R. H. (2007). Environmental enrichment utilizing
stimulus objects for African lions (Panthera leo) and Sumatran tigers (Panthera tigris
sumatrae). Bios. 79 (1): 7-16.
Noonan, B. (1999). Enrichment for African lions. Worley and Hare, editors. The Shape of
Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 8 (2): 6-7.
Pearson, J. (2002). On a roll: novel objects and scent enrichment for Asiatic lions. The Shape of
Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 11 (3): 7-10.
Powell, D.M. (1995). Preoiminary Evaluation of Environmental Enrichment Techniques for
African Lions (Panthera leo). Animal Welfare. 4 (4): 361-370.
Rafacz, M.L. and Santymire, R.M. (2014). Using odor cues to elicit a behavioral and hormonal
response in zoo-housed African wild dogs. Watters, editor. Zoo Biology. 33 (2): 144-149.
Rudnai, J.A. (1973). The Social Life of the Lion: A study of the behavior of wild lions in the
Nairobi National Park, Kenya. Wallingford, Pennsylvania: Washington Square East.
Schuett, E.B. and Frase, B.A. (2001). Making scents: using the olfactory senses for lion
enrichment. Worley and Hare, editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape
of Enrichment, Inc. 10 (3): 1-3.
Sheperdson, D.J., Carlstead K.C., Wielebnowski N. (2004). Cross-institutional assessment of
stress responses in zoo animals using longitudinal monitoring of faecal corticoids and
behavior. Animal Welfare. 13: 105-113.
Jourdan Ringenberg
CZAW Resident
Session II 2014
Skibiel, A.L., Trevino, H.S. & Naugher, K. (2007). Comparison of several types of enrichment
for captive felids. Zoo Biology. 26: 371-381.
Van Metter, J. E., Harriger, M. D. & Bolen, R. H. (2008). Environmental enrichment utilizing
stimulus objects for African lions (Panthera leo leo) and Sumatran tigers (Panthera tigris
sumatrae). BIOS. 79: 7-16.
Wiepkema, P.R. (1984). Stereotypies and endorphins: functional significance of developing
stereotypies in tethered sows. Unshelm, van Putten, and Zeeb, editors. Proceedings of the
International Congress on Applied Ethology in Farm Animals. Dalmstadt: KTBL. p. 93-
96.
Wiepkema, P.R. (1987). Behavioral aspects of stress. Van Adrichem and Wiepkema, editors.
The Biology of Stress in Farm Animals: an Integrated Approach. Dordrecht: Martinus
Nijhoff. p. 113-134.
Wilson, D.E., Mittermeier, R.A. eds. (2009). Handbook of the Mammals of the World.
Barcelona: Lynx Edicions. 1 (1): p. 54-127, 137-139.

More Related Content

What's hot

Lotka volterra model
Lotka   volterra modelLotka   volterra model
Lotka volterra modelSandeep Kumar
 
50; corucia antibiotic therapy
50; corucia antibiotic therapy50; corucia antibiotic therapy
50; corucia antibiotic therapyRoger Meek
 
Animal behavior in changing environment
Animal behavior in changing environmentAnimal behavior in changing environment
Animal behavior in changing environmentAkansha Ganguly
 
Fixed action pattern (fap)
Fixed action pattern (fap)Fixed action pattern (fap)
Fixed action pattern (fap)Jsjahnabi
 
An introduction to ethology
An introduction to ethologyAn introduction to ethology
An introduction to ethologyhasnain akmal
 
Ethology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior is
Ethology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior isEthology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior is
Ethology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior isDillon Jones
 
Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016Kate
 
Ap chap 51 animal behavior
Ap chap 51 animal behaviorAp chap 51 animal behavior
Ap chap 51 animal behaviorsmithbio
 
E3 Innate And Learned Behaviour
E3 Innate And Learned BehaviourE3 Innate And Learned Behaviour
E3 Innate And Learned BehaviourStephen Taylor
 
Western Lowland Gorillas
Western Lowland GorillasWestern Lowland Gorillas
Western Lowland GorillasEmma Thurau
 
Principle of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behavior
Principle of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behaviorPrinciple of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behavior
Principle of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behaviorDIPJYOTIBORAH3
 
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_FinalAlicia Peluso
 
Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.
Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.
Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.vidan biology
 
Ecological interactions chapter 3 class 11th
Ecological interactions chapter 3 class 11thEcological interactions chapter 3 class 11th
Ecological interactions chapter 3 class 11thUrfan Ali
 

What's hot (19)

Lotka volterra model
Lotka   volterra modelLotka   volterra model
Lotka volterra model
 
50; corucia antibiotic therapy
50; corucia antibiotic therapy50; corucia antibiotic therapy
50; corucia antibiotic therapy
 
Animal behavior in changing environment
Animal behavior in changing environmentAnimal behavior in changing environment
Animal behavior in changing environment
 
Fixed action pattern (fap)
Fixed action pattern (fap)Fixed action pattern (fap)
Fixed action pattern (fap)
 
An introduction to ethology
An introduction to ethologyAn introduction to ethology
An introduction to ethology
 
Ethology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior is
Ethology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior isEthology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior is
Ethology: How, Why, and What Animal Behavior is
 
Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016
 
Ap chap 51 animal behavior
Ap chap 51 animal behaviorAp chap 51 animal behavior
Ap chap 51 animal behavior
 
E3 Innate And Learned Behaviour
E3 Innate And Learned BehaviourE3 Innate And Learned Behaviour
E3 Innate And Learned Behaviour
 
Western Lowland Gorillas
Western Lowland GorillasWestern Lowland Gorillas
Western Lowland Gorillas
 
Principle of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behavior
Principle of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behaviorPrinciple of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behavior
Principle of Ethology with special reference to pattern of behavior
 
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
 
Animal behavior powerpoint
Animal behavior powerpointAnimal behavior powerpoint
Animal behavior powerpoint
 
Animal behavior
Animal behaviorAnimal behavior
Animal behavior
 
Habituation
HabituationHabituation
Habituation
 
Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.
Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.
Innate Behavior (Animal Behavior) Taxis, Fixed Action Pattern, reflex.
 
Ecological interactions chapter 3 class 11th
Ecological interactions chapter 3 class 11thEcological interactions chapter 3 class 11th
Ecological interactions chapter 3 class 11th
 
Animal behavior
Animal behavior Animal behavior
Animal behavior
 
Fixed action pattern
Fixed action patternFixed action pattern
Fixed action pattern
 

Similar to The Impact of Olfactory Stimulation on the Behavior and Hormone Levels of the African Lions at the Detroit Zoo

Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior AnalysisAnimal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior AnalysisAglazer1
 
The Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple Species
The Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple SpeciesThe Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple Species
The Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple SpeciesKristina Parker
 
ppt scope of animal behavior.pptx
ppt scope of animal behavior.pptxppt scope of animal behavior.pptx
ppt scope of animal behavior.pptxShivaniSharma217401
 
Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...
Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...
Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...SITIE NADIA ZAKARIA
 
Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...
Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...
Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...Deepa Menon
 
The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio Anubis) in Gash...
The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio  Anubis) in Gash...The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio  Anubis) in Gash...
The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio Anubis) in Gash...Sryahwa Publications
 
wall seeking behaviour in mice.pptx
wall seeking behaviour in mice.pptxwall seeking behaviour in mice.pptx
wall seeking behaviour in mice.pptxSiddharth Rajput
 
Animal and Plant Adaptations
Animal and Plant AdaptationsAnimal and Plant Adaptations
Animal and Plant AdaptationsAlex Noudelman
 
Animal Behavior and it's type to show the
Animal Behavior and it's type to show theAnimal Behavior and it's type to show the
Animal Behavior and it's type to show thehkpatir1996
 
Instinct and Learning Behavior.pdf
Instinct and Learning Behavior.pdfInstinct and Learning Behavior.pdf
Instinct and Learning Behavior.pdfMammalssite
 
adaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptation
adaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptationadaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptation
adaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptationrachitbafna
 

Similar to The Impact of Olfactory Stimulation on the Behavior and Hormone Levels of the African Lions at the Detroit Zoo (20)

Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior AnalysisAnimal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
Animal Behavior: Hippopotamus Behavior Analysis
 
Marina's thesis v1
Marina's thesis v1Marina's thesis v1
Marina's thesis v1
 
Cow beh1
Cow beh1Cow beh1
Cow beh1
 
Zebra fish
Zebra fishZebra fish
Zebra fish
 
Cow beh1
Cow beh1Cow beh1
Cow beh1
 
The Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple Species
The Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple SpeciesThe Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple Species
The Implications of Coprophagy in Multiple Species
 
6th Grade Chapter 5
6th Grade  Chapter 56th Grade  Chapter 5
6th Grade Chapter 5
 
Cat attachment behavior
Cat attachment behaviorCat attachment behavior
Cat attachment behavior
 
ppt scope of animal behavior.pptx
ppt scope of animal behavior.pptxppt scope of animal behavior.pptx
ppt scope of animal behavior.pptx
 
Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...
Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...
Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kela...
 
Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...
Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...
Understanding the behavior of domestic emus a means to improve their manageme...
 
Social organization
Social organizationSocial organization
Social organization
 
The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio Anubis) in Gash...
The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio  Anubis) in Gash...The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio  Anubis) in Gash...
The Social Daily Activity Correlation of Olive Baboon (Papio Anubis) in Gash...
 
wall seeking behaviour in mice.pptx
wall seeking behaviour in mice.pptxwall seeking behaviour in mice.pptx
wall seeking behaviour in mice.pptx
 
Animal and Plant Adaptations
Animal and Plant AdaptationsAnimal and Plant Adaptations
Animal and Plant Adaptations
 
Cow beh
Cow behCow beh
Cow beh
 
PB Habitat Paper
PB Habitat PaperPB Habitat Paper
PB Habitat Paper
 
Animal Behavior and it's type to show the
Animal Behavior and it's type to show theAnimal Behavior and it's type to show the
Animal Behavior and it's type to show the
 
Instinct and Learning Behavior.pdf
Instinct and Learning Behavior.pdfInstinct and Learning Behavior.pdf
Instinct and Learning Behavior.pdf
 
adaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptation
adaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptationadaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptation
adaptation - characteristics of life. types of adaptation
 

The Impact of Olfactory Stimulation on the Behavior and Hormone Levels of the African Lions at the Detroit Zoo

  • 1. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 The Impact of Olfactory Stimulation on the Behavior and Hormone Levels of the African Lions at the Detroit Zoo Introduction African lions (Panthera leo) are found in the open woodlands of Sub-Saharan Africa with a habitat covering a wide range of mixed areas of thick bush, scrub, and grass. An obligatory carnivore, their diet consists of medium to large ungulates (i.e. buffalo, zebra, wildebeest, waterbuck); small rodents and birds; young rhinos, elephants, hippos; and giraffes (McDade 2003). Lions typically are nocturnal hunters, with peak activity levels between 21:30-22:30h and 2:00-4:00h (Wilson et al. 2009). Activity levels also increase in the early morning and late afternoon during the cooler hours of the day. To allow for proper digestion and to avoid the heat of the day, lions spend 20-21 hours resting per day (Estes 1991). Resting periods may be interrupted by brief bouts of activity when engaging in social behaviors or hunting (Wilson et.al. 2009). After stalking their prey to a close range, they make a short dash to catch prey and kill by suffocation, clamping their jaws on the prey’s windpipe or muzzle (McDade 2003). Both male and females work together to catch prey, often alternating who catches prey based on prey size, large and small, respectively. Lions, social animals that live in prides of four to six individuals on average, keep territories anywhere from eight to 200m2 (McDade 2003). During the dry season when water sources are scarce, lions may need to travel further distances to find food, often inducing the need to scavenge. In captivity, zoos are faced with the challenge of inducing the hunting and scavenging behaviors instinctive to a lion, often failing to satisfy the need to utilize these natural behaviors. One study stated that since a natural environment only requires big cats to spend a portion of their daily life stalking and killing prey, the “single greatest period of intensive activity and energy expenditure is during the hunt for food” (Baker 1997). Wild conspecifics often spend three to four hours a day hunting and scavenging for food where they may be forced to travel long distances and utilize their senses to aid in their search. As animals in captivity no longer have the need to provide their own nourishment, there subsequently is an absence of food-finding behaviors and sensory stimulation. While this provides more “free” time, not filling this time with another stimulating activity or behavior could impact the welfare of the animal. The absence of these species-specific behaviors removes the necessity to use sensory input and also poses the potential to replace this time with inactivity or stereotypic behaviors. Mason stated that behavioral inflexibility may indicate an “inadequate ability to suppress natural activities that a captive environment makes impossible” (Mason 2010). This could be detrimental to captive species welfare, as it has the potential to cause chronic frustration and may induce coping methods such as stereotypic behavior or an increased level of inactivity. The inability to express species-specific behaviors such as hunting, scavenging, and using sensory cues to locate food could result in a prolonged negative behavioral and physiological
  • 2. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 response in captive lions. An animal motivated to perform a behavior pattern in a situation, but unable to do so, becomes frustrated (Mason 1991). A repetitive situation that produces chronic frustration may lead to a negative physiological or behavioral response that can be deleterious for the animal (Mason 1991). The presence of prolonged frustration in any animal presents an opportunity to develop a coping strategy. A stereotypy is one behavioral response in which an animal can cope with prolonged frustration (Mason 1991). The most commonly seen stereotypy in big cats is pacing (Mason 2010, Breton 2014); however, upon initial observation of the lions at the Detroit Zoo, pacing seldom is seen. The lack of pacing in the lions suggests that the inability to perform natural behaviors either does not cause a negative behavioral response or that the lions are substituting that time with a different behavior. Initial observations of these lions indicate a high level of inactivity; suggesting that inactivity, or a lack of behavior, may be their form of substitution. However, it is important to note that since the peak activity levels of their wild conspecifics occur in the late night and early morning hours, it is possible that the lions at the Detroit Zoo are active during times when we are not observing them. Both stereotypic behavior and inactivity are potential ways for an animal to substitute for species-specific behaviors they may be less able to perform. However, while stereotypies allow the animal to perform some type of behavior, inactivity is a lack of behavior. Therefore, inactivity may not be as effective of a substitute as a stereotypic behavior. Researchers have found that in some species, animals that display stereotypic patterns have lower glucocorticoid levels than animals who do not display these patterns (Kuhar 2008, Duncan 1970, Barehem 1972, Wiepkema 1984, 1987, Cronin 1985, Kennes et al. 1988, Cabib et al. 1985, Sheperdson 2004). Duncan (1970) found that stereotypic pacing in domestic fowl correlated with a decrease in corticosteroid levels, and adrenal weight showed to be negatively correlated with head- flicking in this species (Barehem 1972). A study by Shepherdson and colleagues (2004) found that polar bears performing stereotypic behaviors had lower peak cortisol levels and greater cortisol level variation than polar bears that did not perform stereotypies. Wiepkema (1984, 1987) determined that older sows that perform stereotypies have no hormonal response, and veal calves showed a significant negative relationship between the severity of abomasal lesions and stereotypic tongue-playing. Conversely, in some species, the absence of stereotypic behaviors correlates with increased stress hormone levels. Young, tethered sows that perform no stereotypic behavior, versus their older conspecifics, have a chronic rise in corticosteroids (Cronin 1985). A study with voles looked at the effects that lowering cage ceilings had on their stereotypic jumping behavior. The lower ceilings removed the ability to perform this behavior and corticosteroid levels subsequently increased (Kennes et al. 1988) Responding to these changes, some voles developed a new type of stereotypy, which returned corticosteroid to normal levels (Kennes et al. 1988). Similarly, a study on mice by Cabib et al. (1985) found that stereotypies “induced by stimulant drugs” are depressed by the introduction of stressors, indicating a lack of behavior when exposed to a source of stress. While stereotypies often are construed as negative reactions to an environment, these studies suggest that for some individuals, stereotypic behaviors do provide some relief to prolonged frustration versus no behavior at all (inactivity).
  • 3. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 While inactivity is a perceived problem in captive felids, there also is often a lack of stimuli necessary to challenge a lion to employ all senses, and according to Powell (1995) “enrichment should take advantage of as many senses as possible”. As hunters and scavengers, lions are innately predisposed to utilize their senses to search for viable food options. As primarily nocturnal hunters, a lion’s primary sense is an acute color vision that reacts quickly to darkness (McDade 2003). However, while being highly equipped visually, all senses are pertinent to a lion’s success to locate food. Olfaction is important in the life of a lion; using scent marking as an indicator of an animal’s sex, how recently that animal was present, and its physiological state (Schuett and Frase 2001). Many studies have focused on olfactory enrichment in captive felids to reduce inactivity and stereotypies (Baker et al. 1997, Lewis 1992, Schuett et al. 2001, Noonan 1999, Pearson 2002). A study by Baker (1997) at the Houston Zoological Gardens looked at the effect prey and adversary feces had on lion behavior. They found that the prey species Cape hartebeest (Alcelaphas buselaphus caama), Dorcas gazelle (Gazella dorcas), Grant’s zebra (Equus burchelli bohmi), greater zebra (Tragelaphus strepsiceros), nyala antelope (Tragelaphus angasi), and white–beared gnu (Connochaetus taurinus albojubatus) samples led to a significant increase in olfaction, territorial, and social behaviors between lions, including “play behavior, rubbing, and scent marking” (Baker 1997). These elevated behavior levels continued for the following 24 hours, whereas investigative behavior, a behavior used in the search for prey, was observed during sample presentation and for 48 hours following the removal of the samples. Other studies have found that zebra feces elicits higher activity levels and behaviors such as scratching, tracking, sniffing, rolling, and social behavior versus no scent presentation that showed very low activity levels and no social behavior (Schuett and Frase 2001, Noonan 1999). The introduction of scents (such as cat nip, peppermint oil, rosemary, all-spice, cinnamon, and chili powder) to large felids have all shown to increase activity levels and behaviors such as rolling, rubbing, solitary play, and allo-grooming (Lewis 1992, Schuett et al. 2001, Pearson 2002). The relationship between cortisol, activity levels, and olfactory enrichment has been poorly studied. Rafacz and colleagues (2014) looked at the hormonal and behavioral response to odor cues as enrichment in captive African wild dogs (AWD: Lycaon pictus), one dominant and one subordinate individual. They presented three odor cues: a competitor (lion), a natural prey (Grant’s gazelle), and an unnatural prey (cattle). Both AWDs showed a significant increase in activity with the presentation of the gazelle cue; and while not significant, activity levels increased during and post presentations of the lion and cattle cues. The dominant AWD demonstrated significantly lower fecal glucocorticoid metabolite (FGM) after the lion cue than before, while the subordinate AWD demonstrated higher FGM after the gazelle cue than before. While the relationship between FGM and activity level was inconsistent, researchers attribute these results to the interaction of the different social ranks.
  • 4. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 No published study has examined the relationship between activity levels, enrichment, and cortisol levels in captive felids. While both the African wild dog and African lion are carnivores and pack species, canids have a significantly more structured social organization that is “phylogenetically more ancient and thus more highly developed” and requires greater cooperation in hunting (Rudnai 1973). As lions have a more relaxed social structure and group cooperation is less vital during a hunt (Rudnai 1973), presenting olfactory cues to lions may not produce the same relationship between hormone and activity level as it did in the African wild dogs. Additionally, the three odor cues in the AWD study were presented only once in one location behind a chain-linked fence and therefore, were not accessible by the dogs. Presenting each olfactory cue in an accessible location may mitigate any negative effects produced by social structure. It is possible that inactivity in the Detroit Zoo lions is the result of the lack of necessity to perform species-specific behaviors, which subsequently could cause physiological side effects. The objectives of this study are to a) determine the fecal cortisol levels in the absence of a stimulus b) understand how presenting olfactory stimuli affects behavior and cortisol levels c) compare the effectivenessof two novel scents: that of a prey species and the oil of a plant that would not be encountered in a lion’s natural habitat but has been shown to increase activity levels in other captive felid studies (Schuett et al. 2001, Pearson 1999). It is expected that the presentation of both scent cues will increase active behaviors and decrease inactive behaviors, which subsequently will decrease fecal cortisol levels. It is important to note that since the peak activity levels of their wild conspecifics occur in the late night and early morning hours, it is possible that the lions at the Detroit Zoo are active during times when we are not observing them. If this is the case, the results may not show a significant difference in activity level before and after enrichment presentation. Nonetheless, the expectation is that the difference still would be evident in fecal cortisol levels. Methods The Detroit Zoo is home to 1.2 African lions (Panthera leo): Simba, Bikira, and Erin. Their habitat consists of a 7500ft2 outdoor enclosure (on exhibit) and a smaller indoor enclosure. Features of the outdoor enclosure includes several trees, a grassy area with a small pool and flat rocks which can be heated, and a large rock wall in the rear of the enclosure that has alcoves and flat areas. The indoor enclosure is made up of 10 individual stalls with concrete floors, mesh barriers, an overhead chute system that allows Simba and Erin access to one another, several rotating toys, elevated platforms within each stall with ramp access to some, movable fire hose beds, and scratch boards. Due to old age and poor health, Bikira is housed separately from her two conspecifics, but still has auditory, visual, and olfactory access to them when all three lions are indoors. Simba and Erin are locked out on exhibit during the day, being let out in the morning at approximately 10:00hr and brought in any time after 17:00hr. Observations will be conducted when the lions are on exhibit, therefore, only Simba and Erin will be subjects of this
  • 5. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 project. All further reference to the lions will exclude Bikira as she is not a part of this study. The lions are fed in the evening when they are brought in for the night. Animals are fed a standard feline diet with the addition of a carcass on occasion. Training sessions are provided in the morning, before the cats are let out, anywhere from one to six times a week, depending on the cat. Part of the lions’ diet is used as reinforcers during training sessions. The lions are trained to perform behaviors pertinent to their husbandry and medical care: tail presentation for blood draw and blood pressure, mouth open for mouth checks, as well as up, down, and side. The keepers of the Detroit Zoo lions do not use an enrichment schedule, rather providing enrichment at a random frequency. Types of enrichment used span from an array of various toys, beddings, and scents. The lions have received various scent enrichment over their time at the Detroit Zoo: a variety of spices and herbs, various perfumes, and also fecal samples from the Southern white rhinoceros (Ceratotherium simum simum) at the Detroit Zoo (L. Butler, personal communication). Fecal samples will be collected during the three weeks prior to experimental data collection. There will be no change in the current enrichment schedule or husbandry activities, with the exception that no scent enrichment will be given outside of study conditions three weeks prior to or during data collection. Behavior, exhibit location (see Figure 1), proximity to conspecific, weather, visitor number and noise level (collected using the NoiseWatch application on an iPhone), and substrate used for inactive behaviors will be observed and recorded for 30 minute intervals. The introduction of olfactory enrichment will begin following three weeks of fecal collections. The two scents will be presented separately over a six week period. Zebra will be presented the first six weeks followed by a one week break. Peppermint oil will be presented for three weeks followed by a two week break, and presented again for another three weeks (see Table 1). Each scent will be given in the morning of the second day of observation each week and taken away at the end of that second day. As the lions’ rotation schedule is often inconsistent, it may not be possible to take out the scents at the end of the second day. In such cases, scents will be taken out as soon as possible and keepers will record appropriately. Enrichment items will be presented in the morning after the lions have entered the outside enclosure. Zebra feces and a peppermint oil-saturated piece of cardboard will be presented in separate paper bags on their corresponding days. Keepers will stand on the roof overlooking the outdoor enclosure and toss each scent bag in the same area. Each scent will have an equal number of presentations (see table 1). Observations will occur on the day before presentation, the day of presentation, and the day following presentation. A four-day break will follow in which no observations will be taken and no new scent presented. The purpose of the break is to reduce the potential continued impact of the previous week’s enrichment on the results of any given week.
  • 6. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 Location will be recorded by these guidelines (all directions will be oriented from an observer’s view point facing the enclosure): Location 1 is from the left most point of the enclosure to the right most point of the pool. Location 2 is from the right most point of the pool to the left side of the large tree trunk towards the middle of the enclosure. Location 3 is from the left point of the tree trunk to the right most point of the two stalagmites. Location 4 is from the right most point of the right stalagmite to the right most point of the enclosure (see Figure 1 for visual representation). Week Scent Given -31 None -2 None -1 None 12 Zebra 2 Zebra 3 Zebra 4 Zebra 5 Zebra 6 Zebra 7 Break 8 Peppermint 9 Peppermint 10 Peppermint 11 Break 12 Break 13 Peppermint 14 Peppermint 15 Peppermint Table 1. A proposed timeline for the presentation of both olfactory enrichments. 1. A negative number indicates a week that data is not collected. 2. A positive number indicates a week that data is collected. Data will be collected on the pair of lions using an instantaneous and an all-occurrence sampling method. Instantaneous sampling will occur at one-minute intervals for activity budget behaviors (see table 3) and all-occurrence sampling will be recorded for the all-occurrence behaviors (see table 4) that occur. Each observation session will be 30 minutes in length. Three observation sessions will occur during three time intervals, respectively (see Table 2) throughout the day. Three days of observations (Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday) will be conducted each week for the duration of 12 weeks.
  • 7. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 Session Time Interval 1 10:00-12:00 2 12:00-14:00 3 14:00-16:00 Table 2. Proposed data collecting time intervals for a given day. Fecal samples for each lion will be collected as often as possible (allowing at least 24 hours between each sample) starting the day of scent presentation and continuing for the following four days, with a total of five potential collections each week per lion. For the three weeks prior to data collection, fecal samples will be collected following the same schedule as the data collection period. Shown by Fuller et al. (2011), it takes approximately one day for a lion to complete a cycle of digestion. Therefore, the proposed fecal collection schedule is chosen to capture the complete realm of digestion, including before the expected effects of scent presentation and several days following. Individual samples will be marked with blue and green food coloring to distinguish between animals. This will allow for a potential total of 15 pre-study samples and 60 study samples for a total of 75 samples per lion. The proposed fecal collection schedule is preferred; however, as the lions’ rotation schedule is often inconsistent, five samples per week may not be possible. In this case, as many samples as possible will be collected. An average level in the pre-study period will be compared to average levels for each scent in the study period. Keeper staff will be responsible for collecting samples. Samples will be stored in a Ziploc bag marked with the lion name, date, and time of collection. After collection, samples will be stored immediately in a -20 degrees C freezer until they are sent to the St. Louis Endocrinology lab for analysis. To account for extraneous husbandry variables, the keepers will complete a form each week in which they will record enrichment given, training sessions, scent time introduction and removal, any diet additions, time the lions are let out in the morning, nights when the lions remained outside, and any additional factors of note. Data Analysis Activity budgets will be calculated by dividing the instances of each behavior by the total number of scans. Activity budgets will be compared between baseline and enrichment conditions (both overall and individual) using G-tests. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite levels (ng/g) will be compared across conditions using mixed effect linear models with lion ID as a random effect and/or non-parametric statistics (Friedman’s two way ANOVA and Wilcoxon signed rank tests). Expected Results It is expected that the presentation of both scent cues will correlate with an increase in active behaviors and a decrease in inactive behaviors. Expectations are such that activity levels will be higher on the day of and following presentation than the day before presentation. Fecal cortisol levels will be higher during the pre-study collection than during study collection. Decreased fecal cortisol levels will be seen starting approximately 24 hours after scent presentation each week.
  • 9. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 Ethogram for African Lion Instantaneous Behaviors Behavior Category Behavior Definition Inactivity2 Rest Cat has all four limbs and body on ground1 , eyes can be open or closed, head up or down Sit Cat has haunches and front paws on ground, chest and forearms are upright Stand Cat has all four paws on ground Locomotion Walk/Run Cat moves from one location to another with an apparent goal Jump/Climb Cat moves from a lower point to a higher point, or vice versa, in a single motion Ingest Eat Cat lowers mouth toward food (mouth is within 6in of food), uses paw(s) to move food to mouth, inserts food into mouth, bites food with mouth, licks food with tongue, includes jaw movement after food insertion Drink Cat lowers mouth toward water source or laps up water into mouth with tongue Other Excretion Cat urinates or defecates, includes vomiting Self-groom Cat licks, scratches, or bites self with the intention of cleaning Stretch Cat extends part(s) of its body to full length Not visible Cat is not visible to observer Table 3. Ethogram for instantaneous behaviors. 1. Ground is defined as any substrate an animal has the opportunity to be on in its enclosure: grass, rock, water, or tree limb. 2. Inactive behaviors are only recorded in absence of other behaviors; if animal is performing an inactive behavior with another behavior, the other behavior takes precedence.
  • 10. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 All-Occurrence Behaviors Behavior Category Behavior Definition Stereotypic Pace Cat walks with no apparent goal; walking becomes pacing after cat completes three rotations of the walking pattern Olfaction Smell/Sniff Cat inhales scent from an object, area, or the air Flehmen Cat opens its mouth, upper lip curled back, teeth are exposed, face is scrunched, eyes are squinted or closed, tongue may or may not be out; position must be held for more than two seconds Rub3 Cat pushes head and/or body against an object Roll3 Cat lowers body to the ground and rotates from side to side while lying down on back Social Allo-groom Cat licks or scratches conspecific with the intention of cleaning Play Cat swats, pounces, “hugs”, or jumps on conspecific Chase Cat runs in pursuit of the other who is simultaneously running and withdrawing Agonistic Dominance/Threat (Strut) Cat stands tall, shows side, walks stiffly, tail erect over back Defense Display Cat has flat ears, barred teeth, marrow eyes, head may or may not be turned, can be crouched or lying on back, may or may not be vocalizing Aggression Cat slaps, grapples, or bites conspecific in a hostile manner Object Manipulation Paw Manipulation Cat moves non-food object between front paws Scratch Cat has front claws protracted and moves claws against an object or substrate, sweeping paw(s) towards body Carry Cat picks up non-food object with mouth and moves it from one location to another Lick Cat drags tongue across non-food object; does not include
  • 11. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 grooming Bite Cat puts mouth around a non-food object and exerts force; may be a form of play Chew Cat grinds a non-food object with mouth Other Investigate Cat moves head toward an object or area to gain more information Vocalize Cat creates any noise with its mouth(e.g. roar, hiss) Mark Cat sprays a vertical object (e.g. on a rock) from posterior, tail raised or rubs paws on ground Stalk Cat walks slowly with legs bent slightly, eyes focused on a specific item or conspecific Table 4. Ethogram for all-occurrence behaviors. 3. This behavior is included in the olfaction category due to the findings of previous studies that the introduction of novel scents produces this behavior in captive felids. (Lewis 1992, Schuett et al. 2001, Pearson 2002).
  • 13. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 Figure 1. Map of the outdoor lion enclosure representing the four possible locations.
  • 14. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 CZAW Lion Study-Keeper Form *This sheet pertains to Simba and Erin only. *Please fill out by the end of the week *Once completed, please return to Jourdan Ringenberg, Stephanie Allard, or Anna Murray. Thank you!!  Please answer #1-4 for Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday. 1. Check if enrichment was given: T___ W___ Th___ 2. Type of enrichment (describe below): Day Type Tues Wed Thurs 3. Training sessions (circle which lion): Tues S E Wed S E Thurs S E 4. Time let out (keep blank if they were already outside): Tues ______ Wed______ Thurs______ 5. Wednesday: Scent enrichment given at ________ AM. 6. Scent enrichment was removed (please indicate day of week and time of day): __________________________________________________________________ Please describe the following conditions that happened this week for #7-10. 7. Diet additions, type and what day(s) (carcass given, etc.):
  • 15. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 ________________________________________________________________________________ _ 8. Simba and Erin stayed outside on which nights: ________________________________________________________________________________ _ 9. Stereotypic behavior witnessed, type and what day(s) (pacing, etc.): ________________________________________________________________________________ _ 10. Any unusual events that happened or other additional comments: ________________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________________ __ References Baker, W.K., et al. (1997). Enriching the pride: scents that make sense. Worley and Hare, editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 6(1): 1-3. Bareham, J.R. (1972). Effects of cages and semi-intensive deep litter pens on the behavior, adrenal responses and production in two strains of laying hens. Br. Vet. J. 128: 153-163. Breton, G., Barrot, S. (2014). Influence of enclosure size on the distances covered and paced by captive tigers (Panthera tigris). Applied Animal Behavior Science. 154: 66-75. Cabib, S., Puglisis-Allegra, S., Oliverio, A. (1985). A genetic analysis of sterotypy in the mouse. Behavioral-neural Biology. 44: 239-248. Cronin, G.M. (1985). The development and significance of abnormal stereotyped behaviours in tethered sows [Ph.D thesis].The Netherlands: Agricultural University of Wageningen. Duncan, I.J.H. (1970). Frustration in the fowl. B.M. Freeman and R.G. Gordon, editors. Aspects of Poultry Behavior. Edinburgh: British Poultry Science. p 15-31. Estes, R.D. (1991). The behavior guide to African mammals: including hoofed mammals, carnivores, primates. Berkeley, CA: The University of California Press. P 369-377 Fuller, G., Margulis, S.W., Santymire, R. (2011). The Effectiveness of Indigestible Markers for Identifying Individual Animal Feces and Their Prevalence of Use in North American Zoos. Zoo Biology. 30 (4): 379-398. Kelling A.S. et.al. (2012). Lion, Ungulate, and Visitor Reactions to Playbacks of Lion Roars at
  • 16. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 Zoo Atlanta. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 15: 313-328. Kuhar, C.W., Bettinger T.L., Laudenslager, M.L. (2008). Using cortisol to assess well-being: limitations and daydreams. Bettinger and Bielitzki, editors. The Well-being of Animals in Zoo and Aquarium-Sponsored Research: Putting Best Practices Forward. Greenbelt, MD: Scientists Center for Animal Welfare. (2): 53-67. Lewis, C. (1992). Cat nips. In: Hare and Woreley, editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 1 (2): 1-2. Mason, G.J. (1991). Stereotypies: a critical review. Animal Behaviour. Roper, editor. London: Academic Press. 41 (6). 1017- Mason, G.J. ed. (2010). Species differences in responses to captivity: stress, welfare and the comparative method. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 25 (12): 713-721. McDade, M.C. (2003). Grzimek’s Animal Life Encyclopedia. Farmington Hills, MI: Gale. 2 (14): 379-381. McPhee, M. E. (2002). Intact carcasses as enrichment for large felids: effects on on- and off- exhibit behaviors. Metter, V. J. E., Harriger, M. D. & Bolen, R. H. (2007). Environmental enrichment utilizing stimulus objects for African lions (Panthera leo) and Sumatran tigers (Panthera tigris sumatrae). Bios. 79 (1): 7-16. Noonan, B. (1999). Enrichment for African lions. Worley and Hare, editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 8 (2): 6-7. Pearson, J. (2002). On a roll: novel objects and scent enrichment for Asiatic lions. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 11 (3): 7-10. Powell, D.M. (1995). Preoiminary Evaluation of Environmental Enrichment Techniques for African Lions (Panthera leo). Animal Welfare. 4 (4): 361-370. Rafacz, M.L. and Santymire, R.M. (2014). Using odor cues to elicit a behavioral and hormonal response in zoo-housed African wild dogs. Watters, editor. Zoo Biology. 33 (2): 144-149. Rudnai, J.A. (1973). The Social Life of the Lion: A study of the behavior of wild lions in the Nairobi National Park, Kenya. Wallingford, Pennsylvania: Washington Square East. Schuett, E.B. and Frase, B.A. (2001). Making scents: using the olfactory senses for lion enrichment. Worley and Hare, editors. The Shape of Enrichment. San Diego: The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. 10 (3): 1-3. Sheperdson, D.J., Carlstead K.C., Wielebnowski N. (2004). Cross-institutional assessment of stress responses in zoo animals using longitudinal monitoring of faecal corticoids and behavior. Animal Welfare. 13: 105-113.
  • 17. Jourdan Ringenberg CZAW Resident Session II 2014 Skibiel, A.L., Trevino, H.S. & Naugher, K. (2007). Comparison of several types of enrichment for captive felids. Zoo Biology. 26: 371-381. Van Metter, J. E., Harriger, M. D. & Bolen, R. H. (2008). Environmental enrichment utilizing stimulus objects for African lions (Panthera leo leo) and Sumatran tigers (Panthera tigris sumatrae). BIOS. 79: 7-16. Wiepkema, P.R. (1984). Stereotypies and endorphins: functional significance of developing stereotypies in tethered sows. Unshelm, van Putten, and Zeeb, editors. Proceedings of the International Congress on Applied Ethology in Farm Animals. Dalmstadt: KTBL. p. 93- 96. Wiepkema, P.R. (1987). Behavioral aspects of stress. Van Adrichem and Wiepkema, editors. The Biology of Stress in Farm Animals: an Integrated Approach. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff. p. 113-134. Wilson, D.E., Mittermeier, R.A. eds. (2009). Handbook of the Mammals of the World. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions. 1 (1): p. 54-127, 137-139.