SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 8
Download to read offline
The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear
in vigilance to pain: a structural equations approach
Liesbet Gouberta,b,*,1
, Geert Crombeza,b
, Stefaan Van Dammea,b
a
Department of Experimental–Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium
b
Research Institute for Psychology and Health, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Received 19 June 2003; received in revised form 27 October 2003; accepted 7 November 2003
Abstract
The present study aimed at clarifying the precise role of pain catastrophizing, pain-related fear and personality dimensions in vigilance to
pain and pain severity by means of structural equation modelling. A questionnaire survey was conducted in 122 patients with chronic or
recurrent low back pain. Results revealed that pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear mediated the relationship between neuroticism and
vigilance to pain. Furthermore, vigilance to pain was found to be associated with heightened pain severity. Finally, we found that neuroticism
moderated the relationship between pain severity and catastrophic thinking about pain. The results strongly support the idea that vigilance to
pain is dependent upon catastrophic thinking and pain-related fear. Neuroticism is best conceived of as a vulnerability factor; it lowers the
threshold at which pain is perceived as threatening, and at which catastrophic thoughts about pain emerge.
q 2003 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Vigilance; Pain catastrophizing; Pain-related fear; Personality; Neuroticism
1. Introduction
The notion of heightened vigilance is often invoked to
explain the heightened experience of pain (Aldrich et al.,
2000; Chapman, 1978; McDermid et al., 1996). Although it
is still a matter of debate which attentional processes are
involved (Eysenck, 1992; Van Damme et al., 2003a,b),
there is a growing consensus that hypervigilance is an
emergent property of the threat value of pain (Chapman,
1978; Eccleston and Crombez, 1999): especially, persons
who appraise bodily sensations as dangerous are believed to
develop a habit of selecting pain over other competing
demands. In line with this idea, self-reported vigilance to
pain has been found to be strongly related to pain
catastrophizing—an exaggerated negative interpretation of
actual and anticipated pain experience—and pain-related
fear—the fear of pain and activities associated with pain—
(Roelofs et al., 2003).
An unresolved issue pertains to the role of personality in
vigilance topain.Ofthefivepersonalitydimensionsidentified
by Costa and McCrae (1987), both neuroticism and extraver-
sion seem to be the most plausible to have an effect upon
vigilance to pain. In particular, neuroticism, which is the trait-
like tendency to experience a broad range of negative feelings
such as distress, worry, and anxiety, has been associated with
a heightened experience of bodily sensations (Watson and
Pennebaker, 1989; Geisser et al., 2000). According to Watson
and Pennebaker (1989), persons scoring high on neuroticism
are more likely to notice and attend to internal physical
sensations and minor aches because their attentional scanning
of both the external and internal environment is fraught with
anxiety and uncertainty. Also the personality dimension
introversion–extraversion may affect selective attention to
pain (Eysenck, 1967). Psychophysiological studies indicated
that introverts exhibit a greater sensitivity to physical stimuli
than extraverts (Stelmack, 1990).
Few studies have directly investigated the interdepen-
dency of the above psychosocial variables in explaining
vigilance to pain. According to the cognitive-affective
model (Eccleston and Crombez, 1999), pain-related fear and
pain catastrophizing are instantiations of the threat value of
pain, and should therefore be considered as crucial
Pain 107 (2004) 234–241
www.elsevier.com/locate/pain
0304-3959/$20.00 q 2003 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.pain.2003.11.005
1
Research Assistant of the Fund for Scientific Research, Flanders,
Belgium, F.W.O.
* Corresponding author. Address: Department of Experimental-Clinical
and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000
Ghent, Belgium. Tel.: þ32-09-2646262; fax: þ32-09-2646489.
E-mail address: liesbet.goubert@ugent.be (L. Goubert).
mediators that account for the relationship between
personality and vigilance to pain. However, to our knowl-
edge, no study has provided a direct test of the putative
mediating role of pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear
in a sample of chronic pain patients. This was, therefore, the
first objective of this cross-sectional study. We designed and
tested a structural equations’ model with the following
assumptions: (1) vigilance to pain leads to the experience of
more severe pain, (2) both pain catastrophizing and pain-
related fear mediate the relationship between personality
dimensions and vigilance to pain, and (3) pain catastrophiz-
ing is a precursor to pain-related fear (Vlaeyen and Linton,
2000). A further objective was to explore the moderating
role of neuroticism within a diathesis-stress framework
(Eysenck, 1992). More specifically, we tested whether high-
neurotic persons who experience pain are more prone to
develop catastrophic thinking about pain and pain-related
fear (see also Wade and Price, 2000).
2. Method
2.1. Participants
One hundred and twenty-two patients with non-specific
chronic or recurrent back pain (mean age ¼ 43.72,
SD ¼ 9.56) were recruited from a physical revalidation
unit and from an orthopaedic unit at two university
hospitals. All participants were white Caucasians. The
majority of participants were female (60.7%), 84.2% were
married or cohabiting, and 22% had a higher education
(longer than the age of 18 years). The average duration of
pain since time of onset was 97.98 months (SD ¼ 104.17;
range ¼ 3 months–45 years). More than half of the patients
(54.1%) reported that their pain started gradually, 74.5%
reported radiation of the pain, and 40.4% of the patients had
undergone at least one spinal surgery. Almost one third of
the patients (30.8%) were in paid employment and 37.5% of
the patients were receiving worker’s compensation.
2.2. Questionnaires
Patients completed a battery of questionnaires designed to
assess the following constructs: pain severity, vigilance to
pain, catastrophic thinking about pain, fear of movement/
(re)injury, and personality characteristics. Pain severity was
assessed by the Dutch version of the Multidimensional Pain
Inventory—Part 1 (MPI; Lousberg et al., 1999). Part 1 of the
MPI-DV consists of five scales: pain severity (3 items),
interference with daily life due to pain (11 items), perceived
life control (4 items), affective distress (3 items) and social
support (3 items). The Dutch version of the MPI has shown to
have good reliability and validity (Lousberg et al., 1999).
Vigilance to pain was assessed by the Dutch version of the
Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire (PVAQ;
McCracken, 1997; Roelofs et al., 2002, 2003). This is
a 6-point 16-item measure of attention to pain that assesses
awareness, consciousness and vigilance to pain. The PVAQ
consists of two subscales: attention to pain (e.g. ‘I pay close
attention to pain’) and attention to changes in pain (e.g. ‘I am
quick to notice changes in pain intensity’) (Roelofs et al.,
2003). The questionnaire can be used in both clinical
(McCracken, 1997; Roelofs et al., 2003) and non-clinical
(McWilliams and Asmundson, 2001; Roelofs et al., 2002)
samples. The Dutch version of the PVAQ is reliable and valid
(Roelofs et al., 2002, 2003).
Catastrophic thinking about pain was assessed by the
Dutch version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS;
Sullivan et al., 1995; Crombez et al., 1998). This is a 13-item
scale developed for both nonclinical and clinical populations.
Participants reflect on past painful experiences and indicate
the degree to which they experienced thoughts or feelings
during pain on a 5-point scale. The Dutch version has been
shown to have good reliability and validity in a student
population and in a clinical population (Van Damme et al.,
2002a). The PCS consists of three subscales (Van Damme
et al., 2002a): rumination (e.g. ‘I can’t seem to keep it out of
my mind’), magnification (e.g. ‘I become afraid that the pain
will get worse’) and helplessness (e.g. ‘I feel I can’t stand it
anymore’).
Fear of (re)injury due to movement was measured by the
Dutch version of the adjusted Tampa Scale for Kinesiopho-
bia (TSK-AV; Kori et al., 1990; Goubert et al., 2003). It
consists of 13 items measured on a 4-point scale with scoring
alternatives ranging from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly
disagree’. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the
adjusted version (in which the inversed items are omitted)
more adequately fitted the data than the original version. The
TSK-AV is sufficiently reliable and valid (Goubert et al.,
2003). Confirmatory factor analyses revealed two valid
subscales, namely harm (e.g. ‘My body is telling me I have
something dangerously wrong’) and fear-avoidance (e.g.,
‘Simply being careful that I do not make any unnecessary
movements is the safest thing I can do to prevent my pain
from worsening’) (Goubert et al., 2003).
Personality was measured by the Dutch version of the
Big Five Personality Questionnaire (NEO-FFI; Costa and
McCrae, 1992; de Fruyt and Mervielde, 1998). This 60-item
questionnaire measures five personality dimensions, i.e.
neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness and
conscientiousness. Research has shown that the NEO-FFI
is sufficiently reliable (a coefficients vary between 0.68 and
0.86) (de Fruyt and Mervielde, 1998). Also the construct-
and concurrent validity has been well documented (de Fruyt
and Mervielde, 1998).
2.3. Statistical analysis
Structural equation modelling (SEM) was performed
using LISREL 8.52 (Jo¨reskog and So¨rbom, 1993) with
maximum likelihood estimation. In line with the recommen-
dations of Bollen and Long (1993), several fit indices were
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 235
used to assess model fit. In the present study, model fit was
assessed using the following goodness-of-fit indices: x2
;
goodness-of-fit index (GFI), adjusted goodness-of-fit index
(AGFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)
and comparative fit index (CFI).
The most frequently used fit index is x2
. This likelihood-
ratio test statistic is used to determine whether a significant
amount of observed covariance between items remains
unexplained by the model. A disadvantage of this index is its
sensitivity to sample size. In a small sample, a poor fit may
result in a non-significant x2
. It is also possible that in a large
sample a good fit results in a statistically significant x2
(Marsh et al., 1988). In the present study, the x2
is the normal
theory weighted least squares x2
as the other fit indices are
based upon this index in the LISREL 8.52 program. The GFI
and the AGFI (Jo¨reskog and So¨rbom, 1984) assess the extent
to which the model provides a better fit compared to no model
at all. These indices generally range between 0 and 1, with
high values (GFI . 0.90 and AGFI . 0.80) reflecting a good
fit of the model. The RMSEA (Browne and Cudeck, 1993) is
a fit measure based on population error of approximation.
The idea behind this is that it is unreasonable to assume that
the model holds exactly in the population. The RMSEA takes
into account the error of approximation in the population.
According to Browne and Cudeck (1993), a RMSEA value of
0.05 indicates a close fit and values up to 0.08 represent
reasonable errors of approximation in the population.
Finally, the CFI (Bentler, 1990a) is an incremental fit index
that produces a statistic in the range between 0 and 1. It
represents the proportionate improvement in model fit by
comparing the target model with a baseline model (usually a
null model in which all the observed variables are
uncorrelated). CFI values larger than 0.90 indicate an
adequate fit (Bentler, 1990b).
Furthermore, we investigated whether neuroticism mod-
erated the relationship between pain severity and both pain
catastrophizing and pain-related fear. To test for neuroticism
as a moderator, it is necessary to enter the cross-product
terms of neuroticism and pain severity in a separate block in a
hierarchical regression analysis, following the entry of pain
severity and neuroticism as first-order terms (Baron and
Kenny, 1986). To reduce the effects of multicollinearity,
variables were centered (Aiken and West, 1991). Statistically
significant interactions are interpreted by plotting regression
lines for high and low values of the moderator variable
(Aiken and West, 1991; Holmbeck, 2002).
3. Results
3.1. Patient characteristics
In comparison with the MPI-DV results of a patient
group entering a cognitive-behavioural rehabilitation pro-
gram (Lousberg et al., 1999), the severity of the pain
complaints in the present patient sample was moderate to
average. The mean score of the pain severity scale was 3.89
(SD ¼ 1.17; mean of comparison group ¼ 4.50;
tð121Þ ¼ 25:51; P , 0:001) and the mean score of the
interference scale was 4.25 (SD ¼ 1.13; mean of compari-
son group ¼ 4.52; tð121Þ ¼ 22:53; P , 0:05Þ:
3.2. Preliminary analyses
Table 1 presents the means, SD, Cronbach’s a and
Pearson intercorrelations for each of the self-report
variables. Vigilance to pain was significantly correlated
with pain severity, pain catastrophizing, fear of movement/
(re)injury and the personality dimensions of neuroticism
and agreeableness. Furthermore, pain catastrophizing was
significantly correlated with pain severity, fear of move-
ment/(re)injury, and the personality dimensions of neuroti-
cism, extraversion, openness to experience and
agreeableness. In addition, fear of movement/(re)injury
was significantly correlated with pain severity, neuroticism,
openness to experience and agreeableness.
3.3. Value of the personality dimensions in predicting
vigilance to pain
A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was per-
formed to investigate the unique contribution of each
personality dimension to the prediction of vigilance to pain.
In a first step, age and gender (males were coded as 0 and
Table 1
Means ðMÞ; standard deviations (SD), internal consistency ðaÞ and intercorrelations of all measures
M (SD) a 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 Vigilance to pain 46.81 (11.02) 0.81 0.21* 0.60*** 0.56*** 0.31** 20.14 20.02 20.19* 0.10
2 Pain severity 3.89 (1.17) 0.84 – 0.30** 0.29** 0.06 20.18 20.21* 20.04 20.02
3 Pain catastrophizing 21.38 (8.18) 0.89 – 0.58*** 0.33*** 20.19* 20.21* 20.24** 20.04
4 Fear of movement/(re)injury 32.71 (7.31) 0.78 – 0.28** 20.15 20.19* 20.28** 20.08
5 Neuroticism 34.25 (7.50) 0.80 – 20.50*** 20.25** 20.28** 20.36***
6 Extraversion 41.56 (6.44) 0.75 – 0.36*** 0.19* 0.39***
7 Openness to experience 36.67 (6.18) 0.69 – 0.19* 0.27**
8 Agreeableness 45.43 (5.84) 0.73 – 0.22*
9 Conscientiousness 47.82 (5.58) 0.79 –
*P , 0.05; **P , 0.01; ***p , 0.0005
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241236
females as 1) were entered into the equation to control for
sociodemographic variables. In a second step, pain severity
and pain duration were entered. Next, the five personality
dimensions were entered. Variance-inflation factors (range
1.00–1.66) were acceptable, suggesting that there was no
problem of collinearity. Our results revealed that of the five
personality dimensions only neuroticism and conscientious-
ness had a unique contribution in predicting vigilance to
pain (see Table 2). As only neuroticism proved (1) to have a
significant zero-order association with vigilance to pain, and
(2) to be a unique predictor of vigilance to pain in the
multiple regression analysis, further analyses were only
performed with this personality dimension.
3.4. Model testing
In order to use SEM, latent constructs of the observed
data were constructed. Fear of movement/(re)injury as a
latent construct was specified by the two subscales of the
TSK-AV: TSK-harm and TSK-fear. The latent construct
pain catastrophizing was indicated by the three subscales of
the PCS: PCS-rumination, PCS-magnification and PCS-
helplessness. Vigilance to pain was specified as a latent
construct measured by the two subscales of the PVAQ:
PVAQ-vigilance to pain and PVAQ-vigilance to changes in
pain. Because neuroticism was measured by one variable we
fixed the error variance to 0 and the loading value to 1.
Finally, pain severity as a latent construct was indicated by
the three items of the pain severity scale of the MPI. Missing
values were omitted, leaving valid data of 121 patients.
Before the structural model was tested we used confirmatory
factor analysis to examine the measurement model. It
showed a good fit of the data, x2
ð35Þ ¼ 53:26 ðP ¼ 0:02Þ;
RMSEA ¼ 0.07 (90% Confidence interval: 0.02–0.10),
CFI ¼ 0.97, GFI ¼ 0.93 and AGFI ¼ 0.86. This indicates
that the subscales of the TSK-AV, PCS, PVAQ and MPI
may be considered as valid operationalisations of the latent
constructs fear of movement/(re)injury, pain catastrophiz-
ing, vigilance to pain and pain severity. All subscales of the
questionnaires loaded significantly on the appropriate latent
construct.
Results revealed that our hypothesized structural model
had a good fit (see Fig. 1): x2
ð40Þ ¼ 66:45 ðP ¼ 0:005Þ;
RMSEA ¼ 0.07 (90% confidence interval: 0.04–0.10),
CFI ¼ 0.96, GFI ¼ 0.91 and AGFI ¼ 0.85. As expected,
our findings show that pain catastrophizing and fear of
movement/(re)injury mediated the relationship between
neuroticism and vigilance to pain. Testing a structural
Fig. 1. Structural equation model showing the role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury in vigilance to pain. NEO-N,
neuroticism subscale of the NEO-FFI; RUM, rumination subscale of the PCS; MAGN, magnification subscale of the PCS; HELP, helplessness subscale of the
PCS; HARM, harm subscale of the TSK-AV; FEAR-AV, fear-avoidance subscale of the TSK-AV; PVAQ-AP, vigilance to pain subscale of the PVAQ; PVAQ-
ACP, vigilance to changes in pain subscale of the PVAQ. *P , 0:05:
Table 2
Hierarchical regression analysis predicting vigilance to pain from age, sex, pain severity, pain duration, neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience,
agreeableness, and conscientiousness
Criterion variable Step Predictor b DR2
Adjusted R2
Vigilance to pain 1 Age 20.10 0.03 0.02
Sex 20.16
2 Pain severity 0.20* 0.06* 0.06
Pain duration 20.09
3 Neuroticism 0.36** 0.16*** 0.19
Extraversion 20.07
Openness to experience 0.08
Agreeableness 20.08
Conscientiousness 0.26**
*P , 0:05; **P , 0:01; ***P , 0:005. Standardized bs are displayed.
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 237
model in which pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/
(re)injury changed places, also revealed a good model fit:
x2
ð40Þ ¼ 61:40 ðP ¼ 0:02Þ; RMSEA ¼ 0.07 (90% confi-
dence interval: 0.03–0.10), CFI ¼ 0.96, GFI ¼ 0.91 and
AGFI ¼ 0.86. The direct link between neuroticism and
vigilance to pain was not significant. Finally, vigilance to
pain was positively associated with the experience of more
severe pain. Standardized b-coefficients and R2
values are
shown in Fig. 1, with R2
values shown above each
endogenous variable.
3.5. Value of the personality dimensions in predicting pain
catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury
To investigate the unique value of the personality
dimensions in predicting pain catastrophizing and fear of
movement/(re)injury, two multiple regression analyses were
performed. Variance-inflation factors (range 1.00–1.66)
were acceptable, suggesting that there was no problem of
collinearity. The first analysis revealed that only neuroti-
cism had a unique contribution in predicting pain catastro-
phizing, beyond age, gender (males were coded as 0 and
females as 1), pain duration and pain severity (see Table 3).
None of the other personality dimensions had a significant
contribution. A similar analysis with fear of movement/
(re)injury as dependent variable revealed identical results.
Only neuroticism had a unique contribution in predicting
fear of movement/(re)injury (see Table 3). Based upon these
results, it was decided to perform only moderation analyses
with the personality dimension ‘neuroticism’.
3.6. Moderation analyses
3.6.1. Moderational role of neuroticism in the relationship
between pain severity and pain catastrophizing
We examined whether the interaction variable (pain
severity £ neuroticism) was a significant predictor of
pain catastrophizing, after controlling for the effects of pain
severity and neuroticism (Aiken and West, 1991; Holmbeck,
2002). Significant main effects were found for pain severity
ðb ¼ 0:31; P , 0:0005Þ and neuroticism ðb ¼ 0:30; P ,
0:0005Þ: Also the interaction pain severity £ neuroticism
was a significant predictor ðb ¼ 0:16; P , 0:05Þ; revealing
that the association between pain severity and pain
catastrophizing is conditional on the values of neuroticism.
To illustrate the pattern reflected in this statistically
significant interaction term, we plotted regression lines for
high (þ1 SD above the mean) and low (21 SD below the
mean) values of the moderator variable (see Aiken and West,
1991; Holmbeck, 2002, Fig. 2). Significance tests for both
slopes indicated that the slope for the high-neuroticism
regression line was significant (b ¼ 0:46; P , 0:0005),
indicating that pain catastrophizing tends to be more
pronounced at higher levels of pain severity when neuroti-
cism is high. The slope for the low-neuroticism regression
line was not significant ðb ¼ 0:15; nsÞ:
3.6.2. Moderational role of neuroticism in the relationship
between pain severity and fear of movement/(re)injury
We entered the cross-product terms of neuroticism and
pain severity in a separate block in a hierarchical regression
Table 3
Hierarchical regression analyses predicting pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury from age, sex, pain severity, pain duration, neuroticism,
extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness
Criterion variable Step Predictor b DR2
Adjusted R2
Pain catastrophizing 1 Age 20.07 0.01 0.00
Sex 20.06
2 Pain severity 0.26*** 0.12*** 0.11
Pain duration 20.14
3 Neuroticism 0.30*** 0.13*** 0.20
Extraversion 20.02
Openness to experience 20.09
Agreeableness 20.13
Conscientiousness 0.13
Fear of movement/(re)injury 1 Age 0.06 0.06* 0.05
Sex 20.21*
2 Pain severity 0.25*** 0.11*** 0.15
Pain duration 20.17
3 Neuroticism 0.27** 0.09* 0.21
Extraversion 0.05
Openness to experience 20.05
Agreeableness 20.12
Conscientiousness 0.05
*P , 0:05; **P , 0:01; ***P , 0:005: Standardized betas are displayed.
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241238
analysis, following the entry of pain severity and neuroticism
as first-order terms. We found significant main effects of pain
severity ðb ¼ 0:28; P , 0:001Þ and neuroticism ðb ¼
0:25; P , 0:01Þ: The interaction term pain severity £
neuroticism, however, was not a significant predictor of
fear of movement/(re)injury ðb ¼ 0:14; P , 0:10Þ: Although
the interaction term was not significant, we performed a post-
hoc probing analysis. The results revealed similar results as
for pain catastrophizing: the slope for the high-neuroticism
regression line was again significant (b ¼ 0.42,
P , 0.0005), indicating that fear of movement/(re)injury
tends to be more pronounced at higher levels of pain severity
when neuroticism is high. The slope for the low-neuroticism
regression line was not significant (b ¼ 0.15, ns).
4. Discussion
The present study aimed at clarifying the role of
catastrophic thinking about pain, pain-related fear and
neuroticism in its relationship with vigilance to pain and
pain severity. The results can be readily summarized. First,
vigilance to pain was related to the experience of more
severe pain. Second, of the five personality dimensions that
were investigated, only neuroticism was found to be
consistently related to vigilance to pain, pain catastrophiz-
ing and fear of movement/(re)injury. Third, pain catastro-
phizing and fear of movement/(re)injury mediated the
relationship between neuroticism and vigilance to pain.
Fourth, neuroticism was found to moderate the relationship
between pain severity and pain catastrophizing. Analyses
predicting pain-related fear were in the same line, but only
showed a trend to significance.
Of particular interest to this study was the finding that the
effect of neuroticism upon pain vigilance is largely
mediated by pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear.
This is not in line with the symptom perception model of
Watson and Pennebaker (1989), which postulates a direct
effect of neuroticism on vigilance to pain. Our results
support the idea that vigilance to pain is critically dependent
upon its immediate threat value (Chapman, 1978; Eccleston
and Crombez, 1999; Price, 1999).
Our findings point out that neuroticism may be conceived
of as a vulnerability factor within a diathesis-stress frame-
work. When a person is confronted with the stressor of
(acute) pain, neuroticism may influence whether the person
appraises the pain as threatening or not (see also Sullivan
et al., 2003; Thorn et al., 2003). Neuroticism may lower the
threshold at which pain is perceived as threatening, and at
which pain elicits catastrophic thoughts. For high-neurotic
individuals, a low pain severity is sufficient to be perceived
as threatening, and consequently, to provoke catastrophic
thoughts about pain. Our results argue for a modern and
dynamic view on personality instead of a traditional
personality approach that stresses cross-situational stability
of personality traits. In a dynamic view, behaviours are not
assumed to be stable across situations, but vary depending
on the specific situation (Mischel and Shoda, 1998).
Personality is then characterized by stable situation-
behaviour (if…then) relations. This dynamic view is similar
to a diathesis-stress framework in which personality traits
influence the way in which people cognitively process the
meanings that chronic pain holds for their life (Wade et al.,
1992).
The personality dimensions ‘extraversion’, ‘openness’,
‘agreeableness’ and ‘conscientiousness’ do not seem to be
of crucial importance in the emergence of vigilance to pain.
We found no evidence for the idea that introverts tended to
be more vigilant to pain (see Stelmack, 1990). The effects of
conscientiousness upon vigilance to pain are preliminary,
and await replication. They are however intriguing. As
conscientious individuals carefully plan before acting and
avoid situations involving risk or danger (Watson et al.,
1994), it is possible that they become frustrated when
confronted with the insolubility of the problem of chronic
pain and its uncontrollable effects upon daily life. Chronic
pain may then be experienced as (partly) taking over
control, and dominating awareness.
Neuroticism was also the only personality dimension
that uniquely accounted for pain catastrophizing and pain-
related fear. Correlational analyses however showed that
pain catastrophizing was negatively related to extraversion,
openness to experience, and agreeableness. The latter two
personality characteristics were also negatively related to
pain-related fear. This is in line with the findings of Jerram
and Coleman (1999), who found that openness to
experience and agreeableness were associated with positive
health perceptions. Regarding extraversion, our results
suggest that extraverted people have fewer catastrophic
thoughts about pain. This is in line with the suggestion of
Phillips and Gatchel (2000), that extraversion–introversion
is a factor that affects how a person experiences and
responds to pain.
Fig. 2. Regression lines for the relationship between pain severity and pain
catastrophizing as moderated by neuroticism. b, unstandardized regression
coefficient (i.e. simple slope); SD, standard deviation. *P , 0:0005:
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 239
The results of this study have a number of important
clinical implications. When pain is perceived as highly
threatening, patients become vigilant to pain, and may
experience difficulties to disengage attention from pain-
related information (Van Damme et al., 2002b, 2003a). As a
consequence, attentional distraction strategies may not be
effective in these patients. It may be more effective to
diminish the threat value of pain by means of cognitive
restructuring techniques (Thorn and Williams, 1993;
Warwick et al., 1996) or exposure techniques that
disconfirm the belief of catastrophic outcomes (Crombez
et al., 2002; Goubert et al., 2002; Vlaeyen et al., 2002).
This study has a number of limitations, each of which
point to directions for future research. First, all findings are
based on cross-sectional and correlational data. Significant
correlations do not indicate causal effects. In future research,
this structural equations model needs to be applied to
longitudinal data, in order to provide firm evidence on the
causal relations between the variables. Second, the variables
in this study were assessed only using self-report instru-
ments. Further research should include multiple methods to
assess vigilance to pain, including experimental and
psychophysiological procedures. A third limitation is that
neuroticism is probably not the only variable that accounts
for pain catastrophizing (see Rainville et al., 2000). Other
pathways may also lead to pain catastrophizing and pain-
related fear. Sullivan et al. (2001) proposed that a learning
history characterized by heightened pain experience, may
itself lead to the development of a pain schema, containing
threatening information and catastrophic expectancies about
painfulstimuli. Fourth, although conscientiousness provedto
be a unique predictor of vigilance to pain in the multiple
regression analysis, the zero-order correlation was not
significant. Further research into the potential role of
personality characteristics, in particular of conscientiousness
and neuroticism, in vigilance to pain and in the development
of pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear is needed.
Acknowledgements
The authors want to thank Petra De Vlieger, Els Persijn,
Leen Van Vlierberghe, Stephen Morley and two anonymous
reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the
manuscript.
References
Aiken LS, West SG, Multiple regression: testing and interpreting
interactions, Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991.
Aldrich S, Eccleston C, Crombez G. Worrying about chronic pain:
vigilance to threat and misdirected problem solving. Behav Res Ther
2000;38:457–70.
Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in
social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical
considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:1173–82.
Bentler PM. Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychol Bull
1990a;107:238–46.
Bentler PM. Fit indices, Lagrange multipliers, constraint changes and
incomplete data in structural models. Multivariate Behav Res 1990b;25:
163–72.
Bollen KA, Long JS. Introduction. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing
structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. p. 1–9.
Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen
KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage; 1993. p. 136–62.
Chapman CR. Pain: the perception of noxious events. In: Sternbach RA,
editor. The psychology of pain. New York: Raven Press; 1978. p.
169–202.
Costa PT, McCrae RR. Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: is the
bark worse than the bite? J Pers 1987;55:299–316.
Costa PT, McCrae RR, Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five
factor inventory professional manual, Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources; 1992.
Crombez G, Eccleston C, Baeyens F, Eelen P. When somatic information
threatens, catastrophic thinking enhances attentional interference. Pain
1998;75:187–98.
Crombez G, Eccleston C, Vlaeyen JWS, Vansteenwegen D, Lysens R,
Eelen P. Exposure to physical movement in low back pain patients:
restricted effects of generalization. Health Psychol 2002;21:573–8.
de Fruyt F, Mervielde I. The assessment of the big five in the Dutch
language domain. Psychol Belg 1998;38:1–22.
Eccleston C, Crombez G. Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective
model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull 1999;125:
356–66.
Eysenck HJ. The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles
C Thomas; 1967.
Eysenck MW. Anxiety: the cognitive perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum;
1992.
Geisser ME, Roth RS, Theisen ME, Robinson ME, Riley JL. Negative
affect, self-report of depressive symptoms, and clinical depression:
relation to the experience of chronic pain. Clin J Pain 2000;16:
110–20.
Goubert L, Crombez G, Van Damme S, Vlaeyen JWS, Bijttebier P, Roelofs
J. Confirmatory factor analysis of the tampa scale for kinesiophobia:
invariant two-factor model across low back pain patients and
fibromyalgia patients. Clin J Pain 2003; in press.
Goubert L, Francken G, Crombez G, Vansteenwegen D, Lysens R.
Exposure to physical movement in chronic back pain patients: no
evidence for generalization across different movements. Behav Res
Ther 2002;40:415–29.
Holmbeck GN. Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and media-
tional effects in studies of pediatric populations. J Pediatr Psychol 2002;
27:87–96.
Jerram KL, Coleman PG. The big five personality traits and reporting of
health problems and health behaviour in old age. Br J Health Psychol
1999;4:181–92.
Jo¨reskog KG, So¨rbom D, LISREL VI user’s guide, Mooresville, IN:
Scientific Software; 1984.
Jo¨reskog KG, So¨rbom D, LISREL 8: structural equation modeling with the
SIMPLIS language, Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc;
1993.
Kori SH, Miller RP, Todd DD. Kinisiophobia: a new view of chronic pain
behavior. Pain Manage 1990;35–43.
Lousberg R, Van Breukelen GJP, Groenman NH, Schmidt AJM, Arntz A,
Winter FAM. Psychometric properties of the multidimensional pain
inventory, Dutch language version (MPI-DLV). Behav Res Ther 1999;
37:167–82.
Marsh HW, Balla JR, McDonald RP. Goodness-of-fit indexes in
confirmatory factor analysis: the effect of sample size. Psychol Bull
1988;103:391–410.
McCracken LM. Attention to pain in persons with chronic pain: a
behavioral approach. Behav Ther 1997;28:271–84.
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241240
McDermid AJ, Rollman GB, McCain GA. Generalized hypervigilance in
fibromyalgia: evidence of perceptual amplification. Pain 1996;66:
133–44.
McWilliams LA, Asmundson GJG. Assessing individual differences in
attention to pain: psychometric properties of the pain vigilance and
awareness questionnaire modified for a non-clinical pain sample. Pers
Individ Differ 2001;31:239–46.
Mischel W, Shoda Y. Reconciling processing dynamics and personality
dispositions. Annu Rev Psychol 1998;49:229–58.
Phillips JM, Gatchel RJ. Extraversion-introversion and chronic pain. In:
Gatchel RJ, Weisberg JN, editors. Personality characteristics of patients
with pain. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association;
2000. p. 181–202.
Price DD. The dimensions of pain experience. In: Price DD, editor.
Psychological mechanisms of pain and analgesia. Seattle, WA: IASP
Press; 1999. p. 43–70.
Rainville J, Carlson N, Polatin P, Gatchel RJ, Indahl A. Exploration of
physicians’ recommendations for activities in chronic low back pain.
Spine 2000;25:2210–20.
Roelofs J, Peters ML, Muris P, Vlaeyen JWS. Dutch version of the pain
vigilance and awareness questionnaire: validity and reliability in a pain-
free population. Behav Res Ther 2002;40:1081–90.
Roelofs J, Peters ML, McCracken L, Vlaeyen JWS. The pain vigilance and
awareness questionnaire (PVAQ): further psychometric evaluation in
fibromyalgiaandother chronic pain syndromes.Pain 2003;101:299–306.
Stelmack RM. Biological bases of extraversion: psychophysiological
evidence. J Pers 1990;1:293–311.
Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale:
development and validation. Psychol Assess 1995;7:524–32.
Sullivan MJL, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, Bradley
LA, Lefebvre JC. Theoretical perspectives on the relation between
catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain 2001;17:52–64.
Sullivan MJL, Thorn BE, Rodgers W, Ward LC. A path model of
psychological antecedents to pain experience: experimental and clinical
findings. Clin J Pain 2003; in press.
Thorn BE, Williams DA. Cognitive-behavioral management of chronic
pain. In: VandeCreek L, Knapp S, Jackson TL, editors. Innovations in
clinical practice: a source book, vol. 12. Sarasota, FL: Professional
Resource Press/Professional Resource Exchange; 1993. p. 169–91.
Thorn BE, Clements KL, Ward C, Dixon KE, Kersh BC, Boothby JL,
Chaplin WF. Personality factors in the explanation of sex differences in
pain catastrophizing and response to experimental pain. Clin J Pain
2003; in press.
Van Damme S, Crombez G, Bijttebier P, Goubert L, Van Houdenhove B. A
confirmatory factor analysis of the pain catastrophizing scale: invariant
factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populations. Pain
2002a;96:319–24.
Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Retarded disengagement from
pain cues: the effects of pain catastrophizing and pain expectancy. Pain
2002b;100:111–8.
Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Impaired disengagement from
pain: the role of catastrophic thinking about pain. Pain 2003a; in press.
Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C, Roelofs J. The role of
hypervigilance in the experience of pain. In: Asmundson GJG, Vlaeyen
JWS, Crombez G, editors. Understanding and treating fear of pain.
Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003b. in press.
Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic
musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain 2000;85:317–32.
Vlaeyen JWS, de Jong J, Geilen M, Heuts PHTG, van Breukelen G. The
treatment of fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain:
further evidence on the effectiveness of exposure in vivo. Clin J Pain
2002;18:251–61.
Wade JB, Price DD. Nonpathological factors in chronic pain: Implications
for assessment and treatment. In: Gatchel RJ, Weisberg JN, editors.
Personality characteristics of patients with pain. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association; 2000. p. 89–108.
Wade JB, Dougherty LM, Hart RP, Rafii A, Price DD. A canonical
correlation analysis of the influence of neuroticism and extraversion on
chronic pain, suffering, and pain behaviour. Pain 1992;51:67–73.
Warwick HMC, Clark DM, Cobb AM, Salkovskis PM. A controlled trial of
cognitive-behavioural treatment of hypochondriasis. Br J Psychiatry
1996;169:189–95.
Watson D, Clark LA, Harkness AR. Structures of personality and their
relevance to psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103:18–31.
Watson D, Pennebaker JW. Health complaints, stress, and distress:
exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychol Rev 1989;
96:234–54.
L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 241

More Related Content

What's hot

Humble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJP
Humble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJPHumble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJP
Humble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJPDr Stephen Humble
 
Soft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic Pain
Soft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic PainSoft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic Pain
Soft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic PainKimberly Burnham, PhD
 
Opioid Induced Hyperalgesia
Opioid Induced HyperalgesiaOpioid Induced Hyperalgesia
Opioid Induced HyperalgesiaAde Wijaya
 
Postmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy Pain
Postmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy PainPostmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy Pain
Postmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy PainJason Attaman
 
article
articlearticle
articleLin T
 
EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...
EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...
EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...EWMA
 
The effect of the “laying on of hands
The effect of the “laying on of handsThe effect of the “laying on of hands
The effect of the “laying on of handsenergiaprimordialreiki
 
1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main
1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main
1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-mainGuillem Feixas
 
Somatic problems with biofeedback
Somatic problems with biofeedbackSomatic problems with biofeedback
Somatic problems with biofeedbackDr. ARNON ROLNICK
 
Common Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & Addiction
Common Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & AddictionCommon Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & Addiction
Common Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & AddictionPaul Coelho, MD
 
Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice
Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice
Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice MaxiMedRx
 
Central Sensitization in EDS
Central Sensitization in EDSCentral Sensitization in EDS
Central Sensitization in EDSPaul Coelho, MD
 

What's hot (19)

Humble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJP
Humble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJPHumble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJP
Humble Syst Rv Periop NeuP 2015 EJP
 
March 2017 vol3 issue1
March 2017 vol3 issue1March 2017 vol3 issue1
March 2017 vol3 issue1
 
Mengenal nyeri untuk peserta ipm ke dua 7 nov 2017 di makassar
Mengenal nyeri untuk peserta ipm ke dua 7 nov 2017 di makassarMengenal nyeri untuk peserta ipm ke dua 7 nov 2017 di makassar
Mengenal nyeri untuk peserta ipm ke dua 7 nov 2017 di makassar
 
Dasar dasar nyeri akut, neuropatik dan kronik
Dasar dasar nyeri akut, neuropatik dan kronik Dasar dasar nyeri akut, neuropatik dan kronik
Dasar dasar nyeri akut, neuropatik dan kronik
 
Soft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic Pain
Soft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic PainSoft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic Pain
Soft Tissue Joint Comfort Range Solutions Tbr Fibromyalgia Chronic Pain
 
Cancer pain concept
Cancer pain concept  Cancer pain concept
Cancer pain concept
 
Opioid Induced Hyperalgesia
Opioid Induced HyperalgesiaOpioid Induced Hyperalgesia
Opioid Induced Hyperalgesia
 
Postmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy Pain
Postmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy PainPostmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy Pain
Postmastectomy and Post Thoracotomy Pain
 
article
articlearticle
article
 
B03520406
B03520406B03520406
B03520406
 
EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...
EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...
EWMA 2014 - EP490 THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WOUND PAIN ASSOCIATED WITH DIABETES-...
 
The effect of the “laying on of hands
The effect of the “laying on of handsThe effect of the “laying on of hands
The effect of the “laying on of hands
 
1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main
1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main
1 s2.0-s1697260018301753-main
 
Somatic problems with biofeedback
Somatic problems with biofeedbackSomatic problems with biofeedback
Somatic problems with biofeedback
 
Multidisciplinary pain management rsuh dr. takdri
Multidisciplinary pain management rsuh dr. takdriMultidisciplinary pain management rsuh dr. takdri
Multidisciplinary pain management rsuh dr. takdri
 
Common Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & Addiction
Common Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & AddictionCommon Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & Addiction
Common Brain Mechanisms Between Pain & Addiction
 
Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice
Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice
Application of Pstim in Clinical Practice
 
Dor cronica
Dor cronicaDor cronica
Dor cronica
 
Central Sensitization in EDS
Central Sensitization in EDSCentral Sensitization in EDS
Central Sensitization in EDS
 

Viewers also liked

Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!
Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!
Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!Joe Noland
 
Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?
Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?
Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?Pierluigi De Rosa
 
supervisorbootcampclean
supervisorbootcampcleansupervisorbootcampclean
supervisorbootcampcleanAshley Andrews
 
5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)
5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)
5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)Secundino vega
 
Geographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMS
Geographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMSGeographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMS
Geographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMSPaul Coelho, MD
 
día 3 Miércoles 2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...
día 3 Miércoles  2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...día 3 Miércoles  2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...
día 3 Miércoles 2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...Diana Vinay
 
Цветотерапия
ЦветотерапияЦветотерапия
Цветотерапияsad-raduga
 
January 25,2015 Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2
January 25,2015  Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2January 25,2015  Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2
January 25,2015 Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2Catherine Lirio
 
2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap
2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap
2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buapDiana Vinay
 
2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay
2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay
2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinayDiana Vinay
 
Topik 9.1 Insurans
Topik 9.1 InsuransTopik 9.1 Insurans
Topik 9.1 InsuransFiza Wahab
 
Jardín n° 111 -------
Jardín n° 111 -------Jardín n° 111 -------
Jardín n° 111 -------anep
 
Diana Vinay RUBRICA Juventus
Diana Vinay RUBRICA JuventusDiana Vinay RUBRICA Juventus
Diana Vinay RUBRICA JuventusDiana Vinay
 
VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...
VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...
VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...Artium Vitoria
 
Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)
Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)
Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)Estanis888
 
La normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilità
La normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilitàLa normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilità
La normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilitàPierluigi De Rosa
 
Jenis-jenis Insurans
Jenis-jenis InsuransJenis-jenis Insurans
Jenis-jenis InsuransFiza Wahab
 

Viewers also liked (20)

hews cert
hews certhews cert
hews cert
 
Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!
Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!
Our Dream - The MORE Revolution!
 
Navidad
NavidadNavidad
Navidad
 
Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?
Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?
Comunicazione di crisi:quali spazi di dialogo tra comunicatori e giornalisti?
 
supervisorbootcampclean
supervisorbootcampcleansupervisorbootcampclean
supervisorbootcampclean
 
5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)
5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)
5.2. teoria neoclasica de la produccion(sin video)
 
Geographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMS
Geographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMSGeographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMS
Geographic Variation in Opioid Use for FMS
 
día 3 Miércoles 2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...
día 3 Miércoles  2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...día 3 Miércoles  2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...
día 3 Miércoles 2014 dic buap taller estrategias de evaluación para el proce...
 
Цветотерапия
ЦветотерапияЦветотерапия
Цветотерапия
 
January 25,2015 Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2
January 25,2015  Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2January 25,2015  Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2
January 25,2015 Sunday Message- Discipline Part 2
 
2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap
2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap
2016 tercera edición buscador estratégico buap
 
2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay
2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay
2014 día 1 taller de microenseñanza buap diana vinay
 
Topik 9.1 Insurans
Topik 9.1 InsuransTopik 9.1 Insurans
Topik 9.1 Insurans
 
Jardín n° 111 -------
Jardín n° 111 -------Jardín n° 111 -------
Jardín n° 111 -------
 
Diana Vinay RUBRICA Juventus
Diana Vinay RUBRICA JuventusDiana Vinay RUBRICA Juventus
Diana Vinay RUBRICA Juventus
 
VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...
VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...
VIII Encuentros de Centros de Documentación de Arte Contemporáneo en Artium -...
 
Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)
Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)
Visita de personal sanitario. Infantil CEIP CARRASCO ALCALDE (Herencia)
 
La normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilità
La normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilitàLa normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilità
La normativa sulla comunicazione di pubblica utilità
 
Product Placement
Product PlacementProduct Placement
Product Placement
 
Jenis-jenis Insurans
Jenis-jenis InsuransJenis-jenis Insurans
Jenis-jenis Insurans
 

Similar to Neuroticism & PCS

For this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions be
For this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions beFor this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions be
For this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions beLilianaJohansen814
 
PainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docx
PainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docxPainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docx
PainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docxkarlhennesey
 
Catastrophizing: Trait or State?
Catastrophizing: Trait or State?Catastrophizing: Trait or State?
Catastrophizing: Trait or State?Paul Coelho, MD
 
Treatment of Catastrophizing
Treatment of CatastrophizingTreatment of Catastrophizing
Treatment of CatastrophizingPaul Coelho, MD
 
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docxCritical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docxstudywriters
 
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docxCritical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docxstudywriters
 
Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.
Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.
Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.Paul Coelho, MD
 
Edwards et al-2006-arthritis_care
Edwards et al-2006-arthritis_careEdwards et al-2006-arthritis_care
Edwards et al-2006-arthritis_carePaul Coelho, MD
 
éMotion
 éMotion éMotion
éMotionKeyhaku
 
éMotion
 éMotion éMotion
éMotionKeyhaku
 
Pain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaning
Pain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaningPain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaning
Pain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaningMark Sullivan
 
EDS & Central Sensitization
EDS & Central SensitizationEDS & Central Sensitization
EDS & Central SensitizationPaul Coelho, MD
 
The Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make Decisions
The Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make DecisionsThe Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make Decisions
The Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make DecisionsAbbie Frank
 
pain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptx
pain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptxpain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptx
pain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptxswetamohanty43
 

Similar to Neuroticism & PCS (20)

Hm eand pain
Hm eand painHm eand pain
Hm eand pain
 
Pain pathway
Pain pathwayPain pathway
Pain pathway
 
For this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions be
For this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions beFor this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions be
For this assignment activity,  I want you to answer the questions be
 
Biopsychosocial pain 2019
Biopsychosocial pain 2019Biopsychosocial pain 2019
Biopsychosocial pain 2019
 
P 517
P 517P 517
P 517
 
PainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docx
PainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docxPainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docx
PainPathophysiology of Acute PainAcute pain, also called noci.docx
 
Catastrophizing: Trait or State?
Catastrophizing: Trait or State?Catastrophizing: Trait or State?
Catastrophizing: Trait or State?
 
Treatment of Catastrophizing
Treatment of CatastrophizingTreatment of Catastrophizing
Treatment of Catastrophizing
 
Meu 2
Meu 2Meu 2
Meu 2
 
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docxCritical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
 
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docxCritical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
Critical Appraisal of Pain Assessment Tools Essay.docx
 
Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.
Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.
Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, FMS, & other rheumatic diseases.
 
Edwards et al-2006-arthritis_care
Edwards et al-2006-arthritis_careEdwards et al-2006-arthritis_care
Edwards et al-2006-arthritis_care
 
Central Neuropathic Pain after Acute Spinal Cord Injury (NP in SCI): A Case s...
Central Neuropathic Pain after Acute Spinal Cord Injury (NP in SCI): A Case s...Central Neuropathic Pain after Acute Spinal Cord Injury (NP in SCI): A Case s...
Central Neuropathic Pain after Acute Spinal Cord Injury (NP in SCI): A Case s...
 
éMotion
 éMotion éMotion
éMotion
 
éMotion
 éMotion éMotion
éMotion
 
Pain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaning
Pain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaningPain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaning
Pain and Opioids: damage and danger, mechanism and meaning
 
EDS & Central Sensitization
EDS & Central SensitizationEDS & Central Sensitization
EDS & Central Sensitization
 
The Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make Decisions
The Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make DecisionsThe Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make Decisions
The Reduction of Anxiety on the Ability to Make Decisions
 
pain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptx
pain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptxpain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptx
pain-behvaioral medicine management overview.pptx
 

More from Paul Coelho, MD

Suicidality Poster References.docx
Suicidality Poster References.docxSuicidality Poster References.docx
Suicidality Poster References.docxPaul Coelho, MD
 
Opioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDC
Opioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDCOpioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDC
Opioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDCPaul Coelho, MD
 
Labeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of Fibromyalgia
Labeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of FibromyalgiaLabeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of Fibromyalgia
Labeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of FibromyalgiaPaul Coelho, MD
 
Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...
Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...
Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...Paul Coelho, MD
 
Pain Phenotyping Tool For Primary Care
Pain Phenotyping Tool For Primary CarePain Phenotyping Tool For Primary Care
Pain Phenotyping Tool For Primary CarePaul Coelho, MD
 
Fibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient Handout
Fibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient HandoutFibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient Handout
Fibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient HandoutPaul Coelho, MD
 
Community Catastrophizing
Community CatastrophizingCommunity Catastrophizing
Community CatastrophizingPaul Coelho, MD
 
Risk-Benefit High-Dose LTOT
Risk-Benefit High-Dose LTOTRisk-Benefit High-Dose LTOT
Risk-Benefit High-Dose LTOTPaul Coelho, MD
 
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...Paul Coelho, MD
 
Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.
Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.
Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.Paul Coelho, MD
 
Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084
Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084
Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084Paul Coelho, MD
 
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...Paul Coelho, MD
 
Gao recommendations to CMS
Gao recommendations to CMSGao recommendations to CMS
Gao recommendations to CMSPaul Coelho, MD
 
Prescribing by specialty
Prescribing by specialtyPrescribing by specialty
Prescribing by specialtyPaul Coelho, MD
 
The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...
The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...
The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...Paul Coelho, MD
 
Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases
Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases
Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases Paul Coelho, MD
 
Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...
Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...
Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...Paul Coelho, MD
 
The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.
The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.
The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.Paul Coelho, MD
 
Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417
Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417
Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417Paul Coelho, MD
 

More from Paul Coelho, MD (20)

Suicidality Poster References.docx
Suicidality Poster References.docxSuicidality Poster References.docx
Suicidality Poster References.docx
 
Opioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDC
Opioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDCOpioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDC
Opioid Milli-equivalence Conversion Factors CDC
 
Labeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of Fibromyalgia
Labeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of FibromyalgiaLabeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of Fibromyalgia
Labeling Woefulness: The Social Construction of Fibromyalgia
 
Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...
Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...
Outcomes in Long-term Opioid Tapering and Buprenorphine Transition: A Retrosp...
 
Pain Phenotyping Tool For Primary Care
Pain Phenotyping Tool For Primary CarePain Phenotyping Tool For Primary Care
Pain Phenotyping Tool For Primary Care
 
Fibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient Handout
Fibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient HandoutFibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient Handout
Fibromyalgia & Somatic Symptom Disorder Patient Handout
 
Community Catastrophizing
Community CatastrophizingCommunity Catastrophizing
Community Catastrophizing
 
Risk-Benefit High-Dose LTOT
Risk-Benefit High-Dose LTOTRisk-Benefit High-Dose LTOT
Risk-Benefit High-Dose LTOT
 
Space Trial
Space TrialSpace Trial
Space Trial
 
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
 
Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.
Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.
Prescriptions filled following an opioid-related hospitalization.
 
Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084
Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084
Jamapsychiatry santavirta 2017_oi_170084
 
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
 
Gao recommendations to CMS
Gao recommendations to CMSGao recommendations to CMS
Gao recommendations to CMS
 
Prescribing by specialty
Prescribing by specialtyPrescribing by specialty
Prescribing by specialty
 
The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...
The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...
The place-of-antipsychotics-in-the-therapy-of-anxiety-disorders-and-obsessive...
 
Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases
Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases
Structured opioid refill clinic epic smartphrases
 
Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...
Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...
Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, an...
 
The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.
The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.
The potential adverse influence of physicians’ words.
 
Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417
Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417
Ctaf chronic pain__evidence_report_100417
 

Recently uploaded

VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171
VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171
VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171Call Girls Service Gurgaon
 
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service available
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service availableCall Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service available
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service availablegragmanisha42
 
Chandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in Chandigarh
Chandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in ChandigarhChandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in Chandigarh
Chandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in ChandigarhSheetaleventcompany
 
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In Raipur
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In RaipurCall Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In Raipur
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In Raipurgragmanisha42
 
Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★
Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★
Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★indiancallgirl4rent
 
Udaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in Udaipur
Udaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in UdaipurUdaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in Udaipur
Udaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in Udaipurseemahedar019
 
Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...
Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...
Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...Niamh verma
 
Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...
Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...
Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...Sheetaleventcompany
 
Mangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Mangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetMangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Mangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetCall Girls Service
 
Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...
Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...
Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...Call Girls Noida
 
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR Call G...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR   Call G...❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR   Call G...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR Call G...Gfnyt.com
 
VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591adityaroy0215
 
Dehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking Models
Dehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking ModelsDehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking Models
Dehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking Modelsindiancallgirl4rent
 
ooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
ooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meetooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
ooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetCall Girls Service
 
pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...
pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...
pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...Call Girls Noida
 
Ozhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Ozhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetOzhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Ozhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetCall Girls Service
 
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF ...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF  ...❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF  ...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF ...Gfnyt.com
 
Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510
Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510
Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510Vipesco
 
Call Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Call Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetCall Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Call Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meetpriyashah722354
 
VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591adityaroy0215
 

Recently uploaded (20)

VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171
VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171
VIP Call Girl Sector 10 Noida Call Me: 9711199171
 
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service available
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service availableCall Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service available
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 whatsapp live cam sex service available
 
Chandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in Chandigarh
Chandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in ChandigarhChandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in Chandigarh
Chandigarh Escorts, 😋9988299661 😋50% off at Escort Service in Chandigarh
 
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In Raipur
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In RaipurCall Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In Raipur
Call Girl Raipur 📲 9999965857 ヅ10k NiGhT Call Girls In Raipur
 
Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★
Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★
Enjoyment ★ 8854095900 Indian Call Girls In Dehradun 🍆🍌 By Dehradun Call Girl ★
 
Udaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in Udaipur
Udaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in UdaipurUdaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in Udaipur
Udaipur Call Girls 📲 9999965857 Call Girl in Udaipur
 
Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...
Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...
Call Girls Service Chandigarh Gori WhatsApp ❤7710465962 VIP Call Girls Chandi...
 
Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...
Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...
Call Girl Amritsar ❤️♀️@ 8725944379 Amritsar Call Girls Near Me ❤️♀️@ Sexy Ca...
 
Mangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Mangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetMangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Mangalore Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
 
Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...
Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...
Vip sexy Call Girls Service In Sector 137,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Ser...
 
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR Call G...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR   Call G...❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR   Call G...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Meghna Jaipur Call Girls Number CRTHNR Call G...
 
VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 25 Gurgaon Just Call Me 9899900591
 
Dehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking Models
Dehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking ModelsDehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking Models
Dehradun Call Girls Service 08854095900 Real Russian Girls Looking Models
 
ooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
ooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meetooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
ooty Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
 
pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...
pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...
pOOJA sexy Call Girls In Sector 49,9999965857 Young Female Escorts Service In...
 
Ozhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Ozhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetOzhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Ozhukarai Call Girls 👙 6297143586 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
 
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF ...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF  ...❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF  ...
❤️♀️@ Jaipur Call Girls ❤️♀️@ Jaispreet Call Girl Services in Jaipur QRYPCF ...
 
Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510
Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510
Krishnagiri call girls Tamil aunty 7877702510
 
Call Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Call Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real MeetCall Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
Call Girls Chandigarh 👙 7001035870 👙 Genuine WhatsApp Number for Real Meet
 
VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591
VIP Call Girl Sector 88 Gurgaon Delhi Just Call Me 9899900591
 

Neuroticism & PCS

  • 1. The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: a structural equations approach Liesbet Gouberta,b,*,1 , Geert Crombeza,b , Stefaan Van Dammea,b a Department of Experimental–Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium b Research Institute for Psychology and Health, Utrecht, The Netherlands Received 19 June 2003; received in revised form 27 October 2003; accepted 7 November 2003 Abstract The present study aimed at clarifying the precise role of pain catastrophizing, pain-related fear and personality dimensions in vigilance to pain and pain severity by means of structural equation modelling. A questionnaire survey was conducted in 122 patients with chronic or recurrent low back pain. Results revealed that pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear mediated the relationship between neuroticism and vigilance to pain. Furthermore, vigilance to pain was found to be associated with heightened pain severity. Finally, we found that neuroticism moderated the relationship between pain severity and catastrophic thinking about pain. The results strongly support the idea that vigilance to pain is dependent upon catastrophic thinking and pain-related fear. Neuroticism is best conceived of as a vulnerability factor; it lowers the threshold at which pain is perceived as threatening, and at which catastrophic thoughts about pain emerge. q 2003 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Vigilance; Pain catastrophizing; Pain-related fear; Personality; Neuroticism 1. Introduction The notion of heightened vigilance is often invoked to explain the heightened experience of pain (Aldrich et al., 2000; Chapman, 1978; McDermid et al., 1996). Although it is still a matter of debate which attentional processes are involved (Eysenck, 1992; Van Damme et al., 2003a,b), there is a growing consensus that hypervigilance is an emergent property of the threat value of pain (Chapman, 1978; Eccleston and Crombez, 1999): especially, persons who appraise bodily sensations as dangerous are believed to develop a habit of selecting pain over other competing demands. In line with this idea, self-reported vigilance to pain has been found to be strongly related to pain catastrophizing—an exaggerated negative interpretation of actual and anticipated pain experience—and pain-related fear—the fear of pain and activities associated with pain— (Roelofs et al., 2003). An unresolved issue pertains to the role of personality in vigilance topain.Ofthefivepersonalitydimensionsidentified by Costa and McCrae (1987), both neuroticism and extraver- sion seem to be the most plausible to have an effect upon vigilance to pain. In particular, neuroticism, which is the trait- like tendency to experience a broad range of negative feelings such as distress, worry, and anxiety, has been associated with a heightened experience of bodily sensations (Watson and Pennebaker, 1989; Geisser et al., 2000). According to Watson and Pennebaker (1989), persons scoring high on neuroticism are more likely to notice and attend to internal physical sensations and minor aches because their attentional scanning of both the external and internal environment is fraught with anxiety and uncertainty. Also the personality dimension introversion–extraversion may affect selective attention to pain (Eysenck, 1967). Psychophysiological studies indicated that introverts exhibit a greater sensitivity to physical stimuli than extraverts (Stelmack, 1990). Few studies have directly investigated the interdepen- dency of the above psychosocial variables in explaining vigilance to pain. According to the cognitive-affective model (Eccleston and Crombez, 1999), pain-related fear and pain catastrophizing are instantiations of the threat value of pain, and should therefore be considered as crucial Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 www.elsevier.com/locate/pain 0304-3959/$20.00 q 2003 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2003.11.005 1 Research Assistant of the Fund for Scientific Research, Flanders, Belgium, F.W.O. * Corresponding author. Address: Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium. Tel.: þ32-09-2646262; fax: þ32-09-2646489. E-mail address: liesbet.goubert@ugent.be (L. Goubert).
  • 2. mediators that account for the relationship between personality and vigilance to pain. However, to our knowl- edge, no study has provided a direct test of the putative mediating role of pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in a sample of chronic pain patients. This was, therefore, the first objective of this cross-sectional study. We designed and tested a structural equations’ model with the following assumptions: (1) vigilance to pain leads to the experience of more severe pain, (2) both pain catastrophizing and pain- related fear mediate the relationship between personality dimensions and vigilance to pain, and (3) pain catastrophiz- ing is a precursor to pain-related fear (Vlaeyen and Linton, 2000). A further objective was to explore the moderating role of neuroticism within a diathesis-stress framework (Eysenck, 1992). More specifically, we tested whether high- neurotic persons who experience pain are more prone to develop catastrophic thinking about pain and pain-related fear (see also Wade and Price, 2000). 2. Method 2.1. Participants One hundred and twenty-two patients with non-specific chronic or recurrent back pain (mean age ¼ 43.72, SD ¼ 9.56) were recruited from a physical revalidation unit and from an orthopaedic unit at two university hospitals. All participants were white Caucasians. The majority of participants were female (60.7%), 84.2% were married or cohabiting, and 22% had a higher education (longer than the age of 18 years). The average duration of pain since time of onset was 97.98 months (SD ¼ 104.17; range ¼ 3 months–45 years). More than half of the patients (54.1%) reported that their pain started gradually, 74.5% reported radiation of the pain, and 40.4% of the patients had undergone at least one spinal surgery. Almost one third of the patients (30.8%) were in paid employment and 37.5% of the patients were receiving worker’s compensation. 2.2. Questionnaires Patients completed a battery of questionnaires designed to assess the following constructs: pain severity, vigilance to pain, catastrophic thinking about pain, fear of movement/ (re)injury, and personality characteristics. Pain severity was assessed by the Dutch version of the Multidimensional Pain Inventory—Part 1 (MPI; Lousberg et al., 1999). Part 1 of the MPI-DV consists of five scales: pain severity (3 items), interference with daily life due to pain (11 items), perceived life control (4 items), affective distress (3 items) and social support (3 items). The Dutch version of the MPI has shown to have good reliability and validity (Lousberg et al., 1999). Vigilance to pain was assessed by the Dutch version of the Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire (PVAQ; McCracken, 1997; Roelofs et al., 2002, 2003). This is a 6-point 16-item measure of attention to pain that assesses awareness, consciousness and vigilance to pain. The PVAQ consists of two subscales: attention to pain (e.g. ‘I pay close attention to pain’) and attention to changes in pain (e.g. ‘I am quick to notice changes in pain intensity’) (Roelofs et al., 2003). The questionnaire can be used in both clinical (McCracken, 1997; Roelofs et al., 2003) and non-clinical (McWilliams and Asmundson, 2001; Roelofs et al., 2002) samples. The Dutch version of the PVAQ is reliable and valid (Roelofs et al., 2002, 2003). Catastrophic thinking about pain was assessed by the Dutch version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS; Sullivan et al., 1995; Crombez et al., 1998). This is a 13-item scale developed for both nonclinical and clinical populations. Participants reflect on past painful experiences and indicate the degree to which they experienced thoughts or feelings during pain on a 5-point scale. The Dutch version has been shown to have good reliability and validity in a student population and in a clinical population (Van Damme et al., 2002a). The PCS consists of three subscales (Van Damme et al., 2002a): rumination (e.g. ‘I can’t seem to keep it out of my mind’), magnification (e.g. ‘I become afraid that the pain will get worse’) and helplessness (e.g. ‘I feel I can’t stand it anymore’). Fear of (re)injury due to movement was measured by the Dutch version of the adjusted Tampa Scale for Kinesiopho- bia (TSK-AV; Kori et al., 1990; Goubert et al., 2003). It consists of 13 items measured on a 4-point scale with scoring alternatives ranging from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the adjusted version (in which the inversed items are omitted) more adequately fitted the data than the original version. The TSK-AV is sufficiently reliable and valid (Goubert et al., 2003). Confirmatory factor analyses revealed two valid subscales, namely harm (e.g. ‘My body is telling me I have something dangerously wrong’) and fear-avoidance (e.g., ‘Simply being careful that I do not make any unnecessary movements is the safest thing I can do to prevent my pain from worsening’) (Goubert et al., 2003). Personality was measured by the Dutch version of the Big Five Personality Questionnaire (NEO-FFI; Costa and McCrae, 1992; de Fruyt and Mervielde, 1998). This 60-item questionnaire measures five personality dimensions, i.e. neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Research has shown that the NEO-FFI is sufficiently reliable (a coefficients vary between 0.68 and 0.86) (de Fruyt and Mervielde, 1998). Also the construct- and concurrent validity has been well documented (de Fruyt and Mervielde, 1998). 2.3. Statistical analysis Structural equation modelling (SEM) was performed using LISREL 8.52 (Jo¨reskog and So¨rbom, 1993) with maximum likelihood estimation. In line with the recommen- dations of Bollen and Long (1993), several fit indices were L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 235
  • 3. used to assess model fit. In the present study, model fit was assessed using the following goodness-of-fit indices: x2 ; goodness-of-fit index (GFI), adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and comparative fit index (CFI). The most frequently used fit index is x2 . This likelihood- ratio test statistic is used to determine whether a significant amount of observed covariance between items remains unexplained by the model. A disadvantage of this index is its sensitivity to sample size. In a small sample, a poor fit may result in a non-significant x2 . It is also possible that in a large sample a good fit results in a statistically significant x2 (Marsh et al., 1988). In the present study, the x2 is the normal theory weighted least squares x2 as the other fit indices are based upon this index in the LISREL 8.52 program. The GFI and the AGFI (Jo¨reskog and So¨rbom, 1984) assess the extent to which the model provides a better fit compared to no model at all. These indices generally range between 0 and 1, with high values (GFI . 0.90 and AGFI . 0.80) reflecting a good fit of the model. The RMSEA (Browne and Cudeck, 1993) is a fit measure based on population error of approximation. The idea behind this is that it is unreasonable to assume that the model holds exactly in the population. The RMSEA takes into account the error of approximation in the population. According to Browne and Cudeck (1993), a RMSEA value of 0.05 indicates a close fit and values up to 0.08 represent reasonable errors of approximation in the population. Finally, the CFI (Bentler, 1990a) is an incremental fit index that produces a statistic in the range between 0 and 1. It represents the proportionate improvement in model fit by comparing the target model with a baseline model (usually a null model in which all the observed variables are uncorrelated). CFI values larger than 0.90 indicate an adequate fit (Bentler, 1990b). Furthermore, we investigated whether neuroticism mod- erated the relationship between pain severity and both pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear. To test for neuroticism as a moderator, it is necessary to enter the cross-product terms of neuroticism and pain severity in a separate block in a hierarchical regression analysis, following the entry of pain severity and neuroticism as first-order terms (Baron and Kenny, 1986). To reduce the effects of multicollinearity, variables were centered (Aiken and West, 1991). Statistically significant interactions are interpreted by plotting regression lines for high and low values of the moderator variable (Aiken and West, 1991; Holmbeck, 2002). 3. Results 3.1. Patient characteristics In comparison with the MPI-DV results of a patient group entering a cognitive-behavioural rehabilitation pro- gram (Lousberg et al., 1999), the severity of the pain complaints in the present patient sample was moderate to average. The mean score of the pain severity scale was 3.89 (SD ¼ 1.17; mean of comparison group ¼ 4.50; tð121Þ ¼ 25:51; P , 0:001) and the mean score of the interference scale was 4.25 (SD ¼ 1.13; mean of compari- son group ¼ 4.52; tð121Þ ¼ 22:53; P , 0:05Þ: 3.2. Preliminary analyses Table 1 presents the means, SD, Cronbach’s a and Pearson intercorrelations for each of the self-report variables. Vigilance to pain was significantly correlated with pain severity, pain catastrophizing, fear of movement/ (re)injury and the personality dimensions of neuroticism and agreeableness. Furthermore, pain catastrophizing was significantly correlated with pain severity, fear of move- ment/(re)injury, and the personality dimensions of neuroti- cism, extraversion, openness to experience and agreeableness. In addition, fear of movement/(re)injury was significantly correlated with pain severity, neuroticism, openness to experience and agreeableness. 3.3. Value of the personality dimensions in predicting vigilance to pain A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was per- formed to investigate the unique contribution of each personality dimension to the prediction of vigilance to pain. In a first step, age and gender (males were coded as 0 and Table 1 Means ðMÞ; standard deviations (SD), internal consistency ðaÞ and intercorrelations of all measures M (SD) a 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 Vigilance to pain 46.81 (11.02) 0.81 0.21* 0.60*** 0.56*** 0.31** 20.14 20.02 20.19* 0.10 2 Pain severity 3.89 (1.17) 0.84 – 0.30** 0.29** 0.06 20.18 20.21* 20.04 20.02 3 Pain catastrophizing 21.38 (8.18) 0.89 – 0.58*** 0.33*** 20.19* 20.21* 20.24** 20.04 4 Fear of movement/(re)injury 32.71 (7.31) 0.78 – 0.28** 20.15 20.19* 20.28** 20.08 5 Neuroticism 34.25 (7.50) 0.80 – 20.50*** 20.25** 20.28** 20.36*** 6 Extraversion 41.56 (6.44) 0.75 – 0.36*** 0.19* 0.39*** 7 Openness to experience 36.67 (6.18) 0.69 – 0.19* 0.27** 8 Agreeableness 45.43 (5.84) 0.73 – 0.22* 9 Conscientiousness 47.82 (5.58) 0.79 – *P , 0.05; **P , 0.01; ***p , 0.0005 L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241236
  • 4. females as 1) were entered into the equation to control for sociodemographic variables. In a second step, pain severity and pain duration were entered. Next, the five personality dimensions were entered. Variance-inflation factors (range 1.00–1.66) were acceptable, suggesting that there was no problem of collinearity. Our results revealed that of the five personality dimensions only neuroticism and conscientious- ness had a unique contribution in predicting vigilance to pain (see Table 2). As only neuroticism proved (1) to have a significant zero-order association with vigilance to pain, and (2) to be a unique predictor of vigilance to pain in the multiple regression analysis, further analyses were only performed with this personality dimension. 3.4. Model testing In order to use SEM, latent constructs of the observed data were constructed. Fear of movement/(re)injury as a latent construct was specified by the two subscales of the TSK-AV: TSK-harm and TSK-fear. The latent construct pain catastrophizing was indicated by the three subscales of the PCS: PCS-rumination, PCS-magnification and PCS- helplessness. Vigilance to pain was specified as a latent construct measured by the two subscales of the PVAQ: PVAQ-vigilance to pain and PVAQ-vigilance to changes in pain. Because neuroticism was measured by one variable we fixed the error variance to 0 and the loading value to 1. Finally, pain severity as a latent construct was indicated by the three items of the pain severity scale of the MPI. Missing values were omitted, leaving valid data of 121 patients. Before the structural model was tested we used confirmatory factor analysis to examine the measurement model. It showed a good fit of the data, x2 ð35Þ ¼ 53:26 ðP ¼ 0:02Þ; RMSEA ¼ 0.07 (90% Confidence interval: 0.02–0.10), CFI ¼ 0.97, GFI ¼ 0.93 and AGFI ¼ 0.86. This indicates that the subscales of the TSK-AV, PCS, PVAQ and MPI may be considered as valid operationalisations of the latent constructs fear of movement/(re)injury, pain catastrophiz- ing, vigilance to pain and pain severity. All subscales of the questionnaires loaded significantly on the appropriate latent construct. Results revealed that our hypothesized structural model had a good fit (see Fig. 1): x2 ð40Þ ¼ 66:45 ðP ¼ 0:005Þ; RMSEA ¼ 0.07 (90% confidence interval: 0.04–0.10), CFI ¼ 0.96, GFI ¼ 0.91 and AGFI ¼ 0.85. As expected, our findings show that pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury mediated the relationship between neuroticism and vigilance to pain. Testing a structural Fig. 1. Structural equation model showing the role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury in vigilance to pain. NEO-N, neuroticism subscale of the NEO-FFI; RUM, rumination subscale of the PCS; MAGN, magnification subscale of the PCS; HELP, helplessness subscale of the PCS; HARM, harm subscale of the TSK-AV; FEAR-AV, fear-avoidance subscale of the TSK-AV; PVAQ-AP, vigilance to pain subscale of the PVAQ; PVAQ- ACP, vigilance to changes in pain subscale of the PVAQ. *P , 0:05: Table 2 Hierarchical regression analysis predicting vigilance to pain from age, sex, pain severity, pain duration, neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness Criterion variable Step Predictor b DR2 Adjusted R2 Vigilance to pain 1 Age 20.10 0.03 0.02 Sex 20.16 2 Pain severity 0.20* 0.06* 0.06 Pain duration 20.09 3 Neuroticism 0.36** 0.16*** 0.19 Extraversion 20.07 Openness to experience 0.08 Agreeableness 20.08 Conscientiousness 0.26** *P , 0:05; **P , 0:01; ***P , 0:005. Standardized bs are displayed. L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 237
  • 5. model in which pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/ (re)injury changed places, also revealed a good model fit: x2 ð40Þ ¼ 61:40 ðP ¼ 0:02Þ; RMSEA ¼ 0.07 (90% confi- dence interval: 0.03–0.10), CFI ¼ 0.96, GFI ¼ 0.91 and AGFI ¼ 0.86. The direct link between neuroticism and vigilance to pain was not significant. Finally, vigilance to pain was positively associated with the experience of more severe pain. Standardized b-coefficients and R2 values are shown in Fig. 1, with R2 values shown above each endogenous variable. 3.5. Value of the personality dimensions in predicting pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury To investigate the unique value of the personality dimensions in predicting pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury, two multiple regression analyses were performed. Variance-inflation factors (range 1.00–1.66) were acceptable, suggesting that there was no problem of collinearity. The first analysis revealed that only neuroti- cism had a unique contribution in predicting pain catastro- phizing, beyond age, gender (males were coded as 0 and females as 1), pain duration and pain severity (see Table 3). None of the other personality dimensions had a significant contribution. A similar analysis with fear of movement/ (re)injury as dependent variable revealed identical results. Only neuroticism had a unique contribution in predicting fear of movement/(re)injury (see Table 3). Based upon these results, it was decided to perform only moderation analyses with the personality dimension ‘neuroticism’. 3.6. Moderation analyses 3.6.1. Moderational role of neuroticism in the relationship between pain severity and pain catastrophizing We examined whether the interaction variable (pain severity £ neuroticism) was a significant predictor of pain catastrophizing, after controlling for the effects of pain severity and neuroticism (Aiken and West, 1991; Holmbeck, 2002). Significant main effects were found for pain severity ðb ¼ 0:31; P , 0:0005Þ and neuroticism ðb ¼ 0:30; P , 0:0005Þ: Also the interaction pain severity £ neuroticism was a significant predictor ðb ¼ 0:16; P , 0:05Þ; revealing that the association between pain severity and pain catastrophizing is conditional on the values of neuroticism. To illustrate the pattern reflected in this statistically significant interaction term, we plotted regression lines for high (þ1 SD above the mean) and low (21 SD below the mean) values of the moderator variable (see Aiken and West, 1991; Holmbeck, 2002, Fig. 2). Significance tests for both slopes indicated that the slope for the high-neuroticism regression line was significant (b ¼ 0:46; P , 0:0005), indicating that pain catastrophizing tends to be more pronounced at higher levels of pain severity when neuroti- cism is high. The slope for the low-neuroticism regression line was not significant ðb ¼ 0:15; nsÞ: 3.6.2. Moderational role of neuroticism in the relationship between pain severity and fear of movement/(re)injury We entered the cross-product terms of neuroticism and pain severity in a separate block in a hierarchical regression Table 3 Hierarchical regression analyses predicting pain catastrophizing and fear of movement/(re)injury from age, sex, pain severity, pain duration, neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness Criterion variable Step Predictor b DR2 Adjusted R2 Pain catastrophizing 1 Age 20.07 0.01 0.00 Sex 20.06 2 Pain severity 0.26*** 0.12*** 0.11 Pain duration 20.14 3 Neuroticism 0.30*** 0.13*** 0.20 Extraversion 20.02 Openness to experience 20.09 Agreeableness 20.13 Conscientiousness 0.13 Fear of movement/(re)injury 1 Age 0.06 0.06* 0.05 Sex 20.21* 2 Pain severity 0.25*** 0.11*** 0.15 Pain duration 20.17 3 Neuroticism 0.27** 0.09* 0.21 Extraversion 0.05 Openness to experience 20.05 Agreeableness 20.12 Conscientiousness 0.05 *P , 0:05; **P , 0:01; ***P , 0:005: Standardized betas are displayed. L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241238
  • 6. analysis, following the entry of pain severity and neuroticism as first-order terms. We found significant main effects of pain severity ðb ¼ 0:28; P , 0:001Þ and neuroticism ðb ¼ 0:25; P , 0:01Þ: The interaction term pain severity £ neuroticism, however, was not a significant predictor of fear of movement/(re)injury ðb ¼ 0:14; P , 0:10Þ: Although the interaction term was not significant, we performed a post- hoc probing analysis. The results revealed similar results as for pain catastrophizing: the slope for the high-neuroticism regression line was again significant (b ¼ 0.42, P , 0.0005), indicating that fear of movement/(re)injury tends to be more pronounced at higher levels of pain severity when neuroticism is high. The slope for the low-neuroticism regression line was not significant (b ¼ 0.15, ns). 4. Discussion The present study aimed at clarifying the role of catastrophic thinking about pain, pain-related fear and neuroticism in its relationship with vigilance to pain and pain severity. The results can be readily summarized. First, vigilance to pain was related to the experience of more severe pain. Second, of the five personality dimensions that were investigated, only neuroticism was found to be consistently related to vigilance to pain, pain catastrophiz- ing and fear of movement/(re)injury. Third, pain catastro- phizing and fear of movement/(re)injury mediated the relationship between neuroticism and vigilance to pain. Fourth, neuroticism was found to moderate the relationship between pain severity and pain catastrophizing. Analyses predicting pain-related fear were in the same line, but only showed a trend to significance. Of particular interest to this study was the finding that the effect of neuroticism upon pain vigilance is largely mediated by pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear. This is not in line with the symptom perception model of Watson and Pennebaker (1989), which postulates a direct effect of neuroticism on vigilance to pain. Our results support the idea that vigilance to pain is critically dependent upon its immediate threat value (Chapman, 1978; Eccleston and Crombez, 1999; Price, 1999). Our findings point out that neuroticism may be conceived of as a vulnerability factor within a diathesis-stress frame- work. When a person is confronted with the stressor of (acute) pain, neuroticism may influence whether the person appraises the pain as threatening or not (see also Sullivan et al., 2003; Thorn et al., 2003). Neuroticism may lower the threshold at which pain is perceived as threatening, and at which pain elicits catastrophic thoughts. For high-neurotic individuals, a low pain severity is sufficient to be perceived as threatening, and consequently, to provoke catastrophic thoughts about pain. Our results argue for a modern and dynamic view on personality instead of a traditional personality approach that stresses cross-situational stability of personality traits. In a dynamic view, behaviours are not assumed to be stable across situations, but vary depending on the specific situation (Mischel and Shoda, 1998). Personality is then characterized by stable situation- behaviour (if…then) relations. This dynamic view is similar to a diathesis-stress framework in which personality traits influence the way in which people cognitively process the meanings that chronic pain holds for their life (Wade et al., 1992). The personality dimensions ‘extraversion’, ‘openness’, ‘agreeableness’ and ‘conscientiousness’ do not seem to be of crucial importance in the emergence of vigilance to pain. We found no evidence for the idea that introverts tended to be more vigilant to pain (see Stelmack, 1990). The effects of conscientiousness upon vigilance to pain are preliminary, and await replication. They are however intriguing. As conscientious individuals carefully plan before acting and avoid situations involving risk or danger (Watson et al., 1994), it is possible that they become frustrated when confronted with the insolubility of the problem of chronic pain and its uncontrollable effects upon daily life. Chronic pain may then be experienced as (partly) taking over control, and dominating awareness. Neuroticism was also the only personality dimension that uniquely accounted for pain catastrophizing and pain- related fear. Correlational analyses however showed that pain catastrophizing was negatively related to extraversion, openness to experience, and agreeableness. The latter two personality characteristics were also negatively related to pain-related fear. This is in line with the findings of Jerram and Coleman (1999), who found that openness to experience and agreeableness were associated with positive health perceptions. Regarding extraversion, our results suggest that extraverted people have fewer catastrophic thoughts about pain. This is in line with the suggestion of Phillips and Gatchel (2000), that extraversion–introversion is a factor that affects how a person experiences and responds to pain. Fig. 2. Regression lines for the relationship between pain severity and pain catastrophizing as moderated by neuroticism. b, unstandardized regression coefficient (i.e. simple slope); SD, standard deviation. *P , 0:0005: L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 239
  • 7. The results of this study have a number of important clinical implications. When pain is perceived as highly threatening, patients become vigilant to pain, and may experience difficulties to disengage attention from pain- related information (Van Damme et al., 2002b, 2003a). As a consequence, attentional distraction strategies may not be effective in these patients. It may be more effective to diminish the threat value of pain by means of cognitive restructuring techniques (Thorn and Williams, 1993; Warwick et al., 1996) or exposure techniques that disconfirm the belief of catastrophic outcomes (Crombez et al., 2002; Goubert et al., 2002; Vlaeyen et al., 2002). This study has a number of limitations, each of which point to directions for future research. First, all findings are based on cross-sectional and correlational data. Significant correlations do not indicate causal effects. In future research, this structural equations model needs to be applied to longitudinal data, in order to provide firm evidence on the causal relations between the variables. Second, the variables in this study were assessed only using self-report instru- ments. Further research should include multiple methods to assess vigilance to pain, including experimental and psychophysiological procedures. A third limitation is that neuroticism is probably not the only variable that accounts for pain catastrophizing (see Rainville et al., 2000). Other pathways may also lead to pain catastrophizing and pain- related fear. Sullivan et al. (2001) proposed that a learning history characterized by heightened pain experience, may itself lead to the development of a pain schema, containing threatening information and catastrophic expectancies about painfulstimuli. Fourth, although conscientiousness provedto be a unique predictor of vigilance to pain in the multiple regression analysis, the zero-order correlation was not significant. Further research into the potential role of personality characteristics, in particular of conscientiousness and neuroticism, in vigilance to pain and in the development of pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear is needed. Acknowledgements The authors want to thank Petra De Vlieger, Els Persijn, Leen Van Vlierberghe, Stephen Morley and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. References Aiken LS, West SG, Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions, Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991. Aldrich S, Eccleston C, Crombez G. Worrying about chronic pain: vigilance to threat and misdirected problem solving. Behav Res Ther 2000;38:457–70. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:1173–82. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychol Bull 1990a;107:238–46. Bentler PM. Fit indices, Lagrange multipliers, constraint changes and incomplete data in structural models. Multivariate Behav Res 1990b;25: 163–72. Bollen KA, Long JS. Introduction. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. p. 1–9. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. p. 136–62. Chapman CR. Pain: the perception of noxious events. In: Sternbach RA, editor. The psychology of pain. New York: Raven Press; 1978. p. 169–202. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: is the bark worse than the bite? J Pers 1987;55:299–316. Costa PT, McCrae RR, Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five factor inventory professional manual, Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. Crombez G, Eccleston C, Baeyens F, Eelen P. When somatic information threatens, catastrophic thinking enhances attentional interference. Pain 1998;75:187–98. Crombez G, Eccleston C, Vlaeyen JWS, Vansteenwegen D, Lysens R, Eelen P. Exposure to physical movement in low back pain patients: restricted effects of generalization. Health Psychol 2002;21:573–8. de Fruyt F, Mervielde I. The assessment of the big five in the Dutch language domain. Psychol Belg 1998;38:1–22. Eccleston C, Crombez G. Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull 1999;125: 356–66. Eysenck HJ. The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas; 1967. Eysenck MW. Anxiety: the cognitive perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992. Geisser ME, Roth RS, Theisen ME, Robinson ME, Riley JL. Negative affect, self-report of depressive symptoms, and clinical depression: relation to the experience of chronic pain. Clin J Pain 2000;16: 110–20. Goubert L, Crombez G, Van Damme S, Vlaeyen JWS, Bijttebier P, Roelofs J. Confirmatory factor analysis of the tampa scale for kinesiophobia: invariant two-factor model across low back pain patients and fibromyalgia patients. Clin J Pain 2003; in press. Goubert L, Francken G, Crombez G, Vansteenwegen D, Lysens R. Exposure to physical movement in chronic back pain patients: no evidence for generalization across different movements. Behav Res Ther 2002;40:415–29. Holmbeck GN. Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and media- tional effects in studies of pediatric populations. J Pediatr Psychol 2002; 27:87–96. Jerram KL, Coleman PG. The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age. Br J Health Psychol 1999;4:181–92. Jo¨reskog KG, So¨rbom D, LISREL VI user’s guide, Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software; 1984. Jo¨reskog KG, So¨rbom D, LISREL 8: structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS language, Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc; 1993. Kori SH, Miller RP, Todd DD. Kinisiophobia: a new view of chronic pain behavior. Pain Manage 1990;35–43. Lousberg R, Van Breukelen GJP, Groenman NH, Schmidt AJM, Arntz A, Winter FAM. Psychometric properties of the multidimensional pain inventory, Dutch language version (MPI-DLV). Behav Res Ther 1999; 37:167–82. Marsh HW, Balla JR, McDonald RP. Goodness-of-fit indexes in confirmatory factor analysis: the effect of sample size. Psychol Bull 1988;103:391–410. McCracken LM. Attention to pain in persons with chronic pain: a behavioral approach. Behav Ther 1997;28:271–84. L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241240
  • 8. McDermid AJ, Rollman GB, McCain GA. Generalized hypervigilance in fibromyalgia: evidence of perceptual amplification. Pain 1996;66: 133–44. McWilliams LA, Asmundson GJG. Assessing individual differences in attention to pain: psychometric properties of the pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire modified for a non-clinical pain sample. Pers Individ Differ 2001;31:239–46. Mischel W, Shoda Y. Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annu Rev Psychol 1998;49:229–58. Phillips JM, Gatchel RJ. Extraversion-introversion and chronic pain. In: Gatchel RJ, Weisberg JN, editors. Personality characteristics of patients with pain. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2000. p. 181–202. Price DD. The dimensions of pain experience. In: Price DD, editor. Psychological mechanisms of pain and analgesia. Seattle, WA: IASP Press; 1999. p. 43–70. Rainville J, Carlson N, Polatin P, Gatchel RJ, Indahl A. Exploration of physicians’ recommendations for activities in chronic low back pain. Spine 2000;25:2210–20. Roelofs J, Peters ML, Muris P, Vlaeyen JWS. Dutch version of the pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire: validity and reliability in a pain- free population. Behav Res Ther 2002;40:1081–90. Roelofs J, Peters ML, McCracken L, Vlaeyen JWS. The pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire (PVAQ): further psychometric evaluation in fibromyalgiaandother chronic pain syndromes.Pain 2003;101:299–306. Stelmack RM. Biological bases of extraversion: psychophysiological evidence. J Pers 1990;1:293–311. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess 1995;7:524–32. Sullivan MJL, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, Bradley LA, Lefebvre JC. Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain 2001;17:52–64. Sullivan MJL, Thorn BE, Rodgers W, Ward LC. A path model of psychological antecedents to pain experience: experimental and clinical findings. Clin J Pain 2003; in press. Thorn BE, Williams DA. Cognitive-behavioral management of chronic pain. In: VandeCreek L, Knapp S, Jackson TL, editors. Innovations in clinical practice: a source book, vol. 12. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press/Professional Resource Exchange; 1993. p. 169–91. Thorn BE, Clements KL, Ward C, Dixon KE, Kersh BC, Boothby JL, Chaplin WF. Personality factors in the explanation of sex differences in pain catastrophizing and response to experimental pain. Clin J Pain 2003; in press. Van Damme S, Crombez G, Bijttebier P, Goubert L, Van Houdenhove B. A confirmatory factor analysis of the pain catastrophizing scale: invariant factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populations. Pain 2002a;96:319–24. Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Retarded disengagement from pain cues: the effects of pain catastrophizing and pain expectancy. Pain 2002b;100:111–8. Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Impaired disengagement from pain: the role of catastrophic thinking about pain. Pain 2003a; in press. Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C, Roelofs J. The role of hypervigilance in the experience of pain. In: Asmundson GJG, Vlaeyen JWS, Crombez G, editors. Understanding and treating fear of pain. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003b. in press. Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain 2000;85:317–32. Vlaeyen JWS, de Jong J, Geilen M, Heuts PHTG, van Breukelen G. The treatment of fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain: further evidence on the effectiveness of exposure in vivo. Clin J Pain 2002;18:251–61. Wade JB, Price DD. Nonpathological factors in chronic pain: Implications for assessment and treatment. In: Gatchel RJ, Weisberg JN, editors. Personality characteristics of patients with pain. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2000. p. 89–108. Wade JB, Dougherty LM, Hart RP, Rafii A, Price DD. A canonical correlation analysis of the influence of neuroticism and extraversion on chronic pain, suffering, and pain behaviour. Pain 1992;51:67–73. Warwick HMC, Clark DM, Cobb AM, Salkovskis PM. A controlled trial of cognitive-behavioural treatment of hypochondriasis. Br J Psychiatry 1996;169:189–95. Watson D, Clark LA, Harkness AR. Structures of personality and their relevance to psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103:18–31. Watson D, Pennebaker JW. Health complaints, stress, and distress: exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychol Rev 1989; 96:234–54. L. Goubert et al. / Pain 107 (2004) 234–241 241