SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 95
Title:
The perils of merit pay: linking teacher pay to performance
can't move forward until resolution of questions regarding
fairness, teacher evaluation, and the relationship of test scores
to teaching quality
Author(s):
Thomas Toch
Source:
Phi Delta Kappan. 91.2 (Oct. 2009): p99.
Document Type:
Article
Copyright:
COPYRIGHT 2009 Phi Delta Kappa, Inc.
Full Text:
The U.S. Senate wants the federal government to make $300
million in grants next year to spur a movement to link teacher
pay to performance. The House wants to spend $445 million on
the effort. And the Obama Administration wants to up the ante
to $487 million in appropriations and another $200 million in
stimulus spending. Secretary of Education Arne Duncan
recently called performance pay "my highest priority."
Such investments make sense. The long tradition in public
education of paying teachers mainly on the basis of the college
credits they've amassed and the years they've taught results in
bad teachers earning the same as good ones--making it
seemingly tougher to recruit and retain talent in public schools
at a time when research suggests that teacher quality is critical
to raising student achievement.
But if the logic of performance pay for teachers is compelling,
reformers have been trying unsuccessfully for decades to
introduce the practice in public education, practically since the
concept of a salary system based exclusively on credentials and
seniority gained favor in the 1920s as a way to counter the
favoritism and other inequities that plagued teacher pay at the
time. "Every effort must be made to devise ways to reward
teachers according to their ability without opening the school
door to unfair personnel practices," reformers warned President
Eisenhower in 1955. So if Duncan and today's school reformers
want to bring performance pay to the public school teaching
profession, they'll have to avoid their predecessors' mistakes.
The last, and largest, push for performance pay came in the
1980s. Ted Bell, Ronald Reagan's secretary of education,
protested in 1981 that public education's single salary schedule
"demands that we pay the worst at the level of the best if we
want to pay the best what they are worth." Two years later, a
Bell-appointed national commission on school reform
recommended performance pay in A Nation at Risk, its stinging
indictment of public education. The result was new reward
systems for teachers in Tennessee, California, Florida, Texas,
New Jersey, and a host of other school systems. But those pay
reforms didn't last long.
Teacher unions attacked merit pay, as President Reagan was
fond of calling the reform, largely because it violated the
collectivism at the heart of the industrial-style unionism in
public education. But the teacher pay experiments of the 1980s
also failed because they were, at bottom, capricious. In many
instances, they offered teachers the possibility of small
additional amounts of money, not enough to mean much to those
who got it, but just enough to irk those who didn't. The number
of teachers receiving the rewards shifted with annual
appropriations, regardless of how many teachers were rated high
enough to receive them. And the eligibility standards for the
rewards were frequently left to the whims of principals.
The absence of credible systems of evaluating teachers'
performance remains a major barrier to performance pay today,
no less of a barrier than continued union resistance to tying pay
to performance. The typical teacher evaluation in public
education consists of a single, fleeting classroom visit by a
harried principal untrained in evaluation who is often more
interested in classroom comportment than the quality of
instruction. The result is statistics like those in Chicago, where
the nonprofit New Teacher Project found that 88% of the city's
600 schools did not issue a single "unsatisfactory" teacher
rating between 2003 and 2006.
The Obama Administration and the Bill & Melinda Gates
Foundation, which is planning to spend upwards of $500 million
on performance pay and other teaching reforms, want to use
student test scores in teacher evaluations, a step the New York
Times endorsed in a recent editorial. There's a clear logic in
giving student test scores a role in teacher evaluations: It's
inexpensive and easy to administer and seemingly measures
what matters most--student achievement.
Why Not Test Scores?
But test scores aren't the simple solution they seem to be. Less
than half of public school teachers teach the subjects or grade
levels in which students are tested, eliminating the prospect of a
system that's applied fairly to all teachers. Most standardized
tests today measure only a narrow band of mostly low-level
skills, such as recalling or restating facts, rather than the ability
to analyze information and other advanced skills. As a result,
the tests tend to privilege low-level pedagogy, leaving the best
teachers, those with wider teaching repertoires and the ability to
move students beyond the basics, at a disadvantage.
And then there's the daunting challenge of separating out
individual teachers' impact on their students' reading and math
scores from the myriad other influences on student achievement
and the difficulty of drawing the right conclusions about teacher
performance from very small numbers of student test scores, a
particular challenge in elementary schools, where teachers work
with a single classroom's worth of students most of the day.
As a result, student test scores should play a supporting rather
than a lead role in teacher evaluations, and any credible
performance pay plan is going to require more effective
evaluations of teachers in classrooms--multiple evaluations by
multiple evaluators and based on multiple indicators of how
well teachers plan, teach, test, manage, and motivate.
Comprehensive evaluations are especially important for making
key decisions, such as granting teachers tenure, and for making
judgments about the majority of teachers who are neither the
very best nor the very worst. And they're key to winning teacher
support of performance pay. In surveys, only a tiny fraction of
teachers are willing to have student test scores play any role in
pay levels. But their opposition to performance pay drops
significantly when ratings are based on comprehensive
evaluations. Encouragingly, the Gates Foundation is planning to
make the evaluation issue a cornerstone of its teacher-reform
work.
Because there have been so few sustained performance pay
systems in public education over the years, we don't know for
sure whether the reform would indeed improve the teaching
profession. James Guthrie and Patrick Schuermann at the
National Center on Performance Incentives in Education at
Vanderbilt's Peabody College, an enterprise funded by the
federal government during the George W. Bush Administration
to study performance pay, warn that there's not yet conclusive
evidence on "the power of financial awards in promoting more
effective teaching and elevating student performance" or on "the
long-term effect of performance awards on the supply of
effective teachers." Nor, they write, do we know the "effects of
group awards relative to individual performance" or the
"preferable mix of financial and nonpecuniary awards"--
important secondary questions.
We need to answer these big questions about performance pay
rather than assume that answers already exist. We know from
surveys, for example, that bigger salaries are less important to
teachers than professional work environments where they feel
supported and are helped to become better at their craft. A 2007
national survey of teachers by the nonprofits Public Agenda and
the National Comprehensive Center for Teacher Quality found
that, if given a choice between two otherwise identical schools,
76% of secondary teachers and 81% of elementary teachers
would rather be at a school where administrators supported
teachers strongly than at a school that paid significantly higher
salaries.
Performance pay may be one tool with which to create a more
professional culture in public school teaching, but it is no more
than that.
THOMAS TOCH is executive director of the Association of
Independent Schools of Greater Washington and a former guest
scholar at the Brookings Institution.
Toch, Thomas
Source Citation (MLA 7th Edition)
Toch, Thomas. "The perils of merit pay: linking teacher pay to
performance can't move forward until resolution of questions
regarding fairness, teacher evaluation, and the relationship of
test scores to teaching quality." Phi Delta Kappan 91.2 (2009):
99+. Academic OneFile. Web. 12 June 2014.
Document URL
http://go.galegroup.com/ps/i.do?id=GALE%7CA210599572&v=
2.1&u=tel_a_ttul&it=r&p=AONE&sw=w&asid=7f62baa2c30422
39e4e1943181d02c70
Gale Document Number: GALE|A210599572
ED 343 191 CS 507 779
AUTROR Kearney, Patricia; And Others
TITLE What Students Don't Like about What Teachers Say and
Do.
PUB DATE Nov 91
NOTE 44p.; Revised version of a paper presented at the
Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication
Association (77th, Atlanta, GA, October 31 -Nomemster
3, 1991).
PUB TIPE Speeches/Conference Papers (150)
EDRS PRICE MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage.
DESCRIPTORS *Behavior Problems; *Classroom
Communication;
*Classroom Environment; Classroom Research; Factor
Analysis; Higher Education; *Student Eialuation of
Teacher Performance; Student Reaction; *Teacher
Behavior; Teacher Characteristics; *Teacher Student
Relationship; Undergraduate Students
IDENTIFIERS *Communication Behavior: Communication
Patterns
ABSTRACT
To change the way classroom discipline and student
discipline are examined, a two-study investigation, rather than
focustLg on student noncompliance and other types of student
misbehaviors, examined teachers themselves as potential
sources of
instructional and/or motivational problems in the college
classroom.
The first study was designed to elicit inductively college
student
reports of teacher misbehaviors. Participants were 254
undergraduates
at a large Western university who were enrolled in two sections
of a
course on interpersonal communication. Responses to an open-
ended
questionnaire indicated 28 different categories of teacher
misbehaviors. The second study was structured to validate the
obtained categories of teacher misbehavior types and to
deternine
whether or not a conceptually meaningful factor structure
underlies
the categories. Even though most students (subjects were 261
undergraduates enrolled in introductory communication classes
at a
large Western university) reported that the teachers referenced
for
this study infrequently engaged in each misbehavtor type, a
representative number of other teachers did. Importantly, the
full
range of frequencies was obtained across all 28 categories.
Results
were further collaborated with qualitative data. Factor analyses
and
factor matching procedures revealed that the teacher
misbehavior
categories could be both meaningfully and reliably reduced to
three
factors: teacher incompetence, offensiveness, and indolence.
Recommendations include that teachers examine the list of 28
behavior
categories in light of their own classroom behaviors. (Four
tables of
data and 24 references are included.) (SG)
*M**************************************************
*******************
* Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be
made
from the original document.
*****************************************************
******************
"FERMISSMN TO REPRODUCE THIS
MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
1.2)11Aile
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC).-
U.& ØIEMIIIWIT OF ESUCITION
Me fit Educeaorel teasesett and tagrameaset
EDUCATIONALAIMPOORMATtOte
0 The docatase ass low repooduced se
teamed tam las sewn or ageassetton
orynatteg
0 Mews changes %see teen mad* to taterove
reptedectoon Seats
Poets of woo or wantons stated la this deco-
meet do not enemata, rep/mem °noel
OM Position Or mac,
Teacher Misbehavior
UMW STIMENTS
DON'T LIKE ABOUT MAT WADERS SAY AIM DO
*Patricia Kearney Timothy G. Plax
Ellis R. Hays Marilyn J. Ivey
Running Head: TEACHER MISBEHAVIOR
*Patricia Kearney (Ed.D., West Virginia University, 1979),
Timothy G. Plax (Ph.D., University of Southern California,
1974)
and Ellis R. Hays (Ph.D., Purdue University, 1967) are
Professors
and Marilyn J. Ivey (B.A., California State University, Long
Beach, 1999) is a graduate student in the Department of
Spiffech
Communication at California State University, Long Beach,
1250
Bellflower Blvd., Long Beach, CA 90940-2407 (714-897-5262).
This research was funded, in part, by a Research Assigned Time
grant from California State University, Long Beach.
An initial analysis of the data reported in Study 1 should also
appear in the M.A. thesis of Marilyn J. Ivey under the direction
of Timothy G. Plax.
An earlier version of this paper was presented on the Top 3
research report panel of the Instructional and Developmental
Division of the Spewch Communication Association, Atlanta,
1991.
2 BEST COPYAMIE
Teacher Misbehavior
a
. WHAT STUDENTS
DON'T LIKE MOOT WHAT TEACHERS SAY AND 00
Abstract
This investigation represents a substantial change in the
way vaL examine classroom discipline and student resistance.
Rather than focusing on student non-compliance and other types
of
student misbehaviors, we examined teachers themselves as
pote tial sources of instructional and/or motivational problems
in the college classroom. Study I was designed to elicit
inductively, college student reports of teacher misbehaviors.
Results indicated 28 different categories of teacher
misbehavinrs. Study 2 was structured to (1) validate the
obtained categories of teacher misbehirvior types and (2) to
determine whether or not a conceptually meaningful, factor
structure underlies the categories. Even though most students
reported that the teachers referenced in study 2 infrequently
engaged in each misbehavior type, a represents ive number of
other teachers did: Importantly, the full range of frequencies
was obtained across all 28 categories. Results were further
corroborated with qualitative data. Factor analyses and factor
matching procedures revealed that the teacher misbehavior
categories could be both meaningfully and reliably reduced to 3
factorst Teacher Incompetence, Offensiveness and Indolence.
Implications for managing student resistance in the classroom
are
discussed.
4 4
Teacher Misbehavior
3
WHAT SIMMS
DOMPT LIKE ABOUT WHAT TEACHERSLOAV AMD DO
A large body of literature examines students as instigators
of a variety of problems for the classroom teacher (see, for
instance, Doyle's 1986 review). Students are frequently accused
of talking out-of-turn, disrupting teacher talk, not paying
attention and a whole host of other classroom misbehaviors. As
a
result, a great deal of attention has been focused on the causes
of student disruptions and the intervention strategies that
teachers can employ to handle these misbehaviors.
Unfortunately,
the research and advice offered in this tradition '2ften overlooks
teachers themselves as a potential source of problems in the
classroom. Not surprisingly, this oversight might be anticipated
from researchers who happen to be teachers as well. In this
study, we depart from that tradition by assuming that (I)
teachers themselves may "misbehave" and (2) these
misbehaviors
can become potential sources
resistance.
Conceptually,
of student dissatisfaction and
student misbehaviors are defined as those
student behaviors that interfere with learning (c.f., Kearney,
Plax, Richmond, & McCroskey, 1984). Similarly, we define
teacher
misbehaviors as those teacher behaviors that interfere with
instruction and thus, learning. Repeatedly letting students out
of class early, failing to keep office hours, returning papers
late, providing nonspecific evaluations on homework
assignments,
making the test too hard (or too easy), or delivering humorless,
4
LI
Teacher Misbehavior
4
monotonous lectures all interfere with our ability to teach
effectively and thus, can all be classified as teacher
misbehaviors (Plax IL Kearney, 199(70). In this paper) we
argue
that these and other teacher misbehaviors can influence the way
students think and act.
A large body of literature substantiates a relationship
between what teachers say and do with students' behaviors.
In the tradition of the process/product paradigm, research-
based conclusions about those specific teacher behaviors that
influence student achievement, feedback, time speAt on-task,
classroom order, student affect, good work habits, social skills,
independence and other outcome variables are reviewed
elsewhere
(Brophy ti Good, 1986; Sage I Needels, 1989; Good Is Brophy,
1986;
Rosenshine Si Stevens, 1986). More recently, research on
students' thought processes emphas.zes the critical role that
students' perceptions of what teachers say and do play in
influencing students' motivation, achievement, attitudes and
related student reactions. In other words, what teachers do
influence students' thinking. That thinking, in turn, mediates
student behavior (Wittrock, 1986). Following from this
mediational perspective then, we might expect teacher
misbehaviors to indirectly affect student* behavior by
influencing how students think about and act towards the
teacher,
school and themselves.
Because we know that what teachers say and do can
significantly affect halt students think and behave, we might
Teacher Misbehavior
5
expect teacher misbehaviors to act as potential antecedents to a
number of undesirable student consequet.t1;. In other words,
teacher misbehaviors may be a primary, albeit indirect,
determinant of student disruptions. This report describes two
studies identifying ways that tlachers themselves may contribute
to the occurrence of problems in the classroom. Study 1 was
structured to elicit inductively- college student reports of
teacher misbehaviors. Study 2 was designed to validate the
obtained categories of teacher misbehavior types and to
determine
whether or not a conceptually meaningful factor structure
underlies the categories. The research and thinking on
classroom
management and student resistance provide the rationale for this
investigation.
Classmaa Management
Within the classroom management perspective the primary
responsibility for classroom control and student engagement lies
not with the student, but with the teacher. Instead of
highlighting student misbehavior problems, this alternative
advocates a preventative stance toward discipline. The appeal of
classroom management has its -oots in a line of research which
demonstrates that the sifulle best predictor of learning is simply
"academic engagement time" (Woolfolk & McCune-Nicolich,
1994;
Woolfolk, 1987). No matter what instrucUonal strategies or
methods are used, the teacher who keeps hr.:T./his students
actively
involved in the learning process is more likely to be effective
(Woolfolk & McCune-Nicolich, 1984, p. 442).
a
Teacher Misbehavior
This fundamental principle has led a number of researchers
to identify those teacher behaviors which influence students'
time spent on task (Emmer, Evertson, Sanford, Clements, &
Worsham, 1984; Evertson, Emmer, Clements, Sanford, 8
Worsham,
1984). Based on classroom observations of elementary and
secondary instruction, Emmer et al. (1984) and Evertson et al.
(1984) differentiate effective from ineffective classroom
managers. These researchers report that good managers
regularly
rely on positive questioning techniques and motivational
messages
(cues and prompts), attend more often to positive than negative
student behaviors, provide students with good role models, give
frequent and specific feedback, hold students accountable, and
plan su,:cess-oriented learning experiences. The end result is
that effective classroom managers increase studem.s' time spent
on task (Brophy & Evertson, 1976; Cantrell, Stenner, &
Katzenmeyer, 1977; Emmer et al., 1984; Evertson et al., 1984).
Consistent with the classroom management perspective,
instructional communication researchers argue that managing
students successfully also requires that we "pErrreget" our
students that learning is important, enjoyable and beneficial to
their overall well-being (c.f., Kearney, 1987; Plax fis Kearney,
1990). In response to the need to identify those communication
strategies which contribute to teacher influence in the
classroom, an initial series of seven "studies was designed that
isolated and validated 22 separate behavior alteration
techn.ques
and representative, sample messages for classroom use (c.f.,
7
Teacher Oftebehavtar
7
Kearney, Plax, Richmond, & McCroskey, 1984, 1905). The
results
of these and subsequent investigations in the same program of
research (c.f., Kearney, Plax, Smith, & Sorensen, 19618;
Kearney,
Plax, Sorensen, & Smith, 1988) indicate that both teachers and
students readily agree on the preferred use of prosocial or
reward-oriented, as opposed to antisocial or punishment-based,
influence techniques. That is, teachers perceived them to be
useful in managing students' behavior and, in turn, students
reported that they enjoy the class and learn more content when
their teachers rely on prosocial means of influence.
With rare exception the classroom management behaviors and
strategies reported in the educational and communication
literature are success-oriented or prosocial. The converse or
absence of those behaviors would seem to contribute negatively
to
students' involvement with learning. An overview of recent
research on student resistance supports and extends that
position.
Student Resistance
Rather than attend solely to what teachers strategically
communicate in their efforts to manage or influence students,
Burroughs, Kearney and Plax (1989) acknowledged the role of
the
student in the teacher/student exchange. Experienced teachers
recognize that students often fail to concede the teacher's right
to assume a power role. Moreover, a number of students may be
reluctant or openly defiant, to assume their expected role of
conciliation, cooperation and submission. In an effort to
Teacher Mambehavior
isolate those strategies college students might use to resist
teachers' influence or compliance-gaining attempts, Burroughs
et
al. (1989) asked students to construct messages they would use
ti
resist their teachers in the classroom. Nineteen separate
categories of techniques and messages wure identified in that
research.
In a follow-up study, Kearney, Plax and Burroughs (in press)
validated the 19 categories and explicated two theoretically
meaningful dimensions underlying the resistance categories:
Teacher-Owned and Student-Owned. In explanation, problem-
ownership refers to the degree to which the problem apparently
originates with the student or the teacher. Kearney et al. (in
press) reasoned that students blame two primary sources for
their
own resistance decisions: Either the teacher "owns" the problem
or the student does. Confirming that explanation, the techniques
that comprise the Teacher-Owned dimension imply that the
teacher
is somehow behaving inappropriately or inconsistently with
student expectations of what instructors should or should not
do.
Drawiny from the sample messages that represent Teacher-
Ownership, students were more likely to resist by accusing the
teacher of being "unenthused, boring, unprepared and doesn't
seem
to care." In other words, we might conclude that the teachers
referenced by students in that study had "misbehaved."
In contrast, strategies reflected in the second dimension
suggest that students themselves actually Rma the reasons for
their resistance. Students who selected Student-Owned
techniques
Teacher Misbehavior
9
were likely to justify their resistance by making excuses,
claiming to have other priorities, or asserting the right to make
their own decisions. Specifically, students might say, "I have
homework so I can't prepare well for this claws" or "Right or
wrong, that's the way I am." These statements and others
suggest
that students hold themselves, not the teacher, responsible for
their resistance decisions.
In that same study Kearney, Plax and Burroughs (in press)
found that college students' selections of either Teacher-Owned
or Student-Owned resistance were influenced by teacher
nonverbal
immediacy. When presented with scenarios depicting a warm,
approachable, friendly teacher (immediate), students were more
likely to select Student-Owned strategies in their resistance
alitempts. Conversely, when presented with descriptio,Is of a
cold, aloof, distant teacher (nonimmediate), students selected
Teacher-Owned techniques.
Apparently, judgments of teacher im-tediacy direct students'
subsequent attributions of problem ownership. In turn, these
attributions govern students' selections of either Teacher-Owned
or Student-Owned resistance techniques. Within the context; of
this investigation, it is reasonable to assume that while
immed:ate teacher behaviors are appropriate and preferred for
the
classroom, nonimmediate behaviors would correspond more
closely
with those teacher misOehaviors that students' perceive as
interfering with instruction. Whether or not nonimmediacy can
be
equated directly with student reports of teacher misbehaviors
0
114
Teacher Misbehavior
MD
remains an empirical question. We do know, however, that
students explain or justify their own resistance, at least in
part, by what their own teachers do or say (Kearney et al., in
press).
In an effort to more fully understand why students resist
teachers, this two-study investigation shifted the focus from
student-centered reasons to conceiving teachers themselves as
potential antecedents to student problems in the classroom. In
other words, we were interested in identifying teacher behaviors
that students' report being detrimental to instruction and thus,
demotivating to them. Pertinert to this change in focus, the
first study asked:
R01: What do college teachers say and do that students
perceive as "misbehaviors?"
Recognizing that teacher misbehaviors are likely zo vary
widely in frequency of occurrence and type depending on the
particular teacher, the second study was designed to validate
across a diversity of university teachers, the categories of
misbehavior identified in Study 1. Moreover, we assumed that
further examination of these data would help to determine
whether
or not the misbehavior categories isolated in Study I could be
reduced to a set of conceptually meaningful underlying
dimensions. For these reasons, research questions in Study 2
asked:
RO2: How frequently do students report their college
teachers engaging in each misbehavior type?
44
Teacher Misbehlxvlor
11
R123: What meaningful factor structure underlies the teacher
misbehavior categories?
Study 1
This study was designed to derive empirically both a broad-
based and representative classification of teacher classroom
behaviors that college students report as misbehaviors. In order
to derive such an inductive scheme, the research design was
structured to generate as many student descriptions of teacher
misbehaviors as possible. These data were used to answer
Research Question 1: "What do college teachers say and do that
students perc,,ive as 'misbehaviors?'"
METHODS
Subiectl. Participants were 254 (110 males, 144 females)
undergraduate students enrolled in two large sertions of
interpersonal communication at a large Western University.
Approximately 36% of the sample were freqhmar, 25% were
sophomores, 25% were juniors, and 14% were seniors. The
mean age
for this sample was 24. This course fulfilled general education
requirements across the university and therefore, students
reprasented a diversity of major fields.
Etalitshitak. In order to identify the wide variety of
teacher misbehaviors that can occur in college classrooms, an
open-ended questionnaire was distributed to the student
participants. Instructions on the questionnaire asked
participants "to think back over their college career and to
recall specific instances where teachers had said or done
12 BEST COPY AVAILABLE
Toschar Misbehavior
12
something that had irritated: demotivated or substantially
distracted them in an aversive way during a course." Students
were then asked to provide brief written descriptions of as many
teacher misbehaviors as they could and to be as specific in their
depictions as possible. In order to stimulate students' rec.:al
of the illustrations, examples of teacher misbehaviors were
included in the questionnaire (i.e, "Not showing up for class,"
"Making fun of a student," "Using sarcasm to get even with a
student," or "Teaching the wrong thing"). Space was provided
following the examples for students to write out their
descriptions of the various teacher misbehaviors. A total of
1762 brief teacher misbehavior descriptions was generated
across
the sample. The average number of misbehaviors described per
student participant was 6.9.
Results. All 1762 descTiptions generated by the students
were included in the unitizing, coding, defining and labeling of
the teacher misbehavior categories. These activities were
completed in seven stages. In stage one, the raw data were
unitized into separate and discrete misbehaviors. A
unitizer/coder read a sample of the raw units in order to become
familiar with the data. Im stage two, this same individual read
each and every descriptive unit and placed them into categories
containing both conceptually and/or operationally similar words
and phrases. Units which were the easiest to categorize were
sorted first; more difficult units were initially set aside and
then sorted into categories at a later time.
Teacher Misbehavior
13
In phase three the same coder reread all of the teacher
misbehavior units in each of the categories to check for
consistency and to make sure that all the units were sorted into
their appropriate categories. Tentative labels were given to
each separate classification of units and preliminary category
definitions were formulated. In phase four, the coder again
reread the descriptions in each category and based on the
tentative labels and definitions made any necessary adjustments
and revisions in the composition of any of the misbehavior
categories. In phase five the coder refined and made revisions
in the category labels and definitions.
Phases six and seven involved two additional coders. In
phase six, the second and third coders were familiarized with
the
data. In phase seven, both coders re-categorized sample units
from each of the categories in an effort to ensure category
appropriateness and to determine the degree of coder agreement.
Percent of unit-by-unit agreement between the original coder
and
the two additional coders ranged from 68% to 100% depending
on
the particular category. Intercoder agreement among all three
coders, assessed by unit-by-unit agreement, was .91.
Because of the relatively close agreement acr'oss the three
coders only light adjustr-ents needed to be made in finalizing
the categories. The resulting inductive classification of
teacher misbehaviors was organized into 28 categories. Table 1
presents the categories with sample teacher misbehavior
descriptions obtained with this procedure. This table also
14
Tescher Misbehavior
14
presents the rankings of these 28 categories including
frequencies and category percentages against the total number
of
descriptions analyzed in this study. The following section
describes conceptually the 28 teacher misbehavior categories.
insert Table 1 about here
Ilachat GA112011E1M1* Four categories, absent,
tardy, keeos students overtime, and early dismissal, categories
address the issue of teacher punctuality and absenteeism.
Teachers in these categories are depicted as insensitive either
to the time demands placed on students or to students' desire to
have their time in the classroom be a complete and constructive
experience. The 5 categories of strays from *0-diegt,
confusina/unc leer lac ture_s, unornared/d sori2ani zed , slev
ifstes
from svillikatm, and ,late returnina work emphasize teacher
organization and structure. These categories portray teachers as
who lack focus and pay little or no attention to the
instructional process. Pprcasm and ou/dpwns,
ver0411v_pbusive,
imitaremagia_ang_arkurAtx_raita, and namAALINmalamtni are
4
categories that capture teachers' contempt of students. These
teachers are characterized as individuals who publicly degrade
students, appear unreasonable and highly structured, and are
chauvinistic in the classroom. Vnresoonsive to students'
Questions, apathetic to studentligand j.naccessible to students
outside of class are 3 categories that speak to teacher
indifference. Instructors described in these categories are
15
it
Teacher Misbehavior
15
unapproachable and impervious to questions, showing little
concern for students.
The 2 categories of unfair tesI*no and unfair .1rodinq
capture teachers who employ unjust methods of evaluation.
Teachers represented in these categories are ambiguous testers
and inconsistent, temperamental graders. The Oprino lectures
category characterizes those teachers who are unenthusiastic,
overly repetitive and much too serious du.ring their classroom
presentations. Information overload depicts teachers who are
either overly demanding of sttudents or noticeably unreasonable
in
their instructional demands. Information underload
characterizes
those teachers who are too easy; those from whom students feel
they have learned very little or absolutely nothing. The 2
categories of negative perlionalitv, and neaative physical
appearance illustrate teachers who possess negative personal
attributes. Teachers described in these categories tend to be
moody and self-centered and often dress or act inappropriately
in
class. The doel not know subiect matter category illustrates
those instructors who are obviously either unqualified to tear_h
the subject matter or simply do not know the course content.
Shows favoritism or oretudice characterizes those teachers who
show preferences to particular students and who reinforce the
concept of stereotypes in the classroom. Formion or reuional
ficcentfh &naooropriate_ vo I tap and toad orammar4ppellino
are 3
categories which capture teachers' misuse of language. Such
teachers are described as unintelligible and/or hard to hear
16
Teacher Misbehavior
lb
during lectures and often display poor language skills.
Study 2
This study was designed to validate the categories of
teacher misbehavior types obtained in Study 1 and to determine
whether or not a conceptually meaningful 'factor structure
.nderlies the original 28 categories. Quantitative data
collected in tt's second study were employed to answer Research
Questions 2 And 3: "How frequently do students report their
college teacher engaging in each misbrhavior type?" and "What
meaningful factor structure underlies the teacher misbehavior
categories?" Qualitative data were also collected which assisted
in our validation and .nterpretation of findings.
IIETIODS
PartIcipants were 261 (150 females, 111 males)
undergraduate students enrolled in introductory communication
classes satisfying general education electives at a large Western
university. Approximately 26% of the sample were freshman,
31%
sophomores, 28% juniors and 15% seniors. The mean age of the
students in this sample was 25.
Research Design. Whereas in Study 1 a research design was
employed to maximize students' generation of teacher
descriptions
across teachers more generally, in this study the design was
structured for each student to focus on a particular college
teacher. While the former results reflect an accumulation of
both numerous and disparate teacher misbrihavior types, the
design
1 7
Teacher Misbehavior
17
for Study 2 essentially minimizes the reported diversity and
frequency of teacher misbehaviors. That is, as a collective
group, teachers may engage in a variety of different
misbehavior
types; however, we would not expect any individual teacher to
exhibit all 28 types. By anchoring each student's perceptions to
her/his respective teacher then, the design of this validational
study allows for a rigorous assessment of the original 28
categories of misbehaviors.
Procedures. Students were given questionnaires which
explained that the instrument included "descriptions of things
teachers have been observed doing or saying in some classes"
which "college students have previously identified as teacher
'misbehaviors.'" They were also told that this study assessed
"how often teachers engage in one or more of those behavior
types
or a behavior similar to those included in the descriptions."
Students were instructed to complete the research instrument
with
reference to "on1v the teacher you have in the course you are
taking that meets just before this class." This anchoring
technique devuloped originally by Flax, Kearney, Richmond,
and
McCroskey (1986), maximized the variability in subject matter
fields represented and allowed for a broad sample of instructors
at the ,iniversity. In this way, data relating to over 250
different classes/teachers were obtained.
After indicating their gender, age and year in school,
students were provided with sets of multiple teacher
misbehaviors
representing each co" the 28 categories derived in Study 1 (see
s
Teacher Misbehavior
10
Table 1 for these descriptions). Category labels were not
included on the questionnaire. Students were asked to indicate
on a 0-4 scale "how frequently your teacher in that class
exhibits the same or similar behaviors" with 0 Never and 4 m
Very Often.
Rweiutts. As expected, descriptive statistics revealed that
even though most of the teachers sampled in this study never (0)
or rarely (1) engaged in the sample misbehavior (M A.1), a
number of others did. Importantly, the full range of student
responses (0 to 4) was obtained across all 28 categories.
Frequency percentages of those scoring 2 or higher ranged from
3.4% to 29.1% per category. Although some categories are more
representative than others, these data provide evidence for the
perceived occurrence of all ee misbehavior types. Table 2
provides the means, standard devitations and frequency
percentages
for those scores.
Insert Table 2 about here
Table 2 also provides a ranking of the misbehavior
categories. Delivering boring lectures, straying from the
subject matter, employing unfair testing procedures, presenting
lectures which are confusing and unclear, and returning
students'
work late were the 5 most frequently cited teacher
misbehaviors.
Correspondingly, inductively-derived data from Study 1
revealed
that 3 of those same misbehaviors were ranked in the top 5:
Strays from subject, unfair testing, and boring lectures. The
43
Teacher Misbuthavior
19
two other misbehaviors ranked high in Study 1 were
sarcasm/putdowns and absent from class.
SLOOLLIalltratrx Rata arderaiisia. To assist us in validating
the data repl3rted in Table 2, we asked each student ". . to
explain hity you think your teacher behaves in the ways you've
indicated. There may be a single reason or there may be several
reasons for your teacher's behavior. Indicate the reason or
reasons you think apply." Students were provided with enough
writing space to briefly describe up to three prenumbered
reasons
for their teacher's behavior. Previous research (Kearney et al.,
in press) has shown that collecting these types of supplemental
responses provide valuable and corroborating information. Sucn
additional information allows for the triangulation of primary
and secondary data sets---a powerful method (Morine-
Dershimer,
1983) for increasing the overall validity of findings.
Examination of students' reported reasons for their
teachers' behavior proved to be revealing. Of the 261 students
who participated, 117 indicated reasons why their current
teacher
misbehaves; 111 described reasons why their current teacher did
not misbehave and/or why their teacher was so effective in the
classroom; and 33 gave no reasons for either their teacher's
misbehavior or effectiveness.
It is particularly interesting that without being directly
asked, almost 43% of the students indicated reasons why they
felt
their teacher was so effective in the classroom. Many of these
same students also indicated that they could only say positive
20
Teacher Mlubehavior
20
things about the teacher for the course they had before this
class. However, they also indicated that they either currently
had another teacher who frequently misbehaved or that they had
had teachers during their college career who had misbehaved in
the variety of ways described on the questionnaire. As for
teacher misbehaviors, over one-half of the responses either
directly or in a restated form, included many of the actual
teacher misbehaviors referenced in the original 28 categories.
Table 3 provides representative samples of the reasons
students gave for their particular teacher's misbehavior as well
as those given for their teacher's effectiveness in the
classroom.
Insert Table 3 about here
To summarize what was illustrated across these data, students
who
indicated that their teacher misbehaved described reasons that
depicted their teacher as unable to relate to students, uncaring,
preoccupied with other work, uninformed about course content,
fearful about initiating personal relationships with students,
outdated, selfish and self-centered, and not being committed to
the teaching profession. In short, the reasons given for
misbehavior suggest that students were less than satisfied with
the way their teachers were behaving. On the other hand,
students' explanations for their particular teacher's
effectiveness portrayed teachers in quite the opposite direction.
That is, the effectiveness of teachers was associated with
Teacher MiebehavMor
24
attributes like a love for the teaching profession, the ability
to establish a rapport with students, a solid knowledge of the
subject matter, a sincere concern for students, a high level of
professionalism, self confidence about teaching the course, an
open and friendly nature, and the ability to create a challenging
classroom environment. Effective teachers then, were perceived
by students as doing a good job and as doing and saying things
correctly in the classroom.
These interview-type data both corroborate and elaborate on
our other findings illustrating the validity of the teacher
misbehavior categories derived in Study 1. Correspondingly,
these data indicate that the majority of the students in this
sample either currently or previously had a least one teacher
who
they perceived as behaving inappropriately. Even the students
responding to a teacher they described in very positive ways
described reasons which illustrated that the students in this
sample were able to make a clear distinction between the
effectiveness and ineffectiveness of what their college tearcs
said and did in the classroom.
Reaucing the Structure of the Categories
Next, we determined whether the students' responses to the
28 categories as presented on the questionnaire could be
reduced
to a meaningful underlying factor structure. An overall default
factor analysis (eigenvalue ( 1.0) resulted in an initial 7-
factor solution. However, factors 1. 2 and 3 accounted for most
of the variance (44.7%). Moreover, these first 3 factors were
22 BEST COPY AVAILABLE
04
Teacher Mlimbehavior
22
conceptually consistent. Subsequent analysis with 3-factor
extractions produced stable factors with all items loading on
their respective factor. An examination of the item loadings
revealed that 7 items failed to meet a liberal 50/30 criterion.
With those items eliminated, our second 3-factor solution
increased the variance accounted for to 50.6%. The results of
this 3-factor solution are reported in Table 4. Interfactor
correlations between Factors 1 and 2 were .25, Factors 1 and 3
=
.26 and Factors 2 and 3 = .18. Alpha reliabilities obtained for
Factor 1 were .86 (M = 5.70, s.d. = 6.31, range = 0-32), Factor 2
= .130 = 2.179 s.d. = 3.51, range = 0-20) and Factor 3 = .80 (PI
= 3.97, s.d. = 4.04, range = 0-23).
Insert Table 4 about here
Nine items comprised Factor is Confusing/unclear lectures,
apat'netic to students, unfair testing, boring lectures,
information overload, does not know subject matter, foreign or
regional accents, inappropriate volume, and bad
grammar/spelling.
This factor was labeled "incompetence." Factor 2, labeled
"offensiveness," consisted of 6 misbehavior categories:
Sarcasm/putdowns, verbally abusive, unreasonable/arbitrary
rules,
sexual harassment, negative personality, and shows
favoritism/prejudice. Items included in Factor 3, labeled
"indolence," included 6 misbehavior types: Absent, tardy,
unprepared/disorganized, deviates from syllabus, late returning
work, and information underload.
Teacher 'Misbehavior
23
lamals end Emrstac nmAshing Etassdurns. In
order to substantiate the reliability of the 3-factor solution we
completed two additional procedures. Firsti we employed a
"random split sample" procedure (Armstrong and Soelberg,
1988) to
create two within sample subsets. These randomly chosen
subsets,
each consisting of 130 student responses, were used to compute
separate forced 3-factor extractions. Descriptively: the results
of these additional factor analyses were virtually identical to
those produced with the entire sample of students. These results
are available upon request.
Secondly, we followed up the randomized split sample
procedure by computing similpritv concordant coefficients
(Nesselroade and Baltes: 1970). This factor matching procedure
was computed between the pairs of loadings produced on factors
1,
2, and 3 when the entire sample was included and those loadings
produced on factors 1, 2, and 3 with a random split sample. The
resulting concordant coefficient for factor 1 between the total
and the split sample was .999; for factor 2 this index was .995;
and for factor 3, .995. These indices provide strong descriptive
support for a claim of factor invariance across each set of
paired factor loadings.
tsi ftnairses. With the reliability of the 3-factor
solution substantiated, we attempted to determine potential
effects of student age, gender and year in school on students'
reports of teacher misbehavior across the dimensions of teacher
incompetence, offensiveness, and indolence. We computed a
24
Teacher Misbehavior
24
regression-type 2 (college students' gender) X 4 (year in school)
fixed effects multivariate analysis of covariance including
student age as a covariate. The criterion variables were
operationalized to include students' summed responses across
each
of the three dimensions of teacher misbehavior. Results
indicated that neither student age (I:s of the covariate for each
of the three dimensions of teacher misbehavior were ( = 1) nor
gender and year in school were significant (all complex
interactions or main effects a > .05; overall power estimates for
all simple main effects were above .90). Identical results were
obtained when the data were transformed into L scores prior to
computing the MANCOVA. Computations on the standardized
data
indicate that the shapes of the distributions of the data
reported in Table 2 in no way affected either the results of the
MANCOVA or the factor analytic and factor matching
procedures.
Complete results are available upon request. Based on these
findings then, at least for this sample of college students, age,
gender and year in school have little influence on students'
reports of teacher misbehavior.
DISCUSSION NITH IMPLICATIONS FOR THE CLASSROOM
This study represents a shift from the research tradition
which focused on student misbehaviors in the classroom to a
new
perspective which examines teacher misbehaviors. We began
this
investigation by assuming that teachers can and do misbehave
and
that these misbehaviors can become potential sources of student
dissatisfaction and resistance. In the first of two studies, we
Teacher Mlubwhawier
25
asked college students to identify teacher misbehaviors they had
observed during their college career. Over 1700 misbehaviors
were inductively derived and then categorized into 28 different
teacher misbehavior types. Clearly, students perceived their
college teachers to "misbehave."
Categories of teacher misbehaviors ranged from using bad
grammar or misspelled words to sexual harassment and verbal
abuse. The most frequently cited misbehavior types were (1)
Sarcasm and Putdowns, (2) Absent, (3) Strays from Subject, (4)
Unfair Testing and (5) Boring Lectures. Perhaps we have all
been
guilty of one or more of these misbehaviors and perhaps we
have
"justified" each and every transgression. Even so, from the
students' point of view, teachers who cancel class or make their
exams too difficult are "misbehaving."
In our second study we presented another sample of college
students with multiple misbehaviors representing each of the 28
categories identified in Study 1. interested in validating the
existence of the misbehavior categories, we asked students to
indicate how frequently a teacher they had currently engaged in
each misbehavior type. Unlike Study 1 which was designed to
maximize the generation of a number of different misbehaviors
across teachers more generally, in Study 2 we anchored
students'
perceptions to specific target teachers. In this way, we were
able to assess more realistically the range and frequency of each
misbehavior actually occurring in the college classroom.
As expected, most of the students reported that their own
26
Teacher Misbehavior
teacher rarely engaged in the diversity of misbehavior types
indexed. Gratefully then, it appears that most students find
their teachers to "behave" appropriately. fisiore we become too
relieved, however, it is important to note that the full range of
frequencies was reported for each and every category. For
instance, almost 30% of the students reported that their teachers
frequently (occasionally to very often) spoke in monotone and
rambled throughout the lectures. One-fourth indicated that their
ozachers were often late in returning papers and exams, wasted
class time with personal stories and opinions, asked trick
questions on tests or made the items too ambiguous, talked too
fast or lectured over students' heads, and confused students by
being unclear or inconsistent in their expectations. Apparently,
these and other misbehavior types occur frflquently enough for
students to notice and for teachers to take pause. Moreover, our
analysis of the reasons students' give to explain their teachers'
behavior tends to corroborate the validity of the 28 misbehavior
categories. Examination of these data also indicate that the
majority of students were able to discriminate their particular
teacher as either misbehaving or 12ehaving effectively based on
the 28 categories.
In an attempt to determine if a meLolingful factor structure
underlied the 28 categories, we were able to reduce all but 7
categories into 3 interpretable dimensions: (l) Incompetence,
(2) Offensiveness and (3) Indolence. The reliability of the
three dimensions of teacher misbehavior was affirmed with
Fandom
27
Teacher Maabehavier
27
split sample and factor matching procedures. Moreover,
additional analyses of the factors indicated that students' age,
gender, and year in school do not influence the way students
report the misbehavior of their teachers.
Misbehaviors represented by Incompetence reflect the lack of
very basic teaching skills. Teachers who assign excessive work
and rush through the material "to get it all done" may fail to
recognize the importance of incremental methods of instruction.
These same teachers may also be accused of making their tests
too
difficult and, at the same time, be unable or unwilling to help
students succeed. Specifically, the misbehaviors included in
this factor suggest that Incompetent teachers do not seem to
care
about either the course or the students themselves, do not know
their students' names, will not review for exams and fail to
allow for student input during class.
The profile of Incompetence is extended further to those
teachers who are unenthused about the material, speak in a
monotone, enunciate poorly (or speak with difficult foreign or
regional accents), and talk too loudly (or softly). Not only
does Incompetence refer to instructirmal ineptitude, but this
factor also implies that stuLdents perceive Incompetent teachers
as ignorant anl confused. In other words, students report that
teachers of this type are unable to answer questions in class,
provide students with incorrect information when they do, lack
currency in their area--and then compound the problem by
presenting vague, confusing lectures and contradicting
themselves
26
Teacher Milashavior
BB
in front of class. In short, teacher Incompetence reflects a
number of teacher misbehaviors that clearly interfere with
instructional goals and student learning.
Teacher Offenkiveness included a number of misbehaviors that
implied teachers could be mean, cruel and ugly. Apparently,
offensive teachers humiiiate students in front of the class:
insult and publicly embarrasses them. Offensive teachers may
use
profanity, become angry or yell and scream in their efforts to
intimidate students. These same teachers are rude, self-
centered, moody, and whiners; moreover, they condescend to
students by acting superior and arrogant.
If those characteristics and behaviors appear insufficient
to label teachers of this type as Offensive, consider also
reported misbehaviors of sexual harassment and prejudice.
Students identify Offensive teachers as those who are
chauvinistic, make sexual remarks and flirt with students. These
teachers reportedly play "favorites" with their students and/or
act prejudicial toward others. Finally, Offensive teachers
appear unreasonable and arbitrary; they refuse to accept late
work: punish the whole class for one student's infraction, and
present themselves as rigid, inflexible and authoritarian.
The third dimension underlying teacher misbehavior types,
Indoler%e, best exemplifies the profile of the stereotypic,
absent-minded college professor. Teachers who are considered
Indolent are those who fail to show up for class, are late when
they do, and offer poor excuses for their truancy. They might
Teacher Milibihevior
29
forget test datas and neglect to collect and grade students'
homework. Indolent teachers are late in returning students'
papers and exams. Because they are so disorganized, they fall
behind in their schedules, change due daf-es for assignments
and
are forced to adjust their- syllabi. Students further report that
indolent teachers "underwhelm" them with information by
making
their classes and tests too easy. Apparently, with indolent
teachers, students do not feel they are learning as much as they
should.
Earlier research (Kearney, Plax, & Burroughs, in press)
indicated that students blame one of two sources for their own
resistance decisions: Teachers or students. The results of this
investigation suggest that students may have legitimate cause
for
those attributions. That is, our findings reveal that teachers
themtielves "misbehave" in the college classroom. While the
degree or frequency of those misbehaviors may vary widely
across
college teachers, students do, in fact, perceive all 28 different
misbehavior types to occur. Whether or not misbehaviors of
incompetence, offensiveness and indolence actually are causally
antecedent to student resistance or other misbehaviors needs
further examination. In this way, future res:earch should
examine
the interactive nature of teacher ant student resistance in the
classroom.
IMPLICATIONS FOR THE TEACHER
Our results demonstrate that there are a variety of teacher
misbehaviors which are likely to influence and potentially,
30
o II
Teacher Misbehavior
30
stimulate student problems in the classroom. The existence of
these misbehaviors was reaffirmed by students' reports of
current
and previous experiences with teachers. We recommend that
teachers examine the list of 28 misbehavior categories in light
of their own classroom behaviors. Many of the categories
represent misbehaviors instructors do almost unknowingly. In
fact, we are all guilty of engaging in one or more of these
behaviors from time to time.
The decision to label what we say and do as "misbehaviors"
has important :.nstructional consequences. While we may be
reluctant or unwilling to view our grading procedures as unfair,
our accent as incomprehensible, and our attendance rules as
unreasonable, students may disagree. While we may feel
justified in changing the syllabus unexpectedly, embarrassing a
student who interrupts the class, and returning graded papers
and
exams late, students may disagree. And when they dot
undesirable student responses may result. Such responses can
take many forms, including negative teacher evaluations, poor
attendance, classroom disruptions, and lower achievement.
Recognizing these potential consequences, we need to consider
students' perceptions as well as our own in our decisions about
what we do and say in the classroom.
60
Teacher Niabehavlar
31
REFERENCES
Armstrong, J. S., 8, SoelOerg, P. (1968). On the interpretation
of factor analysis. exyaftgiczajtsAL Bulletin, Mg 361-364.
Brophy, J. E., 8 Evertson, C. M. (1976). Learnino fr_om
teaching:
a gmtnimmEnntal perspective. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Brophy, J. E., & Good, T. L. (1986). Teacher behavior and
student achievement. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of
research Ea teaching (3rd Ed.) (pp. 32B-375). New York:
Macmillan.
Burroughs, N. F., Kearney, P., & Plax, T. G. (1989).
Compliance-
resistance in the college classroom. Communication
219 214-e29.
Cantrell, R. P., Stenner, A. Jo, 8 Katzenmeyer, W. G. (1977).
Teacher knowledge, attitudes, and classroom teaching
correlates of student achic:vement. Journal pi gOucational
elystiolpox, 69, 180-190.
Doyle, W. (1986). Classroom ornization and management. In M.
C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook pl,resturct teachino (3rd
Ed.) (pp. 392-431). New York: Macmillan.
Emmer, E. T., Evertson, C. M., Sanford, J. Pm, Clements, B. S.,
t%
Worsham, M. E. (1984). Classroom mar:Element ism seconsiory
IMIREARLI. Englewood Cliffs, N3s Prentice-Hall.
Evertson, C. M., Emmer, E. T., Clements, B. S., Sanford, J. P.,
Worsham, J. E. (1984). Classroom manamement fgr.plementory
teachers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Gage, N. Le, I Needels, M. C. (1989). Process-product research
.* 41, A
Teacher Misbehavior
3e
on teachings A review of criticisms. Ths. flementarv, School
Journal, 253-300.
Good, T. Log ett Brophy, J. E. (1986). School effects. In M. C.
Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook gf research wi teechino (3rd Ed.)
(pp. 570-602). New York: Macmillan.
Kearney, P. (1987). Power in the classroom. Journal 211-
houahto
gg, 45-50.
Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., 8, Burroughs, N. F. (ir press). An
attributional analysis of college students' resistance
decisions. Communication Education.
Kearney, P., Plax, T.G., Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C.
(1984). Power in the classroom IV: Alternatives to
discipline. In R. Bostrom (Ed.), Communicatipn yearbook g,
(pp. 724-746). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., Richmond, V. P., L McCroskey, J. C.
(1985). Power in the classroom Ills Teacher communication
techniques and messages. Cpmmunica ion, gdpgation, WI, 19-
28.
Kearney, P., Plax, T. S., Smith, V. R., & Sorensen, G. (1988).
Effects of teacher immediacy and strategy type on college
student resistance to on-task demands. Communication
Education, 225 54-67.
Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., Sorensen, G., 11. Smith, V. R. (1988).
Experienced and prospective teachers' selections of
compliance-gaining messages for "common" student
misbehaviors. CommunicAtion gducation!, 22., 150-164.
.*
Teacher Mlobehavior
33
Morine-Dershimer, S. (1983). Taming ITRArabsmthinkino
throuoh
trianoulation cri data pets. Austin, TX: The Research and
Development Center for Teacher Education, University of
Texas, Report No. 8014.
Nesselroade, J. R., 8 Baltes, P. B. (1970). On a dilemma of
comparative factor analysis: A study of factor matching
based on random data. Educational and Psvcholooical
Measurement, 29.11 935-948.
Plax, T. S., 8 Kearney, P. (1990). Classroom management;
Structuring the classroom for work. In J. Daly, G.
Friedrich, & A. Vangelisti (Eds.), Teaching communication:
Theory, research, and,methgqs (pp. 223-236). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum,
Flax, T. S., Kearney, P., McCroskey, J. C., 8 Richmond, V. P.
(1986). Power in the classroom.VI: Verbal control
strategies, nonverbal immediacy and affective learning.
Communication Educatign, 25., 43-55.
Rosenshine, B., & Stevens, R. (1986). Teaching functions. In
M.
-C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook gi: resparch aateachino (3rd
Ed.) (pp. 376-391). New Yorks Macmillan.
Wittrock, M. C. (1986). Students' thought processes. In M. C.
Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook 21 research Im teaching (3rd Ed.)
(pp. 297-314). New York: Macmillan.
Woolfolk, A. E. (1987). Fducational psycholocv (3rd Ed.).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall,
3 4'
Woolfolk, A. E., I McCune-Nicolich,
gsvcholoov far. teaGhers (2nd
Prentice-Hal1.
3r
Teacher Misbehavior
34
L. (1984). Educational
Ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Table 1
"k.
Mamba-vim
Teacher Misbmhavior
35
h.', A- L.'
Enniumrat. 711103k
ABSENT
Does not show up for class, cancels
class without notification, and/or
offers poor aacuses for being absunt.
TARDY
Is late for class or tardy.
142
93 5
a 2
KEEPS STUDENTS OVERTIME
gawps class overtime, talks too long
or starts class early before all thm
students are thore.
90 5 7
EARLY DISMISSAL 22 1 23
Lets class out early, rushes through
the material to get done early.
STRAYS "F- SUIL3ECI7 117 7 3
Uses the class as a forum for her/hts
personal opinions, goms off an
tangents, talks about family and
personal life and/or gmnarally Hostas
class time.
CONFUSINEWUPCLEAR LECTURES
Unclmar about what is aspectod.
lectures are confusime end vague.
contradicts hins/harself sumps from
onm subject to another and/or lectures
are inconsistent with assigned
readings.
IDISORBANIZED
Twachwr fasbehavior
4 12
Is not prepared for class,
umrganized, forgets tsst dates,
and/or makes assignments but does not
collect them.
DEVIATES FROM SYLLABUS 35 2 20
Changes due datss for assignmnints,
behind schindule, doss not follow the
syllabus, Changes assignments, and/or
assigns books but does not usw them.
21 1 24LATE RETURNINS MORK
Late in returning papers, late in
grading and turning back Imams, and/or
forgets to bring graded papers to
class.
SARCASM AND PUTDOIINS 154 9 1
I. sarcastic and rude, maks, fun of
and humiliates students, picks on
students, and/or insults and
embarrasses students.
VERBALLY ABUSIVE 69 4 11
Uses profanity, is angry and Amman,
yells and screams, interrupts and/or
intisidatss students.
UNREASONABLE AND ARBITRARY RULES 23 1 22
Rifuses to accept late mark, gives no
breaks in 31-bour classes. punishing"
inntirs class for ono student's
'misbehavior, and/or is rigid,
inflexible end authoritarian.
SEXUAL HARASSMENT IS 1 25
Makes sexual remarks to students,
flirts with thee, sakes sexual
innuendos and/or is chauvinistic.
37
UNRESPONSIVE TO STUDENTS' QUESTIONS
Dom. not encourage students to ask
questions, does not answer questions
or recognize raised hands, and/or
seems °put out" to have to explain or
ropeat himiheroelf.
APATHETIC TO STUDENTS
Doesn't seam to care about time course
or show concern for students, dome not
know the students' name's, rejects
students' opinions and/or does not
allow for class discussion.
INACCESSIBLE TO STUDENTS OUTSIDE OF CLASS
Does not show up for appointmmnts or
schedutmd office hours, I. hard to
contact, will not meet with students
outside of office time and/or doesn't
make time for studonts when they need
help.
UNFAIR TESTINS
Asks trick westions on tests, exams
do not relate to the lectures, tests
are too difftcult, qumstions are too
ambiguous, and/or teachar does not
review for exams.
UNFAIR GRADING
grades unfairly, changes grading
policy during the semester, does nat
believe in giving A's, makes mistakes
when grading and/or does not have a
prefttermined grading scale.
WINING LECTURES
I. not an enthusiastic lectr-er,
speaks in monotone and rambles, is
boring, too much rspetition and/or
employs no variety in lectures.
35
BEST COPY AURAE
Teacher PlisbWhavior
37
76 4
73 4
50 3
110 6
62 4
a
10
17
13
INFOINIATION OVEFILOAD
INFORMATION UNDERLOAD
NEGATIVE PERSONALITY
Teacher is impatient, self-centered,
complains, acts superior and/or is
moody.
NEGATIVE PHYSICAL APPEARANCE
Talks too fast and rushes through the
material, talks over the students'
heads, uses obscure terms and/or
assigns excessive work.
The class is too easy, students feel
they have not learned anything, and/or
tests are too easy.
Teacher drosses sloppy, smells bad,
clothes are out of style, and cares
little About hisfher overall
appearance.
DOES NOT KNOM SUBJECT MATTER
Doesn't know the material, unmble to
answer questions, provides incorrect
information, and/or isn't current.
MOMS FAVORITISM OR PREJUDICE
Plays favorites with students or acts
prejudiced against others, is narrow-
minded or close-minded, and/or makes
prejudicial remarks.
FOREIGN OR RESIONAL ACCENTS
reacher is hard to understand,
enunciates poorly, and has a strong
accent that makes it difficult to
understand.
INAPPROPRIATE VOLUME
Doesn't speak loudly enough or speaks
too loud.
Teacher Misbehavior
30
46 3 18
45 2 19
57 3 15
24 1 21
62 4 14
52 3 16
16 0.9 26
9 0.5 27
BAD BRAMIAR/SPELL INF;
Uses bad grammars writes illegibly,
miumpells words on the exam (or on the
board) and/or generally uses poor
English.
ALL OTHERS NOT CATEGORIZED
61=.1=0.M.....M.M...MNMWAM11
4 (
Teacher Misbehavior
39
7 0.4
50 3
Teacher Misbehavior
40
Table 2
. -1 t . ' t t 4. iL k'
, Lt t t .1: _ t
Category Mean SD Z Rank
Absent .46 .83 10.7 17.5
Tardy .87 1.09 20.7 8
Keeps students overtime .79 1.09 18.7 9
Early dismissal .72 .87 15.0 10
"trays from subject .98 1.15 27.6 2
Confusing/unclear lectures .94 1.15 24.6 4
UMprepared/disorganized .40 .79 6.8 25
Deviates from syllabus .84 1.10 21.1 7
Late returning work .87 1.16 23.8 5
Sarcasm and putdowns .49 .46 11.6 15
Verbally Abusive .26 .75 6.1 26
Unreasonable/arbitrary rules .39 .89 9.0 21
Sexual harassment .15 .55 3.4 28
Wresponsive to students'
questions .34 .73 8.0 22.5
Apathetic to students .45 .91 10.7 17.5
Inaccessible to students .37 .81 7.7 24
Unfair testing .93 1.18 27.0 3
Unfair grading .52 1.01 13.4 11
Boring lectures 1.08 1.34 29.1 1
Informmtion overload .82 1.11 23.4 6
Information underload .52 .92 12.2 14
Negative personality .46 .95 12.7 12.5
Negative physical appearance .36 .82 8.0 22.5
Does not know subject matter .25 .60 1.0 27
Shows favoritism or prejudice .41 .85 9.2 20
Foreign or regional accents .48 .93 12.7 12.5
Inappropriate volume .36 .62 7.9 19
Bad grammar/spelling .40 .87 11.1 16
*Absolute mean = 2.0, with 0 = never and 4 = very often.
Teacher Misbshavior
41
TABLE 3
Sludent Reasons for Teacher Misbehavior,
ang_for_IssishscAlfisstiturasi_in_Abaraininnis
I. ReasoneLfor Teishmr Misbehavior
°She doesn't relate with our culture."
"She is so well Educated that she can't relate to students."
"He says that this jiab is just stepping stone for him before he
gets to temch at a better univErsity."
"He behaves this way to get it across to the !students not to F--
K
with him.'
"I think the acts tn these mays because as she says: 'I'm not a
Educator, I's a sathematician'."
'I think my Religious Studies teacher would be happiEr writing
a
book thandtctating to aur class.'
"My teacher doesn't understand what she is trying to teach us."
'In order to sake a tmst more challenging he auks trick
questions.'
"He is late because he is so busy and puts the class behind his
other interests.'
"I think my teacher is thy and is afraid to be a real person with
us."
'He thinks everyone tn his B.E. class is rnrolled because it if
their major.'
'The instructor is mostly into research and those not to care
about students.'
'She is mad at the university and takes it out on us.'
'Has gan I don't care if you come to class or mat attitude'.'
"Bmcause the is a very oWionated feminist."
'As far as his dress is concerned, he feels that has nothing to do
with what he is trying to tooth.'
4S 440
Teacher Misbehavior
II. Reasons far Teacher_Effnctivenwes
"My teacher likes what be is teaching."
"Hoe sincerely likes students and loves to express himself
clearly."
'She likes teaching and enjoys the rapport she has with her
students."
"She reolly cares about the information being delivered to the
class."
"He does keep us over sometimes, but that's only because he
gets
so excited about the material."
"She's a great teacher. I think it is because she has her masters
in communication. She knows haw to be an effective instructor.'
"Ikecause he's fair and truthful to students."
"He definiteiy hes the desire for making each student
understand
the materi41."
"He's always prepared and explains the subject well."
"because she teaches what is useful."
"He iv very open, warm. and kind to every student in the class
regardless of sem or race."
"She loves her job and it shows."
'He really encourages discussion and takes student's opinions as
valid and equal to his own."
"He is a powerful speaker. From the first day of class I told
myself I'd like to be like him."
"My teacher is challenging but I like her that may. She wants us
to learn what she knows."
*These examples illustrate reoccurring themes. More complete
lists are available upon request.
Teacher Misbehavior
43
Table 4
Factor Affillysis_of Teacher Misbehavior TYPWs.
MISBEHAVIOR YWCOMPETENCE OFFENSIVENESS
INMOLENCE
Absent .05 -.01 .60
Tardy .09 02 .62
Confusing/Unclear lecturms .68 .08 .39
Unprepared/Disorganized .37 .08 .73
arviates from syllabus .09 .15 .70
Late returning mark .23 .29 .75
Sarcaem and putdomns .11 .82 .05
Verbally abusive .04 .79 .07
Uhreasonaiblm/arbitrary rules .16 .62 .02
Sexual harassment -.10 .52 .15
Apathmtic to students .61 .31 .10
Unfair testing .68 .11 .07
Sering lecture* .69 -.01 .19
Information overload .73 .17 .17
Information umderload .03 .02 .54
Negative personality .43 .63 .19
Does not know subject matter .57 .03 .19
Shows favoritism/prejudice .27 .64 .01
Foreigndragional accents .70 .02 00
Inappropriate volume .70 .17 -.06
Sad grammar/spelling .70 .09 .11
Eigenvalume 6.22 2.27 2.13
Variance 29.60 10.80 10.10
Alpha Reliabilities .86 .80 .00
c14
. '.
.,.. .'.-. _'_'_-~"">'-"" r:::, ",,-,-_¥,~ ~L'-,-_"',-- J.!.""
Teacher Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior:
Unintentional Negative Communication
Katherine S. Thweatt
West Virginia University
James C. McCroskey
West Virginia University
This study involved the manipulation of teacher immediacy and
teacher
misbehaviors to determine their individual and combined
impacts on perceived
teacher immediacy and teacher misbehavior. Participants in the
study were
students enrolled in undergraduate communication classes. The
results indicated
that teacher nonimmediacy and teacher misbehaviors could not
be manipulated
independently. Analysis of variance indicated that
nonimmediate teachers were
perceived to be misbehaving even when no misbehaviors were
induced in the
experiments, It was concluded that students perceive teachers
who communicate
in nonimmediate ways as misbehaving.
Although immediacy has been clearly defined within the field of
communication (see
Mehrabian, 1969; Andersen, 1978), nonimmediacy cues have
been viewed as nothing more
than the opposite of immediacy cues. For the purposes of this
research, immediacy is
defined as "those communication behaviors that reduce
perceived distance between
people." Immediate behaviors may actually decrease the
physical distance, or they may
decrease the psychological distance, The more immediate a
person is, the more likely he/
she is to communicate at close distances, smile, engage in eye
contact use direct body
orientations, use overall body movement and gestures, touch
others, relax, and be vocally
expressive. In other words, we might say that an immediate
person is perceived as overtly
friendly and warm" (Andersen, 1979). In instructional research,
some immediacy cues that
have been manipulated are eye contact, smiling, movement in
the classroom, general
Katherine S. Thweatt (M.A., West Virginia University, 1996) is
a doctoral student at West Virginia
University, Morgantown, WV 26506-6293. James C. McCroskey
(Ed,D., Pennsylvania State
University, 1966) is a Professor and Chairperson of the
Department of Communication Studies at
West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26506-6293. This
paper is based on research conducted
by the first author as part of her M.A. thesis project which was
directed by the second author.
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH REPORTS, Volume 13,
Number 2, pages 198-204
Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior -Page 199
friendliness/ approachability, and enthusiasm. Conversely,
nonimmediacy has been
manipulated as not smiling, lack of eye contact, lack of
movement in the classroom, etc. The
impact of immediate and nonimmediate cues have been the
focus of a considerable amount
of research in which it was found that immediacy has positive
impacts in the classroom and
nonimmediacy less positive impacts. Students have been shown
to have more positive
affect for teachers high in immediacy which leads to increased
affective learning
(Andersen, 1979; Kearney, Plax, & Wendt-Wasco, 1985; Plax,
Kearney, McCroskey, &
Richmond, 1986; Richmond, McCroskey, Plax, & Kearney,
1986). Research also indicates
that students are unable to differentiate prosocial from
antisocial Behavior Alteration
Techniques when used by more immediate teachers (Kearney,
Plax, and Burroughs, 1991).
Extant research on immediacy has not identified precisely what
students' perceive
nonimmediate communication behaviors to be. In a study
investigating the impact of
teacher immediacy on teacher misbehaviors, some evidence was
uncovered that may help
classify certain behaviors as nonimmediate cues and, hence, aid
in the explanation of the
less than positive impact of nonimmediacy in the classroom.
Teacher misbehaviors have
been defined as "those teacher behaviors that interfere with
student learning" (Kearney,
Plax, Hays & Ivey, 1991). As a result of her research, Dolin
(1995) suggested that
nonimmediate communication behaviors of teachers may be
perceived by students as
misbehaviors, because they clearly interfere with their learning.
The present study sought
to investigate that speculation directly under controlled
experimental conditions. Since
this research was based more on informed speculation than on
firm theory, and reasonable
explanations for divergent outcomes were clearly possible, we
chose to pose a research
question rather than an hypotheses. The following question was
posed: "Can teacher
immediacy /nonimmediacy be manipulated independently of
teacher misbehaviors?" An
affirmative answer to this question would indicate that teacher
nonimmediacy is seen as
just that, not as a form of teacher misbehavior. A negative
answer to the question would
confirm Dolin's (1995) speculation that students perceive
teacher nonimmediacy as
misbehavior. In agreeing with Dolin, we anticipated the
negative outcome. However, since
the misbehavior and immediacy constructs were developed
independently, it was seen as
quite possible they might be completely independent constructs.
METHOD
Design
In a 2 x 2 factorial design, participants were exposed to one of
four scenarios. Teacher
immediacy (immediate/nonimmediate) and teacher misbehavior
(appropriate behavior/
misbehavior) were manipulated across four scenarios. After
reading a scenario,
participants were asked to complete scales measuring level of
perceived immediacy and
level of perceived teacher misbehavior. This research also
included a built-in replication.
Procedure
In large lecture classes at West Virginia University participants
were exposed to one of
four stimulus behavior scenarios (See Appendix A). After
reading the scenario, the
participant was asked to complete scales measuring perceived
teacher immediacy and
perceived teacher misbehavior. The replication was identical to
the original study except
that different scenarios representing different
operationalizations of immediacy and
misbehavior were employed (See Appendix A).
Page 200 - Communication Research ReportsjFall1996
Participants
Participants were 382 undergraduate students enrolled in
communication classes.
These classes were ones which serve all segments of the
undergraduate student body and
enroll approximately equal numbers of females and males. All
were given credit for a
course assignment for participation. There were 195 participants
in study one and 187 in
study two.
Manipulations
Immediacy. Teacher immediacy was manipulated in the four
scenarios. All immediacy
and nonimmediacy cues were derived from the work of
Andersen (1979). Six nonverbal
behaviors were manipulated to create two levels of immediacy.
In the high immediacy
condition, the teacher was immediate in all six nonverbal
behaviors. In the low immediacy
condition, the teacher engaged in no immediate behavior.
Different immediacy behaviors
were included in study two than in study one.
Misbehavior. Teacher misbehavior was also manipulated in the
four scenarios. Three
behaviors were manipulated that were considered either
appropriate behavior or
misbehavior. Two levels of behavior were created by varying
the amount of appropriate
and inappropriate behaviors. In the misbehavior condition, the
teacher engaged in only
inappropriate behaviors. In the nonmisbehavior condition, the
teacher engaged in all
appropriate behaviors. Different misbehaviors were employed in
study two than in study
one.
Measures
Subjects completed rating scales which measured perceived
teacher immediacy and
misbehaviors. Both scales employed two-item, seven-point
response formats.
Subjects were provided with the definition of immediacy. The
level of immediacy of
the teacher in the scenario was measured with two adjective
pairs: "very immediate/very
nonimmediate" and" approachable/ unapproachable." Scores
could range from 2 to 14
with a higher score indicating a more positive answer. The
obtained alpha reliability for this
two-item measure was .94.
A definition of teacher misbehavior was also provided for the
subjects. Perceived
frequency of misbehavior was measured by responses to two
adjective pairs: "never/very
often" and" frequently/seldom." Scores could range from 2 to 14
with a higher score
indicating more frequent misbehavior. The obtained alpha
reliability for this two-item
measure was. 98. .
Data Analyses
The dependent variables and manipulation checks were
subjected to two-way analyses
of variance. Post-hoc cell comparisons were made to probe
significant interaction effects
obtained in both studies. Alpha for all tests was set at .05.
Immediacy. Analysis of variance for the manipulation check for
immediacy indicated
the inductions in both studies were generally successful. In
Study One, the highly
immeqiate teacher was perceived as significantly more
immediate (F{1,188} = 113.58, P <
.05, eta2 = .37) than the less immediate teacher, m = 11.5 and m
= 5.9 respectively. In Study
Two, the highly immediate teacher (m = 11.4) also was
perceived as significantly more
immediate (F{1,180}= 151.4, P < .05, eta2 = .44) than the
teacher low in immediacy (m = 5.3).
..r '':'''.' -'._/.~~..c' ..: '... .'. " - ,_.. .
Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior - Page 201
The impact of the manipulation of misbehavior did not impact
perceived immediacy in
either study (F < 1.0).
There was a significant cross-over interaction observed in both
studies (Study 1, F(l,
188) = 4.68, P < .05, eta2 = .02; Study 2, F(l, 180) = 11.39, P <
.05, eta2 = .03). As noted in Table
1, the perceived immediacy in the low immediacy condition was
somewhat higher in the
"misbehavior" condition than the "no misbehavior" condition in
both studies. In contrast,
the perceived immediacy in the high immediacy condition was
somewhat higher in the "no
misbehavior" condition than in the "misbehavior" condition for
both studies. Tests
between cell means reflecting the interaction were significant
for the second study, but
were not significant in the first study. Given the low effect sizes
for these tests, this
interaction probably should not be seen as particularly
important, particularly considering
the very large main effects observed.
TABLE 1
Means for Immediacy Conditions
Dependent
Variable
Study
Number
Low Immediacy
Appropriate Misbehavior
High Immediacy
Appropriate Misbehavior
Immediacy 5.3ac 6.5bd 12.0ab 10.9cd
2 4.5adf 6.lbef 12.3abc 1O.6cde
Note: Means with the same subscript in the same study are
significantly different at (at least) the .05 level.
Behavioro The analysis of variance for the measures of teacher
misbehavior indicated
that a significant amount of variance was accounted for on
perceived teacher misbehavior
by the two induction variables in both Study 1 (F{3, 190} =
20.8, P < .05, eta2= .25) and Study
2 (F {3, 186} = 23.8, P < .05, eta2 = .28). The interaction effect
was significant in both studies
and, as indicated in Table 2, it was the primary explanation for
the overall significant AOV
models. In both studies the "high immediacy I appropriate
behavior" condition was
reported as having significantly less teacher misbehavior than
the other three conditions
(which did not significantly differ from each other). Thus, the
condition in each study in
which no teacher misbehaviors were induced was seen as having
a high level of teacher
misbehavior if the teacher was described as nonimmediate. For
the participants in these
studies, then, teachers who allegedly behaved in nonimmediate
ways were seen as
misbehaving, even if they were performing none of the
behaviors described as misbehavior
in previous research.
DISCUSSION
The primary purpose of these studies was to investigate the
mediating effect of
immediacy on perceived teacher misbehaviors. While subjects
did perceive differences in
immediacy and misbehaviors for all scenarios, the results
indicated that immediate teacher
behaviors and teacher misbehaviors cannot be manipulated fully
independently (or, at
least, were not in these studies). The low immediate, non-
misbehaving teacher was
perceived as misbehaving as much as the low immediate teacher
that was misbehaving.
Page 202 -Communication Research ReportsjFall1996
TABLE 2
Means for Misbehavior Conditions
Dependent
Variable
Study
Number
Low Immediacy
Appropriate Misbehavior
High Immediacy
Appropriate Misbehavior
Misbehavior 9.8a 8.7b 4.9abc 9.5c
2 9.3a IO.Ib 5.0abc 9.3c
Note: Means with the same subscript in the same study are
significantly different at (at least) the .05 level.
This particular result is counter-intuitive in that it was expected
that the low immediate,
misbehaving teacher would be rated as misbehaving more than
all of the other teacher
types. Apparently there is an upper limit on perceptions of
teacher misbehavior, and
nonimmediacy reaches that limit all by itself.
This leads to the most important conclusion we may draw from
this research:
Nonimmediacy is seen by students as misbehavior. Dolin's
(1995) research indicated that
some misbehaviors previously identified by students are
actually nonimmediate
behaviors. This research indicates that the reverse also appears
to be true. In the present
studies nonimmediacy overwhelmed the impact of good
behaviors. This suggests that
nonimmediacy acts as a misbehavior, even though students do
not necessarily recognize it
as a misbehavior at a conscious level. Students did not, in
Kearney, Plax, Hays, and Ivey
(1991) research, generate nonimmediacy as an actual
misbehavior. One explanation for
this is that students may perceive this simply as "normal" bad
teaching behaviors as a result
of the numerous teachers they have seen engaging in these
behaviors. Such common-place
acts of nonimmediacy are actually behaviors or misbehaviors
that serve to interfere with the
learning process. It can be concluded that, for the most part,
teachers who engage in
misbehaviors and are low in immediacy do not produce greater
perceptions of misbehavior
than the teacher who is low in immediacy, but engages in no
other misbehaviors, at least of
the type investigated in this research.
CONCLUSIONS
From the results of this investigation, it can be concluded that
nonimmediate teacher
behaviors can be classified as teacher misbehaviors. Thus,
teachers who, either by choice or
ignorance, fail to engage in immediate behaviors are
misbehaving, at least in the eyes of
their students. This may explain why students perceive the
nonimmediate teacher, who
functions in an otherwise efficient manner, less positively than
the immediate teacher who
may not even be efficient in accomplishing his/her duties. At
least in these studies,
students consistently evaluated the immediate teacher more
positively than the
nonimmediate teacher who engaged in no misbehaviors, even if
the teacher was
misbehaving. Students consistently evaluated the nonimmediate
teacher as engaging in a
high level of misbehavior, even if that teacher was credited with
otherwise only
appropriate behavior. For teachers, it is necessary to realize that
simply doings one's job
correctly is not enough. Teachers must make students
comfortable about approaching her /
him and ensure students that he/ she is comfortable with
approaching students. Creating a
,< . ....
Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior -Page 203
feeling of closeness, whether it is psychological or physical,
ensures the teacher who may
be forced to engage in what students perceive to be
inappropriate behaviors (misbehave)
on occasion that hel she will not lose the positive affect of the
students. Future researchers
may wish to consider extending this research by manipulating
nonimmediacy cues as
misbehaviors rather than just nonimmediate behaviors.
REFERENCES
Andersen, J. F. (1978). Teacher immediacy and teaching
effectiveness.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, West Virginia University,
Morgantown.
Andersen, J. F. (1979). Teacher immediacy as a predictor of
teaching
effectiveness. In D. Nimmo (Ed.), Communication Yearbook, 3,
(pp. 543-559). new
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
Dolin, D. J. (1995). Ain't Misbehavin'; A study of teacher
misbehaviors, related to
communication behaviors, and student resistance. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation,
West Virginia University, Morgantown.
Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., & Burroughs, N. F. (1991)
Anattributionalanalysis of
college students resistance decisions. Communication
Education, 40, 325-342.
Kearney, P., Pia x, T. G., Hays, E. R., & Ivey, M. J. (1991).
College teacher
misbehaviors: What students don't like about what teachers say
and do.
Communication Quarterly, 39, 309-324.
Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., & Wendt-Wasco, N. J. (1985). Teacher
immediacy for
affective learning in divergent college classes. Communication
Quarterly, 33, 61-74.
Mehrabian, A. (1969). Some referents and measures of
nonverbal behavior.
Behavioral Research Methods and Instruments, 1,213-217.
Pia x, T. G., Kearney, P., McCroskey, J. c., & Richmond, V. P.
(1986). Power in
the classroom VI: Verbal control strategies, nonverbal
immediacy and affective
learning. Communication Education, 35, 43-55.
Richmond, V. P., McCroskey, J. c., Pia x, T. G., & Kearney P.
(1986). Teacher
nonverbal immediacy training and student affect. World
Communication, 15, 181-
194.
APPENDIX A EXPERIMENTAL MANIPULATIONS
Scenarios -Study 1
Scenario 1: Immediate Teacher/Misbehavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who smiles frequently,
uses vocal variety, and gestures.
Moreover, this teacher is enthusiastic, walks around the
classroom, and seems relaxed in the
classroom. He/ she frequently cancels class without notice, does
not follow the syllabus, and is
usually unprepared for class.
Scenario 2: Immediate Teacher/ Appropriate BehaviOr
You are taking a class from a teacher who smiles frequently,
uses vocal variety, and gestures.
Moreover, this teacher is enthusiastic, walks around the
classroom, and seems relaxed in the
classroom. He/ she is always on time for class, follows the
syllabus, and arrives at class prepared.
Scenario 3: Nonimmediate Teacher/Misbehavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who lacks facial
expression, uses monotone, and doesn't
..,~,':: ....'... ;:'.2;:'1'!A~:d."./
:);.L.J::.t:.t.&;'j;''>.')::''';k'..::~'''::.>1..'.L1.:..~ n:",:..L~..
.. ~
Page 204 - Communication Research Reports/Fall 1996
gesture. Moreover, this teacher is unenthusiastic, stands behind
the podium throughout class, and
seems tense. He/ she frequently cancels class without notice,
does not follow the syllabus, and is
usually unprepared for class.
Scenario 4: Nonimmediate Teacher/ Appropriate Behavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who lacks facial
expression, uses monotone, and doesn't
gesture. Moreover, this teacher is unenthusiastic, stands behind
the podium throughout class, and
seems tense. He/ she is always on time for class, follows the
syllabus, and arrives at class prepared.
Scenarios - Study 2
Scenario 1: Immediate Teacher/Misbehavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very relaxed,
looks at the class when teaching,
and walks around the room during the lecture. Moreover, this
teacher engages in a lot of eye contact
and is generally perceived as friendly and approachable. He/she
assigns an excessive amount of
homework, lectures in a confusing manner, and is generally
unprepared for class.
Scenario 2: Immediate Teacher/ Appropriate Behavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very relaxed,
looks at the class when teaching,
and walks around the room during the lecture. Moreover, this
teacher engages in a lot of eye contact
and is generally perceived as friendly and approachable. He/she
assigns a reasonable amount of
homework, lectures in an organized manner, and is generally
prepared for class.
Scenario 3: Nonimmediate Teacher/Misbehavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very tense,
looks at the board while talking,
and stands behind the podium throughout the lecture. Moreover,
this teacher looks at the board
when lecturing and is generally perceived as unfriendly and
unapproachable. He/she assigns an
excessive amount of homework, lectures in a confusing manner,
and is generally unprepared for
class.
Scenario 4: Nonimmediate Teacher/ Appropriate Behavior
You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very tense,
looks at the board while talking,
and stands behind the podium throughout the lecture. Moreover,
this teacher looks at the board
when lecturing and is generally perceived as unfriendly and
unapproachable. He/ she assigns a
reasonable amount of homework, lectures in an organized
manner, and is generally prepared for
class.

More Related Content

Similar to TitleThe perils of merit pay linking teacher pay to performanc.docx

Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015 (1)
Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015  (1)Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015  (1)
Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015 (1)Peter Hofman
 
Performance assessment
Performance assessmentPerformance assessment
Performance assessmentXINYOUWANZ
 
What Makes a Good Principal
What Makes a Good PrincipalWhat Makes a Good Principal
What Makes a Good PrincipalAzreen5520
 
20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska
20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska
20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in NebraskaVicki Alger
 
Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models
Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models
Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models llee18
 
Fixing America’s Standardized Testing
Fixing America’s Standardized TestingFixing America’s Standardized Testing
Fixing America’s Standardized TestingAlex Cortez
 
Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...
Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...
Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...William Kritsonis
 
Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...
Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...
Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...Max Trezil
 
Alison mc bride doctoral forum
Alison mc bride doctoral forumAlison mc bride doctoral forum
Alison mc bride doctoral forumWilliam Kritsonis
 
Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...
Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...
Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...William Kritsonis
 
Running Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES 1.docx
Running Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES    1.docxRunning Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES    1.docx
Running Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES 1.docxcharisellington63520
 
Rolling Educational Rewards....
Rolling Educational Rewards....Rolling Educational Rewards....
Rolling Educational Rewards....Nupur Srivastava
 
Running Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docx
Running Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docxRunning Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docx
Running Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docxtoltonkendal
 
The Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. Document
The Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. DocumentThe Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. Document
The Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. DocumentDoug Reznicek M.Ed.
 
The Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate Earnings
The Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate EarningsThe Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate Earnings
The Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate EarningsGregory Pimentel
 
20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works
20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works
20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that WorksVicki Alger
 
What makes a good secondary assessment on achieving the aims of assessment
What makes a good secondary assessment  on achieving the aims of assessmentWhat makes a good secondary assessment  on achieving the aims of assessment
What makes a good secondary assessment on achieving the aims of assessmentAlexander Decker
 

Similar to TitleThe perils of merit pay linking teacher pay to performanc.docx (20)

Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015 (1)
Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015  (1)Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015  (1)
Re-balancing Assessment_CEPA whitepaper_HofmanGoodwinKahl_Feb2015 (1)
 
Performance assessment
Performance assessmentPerformance assessment
Performance assessment
 
Grading Ethical Issues
Grading Ethical IssuesGrading Ethical Issues
Grading Ethical Issues
 
What Makes a Good Principal
What Makes a Good PrincipalWhat Makes a Good Principal
What Makes a Good Principal
 
20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska
20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska
20120109 Teacher Selection and Evaluation in Nebraska
 
Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models
Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models
Teacher opinions about the use of Value-Added models
 
Fixing America’s Standardized Testing
Fixing America’s Standardized TestingFixing America’s Standardized Testing
Fixing America’s Standardized Testing
 
Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...
Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...
Eunetra Ellison Simpson, PhD Proposal Defense, Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, D...
 
Eunetra Ellison-Simpson
Eunetra Ellison-SimpsonEunetra Ellison-Simpson
Eunetra Ellison-Simpson
 
Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...
Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...
Reducing the Number of Under-performing Teachers in K-12 Schools in New York ...
 
Alison mc bride doctoral forum
Alison mc bride doctoral forumAlison mc bride doctoral forum
Alison mc bride doctoral forum
 
Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...
Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...
Alison Coates-McBridge and William Allan Kritsonis, PhD - Article: The M&M Ef...
 
Running Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES 1.docx
Running Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES    1.docxRunning Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES    1.docx
Running Head REWARDING STUDENTS WITH FINANCIAL INCENTIVES 1.docx
 
Rolling Educational Rewards....
Rolling Educational Rewards....Rolling Educational Rewards....
Rolling Educational Rewards....
 
Running Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docx
Running Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docxRunning Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docx
Running Head Target of Program Evaluation Plan, Part 11TARG.docx
 
The Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. Document
The Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. DocumentThe Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. Document
The Key to Enhancing Educator Effectivness. Document
 
The Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate Earnings
The Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate EarningsThe Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate Earnings
The Effect of Educational Factors on Graduate Earnings
 
20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works
20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works
20140908 Alger Teacher Incentive Pay that Works
 
TEST PREPARATION.pdf
TEST PREPARATION.pdfTEST PREPARATION.pdf
TEST PREPARATION.pdf
 
What makes a good secondary assessment on achieving the aims of assessment
What makes a good secondary assessment  on achieving the aims of assessmentWhat makes a good secondary assessment  on achieving the aims of assessment
What makes a good secondary assessment on achieving the aims of assessment
 

More from edwardmarivel

deadline 6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docx
deadline  6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docxdeadline  6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docx
deadline 6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docxedwardmarivel
 
Deadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docx
Deadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docxDeadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docx
Deadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docxedwardmarivel
 
DEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docx
DEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docxDEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docx
DEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docxedwardmarivel
 
De nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docx
De nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docxDe nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docx
De nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docxedwardmarivel
 
DDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docx
DDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docxDDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docx
DDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docxedwardmarivel
 
DDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docx
DDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docxDDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docx
DDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docxedwardmarivel
 
DCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docx
DCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docxDCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docx
DCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docxedwardmarivel
 
DDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docx
DDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docxDDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docx
DDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docxedwardmarivel
 
DBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docx
DBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docxDBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docx
DBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docxedwardmarivel
 
DBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docx
DBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docxDBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docx
DBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docxedwardmarivel
 
DB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docx
DB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docxDB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docx
DB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docxedwardmarivel
 
DB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docx
DB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docxDB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docx
DB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docxedwardmarivel
 
DB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docx
DB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docxDB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docx
DB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docxedwardmarivel
 
DB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docx
DB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docxDB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docx
DB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docxedwardmarivel
 
DB Response prompt ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docx
DB Response prompt  ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docxDB Response prompt  ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docx
DB Response prompt ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docxedwardmarivel
 
DB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docx
DB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docxDB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docx
DB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docxedwardmarivel
 
DB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docx
DB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docxDB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docx
DB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docxedwardmarivel
 
DB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docx
DB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docxDB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docx
DB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docxedwardmarivel
 
DAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docx
DAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docxDAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docx
DAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docxedwardmarivel
 

More from edwardmarivel (20)

deadline 6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docx
deadline  6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docxdeadline  6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docx
deadline 6 hours 7.3 y 7.47.4.docx
 
Deadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docx
Deadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docxDeadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docx
Deadline 6 PM Friday September 27, 201310 Project Management Que.docx
 
DEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docx
DEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docxDEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docx
DEADLINE 15 HOURS6 PAGES UNDERGRADUATECOURSEWORKHARV.docx
 
De nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docx
De nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docxDe nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docx
De nada.El gusto es mío.Encantada.Me llamo Pepe.Muy bien, grac.docx
 
DDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docx
DDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docxDDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docx
DDBA 8307 Week 4 Assignment TemplateJohn DoeDDBA 8.docx
 
DDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docx
DDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docxDDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docx
DDL 24 hours reading the article and writing a 1-page doubl.docx
 
DCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docx
DCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docxDCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docx
DCF valuation methodSuper-normal growth modelApplicatio.docx
 
ddr-.docx
ddr-.docxddr-.docx
ddr-.docx
 
DDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docx
DDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docxDDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docx
DDBA 8307 Week 2 Assignment ExemplarJohn Doe[footnoteRef1] .docx
 
DBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docx
DBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docxDBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docx
DBM380 v14Create a DatabaseDBM380 v14Page 2 of 2Create a D.docx
 
DBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docx
DBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docxDBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docx
DBA CAPSTONE TEMPLATEThe pages in this template are correctl.docx
 
DB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docx
DB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docxDB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docx
DB3.1 Mexico corruptionDiscuss the connection between pol.docx
 
DB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docx
DB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docxDB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docx
DB2Pepsi Co and Coke American beverage giants, must adhere to th.docx
 
DB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docx
DB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docxDB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docx
DB1 What Ive observedHave you ever experienced a self-managed .docx
 
DB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docx
DB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docxDB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docx
DB Response 1I agree with the decision to search the house. Ther.docx
 
DB Response prompt ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docx
DB Response prompt  ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docxDB Response prompt  ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docx
DB Response prompt ZAKChapter 7, Q1.Customers are expecting.docx
 
DB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docx
DB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docxDB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docx
DB Topic of Discussion Information-related CapabilitiesAnalyze .docx
 
DB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docx
DB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docxDB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docx
DB Instructions Each reply must be 250–300 words with a minim.docx
 
DB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docx
DB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docxDB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docx
DB Defining White Collar CrimeHow would you define white co.docx
 
DAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docx
DAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docxDAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docx
DAVID H. ROSENBLOOMSECOND EDITIONAdministrative Law .docx
 

Recently uploaded

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 

Recently uploaded (20)

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 

TitleThe perils of merit pay linking teacher pay to performanc.docx

  • 1. Title: The perils of merit pay: linking teacher pay to performance can't move forward until resolution of questions regarding fairness, teacher evaluation, and the relationship of test scores to teaching quality Author(s): Thomas Toch Source: Phi Delta Kappan. 91.2 (Oct. 2009): p99. Document Type: Article Copyright: COPYRIGHT 2009 Phi Delta Kappa, Inc. Full Text: The U.S. Senate wants the federal government to make $300 million in grants next year to spur a movement to link teacher pay to performance. The House wants to spend $445 million on the effort. And the Obama Administration wants to up the ante to $487 million in appropriations and another $200 million in stimulus spending. Secretary of Education Arne Duncan recently called performance pay "my highest priority." Such investments make sense. The long tradition in public education of paying teachers mainly on the basis of the college credits they've amassed and the years they've taught results in bad teachers earning the same as good ones--making it seemingly tougher to recruit and retain talent in public schools at a time when research suggests that teacher quality is critical to raising student achievement. But if the logic of performance pay for teachers is compelling, reformers have been trying unsuccessfully for decades to introduce the practice in public education, practically since the concept of a salary system based exclusively on credentials and seniority gained favor in the 1920s as a way to counter the
  • 2. favoritism and other inequities that plagued teacher pay at the time. "Every effort must be made to devise ways to reward teachers according to their ability without opening the school door to unfair personnel practices," reformers warned President Eisenhower in 1955. So if Duncan and today's school reformers want to bring performance pay to the public school teaching profession, they'll have to avoid their predecessors' mistakes. The last, and largest, push for performance pay came in the 1980s. Ted Bell, Ronald Reagan's secretary of education, protested in 1981 that public education's single salary schedule "demands that we pay the worst at the level of the best if we want to pay the best what they are worth." Two years later, a Bell-appointed national commission on school reform recommended performance pay in A Nation at Risk, its stinging indictment of public education. The result was new reward systems for teachers in Tennessee, California, Florida, Texas, New Jersey, and a host of other school systems. But those pay reforms didn't last long. Teacher unions attacked merit pay, as President Reagan was fond of calling the reform, largely because it violated the collectivism at the heart of the industrial-style unionism in public education. But the teacher pay experiments of the 1980s also failed because they were, at bottom, capricious. In many instances, they offered teachers the possibility of small additional amounts of money, not enough to mean much to those who got it, but just enough to irk those who didn't. The number of teachers receiving the rewards shifted with annual appropriations, regardless of how many teachers were rated high enough to receive them. And the eligibility standards for the rewards were frequently left to the whims of principals. The absence of credible systems of evaluating teachers' performance remains a major barrier to performance pay today, no less of a barrier than continued union resistance to tying pay to performance. The typical teacher evaluation in public education consists of a single, fleeting classroom visit by a harried principal untrained in evaluation who is often more
  • 3. interested in classroom comportment than the quality of instruction. The result is statistics like those in Chicago, where the nonprofit New Teacher Project found that 88% of the city's 600 schools did not issue a single "unsatisfactory" teacher rating between 2003 and 2006. The Obama Administration and the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, which is planning to spend upwards of $500 million on performance pay and other teaching reforms, want to use student test scores in teacher evaluations, a step the New York Times endorsed in a recent editorial. There's a clear logic in giving student test scores a role in teacher evaluations: It's inexpensive and easy to administer and seemingly measures what matters most--student achievement. Why Not Test Scores? But test scores aren't the simple solution they seem to be. Less than half of public school teachers teach the subjects or grade levels in which students are tested, eliminating the prospect of a system that's applied fairly to all teachers. Most standardized tests today measure only a narrow band of mostly low-level skills, such as recalling or restating facts, rather than the ability to analyze information and other advanced skills. As a result, the tests tend to privilege low-level pedagogy, leaving the best teachers, those with wider teaching repertoires and the ability to move students beyond the basics, at a disadvantage. And then there's the daunting challenge of separating out individual teachers' impact on their students' reading and math scores from the myriad other influences on student achievement and the difficulty of drawing the right conclusions about teacher performance from very small numbers of student test scores, a particular challenge in elementary schools, where teachers work with a single classroom's worth of students most of the day. As a result, student test scores should play a supporting rather than a lead role in teacher evaluations, and any credible performance pay plan is going to require more effective evaluations of teachers in classrooms--multiple evaluations by multiple evaluators and based on multiple indicators of how
  • 4. well teachers plan, teach, test, manage, and motivate. Comprehensive evaluations are especially important for making key decisions, such as granting teachers tenure, and for making judgments about the majority of teachers who are neither the very best nor the very worst. And they're key to winning teacher support of performance pay. In surveys, only a tiny fraction of teachers are willing to have student test scores play any role in pay levels. But their opposition to performance pay drops significantly when ratings are based on comprehensive evaluations. Encouragingly, the Gates Foundation is planning to make the evaluation issue a cornerstone of its teacher-reform work. Because there have been so few sustained performance pay systems in public education over the years, we don't know for sure whether the reform would indeed improve the teaching profession. James Guthrie and Patrick Schuermann at the National Center on Performance Incentives in Education at Vanderbilt's Peabody College, an enterprise funded by the federal government during the George W. Bush Administration to study performance pay, warn that there's not yet conclusive evidence on "the power of financial awards in promoting more effective teaching and elevating student performance" or on "the long-term effect of performance awards on the supply of effective teachers." Nor, they write, do we know the "effects of group awards relative to individual performance" or the "preferable mix of financial and nonpecuniary awards"-- important secondary questions. We need to answer these big questions about performance pay rather than assume that answers already exist. We know from surveys, for example, that bigger salaries are less important to teachers than professional work environments where they feel supported and are helped to become better at their craft. A 2007 national survey of teachers by the nonprofits Public Agenda and the National Comprehensive Center for Teacher Quality found that, if given a choice between two otherwise identical schools, 76% of secondary teachers and 81% of elementary teachers
  • 5. would rather be at a school where administrators supported teachers strongly than at a school that paid significantly higher salaries. Performance pay may be one tool with which to create a more professional culture in public school teaching, but it is no more than that. THOMAS TOCH is executive director of the Association of Independent Schools of Greater Washington and a former guest scholar at the Brookings Institution. Toch, Thomas Source Citation (MLA 7th Edition) Toch, Thomas. "The perils of merit pay: linking teacher pay to performance can't move forward until resolution of questions regarding fairness, teacher evaluation, and the relationship of test scores to teaching quality." Phi Delta Kappan 91.2 (2009): 99+. Academic OneFile. Web. 12 June 2014. Document URL http://go.galegroup.com/ps/i.do?id=GALE%7CA210599572&v= 2.1&u=tel_a_ttul&it=r&p=AONE&sw=w&asid=7f62baa2c30422 39e4e1943181d02c70 Gale Document Number: GALE|A210599572 ED 343 191 CS 507 779 AUTROR Kearney, Patricia; And Others TITLE What Students Don't Like about What Teachers Say and Do. PUB DATE Nov 91 NOTE 44p.; Revised version of a paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication
  • 6. Association (77th, Atlanta, GA, October 31 -Nomemster 3, 1991). PUB TIPE Speeches/Conference Papers (150) EDRS PRICE MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Behavior Problems; *Classroom Communication; *Classroom Environment; Classroom Research; Factor Analysis; Higher Education; *Student Eialuation of Teacher Performance; Student Reaction; *Teacher Behavior; Teacher Characteristics; *Teacher Student Relationship; Undergraduate Students IDENTIFIERS *Communication Behavior: Communication Patterns ABSTRACT To change the way classroom discipline and student discipline are examined, a two-study investigation, rather than focustLg on student noncompliance and other types of student misbehaviors, examined teachers themselves as potential sources of instructional and/or motivational problems in the college classroom. The first study was designed to elicit inductively college student reports of teacher misbehaviors. Participants were 254 undergraduates at a large Western university who were enrolled in two sections of a course on interpersonal communication. Responses to an open- ended questionnaire indicated 28 different categories of teacher
  • 7. misbehaviors. The second study was structured to validate the obtained categories of teacher misbehavior types and to deternine whether or not a conceptually meaningful factor structure underlies the categories. Even though most students (subjects were 261 undergraduates enrolled in introductory communication classes at a large Western university) reported that the teachers referenced for this study infrequently engaged in each misbehavtor type, a representative number of other teachers did. Importantly, the full range of frequencies was obtained across all 28 categories. Results were further collaborated with qualitative data. Factor analyses and factor matching procedures revealed that the teacher misbehavior categories could be both meaningfully and reliably reduced to three factors: teacher incompetence, offensiveness, and indolence. Recommendations include that teachers examine the list of 28 behavior categories in light of their own classroom behaviors. (Four tables of data and 24 references are included.) (SG) *M************************************************** ******************* * Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document. ***************************************************** ******************
  • 8. "FERMISSMN TO REPRODUCE THIS MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY 1.2)11Aile TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC).- U.& ØIEMIIIWIT OF ESUCITION Me fit Educeaorel teasesett and tagrameaset EDUCATIONALAIMPOORMATtOte 0 The docatase ass low repooduced se teamed tam las sewn or ageassetton orynatteg 0 Mews changes %see teen mad* to taterove reptedectoon Seats Poets of woo or wantons stated la this deco- meet do not enemata, rep/mem °noel OM Position Or mac, Teacher Misbehavior UMW STIMENTS DON'T LIKE ABOUT MAT WADERS SAY AIM DO *Patricia Kearney Timothy G. Plax Ellis R. Hays Marilyn J. Ivey
  • 9. Running Head: TEACHER MISBEHAVIOR *Patricia Kearney (Ed.D., West Virginia University, 1979), Timothy G. Plax (Ph.D., University of Southern California, 1974) and Ellis R. Hays (Ph.D., Purdue University, 1967) are Professors and Marilyn J. Ivey (B.A., California State University, Long Beach, 1999) is a graduate student in the Department of Spiffech Communication at California State University, Long Beach, 1250 Bellflower Blvd., Long Beach, CA 90940-2407 (714-897-5262). This research was funded, in part, by a Research Assigned Time grant from California State University, Long Beach. An initial analysis of the data reported in Study 1 should also appear in the M.A. thesis of Marilyn J. Ivey under the direction of Timothy G. Plax. An earlier version of this paper was presented on the Top 3 research report panel of the Instructional and Developmental Division of the Spewch Communication Association, Atlanta, 1991. 2 BEST COPYAMIE Teacher Misbehavior a . WHAT STUDENTS DON'T LIKE MOOT WHAT TEACHERS SAY AND 00
  • 10. Abstract This investigation represents a substantial change in the way vaL examine classroom discipline and student resistance. Rather than focusing on student non-compliance and other types of student misbehaviors, we examined teachers themselves as pote tial sources of instructional and/or motivational problems in the college classroom. Study I was designed to elicit inductively, college student reports of teacher misbehaviors. Results indicated 28 different categories of teacher misbehavinrs. Study 2 was structured to (1) validate the obtained categories of teacher misbehirvior types and (2) to determine whether or not a conceptually meaningful, factor structure underlies the categories. Even though most students reported that the teachers referenced in study 2 infrequently engaged in each misbehavior type, a represents ive number of other teachers did: Importantly, the full range of frequencies was obtained across all 28 categories. Results were further
  • 11. corroborated with qualitative data. Factor analyses and factor matching procedures revealed that the teacher misbehavior categories could be both meaningfully and reliably reduced to 3 factorst Teacher Incompetence, Offensiveness and Indolence. Implications for managing student resistance in the classroom are discussed. 4 4 Teacher Misbehavior 3 WHAT SIMMS DOMPT LIKE ABOUT WHAT TEACHERSLOAV AMD DO A large body of literature examines students as instigators of a variety of problems for the classroom teacher (see, for instance, Doyle's 1986 review). Students are frequently accused of talking out-of-turn, disrupting teacher talk, not paying attention and a whole host of other classroom misbehaviors. As a result, a great deal of attention has been focused on the causes
  • 12. of student disruptions and the intervention strategies that teachers can employ to handle these misbehaviors. Unfortunately, the research and advice offered in this tradition '2ften overlooks teachers themselves as a potential source of problems in the classroom. Not surprisingly, this oversight might be anticipated from researchers who happen to be teachers as well. In this study, we depart from that tradition by assuming that (I) teachers themselves may "misbehave" and (2) these misbehaviors can become potential sources resistance. Conceptually, of student dissatisfaction and student misbehaviors are defined as those student behaviors that interfere with learning (c.f., Kearney, Plax, Richmond, & McCroskey, 1984). Similarly, we define teacher misbehaviors as those teacher behaviors that interfere with
  • 13. instruction and thus, learning. Repeatedly letting students out of class early, failing to keep office hours, returning papers late, providing nonspecific evaluations on homework assignments, making the test too hard (or too easy), or delivering humorless, 4 LI Teacher Misbehavior 4 monotonous lectures all interfere with our ability to teach effectively and thus, can all be classified as teacher misbehaviors (Plax IL Kearney, 199(70). In this paper) we argue that these and other teacher misbehaviors can influence the way students think and act. A large body of literature substantiates a relationship between what teachers say and do with students' behaviors. In the tradition of the process/product paradigm, research- based conclusions about those specific teacher behaviors that
  • 14. influence student achievement, feedback, time speAt on-task, classroom order, student affect, good work habits, social skills, independence and other outcome variables are reviewed elsewhere (Brophy ti Good, 1986; Sage I Needels, 1989; Good Is Brophy, 1986; Rosenshine Si Stevens, 1986). More recently, research on students' thought processes emphas.zes the critical role that students' perceptions of what teachers say and do play in influencing students' motivation, achievement, attitudes and related student reactions. In other words, what teachers do influence students' thinking. That thinking, in turn, mediates student behavior (Wittrock, 1986). Following from this mediational perspective then, we might expect teacher misbehaviors to indirectly affect student* behavior by influencing how students think about and act towards the teacher, school and themselves. Because we know that what teachers say and do can
  • 15. significantly affect halt students think and behave, we might Teacher Misbehavior 5 expect teacher misbehaviors to act as potential antecedents to a number of undesirable student consequet.t1;. In other words, teacher misbehaviors may be a primary, albeit indirect, determinant of student disruptions. This report describes two studies identifying ways that tlachers themselves may contribute to the occurrence of problems in the classroom. Study 1 was structured to elicit inductively- college student reports of teacher misbehaviors. Study 2 was designed to validate the obtained categories of teacher misbehavior types and to determine whether or not a conceptually meaningful factor structure underlies the categories. The research and thinking on classroom management and student resistance provide the rationale for this investigation. Classmaa Management
  • 16. Within the classroom management perspective the primary responsibility for classroom control and student engagement lies not with the student, but with the teacher. Instead of highlighting student misbehavior problems, this alternative advocates a preventative stance toward discipline. The appeal of classroom management has its -oots in a line of research which demonstrates that the sifulle best predictor of learning is simply "academic engagement time" (Woolfolk & McCune-Nicolich, 1994; Woolfolk, 1987). No matter what instrucUonal strategies or methods are used, the teacher who keeps hr.:T./his students actively involved in the learning process is more likely to be effective (Woolfolk & McCune-Nicolich, 1984, p. 442). a Teacher Misbehavior This fundamental principle has led a number of researchers to identify those teacher behaviors which influence students'
  • 17. time spent on task (Emmer, Evertson, Sanford, Clements, & Worsham, 1984; Evertson, Emmer, Clements, Sanford, 8 Worsham, 1984). Based on classroom observations of elementary and secondary instruction, Emmer et al. (1984) and Evertson et al. (1984) differentiate effective from ineffective classroom managers. These researchers report that good managers regularly rely on positive questioning techniques and motivational messages (cues and prompts), attend more often to positive than negative student behaviors, provide students with good role models, give frequent and specific feedback, hold students accountable, and plan su,:cess-oriented learning experiences. The end result is that effective classroom managers increase studem.s' time spent on task (Brophy & Evertson, 1976; Cantrell, Stenner, & Katzenmeyer, 1977; Emmer et al., 1984; Evertson et al., 1984). Consistent with the classroom management perspective, instructional communication researchers argue that managing
  • 18. students successfully also requires that we "pErrreget" our students that learning is important, enjoyable and beneficial to their overall well-being (c.f., Kearney, 1987; Plax fis Kearney, 1990). In response to the need to identify those communication strategies which contribute to teacher influence in the classroom, an initial series of seven "studies was designed that isolated and validated 22 separate behavior alteration techn.ques and representative, sample messages for classroom use (c.f., 7 Teacher Oftebehavtar 7 Kearney, Plax, Richmond, & McCroskey, 1984, 1905). The results of these and subsequent investigations in the same program of research (c.f., Kearney, Plax, Smith, & Sorensen, 19618; Kearney, Plax, Sorensen, & Smith, 1988) indicate that both teachers and students readily agree on the preferred use of prosocial or
  • 19. reward-oriented, as opposed to antisocial or punishment-based, influence techniques. That is, teachers perceived them to be useful in managing students' behavior and, in turn, students reported that they enjoy the class and learn more content when their teachers rely on prosocial means of influence. With rare exception the classroom management behaviors and strategies reported in the educational and communication literature are success-oriented or prosocial. The converse or absence of those behaviors would seem to contribute negatively to students' involvement with learning. An overview of recent research on student resistance supports and extends that position. Student Resistance Rather than attend solely to what teachers strategically communicate in their efforts to manage or influence students, Burroughs, Kearney and Plax (1989) acknowledged the role of the student in the teacher/student exchange. Experienced teachers
  • 20. recognize that students often fail to concede the teacher's right to assume a power role. Moreover, a number of students may be reluctant or openly defiant, to assume their expected role of conciliation, cooperation and submission. In an effort to Teacher Mambehavior isolate those strategies college students might use to resist teachers' influence or compliance-gaining attempts, Burroughs et al. (1989) asked students to construct messages they would use ti resist their teachers in the classroom. Nineteen separate categories of techniques and messages wure identified in that research. In a follow-up study, Kearney, Plax and Burroughs (in press) validated the 19 categories and explicated two theoretically meaningful dimensions underlying the resistance categories: Teacher-Owned and Student-Owned. In explanation, problem- ownership refers to the degree to which the problem apparently
  • 21. originates with the student or the teacher. Kearney et al. (in press) reasoned that students blame two primary sources for their own resistance decisions: Either the teacher "owns" the problem or the student does. Confirming that explanation, the techniques that comprise the Teacher-Owned dimension imply that the teacher is somehow behaving inappropriately or inconsistently with student expectations of what instructors should or should not do. Drawiny from the sample messages that represent Teacher- Ownership, students were more likely to resist by accusing the teacher of being "unenthused, boring, unprepared and doesn't seem to care." In other words, we might conclude that the teachers referenced by students in that study had "misbehaved." In contrast, strategies reflected in the second dimension suggest that students themselves actually Rma the reasons for their resistance. Students who selected Student-Owned techniques
  • 22. Teacher Misbehavior 9 were likely to justify their resistance by making excuses, claiming to have other priorities, or asserting the right to make their own decisions. Specifically, students might say, "I have homework so I can't prepare well for this claws" or "Right or wrong, that's the way I am." These statements and others suggest that students hold themselves, not the teacher, responsible for their resistance decisions. In that same study Kearney, Plax and Burroughs (in press) found that college students' selections of either Teacher-Owned or Student-Owned resistance were influenced by teacher nonverbal immediacy. When presented with scenarios depicting a warm, approachable, friendly teacher (immediate), students were more likely to select Student-Owned strategies in their resistance alitempts. Conversely, when presented with descriptio,Is of a cold, aloof, distant teacher (nonimmediate), students selected
  • 23. Teacher-Owned techniques. Apparently, judgments of teacher im-tediacy direct students' subsequent attributions of problem ownership. In turn, these attributions govern students' selections of either Teacher-Owned or Student-Owned resistance techniques. Within the context; of this investigation, it is reasonable to assume that while immed:ate teacher behaviors are appropriate and preferred for the classroom, nonimmediate behaviors would correspond more closely with those teacher misOehaviors that students' perceive as interfering with instruction. Whether or not nonimmediacy can be equated directly with student reports of teacher misbehaviors 0 114 Teacher Misbehavior MD remains an empirical question. We do know, however, that
  • 24. students explain or justify their own resistance, at least in part, by what their own teachers do or say (Kearney et al., in press). In an effort to more fully understand why students resist teachers, this two-study investigation shifted the focus from student-centered reasons to conceiving teachers themselves as potential antecedents to student problems in the classroom. In other words, we were interested in identifying teacher behaviors that students' report being detrimental to instruction and thus, demotivating to them. Pertinert to this change in focus, the first study asked: R01: What do college teachers say and do that students perceive as "misbehaviors?" Recognizing that teacher misbehaviors are likely zo vary widely in frequency of occurrence and type depending on the particular teacher, the second study was designed to validate across a diversity of university teachers, the categories of misbehavior identified in Study 1. Moreover, we assumed that
  • 25. further examination of these data would help to determine whether or not the misbehavior categories isolated in Study I could be reduced to a set of conceptually meaningful underlying dimensions. For these reasons, research questions in Study 2 asked: RO2: How frequently do students report their college teachers engaging in each misbehavior type? 44 Teacher Misbehlxvlor 11 R123: What meaningful factor structure underlies the teacher misbehavior categories? Study 1 This study was designed to derive empirically both a broad- based and representative classification of teacher classroom behaviors that college students report as misbehaviors. In order to derive such an inductive scheme, the research design was
  • 26. structured to generate as many student descriptions of teacher misbehaviors as possible. These data were used to answer Research Question 1: "What do college teachers say and do that students perc,,ive as 'misbehaviors?'" METHODS Subiectl. Participants were 254 (110 males, 144 females) undergraduate students enrolled in two large sertions of interpersonal communication at a large Western University. Approximately 36% of the sample were freqhmar, 25% were sophomores, 25% were juniors, and 14% were seniors. The mean age for this sample was 24. This course fulfilled general education requirements across the university and therefore, students reprasented a diversity of major fields. Etalitshitak. In order to identify the wide variety of teacher misbehaviors that can occur in college classrooms, an open-ended questionnaire was distributed to the student participants. Instructions on the questionnaire asked participants "to think back over their college career and to
  • 27. recall specific instances where teachers had said or done 12 BEST COPY AVAILABLE Toschar Misbehavior 12 something that had irritated: demotivated or substantially distracted them in an aversive way during a course." Students were then asked to provide brief written descriptions of as many teacher misbehaviors as they could and to be as specific in their depictions as possible. In order to stimulate students' rec.:al of the illustrations, examples of teacher misbehaviors were included in the questionnaire (i.e, "Not showing up for class," "Making fun of a student," "Using sarcasm to get even with a student," or "Teaching the wrong thing"). Space was provided following the examples for students to write out their descriptions of the various teacher misbehaviors. A total of 1762 brief teacher misbehavior descriptions was generated across the sample. The average number of misbehaviors described per
  • 28. student participant was 6.9. Results. All 1762 descTiptions generated by the students were included in the unitizing, coding, defining and labeling of the teacher misbehavior categories. These activities were completed in seven stages. In stage one, the raw data were unitized into separate and discrete misbehaviors. A unitizer/coder read a sample of the raw units in order to become familiar with the data. Im stage two, this same individual read each and every descriptive unit and placed them into categories containing both conceptually and/or operationally similar words and phrases. Units which were the easiest to categorize were sorted first; more difficult units were initially set aside and then sorted into categories at a later time. Teacher Misbehavior 13 In phase three the same coder reread all of the teacher misbehavior units in each of the categories to check for
  • 29. consistency and to make sure that all the units were sorted into their appropriate categories. Tentative labels were given to each separate classification of units and preliminary category definitions were formulated. In phase four, the coder again reread the descriptions in each category and based on the tentative labels and definitions made any necessary adjustments and revisions in the composition of any of the misbehavior categories. In phase five the coder refined and made revisions in the category labels and definitions. Phases six and seven involved two additional coders. In phase six, the second and third coders were familiarized with the data. In phase seven, both coders re-categorized sample units from each of the categories in an effort to ensure category appropriateness and to determine the degree of coder agreement. Percent of unit-by-unit agreement between the original coder and the two additional coders ranged from 68% to 100% depending on the particular category. Intercoder agreement among all three
  • 30. coders, assessed by unit-by-unit agreement, was .91. Because of the relatively close agreement acr'oss the three coders only light adjustr-ents needed to be made in finalizing the categories. The resulting inductive classification of teacher misbehaviors was organized into 28 categories. Table 1 presents the categories with sample teacher misbehavior descriptions obtained with this procedure. This table also 14 Tescher Misbehavior 14 presents the rankings of these 28 categories including frequencies and category percentages against the total number of descriptions analyzed in this study. The following section describes conceptually the 28 teacher misbehavior categories. insert Table 1 about here Ilachat GA112011E1M1* Four categories, absent, tardy, keeos students overtime, and early dismissal, categories
  • 31. address the issue of teacher punctuality and absenteeism. Teachers in these categories are depicted as insensitive either to the time demands placed on students or to students' desire to have their time in the classroom be a complete and constructive experience. The 5 categories of strays from *0-diegt, confusina/unc leer lac ture_s, unornared/d sori2ani zed , slev ifstes from svillikatm, and ,late returnina work emphasize teacher organization and structure. These categories portray teachers as who lack focus and pay little or no attention to the instructional process. Pprcasm and ou/dpwns, ver0411v_pbusive, imitaremagia_ang_arkurAtx_raita, and namAALINmalamtni are 4 categories that capture teachers' contempt of students. These teachers are characterized as individuals who publicly degrade students, appear unreasonable and highly structured, and are chauvinistic in the classroom. Vnresoonsive to students' Questions, apathetic to studentligand j.naccessible to students
  • 32. outside of class are 3 categories that speak to teacher indifference. Instructors described in these categories are 15 it Teacher Misbehavior 15 unapproachable and impervious to questions, showing little concern for students. The 2 categories of unfair tesI*no and unfair .1rodinq capture teachers who employ unjust methods of evaluation. Teachers represented in these categories are ambiguous testers and inconsistent, temperamental graders. The Oprino lectures category characterizes those teachers who are unenthusiastic, overly repetitive and much too serious du.ring their classroom presentations. Information overload depicts teachers who are either overly demanding of sttudents or noticeably unreasonable in their instructional demands. Information underload characterizes
  • 33. those teachers who are too easy; those from whom students feel they have learned very little or absolutely nothing. The 2 categories of negative perlionalitv, and neaative physical appearance illustrate teachers who possess negative personal attributes. Teachers described in these categories tend to be moody and self-centered and often dress or act inappropriately in class. The doel not know subiect matter category illustrates those instructors who are obviously either unqualified to tear_h the subject matter or simply do not know the course content. Shows favoritism or oretudice characterizes those teachers who show preferences to particular students and who reinforce the concept of stereotypes in the classroom. Formion or reuional ficcentfh &naooropriate_ vo I tap and toad orammar4ppellino are 3 categories which capture teachers' misuse of language. Such teachers are described as unintelligible and/or hard to hear 16
  • 34. Teacher Misbehavior lb during lectures and often display poor language skills. Study 2 This study was designed to validate the categories of teacher misbehavior types obtained in Study 1 and to determine whether or not a conceptually meaningful 'factor structure .nderlies the original 28 categories. Quantitative data collected in tt's second study were employed to answer Research Questions 2 And 3: "How frequently do students report their college teacher engaging in each misbrhavior type?" and "What meaningful factor structure underlies the teacher misbehavior categories?" Qualitative data were also collected which assisted in our validation and .nterpretation of findings. IIETIODS PartIcipants were 261 (150 females, 111 males) undergraduate students enrolled in introductory communication classes satisfying general education electives at a large Western
  • 35. university. Approximately 26% of the sample were freshman, 31% sophomores, 28% juniors and 15% seniors. The mean age of the students in this sample was 25. Research Design. Whereas in Study 1 a research design was employed to maximize students' generation of teacher descriptions across teachers more generally, in this study the design was structured for each student to focus on a particular college teacher. While the former results reflect an accumulation of both numerous and disparate teacher misbrihavior types, the design 1 7 Teacher Misbehavior 17 for Study 2 essentially minimizes the reported diversity and frequency of teacher misbehaviors. That is, as a collective group, teachers may engage in a variety of different misbehavior types; however, we would not expect any individual teacher to
  • 36. exhibit all 28 types. By anchoring each student's perceptions to her/his respective teacher then, the design of this validational study allows for a rigorous assessment of the original 28 categories of misbehaviors. Procedures. Students were given questionnaires which explained that the instrument included "descriptions of things teachers have been observed doing or saying in some classes" which "college students have previously identified as teacher 'misbehaviors.'" They were also told that this study assessed "how often teachers engage in one or more of those behavior types or a behavior similar to those included in the descriptions." Students were instructed to complete the research instrument with reference to "on1v the teacher you have in the course you are taking that meets just before this class." This anchoring technique devuloped originally by Flax, Kearney, Richmond, and McCroskey (1986), maximized the variability in subject matter
  • 37. fields represented and allowed for a broad sample of instructors at the ,iniversity. In this way, data relating to over 250 different classes/teachers were obtained. After indicating their gender, age and year in school, students were provided with sets of multiple teacher misbehaviors representing each co" the 28 categories derived in Study 1 (see s Teacher Misbehavior 10 Table 1 for these descriptions). Category labels were not included on the questionnaire. Students were asked to indicate on a 0-4 scale "how frequently your teacher in that class exhibits the same or similar behaviors" with 0 Never and 4 m Very Often. Rweiutts. As expected, descriptive statistics revealed that even though most of the teachers sampled in this study never (0) or rarely (1) engaged in the sample misbehavior (M A.1), a
  • 38. number of others did. Importantly, the full range of student responses (0 to 4) was obtained across all 28 categories. Frequency percentages of those scoring 2 or higher ranged from 3.4% to 29.1% per category. Although some categories are more representative than others, these data provide evidence for the perceived occurrence of all ee misbehavior types. Table 2 provides the means, standard devitations and frequency percentages for those scores. Insert Table 2 about here Table 2 also provides a ranking of the misbehavior categories. Delivering boring lectures, straying from the subject matter, employing unfair testing procedures, presenting lectures which are confusing and unclear, and returning students' work late were the 5 most frequently cited teacher misbehaviors. Correspondingly, inductively-derived data from Study 1 revealed that 3 of those same misbehaviors were ranked in the top 5:
  • 39. Strays from subject, unfair testing, and boring lectures. The 43 Teacher Misbuthavior 19 two other misbehaviors ranked high in Study 1 were sarcasm/putdowns and absent from class. SLOOLLIalltratrx Rata arderaiisia. To assist us in validating the data repl3rted in Table 2, we asked each student ". . to explain hity you think your teacher behaves in the ways you've indicated. There may be a single reason or there may be several reasons for your teacher's behavior. Indicate the reason or reasons you think apply." Students were provided with enough writing space to briefly describe up to three prenumbered reasons for their teacher's behavior. Previous research (Kearney et al., in press) has shown that collecting these types of supplemental responses provide valuable and corroborating information. Sucn additional information allows for the triangulation of primary and secondary data sets---a powerful method (Morine-
  • 40. Dershimer, 1983) for increasing the overall validity of findings. Examination of students' reported reasons for their teachers' behavior proved to be revealing. Of the 261 students who participated, 117 indicated reasons why their current teacher misbehaves; 111 described reasons why their current teacher did not misbehave and/or why their teacher was so effective in the classroom; and 33 gave no reasons for either their teacher's misbehavior or effectiveness. It is particularly interesting that without being directly asked, almost 43% of the students indicated reasons why they felt their teacher was so effective in the classroom. Many of these same students also indicated that they could only say positive 20 Teacher Mlubehavior 20 things about the teacher for the course they had before this
  • 41. class. However, they also indicated that they either currently had another teacher who frequently misbehaved or that they had had teachers during their college career who had misbehaved in the variety of ways described on the questionnaire. As for teacher misbehaviors, over one-half of the responses either directly or in a restated form, included many of the actual teacher misbehaviors referenced in the original 28 categories. Table 3 provides representative samples of the reasons students gave for their particular teacher's misbehavior as well as those given for their teacher's effectiveness in the classroom. Insert Table 3 about here To summarize what was illustrated across these data, students who indicated that their teacher misbehaved described reasons that depicted their teacher as unable to relate to students, uncaring, preoccupied with other work, uninformed about course content, fearful about initiating personal relationships with students,
  • 42. outdated, selfish and self-centered, and not being committed to the teaching profession. In short, the reasons given for misbehavior suggest that students were less than satisfied with the way their teachers were behaving. On the other hand, students' explanations for their particular teacher's effectiveness portrayed teachers in quite the opposite direction. That is, the effectiveness of teachers was associated with Teacher MiebehavMor 24 attributes like a love for the teaching profession, the ability to establish a rapport with students, a solid knowledge of the subject matter, a sincere concern for students, a high level of professionalism, self confidence about teaching the course, an open and friendly nature, and the ability to create a challenging classroom environment. Effective teachers then, were perceived by students as doing a good job and as doing and saying things correctly in the classroom. These interview-type data both corroborate and elaborate on
  • 43. our other findings illustrating the validity of the teacher misbehavior categories derived in Study 1. Correspondingly, these data indicate that the majority of the students in this sample either currently or previously had a least one teacher who they perceived as behaving inappropriately. Even the students responding to a teacher they described in very positive ways described reasons which illustrated that the students in this sample were able to make a clear distinction between the effectiveness and ineffectiveness of what their college tearcs said and did in the classroom. Reaucing the Structure of the Categories Next, we determined whether the students' responses to the 28 categories as presented on the questionnaire could be reduced to a meaningful underlying factor structure. An overall default factor analysis (eigenvalue ( 1.0) resulted in an initial 7- factor solution. However, factors 1. 2 and 3 accounted for most of the variance (44.7%). Moreover, these first 3 factors were
  • 44. 22 BEST COPY AVAILABLE 04 Teacher Mlimbehavior 22 conceptually consistent. Subsequent analysis with 3-factor extractions produced stable factors with all items loading on their respective factor. An examination of the item loadings revealed that 7 items failed to meet a liberal 50/30 criterion. With those items eliminated, our second 3-factor solution increased the variance accounted for to 50.6%. The results of this 3-factor solution are reported in Table 4. Interfactor correlations between Factors 1 and 2 were .25, Factors 1 and 3 = .26 and Factors 2 and 3 = .18. Alpha reliabilities obtained for Factor 1 were .86 (M = 5.70, s.d. = 6.31, range = 0-32), Factor 2 = .130 = 2.179 s.d. = 3.51, range = 0-20) and Factor 3 = .80 (PI = 3.97, s.d. = 4.04, range = 0-23). Insert Table 4 about here
  • 45. Nine items comprised Factor is Confusing/unclear lectures, apat'netic to students, unfair testing, boring lectures, information overload, does not know subject matter, foreign or regional accents, inappropriate volume, and bad grammar/spelling. This factor was labeled "incompetence." Factor 2, labeled "offensiveness," consisted of 6 misbehavior categories: Sarcasm/putdowns, verbally abusive, unreasonable/arbitrary rules, sexual harassment, negative personality, and shows favoritism/prejudice. Items included in Factor 3, labeled "indolence," included 6 misbehavior types: Absent, tardy, unprepared/disorganized, deviates from syllabus, late returning work, and information underload. Teacher 'Misbehavior 23 lamals end Emrstac nmAshing Etassdurns. In order to substantiate the reliability of the 3-factor solution we
  • 46. completed two additional procedures. Firsti we employed a "random split sample" procedure (Armstrong and Soelberg, 1988) to create two within sample subsets. These randomly chosen subsets, each consisting of 130 student responses, were used to compute separate forced 3-factor extractions. Descriptively: the results of these additional factor analyses were virtually identical to those produced with the entire sample of students. These results are available upon request. Secondly, we followed up the randomized split sample procedure by computing similpritv concordant coefficients (Nesselroade and Baltes: 1970). This factor matching procedure was computed between the pairs of loadings produced on factors 1, 2, and 3 when the entire sample was included and those loadings produced on factors 1, 2, and 3 with a random split sample. The resulting concordant coefficient for factor 1 between the total and the split sample was .999; for factor 2 this index was .995; and for factor 3, .995. These indices provide strong descriptive
  • 47. support for a claim of factor invariance across each set of paired factor loadings. tsi ftnairses. With the reliability of the 3-factor solution substantiated, we attempted to determine potential effects of student age, gender and year in school on students' reports of teacher misbehavior across the dimensions of teacher incompetence, offensiveness, and indolence. We computed a 24 Teacher Misbehavior 24 regression-type 2 (college students' gender) X 4 (year in school) fixed effects multivariate analysis of covariance including student age as a covariate. The criterion variables were operationalized to include students' summed responses across each of the three dimensions of teacher misbehavior. Results indicated that neither student age (I:s of the covariate for each of the three dimensions of teacher misbehavior were ( = 1) nor
  • 48. gender and year in school were significant (all complex interactions or main effects a > .05; overall power estimates for all simple main effects were above .90). Identical results were obtained when the data were transformed into L scores prior to computing the MANCOVA. Computations on the standardized data indicate that the shapes of the distributions of the data reported in Table 2 in no way affected either the results of the MANCOVA or the factor analytic and factor matching procedures. Complete results are available upon request. Based on these findings then, at least for this sample of college students, age, gender and year in school have little influence on students' reports of teacher misbehavior. DISCUSSION NITH IMPLICATIONS FOR THE CLASSROOM This study represents a shift from the research tradition which focused on student misbehaviors in the classroom to a new perspective which examines teacher misbehaviors. We began this
  • 49. investigation by assuming that teachers can and do misbehave and that these misbehaviors can become potential sources of student dissatisfaction and resistance. In the first of two studies, we Teacher Mlubwhawier 25 asked college students to identify teacher misbehaviors they had observed during their college career. Over 1700 misbehaviors were inductively derived and then categorized into 28 different teacher misbehavior types. Clearly, students perceived their college teachers to "misbehave." Categories of teacher misbehaviors ranged from using bad grammar or misspelled words to sexual harassment and verbal abuse. The most frequently cited misbehavior types were (1) Sarcasm and Putdowns, (2) Absent, (3) Strays from Subject, (4) Unfair Testing and (5) Boring Lectures. Perhaps we have all been guilty of one or more of these misbehaviors and perhaps we have
  • 50. "justified" each and every transgression. Even so, from the students' point of view, teachers who cancel class or make their exams too difficult are "misbehaving." In our second study we presented another sample of college students with multiple misbehaviors representing each of the 28 categories identified in Study 1. interested in validating the existence of the misbehavior categories, we asked students to indicate how frequently a teacher they had currently engaged in each misbehavior type. Unlike Study 1 which was designed to maximize the generation of a number of different misbehaviors across teachers more generally, in Study 2 we anchored students' perceptions to specific target teachers. In this way, we were able to assess more realistically the range and frequency of each misbehavior actually occurring in the college classroom. As expected, most of the students reported that their own 26 Teacher Misbehavior
  • 51. teacher rarely engaged in the diversity of misbehavior types indexed. Gratefully then, it appears that most students find their teachers to "behave" appropriately. fisiore we become too relieved, however, it is important to note that the full range of frequencies was reported for each and every category. For instance, almost 30% of the students reported that their teachers frequently (occasionally to very often) spoke in monotone and rambled throughout the lectures. One-fourth indicated that their ozachers were often late in returning papers and exams, wasted class time with personal stories and opinions, asked trick questions on tests or made the items too ambiguous, talked too fast or lectured over students' heads, and confused students by being unclear or inconsistent in their expectations. Apparently, these and other misbehavior types occur frflquently enough for students to notice and for teachers to take pause. Moreover, our analysis of the reasons students' give to explain their teachers' behavior tends to corroborate the validity of the 28 misbehavior categories. Examination of these data also indicate that the
  • 52. majority of students were able to discriminate their particular teacher as either misbehaving or 12ehaving effectively based on the 28 categories. In an attempt to determine if a meLolingful factor structure underlied the 28 categories, we were able to reduce all but 7 categories into 3 interpretable dimensions: (l) Incompetence, (2) Offensiveness and (3) Indolence. The reliability of the three dimensions of teacher misbehavior was affirmed with Fandom 27 Teacher Maabehavier 27 split sample and factor matching procedures. Moreover, additional analyses of the factors indicated that students' age, gender, and year in school do not influence the way students report the misbehavior of their teachers. Misbehaviors represented by Incompetence reflect the lack of very basic teaching skills. Teachers who assign excessive work
  • 53. and rush through the material "to get it all done" may fail to recognize the importance of incremental methods of instruction. These same teachers may also be accused of making their tests too difficult and, at the same time, be unable or unwilling to help students succeed. Specifically, the misbehaviors included in this factor suggest that Incompetent teachers do not seem to care about either the course or the students themselves, do not know their students' names, will not review for exams and fail to allow for student input during class. The profile of Incompetence is extended further to those teachers who are unenthused about the material, speak in a monotone, enunciate poorly (or speak with difficult foreign or regional accents), and talk too loudly (or softly). Not only does Incompetence refer to instructirmal ineptitude, but this factor also implies that stuLdents perceive Incompetent teachers as ignorant anl confused. In other words, students report that teachers of this type are unable to answer questions in class,
  • 54. provide students with incorrect information when they do, lack currency in their area--and then compound the problem by presenting vague, confusing lectures and contradicting themselves 26 Teacher Milashavior BB in front of class. In short, teacher Incompetence reflects a number of teacher misbehaviors that clearly interfere with instructional goals and student learning. Teacher Offenkiveness included a number of misbehaviors that implied teachers could be mean, cruel and ugly. Apparently, offensive teachers humiiiate students in front of the class: insult and publicly embarrasses them. Offensive teachers may use profanity, become angry or yell and scream in their efforts to intimidate students. These same teachers are rude, self- centered, moody, and whiners; moreover, they condescend to
  • 55. students by acting superior and arrogant. If those characteristics and behaviors appear insufficient to label teachers of this type as Offensive, consider also reported misbehaviors of sexual harassment and prejudice. Students identify Offensive teachers as those who are chauvinistic, make sexual remarks and flirt with students. These teachers reportedly play "favorites" with their students and/or act prejudicial toward others. Finally, Offensive teachers appear unreasonable and arbitrary; they refuse to accept late work: punish the whole class for one student's infraction, and present themselves as rigid, inflexible and authoritarian. The third dimension underlying teacher misbehavior types, Indoler%e, best exemplifies the profile of the stereotypic, absent-minded college professor. Teachers who are considered Indolent are those who fail to show up for class, are late when they do, and offer poor excuses for their truancy. They might Teacher Milibihevior 29
  • 56. forget test datas and neglect to collect and grade students' homework. Indolent teachers are late in returning students' papers and exams. Because they are so disorganized, they fall behind in their schedules, change due daf-es for assignments and are forced to adjust their- syllabi. Students further report that indolent teachers "underwhelm" them with information by making their classes and tests too easy. Apparently, with indolent teachers, students do not feel they are learning as much as they should. Earlier research (Kearney, Plax, & Burroughs, in press) indicated that students blame one of two sources for their own resistance decisions: Teachers or students. The results of this investigation suggest that students may have legitimate cause for those attributions. That is, our findings reveal that teachers themtielves "misbehave" in the college classroom. While the degree or frequency of those misbehaviors may vary widely across
  • 57. college teachers, students do, in fact, perceive all 28 different misbehavior types to occur. Whether or not misbehaviors of incompetence, offensiveness and indolence actually are causally antecedent to student resistance or other misbehaviors needs further examination. In this way, future res:earch should examine the interactive nature of teacher ant student resistance in the classroom. IMPLICATIONS FOR THE TEACHER Our results demonstrate that there are a variety of teacher misbehaviors which are likely to influence and potentially, 30 o II Teacher Misbehavior 30 stimulate student problems in the classroom. The existence of these misbehaviors was reaffirmed by students' reports of current
  • 58. and previous experiences with teachers. We recommend that teachers examine the list of 28 misbehavior categories in light of their own classroom behaviors. Many of the categories represent misbehaviors instructors do almost unknowingly. In fact, we are all guilty of engaging in one or more of these behaviors from time to time. The decision to label what we say and do as "misbehaviors" has important :.nstructional consequences. While we may be reluctant or unwilling to view our grading procedures as unfair, our accent as incomprehensible, and our attendance rules as unreasonable, students may disagree. While we may feel justified in changing the syllabus unexpectedly, embarrassing a student who interrupts the class, and returning graded papers and exams late, students may disagree. And when they dot undesirable student responses may result. Such responses can take many forms, including negative teacher evaluations, poor attendance, classroom disruptions, and lower achievement. Recognizing these potential consequences, we need to consider
  • 59. students' perceptions as well as our own in our decisions about what we do and say in the classroom. 60 Teacher Niabehavlar 31 REFERENCES Armstrong, J. S., 8, SoelOerg, P. (1968). On the interpretation of factor analysis. exyaftgiczajtsAL Bulletin, Mg 361-364. Brophy, J. E., 8 Evertson, C. M. (1976). Learnino fr_om teaching: a gmtnimmEnntal perspective. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Brophy, J. E., & Good, T. L. (1986). Teacher behavior and student achievement. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research Ea teaching (3rd Ed.) (pp. 32B-375). New York: Macmillan. Burroughs, N. F., Kearney, P., & Plax, T. G. (1989). Compliance- resistance in the college classroom. Communication
  • 60. 219 214-e29. Cantrell, R. P., Stenner, A. Jo, 8 Katzenmeyer, W. G. (1977). Teacher knowledge, attitudes, and classroom teaching correlates of student achic:vement. Journal pi gOucational elystiolpox, 69, 180-190. Doyle, W. (1986). Classroom ornization and management. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook pl,resturct teachino (3rd Ed.) (pp. 392-431). New York: Macmillan. Emmer, E. T., Evertson, C. M., Sanford, J. Pm, Clements, B. S., t% Worsham, M. E. (1984). Classroom mar:Element ism seconsiory IMIREARLI. Englewood Cliffs, N3s Prentice-Hall. Evertson, C. M., Emmer, E. T., Clements, B. S., Sanford, J. P., Worsham, J. E. (1984). Classroom manamement fgr.plementory teachers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Gage, N. Le, I Needels, M. C. (1989). Process-product research .* 41, A Teacher Misbehavior
  • 61. 3e on teachings A review of criticisms. Ths. flementarv, School Journal, 253-300. Good, T. Log ett Brophy, J. E. (1986). School effects. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook gf research wi teechino (3rd Ed.) (pp. 570-602). New York: Macmillan. Kearney, P. (1987). Power in the classroom. Journal 211- houahto gg, 45-50. Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., 8, Burroughs, N. F. (ir press). An attributional analysis of college students' resistance decisions. Communication Education. Kearney, P., Plax, T.G., Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1984). Power in the classroom IV: Alternatives to discipline. In R. Bostrom (Ed.), Communicatipn yearbook g, (pp. 724-746). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., Richmond, V. P., L McCroskey, J. C. (1985). Power in the classroom Ills Teacher communication techniques and messages. Cpmmunica ion, gdpgation, WI, 19-
  • 62. 28. Kearney, P., Plax, T. S., Smith, V. R., & Sorensen, G. (1988). Effects of teacher immediacy and strategy type on college student resistance to on-task demands. Communication Education, 225 54-67. Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., Sorensen, G., 11. Smith, V. R. (1988). Experienced and prospective teachers' selections of compliance-gaining messages for "common" student misbehaviors. CommunicAtion gducation!, 22., 150-164. .* Teacher Mlobehavior 33 Morine-Dershimer, S. (1983). Taming ITRArabsmthinkino throuoh trianoulation cri data pets. Austin, TX: The Research and Development Center for Teacher Education, University of Texas, Report No. 8014. Nesselroade, J. R., 8 Baltes, P. B. (1970). On a dilemma of
  • 63. comparative factor analysis: A study of factor matching based on random data. Educational and Psvcholooical Measurement, 29.11 935-948. Plax, T. S., 8 Kearney, P. (1990). Classroom management; Structuring the classroom for work. In J. Daly, G. Friedrich, & A. Vangelisti (Eds.), Teaching communication: Theory, research, and,methgqs (pp. 223-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, Flax, T. S., Kearney, P., McCroskey, J. C., 8 Richmond, V. P. (1986). Power in the classroom.VI: Verbal control strategies, nonverbal immediacy and affective learning. Communication Educatign, 25., 43-55. Rosenshine, B., & Stevens, R. (1986). Teaching functions. In M. -C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook gi: resparch aateachino (3rd Ed.) (pp. 376-391). New Yorks Macmillan. Wittrock, M. C. (1986). Students' thought processes. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook 21 research Im teaching (3rd Ed.)
  • 64. (pp. 297-314). New York: Macmillan. Woolfolk, A. E. (1987). Fducational psycholocv (3rd Ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 3 4' Woolfolk, A. E., I McCune-Nicolich, gsvcholoov far. teaGhers (2nd Prentice-Hal1. 3r Teacher Misbehavior 34 L. (1984). Educational Ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Table 1 "k. Mamba-vim Teacher Misbmhavior 35
  • 65. h.', A- L.' Enniumrat. 711103k ABSENT Does not show up for class, cancels class without notification, and/or offers poor aacuses for being absunt. TARDY Is late for class or tardy. 142 93 5 a 2 KEEPS STUDENTS OVERTIME gawps class overtime, talks too long or starts class early before all thm students are thore. 90 5 7 EARLY DISMISSAL 22 1 23 Lets class out early, rushes through the material to get done early. STRAYS "F- SUIL3ECI7 117 7 3 Uses the class as a forum for her/hts
  • 66. personal opinions, goms off an tangents, talks about family and personal life and/or gmnarally Hostas class time. CONFUSINEWUPCLEAR LECTURES Unclmar about what is aspectod. lectures are confusime end vague. contradicts hins/harself sumps from onm subject to another and/or lectures are inconsistent with assigned readings. IDISORBANIZED Twachwr fasbehavior 4 12 Is not prepared for class, umrganized, forgets tsst dates, and/or makes assignments but does not collect them. DEVIATES FROM SYLLABUS 35 2 20 Changes due datss for assignmnints, behind schindule, doss not follow the syllabus, Changes assignments, and/or assigns books but does not usw them. 21 1 24LATE RETURNINS MORK
  • 67. Late in returning papers, late in grading and turning back Imams, and/or forgets to bring graded papers to class. SARCASM AND PUTDOIINS 154 9 1 I. sarcastic and rude, maks, fun of and humiliates students, picks on students, and/or insults and embarrasses students. VERBALLY ABUSIVE 69 4 11 Uses profanity, is angry and Amman, yells and screams, interrupts and/or intisidatss students. UNREASONABLE AND ARBITRARY RULES 23 1 22 Rifuses to accept late mark, gives no breaks in 31-bour classes. punishing" inntirs class for ono student's 'misbehavior, and/or is rigid, inflexible end authoritarian. SEXUAL HARASSMENT IS 1 25 Makes sexual remarks to students, flirts with thee, sakes sexual innuendos and/or is chauvinistic. 37
  • 68. UNRESPONSIVE TO STUDENTS' QUESTIONS Dom. not encourage students to ask questions, does not answer questions or recognize raised hands, and/or seems °put out" to have to explain or ropeat himiheroelf. APATHETIC TO STUDENTS Doesn't seam to care about time course or show concern for students, dome not know the students' name's, rejects students' opinions and/or does not allow for class discussion. INACCESSIBLE TO STUDENTS OUTSIDE OF CLASS Does not show up for appointmmnts or schedutmd office hours, I. hard to contact, will not meet with students outside of office time and/or doesn't make time for studonts when they need help. UNFAIR TESTINS Asks trick westions on tests, exams do not relate to the lectures, tests are too difftcult, qumstions are too ambiguous, and/or teachar does not review for exams. UNFAIR GRADING grades unfairly, changes grading
  • 69. policy during the semester, does nat believe in giving A's, makes mistakes when grading and/or does not have a prefttermined grading scale. WINING LECTURES I. not an enthusiastic lectr-er, speaks in monotone and rambles, is boring, too much rspetition and/or employs no variety in lectures. 35 BEST COPY AURAE Teacher PlisbWhavior 37 76 4 73 4 50 3 110 6 62 4 a 10 17 13
  • 70. INFOINIATION OVEFILOAD INFORMATION UNDERLOAD NEGATIVE PERSONALITY Teacher is impatient, self-centered, complains, acts superior and/or is moody. NEGATIVE PHYSICAL APPEARANCE Talks too fast and rushes through the material, talks over the students' heads, uses obscure terms and/or assigns excessive work. The class is too easy, students feel they have not learned anything, and/or tests are too easy. Teacher drosses sloppy, smells bad, clothes are out of style, and cares little About hisfher overall appearance. DOES NOT KNOM SUBJECT MATTER Doesn't know the material, unmble to answer questions, provides incorrect information, and/or isn't current. MOMS FAVORITISM OR PREJUDICE
  • 71. Plays favorites with students or acts prejudiced against others, is narrow- minded or close-minded, and/or makes prejudicial remarks. FOREIGN OR RESIONAL ACCENTS reacher is hard to understand, enunciates poorly, and has a strong accent that makes it difficult to understand. INAPPROPRIATE VOLUME Doesn't speak loudly enough or speaks too loud. Teacher Misbehavior 30 46 3 18 45 2 19 57 3 15 24 1 21 62 4 14 52 3 16 16 0.9 26 9 0.5 27
  • 72. BAD BRAMIAR/SPELL INF; Uses bad grammars writes illegibly, miumpells words on the exam (or on the board) and/or generally uses poor English. ALL OTHERS NOT CATEGORIZED 61=.1=0.M.....M.M...MNMWAM11 4 ( Teacher Misbehavior 39 7 0.4 50 3 Teacher Misbehavior 40 Table 2 . -1 t . ' t t 4. iL k' , Lt t t .1: _ t Category Mean SD Z Rank Absent .46 .83 10.7 17.5
  • 73. Tardy .87 1.09 20.7 8 Keeps students overtime .79 1.09 18.7 9 Early dismissal .72 .87 15.0 10 "trays from subject .98 1.15 27.6 2 Confusing/unclear lectures .94 1.15 24.6 4 UMprepared/disorganized .40 .79 6.8 25 Deviates from syllabus .84 1.10 21.1 7 Late returning work .87 1.16 23.8 5 Sarcasm and putdowns .49 .46 11.6 15 Verbally Abusive .26 .75 6.1 26 Unreasonable/arbitrary rules .39 .89 9.0 21 Sexual harassment .15 .55 3.4 28 Wresponsive to students' questions .34 .73 8.0 22.5 Apathetic to students .45 .91 10.7 17.5 Inaccessible to students .37 .81 7.7 24 Unfair testing .93 1.18 27.0 3 Unfair grading .52 1.01 13.4 11 Boring lectures 1.08 1.34 29.1 1 Informmtion overload .82 1.11 23.4 6 Information underload .52 .92 12.2 14 Negative personality .46 .95 12.7 12.5 Negative physical appearance .36 .82 8.0 22.5 Does not know subject matter .25 .60 1.0 27 Shows favoritism or prejudice .41 .85 9.2 20 Foreign or regional accents .48 .93 12.7 12.5 Inappropriate volume .36 .62 7.9 19 Bad grammar/spelling .40 .87 11.1 16 *Absolute mean = 2.0, with 0 = never and 4 = very often. Teacher Misbshavior 41
  • 74. TABLE 3 Sludent Reasons for Teacher Misbehavior, ang_for_IssishscAlfisstiturasi_in_Abaraininnis I. ReasoneLfor Teishmr Misbehavior °She doesn't relate with our culture." "She is so well Educated that she can't relate to students." "He says that this jiab is just stepping stone for him before he gets to temch at a better univErsity." "He behaves this way to get it across to the !students not to F-- K with him.' "I think the acts tn these mays because as she says: 'I'm not a Educator, I's a sathematician'." 'I think my Religious Studies teacher would be happiEr writing a book thandtctating to aur class.' "My teacher doesn't understand what she is trying to teach us." 'In order to sake a tmst more challenging he auks trick questions.' "He is late because he is so busy and puts the class behind his other interests.' "I think my teacher is thy and is afraid to be a real person with
  • 75. us." 'He thinks everyone tn his B.E. class is rnrolled because it if their major.' 'The instructor is mostly into research and those not to care about students.' 'She is mad at the university and takes it out on us.' 'Has gan I don't care if you come to class or mat attitude'.' "Bmcause the is a very oWionated feminist." 'As far as his dress is concerned, he feels that has nothing to do with what he is trying to tooth.' 4S 440 Teacher Misbehavior II. Reasons far Teacher_Effnctivenwes "My teacher likes what be is teaching." "Hoe sincerely likes students and loves to express himself clearly." 'She likes teaching and enjoys the rapport she has with her students." "She reolly cares about the information being delivered to the class."
  • 76. "He does keep us over sometimes, but that's only because he gets so excited about the material." "She's a great teacher. I think it is because she has her masters in communication. She knows haw to be an effective instructor.' "Ikecause he's fair and truthful to students." "He definiteiy hes the desire for making each student understand the materi41." "He's always prepared and explains the subject well." "because she teaches what is useful." "He iv very open, warm. and kind to every student in the class regardless of sem or race." "She loves her job and it shows." 'He really encourages discussion and takes student's opinions as valid and equal to his own." "He is a powerful speaker. From the first day of class I told myself I'd like to be like him." "My teacher is challenging but I like her that may. She wants us to learn what she knows." *These examples illustrate reoccurring themes. More complete lists are available upon request.
  • 77. Teacher Misbehavior 43 Table 4 Factor Affillysis_of Teacher Misbehavior TYPWs. MISBEHAVIOR YWCOMPETENCE OFFENSIVENESS INMOLENCE Absent .05 -.01 .60 Tardy .09 02 .62 Confusing/Unclear lecturms .68 .08 .39 Unprepared/Disorganized .37 .08 .73 arviates from syllabus .09 .15 .70 Late returning mark .23 .29 .75 Sarcaem and putdomns .11 .82 .05 Verbally abusive .04 .79 .07 Uhreasonaiblm/arbitrary rules .16 .62 .02 Sexual harassment -.10 .52 .15 Apathmtic to students .61 .31 .10 Unfair testing .68 .11 .07 Sering lecture* .69 -.01 .19 Information overload .73 .17 .17 Information umderload .03 .02 .54 Negative personality .43 .63 .19 Does not know subject matter .57 .03 .19 Shows favoritism/prejudice .27 .64 .01 Foreigndragional accents .70 .02 00 Inappropriate volume .70 .17 -.06 Sad grammar/spelling .70 .09 .11 Eigenvalume 6.22 2.27 2.13 Variance 29.60 10.80 10.10
  • 78. Alpha Reliabilities .86 .80 .00 c14 . '. .,.. .'.-. _'_'_-~"">'-"" r:::, ",,-,-_¥,~ ~L'-,-_"',-- J.!."" Teacher Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior: Unintentional Negative Communication Katherine S. Thweatt West Virginia University James C. McCroskey West Virginia University This study involved the manipulation of teacher immediacy and teacher misbehaviors to determine their individual and combined impacts on perceived teacher immediacy and teacher misbehavior. Participants in the study were students enrolled in undergraduate communication classes. The results indicated that teacher nonimmediacy and teacher misbehaviors could not be manipulated independently. Analysis of variance indicated that nonimmediate teachers were perceived to be misbehaving even when no misbehaviors were
  • 79. induced in the experiments, It was concluded that students perceive teachers who communicate in nonimmediate ways as misbehaving. Although immediacy has been clearly defined within the field of communication (see Mehrabian, 1969; Andersen, 1978), nonimmediacy cues have been viewed as nothing more than the opposite of immediacy cues. For the purposes of this research, immediacy is defined as "those communication behaviors that reduce perceived distance between people." Immediate behaviors may actually decrease the physical distance, or they may decrease the psychological distance, The more immediate a person is, the more likely he/ she is to communicate at close distances, smile, engage in eye contact use direct body orientations, use overall body movement and gestures, touch others, relax, and be vocally expressive. In other words, we might say that an immediate person is perceived as overtly friendly and warm" (Andersen, 1979). In instructional research, some immediacy cues that have been manipulated are eye contact, smiling, movement in the classroom, general Katherine S. Thweatt (M.A., West Virginia University, 1996) is a doctoral student at West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26506-6293. James C. McCroskey (Ed,D., Pennsylvania State University, 1966) is a Professor and Chairperson of the Department of Communication Studies at West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26506-6293. This
  • 80. paper is based on research conducted by the first author as part of her M.A. thesis project which was directed by the second author. COMMUNICATION RESEARCH REPORTS, Volume 13, Number 2, pages 198-204 Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior -Page 199 friendliness/ approachability, and enthusiasm. Conversely, nonimmediacy has been manipulated as not smiling, lack of eye contact, lack of movement in the classroom, etc. The impact of immediate and nonimmediate cues have been the focus of a considerable amount of research in which it was found that immediacy has positive impacts in the classroom and nonimmediacy less positive impacts. Students have been shown to have more positive affect for teachers high in immediacy which leads to increased affective learning (Andersen, 1979; Kearney, Plax, & Wendt-Wasco, 1985; Plax, Kearney, McCroskey, & Richmond, 1986; Richmond, McCroskey, Plax, & Kearney, 1986). Research also indicates that students are unable to differentiate prosocial from antisocial Behavior Alteration Techniques when used by more immediate teachers (Kearney, Plax, and Burroughs, 1991). Extant research on immediacy has not identified precisely what students' perceive nonimmediate communication behaviors to be. In a study investigating the impact of
  • 81. teacher immediacy on teacher misbehaviors, some evidence was uncovered that may help classify certain behaviors as nonimmediate cues and, hence, aid in the explanation of the less than positive impact of nonimmediacy in the classroom. Teacher misbehaviors have been defined as "those teacher behaviors that interfere with student learning" (Kearney, Plax, Hays & Ivey, 1991). As a result of her research, Dolin (1995) suggested that nonimmediate communication behaviors of teachers may be perceived by students as misbehaviors, because they clearly interfere with their learning. The present study sought to investigate that speculation directly under controlled experimental conditions. Since this research was based more on informed speculation than on firm theory, and reasonable explanations for divergent outcomes were clearly possible, we chose to pose a research question rather than an hypotheses. The following question was posed: "Can teacher immediacy /nonimmediacy be manipulated independently of teacher misbehaviors?" An affirmative answer to this question would indicate that teacher nonimmediacy is seen as just that, not as a form of teacher misbehavior. A negative answer to the question would confirm Dolin's (1995) speculation that students perceive teacher nonimmediacy as misbehavior. In agreeing with Dolin, we anticipated the negative outcome. However, since the misbehavior and immediacy constructs were developed independently, it was seen as quite possible they might be completely independent constructs.
  • 82. METHOD Design In a 2 x 2 factorial design, participants were exposed to one of four scenarios. Teacher immediacy (immediate/nonimmediate) and teacher misbehavior (appropriate behavior/ misbehavior) were manipulated across four scenarios. After reading a scenario, participants were asked to complete scales measuring level of perceived immediacy and level of perceived teacher misbehavior. This research also included a built-in replication. Procedure In large lecture classes at West Virginia University participants were exposed to one of four stimulus behavior scenarios (See Appendix A). After reading the scenario, the participant was asked to complete scales measuring perceived teacher immediacy and perceived teacher misbehavior. The replication was identical to the original study except that different scenarios representing different operationalizations of immediacy and misbehavior were employed (See Appendix A). Page 200 - Communication Research ReportsjFall1996 Participants Participants were 382 undergraduate students enrolled in communication classes.
  • 83. These classes were ones which serve all segments of the undergraduate student body and enroll approximately equal numbers of females and males. All were given credit for a course assignment for participation. There were 195 participants in study one and 187 in study two. Manipulations Immediacy. Teacher immediacy was manipulated in the four scenarios. All immediacy and nonimmediacy cues were derived from the work of Andersen (1979). Six nonverbal behaviors were manipulated to create two levels of immediacy. In the high immediacy condition, the teacher was immediate in all six nonverbal behaviors. In the low immediacy condition, the teacher engaged in no immediate behavior. Different immediacy behaviors were included in study two than in study one. Misbehavior. Teacher misbehavior was also manipulated in the four scenarios. Three behaviors were manipulated that were considered either appropriate behavior or misbehavior. Two levels of behavior were created by varying the amount of appropriate and inappropriate behaviors. In the misbehavior condition, the teacher engaged in only inappropriate behaviors. In the nonmisbehavior condition, the teacher engaged in all appropriate behaviors. Different misbehaviors were employed in study two than in study one.
  • 84. Measures Subjects completed rating scales which measured perceived teacher immediacy and misbehaviors. Both scales employed two-item, seven-point response formats. Subjects were provided with the definition of immediacy. The level of immediacy of the teacher in the scenario was measured with two adjective pairs: "very immediate/very nonimmediate" and" approachable/ unapproachable." Scores could range from 2 to 14 with a higher score indicating a more positive answer. The obtained alpha reliability for this two-item measure was .94. A definition of teacher misbehavior was also provided for the subjects. Perceived frequency of misbehavior was measured by responses to two adjective pairs: "never/very often" and" frequently/seldom." Scores could range from 2 to 14 with a higher score indicating more frequent misbehavior. The obtained alpha reliability for this two-item measure was. 98. . Data Analyses The dependent variables and manipulation checks were subjected to two-way analyses of variance. Post-hoc cell comparisons were made to probe significant interaction effects obtained in both studies. Alpha for all tests was set at .05.
  • 85. Immediacy. Analysis of variance for the manipulation check for immediacy indicated the inductions in both studies were generally successful. In Study One, the highly immeqiate teacher was perceived as significantly more immediate (F{1,188} = 113.58, P < .05, eta2 = .37) than the less immediate teacher, m = 11.5 and m = 5.9 respectively. In Study Two, the highly immediate teacher (m = 11.4) also was perceived as significantly more immediate (F{1,180}= 151.4, P < .05, eta2 = .44) than the teacher low in immediacy (m = 5.3). ..r '':'''.' -'._/.~~..c' ..: '... .'. " - ,_.. . Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior - Page 201 The impact of the manipulation of misbehavior did not impact perceived immediacy in either study (F < 1.0). There was a significant cross-over interaction observed in both studies (Study 1, F(l, 188) = 4.68, P < .05, eta2 = .02; Study 2, F(l, 180) = 11.39, P < .05, eta2 = .03). As noted in Table 1, the perceived immediacy in the low immediacy condition was somewhat higher in the "misbehavior" condition than the "no misbehavior" condition in both studies. In contrast, the perceived immediacy in the high immediacy condition was somewhat higher in the "no misbehavior" condition than in the "misbehavior" condition for both studies. Tests
  • 86. between cell means reflecting the interaction were significant for the second study, but were not significant in the first study. Given the low effect sizes for these tests, this interaction probably should not be seen as particularly important, particularly considering the very large main effects observed. TABLE 1 Means for Immediacy Conditions Dependent Variable Study Number Low Immediacy Appropriate Misbehavior High Immediacy Appropriate Misbehavior Immediacy 5.3ac 6.5bd 12.0ab 10.9cd 2 4.5adf 6.lbef 12.3abc 1O.6cde Note: Means with the same subscript in the same study are significantly different at (at least) the .05 level. Behavioro The analysis of variance for the measures of teacher misbehavior indicated that a significant amount of variance was accounted for on perceived teacher misbehavior by the two induction variables in both Study 1 (F{3, 190} =
  • 87. 20.8, P < .05, eta2= .25) and Study 2 (F {3, 186} = 23.8, P < .05, eta2 = .28). The interaction effect was significant in both studies and, as indicated in Table 2, it was the primary explanation for the overall significant AOV models. In both studies the "high immediacy I appropriate behavior" condition was reported as having significantly less teacher misbehavior than the other three conditions (which did not significantly differ from each other). Thus, the condition in each study in which no teacher misbehaviors were induced was seen as having a high level of teacher misbehavior if the teacher was described as nonimmediate. For the participants in these studies, then, teachers who allegedly behaved in nonimmediate ways were seen as misbehaving, even if they were performing none of the behaviors described as misbehavior in previous research. DISCUSSION The primary purpose of these studies was to investigate the mediating effect of immediacy on perceived teacher misbehaviors. While subjects did perceive differences in immediacy and misbehaviors for all scenarios, the results indicated that immediate teacher behaviors and teacher misbehaviors cannot be manipulated fully independently (or, at least, were not in these studies). The low immediate, non- misbehaving teacher was perceived as misbehaving as much as the low immediate teacher that was misbehaving.
  • 88. Page 202 -Communication Research ReportsjFall1996 TABLE 2 Means for Misbehavior Conditions Dependent Variable Study Number Low Immediacy Appropriate Misbehavior High Immediacy Appropriate Misbehavior Misbehavior 9.8a 8.7b 4.9abc 9.5c 2 9.3a IO.Ib 5.0abc 9.3c Note: Means with the same subscript in the same study are significantly different at (at least) the .05 level. This particular result is counter-intuitive in that it was expected that the low immediate, misbehaving teacher would be rated as misbehaving more than all of the other teacher types. Apparently there is an upper limit on perceptions of teacher misbehavior, and nonimmediacy reaches that limit all by itself. This leads to the most important conclusion we may draw from this research:
  • 89. Nonimmediacy is seen by students as misbehavior. Dolin's (1995) research indicated that some misbehaviors previously identified by students are actually nonimmediate behaviors. This research indicates that the reverse also appears to be true. In the present studies nonimmediacy overwhelmed the impact of good behaviors. This suggests that nonimmediacy acts as a misbehavior, even though students do not necessarily recognize it as a misbehavior at a conscious level. Students did not, in Kearney, Plax, Hays, and Ivey (1991) research, generate nonimmediacy as an actual misbehavior. One explanation for this is that students may perceive this simply as "normal" bad teaching behaviors as a result of the numerous teachers they have seen engaging in these behaviors. Such common-place acts of nonimmediacy are actually behaviors or misbehaviors that serve to interfere with the learning process. It can be concluded that, for the most part, teachers who engage in misbehaviors and are low in immediacy do not produce greater perceptions of misbehavior than the teacher who is low in immediacy, but engages in no other misbehaviors, at least of the type investigated in this research. CONCLUSIONS From the results of this investigation, it can be concluded that nonimmediate teacher behaviors can be classified as teacher misbehaviors. Thus, teachers who, either by choice or ignorance, fail to engage in immediate behaviors are misbehaving, at least in the eyes of
  • 90. their students. This may explain why students perceive the nonimmediate teacher, who functions in an otherwise efficient manner, less positively than the immediate teacher who may not even be efficient in accomplishing his/her duties. At least in these studies, students consistently evaluated the immediate teacher more positively than the nonimmediate teacher who engaged in no misbehaviors, even if the teacher was misbehaving. Students consistently evaluated the nonimmediate teacher as engaging in a high level of misbehavior, even if that teacher was credited with otherwise only appropriate behavior. For teachers, it is necessary to realize that simply doings one's job correctly is not enough. Teachers must make students comfortable about approaching her / him and ensure students that he/ she is comfortable with approaching students. Creating a ,< . .... Nonimmediacy and Misbehavior -Page 203 feeling of closeness, whether it is psychological or physical, ensures the teacher who may be forced to engage in what students perceive to be inappropriate behaviors (misbehave) on occasion that hel she will not lose the positive affect of the students. Future researchers may wish to consider extending this research by manipulating nonimmediacy cues as misbehaviors rather than just nonimmediate behaviors.
  • 91. REFERENCES Andersen, J. F. (1978). Teacher immediacy and teaching effectiveness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, West Virginia University, Morgantown. Andersen, J. F. (1979). Teacher immediacy as a predictor of teaching effectiveness. In D. Nimmo (Ed.), Communication Yearbook, 3, (pp. 543-559). new Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. Dolin, D. J. (1995). Ain't Misbehavin'; A study of teacher misbehaviors, related to communication behaviors, and student resistance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, West Virginia University, Morgantown. Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., & Burroughs, N. F. (1991) Anattributionalanalysis of college students resistance decisions. Communication Education, 40, 325-342. Kearney, P., Pia x, T. G., Hays, E. R., & Ivey, M. J. (1991). College teacher misbehaviors: What students don't like about what teachers say and do. Communication Quarterly, 39, 309-324. Kearney, P., Plax, T. G., & Wendt-Wasco, N. J. (1985). Teacher immediacy for affective learning in divergent college classes. Communication Quarterly, 33, 61-74.
  • 92. Mehrabian, A. (1969). Some referents and measures of nonverbal behavior. Behavioral Research Methods and Instruments, 1,213-217. Pia x, T. G., Kearney, P., McCroskey, J. c., & Richmond, V. P. (1986). Power in the classroom VI: Verbal control strategies, nonverbal immediacy and affective learning. Communication Education, 35, 43-55. Richmond, V. P., McCroskey, J. c., Pia x, T. G., & Kearney P. (1986). Teacher nonverbal immediacy training and student affect. World Communication, 15, 181- 194. APPENDIX A EXPERIMENTAL MANIPULATIONS Scenarios -Study 1 Scenario 1: Immediate Teacher/Misbehavior You are taking a class from a teacher who smiles frequently, uses vocal variety, and gestures. Moreover, this teacher is enthusiastic, walks around the classroom, and seems relaxed in the classroom. He/ she frequently cancels class without notice, does not follow the syllabus, and is usually unprepared for class. Scenario 2: Immediate Teacher/ Appropriate BehaviOr You are taking a class from a teacher who smiles frequently, uses vocal variety, and gestures. Moreover, this teacher is enthusiastic, walks around the classroom, and seems relaxed in the
  • 93. classroom. He/ she is always on time for class, follows the syllabus, and arrives at class prepared. Scenario 3: Nonimmediate Teacher/Misbehavior You are taking a class from a teacher who lacks facial expression, uses monotone, and doesn't ..,~,':: ....'... ;:'.2;:'1'!A~:d."./ :);.L.J::.t:.t.&;'j;''>.')::''';k'..::~'''::.>1..'.L1.:..~ n:",:..L~.. .. ~ Page 204 - Communication Research Reports/Fall 1996 gesture. Moreover, this teacher is unenthusiastic, stands behind the podium throughout class, and seems tense. He/ she frequently cancels class without notice, does not follow the syllabus, and is usually unprepared for class. Scenario 4: Nonimmediate Teacher/ Appropriate Behavior You are taking a class from a teacher who lacks facial expression, uses monotone, and doesn't gesture. Moreover, this teacher is unenthusiastic, stands behind the podium throughout class, and seems tense. He/ she is always on time for class, follows the syllabus, and arrives at class prepared. Scenarios - Study 2 Scenario 1: Immediate Teacher/Misbehavior
  • 94. You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very relaxed, looks at the class when teaching, and walks around the room during the lecture. Moreover, this teacher engages in a lot of eye contact and is generally perceived as friendly and approachable. He/she assigns an excessive amount of homework, lectures in a confusing manner, and is generally unprepared for class. Scenario 2: Immediate Teacher/ Appropriate Behavior You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very relaxed, looks at the class when teaching, and walks around the room during the lecture. Moreover, this teacher engages in a lot of eye contact and is generally perceived as friendly and approachable. He/she assigns a reasonable amount of homework, lectures in an organized manner, and is generally prepared for class. Scenario 3: Nonimmediate Teacher/Misbehavior You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very tense, looks at the board while talking, and stands behind the podium throughout the lecture. Moreover, this teacher looks at the board when lecturing and is generally perceived as unfriendly and unapproachable. He/she assigns an excessive amount of homework, lectures in a confusing manner, and is generally unprepared for class. Scenario 4: Nonimmediate Teacher/ Appropriate Behavior You are taking a class from a teacher who seems very tense,
  • 95. looks at the board while talking, and stands behind the podium throughout the lecture. Moreover, this teacher looks at the board when lecturing and is generally perceived as unfriendly and unapproachable. He/ she assigns a reasonable amount of homework, lectures in an organized manner, and is generally prepared for class.