SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 69
EARLY MORTALITY 8
Early Mortality: Review of the Charleston Heart Study
A Critiqué Submitted by
[Your Name]
El Centro College
Psychology 23xx, Section 5xxxx, Fall 2013
Running head: EARLY MORTALITY 1
Abstract
This essay provides a review of forty years of follow-up data
from the Charleston Heart Study (CHS) in their article Divorce
and Death: Forty Years of the Charleston Heart Study. This
includes a longitudinal CHS, which included data collected
from more than 1300 adults from 1960 to 2000, Sbarra and
Nietert explored the relationship between social connectedness
and health using the CHS data and attempted to provide insight
into the long term health consequences of becoming separated
or divorced (2009). Being separated or divorced during the
follow-up window appeared to be one of the strongest predictors
of early mortality. In addition to the review of the actual study,
a critique is included that provides a critical analysis of the
quality of the researchers’ study and article as published in the
Psychological Science journal. The critique addresses such
items as ethics, usefulness, sample size and diversification as
well as a plethora of other interesting items useful to provide
collegiate feedback of the work by Sbarra and Nietert.
Early Mortality: Review of the Charleston Heart
StudyIntroduction
The researchers posed that recent research in social
epidemiology has spurred advances into the association between
interpersonal relationships and health (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009)
but there were limits to the overall findings. It is suggested that
relationship can play a vital role in an individual’s lifespan
development. Sbarra and Nietert share this sentiment. More
specifically, that social integration (i.e. a close relationship in
which one feels close to others and that the relationship is
reciprocally dependable) is positively correlated with mortality;
however, a life deficient in social integration may be a strong
predictor of early mortality (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). The
researchers carefully reviewed the data from the CHS to help
explain the mechanism linking social connectedness and health.
The authors were interested in providing new insights into the
long-term health consequences of divorce or separation. Many
previous studies on the link between divorce and health have
failed to present marital status as a dynamic variable. However,
the researchers took into account both the length of time the
participants were divorced and eventual remarriage rates which
appears to have significantly improved their ability to make the
link.Review
In order to present marital status as dynamically as possible, the
researchers classified participants in the study in three ways.
First, marital status (married, separated-divorced, widowed, or
never married) at the first assessment was examined as a
predictor of long-term mortality (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009).
Second, the researchers calculated the hazard ratio (HR) of
early death for adults who were separated or divorced at each
assessment relative to all other participants who contributed at
least one marital-status entry (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). Finally,
to determine if the effect of remaining separated or divorced
differed from the effect of having ever experienced a separation
or divorce, the researchers reclassified the sample to calculate
the HR of adults who simply experienced a marital separation or
divorce at some point during the CHS follow-up period relative
to all other participants (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009).
The researchers used data collected in the Charleston Heart
Study to address the possible link between marital status and
health. The CHS was a community-based cohort study designed
to assess the normal course of health and aging for adults over
age 35 residing in Charleston County, South Carolina and while
the study began in 1960, marital-status data were collected
during several follow-up phases: 1962–1964, 1974–1975, 1984–
1985, 1987–1989, and 1990–1991 (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009).
Mortality data were updated throughout the study, with the final
revision spanning the entire 41-year period.
The CHS began with an initial sample of 2181 adults, 1195
women and 986 men, 61% were of Caucasian decent, while the
remainder of the sample was of African American decent
(Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). At the onset of the study, the average
age of the participants were 48 years 9 months. The fact that the
average age of the participants was not exceptionally young,
combined with the extended length of the study, provided an
opportunity for the researchers to capture data to the end of life
for the majority of the participants (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009).
In the CHS, information on marital status was collected by self-
report, and participants were classified as married, widowed,
separated, divorced, or never married at each assessment. In the
researchers’ analysis, the separated and divorced categories
were merged to create a single category defined by the
experience of marital separation (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). At
the 1962–1964 assessment, the first-time marital status data was
captured, 81.2% of the sample was married, 10% was widowed,
5.5% was separated or divorced, and 3.3% had never been
married (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009).
In-person medical interviews at the beginning of the CHS
provided a large amount of medical data, such as blood
pressure, smoking status, and body mass index, that helped the
researchers predict mortality. To predict mortality over the 41-
year study period, the researchers used Cox proportional
hazards models, a regression approach commonly referred to as
survival analysis. The researchers were able to create predictors
for each variable to isolate the effect of the variable of interest
– time spent divorced or separated on age of death.
Of the 1,376 adults in the restricted sample assessed at baseline,
74% had died by 2000, with the remaining 26% excluded from
the research (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). The researchers found
that separated and divorced participants had a 55% greater
likelihood of death in the follow-up period than participants
from the other three categories of relationship status (Sbarra &
Nietert, 2009).
The analysis of the CHS data by the researchers suggests that
longevity of life is positively correlated to healthy and
cherished relationships and that living a large portion of life as
a separated or divorced adult may add considerable risk for all-
cause of mortality (as cited in Sbarra & Nietert). It may be
further suggested that the process of a marital break may not
present the highest mortality risk; rather it is more likely the
time spent without a significant other that is the most crucial
factor. The results do not lend to reasons of cause but suggest
results of significant correlation. This agrees with Sbarra and
Nietert, who also suggest that additional research is necessary
to fully understand the mechanisms behind the elevated risk of
early mortality associated with time spent as a separated or
divorced individual (2009).
Critique
The Charleston Heart Study, on which the article is based,
appeared to be conducted in an ethical and scientific manner. In
addition, the researchers appeared to be interested in conducting
a study that included a diverse sample. After the initial 1960
phase of the study, 102 Black men of high socioeconomic status
were added to increase the diversity component, which could be
considered “forward-thinking” for 1962-era America. The fact
that this was a longitudinal study (i.e. conducted over a span of
41 years) proved to be beneficial. Since the original study was
conducted with cardiovascular health in mind, the fact that the
authors had the insight of gathering other useful data for later
use, is truly impressive.
Still, there are some shortcomings with the data, as Sbarra and
Nietert admit (2009). The incident of divorce was less
customary at the onset of the CHS, though it would become
significantly more common as the study progressed.
Consequently, this presents one of the limits of a cohort study
(i.e. how the customs of a single generation can affect the
results of a study unlike another generation). The number of
participants that remained separated or divorced throughout the
follow-up period was relatively small, limiting the ability of the
sample to be generalized across the entire population. It may
have also been more efficacious to delineate between those who
were separated and those who were divorced in the original
study. Separating this group into two variables may have
expanded the results and provided more insight.
Despite these limits, the work of the authors has provided
contemporary researchers with a good start in understanding the
link between marriage status and longevity of life. A more
modern approach to this type of study, with more nuanced
relationship status categories and more frequent follow-up
occasions, might benefit the subject matter. A new study, with a
more contemporary cohort sample and the opportunity to collect
data specifically useful to psychological research, may proffer
more insight for the avid researcher. Additional follow up
studies may also include individuals from select cultures or
ethnicities as well as individuals from a variety of
socioeconomic statuses.
References
Sbarra, D. A. & Nietert, P. J. (2009). Divorce and death: Forty
years of the Charleston heart study. Psychological Science,
20(1), pp. 107-113.
HUMN 100: INTERACTING WITH TEXT
DUE DATES & DETAILS
Length & Format: 800-1000 words
Audience: An academic audience at Emily Carr
Initial draft due: Week 9
Final draft (with evidence of revision) due: Week 11
ASSIGNMENT DESCRIPTION
This assignment asks you to respond to a single written text
based on your own contexts
and experiences. You will be writing this time, however, for an
academic audience—best
imagined, perhaps, as your professors and other university-
educated people—who have
certain expectations about what makes a “critical” response to a
written text. There are
many ways to react to a text (many of which we will discuss in
class), but academic writing
values certain kinds of response that are both respectful of a
text’s author and reflectively
address the points the author is making. As a result, this
assignment asks you to write a
balanced essay which fairly summarizes and discusses a
published text, while offering your
own response to the ideas presented in the text. Rather than a
pure summary of the text or
a pure reaction based only on your opinions and experiences,
aim for more of a
“conversation” between you and the author—one that takes your
own ideas and the writer’s
seriously.
You also need to choose a focus for your own response that may
not (and probably should
not) “cover” every point the author makes. While you are asked
to summarize the text’s
main theme or argument, also let your own interests, ideas,
experiences, and reactions be
the guides for what positions or ideas in the text you choose to
respond to. Imagine this
essay, then, as a response to a text for an academic audience
who is interested in what you
think about the author’s ideas. The center of the essay is you
and your ideas, but this time
we will also work on integrating your experience with that of
the different
contexts/opinions/experiences offered in the published text in
ways that will appeal to an
academic audience.
WRITING GOALS
• To examine your own position in relation to a published text
and to write both with
and against this text.
• To explore (through reading) and practice (through writing)
the various options
that writers have for positioning themselves in relation to the
writing of another.
• To ethically represent through paraphrase, summary and
quotation the ideas of
another writer/text. To understand and use MLA citation.
• To engage in critical analysis of text. To present your own
views with this analysis.
• To continue to practice the writing process (generative
writing, drafting, revision,
editing.)
Surname 1
Name
Professor
Course
Date
American slavery
China permitted slavery since it recognized it as an institution
and thus needed to be protected especially in America. The
large settlement in San Francisco encourages human trafficking.
Slavery has existed unrestricted for years among the Chinese in
California. Chinese women were sold as animals. Modern
slavery in China is linked to poverty. Lack of education,
economic freedom, poor societal structure and the rule of law
allow slavery. Forced labor is common in China. This is what is
experienced today as a form of slavery.
“American Slavery As It Is, Testimony of a Thousand
Witnesses”, was written in1839 by the Anti-Slavery Activist
Theodore Dwight Weld. This book gathers information from
witnesses, active and former slave owners from various
plantations in Southern states. Information on how slaves were
treated aided in building case for the Abolitionist Movement.
The narrative described the conditions experienced by slaves in
the United States. Weld argued that prisoners in United States
and other countries were fed better than American slaves. The
narrative focused on afflictions faced by slaves which covered
housing, clothing, diet and working conditions (McBride 38).
Medicine started in the era of slavery when people started
struggling with disease. It changed slowly into a social
institution becoming competitor of traditional health practices.
In the early colonies of North America, every individual termed
himself as a doctor. In the mid nineteenth century, doctors made
professional medicine to differentiate themselves from the past.
Therefore, the history of medicine is traced from the struggle
for health during slavery. The plantation owners and doctors
coerced much labor from slaves. This forced them to provide
sufficient resources to make these slaves stay productive and
give birth many children. The preference of illness and
mortality in slave people indicated their environmental
experiences and living conditions. Slaves were able to maintain
their health and stabilize their families despite harsh
enslavement. Slaves medicine sets the background of public
health and clap social circumstances in the era of Jim crow.
Weld wrote this narrative as a plain case where the reader acted
as a juror for the purpose of coming up with an honest verdict.
The aim was to show actual situation of the slaves in the United
States. They were made slaves and owned by force by being put
in fear. The slaveholders painted that their slaves were treated
humanely, well fed, well housed, well clothed, well lodged and
worked moderately. They also argued that they provided slaves
with all things they needed for their comfort. Their assertion
was disproved by the testimony of many unbiased witnesses.
This put slaveholders through a course of questioning which led
to condemnation out of their own mouths (McBride 40).
According to testimonies of witnesses, the slaves were treated
inhumane since they were underfed, overworked, wretchedly
lodged and clad, they lacked enough sleep, they dragged heavy
chains while working on the field, they also wore iron collars
round their necks in order to be easily detected when they
escaped. They also wore yokes, bells and iron horns. Some had
their front teeth broken off or torn out for easy identification
when they ran away. They faced torture and brutality. They
were stripped naked, their limbs and backs cut with knives, red
pepper was rubbed into their torn flesh and spirits of turpentine
poured on their wounds to increase the severity of torture. They
were searched with blood hounds and shot down like beasts.
They were also beaten and whipped till they fainted and
sometimes till they die. Their ears were cut off, their bones
broken and their flesh marked with red hot irons. They were
also burned to death over slow fires. Weld established all these
facts from testimony form slaveholders, eye witnesses and
slaves themselves (Weld 13).
This book was influential American abolitionist publication. It
laid bare the cruelty and violence associated with slavery. Most
of evidence was pulled from Southern newspapers and
testimony of former slaves and slave owners. During this
period, opposition of slavery had become more outspoken. The
slaves had been concentrated in more productive agricultural
plantations especially the cotton lands of the South. Slave trade
which was inhumane business also existed and it was criticized
by domestic and foreign travellers, emancipationists,
abolitionists and modern authors. The slave dealer
advertisements in newspapers documented the evils of the
institution of slavery. The narrative encouraged active
antislavery movements which advocated various plans of
emancipation. The intensive opposition of slavery occurred
during decades of 1830-60. Attempts to reduce slave trade led
to enactment of anti-importation law which constrained the
bringing in of slaves from other states.
Much of the content from Theodore Weld publication was from
Kentucky newspapers, slave auction handbills, personal
interviews and personal observations. The enactment of fugitive
slave law by US Congress in 1950 was based on Kentucky.
Mush of setting and many of the characters were drawn from
Kentucky. Increased opposition of slavery was associated with
prosperity in Kentucky.
Weld’s publication turned into extensive antislavery movement
in American history. It is remembered in the abolitionist
movement and future antislavery literary works. This means that
Weld used persuasion means to argue to the northern audiences
concerning evils of slavery and the need to abolish it. The truth
is that Weld used judicial motif where he acted as a prosecutor
and requested his readers to perform as jurors in judging the
legality of slavery in the United States. Weld did this by
depending on evidence from testimony of witnesses and
newspapers advertisements to prove his arguments. Weld
showed that slavery resulted from individuals who were
encouraged by absolute arbitrary power. The South supported
slavery based on the argument that institution of slavery was
positive good and justified necessary evil. Weld rejected these
justifications and established that slavery which aimed at
establishing absolute power over others. According to Weld,
human nature is contrary to slavery and thus it should be ended.
Weld also held that abolitionists were justified in condemning
slavery. Weld accomplished his role of prosecutor in the case by
combining the logic and emotion of his jurors. The jurors acted
as members of humanity and perceived slaves in the same level
as them and this forced them to pass a just judgment of guilty
since slavery is in contradiction of the humanity (Trudeau 16).
Conclusion
Theodore Weld’s publication was the most influential American
antislavery tract. It used judicial motive to show the evils
associated with slavery. The evidence was drawn from
testimony of witnesses, the slaveholders and slaves themselves.
The collection of first hand testimonials and personal narratives
from both whites and freedmen was used to portray the horrors
of American slavery. The work covered the slave diet, clothing,
housing, working conditions and torture and brutality.
Work Cited
McBride, David. "“Slavery As It Is:” Medicine and Slaves of
the Plantation South." OAH Magazine of History 19.5 (2005):
36-40.
Trudeau, Justin Thomas. "Theodore Dwight Weld's use of the
judicial motif in American Slavery As It Is: Testimony of a
Thousand Witnesses." (1998).
Weld, Theodore Dwight, ed. American Slavery as it is:
Testimony of a Thousand Witnesses. No. 10. American Anti-
Slavery Society, 1839.
ABBREVIATED TITLE IN CAPITAL LETTERS 8
Full Title in Upper and Lower Case Letters
A Critiqué Submitted by:
Name of Student
El Centro College
Psychology 2301, Section 52401, Spring 2020
Running head: ABBREVIATED TITLE IN CAPITAL
LETTERS1
Abstract
The abstract is a summary of YOUR paper. In essence, the
abstract section for this paper will include a summary of both
the Review and Critique sections of YOUR paper. It is similar
to reading the back cover of a book to get a snapshot of what
the book is about. Read the author’s abstract as an example of
how an abstract is written but DO NOT copy their abstract. If
you write something in your abstract or any other portion of
your paper that is taken from someone else (including the
author of the article you are critiquing), you must cite them in
or at the end of that sentence and later (on the References page)
provide a full “address” of where the source can be found.
Please be aware, for your purposes in this class, plagiarism will
earn you an automatic zero (Ø) on the assignment. The abstract
is generally written last or after you have completed the
assignment, as it is a summary of what you have written. For
the Abstract, you will be required to write 150-250 words. This
is the only page that is NOT first-line indented and is should be
flush with the left margin of your page (i.e. left justified). Note:
When you submit your paper for grading, it will be
automatically sent to SafeAssign which analyses your paper for
similarities found in other papers that have been submitted,
research articles and websites. It is highly functional and will
most often catch plagiarism, so please do not plagiarize. Also, it
is advisable for you to use this template as much of the
formatting has already been done.
Full Title in Upper and Lower Case LettersIntroduction
Beginning on page three, you will write the headings
Introduction, Review, and Critique and you will begin using
indentions for each paragraph. In APA writing, there are
specific ways to write headings depending on how many levels
are used. This is the one time you ARE allowed to use font
effects (i.e., bolds or italics).
The Introduction is ONE paragraph of 150-250 words that will
first provide a PROBLEM STATEMENT in a single sentence
and then summarizes the article you chose from the list. As
soon as you state something that was taken from the article, you
must IMMEDIATELY cite your source(s) within the text or at
the end of that sentence (whether quoting or paraphrasing).
Here are a couple examples of how to use “in text citations:”
· Smith suggests that the average dog lives eight years (2011).
· It is believed that the average dog lives eight years (Smith,
2011). The author’s name was not listed in the sentence and
needs to be added to the end.
· “After careful review of over 10,000 records, it was
determined that the average dog’s lifespan covers eight years”
(Smith, 2011, p154). Quotes should include quotation marks and
the citation identifies the specific page(s) on which the quote
was found.
In any case, the period for the sentence is after the citation.
… lives eight years (2011). Not …lives eight years.
(2011)
If it is found that you have plagiarized without having given due
credit, you will receive a zero (Ø) on the assignment, and this
activity may be reported to the Dean of the College of Arts and
Sciences division. Plagiarism is a serious issue and will NOT be
tolerated. Review
When you write content on the next heading, do not add extra
lines before the next heading. Notice there is not an additional
line between the last sentence of the Introduction section and
the beginning of the Review section of this template.
After you have read the material for your assignment and
thought about it, give an overview of its contents. This differs
from the Introduction section in that it is more comprehensive
and consequently a page or more in length and that it must
identify assumptions made by the researcher of the article.
Briefly explain WHAT types of research or experiments were
conducted, WHO were these conducted on, WHERE were they
conducted, WHEN were they conducted, and HOW were they
conducted. For example, you might list the organization by
name (Western University) or location (“an inner city high
school in Chicago, Illinois”). Include ages and gender of the
subjects, and whether or not parental consent was required in
order to assess this sample.
Include details of how much research was investigated and the
conclusions of the research.
Do NOT use I, my, he, she, them, we, us, or other pronouns. In
fact, the assignment should be written completely in the third
person which helps remove subjectiveopinion (i.e., your
personal thoughts or opinions) and favors an OBJECTIVE
approach (spoken from the perspective of an uninvolved
observer) by such statements as, “The researchers determined
that a multiple regression analysis of the data was sufficient to
assess the validity of the research methodology.” Third person
makes the writer a reporter of the facts. Personal opinions are
simply not relevant or appropriate for this paper.
Further, when writing about an experiment, you must also
indicate what the hypothesis’ were (there is rarely only one
hypothesis, so include them all), and what the researchers
expected to find.
State the findings (conclusions) and whether or not they were
consistent with what the researchers had anticipated. “Negative
results” are okay…we do not always find what we hope to, but
we DO report everything. It is not necessary for you to give a
synopsis of the statistical and data analysis methods used by the
researchers. Critique
In this section you will write a page or more about the
quality of the author’s article and the quality of their research
(if they conducted research). Do not write a review of the
subject itself. This is your opportunity to express your objective
findings on the article. Think of it as if you were a food critic
and evaluating a restaurant. You would not report to your
readers that the food was “very hot” because that would be
subjective. You would possibly say the plate was seasoned with,
reporting a list of spices, and most likely more spicy than the
average person would prefer. It may also be reported that one
might expect runny nose, watery eyes and similar types of
reactions that are common when eating “spicy foods.” This
provides a more objective view of the spicy level of the food.
Again, the assignment requires writing in third person, NOT
first person (i.e., I think they failed to comply with the
Scientific Method, etc.) nor use phrases like “in MY opinion.”
It would be better to say, the authors did not comply with the
Scientific Method as they did not ….
This is your rare opportunity to actually criticize (providing
objective findings, positive and/or negative) what a researcher
has done, so make the most of it. If you disagree with how the
research was conducted, say so, but TELL WHY this is so,
BASED ON THE SCIENTIFIC METHOD as discussed in
lecture and your textbook. Such comments on your part show
that you have applied critical thinking to your review of the
article. Again, this is not a forum for your opinions or for
feedback of the subject itself but for your collegiate critical
thinking on the quality of their research and/or article. Then end
your Critique section with a conclusion statement.
Here are more items to consider for your Critique section.
· Did the authors:
· follow all appropriate scientific protocols such as:
· identify the target population;
· obtain a random sample or more specifically a representative
sample;
· provide clear definitions (operationalization) of their
dependent and independent variables;
· indicate their specific methodologies;
· give adequate consideration to various aspects of their topic
by having read the research of a number of researchers in this
area.
· Did the findings leave the typical reader more or less
interested in this topic?
· Would it be helpful for the reader to see more articles in this
area of research or meta-analysis …perhaps with additional
questions (i.e., hypotheses) in the future?
· Did the researchers violate the principles of scientific
methodology (e.g., the Scientific Method) in some way?
· Did the researchers obtain Informed Consent from all
participants? Was parental consent (or that of a legal guardian)
required if the subjects were minors?
· Did the researcher miss something that was obvious?
· Would adding additional groups of people to the sample alter
the results?
· Were the researcher’s findings significant, and if so, why?
· Are there additional questions one might have after reading
this article?
· Should additional research (or follow up studies) be
conducted– perhaps with additional questions or specific
parameters or variables? If your article says the average dog
can do three tricks, does that vary in other parts of the world or
is there a significant difference in the abilities of different
breeds of dogs to do tricks?
· What are potential follow-up studies that could be conducted
to expand knowledge in this area?
Your responses to the questions and issues in this document will
tell whether or not you have actually THOUGHT ABOUT the
article at issue.
References
1st Author’s Last Name, First Initial. Second Initial. & 2nd
Author’s Last Name, First Initial. Second Initial (year
published). Write the full title with this type of capitalization.
Write theFull Journal Name in Italics in Regular Capitalization
12(1). pp. 120-151.
Note: Use a hanging indention as seen above (i.e. do not indent
the first line of the reference but indent each line after for that
reference). For the names, do not change the order of the name
but also do not list first names, only initials. For the year, do
not add season or month. The 12 in italics is the volume number
and the 1 in parenthesis is the edition/issue number which is not
in italics. Lastly end with the page range of the article followed
by a period.
Here is an example of an APA formatted article:
Copeland, R. D. (2017). A comprehensive study of different dog
breeds. Journal of Canine Friends, 34(2), pp. 123-145.
Evaluating a Student-Led Mental Health Awareness Campaign
Dominique Giroux and Elisa Geiss* Olivet College
ABSTRACT. The present study evaluated if a week-long mental
health awareness campaign on a college
campus would decrease self-stigma toward seeking help.
Participants were 204 full-time undergraduate
students attending a small private liberal arts college in the
Midwest (October, 2017). The mental health
awareness campaign offered activities where students were
exposed to interactive events and education
about campus crisis resources. Researchers measured
self-stigma and attitudes toward seeking help
through the Self-Stigma of Seeking Help Scale (SSOSH) and
Mental Help Seeking Attitude Scale
(MHSAS) pre- and postawareness week. Results showed that
student self-stigma toward seeking help
decreased after a week-long mental health awareness campaign.
Specifically, we found a decrease in
SSOSH scores, t(52) = 2.66, p = .01, d = 0.25, and an increase
in MHSAS scores from pretest to posttest,
t(56) = -2.72, p = .009, d = -0.29, indicating a reduction of
self-stigma. We discuss results in the context
of reducing stigma from a student-led mental health campaign
and further provide suggestions on how to
conduct an awareness campaign and test results at small
colleges.
Keywords: mental health awareness campaign, college students,
stigma
Mental illness among college students is becoming a serious
public health problem, with the onset of
illness often developing during early college years (Garlow et
al., 2008). One third of college students
self-report symptoms of anxiety, depression, and high rates of
stress (Lipson, Gaddis, Heinze, Beck, &
Eisenberg, 2015). Further, distressed students have worse
academic performance and exhibit high
drop-out rates (Kitzrow, 2003). Seeking and receiving
treatment is one way to decrease mental health
disorders. Those who do not seek treatment are more likely to
develop a longer, more intensified
comorbidity with other mental illnesses (Wang et al., 2005).
When this population seeks treatment,
functioning and academic performance improves (Zivin,
Eisenberg,
Gollust, & Golberstein, 2009). However, not all students will
seek treatment for a mental health disorder.
One of the most common barriers to seeking help for a
psychological disorder among young adult
populations is stigma. College student populations deem stigma
as a barrier to involvement in the
community, social relationships, and seeking proper treatment
(Eisenberg, Downs, Golberstein, & Zivin,
2009). Both public and self-stigma may affect whether a student
decides to seek treatment for a mental
disorder. Public stigma, the stereotyping and discriminative
actions toward those who are known to have
a mental illness (Corrigan, 2004), may be an external barrier to
treatment. For example, individuals who
seek treatment for a mental disorder are deemed more
“emotionally unstable,
Page 61
less interesting, and less confident” than persons seeking
treatment for a physical ailment (Vogel, Wade,
& Ascheman, 2009, p. 301). However, selfstigma or the
internalized public stigma, can lead individuals
to not try to seek care due to shame of being labeled as having a
mental illness (Corrigan, Druss, &
Perlick, 2014; Vogel, Wade, & Haake, 2006). Both public and
self-stigma introduce barriers toward
seeking help, and reducing both types of stigma may improve
mental health on a college campus.
Colleges have tried multiple interventions to reduce incidences
of mental health crises on campus by
targeting psychoeducation and improving awareness.
Specifically, psychoeducational interventions can
reduce stigma, increase the mental health literacy of a
population (Jorm et al., 2003), and dispel myths
about mental illnesses (Yanos, Lucksted, Drapalski, Roe, &
Lysaker, 2015). These types of interventions
have been effective in decreasing the stigma of seeking
psychological help (Brown & Bradley, 2002;
Komiya, Good, & Sherrod, 2000), and furthermore have the
potential to reduce barriers to help-seeking.
In fact, student-led campaigns such as the Depression OutReach
Alliance college program uses
peer-topeer psychoeducation and intervention to educate and
increase help-seeking among undergraduate
college students. In particular, Funkhouser, Zakriski, and
Spoltore (2017) measured responses from
participants regarding at-risk peers and stigma and found
increased crisis response skills, less desire for
social distance from peers in distress, and less social stigma
toward seeking help. Thus, a student-led
mental health awareness campaign has shown potential to both
target stigma and increase help-seeking
among college students.
Given the potential benefit of student-led mental health
campaigns to reduce stigma, we aimed to test
whether a campaign at a small liberal arts college would be
effective in decreasing stigma toward seeking
help. In a previous study conducted on our campus, we found
that 47% of student respondents did not
know where to go in crisis, only 22% knew about student
services or a counselor on campus, and 88% of
students selfreported that more awareness on campus is needed
(Giroux & Geiss, 2017). Given this data,
providing psychoeducation about mental health concerns and
improving knowledge about treatment
resources on campus were appropriate targets of intervention.
Thus, we created a mental health awareness
campaign focused on giving general information about mental
health disorders and access to campus
resources.
The current study was unique in its approach because it
examined a student-led and-run intervention at a
small liberal arts college without many mental health awareness
resources available to students. First, we
hypothesized that students’ self­reported stigma about seeking
mental health help would lessen after the
mental health awareness week given that previous studies have
found improvement of stigma after
psychoeducation. Furthermore, we hypothesized that those who
actively participated and attended mental
health week events would experience a larger decrease in
self-stigma toward seeking help compared to
those who did not actively participate or attend.
This study addressed the feasibility of conducting an
intervention while testing students empirically, and
aimed to improve mental health awareness on a small campus.
We created a mental health awareness
week and tested stigma before and after the week, and also
gathered demographic data about the
participants.
Method
Procedure: After we gained approval from the Olivet College
institutional review board, we recruited
students at a small, Midwestern, liberal arts college to
participate in two surveys via e-mail. Those who
completed the online consent form completed de-identified
questionnaires on Google Forms a week prior
to the mental health awareness week and immediately afterward.
Because this was a universal
intervention, students who took the surveys could choose to
participate or not in the mental health
awareness intervention. Participants were entered into a raffle
for a FitBit Charge 2 or a $50 voucher to
the college bookstore if they completed both pre- and
postintervention surveys.
Questionnaires
Demographics Questionnaire. Participants were asked to create
a unique identifier to track their
participation from the pretest to the posttest, and to ensure
confidentiality. Participants were then asked to
complete questions about gender, ethnicity, year in school, area
of study, and participation in college
athletics. In addition, during the posttest, participants reported
whether they participated in the mental
health awareness week, which included picking up brochures or
attending mental health week events.
Page 62
Mental Help Seeking Attitudes Scale (MHSAS). The MHSAS is
a 9-item instrument that examines
attitudes toward seeking help from a mental health professional
if respondents hypothetically had a
mental health concern (Hammer, Parent, & Spiker, 2018).
Participants were asked to respond to a single
statement on a 7-point Likert-type scale. For example, they
were asked to rate how important or how
healing it is to seek help from a mental health professional. A
higher score indicates a more positive
attitude toward seeking help. These scores showed strong
internal reliability for both the pretest and
posttest; Cronbach’s α = .89 and α = .91, respectively.
Self-Stigma of Seeking Help (SSOSH). The SSOSH is a
10-item scale designed to understand
how a participant views seeking mental health help, and ideas
about public stigma toward seeking
psychological help (Vogel et al., 2006). This scale asked
participants to rate the degree to which each item
describes how one would react in a situation. Examples of
statements are “I would feel inadequate if I
went to a therapist,” “My view of myself would not change just
because I made the choice to see a
therapist,” and “I would feel worse about myself if I could not
solve my own problems.” A higher score
represents higher levels of self-stigma. These scores showed
strong reliability for both the pretest and
posttest; Cronbach’s α = .87 and α = .84, respectively.
Mental Health Week Intervention The inaugural mental health
awareness week took place in the
fall 2017. The college’s Psi Chi chapter as well as a 3­credit
Abnormal Psychology class contributed to
the information and events held throughout the week. Tangible
items such as stress balls, mental health
awareness ribbons, informational pamphlets describing common
disorders among students, and
interactive displays were freely made available to the public in
the main academic building.
The college’s Psi Chi chapter and the Black Student Union
hosted the first Mental Health Open
Mic Night in the middle of the week for students to gather and
talk about multiple issues faced on campus
or at home in regard to stressors or mental health disorders. The
Open Mic Night was a free event for
students, and Psi Chi provided informational slideshows and
hosted the college’s school counselor for
familiarity. A faculty member from the psychology department
also attended the event, providing
handouts regarding particular disorders that are commonly faced
by students, such as alcoholism. Once
students finished talking to the audience about personal
experiences, the school counselor held a short
debrief and educated the attendees on what to do when
distressed. The school counselor also handed out
her business card to attendees for further contact if needed.
Once the event ended, the mental health
awareness week intervention was considered complete.
Results
Participants
After conducting the college’s inaugural mental health
awareness week, we received 166
responses in total. Both women (n = 111), men (n = 54), and
self-reported other gender (n = 1) responded
to our questionnaires. We received responses from students
across all class ranks: first-year students (n =
52), sophomores (n = 39), juniors (n = 31), seniors (n = 39), and
nondegree seeking (n = 4). Although one
student did not self-identify an ethnicity, the students
responding to our surveys self-identified as White
or European American (n = 145), African American or Black (n
= 11), Hispanic or Latino (n = 8), or
Asian (n = 1).
Analyses in this article only included the 57 participants who
completed both the pretest and
posttest surveys. Demographic makeup of this final participant
list was mostly female (n = 40) compared
to male (n = 16) or other gender (n = 1), mostly White or
European American (n = 50) compared to
African American or Black (n = 4), and Hispanic or Latino (n =
3), and mostly first-year students
(n = 20) compared to sophomores (n = 11), juniors (n = 12),
seniors (n = 13) or nondegree seeking
(n = 1). Prior to excluding those who did not participate in both
surveys, we conducted analyses to ensure
that our final participants were not significantly different from
those who only completed one survey.
Participant Analysis
We ran a Chi Square to determine whether there was a
difference in gender and student class rank
across those participants who took only one (pretest or posttest)
or two surveys (both pretest and posttest).
There was no difference in response rate of genders, χ2 = 0.95,
p = .62, d = 0.15, or year in school across
pretest, posttest, or both tests, χ2 = 2.7, p = .95, d = 0.26. Given
the low response rate for some ethnicities
who completed only the posttest (no African American students
and only one Hispanic/ Latino student),
we could not run a Chi Square test
Page 63
on this data. However, the relative percentage of students
identifying as European American, African
American, or Hispanic/Latino was similar across the entire
sample and final sample.
In addition, we examined if there was a difference in scores on
the SSOSH and MHSAS based on
participation in just one survey or across both. Results showed
that there was no significant difference
between participants who completed the pretest survey and
those who completed both. However, we
found a significant difference between participants who
completed the posttest survey only and those who
completed both on the SSOSH, t(90) = 2.6, p = .01, d = 0.54.
Specifically, participants who completed
only the posttest survey self-reported higher levels of stigma (M
= 26.8, SD = 6.4) compared to those who
completed both (M = 23.5, SD = 5.7).
Table 1 Correlation Table for Pretest and Posttest Scores on the
MHSAS and SSOSH
Correlations N M (SD) MHSAS
pretest
SSOSH
pretest
MHSAS
posttest
SSOSH
posttest
MHSAS
pretest
57 5.7 (1.06) 1
SSOSH
pretest
55 25 (6.6) -.41* 1
MHSAS
posttest
57 6.0 (0.98) .62** -.24 1
SSOSH
posttest
55 23.5 (5.73) 1.19 .77** -3.7 1
**. Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed). Note.
MHSAS pretest = Mental Health
Seeking Attitude Scale pretest; SSOSH pretest = Self-Stigma of
Seeking Help pretest; MHSAS posttest =
Mental Health Seeking Attitude Scale posttest; SSOSH posttest
= Self-Stigma of Seeking Help posttest;
N = number of respondents.
Main Analyses Change in stigma after mental health awareness
week. Before examining the
change in pretest to posttest scores, we first ran correlations
between the variables of interest in
participants who took both the pretest- and posttest (see Table
1). As expected, scores on both stigma
surveys reported at the same time (either pretest or posttest)
were negatively related to each other (i.e.,
higher scores on the SSOSH and lower scores on the MHSAS
both reflect greater stigma). In addition,
scores on the same surveys taken before and after the
intervention were positively correlated with each
other.
Next, we used a paired-samples t test to examine change in
MHSAS and SSOSH scores before
and after the intervention. We found an overall decrease in
SSOSH scores as seen in SSOSH pretest (M =
25.0, SD = 6.8) and SSOSH posttest (M = 23.4, SD = 5.8), t(52)
= 2.66, p = .01, d = 0.25. We then found
an increase in MHSAS scores from pretest (M = 5.7, SD = 1.1)
to posttest (M = 6.0, SD = 1.0), t(56) =
-2.72, p = .009, d = - 0.29. Thus, over the week, there was
decreased mental health seeking stigma as
indicated on both stigma surveys.
Participation in awareness week on change in self-stigma
scores. Because we collected data from
students who might not have actively participated in the mental
health awareness week events, we
examined whether changes in self-stigma differed based on
self-report of having participated in the
mental health week activities. Participation in the awareness
week was defined as picking up brochures or
attending an event on a given night.
Although the scores for SSOSH did not differ, we found that
changes in MHSAS scores were
dependent upon participation, F(1) = 5.28, p = .03,
g = 0.68. Using a two-way repeated-measures Analysis of
Variance, we found that those
individuals who participated in the Mental Health Week
activities had an increase in MHSAS scores (N =
16, Mpre = 5.23, SDpre = 1.01, Mpost = 5.97, SDpost = 1.05),
t(15) = 2.45, p = .03, whereas those who
did not participate did not have a change in scores (N = 41,
Mpre = 5.88, SDpre = 1.04, Mpost = 6.03,
SDpost = 0.97), t(40) = 1.47, p = .15. Yet, we also found that
MHSAS scores were higher during the
pretest in those individuals who did not participate (Mnon
participating = 5.88, SD = 1.04) compared to
those who did participate (Mparticipating = 5.22, SD = 1.01),
t(55) = 2.14, p = .04, d = 0.64. This may
indicate that those people who participated in the mental health
activities held more stigmatized beliefs,
and they improved upon their stigma over time (see Figure 1).
Page 64
Discussion
On our small sized college campus, students have expressed a
need for more visible mental
health awareness on campus. With only 22% of students on our
campus knowing where to go if in
distress, we sought to educate students about local resources
offered on our campus (Giroux & Geiss,
2017). After a week-long mental health awareness campaign,
this sample of college students reported a
decrease in help-seeking stigma on both measures collected.
Furthermore, stigmatized attitudes improved
the most for those who actively participated in the weeklong
events, especially because these students
also started with higher stigma to begin with. Thus, this may be
an intervention that holds promise to
reduce stigma toward seeking help, especially in those who
actively participate.
While exploring the effect of participation in the events on
self-stigma, we found that students
who participated had increased positive attitudes toward seeking
help, but these participants also
self-reported high levels of stigma during the pretest. Similar to
prior research, interventions such as
psychoeducational programming may be most beneficial for
those who actively participate in mental
health awareness events (Lannin, Vogel, Brenner, Abraham, &
Heath, 2015). Yet, it also seemed puzzling
why students who participated had higher levels of stigma. One
reason for this result could be that those
who actively attended and participated in mental health
awareness week events may self-report high
self-stigma because of experienced negative psychological
feelings such as symptoms of depression
(Busby Grant, Bruce, & Batterham, 2016). However, another
study suggested that self-stigma was
significantly associated with decreased likeliness to engage in
mental health related activities, seek
psychological help, or seek counseling (Lannin et al., 2015).
The results from the present study support the importance of
evaluating a mental health
awareness campaign to understand how stigma may decrease in
specific populations (Kelly, Jorm, &
Wright, 2007). In the process of designing and conducting a
mental health awareness week, we were able
to provide this type of service while also investigating how it
changed stigma perceptions. In running this
study, we received promising preliminary results that suggest a
student-led and student-run mental health
week can decrease stigma toward seeking help. Specifically,
there needs to be considerations of methods
of administration of surveys, who to sample, and when during
the semester to sample students.
First, when thinking about administration of surveys to
undergraduate college students, it is
important to understand what method of administration is
effective for the demographic. Our results
showed significant attrition rates from pretest to posttest using
online surveys via college e-mail
addresses. Thus, future research may include testing different
types of survey administration (e.g., paper
and pencil, online) among college students that may improve
attrition rates and provide a more
representative sample of the student population.
Second, we found that time of academic school year impacted
student engagement. Attrition rates
may be linked to the fact that students were asked to participate
in the mental health awareness activities
during midterm exams, which may increase levels of stress.
Therefore, teasing out the effect of the
intervention and timing of the school year is an important
consideration for those running studies on a
college campus.
Last, it is important for pre- and postsurveys to be filled out by
students who actively participate
in mental health awareness events. One suggestion to capture
this would be to provide an incentive for
active participants in the weeklong events to participate in the
pretest and posttest surveys. By doing this,
data may capture the empirical evidence that incorporates all
student perceptions.
This study may be helpful to student-led mental health
awareness campaigns. An undergraduate
student leading a mental health awareness campaign on campus
may consider survey administration, time
of academic year, and more consistent incentives to better
understand the implications of awareness for
mental health help-seeking on a small sized campus.
References
• Brown, K., & Bradley, L. J. (2002). Reducing the stigma of
mental illness. Journal of Mental
Health Counseling, 81–87. Retrieved from
http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-10626-006
• Busby Grant, J., Bruce, C. P., & Batterham, P. J. (2016).
Predictors of personal, perceived, and
self-stigma towards anxiety and depression. Epidemiology and
Psychiatric Sciences, 25, 247–
254. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796015000220
• Corrigan, P. W. (2004). How stigma interferes with mental
health care. American Psychologist,
59, 614–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614
• Corrigan, P. W., Druss, B. G., & Perlick, D. A. (2014). The
impact of mental illness stigma on
seeking and participating in mental health care. Psychological
Science in the Public Interest, 15,
37–70. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100614531398
• Eisenberg, D., Downs, M. F., Golberstein, E., & Zivin, K.
(2009). Stigma and help seeking for
mental health among college students. Medical Care Research
and Review, 66, 522–541.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077558709335173
• Funkhouser, C. J., Zakriski, A. L., & Spoltore, J. D. (2017).
Evaluating peer-peer depression
outreach: College students helping peers approach and respond
to students in crisis. Psi Chi
Journal of Psychological Research, 22, 19–28.
https://doi.org/10.24839/2325-7342.JN22.1.19
Page 65
• Garlow, S. J., Rosenberg, J., Moore, J. D., Haas, A. P.,
Koestner, B., Hendin, H., & Nemeroff, C.
B. (2008). Depression, desperation, and suicidal ideation in
college students: Results from the
American Foundation for Suicide Prevention College Screening
Project at Emory University.
Depression and Anxiety, 25, 482–488.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20321
• Giroux, D., & Geiss, E. (2017, April). Students on campus:
Relationships, mental health, and help
seeking. Paper presented at Michigan Undergraduate
Psychology Research Conference, Hillsdale,
MI.
• Hammer, J. H., Parent, M. C., & Spiker, D. A. (2018). Mental
Help Seeking Attitudes Scale
(MHSAS): Development, reliability, validity, and comparison
with the ATSPPH-SF and
IASMHS-PO. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 65, 74–85.
https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000248
• Jorm, A. F., Griffiths, K. M., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E.,
Parslow, R. A., & Rodgers, B.
(2003). Providing information about the effectiveness of
treatment options to depressed people in
the community: A randomized controlled trial of effects on
mental health literacy, help-seeking
and symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1071–1079.
• Kelly, C. M., Jorm, A. F., & Wright, A. (2007). Improving
mental health literacy as a strategy to
facilitate early intervention for mental disorders. Medical
Journal of Australia, 187(7), S26–S30.
Retrieved from https://www.mja.com.
au/system/files/issues/187_07_011007/kel10278_fm.pdf
• Kitzrow, M. A. (2003). The mental health needs of today's
college students: Challenges and
recommendations. NASPA Journal, 41, 167–181.
https://doi.org/10.2202/0027-6014.1310
• Komiya, N., Good, G. E., & Sherrod, N. B. (2000). Emotional
openness as a predictor of college
students' attitudes toward seeking psychological help. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 47,
138–143. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-0167.47.1.138
• Lannin, D. G., Vogel, D. L., Brenner, R. E., Abraham, W. T.,
& Heath, P. J. (2015). Does self-
stigma reduce the probability of seeking mental health
information? Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 63, 351–358.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000108
• Lipson, S. K., Gaddis, S. M., Heinze, J., Beck, K., &
Eisenberg, D. (2015). Variations in student
http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-10626-006
https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796015000220
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077558709335173
https://doi.org/10.24839/2325-7342.JN22.1.19
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20321
https://doi.org/10.2202/0027-6014.1310
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.47.1.138
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000108
mental health and treatment utilization across US colleges and
universities. Journal of American
College Health, 63, 388–396.
https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2015.1040411
• Vogel, D. L., Wade, N. G., & Ascheman, P. L. (2009).
Measuring perceptions of stigmatization
by others for seeking psychological help: Reliability and
validity of a new stigma scale with
college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 301–
308.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014903
• Vogel, D. L., Wade, N. G., & Haake, S. (2006). Measuring the
self-stigma associated with
seeking psychological help. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
53, 325–337.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.325
• Wang, P. S., Berglund, P., Olfson, M., Pincus, H. A., Wells,
K. B., & Kessler, R. C. (2005).
Failure and delay in initial treatment contact after first onset of
mental disorders in the National
Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 62, 603–613.
https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.603
• Yanos, P. T., Lucksted, A., Drapalski, A. L., Roe, D., &
Lysaker, P. (2015). Interventions
targeting mental health self-stigma: A review and comparison.
Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal,
38, 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000100
• Zivin, K., Eisenberg, D., Gollust, S. E., & Golberstein, E.
(2009). Persistence of mental health
problems and needs in a college student population. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 117, 180–
185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.001
Author Note. Dominique Giroux,
https://orcid.org/00000002-2487-7777, Olivet College; Elisa
Geiss,
Department of Psychology, Olivet College. This study was
supported by the Olivet College Student
Government Association. Special thanks to Psi Chi Journal
reviewers for their support. Correspondence
concerning this article should be addressed to Dominique
Giroux, E: [email protected]
Page 66
Copyright of Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research is the
property of Psi Chi Journal of
Undergraduate Research and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download,
or email articles for individual use.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.325
https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.603
https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000100
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.001
mailto:[email protected]Evaluating a Student-Led Mental Health
Awareness CampaignMethodResultsTable 1 Correlation Table
for Pretest and Posttest Scores on the MHSAS and
SSOSHDiscussionReferences
Enhancing School Success with Mnemonic Strategies
MARGO A. MASTROPIERI AND THOMAS E. SCRUCGGS
Many students with disabilities and those at risk for educational
failure exhibit problems
with remembering materials covered in school. Suggestions for
helping these students
improve memory for school content are described in this article.
In particular, mnemonic
instruction is described and examples are provided of how it can
be used to increase
school learning and memory of students with learning
difficulties.
A few years ago, we revisited an inner-city middle school
where-about a year before we had
conducted an investigation on the effectiveness of mnemonic
techniques in helping students
with mild cognitive disabilities remember U.S. states and
capitals. As we entered the classroom,
Crystal, a student classified as mildly mentally handicapped,
recognized one of our graduate
students immediately.
"Hay! I remember you!" she exclaimed enthusiastically.
"You were here last year-you taught us states and capitals! I
remember, go ahead, ask me
one!"
"Well, uh," replied our colleague, taken slightly by surprise,
"How about ... Florida? What's the
capital of Florida?"
"That's too easy!" she said, smiling. "Here it is: Florida, the
keyword is flower-the flower is on a
television set, and television is the keyword for Tallahassee!"
In this scenario, a student classified as mentally retarded
effectively remembered information
she had been taught 1 year previously. Even more impressive
was the fact that she had not
reviewed or rehearsed this information with any teacher since
the last time we had seen her! As
startling as this scenario is, it underscores something we have
been witnessing for many years:
the incredible power of mnemonic strategies to increase
dramatically the amount of information
students remember, even students with learning problems. In
this article, we describe the need
for effective memory strategies for school learning. Next, we
provide a brief description of what
mnemonic strategies are and what they are not. Following that,
we describe how you can use
these powerful learning tools to enhance the school success of
your own students.
THE NEED FOR MNEMONIC STRATEGIES
According to the Sixteenth Annual Report to Congress on the
Implementation of the Individuals
with Disabilities Act (U.S. Department of Education, I 994),
71.2 % of all students now spend all
or a substantial part of their school day in general education
classrooms. As many as 78.9% of
students with learning disabilities spend all or most of their
educational time in general
education classrooms. On the secondary level, students with
disabilities are included in content
area classrooms, such as English, science, and social studies
classes. Largely, ability to
succeed in these classrooms determines their level of success in
school.
What factors determine whether a student will succeed in
secondary content-area classrooms?
Putnam (1992b) surveyed 7th- and 10th-grade teachers in
Kansas,
INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC VOL. 33, No. 4,
MARCH 1996 (PP. 201-208) 201
Indiana, and Florida, and reported that an average of nearly half
of a student's report card
grades depended on test performance. Clearly, such factors as
attendance, punctuality,
participation, and homework completion are also important.
However, teachers made it clear
that test scores were the single most important factor in report
card grades. Teachers gave an
average of 11 of these tests over the course of a single 9-week
grading period. Clearly,
students' academic survival is tied very closely with
performance on academic tests.
In addition to the number of tests teachers give, Putnam (1992a)
also examined the types of
questions teachers asked on tests. He found that the
overwhelming majority of test questions
students were asked required facn1al recall:
The majority of questions on tests administered by main- stream
secondary classroom teachers
required the student to recall a specific foct-2 S.3 per test. A
sample question that asked for a
specific fact was "Who discovered America?" Other possible
responses, such as conclusion,
sequence, opinion, discrimination, compare and contrast,
purpose, correct an example, and
summary, appeared about one per test. (p. 131)
Thus it can be seen that memory for factual information is
absolutely essential for success in
school, particularly at the secondary level. Unfortunately, it is
also true that students with
learning disabilities and other learning problems have been
consistently shown to have
particular difficulties remembering academic content (e.g.,
Cooney & Swanson, 1987). Our work
in the area of mnemonic (memory-enhancing) strategies has
been devoted to finding ways of
increasing the amount of content-area information students are
able to remember. This article
provides information on the utility, and effectiveness, of
mnemonic strategies in enhancing
memory for school learning.
WHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES ARE
A1nemonic strategies are systematic procedures for enhancing
memory. Their particular use is
in developing better ways to take in (encode) information so
that it will be much easier to
remember (retrieve). Although there are retrieval strategies that
can be employed to attempt to
retrieve information that has been forgotten, research has
demonstrated that the way we
encode information when we first study facilitates memory
better. The particular task in
developing mnemonic strategies is to find a way to relate new
information to information
students already have locked in long-term memory. If we can
make a firm enough connection,
the memory will last a very long time. For example, Crystal had
learned the capital of Florida so
well because the mnemonic strategy had carefully linked it to
things she was very familiar with.
Because Florida sounds like flower (the keyword), it was easy
to teach her to make an
automatic connection between Florida and flower:
What's the keyword for Florida? Flower, good! And, what state
is flower the keyword for? Good,
Florida!
It was also easy to teach her to establish a firm association
between Tallahassee and television
because television was very familiar to Crystal and the two
words, again, sound very similar:
What’s the keyword for Tallahassee? Television, good! And,
what capital is television the
keyword for? Good, Tallahassee!
Having linked the two words (Florida, Tallahassee) to concrete,
familiar words that sound similar
(flower, television), all that remains is to link the two familiar
words together. And although
memory experts Harry Lorayne and Jerry Lucas have suggested
that these words must be
associated "in some ridiculous way" (1974, p. 2 3), in fact all
that is necessary is that the two
keywords be pictured in some meaningful interaction. In the
Florida instance, a picture had
been created and displayed on an overhead projector of a flower
sitting on a television set, as
shown in Figure I.
Although recalling that Tallahassee is the capital of Florida may
be difficult for a student with
learning problems, remembering a picture of a flower on a
television set was much simpler, and-
if the keywords had been learned-contained the same
information. For this rea- son, we have
found mnemonic strategies constructed according to these
procedures to be extraordinarily
effective (Mastropieri, Scruggs, Bakken, & Brigham, 1992).
Figure 1. Mnemonic representation of Tallahassee, capital of
Florida. (Copyright I 993 by M.A.
Mastropieri and TE. Scruggs.)
202 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC
WHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES ARE NOT
It might be helpful, at this point, to also mention briefly what
mnemonic strategies are not.
Mnemonic strategies do not represent a "philosophy" of
education. We do not use, or
recommend the use of, mnemonic strategies because they arc
compatible with someone's
particular philosophy or because they are a part of some- one's
theory about what education
should be. We recommend mnemonic strategies for only one
reason: Over and over again, they
have been proven to be extremely effective in helping people
remember things (Bulgren,
Schumaker, & Deshler, 1994; Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1989).
It is also true that mnemonic strategies are not an overall
teaching method or curricular
approach. The focus of mnemonic strategies is so specific that
they are intended to be used to
enhance the recall of the components of any lesson for which
memory is needed. We have
found, for example, that mnemonic strategies can be ·used to
enhance science learning when
the curriculum involves a textbook/lecture format (Scruggs &
Mastropicri, 1992) or when the
curriculum involves a hands-on, inquiry learning format
(Mastropicri, Scruggs, & Chung, 1997).
Even though these approaches to science learning are very
different (Mastropieri & Scruggs,
1994), mnemonic strategies can still be incorporated for the
elements that require recall.
It is also important to consider that mnemonic strategies are
memory strategies, and not
comprehension strategies. Students who are trained
mnemonically also perform better on
comprehension tests of that content (e.g., Mastropieri, Scruggs,
& Fulk, 1990; Scruggs,
Mastropicri, McLoone, Levin, & Morrison, 1987), but that is
generally because they remember
more information that can be applied on comprehension tests.
Nevertheless, when
comprehension enhancement is called for, it is important to
consider using specific
comprehension strategies, such as content elaboration, prior
knowledge activation,
manipulation, coaching and questioning, or prediction and
verification (e.g., Mastropieri &
Scruggs, 1997; Scruggs, Mastropieri, Sullivan, & Hesser, 1993).
Nevertheless, mnemonic
strategies do not inhibit com- prehension, as suggested by some
(e.g., Kilpatrick, 1985), and,
more importantly, there arc many instances in school of
students who have achieved adequate
com- prehension of a concept, but who have forgotten the facts
associated with it. For example,
it is completely possible to comprehend the nature of states and
capitals while being unable to
retrieve the capital of Florida. For another example, it is
altogether possible to comprehend the
concepts of a cell having a nucleus and consuming other
organisms while being unable to
retrieve the related verbal label (prokaryotic heterotroph).
These are the areas where mnemonic
strategies can help.
Finally, it should be emphasized that mnemonic strategies do
not represent an educational
panacea. There are many things that students must do to succeed
in school, and remembering
content information is only one part of the entire picture.
However, when there is academic
content to be remembered, mnemonic strategies may be an
important instructional component.
In the next section, we discuss some general procedures for
improving memory, followed by a
description of how to create specific mnemonic strategies.
GENERAL TECHNIQUES FOR IMPROVING MEMORY
Mnemonic strategies as described in this article arc not the only
way of improving memory in
students who exhibit difficulty remembering things. It is
important that you consider all possible
methods for improving memory and not assume that mnemonic
strategies are your only option.
In other publications (Mastropieri & Scruggs, l 993; Mastropieri
& Scruggs, in press), we have
described more general methods for improving memory. These
include the following:
1. INCREASE AITENTION. Students will not remember
something that they did not pay
attention to in the first place. Be sure your students' memory
problems are not really
attention problems. Use strategies for enhancing attention, such
as intensifying
instruction, teaching enthusiastically, using more visual aids
and activities, and
reinforcing attending.
2. PROMOTE EXTERNAL MEMORY. Many things that need to
be remembered can be
written down, a practice known as "external memory." Practices
such as keeping an
assignment notebook and maintaining a student calendar can be
helpful in remembering
to do things. Unfortunately, external memory is usually of little
use (ethically, anyway) on
tests.
3. ENHANCE MEANINGFULNFSS. Find ways to relate the
content being discussed to the
student's prior knowledge. Draw parallels to the students' own
lives. Bring in concrete,
meaningful examples for students to explore so the content
becomes more a part of their
experience.
4. USE PICTURES. Pictures can provide a substantial memory
advantage. Use pictures on
the chalkboard or on the overhead projector. Bring in
photographs or other illustrations.
Show concrete images on video- tape, when appropriate. If
pictures are simply
unavailable, ask students to create images, or "pictures in their
heads."
VOL. 33. No. 4. MARCH 1998 203
5. MINIMIZE INTERFERENCE. Avoid digressions and
emphasize only the critical features
of a new topic. Make sure all examples relate directly to the
content being covered.
6. PROMOTE ACTIVE REASONING. Students re- member
content better when they
experience it for themselves (Scruggs, Mastropieri, Bakken, &
Brigham, 1993). For
example, rather than lecturing the class on the effect of weak
acid (such as vinegar) on
calcite, allow students to place calcite in a glass of vinegar and
see for themselves.
7. PROMOTE ACTIVE REASONING. Students remember
better if they actively think
through new information, rather than simply repeating it. For
example, rather than simply
telling students that penguins carry their eggs on the tops of
their feet, ask students why
it makes sense that penguins would carry their eggs on the tops
of their feet.
8. INCREASE THE AMOUNT OF PRACTICE. Students
remember information better if
they have practice using it more frequently. Use lots of review
in your teaching; do not
simply finish one topic and then never mention it again. Remind
the class, and have
students practice previous information frequently.
All these strategies can be used to improve memory, and all
should be considered.
Unfortunately, none of these strategies specifically targets
recall of information contained in new
or unfamiliar words, and this is the aspect of memory where
students most often fail. For
example, in the Scruggs, Mastropieri, Bakken, and Brig- ham
(1993) investigation, students who
engaged in active manipulation remembered more information
about electricity and rocks and
minerals than students who studied from textbooks. However,
neither condition improved recall
of critical vocabulary or terminology, the area where mnemonic
strategies are most effective. In
the following section, we describe several different types of
mnemonic strategies that can be
used to improve students' memory.
SPECIFIC MNEMONIC TECHNIQUES
The Keyword Method
The keyword method has already been described for helping
students remember states and
capitals. However, the keyword method is extremely versatile
and has a variety of helpful
applications. One possibility is in teaching new vocabulary
words. For example, to help students
remember that barrister is another word for lawyer, first create a
keyword for the unfamiliar
word, banister. Remember, a keyword is a word that sounds like
the new word and is easily
pictured. A good keyword for barrister,
then, is bear. Then, you create a picture of the keyword and the
definition doing something
together. It is important that these two things actually interact
and are not simply presented in
the same picture. Therefore, a picture of a bear and a lawyer in
one picture is not a good
mnemonic, because the elements are not interacting. A better
picture would be a bear who is
acting as a lawyer in a courtroom, for example, pleading his
client's innocence. We have
created pictures and shown them on overhead projectors, but
you could show them in other
ways as well. When you practice this strategy, be certain
students understand all parts of it:
Class, barrister is another word for lawye1: To remember what a
barrister is, first think of the
keyword for barrister: bear. Whats the keyword for barrister?
[bear] Good, the keyword for
barrister is bear, and barrister means lawyer. Now [displays
overhead] look at this pic- ture of a
bear acting like a lawyer. The bear is the keyword for. .?
[barrister/ Barrister, good. So
remember this picture of a bear acting like a lawyer. When you
hear the word barrister, you first
think of the keyword...? [Bear] Good, and remember, what is
the bear doing in the picture?
/being a lawyer}. Right, being a lawyer. So what does barrister
mean? [lawyer] Lawyer, good.
The keyword method can also be used for more specialized
vocabulary such as ranidae, the
scientific term for common frogs. A good keyword for ranidae
could be rain, and you could show
a picture of frogs sitting in the rain. Practice the strategy as in
the barrister example. When you
question individual students, ask them to give the answer and
then describe how they
remembered. You should get an answer something like:
Ranidae is the word for common frogs. I remembered because
the keyword is rain and it was
raining on the frogs.
If you practice the strategy carefully and frequently, students
should remember this information
very well. At early stages of learning, you might find some
students give the answer rain when
you ask what ranid(le means. In these cases, you simply remind
the student,
No, rain is the keyword it just helps us remember the answer.
Now, think in the picture,
what is it raining on? [frogs] Right, frogs. So what does
ranidae mean? [common frogs].
Correct, common frogs.
Mnemonics can also be used in acquiring foreign language
vocabulary. A list of some Italian
vocabulary words (from Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1991, p. 24)
and corresponding mnemonic
strategies are given in Table l. Before you read, cover up the
keywords and strategies and see if
you can come up with your own.
Keywords have also been used to improve recall of map
locations. For example, students with
learning dis- abilities were much more successful in locating
Revolutionary War battle locations
on a map when they
204 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC
Table 1. Sample Italian Vocabulary Words and Corresponding
Mnemonic Strategies
Word & meaning Keyword Strategy
meta (apple) mail an apple in a mailbox
capre (goat) cop a goat dressed like a
cop
Iago (lake) log a log in a lake
carta (letter) cart a cart with a letter in
it
Word & meaning Keyword Strategy
fonda (bag) phone a phone in a bag
were mnemonically encoded (e.g., a picture of a tiger, keyword
for Fort Ticonderoga) than when
representational pictures were used. When asked for the
location of Fort Ticonderoga, students
proved much more able to identify where on the map the tiger
had been than they were to
identify the location of a more traditional illustration. Further,
if the tiger was shown tending a
cannon, students were more likely to remember that at Fort
Ticonderoga, cannons were
captured that were helpful in the American war effort (Brigham,
Scruggs, & Mastropieri, 1995).
The Pegword Method
Pegwords can be used when numbered or ordered information
needs to be remembered.
Pegwords are rhyming words for numbers and include the
following:
One is bun, six is sticks,
two is shoe, seven is heaven,
three is tree, eight is gate,
four is door, nine is vine,
five is hive, ten is hen.
Pegwords are substituted for the number to be remembered and
associated with the other
information. For instance, to remember that insects have six
legs whereas spiders have eight
legs, create a picture of insects on sticks (see Figure 2) and
another picture of a spider on a
gate. To remember Newton's first law of motion (objects at rest
tend to remain at rest unless
acted on by another force), create a picture of a bun (pegword
for one) resting. To remember
that a garden rake is an example of a third-class lever, create a
picture of a rake leaning against
a tree (pegword for three, or third).
Pegwords can also be combined with keywords. To teach that
crocoite is a mineral that is
number 2 on the Mohs hardness scale, create a picture of
crocodiles (key- word for crocoite)
wearing shoes (pegword for 2). To remember that the mineral
wolframite is hardness num- ber
4, black in color, and used in making filaments for lightbulbs,
create a picture of a black wolf
(keyword for wolframite), looking in a door (pegword for 4),
and turn- ing on a lightbulb.
Research has shown that elaborate strategies such as this are
very effective, and that color,
appropriately encoded, can also be easily remembered (Scruggs,
Mastropieri, Levin, & Gaffney,
1985). That is, a picture of a black wolf is much more likely to
be remembered than a picture of
wolframite colored black. Pegwords can also be extended
beyond the number 10 (11 is lever,
12 is elf, etc.). For instance, to remember that the 19th
amendment of the U.S. Constitution
guar- anteed women the right to vote, create a picture of a
woman dressed as a knight (19 =
knighting) riding to a voting booth. To remember that James K.
Polk was the 11th American
president, create a picture of a polka dotted (keyword for
Polk) lever (pegword for 11) as
shown in Figure 3 (Mastropieri, Scruggs, & Whedon,1997).
INSECTS 6 (sticks) legs
Figure 2. Mnemonic representation of insects having six legs.
(Copyright 1993 by M.A.
Mastropieri and TE. Scruggs.)
Figure 3. Mnemonic representation of Polk as the 11th U.S.
president. (Copyright 1993 by M.A.
Mastropieri and TE. Scruggs.)
VOL. 33. No. 4. MARCH 1998 205
Pegwords can also be extended beyond the number 19, by using,
for example, twenty is twinty
(twins), thirty is thirsty, forty is party, and fifty is "gifty," or
gift-wrapped. For example, to
remember the math fact, 7 x 8 = 56, create a picture and
practice the pegword phrase,
"Heaven's (7) gate (8) holds gifty sticks" (pegword for 56). To
remember that Taft was the 27th
president, create a picture of a taffy (keyword for Taft), being
pulled between twin heavens
(pegword for 27).
Letter Strategies
Letter strategies, which involve using letter prompts to
remember lists of things, arc the most
familiar to students. Most former students remember using the
acronym HOMES to remember
the names of the Great Lakes and FACE to remember the notes
represented in the spaces of
the treble clef, from bottom to top. Except for the FACE
strategy, however, most acronyms
assume that a name of something will be remembered when the
first letter is retrieved.
However, this may not always be true. For example, if a student
is unfamiliar with Lake Ontario,
remembering simply that the first letter is O is insufficient to
prompt recall. The names of the
individual lakes must be practiced until they have become
familiar.
Acronyms are most helpful when the first letters of a list can be
used to create an entire word;
however, sometimes modifications can be made. For instance,
consider the acronym FARM-B,
which represents the five classes of vertebrate animals: fish,
amphibian, reptile, mammal, and
bird, as shown in Figure 4. The B for bird does not really fit,
but it can be added to FARM and
practiced until it becomes automatic. In other cases, appropriate
words cannot be easily con-
structed from the first letters of the words to be remembered.
For example, if you wished to
remember the names of the planets in their order from the sun,
the letters would be M-V-E-M-J-
S-U-N-P, from which a word cannot be made. In these cases, an
acrostic can be created, in
which the first letters are reconstructed to represent the words
in a sentence. In this case, the
sentence could be "My very educated mother just sent us nine
pizzas" (Mastropieri & Scruggs,
1994, p. 271). Again, the names of the planets must be
sufficiently familiar so that students can
retrieve a planet name, given only the first letter. Also, students
should be sufficiently familiar
with the solar system to know that the first M stands for
Mercury, and not Mars.
For another example, to remember the classification taxonomy
of living things, remember the
sentence, "King Philip's class ordered a family of gentle
spaniels." This sentence helps prompt
kingdom, phylum, class, order, family, genus, and species, in
order.
Figure 4. Mnemonic representation of the vertebrates: fish,
amphibians, reptiles, mammals, and
birds. (Note: From A Practical Guide for Teaching Science to
Students with Special Needs in
Inclusive Settings [p. 158}, by M. A. Mastropieri and T E.
Scruggs, 1991, Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Reprinted with per- 111frsion.)
TRAINING INDEPENDENT STRATEGY USE
Several research studies have described the effects of training
students with memory problems
how to use mnemonic strategies independently (Fulk,
Mastropieri, & Scruggs, 1992; King-
Sears, Mercer, & Sindelar, 1992; Mastropieri, Scruggs, Levin,
Gaffney, & McLoone, 1985;
McLoone, Scruggs, Mastropieri, & Zucker, 1986; Scruggs &
Mastropieri, 1992). The earlier
studies successfully trained students with disabilities to use the
mnemonic procedures and then
to generalize the procedures for learning new vocabulary words
(Mastropieri et al., 1985;
McLoone et al., 1986). More recent studies trained students
with disabilities to use the
strategies across different content areas, including science and
social studies (Fulk et al., 1992;
King-Sears et al., I 992; Scruggs & Mastropieri, 1992). All of
these studies demonstrated some
positive benefits for training students to use mnemonic
strategies independently. More
importantly, however, studies shared seven common elements
during the training sessions with
students with disabilities, as described by Fulk (1994) and
Bulgren et al. (1994) and
summarized next.
STEP 1. Inform students about the purpose of the instruction
and the rationale for the strategy
training. Tell students that strategy training will be beneficial
for them and that their efforts at
using the strategies will result in better performance. Provide
examples of how the strategy can
be beneficial across a variety of learning situations and content
areas. For example, in teaching
students how to implement the strategy to learn science
vocabulary, show them how the
method can also be helpful in learning social studies content
and for learning English and
foreign language vocabulary.
STEP 2. Provide instruction in the strategy and in positive
attributions toward strategy usage.
Demonstrate,
206 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC
model, and lead students using many examples to ensure their
complete understanding of the
mnemonic process during the strategy component training.
Provide instances and noninstances
of correct usage and have them identify and correct any
incorrect examples.
Make charts listing the steps involved in generating strategies.
King-Sears ct al. (1992) taught
students the IT FITS strategy:
Identify the term.
Tell the definition of the term. Find a keyword.
Imagine the definition doing something with the keyword.
Think about the definition doing something with the keyword.
Study what you imagined until you know the definition.
(King-Sears et al., 1992, p. 27)
Provide ample modeling and practice with students, attributing
their successes to strategy use.
Say, for example, "When I try hard and use the strategy, I will
remember more information."
Reinforce students for trying hard to use the strategy and
attribute success and failure to
strategy usage.
STEP 3. Provide models during which examples and thinking
processes are said aloud.
Demonstrate how you proceed with your thinking while
generating a strategy for specific
examples. Include statements attributing your success to the
hard effort and use of the strategy.
STEP 4. Allow students opportunities to practice orally and
provide corrective feedback.
Practice several examples with the class as a whole. Encourage
brainstorming during the
development of the keyword and interactive picture phase.
Allow students to work in small
groups and practice generating strategies and brainstorming.
Then, have students work with
partners to develop strategies before working independently.
STEP 5. Arrange guided practice with relevant feed- back on
both strategy usage and attribution
feedback. Give students additional items to practice using the
mnemonic and attribution
strategies. Provide corrective feedback and allow opportunities
for students to share their
thinking with one another about how they developed their
strategies.
STEP 6. Provide generalization instruction, practice, and
feedback. Use different types of
materials to demonstrate how the strategy can be applied across
content areas and various
types of factual information. Have students practice generating
strategies for vocabulary words
in English, for names of famous people and their accomplished
in history, for minerals and their
associated attributes in science, and other associated factual
information they may need to
learn in school
STEP 7. Include pos1t1ve reinforcement and pos1t1ve
attribution training for completing the
tasks and for remembering the information correctly. Provide
review and practice with
information that was learned using strategies. Students will still
need to practice retrieving
information learned with strategies. When implemented as a
package of training, students with
disabilities may be more likely to learn to use and generate
these strategies independently.
Limitations
Research has indicated that students who have been taught
strategies for creating their own
mnemonics out- perform comparison students in free-study
conditions. Unfortunately, however,
when students generate their own strategies, instruction may
proceed at a much slower rate
and students' performances may be lower than when teachers
supply the strategies (Scruggs &
Mastropieri, 1992). During a given unit of instruction, teachers
should consider whether learning
a strategy or learning the content is the priority.
Moreover, initial development of many of these strategies can
be difficult for anyone. Try
developing several strategies yourself before teaching your
students with disabilities. If you
experience difficulties, imagine that the task will be much more
difficult for your students. Each
year develop a few strategies to accompany the content areas
that you teach most frequently.
Over time you will have a great number of effective strategies
that you can teach your students.
This does not mean that you should not teach your students to
develop strategies
independently. You can still encourage active strategy
development on the part of your
students, but if students have difficulties generating strategies,
you can supply the ones
developed by you or your co-teachers. Perhaps you will find
that a combination of teacher-
created and student-generated mnemonic strategies is the best
way to enhance recall and still
promote independent strategy use.
SUMMARY
Although many changes in schooling have taken place in recent
years, memory for academic
content remains an extremely important part of the school
learning experience. Students with
learning disabilities and other special needs may he at particular
risk for failure in this important
arena of school functioning. In order to promote academic
success in school, we recommend
that teachers teach students how to remember as well as what to
remember. This can be done
by a variety of strategies, but by far the most powerful have
been the keyword method, the
pegword method, and letter strategies. Systematic instruction
using mnemonic strategies for
important information to remember, as well as systematic
VQL. 33, No. 4, MARCH 1998 207
instruction in independent use of mnemonic strategies, can be
important factors in determining
school success for students with learning and memory problems.
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Margo A. Mastropieri, PhD, is a professor of special education,
Department of Educational
Studies, Purdue University. Her research interests include ways
to facilitate learning, memory,
and thinking of student with disabilities. Thomas E. Scruggs,
PhD, is a professor of special
education, Department of Educational Studies, Purdue
University. His research emphasizes
research synthesis, science instruction for students with
disabilities, and mnemonic instruction
for student with disabilities. Address: Margo A. Mastropieri,
Department of Educational Studies,
LAF.B 5th floor, Purdue University, West Lafayette IN 47907
e-mail: [email protected]
REFERENCES
Brigham, F. J., Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1995).
Elaborative maps for enhanced
learning of historical information: Uniting spatial, verbal, and
imaginal information. The Journal
of Special Education, 28, 440-460.
Bulgren, J. A., Schumaker, J. B., & Deshler, D. D. (1994). The
effects of a recall enhancement
routine on the test performance of secondary students with and
without learning disabilities.
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 9, 2-11.
Cooney, J. B., & Swanson, H. L. (1987). Memory and learning
disabilities: An overview. In H. L.
Swanson (Ed.), Advances in learning and behavioral
disabilities: Memory and learning
disabilities (pp. I^K)). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Fulk, B. J. M. (1994). Mnemonic keyword strategy training for
students with learning disabilities.
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 9, 179-185.
Fulk, B.J. M., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. F. (1992).
Mnemonic generalization training with
learning disabled students. Learning Disabilities Research &
Practice, 7, 2-10.
Kilpatrick, J. (1985). Doing mathematics without understanding
it: A commentary on Higbee and
Kunihira. Educational Psychologist, 20, 65-68.
King-Sears, M. F., Mercer, C. D., & Sindelar, P. T. (1992).
Toward independence with keyword
mnemonics: A strategy for science vocabulary instruction.
Remedial and Special Education, 13,
22-33.
Lorayne, H., & Lucas, J. (1974). The memory book. New York:
Ballantine. Mastropieri, M. A., &
Scruggs, T. F. (1989). Constructing more meaningful
relationships: Mnemonic instructions for
special populations. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 88-111.
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T E. (1991). Teaching students
ways to remember: Strategies for
learning mnemonically. Cambridge, MA: Brookline.
Mastropieri
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1993). A practical guide
for teaching science to students
with special needs in inclusive settings. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T
E. (1994). Text-based vs. activities oriented science curriculum:
Implications for students with
disabilities. Remedial and Special Education, IS, 72-85.
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1997). Best practices in
promoting reading comprehension
in students with learning disabilities: 1976 to 1996. Remedial
and Special Education, 18(4), 197-
213.
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (in press). Strategies for
teaching in inclusive classrooms.
Columbus, OH: Prentice-Hall/Merrill. Mastropieri, M. A.,
Scruggs, T. E., Bakken J., & Brigham,
F. J. (1992). A complex mnemonic strategy for teaching states
and capitals: Comparing forward
and backward associations. Learning Disabilities Research &
Practice, 7, 96-103.
Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T E., & Chung, S. (1997, April).
Qualitative and quantitative
outcomes associated with inclusive science teaching. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of
the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Fulk, B.J. M. (1990).
Teaching abstract vocabulary with the
keyword method: Effects on recall and comprehension. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 23, 92-
96.
Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T F., Levin, J. R., Gaffney, J., &
McLoone, B. B. (1985). Mnemonic
vocabulary instruction for learning disabled students. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 8, 57-63.
Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Whedon, C. (1997). Using
mnemonic strategies to teach
information about U.S. presidents: A classroom-based
investigation. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 20, 13-21.
McLoone, B. B., Scruggs, T E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Zucker,
S. (1986). Mnemonic instruction
and training with learning disabled adolescents. Learning
Disabilities Research, 2, 45-53.
Putnam, M. L. (1992a). Characteristics of questions on tests
administered by mainstream
secondary classroom teachers. Learning Disabilities Research &
Practice, 7, 129-136.
Putnam, M. L. (1992b). The testing practices of mainstream
secondary classroom teachers.
Remedial and Special Education, 11-21. Scruggs, T. E., &
Mastropieri, M. A. (1992). Classroom
applications of mnemonic instruction: Acquisition,
maintenance, and generalization. Exceptional
Children, 58, 219-229.
Scruggs, T F., Mastropieri, M. A., Bakken, J. P., & Brigham, F
J. (1993). Reading vs. doing:
The relative effectiveness of textbook based and inquiry-
oriented approaches to science
education. The Journal of Special Education, 27, 1-15.
Scruggs, T F., Mastropieri, M. A., Levin, J. R., & Gaffney J. S.
(1985). Facilitating the acquisition
of science facts in learning disabled students. American
Educational Research Journal, 22, 575-
586.
Scruggs, T E., Mastropieri, M. A., McLoone, B. B., Levin, J.
R., & Morrison, C. (1987).
Mnemonic facilitation of learning disabled students' memory for
expository prose. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 79, 27-34.
Scruggs, T E., Mastropieri, M. A., Sullivan, G. S., & Hesser, L.
S. (1993). Improving reasoning
and recall: The differential effects of elaborative interrogation
and mnemonic elaboration.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 16, 233-240.
U.S. Department of Education. (1994). Sixteenth annual report
to Congress on the
implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Education
Act. Washington, DC: Author.
208
Copyright of Intervention in School & Clinic is the property of
Sage Publications Inc. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted
to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
208 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC
Enhancing School Success with Mnemonic StrategiesTHE
NEED FOR MNEMONIC STRATEGIESWHAT MNEMONIC
STRATEGIES AREWHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES ARE
NOTGENERAL TECHNIQUES FOR IMPROVING
MEMORYSPECIFIC MNEMONIC TECHNIQUESTRAINING
INDEPENDENT STRATEGY USESUMMARYABOUT THE
AUTHORSREFERENCES

More Related Content

Similar to EARLY MORTALITY8Early Mortality Review of the Charl.docx

Substance Abuse Vs Suicidal risk report Final Draft 06_04_2015
Substance  Abuse Vs Suicidal risk  report Final Draft 06_04_2015Substance  Abuse Vs Suicidal risk  report Final Draft 06_04_2015
Substance Abuse Vs Suicidal risk report Final Draft 06_04_2015Geoffrey Kip, MPH
 
A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...
A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...
A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...Monica Franklin
 
Author & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docx
Author & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docxAuthor & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docx
Author & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docxrock73
 
Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...
Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...
Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...Susan Tullis
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxClinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxgordienaysmythe
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxClinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxvernettacrofts
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxClinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009CruzIbarra161
 
Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?
Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?
Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?bijsshrjournal
 
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdfBrown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdftytrete
 
Otol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-Merritt
Otol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-MerrittOtol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-Merritt
Otol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-MerrittMichael (Mick) Merritt
 
Nursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docx
Nursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docxNursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docx
Nursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docxpoulterbarbara
 
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pPrepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pamit657720
 
Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...
Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...
Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...Liberty University (LU)
 
Mehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is Sex
Mehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is SexMehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is Sex
Mehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is SexClare Mehta
 
Lesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docx
Lesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docxLesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docx
Lesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docxSHIVA101531
 
Predictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailand
Predictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailandPredictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailand
Predictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailandAlexander Li
 

Similar to EARLY MORTALITY8Early Mortality Review of the Charl.docx (20)

Substance Abuse Vs Suicidal risk report Final Draft 06_04_2015
Substance  Abuse Vs Suicidal risk  report Final Draft 06_04_2015Substance  Abuse Vs Suicidal risk  report Final Draft 06_04_2015
Substance Abuse Vs Suicidal risk report Final Draft 06_04_2015
 
A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...
A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...
A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE RESEARCH LITERATURE CONCERNING SOME BIOLOGICAL AND P...
 
Author & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docx
Author & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docxAuthor & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docx
Author & TitleAuthors Maggie Lawrence & Sue Kinn.Title Need.docx
 
Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...
Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...
Factors That Impacted Effective Diabetes Management Within...
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxClinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxClinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docxClinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009.docx
 
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009
Clinical Case Studies8(6) 417 –423© The Author(s) 2009
 
Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?
Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?
Retired: What Happens When They Aren’t at the Bridge Table?
 
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdfBrown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
 
Biomarker.pdf
Biomarker.pdfBiomarker.pdf
Biomarker.pdf
 
Otol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-Merritt
Otol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-MerrittOtol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-Merritt
Otol HNS Better to be Young-2000-Lacy-Merritt
 
Biomarker.docx
Biomarker.docxBiomarker.docx
Biomarker.docx
 
Nursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docx
Nursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docxNursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docx
Nursing as a profession firmly depends on the ethical principles.docx
 
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pPrepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
 
10.1177_2055102915622928
10.1177_205510291562292810.1177_2055102915622928
10.1177_2055102915622928
 
Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...
Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...
Explanation of how do individuals with multiple sclerosis cope with social is...
 
Mehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is Sex
Mehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is SexMehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is Sex
Mehta, Sunner, Crosby & Shrier 2011_What is Sex
 
Lesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docx
Lesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docxLesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docx
Lesson 3 Epi emiologyof Readings Predictors for the Daily Value.docx
 
Predictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailand
Predictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailandPredictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailand
Predictors of smoking cessation in malaysia and thailand
 

More from madlynplamondon

. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx
. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx
. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx
-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx
-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx
-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx
-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docxmadlynplamondon
 
. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx
. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx
. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docxmadlynplamondon
 
.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx
.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx
.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx
-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx
-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx
-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx
-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docxmadlynplamondon
 
. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx
. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx
. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docxmadlynplamondon
 
. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx
. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx
. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docxmadlynplamondon
 
. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx
. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx
. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx
-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx
-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx
-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx
-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx
-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx
-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx
-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx
-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docxmadlynplamondon
 
-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx
-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx
-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docxmadlynplamondon
 
- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx
- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx
- What are the key differences between national health service (.docxmadlynplamondon
 
--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx
--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx
--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docxmadlynplamondon
 
------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx
------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx
------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docxmadlynplamondon
 
) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx
) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx
) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docxmadlynplamondon
 
- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx
- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx
- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docxmadlynplamondon
 

More from madlynplamondon (20)

. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx
. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx
. According to your textbook, Contrary to a popular misconception.docx
 
-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx
-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx
-How did artwork produced in America from 1945 to 1960 compare to ar.docx
 
-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx
-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx
-Just thoughts and opinion on the reading-Consent and compen.docx
 
. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx
. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx
. The Questioned Documents Unit (QDU) provides forensic support .docx
 
.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx
.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx
.  What is it about the fundamental nature and structure of the Olym.docx
 
-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx
-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx
-Learning objectives for presentation-Brief background o.docx
 
-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx
-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx
-You will need to play a phone game Angry Birds (any version) to mak.docx
 
. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx
. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx
. EDU 571 Week 5 Discussion 1 -Data Collection Please respond .docx
 
. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx
. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx
. What were the causes of World War II Explain how and why the Unit.docx
 
. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx
. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx
. Complete the prewriting for the progress reportPrewriting p.docx
 
-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx
-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx
-in Filomena by Roberta Fernandez the author refers to the Mexican r.docx
 
-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx
-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx
-Write about a violent religious event in history.(Ex. Muslim ex.docx
 
-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx
-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx
-This project is an opportunity to demonstrate the ability to analyz.docx
 
-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx
-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx
-7 Three men are trapped in a cave with no hope of rescue and no foo.docx
 
-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx
-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx
-1. Are the three main elements of compensation systems—internal.docx
 
- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx
- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx
- What are the key differences between national health service (.docx
 
--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx
--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx
--Describe and analyze the ways in which Alfons Heck’s participation.docx
 
------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx
------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx
------ Watch an online speechpresentation of 20 minutes or lo.docx
 
) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx
) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx
) Florida National UniversityNursing DepartmentBSN.docx
 
- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx
- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx
- Please answer question 2 at the end of the case.- cita.docx
 

Recently uploaded

On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxJisc
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxEsquimalt MFRC
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Pooja Bhuva
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024Elizabeth Walsh
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Pooja Bhuva
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentationcamerronhm
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...Amil baba
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structuredhanjurrannsibayan2
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptxREMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptxDr. Ravikiran H M Gowda
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfSherif Taha
 
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptxPlant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptxUmeshTimilsina1
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxannathomasp01
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfDr Vijay Vishwakarma
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Jisc
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 

Recently uploaded (20)

On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptxREMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
REMIFENTANIL: An Ultra short acting opioid.pptx
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
 
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptxPlant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 

EARLY MORTALITY8Early Mortality Review of the Charl.docx

  • 1. EARLY MORTALITY 8 Early Mortality: Review of the Charleston Heart Study A Critiqué Submitted by [Your Name] El Centro College Psychology 23xx, Section 5xxxx, Fall 2013 Running head: EARLY MORTALITY 1 Abstract This essay provides a review of forty years of follow-up data from the Charleston Heart Study (CHS) in their article Divorce and Death: Forty Years of the Charleston Heart Study. This includes a longitudinal CHS, which included data collected from more than 1300 adults from 1960 to 2000, Sbarra and Nietert explored the relationship between social connectedness
  • 2. and health using the CHS data and attempted to provide insight into the long term health consequences of becoming separated or divorced (2009). Being separated or divorced during the follow-up window appeared to be one of the strongest predictors of early mortality. In addition to the review of the actual study, a critique is included that provides a critical analysis of the quality of the researchers’ study and article as published in the Psychological Science journal. The critique addresses such items as ethics, usefulness, sample size and diversification as well as a plethora of other interesting items useful to provide collegiate feedback of the work by Sbarra and Nietert. Early Mortality: Review of the Charleston Heart StudyIntroduction The researchers posed that recent research in social epidemiology has spurred advances into the association between interpersonal relationships and health (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009) but there were limits to the overall findings. It is suggested that relationship can play a vital role in an individual’s lifespan development. Sbarra and Nietert share this sentiment. More specifically, that social integration (i.e. a close relationship in which one feels close to others and that the relationship is reciprocally dependable) is positively correlated with mortality; however, a life deficient in social integration may be a strong predictor of early mortality (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). The researchers carefully reviewed the data from the CHS to help explain the mechanism linking social connectedness and health. The authors were interested in providing new insights into the long-term health consequences of divorce or separation. Many previous studies on the link between divorce and health have failed to present marital status as a dynamic variable. However, the researchers took into account both the length of time the participants were divorced and eventual remarriage rates which appears to have significantly improved their ability to make the link.Review In order to present marital status as dynamically as possible, the
  • 3. researchers classified participants in the study in three ways. First, marital status (married, separated-divorced, widowed, or never married) at the first assessment was examined as a predictor of long-term mortality (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). Second, the researchers calculated the hazard ratio (HR) of early death for adults who were separated or divorced at each assessment relative to all other participants who contributed at least one marital-status entry (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). Finally, to determine if the effect of remaining separated or divorced differed from the effect of having ever experienced a separation or divorce, the researchers reclassified the sample to calculate the HR of adults who simply experienced a marital separation or divorce at some point during the CHS follow-up period relative to all other participants (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). The researchers used data collected in the Charleston Heart Study to address the possible link between marital status and health. The CHS was a community-based cohort study designed to assess the normal course of health and aging for adults over age 35 residing in Charleston County, South Carolina and while the study began in 1960, marital-status data were collected during several follow-up phases: 1962–1964, 1974–1975, 1984– 1985, 1987–1989, and 1990–1991 (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). Mortality data were updated throughout the study, with the final revision spanning the entire 41-year period. The CHS began with an initial sample of 2181 adults, 1195 women and 986 men, 61% were of Caucasian decent, while the remainder of the sample was of African American decent (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). At the onset of the study, the average age of the participants were 48 years 9 months. The fact that the average age of the participants was not exceptionally young, combined with the extended length of the study, provided an opportunity for the researchers to capture data to the end of life for the majority of the participants (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). In the CHS, information on marital status was collected by self- report, and participants were classified as married, widowed, separated, divorced, or never married at each assessment. In the
  • 4. researchers’ analysis, the separated and divorced categories were merged to create a single category defined by the experience of marital separation (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). At the 1962–1964 assessment, the first-time marital status data was captured, 81.2% of the sample was married, 10% was widowed, 5.5% was separated or divorced, and 3.3% had never been married (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). In-person medical interviews at the beginning of the CHS provided a large amount of medical data, such as blood pressure, smoking status, and body mass index, that helped the researchers predict mortality. To predict mortality over the 41- year study period, the researchers used Cox proportional hazards models, a regression approach commonly referred to as survival analysis. The researchers were able to create predictors for each variable to isolate the effect of the variable of interest – time spent divorced or separated on age of death. Of the 1,376 adults in the restricted sample assessed at baseline, 74% had died by 2000, with the remaining 26% excluded from the research (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). The researchers found that separated and divorced participants had a 55% greater likelihood of death in the follow-up period than participants from the other three categories of relationship status (Sbarra & Nietert, 2009). The analysis of the CHS data by the researchers suggests that longevity of life is positively correlated to healthy and cherished relationships and that living a large portion of life as a separated or divorced adult may add considerable risk for all- cause of mortality (as cited in Sbarra & Nietert). It may be further suggested that the process of a marital break may not present the highest mortality risk; rather it is more likely the time spent without a significant other that is the most crucial factor. The results do not lend to reasons of cause but suggest results of significant correlation. This agrees with Sbarra and Nietert, who also suggest that additional research is necessary to fully understand the mechanisms behind the elevated risk of early mortality associated with time spent as a separated or
  • 5. divorced individual (2009). Critique The Charleston Heart Study, on which the article is based, appeared to be conducted in an ethical and scientific manner. In addition, the researchers appeared to be interested in conducting a study that included a diverse sample. After the initial 1960 phase of the study, 102 Black men of high socioeconomic status were added to increase the diversity component, which could be considered “forward-thinking” for 1962-era America. The fact that this was a longitudinal study (i.e. conducted over a span of 41 years) proved to be beneficial. Since the original study was conducted with cardiovascular health in mind, the fact that the authors had the insight of gathering other useful data for later use, is truly impressive. Still, there are some shortcomings with the data, as Sbarra and Nietert admit (2009). The incident of divorce was less customary at the onset of the CHS, though it would become significantly more common as the study progressed. Consequently, this presents one of the limits of a cohort study (i.e. how the customs of a single generation can affect the results of a study unlike another generation). The number of participants that remained separated or divorced throughout the follow-up period was relatively small, limiting the ability of the sample to be generalized across the entire population. It may have also been more efficacious to delineate between those who were separated and those who were divorced in the original study. Separating this group into two variables may have expanded the results and provided more insight. Despite these limits, the work of the authors has provided contemporary researchers with a good start in understanding the link between marriage status and longevity of life. A more modern approach to this type of study, with more nuanced relationship status categories and more frequent follow-up occasions, might benefit the subject matter. A new study, with a more contemporary cohort sample and the opportunity to collect data specifically useful to psychological research, may proffer
  • 6. more insight for the avid researcher. Additional follow up studies may also include individuals from select cultures or ethnicities as well as individuals from a variety of socioeconomic statuses. References Sbarra, D. A. & Nietert, P. J. (2009). Divorce and death: Forty years of the Charleston heart study. Psychological Science, 20(1), pp. 107-113. HUMN 100: INTERACTING WITH TEXT DUE DATES & DETAILS Length & Format: 800-1000 words Audience: An academic audience at Emily Carr Initial draft due: Week 9 Final draft (with evidence of revision) due: Week 11 ASSIGNMENT DESCRIPTION This assignment asks you to respond to a single written text based on your own contexts and experiences. You will be writing this time, however, for an academic audience—best imagined, perhaps, as your professors and other university- educated people—who have certain expectations about what makes a “critical” response to a written text. There are many ways to react to a text (many of which we will discuss in class), but academic writing values certain kinds of response that are both respectful of a text’s author and reflectively
  • 7. address the points the author is making. As a result, this assignment asks you to write a balanced essay which fairly summarizes and discusses a published text, while offering your own response to the ideas presented in the text. Rather than a pure summary of the text or a pure reaction based only on your opinions and experiences, aim for more of a “conversation” between you and the author—one that takes your own ideas and the writer’s seriously. You also need to choose a focus for your own response that may not (and probably should not) “cover” every point the author makes. While you are asked to summarize the text’s main theme or argument, also let your own interests, ideas, experiences, and reactions be the guides for what positions or ideas in the text you choose to respond to. Imagine this essay, then, as a response to a text for an academic audience who is interested in what you think about the author’s ideas. The center of the essay is you and your ideas, but this time we will also work on integrating your experience with that of the different contexts/opinions/experiences offered in the published text in ways that will appeal to an academic audience. WRITING GOALS • To examine your own position in relation to a published text and to write both with and against this text.
  • 8. • To explore (through reading) and practice (through writing) the various options that writers have for positioning themselves in relation to the writing of another. • To ethically represent through paraphrase, summary and quotation the ideas of another writer/text. To understand and use MLA citation. • To engage in critical analysis of text. To present your own views with this analysis. • To continue to practice the writing process (generative writing, drafting, revision, editing.) Surname 1 Name Professor Course Date American slavery China permitted slavery since it recognized it as an institution and thus needed to be protected especially in America. The large settlement in San Francisco encourages human trafficking. Slavery has existed unrestricted for years among the Chinese in California. Chinese women were sold as animals. Modern slavery in China is linked to poverty. Lack of education, economic freedom, poor societal structure and the rule of law allow slavery. Forced labor is common in China. This is what is experienced today as a form of slavery. “American Slavery As It Is, Testimony of a Thousand Witnesses”, was written in1839 by the Anti-Slavery Activist Theodore Dwight Weld. This book gathers information from
  • 9. witnesses, active and former slave owners from various plantations in Southern states. Information on how slaves were treated aided in building case for the Abolitionist Movement. The narrative described the conditions experienced by slaves in the United States. Weld argued that prisoners in United States and other countries were fed better than American slaves. The narrative focused on afflictions faced by slaves which covered housing, clothing, diet and working conditions (McBride 38). Medicine started in the era of slavery when people started struggling with disease. It changed slowly into a social institution becoming competitor of traditional health practices. In the early colonies of North America, every individual termed himself as a doctor. In the mid nineteenth century, doctors made professional medicine to differentiate themselves from the past. Therefore, the history of medicine is traced from the struggle for health during slavery. The plantation owners and doctors coerced much labor from slaves. This forced them to provide sufficient resources to make these slaves stay productive and give birth many children. The preference of illness and mortality in slave people indicated their environmental experiences and living conditions. Slaves were able to maintain their health and stabilize their families despite harsh enslavement. Slaves medicine sets the background of public health and clap social circumstances in the era of Jim crow. Weld wrote this narrative as a plain case where the reader acted as a juror for the purpose of coming up with an honest verdict. The aim was to show actual situation of the slaves in the United States. They were made slaves and owned by force by being put in fear. The slaveholders painted that their slaves were treated humanely, well fed, well housed, well clothed, well lodged and worked moderately. They also argued that they provided slaves with all things they needed for their comfort. Their assertion was disproved by the testimony of many unbiased witnesses. This put slaveholders through a course of questioning which led to condemnation out of their own mouths (McBride 40). According to testimonies of witnesses, the slaves were treated
  • 10. inhumane since they were underfed, overworked, wretchedly lodged and clad, they lacked enough sleep, they dragged heavy chains while working on the field, they also wore iron collars round their necks in order to be easily detected when they escaped. They also wore yokes, bells and iron horns. Some had their front teeth broken off or torn out for easy identification when they ran away. They faced torture and brutality. They were stripped naked, their limbs and backs cut with knives, red pepper was rubbed into their torn flesh and spirits of turpentine poured on their wounds to increase the severity of torture. They were searched with blood hounds and shot down like beasts. They were also beaten and whipped till they fainted and sometimes till they die. Their ears were cut off, their bones broken and their flesh marked with red hot irons. They were also burned to death over slow fires. Weld established all these facts from testimony form slaveholders, eye witnesses and slaves themselves (Weld 13). This book was influential American abolitionist publication. It laid bare the cruelty and violence associated with slavery. Most of evidence was pulled from Southern newspapers and testimony of former slaves and slave owners. During this period, opposition of slavery had become more outspoken. The slaves had been concentrated in more productive agricultural plantations especially the cotton lands of the South. Slave trade which was inhumane business also existed and it was criticized by domestic and foreign travellers, emancipationists, abolitionists and modern authors. The slave dealer advertisements in newspapers documented the evils of the institution of slavery. The narrative encouraged active antislavery movements which advocated various plans of emancipation. The intensive opposition of slavery occurred during decades of 1830-60. Attempts to reduce slave trade led to enactment of anti-importation law which constrained the bringing in of slaves from other states. Much of the content from Theodore Weld publication was from Kentucky newspapers, slave auction handbills, personal
  • 11. interviews and personal observations. The enactment of fugitive slave law by US Congress in 1950 was based on Kentucky. Mush of setting and many of the characters were drawn from Kentucky. Increased opposition of slavery was associated with prosperity in Kentucky. Weld’s publication turned into extensive antislavery movement in American history. It is remembered in the abolitionist movement and future antislavery literary works. This means that Weld used persuasion means to argue to the northern audiences concerning evils of slavery and the need to abolish it. The truth is that Weld used judicial motif where he acted as a prosecutor and requested his readers to perform as jurors in judging the legality of slavery in the United States. Weld did this by depending on evidence from testimony of witnesses and newspapers advertisements to prove his arguments. Weld showed that slavery resulted from individuals who were encouraged by absolute arbitrary power. The South supported slavery based on the argument that institution of slavery was positive good and justified necessary evil. Weld rejected these justifications and established that slavery which aimed at establishing absolute power over others. According to Weld, human nature is contrary to slavery and thus it should be ended. Weld also held that abolitionists were justified in condemning slavery. Weld accomplished his role of prosecutor in the case by combining the logic and emotion of his jurors. The jurors acted as members of humanity and perceived slaves in the same level as them and this forced them to pass a just judgment of guilty since slavery is in contradiction of the humanity (Trudeau 16). Conclusion Theodore Weld’s publication was the most influential American antislavery tract. It used judicial motive to show the evils associated with slavery. The evidence was drawn from testimony of witnesses, the slaveholders and slaves themselves. The collection of first hand testimonials and personal narratives from both whites and freedmen was used to portray the horrors of American slavery. The work covered the slave diet, clothing,
  • 12. housing, working conditions and torture and brutality. Work Cited McBride, David. "“Slavery As It Is:” Medicine and Slaves of the Plantation South." OAH Magazine of History 19.5 (2005): 36-40. Trudeau, Justin Thomas. "Theodore Dwight Weld's use of the judicial motif in American Slavery As It Is: Testimony of a Thousand Witnesses." (1998). Weld, Theodore Dwight, ed. American Slavery as it is: Testimony of a Thousand Witnesses. No. 10. American Anti- Slavery Society, 1839. ABBREVIATED TITLE IN CAPITAL LETTERS 8 Full Title in Upper and Lower Case Letters A Critiqué Submitted by: Name of Student El Centro College Psychology 2301, Section 52401, Spring 2020
  • 13. Running head: ABBREVIATED TITLE IN CAPITAL LETTERS1 Abstract The abstract is a summary of YOUR paper. In essence, the abstract section for this paper will include a summary of both the Review and Critique sections of YOUR paper. It is similar to reading the back cover of a book to get a snapshot of what the book is about. Read the author’s abstract as an example of how an abstract is written but DO NOT copy their abstract. If you write something in your abstract or any other portion of your paper that is taken from someone else (including the author of the article you are critiquing), you must cite them in or at the end of that sentence and later (on the References page) provide a full “address” of where the source can be found. Please be aware, for your purposes in this class, plagiarism will earn you an automatic zero (Ø) on the assignment. The abstract is generally written last or after you have completed the assignment, as it is a summary of what you have written. For the Abstract, you will be required to write 150-250 words. This is the only page that is NOT first-line indented and is should be flush with the left margin of your page (i.e. left justified). Note: When you submit your paper for grading, it will be automatically sent to SafeAssign which analyses your paper for similarities found in other papers that have been submitted,
  • 14. research articles and websites. It is highly functional and will most often catch plagiarism, so please do not plagiarize. Also, it is advisable for you to use this template as much of the formatting has already been done. Full Title in Upper and Lower Case LettersIntroduction Beginning on page three, you will write the headings Introduction, Review, and Critique and you will begin using indentions for each paragraph. In APA writing, there are specific ways to write headings depending on how many levels are used. This is the one time you ARE allowed to use font effects (i.e., bolds or italics). The Introduction is ONE paragraph of 150-250 words that will first provide a PROBLEM STATEMENT in a single sentence and then summarizes the article you chose from the list. As soon as you state something that was taken from the article, you must IMMEDIATELY cite your source(s) within the text or at the end of that sentence (whether quoting or paraphrasing). Here are a couple examples of how to use “in text citations:” · Smith suggests that the average dog lives eight years (2011). · It is believed that the average dog lives eight years (Smith, 2011). The author’s name was not listed in the sentence and needs to be added to the end. · “After careful review of over 10,000 records, it was determined that the average dog’s lifespan covers eight years” (Smith, 2011, p154). Quotes should include quotation marks and the citation identifies the specific page(s) on which the quote was found. In any case, the period for the sentence is after the citation. … lives eight years (2011). Not …lives eight years. (2011) If it is found that you have plagiarized without having given due credit, you will receive a zero (Ø) on the assignment, and this activity may be reported to the Dean of the College of Arts and Sciences division. Plagiarism is a serious issue and will NOT be
  • 15. tolerated. Review When you write content on the next heading, do not add extra lines before the next heading. Notice there is not an additional line between the last sentence of the Introduction section and the beginning of the Review section of this template. After you have read the material for your assignment and thought about it, give an overview of its contents. This differs from the Introduction section in that it is more comprehensive and consequently a page or more in length and that it must identify assumptions made by the researcher of the article. Briefly explain WHAT types of research or experiments were conducted, WHO were these conducted on, WHERE were they conducted, WHEN were they conducted, and HOW were they conducted. For example, you might list the organization by name (Western University) or location (“an inner city high school in Chicago, Illinois”). Include ages and gender of the subjects, and whether or not parental consent was required in order to assess this sample. Include details of how much research was investigated and the conclusions of the research. Do NOT use I, my, he, she, them, we, us, or other pronouns. In fact, the assignment should be written completely in the third person which helps remove subjectiveopinion (i.e., your personal thoughts or opinions) and favors an OBJECTIVE approach (spoken from the perspective of an uninvolved observer) by such statements as, “The researchers determined that a multiple regression analysis of the data was sufficient to assess the validity of the research methodology.” Third person makes the writer a reporter of the facts. Personal opinions are simply not relevant or appropriate for this paper. Further, when writing about an experiment, you must also indicate what the hypothesis’ were (there is rarely only one hypothesis, so include them all), and what the researchers expected to find. State the findings (conclusions) and whether or not they were consistent with what the researchers had anticipated. “Negative
  • 16. results” are okay…we do not always find what we hope to, but we DO report everything. It is not necessary for you to give a synopsis of the statistical and data analysis methods used by the researchers. Critique In this section you will write a page or more about the quality of the author’s article and the quality of their research (if they conducted research). Do not write a review of the subject itself. This is your opportunity to express your objective findings on the article. Think of it as if you were a food critic and evaluating a restaurant. You would not report to your readers that the food was “very hot” because that would be subjective. You would possibly say the plate was seasoned with, reporting a list of spices, and most likely more spicy than the average person would prefer. It may also be reported that one might expect runny nose, watery eyes and similar types of reactions that are common when eating “spicy foods.” This provides a more objective view of the spicy level of the food. Again, the assignment requires writing in third person, NOT first person (i.e., I think they failed to comply with the Scientific Method, etc.) nor use phrases like “in MY opinion.” It would be better to say, the authors did not comply with the Scientific Method as they did not …. This is your rare opportunity to actually criticize (providing objective findings, positive and/or negative) what a researcher has done, so make the most of it. If you disagree with how the research was conducted, say so, but TELL WHY this is so, BASED ON THE SCIENTIFIC METHOD as discussed in lecture and your textbook. Such comments on your part show that you have applied critical thinking to your review of the article. Again, this is not a forum for your opinions or for feedback of the subject itself but for your collegiate critical thinking on the quality of their research and/or article. Then end your Critique section with a conclusion statement. Here are more items to consider for your Critique section. · Did the authors: · follow all appropriate scientific protocols such as:
  • 17. · identify the target population; · obtain a random sample or more specifically a representative sample; · provide clear definitions (operationalization) of their dependent and independent variables; · indicate their specific methodologies; · give adequate consideration to various aspects of their topic by having read the research of a number of researchers in this area. · Did the findings leave the typical reader more or less interested in this topic? · Would it be helpful for the reader to see more articles in this area of research or meta-analysis …perhaps with additional questions (i.e., hypotheses) in the future? · Did the researchers violate the principles of scientific methodology (e.g., the Scientific Method) in some way? · Did the researchers obtain Informed Consent from all participants? Was parental consent (or that of a legal guardian) required if the subjects were minors? · Did the researcher miss something that was obvious? · Would adding additional groups of people to the sample alter the results? · Were the researcher’s findings significant, and if so, why? · Are there additional questions one might have after reading this article? · Should additional research (or follow up studies) be conducted– perhaps with additional questions or specific parameters or variables? If your article says the average dog can do three tricks, does that vary in other parts of the world or is there a significant difference in the abilities of different breeds of dogs to do tricks? · What are potential follow-up studies that could be conducted to expand knowledge in this area? Your responses to the questions and issues in this document will tell whether or not you have actually THOUGHT ABOUT the article at issue.
  • 18. References 1st Author’s Last Name, First Initial. Second Initial. & 2nd Author’s Last Name, First Initial. Second Initial (year published). Write the full title with this type of capitalization. Write theFull Journal Name in Italics in Regular Capitalization 12(1). pp. 120-151. Note: Use a hanging indention as seen above (i.e. do not indent the first line of the reference but indent each line after for that reference). For the names, do not change the order of the name but also do not list first names, only initials. For the year, do not add season or month. The 12 in italics is the volume number and the 1 in parenthesis is the edition/issue number which is not in italics. Lastly end with the page range of the article followed by a period. Here is an example of an APA formatted article: Copeland, R. D. (2017). A comprehensive study of different dog breeds. Journal of Canine Friends, 34(2), pp. 123-145. Evaluating a Student-Led Mental Health Awareness Campaign Dominique Giroux and Elisa Geiss* Olivet College ABSTRACT. The present study evaluated if a week-long mental health awareness campaign on a college campus would decrease self-stigma toward seeking help. Participants were 204 full-time undergraduate students attending a small private liberal arts college in the Midwest (October, 2017). The mental health
  • 19. awareness campaign offered activities where students were exposed to interactive events and education about campus crisis resources. Researchers measured self-stigma and attitudes toward seeking help through the Self-Stigma of Seeking Help Scale (SSOSH) and Mental Help Seeking Attitude Scale (MHSAS) pre- and postawareness week. Results showed that student self-stigma toward seeking help decreased after a week-long mental health awareness campaign. Specifically, we found a decrease in SSOSH scores, t(52) = 2.66, p = .01, d = 0.25, and an increase in MHSAS scores from pretest to posttest, t(56) = -2.72, p = .009, d = -0.29, indicating a reduction of self-stigma. We discuss results in the context of reducing stigma from a student-led mental health campaign and further provide suggestions on how to conduct an awareness campaign and test results at small colleges. Keywords: mental health awareness campaign, college students, stigma Mental illness among college students is becoming a serious public health problem, with the onset of illness often developing during early college years (Garlow et al., 2008). One third of college students self-report symptoms of anxiety, depression, and high rates of stress (Lipson, Gaddis, Heinze, Beck, & Eisenberg, 2015). Further, distressed students have worse academic performance and exhibit high drop-out rates (Kitzrow, 2003). Seeking and receiving treatment is one way to decrease mental health disorders. Those who do not seek treatment are more likely to develop a longer, more intensified comorbidity with other mental illnesses (Wang et al., 2005). When this population seeks treatment,
  • 20. functioning and academic performance improves (Zivin, Eisenberg, Gollust, & Golberstein, 2009). However, not all students will seek treatment for a mental health disorder. One of the most common barriers to seeking help for a psychological disorder among young adult populations is stigma. College student populations deem stigma as a barrier to involvement in the community, social relationships, and seeking proper treatment (Eisenberg, Downs, Golberstein, & Zivin, 2009). Both public and self-stigma may affect whether a student decides to seek treatment for a mental disorder. Public stigma, the stereotyping and discriminative actions toward those who are known to have a mental illness (Corrigan, 2004), may be an external barrier to treatment. For example, individuals who seek treatment for a mental disorder are deemed more “emotionally unstable, Page 61 less interesting, and less confident” than persons seeking treatment for a physical ailment (Vogel, Wade, & Ascheman, 2009, p. 301). However, selfstigma or the internalized public stigma, can lead individuals to not try to seek care due to shame of being labeled as having a mental illness (Corrigan, Druss, & Perlick, 2014; Vogel, Wade, & Haake, 2006). Both public and self-stigma introduce barriers toward seeking help, and reducing both types of stigma may improve mental health on a college campus. Colleges have tried multiple interventions to reduce incidences of mental health crises on campus by
  • 21. targeting psychoeducation and improving awareness. Specifically, psychoeducational interventions can reduce stigma, increase the mental health literacy of a population (Jorm et al., 2003), and dispel myths about mental illnesses (Yanos, Lucksted, Drapalski, Roe, & Lysaker, 2015). These types of interventions have been effective in decreasing the stigma of seeking psychological help (Brown & Bradley, 2002; Komiya, Good, & Sherrod, 2000), and furthermore have the potential to reduce barriers to help-seeking. In fact, student-led campaigns such as the Depression OutReach Alliance college program uses peer-topeer psychoeducation and intervention to educate and increase help-seeking among undergraduate college students. In particular, Funkhouser, Zakriski, and Spoltore (2017) measured responses from participants regarding at-risk peers and stigma and found increased crisis response skills, less desire for social distance from peers in distress, and less social stigma toward seeking help. Thus, a student-led mental health awareness campaign has shown potential to both target stigma and increase help-seeking among college students. Given the potential benefit of student-led mental health campaigns to reduce stigma, we aimed to test whether a campaign at a small liberal arts college would be effective in decreasing stigma toward seeking help. In a previous study conducted on our campus, we found that 47% of student respondents did not know where to go in crisis, only 22% knew about student services or a counselor on campus, and 88% of students selfreported that more awareness on campus is needed
  • 22. (Giroux & Geiss, 2017). Given this data, providing psychoeducation about mental health concerns and improving knowledge about treatment resources on campus were appropriate targets of intervention. Thus, we created a mental health awareness campaign focused on giving general information about mental health disorders and access to campus resources. The current study was unique in its approach because it examined a student-led and-run intervention at a small liberal arts college without many mental health awareness resources available to students. First, we hypothesized that students’ self­reported stigma about seeking mental health help would lessen after the mental health awareness week given that previous studies have found improvement of stigma after psychoeducation. Furthermore, we hypothesized that those who actively participated and attended mental health week events would experience a larger decrease in self-stigma toward seeking help compared to those who did not actively participate or attend. This study addressed the feasibility of conducting an intervention while testing students empirically, and aimed to improve mental health awareness on a small campus. We created a mental health awareness week and tested stigma before and after the week, and also gathered demographic data about the participants. Method Procedure: After we gained approval from the Olivet College institutional review board, we recruited students at a small, Midwestern, liberal arts college to
  • 23. participate in two surveys via e-mail. Those who completed the online consent form completed de-identified questionnaires on Google Forms a week prior to the mental health awareness week and immediately afterward. Because this was a universal intervention, students who took the surveys could choose to participate or not in the mental health awareness intervention. Participants were entered into a raffle for a FitBit Charge 2 or a $50 voucher to the college bookstore if they completed both pre- and postintervention surveys. Questionnaires Demographics Questionnaire. Participants were asked to create a unique identifier to track their participation from the pretest to the posttest, and to ensure confidentiality. Participants were then asked to complete questions about gender, ethnicity, year in school, area of study, and participation in college athletics. In addition, during the posttest, participants reported whether they participated in the mental health awareness week, which included picking up brochures or attending mental health week events. Page 62 Mental Help Seeking Attitudes Scale (MHSAS). The MHSAS is a 9-item instrument that examines attitudes toward seeking help from a mental health professional if respondents hypothetically had a mental health concern (Hammer, Parent, & Spiker, 2018).
  • 24. Participants were asked to respond to a single statement on a 7-point Likert-type scale. For example, they were asked to rate how important or how healing it is to seek help from a mental health professional. A higher score indicates a more positive attitude toward seeking help. These scores showed strong internal reliability for both the pretest and posttest; Cronbach’s α = .89 and α = .91, respectively. Self-Stigma of Seeking Help (SSOSH). The SSOSH is a 10-item scale designed to understand how a participant views seeking mental health help, and ideas about public stigma toward seeking psychological help (Vogel et al., 2006). This scale asked participants to rate the degree to which each item describes how one would react in a situation. Examples of statements are “I would feel inadequate if I went to a therapist,” “My view of myself would not change just because I made the choice to see a therapist,” and “I would feel worse about myself if I could not solve my own problems.” A higher score represents higher levels of self-stigma. These scores showed strong reliability for both the pretest and posttest; Cronbach’s α = .87 and α = .84, respectively. Mental Health Week Intervention The inaugural mental health awareness week took place in the fall 2017. The college’s Psi Chi chapter as well as a 3­credit Abnormal Psychology class contributed to the information and events held throughout the week. Tangible items such as stress balls, mental health awareness ribbons, informational pamphlets describing common disorders among students, and interactive displays were freely made available to the public in
  • 25. the main academic building. The college’s Psi Chi chapter and the Black Student Union hosted the first Mental Health Open Mic Night in the middle of the week for students to gather and talk about multiple issues faced on campus or at home in regard to stressors or mental health disorders. The Open Mic Night was a free event for students, and Psi Chi provided informational slideshows and hosted the college’s school counselor for familiarity. A faculty member from the psychology department also attended the event, providing handouts regarding particular disorders that are commonly faced by students, such as alcoholism. Once students finished talking to the audience about personal experiences, the school counselor held a short debrief and educated the attendees on what to do when distressed. The school counselor also handed out her business card to attendees for further contact if needed. Once the event ended, the mental health awareness week intervention was considered complete. Results Participants After conducting the college’s inaugural mental health awareness week, we received 166 responses in total. Both women (n = 111), men (n = 54), and self-reported other gender (n = 1) responded to our questionnaires. We received responses from students across all class ranks: first-year students (n =
  • 26. 52), sophomores (n = 39), juniors (n = 31), seniors (n = 39), and nondegree seeking (n = 4). Although one student did not self-identify an ethnicity, the students responding to our surveys self-identified as White or European American (n = 145), African American or Black (n = 11), Hispanic or Latino (n = 8), or Asian (n = 1). Analyses in this article only included the 57 participants who completed both the pretest and posttest surveys. Demographic makeup of this final participant list was mostly female (n = 40) compared to male (n = 16) or other gender (n = 1), mostly White or European American (n = 50) compared to African American or Black (n = 4), and Hispanic or Latino (n = 3), and mostly first-year students (n = 20) compared to sophomores (n = 11), juniors (n = 12), seniors (n = 13) or nondegree seeking (n = 1). Prior to excluding those who did not participate in both surveys, we conducted analyses to ensure that our final participants were not significantly different from those who only completed one survey. Participant Analysis We ran a Chi Square to determine whether there was a difference in gender and student class rank across those participants who took only one (pretest or posttest) or two surveys (both pretest and posttest). There was no difference in response rate of genders, χ2 = 0.95, p = .62, d = 0.15, or year in school across pretest, posttest, or both tests, χ2 = 2.7, p = .95, d = 0.26. Given
  • 27. the low response rate for some ethnicities who completed only the posttest (no African American students and only one Hispanic/ Latino student), we could not run a Chi Square test Page 63 on this data. However, the relative percentage of students identifying as European American, African American, or Hispanic/Latino was similar across the entire sample and final sample. In addition, we examined if there was a difference in scores on the SSOSH and MHSAS based on participation in just one survey or across both. Results showed that there was no significant difference between participants who completed the pretest survey and those who completed both. However, we found a significant difference between participants who completed the posttest survey only and those who completed both on the SSOSH, t(90) = 2.6, p = .01, d = 0.54. Specifically, participants who completed only the posttest survey self-reported higher levels of stigma (M = 26.8, SD = 6.4) compared to those who completed both (M = 23.5, SD = 5.7). Table 1 Correlation Table for Pretest and Posttest Scores on the MHSAS and SSOSH Correlations N M (SD) MHSAS
  • 28. pretest SSOSH pretest MHSAS posttest SSOSH posttest MHSAS pretest 57 5.7 (1.06) 1 SSOSH pretest 55 25 (6.6) -.41* 1 MHSAS posttest 57 6.0 (0.98) .62** -.24 1 SSOSH posttest 55 23.5 (5.73) 1.19 .77** -3.7 1 **. Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed). Note. MHSAS pretest = Mental Health Seeking Attitude Scale pretest; SSOSH pretest = Self-Stigma of
  • 29. Seeking Help pretest; MHSAS posttest = Mental Health Seeking Attitude Scale posttest; SSOSH posttest = Self-Stigma of Seeking Help posttest; N = number of respondents. Main Analyses Change in stigma after mental health awareness week. Before examining the change in pretest to posttest scores, we first ran correlations between the variables of interest in participants who took both the pretest- and posttest (see Table 1). As expected, scores on both stigma surveys reported at the same time (either pretest or posttest) were negatively related to each other (i.e., higher scores on the SSOSH and lower scores on the MHSAS both reflect greater stigma). In addition, scores on the same surveys taken before and after the intervention were positively correlated with each other. Next, we used a paired-samples t test to examine change in MHSAS and SSOSH scores before and after the intervention. We found an overall decrease in SSOSH scores as seen in SSOSH pretest (M = 25.0, SD = 6.8) and SSOSH posttest (M = 23.4, SD = 5.8), t(52) = 2.66, p = .01, d = 0.25. We then found an increase in MHSAS scores from pretest (M = 5.7, SD = 1.1) to posttest (M = 6.0, SD = 1.0), t(56) = -2.72, p = .009, d = - 0.29. Thus, over the week, there was decreased mental health seeking stigma as
  • 30. indicated on both stigma surveys. Participation in awareness week on change in self-stigma scores. Because we collected data from students who might not have actively participated in the mental health awareness week events, we examined whether changes in self-stigma differed based on self-report of having participated in the mental health week activities. Participation in the awareness week was defined as picking up brochures or attending an event on a given night. Although the scores for SSOSH did not differ, we found that changes in MHSAS scores were dependent upon participation, F(1) = 5.28, p = .03, g = 0.68. Using a two-way repeated-measures Analysis of Variance, we found that those individuals who participated in the Mental Health Week activities had an increase in MHSAS scores (N = 16, Mpre = 5.23, SDpre = 1.01, Mpost = 5.97, SDpost = 1.05), t(15) = 2.45, p = .03, whereas those who did not participate did not have a change in scores (N = 41, Mpre = 5.88, SDpre = 1.04, Mpost = 6.03, SDpost = 0.97), t(40) = 1.47, p = .15. Yet, we also found that MHSAS scores were higher during the pretest in those individuals who did not participate (Mnon participating = 5.88, SD = 1.04) compared to those who did participate (Mparticipating = 5.22, SD = 1.01), t(55) = 2.14, p = .04, d = 0.64. This may indicate that those people who participated in the mental health activities held more stigmatized beliefs,
  • 31. and they improved upon their stigma over time (see Figure 1). Page 64 Discussion On our small sized college campus, students have expressed a need for more visible mental health awareness on campus. With only 22% of students on our campus knowing where to go if in distress, we sought to educate students about local resources offered on our campus (Giroux & Geiss, 2017). After a week-long mental health awareness campaign, this sample of college students reported a decrease in help-seeking stigma on both measures collected. Furthermore, stigmatized attitudes improved the most for those who actively participated in the weeklong events, especially because these students also started with higher stigma to begin with. Thus, this may be an intervention that holds promise to reduce stigma toward seeking help, especially in those who actively participate. While exploring the effect of participation in the events on self-stigma, we found that students who participated had increased positive attitudes toward seeking help, but these participants also self-reported high levels of stigma during the pretest. Similar to prior research, interventions such as
  • 32. psychoeducational programming may be most beneficial for those who actively participate in mental health awareness events (Lannin, Vogel, Brenner, Abraham, & Heath, 2015). Yet, it also seemed puzzling why students who participated had higher levels of stigma. One reason for this result could be that those who actively attended and participated in mental health awareness week events may self-report high self-stigma because of experienced negative psychological feelings such as symptoms of depression (Busby Grant, Bruce, & Batterham, 2016). However, another study suggested that self-stigma was significantly associated with decreased likeliness to engage in mental health related activities, seek psychological help, or seek counseling (Lannin et al., 2015). The results from the present study support the importance of evaluating a mental health awareness campaign to understand how stigma may decrease in specific populations (Kelly, Jorm, & Wright, 2007). In the process of designing and conducting a mental health awareness week, we were able to provide this type of service while also investigating how it changed stigma perceptions. In running this study, we received promising preliminary results that suggest a student-led and student-run mental health week can decrease stigma toward seeking help. Specifically, there needs to be considerations of methods of administration of surveys, who to sample, and when during the semester to sample students. First, when thinking about administration of surveys to undergraduate college students, it is
  • 33. important to understand what method of administration is effective for the demographic. Our results showed significant attrition rates from pretest to posttest using online surveys via college e-mail addresses. Thus, future research may include testing different types of survey administration (e.g., paper and pencil, online) among college students that may improve attrition rates and provide a more representative sample of the student population. Second, we found that time of academic school year impacted student engagement. Attrition rates may be linked to the fact that students were asked to participate in the mental health awareness activities during midterm exams, which may increase levels of stress. Therefore, teasing out the effect of the intervention and timing of the school year is an important consideration for those running studies on a college campus. Last, it is important for pre- and postsurveys to be filled out by students who actively participate in mental health awareness events. One suggestion to capture this would be to provide an incentive for active participants in the weeklong events to participate in the pretest and posttest surveys. By doing this, data may capture the empirical evidence that incorporates all student perceptions. This study may be helpful to student-led mental health
  • 34. awareness campaigns. An undergraduate student leading a mental health awareness campaign on campus may consider survey administration, time of academic year, and more consistent incentives to better understand the implications of awareness for mental health help-seeking on a small sized campus. References • Brown, K., & Bradley, L. J. (2002). Reducing the stigma of mental illness. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 81–87. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-10626-006 • Busby Grant, J., Bruce, C. P., & Batterham, P. J. (2016). Predictors of personal, perceived, and self-stigma towards anxiety and depression. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25, 247– 254. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796015000220 • Corrigan, P. W. (2004). How stigma interferes with mental health care. American Psychologist, 59, 614–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614 • Corrigan, P. W., Druss, B. G., & Perlick, D. A. (2014). The impact of mental illness stigma on seeking and participating in mental health care. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 15, 37–70. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100614531398
  • 35. • Eisenberg, D., Downs, M. F., Golberstein, E., & Zivin, K. (2009). Stigma and help seeking for mental health among college students. Medical Care Research and Review, 66, 522–541. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077558709335173 • Funkhouser, C. J., Zakriski, A. L., & Spoltore, J. D. (2017). Evaluating peer-peer depression outreach: College students helping peers approach and respond to students in crisis. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 22, 19–28. https://doi.org/10.24839/2325-7342.JN22.1.19 Page 65 • Garlow, S. J., Rosenberg, J., Moore, J. D., Haas, A. P., Koestner, B., Hendin, H., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2008). Depression, desperation, and suicidal ideation in college students: Results from the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention College Screening Project at Emory University. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 482–488. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20321 • Giroux, D., & Geiss, E. (2017, April). Students on campus: Relationships, mental health, and help seeking. Paper presented at Michigan Undergraduate Psychology Research Conference, Hillsdale, MI. • Hammer, J. H., Parent, M. C., & Spiker, D. A. (2018). Mental Help Seeking Attitudes Scale (MHSAS): Development, reliability, validity, and comparison with the ATSPPH-SF and IASMHS-PO. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 65, 74–85. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000248
  • 36. • Jorm, A. F., Griffiths, K. M., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Parslow, R. A., & Rodgers, B. (2003). Providing information about the effectiveness of treatment options to depressed people in the community: A randomized controlled trial of effects on mental health literacy, help-seeking and symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1071–1079. • Kelly, C. M., Jorm, A. F., & Wright, A. (2007). Improving mental health literacy as a strategy to facilitate early intervention for mental disorders. Medical Journal of Australia, 187(7), S26–S30. Retrieved from https://www.mja.com. au/system/files/issues/187_07_011007/kel10278_fm.pdf • Kitzrow, M. A. (2003). The mental health needs of today's college students: Challenges and recommendations. NASPA Journal, 41, 167–181. https://doi.org/10.2202/0027-6014.1310 • Komiya, N., Good, G. E., & Sherrod, N. B. (2000). Emotional openness as a predictor of college students' attitudes toward seeking psychological help. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 138–143. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-0167.47.1.138 • Lannin, D. G., Vogel, D. L., Brenner, R. E., Abraham, W. T., & Heath, P. J. (2015). Does self- stigma reduce the probability of seeking mental health information? Journal of Counseling Psychology, 63, 351–358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000108 • Lipson, S. K., Gaddis, S. M., Heinze, J., Beck, K., & Eisenberg, D. (2015). Variations in student
  • 37. http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-10626-006 https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796015000220 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614 https://doi.org/10.1177/1077558709335173 https://doi.org/10.24839/2325-7342.JN22.1.19 https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20321 https://doi.org/10.2202/0027-6014.1310 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.47.1.138 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000108 mental health and treatment utilization across US colleges and universities. Journal of American College Health, 63, 388–396. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2015.1040411 • Vogel, D. L., Wade, N. G., & Ascheman, P. L. (2009). Measuring perceptions of stigmatization by others for seeking psychological help: Reliability and validity of a new stigma scale with college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 301– 308. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014903 • Vogel, D. L., Wade, N. G., & Haake, S. (2006). Measuring the self-stigma associated with seeking psychological help. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 325–337. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.325 • Wang, P. S., Berglund, P., Olfson, M., Pincus, H. A., Wells, K. B., & Kessler, R. C. (2005). Failure and delay in initial treatment contact after first onset of mental disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General
  • 38. Psychiatry, 62, 603–613. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.603 • Yanos, P. T., Lucksted, A., Drapalski, A. L., Roe, D., & Lysaker, P. (2015). Interventions targeting mental health self-stigma: A review and comparison. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 38, 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000100 • Zivin, K., Eisenberg, D., Gollust, S. E., & Golberstein, E. (2009). Persistence of mental health problems and needs in a college student population. Journal of Affective Disorders, 117, 180– 185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.001 Author Note. Dominique Giroux, https://orcid.org/00000002-2487-7777, Olivet College; Elisa Geiss, Department of Psychology, Olivet College. This study was supported by the Olivet College Student Government Association. Special thanks to Psi Chi Journal reviewers for their support. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dominique Giroux, E: [email protected] Page 66 Copyright of Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research is the property of Psi Chi Journal of Undergraduate Research and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
  • 39. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.325 https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.603 https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000100 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.001 mailto:[email protected]Evaluating a Student-Led Mental Health Awareness CampaignMethodResultsTable 1 Correlation Table for Pretest and Posttest Scores on the MHSAS and SSOSHDiscussionReferences Enhancing School Success with Mnemonic Strategies MARGO A. MASTROPIERI AND THOMAS E. SCRUCGGS Many students with disabilities and those at risk for educational failure exhibit problems with remembering materials covered in school. Suggestions for helping these students improve memory for school content are described in this article. In particular, mnemonic instruction is described and examples are provided of how it can be used to increase school learning and memory of students with learning difficulties. A few years ago, we revisited an inner-city middle school where-about a year before we had conducted an investigation on the effectiveness of mnemonic techniques in helping students with mild cognitive disabilities remember U.S. states and capitals. As we entered the classroom, Crystal, a student classified as mildly mentally handicapped,
  • 40. recognized one of our graduate students immediately. "Hay! I remember you!" she exclaimed enthusiastically. "You were here last year-you taught us states and capitals! I remember, go ahead, ask me one!" "Well, uh," replied our colleague, taken slightly by surprise, "How about ... Florida? What's the capital of Florida?" "That's too easy!" she said, smiling. "Here it is: Florida, the keyword is flower-the flower is on a television set, and television is the keyword for Tallahassee!" In this scenario, a student classified as mentally retarded effectively remembered information she had been taught 1 year previously. Even more impressive was the fact that she had not reviewed or rehearsed this information with any teacher since the last time we had seen her! As startling as this scenario is, it underscores something we have been witnessing for many years: the incredible power of mnemonic strategies to increase dramatically the amount of information students remember, even students with learning problems. In this article, we describe the need for effective memory strategies for school learning. Next, we provide a brief description of what mnemonic strategies are and what they are not. Following that, we describe how you can use
  • 41. these powerful learning tools to enhance the school success of your own students. THE NEED FOR MNEMONIC STRATEGIES According to the Sixteenth Annual Report to Congress on the Implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Act (U.S. Department of Education, I 994), 71.2 % of all students now spend all or a substantial part of their school day in general education classrooms. As many as 78.9% of students with learning disabilities spend all or most of their educational time in general education classrooms. On the secondary level, students with disabilities are included in content area classrooms, such as English, science, and social studies classes. Largely, ability to succeed in these classrooms determines their level of success in school. What factors determine whether a student will succeed in secondary content-area classrooms? Putnam (1992b) surveyed 7th- and 10th-grade teachers in Kansas, INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC VOL. 33, No. 4, MARCH 1996 (PP. 201-208) 201 Indiana, and Florida, and reported that an average of nearly half of a student's report card grades depended on test performance. Clearly, such factors as attendance, punctuality, participation, and homework completion are also important.
  • 42. However, teachers made it clear that test scores were the single most important factor in report card grades. Teachers gave an average of 11 of these tests over the course of a single 9-week grading period. Clearly, students' academic survival is tied very closely with performance on academic tests. In addition to the number of tests teachers give, Putnam (1992a) also examined the types of questions teachers asked on tests. He found that the overwhelming majority of test questions students were asked required facn1al recall: The majority of questions on tests administered by main- stream secondary classroom teachers required the student to recall a specific foct-2 S.3 per test. A sample question that asked for a specific fact was "Who discovered America?" Other possible responses, such as conclusion, sequence, opinion, discrimination, compare and contrast, purpose, correct an example, and summary, appeared about one per test. (p. 131) Thus it can be seen that memory for factual information is absolutely essential for success in school, particularly at the secondary level. Unfortunately, it is also true that students with learning disabilities and other learning problems have been consistently shown to have particular difficulties remembering academic content (e.g., Cooney & Swanson, 1987). Our work in the area of mnemonic (memory-enhancing) strategies has been devoted to finding ways of increasing the amount of content-area information students are able to remember. This article
  • 43. provides information on the utility, and effectiveness, of mnemonic strategies in enhancing memory for school learning. WHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES ARE A1nemonic strategies are systematic procedures for enhancing memory. Their particular use is in developing better ways to take in (encode) information so that it will be much easier to remember (retrieve). Although there are retrieval strategies that can be employed to attempt to retrieve information that has been forgotten, research has demonstrated that the way we encode information when we first study facilitates memory better. The particular task in developing mnemonic strategies is to find a way to relate new information to information students already have locked in long-term memory. If we can make a firm enough connection, the memory will last a very long time. For example, Crystal had learned the capital of Florida so well because the mnemonic strategy had carefully linked it to things she was very familiar with. Because Florida sounds like flower (the keyword), it was easy to teach her to make an automatic connection between Florida and flower: What's the keyword for Florida? Flower, good! And, what state is flower the keyword for? Good, Florida! It was also easy to teach her to establish a firm association
  • 44. between Tallahassee and television because television was very familiar to Crystal and the two words, again, sound very similar: What’s the keyword for Tallahassee? Television, good! And, what capital is television the keyword for? Good, Tallahassee! Having linked the two words (Florida, Tallahassee) to concrete, familiar words that sound similar (flower, television), all that remains is to link the two familiar words together. And although memory experts Harry Lorayne and Jerry Lucas have suggested that these words must be associated "in some ridiculous way" (1974, p. 2 3), in fact all that is necessary is that the two keywords be pictured in some meaningful interaction. In the Florida instance, a picture had been created and displayed on an overhead projector of a flower sitting on a television set, as shown in Figure I. Although recalling that Tallahassee is the capital of Florida may be difficult for a student with learning problems, remembering a picture of a flower on a television set was much simpler, and- if the keywords had been learned-contained the same information. For this rea- son, we have found mnemonic strategies constructed according to these procedures to be extraordinarily effective (Mastropieri, Scruggs, Bakken, & Brigham, 1992).
  • 45. Figure 1. Mnemonic representation of Tallahassee, capital of Florida. (Copyright I 993 by M.A. Mastropieri and TE. Scruggs.) 202 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC WHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES ARE NOT It might be helpful, at this point, to also mention briefly what mnemonic strategies are not. Mnemonic strategies do not represent a "philosophy" of education. We do not use, or recommend the use of, mnemonic strategies because they arc compatible with someone's particular philosophy or because they are a part of some- one's theory about what education should be. We recommend mnemonic strategies for only one reason: Over and over again, they have been proven to be extremely effective in helping people remember things (Bulgren, Schumaker, & Deshler, 1994; Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1989). It is also true that mnemonic strategies are not an overall teaching method or curricular approach. The focus of mnemonic strategies is so specific that they are intended to be used to enhance the recall of the components of any lesson for which memory is needed. We have found, for example, that mnemonic strategies can be ·used to enhance science learning when the curriculum involves a textbook/lecture format (Scruggs & Mastropicri, 1992) or when the curriculum involves a hands-on, inquiry learning format
  • 46. (Mastropicri, Scruggs, & Chung, 1997). Even though these approaches to science learning are very different (Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1994), mnemonic strategies can still be incorporated for the elements that require recall. It is also important to consider that mnemonic strategies are memory strategies, and not comprehension strategies. Students who are trained mnemonically also perform better on comprehension tests of that content (e.g., Mastropieri, Scruggs, & Fulk, 1990; Scruggs, Mastropicri, McLoone, Levin, & Morrison, 1987), but that is generally because they remember more information that can be applied on comprehension tests. Nevertheless, when comprehension enhancement is called for, it is important to consider using specific comprehension strategies, such as content elaboration, prior knowledge activation, manipulation, coaching and questioning, or prediction and verification (e.g., Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1997; Scruggs, Mastropieri, Sullivan, & Hesser, 1993). Nevertheless, mnemonic strategies do not inhibit com- prehension, as suggested by some (e.g., Kilpatrick, 1985), and, more importantly, there arc many instances in school of students who have achieved adequate com- prehension of a concept, but who have forgotten the facts associated with it. For example, it is completely possible to comprehend the nature of states and capitals while being unable to retrieve the capital of Florida. For another example, it is
  • 47. altogether possible to comprehend the concepts of a cell having a nucleus and consuming other organisms while being unable to retrieve the related verbal label (prokaryotic heterotroph). These are the areas where mnemonic strategies can help. Finally, it should be emphasized that mnemonic strategies do not represent an educational panacea. There are many things that students must do to succeed in school, and remembering content information is only one part of the entire picture. However, when there is academic content to be remembered, mnemonic strategies may be an important instructional component. In the next section, we discuss some general procedures for improving memory, followed by a description of how to create specific mnemonic strategies. GENERAL TECHNIQUES FOR IMPROVING MEMORY Mnemonic strategies as described in this article arc not the only way of improving memory in students who exhibit difficulty remembering things. It is important that you consider all possible methods for improving memory and not assume that mnemonic strategies are your only option. In other publications (Mastropieri & Scruggs, l 993; Mastropieri & Scruggs, in press), we have described more general methods for improving memory. These include the following: 1. INCREASE AITENTION. Students will not remember something that they did not pay
  • 48. attention to in the first place. Be sure your students' memory problems are not really attention problems. Use strategies for enhancing attention, such as intensifying instruction, teaching enthusiastically, using more visual aids and activities, and reinforcing attending. 2. PROMOTE EXTERNAL MEMORY. Many things that need to be remembered can be written down, a practice known as "external memory." Practices such as keeping an assignment notebook and maintaining a student calendar can be helpful in remembering to do things. Unfortunately, external memory is usually of little use (ethically, anyway) on tests. 3. ENHANCE MEANINGFULNFSS. Find ways to relate the content being discussed to the student's prior knowledge. Draw parallels to the students' own lives. Bring in concrete, meaningful examples for students to explore so the content becomes more a part of their experience. 4. USE PICTURES. Pictures can provide a substantial memory advantage. Use pictures on the chalkboard or on the overhead projector. Bring in photographs or other illustrations.
  • 49. Show concrete images on video- tape, when appropriate. If pictures are simply unavailable, ask students to create images, or "pictures in their heads." VOL. 33. No. 4. MARCH 1998 203 5. MINIMIZE INTERFERENCE. Avoid digressions and emphasize only the critical features of a new topic. Make sure all examples relate directly to the content being covered. 6. PROMOTE ACTIVE REASONING. Students re- member content better when they experience it for themselves (Scruggs, Mastropieri, Bakken, & Brigham, 1993). For example, rather than lecturing the class on the effect of weak acid (such as vinegar) on calcite, allow students to place calcite in a glass of vinegar and see for themselves. 7. PROMOTE ACTIVE REASONING. Students remember better if they actively think through new information, rather than simply repeating it. For example, rather than simply telling students that penguins carry their eggs on the tops of
  • 50. their feet, ask students why it makes sense that penguins would carry their eggs on the tops of their feet. 8. INCREASE THE AMOUNT OF PRACTICE. Students remember information better if they have practice using it more frequently. Use lots of review in your teaching; do not simply finish one topic and then never mention it again. Remind the class, and have students practice previous information frequently. All these strategies can be used to improve memory, and all should be considered. Unfortunately, none of these strategies specifically targets recall of information contained in new or unfamiliar words, and this is the aspect of memory where students most often fail. For example, in the Scruggs, Mastropieri, Bakken, and Brig- ham (1993) investigation, students who engaged in active manipulation remembered more information about electricity and rocks and minerals than students who studied from textbooks. However, neither condition improved recall of critical vocabulary or terminology, the area where mnemonic strategies are most effective. In the following section, we describe several different types of mnemonic strategies that can be used to improve students' memory. SPECIFIC MNEMONIC TECHNIQUES The Keyword Method
  • 51. The keyword method has already been described for helping students remember states and capitals. However, the keyword method is extremely versatile and has a variety of helpful applications. One possibility is in teaching new vocabulary words. For example, to help students remember that barrister is another word for lawyer, first create a keyword for the unfamiliar word, banister. Remember, a keyword is a word that sounds like the new word and is easily pictured. A good keyword for barrister, then, is bear. Then, you create a picture of the keyword and the definition doing something together. It is important that these two things actually interact and are not simply presented in the same picture. Therefore, a picture of a bear and a lawyer in one picture is not a good mnemonic, because the elements are not interacting. A better picture would be a bear who is acting as a lawyer in a courtroom, for example, pleading his client's innocence. We have created pictures and shown them on overhead projectors, but you could show them in other ways as well. When you practice this strategy, be certain students understand all parts of it: Class, barrister is another word for lawye1: To remember what a barrister is, first think of the keyword for barrister: bear. Whats the keyword for barrister? [bear] Good, the keyword for barrister is bear, and barrister means lawyer. Now [displays
  • 52. overhead] look at this pic- ture of a bear acting like a lawyer. The bear is the keyword for. .? [barrister/ Barrister, good. So remember this picture of a bear acting like a lawyer. When you hear the word barrister, you first think of the keyword...? [Bear] Good, and remember, what is the bear doing in the picture? /being a lawyer}. Right, being a lawyer. So what does barrister mean? [lawyer] Lawyer, good. The keyword method can also be used for more specialized vocabulary such as ranidae, the scientific term for common frogs. A good keyword for ranidae could be rain, and you could show a picture of frogs sitting in the rain. Practice the strategy as in the barrister example. When you question individual students, ask them to give the answer and then describe how they remembered. You should get an answer something like: Ranidae is the word for common frogs. I remembered because the keyword is rain and it was raining on the frogs. If you practice the strategy carefully and frequently, students should remember this information very well. At early stages of learning, you might find some students give the answer rain when you ask what ranid(le means. In these cases, you simply remind the student, No, rain is the keyword it just helps us remember the answer. Now, think in the picture, what is it raining on? [frogs] Right, frogs. So what does ranidae mean? [common frogs]. Correct, common frogs.
  • 53. Mnemonics can also be used in acquiring foreign language vocabulary. A list of some Italian vocabulary words (from Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1991, p. 24) and corresponding mnemonic strategies are given in Table l. Before you read, cover up the keywords and strategies and see if you can come up with your own. Keywords have also been used to improve recall of map locations. For example, students with learning dis- abilities were much more successful in locating Revolutionary War battle locations on a map when they 204 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC Table 1. Sample Italian Vocabulary Words and Corresponding Mnemonic Strategies Word & meaning Keyword Strategy meta (apple) mail an apple in a mailbox capre (goat) cop a goat dressed like a cop Iago (lake) log a log in a lake carta (letter) cart a cart with a letter in it Word & meaning Keyword Strategy fonda (bag) phone a phone in a bag
  • 54. were mnemonically encoded (e.g., a picture of a tiger, keyword for Fort Ticonderoga) than when representational pictures were used. When asked for the location of Fort Ticonderoga, students proved much more able to identify where on the map the tiger had been than they were to identify the location of a more traditional illustration. Further, if the tiger was shown tending a cannon, students were more likely to remember that at Fort Ticonderoga, cannons were captured that were helpful in the American war effort (Brigham, Scruggs, & Mastropieri, 1995). The Pegword Method Pegwords can be used when numbered or ordered information needs to be remembered. Pegwords are rhyming words for numbers and include the following: One is bun, six is sticks, two is shoe, seven is heaven, three is tree, eight is gate, four is door, nine is vine, five is hive, ten is hen. Pegwords are substituted for the number to be remembered and associated with the other information. For instance, to remember that insects have six legs whereas spiders have eight legs, create a picture of insects on sticks (see Figure 2) and
  • 55. another picture of a spider on a gate. To remember Newton's first law of motion (objects at rest tend to remain at rest unless acted on by another force), create a picture of a bun (pegword for one) resting. To remember that a garden rake is an example of a third-class lever, create a picture of a rake leaning against a tree (pegword for three, or third). Pegwords can also be combined with keywords. To teach that crocoite is a mineral that is number 2 on the Mohs hardness scale, create a picture of crocodiles (key- word for crocoite) wearing shoes (pegword for 2). To remember that the mineral wolframite is hardness num- ber 4, black in color, and used in making filaments for lightbulbs, create a picture of a black wolf (keyword for wolframite), looking in a door (pegword for 4), and turn- ing on a lightbulb. Research has shown that elaborate strategies such as this are very effective, and that color, appropriately encoded, can also be easily remembered (Scruggs, Mastropieri, Levin, & Gaffney, 1985). That is, a picture of a black wolf is much more likely to be remembered than a picture of wolframite colored black. Pegwords can also be extended beyond the number 10 (11 is lever, 12 is elf, etc.). For instance, to remember that the 19th amendment of the U.S. Constitution guar- anteed women the right to vote, create a picture of a woman dressed as a knight (19 = knighting) riding to a voting booth. To remember that James K. Polk was the 11th American president, create a picture of a polka dotted (keyword for Polk) lever (pegword for 11) as shown in Figure 3 (Mastropieri, Scruggs, & Whedon,1997).
  • 56. INSECTS 6 (sticks) legs Figure 2. Mnemonic representation of insects having six legs. (Copyright 1993 by M.A. Mastropieri and TE. Scruggs.) Figure 3. Mnemonic representation of Polk as the 11th U.S. president. (Copyright 1993 by M.A. Mastropieri and TE. Scruggs.) VOL. 33. No. 4. MARCH 1998 205 Pegwords can also be extended beyond the number 19, by using, for example, twenty is twinty (twins), thirty is thirsty, forty is party, and fifty is "gifty," or gift-wrapped. For example, to remember the math fact, 7 x 8 = 56, create a picture and practice the pegword phrase, "Heaven's (7) gate (8) holds gifty sticks" (pegword for 56). To remember that Taft was the 27th president, create a picture of a taffy (keyword for Taft), being pulled between twin heavens (pegword for 27).
  • 57. Letter Strategies Letter strategies, which involve using letter prompts to remember lists of things, arc the most familiar to students. Most former students remember using the acronym HOMES to remember the names of the Great Lakes and FACE to remember the notes represented in the spaces of the treble clef, from bottom to top. Except for the FACE strategy, however, most acronyms assume that a name of something will be remembered when the first letter is retrieved. However, this may not always be true. For example, if a student is unfamiliar with Lake Ontario, remembering simply that the first letter is O is insufficient to prompt recall. The names of the individual lakes must be practiced until they have become familiar. Acronyms are most helpful when the first letters of a list can be used to create an entire word; however, sometimes modifications can be made. For instance, consider the acronym FARM-B, which represents the five classes of vertebrate animals: fish, amphibian, reptile, mammal, and bird, as shown in Figure 4. The B for bird does not really fit, but it can be added to FARM and practiced until it becomes automatic. In other cases, appropriate words cannot be easily con- structed from the first letters of the words to be remembered. For example, if you wished to remember the names of the planets in their order from the sun, the letters would be M-V-E-M-J- S-U-N-P, from which a word cannot be made. In these cases, an
  • 58. acrostic can be created, in which the first letters are reconstructed to represent the words in a sentence. In this case, the sentence could be "My very educated mother just sent us nine pizzas" (Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1994, p. 271). Again, the names of the planets must be sufficiently familiar so that students can retrieve a planet name, given only the first letter. Also, students should be sufficiently familiar with the solar system to know that the first M stands for Mercury, and not Mars. For another example, to remember the classification taxonomy of living things, remember the sentence, "King Philip's class ordered a family of gentle spaniels." This sentence helps prompt kingdom, phylum, class, order, family, genus, and species, in order. Figure 4. Mnemonic representation of the vertebrates: fish, amphibians, reptiles, mammals, and birds. (Note: From A Practical Guide for Teaching Science to Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings [p. 158}, by M. A. Mastropieri and T E. Scruggs, 1991, Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Reprinted with per- 111frsion.)
  • 59. TRAINING INDEPENDENT STRATEGY USE Several research studies have described the effects of training students with memory problems how to use mnemonic strategies independently (Fulk, Mastropieri, & Scruggs, 1992; King- Sears, Mercer, & Sindelar, 1992; Mastropieri, Scruggs, Levin, Gaffney, & McLoone, 1985; McLoone, Scruggs, Mastropieri, & Zucker, 1986; Scruggs & Mastropieri, 1992). The earlier studies successfully trained students with disabilities to use the mnemonic procedures and then to generalize the procedures for learning new vocabulary words (Mastropieri et al., 1985; McLoone et al., 1986). More recent studies trained students with disabilities to use the strategies across different content areas, including science and social studies (Fulk et al., 1992; King-Sears et al., I 992; Scruggs & Mastropieri, 1992). All of these studies demonstrated some positive benefits for training students to use mnemonic strategies independently. More importantly, however, studies shared seven common elements during the training sessions with students with disabilities, as described by Fulk (1994) and Bulgren et al. (1994) and summarized next. STEP 1. Inform students about the purpose of the instruction and the rationale for the strategy training. Tell students that strategy training will be beneficial for them and that their efforts at using the strategies will result in better performance. Provide examples of how the strategy can be beneficial across a variety of learning situations and content
  • 60. areas. For example, in teaching students how to implement the strategy to learn science vocabulary, show them how the method can also be helpful in learning social studies content and for learning English and foreign language vocabulary. STEP 2. Provide instruction in the strategy and in positive attributions toward strategy usage. Demonstrate, 206 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC model, and lead students using many examples to ensure their complete understanding of the mnemonic process during the strategy component training. Provide instances and noninstances of correct usage and have them identify and correct any incorrect examples. Make charts listing the steps involved in generating strategies. King-Sears ct al. (1992) taught students the IT FITS strategy: Identify the term. Tell the definition of the term. Find a keyword. Imagine the definition doing something with the keyword. Think about the definition doing something with the keyword. Study what you imagined until you know the definition. (King-Sears et al., 1992, p. 27) Provide ample modeling and practice with students, attributing their successes to strategy use. Say, for example, "When I try hard and use the strategy, I will
  • 61. remember more information." Reinforce students for trying hard to use the strategy and attribute success and failure to strategy usage. STEP 3. Provide models during which examples and thinking processes are said aloud. Demonstrate how you proceed with your thinking while generating a strategy for specific examples. Include statements attributing your success to the hard effort and use of the strategy. STEP 4. Allow students opportunities to practice orally and provide corrective feedback. Practice several examples with the class as a whole. Encourage brainstorming during the development of the keyword and interactive picture phase. Allow students to work in small groups and practice generating strategies and brainstorming. Then, have students work with partners to develop strategies before working independently. STEP 5. Arrange guided practice with relevant feed- back on both strategy usage and attribution feedback. Give students additional items to practice using the mnemonic and attribution strategies. Provide corrective feedback and allow opportunities for students to share their thinking with one another about how they developed their strategies. STEP 6. Provide generalization instruction, practice, and feedback. Use different types of
  • 62. materials to demonstrate how the strategy can be applied across content areas and various types of factual information. Have students practice generating strategies for vocabulary words in English, for names of famous people and their accomplished in history, for minerals and their associated attributes in science, and other associated factual information they may need to learn in school STEP 7. Include pos1t1ve reinforcement and pos1t1ve attribution training for completing the tasks and for remembering the information correctly. Provide review and practice with information that was learned using strategies. Students will still need to practice retrieving information learned with strategies. When implemented as a package of training, students with disabilities may be more likely to learn to use and generate these strategies independently. Limitations Research has indicated that students who have been taught strategies for creating their own mnemonics out- perform comparison students in free-study conditions. Unfortunately, however, when students generate their own strategies, instruction may proceed at a much slower rate and students' performances may be lower than when teachers supply the strategies (Scruggs & Mastropieri, 1992). During a given unit of instruction, teachers should consider whether learning a strategy or learning the content is the priority. Moreover, initial development of many of these strategies can be difficult for anyone. Try
  • 63. developing several strategies yourself before teaching your students with disabilities. If you experience difficulties, imagine that the task will be much more difficult for your students. Each year develop a few strategies to accompany the content areas that you teach most frequently. Over time you will have a great number of effective strategies that you can teach your students. This does not mean that you should not teach your students to develop strategies independently. You can still encourage active strategy development on the part of your students, but if students have difficulties generating strategies, you can supply the ones developed by you or your co-teachers. Perhaps you will find that a combination of teacher- created and student-generated mnemonic strategies is the best way to enhance recall and still promote independent strategy use. SUMMARY Although many changes in schooling have taken place in recent years, memory for academic content remains an extremely important part of the school learning experience. Students with learning disabilities and other special needs may he at particular risk for failure in this important arena of school functioning. In order to promote academic success in school, we recommend that teachers teach students how to remember as well as what to remember. This can be done by a variety of strategies, but by far the most powerful have
  • 64. been the keyword method, the pegword method, and letter strategies. Systematic instruction using mnemonic strategies for important information to remember, as well as systematic VQL. 33, No. 4, MARCH 1998 207 instruction in independent use of mnemonic strategies, can be important factors in determining school success for students with learning and memory problems. ABOUT THE AUTHORS Margo A. Mastropieri, PhD, is a professor of special education, Department of Educational Studies, Purdue University. Her research interests include ways to facilitate learning, memory, and thinking of student with disabilities. Thomas E. Scruggs, PhD, is a professor of special education, Department of Educational Studies, Purdue University. His research emphasizes research synthesis, science instruction for students with disabilities, and mnemonic instruction for student with disabilities. Address: Margo A. Mastropieri, Department of Educational Studies, LAF.B 5th floor, Purdue University, West Lafayette IN 47907 e-mail: [email protected] REFERENCES Brigham, F. J., Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1995).
  • 65. Elaborative maps for enhanced learning of historical information: Uniting spatial, verbal, and imaginal information. The Journal of Special Education, 28, 440-460. Bulgren, J. A., Schumaker, J. B., & Deshler, D. D. (1994). The effects of a recall enhancement routine on the test performance of secondary students with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 9, 2-11. Cooney, J. B., & Swanson, H. L. (1987). Memory and learning disabilities: An overview. In H. L. Swanson (Ed.), Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities: Memory and learning disabilities (pp. I^K)). Greenwich, CT: JAI. Fulk, B. J. M. (1994). Mnemonic keyword strategy training for students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 9, 179-185. Fulk, B.J. M., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. F. (1992). Mnemonic generalization training with learning disabled students. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 7, 2-10. Kilpatrick, J. (1985). Doing mathematics without understanding it: A commentary on Higbee and Kunihira. Educational Psychologist, 20, 65-68. King-Sears, M. F., Mercer, C. D., & Sindelar, P. T. (1992). Toward independence with keyword mnemonics: A strategy for science vocabulary instruction.
  • 66. Remedial and Special Education, 13, 22-33. Lorayne, H., & Lucas, J. (1974). The memory book. New York: Ballantine. Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. F. (1989). Constructing more meaningful relationships: Mnemonic instructions for special populations. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 88-111. Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T E. (1991). Teaching students ways to remember: Strategies for learning mnemonically. Cambridge, MA: Brookline. Mastropieri Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1993). A practical guide for teaching science to students with special needs in inclusive settings. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T E. (1994). Text-based vs. activities oriented science curriculum: Implications for students with disabilities. Remedial and Special Education, IS, 72-85. Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1997). Best practices in promoting reading comprehension in students with learning disabilities: 1976 to 1996. Remedial and Special Education, 18(4), 197- 213. Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (in press). Strategies for teaching in inclusive classrooms. Columbus, OH: Prentice-Hall/Merrill. Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., Bakken J., & Brigham, F. J. (1992). A complex mnemonic strategy for teaching states and capitals: Comparing forward and backward associations. Learning Disabilities Research &
  • 67. Practice, 7, 96-103. Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T E., & Chung, S. (1997, April). Qualitative and quantitative outcomes associated with inclusive science teaching. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago. Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Fulk, B.J. M. (1990). Teaching abstract vocabulary with the keyword method: Effects on recall and comprehension. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 92- 96. Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T F., Levin, J. R., Gaffney, J., & McLoone, B. B. (1985). Mnemonic vocabulary instruction for learning disabled students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 8, 57-63. Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Whedon, C. (1997). Using mnemonic strategies to teach information about U.S. presidents: A classroom-based investigation. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20, 13-21. McLoone, B. B., Scruggs, T E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Zucker, S. (1986). Mnemonic instruction and training with learning disabled adolescents. Learning Disabilities Research, 2, 45-53. Putnam, M. L. (1992a). Characteristics of questions on tests administered by mainstream secondary classroom teachers. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 7, 129-136. Putnam, M. L. (1992b). The testing practices of mainstream secondary classroom teachers.
  • 68. Remedial and Special Education, 11-21. Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1992). Classroom applications of mnemonic instruction: Acquisition, maintenance, and generalization. Exceptional Children, 58, 219-229. Scruggs, T F., Mastropieri, M. A., Bakken, J. P., & Brigham, F J. (1993). Reading vs. doing: The relative effectiveness of textbook based and inquiry- oriented approaches to science education. The Journal of Special Education, 27, 1-15. Scruggs, T F., Mastropieri, M. A., Levin, J. R., & Gaffney J. S. (1985). Facilitating the acquisition of science facts in learning disabled students. American Educational Research Journal, 22, 575- 586. Scruggs, T E., Mastropieri, M. A., McLoone, B. B., Levin, J. R., & Morrison, C. (1987). Mnemonic facilitation of learning disabled students' memory for expository prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 27-34. Scruggs, T E., Mastropieri, M. A., Sullivan, G. S., & Hesser, L. S. (1993). Improving reasoning and recall: The differential effects of elaborative interrogation and mnemonic elaboration. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16, 233-240. U.S. Department of Education. (1994). Sixteenth annual report to Congress on the implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Education
  • 69. Act. Washington, DC: Author. 208 Copyright of Intervention in School & Clinic is the property of Sage Publications Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. 208 INTERVENTION IN SCHOOL AND CLINIC Enhancing School Success with Mnemonic StrategiesTHE NEED FOR MNEMONIC STRATEGIESWHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES AREWHAT MNEMONIC STRATEGIES ARE NOTGENERAL TECHNIQUES FOR IMPROVING MEMORYSPECIFIC MNEMONIC TECHNIQUESTRAINING INDEPENDENT STRATEGY USESUMMARYABOUT THE AUTHORSREFERENCES