SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable for
Maintaining Problem Behavior in the School Setting
Jana M. Sarno and Heather E. Sterling
The University of Southern Mississippi
Michael M. Mueller
Southern Behavioral Group
Brad Dufrene, Daniel H. Tingstrom, and D. Joe Olmi
The University of Southern Mississippi
Abstract. Mueller, Sterling-Turner, and Moore (2005) reported a
novel escape-
to-attention (ETA) functional analysis condition in a school
setting with one child.
The current study replicates Mueller et al.’s functional analysis
procedures with
three elementary school-age boys referred for problem behavior.
Functional
analysis verified the participant’s problem behavior was
maintained by escape
from academic demands. Follow-up functional analyses in
which target behaviors
in escape versus ETA conditions were compared resulted in
higher levels of target
behavior in the ETA condition for 2 of the 3 participants. The
current study also
extended previous research by including a treatment analysis.
Treatments de-
signed to address escape and attention functions were more
effective at reducing
the target behaviors than treatments designed to target escape
alone for all 3
participants. Results and implications for future research are
discussed.
Incorporating experimental analyses
into a functional behavioral assessment is an
effective and time-efficient approach for the
assessment and treatment of problem behavior
(Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003; Mueller,
Sterling-Turner, & Moore, 2005; Mueller,
Nkosi, & Hine, in press). The functional anal-
ysis methodology developed by Iwata, Dorsey,
Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982) is an
analogue evaluation of problem behavior in
which purported reinforcers are withheld and
then delivered contingent upon target behav-
ior. In their original work, Iwata and col-
leagues measured levels of target behaviors
during experimental conditions (i.e., attention,
escape, alone) and compared the data to levels
of target behavior in a control condition in
which the reinforcers were available noncon-
tingently. Iwata et al.’s methodology has been
used extensively to identify the behavioral
function of self-injurious behavior in clinical
settings and has been used with a variety of
behaviors and in other nonclinical settings.
Although use of functional analysis proce-
This article was taken, in part, from the first author’s thesis.
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to
Heather E. Sterling, The University of
Southern Mississippi, 118 College Drive, #5025, Hattiesburg,
MS 39406; E-mail: [email protected]
Copyright 2011 by the National Association of School
Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015
School Psychology Review,
2011, Volume 40, No. 1, pp. 57–71
57
dures is reported less commonly in school
settings (Hanley et al., 2003), studies have
been reported with examples of disruptive
school-based behaviors reinforced by peer at-
tention (e.g., Broussard & Northup, 1997),
teacher attention, (e.g., Gunter, Jack, Shores,
Carrell, & Flowers, 1993), access to tangible
items (e.g., Moore, Mueller, Dubard, Roberts,
& Sterling-Turner, 2002), and escape from
academic demands (e.g., Broussard & Nor-
thup, 1995).
Although a functional behavioral assess-
ment, including experimental analysis, may
not be necessary to address all disruptive be-
haviors in school settings (Gresham et al.,
2004), additional research on the effect of
idiosyncratic variables is needed. In school
settings, as in other nonclinical settings,
unique environmental variables (e.g., setting,
personnel, physical) could require modifica-
tions to the standard functional analysis con-
ditions typically reported. For example, in
school settings, tasks in the form of academic
demands (e.g., ongoing instruction, indepen-
dent practice worksheets) are, at least theoret-
ically, present throughout the majority of the
day. Likewise, concurrent and potentially
competing reinforcers in the form of peer at-
tention, teacher attention, or preferred activi-
ties (e.g., reading a more desirable book) or
items (e.g., playing with a toy hidden in a
desk) for inappropriate behavior may be pres-
ent. Thus, students may be provided with es-
cape from academic demands, while subse-
quently being provided with an additional re-
inforcer for problem behavior. Because
student behavior can be under the discrimina-
tive control of multiple antecedent events or
reinforced by multiple variables (e.g., teacher
and peer attention, access to preferred materials,
breaks from work), it is important to examine a
combination of factors that may be maintaining
problem behavior in the classroom.
Over the past few years, investigations
of the effects of multiple variables have begun
in and out of the classroom. For example,
Hoff, Ervin, and Friman (2005) examined the
separate and combined effects of escape and
peer attention on disruptive behavior in the
general education classroom. Following a de-
scriptive assessment, including interviews and
direct observations, Hoff and colleagues for-
mulated three hypotheses to test in an alter-
nating treatments design: access to peer atten-
tion, escape from a nonpreferred activity, and
access to peer attention and escape from a
nonpreferred activity. Treatment analysis data
verified the initial hypothesis of access to peer
attention and escape from academic demands.
In addition, a combined intervention targeting
both attention and escape decreased problem
behaviors to near zero levels.
Moore, Mueller et al. (2002) investi-
gated the influence of the simultaneous deliv-
ery of therapist attention on self-injurious be-
havior in a tangible condition. Following the
initial functional analysis, attention in the tan-
gible condition was evaluated using a reversal
design. In one phase, juice and brief attention
were delivered contingent on self-injurious be-
havior. In the second phase, the delivery of the
preferred stimulus (juice) was returned contin-
gent on problem behavior and attention was
withheld. The results of the follow-up analysis
demonstrated that self-injurious behavior oc-
curred at higher rates when the juice and at-
tention were delivered concurrently than when
the juice was presented alone.
By incorporating procedural variations
in the functional analysis methodology,
Moore, Mueller et al. (2002) demonstrated
that the presence of attention could confound
the outcomes of functional analysis condi-
tions. Moore and colleagues hypothesized that
“practical solutions for the tangible condition
might be to restrict attention as much as pos-
sible or to weaken the dependency between
problem behavior and therapist attention by
delivering attention on a response-independent
schedule” (p. 284). However, Moore, Mueller
et al. did not present treatment data to support
their hypothesis. It is conceivable, though, as
the authors suggested, that the influence of
attention might affect other consequent analy-
sis conditions.
In Mann and Mueller (2009), the func-
tional analysis results of a girl’s aggression
appeared to be maintained by attention. The
results of the functional analysis of her behav-
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
58
ior showed high levels of aggression in the
attention condition and low levels in an escape
from academic demand, access to tangibles,
and a toy play control. When she failed to
acquire a functional communicative response
to replace aggression for attention, a follow-up
functional analysis was used to evaluate
whether access to attention was part of a chain
of reinforcers maintaining aggression. In the
follow-up analysis, attention-to-tangibles, at-
tention-to-escape, and attention alone as a
control condition were each used. Aggression
was high in the attention-to-tangibles condi-
tion and low in the attention-to-escape and the
attention-only control. When functional com-
munication training was used to address the
attention-to-tangibles (i.e., manding for access
to tangibles and attention, rather than attention
only), the response was acquired and the ag-
gression decreased. These results highlight
two issues relevant for school-based func-
tional analysis. First, Iwata et al.’s (1982)
methodology is useful, even if structural vari-
ants to assess multiple reinforcers are re-
quired. Second, for behaviors maintained by
multiple reinforcers, matching treatments to
both reinforcers may be required to reduce
target behaviors substantially.
Mueller et al. (2005) provided pilot data
for an ETA condition used in a school setting.
For one child, a functional analysis with an
escape, attention, and toy play conditions was
conducted using the procedures described by
Iwata et al. (1982). Results of initial functional
analysis showed that problem behavior only
occurred in the escape from academic demand
condition, however, lower than that typically
was observed in the classroom setting. After
escape was identified by the initial functional
analysis, the researchers assessed a combina-
tion of variables to determine whether differ-
ential levels of problem behavior would occur
with the addition of attention during the break
from academic demands, as this was observed
in the direct behavioral observation prior to
the initial functional analysis. In the follow-up
functional analysis, the escape-only, ETA, and
control conditions were presented. A substan-
tially higher level of tantrums was demon-
strated in the ETA condition than in the es-
cape-alone condition or the control conditions.
Mueller et al. (2005) hypothesized that
without the information derived from the fol-
low-up analysis, an intervention based on the
escape-only hypothesis would have failed. Al-
though the results of Mann and Mueller (2009)
provide some support for this hypothesis, Mu-
eller et al. (2005) did not provide any inter-
vention data. Other limitations should be ad-
dressed as well. First, the investigation was a
pilot study of the ETA condition and involved
only one participant. Another limitation was
that the consultant collected all data and, be-
cause of staffing issues, no interobserver
agreement data (IOA) were collected.
Given the limitations of Mueller et al.
(2005), the current study was undertaken with
two primary goals. First, we replicated Muel-
ler et al.’s ETA investigation with additional
participants and in a more controlled manner,
including IOA data and multiple behavioral
observers, to determine whether the ETA
function would emerge in additional partici-
pants. The second goal was to extend Mueller
et al. by evaluating two different behavioral
interventions, one that presented an escape-
only treatment and one that was matched to
both functions (escape and teacher attention).
We predicted that differential treatment results
would emerge for students who showed higher
levels of problem behavior in the ETA condi-
tion, with stronger treatment effects favoring
the combined treatment for children with an
ETA function when compared to children with
escape-maintained problem only.
Method
Participants and Setting
Three elementary school-age boys re-
ferred for problem classroom behavior partic-
ipated. All students were enrolled in public
schools and were placed in general education
classrooms in a rural Southeastern school dis-
trict. Teacher and parental consent were se-
cured for participation; participant names used
hereafter are pseudonyms. Brandon was a
6-year-old Caucasian male enrolled in a gen-
eral education first-grade classroom. Brandon
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
59
was diagnosed with attention deficit hyperac-
tivity disorder (combined type) when he was
5-years-old and was prescribed a 10-mg dose
patch of methylphenidate (Daytrana). Franklin
and J’Marcus were 5-year-old African Amer-
ican males enrolled in separate general educa-
tion kindergarten classrooms. J’Marcus and
Franklin had no medical diagnoses and were
prescribed no medications.
All sessions were conducted in the par-
ticipants’ classrooms during typically sched-
uled activities that corresponded to teacher-
reported times when problem behaviors were
most frequent. The students’ classroom teach-
ers implemented all functional analyses and
treatment evaluation sessions.
Measures
Functional Assessment Informant Re-
cord for Teachers (FAIR-T). The FAIR-T is
an instrument administered to teachers to gen-
erate hypotheses concerning the function of
problem behavior (Edwards, 2002). The
FAIR-T is designed with four components to
achieve this purpose: (a) general referral in-
formation, (b) identification and description of
problem behavior, (c) potential antecedents
for problem behavior, and (d) potential conse-
quences that follow the problem behavior
most frequently. Researchers have demon-
strated that the hypotheses generated from in-
formation gathered via the FAIR-T correspond
with behavioral function identified in experi-
mental analyses (e.g., Doggett, Edwards,
Moore, Tingstrom, & Wilczynski, 2001; Du-
frene, Doggett, Henington, & Watson, 2007).
Intervention Rating Profile-15 (IRP-
15). The Intervention Rating Profile-15 (IRP-
15; Martens, Witt, Elliott, & Darveaux, 1985)
was used as a social validity measure of the
treatment conditions. The IRP-15 is composed
of 15 questions that the respondent rates on a
Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly dis-
agree) to 6 (strongly agree). Ratings range
from a total score of 15–90, where a total
score above 52.50 represents a rating of “ac-
ceptable” (Von Brock & Elliott, 1987). The
IRP-15 has high reported internal consistency
(Cronbach � � .98), and all items load on a
General Acceptability Factor (ranging from 0.82
to 0.95; Martens et al., 1985).
Problem behavior. Child problem be-
havior served as the primary dependent vari-
able and was reported as the percentage of
intervals in which the behavior occurred.
Problem behavior included: inappropriate vo-
calizations (Brandon, J’Marcus, Franklin),
which was defined as talking or yelling with-
out teacher permission; elopement (J’Marcus),
which was defined as any movement 1 m away
from the teacher or teacher-designated area
without permission; and banging on surfaces
(Franklin), which was defined as throwing ac-
ademic materials in a downward motion to the
desk, and/or floor so that it made an audible
sound on impact. Additional data were also
collected for task engagement during the treat-
ment evaluation phases. Task engagement was
defined as the student’s eyes directed at work
materials and/or manipulating objects associ-
ated with the teacher command. Task engage-
ment is presented as the percentage of inter-
vals in which behavior was observed during a
session. A 10-s partial interval recording sys-
tem was used for all observations. All sessions
were 10 min in length.
Procedures
First, functional behavioral assessments
that included teacher interview and direct
classroom observations were conducted to
generate hypotheses of behavioral function.
Second, functional analyses were used to ver-
ify escape from task demands as the maintain-
ing variable for referred behaviors. Third, fol-
low-up functional analyses were used to in-
vestigate the additive effects of attention
delivered during the break from academic de-
mands (i.e., ETA). Finally, two different treat-
ments were compared to examine the effects
on target behavior when an escape-only treat-
ment was alternated with an intervention pack-
age that targeted escape and attention.
Functional behavior assessment.
Each teacher was administered the FAIR-T as
a semistructured interview in order to define
target behaviors and their immediate anteced-
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
60
ent and consequent events. Next, direct behav-
ioral observations were conducted in the stu-
dents’ classrooms. The information obtained
from the functional assessment was used to
form hypotheses about potential behavior-re-
inforcer relationships. Conditional probabili-
ties (VanDerHeyden, Witt, & Gatti, 2001)
were also calculated from the observational
data to determine the temporal proximity of
specific consequences and target behaviors.
Conditional probabilities for each participant
were as follows: Brandon—escape � 74%,
teacher attention � 24%, peer attention � 0%;
Franklin—escape � 66%, teacher attention �
25%; peer attention � 5%; J’Marcus—es-
cape � 59%, teacher attention � 29%, peer
attention � 9%. Descriptive data suggested
escape from academic demands or teacher at-
tention in the form of reprimands and redirec-
tion to work might be reinforcing the target
behaviors identified for each child. Thus, each
child proceeded to the experimental phases of
the study reported below.
Functional analysis. A hypothesis-
driven (Repp, Felce, & Barton, 1988) func-
tional analysis was used to identify the rein-
forcers for problem behaviors. For each par-
ticipant, escape from academic demands and
teacher attention were tested. A play condition
was included as an experimental control. Con-
ditions were presented in a random order and
results were evaluated using a multi-element
design. All conditions were 10 min in dura-
tion. A 2-min break was given between con-
ditions. During the break, the student and
teacher continued with the naturally occurring
classroom activities (e.g., read a book, transi-
tioned between activities). Students were not
informed of changes in contingencies across
sessions and different stimuli were used were
used across conditions (e.g., academic work-
sheets during demand conditions; leisure items
during attention conditions). Each partici-
pant’s teacher implemented the functional
analysis and conducted between 2 and 4 ses-
sions per day.
Control (play) condition. The student
was provided free access to attention and pre-
ferred play materials available in the class-
room. The teacher engaged in interactive play
with the student and delivered attention at
least every 30 s. No programmed conse-
quences or demands were delivered for target
behaviors.
Attention condition. The student was
allowed unrestricted access to activities/items
typically available in the classroom. The
teacher interacted with the student until he was
engaged in an activity. Next, the teacher re-
moved herself from the activity, saying she
needed to do work at her desk. Contingent on
target behavior(s), the teacher delivered verbal
attention in the form of reprimands or redirec-
tion to work, consistent with verbalizations
noted in the descriptive observations. Follow-
ing the delivery of attention, the teacher re-
turned to work and the student continued to
have free access to preferred items.
Escape from academic demand con-
dition. During the escape condition, the
student was presented with work materials
identified by the teacher as associated with
problem behavior in the past. A graduated
prompting (i.e., verbal, gestural, physical) se-
quence was used to deliver academic de-
mands. If problem behavior occurred, the
teacher removed the academic demand and
walked away from the student. No attention
was provided to the student. Following the
30-s break period, the teacher returned to the
student and delivered another demand and re-
peated the procedure described above.
Follow-up functional analysis. Fol-
lowing the initial functional analysis, a fol-
low-up functional analysis was conducted to
investigate the additive effects of attention
during the escape condition. All conditions
were 10 min in duration, and 2-min breaks
were given between conditions. The teacher
implemented between 2 and 4 experimental
conditions per day. The escape from academic
demands and control/play conditions were im-
plemented in an identical manner as in the
initial functional analysis.
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
61
Escape-to-attention condition. Dur-
ing the ETA condition, the student was pre-
sented with work materials identified by the
teacher as associated with problem behavior in
the past. A graduated prompting (i.e., verbal,
gestural, physical) sequence was used to de-
liver academic demands. Contingent on target
behavior, the teacher removed the task mate-
rials and provided verbal attention during the
30-s break. The quality of teacher attention
during the escape break and the nature of
teacher attention were based on information
obtained in the descriptive assessment (i.e.,
reprimands, redirections, physical attention).
During the 30-s break, the teacher continued
to deliver attention to the student in the typical
manner for that classroom (e.g., “You need to
get back to work”, “I told you no screaming,
you have to work.”). Following the 30-s break,
the teacher represented the task and the
prompting sequence continued.
Treatment evaluations. A treatment
comparison was employed to evaluate the tar-
get behavior under two different treatment
types. One treatment, Escape Extinction, tar-
geted escape only. The other treatment (Es-
cape Extinction � Differential Reinforcement
of Alternative Behaviors), targeted escape and
teacher attention. The ETA treatment condi-
tions were evaluated using a B/C/B/C design
for Brandon and C/B/C designs for Franklin
and J’Marcus.
Escape extinction (EE). The EE con-
dition was identical to the escape condition
from the functional analysis with the excep-
tion that no break was delivered for target
behavior. Difficult academic materials were
presented using a graduated prompting se-
quence. No attention was delivered for target
behaviors.
Escape extinction � differential re-
inforcement of alternative behaviors
(EE�DRA). The EE�DRA condition was
implemented identical to the EE condition
with one exception. During EE�DRA phase,
the schedule of attention was based on the
descriptive data obtained during baseline ob-
servations. For Brandon and J’Marcus, atten-
tion was delivered every 30 s contingent on
demonstrating appropriate behavior. For
Franklin, teacher attention was delivered ev-
ery 15 s. In this phase, teacher attention con-
sisted of descriptive praise for appropriate be-
havior (i.e., task engagement; “Great job
working.”) and/or physical attention (e.g., pats
on the back). If problem behavior occurred
when the interval elapsed, the interval was
reset and the teacher did not deliver attention
to the student. That is, problem behavior did
not result in the delivery of teacher attention.
Interobserver Agreement
Two observers were assigned to one stu-
dent: one observer served as the primary data
collector and the other for IOA. Agreement
coefficients were calculated by dividing the
total number of agreements by the number of
agreements plus disagreements and multiply-
ing by 100. IOA data were collected across a
minimum of 30% of sessions during all phases
of the study. IOA data during the initial func-
tional analysis were: Brandon, M � 95%
(range � 85%–100%); Franklin, M � 96%
(range � 92%–100%); and J’Marcus, M �
95% (range � 90%–100%). IOA data during
the follow-up functional analysis were: Bran-
don, M � 98% (range � 90%–100%); Frank-
lin, M � 91% (range � 85%–100%); and
J’Marcus, M � 98% (range � 95%–100%).
IOA data during the treatment sessions were:
Brandon, M � 96% (range � 90%–100%);
Franklin, M � 92% (range � 84%–100%);
and J’Marcus, M � 93% (range � 90%–97%).
Procedural and Treatment Integrity
All teachers were trained to implement
to implement the functional analysis and treat-
ment evaluation conditions, based on proce-
dures outlined by Moore, Edwards et al.
(2002). For all activities for which teachers
were trained, a series of steps was created.
Procedural integrity was calculated each ses-
sion and by dividing the number of correctly
implemented steps by the total number of
steps for that condition. Procedural integrity
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
62
data (see Appendix A) were collected during
each functional analysis session and ranged
from 90% to 100% across all teachers.
Procedural integrity for individual com-
ponents of each treatment condition was cal-
culated for a minimum of 50% of the treat-
ment evaluation sessions. During the EE con-
dition, treatment integrity averaged 98%
(range � 96%–100%) for Brandon’s teacher,
80% (range � 65%–91%) for Franklin’s
teacher, and 89% (range � 86%–92%) for
J’Marcus’s teacher. During the EE � DRA
treatment, treatment integrity averaged 98%
(range � 92%–100%), 86% (range � 58%–
100%), and 95% (range � 92%–100%) for
Brandon’s, Franklin’s, and J’Marcus’s teach-
ers, respectively.
Results
Initial Functional Analysis
Initial functional analysis results are de-
picted in Figure 1. Data supported the hypoth-
esis that each participant’s behavior was rein-
forced by escape from academic demands.
Brandon (top panel) exhibited problem behav-
ior in an average of 14% (range � 12%–19%)
of the observed intervals during the escape
condition. The mean percentage of intervals
with problem behavior during the teacher at-
tention condition was 0.67% (range � 0%–
2%). No problem behavior was observed dur-
ing the control sessions. Franklin’s (middle
panel) mean percentage of intervals with prob-
lem behavior during the escape condition was
11.33% (range � 8%–16%) and less than 1%
during the attention sessions (range � 0%–
2%). No problem behavior was observed dur-
ing control sessions. The bottom panel of Fig-
ure 1 depicts the results of the initial func-
tional analysis for J’Marcus. The mean
percentage of intervals containing problem be-
havior during the escape condition was
32.67% (range � 29%–37%). No problem
behavior was observed during control and at-
tention conditions.
Follow-up Functional Analysis
The top panel of Figure 2 depicts the
results of Brandon’s follow-up functional
analysis. The mean percentage of intervals
containing problem behavior during the es-
cape condition was 10.5% (range � 7%–
15%), and no problem behavior occurred dur-
ing the control condition. The mean percent-
age of intervals with problem behavior in the
ETA condition was 11.5% (range � 7%–
14%). The ETA condition resulted in slightly
more problem behavior than the escape con-
dition. However, given the substantial overlap
of the level of behavior between the escape
and ETA conditions, the addition of teacher
attention during the escape interval did not
produce differential levels of responding be-
havior for Brandon across the two conditions.
As shown in the middle panel of Fig-
ure 2, Franklin’s mean percentage of intervals
Figure 1. Percentage of intervals con-
taining problem behavior during the
initial functional analysis for Brandon
(top panel), Franklin (middle panel),
and J’Marcus (bottom panel).
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
63
with problem behavior during the escape con-
dition was 11.33% (range � 7%–17%). Low
levels of problem behavior occurred during
the control condition (range � 0%–3%). Prob-
lem behavior in the ETA condition occurred in
an average of 36.67% (range � 31%–44%) of
intervals. The high level of behavior in the
ETA and low level of behavior in the other
two conditions suggests that Franklin’s prob-
lem behavior was reinforced by attention dur-
ing the escape period.
The results of the follow-up functional
analysis for J’Marcus are depicted in the bot-
tom panel of Figure 2. The mean percentage of
intervals with problem behavior during the
escape condition was 23.33% (range � 22%–
25%), and no problem behavior occurred dur-
ing the control condition. A substantial in-
crease in problem behavior was observed
during the ETA condition, (M � 46.67%;
range � 40%–58%), suggesting that J’Marcus’s
problem behavior was reinforced by attention
delivered during breaks from work.
ETA Treatment Evaluations
Brandon. The top panel of Figure 3
depicts the percentage of intervals with prob-
lem behavior and task engagement observed
during Brandon’s treatment evaluation. Dur-
ing the first EE treatment phase, an increasing
trend in Brandon’s problem behavior was ob-
served. (M � 38.4%; range � 17%–60%).
Following the implementation of the EE�
DRA treatment condition, an immediate de-
crease was observed in Brandon’s problem
behavior. The mean percentage of intervals
with problem behavior was 11.8% (range �
Figure 2. Percentage of intervals con-
taining problem behavior during the
modified functional analysis for Bran-
don (top panel), Franklin (middle
panel), and J’Marcus (bottom panel).
Figure 3. Percentage of intervals with
problem behavior and task engage-
ment for Brandon, (top panel), Frank-
lin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bot-
tom panel) during the escape-to-
attention treatment evaluations with
escape extinction (EE) and escape ex-
tinction � differential reinforcement
of alternative behaviors (DRA).
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
64
1%–23%) of intervals. When the EE treatment
was reintroduced, Brandon’s problem behav-
ior increased slightly to a mean percentage of
intervals of 12.33% (range � 12%–13%). The
level of problem behavior observed in the
second EE phase was relatively stable but did
not reach the levels observed in the first EE
phase. Finally, the reintroduction of the com-
bined treatment of EE�DRA produced stable,
low levels of problem behavior (M � 4.5%;
range � 2%–6%).
The top panel of Figure 3 also depicts
Brandon’s task engagement data during the
ETA treatment evaluation sessions. During the
first EE treatment phase, Brandon’s task en-
gagement had a decreasing trend as problem
behavior increased. Brandon was engaged
with task materials, on average, during 27.2%
(range � 0%–58%) of the intervals. When the
combined EE�DRA treatment was imple-
mented, Brandon’s level of task engagement
increased immediately and continued on an
upward trend over the phase (M � 73%;
range � 40%–95%). Following the reintro-
duction of the EE treatment, Brandon’s mean
task engagement was stabilized at 78.67%
(range � 78%–80%) of the intervals. In the
final EE�DRA treatment phase, Brandon’s
task engagement increased slightly to a mean
of 86.5% (range � 83%–95%).
Franklin. The results for Franklin’s
ETA treatment evaluations are depicted in the
middle panel of Figure 3. During the first
EE�DRA treatment phase, Franklin’s prob-
lem behavior decreased slightly across the
phase (M � 9.17%; range � 2%–19%). When
the EE treatment was implemented, a large
and immediate increase in problem behavior
was observed, averaging 46% (range � 38%–
45%) of the intervals. When the EE�DRA
treatment was reintroduced, a large and imme-
diate decrease was observed in Franklin’s
problem behavior (M � 25.5%; range �
25%–26%).
Franklin engaged with academic mate-
rials during an average of 77.67% (range �
65%–95%) of intervals during the first
EE�DRA treatment evaluation phase. With
the implementation of the EE treatment, a
large and immediate decrease was observed in
Franklin’s task engagement (M � 47.33%;
range � 42%–55%) of observed intervals. Fi-
nally with the reintroduction of the EE�DRA,
Franklin’s task engagement increased slightly
to a mean of 61% (range � 52%–70%) of the
observed intervals.
J’Marcus. The bottom panel of Figure 3
depicts J’Marcus’s ETA treatment evaluation
results for problem behavior and task engage-
ment. During the initial EE�DRA treatment
phase, J’Marcus exhibited low levels (M �
15%; range � 10%–20%) of problem behav-
ior with a decreasing trend across the phase.
Following the implementation of the EE treat-
ment phase, problem behavior showed an
immediate increase and continued trending
upward (M � 51%; range � 33%–65%).
Finally, after the reimplementation of the
EE�DRA treatment, an immediate and large
decrease was observed in J’Marcus’s problem
behavior. Low and stable levels of problem
behavior were observed in the final EE�DRA
treatment condition, with a mean level
of 3.75% (range � 2%–5%).
During the EE�DRA treatment phase,
J’Marcus was appropriately engaged with ac-
ademic work materials during a mean
of 78.67% (range � 73%–83%) of the in-
tervals. During the EE treatment phase,
J’Marcus’s task engagement decreased sharply
over the phase with a mean percentage
of 33.33% (range � 16%–62%) of intervals
coded with problem behavior. When the
EE�DRA treatment was reimplemented, an
immediate increase in J’Marcus’s task engage-
ment was observed, and behavior levels were
stable throughout the phase (M � 97.5%;
range � 94%–100%).
Treatment Acceptability
Each classroom teacher completed the
IRP-15 at the conclusion of each treatment
phase. Overall, all teachers rated the EE�
DRA treatment condition as more acceptable
than the EE treatment condition. For the
EE�DRA condition, the total scores were 89
for all participants. For the EE condition, the
following total scores were obtained: 71, 30,
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
65
and 16 for Brandon, Franklin, and J’Marcus’s
teachers, respectively. Thus, two of the three
teachers responded that the EE treatment was
an “unacceptable” treatment evidenced by to-
tal scores substantially lower than the tradi-
tionally used cutoff score of 52.50.
Discussion
The purpose of the current investigation
was to replicate and extend Mueller et al.’s
(2005) case study of a novel functional anal-
ysis condition designed to assess the additive
effects of attention as a reinforcer during
breaks from academic tasks. That is, the pres-
ent study sought to determine whether the
addition of teacher attention to an escape in-
terval (i.e., ETA) would result in elevated lev-
els of problem behavior when compared to a
standard escape condition for 3 children. The
second purpose of the study was to evaluate
whether a treatment package that targeted es-
cape and attention functions would reduce tar-
get behavior better than a treatment targeting
only escape.
The descriptive data from the functional
assessment suggested that problem behavior
led to escape from task demands for all par-
ticipants. Classroom observations revealed
that teachers also provided attention (e.g., rep-
rimands, requests to return to work) when the
students were not engaged in work; low levels
of peer attention for problem behavior were
observed. The results of the initial functional
analyses verified that all three participant’s
problem behavior was maintained by escape
from task demands. The first research question
evaluated whether the addition of teacher at-
tention during the escape interval would pro-
duce elevated levels of problem behavior
when compared to an escape-only condition.
The results supported Mueller et al.’s (2005)
findings, as the follow-up functional analysis
showed increases in problem behavior in the
ETA condition for 2 of the 3 participants rel-
ative to the escape-only and play/control con-
dition. Responding during the escape-only and
ETA functional analyses was similar for
Brandon.
As hypothesized by Mueller et al.
(2005), the findings that attention can rein-
force problem behavior during a work task
suggest that the presentation of task demands
may motivate problem behavior reinforced by
escape from an aversive task and by teacher
attention. In the conditions described by Iwata
et al. (1982), the only establishing operation
for the assessment of attention as a reinforcer
was the deprivation of attention. As seen in the
current analyses, attention functioned as a re-
inforcer in contexts other than those in which
a child was being ignored.
The second research question investi-
gated treatment implications for ETA-main-
tained problem behavior. A treatment program
designed to match the escape function only
(i.e., EE) versus a treatment targeting escape
with the addition of teacher attention (i.e.,
EE�DRA) were evaluated. Problem behavior
decreased for all participants during the
EE�DRA treatment, and a general increasing
trend in problem behavior was found during
the standard EE treatment. Likewise, concom-
itant increases in task engagement and de-
creases in problem behavior were observed
across all participants. Thus, differential re-
sponsiveness to treatment based on behavioral
function was observed, although differences
for Brandon were minimal by the end of
treatment.
The present study adds to a growing
literature base of studies investigating the ef-
fect of multiple reinforcing variables delivered
together (compound reinforcers) or in se-
quential arrangement (chained reinforcers).
Golonka et al. (2000) described the additive
effects of escape to an enriched environment
contingent upon appropriate behavior as treat-
ment for problem behavior in a classroom.
Although a compound or chained reinforcer
was not used in their functional analysis, the
treatment results supported the reductive ef-
fects of contingent escape to an enriched en-
vironment as more effective than contingent
escape alone. Mueller et al. (2005) used
Golonka et al.’s (2000) findings as the basis
for their ETA condition. However, no treat-
ment data were described in their one-partici-
pant case study. The present results add to
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
66
Mueller et al.’s (2005) findings by replicating
effects across additional participants. In addi-
tion, preliminary treatment findings showed
that when a behavior is reinforced by ETA,
providing attention combined with EE inter-
vention was more effective than escape extinc-
tion alone.
Although treatment phases were trun-
cated for some participants, the treatment
comparisons data provide some intriguing in-
formation for future study. Immediate changes
of a substantial magnitude were observed for
all participants’ problem behavior during the
initial EE�DRA condition, regardless of the
ordering of treatments. In subsequent itera-
tions of the treatment comparisons, problem
behavior levels for J’Marcus and Franklin
continued to show differential responding,
with substantially lower levels observed in the
combined treatment phase. Brandon, who did
not exhibit differential responding during the
modified ETA functional analysis, showed
less substantial treatment differences across
the treatment conditions in the latter treatment
phase comparisons. It is possible that the ini-
tial differences between the EE and EE�DRA
treatments could reflect an extinction burst
associated with the EE treatment, rather than
an actual difference between the two treat-
ments. It is also possible that the inclusion of
prompts in the EE condition may have pro-
vided students with preferred attention, there-
fore making the condition functionally similar
to the EE�DRA condition. However, given
the observed differences between the two
treatment conditions, the effects of prompts
were likely minimal. Additional treatment
comparisons may provide additional support
for matching treatment programs to behavioral
function, the ultimate goal behind conducting
a functional behavioral assessment.
The simplest explanation of the current
treatment results is that the EE�DRA treat-
ment worked better than EE for Franklin and
J’Marcus because EE�DRA addressed both
the escape and the attention aspects of the
compound function. For Brandon, although
EE initially produced higher level of problem
behavior, each treatment reduced the behavior
to similar levels by the end of the treatment
evaluation. The explanation of Brandon’s out-
comes is also straightforward, but different
from the reasons why the EE�DRA worked
so well with Franklin and J’Marcus. That is,
the addition of a positive reinforcer into Bran-
don’s demand context most likely reduced the
aversiveness of the task and therefore reduced
the motivation to demonstrate escape-main-
tained behavior.
The benefits of using positive reinforce-
ment techniques to reduce behaviors main-
tained by escape have been used successfully
for over 30 years. Carr, Newsom, and Binkoff
(1980) first demonstrated that attenuating
the aversiveness of task demand situations
through the delivery of highly preferred edi-
bles during work tasks reduced escape-main-
tained problem behavior. Several studies fol-
lowed replicating and supporting the aver-
siveness-attenuating benefits of introducing
preferred tangibles or food items into demand
contexts (e.g., Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski,
1997; Mazaleski, Iwata, Vollmer, Zarcone, &
Smith, 1993; Mueller, Edwards, & Trahant,
2003). Adding preferred tangibles to a demand
context makes use of reinforcers not identified
during the functional assessment. That is, the
addition of positive reinforcers into demand
contexts can reduce escape-maintained behav-
ior by using noncontingent reinforcement with
a functional or arbitrary reinforcer, or by pos-
itively reinforcing behaviors such as task en-
gagement or compliance through differential
reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA).
The results of the present study apply this
well-known concept to a treatment in which
escape and attention functions required sup-
port. By interjecting positive reinforcers into the
demand context using DRA procedures, the
aversiveness of the task was reduced through the
provision of a functional reinforcer.
Data also were collected for treatment
acceptability, which has not been commonly
reported in previous FBA research (Ervin et
al., 2001). In the present study, all three teach-
ers rated the EE�DRA treatment as more
acceptable treatment than the EE alone. Sur-
prisingly, two of the three teachers responded
that the EE treatment was an “unacceptable”
treatment; as such, low ratings of treatment
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
67
acceptability are often not reported in the pub-
lished literature. Teachers specifically re-
ported that they strongly disagreed that the EE
treatment was effective for changing problem
behavior, acceptable to use in the classroom,
and consistent with interventions they had
used in the past. These ratings may have been
influenced by the fact that teachers completed
acceptability ratings post-treatment use, after
they had experience implementing the two
interventions and had seen graphed data sup-
porting the relative effectiveness of the
EE�DRA treatment to the EE treatment
alone, a finding reported in analogue treatment
acceptability research (e.g., Tingstrom, 1989;
Tingstrom, McPhail, & Bolton, 1989; Von-
Brock & Elliott, 1987). Likewise, the higher
ratings for the combined treatment may have
been influenced by the addition of differential
reinforcement for task engagement (i.e.,
praise), as previous researchers have reported
that reinforcement-based interventions are
generally rated as more acceptable than pun-
ishment-based interventions (e.g., Blampied &
Kahan, 1992; Elliott, Witt, Galvin, & Peter-
son, 1984). Although extinction-based proce-
dures are not technically classified as punish-
ment in the behavioral literature, the teachers
may have perceived the treatment as such, and
thus rated it less favorably vis-à-vis a treat-
ment that included a richer schedule of
reinforcement.
A few limitations in the current study
are worth noting. Data collection was discon-
tinued early for Franklin because of a 9-day
suspension from school for fighting at end of
the school year. Thus, only two data points
were collected during the final treatment
phase. As such, definitive information about
the long-term effects of the EE�DRA treat-
ment is unknown. Additional replications of
treatment phases, counterbalancing treatment
order across students with escape-only and
ETA functions, extended data collection
within phases, and follow-up data would
strengthen the experimental design of such
studies and add information about long-term
treatment effects. Next, the current treatment
comparisons, although successful in reducing
target behavior, did not allow for a full anal-
ysis of the mechanisms underlying the change.
As described above, multiple theoretical pos-
tulates are reasonable and plausible. Future
studies may seek to determine exactly which
mechanisms of behavior change are at play to
reduce the behaviors. For example, a study
might compare the reductive effects of EE
with those of noncontingent reinforcement
with an arbitrary reinforcer such as preferred
tangible items to discover whether the addition
of the attention in the current study reduced
target behavior because it was functionally
related to the target behavior or whether it
simply reduced the aversiveness of the tasks
presented.
Notwithstanding the limitations, the
present study provides additional support for
translating FBA research into practice in the
schools. One criticism of previous FBA re-
search is that individuals with specialized
training (e.g., researchers, behavior special-
ists) conducted the functional analysis condi-
tions. In the current investigation, the stu-
dents’ teachers implemented all assessment
and treatment conditions, albeit with supervi-
sion and feedback. Another strength is that the
functional analysis occurred during standard
classroom activities, rather than an analogue
setting. In addition, all participants were gen-
eral education students, unlike the majority of
FBA studies conducted with participants with
disabilities (Ervin et al., 2001; Hanley et al.,
2003; Hoff et al., 2005).
The present study is in line with previ-
ous research documenting the importance of
identifying idiosyncratic variables during
FBAs, in an effort to develop the most effec-
tive treatment plan (Hoff et al., 2005). The
results also add to a growing literature of
school-based studies investigating the effect of
idiosyncratic variables on problem behavior.
The findings provide a heuristic and experi-
mental example for incorporating descriptive
assessment information into the development
of functional analysis conditions in the school
setting. Similar to previous studies, the results
show descriptive assessment information was
vital to the precise identification of behavioral
function (Galiatsatos & Graff, 2003; Lalli &
Casey, 1996; Mueller et al., 2005; Richman &
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
68
Hagopian, 1999; Tiger, Hanley, & Bessette,
2006). The ETA condition was designed spe-
cifically to assess the relative contributions of
escape from academic demands and the addi-
tive effects of teacher attention following
problem behavior, a consequent frequently ob-
served in classroom settings (Kurtz et al.,
2003; McKerchar & Thompson, 2004). The
results suggest the ETA condition may have
utility for FBAs and treatment planning in the
school setting. In future research, researchers
may wish to investigate other sources of rein-
forcement that may occur during the escape
interval (i.e., peer attention, access to preferred
activities/items). Also, researchers should focus
on additional ETA-based treatment options.
Future researchers may wish to investigate the
addition of teacher attention with other es-
cape-maintained treatment options.
References
Blampied, N. M., Kahan, E. (1992). Acceptability of
alternative punishments: A community survey. Behav-
ior Modification, 16, 400–413.
Broussard, C. D., & Northup, J. (1995). An approach to
functional assessment and analysis of disruptive be-
havior in regular education classrooms. School Psy-
chology Quarterly, 10, 154–164.
Broussard, C. D., & Northup, J. (1997). The use of func-
tional analysis to develop peer interventions for dis-
ruptive classroom behavior. School Psychology Quar-
terly, 12, 65–76.
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980).
Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior in two
retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy-
sis, 13, 101–117.
Doggett, R. A., Edwards, R. P., Moore, J. W., Tingstrom,
D. H., & Wilczynski, S. M. (2001). An approach to
functional assessment in general education classroom
settings. School Psychology Review, 30, 313–328.
Dufrene, B. A., Doggett, R. A., Henington, C., & Watson,
T. S. (2007). Functional assessment and intervention
for disruptive classroom behaviors in preschool and
Head Start classrooms. Journal of Behavioral Educa-
tion, 16, 368–388.
Edwards, R. P. (2002). A tutorial for using the functional
assessment informant record for teachers (FAIR-T).
Proven Practice, 4, 31–33.
Elliott, S. N., Witt, J. C., Galvin, G. A., & Peterson, R.
(1984). Acceptability of positive and reductive behav-
ioral interventions: Factors that influence teachers’ de-
cisions. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 353–360.
Ellis, J., & Magee, S. (2004). Modifications to basic
functional analysis procedures in school settings: A
selective review. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 205–
228.
Ervin, R. A., Radford, P. M., Bertsch, K., Piper, A. L.,
Ehrhardt, K. E., & Poling, A. (2001). A descriptive
analysis and critique of the empirical literature on
school-based functional assessment. School Psychol-
ogy Review, 30, 193–210.
Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. A. (1997).
Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as a
treatment of self-injurious behavior. Journal of Ap-
plied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249.
Galiatsatos, G. T., & Graff, R. B. (2003). Combining
descriptive and functional analyses to assess and treat
screaming. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 123–128.
Golonka, Z., Wacker, D., Berg, W., Derby, M., K., Hard-
ing, J., & Peck, S. (2000). Effects of escape to alone
versus escape to enriched environments on adaptive
and aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 33, 243–246.
Gresham, F. M., McIntyre, L. L., Olson-Tinker, H., Dol-
stra, L., McLaughlin, V., & Van, M. (2004). Relevance
of functional behavioral assessment research for
school-based intervention and positive behavioral sup-
port. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 19–
37.
Gunter, P. L., Jack, S. L., Shores, R. E., Carrell, D. E., &
Flowers, J. (1993). Lag sequential analysis as a tool for
functional analysis of student disruptive behavior in
classrooms. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Dis-
orders, 1, 138–148.
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
Hoff, K. E., Ervin, R. A., & Friman, P. C. (2005). Refining
functional behavior assessment: Analyzing the sepa-
rate and combined effects of hypothesized controlling
variables during ongoing classroom routines. School
Psychology Review, 34, 45–57.
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
& Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional analysis
of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Develop-
mental Disabilities, 2, 3–20.
Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg, J. S.,
Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., et al. (2000). Skill acqui-
sition in the implementation of functional analysis
methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
33, 181–194.
Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S.,
O’Conner, J. T., Paclawskyj, T., et al. (2003). Func-
tional analysis and treatment of self-injurious behavior
in young children: A summary of 30 cases. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219.
Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply
controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 29, 391–396.
Mann, A. J., & Mueller, M. M. (2009). False positive
functional analysis results as a contributor of treatment
failure during functional communication training. Ed-
ucation and Treatment of Children, 32, 121–149.
Martens, B. K., Witt, J. C., Elliot, S. N., & Darveaux,
D. X. (1985). Teacher judgments concerning the ac-
ceptability of school-based interventions. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 16, 191–198.
Mazaleski, J. A., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone,
J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the reinforce-
ment and extinction components in DRO contingencies
with self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
26, 143–156.
McKerchar, P. M., & Thompson, R. H. (2004). A descrip-
tive analysis of potential reinforcement contingencies
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
69
in the preschool classroom. Journal of Applied Behav-
ior Analysis, 37, 431–443.
Moore, J. W., Edwards, R. P., Sterling-Turner, H. E.,
Riley, J., DuBard, M., & McGeorge, A. (2002).
Teacher acquisition of functional analysis methodol-
ogy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 73–77.
Moore, J. W., Mueller, M. M., Dubard, M., Roberts, D. S.,
Sterling-Turner, H. E. (2002). The influence of thera-
pist attention on self-injury during a tangible condition.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 283–286.
Mueller, M. M., Edwards, R. P., & Trahant, D. (2003).
Translating multiple assessment techniques into an in-
tervention selection model for classrooms. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 563–573.
Mueller, M. M., Nkosi, A., & Hine, J. F. (in press). Functional
analysis in public school settings: A review of 90 functional
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis.
Mueller, M. M., Sterling-Turner, H. E., & Moore, J. M.
(2005). Towards developing a classroom-based func-
tional analysis condition to assess escape-to-attention
as a variable maintaining problem behavior. School
Psychology Review, 34, 425–431.
Repp, A. C., Felce, D., & Barton, L. E. (1988). Basing the
treatment of stereotypic and self-injurious behaviors on
hypotheses of their causes. Journal of Applied Behav-
ior Analysis, 21, 281–289.
Richman, D. M., & Hagopian, L. P. (1999). On the effects
of “quality” of attention in the functional analysis of
destructive behavior. Research in Developmental Dis-
abilities, 20, 51–62.
Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bessette, K. K. (2006).
Incorporating descriptive assessment results into the
design of a functional analysis: Case example involv-
ing a preschooler’s handmouthing. Education & Treat-
ment of Children, 29, 107–124.
Tingstrom, D. H. (1990). Acceptability of time-out: The
influence of problem behavior severity, interventionist,
and reported effectiveness. Journal of School Psychol-
ogy, 28, 165–169.
Tingstrom, D. H., McPhail, R. L., & Bolton, A. B. (1989).
Acceptability of alternative school-based interven-
tions: The influence of reported effectiveness and age
of target child. The Journal of Psychology, 123, 133–
140.
VanDerHeyden, A. M., Witt, J. C., & Gatti, S. (2001).
Descriptive assessment method to reduce overall dis-
ruptive behavior in a preschool classroom. School Psy-
chology Review, 30(4), 548–567.
Von Brock, M. B., & Elliott, S. N. (1987). Influence of
treatment effectiveness information on the acceptabil-
ity of classroom interventions. Journal of School Psy-
chology, 25, 131–144.
Date Received: August 27, 2009
Date Accepted: January 15, 2011
Action Editor: Tanya Eckert �
Jana Sarno, MA, BCBA, is a senior program supervisor with
Coyne and Associates
Education Corporation in San Diego, California, and a doctoral
candidate at The Uni-
versity of Southern Mississippi. Her areas of interest include
early intervention with
children diagnosed with autism, verbal behavior, and functional
analysis.
Heather E. Sterling, PhD, is an associate professor of
psychology and the director of
training for the school psychology program at The University of
Southern Mississippi.
Her research areas include functional assessment and analysis,
single-case research
design, school-based consultation, and intellectual and
developmental disabilities.
Michael Mueller, PhD, BCBA-D, received his doctorate in
school psychology from The
University of Southern Mississippi. He is currently the director
of behavioral services for
Southern Behavioral Group in Atlanta, Georgia.
Brad A. Dufrene, PhD, is an assistant professor and director of
the School Psychology
Service Center at The University of Southern Mississippi. His
research interests include
functional assessment and school-based consultation.
Daniel H. Tingstrom, PhD, is a professor of psychology and is
affiliated with the school
psychology program in the Department of Psychology at The
University of Southern
Mississippi. His research interests include applied behavior
analysis, and the implemen-
tation and evaluation of behavioral and academic interventions.
D. Joe Olmi, PhD, is a professor of psychology and
departmental chairperson at The
University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests
include positive behavioral
interventions and supports, compliance training, and school-
based consultation.
School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1
70
APPENDIX A
PROCEDURAL INTEGRITY FOR ETA CONDITION
Student: Session:
Teacher: Date:
Observer: Condition: ETA
This form is used to assess the level of procedural integrity for
each teacher-implemented functional analysis ETA
condition. Record if the teacher behaviors were implemented as
planned (Yes) or not implemented as planned (No)
during each FA control condition.
YES NO N/A
1. Seats student at his/her desk or table
2. Teacher places academic materials on the student’s desk
3. Teacher provides verbal instructions to student to complete
the academic work
4. Teacher waits 5 seconds for compliance
a. The student complies
i. Teacher provides descriptive praise
ii. Teacher moves to the next demand
b. The student does not comply
i. Teacher restates the instructions with verbal and
gestural prompts
ii. Teacher waits 5 seconds for compliance
A. Student complies
1. Teacher provides descriptive praise
2. Teacher moves to the next demand
B. Student does not comply
1. Teacher restates the instructions and
provides hand-over-hand guidance
5. Teacher does not respond to any other problem behavior
6. Contingent on problem behavior
a. Teacher removes task demand for 30 s
b. Teacher provides attention during escape period
Repeat steps 3–6 for each demand sequence
Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable
71
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.

More Related Content

Similar to Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable forMaintaining P.docx

Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...
Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...
Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...
Alexander Decker
 
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docxPsychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
denneymargareta
 
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docxPsychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
woodruffeloisa
 
1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx
1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx
1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx
jackiewalcutt
 
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
Liz Adams
 
Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01
Kishor Ade
 
Rubric Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course .docx
Rubric  Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course  .docxRubric  Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course  .docx
Rubric Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course .docx
joellemurphey
 
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154 137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
AnastaciaShadelb
 
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154 137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
ChantellPantoja184
 
Writing the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney Luang
Writing the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney LuangWriting the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney Luang
Writing the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney Luangdabneyluang
 
6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx
6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx
6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx
priestmanmable
 
Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docx
Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docxJournal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docx
Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docx
tawnyataylor528
 
Unit 2 types of research
Unit 2 types of researchUnit 2 types of research
Unit 2 types of research
Asima shahzadi
 
College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx
                College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx                College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx
College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx
joyjonna282
 
1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures
1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures
1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures
AnastaciaShadelb
 
A Case Study With An Identified Bully Policy And Practice Implications
A Case Study With An Identified Bully  Policy And Practice ImplicationsA Case Study With An Identified Bully  Policy And Practice Implications
A Case Study With An Identified Bully Policy And Practice Implications
Kristen Flores
 

Similar to Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable forMaintaining P.docx (20)

Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...
Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...
Influence of gender, types of school and occupational stress on pupil control...
 
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docxPsychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
 
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docxPsychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 52(2), 2015 C© 2014 Wiley Peri.docx
 
Presentation
PresentationPresentation
Presentation
 
1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx
1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx
1. Need all 3 article read and compared answering the questions I .docx
 
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
 
Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01
 
paper-25092016160947
paper-25092016160947paper-25092016160947
paper-25092016160947
 
2014 yesulyurt
2014 yesulyurt2014 yesulyurt
2014 yesulyurt
 
Rubric Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course .docx
Rubric  Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course  .docxRubric  Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course  .docx
Rubric Analysis of a case studyStudentGroup Name Course .docx
 
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154 137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
 
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154 137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
137JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2002, 35, 137–154
 
Writing the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney Luang
Writing the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney LuangWriting the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney Luang
Writing the review research(My Research Review ) Dabney Luang
 
6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx
6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx
6Websites, Books, Independent Studies 6 Journal Articles Summ.docx
 
Proposal
ProposalProposal
Proposal
 
Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docx
Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docxJournal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docx
Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2254–64,.docx
 
Unit 2 types of research
Unit 2 types of researchUnit 2 types of research
Unit 2 types of research
 
College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx
                College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx                College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx
College of Doctoral StudiesExpanded Comparison.docx
 
1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures
1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures
1Methodology AssignmentParticipantProcedures
 
A Case Study With An Identified Bully Policy And Practice Implications
A Case Study With An Identified Bully  Policy And Practice ImplicationsA Case Study With An Identified Bully  Policy And Practice Implications
A Case Study With An Identified Bully Policy And Practice Implications
 

More from russelldayna

Essay for ENG 213 21st century and e.docx
Essay for ENG 213 21st  century and e.docxEssay for ENG 213 21st  century and e.docx
Essay for ENG 213 21st century and e.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docx
Essay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docxEssay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docx
Essay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docx
russelldayna
 
essay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docx
essay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docxessay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docx
essay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docx
Essay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docxEssay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docx
Essay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docx
Essay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docxEssay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docx
Essay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docx
russelldayna
 
Essay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docx
Essay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docxEssay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docx
Essay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docx
russelldayna
 
essay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docx
essay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docxessay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docx
essay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docx
Essay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docxEssay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docx
Essay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docx
Essay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docxEssay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docx
Essay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docx
Essay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docxEssay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docx
Essay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docx
Essay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docxEssay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docx
Essay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docx
Essay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docxEssay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docx
Essay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docx
Essay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docxEssay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docx
Essay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docx
russelldayna
 
ESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docx
ESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docxESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docx
ESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Assignments Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docx
Essay Assignments   Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docxEssay Assignments   Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docx
Essay Assignments Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docx
Essay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docxEssay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docx
Essay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docx
russelldayna
 
essay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docx
essay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docxessay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docx
essay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docx
Essay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docxEssay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docx
Essay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docx
Essay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docxEssay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docx
Essay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docx
russelldayna
 
Essay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docx
Essay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docxEssay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docx
Essay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docx
russelldayna
 

More from russelldayna (20)

Essay for ENG 213 21st century and e.docx
Essay for ENG 213 21st  century and e.docxEssay for ENG 213 21st  century and e.docx
Essay for ENG 213 21st century and e.docx
 
Essay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docx
Essay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docxEssay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docx
Essay for ENG Unit 3 Breaking out Poe.docx
 
essay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docx
essay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docxessay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docx
essay I wrote need to be fixedHEADING-should write Essay One  S.docx
 
Essay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docx
Essay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docxEssay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docx
Essay help on congressional committesslegislation assignmentI.docx
 
Essay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docx
Essay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docxEssay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docx
Essay Format-Persuasive EssayIntroduction Paragraph with Thesis .docx
 
Essay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docx
Essay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docxEssay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docx
Essay format APA Style of Writing. No less than 150 words per answ.docx
 
essay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docx
essay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docxessay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docx
essay format that discuss all three  has to be 3-4 pagesDiscus.docx
 
Essay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docx
Essay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docxEssay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docx
Essay Exponents and PolynomialsWrite an essay of at least t.docx
 
Essay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docx
Essay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docxEssay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docx
Essay Exam #3 The essays generated by this assignment will be co.docx
 
Essay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docx
Essay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docxEssay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docx
Essay ElectionVoting ReformWrite a 4 page essay in which y.docx
 
Essay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docx
Essay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docxEssay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docx
Essay contentWhere did your storypoetry come from What specif.docx
 
Essay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docx
Essay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docxEssay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docx
Essay contains at least six pages and includes an introduction, bo.docx
 
Essay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docx
Essay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docxEssay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docx
Essay at least 1 page each paper, APA, with references. 1). Clif.docx
 
ESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docx
ESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docxESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docx
ESSAY CHOICE 1Develop a communication strategy to resolve confli.docx
 
Essay Assignments Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docx
Essay Assignments   Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docxEssay Assignments   Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docx
Essay Assignments Topic (How Should I be Governed d.docx
 
Essay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docx
Essay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docxEssay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docx
Essay Assignment     Students are tasked with completing an e.docx
 
essay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docx
essay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docxessay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docx
essay assignmentTechnology and Education”How have new tec.docx
 
Essay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docx
Essay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docxEssay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docx
Essay Assignment – A Textual Analysis of the Royal Proclamation of 1.docx
 
Essay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docx
Essay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docxEssay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docx
Essay Arugment on Julius Caesar play. essay question is 2In.docx
 
Essay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docx
Essay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docxEssay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docx
Essay Assignment #3Required length 5-7 pages, not including th.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp NetworkIntroduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
TechSoup
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
JosvitaDsouza2
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Balvir Singh
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
siemaillard
 
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdfLapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Jean Carlos Nunes Paixão
 
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and ResearchDigital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Vikramjit Singh
 
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdfWelcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
TechSoup
 
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9  .docxAcetabularia Information For Class 9  .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
vaibhavrinwa19
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
MIRIAMSALINAS13
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Peter Windle
 
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
CarlosHernanMontoyab2
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Atul Kumar Singh
 
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
beazzy04
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
DhatriParmar
 
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdfspecial B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
Special education needs
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Po-Chuan Chen
 
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideasThe geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
GeoBlogs
 
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.pptThesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
EverAndrsGuerraGuerr
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
 
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp NetworkIntroduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdfLapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
 
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and ResearchDigital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
Digital Tools and AI for Teaching Learning and Research
 
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdfWelcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
 
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9  .docxAcetabularia Information For Class 9  .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
 
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
 
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
 
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdfspecial B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
 
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideasThe geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
The geography of Taylor Swift - some ideas
 
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
 
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.pptThesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
 

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable forMaintaining P.docx

  • 1. Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable for Maintaining Problem Behavior in the School Setting Jana M. Sarno and Heather E. Sterling The University of Southern Mississippi Michael M. Mueller Southern Behavioral Group Brad Dufrene, Daniel H. Tingstrom, and D. Joe Olmi The University of Southern Mississippi Abstract. Mueller, Sterling-Turner, and Moore (2005) reported a novel escape- to-attention (ETA) functional analysis condition in a school setting with one child. The current study replicates Mueller et al.’s functional analysis procedures with three elementary school-age boys referred for problem behavior. Functional analysis verified the participant’s problem behavior was maintained by escape from academic demands. Follow-up functional analyses in which target behaviors in escape versus ETA conditions were compared resulted in higher levels of target behavior in the ETA condition for 2 of the 3 participants. The current study also extended previous research by including a treatment analysis. Treatments de- signed to address escape and attention functions were more effective at reducing
  • 2. the target behaviors than treatments designed to target escape alone for all 3 participants. Results and implications for future research are discussed. Incorporating experimental analyses into a functional behavioral assessment is an effective and time-efficient approach for the assessment and treatment of problem behavior (Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003; Mueller, Sterling-Turner, & Moore, 2005; Mueller, Nkosi, & Hine, in press). The functional anal- ysis methodology developed by Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982) is an analogue evaluation of problem behavior in which purported reinforcers are withheld and then delivered contingent upon target behav- ior. In their original work, Iwata and col- leagues measured levels of target behaviors during experimental conditions (i.e., attention, escape, alone) and compared the data to levels of target behavior in a control condition in which the reinforcers were available noncon- tingently. Iwata et al.’s methodology has been used extensively to identify the behavioral function of self-injurious behavior in clinical settings and has been used with a variety of behaviors and in other nonclinical settings. Although use of functional analysis proce- This article was taken, in part, from the first author’s thesis. Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Heather E. Sterling, The University of Southern Mississippi, 118 College Drive, #5025, Hattiesburg,
  • 3. MS 39406; E-mail: [email protected] Copyright 2011 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015 School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1, pp. 57–71 57 dures is reported less commonly in school settings (Hanley et al., 2003), studies have been reported with examples of disruptive school-based behaviors reinforced by peer at- tention (e.g., Broussard & Northup, 1997), teacher attention, (e.g., Gunter, Jack, Shores, Carrell, & Flowers, 1993), access to tangible items (e.g., Moore, Mueller, Dubard, Roberts, & Sterling-Turner, 2002), and escape from academic demands (e.g., Broussard & Nor- thup, 1995). Although a functional behavioral assess- ment, including experimental analysis, may not be necessary to address all disruptive be- haviors in school settings (Gresham et al., 2004), additional research on the effect of idiosyncratic variables is needed. In school settings, as in other nonclinical settings, unique environmental variables (e.g., setting, personnel, physical) could require modifica- tions to the standard functional analysis con- ditions typically reported. For example, in school settings, tasks in the form of academic demands (e.g., ongoing instruction, indepen-
  • 4. dent practice worksheets) are, at least theoret- ically, present throughout the majority of the day. Likewise, concurrent and potentially competing reinforcers in the form of peer at- tention, teacher attention, or preferred activi- ties (e.g., reading a more desirable book) or items (e.g., playing with a toy hidden in a desk) for inappropriate behavior may be pres- ent. Thus, students may be provided with es- cape from academic demands, while subse- quently being provided with an additional re- inforcer for problem behavior. Because student behavior can be under the discrimina- tive control of multiple antecedent events or reinforced by multiple variables (e.g., teacher and peer attention, access to preferred materials, breaks from work), it is important to examine a combination of factors that may be maintaining problem behavior in the classroom. Over the past few years, investigations of the effects of multiple variables have begun in and out of the classroom. For example, Hoff, Ervin, and Friman (2005) examined the separate and combined effects of escape and peer attention on disruptive behavior in the general education classroom. Following a de- scriptive assessment, including interviews and direct observations, Hoff and colleagues for- mulated three hypotheses to test in an alter- nating treatments design: access to peer atten- tion, escape from a nonpreferred activity, and access to peer attention and escape from a nonpreferred activity. Treatment analysis data verified the initial hypothesis of access to peer
  • 5. attention and escape from academic demands. In addition, a combined intervention targeting both attention and escape decreased problem behaviors to near zero levels. Moore, Mueller et al. (2002) investi- gated the influence of the simultaneous deliv- ery of therapist attention on self-injurious be- havior in a tangible condition. Following the initial functional analysis, attention in the tan- gible condition was evaluated using a reversal design. In one phase, juice and brief attention were delivered contingent on self-injurious be- havior. In the second phase, the delivery of the preferred stimulus (juice) was returned contin- gent on problem behavior and attention was withheld. The results of the follow-up analysis demonstrated that self-injurious behavior oc- curred at higher rates when the juice and at- tention were delivered concurrently than when the juice was presented alone. By incorporating procedural variations in the functional analysis methodology, Moore, Mueller et al. (2002) demonstrated that the presence of attention could confound the outcomes of functional analysis condi- tions. Moore and colleagues hypothesized that “practical solutions for the tangible condition might be to restrict attention as much as pos- sible or to weaken the dependency between problem behavior and therapist attention by delivering attention on a response-independent schedule” (p. 284). However, Moore, Mueller et al. did not present treatment data to support their hypothesis. It is conceivable, though, as
  • 6. the authors suggested, that the influence of attention might affect other consequent analy- sis conditions. In Mann and Mueller (2009), the func- tional analysis results of a girl’s aggression appeared to be maintained by attention. The results of the functional analysis of her behav- School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 58 ior showed high levels of aggression in the attention condition and low levels in an escape from academic demand, access to tangibles, and a toy play control. When she failed to acquire a functional communicative response to replace aggression for attention, a follow-up functional analysis was used to evaluate whether access to attention was part of a chain of reinforcers maintaining aggression. In the follow-up analysis, attention-to-tangibles, at- tention-to-escape, and attention alone as a control condition were each used. Aggression was high in the attention-to-tangibles condi- tion and low in the attention-to-escape and the attention-only control. When functional com- munication training was used to address the attention-to-tangibles (i.e., manding for access to tangibles and attention, rather than attention only), the response was acquired and the ag- gression decreased. These results highlight two issues relevant for school-based func-
  • 7. tional analysis. First, Iwata et al.’s (1982) methodology is useful, even if structural vari- ants to assess multiple reinforcers are re- quired. Second, for behaviors maintained by multiple reinforcers, matching treatments to both reinforcers may be required to reduce target behaviors substantially. Mueller et al. (2005) provided pilot data for an ETA condition used in a school setting. For one child, a functional analysis with an escape, attention, and toy play conditions was conducted using the procedures described by Iwata et al. (1982). Results of initial functional analysis showed that problem behavior only occurred in the escape from academic demand condition, however, lower than that typically was observed in the classroom setting. After escape was identified by the initial functional analysis, the researchers assessed a combina- tion of variables to determine whether differ- ential levels of problem behavior would occur with the addition of attention during the break from academic demands, as this was observed in the direct behavioral observation prior to the initial functional analysis. In the follow-up functional analysis, the escape-only, ETA, and control conditions were presented. A substan- tially higher level of tantrums was demon- strated in the ETA condition than in the es- cape-alone condition or the control conditions. Mueller et al. (2005) hypothesized that without the information derived from the fol- low-up analysis, an intervention based on the
  • 8. escape-only hypothesis would have failed. Al- though the results of Mann and Mueller (2009) provide some support for this hypothesis, Mu- eller et al. (2005) did not provide any inter- vention data. Other limitations should be ad- dressed as well. First, the investigation was a pilot study of the ETA condition and involved only one participant. Another limitation was that the consultant collected all data and, be- cause of staffing issues, no interobserver agreement data (IOA) were collected. Given the limitations of Mueller et al. (2005), the current study was undertaken with two primary goals. First, we replicated Muel- ler et al.’s ETA investigation with additional participants and in a more controlled manner, including IOA data and multiple behavioral observers, to determine whether the ETA function would emerge in additional partici- pants. The second goal was to extend Mueller et al. by evaluating two different behavioral interventions, one that presented an escape- only treatment and one that was matched to both functions (escape and teacher attention). We predicted that differential treatment results would emerge for students who showed higher levels of problem behavior in the ETA condi- tion, with stronger treatment effects favoring the combined treatment for children with an ETA function when compared to children with escape-maintained problem only. Method Participants and Setting
  • 9. Three elementary school-age boys re- ferred for problem classroom behavior partic- ipated. All students were enrolled in public schools and were placed in general education classrooms in a rural Southeastern school dis- trict. Teacher and parental consent were se- cured for participation; participant names used hereafter are pseudonyms. Brandon was a 6-year-old Caucasian male enrolled in a gen- eral education first-grade classroom. Brandon Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 59 was diagnosed with attention deficit hyperac- tivity disorder (combined type) when he was 5-years-old and was prescribed a 10-mg dose patch of methylphenidate (Daytrana). Franklin and J’Marcus were 5-year-old African Amer- ican males enrolled in separate general educa- tion kindergarten classrooms. J’Marcus and Franklin had no medical diagnoses and were prescribed no medications. All sessions were conducted in the par- ticipants’ classrooms during typically sched- uled activities that corresponded to teacher- reported times when problem behaviors were most frequent. The students’ classroom teach- ers implemented all functional analyses and treatment evaluation sessions.
  • 10. Measures Functional Assessment Informant Re- cord for Teachers (FAIR-T). The FAIR-T is an instrument administered to teachers to gen- erate hypotheses concerning the function of problem behavior (Edwards, 2002). The FAIR-T is designed with four components to achieve this purpose: (a) general referral in- formation, (b) identification and description of problem behavior, (c) potential antecedents for problem behavior, and (d) potential conse- quences that follow the problem behavior most frequently. Researchers have demon- strated that the hypotheses generated from in- formation gathered via the FAIR-T correspond with behavioral function identified in experi- mental analyses (e.g., Doggett, Edwards, Moore, Tingstrom, & Wilczynski, 2001; Du- frene, Doggett, Henington, & Watson, 2007). Intervention Rating Profile-15 (IRP- 15). The Intervention Rating Profile-15 (IRP- 15; Martens, Witt, Elliott, & Darveaux, 1985) was used as a social validity measure of the treatment conditions. The IRP-15 is composed of 15 questions that the respondent rates on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly dis- agree) to 6 (strongly agree). Ratings range from a total score of 15–90, where a total score above 52.50 represents a rating of “ac- ceptable” (Von Brock & Elliott, 1987). The IRP-15 has high reported internal consistency (Cronbach � � .98), and all items load on a General Acceptability Factor (ranging from 0.82
  • 11. to 0.95; Martens et al., 1985). Problem behavior. Child problem be- havior served as the primary dependent vari- able and was reported as the percentage of intervals in which the behavior occurred. Problem behavior included: inappropriate vo- calizations (Brandon, J’Marcus, Franklin), which was defined as talking or yelling with- out teacher permission; elopement (J’Marcus), which was defined as any movement 1 m away from the teacher or teacher-designated area without permission; and banging on surfaces (Franklin), which was defined as throwing ac- ademic materials in a downward motion to the desk, and/or floor so that it made an audible sound on impact. Additional data were also collected for task engagement during the treat- ment evaluation phases. Task engagement was defined as the student’s eyes directed at work materials and/or manipulating objects associ- ated with the teacher command. Task engage- ment is presented as the percentage of inter- vals in which behavior was observed during a session. A 10-s partial interval recording sys- tem was used for all observations. All sessions were 10 min in length. Procedures First, functional behavioral assessments that included teacher interview and direct classroom observations were conducted to generate hypotheses of behavioral function. Second, functional analyses were used to ver- ify escape from task demands as the maintain-
  • 12. ing variable for referred behaviors. Third, fol- low-up functional analyses were used to in- vestigate the additive effects of attention delivered during the break from academic de- mands (i.e., ETA). Finally, two different treat- ments were compared to examine the effects on target behavior when an escape-only treat- ment was alternated with an intervention pack- age that targeted escape and attention. Functional behavior assessment. Each teacher was administered the FAIR-T as a semistructured interview in order to define target behaviors and their immediate anteced- School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 60 ent and consequent events. Next, direct behav- ioral observations were conducted in the stu- dents’ classrooms. The information obtained from the functional assessment was used to form hypotheses about potential behavior-re- inforcer relationships. Conditional probabili- ties (VanDerHeyden, Witt, & Gatti, 2001) were also calculated from the observational data to determine the temporal proximity of specific consequences and target behaviors. Conditional probabilities for each participant were as follows: Brandon—escape � 74%, teacher attention � 24%, peer attention � 0%; Franklin—escape � 66%, teacher attention � 25%; peer attention � 5%; J’Marcus—es-
  • 13. cape � 59%, teacher attention � 29%, peer attention � 9%. Descriptive data suggested escape from academic demands or teacher at- tention in the form of reprimands and redirec- tion to work might be reinforcing the target behaviors identified for each child. Thus, each child proceeded to the experimental phases of the study reported below. Functional analysis. A hypothesis- driven (Repp, Felce, & Barton, 1988) func- tional analysis was used to identify the rein- forcers for problem behaviors. For each par- ticipant, escape from academic demands and teacher attention were tested. A play condition was included as an experimental control. Con- ditions were presented in a random order and results were evaluated using a multi-element design. All conditions were 10 min in dura- tion. A 2-min break was given between con- ditions. During the break, the student and teacher continued with the naturally occurring classroom activities (e.g., read a book, transi- tioned between activities). Students were not informed of changes in contingencies across sessions and different stimuli were used were used across conditions (e.g., academic work- sheets during demand conditions; leisure items during attention conditions). Each partici- pant’s teacher implemented the functional analysis and conducted between 2 and 4 ses- sions per day. Control (play) condition. The student was provided free access to attention and pre-
  • 14. ferred play materials available in the class- room. The teacher engaged in interactive play with the student and delivered attention at least every 30 s. No programmed conse- quences or demands were delivered for target behaviors. Attention condition. The student was allowed unrestricted access to activities/items typically available in the classroom. The teacher interacted with the student until he was engaged in an activity. Next, the teacher re- moved herself from the activity, saying she needed to do work at her desk. Contingent on target behavior(s), the teacher delivered verbal attention in the form of reprimands or redirec- tion to work, consistent with verbalizations noted in the descriptive observations. Follow- ing the delivery of attention, the teacher re- turned to work and the student continued to have free access to preferred items. Escape from academic demand con- dition. During the escape condition, the student was presented with work materials identified by the teacher as associated with problem behavior in the past. A graduated prompting (i.e., verbal, gestural, physical) se- quence was used to deliver academic de- mands. If problem behavior occurred, the teacher removed the academic demand and walked away from the student. No attention was provided to the student. Following the 30-s break period, the teacher returned to the student and delivered another demand and re- peated the procedure described above.
  • 15. Follow-up functional analysis. Fol- lowing the initial functional analysis, a fol- low-up functional analysis was conducted to investigate the additive effects of attention during the escape condition. All conditions were 10 min in duration, and 2-min breaks were given between conditions. The teacher implemented between 2 and 4 experimental conditions per day. The escape from academic demands and control/play conditions were im- plemented in an identical manner as in the initial functional analysis. Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 61 Escape-to-attention condition. Dur- ing the ETA condition, the student was pre- sented with work materials identified by the teacher as associated with problem behavior in the past. A graduated prompting (i.e., verbal, gestural, physical) sequence was used to de- liver academic demands. Contingent on target behavior, the teacher removed the task mate- rials and provided verbal attention during the 30-s break. The quality of teacher attention during the escape break and the nature of teacher attention were based on information obtained in the descriptive assessment (i.e., reprimands, redirections, physical attention). During the 30-s break, the teacher continued to deliver attention to the student in the typical
  • 16. manner for that classroom (e.g., “You need to get back to work”, “I told you no screaming, you have to work.”). Following the 30-s break, the teacher represented the task and the prompting sequence continued. Treatment evaluations. A treatment comparison was employed to evaluate the tar- get behavior under two different treatment types. One treatment, Escape Extinction, tar- geted escape only. The other treatment (Es- cape Extinction � Differential Reinforcement of Alternative Behaviors), targeted escape and teacher attention. The ETA treatment condi- tions were evaluated using a B/C/B/C design for Brandon and C/B/C designs for Franklin and J’Marcus. Escape extinction (EE). The EE con- dition was identical to the escape condition from the functional analysis with the excep- tion that no break was delivered for target behavior. Difficult academic materials were presented using a graduated prompting se- quence. No attention was delivered for target behaviors. Escape extinction � differential re- inforcement of alternative behaviors (EE�DRA). The EE�DRA condition was implemented identical to the EE condition with one exception. During EE�DRA phase, the schedule of attention was based on the descriptive data obtained during baseline ob- servations. For Brandon and J’Marcus, atten-
  • 17. tion was delivered every 30 s contingent on demonstrating appropriate behavior. For Franklin, teacher attention was delivered ev- ery 15 s. In this phase, teacher attention con- sisted of descriptive praise for appropriate be- havior (i.e., task engagement; “Great job working.”) and/or physical attention (e.g., pats on the back). If problem behavior occurred when the interval elapsed, the interval was reset and the teacher did not deliver attention to the student. That is, problem behavior did not result in the delivery of teacher attention. Interobserver Agreement Two observers were assigned to one stu- dent: one observer served as the primary data collector and the other for IOA. Agreement coefficients were calculated by dividing the total number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiply- ing by 100. IOA data were collected across a minimum of 30% of sessions during all phases of the study. IOA data during the initial func- tional analysis were: Brandon, M � 95% (range � 85%–100%); Franklin, M � 96% (range � 92%–100%); and J’Marcus, M � 95% (range � 90%–100%). IOA data during the follow-up functional analysis were: Bran- don, M � 98% (range � 90%–100%); Frank- lin, M � 91% (range � 85%–100%); and J’Marcus, M � 98% (range � 95%–100%). IOA data during the treatment sessions were: Brandon, M � 96% (range � 90%–100%); Franklin, M � 92% (range � 84%–100%); and J’Marcus, M � 93% (range � 90%–97%).
  • 18. Procedural and Treatment Integrity All teachers were trained to implement to implement the functional analysis and treat- ment evaluation conditions, based on proce- dures outlined by Moore, Edwards et al. (2002). For all activities for which teachers were trained, a series of steps was created. Procedural integrity was calculated each ses- sion and by dividing the number of correctly implemented steps by the total number of steps for that condition. Procedural integrity School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 62 data (see Appendix A) were collected during each functional analysis session and ranged from 90% to 100% across all teachers. Procedural integrity for individual com- ponents of each treatment condition was cal- culated for a minimum of 50% of the treat- ment evaluation sessions. During the EE con- dition, treatment integrity averaged 98% (range � 96%–100%) for Brandon’s teacher, 80% (range � 65%–91%) for Franklin’s teacher, and 89% (range � 86%–92%) for J’Marcus’s teacher. During the EE � DRA treatment, treatment integrity averaged 98% (range � 92%–100%), 86% (range � 58%– 100%), and 95% (range � 92%–100%) for
  • 19. Brandon’s, Franklin’s, and J’Marcus’s teach- ers, respectively. Results Initial Functional Analysis Initial functional analysis results are de- picted in Figure 1. Data supported the hypoth- esis that each participant’s behavior was rein- forced by escape from academic demands. Brandon (top panel) exhibited problem behav- ior in an average of 14% (range � 12%–19%) of the observed intervals during the escape condition. The mean percentage of intervals with problem behavior during the teacher at- tention condition was 0.67% (range � 0%– 2%). No problem behavior was observed dur- ing the control sessions. Franklin’s (middle panel) mean percentage of intervals with prob- lem behavior during the escape condition was 11.33% (range � 8%–16%) and less than 1% during the attention sessions (range � 0%– 2%). No problem behavior was observed dur- ing control sessions. The bottom panel of Fig- ure 1 depicts the results of the initial func- tional analysis for J’Marcus. The mean percentage of intervals containing problem be- havior during the escape condition was 32.67% (range � 29%–37%). No problem behavior was observed during control and at- tention conditions. Follow-up Functional Analysis The top panel of Figure 2 depicts the
  • 20. results of Brandon’s follow-up functional analysis. The mean percentage of intervals containing problem behavior during the es- cape condition was 10.5% (range � 7%– 15%), and no problem behavior occurred dur- ing the control condition. The mean percent- age of intervals with problem behavior in the ETA condition was 11.5% (range � 7%– 14%). The ETA condition resulted in slightly more problem behavior than the escape con- dition. However, given the substantial overlap of the level of behavior between the escape and ETA conditions, the addition of teacher attention during the escape interval did not produce differential levels of responding be- havior for Brandon across the two conditions. As shown in the middle panel of Fig- ure 2, Franklin’s mean percentage of intervals Figure 1. Percentage of intervals con- taining problem behavior during the initial functional analysis for Brandon (top panel), Franklin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bottom panel). Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 63 with problem behavior during the escape con- dition was 11.33% (range � 7%–17%). Low levels of problem behavior occurred during the control condition (range � 0%–3%). Prob-
  • 21. lem behavior in the ETA condition occurred in an average of 36.67% (range � 31%–44%) of intervals. The high level of behavior in the ETA and low level of behavior in the other two conditions suggests that Franklin’s prob- lem behavior was reinforced by attention dur- ing the escape period. The results of the follow-up functional analysis for J’Marcus are depicted in the bot- tom panel of Figure 2. The mean percentage of intervals with problem behavior during the escape condition was 23.33% (range � 22%– 25%), and no problem behavior occurred dur- ing the control condition. A substantial in- crease in problem behavior was observed during the ETA condition, (M � 46.67%; range � 40%–58%), suggesting that J’Marcus’s problem behavior was reinforced by attention delivered during breaks from work. ETA Treatment Evaluations Brandon. The top panel of Figure 3 depicts the percentage of intervals with prob- lem behavior and task engagement observed during Brandon’s treatment evaluation. Dur- ing the first EE treatment phase, an increasing trend in Brandon’s problem behavior was ob- served. (M � 38.4%; range � 17%–60%). Following the implementation of the EE� DRA treatment condition, an immediate de- crease was observed in Brandon’s problem behavior. The mean percentage of intervals with problem behavior was 11.8% (range �
  • 22. Figure 2. Percentage of intervals con- taining problem behavior during the modified functional analysis for Bran- don (top panel), Franklin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bottom panel). Figure 3. Percentage of intervals with problem behavior and task engage- ment for Brandon, (top panel), Frank- lin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bot- tom panel) during the escape-to- attention treatment evaluations with escape extinction (EE) and escape ex- tinction � differential reinforcement of alternative behaviors (DRA). School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 64 1%–23%) of intervals. When the EE treatment was reintroduced, Brandon’s problem behav- ior increased slightly to a mean percentage of intervals of 12.33% (range � 12%–13%). The level of problem behavior observed in the second EE phase was relatively stable but did not reach the levels observed in the first EE phase. Finally, the reintroduction of the com- bined treatment of EE�DRA produced stable, low levels of problem behavior (M � 4.5%; range � 2%–6%). The top panel of Figure 3 also depicts
  • 23. Brandon’s task engagement data during the ETA treatment evaluation sessions. During the first EE treatment phase, Brandon’s task en- gagement had a decreasing trend as problem behavior increased. Brandon was engaged with task materials, on average, during 27.2% (range � 0%–58%) of the intervals. When the combined EE�DRA treatment was imple- mented, Brandon’s level of task engagement increased immediately and continued on an upward trend over the phase (M � 73%; range � 40%–95%). Following the reintro- duction of the EE treatment, Brandon’s mean task engagement was stabilized at 78.67% (range � 78%–80%) of the intervals. In the final EE�DRA treatment phase, Brandon’s task engagement increased slightly to a mean of 86.5% (range � 83%–95%). Franklin. The results for Franklin’s ETA treatment evaluations are depicted in the middle panel of Figure 3. During the first EE�DRA treatment phase, Franklin’s prob- lem behavior decreased slightly across the phase (M � 9.17%; range � 2%–19%). When the EE treatment was implemented, a large and immediate increase in problem behavior was observed, averaging 46% (range � 38%– 45%) of the intervals. When the EE�DRA treatment was reintroduced, a large and imme- diate decrease was observed in Franklin’s problem behavior (M � 25.5%; range � 25%–26%). Franklin engaged with academic mate- rials during an average of 77.67% (range �
  • 24. 65%–95%) of intervals during the first EE�DRA treatment evaluation phase. With the implementation of the EE treatment, a large and immediate decrease was observed in Franklin’s task engagement (M � 47.33%; range � 42%–55%) of observed intervals. Fi- nally with the reintroduction of the EE�DRA, Franklin’s task engagement increased slightly to a mean of 61% (range � 52%–70%) of the observed intervals. J’Marcus. The bottom panel of Figure 3 depicts J’Marcus’s ETA treatment evaluation results for problem behavior and task engage- ment. During the initial EE�DRA treatment phase, J’Marcus exhibited low levels (M � 15%; range � 10%–20%) of problem behav- ior with a decreasing trend across the phase. Following the implementation of the EE treat- ment phase, problem behavior showed an immediate increase and continued trending upward (M � 51%; range � 33%–65%). Finally, after the reimplementation of the EE�DRA treatment, an immediate and large decrease was observed in J’Marcus’s problem behavior. Low and stable levels of problem behavior were observed in the final EE�DRA treatment condition, with a mean level of 3.75% (range � 2%–5%). During the EE�DRA treatment phase, J’Marcus was appropriately engaged with ac- ademic work materials during a mean of 78.67% (range � 73%–83%) of the in- tervals. During the EE treatment phase,
  • 25. J’Marcus’s task engagement decreased sharply over the phase with a mean percentage of 33.33% (range � 16%–62%) of intervals coded with problem behavior. When the EE�DRA treatment was reimplemented, an immediate increase in J’Marcus’s task engage- ment was observed, and behavior levels were stable throughout the phase (M � 97.5%; range � 94%–100%). Treatment Acceptability Each classroom teacher completed the IRP-15 at the conclusion of each treatment phase. Overall, all teachers rated the EE� DRA treatment condition as more acceptable than the EE treatment condition. For the EE�DRA condition, the total scores were 89 for all participants. For the EE condition, the following total scores were obtained: 71, 30, Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 65 and 16 for Brandon, Franklin, and J’Marcus’s teachers, respectively. Thus, two of the three teachers responded that the EE treatment was an “unacceptable” treatment evidenced by to- tal scores substantially lower than the tradi- tionally used cutoff score of 52.50. Discussion
  • 26. The purpose of the current investigation was to replicate and extend Mueller et al.’s (2005) case study of a novel functional anal- ysis condition designed to assess the additive effects of attention as a reinforcer during breaks from academic tasks. That is, the pres- ent study sought to determine whether the addition of teacher attention to an escape in- terval (i.e., ETA) would result in elevated lev- els of problem behavior when compared to a standard escape condition for 3 children. The second purpose of the study was to evaluate whether a treatment package that targeted es- cape and attention functions would reduce tar- get behavior better than a treatment targeting only escape. The descriptive data from the functional assessment suggested that problem behavior led to escape from task demands for all par- ticipants. Classroom observations revealed that teachers also provided attention (e.g., rep- rimands, requests to return to work) when the students were not engaged in work; low levels of peer attention for problem behavior were observed. The results of the initial functional analyses verified that all three participant’s problem behavior was maintained by escape from task demands. The first research question evaluated whether the addition of teacher at- tention during the escape interval would pro- duce elevated levels of problem behavior when compared to an escape-only condition. The results supported Mueller et al.’s (2005) findings, as the follow-up functional analysis showed increases in problem behavior in the
  • 27. ETA condition for 2 of the 3 participants rel- ative to the escape-only and play/control con- dition. Responding during the escape-only and ETA functional analyses was similar for Brandon. As hypothesized by Mueller et al. (2005), the findings that attention can rein- force problem behavior during a work task suggest that the presentation of task demands may motivate problem behavior reinforced by escape from an aversive task and by teacher attention. In the conditions described by Iwata et al. (1982), the only establishing operation for the assessment of attention as a reinforcer was the deprivation of attention. As seen in the current analyses, attention functioned as a re- inforcer in contexts other than those in which a child was being ignored. The second research question investi- gated treatment implications for ETA-main- tained problem behavior. A treatment program designed to match the escape function only (i.e., EE) versus a treatment targeting escape with the addition of teacher attention (i.e., EE�DRA) were evaluated. Problem behavior decreased for all participants during the EE�DRA treatment, and a general increasing trend in problem behavior was found during the standard EE treatment. Likewise, concom- itant increases in task engagement and de- creases in problem behavior were observed across all participants. Thus, differential re- sponsiveness to treatment based on behavioral function was observed, although differences
  • 28. for Brandon were minimal by the end of treatment. The present study adds to a growing literature base of studies investigating the ef- fect of multiple reinforcing variables delivered together (compound reinforcers) or in se- quential arrangement (chained reinforcers). Golonka et al. (2000) described the additive effects of escape to an enriched environment contingent upon appropriate behavior as treat- ment for problem behavior in a classroom. Although a compound or chained reinforcer was not used in their functional analysis, the treatment results supported the reductive ef- fects of contingent escape to an enriched en- vironment as more effective than contingent escape alone. Mueller et al. (2005) used Golonka et al.’s (2000) findings as the basis for their ETA condition. However, no treat- ment data were described in their one-partici- pant case study. The present results add to School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 66 Mueller et al.’s (2005) findings by replicating effects across additional participants. In addi- tion, preliminary treatment findings showed that when a behavior is reinforced by ETA, providing attention combined with EE inter- vention was more effective than escape extinc- tion alone.
  • 29. Although treatment phases were trun- cated for some participants, the treatment comparisons data provide some intriguing in- formation for future study. Immediate changes of a substantial magnitude were observed for all participants’ problem behavior during the initial EE�DRA condition, regardless of the ordering of treatments. In subsequent itera- tions of the treatment comparisons, problem behavior levels for J’Marcus and Franklin continued to show differential responding, with substantially lower levels observed in the combined treatment phase. Brandon, who did not exhibit differential responding during the modified ETA functional analysis, showed less substantial treatment differences across the treatment conditions in the latter treatment phase comparisons. It is possible that the ini- tial differences between the EE and EE�DRA treatments could reflect an extinction burst associated with the EE treatment, rather than an actual difference between the two treat- ments. It is also possible that the inclusion of prompts in the EE condition may have pro- vided students with preferred attention, there- fore making the condition functionally similar to the EE�DRA condition. However, given the observed differences between the two treatment conditions, the effects of prompts were likely minimal. Additional treatment comparisons may provide additional support for matching treatment programs to behavioral function, the ultimate goal behind conducting a functional behavioral assessment.
  • 30. The simplest explanation of the current treatment results is that the EE�DRA treat- ment worked better than EE for Franklin and J’Marcus because EE�DRA addressed both the escape and the attention aspects of the compound function. For Brandon, although EE initially produced higher level of problem behavior, each treatment reduced the behavior to similar levels by the end of the treatment evaluation. The explanation of Brandon’s out- comes is also straightforward, but different from the reasons why the EE�DRA worked so well with Franklin and J’Marcus. That is, the addition of a positive reinforcer into Bran- don’s demand context most likely reduced the aversiveness of the task and therefore reduced the motivation to demonstrate escape-main- tained behavior. The benefits of using positive reinforce- ment techniques to reduce behaviors main- tained by escape have been used successfully for over 30 years. Carr, Newsom, and Binkoff (1980) first demonstrated that attenuating the aversiveness of task demand situations through the delivery of highly preferred edi- bles during work tasks reduced escape-main- tained problem behavior. Several studies fol- lowed replicating and supporting the aver- siveness-attenuating benefits of introducing preferred tangibles or food items into demand contexts (e.g., Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski, 1997; Mazaleski, Iwata, Vollmer, Zarcone, & Smith, 1993; Mueller, Edwards, & Trahant, 2003). Adding preferred tangibles to a demand
  • 31. context makes use of reinforcers not identified during the functional assessment. That is, the addition of positive reinforcers into demand contexts can reduce escape-maintained behav- ior by using noncontingent reinforcement with a functional or arbitrary reinforcer, or by pos- itively reinforcing behaviors such as task en- gagement or compliance through differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA). The results of the present study apply this well-known concept to a treatment in which escape and attention functions required sup- port. By interjecting positive reinforcers into the demand context using DRA procedures, the aversiveness of the task was reduced through the provision of a functional reinforcer. Data also were collected for treatment acceptability, which has not been commonly reported in previous FBA research (Ervin et al., 2001). In the present study, all three teach- ers rated the EE�DRA treatment as more acceptable treatment than the EE alone. Sur- prisingly, two of the three teachers responded that the EE treatment was an “unacceptable” treatment; as such, low ratings of treatment Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 67 acceptability are often not reported in the pub- lished literature. Teachers specifically re- ported that they strongly disagreed that the EE
  • 32. treatment was effective for changing problem behavior, acceptable to use in the classroom, and consistent with interventions they had used in the past. These ratings may have been influenced by the fact that teachers completed acceptability ratings post-treatment use, after they had experience implementing the two interventions and had seen graphed data sup- porting the relative effectiveness of the EE�DRA treatment to the EE treatment alone, a finding reported in analogue treatment acceptability research (e.g., Tingstrom, 1989; Tingstrom, McPhail, & Bolton, 1989; Von- Brock & Elliott, 1987). Likewise, the higher ratings for the combined treatment may have been influenced by the addition of differential reinforcement for task engagement (i.e., praise), as previous researchers have reported that reinforcement-based interventions are generally rated as more acceptable than pun- ishment-based interventions (e.g., Blampied & Kahan, 1992; Elliott, Witt, Galvin, & Peter- son, 1984). Although extinction-based proce- dures are not technically classified as punish- ment in the behavioral literature, the teachers may have perceived the treatment as such, and thus rated it less favorably vis-à-vis a treat- ment that included a richer schedule of reinforcement. A few limitations in the current study are worth noting. Data collection was discon- tinued early for Franklin because of a 9-day suspension from school for fighting at end of the school year. Thus, only two data points were collected during the final treatment
  • 33. phase. As such, definitive information about the long-term effects of the EE�DRA treat- ment is unknown. Additional replications of treatment phases, counterbalancing treatment order across students with escape-only and ETA functions, extended data collection within phases, and follow-up data would strengthen the experimental design of such studies and add information about long-term treatment effects. Next, the current treatment comparisons, although successful in reducing target behavior, did not allow for a full anal- ysis of the mechanisms underlying the change. As described above, multiple theoretical pos- tulates are reasonable and plausible. Future studies may seek to determine exactly which mechanisms of behavior change are at play to reduce the behaviors. For example, a study might compare the reductive effects of EE with those of noncontingent reinforcement with an arbitrary reinforcer such as preferred tangible items to discover whether the addition of the attention in the current study reduced target behavior because it was functionally related to the target behavior or whether it simply reduced the aversiveness of the tasks presented. Notwithstanding the limitations, the present study provides additional support for translating FBA research into practice in the schools. One criticism of previous FBA re- search is that individuals with specialized training (e.g., researchers, behavior special- ists) conducted the functional analysis condi-
  • 34. tions. In the current investigation, the stu- dents’ teachers implemented all assessment and treatment conditions, albeit with supervi- sion and feedback. Another strength is that the functional analysis occurred during standard classroom activities, rather than an analogue setting. In addition, all participants were gen- eral education students, unlike the majority of FBA studies conducted with participants with disabilities (Ervin et al., 2001; Hanley et al., 2003; Hoff et al., 2005). The present study is in line with previ- ous research documenting the importance of identifying idiosyncratic variables during FBAs, in an effort to develop the most effec- tive treatment plan (Hoff et al., 2005). The results also add to a growing literature of school-based studies investigating the effect of idiosyncratic variables on problem behavior. The findings provide a heuristic and experi- mental example for incorporating descriptive assessment information into the development of functional analysis conditions in the school setting. Similar to previous studies, the results show descriptive assessment information was vital to the precise identification of behavioral function (Galiatsatos & Graff, 2003; Lalli & Casey, 1996; Mueller et al., 2005; Richman & School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 68
  • 35. Hagopian, 1999; Tiger, Hanley, & Bessette, 2006). The ETA condition was designed spe- cifically to assess the relative contributions of escape from academic demands and the addi- tive effects of teacher attention following problem behavior, a consequent frequently ob- served in classroom settings (Kurtz et al., 2003; McKerchar & Thompson, 2004). The results suggest the ETA condition may have utility for FBAs and treatment planning in the school setting. In future research, researchers may wish to investigate other sources of rein- forcement that may occur during the escape interval (i.e., peer attention, access to preferred activities/items). Also, researchers should focus on additional ETA-based treatment options. Future researchers may wish to investigate the addition of teacher attention with other es- cape-maintained treatment options. References Blampied, N. M., Kahan, E. (1992). Acceptability of alternative punishments: A community survey. Behav- ior Modification, 16, 400–413. Broussard, C. D., & Northup, J. (1995). An approach to functional assessment and analysis of disruptive be- havior in regular education classrooms. School Psy- chology Quarterly, 10, 154–164. Broussard, C. D., & Northup, J. (1997). The use of func- tional analysis to develop peer interventions for dis- ruptive classroom behavior. School Psychology Quar- terly, 12, 65–76.
  • 36. Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980). Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior in two retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy- sis, 13, 101–117. Doggett, R. A., Edwards, R. P., Moore, J. W., Tingstrom, D. H., & Wilczynski, S. M. (2001). An approach to functional assessment in general education classroom settings. School Psychology Review, 30, 313–328. Dufrene, B. A., Doggett, R. A., Henington, C., & Watson, T. S. (2007). Functional assessment and intervention for disruptive classroom behaviors in preschool and Head Start classrooms. Journal of Behavioral Educa- tion, 16, 368–388. Edwards, R. P. (2002). A tutorial for using the functional assessment informant record for teachers (FAIR-T). Proven Practice, 4, 31–33. Elliott, S. N., Witt, J. C., Galvin, G. A., & Peterson, R. (1984). Acceptability of positive and reductive behav- ioral interventions: Factors that influence teachers’ de- cisions. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 353–360. Ellis, J., & Magee, S. (2004). Modifications to basic functional analysis procedures in school settings: A selective review. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 205– 228. Ervin, R. A., Radford, P. M., Bertsch, K., Piper, A. L., Ehrhardt, K. E., & Poling, A. (2001). A descriptive analysis and critique of the empirical literature on school-based functional assessment. School Psychol- ogy Review, 30, 193–210.
  • 37. Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. A. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as a treatment of self-injurious behavior. Journal of Ap- plied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249. Galiatsatos, G. T., & Graff, R. B. (2003). Combining descriptive and functional analyses to assess and treat screaming. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 123–128. Golonka, Z., Wacker, D., Berg, W., Derby, M., K., Hard- ing, J., & Peck, S. (2000). Effects of escape to alone versus escape to enriched environments on adaptive and aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 243–246. Gresham, F. M., McIntyre, L. L., Olson-Tinker, H., Dol- stra, L., McLaughlin, V., & Van, M. (2004). Relevance of functional behavioral assessment research for school-based intervention and positive behavioral sup- port. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 19– 37. Gunter, P. L., Jack, S. L., Shores, R. E., Carrell, D. E., & Flowers, J. (1993). Lag sequential analysis as a tool for functional analysis of student disruptive behavior in classrooms. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Dis- orders, 1, 138–148. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. Hoff, K. E., Ervin, R. A., & Friman, P. C. (2005). Refining functional behavior assessment: Analyzing the sepa- rate and combined effects of hypothesized controlling
  • 38. variables during ongoing classroom routines. School Psychology Review, 34, 45–57. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Develop- mental Disabilities, 2, 3–20. Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg, J. S., Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., et al. (2000). Skill acqui- sition in the implementation of functional analysis methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 181–194. Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S., O’Conner, J. T., Paclawskyj, T., et al. (2003). Func- tional analysis and treatment of self-injurious behavior in young children: A summary of 30 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219. Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied Be- havior Analysis, 29, 391–396. Mann, A. J., & Mueller, M. M. (2009). False positive functional analysis results as a contributor of treatment failure during functional communication training. Ed- ucation and Treatment of Children, 32, 121–149. Martens, B. K., Witt, J. C., Elliot, S. N., & Darveaux, D. X. (1985). Teacher judgments concerning the ac- ceptability of school-based interventions. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 16, 191–198. Mazaleski, J. A., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the reinforce-
  • 39. ment and extinction components in DRO contingencies with self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 143–156. McKerchar, P. M., & Thompson, R. H. (2004). A descrip- tive analysis of potential reinforcement contingencies Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 69 in the preschool classroom. Journal of Applied Behav- ior Analysis, 37, 431–443. Moore, J. W., Edwards, R. P., Sterling-Turner, H. E., Riley, J., DuBard, M., & McGeorge, A. (2002). Teacher acquisition of functional analysis methodol- ogy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 73–77. Moore, J. W., Mueller, M. M., Dubard, M., Roberts, D. S., Sterling-Turner, H. E. (2002). The influence of thera- pist attention on self-injury during a tangible condition. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 283–286. Mueller, M. M., Edwards, R. P., & Trahant, D. (2003). Translating multiple assessment techniques into an in- tervention selection model for classrooms. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 563–573. Mueller, M. M., Nkosi, A., & Hine, J. F. (in press). Functional analysis in public school settings: A review of 90 functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. Mueller, M. M., Sterling-Turner, H. E., & Moore, J. M.
  • 40. (2005). Towards developing a classroom-based func- tional analysis condition to assess escape-to-attention as a variable maintaining problem behavior. School Psychology Review, 34, 425–431. Repp, A. C., Felce, D., & Barton, L. E. (1988). Basing the treatment of stereotypic and self-injurious behaviors on hypotheses of their causes. Journal of Applied Behav- ior Analysis, 21, 281–289. Richman, D. M., & Hagopian, L. P. (1999). On the effects of “quality” of attention in the functional analysis of destructive behavior. Research in Developmental Dis- abilities, 20, 51–62. Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bessette, K. K. (2006). Incorporating descriptive assessment results into the design of a functional analysis: Case example involv- ing a preschooler’s handmouthing. Education & Treat- ment of Children, 29, 107–124. Tingstrom, D. H. (1990). Acceptability of time-out: The influence of problem behavior severity, interventionist, and reported effectiveness. Journal of School Psychol- ogy, 28, 165–169. Tingstrom, D. H., McPhail, R. L., & Bolton, A. B. (1989). Acceptability of alternative school-based interven- tions: The influence of reported effectiveness and age of target child. The Journal of Psychology, 123, 133– 140. VanDerHeyden, A. M., Witt, J. C., & Gatti, S. (2001). Descriptive assessment method to reduce overall dis- ruptive behavior in a preschool classroom. School Psy-
  • 41. chology Review, 30(4), 548–567. Von Brock, M. B., & Elliott, S. N. (1987). Influence of treatment effectiveness information on the acceptabil- ity of classroom interventions. Journal of School Psy- chology, 25, 131–144. Date Received: August 27, 2009 Date Accepted: January 15, 2011 Action Editor: Tanya Eckert � Jana Sarno, MA, BCBA, is a senior program supervisor with Coyne and Associates Education Corporation in San Diego, California, and a doctoral candidate at The Uni- versity of Southern Mississippi. Her areas of interest include early intervention with children diagnosed with autism, verbal behavior, and functional analysis. Heather E. Sterling, PhD, is an associate professor of psychology and the director of training for the school psychology program at The University of Southern Mississippi. Her research areas include functional assessment and analysis, single-case research design, school-based consultation, and intellectual and developmental disabilities. Michael Mueller, PhD, BCBA-D, received his doctorate in school psychology from The University of Southern Mississippi. He is currently the director of behavioral services for Southern Behavioral Group in Atlanta, Georgia.
  • 42. Brad A. Dufrene, PhD, is an assistant professor and director of the School Psychology Service Center at The University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests include functional assessment and school-based consultation. Daniel H. Tingstrom, PhD, is a professor of psychology and is affiliated with the school psychology program in the Department of Psychology at The University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests include applied behavior analysis, and the implemen- tation and evaluation of behavioral and academic interventions. D. Joe Olmi, PhD, is a professor of psychology and departmental chairperson at The University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests include positive behavioral interventions and supports, compliance training, and school- based consultation. School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1 70 APPENDIX A PROCEDURAL INTEGRITY FOR ETA CONDITION Student: Session: Teacher: Date: Observer: Condition: ETA
  • 43. This form is used to assess the level of procedural integrity for each teacher-implemented functional analysis ETA condition. Record if the teacher behaviors were implemented as planned (Yes) or not implemented as planned (No) during each FA control condition. YES NO N/A 1. Seats student at his/her desk or table 2. Teacher places academic materials on the student’s desk 3. Teacher provides verbal instructions to student to complete the academic work 4. Teacher waits 5 seconds for compliance a. The student complies i. Teacher provides descriptive praise ii. Teacher moves to the next demand b. The student does not comply i. Teacher restates the instructions with verbal and gestural prompts ii. Teacher waits 5 seconds for compliance A. Student complies 1. Teacher provides descriptive praise 2. Teacher moves to the next demand
  • 44. B. Student does not comply 1. Teacher restates the instructions and provides hand-over-hand guidance 5. Teacher does not respond to any other problem behavior 6. Contingent on problem behavior a. Teacher removes task demand for 30 s b. Teacher provides attention during escape period Repeat steps 3–6 for each demand sequence Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable 71 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.