SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 162
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With
Family
Conflict
Emma Armstrong-Carter
Stanford University
Eva H. Telzer
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Family meals have been associated with positive adolescent
outcomes in cross-sectional and longitudinal
research. However, it is not known how adolescents experience
family meals on a daily basis, and
whether family meals buffer stresses associated with
interpersonal conflicts on the daily level. To address
this gap in the literature, adolescents (N � 396, 58% female,
Mage � 14.57 years) completed diary
checklists for up to 14 days, reporting their emotions,
experiences of family and peer conflict, and
whether they ate with their family that day. On days that
adolescents shared a family meal, they felt
greater happiness and role fulfillment, and less burnout and
distress. Moreover, family conflict was
associated with more negative emotionality only on days that
adolescents did not also eat with the family.
Findings suggest that family meals buffer daily risks associated
with familial conflicts. Follow-up
analyses suggest that these processes may be particularly
important among older adolescents.
Keywords: adolescence, family meal, emotions, family conflict,
peer conflict
During busy daily life, meals are often the only time when
family members come together to engage, and provide and
receive
emotional support (Larson, Branscomb, & Wiley, 2006).
Perhaps
in part because daily family meals represent a stable, routine,
and
context for emotional connectedness (Goldfarb, Tarver, & Sen,
2014; Jones, 2018), family meals have been associated with
many
positive outcomes across development. For example,
adolescents
who more frequently eat with the family are less likely to be
overweight or underweight, have substance use problems, and
struggle with clinical depression (Fulkerson et al., 2006), and
tend
to feel more emotionally close to parents and siblings (Fiese et
al.,
2002) compared with their peers. The benefits associated with
family meals also extend beyond the home. Adolescents who
more
frequently eat with the family tend to exhibit higher academic
performance (Eisenberg, Olson, Neumark-Sztainer, Story, &
Bear-
inger, 2004), fewer antisocial behaviors (Fulkerson et al., 2006;
Prior & Limbert, 2013; Sen, 2010), and increased social compe-
tencies with peers (Fulkerson et al., 2006).
Prior research has been almost entirely cross-sectional, retro-
spective, or longitudinal, which can only tell us about average
meal eating behaviors between adolescents. This work has
exam-
ined how average family meals at one time point relate to
average
well-being at another time point (Goldfarb et al., 2014). To
extend
prior research, it is important to clarify whether family meals
are
associated with positive or negative emotions on the daily level.
Examining temporal relations at the daily level may help us to
understand the processes by which family meals promote long-
term well-being (Offer, 2013a, 2013b), as has been observed in
prior research. For example, if adolescents feel happier on days
that they eat with the family, this could partially explain why
family meals are associated with later positive social and emo-
tional adaptation.
Family Meals as a Protective Factor Against
Interpersonal Conflict
Family meals may translate into greater well-being by protect-
ing against the negative effects of daily stressors, such as
conflict
in the home. On the daily level, sharing a family meal may
mitigate the negative impacts of family conflict by providing an
opportunity to make amends, internalize disputes less, engage
positively, and recover; thereby, offsetting distress caused by
conflict during the day. Supporting this notion, Family Systems
Theory and developmental theories of risk and resilience (Brod-
erick, 1993; Labella & Masten, 2018) have suggested that
spend-
ing positive time together as a family after experiencing
emotional
challenges can increase adolescents’ feelings of family cohesion
and emotional security, and promote resilience (Fiese et al.,
2002;
Jones, 2018). Consistent with these theories, family meals have
been associated with greater emotional well-being among
adoles-
cents experiencing ongoing difficult family relationships (Meier
&
Musick, 2014). Indeed, the experience of coming together as a
This article was published Online First September 28, 2020
X Emma Armstrong-Carter, Graduate School of Education,
Stanford
University; Eva H. Telzer, Department of Psychology and
Neuroscience,
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
This article was prepared with support from the National
Institutes of
Health Grant R01DA039923 and National Science Foundation
Grant SES
1459719 provided to Eva H. Telzer, the Department of
Psychology at the
University of Illinois, and the Department of Psychology and
Neuroscience
at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Emma
Armstrong-Carter, Graduate School of Education, Stanford
University,
520 Galvez Mall, Stanford, CA 94305, or to Eva H. Telzer,
Department
of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina
at Chapel
Hill, 235 East Cameron Avenue, Chapel Hill, NC 27510. E-
mail:
[email protected] or [email protected]
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Developmental Psychology
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 56, No.
11, 2110 –2120
ISSN: 0012-1649 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001111
2110
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5847-9486
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001111
family to share a meal may be more important than the specific
meal itself (Larson et al., 2006). However, it remains unclear
whether family meals protect against emotional risks on the
daily
level as well. If family meals reduce the distress that is asso-
ciated with family conflict the same day, this could represent
one daily pathway through which family meals ultimately con-
fer the greater well-being. Moreover, this could reveal a key
experience in the home that helps to support adolescents’ daily
emotional health.
In addition to buffering the negative costs of family conflict,
family meals may also protect youth in the face of conflict with
peers. Family meals can provide adolescents the opportunity to
express difficult feelings, receive emotional support from
parents
and siblings, and distract from difficult emotions by engaging
positively with family (Fiese, Foley, & Spagnola, 2006, 2012;
Larson et al., 2006). For example, adolescents whose family
mem-
bers express concern and emotional support during family
dinners
tend to feel greater trust and belonging in the family, which
may
foster resiliency even outside the home (Fiese et al., 2006;
Larson
et al., 2006). Indeed, parents often use meal times to encourage
and
coach youth through difficult experiences (Larson et al., 2006),
and this may include social challenges with peers. Consistent
with
this notion, a family meal may provide adolescents the
opportunity
to share their experiences of negative peer interactions or
conflict
they encountered that day, receive emotional support,
encourage-
ment and ideas for how to cope. In this way, family meals may
protect adolescents from emotional risk the same day; that is,
adolescents may go to bed feeling relatively happier and more
fulfilled after having a chance to express their challenges and
receive support (Ho et al., 2018).
Adolescence as a Sensitive Developmental Period
Adolescence is an essential developmental period for studying
processes related to family meals, interpersonal conflict, and
youth
adaptation for several reasons. First, adolescents may benefit
more
from family meals compared with children, if meals help to
main-
tain connection with the family during the adolescent transition
toward increased autonomy and participation in activities
outside
the home with peers (Brannen, 2002). Second, adolescents expe-
rience higher levels of daily interpersonal conflicts, which
incurs
increased emotional risk (Chung, Flook, & Fuligni, 2011; Holm-
beck, 2018). In addition, adolescents also become more
sensitive
to the negative effects of interpersonal conflicts, because they
experience hormonal shifts (Romeo, 2013), begin to develop
their
own social identities that are highly influenced by their interac-
tions with others (Fuligni, 2019) and place increasing
importance
on relationships with peers (Chung et al., 2011). These shifts
may
be particularly pronounced in older compared with younger ado-
lescents (Holmbeck, 2018), who spend more of their day away
from the family with peers (Brannen, 2002). In light of this,
family
meals might be even more of a protective factor during later
adolescence, compared with earlier adolescence. It is important
to
understand if the interplay between family meals, conflict, and
adjustment varies across adolescent development, as this may
reveal key, specific developmental periods during which family
meals are more or less beneficial and protective.
Current Study
In the current study, we capitalized on the daily diary method in
a large and diverse sample of adolescents. We examined
whether
family meals and family and peer conflict contribute to adoles -
cents’ positive or negative emotions on the daily level, and
whether family meals mitigate the emotional risks associated
with
family and peer conflict. The daily diary method is uniquely
useful
for examining questions related to adolescents’ emotions and
daily
routines in the home (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). Youths’ reports
of
their daily activities, behaviors, and feelings are more reliable
and
accurate than when these processes are assessed using
traditional
retrospective accounts from a single questionnaire (Bolger,
Davis,
& Rafaeli, 2003). Daily diary methods also allow researchers to
examine whether specific events that occur on one day are asso-
ciated with feelings measured the same day. For example, on
days
that adolescents share a meal with their family, are they more
likely to feel happy or distressed? While not causal, data of this
nature allow us to test within the same adolescent, whether
family
meals and positive and negative emotions co-occur with each
other
on the same day. In this way, we can hold constant the
extraneous
traits and characteristics of both the individual adolescent and
the
family. Daily diary methods also enable us to examine
interactions
between multiple processes that occur on the daily level. For
example, does the daily association between interpersonal
conflict
and emotions depend on whether the adolescent ate a meal with
their family that day? Prior literature that has examined links
between family meals and adolescent outcomes has used indexes
that are averaged across days or retrospective (Goldfarb et al.,
2014). More recent studies emphasize the importance of
examin-
ing family experiences on the daily level, as family routines
have
been shown to temporally fluctuate with emotions within days
(Armstrong-Carter, Ivory, Lin, Muscatell, & Telzer, 2020;
Telzer
& Fuligni, 2009b). In particular, recent work has revealed
signif-
icant daily fluctuations in adolescents’ happiness, distress,
burn-
out, and role fulfillment (i.e., feeling like a good son, daughter,
or
sibling), suggesting that these measures of emotions capture
dis-
tinctive variability across days within adolescents, and are
mean-
ingfully related to same-day family experiences. For example,
role-fulfillment has been linked to family behaviors with effect
sizes twice the size of more commonly measured constructs
such
as happiness or distress (Armstrong-Carter et al., 2020),
indicating
that it may be robustly and uniquely related to adolescents’
expe-
riences in the home the same day.
Using the daily diary method, we tested the following key
questions: (1) Is eating a meal with the family associated with
positive or negative emotions the same day? Given the observed
associations between family meals and positive developmental
outcomes in prior cross-sectional and longitudinal work, we hy-
pothesized that family meals would be associated with more
pos-
itive emotions (e.g., happiness and role-fulfillment) and fewer
negative emotions (e.g., distress and burnout) the same day. We
tested multiple positive and negative emotions to both to allow
for
the possibility that family meals might relate to different
measures
simultaneously and divergently (e.g., increase happiness but
also
increase distress, or increase happiness but decrease role -
fulfillment) and to be consistent with prior work (Armstrong-
Carter et al., 2020; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). (2) Are family and
peer conflict associated with positive or negative emotions on
the
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2111FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS
daily level, and does eating a meal with the family buffer (i.e.,
moderate) this association? Informed by developmental theories
of
youth risk and resilience, which suggest that positive family ex-
periences can counteract the risks of negative experiences
(Labella
& Masten, 2018), we hypothesized that family meals would
buffer
the negative effects of family and peer conflict. (3) Do these
associations vary by age across early to late adolescence?
Drawing
on evidence of increases in daily conflict, emotional risk, and
interpersonal sensitivity across adolescence (Holmbeck, 2018),
we
hypothesized that older adolescents may benefit even more from
family meals, compared with younger adolescents. To follow up
and provide further evidence of directionality of effects, we
con-
ducted additional sensitivity analyses. Specifically, we first
con-
trolled for previous day emotions, and then tested potential
spill-
over into emotions the next day.
Method
Participants
Participants were 396 adolescents (57.92% female) between the
ages of 11 and 18 years (Mage � 14.57 years, SD � 1.39 years).
The sample was racially and ethnically diverse: 38.89%
identified
as non-Hispanic White (from here on referred to as White, N �
154), 26.77% Asian (N � 106, 13 of whom were mixed [e.g.,
Asian and White]), 16.67% Hispanic/Latinx (N � 66, 11 of
whom
were mixed [e.g., Hispanic and White]), 10.8% African
American
(N � 45, 9 of whom were mixed [e.g., African American and
White]), and 6.31% other race (N � 27, 16 of whom self-
identified
as other and nine were mixed race). Approximately 10% of
moth-
ers had less than an 8th grade education, 13% did not complete
high school, 24% completed high school, 27% completed
postsec-
ondary education (college, trade, or vocational school), and
23%
completed graduate school (3% declined to answer).
Participants
were recruited from the community using convenience
sampling,
including posting flyers at schools, posting on listservs serving
ethnic minority families, recruiting participants from previous
studies who agreed to be contacted for other research studies,
and
word of mouth. Participants were compensated $10 in total for
completing the daily diaries as well as a $20 bonus if inspection
of
the data indicated that they had completed all the diaries on
time.
Participants provided written consent/assent and procedures
were
approved by the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
Com-
mittee on Human Subjects (Protocol #13378; Development of
Decision Making and Social Cognition).
Procedure
Participants were provided with diary checklists; most partici -
pants (80%) were provided 14 days of diaries, whereas 20% of
participants (N � 83) were only provided with 7 days of diaries.
Most participants (89.82%) completed all days of their dairies
(M � 93.87% of days, SD � 15.51% of days, range � 14.29 –
100%). There were 4,369 total person-day observations (Level 1
reports). Diaries included both weekdays and weekends. The
order
of days differed between participants depending on the day of
the
week that they started, but all participants had the same
proportion
of weekday to weekend data if they completed all of the diaries.
Participants chose to complete the diaries either on paper
(63.20%)
or via a secure website (36.80%). Participants who responded
with
paper were given 14 manila envelopes and an electronic time
stamper (Dymo Corporation, Stamford, CT), which verified the
time that checklists were completed. The time stamper is a
small
device that imprints the current date and time and is
programmed
with a security code so that the correct date and time cannot be
changed. Participants were instructed to place their completed
checklists into an envelope each night and to stamp the seal of
the
envelope with the time stamper. Participants who completed
sur-
veys online were sent an e-mail with the link to each daily diary
survey, and the time and date of completion were recorded via
the
website. The daily diary checklists were three pages long and
each
took approximately 5–10 min to complete.
Measures
Family meals. Participants indicated on the daily checklist
whether or not they ate any meal with their family each day. We
chose this broad measure because the process of coming
together
as a family has been associated with positive outcomes, and the
specific meal (i.e., breakfast, lunch, or dinner) may vary by
family
and day and may be less important (Larson et al., 2006). To
allow
for the possibility that the structure and timing of family meals
vary across different households, we did not specify or define
“family meal” any further in the daily checklist. As such,
adoles-
cents were able to define family meals for themselves in the
context of their own experience. This item yielded a single
dichot-
omous index of family meals, which was coded 0 � no family
meal, 1 � family meal.
Daily family conflict. Items on the daily checklist asked par -
ticipants to indicate whether they had engaged in different
behav-
iors with family members each day. Each item was coded as 0 �
no, 1 � yes. Our measure of Family conflict was based on
family
systems theory (Broderick, 1993), general self-report measures
of
family conflict (e.g., Bloom, 1985), and other daily diary
studies
(Chung et al., 2011). This was the mean of four items: you
argued
with a sibling, you got into trouble or were punished by your
parents, you argued with a parent, you lied to parent (R1F �
.77;
see Cranford et al., 2006 for more information on this reliability
statistic).
Daily peer conflict. Items on the daily checklist asked partic-
ipants to indicate whether they had engaged in different
behaviors
with peers each day. Each item was coded as 0 � no, 1 � yes.
Our
measure of Peer conflict was based on social relational theories
of
adolescent development (Laursen & Collins, 1994), and other
self-reported measures of peer conflict (e.g., Marsee et al.,
2011)
including those from prior daily diary studies (Chung et al.,
2011).
It was the mean of 11 items: you hit, kicked, or shoved a peer,
you
threatened, insulted, or made fun of a peer, you said something
mean behind a friend’s back, you excluded or left a friend out,
you
lied to a friend, someone online or in a text message threatened,
insulted or made fun of you, you argued with a friend, you
argued
with a boyfriend or girlfriend, you were excluded or left out by
friends, a peer said something mean behind your back, a peer
threatened, insulted, or made fun of you, R1F � .97. This
measure
demonstrated acceptable within-person reliability of change
(RC � .71; Cranford et al., 2006).
Daily emotions. Daily emotions were assessed with items on
the daily checklist that were drawn from the Profile of Mood
States
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2112 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER
(McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971) and used in prior research
(Armstrong-Carter et al., 2020; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a).
Adoles-
cents used a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (ex-
tremely) to indicate “the extent to which they felt each emotion
that day.” The time of day for each emotion was not specified.
Happiness was calculated from two items: joyful and happy,
R1F � .87. Distress was calculated from nine items: sad,
hopeless,
discouraged, on edge, unable to concentrate, uneasy, nervous,
stressed, worried, R1F � .88. Burnout was calculated from three
items: fatigue, exhausted, and worn-out, R1F � .87. Role
Fulfill-
ment was calculated from two additional items in the daily diary
in
which participants responded on a 7-point Likert-scale ranging
from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely) to report the extent to which
they felt like “a good son or daughter” and “a good brother or
sister” that day. Role fulfillment was calculated as the mean of
these two items, R1F � .86. Because adolescents answered the
daily diaries at the end of the day just before bedtime, family
meals
occurred before adolescents reported their emotions. All
emotion
composites demonstrated acceptable within-person reliability of
change (RCs � .71–.79). For all daily diary measures (i.e.,
family
and peer conflict, emotions), there was substantial within-
person
variability (intraclass correlation coefficient, ICC � .43–.79)
and
between-person variability across the diary days (ICC � .36 –
.64).
Data Analysis
Our aim was to understand the daily association between eating
a meal with the family and positive and negative emotions, and
whether family meals buffer emotional reactivity associated
with
daily interpersonal conflicts. We conducted linear mixed effect
models that nested days (Level 1) within participants (Level 2).
Fixed effects were tested at the level of participants (i.e., Level
2).
This statistical approach accounts for dependency within
partici-
pants and introduces less bias because of missing data compared
with traditional statistical analyses, such as repeated measures
analysis of variance (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). We person-
centered all Level-1 predictors, and we included on the
intercept
person-mean values for each of our daily predictors (i.e., family
meals, family conflict, and peer conflict; Curran & Bauer,
2011).
Model 1 tested family meals and interpersonal conflict (family
and peer) as simultaneous Level 1 predictors of each emotional
experience the same day. Model 2 additionally included an
inter-
action term that was the product of family meal and each inter -
personal conflict variable (i.e., family conflict and peer
conflict).
To test potential differences in observed associations by
develop-
mental period, Model 3 included an additional three-way
interac-
tion term between family meals, adolescent age, and each
conflict
variable. We conducted additional sensitivity analyses first con-
trolling for prior day emotions and then testing next day
emotions
as a dependent variable.
To probe significant cross-level interactions, we used the simple
slopes technique (Aiken, West, & Reno, 1991) to test the
associ-
ations between interpersonal conflict and emotions on days
when
there was or was not a family meal. For three-way interactions,
we
split adolescents by the mean age for the sample, and plotted
two-way interactions within younger and older adolescents. All
analyses were conducted using Stata Software (StataSE, Version
15.1.632).
Results
Sample Characteristics
Table 1 displays descriptive statistics for sample constructs for
the full sample and by sex and race/ethnicity. On average,
adoles-
cents ate a meal with the family on 61% of days. Family meals
were most common among Asian youth and least common
among
Black youth, but did not differ by sex. Girls reported higher
peer
conflict than boys, but family conflict did not vary by sex.
Bivariate correlations of mean values across days showed that
adolescents who ate more frequent family meals were younger,
r � �.09, p � .001 and experienced more family conflict, r �
.05,
p � .002, and less peer conflict, r � �.04, p � .001.
Adolescents
who experienced more family conflict were younger, r � �.08,
p � .001, and experienced more peer conflict, r � .21, p � .001.
There were no other significant correlations across days.
Multilevel analyses at the daily level showed that family meals
were not related to family conflict or peer conflict on the dai ly
level (p � .078). Family and peer conflict co-occurred on the
same
days (B � 0.06, SE � 0.01, p � .000), consistent with prior
research (Chung et al., 2011).
Family Meals Are Associated With More Positive and
Fewer Negative Emotions
We first tested whether family meals, family conflict, and peer
conflict uniquely predict positive and negative emotions. As
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Study Constructs
Study
constructs
Full sample Boys Girls Black Asian Hispanic Other White
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Family meal 0.61 (0.49) 0.62 (0.48)1 0.60 (0.49)1 0.35 (0.48)A
0.74 (0.44)B 0.60 (0.49)C 0.54 (0.50)C 0.59 (0.49)C
Family conflict 0.14 (0.20) 0.13 (0.20)1 0.14 (0.21)1 0.13
(0.19)A,B 0.14 (0.19)A,B 0.10 (0.22)A 0.16 (0.22)B 0.14
(0.21)A,B
Peer conflict 0.04 (0.10) 0.03 (0.08)1 0.04 (0.11)2 0.07 (0.13)A
0.03 (0.07)B 0.06 (0.17)A,C 0.05 (0.10)C,D 0.04 (0.09)D
Happiness 3.25 (1.08) 3.23 (1.07)1 3.25 (1.09)1 3.07 (1.25)A,D
3.08 (0.99)A 3.35 (1.08)B,C 3.27 (1.19)C,D 3.40 (1.06)C
Distress 1.65 (0.79) 1.54 (0.65)1 1.73 (0.87)2 1.74 (0.85)A 1.60
(0.75)B 1.45 (0.99)A,B 1.68 (0.92)A,B 1.67 (0.78)A,B
Burnout 2.09 (1.05) 1.96 (0.95)1 2.19 (1.11)2 2.33 (1.12)A 1.90
(0.98)B 1.92 (0.99)B 2.16 (1.06)A 2.20 (1.07)A
Role fulfillment 4.87 (1.36) 4.80 (1.35)1 4.92 (1.36)2 5.01
(1.56)A 4.56 (1.22)B 4.92 (1.48)A 5.11 (1.34)A 5.02 (1.35)A
Note. For sex, means with the same number are not significantly
different at the p � .05 level. For race, means with the same
letter are not significantly
different.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2113FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS
shown in Model 1 of Table 2, on days that adolescents ate a
meal
with the family, they reported significantly greater happiness
and
role fulfillment (ps �.001), and less distress and burnout (p �
.005
and p � .010, respectively). Conversely, on days that
adolescents
experienced greater family conflict, they reported significantly
less
happiness and role fulfillment (p � .001 and p � .001), and
more
distress and burnout (p � .001 and p � .030). On days that
adolescents experienced greater peer conflict, they reported sig-
nificantly less happiness (p � .001) and more burnout and
distress
(ps �.001).
Family Meals Buffer the Associations Between Family
Conflict and Negative Emotions
Our next model tested whether family meals moderates the
associations between family and peer conflict and emotions. For
family conflict, as shown in Model 2 of Table 2, family meals
significantly interacted with family conflict to predict happiness
(p � .003), burnout (p � .010), role fulfilment (p � .000), and
distress (p � .030). As shown in Figures 1 and 2, family
conflict
was associated with lower levels of happiness and higher levels
of
burnout only on days when adolescents did not eat a family
meal.
On days when adolescents ate a family meal, family conflict
was
not associated with happiness or burnout. Similarly, as shown in
Figures 3 and 4, family conflict was associated with lower role
fulfillment and higher levels of distress, particularly on days
that
there was no family meal. Together, these findings suggest that
family meals buffer the daily emotional toll of experiencing
family
conflict.
In addition, family meals significantly interacted with peer
conflict to predict role fulfilment (p � .045). As shown in
Figure
5, peer conflict was associated with marginally lower levels of
role
fulfillment only on days when adolescents did not eat a family
meal. On days when adolescents ate a family meal, peer conflict
was not associated with role fulfillment. There were no other
significant associations between family meals, peer conflict,
and
emotions (ps � .050�.832).
Sensitivity Analyses
Bonferroni correction. As follow up, we conducted Bonfer-
roni correction for four analyses (four emotion outcomes).
Results
all retained significance at the p � .013 level except for the
direct
positive association between family conflict and burnout, the in-
teraction between family conflict and family meals in predicti ng
burnout, and the interaction between peer conflict and family
meals in predicting role fulfillment.
Controlling for frequency of family meals across days. We
also tested whether family meals buffered the negative effects
of
conflict more or less for adolescents who routinely had more or
fewer family meals by entering average family meals as a cross
level interaction term at Level 2. We found no evidence for this
(p � .150 –723).
Controlling for prior day emotions. To provide further evi -
dence of directionality, we conducted two additional models.
First, we
examined whether family meals buffered emotional risks
associated
with family and peer conflict the same day, controlling for
emotions
the previous day. To do this, we ran the same models as before,
but
included the same emotion on the previous day. Consistent with
our T
ab
le
2
D
ir
ec
t
a
n
d
In
te
ra
ct
iv
e
D
a
il
y
A
ss
o
ci
a
ti
o
n
s
B
et
w
ee
n
F
a
m
il
y
M
ea
ls
a
n
d
F
a
m
il
y
a
n
d
P
ee
r
C
o
n
fl
ic
t
P
re
d
ic
ti
n
g
P
sy
ch
o
lo
g
ic
a
l
E
xp
er
ie
n
ce
s
H
ap
pi
ne
ss
D
is
tr
es
s
B
ur
no
ut
R
ol
e
fu
lf
il
lm
en
t
In
de
pe
nd
en
t
va
ri
ab
le
s
M
od
el
1
M
od
el
2
M
od
el
1
M
od
el
2
M
od
el
1
M
od
el
2
M
od
el
1
M
od
el
2
P
er
so
n-
ce
nt
er
ed
va
ri
ab
le
s
F
am
il
y
m
ea
l
0.
11
�
�
�
(0
.0
3)
0.
16
�
�
�
(0
.0
3)
�
0.
05
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
08
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
06
�
(0
.0
3)
�
0.
10
�
�
(0
.0
3)
0.
23
�
�
�
(0
.0
3)
0.
28
�
�
�
(0
.0
4)
F
am
il
y
co
nf
li
ct
�
0.
06
�
�
�
(0
.0
1)
�
0.
11
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
0.
07
�
�
�
(0
.0
1)
0.
10
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
0.
02
(0
.0
1)
0.
07
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
25
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
33
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
P
ee
r
co
nf
li
ct
�
0.
08
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
08
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
0.
11
�
�
�
(0
.0
1)
0.
11
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
0.
08
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
0.
08
�
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
00
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
04
†
(0
.0
2)
P
er
so
n-
m
ea
n
va
ri
ab
le
s
F
am
il
y
m
ea
l
0.
24
†
(0
.1
4)
0.
24
†
(0
.1
4)
�
0.
38
�
�
�
(0
.1
1)
�
0.
38
�
�
�
(0
.1
1)
�
0.
47
�
�
�
(0
.1
3)
�
0.
48
�
�
�
(0
.1
3)
0.
33
†
(0
.1
9)
0.
34
†
(0
.1
9)
F
am
il
y
co
nf
li
ct
0.
12
†
(0
.0
7)
0.
12
†
(0
.0
7)
0.
14
�
(0
.0
6)
0.
14
�
(0
.0
6)
0.
18
�
�
(0
.0
6)
0.
18
�
�
(0
.0
6)
0.
07
(0
.0
9)
0.
07
(0
.0
9)
P
ee
r
co
nf
li
ct
0.
04
(0
.0
5)
0.
04
(0
.0
5)
0.
16
�
�
(0
.0
6)
0.
16
�
�
(0
.0
6)
0.
08
(0
.0
5)
0.
07
(0
.0
5)
�
0.
02
(0
.0
7)
�
0.
02
(0
.0
7)
In
te
ra
ct
io
n
va
ri
ab
le
s
M
ea
l
�
F
am
il
y
C
on
fl
ic
t
0.
09
�
�
�
(0
.0
3)
�
0.
05
�
�
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
07
�
�
(0
.0
3)
0.
12
�
�
�
(0
.0
3)
M
ea
l
�
P
ee
r
C
on
fl
ic
t
�
0.
01
(0
.0
3)
0.
00
(0
.0
2)
�
0.
01
(0
.0
3)
0.
06
�
(0
.0
3)
C
on
st
an
t
�
0.
05
(0
.0
9)
�
0.
09
(0
.0
9)
0.
26
�
�
�
(0
.0
7)
0.
28
�
�
�
(0
.0
7)
�
0.
79
�
�
�
(0
.0
9)
�
0.
76
�
�
�
(0
.0
9)
�
1.
36
�
�
�
(0
.1
3)
�
1.
39
�
�
�
(0
.1
3)
O
bs
er
va
ti
on
s
4,
03
5
4,
03
5
3,
72
5
3,
72
5
4,
01
7
4,
01
7
4,
00
1
4,
00
1
G
ro
up
s
39
7
39
7
33
8
33
8
39
7
39
7
39
0
39
0
N
o
te
.
S
ta
nd
ar
d
er
ro
rs
in
pa
re
nt
he
se
s.
†
p
�
.1
.
�
p
�
.0
5.
�
�
p
�
.0
1.
�
�
�
p
�
.0
01
.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2114 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER
previous results, we found that family meals significantly
buffered the
effect of family conflict on happiness (B � 0.08, SE � 0.03, p
�
.007), burnout (B � �0.06, SE � 0.03, p � .021), and role
fulfillment
(B � 1.43, SE � 0.03, p � .001). Family meals did not buffer
the
effect of family conflict on distress (p � .174) or the effects of
peer
conflict on any emotion (p � .077–.730). With Bonferroni
correction,
these results retained significance at the p � .023 level except
for the
interaction between family conflict and family meals in
predicting
burnout above prior day levels.
Spillover into next day emotions. As a second more conser-
vative follow-up analysis, we examined whether family meals
buffered negative emotions experienced the day after family and
peer conflict (i.e., a spillover effect). We again ran the same
models but with each next day emotion as the dependent
variable,
and controlled for that same emotion the same day (i.e., the day
of
the conflict). Consistent with our same-day results, we found
that
family meals significantly buffered the effect of family conflict
on
distress the next day (B � �0.06, SE � 0.02, p � .002). Specif-
ically, family conflict was associated with marginally higher
levels
of distress only on days when adolescents did not eat a family
meal
(B � 0.14, SE � 0.08, p � .069), but significantly lower levels
of
distress on days when adolescents did eat a family meal
(B � �0.17, SE � 0.07, p � .012). This result remained signif-
icant after Bonferroni correction.
Variation Across Adolescent Years
To investigate whether the observed associations varied across
adolescence, our final models added a three-way interaction be-
tween family meals, age, and each conflict variable (i.e., family
conflict or peer conflict) predicting each emotion. We first
added
this interaction term to our primary model (same day emotion as
the dependent variable) then to our two sensitivity analyses
(same
day emotion as the dependent variable while controlling for pre -
vious day emotion, and next day emotion as the dependent
variable
while controlling for same day emotion). The associations
between
family meal, family and peer conflict, and emotions the same
day
did not vary by adolescent age in either of the two models with
same day emotions as the outcomes. However, family meals
significantly interacted with family conflict and age to predict
distress the next day (B � �0.06, SE � 0.02, p � .005). As
shown
in Figure 6, family conflict spilled over into more distress the
day
after there was no family meal, but only for older adolescents,
and
not for younger adolescents. Similarly, family meals
significantly
interacted with peer conflict and age to predict distress the next
day (B � 0.06, SE � 0.02, p � .010). As shown in Figure 7,
peer
conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there was
no
family meal, but only for younger adolescents, and not for older
adolescents.
B = -0.50, p < .001
B = -0.08, p = .36
Figure 1. Family meal buffers the daily negative association
between
family conflict and happiness.
B = 0.30, p = .002
B = -0.06, p =.54
Figure 2. Family meal buffers the daily positive association
between
family conflict and burnout.
B = -1.52, p < .001
B = -0.93, p <.001
Figure 3. Family meal buffers the daily negative association
between
family conflict and less role fulfillment.
B = 0.45, p =.002
B = 0.22, p <.001
Figure 4. Family meal buffers the daily positive association
between
family conflict and increased distress.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2115FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS
Discussion
The goal of this study was to understand how family meals are
related to adolescents’ emotions on the daily level, and whether
family meals moderate how family and peer conflict relate to
positive and challenging emotions. In a large and diverse
sample of
adolescents, we investigated these direct and interactive effects.
Since family meals might represent an opportunity for
adolescents
to express themselves and receive emotional support after
experi-
encing challenges during the day, we hypothesized that adoles-
cents would end the day relatively happier and more fulfilled,
and
less upset after having a family meal. Consistent with this
hypoth-
esis, we found that on days that adolescents shared a meal with
the
family, they reported more positive emotions and fewer
negative
emotions. Moreover, family meals buffered the negative effects
of
family conflict on increased negative emotions and decreased
positive emotions. These findings suggest that family meals
may
serve as a daily protective factor against the negative effects of
daily familial disputes. Sensitivity analyses controlling for
previ-
ous day emotions and examining links with emotions the next
day
strengthened this interpretation of the direction of this pathway.
Our first key finding was that on days that adolescents shared a
meal with the family, they reported greater happiness and role
fulfillment, and less burnout and distress, compared with days
that
they did not eat with the family. These results are consistent
with
cross-sectional reports that the majority of adolescents
retrospec-
tively view family meals as one of their favorite family
activities
(Zollo, 1995), and evidence that more frequent family meals on
average are associated with more optimal physical and mental
health (Goldfarb et al., 2014; Jones, 2018). Our study extends
this
work by demonstrating that the positive associations with
family
meals are observable on the daily level as well. Moreover, this
pattern appears consistent across adolescents, whether they rou-
tinely have more or fewer family meals, underscoring that it is
not
only the overall amount of family meals that is protective (as
assessed in prior research), but the temporal co-occurrence
within
individual adolescents. Daily family meals provide an
opportunity
to share experiences from the day, enjoy the company of loved-
ones, and experience a stable family routine that reminds
adoles-
cents of their belonging and trust in the family (Fiese et al.,
2006).
In particular, family meals may provide more of a context for
youth to share their experiences and receive support, compared
with family time in other settings which may be more structured
around an activity that facilitates less communication (e.g.,
watch-
ing a movie).
A reduction in distress and burnout and an increase in happiness
and role fulfillment as a function of eating with the family are
meaningful outcomes in and of themselves, as they are key
indices
of psychological well-being (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). Further-
more, these psychological experiences are important because
they
contribute to a variety of other positive outcomes in youth,
ranging
from school achievement and school behaviors (Quinn & Duck-
B = -0.04, p = .060
B = 0.04, p =.108
Figure 5. Family meal buffers the daily negative association
between
peer conflict and role fulfillment.
B = -0.07, p =.029
B = -0.05, p =.126
B = -0.03, p =.245
B = -0.04, p =.154
Figure 6. Family meal buffers the association between family
conflict and increased distress the next day for
adolescents who were older (ages 14.5–18 years), but not
younger (ages 12–14.5 years).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2116 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER
worth, 2007) to physical health, both in the short and long term
(Mahon, Yarcheski, & Yarcheski, 2005; Trzesniewski et al.,
2006). Thus, the daily links between family meals and increased
well-being may also have effects downstream on overall
psycho-
logical and physical well-being.
Our interactive models revealed that family meals buffered
adolescents from the daily emotional risks associated with
family
conflict. In particular, family conflict was linked with greater
distress and burnout, and less happiness and role fulfillment
only
on days that adolescents did not eat a meal with their family. In
contrast, family conflict was not associated with negative
emotions
on days that adolescents did eat a meal with their family. These
results were particularly robust for daily happiness, role
fulfillment
and distress— compared with burnout, which was no longer sig-
nificant after correcting for multiple tests.
A shared meal may provide an opportunity to make amends,
internalize negative interactions less, engage positively and re -
cover, thereby offsetting the negative impacts of conflict during
the day. After experiencing family conflict, adolescents may
feel
more positive at the end of the day if they also shared family
meal,
because the meal reminded them consciously or unconsciously
that
“everything is still okay.” Thus, family meal routines may
facili-
tate feelings of family cohesion, belonging, and emotional
security
(Fiese et al., 2006) even the same day, which could in turn help
to
mitigate emotional distress caused by interpersonal conflicts
ear-
lier in the day. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis
that
the routine of consistent family meals provides a sense of
familial
organization and clear expectations, which are integral for
adoles-
cents’ psychological resilience (Larson et al., 2006). This
interpre-
tation was also supported by sensitivity analysis which revealed
that family meals buffered the negative effects of conflict on
emotionality over and above emotions the previous day.
Our investigation of age-related variation revealed that family
conflict was associated with higher levels of distress the day
after
there was no family meal only for older adolescents, but not for
younger adolescents. Older adolescents may benefit more from
family meals in part because they had fewer family meals on
average in our sample. Older adolescents also tend to have rela-
tively greater independence and are more often outside the
home
or away from family, interacting with peers (Holmbeck, 2018),
so
family meals may be the only time when they come together
with
their family to engage and receive emotional support (Larson et
al.,
2006). In part because older adolescents have fewer family
meals
during the week, they may be more inclined to share their expe-
riences and receive emotional support when they do come
together
to eat with the family. In contrast, younger adolescents may
spend
more time around the home engaging with parents and siblings
during the afternoon after school or on weekends, and receive
emotional support during those times. These results are particu-
larly important since late adolescence marks the transition to
early
adulthood, a time of increasing autonomy and emotional risk
that
paves the way for lifelong adjustment.
Results revealed one significant interaction with peer conflict,
suggesting that peer conflict reduced positive feelings of role
fulfillment only when adolescents did not eat a meal with the
family. However, this result was no longer significant after cor -
rection for multiple testing and should be interpreted with
caution.
More commonly—and in contrast to family conflict—we found
that peer conflict was consistently associated with less
happiness,
more burnout and more distress the same day, even when
adoles-
cents ate with the family. It is possible that adolescents may
feel
unwilling or unable to open up and confide in their family (Ca-
mara, Bacigalupe, & Padilla, 2017), even though the meal may
provide an opportunity to do so (Fiese et al., 2006). Thus,
adoles-
cents may not receive the emotional support and guidance that
the
family members could offer during the meal (Fiese et al., 2006).
Another possible explanation is that even if the adolescent con-
fides in family members, family members may not know how
best
B = -0.07, p = .022
B = -0.05, p =.126
B = -0.02, p =.608
B = -0.02, p =.656
Figure 7. Family meal buffers the association between peer
conflict and increased distress the next day for
adolescents who were younger (ages 12–14.5 years) but not
older (ages 14.5–18 years). We split the two groups
by mean age in our sample.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2117FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS
to provide support for relationships that occur outside of the
home.
Parents, guardians and siblings may not say the “right” thing to
help the adolescent resolve their negative emotions from peer
conflict and experience relief (Camara et al., 2017). It is also
possible that despite the adolescent confiding and receiving
emo-
tional support during family meals, the support may not
alleviate
the stresses of peer conflict because it does not involve
positively
engaging with the same person or people with whom the conflict
occurred. Families’ attempts to offset conflict by providing
warmth and structure may be uniquely valuable for
counteracting
previous upsetting interactions that occurred within the family,
as
opposed to those that occurred outside the home (Labella &
Masten, 2018).
Follow up sensitivity analysis revealed an additional buffering
effect for peer conflict and emotional risk the next day. Peer
conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there was
no
family meal, but only for younger adolescents, and not for older
adolescents. This contrasts our prior findings in which family
conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there were
no
family meal only for older adolescents, and not for younger
ado-
lescents. One possible explanation is that younger adolescents
may
be more likely to process their experiences of peer conflict with
their families at mealtime, compared with older adolescents. In
contrast, older adolescents may discuss experiences of peer con-
flict with friends more and receive more support from friends,
because older adolescents on average spend more time with
peers
away from home (Brannen, 2002). In this way, family meals
may
buffer the association between peer conflict and heightened dis -
tress the next day for younger adolescents only because they are
more likely to open up to their family and receive support for
peer
conflict when it occurs. These results support a growing body of
literature documenting how family mealtime communication is
beneficial to adolescents’ emotional well-being (Offer, 2013a).
Limitations and Future Directions
Several limitations should be acknowledged. First, because of
the rich nature of daily diary data, there was some missing data,
which may or may not have been random. Days that adolescents
did not respond to the diaries might have been most difficult,
with
the highest levels of family and peer conflict, and these could
be
missing from analysis. In addition, our measure of family meals
was dichotomous, indexing whether or not the adolescent ate
any
meal with the family that day. We are unable to shed light on
the
chronological order of events. Specifically, we are unable to de-
termine whether family meals and interpersonal conflict
occurred
before, during or after adolescents’ emotions. Similarly,
although
we did not distinguish between breakfast, lunch and dinner, the
specific meal might also matter. For example, family dinners
may
more effectively buffer the negative effect of conflicts during
the
day, compared with breakfast which likely occurs before
potential
conflicts. However, the observed covariation of these
experiences
within days highlight a dynamic interplay within days and
across
days, regardless of the specific order in which they occurred.
Our measure of family meals was also unable to shed light on
the quality of the interaction during the meals, for example
which
topics were discussed and if emotional support was provided.
As
such, it is unclear from the current study the specific
interpersonal
processes that occurred during family meals (e.g., personal
disclo-
sure, emotional support) that may mediate or impact the
observed
daily associations between family meals, interpersonal conflict,
and emotions. Future research should measure the specific con-
versation topics and behaviors during family meals, to specify
which aspects of the quality of the interaction during family
meals
promote positive emotionality, even in the context of family
dis-
putes. Measuring conversations and behaviors during family
meals
may also reveal more individual differences (e.g., Meier & Mu-
sick, 2014). For example, it is possible that family meals are
only
protective for adolescents who experience relatively more
positive
familial relationships, but are not protective— or perhaps even
exacerbate the effects of conflict—among adolescents who
expe-
rience family interaction as more stressful (Meier & Musick,
2014).
Future research should also compare the effects of family meals
to other family quality time, to clarify whether the process of
eating together is uniquely valuable or if similar benefits may
be
derived from other consistent, daily family activities. For
example,
an alternative explanation to our findings may be that quality
time
with the family—rather than in particular the act of eating
togeth-
er— has a protective effect for adolescents. Conversely, it is
also
feasible that simply “slowing down,” and taking time to attend
to
basic human needs (i.e., eating) is protective—regardless of
whether or not family members are present.
In addition, although the present study utilizes daily diary meth-
ods that account for individual differences and, thus, offers
robust
predictive power (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b), causality cannot be
determined in this study. Future experimental work could
address
this issue by randomly assigning adolescents to eat with their
families (or not) and examine the psychological outcomes of
such
a manipulation. For example, randomized control trials have
shown that family meals can improve psychological symptoms
in
clinical samples of adolescents (Herscovici, Kovalskys, & Orel -
lana, 2017), and increase healthy eating habits in small samples
of
preadolescents (Flattum et al., 2015). Consistent with these
find-
ings, our results suggest that family meals could be valuable
components of interventions designed to increase positive emo-
tions, improve familial relationships, and reduce adolescent risk
in
families who experience high levels of conflict. Moreover, such
interventions may be more effective for older compared with
younger adolescents.
Conclusion
The current study extends long-standing interest in understand-
ing how family meals impact youths’ wellbeing and
development.
Cross-sectional and longitudinal research have shown that more
frequent family meals on average during childhood are
associated
with better physical and mental health. This study fills an
impor-
tant gap in the literature: it addresses how adolescents
experience
family meals on a day to day basis, and the extent to which
family
meals serve as a daily protective factor. Consistent with prior
evidence that more frequent family meals on average are a
positive
and stable routine that is integral for positive adolescent
develop-
ment (Goldfarb et al., 2014; Jones, 2018), our findings highlight
important daily associations between family meals and positive
emotionality— even in the context of conflict—that in turn may
partially explain how family meals are related to long-term
well-
being downstream.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2118 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER
References
Aiken, L., West, S., & Reno, R. (1991). Multiple regression:
Testing and
interpreting interactions. Atlanta, GA: Sage.
Armstrong-Carter, E., Ivory, S., Lin, L. C., Muscatell, K. A., &
Telzer,
E. H. (2020). Role Fulfillment Mediates the Association
Between Daily
Family Assistance and Cortisol Awakening Response in
Adolescents.
Child Development, 91, 754 –768.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13213
Bloom, B. L. (1985). A factor analysis of self-report measures
of family
functioning. Family Process, 24, 225–239.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1545-5300.1985.00225.x
Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods:
Capturing life
as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579 – 616.
http://dx.doi
.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030
Brannen, J. (2002). Discourses of Adolescence: Young People’s
Indepen-
dence and Autonomy within Families. Children in families:
Research
and policy. Retrieved from
https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/
9780203453803/chapters/10.4324/9780203453803-12
Broderick, C. B. (1993). Understanding family process: Basics
of family
systems theory. Atlanta, GA: Sage.
Camara, M., Bacigalupe, G., & Padilla, P. (2017). The role of
social
support in adolescents: Are you helping me or stressing me out?
Inter-
national Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 22, 123–136.
http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.875480
Chung, G. H., Flook, L., & Fuligni, A. J. (2011). Reciprocal
associations
between family and peer conflict in adolescents’ daily lives.
Child
Development, 82, 1390 –1396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624
.2011.01625.x
Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., &
Bolger, N.
(2006). A procedure for evaluating sensitivity to within-person
change:
Can mood measures in diary studies detect change reliably?
Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 917–929.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0146167206287721
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of
within-person
and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change.
Annual
Review of Psychology, 62, 583– 619.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev
.psych.093008.100356
Eisenberg, M. E., Olson, R. E., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story,
M., &
Bearinger, L. H. (2004). Correlations between family meals and
psy-
chosocial well-being among adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics
& Ad-
olescent Medicine, 158, 792–796.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi
.158.8.792
Fiese, B. H., Foley, K. P., & Spagnola, M. (2006). Routine and
ritual
elements in family mealtimes: Contexts for child well-being and
family
identity. New Directions for Child and Adolescent
Development, 2006,
67– 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.156
Fiese, B. H., Hammons, A., & Grigsby-Toussaint, D. (2012).
Family
mealtimes: A contextual approach to understanding childhood
obesity.
Economics and Human Biology, 10, 365–374.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.ehb.2012.04.004
Fiese, B. H., Tomcho, T. J., Douglas, M., Josephs, K., Poltrock,
S., &
Baker, T. (2002). A review of 50 years of research on naturally
occurring
family routines and rituals: Cause for celebration? Journal of
Family
Psychology, 16, 381–390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-
3200.16.4.381
Flattum, C., Draxten, M., Horning, M., Fulkerson, J. A.,
Neumark-
Sztainer, D., Garwick, A., . . . Story, M. (2015). HOME Plus:
Program
design and implementation of a family-focused, community-
based in-
tervention to promote the frequency and healthfulness of family
meals,
reduce children’s sedentary behavior, and prevent obesity. The
Interna-
tional Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 12,
53.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0211-7
Fuligni, A. J. (2019). The need to contribute during
adolescence. Perspec-
tives on Psychological Science, 14, 331–343.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1745691618805437
Fulkerson, J. A., Story, M., Mellin, A., Leffert, N., Neumark-
Sztainer, D.,
& French, S. A. (2006). Family dinner meal frequency and
adolescent
development: Relationships with developmental assets and high-
risk
behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 337–345.
http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.026
Goldfarb, S., Tarver, W. L., & Sen, B. (2014). Family structure
and risk
behaviors: The role of the family meal in assessing likelihood
of ado-
lescent risk behaviors. Psychology Research and Behavior
Management,
7, 53– 66.
Herscovici, C. R., Kovalskys, I., & Orellana, L. (2017). An
exploratory
evaluation of the family meal intervention for adolescent
anorexia ner-
vosa. Family Process, 56, 364 –375.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp
.12199
Ho, H. C. Y., Mui, M., Wan, A., Yew, C., Lam, T. H., Chan, S.
S., &
Stewart, S. M. (2018). Family meal practices and well-being in
Hong
Kong: The mediating effect of family communication. Journal
of Family
Issues, 39, 3835–3856.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18800787
Holmbeck, G. N. (2018, October 24). A model of family
relational trans-
formations during the transition to adolescence: Parent–
adolescent con-
flict and adaptation. In J. A. Graber, J. Brooks-Gunn, & A. C.
Petersen
(Eds.), Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains
and
context (pp. 167–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
http://dx.doi
.org/10.4324/9781315789286-7
Jones, B. L. (2018). Making time for family meals: Parental
influences,
home eating environments, barriers and protective factors.
Physiology &
Behavior, 193, 248 –251.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03
.035
Labella, M. H., & Masten, A. S. (2018). Family influences on
the devel-
opment of aggression and violence. Current Opinion in
Psychology, 19,
11–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.028
Larson, R. W., Branscomb, K. R., & Wiley, A. R. (2006). Forms
and
functions of family mealtimes: Multidisciplinary perspectives.
New Di-
rections for Child and Adolescent Development, 2006, 1–15.
http://dx
.doi.org/10.1002/cd.152
Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (1994). Interpersonal conflict
during ado-
lescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 197–209.
http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0033-2909.115.2.197
Mahon, N. E., Yarcheski, A., & Yarcheski, T. J. (2005).
Happiness as
related to gender and health in early adolescents. Clinical
Nursing
Research, 14, 175–190.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1054773804271936
Marsee, M. A., Barry, C. T., Childs, K. K., Frick, P. J.,
Kimonis, E. R.,
Muñoz, L. C., . . . Lau, K. S. L. (2011). Assessing the forms and
functions of aggression using self-report: Factor structure and
invariance
of the Peer Conflict Scale in youths. Psychological Assessment,
23,
792– 804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023369
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual
for the
profile of mood states (POMS). San Diego, CA: EdITS.
Meier, A., & Musick, K. (2014). Variation in associations
between family
dinners and adolescent well-being. Journal of Marriage and
Family, 76,
13–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12079
Offer, S. (2013a). Assessing the relationship between family
mealtime
communication and adolescent emotional well-being using the
experi-
ence sampling method. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 577–585.
http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.007
Offer, S. (2013b). Family time activities and adolescents’
emotional well-
being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75, 26 – 41.
http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01025.x
Prior, A.-L., & Limbert, C. (2013). Adolescents’ perceptions
and experi-
ences of family meals. Journal of Child Health Care, 17, 354 –
365.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367493512 462261
Quinn, P. D., & Duckworth, A. L. (2007). Happiness and
academic
achievement: Evidence for reciprocal causality. The Annual
Meeting of
the American Psychological Society, 24, 2007.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2119FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13213
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1985.00225.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1985.00225.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030
https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9780203453803/chapters
/10.4324/9780203453803-12
https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9780203453803/chapters
/10.4324/9780203453803-12
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.875480
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.875480
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01625.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01625.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206287721
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206287721
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.158.8.792
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.158.8.792
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.156
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2012.04.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2012.04.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.16.4.381
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0211-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805437
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805437
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.026
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.026
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp.12199
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp.12199
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18800787
http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315789286-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315789286-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.035
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.035
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.028
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.152
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.152
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.197
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.197
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1054773804271936
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023369
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12079
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01025.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01025.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367493512462261
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
models:
Applications and data analysis methods (Vol. 1). Thousand
Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Romeo, R. D. (2013). The teenage brain: The stress response
and the
adolescent brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
22, 140 –
145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721413475445
Sen, B. (2010). The relationship between frequency of family
dinner and
adolescent problem behaviors after adjusting for other family
character-
istics. Journal of Adolescence, 33, 187–196.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.adolescence.2009.03.011
Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009a). Daily family assistance
and the
psychological well-being of adolescents from Latin American,
Asian,
and European backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 45,
1177–1189.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014728
Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009b). A longitudinal daily
diary study of
family assistance and academic achievement among adolescents
from
Mexican, Chinese, and European backgrounds. Journal of Youth
and
Adolescence, 38, 560 –571. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-
008-
9391-7
Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Robins,
R. W.,
Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during
adolescence
predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic
prospects
during adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 42, 381–390.
http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381
Zollo, P. (1995). Wise up to teens: Insights into marketing and
advertising
to teenagers. East Patchogue, NY: New Strategist Publications,
Inc.
Received November 10, 2019
Revision received July 7, 2020
Accepted July 28, 2020 �
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2120 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721413475445
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014728
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9391-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9391-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381Family Meals
Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With Family
ConflictFamily Meals as a Protective Factor Against
Interpersonal ConflictAdolescence as a Sensitive
Developmental PeriodCurrent
StudyMethodParticipantsProcedureMeasuresFamily mealsDaily
family conflictDaily peer conflictDaily emotionsData
AnalysisResultsSample CharacteristicsFamily Meals Are
Associated With More Positive and Fewer Negative
EmotionsFamily Meals Buffer the Associations Between Family
Conflict and Negative EmotionsSensitivity AnalysesBonferroni
correctionControlling for frequency of family meals across
daysControlling for prior day emotionsSpillover into next day
emotionsVariation Across Adolescent
YearsDiscussionLimitations and Future
DirectionsConclusionReferences
Social Behavior and Personality , Volume 48, Issue 11, e9120
https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.9120
www.sbp-journal.com
Influence of relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers
on
employees’ voice
Siyuan Chen1, Mingyu Zhang1, Yihua Zhang2, Wen Wu1,
Zhimin Xiao3, Shaoxue Wu4, Pan Liu1, Yuhuan
Xia1
1School of Economics and Management, Beijing Jiaotong
University, People’s Republic of China
2Graduate School of Education and Psychology, Pepperdine
University, United States
3School of International Trade and Economics, University of
International Business and Economics, People’s Republic of
China
4Beijing Shiquan Consulting Company, Beijing, People’s
Republic of China
How to cite: Chen, S., Zhang, M., Zhang, Y., Wu, W., Xiao, Z.,
Wu, S., Liu, P., & Xia, Y. (2020). Influence of relationship
conflicts with
leaders and coworkers on employees’ voice. Social Behavior
and Personality: An international journal, 48(11), e9120
Building on self-determination theory and relational attribution
theory,
in this study we examined how relationship conflicts with
leaders and
coworkers simultaneously affect employee voice behaviors. We
expanded relational attribution theory by developing two new
constructs we labeled leader-relational attribution orientation
and
coworker-relational attribution orientation to describe
employees’
different responses to relationship conflicts with leaders and
coworkers
via psychological needs satisfaction. We surveyed 328
employee–leader
dyads who were employed at a semiconductor manufacturer to
test our
hypotheses. We found that leader-relational attribution
orientation can
strengthen the influences of relationship conflicts with leaders
on
psychological needs satisfaction and its indirect effects on
employee
voice behaviors. Coworker-relational attribution orientation can
strengthen the influences of relationship conflicts with
coworkers on
psychological needs satisfaction and its indirect effects on
employee
voice behaviors. Theoretical and practical implications are
discussed.
Keywords
supportive voice;
constructive voice;
destructive voice;
attribution orientation;
psychological needs
satisfaction; relationship
conflicts
Relationship conflict refers to the perception of
incompatibilities and disagreements among organizational
members regarding issues that are not task-related, for instance,
political beliefs, norms and values, or
gossip (De Dreu & Van Vianen, 2001). In some studies,
findings have shown that relationship conflict can
disrupt groups’ effective functioning and employees’
psychological well-being (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003;
Jehn, 1995). Researchers have explored some factors leading to
relationship conflict and how to prevent it
from happening (Edmondson & Smith, 2006). However, no
studies have included relationship conflicts
with leaders and coworkers in one empirical investigation.
Consequently, few empirical studies have been
conducted to test whether one type of relationship conflict
remains a significant factor when controlling for
the other. Organizational researchers have not developed an
integrated conceptual framework to explore the
influences of relationship conflicts with both leaders and
coworkers on focal employee voice behaviors.
Voice behavior, the expression of constructive opinions,
concerns, or ideas about work-related issues (Van
Dyne et al., 2003) plays a significant role in the running and
effectiveness of an organization (Organ et al.,
2006). Some researchers have explored the antecedents of
employee voice behavior at the individual,
supervisory, or organizational levels (Fuller et al., 2007; Liang
et al., 2012; W. Liu et al., 2010; Van Dyne et
al., 2008). In the current study, we explore the influence of
relationship conflict on employees’ voice
CORRESPONDENCE Wen Wu, School of Economics and
Management, Beijing Jiaotong University, No. 3 Shangyuancun,
Beijing
100044, People’s Republic of China. Email: [email protected] or
Zhimin Xiao, School of International Trade and Economics,
University of International Business and Economics, No. 10,
Huxin Dongjie, Beijing, 100029, People’s Republic of China.
Email:
[email protected]
© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia
behavior.
We develop a framework based on self-determination theory
(Deci & Ryan, 1985) and relational attribution
theory (Eberly et al., 2011) to examine how relationship
conflicts with leaders and coworkers affect
employee voice behaviors, including supportive voice,
destructive voice, and constructive voice. We argue
that relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers can
hinder the satisfaction of psychological needs,
which, in turn, influences employee voice behaviors. For
example, relationship conflict can induce a
coworker to refuse to provide information. We further propose
that such relationships depend on the focal
employee’s relational attributions of relationship conflicts with
leaders and coworkers. Eberly et al. (2011)
proposed a new set of relational attributions, defined as the
focal employee’s acknowledgment of an event in
a relationship. They suggested that individuals might attribute
the causes of events to their relationships
with others. Relationship attribution orientation is individuals’
explanations that locate their success in
their organization within their relationship with others. We
argue that individuals may differ in this
tendency, attributing success in their organization to their
relationships with leaders or with coworkers, and
these attributions may influence their experiences and
expectations (Eberly et al., 2011). Specifically, leader-
relational attribution orientation describes explanations given
by a focal individual that locate the success
of that individual in the organization within their relationship
with their immediate leader, and coworker-
relational attribution orientation describes explanations given
by a focal individual that locate the success
of that individual in the organization within their relationship
with their coworkers.
Theory and Hypotheses
Deci and Ryan (2000) posited that three innate basic
psychological needs affect individuals’ attitudes,
motivation, and behaviors, namely, autonomy, competence, and
relatedness. Need for autonomy refers to
the desire to be “the perceived origin or source of one’s own
behavior” (Deci & Ryan, 2000, p. 8) or to
endorse one’s actions as authentic and consistent with how one
would act regardless of external influences.
Need for competence refers to the desire to feel effective in
one’s activities and to interact with an
environment that supports the expression of one’s abilities.
Need for relatedness refers to the desire to
experience a sense of belonging or connectedness, or to be
valued by other individuals or collectives (Rosen
et al., 2014). Everyone is assumed to have these innate needs
(Baard et al., 2004). Psychological needs
satisfaction is the basis of linking social context (e.g.,
relationship building and information seeking) to
individual growth and well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) is one of the
most influential perspectives for explaining
employees’ behaviors. It explains how psychological needs
satisfaction strengthens and promotes the
internalization of external motivation, and ultimately affects
people’s attitudes and behaviors (Deci et al.,
1989). Factors that can fulfill individual psychological needs
promote intrinsic motivation and personal
growth, whereas factors that impede an individual’s
psychological needs thwart intrinsic motivation
(Greguras & Diefendorff, 2009).
We contend that the motivational routes to individual behavior
could be explained through social
determination theory. We focus on the mediating roles of
psychological needs satisfaction between
relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers, and the
employee voice behaviors of supportive voice,
destructive voice, and constructive voice. Figure 1 illustrates
the conceptual model of this study.
2© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal
Figure 1. Conceptual Model
Note. T1, T2, and T3 refer to the three data collection waves.
Sub = subordinate, Sup = supervisor,
indicating from whom data were collected.
Relationship Conflicts and Psychological Needs Satisfaction
On the basis of self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985),
we established that relationship conflicts
with both leaders and coworkers could hinder the focal
employees’ three psychological needs. In
relationship conflicts with leaders, employees will not be
encouraged to take on challenging and meaningful
tasks through close communication (Erdogan & Liden, 2002).
Deci and Ryan (2000) suggested that the human need for
autonomy could be satisfied by making choices
and fulfilling meaningful requests. Thus, high levels of
relationship conflict with leaders impede the focal
employees’ autonomy need. Moreover, in relationship conflicts
of members with their leaders, leaders tend
not to offer the members rewards, such as resource allocation
and personal development (Bolino & Turnley,
2009), which are instrumental for fulfilling employees’ intrinsic
and extrinsic needs. The poor career
development and negative evaluation arising from employees’
relationship conflicts with leaders may signal
to the employees that they are not valued or respected. This
perception impedes their sense of efficacy and
self-enhancement. In the case of leader relationship conflicts
with employees, the leader might not
encourage employees to question the existing ways of solving
problems (Hetland et al., 2011). Finally, the
poor mutual reciprocation that embodies relationship conflicts
with leaders results in weak employee
affective attachment to leaders (Ferris et al., 2009), and lack of
in-group membership and group
interconnectivity (Nifadkar et al., 2012). These outcomes can
hinder the satisfying of the need for
relatedness. Relationship conflicts with coworkers signify that
coworkers do not have strong bonds with one
another (Kim et al., 2017). They treat each other with a lack of
respect and autonomy (Sherony & Green,
2002; Van den Broeck et al., 2008). Thus, we concluded that if
focal employees had relationship conflicts
with their coworkers, this could lower the focal employees’
psychological needs satisfaction.
Psychological Needs Satisfaction and Employee Voice
Behaviors
3© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia
When employees satisfy their needs for autonomy, relatedness,
and competence, they tend to have a
favorable job attitude and perform well (Gagné & Deci, 2005).
In self-determination theory it is proposed
that satisfying these needs can improve employees’ intrinsic
motivation, which gives them a sense of
satisfaction from performing their work tasks (Gagné & Deci,
2005). Specifically, autonomy means that
individuals can make choices freely and based on a full
understanding of the relevant information (Deci &
Ryan, 2000). Hill et al. (2014) found that employees express
their values and interests through their work
when they can make decisions. Employee voice is an important
proactive behavior that can help leaders
make the right decisions and that benefits the organization.
Supportive voice is employees’ voluntary expression of support
for worthwhile work-related policies,
programs, objectives, procedures, or speaking out in defense of
these behaviors when they are being unfairly
criticized (Maynes & Podsakoff, 2013). According to the self-
determination perspective, employees whose
psychological needs for competence are satisfied are l ikely to
be intrinsically motivated. Such intrinsic
motivation may promote extrarole behaviors, such as supportive
voice (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Hence,
psychological needs satisfaction is likely to predict supportive
voice.
Constructive voice refers to the voluntary expression of ideas,
information, or opinions focused on effecting
organizationally functional change to the work context. (Maynes
& Podsakoff, 2013). Satisfying the
psychological needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness
denotes that employees can strive to meet
challenges, and to achieve success and satisfactory results
(Baard et al., 2004; Deci & Ryan, 2000). As such,
employees whose psychological needs are satisfied are likely to
suggest improvements to enhance
organizational performance.
Destructive voice is the voluntary expression of hurtful, critical,
or debasing opinions regarding work
policies, practices, and procedures (Maynes & Podsakoff,
2013). According to self-determination theory, if
employees’ basic psychological needs are not satisfied, they
may lack intrinsic motivation to control their
negative attitudes and behaviors that may harm the organization
(Gagné & Deci, 2005). Thus, psychological
needs satisfaction may be negatively related to destructive
voice.
Satisfying the three basic psychological needs for autonomy,
relatedness, and competence can promote
employees’ intrinsic motivation and facilitate positive work
attitudes and voice behaviors (Gagné & Deci,
2005). Accordingly, based on self-determination theory, we
proposed that relationship conflicts with leaders
and coworkers would hinder individual employees’ satisfying
their psychological needs and, in turn, would
increase their destructive voice, and would reduce their
constructive voice and supportive voice. Specifically,
we predicted the following:
Hypothesis 1a: Relationship conflicts with leaders have a
significant negative effect on employee
constructive voice through psychological needs satisfaction.
Hypothesis 1b: Relationship conflicts with leaders have a
positive influence on destructive voice through
psychological needs satisfaction.
Hypothesis 1c: Relationship conflicts with leaders will have a
significant negative effect on employee
supportive voice through psychological needs satisfaction.
Hypothesis 2a: Relationship conflicts with coworkers will exert
a significant negative influence on
employee constructive voice through psychological needs
satisfaction.
Hypothesis 2b: Relationship conflicts with coworkers will exert
a positive influence on employee
destructive voice through psychological needs satisfaction.
Hypothesis 2c: Relationship conflicts with coworkers will exert
a significant negative influence on
employee supportive voice through psychological needs
satisfaction.
Relational Attribution Theory
Both relational attribution theory (Eberly et al., 2011) and self-
determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985),
emphasize individual differences in work outcomes, depending
on social context. Individual attributions
4© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal
about the causes of events affect one’s feelings and expectations
(Tomlinson & Mayer, 2009). We further
maintained that employees’ attribution orientation of workplace
life may moderate the influence of
relationship conflicts on their psychological needs satisfaction.
In Hypothesis 1 we predicted that
relationship conflicts with leaders would hinder employees’
psychological needs for competency,
relatedness, and autonomy. When employees interpret conflicts
with leaders as hindering workplace
success, they may believe that bad relationships will provide
poor benefits, resources, and support.
In contrast, when employees believe that relationship conflicts
with leaders are not important in the
workplace, they may also believe that relationship conflicts
with leaders cannot bring about a critical lack of
resources (e.g., promotion, competence, relatedness) to hinder
their psychological needs. Satisfaction of
psychological needs is then less contingent on the presence or
absence of relationship conflict with leaders.
Therefore, we contended that the relationship between
relationship conflicts with leaders and psychological
needs satisfaction can be accentuated by employees’ leader -
relational attributions:
Hypothesis3a: The relationship between relationship conflicts
with leaders and employees’ psychological
needs satisfaction will be strengthened by the employees’
leader-relational attribution orientations: The
relationship will be stronger when employees’ leader-relational
attribution orientation is higher rather than
lower.
Likewise, when employees attribute work-related success to
their relationships with coworkers, they may
believe that relationship conflicts with coworkers will hinder
them in satisfying competence, relatedness,
and autonomy needs. In other words, with other conditions
being equal, the influence of relationship
conflicts with coworkers on employees’ psychological needs
can be strengthened when the employees have
high coworker-relational attribution orientation:
Hypothesis 3b: The relationship between relationship conflicts
with coworkers and employees’
psychological needs satisfaction will be strengthened by the
employees’ coworker-relational attribution
orientations: The relationship will be stronger when employees’
coworker-relational attribution orientations
are higher rather than lower.
An Integrative Moderated Mediation Model
As previously discussed, relationship conflicts with leaders and
with coworkers both influence employee
voice behaviors through the mediating role of psychological
needs satisfaction. When employees have strong
leader-relational attribution orientation (LRAO), they may
believe that their career will be improved via
relationship conflicts with their leader. In the previous
hypotheses we suggested that by augmenting
relationship conflicts with leaders and psychological needs
satisfaction, employees’ LRAO affect the degree
to which relationship conflicts with the leader influence
employee voice behaviors. Similarly, employees’
coworker-relational attribution orientations (CRAO) may
moderate the influence of relationship conflicts
with coworkers on employee voice behaviors and thus may
change the indirect effect of relationship
conflicts with coworkers on employee voice behaviors via
psychological needs satisfaction. We therefore
made two moderated mediation predictions:
Hypothesis 3c: Employees’ leader-relational attribution
orientations will moderate the indirect effect of
relationship conflicts with leaders and employee voice
behaviors via psychological needs satisfaction: The
indirect effect will be stronger when employees’ leader-
relational attribution orientations are higher rather
than lower.
Hypothesis 3d: Employees’ coworker-relational attribution
orientations will moderate the indirect effect
of relationship conflicts with coworkers and employee voice
behaviors via psychological needs satisfaction:
The indirect effect will be stronger when employees’ coworker -
relational attribution orientations are higher
rather than lower.
Pilot Study
In differentiating and validating items of LRAO and CRAO, we
used theoretical evidence as our deductive
5© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia
item-generation approach (Hinkin, 1998). Based on the
definition of relational attribution, we generated
items using two sources. First, we directly generated items in
line with this definition, using concise
sentences to describe employee behaviors that fell into our two
categories of relational attributions. Second,
we referred to measures used in existing studies on relational
attribution for our item pool (Tomlinson &
Mayer, 2009). Thus, we identified 16 items for LRAO and
CRAO. After removing redundant items, three
subject-matter experts matched the items to the definitions of
LRAO and CRAO. From carrying out this
procedure, we identified five representative items for LRAO
and five for CRAO, which we used for pilot
testing.
To test our items of LRAO and CRAO, we distributed a survey
form including potential items of relational
attribution orientation, to 322 employees of a large bank in
China via WeChat, a popular social media
platform in China. Participation was voluntary, and 236
respondents completed the survey. In the final
sample, 53.8% of respondents were men and 46.2% were women
(Mage = 38.39 years, SD = 8.72, ranging
from 26 to 53 years; 84.75% had a bachelor’s degree or above,
and 15.25% had a college education). Items
are set out in Table 1 and were rated by employees on a 5-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely
disagree) and 5 (completely agree).
Then, we conducted a factor analysis with the Oblimin method.
We performed principal component
analysis. The rotation method was Oblimin with Kaiser
normalization, and the rotation converged in four
iterations. The results showed a distinction between LRAO and
CRAO. No item had heavy cross-loadings, so
that the LRAO factor consisted of five items, and all five
emphasized relationship quality with leaders. The
CRAO factor also consisted of five items, and all five
emphasized relationship quality with coworkers. Table
1 shows the pattern matrix using the oblimin method.
Table 1. Pattern Matrix of the Factor Analysis by the Oblimin
Method
Main Study
Sample and Procedure
We recruited participants for the main study from one private
semiconductor manufacturer whose research
and development center is in China, and whose products are
sold locally and abroad. We used a three-wave,
two-source survey method. The general managers notified
potential respondents that the academic survey
was voluntary. We randomly selected one employee from each
work team. We received emails from 745
6© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal
employees, giving consent to participate together with their
employee identification number.
Among the group of employees 58.53% were men, and 41.47%
were women. The average age of the
employees was 35.46 years (ranging from 24 to 47, SD = 4.76).
In terms of position held, 93% were workers
at grass-roots level, 7% were in low management positions. As
regards education level, 68.79% had a
bachelor degree or higher qualification, the rest had a college
education. Among the group, 63.57% had
worked for the company for more than 7 years, and the
remaining 36.43% had less than 7 years’ tenure. At
Time 1, we asked the employees to report on relationship
conflicts with leaders and coworkers, and control
variables. We distributed pencils and survey forms, along with a
cover letter assuring confidentiality, to the
participants. After finishing the survey the respondents gave
them directly to the research team. At Time 2,
which was four weeks later, we asked employees to rate their
psychological needs satisfaction and their
LRAO and CRAO. At Time 3, which was four weeks after Time
2, we distributed a survey to the participants’
supervisors of participants at Times 1 and 2, using a list
provided by the company’s human resources
department, asking the supervisors to rate the participants’
employee voice behaviors.
At Time 1, we gave survey forms to 745 employees, and 601
were completed (80.67%). At Time 2, we gave
survey forms to 601 employees, and 482 were completed
(80.20%). At Time 3, we distributed 481
supervisor survey forms, and 371 were completed (77.13%). In
the end, we had 328 matched supervisor-
subordinate dyads for analysis.
Measures
Relationship Conflict
We asked employees to evaluate their relationship conflicts
with their immediate supervisor and coworkers
using four items adapted from the scale developed by Jehn
(1995). The sample items include “How much
friction is there between you and your immediate supervisor?”
Employees rated the items on a 5-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all), to 5 (very frequently).
Psychological Needs Satisfaction
We used the 21-item scale developed by Kasser et al. (1992) to
measure psychological needs satisfaction.
Employees rated the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely
agree). Sample items include “Most days I feel a sense of
accomplishment from working,” and “I feel like I
can pretty much be myself at work.” In accordance with
previous research (Rosen et al., 2014) and from the
theoretical perspective, we viewed need satisfaction as an
overall index rather than as separate need
satisfaction categories. We did not expect relationship conflicts
with leaders and coworkers to differ in terms
of their relationship to autonomy, competence, and relatedness.
Similarly, we did not expect every need to
relate differentially to employee outcomes.
Constructive Voice
Constructive voice was measured using Maynes and Podsakoff’s
(2013) five-item instrument. Leaders rated
the items for each type of voice on a 5-point Likert scale
ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5
(completely agree). Sample items for constructive voice are
“This employee frequently makes suggestions
about how to do things in new or more effective ways at work,”
and “This employee often suggests changes
to work projects in order to make them better.”
Destructive Voice
The five-item scale from Maynes and Podsakoff (2013) was
employed to measure employee destructive voice
behavior. A sample item is “This employee often bad-mouths
the organization’s policies or objectives.”
Supportive Voice
The five-item scale developed by Maynes and Podsakoff (2013)
was used to measure employee supportive
voice. The sample items include “This employee defends
organizational programs that are worthwhile when
7© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia
others unfairly criticize the programs.”
Leader-Relational Attribution Orientation and Coworker-
Relational Attribution Orientation
Employees’ LRAO and CRAO were measured with the items
developed in the pilot study.
Results
Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics, reliabilities, and
correlations for the study variables.
Table 2. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations of Study
Variables
Note. N = 328; reliabilities are in italics on the diagonal.
Gender was measured using two categories: 1 =
male, and 2 = female. Level of education was measured using
four categories: 1 = below college, 2 = college, 3
= postgraduate, and 4 = doctorate. Position in the organization
was measured using four categories: 1 =
employee, 2 = supervisor, 3 = middle manager, and 4 = senior
manager. Organizational tenure was measured
with four categories: 1 = less than 5 years, 2 = 5–10 years, 3 =
11–15 years, and 4 = more than 15 years. LRAO
= leader-relational attribution orientation, CRAO = coworker-
relational attribution orientation.
* p < .05. ** p < .01 (two-tailed).
Mediation Effects of Psychological Needs Satisfaction
We conducted hierarchical multiple regression analyses to test
Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 1c and 2a, 2b, 2c by
separately entering the control variables, independent variables
(relationship conflicts with leaders and
coworkers), and the mediator (psychological needs satisfaction).
Model 1 in Table 3 indicates that
relationship conflicts with leaders and with coworkers were
both significantly related to psychological needs
satisfaction. As shown by the results of Model 4, Model 7, and
Model 10, relationship conflicts with leaders
and with coworkers were both significantly related to
supportive voice, destructive voice, and constructive
voice. Model 5, Model 8, and Model 11 further revealed that
psychological needs satisfaction was
significantly related to supportive, destructive, and constructive
voice, and that the effects of relationship
conflicts with leaders and with coworkers on supportive,
destructive, and constructive voice were weaker
than the effects when constructive voice was excluded.
8© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights
Reserved.
Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal
Table 3. Main and Interactive Effects Results
Note. N = 328. PNS = psychological needs satisfaction, RCL =
relational conflict with leaders, RCC =
relational conflict with coworkers, LRAO = leader-relational
attribution orientation, CRAO = coworker-
relational attribution orientation.
* p < .05. ** p < .01 (two-tailed).
We also examined those effects using the bootstrap approach
(MacKinnon et al., 2007). The results
indicated that all the mediation effects of psychological needs
satisfaction were significant. Bootstrap results
are reported in Table 4. Particularly for relationship conflicts
with leaders, the 95% confidence interval (CI)
for indirect effects did not include zero; for relationship
conflicts with coworkers, the 95% CI of the indirect
effects also excluded zero. Thus, Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 1c and 2a,
2b, 2c were all supported.
Table 4. Results of Bootstrap Analyses for the Mediating Role
of Psychological Needs Satisfaction
Note. N = 328. Bootstrap sample size = 5,000. CI = confidence
interval, LL = lower limit, UL = upper limit.
Moderating Effects of Employees’ Leader-Relational
Attribution Orientation and
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F
Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F

More Related Content

Similar to Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F

International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Social support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docx
Social support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docxSocial support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docx
Social support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Does Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal Follow
Does Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal FollowDoes Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal Follow
Does Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal FollowDustiBuckner14
 
Freeing Families FLE Project Packet
Freeing Families FLE Project PacketFreeing Families FLE Project Packet
Freeing Families FLE Project PacketJazmin Nagorski
 
ENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docx
ENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docxENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docx
ENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docxgreg1eden90113
 
Research Supporting Sf Protective Factors 02 19 09
Research Supporting  Sf Protective Factors  02 19 09Research Supporting  Sf Protective Factors  02 19 09
Research Supporting Sf Protective Factors 02 19 09k.stepleton
 
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...inventionjournals
 
Current Events and Childhood Paper (1).docx
Current Events and Childhood Paper (1).docxCurrent Events and Childhood Paper (1).docx
Current Events and Childhood Paper (1).docxBanks Fullerton
 
Effects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and Parenting
Effects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and ParentingEffects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and Parenting
Effects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and ParentingRandi Hovey
 
Annotated Bibliography
Annotated BibliographyAnnotated Bibliography
Annotated BibliographyTaylor Panzer
 
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And DistressAdolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And DistressDarian Pruitt
 
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docxComplete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docxwrite22
 
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docxComplete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docxwrite22
 
D Lee Presentation
D Lee PresentationD Lee Presentation
D Lee Presentationddlee82
 
Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Styles
Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting StylesAuthoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Styles
Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Stylesdrunksilhouette51
 
ArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docx
ArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docxArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docx
ArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docxrossskuddershamus
 
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...John Mattscheck, M.A.
 
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...John Mattscheck, M.A.
 

Similar to Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F (20)

International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Social support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docx
Social support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docxSocial support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docx
Social support available for substance‐dependent mothersfrom.docx
 
Does Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal Follow
Does Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal FollowDoes Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal Follow
Does Parental Sexual Orientation Matter A Longitudinal Follow
 
Freeing Families FLE Project Packet
Freeing Families FLE Project PacketFreeing Families FLE Project Packet
Freeing Families FLE Project Packet
 
ENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docx
ENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docxENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docx
ENGL 1302Due Friday, November 18McCourtLab Six As.docx
 
Research Supporting Sf Protective Factors 02 19 09
Research Supporting  Sf Protective Factors  02 19 09Research Supporting  Sf Protective Factors  02 19 09
Research Supporting Sf Protective Factors 02 19 09
 
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
 
Current Events and Childhood Paper (1).docx
Current Events and Childhood Paper (1).docxCurrent Events and Childhood Paper (1).docx
Current Events and Childhood Paper (1).docx
 
Effects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and Parenting
Effects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and ParentingEffects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and Parenting
Effects of Divorce on Children as a Function of Age, Sex, and Parenting
 
Annotated Bibliography
Annotated BibliographyAnnotated Bibliography
Annotated Bibliography
 
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And DistressAdolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
 
aids and behavior
aids and behavioraids and behavior
aids and behavior
 
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docxComplete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
 
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docxComplete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
Complete Advanced Nursing Discussion.docx
 
Student-Outcomes-MFF
Student-Outcomes-MFFStudent-Outcomes-MFF
Student-Outcomes-MFF
 
D Lee Presentation
D Lee PresentationD Lee Presentation
D Lee Presentation
 
Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Styles
Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting StylesAuthoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Styles
Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Styles
 
ArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docx
ArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docxArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docx
ArticleAre parents identifyingpositive aspects to parent.docx
 
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
 
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
 

More from ChereCheek752

please read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docx
please read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docxplease read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docx
please read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docxChereCheek752
 
please read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docx
please read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docxplease read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docx
please read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docxChereCheek752
 
Please read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docx
Please read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docxPlease read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docx
Please read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docxChereCheek752
 
Please read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docx
Please read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docxPlease read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docx
Please read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docxChereCheek752
 
Please read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docx
Please read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docxPlease read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docx
Please read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docxChereCheek752
 
Please Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docx
Please Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docxPlease Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docx
Please Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docxChereCheek752
 
Please provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docx
Please provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docxPlease provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docx
Please provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docxChereCheek752
 
Please provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docx
Please provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docxPlease provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docx
Please provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docxChereCheek752
 
Please provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docx
Please provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docxPlease provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docx
Please provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docxChereCheek752
 
Please provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docx
Please provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docxPlease provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docx
Please provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docxChereCheek752
 
PLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docx
PLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docxPLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docx
PLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docxChereCheek752
 
Please note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docx
Please note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docxPlease note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docx
Please note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docxChereCheek752
 
Please no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docx
Please no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docxPlease no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docx
Please no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docxChereCheek752
 
PLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docx
PLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docxPLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docx
PLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docxChereCheek752
 
Please Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docx
Please Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docxPlease Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docx
Please Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docxChereCheek752
 
Please only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docx
Please only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docxPlease only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docx
Please only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docxChereCheek752
 
Please note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docx
Please note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docxPlease note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docx
Please note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docxChereCheek752
 
PLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docx
PLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docxPLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docx
PLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docxChereCheek752
 
Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docx
Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docxPlease no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docx
Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docxChereCheek752
 
Please need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docx
Please need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docxPlease need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docx
Please need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docxChereCheek752
 

More from ChereCheek752 (20)

please read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docx
please read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docxplease read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docx
please read the attached file cearfully before telling me you can do.docx
 
please read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docx
please read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docxplease read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docx
please read my post carefully.then place handshakei have the wor.docx
 
Please read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docx
Please read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docxPlease read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docx
Please read the attachment.Please write a pure Essay Paper. Plea.docx
 
Please read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docx
Please read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docxPlease read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docx
Please read first because this Assignment is for correction.Plea.docx
 
Please read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docx
Please read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docxPlease read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docx
Please read below, and write esaay.I need 3 pages.Overvi.docx
 
Please Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docx
Please Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docxPlease Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docx
Please Read Before RespondingI need assistance with a .docx
 
Please provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docx
Please provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docxPlease provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docx
Please provide response to the below post. Topic #1) You are an .docx
 
Please provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docx
Please provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docxPlease provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docx
Please provide an annotation for the two articles attached AND ide.docx
 
Please provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docx
Please provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docxPlease provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docx
Please provide a statement that addresses your reasons for transferr.docx
 
Please provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docx
Please provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docxPlease provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docx
Please provide a brief response to the following questions1) How .docx
 
PLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docx
PLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docxPLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docx
PLEASE NOTE OF SOURCESMATERIALS ALSO INCLUDED---USE THEMT.docx
 
Please note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docx
Please note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docxPlease note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docx
Please note that the following vignettes represent samples of the ty.docx
 
Please no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docx
Please no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docxPlease no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docx
Please no plagiarism. I have attached an example to go by. The popul.docx
 
PLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docx
PLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docxPLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docx
PLEASE NO PLAGIARIZE!! Have 10 hours to fullfil this work. 1page or .docx
 
Please Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docx
Please Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docxPlease Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docx
Please Paraphrase the following into a more scholarly toneI f.docx
 
Please only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docx
Please only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docxPlease only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docx
Please only respond if you are familiar with raspberry piIam loo.docx
 
Please note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docx
Please note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docxPlease note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docx
Please note this is 2 ASSIGNMENTS ......Please only orginial work on.docx
 
PLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docx
PLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docxPLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docx
PLEASE NEED TWO RESPONSES TWO HUNDRED WORDS EACHDistinguish b.docx
 
Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docx
Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docxPlease no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docx
Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resour.docx
 
Please need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docx
Please need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docxPlease need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docx
Please need two posts of 200 words each. Discuss the ways in whi.docx
 

Recently uploaded

“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxabhijeetpadhi001
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxUnboundStockton
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 

Recently uploaded (20)

“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 

Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With F

  • 1. Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With Family Conflict Emma Armstrong-Carter Stanford University Eva H. Telzer University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Family meals have been associated with positive adolescent outcomes in cross-sectional and longitudinal research. However, it is not known how adolescents experience family meals on a daily basis, and whether family meals buffer stresses associated with interpersonal conflicts on the daily level. To address this gap in the literature, adolescents (N � 396, 58% female, Mage � 14.57 years) completed diary checklists for up to 14 days, reporting their emotions, experiences of family and peer conflict, and whether they ate with their family that day. On days that adolescents shared a family meal, they felt greater happiness and role fulfillment, and less burnout and distress. Moreover, family conflict was associated with more negative emotionality only on days that adolescents did not also eat with the family. Findings suggest that family meals buffer daily risks associated with familial conflicts. Follow-up analyses suggest that these processes may be particularly important among older adolescents. Keywords: adolescence, family meal, emotions, family conflict,
  • 2. peer conflict During busy daily life, meals are often the only time when family members come together to engage, and provide and receive emotional support (Larson, Branscomb, & Wiley, 2006). Perhaps in part because daily family meals represent a stable, routine, and context for emotional connectedness (Goldfarb, Tarver, & Sen, 2014; Jones, 2018), family meals have been associated with many positive outcomes across development. For example, adolescents who more frequently eat with the family are less likely to be overweight or underweight, have substance use problems, and struggle with clinical depression (Fulkerson et al., 2006), and tend to feel more emotionally close to parents and siblings (Fiese et al., 2002) compared with their peers. The benefits associated with family meals also extend beyond the home. Adolescents who more frequently eat with the family tend to exhibit higher academic performance (Eisenberg, Olson, Neumark-Sztainer, Story, & Bear- inger, 2004), fewer antisocial behaviors (Fulkerson et al., 2006; Prior & Limbert, 2013; Sen, 2010), and increased social compe- tencies with peers (Fulkerson et al., 2006). Prior research has been almost entirely cross-sectional, retro- spective, or longitudinal, which can only tell us about average meal eating behaviors between adolescents. This work has exam- ined how average family meals at one time point relate to average
  • 3. well-being at another time point (Goldfarb et al., 2014). To extend prior research, it is important to clarify whether family meals are associated with positive or negative emotions on the daily level. Examining temporal relations at the daily level may help us to understand the processes by which family meals promote long- term well-being (Offer, 2013a, 2013b), as has been observed in prior research. For example, if adolescents feel happier on days that they eat with the family, this could partially explain why family meals are associated with later positive social and emo- tional adaptation. Family Meals as a Protective Factor Against Interpersonal Conflict Family meals may translate into greater well-being by protect- ing against the negative effects of daily stressors, such as conflict in the home. On the daily level, sharing a family meal may mitigate the negative impacts of family conflict by providing an opportunity to make amends, internalize disputes less, engage positively, and recover; thereby, offsetting distress caused by conflict during the day. Supporting this notion, Family Systems Theory and developmental theories of risk and resilience (Brod- erick, 1993; Labella & Masten, 2018) have suggested that spend- ing positive time together as a family after experiencing emotional challenges can increase adolescents’ feelings of family cohesion and emotional security, and promote resilience (Fiese et al., 2002; Jones, 2018). Consistent with these theories, family meals have been associated with greater emotional well-being among adoles- cents experiencing ongoing difficult family relationships (Meier
  • 4. & Musick, 2014). Indeed, the experience of coming together as a This article was published Online First September 28, 2020 X Emma Armstrong-Carter, Graduate School of Education, Stanford University; Eva H. Telzer, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. This article was prepared with support from the National Institutes of Health Grant R01DA039923 and National Science Foundation Grant SES 1459719 provided to Eva H. Telzer, the Department of Psychology at the University of Illinois, and the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Emma Armstrong-Carter, Graduate School of Education, Stanford University, 520 Galvez Mall, Stanford, CA 94305, or to Eva H. Telzer, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 235 East Cameron Avenue, Chapel Hill, NC 27510. E- mail: [email protected] or [email protected] T hi s
  • 9. br oa dl y. Developmental Psychology © 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 56, No. 11, 2110 –2120 ISSN: 0012-1649 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001111 2110 https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5847-9486 mailto:[email protected] mailto:[email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001111 family to share a meal may be more important than the specific meal itself (Larson et al., 2006). However, it remains unclear whether family meals protect against emotional risks on the daily level as well. If family meals reduce the distress that is asso- ciated with family conflict the same day, this could represent one daily pathway through which family meals ultimately con- fer the greater well-being. Moreover, this could reveal a key experience in the home that helps to support adolescents’ daily emotional health. In addition to buffering the negative costs of family conflict, family meals may also protect youth in the face of conflict with peers. Family meals can provide adolescents the opportunity to express difficult feelings, receive emotional support from parents
  • 10. and siblings, and distract from difficult emotions by engaging positively with family (Fiese, Foley, & Spagnola, 2006, 2012; Larson et al., 2006). For example, adolescents whose family mem- bers express concern and emotional support during family dinners tend to feel greater trust and belonging in the family, which may foster resiliency even outside the home (Fiese et al., 2006; Larson et al., 2006). Indeed, parents often use meal times to encourage and coach youth through difficult experiences (Larson et al., 2006), and this may include social challenges with peers. Consistent with this notion, a family meal may provide adolescents the opportunity to share their experiences of negative peer interactions or conflict they encountered that day, receive emotional support, encourage- ment and ideas for how to cope. In this way, family meals may protect adolescents from emotional risk the same day; that is, adolescents may go to bed feeling relatively happier and more fulfilled after having a chance to express their challenges and receive support (Ho et al., 2018). Adolescence as a Sensitive Developmental Period Adolescence is an essential developmental period for studying processes related to family meals, interpersonal conflict, and youth adaptation for several reasons. First, adolescents may benefit more from family meals compared with children, if meals help to main-
  • 11. tain connection with the family during the adolescent transition toward increased autonomy and participation in activities outside the home with peers (Brannen, 2002). Second, adolescents expe- rience higher levels of daily interpersonal conflicts, which incurs increased emotional risk (Chung, Flook, & Fuligni, 2011; Holm- beck, 2018). In addition, adolescents also become more sensitive to the negative effects of interpersonal conflicts, because they experience hormonal shifts (Romeo, 2013), begin to develop their own social identities that are highly influenced by their interac- tions with others (Fuligni, 2019) and place increasing importance on relationships with peers (Chung et al., 2011). These shifts may be particularly pronounced in older compared with younger ado- lescents (Holmbeck, 2018), who spend more of their day away from the family with peers (Brannen, 2002). In light of this, family meals might be even more of a protective factor during later adolescence, compared with earlier adolescence. It is important to understand if the interplay between family meals, conflict, and adjustment varies across adolescent development, as this may reveal key, specific developmental periods during which family meals are more or less beneficial and protective. Current Study In the current study, we capitalized on the daily diary method in a large and diverse sample of adolescents. We examined whether family meals and family and peer conflict contribute to adoles - cents’ positive or negative emotions on the daily level, and
  • 12. whether family meals mitigate the emotional risks associated with family and peer conflict. The daily diary method is uniquely useful for examining questions related to adolescents’ emotions and daily routines in the home (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). Youths’ reports of their daily activities, behaviors, and feelings are more reliable and accurate than when these processes are assessed using traditional retrospective accounts from a single questionnaire (Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003). Daily diary methods also allow researchers to examine whether specific events that occur on one day are asso- ciated with feelings measured the same day. For example, on days that adolescents share a meal with their family, are they more likely to feel happy or distressed? While not causal, data of this nature allow us to test within the same adolescent, whether family meals and positive and negative emotions co-occur with each other on the same day. In this way, we can hold constant the extraneous traits and characteristics of both the individual adolescent and the family. Daily diary methods also enable us to examine interactions between multiple processes that occur on the daily level. For example, does the daily association between interpersonal conflict and emotions depend on whether the adolescent ate a meal with their family that day? Prior literature that has examined links between family meals and adolescent outcomes has used indexes
  • 13. that are averaged across days or retrospective (Goldfarb et al., 2014). More recent studies emphasize the importance of examin- ing family experiences on the daily level, as family routines have been shown to temporally fluctuate with emotions within days (Armstrong-Carter, Ivory, Lin, Muscatell, & Telzer, 2020; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). In particular, recent work has revealed signif- icant daily fluctuations in adolescents’ happiness, distress, burn- out, and role fulfillment (i.e., feeling like a good son, daughter, or sibling), suggesting that these measures of emotions capture dis- tinctive variability across days within adolescents, and are mean- ingfully related to same-day family experiences. For example, role-fulfillment has been linked to family behaviors with effect sizes twice the size of more commonly measured constructs such as happiness or distress (Armstrong-Carter et al., 2020), indicating that it may be robustly and uniquely related to adolescents’ expe- riences in the home the same day. Using the daily diary method, we tested the following key questions: (1) Is eating a meal with the family associated with positive or negative emotions the same day? Given the observed associations between family meals and positive developmental outcomes in prior cross-sectional and longitudinal work, we hy- pothesized that family meals would be associated with more pos- itive emotions (e.g., happiness and role-fulfillment) and fewer
  • 14. negative emotions (e.g., distress and burnout) the same day. We tested multiple positive and negative emotions to both to allow for the possibility that family meals might relate to different measures simultaneously and divergently (e.g., increase happiness but also increase distress, or increase happiness but decrease role - fulfillment) and to be consistent with prior work (Armstrong- Carter et al., 2020; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). (2) Are family and peer conflict associated with positive or negative emotions on the T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by
  • 18. is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2111FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS daily level, and does eating a meal with the family buffer (i.e., moderate) this association? Informed by developmental theories of youth risk and resilience, which suggest that positive family ex- periences can counteract the risks of negative experiences (Labella & Masten, 2018), we hypothesized that family meals would buffer
  • 19. the negative effects of family and peer conflict. (3) Do these associations vary by age across early to late adolescence? Drawing on evidence of increases in daily conflict, emotional risk, and interpersonal sensitivity across adolescence (Holmbeck, 2018), we hypothesized that older adolescents may benefit even more from family meals, compared with younger adolescents. To follow up and provide further evidence of directionality of effects, we con- ducted additional sensitivity analyses. Specifically, we first con- trolled for previous day emotions, and then tested potential spill- over into emotions the next day. Method Participants Participants were 396 adolescents (57.92% female) between the ages of 11 and 18 years (Mage � 14.57 years, SD � 1.39 years). The sample was racially and ethnically diverse: 38.89% identified as non-Hispanic White (from here on referred to as White, N � 154), 26.77% Asian (N � 106, 13 of whom were mixed [e.g., Asian and White]), 16.67% Hispanic/Latinx (N � 66, 11 of whom were mixed [e.g., Hispanic and White]), 10.8% African American (N � 45, 9 of whom were mixed [e.g., African American and White]), and 6.31% other race (N � 27, 16 of whom self- identified as other and nine were mixed race). Approximately 10% of moth- ers had less than an 8th grade education, 13% did not complete
  • 20. high school, 24% completed high school, 27% completed postsec- ondary education (college, trade, or vocational school), and 23% completed graduate school (3% declined to answer). Participants were recruited from the community using convenience sampling, including posting flyers at schools, posting on listservs serving ethnic minority families, recruiting participants from previous studies who agreed to be contacted for other research studies, and word of mouth. Participants were compensated $10 in total for completing the daily diaries as well as a $20 bonus if inspection of the data indicated that they had completed all the diaries on time. Participants provided written consent/assent and procedures were approved by the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Com- mittee on Human Subjects (Protocol #13378; Development of Decision Making and Social Cognition). Procedure Participants were provided with diary checklists; most partici - pants (80%) were provided 14 days of diaries, whereas 20% of participants (N � 83) were only provided with 7 days of diaries. Most participants (89.82%) completed all days of their dairies (M � 93.87% of days, SD � 15.51% of days, range � 14.29 – 100%). There were 4,369 total person-day observations (Level 1 reports). Diaries included both weekdays and weekends. The order of days differed between participants depending on the day of the
  • 21. week that they started, but all participants had the same proportion of weekday to weekend data if they completed all of the diaries. Participants chose to complete the diaries either on paper (63.20%) or via a secure website (36.80%). Participants who responded with paper were given 14 manila envelopes and an electronic time stamper (Dymo Corporation, Stamford, CT), which verified the time that checklists were completed. The time stamper is a small device that imprints the current date and time and is programmed with a security code so that the correct date and time cannot be changed. Participants were instructed to place their completed checklists into an envelope each night and to stamp the seal of the envelope with the time stamper. Participants who completed sur- veys online were sent an e-mail with the link to each daily diary survey, and the time and date of completion were recorded via the website. The daily diary checklists were three pages long and each took approximately 5–10 min to complete. Measures Family meals. Participants indicated on the daily checklist whether or not they ate any meal with their family each day. We chose this broad measure because the process of coming together as a family has been associated with positive outcomes, and the specific meal (i.e., breakfast, lunch, or dinner) may vary by family
  • 22. and day and may be less important (Larson et al., 2006). To allow for the possibility that the structure and timing of family meals vary across different households, we did not specify or define “family meal” any further in the daily checklist. As such, adoles- cents were able to define family meals for themselves in the context of their own experience. This item yielded a single dichot- omous index of family meals, which was coded 0 � no family meal, 1 � family meal. Daily family conflict. Items on the daily checklist asked par - ticipants to indicate whether they had engaged in different behav- iors with family members each day. Each item was coded as 0 � no, 1 � yes. Our measure of Family conflict was based on family systems theory (Broderick, 1993), general self-report measures of family conflict (e.g., Bloom, 1985), and other daily diary studies (Chung et al., 2011). This was the mean of four items: you argued with a sibling, you got into trouble or were punished by your parents, you argued with a parent, you lied to parent (R1F � .77; see Cranford et al., 2006 for more information on this reliability statistic). Daily peer conflict. Items on the daily checklist asked partic- ipants to indicate whether they had engaged in different behaviors with peers each day. Each item was coded as 0 � no, 1 � yes. Our measure of Peer conflict was based on social relational theories
  • 23. of adolescent development (Laursen & Collins, 1994), and other self-reported measures of peer conflict (e.g., Marsee et al., 2011) including those from prior daily diary studies (Chung et al., 2011). It was the mean of 11 items: you hit, kicked, or shoved a peer, you threatened, insulted, or made fun of a peer, you said something mean behind a friend’s back, you excluded or left a friend out, you lied to a friend, someone online or in a text message threatened, insulted or made fun of you, you argued with a friend, you argued with a boyfriend or girlfriend, you were excluded or left out by friends, a peer said something mean behind your back, a peer threatened, insulted, or made fun of you, R1F � .97. This measure demonstrated acceptable within-person reliability of change (RC � .71; Cranford et al., 2006). Daily emotions. Daily emotions were assessed with items on the daily checklist that were drawn from the Profile of Mood States T hi s do cu m en t
  • 28. 2112 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER (McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971) and used in prior research (Armstrong-Carter et al., 2020; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). Adoles- cents used a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (ex- tremely) to indicate “the extent to which they felt each emotion that day.” The time of day for each emotion was not specified. Happiness was calculated from two items: joyful and happy, R1F � .87. Distress was calculated from nine items: sad, hopeless, discouraged, on edge, unable to concentrate, uneasy, nervous, stressed, worried, R1F � .88. Burnout was calculated from three items: fatigue, exhausted, and worn-out, R1F � .87. Role Fulfill- ment was calculated from two additional items in the daily diary in which participants responded on a 7-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely) to report the extent to which they felt like “a good son or daughter” and “a good brother or sister” that day. Role fulfillment was calculated as the mean of these two items, R1F � .86. Because adolescents answered the daily diaries at the end of the day just before bedtime, family meals occurred before adolescents reported their emotions. All emotion composites demonstrated acceptable within-person reliability of change (RCs � .71–.79). For all daily diary measures (i.e., family and peer conflict, emotions), there was substantial within- person variability (intraclass correlation coefficient, ICC � .43–.79) and between-person variability across the diary days (ICC � .36 –
  • 29. .64). Data Analysis Our aim was to understand the daily association between eating a meal with the family and positive and negative emotions, and whether family meals buffer emotional reactivity associated with daily interpersonal conflicts. We conducted linear mixed effect models that nested days (Level 1) within participants (Level 2). Fixed effects were tested at the level of participants (i.e., Level 2). This statistical approach accounts for dependency within partici- pants and introduces less bias because of missing data compared with traditional statistical analyses, such as repeated measures analysis of variance (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). We person- centered all Level-1 predictors, and we included on the intercept person-mean values for each of our daily predictors (i.e., family meals, family conflict, and peer conflict; Curran & Bauer, 2011). Model 1 tested family meals and interpersonal conflict (family and peer) as simultaneous Level 1 predictors of each emotional experience the same day. Model 2 additionally included an inter- action term that was the product of family meal and each inter - personal conflict variable (i.e., family conflict and peer conflict). To test potential differences in observed associations by develop- mental period, Model 3 included an additional three-way interac- tion term between family meals, adolescent age, and each
  • 30. conflict variable. We conducted additional sensitivity analyses first con- trolling for prior day emotions and then testing next day emotions as a dependent variable. To probe significant cross-level interactions, we used the simple slopes technique (Aiken, West, & Reno, 1991) to test the associ- ations between interpersonal conflict and emotions on days when there was or was not a family meal. For three-way interactions, we split adolescents by the mean age for the sample, and plotted two-way interactions within younger and older adolescents. All analyses were conducted using Stata Software (StataSE, Version 15.1.632). Results Sample Characteristics Table 1 displays descriptive statistics for sample constructs for the full sample and by sex and race/ethnicity. On average, adoles- cents ate a meal with the family on 61% of days. Family meals were most common among Asian youth and least common among Black youth, but did not differ by sex. Girls reported higher peer conflict than boys, but family conflict did not vary by sex. Bivariate correlations of mean values across days showed that adolescents who ate more frequent family meals were younger, r � �.09, p � .001 and experienced more family conflict, r � .05,
  • 31. p � .002, and less peer conflict, r � �.04, p � .001. Adolescents who experienced more family conflict were younger, r � �.08, p � .001, and experienced more peer conflict, r � .21, p � .001. There were no other significant correlations across days. Multilevel analyses at the daily level showed that family meals were not related to family conflict or peer conflict on the dai ly level (p � .078). Family and peer conflict co-occurred on the same days (B � 0.06, SE � 0.01, p � .000), consistent with prior research (Chung et al., 2011). Family Meals Are Associated With More Positive and Fewer Negative Emotions We first tested whether family meals, family conflict, and peer conflict uniquely predict positive and negative emotions. As Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for Study Constructs Study constructs Full sample Boys Girls Black Asian Hispanic Other White M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Family meal 0.61 (0.49) 0.62 (0.48)1 0.60 (0.49)1 0.35 (0.48)A 0.74 (0.44)B 0.60 (0.49)C 0.54 (0.50)C 0.59 (0.49)C Family conflict 0.14 (0.20) 0.13 (0.20)1 0.14 (0.21)1 0.13 (0.19)A,B 0.14 (0.19)A,B 0.10 (0.22)A 0.16 (0.22)B 0.14 (0.21)A,B
  • 32. Peer conflict 0.04 (0.10) 0.03 (0.08)1 0.04 (0.11)2 0.07 (0.13)A 0.03 (0.07)B 0.06 (0.17)A,C 0.05 (0.10)C,D 0.04 (0.09)D Happiness 3.25 (1.08) 3.23 (1.07)1 3.25 (1.09)1 3.07 (1.25)A,D 3.08 (0.99)A 3.35 (1.08)B,C 3.27 (1.19)C,D 3.40 (1.06)C Distress 1.65 (0.79) 1.54 (0.65)1 1.73 (0.87)2 1.74 (0.85)A 1.60 (0.75)B 1.45 (0.99)A,B 1.68 (0.92)A,B 1.67 (0.78)A,B Burnout 2.09 (1.05) 1.96 (0.95)1 2.19 (1.11)2 2.33 (1.12)A 1.90 (0.98)B 1.92 (0.99)B 2.16 (1.06)A 2.20 (1.07)A Role fulfillment 4.87 (1.36) 4.80 (1.35)1 4.92 (1.36)2 5.01 (1.56)A 4.56 (1.22)B 4.92 (1.48)A 5.11 (1.34)A 5.02 (1.35)A Note. For sex, means with the same number are not significantly different at the p � .05 level. For race, means with the same letter are not significantly different. T hi s do cu m en t is co py
  • 36. al us er an d is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2113FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS shown in Model 1 of Table 2, on days that adolescents ate a
  • 37. meal with the family, they reported significantly greater happiness and role fulfillment (ps �.001), and less distress and burnout (p � .005 and p � .010, respectively). Conversely, on days that adolescents experienced greater family conflict, they reported significantly less happiness and role fulfillment (p � .001 and p � .001), and more distress and burnout (p � .001 and p � .030). On days that adolescents experienced greater peer conflict, they reported sig- nificantly less happiness (p � .001) and more burnout and distress (ps �.001). Family Meals Buffer the Associations Between Family Conflict and Negative Emotions Our next model tested whether family meals moderates the associations between family and peer conflict and emotions. For family conflict, as shown in Model 2 of Table 2, family meals significantly interacted with family conflict to predict happiness (p � .003), burnout (p � .010), role fulfilment (p � .000), and distress (p � .030). As shown in Figures 1 and 2, family conflict was associated with lower levels of happiness and higher levels of burnout only on days when adolescents did not eat a family meal. On days when adolescents ate a family meal, family conflict was not associated with happiness or burnout. Similarly, as shown in Figures 3 and 4, family conflict was associated with lower role fulfillment and higher levels of distress, particularly on days
  • 38. that there was no family meal. Together, these findings suggest that family meals buffer the daily emotional toll of experiencing family conflict. In addition, family meals significantly interacted with peer conflict to predict role fulfilment (p � .045). As shown in Figure 5, peer conflict was associated with marginally lower levels of role fulfillment only on days when adolescents did not eat a family meal. On days when adolescents ate a family meal, peer conflict was not associated with role fulfillment. There were no other significant associations between family meals, peer conflict, and emotions (ps � .050�.832). Sensitivity Analyses Bonferroni correction. As follow up, we conducted Bonfer- roni correction for four analyses (four emotion outcomes). Results all retained significance at the p � .013 level except for the direct positive association between family conflict and burnout, the in- teraction between family conflict and family meals in predicti ng burnout, and the interaction between peer conflict and family meals in predicting role fulfillment. Controlling for frequency of family meals across days. We also tested whether family meals buffered the negative effects of conflict more or less for adolescents who routinely had more or fewer family meals by entering average family meals as a cross level interaction term at Level 2. We found no evidence for this
  • 39. (p � .150 –723). Controlling for prior day emotions. To provide further evi - dence of directionality, we conducted two additional models. First, we examined whether family meals buffered emotional risks associated with family and peer conflict the same day, controlling for emotions the previous day. To do this, we ran the same models as before, but included the same emotion on the previous day. Consistent with our T ab le 2 D ir ec t a n d In te ra ct iv e
  • 77. y. 2114 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER previous results, we found that family meals significantly buffered the effect of family conflict on happiness (B � 0.08, SE � 0.03, p � .007), burnout (B � �0.06, SE � 0.03, p � .021), and role fulfillment (B � 1.43, SE � 0.03, p � .001). Family meals did not buffer the effect of family conflict on distress (p � .174) or the effects of peer conflict on any emotion (p � .077–.730). With Bonferroni correction, these results retained significance at the p � .023 level except for the interaction between family conflict and family meals in predicting burnout above prior day levels. Spillover into next day emotions. As a second more conser- vative follow-up analysis, we examined whether family meals buffered negative emotions experienced the day after family and peer conflict (i.e., a spillover effect). We again ran the same models but with each next day emotion as the dependent variable, and controlled for that same emotion the same day (i.e., the day of the conflict). Consistent with our same-day results, we found that family meals significantly buffered the effect of family conflict on
  • 78. distress the next day (B � �0.06, SE � 0.02, p � .002). Specif- ically, family conflict was associated with marginally higher levels of distress only on days when adolescents did not eat a family meal (B � 0.14, SE � 0.08, p � .069), but significantly lower levels of distress on days when adolescents did eat a family meal (B � �0.17, SE � 0.07, p � .012). This result remained signif- icant after Bonferroni correction. Variation Across Adolescent Years To investigate whether the observed associations varied across adolescence, our final models added a three-way interaction be- tween family meals, age, and each conflict variable (i.e., family conflict or peer conflict) predicting each emotion. We first added this interaction term to our primary model (same day emotion as the dependent variable) then to our two sensitivity analyses (same day emotion as the dependent variable while controlling for pre - vious day emotion, and next day emotion as the dependent variable while controlling for same day emotion). The associations between family meal, family and peer conflict, and emotions the same day did not vary by adolescent age in either of the two models with same day emotions as the outcomes. However, family meals significantly interacted with family conflict and age to predict distress the next day (B � �0.06, SE � 0.02, p � .005). As shown in Figure 6, family conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there was no family meal, but only for older adolescents,
  • 79. and not for younger adolescents. Similarly, family meals significantly interacted with peer conflict and age to predict distress the next day (B � 0.06, SE � 0.02, p � .010). As shown in Figure 7, peer conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there was no family meal, but only for younger adolescents, and not for older adolescents. B = -0.50, p < .001 B = -0.08, p = .36 Figure 1. Family meal buffers the daily negative association between family conflict and happiness. B = 0.30, p = .002 B = -0.06, p =.54 Figure 2. Family meal buffers the daily positive association between family conflict and burnout. B = -1.52, p < .001 B = -0.93, p <.001 Figure 3. Family meal buffers the daily negative association between family conflict and less role fulfillment. B = 0.45, p =.002
  • 80. B = 0.22, p <.001 Figure 4. Family meal buffers the daily positive association between family conflict and increased distress. T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by th e A m
  • 84. be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2115FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS Discussion The goal of this study was to understand how family meals are related to adolescents’ emotions on the daily level, and whether family meals moderate how family and peer conflict relate to positive and challenging emotions. In a large and diverse sample of adolescents, we investigated these direct and interactive effects. Since family meals might represent an opportunity for adolescents to express themselves and receive emotional support after experi- encing challenges during the day, we hypothesized that adoles- cents would end the day relatively happier and more fulfilled,
  • 85. and less upset after having a family meal. Consistent with this hypoth- esis, we found that on days that adolescents shared a meal with the family, they reported more positive emotions and fewer negative emotions. Moreover, family meals buffered the negative effects of family conflict on increased negative emotions and decreased positive emotions. These findings suggest that family meals may serve as a daily protective factor against the negative effects of daily familial disputes. Sensitivity analyses controlling for previ- ous day emotions and examining links with emotions the next day strengthened this interpretation of the direction of this pathway. Our first key finding was that on days that adolescents shared a meal with the family, they reported greater happiness and role fulfillment, and less burnout and distress, compared with days that they did not eat with the family. These results are consistent with cross-sectional reports that the majority of adolescents retrospec- tively view family meals as one of their favorite family activities (Zollo, 1995), and evidence that more frequent family meals on average are associated with more optimal physical and mental health (Goldfarb et al., 2014; Jones, 2018). Our study extends this work by demonstrating that the positive associations with family
  • 86. meals are observable on the daily level as well. Moreover, this pattern appears consistent across adolescents, whether they rou- tinely have more or fewer family meals, underscoring that it is not only the overall amount of family meals that is protective (as assessed in prior research), but the temporal co-occurrence within individual adolescents. Daily family meals provide an opportunity to share experiences from the day, enjoy the company of loved- ones, and experience a stable family routine that reminds adoles- cents of their belonging and trust in the family (Fiese et al., 2006). In particular, family meals may provide more of a context for youth to share their experiences and receive support, compared with family time in other settings which may be more structured around an activity that facilitates less communication (e.g., watch- ing a movie). A reduction in distress and burnout and an increase in happiness and role fulfillment as a function of eating with the family are meaningful outcomes in and of themselves, as they are key indices of psychological well-being (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). Further- more, these psychological experiences are important because they contribute to a variety of other positive outcomes in youth, ranging from school achievement and school behaviors (Quinn & Duck- B = -0.04, p = .060 B = 0.04, p =.108
  • 87. Figure 5. Family meal buffers the daily negative association between peer conflict and role fulfillment. B = -0.07, p =.029 B = -0.05, p =.126 B = -0.03, p =.245 B = -0.04, p =.154 Figure 6. Family meal buffers the association between family conflict and increased distress the next day for adolescents who were older (ages 14.5–18 years), but not younger (ages 12–14.5 years). T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh
  • 91. er an d is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2116 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER worth, 2007) to physical health, both in the short and long term (Mahon, Yarcheski, & Yarcheski, 2005; Trzesniewski et al., 2006). Thus, the daily links between family meals and increased well-being may also have effects downstream on overall
  • 92. psycho- logical and physical well-being. Our interactive models revealed that family meals buffered adolescents from the daily emotional risks associated with family conflict. In particular, family conflict was linked with greater distress and burnout, and less happiness and role fulfillment only on days that adolescents did not eat a meal with their family. In contrast, family conflict was not associated with negative emotions on days that adolescents did eat a meal with their family. These results were particularly robust for daily happiness, role fulfillment and distress— compared with burnout, which was no longer sig- nificant after correcting for multiple tests. A shared meal may provide an opportunity to make amends, internalize negative interactions less, engage positively and re - cover, thereby offsetting the negative impacts of conflict during the day. After experiencing family conflict, adolescents may feel more positive at the end of the day if they also shared family meal, because the meal reminded them consciously or unconsciously that “everything is still okay.” Thus, family meal routines may facili- tate feelings of family cohesion, belonging, and emotional security (Fiese et al., 2006) even the same day, which could in turn help to mitigate emotional distress caused by interpersonal conflicts ear- lier in the day. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis
  • 93. that the routine of consistent family meals provides a sense of familial organization and clear expectations, which are integral for adoles- cents’ psychological resilience (Larson et al., 2006). This interpre- tation was also supported by sensitivity analysis which revealed that family meals buffered the negative effects of conflict on emotionality over and above emotions the previous day. Our investigation of age-related variation revealed that family conflict was associated with higher levels of distress the day after there was no family meal only for older adolescents, but not for younger adolescents. Older adolescents may benefit more from family meals in part because they had fewer family meals on average in our sample. Older adolescents also tend to have rela- tively greater independence and are more often outside the home or away from family, interacting with peers (Holmbeck, 2018), so family meals may be the only time when they come together with their family to engage and receive emotional support (Larson et al., 2006). In part because older adolescents have fewer family meals during the week, they may be more inclined to share their expe- riences and receive emotional support when they do come together to eat with the family. In contrast, younger adolescents may spend more time around the home engaging with parents and siblings during the afternoon after school or on weekends, and receive
  • 94. emotional support during those times. These results are particu- larly important since late adolescence marks the transition to early adulthood, a time of increasing autonomy and emotional risk that paves the way for lifelong adjustment. Results revealed one significant interaction with peer conflict, suggesting that peer conflict reduced positive feelings of role fulfillment only when adolescents did not eat a meal with the family. However, this result was no longer significant after cor - rection for multiple testing and should be interpreted with caution. More commonly—and in contrast to family conflict—we found that peer conflict was consistently associated with less happiness, more burnout and more distress the same day, even when adoles- cents ate with the family. It is possible that adolescents may feel unwilling or unable to open up and confide in their family (Ca- mara, Bacigalupe, & Padilla, 2017), even though the meal may provide an opportunity to do so (Fiese et al., 2006). Thus, adoles- cents may not receive the emotional support and guidance that the family members could offer during the meal (Fiese et al., 2006). Another possible explanation is that even if the adolescent con- fides in family members, family members may not know how best B = -0.07, p = .022 B = -0.05, p =.126 B = -0.02, p =.608
  • 95. B = -0.02, p =.656 Figure 7. Family meal buffers the association between peer conflict and increased distress the next day for adolescents who were younger (ages 12–14.5 years) but not older (ages 14.5–18 years). We split the two groups by mean age in our sample. T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by th e
  • 99. t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2117FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS to provide support for relationships that occur outside of the home. Parents, guardians and siblings may not say the “right” thing to help the adolescent resolve their negative emotions from peer conflict and experience relief (Camara et al., 2017). It is also possible that despite the adolescent confiding and receiving emo- tional support during family meals, the support may not alleviate the stresses of peer conflict because it does not involve positively engaging with the same person or people with whom the conflict
  • 100. occurred. Families’ attempts to offset conflict by providing warmth and structure may be uniquely valuable for counteracting previous upsetting interactions that occurred within the family, as opposed to those that occurred outside the home (Labella & Masten, 2018). Follow up sensitivity analysis revealed an additional buffering effect for peer conflict and emotional risk the next day. Peer conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there was no family meal, but only for younger adolescents, and not for older adolescents. This contrasts our prior findings in which family conflict spilled over into more distress the day after there were no family meal only for older adolescents, and not for younger ado- lescents. One possible explanation is that younger adolescents may be more likely to process their experiences of peer conflict with their families at mealtime, compared with older adolescents. In contrast, older adolescents may discuss experiences of peer con- flict with friends more and receive more support from friends, because older adolescents on average spend more time with peers away from home (Brannen, 2002). In this way, family meals may buffer the association between peer conflict and heightened dis - tress the next day for younger adolescents only because they are more likely to open up to their family and receive support for peer conflict when it occurs. These results support a growing body of literature documenting how family mealtime communication is beneficial to adolescents’ emotional well-being (Offer, 2013a).
  • 101. Limitations and Future Directions Several limitations should be acknowledged. First, because of the rich nature of daily diary data, there was some missing data, which may or may not have been random. Days that adolescents did not respond to the diaries might have been most difficult, with the highest levels of family and peer conflict, and these could be missing from analysis. In addition, our measure of family meals was dichotomous, indexing whether or not the adolescent ate any meal with the family that day. We are unable to shed light on the chronological order of events. Specifically, we are unable to de- termine whether family meals and interpersonal conflict occurred before, during or after adolescents’ emotions. Similarly, although we did not distinguish between breakfast, lunch and dinner, the specific meal might also matter. For example, family dinners may more effectively buffer the negative effect of conflicts during the day, compared with breakfast which likely occurs before potential conflicts. However, the observed covariation of these experiences within days highlight a dynamic interplay within days and across days, regardless of the specific order in which they occurred. Our measure of family meals was also unable to shed light on the quality of the interaction during the meals, for example which topics were discussed and if emotional support was provided.
  • 102. As such, it is unclear from the current study the specific interpersonal processes that occurred during family meals (e.g., personal disclo- sure, emotional support) that may mediate or impact the observed daily associations between family meals, interpersonal conflict, and emotions. Future research should measure the specific con- versation topics and behaviors during family meals, to specify which aspects of the quality of the interaction during family meals promote positive emotionality, even in the context of family dis- putes. Measuring conversations and behaviors during family meals may also reveal more individual differences (e.g., Meier & Mu- sick, 2014). For example, it is possible that family meals are only protective for adolescents who experience relatively more positive familial relationships, but are not protective— or perhaps even exacerbate the effects of conflict—among adolescents who expe- rience family interaction as more stressful (Meier & Musick, 2014). Future research should also compare the effects of family meals to other family quality time, to clarify whether the process of eating together is uniquely valuable or if similar benefits may be derived from other consistent, daily family activities. For example, an alternative explanation to our findings may be that quality time
  • 103. with the family—rather than in particular the act of eating togeth- er— has a protective effect for adolescents. Conversely, it is also feasible that simply “slowing down,” and taking time to attend to basic human needs (i.e., eating) is protective—regardless of whether or not family members are present. In addition, although the present study utilizes daily diary meth- ods that account for individual differences and, thus, offers robust predictive power (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b), causality cannot be determined in this study. Future experimental work could address this issue by randomly assigning adolescents to eat with their families (or not) and examine the psychological outcomes of such a manipulation. For example, randomized control trials have shown that family meals can improve psychological symptoms in clinical samples of adolescents (Herscovici, Kovalskys, & Orel - lana, 2017), and increase healthy eating habits in small samples of preadolescents (Flattum et al., 2015). Consistent with these find- ings, our results suggest that family meals could be valuable components of interventions designed to increase positive emo- tions, improve familial relationships, and reduce adolescent risk in families who experience high levels of conflict. Moreover, such interventions may be more effective for older compared with younger adolescents. Conclusion
  • 104. The current study extends long-standing interest in understand- ing how family meals impact youths’ wellbeing and development. Cross-sectional and longitudinal research have shown that more frequent family meals on average during childhood are associated with better physical and mental health. This study fills an impor- tant gap in the literature: it addresses how adolescents experience family meals on a day to day basis, and the extent to which family meals serve as a daily protective factor. Consistent with prior evidence that more frequent family meals on average are a positive and stable routine that is integral for positive adolescent develop- ment (Goldfarb et al., 2014; Jones, 2018), our findings highlight important daily associations between family meals and positive emotionality— even in the context of conflict—that in turn may partially explain how family meals are related to long-term well- being downstream. T hi s do cu m en t
  • 109. References Aiken, L., West, S., & Reno, R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Atlanta, GA: Sage. Armstrong-Carter, E., Ivory, S., Lin, L. C., Muscatell, K. A., & Telzer, E. H. (2020). Role Fulfillment Mediates the Association Between Daily Family Assistance and Cortisol Awakening Response in Adolescents. Child Development, 91, 754 –768. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13213 Bloom, B. L. (1985). A factor analysis of self-report measures of family functioning. Family Process, 24, 225–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j .1545-5300.1985.00225.x Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579 – 616. http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030 Brannen, J. (2002). Discourses of Adolescence: Young People’s Indepen- dence and Autonomy within Families. Children in families: Research and policy. Retrieved from https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/
  • 110. 9780203453803/chapters/10.4324/9780203453803-12 Broderick, C. B. (1993). Understanding family process: Basics of family systems theory. Atlanta, GA: Sage. Camara, M., Bacigalupe, G., & Padilla, P. (2017). The role of social support in adolescents: Are you helping me or stressing me out? Inter- national Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 22, 123–136. http://dx.doi .org/10.1080/02673843.2013.875480 Chung, G. H., Flook, L., & Fuligni, A. J. (2011). Reciprocal associations between family and peer conflict in adolescents’ daily lives. Child Development, 82, 1390 –1396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 8624 .2011.01625.x Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., & Bolger, N. (2006). A procedure for evaluating sensitivity to within-person change: Can mood measures in diary studies detect change reliably? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 917–929. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0146167206287721 Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual
  • 111. Review of Psychology, 62, 583– 619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev .psych.093008.100356 Eisenberg, M. E., Olson, R. E., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & Bearinger, L. H. (2004). Correlations between family meals and psy- chosocial well-being among adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics & Ad- olescent Medicine, 158, 792–796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi .158.8.792 Fiese, B. H., Foley, K. P., & Spagnola, M. (2006). Routine and ritual elements in family mealtimes: Contexts for child well-being and family identity. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2006, 67– 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.156 Fiese, B. H., Hammons, A., & Grigsby-Toussaint, D. (2012). Family mealtimes: A contextual approach to understanding childhood obesity. Economics and Human Biology, 10, 365–374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.ehb.2012.04.004 Fiese, B. H., Tomcho, T. J., Douglas, M., Josephs, K., Poltrock, S., & Baker, T. (2002). A review of 50 years of research on naturally occurring family routines and rituals: Cause for celebration? Journal of Family
  • 112. Psychology, 16, 381–390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893- 3200.16.4.381 Flattum, C., Draxten, M., Horning, M., Fulkerson, J. A., Neumark- Sztainer, D., Garwick, A., . . . Story, M. (2015). HOME Plus: Program design and implementation of a family-focused, community- based in- tervention to promote the frequency and healthfulness of family meals, reduce children’s sedentary behavior, and prevent obesity. The Interna- tional Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 12, 53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0211-7 Fuligni, A. J. (2019). The need to contribute during adolescence. Perspec- tives on Psychological Science, 14, 331–343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 1745691618805437 Fulkerson, J. A., Story, M., Mellin, A., Leffert, N., Neumark- Sztainer, D., & French, S. A. (2006). Family dinner meal frequency and adolescent development: Relationships with developmental assets and high- risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 337–345. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.026 Goldfarb, S., Tarver, W. L., & Sen, B. (2014). Family structure and risk behaviors: The role of the family meal in assessing likelihood
  • 113. of ado- lescent risk behaviors. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 7, 53– 66. Herscovici, C. R., Kovalskys, I., & Orellana, L. (2017). An exploratory evaluation of the family meal intervention for adolescent anorexia ner- vosa. Family Process, 56, 364 –375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp .12199 Ho, H. C. Y., Mui, M., Wan, A., Yew, C., Lam, T. H., Chan, S. S., & Stewart, S. M. (2018). Family meal practices and well-being in Hong Kong: The mediating effect of family communication. Journal of Family Issues, 39, 3835–3856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18800787 Holmbeck, G. N. (2018, October 24). A model of family relational trans- formations during the transition to adolescence: Parent– adolescent con- flict and adaptation. In J. A. Graber, J. Brooks-Gunn, & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains and context (pp. 167–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. http://dx.doi .org/10.4324/9781315789286-7 Jones, B. L. (2018). Making time for family meals: Parental influences,
  • 114. home eating environments, barriers and protective factors. Physiology & Behavior, 193, 248 –251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03 .035 Labella, M. H., & Masten, A. S. (2018). Family influences on the devel- opment of aggression and violence. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 11–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.028 Larson, R. W., Branscomb, K. R., & Wiley, A. R. (2006). Forms and functions of family mealtimes: Multidisciplinary perspectives. New Di- rections for Child and Adolescent Development, 2006, 1–15. http://dx .doi.org/10.1002/cd.152 Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (1994). Interpersonal conflict during ado- lescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 197–209. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/0033-2909.115.2.197 Mahon, N. E., Yarcheski, A., & Yarcheski, T. J. (2005). Happiness as related to gender and health in early adolescents. Clinical Nursing Research, 14, 175–190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1054773804271936 Marsee, M. A., Barry, C. T., Childs, K. K., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., Muñoz, L. C., . . . Lau, K. S. L. (2011). Assessing the forms and
  • 115. functions of aggression using self-report: Factor structure and invariance of the Peer Conflict Scale in youths. Psychological Assessment, 23, 792– 804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023369 McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the profile of mood states (POMS). San Diego, CA: EdITS. Meier, A., & Musick, K. (2014). Variation in associations between family dinners and adolescent well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 76, 13–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12079 Offer, S. (2013a). Assessing the relationship between family mealtime communication and adolescent emotional well-being using the experi- ence sampling method. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 577–585. http://dx .doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.007 Offer, S. (2013b). Family time activities and adolescents’ emotional well- being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75, 26 – 41. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01025.x Prior, A.-L., & Limbert, C. (2013). Adolescents’ perceptions and experi- ences of family meals. Journal of Child Health Care, 17, 354 – 365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367493512 462261
  • 116. Quinn, P. D., & Duckworth, A. L. (2007). Happiness and academic achievement: Evidence for reciprocal causality. The Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Society, 24, 2007. T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by th e A m
  • 120. be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2119FAMILY MEALS, CONFLICT, AND EMOTIONS http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13213 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1985.00225.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1985.00225.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030 https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9780203453803/chapters /10.4324/9780203453803-12 https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9780203453803/chapters /10.4324/9780203453803-12 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.875480 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.875480 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01625.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01625.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206287721 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206287721 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356
  • 121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.158.8.792 http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.158.8.792 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.156 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2012.04.004 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2012.04.004 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.16.4.381 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0211-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805437 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805437 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.026 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.026 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp.12199 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp.12199 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18800787 http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315789286-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315789286-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.035 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.035 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.028 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.152 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.152 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.197 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.197 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1054773804271936 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023369 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12079 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.007 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.007 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01025.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01025.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367493512462261 Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (Vol. 1). Thousand
  • 122. Oaks, CA: Sage. Romeo, R. D. (2013). The teenage brain: The stress response and the adolescent brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 140 – 145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721413475445 Sen, B. (2010). The relationship between frequency of family dinner and adolescent problem behaviors after adjusting for other family character- istics. Journal of Adolescence, 33, 187–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j .adolescence.2009.03.011 Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009a). Daily family assistance and the psychological well-being of adolescents from Latin American, Asian, and European backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1177–1189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014728 Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009b). A longitudinal daily diary study of family assistance and academic achievement among adolescents from Mexican, Chinese, and European backgrounds. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 560 –571. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964- 008- 9391-7
  • 123. Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Robins, R. W., Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 42, 381–390. http://dx .doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381 Zollo, P. (1995). Wise up to teens: Insights into marketing and advertising to teenagers. East Patchogue, NY: New Strategist Publications, Inc. Received November 10, 2019 Revision received July 7, 2020 Accepted July 28, 2020 � T hi s do cu m en t is co py
  • 127. al us er an d is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2120 ARMSTRONG-CARTER AND TELZER http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721413475445 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.011 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.011
  • 128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014728 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9391-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9391-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381Family Meals Buffer the Daily Emotional Risk Associated With Family ConflictFamily Meals as a Protective Factor Against Interpersonal ConflictAdolescence as a Sensitive Developmental PeriodCurrent StudyMethodParticipantsProcedureMeasuresFamily mealsDaily family conflictDaily peer conflictDaily emotionsData AnalysisResultsSample CharacteristicsFamily Meals Are Associated With More Positive and Fewer Negative EmotionsFamily Meals Buffer the Associations Between Family Conflict and Negative EmotionsSensitivity AnalysesBonferroni correctionControlling for frequency of family meals across daysControlling for prior day emotionsSpillover into next day emotionsVariation Across Adolescent YearsDiscussionLimitations and Future DirectionsConclusionReferences Social Behavior and Personality , Volume 48, Issue 11, e9120 https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.9120 www.sbp-journal.com Influence of relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers on employees’ voice Siyuan Chen1, Mingyu Zhang1, Yihua Zhang2, Wen Wu1, Zhimin Xiao3, Shaoxue Wu4, Pan Liu1, Yuhuan Xia1 1School of Economics and Management, Beijing Jiaotong
  • 129. University, People’s Republic of China 2Graduate School of Education and Psychology, Pepperdine University, United States 3School of International Trade and Economics, University of International Business and Economics, People’s Republic of China 4Beijing Shiquan Consulting Company, Beijing, People’s Republic of China How to cite: Chen, S., Zhang, M., Zhang, Y., Wu, W., Xiao, Z., Wu, S., Liu, P., & Xia, Y. (2020). Influence of relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers on employees’ voice. Social Behavior and Personality: An international journal, 48(11), e9120 Building on self-determination theory and relational attribution theory, in this study we examined how relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers simultaneously affect employee voice behaviors. We expanded relational attribution theory by developing two new constructs we labeled leader-relational attribution orientation and coworker-relational attribution orientation to describe employees’ different responses to relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers via psychological needs satisfaction. We surveyed 328 employee–leader dyads who were employed at a semiconductor manufacturer to test our hypotheses. We found that leader-relational attribution orientation can strengthen the influences of relationship conflicts with leaders on psychological needs satisfaction and its indirect effects on
  • 130. employee voice behaviors. Coworker-relational attribution orientation can strengthen the influences of relationship conflicts with coworkers on psychological needs satisfaction and its indirect effects on employee voice behaviors. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. Keywords supportive voice; constructive voice; destructive voice; attribution orientation; psychological needs satisfaction; relationship conflicts Relationship conflict refers to the perception of incompatibilities and disagreements among organizational members regarding issues that are not task-related, for instance, political beliefs, norms and values, or gossip (De Dreu & Van Vianen, 2001). In some studies, findings have shown that relationship conflict can disrupt groups’ effective functioning and employees’ psychological well-being (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; Jehn, 1995). Researchers have explored some factors leading to relationship conflict and how to prevent it from happening (Edmondson & Smith, 2006). However, no studies have included relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers in one empirical investigation. Consequently, few empirical studies have been conducted to test whether one type of relationship conflict remains a significant factor when controlling for the other. Organizational researchers have not developed an integrated conceptual framework to explore the
  • 131. influences of relationship conflicts with both leaders and coworkers on focal employee voice behaviors. Voice behavior, the expression of constructive opinions, concerns, or ideas about work-related issues (Van Dyne et al., 2003) plays a significant role in the running and effectiveness of an organization (Organ et al., 2006). Some researchers have explored the antecedents of employee voice behavior at the individual, supervisory, or organizational levels (Fuller et al., 2007; Liang et al., 2012; W. Liu et al., 2010; Van Dyne et al., 2008). In the current study, we explore the influence of relationship conflict on employees’ voice CORRESPONDENCE Wen Wu, School of Economics and Management, Beijing Jiaotong University, No. 3 Shangyuancun, Beijing 100044, People’s Republic of China. Email: [email protected] or Zhimin Xiao, School of International Trade and Economics, University of International Business and Economics, No. 10, Huxin Dongjie, Beijing, 100029, People’s Republic of China. Email: [email protected] © 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia behavior. We develop a framework based on self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) and relational attribution theory (Eberly et al., 2011) to examine how relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers affect
  • 132. employee voice behaviors, including supportive voice, destructive voice, and constructive voice. We argue that relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers can hinder the satisfaction of psychological needs, which, in turn, influences employee voice behaviors. For example, relationship conflict can induce a coworker to refuse to provide information. We further propose that such relationships depend on the focal employee’s relational attributions of relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers. Eberly et al. (2011) proposed a new set of relational attributions, defined as the focal employee’s acknowledgment of an event in a relationship. They suggested that individuals might attribute the causes of events to their relationships with others. Relationship attribution orientation is individuals’ explanations that locate their success in their organization within their relationship with others. We argue that individuals may differ in this tendency, attributing success in their organization to their relationships with leaders or with coworkers, and these attributions may influence their experiences and expectations (Eberly et al., 2011). Specifically, leader- relational attribution orientation describes explanations given by a focal individual that locate the success of that individual in the organization within their relationship with their immediate leader, and coworker- relational attribution orientation describes explanations given by a focal individual that locate the success of that individual in the organization within their relationship with their coworkers. Theory and Hypotheses Deci and Ryan (2000) posited that three innate basic psychological needs affect individuals’ attitudes, motivation, and behaviors, namely, autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Need for autonomy refers to
  • 133. the desire to be “the perceived origin or source of one’s own behavior” (Deci & Ryan, 2000, p. 8) or to endorse one’s actions as authentic and consistent with how one would act regardless of external influences. Need for competence refers to the desire to feel effective in one’s activities and to interact with an environment that supports the expression of one’s abilities. Need for relatedness refers to the desire to experience a sense of belonging or connectedness, or to be valued by other individuals or collectives (Rosen et al., 2014). Everyone is assumed to have these innate needs (Baard et al., 2004). Psychological needs satisfaction is the basis of linking social context (e.g., relationship building and information seeking) to individual growth and well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) is one of the most influential perspectives for explaining employees’ behaviors. It explains how psychological needs satisfaction strengthens and promotes the internalization of external motivation, and ultimately affects people’s attitudes and behaviors (Deci et al., 1989). Factors that can fulfill individual psychological needs promote intrinsic motivation and personal growth, whereas factors that impede an individual’s psychological needs thwart intrinsic motivation (Greguras & Diefendorff, 2009). We contend that the motivational routes to individual behavior could be explained through social determination theory. We focus on the mediating roles of psychological needs satisfaction between relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers, and the employee voice behaviors of supportive voice, destructive voice, and constructive voice. Figure 1 illustrates the conceptual model of this study.
  • 134. 2© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal Figure 1. Conceptual Model Note. T1, T2, and T3 refer to the three data collection waves. Sub = subordinate, Sup = supervisor, indicating from whom data were collected. Relationship Conflicts and Psychological Needs Satisfaction On the basis of self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), we established that relationship conflicts with both leaders and coworkers could hinder the focal employees’ three psychological needs. In relationship conflicts with leaders, employees will not be encouraged to take on challenging and meaningful tasks through close communication (Erdogan & Liden, 2002). Deci and Ryan (2000) suggested that the human need for autonomy could be satisfied by making choices and fulfilling meaningful requests. Thus, high levels of relationship conflict with leaders impede the focal employees’ autonomy need. Moreover, in relationship conflicts of members with their leaders, leaders tend not to offer the members rewards, such as resource allocation and personal development (Bolino & Turnley, 2009), which are instrumental for fulfilling employees’ intrinsic and extrinsic needs. The poor career development and negative evaluation arising from employees’ relationship conflicts with leaders may signal to the employees that they are not valued or respected. This
  • 135. perception impedes their sense of efficacy and self-enhancement. In the case of leader relationship conflicts with employees, the leader might not encourage employees to question the existing ways of solving problems (Hetland et al., 2011). Finally, the poor mutual reciprocation that embodies relationship conflicts with leaders results in weak employee affective attachment to leaders (Ferris et al., 2009), and lack of in-group membership and group interconnectivity (Nifadkar et al., 2012). These outcomes can hinder the satisfying of the need for relatedness. Relationship conflicts with coworkers signify that coworkers do not have strong bonds with one another (Kim et al., 2017). They treat each other with a lack of respect and autonomy (Sherony & Green, 2002; Van den Broeck et al., 2008). Thus, we concluded that if focal employees had relationship conflicts with their coworkers, this could lower the focal employees’ psychological needs satisfaction. Psychological Needs Satisfaction and Employee Voice Behaviors 3© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia When employees satisfy their needs for autonomy, relatedness, and competence, they tend to have a favorable job attitude and perform well (Gagné & Deci, 2005). In self-determination theory it is proposed that satisfying these needs can improve employees’ intrinsic motivation, which gives them a sense of
  • 136. satisfaction from performing their work tasks (Gagné & Deci, 2005). Specifically, autonomy means that individuals can make choices freely and based on a full understanding of the relevant information (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Hill et al. (2014) found that employees express their values and interests through their work when they can make decisions. Employee voice is an important proactive behavior that can help leaders make the right decisions and that benefits the organization. Supportive voice is employees’ voluntary expression of support for worthwhile work-related policies, programs, objectives, procedures, or speaking out in defense of these behaviors when they are being unfairly criticized (Maynes & Podsakoff, 2013). According to the self- determination perspective, employees whose psychological needs for competence are satisfied are l ikely to be intrinsically motivated. Such intrinsic motivation may promote extrarole behaviors, such as supportive voice (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Hence, psychological needs satisfaction is likely to predict supportive voice. Constructive voice refers to the voluntary expression of ideas, information, or opinions focused on effecting organizationally functional change to the work context. (Maynes & Podsakoff, 2013). Satisfying the psychological needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness denotes that employees can strive to meet challenges, and to achieve success and satisfactory results (Baard et al., 2004; Deci & Ryan, 2000). As such, employees whose psychological needs are satisfied are likely to suggest improvements to enhance organizational performance. Destructive voice is the voluntary expression of hurtful, critical,
  • 137. or debasing opinions regarding work policies, practices, and procedures (Maynes & Podsakoff, 2013). According to self-determination theory, if employees’ basic psychological needs are not satisfied, they may lack intrinsic motivation to control their negative attitudes and behaviors that may harm the organization (Gagné & Deci, 2005). Thus, psychological needs satisfaction may be negatively related to destructive voice. Satisfying the three basic psychological needs for autonomy, relatedness, and competence can promote employees’ intrinsic motivation and facilitate positive work attitudes and voice behaviors (Gagné & Deci, 2005). Accordingly, based on self-determination theory, we proposed that relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers would hinder individual employees’ satisfying their psychological needs and, in turn, would increase their destructive voice, and would reduce their constructive voice and supportive voice. Specifically, we predicted the following: Hypothesis 1a: Relationship conflicts with leaders have a significant negative effect on employee constructive voice through psychological needs satisfaction. Hypothesis 1b: Relationship conflicts with leaders have a positive influence on destructive voice through psychological needs satisfaction. Hypothesis 1c: Relationship conflicts with leaders will have a significant negative effect on employee supportive voice through psychological needs satisfaction. Hypothesis 2a: Relationship conflicts with coworkers will exert a significant negative influence on employee constructive voice through psychological needs satisfaction. Hypothesis 2b: Relationship conflicts with coworkers will exert a positive influence on employee
  • 138. destructive voice through psychological needs satisfaction. Hypothesis 2c: Relationship conflicts with coworkers will exert a significant negative influence on employee supportive voice through psychological needs satisfaction. Relational Attribution Theory Both relational attribution theory (Eberly et al., 2011) and self- determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), emphasize individual differences in work outcomes, depending on social context. Individual attributions 4© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal about the causes of events affect one’s feelings and expectations (Tomlinson & Mayer, 2009). We further maintained that employees’ attribution orientation of workplace life may moderate the influence of relationship conflicts on their psychological needs satisfaction. In Hypothesis 1 we predicted that relationship conflicts with leaders would hinder employees’ psychological needs for competency, relatedness, and autonomy. When employees interpret conflicts with leaders as hindering workplace success, they may believe that bad relationships will provide poor benefits, resources, and support. In contrast, when employees believe that relationship conflicts with leaders are not important in the workplace, they may also believe that relationship conflicts
  • 139. with leaders cannot bring about a critical lack of resources (e.g., promotion, competence, relatedness) to hinder their psychological needs. Satisfaction of psychological needs is then less contingent on the presence or absence of relationship conflict with leaders. Therefore, we contended that the relationship between relationship conflicts with leaders and psychological needs satisfaction can be accentuated by employees’ leader - relational attributions: Hypothesis3a: The relationship between relationship conflicts with leaders and employees’ psychological needs satisfaction will be strengthened by the employees’ leader-relational attribution orientations: The relationship will be stronger when employees’ leader-relational attribution orientation is higher rather than lower. Likewise, when employees attribute work-related success to their relationships with coworkers, they may believe that relationship conflicts with coworkers will hinder them in satisfying competence, relatedness, and autonomy needs. In other words, with other conditions being equal, the influence of relationship conflicts with coworkers on employees’ psychological needs can be strengthened when the employees have high coworker-relational attribution orientation: Hypothesis 3b: The relationship between relationship conflicts with coworkers and employees’ psychological needs satisfaction will be strengthened by the employees’ coworker-relational attribution orientations: The relationship will be stronger when employees’ coworker-relational attribution orientations are higher rather than lower. An Integrative Moderated Mediation Model
  • 140. As previously discussed, relationship conflicts with leaders and with coworkers both influence employee voice behaviors through the mediating role of psychological needs satisfaction. When employees have strong leader-relational attribution orientation (LRAO), they may believe that their career will be improved via relationship conflicts with their leader. In the previous hypotheses we suggested that by augmenting relationship conflicts with leaders and psychological needs satisfaction, employees’ LRAO affect the degree to which relationship conflicts with the leader influence employee voice behaviors. Similarly, employees’ coworker-relational attribution orientations (CRAO) may moderate the influence of relationship conflicts with coworkers on employee voice behaviors and thus may change the indirect effect of relationship conflicts with coworkers on employee voice behaviors via psychological needs satisfaction. We therefore made two moderated mediation predictions: Hypothesis 3c: Employees’ leader-relational attribution orientations will moderate the indirect effect of relationship conflicts with leaders and employee voice behaviors via psychological needs satisfaction: The indirect effect will be stronger when employees’ leader- relational attribution orientations are higher rather than lower. Hypothesis 3d: Employees’ coworker-relational attribution orientations will moderate the indirect effect of relationship conflicts with coworkers and employee voice behaviors via psychological needs satisfaction: The indirect effect will be stronger when employees’ coworker - relational attribution orientations are higher rather than lower. Pilot Study In differentiating and validating items of LRAO and CRAO, we
  • 141. used theoretical evidence as our deductive 5© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia item-generation approach (Hinkin, 1998). Based on the definition of relational attribution, we generated items using two sources. First, we directly generated items in line with this definition, using concise sentences to describe employee behaviors that fell into our two categories of relational attributions. Second, we referred to measures used in existing studies on relational attribution for our item pool (Tomlinson & Mayer, 2009). Thus, we identified 16 items for LRAO and CRAO. After removing redundant items, three subject-matter experts matched the items to the definitions of LRAO and CRAO. From carrying out this procedure, we identified five representative items for LRAO and five for CRAO, which we used for pilot testing. To test our items of LRAO and CRAO, we distributed a survey form including potential items of relational attribution orientation, to 322 employees of a large bank in China via WeChat, a popular social media platform in China. Participation was voluntary, and 236 respondents completed the survey. In the final sample, 53.8% of respondents were men and 46.2% were women (Mage = 38.39 years, SD = 8.72, ranging from 26 to 53 years; 84.75% had a bachelor’s degree or above, and 15.25% had a college education). Items are set out in Table 1 and were rated by employees on a 5-point
  • 142. Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) and 5 (completely agree). Then, we conducted a factor analysis with the Oblimin method. We performed principal component analysis. The rotation method was Oblimin with Kaiser normalization, and the rotation converged in four iterations. The results showed a distinction between LRAO and CRAO. No item had heavy cross-loadings, so that the LRAO factor consisted of five items, and all five emphasized relationship quality with leaders. The CRAO factor also consisted of five items, and all five emphasized relationship quality with coworkers. Table 1 shows the pattern matrix using the oblimin method. Table 1. Pattern Matrix of the Factor Analysis by the Oblimin Method Main Study Sample and Procedure We recruited participants for the main study from one private semiconductor manufacturer whose research and development center is in China, and whose products are sold locally and abroad. We used a three-wave, two-source survey method. The general managers notified potential respondents that the academic survey was voluntary. We randomly selected one employee from each work team. We received emails from 745 6© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal
  • 143. employees, giving consent to participate together with their employee identification number. Among the group of employees 58.53% were men, and 41.47% were women. The average age of the employees was 35.46 years (ranging from 24 to 47, SD = 4.76). In terms of position held, 93% were workers at grass-roots level, 7% were in low management positions. As regards education level, 68.79% had a bachelor degree or higher qualification, the rest had a college education. Among the group, 63.57% had worked for the company for more than 7 years, and the remaining 36.43% had less than 7 years’ tenure. At Time 1, we asked the employees to report on relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers, and control variables. We distributed pencils and survey forms, along with a cover letter assuring confidentiality, to the participants. After finishing the survey the respondents gave them directly to the research team. At Time 2, which was four weeks later, we asked employees to rate their psychological needs satisfaction and their LRAO and CRAO. At Time 3, which was four weeks after Time 2, we distributed a survey to the participants’ supervisors of participants at Times 1 and 2, using a list provided by the company’s human resources department, asking the supervisors to rate the participants’ employee voice behaviors. At Time 1, we gave survey forms to 745 employees, and 601 were completed (80.67%). At Time 2, we gave survey forms to 601 employees, and 482 were completed (80.20%). At Time 3, we distributed 481 supervisor survey forms, and 371 were completed (77.13%). In the end, we had 328 matched supervisor- subordinate dyads for analysis.
  • 144. Measures Relationship Conflict We asked employees to evaluate their relationship conflicts with their immediate supervisor and coworkers using four items adapted from the scale developed by Jehn (1995). The sample items include “How much friction is there between you and your immediate supervisor?” Employees rated the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all), to 5 (very frequently). Psychological Needs Satisfaction We used the 21-item scale developed by Kasser et al. (1992) to measure psychological needs satisfaction. Employees rated the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). Sample items include “Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from working,” and “I feel like I can pretty much be myself at work.” In accordance with previous research (Rosen et al., 2014) and from the theoretical perspective, we viewed need satisfaction as an overall index rather than as separate need satisfaction categories. We did not expect relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers to differ in terms of their relationship to autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Similarly, we did not expect every need to relate differentially to employee outcomes. Constructive Voice Constructive voice was measured using Maynes and Podsakoff’s (2013) five-item instrument. Leaders rated the items for each type of voice on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). Sample items for constructive voice are “This employee frequently makes suggestions
  • 145. about how to do things in new or more effective ways at work,” and “This employee often suggests changes to work projects in order to make them better.” Destructive Voice The five-item scale from Maynes and Podsakoff (2013) was employed to measure employee destructive voice behavior. A sample item is “This employee often bad-mouths the organization’s policies or objectives.” Supportive Voice The five-item scale developed by Maynes and Podsakoff (2013) was used to measure employee supportive voice. The sample items include “This employee defends organizational programs that are worthwhile when 7© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Chen, Zhang, Zhang, Wu, Xiao, Wu, Liu, Xia others unfairly criticize the programs.” Leader-Relational Attribution Orientation and Coworker- Relational Attribution Orientation Employees’ LRAO and CRAO were measured with the items developed in the pilot study. Results Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics, reliabilities, and correlations for the study variables. Table 2. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations of Study Variables
  • 146. Note. N = 328; reliabilities are in italics on the diagonal. Gender was measured using two categories: 1 = male, and 2 = female. Level of education was measured using four categories: 1 = below college, 2 = college, 3 = postgraduate, and 4 = doctorate. Position in the organization was measured using four categories: 1 = employee, 2 = supervisor, 3 = middle manager, and 4 = senior manager. Organizational tenure was measured with four categories: 1 = less than 5 years, 2 = 5–10 years, 3 = 11–15 years, and 4 = more than 15 years. LRAO = leader-relational attribution orientation, CRAO = coworker- relational attribution orientation. * p < .05. ** p < .01 (two-tailed). Mediation Effects of Psychological Needs Satisfaction We conducted hierarchical multiple regression analyses to test Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 1c and 2a, 2b, 2c by separately entering the control variables, independent variables (relationship conflicts with leaders and coworkers), and the mediator (psychological needs satisfaction). Model 1 in Table 3 indicates that relationship conflicts with leaders and with coworkers were both significantly related to psychological needs satisfaction. As shown by the results of Model 4, Model 7, and Model 10, relationship conflicts with leaders and with coworkers were both significantly related to supportive voice, destructive voice, and constructive voice. Model 5, Model 8, and Model 11 further revealed that psychological needs satisfaction was significantly related to supportive, destructive, and constructive voice, and that the effects of relationship conflicts with leaders and with coworkers on supportive, destructive, and constructive voice were weaker than the effects when constructive voice was excluded.
  • 147. 8© 2020 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal Table 3. Main and Interactive Effects Results Note. N = 328. PNS = psychological needs satisfaction, RCL = relational conflict with leaders, RCC = relational conflict with coworkers, LRAO = leader-relational attribution orientation, CRAO = coworker- relational attribution orientation. * p < .05. ** p < .01 (two-tailed). We also examined those effects using the bootstrap approach (MacKinnon et al., 2007). The results indicated that all the mediation effects of psychological needs satisfaction were significant. Bootstrap results are reported in Table 4. Particularly for relationship conflicts with leaders, the 95% confidence interval (CI) for indirect effects did not include zero; for relationship conflicts with coworkers, the 95% CI of the indirect effects also excluded zero. Thus, Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 1c and 2a, 2b, 2c were all supported. Table 4. Results of Bootstrap Analyses for the Mediating Role of Psychological Needs Satisfaction Note. N = 328. Bootstrap sample size = 5,000. CI = confidence interval, LL = lower limit, UL = upper limit. Moderating Effects of Employees’ Leader-Relational Attribution Orientation and