SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 44
Developmental Science. 2018;21:e12610.
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/desc | 1 of 9
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12610
© 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Received: 17 December 2016 | Accepted: 18 July
2017
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12610
S H O R T R E P O R T
Digital disruption? Maternal mobile device use is related to
infant social- emotional functioning
Sarah Myruski1,2 | Olga Gulyayeva2,4 | Samantha Birk2
| Koraly Pérez-Edgar3 |
Kristin A. Buss3 | Tracy A. Dennis-Tiwary1,2
1Department of Psychology, The Graduate
Center, The City University of New York, NY,
USA
2Department of Psychology, Hunter
College, The City University of New York, NY,
USA
3Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania
StateUniversity, University Park, PA, USA
4Department of Psychology, St John’s
University, Queens, NY, USA
Correspondence
Tracy Dennis-Tiwary, 695 Park Avenue,
Department of Psychology, Hunter College,
The City University of New York, New York,
NY 10065, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Funding Information
This research was made possible by grant
R21 MH103627 from the US Department of
Health and Human Services of the National
Institute of Mental Health
Abstract
Mobile device use has become increasingly prevalent, yet its
impact on infant develop-
ment remains largely unknown. When parents use mobile
devices in front of infants,
the parent is physically present but most likely distracted and
unresponsive. Research
using the classic Still Face Paradigm (SFP) suggests that
parental withdrawal and unre-
sponsiveness may have negative consequences for
children’s social- emotional devel-
opment. In the present study, 50 infants aged 7.20 to 23.60
months (M = 15.40, SD =
4.74) and their mothers completed a modified SFP. The SFP
consisted of three phases:
free play (FP; parent and infant play and interact), still face
(SF; parent withdraws at-
tention and becomes unresponsive), and reunion (RU; parent
resumes normal interac-
tion). The modified SFP incorporated mobile device use in the
SF phase. Parents
reported on their typical mobile device use and infant
temperament. Consistent with
the standard SFP, infants showed more negative affect and less
positive affect during
SF versus FP. Infants also showed more toy engagement and
more engagement with
mother during FP versus SF and RU. Infants showed the most
social bids during SF and
more room exploration in SF than RU. More frequent reported
mobile device use was
associated with less room exploration and positive affect during
SF, and less recovery
(i.e., engagement with mother, room exploration positive affect)
during RU, even when
controlling for individual differences in temperament. Findings
suggest that the SFP
represents a promising theoretical framework for understanding
the impact of par-
ent’s mobile device use on infant social-
emotional functioning and parent–infant
interactions.
R E S E A R C H H I G H L I G H T S
• The traditional Still Face Paradigm (SFP) was modified to
include
mobile device use, mimicking typical disruptions
in parent–infant
interactions that may occur in daily life.
• Patterns of child behavior during the modified SFP mirrored
those
of the traditional version, with infants showing the most distress
when mothers were disengaged.
• Greater habitual self-reported maternal mobile device use was
as-
sociated with less infant recovery upon reunion.
• Findings provide support for the use of this modified paradigm
as a
framework for understanding the impact of
parent’s mobile device
use on infant social-emotional functioning and
parent–infant
interactions.
1 | I N T R O D U C T I O N
The exponential increase in mobile device use has transformed
not
only how we communicate remotely, but also how we engage in
www.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/desc
mailto:[email protected]
2 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al.
face- to- face interactions. Greater mobile device use in adults
has
been associated with mental health problems, including anxiety
(Reid
& Reid, 2007; Sapacz, Rockman, & Clark, 2016). However,
little is
known about the impact of device use on
parent–infant interactions.
Frequent use of mobile devices during these interactions may
de-
crease the quality of the social exchange by limiting
opportunities for
the in- the- moment emotional feedback essential for emotion
regula-
tion development (Field, 1994).
Infant behavior during dyadic interactions can be assessed using
the Still Face Paradigm (SFP; Braungart- Rieker et al., 2014;
Fuertes,
Santos, Beeghly, & Tronick, 2006; Montirosso, Casini et al.,
2015;
Montirosso, Provenzi et al., 2015; Provenzi, Borgatti, Menozzi,
&
Montirosso, 2015; Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, &
Brazelton, 1978),
a classic laboratory behavioral task that examines infant
responses to
social cues by a parent, consisting of three phases: Free Play
(FP), Still
Face (SF), and Reunion (RU). The FP phase serves as a baseline
for par-
ent–infant play, while the SF phase disrupts this
interaction by making
the parent cease initiating or responding to social cues, while
main-
taining eye gaze. Infant behavior during the SF phase is
characterized
by decreased positive affect and gaze, and increased negative
affect
(Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Bakermans- Kranenburg,
2009). Maternal
regulation of infant emotion is absent during this phase, and
when
bids for emotional reciprocation are not returned, the infant
tends to
respond with distress and confusion (Montirosso, Casini et al.,
2015;
Montirosso, Provenzi et al., 2015; Provenzi et al., 2015;
Trevarthen,
1977). Finally, the RU phase provides an opportunity to repair
subse-
quent mismatches in dyadic behavior by resuming interactive
play. The
current study created a modified version of the classic SFP by
employ-
ing a novel SF phase that introduced an ecologically valid
mechanism
(mobile device use) that in effect may typically make parents
unavail-
able to infants in daily life.
Infant behavior during the SFP is related to broader patterns of
emo-
tional and social well- being, with greater positive affect and
social bids
during the SF phase predicting secure attachment (Braungart-
Rieker
et al., 2014; Fuertes et al., 2006; Kiser,
Bates, Maslin, & Bayles, 1986;
Tronick, Ricks, & Cohn, 1982). In addition, infant individual
differences
are related to their behavior during the SFP. For example,
infants with
greater parent- rated temperamental negative affectivity showed
re-
duced self- comforting during SF, potentially blunting
regulation and re-
covery during RU (Braungart- Rieker, Garwood, Powers, &
Notaro, 1998;
Mesman et al., 2009). In addition, Rothbart, Ziaie,
and O’Boyle (1992)
found that self- regulatory behaviors during the task were
related to in-
fant temperament. For example, activity level was negatively
related to
oral self- soothing, fear scores were positively related to
inhibited reach
and negatively related to approach, and attention disengagement
was
negatively related to distress and positively related to positive
affect.
A largebody of research has also established the
SFP as an ana-
log for dyadic interactions between a depressed mother and her
child
(Field, 1994; Field et al., 2007). In one study, the SF phase
elicited less
distress in infants of depressed mothers compared to controls
(Field
et al., 2007), potentially since this lack of emotional
responsiveness
tends to be more habitual in day- to- day interactions for infants
of de-
pressed mothers.
Several studies have modified components of the classic SFP.
In one study, modifications included mothers wearing masks
while
maintaining eye contact and vocal interactions with their
children, or
drinking from a bottle while maintaining eye contact and a
neutral,
unresponsive face. Infants only displayed negative affect in
response
to the traditional still face but not to the modified versions
(Legerstee
& Markova, 2007), suggesting that infants may have interpreted
the
mothers’ unresponsiveness differently when tied to
a novel behavior.
In another study using a modified SF phase in which mothers
played
with another infant, 6- month- old infants responded with
heightened
sadness and interest that exceeded that of the traditional SFP
(Hart,
Carrington, Tronick, & Carroll, 2004). A third
study modified the tradi-
tional SFP for use with toddlers by using the same three phases,
but
placing them in the middle of a longer
mother–child free play session
(Weinberg, Beeghly, Olson, & Tronick, 2008). Toddlers showed
similar
response patterns as infants, including the classic still face
effect, but
also exhibited a wider array of responses, including
vocalizations that
expressed an effort to understand the reason
for the mother’s unre-
sponsiveness. Thus, Weinberg and colleagues (2008)
demonstrated
that the SFP can be used to examine behavior across a wider age
range
than previously examined.
Similar to the key components of the classic SFP, parent mobile
de-
vice use in front of infants causes the parents to be physically
present
but putatively distracted and unresponsive. While mobile device
use
is pervasive, only two studies to date have investigated how
engage-
ment with devices may interfere with parent–child
interactions. One
study found that during a structured interaction task, maternal
mobile
device use was common and associatedwith fewer
mother–child in-
teractions (Radesky et al., 2015). In a descriptive observational
study,
Radesky and colleagues (2014) found that parents who were
deeply
absorbed in mobile device use during meal times tended to
respond to
child bids for attention in insensitive or aggressive ways.
However, no
study to date has examined how infant social and emotional
behavior
is influenced by parental device use during dyadic interactions.
The first aim of the current study is to establish whether a mod-
ified SFP that incorporates maternal mobile device use could
serve
as an analog to the original SFP, probing the impact of
distracted or
unresponsive parents on child socioemotional behavior.
Specifically,
given that mobile device use may mimic the social and
emotional dis-
engagement present in the classic SFP, we predict that there
will be
greater negative affect, and less positive affect, during the SF
phase,
compared to the FP and RU phases. Also, we
predict that dyadic
interaction will resume upon the RU phase, with infants
showing
more engagement with mother in the RU phase compared to the
SF
phase. Second, we aim to examine whether or not maternal
device
use habits predict individual differences in infant behavior
during
the SFP. Specifically, based on research using the SFP with
infants
and depressed mothers (Field et al., 2007), we hypothesize that
habitual device use will be associated with less negative
responses
from infants in the SF phase. That is, mothers who frequently
use
devices, particularly in front of their family and infants, may
habit-
ually show a lack of emotional responsiveness during
interactions,
making their infant more accustomed to such disruptions and
thus
| 3 of 9MYRUSKI et al.
eliciting less distress. Third, based on previous studies
suggesting
that temperament may make infants more sensitive to the still
face
disruption (Braungart- Rieker et al., 1998), we predicted that
infants
high in negative affect would show greater disruption during the
SF
and less re- engagement during the RU.
2 | M E T H O D
2.1 | Participants
Fifty infants (25 female) ages 7.20 to 23.60 months (M = 15.40,
SD
= 4.74)1 participated in a modified SFP with their mothers.
Forty-
five (90.0%) parents reported their infant’s ethnicity as
White/Non-
Hispanic, three (6.0 %) reported Hispanic, three (6.0 %)
reported
Asian/Pacific Islander, one (2.0 %) reported African-
American,and
one (2.0 %) reported Native American.All infants
were born within 2
weeks of their due date, reported no major health complications,
and
were within normal birthweight ranges (M = 7.53 lb, SD =
1.14).
2.2 | Materials
2.2.1 | The modified SFP
Infants and their mothers participated in a modified SFP
(Tronick
et al., 1978), which consisted of three phases: a free play phase
(FP; 5
minutes), during which mother and infant interacted as they
naturally
would during play time; a still face phase (SF; 2 minutes),
during which
an alarm signaled the mother to pick up a mobile device (iPod
touch),
interact only with the device, withdraw attention from their
infant, be-
come unresponsive, and allow their infant to play on their own;
and a
reunion phase (RU; 1 minute; signaled by a knock on a
window), during
which the mothers stopped using the device and resumed
interacting
with their infant as they did during FP. This modified SFP
altered the
protocol of the original SFP by instructing mothers to use a
mobile de-
vice during SF, allowing infants to move around freely instead
of con-
fining them to a high chair, and allowing infants to have access
to toys
throughout the task. We also varied the durations for each of the
three
SFP phases. A key difference between the modified
SFP and the origi-
nal SFP is that we did not require mothers to maintain eye
contact while
avoiding any communication with infants, which was a feature
present
in the original SFP. These modifications were intended to
increase the
ecological validity of the SFP by including features that more
closely
mimicked scenarios that may arise in everyday life.
2.2.2 | Behavioral coding
The SFP was video- recorded and scored by three reliable
raters.
Reliability was computed across 20% of participants
using Cohen’s
kappa, and ranged from .71 to .98 (M = .85, SD = .03). Presence
or ab-
sence of each behavior (0, 1) was coded in 15
second epochs. Average
scores were computed (number of epochs each behavior was
per-
formed divided by total number of behaviors) for each phase
(FP, SF,
RU) to account for individual differences between children who
showed
generally higher and lower behavioral frequencies overall.
Behaviors
performed by at least 25% of infants in at least one phase were
selected
for analyses (negative affect, positive affect, toy engagement,
engage-
ment with mother, social bid, room exploration; Table 1).2
2.2.3 | Questionnaires
Parents self- reported their mobile device use including habitual
de-
vice use frequency per day [(1) less than 30 min; (2) 1
hour; (3) 1–3
hours; (4) 3–5 hours], use in front of family,
and use in front of infant
[(1) do not use in presence; (2) less than 30 min;
(3) 1 hour; (4) 1–3
hours; (5) 3–5 hours], as well as the total number
of communication
types used (e. g. texting, email, etc.).
Participants also completed the Revised Infant Behavior
Questionnaire Short Form [IBQ- R; (Putnam, Helbig, Gartstein,
Rothbart, & Leerkes, 2014); n = 11] or the Toddler
Behavior Assessment
Questionnaire [TBAQ; (Goldsmith, 1996); n =
39]. The IBQ- R is a
parent- report measure of infant temperament
for ages 4–12 months
and consists of 91 items about infant behaviors in the past week
on
a 7- pointLikert- type scale. The TBAQ is a
parent- report measure of
infant temperament for ages 12–24 months and
consists of 110 items
about infant behaviors in the past month on a 7- point Likert-
type
scale. To examine temperament across the entire age range of
the cur-
rent sample, within- questionnaire z- scores were computed and
IBQ- R
and TBAQ subscales were combined. Independent
samples t tests
found no significant differences in temperament (z- scores)
between
Infant behavior Description
Negative affect Negative expressions or
vocalizations; infant protesting,
or withdrawn. Must display
negative facial expressions.
Positive affect Displaying facial expressions of
joy particularly smiles or
vocalizations with a positive
tone.
Engagement with toy or other
object
Playing with the toys provided or
in room or playing with non- toy
objects, such as the chair.
Engagement with mother Playing with or engaging with the
parent (except if parent is
ignoring infant for the still face
phase, then it is a social bid).
Social bid Making an attempt to get the
attention of the parent
physically or vocally, either in a
negative way or positive or
neutral way.
Room exploration Playing with objects around the
room or exploring the room in
an attention- seeking manner or
in a manner designed to not
engage the caregiver.
TABLE 1 Coding scheme for infant behaviors during the still
face
paradigm
4 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al.
infants whose parents completedthe infant (IBQ)
or toddler (TBAQ)
version of the questionnaire (ps > .10). Subscales represented
on both
versions of the questionnaire that captured characteristics
relevant to
behavior during the SFP were examined: activity level,
approach/in-
terest, attentional control (duration and orienting from IBQ- R
and ap-
propriate attentional allocation from TBAQ), and
negative affectivity
(higher order subscale consisting of fear, sadness, distress to
limitation,
and falling reactivity).
2.3 | Procedure
This study was approved by the institutional review board of the
Pennsylvania State University. Parents and infants participated
in
either one (2.5 hour duration) or two (to prevent infant fatigue;
1.5
hour duration) lab visits, as part of a larger study. Following
informed
consent, parents completed questionnaires reporting
their infant’s
temperament and demographics. Participants then completed the
modified SFP task with their infant (approximately 8 minutes).
The
modified SFP task was video- recorded to allow for observation
of in-
fant behaviors. Participants were compensated with $50 and
infants
received a T- shirt with the lab logo.
3 | R E S U LT S
3.1 | Descriptive statistics
Descriptive statistics for infant behavior during the SFP,
maternal de-
vice use, and infant temperament are presented in
Tables 2–4.
Pearson correlations were conducted to examine associations
between infant age and sex relative to other study variables.
Infant
age was normally distributed, and there were an even number of
males and females in the sample. There were no differences be-
tween males and females in child temperament (ps > .10),
maternal
mobile device use habits (ps > .10), or infant behaviors during
the
SFP (ps > .10). Older infants showed more engagement with
mother
during FP (r = .46, p = .001), and infant age was positively
correlated
with attentional control (r = .34, p = .02) and approach/interest
(r =
.42, p = .003). Infant age was not significantly correlated with
ma-
ternal mobile device use (ps > .10). Although therewere
only limited
relations between infant age and other measures, due to the
wide
age range of the sample, age was included as a covariate in
subse-
quent analyses to control for the potential influence of
developmen-
tal stage on observed behaviors (e.g., greater mobility, verbal
skills
in older infants).
3.2 | Analytic plan
First, as a manipulation check, we examined within- subject
differ-
ences in behaviors across the three phases of the SFP to confirm
that
the parameters of the task elicited varying levels of child
behaviors.
Second, a series of regressions were conducted to examine the
main
research question regarding the relationship between maternal
device
use and infant behavior in the SFP.
TABLE 2 Descriptive statistics for SFP behaviors
Behavior Min Max Mean (SD)
Free play phase
Negative affect .00 .80 .07 (.16)
Positive affect .00 1.00 .29 (.25)
Toy engagement .15 1.00 .92 (.15)
Engagement with
mother
.00 1.00 .68 (.26)
Room exploration .00 .95 .44 (.29)
Still face phase
Negative affect .00 .78 .17 (.24)
Positive affect .00 .57 .06 (.12)
Toy engagement .20 1.00 .82 (.22)
Social bid .00 1.00 .26 (.27)
Room exploration .00 1.00 .50 (.31)
Reunion phase
Negative affect .00 1.00 .10 (.20)
Positive affect .00 1.00 .21 (.27)
Toy engagement .00 1.00 .70 (.41)
Engagement with
mother
.00 1.00 .63 (.38)
Room exploration .00 1.00 .32 (.37)
Maternal device use Min Max Mean (SD)
How often do you use your device(s)? 1 5 2.74 (.97)
Do you use your device(s) in the
presence of family members? If so, how
often?
2 5 2.92 (.80)
Do you use your device(s) in the
presence of your baby? If so, how
often?
2 4 2.54 (.68)
Communication types 8 14 10.34 (1.24)
Note. Response scalefor device use frequency (per
day) = (1) less than 30 min; (2) 1 hour;
(3) 1–3 hours;
(4) 3–5 hours; (5) more than 5 hours.
Response scale for use in front of family and baby (per day) =
(1) do not use in presence; (2) less than
30 min; (3) 1 hour; (4) 1–3 hours; (5) 3–5
hours.
TABLE 3 Descriptive statistics for
maternal device use
| 5 of 9MYRUSKI et al.
3.3 | Infant behavior during still face paradigm
To examine differences across the phases of the SFP, repeated-
measures ANCOVAs were conducted separately for each
behavior
(negative affect, positive affect, toy engagement, engagement
with
mother, social bids, room exploration) with age in months as a
co-
variate, and Phase (FP, SF, RU) as a within- subjects factor
(Figure 1).
Bonferroni correction was used to control for multiple
comparisons in
post- hoc paired- samples t tests (adjusted p = .017).
Affect: Mirroring the effects of the traditional SFP, infants
showed more negative affect during SF versus FP, [t(49) = 2.98,
p =
.004; F(2, 96) = 5.67, p = .005, ƞp
2= .11]. In contrast, infants showed
less positive affect during SF versus FP [t(49) = −7.14, p
< .001] and
RU [t(49) = −3.85, p < . 001; F(2, 96) = 23.11, p < .001,
ƞp
2 = .33].
Positive affect was also greater during FP in comparison to RU
[t(49)
= 2.64, p = .011].
Toy engagement: Infants showed more toy engagement in FP
com pared
to SF [t(49) = 3.37, p = .001] and RU [t(49) =
2.16, p < .001; F(2, 96) =
11.25, p < .001, ƞp
2 = .19].
Social behaviors: Infants also showed less engagement with
mother
during SF compared to FP [t(49) = −17.64, p <
.001] and RU [t(49) =
−11.37, p < .001; F(2, 96) = 117.83, p < .001, ƞp
2 = .71]. Social bids
were used more in SF compared to FP [t(49) = 6.76, p <
.001] and RU
[t(49) = 5.51, p < .001; F(2, 98) = 37.70, p < .001, ƞp
2 = .44].
Exploration: Finally, infants explored the room less in RU
compared
to SF [t(49) = −3.50, p = .001; F(2, 98) = 6.89, p =
.002, ƞp
2 = .13].
3.4 | Infant temperament and behavior during still
face paradigm
To identify covariates for regression analyses, infant
temperament
was examined in relation to mobile device use and child
behavior dur-
ing the SFP. Pearson correlations revealed that infants with
greater
parent- reported negative affectivity showed less engagement
with
mother during FP (r = −.31,p = .03), and less room
exploration during
RU (r = −.30,p = .03). Infants with greater parent-
reported activity
level expressed more negative affect during SF (r = .29, p =
.04). Infants
with greater approach/interest engaged in more room
exploration
during SF (r = .32, p = .03).
3.5 | Maternal mobile device use and infant behavior
during still face paradigm
Associations between self- report of maternal mobile
device use and
behavior during the SFP were examined using linear regressions
as
follows: 1st step = age, sex, and infant temperament variables
(nega-
tive affectivity, activity level, approach/interest); 2nd step =
maternal
device use (separately for mobile device use variable: overall
habitual
use, use in front of family, use in front of infant);
DV = infant behavior
during SFP, separately for each phase. Infant temperamental
variables
were included as covariates since they correlated with infant
behavior.
The Benjamini- Hochberg correction (Benjamini & Hochberg,
1995)
was applied to correct for multiple comparisons for each
phase.3
This procedure, which involves ranking p- values and accounts
for the
number of tests conducted, was applied separately to each
family of
regressions (i.e., separately for each SFP phase), since this
correction
approach assumes independence of samples. All p-
values reported
below are raw, and were significant using a false discovery rate
crite-
rion of 0.25 which is recommended for research questions that
are a
first, relatively exploratory step (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995).
Based
on these parameters, raw p- values less than approximately .085
were
considered significant.
3.5.1 | Free play phase
Maternal mobile device use did not significantly predict infant
behav-
ior during the SFP (ps > .10).
3.5.2 | Still face phase
Greater habitual device use was associated with less room
exploration
[β = −.39,t(49) = −2.70, p = .01; Figure 2],
and more frequent device
Temperament
Questionnaire Subscale Measure used N Min Max Mean (SD)
Attentional control z- Score 50 −1.87 1.80 –
IBQ 11 2.25 5.27 3.49 (0.99)
TBAQ 39 2.13 5.31 3.77(0.88)
Activity level z- Score 50 −2.10 2.49 –
IBQ 11 3.93 5.67 4.67 (0.62)
TBAQ 39 2.67 6.10 4.24 (0.75)
Approach/interest z- Score 50 −1.94 1.68 –
IBQ 11 3.70 6.75 5.27(1.03)
TBAQ 39 1.60 5.00 3.42(0.94)
Negative affectivity z- Score 50 −1.71 3.06 –
IBQ 11 3.26 4.56 3.93(0.44)
TBAQ 39 2.16 5.03 3.26(0.66)
TABLE 4 Descriptive Statistics for
Temperament Questionnaire Subscales
6 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al.
use in front of infant was associated with less positive affect [β
= −.35,
t(49) = −2.37, p = .022].
3.5.3 | Reunion phase
Greater habitual device use [β = −.38,t(49) = −2.55, p
= .014], and
more specifically greater use in front of infant [β = −.29,
t(49) = −2.05,
p = .047], was associatedwith less roomexploration.
Greater habitual
device use [β = −.30,t(49) = −1.96, p = .057],
as well as use in front
of infant [β = −.29,t(49) = −1.95, p = .057],
was marginally associated
with less positive affect. Greater habitual device use [β =
−.36,t(49) =
−2.54, p = .015;Figure 3], more use in front of
family [β = −.25,t(49) =
−1.79, p = .081], as well as more use in
front of infant [β = −.28,t(49)
= −2.03, p = .048], was associatedwith less
engagement with mother
during this phase.
4 | D I S C U S S I O N
Since infants tend to respond to maternal unresponsiveness
during
the SF phase with distress (Trevarthen, 1977), the SF phase is
char-
acterized by a decrease in positive affect and an increase in
negative
affect (Mesman et al., 2009). Consistent with the extant
literature,
the modified SFP using a mobile device in the current study
produced
robust differential patterns of infant behavior between the three
phases. Infants expressed increased negative affect in the SF
versus
FP, as well as decreased positive affect and engagement with
mother
in SF versus both FP and RU. Infants also increased social bids
dur-
ing the SF in an attempt to obtain their
caregiver’s attention. Infants
displayed more toy engagement during FP versus the other
phases,
likely due to the fact that infants were adjusting to the new
environ-
ment and parents aided in toy engagement when they were
available
to interact, whereas during RU infants were preoccupied with
re-
engaging with the parent and they lacked scaffolding for play
during
SF. Finally, infants explored the room less in RU compared to
SF, pos-
sibly because they were more focused on reuniting with their
mother
following the SF phase. In fact, as predicted, infant engagement
with
mother increased significantly between the SF and RU phases.
Two-
way engagement was greater in FP and RU when the rules of the
task
allowed for it, whereas social bids were greater when the parent
was
unavailable during SF. These observed patterns illustrate that
the
modified SFP may act as a potentially analogous paradigm to
the origi-
nal SFP and can be used to understand the implications of
maternal
device use on infant social- emotional functioning.
Results of the current study also indicated that individual
differ-
ences in infant temperament contribute to behavior during the
SFP
phases. Infants with higher parent- reported temperamental
negative
affectivity engaged less with their caregiver during FP. During
RU, in-
fants with higher negative affectivity displayed less room
exploration,
indicating less recovery. This is consistent with findings that
infants
high in negative affectivity showed less emotion regulation
during SF,
blunting recovery during RU (Braungart- Rieker et al., 1998;
Mesman
et al., 2009). During SF, infants with greater activity level
scores
showed more negative affect. It is possible that it is more
challenging
for more active infants to deal with their nonresponsive parent,
result-
ing in increased negative affect. Finally, infants with greater
approach/
interest scores showed more room exploration during SF.
Increased
approach and interest scores may be related to decreased fear
(Buss,
2011; Rothbart et al., 1992) and increased curiosity about the
environ-
ment, resulting in increased room exploration.
An important goal of the current study was to
examine the impact
of maternal device use on infant emotion
regulation and parent–in-
fant interactions. Although previous research suggests
that physical
FIGURE 1 Infant behavior significantly differed across the
three
phases of the still face paradigm. Notably, the still face phase
elicited
greater negative affect, but less positive affect
Ne
ga
tiv
e A
ffe
ct
Po
sit
ive
A
ffe
ct
To
y E
ng
ag
em
en
t
En
ga
ge
m
en
t w
ith
M
ot
he
r
So
cia
l B
id
Ro
om
E
xp
lo
ra
tio
n
0.0
0.5
1.0
Child Behavior by during the Still Face Paradigm
Fr
eq
ue
nc
y
Still Face ReunionFreeplay
**
*
*** ***
*
***
******
******
*
**
FIGURE 2 Greater parental device use predicts less infant
room
exploration during the still face phase, while controlling for
infant
temperament
| 7 of 9MYRUSKI et al.
and emotional unavailability may decrease the quality of the
social
exchange (Field, 1994), no study has examined the effect of
maternal
device use on infant behavior and emotion regulation. The
results of
the current study suggest that greater maternal mobile use is
associ-
ated with behavior patterns across the SFP phases, even when
infant
temperamental traits are taken into account. Contrary to
predictions,
infants of mothers who frequently used devices did not show
less neg-
ative affect, or more positive affect, during the SF phase. We
did note
decreased room exploration during SF and RU as a function of
greater
habitual use, indicating that these infants were not unaffected
by the
disruption. These findings may suggest that parental habitual
device
use may be associatedwith an infant’s ability to
adjust to their envi-
ronment and highlight the need for future research in this area.
Importantly, during RU, greater habitual device use was
associated
with less positive affect, less engagement with mother, and less
room
exploration. The RU phase is crucial in that it provides an
opportunity
for the parent and infant to reconnect. However, the current
study
showed that with greater habitual device use, the reunion
between
mother and infant was not as successful. Consistent with
previous re-
search showing that greater parental mobile device use was
related to
less interaction with children (Radesky et al., 2015), and
insensitive or
aggressive parent responses to social bids (Radesky et al.,
2014), the
current findings suggest that frequent habitual device use may
reduce
the successful repair of interactions following disruptions.
These results
highlight the importance of research surrounding parental
device use
and its impact on both infant emotional regulation development
and the
quality of social exchange in parent–infant
interactions. The modified
SFP represents a promising theoretical framework for this
research.
Although the results from the current study
confirm the find-
ings that parental mobile device use is associated with infant
social-
emotional functioning and parent–infant interactions,
somelimitations
should be noted. First, the age range included in the current
study
was relatively wide, encompassing developmental stages with
vary-
ing levels of mobility, language ability, and
understanding of others’
intentions. For these reasons, age was included as a covariate in
all
main analyses. Age did not significantly alter the pattern
of results
when examining either infant behaviors across stages or
relations be-
tween maternal device use and infant behavior.
Although the current
findings represent a crucial starting point and previous studies
have
illustrated that the SFP can be used across a wide age range
(Weinberg
et al., 2008), future studies must investigate smaller age ranges
and/
or track changes longitudinally across early childhood to more
con-
cretely understand when and how maternal device use impacts
social-
emotional development. Also, the current study
lacked a comparison
group or comparison condition exposed to the classic SFP at the
same
age. Future studies should aim to address this gap to establish if
the
two versions elicit similar patterns of behavior in the same
child.
In addition, contrary to the classic SFP, in the modified SFP
with
a mobile device, toys were freely available, infant mobility was
not
restricted, and mothers were not instructed to maintain eye
contact
during SF. Importantly, these variations included in the
modified SFP
more closely resemble real- life parent–child
interactions involving
disruptions in social- emotional communication, thus increasing
the
ecological validity of this paradigm. Due to these differences,
the cur-
rent SF may have elicited overall lower levels of negative affect
versus
other behaviors as compared to the classic SF (Braungart-
Rieker et al.,
2014; Fuertes et al., 2006; Montirosso, Casini et al., 2015;
Montirosso,
Provenzi et al., 2015; Provenzi et al., 2015). This difference in
negative
affect frequency could also be due to differences in coding
methods
used (e.g., 30 second epochs versus second- by- second
microanalysis).
It also may be that infants are more accustomed to disruptions
due to
mobile device use and thus were not as distressed as they would
be by
the classic SFP. These subtle, low- level expressions of negative
affect
might be common among children whose parents habitually and
fre-
quently use mobile devices, and future research should examine
their
function, such as serving to re- engage the parent or express
distress.
An additional limitation is that the response scalefor
parental mo-
bile device use only went from (1) less than 30 minutes per day
to (5)
FIGURE 3 Greater parent device use was associated with less
infant positive affect (left) and engagement with mother (right)
during the
reunion phase, while controlling for
temperament, suggesting reduced recovery
following disruption in parent–infant interaction
8 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al.
greater than 5 hours per day for three contexts: general device
use
frequency, use in front of family, and use in front of their
infant. Future
research should track device use in real time or complete in-
home be-
havioral observations to observe how often parents use their
device
and how the infant reacts. This would provide a better
understanding
of how parent device use and how familiarity with a device may
impact
an infant’s responses. It would also be
beneficial to trackinfant social-
emotional functioning over time to determine whether device
use has
a long- term impact on emotion regulation development.
Finally, it is important to note that the durations of the three
phases were altered from the original SFP. Similar to previous
studies
(Weinberg et al., 2008), an extended free play phase was
included to
allow dyads enough time to adjust to the room in which they
had the
freedom to move at will, in contrast to the classic SFP.
However, the
most notable shortcoming of the current SFP design was that the
re-
union phase was only 1 minute long. Despite this short time-
period,
infants exhibited patterns of behavior suggesting dyadic
interaction
repair (e.g., increase in engagement with mother), as well as
significant
individual differences in recovery behaviors, which related to
mobile
device use habits. This suggests that, while this short RU phase
may
not be ideal, it was sufficient to detect notable patterns of infant
be-
havior and lays the groundwork for subsequent investigations.
Future
studies should extend this phase to observe recovery over a
longer pe-
riod, and track individual differences in recovery trajectories in
relation
to patterns of daily device use.
The modified SFP used in the current study may represent a
fruitful
method for examining the use of digital devices
in parent–child rela-
tionships in a controlled, yet ecologically valid manner. Taken
together,
results suggest that parental device use influences the quality of
par-
ent–infant interactions, and highlight the need for
continued research
on the role of technology in infant social- emotional
development.
E N D N O T E S
1 The current sample was a subsample taken from a larger study
examin-
ing broader patterns of socioemotional functioning in the first 2
years
of life (LoBue, Buss, Taber- Thomas, & Pérez- Edgar, 2017;
Morales et al.,
2017; Pérez-Edgar, Morales, LoBue, Taber-Thomas,
Allen, Brown, & Buss,
in press).
2 The distributions for some of the observed behaviors were
significantly
positively or negatively skewed. However, these skewed
behaviors
would be expected to be used either extremely frequently or
infre-
quently due to the nature of the paradigm (e.g., task parameters
would
not be expected to elicit social bids from children during free
play
or reunion). These skewed behaviors were included in the
repeated-
measures analyses only, in order to track differences in
behavior fre-
quency across the three phases.
3 Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was used in
the analyses of
covariance reported above, since Benjamini- Hochberg is not
recommended
for within- subject tests (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995).
R E F E R E N C E S
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg,Y. (1995). Controlling
the false discovery rate: A
practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of
the Royal
Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300.
Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M.M., Powers, B.P., &
Notaro, P.C. (1998).
Infant affect and affect regulation during the still- face
paradigm with
mothers and fathers: The role of infant characteristics and
parental
sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1428.
Braungart-Rieker, J.M.,Zentall, S., Lickenbrock, D.M.,
Ekas, N.V., Oshio, T.,
& Planalp, E. (2014). Attachment in the
making: Mother and father sen-
sitivity and infants’ responses during the still- face
paradigm. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 125, 63–84.
Buss, K.A. (2011). Which fearful toddlers should
we worry about? Context,
fear regulation, and anxiety risk. Developmental Psychology,
47, 804–819.
Field, T. (1994). The effects of mother’s
physical and emotional unavail-
ability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for
Research in
Child Development, 59, 208–227.
Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., Diego, M., Feijo,
L., Vera, Y., Gil, K., & Sanders,
C. (2007). Still- face and separationeffects on
depressed mother–in-
fant interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28, 314–323.
Fuertes, M., Santos, P.L.d., Beeghly, M., & Tronick, E. (2006).
More than ma-
ternal sensitivity shapes attachment. Annals of the New York
Academy of
Sciences, 1094, 292–296.
Goldsmith, H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of
the
Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire. Child
Development, 67,
218–235.
Hart, S.L., Carrington, H.A., Tronick, E., &
Carroll, S.R. (2004). When
infants lose exclusive maternal attention: Is it jealousy?
Infancy, 6,
57–78.
Kiser, L.J., Bates, J.E., Maslin, C.A., & Bayles,
K. (1986). Mother–infant play
at six months as a predictor of attachment security at thirteen
months.
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25,
68–75.
Legerstee, M., & Markova, G. (2007). Intentions make a
difference: Infant
responses to still- face and modified still- face conditions.
Infant Behavior
and Development, 30, 232–250.
LoBue, V., Buss, K.A., Taber-Thomas, B.C.,
& Pérez-Edgar, K. (2017).
Developmental differences in infants’ attention to
social and nonsocial
threats. Infancy, 22, 403–415.
Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Bakermans-
Kranenburg, M.J. (2009).
The many facesof the still- face paradigm: A review
and meta- analysis.
Developmental Review, 29, 120–162.
Montirosso, R., Casini, E., Provenzi, L., Putnam, S.P., Morandi,
F., Fedeli, C., &
Borgatti, R. (2015). A categorical approach to
infants’ individual differ-
ences during the still- face paradigm. Infant Behavior and
Development,
38, 67–76.
Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Tavian, D., Morandi, F.,
Bonanomi, A., Missaglia,
S., & Borgatti, R. (2015). Social stress regulation in 4- month-
old infants:
Contribution of maternal social engagement and
infants’ 5- HTTLPR
genotype. Early Human Development, 91, 173–179.
Morales, S., Brown, K.M., Taber-Thomas, B.C.,
LoBue, V., Buss, K.A.,
& Pérez-Edgar, K.E. (2017). Maternal anxiety
predicts attentional
bias towards threat in infancy. Emotion (Washington, DC), 17,
874–883.
Pérez-Edgar, K., Morales, S., LoBue, V., Taber-
Thomas, B.C., Allen, E.K.,
Brown, K.M., & Buss, K.A. (in press). The
impact of negative affect
on attention patterns to threat across the first two years of life.
Developmental Psychology.
Provenzi, L., Borgatti, R., Menozzi, G., & Montirosso,
R. (2015). A dynamic
system analysis of dyadic flexibility and stability across the
Face- to-
Face Still- Face procedure: Application of the
StateSpace Grid. Infant
Behavior and Development, 38, 1–10.
Putnam, S.P., Helbig, A.L., Gartstein, M.A.,
Rothbart, M.K., & Leerkes, E.
(2014). Development and assessment of short and very short
forms
of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire- Revised. Journal of
Personality
Assessment, 96, 445–458.
Radesky, J.S., Kistin, C.J., Zuckerman, B., Nitzberg,
K., Gross, J., Kaplan-
Sanoff, M., & Silverstein, M. (2014). Patterns of mobile device
use by
caregivers and children during meals in fast food restaurants.
Pediatrics,
133, e843–e849.
| 9 of 9MYRUSKI et al.
Radesky, J., Miller, A.L., Rosenblum, K.L.,
Appugliese, D., Kaciroti,
N., & Lumeng, J.C. (2015). Maternal mobile device
use during a
structured parent–child interaction task. Academic
Pediatrics, 15,
238–244.
Reid, D.J., & Reid, F.J. (2007). Text or talk?
Social anxiety, loneliness, and
divergent preferences for cell phone use. CyberPsychology &
Behavior,
10, 424–435.
Rothbart, M.K., Ziaie, H., & O’Boyle, C.G. (1992).
Self- regulation and emo-
tion in infancy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent
Development, 55,
7–23.
Sapacz, M., Rockman, G., & Clark, J. (2016).
Are we addicted to our cell
phones? Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 153–159.
Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant
communicative be-
havior. In H.R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother–infant
interaction (pp.
227–270). London: Academic Press.
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., &
Brazelton, T.B. (1978). The
infant’s response to entrapment between
contradictory messages
in face- to- face interaction. Journal of the American Academy
of Child
Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.
Tronick, E.Z., Ricks, M., & Cohn, J.F. (1982).
Maternal and infant affective
exchange: Patterns of adaptation. In T. Field& A.
Fogel (Eds.), Emotion
and early interaction (pp. 83–100). London: LEA.
Weinberg,M.K., Beeghly, M., Olson, K.L., &
Tronick, E. (2008). A still- face par-
adigm for young children: 2½ year-olds’reactions to
maternal unavailabil-
ity during the still- face. Journal of Developmental Processes, 3,
4–22.
How to cite this article: Myruski S, Gulyayeva O, Birk S,
Pérez-Edgar K, Buss KA, Dennis-Tiwary TA. Digital
disruption?
Maternal mobile device use is related to infant social-
emotional functioning. Dev Sci. 2018;21:e12610. https://doi.
org/10.1111/desc.12610
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12610
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12610
Copyright of Developmental Science is the property of Wiley-
Blackwell and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for
individual use.

More Related Content

Similar to Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx

EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...
paperpublications3
 
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxRunning title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
agnesdcarey33086
 
RUNNING HEADER COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docx
RUNNING HEADER  COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docxRUNNING HEADER  COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docx
RUNNING HEADER COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docx
agnesdcarey33086
 
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Felicia Nicole Ghrist
 
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON   SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON   SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...
paperpublications3
 
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docxRunning Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
cowinhelen
 
Running head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docx
Running head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docxRunning head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docx
Running head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docx
todd271
 
Vol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docx
Vol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docxVol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docx
Vol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docx
jessiehampson
 
11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Alexander Decker
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
croysierkathey
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
karenahmanny4c
 
Summary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis PaperSummary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis Paper
Bronwyn Thomas
 
This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx
This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docxThis article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx
This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx
christalgrieg
 

Similar to Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx (20)

EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDARY ...
 
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxRunning title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
 
RUNNING HEADER COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docx
RUNNING HEADER  COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docxRUNNING HEADER  COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docx
RUNNING HEADER COURSE PROJECT – INTRODUCTION AND REFERENCES1.docx
 
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
 
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON   SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON   SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...
EFFECT OF PARENTS’ INFLUENCE ON SEXUAL BEHAVIOUR AMONG STUDENTS IN SECONDAR...
 
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
 
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
 
TMA03
TMA03TMA03
TMA03
 
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docxRunning Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
 
Running head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docx
Running head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docxRunning head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docx
Running head CHILD ABUSE Child Abuse Larita SettlesLibe.docx
 
Bully Policy and Prevention
Bully Policy and PreventionBully Policy and Prevention
Bully Policy and Prevention
 
Vol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docx
Vol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docxVol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docx
Vol.(0123456789)1 3J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 492779–279.docx
 
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specifiedPervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
 
11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
11.pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
 
Summary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis PaperSummary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis Paper
 
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
 
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
 
This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx
This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docxThis article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx
This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx
 

More from hcheryl1

Develop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docxDevelop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docx
hcheryl1
 
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docxDevelop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docx
hcheryl1
 
Develop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docx
Develop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docxDevelop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docx
Develop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docx
hcheryl1
 
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docxDevelopmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docx
hcheryl1
 
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docxDevelopmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
hcheryl1
 
Develop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docx
Develop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docxDevelop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docx
Develop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docx
hcheryl1
 
Developmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docx
Developmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docxDevelopmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docx
Developmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docx
hcheryl1
 
Developments New technology that affects policy process • So.docx
Developments New technology that affects policy process • So.docxDevelopments New technology that affects policy process • So.docx
Developments New technology that affects policy process • So.docx
hcheryl1
 
Developmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docx
Developmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docxDevelopmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docx
Developmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docx
hcheryl1
 

More from hcheryl1 (20)

Develop a synopsis of your outcomes for acquiring, developing, train.docx
Develop a synopsis of your outcomes for acquiring, developing, train.docxDevelop a synopsis of your outcomes for acquiring, developing, train.docx
Develop a synopsis of your outcomes for acquiring, developing, train.docx
 
Develop a strategic plan for the company that you selected at the .docx
Develop a strategic plan for the company that you selected at the .docxDevelop a strategic plan for the company that you selected at the .docx
Develop a strategic plan for the company that you selected at the .docx
 
develop a storyboard display. This will be a depiction of a QI proje.docx
develop a storyboard display. This will be a depiction of a QI proje.docxdevelop a storyboard display. This will be a depiction of a QI proje.docx
develop a storyboard display. This will be a depiction of a QI proje.docx
 
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docxDevelop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. Follow th.docx
 
Develop a realistic case and Identify a chronic illness and its .docx
Develop a realistic case and Identify a chronic illness and its .docxDevelop a realistic case and Identify a chronic illness and its .docx
Develop a realistic case and Identify a chronic illness and its .docx
 
Develop a Scholarly written Research Document Report  topic Han.docx
Develop a Scholarly written Research Document Report  topic Han.docxDevelop a Scholarly written Research Document Report  topic Han.docx
Develop a Scholarly written Research Document Report  topic Han.docx
 
Develop a Risk Log (aka Risk Register), identifying ten (10) ris.docx
Develop a Risk Log (aka Risk Register), identifying ten (10) ris.docxDevelop a Risk Log (aka Risk Register), identifying ten (10) ris.docx
Develop a Risk Log (aka Risk Register), identifying ten (10) ris.docx
 
Develop a public relations plan. Make sure to read the chapter on Pr.docx
Develop a public relations plan. Make sure to read the chapter on Pr.docxDevelop a public relations plan. Make sure to read the chapter on Pr.docx
Develop a public relations plan. Make sure to read the chapter on Pr.docx
 
Develop a Resource Audit and a Value System for Tesco. For the.docx
Develop a Resource Audit and a Value System for Tesco. For the.docxDevelop a Resource Audit and a Value System for Tesco. For the.docx
Develop a Resource Audit and a Value System for Tesco. For the.docx
 
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docxDevelop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docx
Develop a research paper on the project charter process. PAR.docx
 
Develop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docx
Develop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docxDevelop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docx
Develop a water sustainability plan for your city, hometown, or ne.docx
 
Develop an 8- to 12-slide Microsoft® PowerPoint® presentat.docx
Develop an 8- to 12-slide Microsoft® PowerPoint® presentat.docxDevelop an 8- to 12-slide Microsoft® PowerPoint® presentat.docx
Develop an 8- to 12-slide Microsoft® PowerPoint® presentat.docx
 
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docxDevelopmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 224–267Contents lists availab.docx
 
Develop a plan to integrate this new data warehouse with an Intern.docx
Develop a plan to integrate this new data warehouse with an Intern.docxDevelop a plan to integrate this new data warehouse with an Intern.docx
Develop a plan to integrate this new data warehouse with an Intern.docx
 
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docxDevelopmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
 
Develop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docx
Develop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docxDevelop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docx
Develop a 6- to 7-page manual using the Security Standards, Po.docx
 
Developmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docx
Developmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docxDevelopmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docx
Developmental and Communication MilestonesOne of the first tas.docx
 
Developments New technology that affects policy process • So.docx
Developments New technology that affects policy process • So.docxDevelopments New technology that affects policy process • So.docx
Developments New technology that affects policy process • So.docx
 
Developmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docx
Developmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docxDevelopmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docx
Developmental Stages Week 7Christina Sierra 1Sub.docx
 
Develop a presentation on the cultural identity with which you align.docx
Develop a presentation on the cultural identity with which you align.docxDevelop a presentation on the cultural identity with which you align.docx
Develop a presentation on the cultural identity with which you align.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
KarakKing
 

Recently uploaded (20)

HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
 
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptxInterdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
 

Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx

  • 1. Developmental Science. 2018;21:e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/desc | 1 of 9 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12610 © 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Received: 17 December 2016 | Accepted: 18 July 2017 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12610 S H O R T R E P O R T Digital disruption? Maternal mobile device use is related to infant social- emotional functioning Sarah Myruski1,2 | Olga Gulyayeva2,4 | Samantha Birk2 | Koraly Pérez-Edgar3 | Kristin A. Buss3 | Tracy A. Dennis-Tiwary1,2 1Department of Psychology, The Graduate Center, The City University of New York, NY, USA 2Department of Psychology, Hunter College, The City University of New York, NY, USA 3Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania StateUniversity, University Park, PA, USA 4Department of Psychology, St John’s University, Queens, NY, USA Correspondence
  • 2. Tracy Dennis-Tiwary, 695 Park Avenue, Department of Psychology, Hunter College, The City University of New York, New York, NY 10065, USA. Email: [email protected] Funding Information This research was made possible by grant R21 MH103627 from the US Department of Health and Human Services of the National Institute of Mental Health Abstract Mobile device use has become increasingly prevalent, yet its impact on infant develop- ment remains largely unknown. When parents use mobile devices in front of infants, the parent is physically present but most likely distracted and unresponsive. Research using the classic Still Face Paradigm (SFP) suggests that parental withdrawal and unre- sponsiveness may have negative consequences for children’s social- emotional devel- opment. In the present study, 50 infants aged 7.20 to 23.60 months (M = 15.40, SD = 4.74) and their mothers completed a modified SFP. The SFP consisted of three phases: free play (FP; parent and infant play and interact), still face (SF; parent withdraws at- tention and becomes unresponsive), and reunion (RU; parent resumes normal interac- tion). The modified SFP incorporated mobile device use in the SF phase. Parents reported on their typical mobile device use and infant temperament. Consistent with the standard SFP, infants showed more negative affect and less positive affect during
  • 3. SF versus FP. Infants also showed more toy engagement and more engagement with mother during FP versus SF and RU. Infants showed the most social bids during SF and more room exploration in SF than RU. More frequent reported mobile device use was associated with less room exploration and positive affect during SF, and less recovery (i.e., engagement with mother, room exploration positive affect) during RU, even when controlling for individual differences in temperament. Findings suggest that the SFP represents a promising theoretical framework for understanding the impact of par- ent’s mobile device use on infant social- emotional functioning and parent–infant interactions. R E S E A R C H H I G H L I G H T S • The traditional Still Face Paradigm (SFP) was modified to include mobile device use, mimicking typical disruptions in parent–infant interactions that may occur in daily life. • Patterns of child behavior during the modified SFP mirrored those of the traditional version, with infants showing the most distress when mothers were disengaged. • Greater habitual self-reported maternal mobile device use was as- sociated with less infant recovery upon reunion. • Findings provide support for the use of this modified paradigm
  • 4. as a framework for understanding the impact of parent’s mobile device use on infant social-emotional functioning and parent–infant interactions. 1 | I N T R O D U C T I O N The exponential increase in mobile device use has transformed not only how we communicate remotely, but also how we engage in www.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/desc mailto:[email protected] 2 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al. face- to- face interactions. Greater mobile device use in adults has been associated with mental health problems, including anxiety (Reid & Reid, 2007; Sapacz, Rockman, & Clark, 2016). However, little is known about the impact of device use on parent–infant interactions. Frequent use of mobile devices during these interactions may de- crease the quality of the social exchange by limiting opportunities for the in- the- moment emotional feedback essential for emotion regula- tion development (Field, 1994). Infant behavior during dyadic interactions can be assessed using
  • 5. the Still Face Paradigm (SFP; Braungart- Rieker et al., 2014; Fuertes, Santos, Beeghly, & Tronick, 2006; Montirosso, Casini et al., 2015; Montirosso, Provenzi et al., 2015; Provenzi, Borgatti, Menozzi, & Montirosso, 2015; Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978), a classic laboratory behavioral task that examines infant responses to social cues by a parent, consisting of three phases: Free Play (FP), Still Face (SF), and Reunion (RU). The FP phase serves as a baseline for par- ent–infant play, while the SF phase disrupts this interaction by making the parent cease initiating or responding to social cues, while main- taining eye gaze. Infant behavior during the SF phase is characterized by decreased positive affect and gaze, and increased negative affect (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Bakermans- Kranenburg, 2009). Maternal regulation of infant emotion is absent during this phase, and when bids for emotional reciprocation are not returned, the infant tends to respond with distress and confusion (Montirosso, Casini et al., 2015; Montirosso, Provenzi et al., 2015; Provenzi et al., 2015; Trevarthen, 1977). Finally, the RU phase provides an opportunity to repair subse- quent mismatches in dyadic behavior by resuming interactive play. The
  • 6. current study created a modified version of the classic SFP by employ- ing a novel SF phase that introduced an ecologically valid mechanism (mobile device use) that in effect may typically make parents unavail- able to infants in daily life. Infant behavior during the SFP is related to broader patterns of emo- tional and social well- being, with greater positive affect and social bids during the SF phase predicting secure attachment (Braungart- Rieker et al., 2014; Fuertes et al., 2006; Kiser, Bates, Maslin, & Bayles, 1986; Tronick, Ricks, & Cohn, 1982). In addition, infant individual differences are related to their behavior during the SFP. For example, infants with greater parent- rated temperamental negative affectivity showed re- duced self- comforting during SF, potentially blunting regulation and re- covery during RU (Braungart- Rieker, Garwood, Powers, & Notaro, 1998; Mesman et al., 2009). In addition, Rothbart, Ziaie, and O’Boyle (1992) found that self- regulatory behaviors during the task were related to in- fant temperament. For example, activity level was negatively related to oral self- soothing, fear scores were positively related to inhibited reach and negatively related to approach, and attention disengagement was
  • 7. negatively related to distress and positively related to positive affect. A largebody of research has also established the SFP as an ana- log for dyadic interactions between a depressed mother and her child (Field, 1994; Field et al., 2007). In one study, the SF phase elicited less distress in infants of depressed mothers compared to controls (Field et al., 2007), potentially since this lack of emotional responsiveness tends to be more habitual in day- to- day interactions for infants of de- pressed mothers. Several studies have modified components of the classic SFP. In one study, modifications included mothers wearing masks while maintaining eye contact and vocal interactions with their children, or drinking from a bottle while maintaining eye contact and a neutral, unresponsive face. Infants only displayed negative affect in response to the traditional still face but not to the modified versions (Legerstee & Markova, 2007), suggesting that infants may have interpreted the mothers’ unresponsiveness differently when tied to a novel behavior. In another study using a modified SF phase in which mothers played with another infant, 6- month- old infants responded with heightened
  • 8. sadness and interest that exceeded that of the traditional SFP (Hart, Carrington, Tronick, & Carroll, 2004). A third study modified the tradi- tional SFP for use with toddlers by using the same three phases, but placing them in the middle of a longer mother–child free play session (Weinberg, Beeghly, Olson, & Tronick, 2008). Toddlers showed similar response patterns as infants, including the classic still face effect, but also exhibited a wider array of responses, including vocalizations that expressed an effort to understand the reason for the mother’s unre- sponsiveness. Thus, Weinberg and colleagues (2008) demonstrated that the SFP can be used to examine behavior across a wider age range than previously examined. Similar to the key components of the classic SFP, parent mobile de- vice use in front of infants causes the parents to be physically present but putatively distracted and unresponsive. While mobile device use is pervasive, only two studies to date have investigated how engage- ment with devices may interfere with parent–child interactions. One study found that during a structured interaction task, maternal mobile device use was common and associatedwith fewer mother–child in-
  • 9. teractions (Radesky et al., 2015). In a descriptive observational study, Radesky and colleagues (2014) found that parents who were deeply absorbed in mobile device use during meal times tended to respond to child bids for attention in insensitive or aggressive ways. However, no study to date has examined how infant social and emotional behavior is influenced by parental device use during dyadic interactions. The first aim of the current study is to establish whether a mod- ified SFP that incorporates maternal mobile device use could serve as an analog to the original SFP, probing the impact of distracted or unresponsive parents on child socioemotional behavior. Specifically, given that mobile device use may mimic the social and emotional dis- engagement present in the classic SFP, we predict that there will be greater negative affect, and less positive affect, during the SF phase, compared to the FP and RU phases. Also, we predict that dyadic interaction will resume upon the RU phase, with infants showing more engagement with mother in the RU phase compared to the SF phase. Second, we aim to examine whether or not maternal device use habits predict individual differences in infant behavior during the SFP. Specifically, based on research using the SFP with
  • 10. infants and depressed mothers (Field et al., 2007), we hypothesize that habitual device use will be associated with less negative responses from infants in the SF phase. That is, mothers who frequently use devices, particularly in front of their family and infants, may habit- ually show a lack of emotional responsiveness during interactions, making their infant more accustomed to such disruptions and thus | 3 of 9MYRUSKI et al. eliciting less distress. Third, based on previous studies suggesting that temperament may make infants more sensitive to the still face disruption (Braungart- Rieker et al., 1998), we predicted that infants high in negative affect would show greater disruption during the SF and less re- engagement during the RU. 2 | M E T H O D 2.1 | Participants Fifty infants (25 female) ages 7.20 to 23.60 months (M = 15.40, SD = 4.74)1 participated in a modified SFP with their mothers. Forty- five (90.0%) parents reported their infant’s ethnicity as
  • 11. White/Non- Hispanic, three (6.0 %) reported Hispanic, three (6.0 %) reported Asian/Pacific Islander, one (2.0 %) reported African- American,and one (2.0 %) reported Native American.All infants were born within 2 weeks of their due date, reported no major health complications, and were within normal birthweight ranges (M = 7.53 lb, SD = 1.14). 2.2 | Materials 2.2.1 | The modified SFP Infants and their mothers participated in a modified SFP (Tronick et al., 1978), which consisted of three phases: a free play phase (FP; 5 minutes), during which mother and infant interacted as they naturally would during play time; a still face phase (SF; 2 minutes), during which an alarm signaled the mother to pick up a mobile device (iPod touch), interact only with the device, withdraw attention from their infant, be- come unresponsive, and allow their infant to play on their own; and a reunion phase (RU; 1 minute; signaled by a knock on a window), during which the mothers stopped using the device and resumed interacting with their infant as they did during FP. This modified SFP altered the
  • 12. protocol of the original SFP by instructing mothers to use a mobile de- vice during SF, allowing infants to move around freely instead of con- fining them to a high chair, and allowing infants to have access to toys throughout the task. We also varied the durations for each of the three SFP phases. A key difference between the modified SFP and the origi- nal SFP is that we did not require mothers to maintain eye contact while avoiding any communication with infants, which was a feature present in the original SFP. These modifications were intended to increase the ecological validity of the SFP by including features that more closely mimicked scenarios that may arise in everyday life. 2.2.2 | Behavioral coding The SFP was video- recorded and scored by three reliable raters. Reliability was computed across 20% of participants using Cohen’s kappa, and ranged from .71 to .98 (M = .85, SD = .03). Presence or ab- sence of each behavior (0, 1) was coded in 15 second epochs. Average scores were computed (number of epochs each behavior was per- formed divided by total number of behaviors) for each phase (FP, SF, RU) to account for individual differences between children who showed
  • 13. generally higher and lower behavioral frequencies overall. Behaviors performed by at least 25% of infants in at least one phase were selected for analyses (negative affect, positive affect, toy engagement, engage- ment with mother, social bid, room exploration; Table 1).2 2.2.3 | Questionnaires Parents self- reported their mobile device use including habitual de- vice use frequency per day [(1) less than 30 min; (2) 1 hour; (3) 1–3 hours; (4) 3–5 hours], use in front of family, and use in front of infant [(1) do not use in presence; (2) less than 30 min; (3) 1 hour; (4) 1–3 hours; (5) 3–5 hours], as well as the total number of communication types used (e. g. texting, email, etc.). Participants also completed the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire Short Form [IBQ- R; (Putnam, Helbig, Gartstein, Rothbart, & Leerkes, 2014); n = 11] or the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire [TBAQ; (Goldsmith, 1996); n = 39]. The IBQ- R is a parent- report measure of infant temperament for ages 4–12 months and consists of 91 items about infant behaviors in the past week on a 7- pointLikert- type scale. The TBAQ is a parent- report measure of infant temperament for ages 12–24 months and
  • 14. consists of 110 items about infant behaviors in the past month on a 7- point Likert- type scale. To examine temperament across the entire age range of the cur- rent sample, within- questionnaire z- scores were computed and IBQ- R and TBAQ subscales were combined. Independent samples t tests found no significant differences in temperament (z- scores) between Infant behavior Description Negative affect Negative expressions or vocalizations; infant protesting, or withdrawn. Must display negative facial expressions. Positive affect Displaying facial expressions of joy particularly smiles or vocalizations with a positive tone. Engagement with toy or other object Playing with the toys provided or in room or playing with non- toy objects, such as the chair. Engagement with mother Playing with or engaging with the parent (except if parent is ignoring infant for the still face phase, then it is a social bid).
  • 15. Social bid Making an attempt to get the attention of the parent physically or vocally, either in a negative way or positive or neutral way. Room exploration Playing with objects around the room or exploring the room in an attention- seeking manner or in a manner designed to not engage the caregiver. TABLE 1 Coding scheme for infant behaviors during the still face paradigm 4 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al. infants whose parents completedthe infant (IBQ) or toddler (TBAQ) version of the questionnaire (ps > .10). Subscales represented on both versions of the questionnaire that captured characteristics relevant to behavior during the SFP were examined: activity level, approach/in- terest, attentional control (duration and orienting from IBQ- R and ap- propriate attentional allocation from TBAQ), and negative affectivity (higher order subscale consisting of fear, sadness, distress to limitation, and falling reactivity).
  • 16. 2.3 | Procedure This study was approved by the institutional review board of the Pennsylvania State University. Parents and infants participated in either one (2.5 hour duration) or two (to prevent infant fatigue; 1.5 hour duration) lab visits, as part of a larger study. Following informed consent, parents completed questionnaires reporting their infant’s temperament and demographics. Participants then completed the modified SFP task with their infant (approximately 8 minutes). The modified SFP task was video- recorded to allow for observation of in- fant behaviors. Participants were compensated with $50 and infants received a T- shirt with the lab logo. 3 | R E S U LT S 3.1 | Descriptive statistics Descriptive statistics for infant behavior during the SFP, maternal de- vice use, and infant temperament are presented in Tables 2–4. Pearson correlations were conducted to examine associations between infant age and sex relative to other study variables. Infant age was normally distributed, and there were an even number of males and females in the sample. There were no differences be- tween males and females in child temperament (ps > .10),
  • 17. maternal mobile device use habits (ps > .10), or infant behaviors during the SFP (ps > .10). Older infants showed more engagement with mother during FP (r = .46, p = .001), and infant age was positively correlated with attentional control (r = .34, p = .02) and approach/interest (r = .42, p = .003). Infant age was not significantly correlated with ma- ternal mobile device use (ps > .10). Although therewere only limited relations between infant age and other measures, due to the wide age range of the sample, age was included as a covariate in subse- quent analyses to control for the potential influence of developmen- tal stage on observed behaviors (e.g., greater mobility, verbal skills in older infants). 3.2 | Analytic plan First, as a manipulation check, we examined within- subject differ- ences in behaviors across the three phases of the SFP to confirm that the parameters of the task elicited varying levels of child behaviors. Second, a series of regressions were conducted to examine the main research question regarding the relationship between maternal device use and infant behavior in the SFP.
  • 18. TABLE 2 Descriptive statistics for SFP behaviors Behavior Min Max Mean (SD) Free play phase Negative affect .00 .80 .07 (.16) Positive affect .00 1.00 .29 (.25) Toy engagement .15 1.00 .92 (.15) Engagement with mother .00 1.00 .68 (.26) Room exploration .00 .95 .44 (.29) Still face phase Negative affect .00 .78 .17 (.24) Positive affect .00 .57 .06 (.12) Toy engagement .20 1.00 .82 (.22) Social bid .00 1.00 .26 (.27) Room exploration .00 1.00 .50 (.31) Reunion phase Negative affect .00 1.00 .10 (.20)
  • 19. Positive affect .00 1.00 .21 (.27) Toy engagement .00 1.00 .70 (.41) Engagement with mother .00 1.00 .63 (.38) Room exploration .00 1.00 .32 (.37) Maternal device use Min Max Mean (SD) How often do you use your device(s)? 1 5 2.74 (.97) Do you use your device(s) in the presence of family members? If so, how often? 2 5 2.92 (.80) Do you use your device(s) in the presence of your baby? If so, how often? 2 4 2.54 (.68) Communication types 8 14 10.34 (1.24) Note. Response scalefor device use frequency (per day) = (1) less than 30 min; (2) 1 hour; (3) 1–3 hours; (4) 3–5 hours; (5) more than 5 hours. Response scale for use in front of family and baby (per day) = (1) do not use in presence; (2) less than 30 min; (3) 1 hour; (4) 1–3 hours; (5) 3–5
  • 20. hours. TABLE 3 Descriptive statistics for maternal device use | 5 of 9MYRUSKI et al. 3.3 | Infant behavior during still face paradigm To examine differences across the phases of the SFP, repeated- measures ANCOVAs were conducted separately for each behavior (negative affect, positive affect, toy engagement, engagement with mother, social bids, room exploration) with age in months as a co- variate, and Phase (FP, SF, RU) as a within- subjects factor (Figure 1). Bonferroni correction was used to control for multiple comparisons in post- hoc paired- samples t tests (adjusted p = .017). Affect: Mirroring the effects of the traditional SFP, infants showed more negative affect during SF versus FP, [t(49) = 2.98, p = .004; F(2, 96) = 5.67, p = .005, ƞp 2= .11]. In contrast, infants showed less positive affect during SF versus FP [t(49) = −7.14, p < .001] and RU [t(49) = −3.85, p < . 001; F(2, 96) = 23.11, p < .001, ƞp 2 = .33].
  • 21. Positive affect was also greater during FP in comparison to RU [t(49) = 2.64, p = .011]. Toy engagement: Infants showed more toy engagement in FP com pared to SF [t(49) = 3.37, p = .001] and RU [t(49) = 2.16, p < .001; F(2, 96) = 11.25, p < .001, ƞp 2 = .19]. Social behaviors: Infants also showed less engagement with mother during SF compared to FP [t(49) = −17.64, p < .001] and RU [t(49) = −11.37, p < .001; F(2, 96) = 117.83, p < .001, ƞp 2 = .71]. Social bids were used more in SF compared to FP [t(49) = 6.76, p < .001] and RU [t(49) = 5.51, p < .001; F(2, 98) = 37.70, p < .001, ƞp 2 = .44]. Exploration: Finally, infants explored the room less in RU compared to SF [t(49) = −3.50, p = .001; F(2, 98) = 6.89, p = .002, ƞp 2 = .13]. 3.4 | Infant temperament and behavior during still face paradigm To identify covariates for regression analyses, infant temperament
  • 22. was examined in relation to mobile device use and child behavior dur- ing the SFP. Pearson correlations revealed that infants with greater parent- reported negative affectivity showed less engagement with mother during FP (r = −.31,p = .03), and less room exploration during RU (r = −.30,p = .03). Infants with greater parent- reported activity level expressed more negative affect during SF (r = .29, p = .04). Infants with greater approach/interest engaged in more room exploration during SF (r = .32, p = .03). 3.5 | Maternal mobile device use and infant behavior during still face paradigm Associations between self- report of maternal mobile device use and behavior during the SFP were examined using linear regressions as follows: 1st step = age, sex, and infant temperament variables (nega- tive affectivity, activity level, approach/interest); 2nd step = maternal device use (separately for mobile device use variable: overall habitual use, use in front of family, use in front of infant); DV = infant behavior during SFP, separately for each phase. Infant temperamental variables were included as covariates since they correlated with infant behavior.
  • 23. The Benjamini- Hochberg correction (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) was applied to correct for multiple comparisons for each phase.3 This procedure, which involves ranking p- values and accounts for the number of tests conducted, was applied separately to each family of regressions (i.e., separately for each SFP phase), since this correction approach assumes independence of samples. All p- values reported below are raw, and were significant using a false discovery rate crite- rion of 0.25 which is recommended for research questions that are a first, relatively exploratory step (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Based on these parameters, raw p- values less than approximately .085 were considered significant. 3.5.1 | Free play phase Maternal mobile device use did not significantly predict infant behav- ior during the SFP (ps > .10). 3.5.2 | Still face phase Greater habitual device use was associated with less room exploration [β = −.39,t(49) = −2.70, p = .01; Figure 2], and more frequent device Temperament
  • 24. Questionnaire Subscale Measure used N Min Max Mean (SD) Attentional control z- Score 50 −1.87 1.80 – IBQ 11 2.25 5.27 3.49 (0.99) TBAQ 39 2.13 5.31 3.77(0.88) Activity level z- Score 50 −2.10 2.49 – IBQ 11 3.93 5.67 4.67 (0.62) TBAQ 39 2.67 6.10 4.24 (0.75) Approach/interest z- Score 50 −1.94 1.68 – IBQ 11 3.70 6.75 5.27(1.03) TBAQ 39 1.60 5.00 3.42(0.94) Negative affectivity z- Score 50 −1.71 3.06 – IBQ 11 3.26 4.56 3.93(0.44) TBAQ 39 2.16 5.03 3.26(0.66) TABLE 4 Descriptive Statistics for Temperament Questionnaire Subscales 6 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al. use in front of infant was associated with less positive affect [β = −.35, t(49) = −2.37, p = .022].
  • 25. 3.5.3 | Reunion phase Greater habitual device use [β = −.38,t(49) = −2.55, p = .014], and more specifically greater use in front of infant [β = −.29, t(49) = −2.05, p = .047], was associatedwith less roomexploration. Greater habitual device use [β = −.30,t(49) = −1.96, p = .057], as well as use in front of infant [β = −.29,t(49) = −1.95, p = .057], was marginally associated with less positive affect. Greater habitual device use [β = −.36,t(49) = −2.54, p = .015;Figure 3], more use in front of family [β = −.25,t(49) = −1.79, p = .081], as well as more use in front of infant [β = −.28,t(49) = −2.03, p = .048], was associatedwith less engagement with mother during this phase. 4 | D I S C U S S I O N Since infants tend to respond to maternal unresponsiveness during the SF phase with distress (Trevarthen, 1977), the SF phase is char- acterized by a decrease in positive affect and an increase in negative affect (Mesman et al., 2009). Consistent with the extant literature, the modified SFP using a mobile device in the current study produced
  • 26. robust differential patterns of infant behavior between the three phases. Infants expressed increased negative affect in the SF versus FP, as well as decreased positive affect and engagement with mother in SF versus both FP and RU. Infants also increased social bids dur- ing the SF in an attempt to obtain their caregiver’s attention. Infants displayed more toy engagement during FP versus the other phases, likely due to the fact that infants were adjusting to the new environ- ment and parents aided in toy engagement when they were available to interact, whereas during RU infants were preoccupied with re- engaging with the parent and they lacked scaffolding for play during SF. Finally, infants explored the room less in RU compared to SF, pos- sibly because they were more focused on reuniting with their mother following the SF phase. In fact, as predicted, infant engagement with mother increased significantly between the SF and RU phases. Two- way engagement was greater in FP and RU when the rules of the task allowed for it, whereas social bids were greater when the parent was unavailable during SF. These observed patterns illustrate that the modified SFP may act as a potentially analogous paradigm to the origi- nal SFP and can be used to understand the implications of
  • 27. maternal device use on infant social- emotional functioning. Results of the current study also indicated that individual differ- ences in infant temperament contribute to behavior during the SFP phases. Infants with higher parent- reported temperamental negative affectivity engaged less with their caregiver during FP. During RU, in- fants with higher negative affectivity displayed less room exploration, indicating less recovery. This is consistent with findings that infants high in negative affectivity showed less emotion regulation during SF, blunting recovery during RU (Braungart- Rieker et al., 1998; Mesman et al., 2009). During SF, infants with greater activity level scores showed more negative affect. It is possible that it is more challenging for more active infants to deal with their nonresponsive parent, result- ing in increased negative affect. Finally, infants with greater approach/ interest scores showed more room exploration during SF. Increased approach and interest scores may be related to decreased fear (Buss, 2011; Rothbart et al., 1992) and increased curiosity about the environ- ment, resulting in increased room exploration. An important goal of the current study was to
  • 28. examine the impact of maternal device use on infant emotion regulation and parent–in- fant interactions. Although previous research suggests that physical FIGURE 1 Infant behavior significantly differed across the three phases of the still face paradigm. Notably, the still face phase elicited greater negative affect, but less positive affect Ne ga tiv e A ffe ct Po sit ive A ffe ct To y E ng ag
  • 30. tio n 0.0 0.5 1.0 Child Behavior by during the Still Face Paradigm Fr eq ue nc y Still Face ReunionFreeplay ** * *** *** * *** ****** ****** *
  • 31. ** FIGURE 2 Greater parental device use predicts less infant room exploration during the still face phase, while controlling for infant temperament | 7 of 9MYRUSKI et al. and emotional unavailability may decrease the quality of the social exchange (Field, 1994), no study has examined the effect of maternal device use on infant behavior and emotion regulation. The results of the current study suggest that greater maternal mobile use is associ- ated with behavior patterns across the SFP phases, even when infant temperamental traits are taken into account. Contrary to predictions, infants of mothers who frequently used devices did not show less neg- ative affect, or more positive affect, during the SF phase. We did note decreased room exploration during SF and RU as a function of greater habitual use, indicating that these infants were not unaffected by the disruption. These findings may suggest that parental habitual device use may be associatedwith an infant’s ability to adjust to their envi-
  • 32. ronment and highlight the need for future research in this area. Importantly, during RU, greater habitual device use was associated with less positive affect, less engagement with mother, and less room exploration. The RU phase is crucial in that it provides an opportunity for the parent and infant to reconnect. However, the current study showed that with greater habitual device use, the reunion between mother and infant was not as successful. Consistent with previous re- search showing that greater parental mobile device use was related to less interaction with children (Radesky et al., 2015), and insensitive or aggressive parent responses to social bids (Radesky et al., 2014), the current findings suggest that frequent habitual device use may reduce the successful repair of interactions following disruptions. These results highlight the importance of research surrounding parental device use and its impact on both infant emotional regulation development and the quality of social exchange in parent–infant interactions. The modified SFP represents a promising theoretical framework for this research. Although the results from the current study confirm the find- ings that parental mobile device use is associated with infant
  • 33. social- emotional functioning and parent–infant interactions, somelimitations should be noted. First, the age range included in the current study was relatively wide, encompassing developmental stages with vary- ing levels of mobility, language ability, and understanding of others’ intentions. For these reasons, age was included as a covariate in all main analyses. Age did not significantly alter the pattern of results when examining either infant behaviors across stages or relations be- tween maternal device use and infant behavior. Although the current findings represent a crucial starting point and previous studies have illustrated that the SFP can be used across a wide age range (Weinberg et al., 2008), future studies must investigate smaller age ranges and/ or track changes longitudinally across early childhood to more con- cretely understand when and how maternal device use impacts social- emotional development. Also, the current study lacked a comparison group or comparison condition exposed to the classic SFP at the same age. Future studies should aim to address this gap to establish if the two versions elicit similar patterns of behavior in the same child.
  • 34. In addition, contrary to the classic SFP, in the modified SFP with a mobile device, toys were freely available, infant mobility was not restricted, and mothers were not instructed to maintain eye contact during SF. Importantly, these variations included in the modified SFP more closely resemble real- life parent–child interactions involving disruptions in social- emotional communication, thus increasing the ecological validity of this paradigm. Due to these differences, the cur- rent SF may have elicited overall lower levels of negative affect versus other behaviors as compared to the classic SF (Braungart- Rieker et al., 2014; Fuertes et al., 2006; Montirosso, Casini et al., 2015; Montirosso, Provenzi et al., 2015; Provenzi et al., 2015). This difference in negative affect frequency could also be due to differences in coding methods used (e.g., 30 second epochs versus second- by- second microanalysis). It also may be that infants are more accustomed to disruptions due to mobile device use and thus were not as distressed as they would be by the classic SFP. These subtle, low- level expressions of negative affect might be common among children whose parents habitually and fre- quently use mobile devices, and future research should examine
  • 35. their function, such as serving to re- engage the parent or express distress. An additional limitation is that the response scalefor parental mo- bile device use only went from (1) less than 30 minutes per day to (5) FIGURE 3 Greater parent device use was associated with less infant positive affect (left) and engagement with mother (right) during the reunion phase, while controlling for temperament, suggesting reduced recovery following disruption in parent–infant interaction 8 of 9 | MYRUSKI et al. greater than 5 hours per day for three contexts: general device use frequency, use in front of family, and use in front of their infant. Future research should track device use in real time or complete in- home be- havioral observations to observe how often parents use their device and how the infant reacts. This would provide a better understanding of how parent device use and how familiarity with a device may impact an infant’s responses. It would also be beneficial to trackinfant social- emotional functioning over time to determine whether device use has
  • 36. a long- term impact on emotion regulation development. Finally, it is important to note that the durations of the three phases were altered from the original SFP. Similar to previous studies (Weinberg et al., 2008), an extended free play phase was included to allow dyads enough time to adjust to the room in which they had the freedom to move at will, in contrast to the classic SFP. However, the most notable shortcoming of the current SFP design was that the re- union phase was only 1 minute long. Despite this short time- period, infants exhibited patterns of behavior suggesting dyadic interaction repair (e.g., increase in engagement with mother), as well as significant individual differences in recovery behaviors, which related to mobile device use habits. This suggests that, while this short RU phase may not be ideal, it was sufficient to detect notable patterns of infant be- havior and lays the groundwork for subsequent investigations. Future studies should extend this phase to observe recovery over a longer pe- riod, and track individual differences in recovery trajectories in relation to patterns of daily device use. The modified SFP used in the current study may represent a fruitful method for examining the use of digital devices
  • 37. in parent–child rela- tionships in a controlled, yet ecologically valid manner. Taken together, results suggest that parental device use influences the quality of par- ent–infant interactions, and highlight the need for continued research on the role of technology in infant social- emotional development. E N D N O T E S 1 The current sample was a subsample taken from a larger study examin- ing broader patterns of socioemotional functioning in the first 2 years of life (LoBue, Buss, Taber- Thomas, & Pérez- Edgar, 2017; Morales et al., 2017; Pérez-Edgar, Morales, LoBue, Taber-Thomas, Allen, Brown, & Buss, in press). 2 The distributions for some of the observed behaviors were significantly positively or negatively skewed. However, these skewed behaviors would be expected to be used either extremely frequently or infre- quently due to the nature of the paradigm (e.g., task parameters would not be expected to elicit social bids from children during free play or reunion). These skewed behaviors were included in the repeated- measures analyses only, in order to track differences in behavior fre-
  • 38. quency across the three phases. 3 Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was used in the analyses of covariance reported above, since Benjamini- Hochberg is not recommended for within- subject tests (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). R E F E R E N C E S Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg,Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300. Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M.M., Powers, B.P., & Notaro, P.C. (1998). Infant affect and affect regulation during the still- face paradigm with mothers and fathers: The role of infant characteristics and parental sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1428. Braungart-Rieker, J.M.,Zentall, S., Lickenbrock, D.M., Ekas, N.V., Oshio, T., & Planalp, E. (2014). Attachment in the making: Mother and father sen- sitivity and infants’ responses during the still- face paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125, 63–84. Buss, K.A. (2011). Which fearful toddlers should we worry about? Context, fear regulation, and anxiety risk. Developmental Psychology, 47, 804–819.
  • 39. Field, T. (1994). The effects of mother’s physical and emotional unavail- ability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 208–227. Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., Diego, M., Feijo, L., Vera, Y., Gil, K., & Sanders, C. (2007). Still- face and separationeffects on depressed mother–in- fant interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28, 314–323. Fuertes, M., Santos, P.L.d., Beeghly, M., & Tronick, E. (2006). More than ma- ternal sensitivity shapes attachment. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 292–296. Goldsmith, H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire. Child Development, 67, 218–235. Hart, S.L., Carrington, H.A., Tronick, E., & Carroll, S.R. (2004). When infants lose exclusive maternal attention: Is it jealousy? Infancy, 6, 57–78. Kiser, L.J., Bates, J.E., Maslin, C.A., & Bayles, K. (1986). Mother–infant play at six months as a predictor of attachment security at thirteen months. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25,
  • 40. 68–75. Legerstee, M., & Markova, G. (2007). Intentions make a difference: Infant responses to still- face and modified still- face conditions. Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 232–250. LoBue, V., Buss, K.A., Taber-Thomas, B.C., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2017). Developmental differences in infants’ attention to social and nonsocial threats. Infancy, 22, 403–415. Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Bakermans- Kranenburg, M.J. (2009). The many facesof the still- face paradigm: A review and meta- analysis. Developmental Review, 29, 120–162. Montirosso, R., Casini, E., Provenzi, L., Putnam, S.P., Morandi, F., Fedeli, C., & Borgatti, R. (2015). A categorical approach to infants’ individual differ- ences during the still- face paradigm. Infant Behavior and Development, 38, 67–76. Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Tavian, D., Morandi, F., Bonanomi, A., Missaglia, S., & Borgatti, R. (2015). Social stress regulation in 4- month- old infants: Contribution of maternal social engagement and infants’ 5- HTTLPR genotype. Early Human Development, 91, 173–179.
  • 41. Morales, S., Brown, K.M., Taber-Thomas, B.C., LoBue, V., Buss, K.A., & Pérez-Edgar, K.E. (2017). Maternal anxiety predicts attentional bias towards threat in infancy. Emotion (Washington, DC), 17, 874–883. Pérez-Edgar, K., Morales, S., LoBue, V., Taber- Thomas, B.C., Allen, E.K., Brown, K.M., & Buss, K.A. (in press). The impact of negative affect on attention patterns to threat across the first two years of life. Developmental Psychology. Provenzi, L., Borgatti, R., Menozzi, G., & Montirosso, R. (2015). A dynamic system analysis of dyadic flexibility and stability across the Face- to- Face Still- Face procedure: Application of the StateSpace Grid. Infant Behavior and Development, 38, 1–10. Putnam, S.P., Helbig, A.L., Gartstein, M.A., Rothbart, M.K., & Leerkes, E. (2014). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire- Revised. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96, 445–458. Radesky, J.S., Kistin, C.J., Zuckerman, B., Nitzberg, K., Gross, J., Kaplan- Sanoff, M., & Silverstein, M. (2014). Patterns of mobile device use by caregivers and children during meals in fast food restaurants. Pediatrics,
  • 42. 133, e843–e849. | 9 of 9MYRUSKI et al. Radesky, J., Miller, A.L., Rosenblum, K.L., Appugliese, D., Kaciroti, N., & Lumeng, J.C. (2015). Maternal mobile device use during a structured parent–child interaction task. Academic Pediatrics, 15, 238–244. Reid, D.J., & Reid, F.J. (2007). Text or talk? Social anxiety, loneliness, and divergent preferences for cell phone use. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10, 424–435. Rothbart, M.K., Ziaie, H., & O’Boyle, C.G. (1992). Self- regulation and emo- tion in infancy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 55, 7–23. Sapacz, M., Rockman, G., & Clark, J. (2016). Are we addicted to our cell phones? Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 153–159. Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative be- havior. In H.R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 227–270). London: Academic Press.
  • 43. Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T.B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face- to- face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13. Tronick, E.Z., Ricks, M., & Cohn, J.F. (1982). Maternal and infant affective exchange: Patterns of adaptation. In T. Field& A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction (pp. 83–100). London: LEA. Weinberg,M.K., Beeghly, M., Olson, K.L., & Tronick, E. (2008). A still- face par- adigm for young children: 2½ year-olds’reactions to maternal unavailabil- ity during the still- face. Journal of Developmental Processes, 3, 4–22. How to cite this article: Myruski S, Gulyayeva O, Birk S, Pérez-Edgar K, Buss KA, Dennis-Tiwary TA. Digital disruption? Maternal mobile device use is related to infant social- emotional functioning. Dev Sci. 2018;21:e12610. https://doi. org/10.1111/desc.12610 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12610 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12610 Copyright of Developmental Science is the property of Wiley- Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
  • 44. without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.