SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 15
Download to read offline
This article was downloaded by: [Novo Nordisk A/S]
On: 13 March 2015, At: 06:51
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Social Epistemology: A Journal of
Knowledge, Culture and Policy
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tsep20
The Challenges of Cross‐disciplinary
Research
Jens Aagaard‐Hansen
Published online: 05 Dec 2007.
To cite this article: Jens Aagaard‐Hansen (2007) The Challenges of Cross‐disciplinary
Research, Social Epistemology: A Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy, 21:4, 425-438, DOI:
10.1080/02691720701746540
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02691720701746540
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Social Epistemology
Vol. 21, No. 4, October–December 2007, pp. 425–438
ISSN 0269–1728 (print)/ISSN 1464–5297 (online) © 2007 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/02691720701746540
The Challenges of Cross-disciplinary
Research
Jens Aagaard-HansenTaylor and FrancisTSEP_A_274611.sgm10.1080/02691720701746540Social Epistemology0269-1728 (print)/1464-5297 (online)Original Article2007Taylor & Francis214000000October–December 2007
During the past decades, research collaboration between researchers from different disci-
plines has become more frequent. However, there is a need to look into the generic modalities
and challenges. The article explores a series of potential obstructions to cross-disciplinary
collaboration of methodological and epistemological nature. Furthermore, a number of
contextual, inhibiting factors are outlined. As means of overcoming the obstacles, the
importance of mutual knowledge, allocation of adequate time and conducive research
management is emphasised. New teams may benefit from tutoring by facilitators, who can
help to make problem areas explicit and negotiate solutions. Owing to the training back-
ground of the author, most of the examples are drawn from the interface between biomed-
icine/naturalscienceandappliedmedicalanthropology.However,theissuesraisedbasically
apply to all sorts of cross-disciplinary research collaboration in various combinations.
Keywords: Applied Research; Biomedicine; Cross-disciplinarity; Epistemology;
Facilitators; Medical Anthropology; Methodology; Paradigms; Research Management
Introduction
Thetopicofthisarticleistheclusterofdifferencesthatshouldbeovercomeifresearchers
with different disciplinary background are to collaborate. It will be guided by the defi-
nitions of Rosenfield (1992, 1351), who ventured a taxonomy of the level of integration
between disciplines:
Level one: Multidisciplinary. Researchers work in parallel or sequentially from disciplinary-
specific base to address common problem.
Level two: Interdisciplinary. Researchers work jointly but still from disciplinary-specific
basis to address common problem.
Jens Aagaard-Hansen is a senior researcher with a double training background as medical doctor and anthropol-
ogist. For the past 14 years he has been based at DBL—Institute for Health Research and Development (formerly
Danish Bilharziasis Laboratory). He has been involved in cross-disciplinary research, applied medical anthropol-
ogy, research management and research capacity strengthening mainly in developing countries. Correspondence
to: Jens Aagaard-Hansen, DBL—Centre for Health Research and Development (formerly Danish Bilharziasis
Laboratory), Jaegersborg Allé 1D, DK-2920 Charlottenlund, Denmark. Email: jah@life.ku.dk
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
426 J. Aagaard-Hansen
Level three: Transdisciplinary. Researchers work jointly using shared conceptual frame-
work drawing together disciplinary-specific theories, concepts, and approaches to address
common problem.
The general perception of and distinction between multidisciplinarity and interdiscipli-
narity is shared by a majority of relevant scholars (for example, Albrecht, Higginbotham
and Freeman 2001, 73; Friedman and Friedman 1985, 77; Gilbert 1998, 6; Kilburn 1990,
132 ; Kline 1995, 2; Maina-Ahlberg, Nordberg and Tomson 1997, 1230; McNeill 1999,
314). Although they phrase it in different ways and may even have slightly different
views, there is a general agreement that interdisciplinarity entails more integration than
multidisciplinarity. Even Rosenfield’s addition of transdisciplinarity is endorsed by
many (Albrecht, Higginbotham and Freeman 2001, 73; Maina-Ahlberg, Nordberg and
Tomson 1997, 1230; McNeill 1999, 314). In this article the term cross-disciplinarity will
be used according to Rosenfield (1992, 1351); that is, as a general designation for all the
three terms (multidisciplinarity, interdisciplinarity and transdisciplinarity). Aagaard-
Hansen and Ouma draw attention to the time dimension in the sense that cross-disci-
plinarity “is a gradual process in which the research group little by little moves in the
direction of integration—from multi- to transdisciplinarity and which is taking place
at different paces” (2002, 206).
There are basically two motives for cross-disciplinary research, either to provide
results that can be applied or as a source of inspiration for new, overarching research
questions and exchange of methods and conceptual frameworks (Aagaard-Hansen
2003, 32–36).
When the extent of integration of disciplines is considered, it also plays a role how
different they are. McNeill explains “the nature of both bridge-building and restructur-
ing will differ according to what I call the scope that is, the number of disciplines
involved and the distance between them” (1999, 314). He continues, “the distance
between disciplines can in some cases appear small, in terms of subject matter, but
be large in terms of methodology or perspective” (McNeill 1999, 315). Based on the
training background of the author, the examples used in this article are derived from
collaboration between anthropology and biomedicine. However, the issues raised are
of a general nature and are to various extents relevant to interaction between other
disciplines as well. Many of the points are hardly new as such, but are brought together
here in an overview that may be of use to research teams who venture into the field or
programme managers who want to commission research of this kind. The article is
making its points by setting up a series of dichotomies. Evidently, this is an oversimpli-
fication of the many ongoing, cross-disciplinary projects that are rather positioned at
continua between the extremes.1
However, there are two justifications for this: one is
didactic; the other is that a certain proportion of our colleagues are still bound by these
polarised positions.
Challenges for Cross-disciplinary Research
However good the intentions, cross-disciplinarity is difficult to operationalise. In the
process, researchers may face many situations in which collaboration is impeded
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
Social Epistemology 427
either by lack of knowledge, divergent standards, different approaches, or simply
negative attitudes and prejudices. Below is a brief outline of the main areas in which
problems may arise. The issues are intimately interlinked, but described separately for
the sake of clarity.
Quantitative Versus Qualitative Methods
The difference in data collection methods is usually the most conspicuous, and
consequently the first, issue that comes to mind. For scientists having been socialised
within particular discourses, it can be rather difficult to come to terms with alternative
approaches. Either figures or big chunks of text and observation notes may be consid-
ered the only appropriate way of representing the reality. In the process of establishing
a cross-disciplinary team it is necessary to reach an initial, common understanding that
the data collection method of choice is directly derived from the character of the
research question and that no method is better than any other per se. However, if the
time and funds allow, it is possible to get the best of both worlds by combined use of
quantitative and qualitative data collection methods.
Closed Versus Open Approach
The choice of data collection methods often has a direct bearing on the degree of open-
ness. If the aim is to end up with a concise figure describing a certain occurrence
(combination of variables), a closed approach is needed—irrespective of whether it is
the measurement of a chemical substance or choices between pre-defined responses to
distinct questions in a questionnaire. If the idea is to obtain a balanced picture of a
phenomenon as it is seen through the eyes of an individual, a more open approach is
suitable. Both are appropriate under certain circumstances—the former when it is
important to explore tangible, quantified correlations between different variables; the
latter when the main aim is to provide a multi-faceted, thick description of the new
field. The former is necessarily based on the researchers’ preconceived categorisation;
the latter on trying to avoid exactly that and letting the categories emerge from the data.
Both are valid approaches depending on the circumstances.
However, there is another level at which the degree of openness is relevant—the level
of study design. It is considered a virtue for many biomedical studies that as much as
possible is planned in advance (e.g. specific research questions, study populations,
sampling procedures, design, exclusion criteria and data collection methods), in order
to be able to control bias and confounders influencing the outcomes under study.
Although the same applies to a certain extent to anthropological studies as well, it is not
quite as rigid. A change of focus, study population or data collection method halfway
through a study would usually mean a major threat to a biomedical trial, whereas it
would be more legitimate for an anthropologist. In fact it is considered a virtue for an
anthropologist to be receptive to unforeseen events in the field and adapt accordingly,
and it does not necessarily jeopardise the study per se. As with data collection methods
there are no good or bad, but simply more or less appropriate, designs.
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
428 J. Aagaard-Hansen
Objectivism Versus Subjectivism
The issue of openness described above is linked to one of the predominant sets of prej-
udices. The fact that anthropologists function as their own data collection instruments
and that they have more degrees of freedom when it comes to introducing changes in
the research process makes some biomedical colleagues wonder about the objectivism
of the data produced. Where is the borderline between scientific findings and the
researcher’s personal opinions? The opposite position constitutes scepticism towards
the biomedical science’s claim for objectivity. Whereas the archetypal, medical stand is
a quest for objectivism based on traditional positivistic virtues, anthropologists see
subjectivism as a basic condition for all research and that anthropology is based on a
process of inter-subjectivity (Jackson 1998; Schutz and Luckmann 1974). In brief, this
refers to a scientific position that acknowledges subjectivism is a basic precondition for
research per se. Thus, the aim should not be to eliminate subjectivism, but to take it into
considerationasacharacteristicoftheprocessofdataproductioninvolvingtwosubjects.
Causality Versus Description
Much outstanding research has simply aimed at describing phenomena as they are; for
example, descriptive, epidemiological studies of disease patterns in a given population
or cultural studies of perceptions and practices in a certain ethnic group. Other studies
have had the aim of illuminating a specific causal relationship or a cluster of the same;
for example, whether a high level of education makes women have other treatment-
seeking practices for their infants than women with a low educational level. Notwith-
standing the importance of both ventures, this is an area where the two scientific
discourses often differ. Hence, it is more legitimate to conduct non-causality studies
within anthropology than it is within biomedical science. A research question that does
not deal with causal relationships is often considered a bit shallow by medical scientists,
whereas it is the rule rather than the exception for anthropologists.
Apart from the implicit attitudes attached to these positions, there is a much more
tangible consequence as well. The nature of such a research question has major conse-
quences for the design. The quest to answer the specific causality type of questions
necessitates a certain sample size, sampling procedure, choice of closed data collection
method and an appropriate study design.2 This is related to the section on closed versus
open approach above.
Text Versus Context
There is another dichotomy that characterises the two scientific discourses: the focus
on either text or context. The terms, which are derived from linguistics, refer to the text
(a phenomenon, a sign, a sentence, an item, an act) and the context (the setting, the
socio-cultural environment, the biotope in which the text is situated). Put simply,
biomedical research focuses on the text and tries to control context as a source of noise
(confounders) that threatens to disrupt the research. It can be the aim to study a certain
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
Social Epistemology 429
dependent variable (the growth rate of bacteria or incidence of a disease) by controlling
every factor in the environment (the media and temperature or other potential risk
factors) except from the independent variable in question. In contrast, anthropologists
make context a main area of research in its own right. Context in its multiplicity is stud-
ied as a necessary part of a given phenomenon. Only by seeing the variations of the text
in the light of the complexity of and changes in the context can a valid picture of socio-
cultural issues be obtained. Among these two basic positions, one is not superior to the
other per se. But as is the case for data collection methods, one or the other is preferable
in order to conduct good research on different kinds of research questions. However,
if this basic difference in approach is not realised, it can cause major problems in cross-
disciplinary teams.
Absolute Versus Relative Perspective
Research is conducted in order to provide new knowledge. Some (more positivistic
inclined)scientiststakeitforgrantedthatthereisatruthoutthere,andthatthechallenge
is to get as close as possible to it by conducting good research. Other (more phenome-
nologically inclined) colleagues do not see their aim to be a search for a golden truth,
but rather a quest for providing valid representations of local lifeworlds (Schutz and
Luckmann 1974, 59–92). Whereas the biomedical researchers explore universal causal
relationships (e.g. smoking as a cause for lung cancer), the anthropologists have the
ambition of describing the world as seen through the eyes of the particular study
population in question (i.e. if witchcraft is considered an important cause of misfortune
in a certain ethnic group, the challenge for the anthropologist is to describe that in a
fair manner—not to evaluate whether it is real or not). It is crucial that this anthropo-
logicalendeavourisnotlookeddownuponbybiomedicalcolleagues,butisappreciated,
partly because it is based on sound scientific (but different) ground and partly because
it is exactly the source of the potential usefulness of anthropology to public health. On
the other hand, anthropologists have to realise that there is a need for closed and rigidly
designed studies in order to provide knowledge about the effect of various interventions
without a number of confounding factors. This divide is often expressed as the etic
(global scientific standard, in this case biomedicine) versus the emic (local, indigenous
view as described by the anthropologists) position (Pike 1971).
Representativity and Validity
The issue of representativity plays a major role in most public health research. It is the
issue of whether the study sample (e.g. 200 people from which a specific blood sample
has been taken) share the same characteristics as the larger population about which
inferences are made (e.g. the people of Finland). Only if the sample and the popula-
tion have the same proportions of all relevant variables (such as sex, age, ethnicity,
class or whatever else plays a role with regard to the research question) are broader
conclusions justified beyond the borders of the sample. The issue of representativity
should be considered already in the planning phase.3
In this article, validity is defined
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
430 J. Aagaard-Hansen
as “the extent to which a study finding reflects a real fact of life”.4
In this broad defini-
tion, many things can contribute to reduced validity. For instance various sorts of
systematic error can play a role, such as selection bias (i.e. skewness as a result of
mismatch between sample and population) or information bias (i.e. the phenomenon
that the source of information—a thermometer or an interview—does not give a true
picture of the phenomenon the researcher wants to study). But it can also be caused
by random error, which is a reflection of natural variation in data collection methods
(and hence imprecise data). Whatever the case, a high degree of validity is the aim of
every researcher.
Usually anthropological studies deal with relatively few informants, with whom the
researcher interacts for long periods of time typically during series of in-depth
interviews and observations. The in-depth data collection takes place at the expense
of having a relatively small sample size, and is consequently more exposed to be influ-
enced by individual variation among the informants. On the other hand, biomedical
studies, however complex they may be, typically take relatively simpler measures (e.g.
blood samples, questions, weight, etc.) from each individual, which permits such
studies to have large sample sizes. Thus, an anthropological study will tend to provide
very valid data (exactly because the researcher has interacted with the same infor-
mants for a long time) whereby more hidden and sensitive issues are more likely to
come to the fore and be seen in changing contexts, whereas the standardised way in
which biomedical data are collected does not offer the same opportunity. In short,
anthropological studies are typically relatively weak when it comes to representativity
and are strong in validity, whereas the opposite is often the case for biomedical
studies. Again one is not better than the other per se, but is simply suitable for differ-
ent purposes. As with data collection methods, it is here possible to design studies
that are strong in both aspects if resources allows.
Role of Theory
All scientific disciplines are grounded in theoretical foundations and paradigms.
Biomedicine has developed a tightly knit set of anatomical, physiological, genetic and
biochemical models that have proved extremely useful when it comes to explaining
disease processes. Apart from a few philosophically inclined public health scientists,
these theories and models are taken for granted as absolute, fixed points in the episte-
mological universe. In contrast to this stance, the average anthropologist spends quite
a bit of time discussing the theories on which studies are based. The extent to which
theoretical issues are of concern is a major difference between the two disciplines and
a potential source of prejudices. The anthropologists may be perceived as wordy people
making a lot of fuss about nothing, whereas the opposite prejudice is that biomedical
researchers are turning a blind eye to the shaky, epistemological ground of their own
endeavour. For the anthropologists working in the applied field, this poses a particular
challenge. Some claim that it is not possible to live up to the academic, disciplinary
demands of dealing adequately with theoretical issues and being applied at the same
time. It is indeed difficult, but not impossible.
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
Social Epistemology 431
Research Ethics
Biomedicine has a tradition for detailed ethical codes whereas some anthropological
guidelines are held in more generalised terms (Aagaard-Hansen, Johansen and Riis
2004). For instance, the key element of informed consent is dealt with much more
explicitly by the biomedical codes (Fluehr-Lobban 2003). This is not to say that some
disciplines are unethical. However, when disciplines are to collaborate it may take some
effort to harmonise the approaches to ethical issues as well.
Terminology
Many of the methodological and epistemological differences are expressed in different
terms. In the cases where there are distinct jargons, this may pose an additional hurdle
to overcome. More subtly, however, there are sometimes varying meanings of the same
terms within different disciplinary discourses. McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker
also point to language variations in different disciplines as a potential barrier, and say
that “sometimes there appears to be common understanding and it is not for some time
that serious divergence in understanding is revealed” (2001, 17). Svedin et al. (1999)
point out terminology as an important element of assessing cross-disciplinary projects.
Are key concepts used consistently within the “project language”? The relation to the
use of facilitators (see below) is evident.
Power Balance
As anything else in life, research is influenced by power and status. Research projects
can be seen as the potential battlefield of individuals as well as disciplines. Historically,
some disciplines can be perceived as being more powerful than others—either because
of access to funds or simply due to status. Disciplinary boundaries are often propped
up by attitudes, the origins of which are rarely rational. A strong influence comes from
the positivist philosophy, which was launched by Auguste Comte (1798–1857) and
which still frames the thinking of many. As expressed by Kline, “the hierarchy that
Comte suggested went in descending order: math, astronomy, physics, chemistry,
biology (including physiology), sociology” (1995, 208). Another view on the prestige
heldbyvariousbranchesofresearch,accordingtoFriedmanandFriedman,isthat“there
is an informal hierarchy of disciplines and fields in academe in which the more presti-
gious theoretical basic disciplines regard scholarly interaction with colleagues in the
applied, less theoretical fields as an unrewarding experience” (1985, 79). Traditionally,
biomedicine has had a stronger position than social science. In cases where disciplines
have collaborated, the biomedical researchers have typically been the principal investi-
gators whereas the social scientists have been invited to solve specific problems, such as
mobilisationofthestudypopulationorprovisionofdataoncertainconfoundingfactors
suchassocio-economicbackground.Aagaard-HansenandOumaemphasisetheimpor-
tance of the balance between the disciplines, a notion that encompassed two elements;
firstly the methodological differences between the participating disciplines (“how to do
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
432 J. Aagaard-Hansen
research”), and secondly the power balance (“who decides what to study”). The authors
state that “managing an interdisciplinary project in such a way that it strikes a balance
between common reference to main objectives and disciplinary freedom is maybe one
of the main challenges to research managers in positions like ours” (Aagaard-Hansen
and Ouma 2002, 203).5
Bridging the Gap
The many potential challenges listed above may be overcome. In this section, some
practical remedies are outlined.
Mutual Knowledge
An obvious first step when cross-disciplinary teams are to succeed is to obtain mutual
knowledge. Insight into the basics of methodology, theories, epistemological and
historical aspects of the others’ disciplinary discourse (as have been described in detail
above) is essential for understanding and respecting the position of collaborators from
other fields. Conceptual compatibility is the basis for understanding and overcoming
negative prejudices and creating respect. Furthermore, knowledge about alternatives
gives a healthy perspective on one’s own habitual thinking, which is quite often taken
for granted.
Adequate Time
Several groups emphasise the need for a long-time perspective for cross-disciplinarity
to succeed (McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker 2001, 26; Silva, Peeters and Lewis
1994, 184). Rosenfield expresses it as follows: “with continuing support to the same
team over a sufficiently long period of time and covering several types of problems, it
is more likely that disciplinary barriers can be transcended and increased understanding
and confidence about the value of other disciplines can be achieved” (1992, 1345).
Allwood and Bärmark state it more daringly: “one lesson to be learned from this is that
it might take a research project or two in order for a group of researchers to develop the
level of competence necessary for an interdisciplinary generation of problems” (1999,
78). Hence, researchers as well as funding agencies should allocate additional time for
the collaboration to succeed.
Conducive Research Management
The topic of research management is to a large extent neglected. If research manage-
ment as such is difficult, management of cross-disciplinary research is even more
challenging because of its complex nature and the particular problems it poses. The
ability of the team leader to create a conducive working environment in which
synergism can blossom between the disciplines is crucial. In their book on “mode 2
knowledge production”, Gibbons et al. point out the challenge of the “management
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
Social Epistemology 433
of disciplinary identities in transdisciplinary settings and the development of trans-
disciplinary capacities” (1994, 138–139). The importance of the human factor for
cross-disciplinary research has been explored by Gold and Gold (1983, 86),
Øvretveit (1993, 139–158) and Allwood and Bärmark (1999, 79). Aagaard-Hansen
and Ouma (2002) have analysed various aspects of management in relation to cross-
disciplinarity.
Facilitators
Severalauthorsrecommendthattheintricateprocessofestablishingacross-disciplinary
team and developing a proposal should be facilitated by an individual with adequate
skills. Gold and Gold recommend that “the magnitude of the burden depends upon the
number of different disciplines, the type and degree of their differences, and upon
the depth of integration required. When this burden is especially great, the network
(i.e. the group) may benefit from including individuals whose primary function is inte-
gration and whose specialised training deals with integrative methodologies (e.g. system
science)” (1983, 87). Such an individual has been termed “generalist” (Bärmark and
Wallén 1980, 231) or “bridge-scientist” (Epton, Payne and Pearson 1983, 41). In the
present article, the term facilitator will be preferred.
Some authors indicate more specifically what the role of such a facilitator might be.
Gilbert indicates the potential role of a facilitator, stating that “while that individual
may not have the disciplinary depth of any of the other members of the group, that
person will be able to translate between the other experts and can serve as a catalyst
for the development of the vocabulary and communication skills necessary for the
group to move from the production of multidisciplinary output to interdisciplinary
knowledge” (i.e. in the direction of increased integration) (Gilbert 1998, 10). Sharp
states that “the ideal bridge-scientist must have acquired a familiarity with a number
of sciences or technologies, their basic paradigms, methodologies, and ways of work-
ing; moreover, he or she must be sensitive to the motivations, aspirations, and value
orientations of people in the several disciplines” (1983 cited in Epton, Payne and
Pearson 1983, 19). McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker point out the challenge of
establishing a common language and common standards, and suggest that “social
sciences could have this ‘glue’ function both at an epistemological level and a practical
level” (2001, 13–14). This concurs with the importance of common terminology
described above.
Contextualising the Cross-disciplinary Gap
Until now the article has dealt with the potential obstacles that may exist between
researchers of different disciplines, and some preconditions for how they may be over-
come. However, in addition to these intra-project challenges, there are a number of
contextual factors at play—factors that are beyond the control of the individual
researcher or project, but nevertheless have a strong (and usually negative) influence
on cross-disciplinary endeavours.
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
434 J. Aagaard-Hansen
Organisation of Institutions of Higher Education
Several authors point to the need for structural changes at the universities in order to
facilitate cross-disciplinarity (Chen 1969, 353; McNeill 1999, 312; Silva, Peeters and
Lewis 1994, 184). Others concentrate more on the technical, organisational patterns
(Epton, Payne and Pearson 1983, 22–25). Hull (1994) provides a very illuminating
example from Indonesia of how institutional structures can constrain collaboration in
general, and cross-disciplinarity in particular. Gibbons et al., who deal with the issue of
cross-disciplinarity within the discourse of mode 2, have this to say about the hetero-
geneity and organisational diversity: “Over time, knowledge production moves
increasingly away from traditional disciplinary activity into new societal contexts. … in
Mode 2 research groups are less firmly institutionalised; … Though problems may be
transient and groups short-lived, the organisation and communication pattern persists
as a matrix from which further groups and networks, dedicated to different problems,
will be formed” (Gibbons et al. 1994, 6). The way institutions of higher education are
organised is of course closely related to the issue of learning cross-disciplinarity. How
should students be introduced to the potential and modalities of cross-disciplinarity?
Evaluation Procedures
Evaluationisanintegratedelementofresearch,thestandardassessmentprocedurebeing
peerreview.“Thecompetitivenatureofacademiclifeisafunctionoftheemphasisplaced
ongainingaprofessionalreputation…”(BecherandTrowler2001,118),“andthepeers
are the judges” (Becher and Trowler 2001, 86). However, the fairness of the principle
of peer reviews has been questioned when it comes to cross-disciplinarity (Epton, Payne
and Pearson 1983, 44; Porter and Rossini 1985). Hence, there is a need to take a critical
look at the criteria for evaluating cross-disciplinary research (Friedman and Friedman
1985, 81), both with regard to project applications and publications (see below).
Funding Agencies
The priorities of the calls for proposals provide forceful incentives for the formulation
of research questions. McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker call cross-disciplinary
research “a high risk/high return endeavour” (2001, 8). Hence, the sponsors should
not only proactively encourage cross-disciplinary research, but also be willing to
allocate more time and more funds to such kind of projects. In addition, the funding
agencies should have access to appropriate expertise when it comes to evaluating
cross-disciplinary research applications.
Career Paths
The standard pathways laid down for academic careers are mainly confined within
disciplines. Researchers are accumulating credit and constructing networks within
their own discipline that help them in their careers. Accordingly, there are very few
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
Social Epistemology 435
positions (e.g. professorships) that are cross-disciplinary by nature. Trespassing into
collaboration with other disciplines does not usually promote the progress. It may be
seen as waste of time at best, or sometimes directly negative. Silva, Peeters and Lewis
(1994, 184) and Rosenfield (1992, 1353) provide examples from the public health field
in developing countries.
Publication Traditions
McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker (2001, 34) state that publication of cross-
disciplinary research is often ascribed low status; and Epton, Payne and Pearson go
further by stating that this ‘is not surprising if one considers that most of the highest
quality journals are mono-disciplinary and editors are probably biased against cross-
disciplinary papers’ (1983, 44). Birnbaum, who ventured a very quantitative investiga-
tion of the success of Research and Development teams, reached a similar conclusion
in the sense that “the tight integration of activities, usually associated with interdisci-
plinary research, is negatively related to article production” (Birnbaum 1983, 57). This
may to some extent reflect the epistemological and methodological differences
outlined above. Becher and Trowler point out the different media preferred for
publication within the various disciplines. For instance, there are marked differences
between disciplines that value book publications (e.g. history) and articles (e.g. phys-
ics), or even more recent electronic preprints and on-line discussion groups, modalities
that evidently have major implications for the speed of information exchange (Becher
and Trowler 2001, 108–114).
Intra-disciplinary Differences
For obvious reasons, the focus of the present article is predominantly on the interac-
tions between the disciplines. However, according to the findings of Aagaard-Hansen
and Ouma (2002), the intra-disciplinary differences are also significant (for instance,
between colleagues at different continents or from different language groups)—an
issue that indirectly has a bearing on the cross-disciplinary processes within a given
project. The process of developing intra-disciplinary collaboration is influenced by the
time factor in the same way as cross-disciplinary research (Aagaard-Hansen and Ouma
2002; Suda and Aagaard-Hansen 2003). Becher and Trowler have dealt with the intra-
disciplinary diversity as well (2001, 43–44).
Conclusion
The article has explored aspects of cross-disciplinary research collaboration. Issues such
as the choice of data collection methods and design (quantitative versus qualitative and
closed versus open approach) may be sources of divergence. The researchers’ perceptions
of causality versus descriptive studies and representativity and validity should likewise be
sorted out in cross-disciplinary teams. The attitudes towards the absolute versus relative
perspective and objectivism versus subjectivism are also important. Crucial differences
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
436 J. Aagaard-Hansen
may be derived from different emphasis on text versus context, role of theory and the
varying approaches to research ethics. Finally, the terminology and the power balance
within the cross-disciplinary team are of outmost importance for a successful result.
Mutual knowledge (conceptual compatibility) is a necessary first step towards smooth
cross-disciplinary collaboration. Hence, researchers working in cross-disciplinary
teams should have a certain minimum of knowledge about the other participating disci-
plines as a means to overcome prejudices and discover possibilities. The importance
of allocating adequate time should be emphasised. The way research management is
handled is crucial to the success of cross-disciplinary team work. In order not to rein-
vent the wheel, new cross-disciplinary teams are well advised to consult with someone
who has previous experience. So-called facilitators can assist the initial negotiations and
ensure that the necessary discussion and definition are addressed and clarified from the
start, so that implicit conflicts and misunderstandings are not continuously hampering
the collaboration.
Separate but nevertheless important are the contextual factors (organisation of
institutions of higher education, evaluation procedures, funding agencies, career paths,
publication traditions and intra-disciplinary differences) that are often serving as obsta-
cles and disincentives to cross-disciplinary work.
In spite of the complexity, there is a need for more researchers to enter this field. The
call for relevant and innovative, cross-disciplinary research makes it necessary. There
aremanychallengestocross-disciplinaryresearch,whichhavebeenoutlinedabove.The
examples have been drawn from the collaboration between biomedicine and anthro-
pology, and have been set up in a simplified and polarised way. However, I contend that
the issues are of relevance to other cross-disciplinary collaborations as well, although
their relative importance will vary depending on the specific disciplines involved. The
challenges should be carefully dealt with in the initial project planning phase.
Acknowledgements
ThankstothemanyresearchersatDBL—CentreforHealthResearchandDevelopment,
theInstituteofAnthropology(UniversityofCopenhagen),theInstituteofAfricanStud-
ies (University of Nairobi), the Division of Vector Borne Diseases (Ministry of Health,
Kenya) and the Danish University of Education (Copenhagen) and other research insti-
tutions, amongst whom I learned the practical steps of cross-disciplinarity.
Notes
1
[1] The article is based on another over-simplification. Medicine is presented as a well-defined
and homogeneous entity. However, medicine is in itself a complex discipline with strong
elements of natural science based on a positivistic paradigm as well as the public health
elements, many of which are strongly influenced by social science and which refer more to
hermeneutic traditions. In this article, the term biomedicine is used to emphasise the natural
science aspects.
2
[2] In addition to this, there is of course the often-committed sin of public health research to
draw causal conclusion based on data obtained from cross-sectional studies.
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
Social Epistemology 437
3
[3] Unfortunately this does not always take place. Either it may only be realised during the imple-
mentation of the study that there is a confounding background variable that was not considered
in the original design, and that weakens the study, or, even worse, it is never found out, with
the consequence that conclusions are wrongly believed to be true. In either case, such selection
bias should rightly be feared as a major threat to good research.
4
[4] The terminological details and subdivisions of validity are very complex and beyond the scope
of this article.
5
[5] In relation to this, the issue of the study population’s participation in the various phases of the
research process should also be considered. Thus, some may see the real issue not to be the
power balance within the research team, but that between the team and the study population.
However, this discussion is beyond the scope of the present article.
References
Aagaard-Hansen, J. 2003. Barriers and possibilities for cross-disciplinary health research—The
example of Scandinavian, bilateral programmes in developing countries. Thesis., Master’s of
Public Health, Nordic School of Public Health, Gothenburg
Aagaard-Hansen, J., and J. H. Ouma. 2002. Managing interdisciplinary health research—Theoretical
and practical aspects. International Journal of Health Planning and Management 17 (3): 195–212
Aagaard-Hansen,J.,M.V.Johansen,andP.Riis.2004.Researchethicalchallengesincross-disciplinary
andcross-culturalhealthresearch:Thediversityofcodes.DanishMedicalBulletin51(1):117–20.
Albrecht, G.,N. Higginbotham, and S. Freeman. 2001. Transdisciplinary thinking in health social
science research: Definition, rationale, and procedures. In Health social science. A transdisci-
plinary and complexity perspective, edited by N. Higginbotham, G. Albrecht, and L. Connor.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Allwood, C. M., and J. Bärmark. 1999. The role of research problems in the process of research.
Social Epistemology 13 (1): 59–83.
Bärmark, B., and G. Wallén. 1980. The development of an interdisciplinary project. In The social
process of scientific investigation. Sociology of the sciences, edited by K. D. Knorr, R. Krohn, and
R. Whitley. Vol. IV. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing Company.
Becher, T., and P. R. Trowler. 2001. Academic tribes and territories. Buckingham: The Society for
Research into Higher Education & Open University Press.
Birnbaum, P. H. 1983. Predictors of long-term research performance. In Managing interdisciplinary
research, edited by S. R. Epton, R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Chen, K., 1969, An interdisciplinary team approach from the nonengineering point of view. In
Industrialization and development, edited by H. E. Hoelscher and M. C. Hawk. San Francisco,
CA: San Francisco Press Inc.
Epton, S. R., R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. 1983. Introduction. In Managing interdisciplinary
research, edited by S. R. Epton, R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Fluehr-Lobban, C. 2003. Informed consent in anthropological research. We are not exempt. In
Ethics and the profession of anthropology. Dialogue for ethically conscious practice, edited by
C. Fluehr-Lobban. 2nd ed. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press.
Friedman, R. S., and R. C. Friedman. 1985. Organized research units in academe revisited. In
Managing high technology: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by B. W. Mar, W. T. Newell,
and B. O. Saxberg. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., North-Holland.
Gibbons, M., C. Limoges, H. Nowotny, S. Schwartzman, P. Scott, and M. Trow. 1994. The new
production of knowledge. London: Sage Publications.
Gilbert, L. E., 1998, Disciplinary breadth and interdisciplinary knowledge production. Knowledge,
Technology, and Policy 11 (1&2): 4–15.
Gold, S. E., and H. J. Gold. 1983. Some elements of a model to improve productivity of interdisci-
plinary groups. In Managing interdisciplinary research, edited by S. R. Epton, R. L. Payne, and
A. W. Pearson. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
438 J. Aagaard-Hansen
Hull, T. H., 1994, Institutional constraints to building social science capability in public health
research: A case study from Indonesia. Acta Tropica 57 (2/3): 211–27.
Jackson, M. 1998. Minima ethnographica. Intersubjectivity and the anthropological project. Chicago,
IL: University of Chicago Press.
Kilburn, A. D. 1990. Creating and maintaining an effective interdisciplinary research team. R&D
Management 20 (2): 131–138.
Kline, S. J. 1995. Conceptual foundations for multidisciplinary thinking. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Maina-Ahlberg, B., E. Nordberg, and G. Tomson. 1997. North–South health research collaboration:
Challenges in institutional interaction. Social Science and Medicine 44 (8): 1229–38.
McNeill, D. 1999. On interdisciplinary research: With particular reference to the field of environment
and development. Higher Education Quarterly 53 (4): 312–32.
McNeill, D., J. García-Godos, and A. Gjerdåker, eds. 2001. Interdisciplinary research on development
and the environment. SUM Report No. 10. Oslo: Centre for Development and the Environment,
University of Oslo.
Øvretveit J. 1993. Coordinating community care. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Pike, K. L. 1971. Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human behavior. The
Haague: Mouton & Co.
Porter, A. L., and F. A. Rossini. 1985. Forty interdisciplinary research projects: Multiple skills
and peer review. In Managing high technology: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by
B. W. Mar, W. T. Newell, and B. O. Saxberg. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.,
North-Holland.
Rosenfield, P. L. 1992. The potential of transdisciplinary research for sustaining and extending link-
ages between the health and social sciences. Social Science and Medicine 35 (11): 1343–57.
Schutz, A., and T. Luckmann. 1974. The structure of the life-world. London: Heinemann.
Silva, K. T., R. Peeters, and J. Lewis. 1994. Promoting interdisciplinary collaboration in health
research in developing countries: Lessons from the Triangle Programme in Sri Lanka. Acta
Tropica 57 (2/3): 175–84
Svedin, U., A.-L. Lindén, D. Magnusson, O. Stendahl, G. Tibell, D. Vågerö, G. Öqvist, and J. Larsson.
1999. The Workgroup on Interdisciplinarity, Gender Research and Equity, Stockholm.
Tvärvetenskap—Hur, av Vem och Varför [Cross-disciplinarity—How, by whom and why].
Unpublished report, Stockholm
Suda, C., and J. Aagaard-Hansen. 2003. Research capacity strengthening and applied medical anthro-
pology within the Kenyan–Danish Health Research Project (KEDAHR): The background.
MILA (N.S.) 5: 1–8
Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015

More Related Content

What's hot

Open Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNet
Open Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNetOpen Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNet
Open Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNetLeslie Chan
 
Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...
Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...
Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...QUESTJOURNAL
 
DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...
DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...
DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...Dr. Hanningtone Gaya PhD.,EBS
 
Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...
Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...
Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...Peachy Essay
 
Education as a tool for health policy making
Education as a tool for health policy makingEducation as a tool for health policy making
Education as a tool for health policy makingCarlos Javier Regazzoni
 
A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...
A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...
A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...Crimsonpublisherscojnh
 
Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...
Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...
Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...IJERA Editor
 

What's hot (9)

Open Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNet
Open Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNetOpen Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNet
Open Access in the Global South: Perspectives from the OCSDNet
 
The Sustainable Value of Open Data
The Sustainable Value of Open DataThe Sustainable Value of Open Data
The Sustainable Value of Open Data
 
Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...
Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...
Narrations of Work-Life Balance among Academic Staff in an Open Distance Lear...
 
Plomo en el pelo
Plomo en el peloPlomo en el pelo
Plomo en el pelo
 
DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...
DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...
DEVELOPING A QUALITATIVE SINGLE CASE STUDY IN THE REALM-AN APPROPRIATE RESEAR...
 
Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...
Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...
Urban Car Parks as Fashion Event Venue: A Reflection Study of London Fashion ...
 
Education as a tool for health policy making
Education as a tool for health policy makingEducation as a tool for health policy making
Education as a tool for health policy making
 
A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...
A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...
A Concept Analysis of Interdisciplinary Collaboration in Mental Healthcare by...
 
Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...
Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...
Identifying Structures in Social Conversations in NSCLC Patients through the ...
 

Viewers also liked (8)

art-STENO-GP-prediab
art-STENO-GP-prediabart-STENO-GP-prediab
art-STENO-GP-prediab
 
art-etik-AT
art-etik-ATart-etik-AT
art-etik-AT
 
Summary-ver0215
Summary-ver0215Summary-ver0215
Summary-ver0215
 
art-STENO-Grainne-1
art-STENO-Grainne-1art-STENO-Grainne-1
art-STENO-Grainne-1
 
art-IAS-RCS
art-IAS-RCSart-IAS-RCS
art-IAS-RCS
 
art-Suda-RCS
art-Suda-RCSart-Suda-RCS
art-Suda-RCS
 
art-cross-eval
art-cross-evalart-cross-eval
art-cross-eval
 
Cross-disciplinarity
Cross-disciplinarityCross-disciplinarity
Cross-disciplinarity
 

Similar to art-cross-challenges

10 heuristics for modeling decision making
10 heuristics for modeling decision making10 heuristics for modeling decision making
10 heuristics for modeling decision makingBarney Stacher
 
CHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed m
CHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed mCHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed m
CHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed mEstelaJeffery653
 
Using Grounded Theory In Research
Using Grounded Theory In ResearchUsing Grounded Theory In Research
Using Grounded Theory In ResearchAlyssa Dennis
 
52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx
52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx
52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docxalinainglis
 
Chapter 5 theory and methodology
Chapter 5 theory and methodology Chapter 5 theory and methodology
Chapter 5 theory and methodology grainne
 
A Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical Frameworks
A Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical FrameworksA Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical Frameworks
A Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical FrameworksCynthia King
 
The case study approach
The case study approachThe case study approach
The case study approachBAPPENAS
 
1 8Annotated Bib
1                                         8Annotated Bib1                                         8Annotated Bib
1 8Annotated BibVannaJoy20
 
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docxThe Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docxmamanda2
 
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docxThe Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
An Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge Translation
An Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge TranslationAn Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge Translation
An Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge TranslationSara Alvarez
 
Readings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docx
Readings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docxReadings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docx
Readings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docxcatheryncouper
 
Research methodolgy & methods.docx
Research methodolgy & methods.docxResearch methodolgy & methods.docx
Research methodolgy & methods.docxAli jili'ow
 
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docxCombining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docxdrandy1
 
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docxCombining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docxcargillfilberto
 
Mixed Method Research by bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research by bangladesh-.pptMixed Method Research by bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research by bangladesh-.pptsudiptasarker6
 
Mixed Method Research of bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research of bangladesh-.pptMixed Method Research of bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research of bangladesh-.pptsudiptasarker6
 

Similar to art-cross-challenges (20)

STDEV . I3.pdf
STDEV . I3.pdfSTDEV . I3.pdf
STDEV . I3.pdf
 
B0740410
B0740410B0740410
B0740410
 
10 heuristics for modeling decision making
10 heuristics for modeling decision making10 heuristics for modeling decision making
10 heuristics for modeling decision making
 
CHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed m
CHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed mCHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed m
CHAPTER 10 MIXED METHODS PROCEDURESHow would you write a mixed m
 
Using Grounded Theory In Research
Using Grounded Theory In ResearchUsing Grounded Theory In Research
Using Grounded Theory In Research
 
52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx
52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx
52 NURSERESEARCHER 2011, 18, 2issues in researchQualit.docx
 
Chapter 5 theory and methodology
Chapter 5 theory and methodology Chapter 5 theory and methodology
Chapter 5 theory and methodology
 
A Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical Frameworks
A Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical FrameworksA Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical Frameworks
A Model For Developing Interdisciplinary Research Theoretical Frameworks
 
Hampson "Our Open Future"
Hampson "Our Open Future"Hampson "Our Open Future"
Hampson "Our Open Future"
 
The case study approach
The case study approachThe case study approach
The case study approach
 
1 8Annotated Bib
1                                         8Annotated Bib1                                         8Annotated Bib
1 8Annotated Bib
 
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docxThe Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
 
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docxThe Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
The Case StudyMany disciplines use various forms of the ca.docx
 
An Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge Translation
An Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge TranslationAn Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge Translation
An Interdisciplinary And Development Lens On Knowledge Translation
 
Readings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docx
Readings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docxReadings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docx
Readings Resources· Adams, R., Tranfield, D., & Denyer, D. (2011.docx
 
Research methodolgy & methods.docx
Research methodolgy & methods.docxResearch methodolgy & methods.docx
Research methodolgy & methods.docx
 
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docxCombining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
 
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docxCombining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
Combining Qualitative and Quantitative ApproachesSome Argum.docx
 
Mixed Method Research by bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research by bangladesh-.pptMixed Method Research by bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research by bangladesh-.ppt
 
Mixed Method Research of bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research of bangladesh-.pptMixed Method Research of bangladesh-.ppt
Mixed Method Research of bangladesh-.ppt
 

art-cross-challenges

  • 1. This article was downloaded by: [Novo Nordisk A/S] On: 13 March 2015, At: 06:51 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Social Epistemology: A Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tsep20 The Challenges of Cross‐disciplinary Research Jens Aagaard‐Hansen Published online: 05 Dec 2007. To cite this article: Jens Aagaard‐Hansen (2007) The Challenges of Cross‐disciplinary Research, Social Epistemology: A Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy, 21:4, 425-438, DOI: 10.1080/02691720701746540 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02691720701746540 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions
  • 2. Social Epistemology Vol. 21, No. 4, October–December 2007, pp. 425–438 ISSN 0269–1728 (print)/ISSN 1464–5297 (online) © 2007 Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/02691720701746540 The Challenges of Cross-disciplinary Research Jens Aagaard-HansenTaylor and FrancisTSEP_A_274611.sgm10.1080/02691720701746540Social Epistemology0269-1728 (print)/1464-5297 (online)Original Article2007Taylor & Francis214000000October–December 2007 During the past decades, research collaboration between researchers from different disci- plines has become more frequent. However, there is a need to look into the generic modalities and challenges. The article explores a series of potential obstructions to cross-disciplinary collaboration of methodological and epistemological nature. Furthermore, a number of contextual, inhibiting factors are outlined. As means of overcoming the obstacles, the importance of mutual knowledge, allocation of adequate time and conducive research management is emphasised. New teams may benefit from tutoring by facilitators, who can help to make problem areas explicit and negotiate solutions. Owing to the training back- ground of the author, most of the examples are drawn from the interface between biomed- icine/naturalscienceandappliedmedicalanthropology.However,theissuesraisedbasically apply to all sorts of cross-disciplinary research collaboration in various combinations. Keywords: Applied Research; Biomedicine; Cross-disciplinarity; Epistemology; Facilitators; Medical Anthropology; Methodology; Paradigms; Research Management Introduction Thetopicofthisarticleistheclusterofdifferencesthatshouldbeovercomeifresearchers with different disciplinary background are to collaborate. It will be guided by the defi- nitions of Rosenfield (1992, 1351), who ventured a taxonomy of the level of integration between disciplines: Level one: Multidisciplinary. Researchers work in parallel or sequentially from disciplinary- specific base to address common problem. Level two: Interdisciplinary. Researchers work jointly but still from disciplinary-specific basis to address common problem. Jens Aagaard-Hansen is a senior researcher with a double training background as medical doctor and anthropol- ogist. For the past 14 years he has been based at DBL—Institute for Health Research and Development (formerly Danish Bilharziasis Laboratory). He has been involved in cross-disciplinary research, applied medical anthropol- ogy, research management and research capacity strengthening mainly in developing countries. Correspondence to: Jens Aagaard-Hansen, DBL—Centre for Health Research and Development (formerly Danish Bilharziasis Laboratory), Jaegersborg Allé 1D, DK-2920 Charlottenlund, Denmark. Email: jah@life.ku.dk Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 3. 426 J. Aagaard-Hansen Level three: Transdisciplinary. Researchers work jointly using shared conceptual frame- work drawing together disciplinary-specific theories, concepts, and approaches to address common problem. The general perception of and distinction between multidisciplinarity and interdiscipli- narity is shared by a majority of relevant scholars (for example, Albrecht, Higginbotham and Freeman 2001, 73; Friedman and Friedman 1985, 77; Gilbert 1998, 6; Kilburn 1990, 132 ; Kline 1995, 2; Maina-Ahlberg, Nordberg and Tomson 1997, 1230; McNeill 1999, 314). Although they phrase it in different ways and may even have slightly different views, there is a general agreement that interdisciplinarity entails more integration than multidisciplinarity. Even Rosenfield’s addition of transdisciplinarity is endorsed by many (Albrecht, Higginbotham and Freeman 2001, 73; Maina-Ahlberg, Nordberg and Tomson 1997, 1230; McNeill 1999, 314). In this article the term cross-disciplinarity will be used according to Rosenfield (1992, 1351); that is, as a general designation for all the three terms (multidisciplinarity, interdisciplinarity and transdisciplinarity). Aagaard- Hansen and Ouma draw attention to the time dimension in the sense that cross-disci- plinarity “is a gradual process in which the research group little by little moves in the direction of integration—from multi- to transdisciplinarity and which is taking place at different paces” (2002, 206). There are basically two motives for cross-disciplinary research, either to provide results that can be applied or as a source of inspiration for new, overarching research questions and exchange of methods and conceptual frameworks (Aagaard-Hansen 2003, 32–36). When the extent of integration of disciplines is considered, it also plays a role how different they are. McNeill explains “the nature of both bridge-building and restructur- ing will differ according to what I call the scope that is, the number of disciplines involved and the distance between them” (1999, 314). He continues, “the distance between disciplines can in some cases appear small, in terms of subject matter, but be large in terms of methodology or perspective” (McNeill 1999, 315). Based on the training background of the author, the examples used in this article are derived from collaboration between anthropology and biomedicine. However, the issues raised are of a general nature and are to various extents relevant to interaction between other disciplines as well. Many of the points are hardly new as such, but are brought together here in an overview that may be of use to research teams who venture into the field or programme managers who want to commission research of this kind. The article is making its points by setting up a series of dichotomies. Evidently, this is an oversimpli- fication of the many ongoing, cross-disciplinary projects that are rather positioned at continua between the extremes.1 However, there are two justifications for this: one is didactic; the other is that a certain proportion of our colleagues are still bound by these polarised positions. Challenges for Cross-disciplinary Research However good the intentions, cross-disciplinarity is difficult to operationalise. In the process, researchers may face many situations in which collaboration is impeded Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 4. Social Epistemology 427 either by lack of knowledge, divergent standards, different approaches, or simply negative attitudes and prejudices. Below is a brief outline of the main areas in which problems may arise. The issues are intimately interlinked, but described separately for the sake of clarity. Quantitative Versus Qualitative Methods The difference in data collection methods is usually the most conspicuous, and consequently the first, issue that comes to mind. For scientists having been socialised within particular discourses, it can be rather difficult to come to terms with alternative approaches. Either figures or big chunks of text and observation notes may be consid- ered the only appropriate way of representing the reality. In the process of establishing a cross-disciplinary team it is necessary to reach an initial, common understanding that the data collection method of choice is directly derived from the character of the research question and that no method is better than any other per se. However, if the time and funds allow, it is possible to get the best of both worlds by combined use of quantitative and qualitative data collection methods. Closed Versus Open Approach The choice of data collection methods often has a direct bearing on the degree of open- ness. If the aim is to end up with a concise figure describing a certain occurrence (combination of variables), a closed approach is needed—irrespective of whether it is the measurement of a chemical substance or choices between pre-defined responses to distinct questions in a questionnaire. If the idea is to obtain a balanced picture of a phenomenon as it is seen through the eyes of an individual, a more open approach is suitable. Both are appropriate under certain circumstances—the former when it is important to explore tangible, quantified correlations between different variables; the latter when the main aim is to provide a multi-faceted, thick description of the new field. The former is necessarily based on the researchers’ preconceived categorisation; the latter on trying to avoid exactly that and letting the categories emerge from the data. Both are valid approaches depending on the circumstances. However, there is another level at which the degree of openness is relevant—the level of study design. It is considered a virtue for many biomedical studies that as much as possible is planned in advance (e.g. specific research questions, study populations, sampling procedures, design, exclusion criteria and data collection methods), in order to be able to control bias and confounders influencing the outcomes under study. Although the same applies to a certain extent to anthropological studies as well, it is not quite as rigid. A change of focus, study population or data collection method halfway through a study would usually mean a major threat to a biomedical trial, whereas it would be more legitimate for an anthropologist. In fact it is considered a virtue for an anthropologist to be receptive to unforeseen events in the field and adapt accordingly, and it does not necessarily jeopardise the study per se. As with data collection methods there are no good or bad, but simply more or less appropriate, designs. Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 5. 428 J. Aagaard-Hansen Objectivism Versus Subjectivism The issue of openness described above is linked to one of the predominant sets of prej- udices. The fact that anthropologists function as their own data collection instruments and that they have more degrees of freedom when it comes to introducing changes in the research process makes some biomedical colleagues wonder about the objectivism of the data produced. Where is the borderline between scientific findings and the researcher’s personal opinions? The opposite position constitutes scepticism towards the biomedical science’s claim for objectivity. Whereas the archetypal, medical stand is a quest for objectivism based on traditional positivistic virtues, anthropologists see subjectivism as a basic condition for all research and that anthropology is based on a process of inter-subjectivity (Jackson 1998; Schutz and Luckmann 1974). In brief, this refers to a scientific position that acknowledges subjectivism is a basic precondition for research per se. Thus, the aim should not be to eliminate subjectivism, but to take it into considerationasacharacteristicoftheprocessofdataproductioninvolvingtwosubjects. Causality Versus Description Much outstanding research has simply aimed at describing phenomena as they are; for example, descriptive, epidemiological studies of disease patterns in a given population or cultural studies of perceptions and practices in a certain ethnic group. Other studies have had the aim of illuminating a specific causal relationship or a cluster of the same; for example, whether a high level of education makes women have other treatment- seeking practices for their infants than women with a low educational level. Notwith- standing the importance of both ventures, this is an area where the two scientific discourses often differ. Hence, it is more legitimate to conduct non-causality studies within anthropology than it is within biomedical science. A research question that does not deal with causal relationships is often considered a bit shallow by medical scientists, whereas it is the rule rather than the exception for anthropologists. Apart from the implicit attitudes attached to these positions, there is a much more tangible consequence as well. The nature of such a research question has major conse- quences for the design. The quest to answer the specific causality type of questions necessitates a certain sample size, sampling procedure, choice of closed data collection method and an appropriate study design.2 This is related to the section on closed versus open approach above. Text Versus Context There is another dichotomy that characterises the two scientific discourses: the focus on either text or context. The terms, which are derived from linguistics, refer to the text (a phenomenon, a sign, a sentence, an item, an act) and the context (the setting, the socio-cultural environment, the biotope in which the text is situated). Put simply, biomedical research focuses on the text and tries to control context as a source of noise (confounders) that threatens to disrupt the research. It can be the aim to study a certain Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 6. Social Epistemology 429 dependent variable (the growth rate of bacteria or incidence of a disease) by controlling every factor in the environment (the media and temperature or other potential risk factors) except from the independent variable in question. In contrast, anthropologists make context a main area of research in its own right. Context in its multiplicity is stud- ied as a necessary part of a given phenomenon. Only by seeing the variations of the text in the light of the complexity of and changes in the context can a valid picture of socio- cultural issues be obtained. Among these two basic positions, one is not superior to the other per se. But as is the case for data collection methods, one or the other is preferable in order to conduct good research on different kinds of research questions. However, if this basic difference in approach is not realised, it can cause major problems in cross- disciplinary teams. Absolute Versus Relative Perspective Research is conducted in order to provide new knowledge. Some (more positivistic inclined)scientiststakeitforgrantedthatthereisatruthoutthere,andthatthechallenge is to get as close as possible to it by conducting good research. Other (more phenome- nologically inclined) colleagues do not see their aim to be a search for a golden truth, but rather a quest for providing valid representations of local lifeworlds (Schutz and Luckmann 1974, 59–92). Whereas the biomedical researchers explore universal causal relationships (e.g. smoking as a cause for lung cancer), the anthropologists have the ambition of describing the world as seen through the eyes of the particular study population in question (i.e. if witchcraft is considered an important cause of misfortune in a certain ethnic group, the challenge for the anthropologist is to describe that in a fair manner—not to evaluate whether it is real or not). It is crucial that this anthropo- logicalendeavourisnotlookeddownuponbybiomedicalcolleagues,butisappreciated, partly because it is based on sound scientific (but different) ground and partly because it is exactly the source of the potential usefulness of anthropology to public health. On the other hand, anthropologists have to realise that there is a need for closed and rigidly designed studies in order to provide knowledge about the effect of various interventions without a number of confounding factors. This divide is often expressed as the etic (global scientific standard, in this case biomedicine) versus the emic (local, indigenous view as described by the anthropologists) position (Pike 1971). Representativity and Validity The issue of representativity plays a major role in most public health research. It is the issue of whether the study sample (e.g. 200 people from which a specific blood sample has been taken) share the same characteristics as the larger population about which inferences are made (e.g. the people of Finland). Only if the sample and the popula- tion have the same proportions of all relevant variables (such as sex, age, ethnicity, class or whatever else plays a role with regard to the research question) are broader conclusions justified beyond the borders of the sample. The issue of representativity should be considered already in the planning phase.3 In this article, validity is defined Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 7. 430 J. Aagaard-Hansen as “the extent to which a study finding reflects a real fact of life”.4 In this broad defini- tion, many things can contribute to reduced validity. For instance various sorts of systematic error can play a role, such as selection bias (i.e. skewness as a result of mismatch between sample and population) or information bias (i.e. the phenomenon that the source of information—a thermometer or an interview—does not give a true picture of the phenomenon the researcher wants to study). But it can also be caused by random error, which is a reflection of natural variation in data collection methods (and hence imprecise data). Whatever the case, a high degree of validity is the aim of every researcher. Usually anthropological studies deal with relatively few informants, with whom the researcher interacts for long periods of time typically during series of in-depth interviews and observations. The in-depth data collection takes place at the expense of having a relatively small sample size, and is consequently more exposed to be influ- enced by individual variation among the informants. On the other hand, biomedical studies, however complex they may be, typically take relatively simpler measures (e.g. blood samples, questions, weight, etc.) from each individual, which permits such studies to have large sample sizes. Thus, an anthropological study will tend to provide very valid data (exactly because the researcher has interacted with the same infor- mants for a long time) whereby more hidden and sensitive issues are more likely to come to the fore and be seen in changing contexts, whereas the standardised way in which biomedical data are collected does not offer the same opportunity. In short, anthropological studies are typically relatively weak when it comes to representativity and are strong in validity, whereas the opposite is often the case for biomedical studies. Again one is not better than the other per se, but is simply suitable for differ- ent purposes. As with data collection methods, it is here possible to design studies that are strong in both aspects if resources allows. Role of Theory All scientific disciplines are grounded in theoretical foundations and paradigms. Biomedicine has developed a tightly knit set of anatomical, physiological, genetic and biochemical models that have proved extremely useful when it comes to explaining disease processes. Apart from a few philosophically inclined public health scientists, these theories and models are taken for granted as absolute, fixed points in the episte- mological universe. In contrast to this stance, the average anthropologist spends quite a bit of time discussing the theories on which studies are based. The extent to which theoretical issues are of concern is a major difference between the two disciplines and a potential source of prejudices. The anthropologists may be perceived as wordy people making a lot of fuss about nothing, whereas the opposite prejudice is that biomedical researchers are turning a blind eye to the shaky, epistemological ground of their own endeavour. For the anthropologists working in the applied field, this poses a particular challenge. Some claim that it is not possible to live up to the academic, disciplinary demands of dealing adequately with theoretical issues and being applied at the same time. It is indeed difficult, but not impossible. Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 8. Social Epistemology 431 Research Ethics Biomedicine has a tradition for detailed ethical codes whereas some anthropological guidelines are held in more generalised terms (Aagaard-Hansen, Johansen and Riis 2004). For instance, the key element of informed consent is dealt with much more explicitly by the biomedical codes (Fluehr-Lobban 2003). This is not to say that some disciplines are unethical. However, when disciplines are to collaborate it may take some effort to harmonise the approaches to ethical issues as well. Terminology Many of the methodological and epistemological differences are expressed in different terms. In the cases where there are distinct jargons, this may pose an additional hurdle to overcome. More subtly, however, there are sometimes varying meanings of the same terms within different disciplinary discourses. McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker also point to language variations in different disciplines as a potential barrier, and say that “sometimes there appears to be common understanding and it is not for some time that serious divergence in understanding is revealed” (2001, 17). Svedin et al. (1999) point out terminology as an important element of assessing cross-disciplinary projects. Are key concepts used consistently within the “project language”? The relation to the use of facilitators (see below) is evident. Power Balance As anything else in life, research is influenced by power and status. Research projects can be seen as the potential battlefield of individuals as well as disciplines. Historically, some disciplines can be perceived as being more powerful than others—either because of access to funds or simply due to status. Disciplinary boundaries are often propped up by attitudes, the origins of which are rarely rational. A strong influence comes from the positivist philosophy, which was launched by Auguste Comte (1798–1857) and which still frames the thinking of many. As expressed by Kline, “the hierarchy that Comte suggested went in descending order: math, astronomy, physics, chemistry, biology (including physiology), sociology” (1995, 208). Another view on the prestige heldbyvariousbranchesofresearch,accordingtoFriedmanandFriedman,isthat“there is an informal hierarchy of disciplines and fields in academe in which the more presti- gious theoretical basic disciplines regard scholarly interaction with colleagues in the applied, less theoretical fields as an unrewarding experience” (1985, 79). Traditionally, biomedicine has had a stronger position than social science. In cases where disciplines have collaborated, the biomedical researchers have typically been the principal investi- gators whereas the social scientists have been invited to solve specific problems, such as mobilisationofthestudypopulationorprovisionofdataoncertainconfoundingfactors suchassocio-economicbackground.Aagaard-HansenandOumaemphasisetheimpor- tance of the balance between the disciplines, a notion that encompassed two elements; firstly the methodological differences between the participating disciplines (“how to do Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 9. 432 J. Aagaard-Hansen research”), and secondly the power balance (“who decides what to study”). The authors state that “managing an interdisciplinary project in such a way that it strikes a balance between common reference to main objectives and disciplinary freedom is maybe one of the main challenges to research managers in positions like ours” (Aagaard-Hansen and Ouma 2002, 203).5 Bridging the Gap The many potential challenges listed above may be overcome. In this section, some practical remedies are outlined. Mutual Knowledge An obvious first step when cross-disciplinary teams are to succeed is to obtain mutual knowledge. Insight into the basics of methodology, theories, epistemological and historical aspects of the others’ disciplinary discourse (as have been described in detail above) is essential for understanding and respecting the position of collaborators from other fields. Conceptual compatibility is the basis for understanding and overcoming negative prejudices and creating respect. Furthermore, knowledge about alternatives gives a healthy perspective on one’s own habitual thinking, which is quite often taken for granted. Adequate Time Several groups emphasise the need for a long-time perspective for cross-disciplinarity to succeed (McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker 2001, 26; Silva, Peeters and Lewis 1994, 184). Rosenfield expresses it as follows: “with continuing support to the same team over a sufficiently long period of time and covering several types of problems, it is more likely that disciplinary barriers can be transcended and increased understanding and confidence about the value of other disciplines can be achieved” (1992, 1345). Allwood and Bärmark state it more daringly: “one lesson to be learned from this is that it might take a research project or two in order for a group of researchers to develop the level of competence necessary for an interdisciplinary generation of problems” (1999, 78). Hence, researchers as well as funding agencies should allocate additional time for the collaboration to succeed. Conducive Research Management The topic of research management is to a large extent neglected. If research manage- ment as such is difficult, management of cross-disciplinary research is even more challenging because of its complex nature and the particular problems it poses. The ability of the team leader to create a conducive working environment in which synergism can blossom between the disciplines is crucial. In their book on “mode 2 knowledge production”, Gibbons et al. point out the challenge of the “management Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 10. Social Epistemology 433 of disciplinary identities in transdisciplinary settings and the development of trans- disciplinary capacities” (1994, 138–139). The importance of the human factor for cross-disciplinary research has been explored by Gold and Gold (1983, 86), Øvretveit (1993, 139–158) and Allwood and Bärmark (1999, 79). Aagaard-Hansen and Ouma (2002) have analysed various aspects of management in relation to cross- disciplinarity. Facilitators Severalauthorsrecommendthattheintricateprocessofestablishingacross-disciplinary team and developing a proposal should be facilitated by an individual with adequate skills. Gold and Gold recommend that “the magnitude of the burden depends upon the number of different disciplines, the type and degree of their differences, and upon the depth of integration required. When this burden is especially great, the network (i.e. the group) may benefit from including individuals whose primary function is inte- gration and whose specialised training deals with integrative methodologies (e.g. system science)” (1983, 87). Such an individual has been termed “generalist” (Bärmark and Wallén 1980, 231) or “bridge-scientist” (Epton, Payne and Pearson 1983, 41). In the present article, the term facilitator will be preferred. Some authors indicate more specifically what the role of such a facilitator might be. Gilbert indicates the potential role of a facilitator, stating that “while that individual may not have the disciplinary depth of any of the other members of the group, that person will be able to translate between the other experts and can serve as a catalyst for the development of the vocabulary and communication skills necessary for the group to move from the production of multidisciplinary output to interdisciplinary knowledge” (i.e. in the direction of increased integration) (Gilbert 1998, 10). Sharp states that “the ideal bridge-scientist must have acquired a familiarity with a number of sciences or technologies, their basic paradigms, methodologies, and ways of work- ing; moreover, he or she must be sensitive to the motivations, aspirations, and value orientations of people in the several disciplines” (1983 cited in Epton, Payne and Pearson 1983, 19). McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker point out the challenge of establishing a common language and common standards, and suggest that “social sciences could have this ‘glue’ function both at an epistemological level and a practical level” (2001, 13–14). This concurs with the importance of common terminology described above. Contextualising the Cross-disciplinary Gap Until now the article has dealt with the potential obstacles that may exist between researchers of different disciplines, and some preconditions for how they may be over- come. However, in addition to these intra-project challenges, there are a number of contextual factors at play—factors that are beyond the control of the individual researcher or project, but nevertheless have a strong (and usually negative) influence on cross-disciplinary endeavours. Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 11. 434 J. Aagaard-Hansen Organisation of Institutions of Higher Education Several authors point to the need for structural changes at the universities in order to facilitate cross-disciplinarity (Chen 1969, 353; McNeill 1999, 312; Silva, Peeters and Lewis 1994, 184). Others concentrate more on the technical, organisational patterns (Epton, Payne and Pearson 1983, 22–25). Hull (1994) provides a very illuminating example from Indonesia of how institutional structures can constrain collaboration in general, and cross-disciplinarity in particular. Gibbons et al., who deal with the issue of cross-disciplinarity within the discourse of mode 2, have this to say about the hetero- geneity and organisational diversity: “Over time, knowledge production moves increasingly away from traditional disciplinary activity into new societal contexts. … in Mode 2 research groups are less firmly institutionalised; … Though problems may be transient and groups short-lived, the organisation and communication pattern persists as a matrix from which further groups and networks, dedicated to different problems, will be formed” (Gibbons et al. 1994, 6). The way institutions of higher education are organised is of course closely related to the issue of learning cross-disciplinarity. How should students be introduced to the potential and modalities of cross-disciplinarity? Evaluation Procedures Evaluationisanintegratedelementofresearch,thestandardassessmentprocedurebeing peerreview.“Thecompetitivenatureofacademiclifeisafunctionoftheemphasisplaced ongainingaprofessionalreputation…”(BecherandTrowler2001,118),“andthepeers are the judges” (Becher and Trowler 2001, 86). However, the fairness of the principle of peer reviews has been questioned when it comes to cross-disciplinarity (Epton, Payne and Pearson 1983, 44; Porter and Rossini 1985). Hence, there is a need to take a critical look at the criteria for evaluating cross-disciplinary research (Friedman and Friedman 1985, 81), both with regard to project applications and publications (see below). Funding Agencies The priorities of the calls for proposals provide forceful incentives for the formulation of research questions. McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker call cross-disciplinary research “a high risk/high return endeavour” (2001, 8). Hence, the sponsors should not only proactively encourage cross-disciplinary research, but also be willing to allocate more time and more funds to such kind of projects. In addition, the funding agencies should have access to appropriate expertise when it comes to evaluating cross-disciplinary research applications. Career Paths The standard pathways laid down for academic careers are mainly confined within disciplines. Researchers are accumulating credit and constructing networks within their own discipline that help them in their careers. Accordingly, there are very few Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 12. Social Epistemology 435 positions (e.g. professorships) that are cross-disciplinary by nature. Trespassing into collaboration with other disciplines does not usually promote the progress. It may be seen as waste of time at best, or sometimes directly negative. Silva, Peeters and Lewis (1994, 184) and Rosenfield (1992, 1353) provide examples from the public health field in developing countries. Publication Traditions McNeill, García-Godos and Gjerdåker (2001, 34) state that publication of cross- disciplinary research is often ascribed low status; and Epton, Payne and Pearson go further by stating that this ‘is not surprising if one considers that most of the highest quality journals are mono-disciplinary and editors are probably biased against cross- disciplinary papers’ (1983, 44). Birnbaum, who ventured a very quantitative investiga- tion of the success of Research and Development teams, reached a similar conclusion in the sense that “the tight integration of activities, usually associated with interdisci- plinary research, is negatively related to article production” (Birnbaum 1983, 57). This may to some extent reflect the epistemological and methodological differences outlined above. Becher and Trowler point out the different media preferred for publication within the various disciplines. For instance, there are marked differences between disciplines that value book publications (e.g. history) and articles (e.g. phys- ics), or even more recent electronic preprints and on-line discussion groups, modalities that evidently have major implications for the speed of information exchange (Becher and Trowler 2001, 108–114). Intra-disciplinary Differences For obvious reasons, the focus of the present article is predominantly on the interac- tions between the disciplines. However, according to the findings of Aagaard-Hansen and Ouma (2002), the intra-disciplinary differences are also significant (for instance, between colleagues at different continents or from different language groups)—an issue that indirectly has a bearing on the cross-disciplinary processes within a given project. The process of developing intra-disciplinary collaboration is influenced by the time factor in the same way as cross-disciplinary research (Aagaard-Hansen and Ouma 2002; Suda and Aagaard-Hansen 2003). Becher and Trowler have dealt with the intra- disciplinary diversity as well (2001, 43–44). Conclusion The article has explored aspects of cross-disciplinary research collaboration. Issues such as the choice of data collection methods and design (quantitative versus qualitative and closed versus open approach) may be sources of divergence. The researchers’ perceptions of causality versus descriptive studies and representativity and validity should likewise be sorted out in cross-disciplinary teams. The attitudes towards the absolute versus relative perspective and objectivism versus subjectivism are also important. Crucial differences Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 13. 436 J. Aagaard-Hansen may be derived from different emphasis on text versus context, role of theory and the varying approaches to research ethics. Finally, the terminology and the power balance within the cross-disciplinary team are of outmost importance for a successful result. Mutual knowledge (conceptual compatibility) is a necessary first step towards smooth cross-disciplinary collaboration. Hence, researchers working in cross-disciplinary teams should have a certain minimum of knowledge about the other participating disci- plines as a means to overcome prejudices and discover possibilities. The importance of allocating adequate time should be emphasised. The way research management is handled is crucial to the success of cross-disciplinary team work. In order not to rein- vent the wheel, new cross-disciplinary teams are well advised to consult with someone who has previous experience. So-called facilitators can assist the initial negotiations and ensure that the necessary discussion and definition are addressed and clarified from the start, so that implicit conflicts and misunderstandings are not continuously hampering the collaboration. Separate but nevertheless important are the contextual factors (organisation of institutions of higher education, evaluation procedures, funding agencies, career paths, publication traditions and intra-disciplinary differences) that are often serving as obsta- cles and disincentives to cross-disciplinary work. In spite of the complexity, there is a need for more researchers to enter this field. The call for relevant and innovative, cross-disciplinary research makes it necessary. There aremanychallengestocross-disciplinaryresearch,whichhavebeenoutlinedabove.The examples have been drawn from the collaboration between biomedicine and anthro- pology, and have been set up in a simplified and polarised way. However, I contend that the issues are of relevance to other cross-disciplinary collaborations as well, although their relative importance will vary depending on the specific disciplines involved. The challenges should be carefully dealt with in the initial project planning phase. Acknowledgements ThankstothemanyresearchersatDBL—CentreforHealthResearchandDevelopment, theInstituteofAnthropology(UniversityofCopenhagen),theInstituteofAfricanStud- ies (University of Nairobi), the Division of Vector Borne Diseases (Ministry of Health, Kenya) and the Danish University of Education (Copenhagen) and other research insti- tutions, amongst whom I learned the practical steps of cross-disciplinarity. Notes 1 [1] The article is based on another over-simplification. Medicine is presented as a well-defined and homogeneous entity. However, medicine is in itself a complex discipline with strong elements of natural science based on a positivistic paradigm as well as the public health elements, many of which are strongly influenced by social science and which refer more to hermeneutic traditions. In this article, the term biomedicine is used to emphasise the natural science aspects. 2 [2] In addition to this, there is of course the often-committed sin of public health research to draw causal conclusion based on data obtained from cross-sectional studies. Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 14. Social Epistemology 437 3 [3] Unfortunately this does not always take place. Either it may only be realised during the imple- mentation of the study that there is a confounding background variable that was not considered in the original design, and that weakens the study, or, even worse, it is never found out, with the consequence that conclusions are wrongly believed to be true. In either case, such selection bias should rightly be feared as a major threat to good research. 4 [4] The terminological details and subdivisions of validity are very complex and beyond the scope of this article. 5 [5] In relation to this, the issue of the study population’s participation in the various phases of the research process should also be considered. Thus, some may see the real issue not to be the power balance within the research team, but that between the team and the study population. However, this discussion is beyond the scope of the present article. References Aagaard-Hansen, J. 2003. Barriers and possibilities for cross-disciplinary health research—The example of Scandinavian, bilateral programmes in developing countries. Thesis., Master’s of Public Health, Nordic School of Public Health, Gothenburg Aagaard-Hansen, J., and J. H. Ouma. 2002. Managing interdisciplinary health research—Theoretical and practical aspects. International Journal of Health Planning and Management 17 (3): 195–212 Aagaard-Hansen,J.,M.V.Johansen,andP.Riis.2004.Researchethicalchallengesincross-disciplinary andcross-culturalhealthresearch:Thediversityofcodes.DanishMedicalBulletin51(1):117–20. Albrecht, G.,N. Higginbotham, and S. Freeman. 2001. Transdisciplinary thinking in health social science research: Definition, rationale, and procedures. In Health social science. A transdisci- plinary and complexity perspective, edited by N. Higginbotham, G. Albrecht, and L. Connor. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Allwood, C. M., and J. Bärmark. 1999. The role of research problems in the process of research. Social Epistemology 13 (1): 59–83. Bärmark, B., and G. Wallén. 1980. The development of an interdisciplinary project. In The social process of scientific investigation. Sociology of the sciences, edited by K. D. Knorr, R. Krohn, and R. Whitley. Vol. IV. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing Company. Becher, T., and P. R. Trowler. 2001. Academic tribes and territories. Buckingham: The Society for Research into Higher Education & Open University Press. Birnbaum, P. H. 1983. Predictors of long-term research performance. In Managing interdisciplinary research, edited by S. R. Epton, R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Chen, K., 1969, An interdisciplinary team approach from the nonengineering point of view. In Industrialization and development, edited by H. E. Hoelscher and M. C. Hawk. San Francisco, CA: San Francisco Press Inc. Epton, S. R., R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. 1983. Introduction. In Managing interdisciplinary research, edited by S. R. Epton, R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Fluehr-Lobban, C. 2003. Informed consent in anthropological research. We are not exempt. In Ethics and the profession of anthropology. Dialogue for ethically conscious practice, edited by C. Fluehr-Lobban. 2nd ed. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press. Friedman, R. S., and R. C. Friedman. 1985. Organized research units in academe revisited. In Managing high technology: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by B. W. Mar, W. T. Newell, and B. O. Saxberg. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., North-Holland. Gibbons, M., C. Limoges, H. Nowotny, S. Schwartzman, P. Scott, and M. Trow. 1994. The new production of knowledge. London: Sage Publications. Gilbert, L. E., 1998, Disciplinary breadth and interdisciplinary knowledge production. Knowledge, Technology, and Policy 11 (1&2): 4–15. Gold, S. E., and H. J. Gold. 1983. Some elements of a model to improve productivity of interdisci- plinary groups. In Managing interdisciplinary research, edited by S. R. Epton, R. L. Payne, and A. W. Pearson. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015
  • 15. 438 J. Aagaard-Hansen Hull, T. H., 1994, Institutional constraints to building social science capability in public health research: A case study from Indonesia. Acta Tropica 57 (2/3): 211–27. Jackson, M. 1998. Minima ethnographica. Intersubjectivity and the anthropological project. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Kilburn, A. D. 1990. Creating and maintaining an effective interdisciplinary research team. R&D Management 20 (2): 131–138. Kline, S. J. 1995. Conceptual foundations for multidisciplinary thinking. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Maina-Ahlberg, B., E. Nordberg, and G. Tomson. 1997. North–South health research collaboration: Challenges in institutional interaction. Social Science and Medicine 44 (8): 1229–38. McNeill, D. 1999. On interdisciplinary research: With particular reference to the field of environment and development. Higher Education Quarterly 53 (4): 312–32. McNeill, D., J. García-Godos, and A. Gjerdåker, eds. 2001. Interdisciplinary research on development and the environment. SUM Report No. 10. Oslo: Centre for Development and the Environment, University of Oslo. Øvretveit J. 1993. Coordinating community care. Buckingham: Open University Press. Pike, K. L. 1971. Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human behavior. The Haague: Mouton & Co. Porter, A. L., and F. A. Rossini. 1985. Forty interdisciplinary research projects: Multiple skills and peer review. In Managing high technology: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by B. W. Mar, W. T. Newell, and B. O. Saxberg. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., North-Holland. Rosenfield, P. L. 1992. The potential of transdisciplinary research for sustaining and extending link- ages between the health and social sciences. Social Science and Medicine 35 (11): 1343–57. Schutz, A., and T. Luckmann. 1974. The structure of the life-world. London: Heinemann. Silva, K. T., R. Peeters, and J. Lewis. 1994. Promoting interdisciplinary collaboration in health research in developing countries: Lessons from the Triangle Programme in Sri Lanka. Acta Tropica 57 (2/3): 175–84 Svedin, U., A.-L. Lindén, D. Magnusson, O. Stendahl, G. Tibell, D. Vågerö, G. Öqvist, and J. Larsson. 1999. The Workgroup on Interdisciplinarity, Gender Research and Equity, Stockholm. Tvärvetenskap—Hur, av Vem och Varför [Cross-disciplinarity—How, by whom and why]. Unpublished report, Stockholm Suda, C., and J. Aagaard-Hansen. 2003. Research capacity strengthening and applied medical anthro- pology within the Kenyan–Danish Health Research Project (KEDAHR): The background. MILA (N.S.) 5: 1–8 Downloadedby[NovoNordiskA/S]at06:5113March2015