SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 39
Common Mistakes I see on this paper are
1. Using summaries and reviews written by other authors rather
than using the actual IOM report and viewing each section, or
viewing the IOM's summaries of it's report.
2. Forgetting to include information on how the IOM report will
or has impacted one's practice. This is worth a high percentage
of points
3. Not formatting the paper in APA, citing sources or using
current sources like those supplied in our course resources for
the week (since I am supplying a template APA should be
flawless)
4. Going over the allowed length (papers that are over the
criteria for length will be returned for revision)
5. Including background information, losing focus - remember
the goal is to discuss the recommendations for education,
practice, leadership and discuss how they can be achieved.
6. Ineffective introduction, too long, does not introduce the
topic briefly, does not include a summary of what the paper will
cover
LINKS TO THE IOM REPORT AND IOM SUMMARIES OF
THE REPORT (These should be your main references for the
report, don’t use a summary written about the report, use the
IOMs summaries)
Download whole report as guest
Report Brief
Summary of Report on Education
Summary of Report on Practice
Summary of recommendations
Great infograph that highlights IOM recommendations. Here is
the link. This link gives an outline of the IOM's
recommendations. Both offer an option that provides quick
access to needed information in a brief and easy to follow
format. Hope these are helpful.
How to Reference and Cite the IOM Report
IOM will each have the same author (the Institute of Medicine
is the author) and year you will differentiate them by adding a
small case letter after the year of publication in both your
reference and citation. The reference would be listed in
alphabetical order using the title. I have posted examples
below.
Institute of Medicine [IOM]. (2010). Future of nursing: Focus
on education. Retrieved from
www.nationalacademies.org/hmd/~/media/Files/Report%20Files
/2010/The-Future-of-
Nursing/Nursing%20Education%202010%20Brief.pdf
Research Article
The Cross-Category Effect
Mere Social Categorization Is Sufficient to Elicit an
Own-Group Bias in Face Recognition
Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg
Miami University
ABSTRACT—Although the cross-race effect (CRE) is a well-
established phenomenon, both perceptual-expertise and
social-categorization models have been proposed to ex-
plain the effect. The two studies reported here investigated
the extent to which categorizing other people as in-group
versus out-group members is sufficient to elicit a pattern of
face recognition analogous to that of the CRE, even when
perceptual expertise with the stimuli is held constant. In
Study 1, targets were categorized as members of real-life
in-groups and out-groups (based on university affiliation),
whereas in Study 2, targets were categorized into experi-
mentally created minimal groups. In both studies, recog-
nition performance was better for targets categorized as
in-group members, despite the fact that perceptual ex-
pertise was equivalent for in-group and out-group faces.
These results suggest that social-cognitive mechanisms of
in-group and out-group categorization are sufficient to
elicit performance differences for in-group and out-group
face recognition.
The cross-race recognition deficit, known more simply as the
cross-race effect (CRE), is one of the best-replicated
phenomena
in face perception (Anthony, Copper, & Mullen, 1992; Chance
&
Goldstein, 1981; Cross, Cross, & Daly, 1971; Malpass &
Kravitz,
1969). Explained simply, the CRE is a tendency for recogni-
tion accuracy to be better for same-race faces than for cross-
race faces, an effect that has been shown to be surprisingly
robust across numerous racial groups and research paradigms
(Meissner & Brigham, 2001).
Generally, two different kinds of theoretical models have been
proposed to explain the CRE: perceptual-expertise models and
social-categorization models. Perceptual-expertise models are
perhaps the longest-standing explanations for the CRE (see
Meissner & Brigham, 2001). Although there are many variations
of this hypothesis (Ng & Lindsay, 1994), the core argument
is that de facto racial segregation leads perceivers to have dif-
ferential expertise in processing same-race versus cross-race
faces. This differential expertise then leads to differential rec-
ognition accuracy. The lesser contact with individuals of other
races than with individuals of the same race yields fewer op-
portunities for distinguishing between cross-race faces, mean-
ing perceivers are relatively inexpert at distinguishing between
cross-race faces. Although there is general consensus about why
differential expertise occurs, the specific mechanism by which
this differential expertise elicits differential recognition of
cross-race faces is a matter of some debate (see Sporer, 2001,
for
a review). For example, a proposal in line with popular models
of
recognition memory (e.g., McClelland & Chappell, 1998) is that
a lack of contact may lead to a lack of expertise with the di-
mensions on which cross-race faces actually vary (see MacLin
&
Malpass, 2001). Alternatively, lower levels of expertise with
cross-race faces may elicit less holistic and more feature-based
processing of cross-race faces relative to same-race faces (Mi-
chel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006; Rhodes, Brake,
Taylor, & Tan, 1989).
Mechanisms aside, this perceptual-expertise hypothesis has a
history of empirical support. For example, Malpass, Lavigueur,
and Weldon (1973) found that practice at perceptual discrimi-
nation between same-race and cross-race faces in the laboratory
can at least temporarily reduce the magnitude of the CRE (see
also Elliott, Wills, & Goldstein, 1973; Goldstein & Chance,
1985). In addition, cross-race faces are perceptually discrimi-
nated with less facility than are same-race faces (e.g., Byatt &
Rhodes, 1998; Walker & Tanaka, 2003). More recently, San-
grigoli, Pallier, Argenti, Ventureyra, and de Schonen (2005)
found that lifelong training with cross-race faces can even re-
verse the direction of the CRE. In their study, individuals of
Korean heritage who were adopted as children by Caucasian
families in Europe showed a reversal of the CRE by adulthood.
That is, despite their Korean heritage, these adoptees who grew
to maturity among Caucasian families were like their adoptive
Address correspondence to Kurt Hugenberg, Department of Psy-
chology, Psychology Building, Miami University, Oxford, OH
45056,
e-mail: [email protected]
P S Y C H O L O G I C A L S C I E N C E
706 Volume 18—Number 8Copyright r 2007 Association for
Psychological Science
Caucasian parents in finding Asian faces more difficult to rec-
ognize than White faces.
Despite this corpus of supportive empirical evidence, some
recent studies have begun to call into question the extent to
which differential expertise alone is sufficient to explain the
CRE. For example, Hugenberg, Miller, and Claypool (2007)
recently found that instructing subjects at encoding that they
were likely to show a racial bias in recognition, and that they
should attend closely to the individuating characteristics of the
faces, was sufficient to eliminate the CRE. The sufficiency of
instructions to eliminate the CRE suggests that the CRE may
partly originate from motivational differences elicited by social
categorization. In related research, MacLin and Malpass (2001,
2003) found that adding Latino- or Black-stereotypic hairstyles
to racially ambiguous Latino-Black faces not only influenced
race categorization, but also was sufficient to elicit the CRE.
For
example, ambiguous-race faces with Latino-stereotypic hair-
styles were not only categorized as Latino, but were also better
recognized by Latino perceivers than were faces with Black-
stereotypic hairstyles.
Taken together, this research falls in line with a number of re-
cent models that in part explain the CRE via social-cognitive
mechanisms of categorization versus individuation (Sporer,
2001).
At the core of social-cognitive theory is the ubiquitous tendency
of
perceivers to think categorically about out-group members (e.g.,
Bodenhausen, Macrae, & Hugenberg, 2003). Thus, according to
social-categorization models, the CRE is not due to differential
expertise with cross-race faces per se, but rather is due to
differ-
ences in social cognitions typically elicited when processing in-
group and out-group members (see Sporer, 2001, for a review).
Indeed, a large body of research has shown that merely catego-
rizing a stimulus as an in-group or an out-group member has a
host of important cognitive, motivational, and behavioral
sequelae
(Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel, Billing, Brundy, & Flament, 1971; Tajfel
&
Turner, 1986). As is the case with perceptual-expertise models,
social-categorization models vary in the specific proposed
mech-
anisms underlying the CRE. For example, in Levin’s (1996,
2000)
feature-selection model, thinking categorically about out-group
members leads individuals to search for category-specifying
features (e.g., skin tone) in cross-race faces, instead of the in-
dividuating features that distinguish one face from another (see
also MacLin & Malpass, 2001, 2003). Alternatively,
categorizing a
target as an out-group member might reduce processing motiva-
tion, leading to weaker encoding of the individuating features of
cross-race faces relative to same-race faces (Rodin, 1987).
Regardless of specific mechanisms, these recent findings
suggest that the CRE may have its origins, at least in part, in
social categorization. If social categorization is implicated in
the CRE, the ramifications are quite profound. First, the CRE
should be reduced or even eliminated by factors that tend to
reduce reliance on social categories (Hugenberg et al., 2007).
Second, if the CRE is due at least in part to social
categorization,
then social categorization alone, absent any differences in ex-
pertise, should be sufficient to elicit recognition differences
between in-group and out-group targets.
Drawing on this logic, we hypothesized that merely labeling or
categorizing faces as belonging to an in-group should facilitate
recognition, relative to recognition of faces believed to belong
to
an out-group, even when perceivers’ expertise with the stimuli
is
held constant. We designed two studies to test the hypothesis
that beliefs about targets’ group membership alone, independent
of race or a priori expertise or exposure, are sufficient to elicit
recognition differences. In essence, we hypothesized that the
CRE may be, in part, a subset of a broader cross-category
effect,
such that mere social categorization is sufficient to lead to dif-
ferences in recognition.
STUDY 1
Overview
Study 1 tested the hypothesis that individuals are better able to
recognize in-group than out-group faces, even when perceptual
expertise with the in-group and out-group faces is equal. White
subjects saw a series of White faces presented on red and green
backgrounds. Subjects in the category-label condition were in-
structed that faces on the red background were university in-
group members (i.e., fellow Miami University students) and that
faces on the green background were university out-group mem-
bers (i.e., students at Marshall University, a perennial football
rival). Subjects in the control condition were given no specific
instructions regarding the background color. Subjects in the
control condition were expected to show equivalent
performance
for faces on the red and green backgrounds. However, if merely
categorizing targets as in-group and out-group members is suf-
ficient to elicit cross-race-like effects (i.e., cross-category ef-
fects), subjects told that the background was indicative of group
affiliation would be expected to show better recognition perfor-
mance for faces on red backgrounds than for faces on green
backgrounds. Because all subjects and targets were of the same
race, and targets were counterbalanced across backgrounds, a
perceptual-expertise model would predict no differences in face
recognition across backgrounds in the category-label condition.
Method
Subjects and Design
Sixty White Miami University undergraduates (22 women)
participated for course credit. A 2 (background color: red,
green)
� 2 (category label: present, not present) mixed-model experi-
mental design was used, with repeated measures on the first
factor.
Materials
Eighty gray-scale faces of White college-age males displaying
neutral expressions were used as the stimuli (no stimuli were
Miami or Marshall University students). Adobe Photoshop was
Volume 18—Number 8 707
Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg
used to resize the images to approximately 2.25 � 1.5 in., and
each face was then placed on both red and green backgrounds
measuring 3 � 3 in. For the control condition, the words ‘‘Red’’
and ‘‘Green’’ were inscribed in white letters at the bottom of
the
red and green backgrounds, respectively. For the category-label
condition, the university name (‘‘Miami University’’ for red;
‘‘Marshall University’’ for green) was inscribed in white at
the bottom of the background (red and green are the school
colors for Miami and Marshall, respectively; see Fig. 1 for ex-
ample stimuli).
Procedure
After providing informed consent, subjects were seated at
computers in individual cubicles and instructed that they would
complete a face recognition experiment consisting of a learning
phase and a recognition phase. All instructions and stimuli were
presented via computer. Subjects were instructed that during the
learning phase, they would see 40 faces on the computer screen
and should attend closely to these faces in order to recognize
them later. Subjects in the control condition received no in-
structions regarding the background colors. Subjects in the
category-label condition, however, were instructed that the
faces
on red backgrounds were fellow Miami University students,
whereas the faces on green backgrounds were Marshall Uni-
versity students. Subjects then began the learning phase, during
which 40 target faces (20 on the green background and 20 on
the
red background) were displayed in a randomized order. Each
face was displayed for 2 s, and the interstimulus interval was
500
ms. All faces were counterbalanced across background color
and for presence/absence during the learning phase on a be-
tween-subjects basis, such that for each subject, each face was
equally likely to be on a red or green background and was
equally likely to be seen or not seen during the learning phase.
Preliminary analyses found no effects of the counterbalancing;
therefore, it is not discussed further.
After completing a 5- to 7-min unrelated distractor task,
subjects engaged in the recognition phase. They were instructed
that they would see a series of faces, some of which they had
seen
during the learning phase (old faces) and some of which they
had
not seen (new faces). Subjects were instructed that as each face
appeared on the screen, they should report if they had or had not
seen it during the learning phase. Each face remained on the
screen until a decision was rendered, at which point the next
face appeared. The 80 faces presented during the recognition
phase included the 40 faces previously seen during learning and
40 new faces (20 on the green background and 20 on the red
background), displayed in a separate random order for each
subject. Each face remained on the screen until a response was
made, and then the next trial began. After completing all tasks,
subjects were probed for suspicion, thanked, and debriefed.
Results and Discussion
Of interest was the extent to which the presence of the category
labels influenced face recognition. Within the signal detection
framework, the CRE is observed as lower sensitivity (d0) for
cross-race compared with same-race faces. Thus, hit rates and
false alarm rates were calculated separately for targets on red
and green backgrounds, and these rates were then used to cal-
culate the separate sensitivity (d0) parameters for red and green
targets.
1
To test whether the mere presence of social-category labels
influenced face recognition, we subjected sensitivity scores for
red- and green-background targets to a 2 (background color) �
2 (category label) mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA),
with repeated measures on the first factor. Results were in line
with the social-categorization account, as the ANOVA revealed
the predicted Background Color � Category Label interaction,
F(1, 58) 5 5.16, p < .05, prep 5 .91, Z
2
5 .082 (see Fig. 2, top
panel). When no social-category labels were present, recogni-
tion performance was equivalent for the red (M 5 1.08, SD 5
0.58) and green (M 5 1.14, SD 5 0.56) backgrounds, t(27) < 1.
However, when category labels were present, faces on the red
background (i.e., in-group members; M 5 1.23, SD 5 0.66) were
better recognized than were faces on the green background (i.e.,
out-group members; M 5 0.94, SD 5 0.77), t(31) 5 2.97, p <
.01, prep 5 .96, d 5 0.40. Thus, when perceivers did not believe
the background color was diagnostic of group membership, no
recognition differences emerged. However, as predicted by the
social-categorization account of the CRE, when background
color was indicative of group status, faces on red (in-group)
backgrounds were better recognized than were faces on green
Fig. 1. Example of stimuli in the experimental (top) and control
(bottom)
conditions in Study 1.
1
For both studies, we conducted preliminary analyses on
criterion, but no
reliable interaction was found in either study. Therefore,
criterion is not dis-
cussed further.
708 Volume 18—Number 8
Cross-Category Effect
(out-group) backgrounds. Because the race of the faces was held
constant and the background color for each target was coun-
terbalanced, subjects’ perceptual expertise with the targets was
equated across conditions. Thus, the recognition differences
between in-group and out-group targets seem difficult to
explain
using solely an expertise mechanism. Instead, the mere pres-
ence of category labels seems sufficient to have elicited a
pattern
of recognition analogous to the CRE, even without manipulating
race.
Although the current results are consistent with social-cate-
gorization accounts of the CRE, this study did rely on preex-
isting groups (i.e., university affiliations), which itself could be
problematic. For example, insofar as the out-group label may
have been infrequently seen relative to the in-group label prior
to this study, this novelty may have competed for participants’
attention during encoding. Alternatively, specific stereotype
content about the out-group may have made subjects unwilling
to attend to faces of that out-group. To eliminate problems that
may arise because of preexisting in-group/out-group distinc-
tions, we turned to the well-established minimal-group para-
digm. In the social-cognitive literature, research employing this
paradigm has shown strong cognitive, motivational, and be-
havioral differences in responses to relatively arbitrarily con-
structed in-groups and out-groups (e.g., DeSteno, Dasgupta,
Bartlett, & Cajdric, 2004; Tajfel et al., 1971). The strong
version
of the social-categorization explanation for the CRE suggests
that even categorizing perceivers into relatively minimalistic
groups with no previous history of between-groups distinctions
should be sufficient to lead to differences in face recognition. If
mere categorization into in-groups and out-groups is sufficient
to
elicit cross-race-like effects, or cross-category effects, then
similar results should be observed when subjects are separated
into artificially constructed social groups as well. Therefore, we
performed a second study using a variant of the minimal-group
paradigm to extend the current results and to ensure that in-
group/out-group categorizations alone are sufficient to elicit
differences in face recognition accuracy.
STUDY 2
Overview
Study 2 used a design similar to that of Study 1; however,
before
completing the learning phase, all subjects completed a bogus
personality test that categorized them as having either ‘‘red’’ or
‘‘green’’ personality types. No description of the personality
types was provided. Subjects then saw faces of 40 people
labeled
as belonging to these personality types (i.e., 20 people per
personality type). If social categorization is sufficient to elicit
differences in face recognition, subjects would be expected to
recognize members of their newly found personality type better
than members of the other personality type.
Method
Subjects and Design
Forty-three White Miami University undergraduates (39 wom-
en) participated for course credit. Four subjects were removed
from the analysis; 2 did not receive the group manipulation
because of experimenter error, and 2 admitted not following
instructions or not understanding the task. Analyses were con-
ducted on data from the remaining 39 subjects. A 2 (background
color: red, green) � 2 (in-group: red, green) mixed-model ex-
perimental design was used, with repeated measures on the first
factor.
Materials
The faces used in the previous study were used in this study,
except that all faces had ‘‘Red’’ or ‘‘Green’’ category labels.
A bogus personality test was used to create the minimal
groups. Forty questions taken from the Big Five Personality
Test
(Goldberg, 1993; John & Srivastava, 1999) were presented to
subjects one at a time on the computer screen. Each question
remained on the screen until a response was made. Responses
were given on 7-point Likert scales, with higher values indi-
Fig. 2. Recognition accuracy (d0) for faces with red and green
back-
grounds as a function of category label (Study 1, top panel) and
minimal-
group manipulation (Study 2, bottom panel).
Volume 18—Number 8 709
Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg
cating greater agreement. The questions were not systematically
representative of personality dimensions; therefore, these data
were not analyzed.
Procedure
The procedure was identical to that of Study 1, except as noted.
After providing informed consent, subjects were instructed that
they would take a computerized personality test. After subjects
completed this test, the computer ostensibly analyzed their re-
sponses, and informed them that they were either a ‘‘red’’ or a
‘‘green’’ personality type. Subjects were then instructed:
This personality measure has been found to be very good at pre-
dicting future success both socially and monetarily. The
measure
itself is often used by businesses and organizations as a means
of
identifying strong candidates for competitive positions. Further,
psychologists who study relationships often use this personality
inventory to identify future success in relationships.
Subjects were given no further description of the personality
types. They were then given a green or red wristband to wear,
and
told it was to identify them as a member of their particular
group
(see DeSteno et al., 2004, for a similar procedure). Subjects
were then instructed that they would view faces on the
computer,
and that the background color for each face would denote
whether that person had the red personality type or the green
personality type. As in Study 1, all faces were counterbalanced
across background color and for presence/absence in the
learning phase on a between-subjects basis, such that for each
subject, each face was equally likely to be on a red or green
background and equally likely to be seen or not seen during the
learning phase.
Results and Discussion
As in Study 1, sensitivity scores (d0) for red and green targets
were subjected to a 2 (background color) � 2 (in-group) mixed-
model ANOVA, with repeated measures on the first factor. Re-
sults were in line with the social-categorization model, as the
ANOVA yielded the predicted Background Color � In-Group
interaction, F(1, 37) 5 4.36, p < .05, prep 5 .89, Z
2
5 .11 (see
Fig. 2, bottom panel). Critically, this study tested whether a
relatively arbitrarily constructed in-group is sufficient to elicit
cross-race-like effects. To test this hypothesis, we collapsed the
data across in-group color, to directly compare performance for
in-group and out-group faces. As predicted, in-group faces (M 5
1.55, SD 5 0.56) were better recognized than out-group faces
(M 5 1.34, SD 5 0.56), t(38) 5 2.06, p < .05, prep 5 .88, d 5
0.38, replicating the pattern of recognition shown in Study 1.
Thus, even when subjects have equivalent perceptual expertise
with in-group and out-group targets, and the in-groups and out-
groups are constructed in the experimental session itself, merely
categorizing targets as in-group and out-group members is suf-
ficient to elicit better face recognition for in-group targets.
Moreover, insofar as this study experimentally created in-
groups
and out-groups, it rules out alternate explanations that involve
properties of preexisting groups.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Our results fall clearly in line with social-categorization models
of the CRE. That is, we found that merely categorizing faces as
belonging to an in-group facilitates their recognition, relative to
faces believed to belong to an out-group. Across two studies
using both real and minimal groups, faces categorized as in-
group members were better recognized than those categorized as
out-group members. Critically, this phenomenon occurred even
though perceivers’ expertise with the stimuli was held constant.
In our studies, subjects and targets were always of the same
race,
and in all cases, stimuli were counterbalanced to equate ex-
pertise with the stimuli across conditions. Thus, perceptual-
expertise models do not seem entirely adequate to explain the
current findings. Instead, it appears that merely categorizing a
face as a member of the in-group or out-group is sufficient to
influence recognition accuracy, as predicted by social-catego-
rization models of the CRE.
Although the findings of these two studies are in line with
other recent findings suggesting that the CRE may be due, in
part, to social-cognitive phenomena (e.g., Hugenberg et al.,
2007; Levin, 1996, 2000; MacLin & Malpass, 2001, 2003;
Sporer, 2001), the current research is one of only a handful of
studies to show that social categorization alone, absent differ-
ences in expertise, is sufficient to elicit deficits in face recog-
nition. Moreover, considering the current findings in light of
the
CRE may also offer parsimonious explanations for other phe-
nomena previously attributed to differential expertise with
same-race and cross-race faces. For example, Sangrigoli et al.
(2005) showed that Korean children adopted by French Cau-
casian parents had better recognition for White than Asian
targets. Sangrigoli et al. interpreted their findings to indicate
that immersion in a cross-race environment yields substantial
practice with cross-race faces, leading to better cross-race than
same-race recognition. Though we agree that the experience of
these adoptees yielded substantial expertise with cross-race
faces, this experience may also have changed the adoptees’
manner of self-categorization. Adopted Korean children living
in a primarily European Caucasian environment may implicitly
categorize themselves as belonging to the predominant White
group in their community. If so, just as Miami University stu-
dents recognized supposed Miami students better than supposed
students from other universities, so too may Koreans adopted by
Whites recognize White faces better than Asian faces. This
same
process could also explain other between-groups biases in face
recognition, such as the ‘‘own-age bias’’ (Anastasi & Rhodes,
2006) and the ‘‘own-sex bias’’ (Wright & Sladden, 2003).
Although the current results are difficult to explain using a
pure expertise mechanism, this does not mean that perceptual
710 Volume 18—Number 8
Cross-Category Effect
expertise plays no role in face-processing biases such as the
CRE. To the contrary, we argue that perceptual expertise is
certainly a necessary condition for strong recognition (Hugen-
berg et al., 2007). Perceivers who lack facility with the dimen-
sions on which stimuli (faces included) differ are certain to
have
substantial difficulties in recognizing those stimuli (Tanaka &
Farah, 1993; Tanaka, Kiefer, & Bukach, 2004). Indeed, the in-
group/out-group model (IOM; Sporer, 2001), a theoretical
model
designed to explain the CRE by integrating social-categoriza-
tion and perceptual-expertise mechanisms, makes predictions
quite similar to our findings. In this model, as a result of greater
expertise, in-group faces are processed in a default, automatic
manner, characterized by holistic processing and superior rec-
ognition. When out-group cues are detected, however, social
categorization disrupts default holistic processing, and may be a
cue to disregard the stimulus, resulting in poor recognition. A
significant implication of the IOM is that recognition will suffer
not just for cross-race faces, but for out-group faces more gen-
erally. Thus, the current findings are consistent with the IOM
and other models designed to synthesize social-cognitive and
perceptual-expertise explanations for the CRE. When we strip
away differences in perceptual expertise, a recognition deficit
still occurs for out-group faces, strongly suggesting that social
categorization is at play in the CRE, as well as in similar cross-
age and cross-sex effects.
It is important to note that the observed advantage for in-group
recognition in these studies (i.e., the cross-category effect) may
involve mechanisms that are partially different from those that
are typically employed to explain the CRE. Although this work
extends study of face recognition biases beyond the CRE, it
does
not yet provide a mechanism to account for these biases. Al-
though it is possible that our subjects employed greater holistic
processing for in-group than for out-group faces, a plausible
alternative is that they differentially attended to in-group and
out-group targets during encoding. For example, perhaps per-
ceivers attended to the category-specifying information (i.e., the
category labels), rather than the individuating features of the
faces, when processing out-group targets (Levin, 1996, 2000).
Alternatively, perhaps faces categorized as out-group members
are cognitively disregarded (Rodin, 1987), deemed as less
deserving of attention than faces of in-group members, leading
to worse out-group recognition. Or perhaps the evaluative pos-
itivity elicited by in-group targets (Claypool, Hugenberg,
Housley, & Mackie, 2007; Tajfel & Turner, 1986) facilitates
deeper encoding. Such positivity itself may elicit more holistic
processing (Gasper & Clore, 2002; see Johnson & Fredrickson,
2005, for a similar argument), thereby facilitating later recog-
nition (Michel et al., 2006; Rhodes et al., 1989). Current work
in
our lab is investigating these possibilities.
Although the CRE certainly involves elements of differential
perceptual expertise with same-race and cross-race faces, the
current research provides novel evidence that in-group and out-
group social-category distinctions are sufficient to elicit differ-
ences in face recognition, even when perceptual expertise is
held constant. More research is certainly needed to show how
perceptual-expertise and social-categorization mechanisms act
together to elicit biases in face recognition. However, the
current
results, taken together with other recent findings (e.g., Hugen-
berg et al., 2007; Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005), suggest that
researchers should take seriously the possibility that cross-
category biases in face recognition, such as the CRE, may be
due
in part to social categorization.
REFERENCES
Anastasi, J.S., & Rhodes, M.G. (2006). Evidence for an own-
age bias
in face recognition. North American Journal of Psychology, 8,
237–252.
Anthony, T., Copper, C., & Mullen, B. (1992). Cross-racial
facial iden-
tification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 18, 296–301.
Bodenhausen, G.V., Macrae, C.N., & Hugenberg, K. (2003).
Social
cognition. In T. Millon & M.J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of
psychol-
ogy: Personality and social psychology (pp. 257–282).
Hoboken,
NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Byatt, G., & Rhodes, G. (1998). Recognition of own-race and
other-
race caricatures: Implications for models of face recognition.
Vision Research, 38, 2455–2468.
Chance, J.E., & Goldstein, A.G. (1981). Depth of processing in
response
to own- and other-race faces. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 7, 475–480.
Claypool, H., Hugenberg, K., Housley, M., & Mackie, D.
(2007). Us
and them: Familiarity disambiguates group membership. Manu-
script submitted for publication.
Cross, J.F., Cross, J., & Daly, J. (1971). Sex, race, age, and
beauty as
factors in recognition of faces. Perception & Psychophysics, 10,
393–396.
DeSteno, D., Dasgupta, N., Bartlett, M.Y., & Cajdric, A.
(2004).
Prejudice from thin air: The effect of emotion on automatic in-
tergroup attitudes. Psychological Science, 15, 319–324.
Elliott, E.A., Wills, E.J., & Goldstein, A.G. (1973). The effects
of
discrimination training on the recognition of white and oriental
faces. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 71–73.
Gasper, K., & Clore, G.L. (2002). Attending to the big picture:
Mood
and global versus local processing of visual information. Psy-
chological Science, 13, 34–40.
Goldberg, L.R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality
traits.
American Psychologist, 48, 26–34.
Goldstein, A.G., & Chance, J.E. (1985). Effects of training on
Japanese
face recognition: Reduction of the other-race effect. Bulletin of
the Psychonomic Society, 23, 211–214.
Hugenberg, K., Miller, J., & Claypool, H. (2007).
Categorization and
individuation in the cross-race recognition deficit: Toward a so-
lution to an insidious problem. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 43, 334–340.
John, O.P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big-five trait
taxonomy: His-
tory, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. Pervin &
O.P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research
(pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
Johnson, K.J., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2005). ‘‘We all look the
same to
me’’: Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face rec-
ognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875–881.
Volume 18—Number 8 711
Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg
Levin, D.T. (1996). Classifying faces by race: The structure of
face
categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Mem-
ory, and Cognition, 22, 1364–1382.
Levin, D.T. (2000). Race as a visual feature: Using visual
search and
perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories
and
the cross-race recognition deficit. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: General, 129, 559–574.
MacLin, O.H., & Malpass, R.S. (2001). Racial categorization of
faces:
The ambiguous-race face effect. Psychology, Public Policy, and
Law, 7, 98–118.
MacLin, O.H., & Malpass, R.S. (2003). The ambiguous-race
face il-
lusion. Perception, 32, 249–252.
Malpass, R.S., & Kravitz, J. (1969). Recognition for faces of
own and
other race. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13,
330–334.
Malpass, R.S., Lavigueur, H., & Weldon, D.E. (1973). Verbal
and visual
training in face recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 14,
285–
292.
McClelland, J.L., & Chappell, M. (1998). Familiarity breeds
differ-
entiation: A subjective-likelihood approach to the effects of
expe-
rience in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 105, 734–
760.
Meissner, C.A., & Brigham, J.C. (2001). Thirty years of
investigating
the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review.
Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3–35.
Michel, C., Rossion, B., Han, J., Chung, C.S., & Caldara, R.
(2006).
Holistic processing is finely tuned for faces of one’s own race.
Psychological Science, 17, 608–615.
Ng, W., & Lindsay, R. (1994). Cross-race recognition: Failure
of the
contact hypothesis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 25,
217–232.
Rhodes, G., Brake, S., Taylor, K., & Tan, S. (1989). Expertise
and
configurable coding in face recognition. British Journal of Psy-
chology, 80, 313–331.
Rodin, M.J. (1987). Who is memorable to whom: A study of
cognitive
disregard. Social Cognition, 5, 144–165.
Sangrigoli, S., Pallier, C., Argenti, A.-M., Ventureyra, V.A.G.,
& de
Schonen, S. (2005). Reversibility of the other-race effect in face
recognition during childhood. Psychological Science, 16, 440–
444.
Sporer, S.L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups:
An inte-
gration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 36–
97.
Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations.
Annual
Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39.
Tajfel, H., Billing, M., Brundy, R., & Flament, C. (1971).
Social cat-
egorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 1, 149–178.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of
inter-
group behavior. In S. Worchek & W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology
of
intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson Hall.
Tanaka, J., & Farah, M. (1993). Parts and wholes in face
recognition.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 397–405.
Tanaka, J., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C.M. (2004). A holistic
account of
the own-race effect in face recognition: Evidence from a cross-
cultural study. Cognition, 93, 1–9.
Walker, P.M., & Tanaka, J.W. (2003). An encoding advantage
for own-
race versus other-race faces. Perception, 32, 1117–1125.
Wright, D.B., & Sladden, B. (2003). An own gender bias and
the im-
portance of hair in face recognition. Acta Psychologica, 114,
101–
114.
(RECEIVED 10/19/06; REVISION ACCEPTED 12/8/06;
FINAL MATERIALS RECEIVED 12/15/06)
712 Volume 18—Number 8
Cross-Category Effect

More Related Content

Similar to Common Mistakes I see on this paper are1. Using summaries and .docx

Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docx
Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docxStereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docx
Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docx
dessiechisomjj4
 
PSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docx
PSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docxPSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docx
PSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docx
woodruffeloisa
 
Overview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docx
Overview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docxOverview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docx
Overview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docx
alfred4lewis58146
 
INTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report. The .docx
INTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report.  The .docxINTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report.  The .docx
INTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report. The .docx
normanibarber20063
 
Intergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docx
Intergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docxIntergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docx
Intergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docx
doylymaura
 
How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...
How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...
How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...
Crystal Chapman
 
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docxExample 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
elbanglis
 
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docxExample 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
cravennichole326
 
A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...
A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...
A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...
Tiffany Sandoval
 
EBSCO Publishing Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docx
EBSCO Publishing   Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docxEBSCO Publishing   Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docx
EBSCO Publishing Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docx
tidwellveronique
 
Reading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docx
Reading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docxReading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docx
Reading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docx
sodhi3
 
SOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docx
SOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docxSOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docx
SOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docx
rosemariebrayshaw
 

Similar to Common Mistakes I see on this paper are1. Using summaries and .docx (17)

Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docx
Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docxStereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docx
Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self.docx
 
Action research meta analysis
Action research meta analysisAction research meta analysis
Action research meta analysis
 
PSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docx
PSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docxPSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docx
PSY 294 RESEARCH DESIGN &ANALYSIS IILECTURE 4Research Pr.docx
 
Overview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docx
Overview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docxOverview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docx
Overview Write 5–6 pages in which you discuss practical ways to .docx
 
INTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report. The .docx
INTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report.  The .docxINTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report.  The .docx
INTERCULTURAL RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS PROJECTWritten Report. The .docx
 
Intergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docx
Intergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docxIntergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docx
Intergroup Bias and Dual ProcessingResearch on stereotypes is re.docx
 
How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...
How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...
How To Write A Classification Essay. Classification Essay Examples, Definitio...
 
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docxExample 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
 
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docxExample 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
Example 1Methodological Design PhenomenologyResearch Questi.docx
 
Lanning ICPS: Community structure of JPSP
Lanning ICPS: Community structure of JPSPLanning ICPS: Community structure of JPSP
Lanning ICPS: Community structure of JPSP
 
A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...
A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...
A Grounded Theory Approach Into The Development Of Career...
 
Reflective Essay Sample Paper
Reflective Essay Sample PaperReflective Essay Sample Paper
Reflective Essay Sample Paper
 
Morality
MoralityMorality
Morality
 
Shifting landscape of lgbt org research
Shifting landscape of lgbt org researchShifting landscape of lgbt org research
Shifting landscape of lgbt org research
 
EBSCO Publishing Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docx
EBSCO Publishing   Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docxEBSCO Publishing   Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docx
EBSCO Publishing Citation Format APA (American Psychologica.docx
 
Reading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docx
Reading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docxReading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docx
Reading ResponseBy R.C. Lewontin, Confusions about Human Races.docx
 
SOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docx
SOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docxSOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docx
SOCW 6210 Week 5 discussion post responses.Respond to the coll.docx
 

More from drandy1

COMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docx
COMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docxCOMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docx
COMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docx
drandy1
 
COMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docx
COMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docxCOMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docx
COMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docx
drandy1
 
COMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docx
COMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docxCOMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docx
COMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docx
drandy1
 
COMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docx
COMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docxCOMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docx
COMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docx
drandy1
 
Comment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docx
Comment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docxComment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docx
Comment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docx
drandy1
 
Comments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docx
Comments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docxComments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docx
Comments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docx
drandy1
 
Community Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docx
Community Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docxCommunity Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docx
Community Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docx
drandy1
 
Comment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docx
Comment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docxComment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docx
Comment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docx
drandy1
 
Communication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docx
Communication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docxCommunication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docx
Communication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docx
drandy1
 
COMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docx
COMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docxCOMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docx
COMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docx
drandy1
 

More from drandy1 (20)

COMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docx
COMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docxCOMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docx
COMM 166 Final Research Proposal GuidelinesThe proposal should.docx
 
COMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docx
COMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docxCOMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docx
COMMENTS You wrote an interesting essay; however, it is lacking t.docx
 
Commercial Space TravelThere are about a half dozen commercial s.docx
Commercial Space TravelThere are about a half dozen commercial s.docxCommercial Space TravelThere are about a half dozen commercial s.docx
Commercial Space TravelThere are about a half dozen commercial s.docx
 
CommentsPrice is the easiest way to make profit – all you.docx
CommentsPrice is the easiest way to make profit – all you.docxCommentsPrice is the easiest way to make profit – all you.docx
CommentsPrice is the easiest way to make profit – all you.docx
 
COMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docx
COMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docxCOMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docx
COMM 1110 Library Research Assignment Objective To ensu.docx
 
COMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docx
COMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docxCOMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docx
COMM 1110 Persuasive Speech Evaluation Objective To lea.docx
 
Comment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docx
Comment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docxComment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docx
Comment The ANA is such an astonishing association. They help .docx
 
Comments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docx
Comments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docxComments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docx
Comments Excellent paper. It’s obvious that you put quite a bit of .docx
 
Community Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docx
Community Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docxCommunity Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docx
Community Assessment and Analysis PresentationThis assignment co.docx
 
Comment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docx
Comment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docxComment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docx
Comment Commentonat least 3 Classmates’Posts (approximately  150.docx
 
Communication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docx
Communication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docxCommunication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docx
Communication permeates all that we do, no matter who we are. In thi.docx
 
Combating BriberyIn May 2011, the Commission for Eradication of .docx
Combating BriberyIn May 2011, the Commission for Eradication of .docxCombating BriberyIn May 2011, the Commission for Eradication of .docx
Combating BriberyIn May 2011, the Commission for Eradication of .docx
 
Comment using your own words but please provide at least one referen.docx
Comment using your own words but please provide at least one referen.docxComment using your own words but please provide at least one referen.docx
Comment using your own words but please provide at least one referen.docx
 
Communicating and Collaborating Family InvolvementIn this uni.docx
Communicating and Collaborating Family InvolvementIn this uni.docxCommunicating and Collaborating Family InvolvementIn this uni.docx
Communicating and Collaborating Family InvolvementIn this uni.docx
 
Community Health Assessment and Health Promotion-1000 words-due .docx
Community Health Assessment and Health Promotion-1000 words-due .docxCommunity Health Assessment and Health Promotion-1000 words-due .docx
Community Health Assessment and Health Promotion-1000 words-due .docx
 
COMMUNITY HEALTH ASSESSMENTWINSHIELD SURVEYGUIDELINES1.  C.docx
COMMUNITY HEALTH ASSESSMENTWINSHIELD SURVEYGUIDELINES1.  C.docxCOMMUNITY HEALTH ASSESSMENTWINSHIELD SURVEYGUIDELINES1.  C.docx
COMMUNITY HEALTH ASSESSMENTWINSHIELD SURVEYGUIDELINES1.  C.docx
 
COMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docx
COMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docxCOMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docx
COMMUNITY CORRECTIONSPrepared ByDatePROBATIONDescr.docx
 
Community Concerns  Please respond to the followingIn your.docx
Community Concerns  Please respond to the followingIn your.docxCommunity Concerns  Please respond to the followingIn your.docx
Community Concerns  Please respond to the followingIn your.docx
 
Community Engagement InstructionsPart I PlanStudents wi.docx
Community Engagement InstructionsPart I PlanStudents wi.docxCommunity Engagement InstructionsPart I PlanStudents wi.docx
Community Engagement InstructionsPart I PlanStudents wi.docx
 
Community Career DevelopmentRead the following case study an.docx
Community Career DevelopmentRead the following case study an.docxCommunity Career DevelopmentRead the following case study an.docx
Community Career DevelopmentRead the following case study an.docx
 

Recently uploaded

1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
QucHHunhnh
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
negromaestrong
 
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseSpellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
AnaAcapella
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
ciinovamais
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Magic bus Group work1and 2 (Team 3).pptx
Magic bus Group work1and 2 (Team 3).pptxMagic bus Group work1and 2 (Team 3).pptx
Magic bus Group work1and 2 (Team 3).pptx
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please PractiseSpellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
Spellings Wk 3 English CAPS CARES Please Practise
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Third Battle of Panipat detailed notes.pptx
Third Battle of Panipat detailed notes.pptxThird Battle of Panipat detailed notes.pptx
Third Battle of Panipat detailed notes.pptx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptxAsian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
 

Common Mistakes I see on this paper are1. Using summaries and .docx

  • 1. Common Mistakes I see on this paper are 1. Using summaries and reviews written by other authors rather than using the actual IOM report and viewing each section, or viewing the IOM's summaries of it's report. 2. Forgetting to include information on how the IOM report will or has impacted one's practice. This is worth a high percentage of points 3. Not formatting the paper in APA, citing sources or using current sources like those supplied in our course resources for the week (since I am supplying a template APA should be flawless) 4. Going over the allowed length (papers that are over the criteria for length will be returned for revision) 5. Including background information, losing focus - remember the goal is to discuss the recommendations for education, practice, leadership and discuss how they can be achieved. 6. Ineffective introduction, too long, does not introduce the topic briefly, does not include a summary of what the paper will cover LINKS TO THE IOM REPORT AND IOM SUMMARIES OF THE REPORT (These should be your main references for the report, don’t use a summary written about the report, use the IOMs summaries) Download whole report as guest Report Brief Summary of Report on Education Summary of Report on Practice
  • 2. Summary of recommendations Great infograph that highlights IOM recommendations. Here is the link. This link gives an outline of the IOM's recommendations. Both offer an option that provides quick access to needed information in a brief and easy to follow format. Hope these are helpful. How to Reference and Cite the IOM Report IOM will each have the same author (the Institute of Medicine is the author) and year you will differentiate them by adding a small case letter after the year of publication in both your reference and citation. The reference would be listed in alphabetical order using the title. I have posted examples below. Institute of Medicine [IOM]. (2010). Future of nursing: Focus on education. Retrieved from www.nationalacademies.org/hmd/~/media/Files/Report%20Files /2010/The-Future-of- Nursing/Nursing%20Education%202010%20Brief.pdf Research Article The Cross-Category Effect Mere Social Categorization Is Sufficient to Elicit an Own-Group Bias in Face Recognition Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg
  • 3. Miami University ABSTRACT—Although the cross-race effect (CRE) is a well- established phenomenon, both perceptual-expertise and social-categorization models have been proposed to ex- plain the effect. The two studies reported here investigated the extent to which categorizing other people as in-group versus out-group members is sufficient to elicit a pattern of face recognition analogous to that of the CRE, even when perceptual expertise with the stimuli is held constant. In Study 1, targets were categorized as members of real-life in-groups and out-groups (based on university affiliation), whereas in Study 2, targets were categorized into experi- mentally created minimal groups. In both studies, recog- nition performance was better for targets categorized as in-group members, despite the fact that perceptual ex- pertise was equivalent for in-group and out-group faces. These results suggest that social-cognitive mechanisms of in-group and out-group categorization are sufficient to
  • 4. elicit performance differences for in-group and out-group face recognition. The cross-race recognition deficit, known more simply as the cross-race effect (CRE), is one of the best-replicated phenomena in face perception (Anthony, Copper, & Mullen, 1992; Chance & Goldstein, 1981; Cross, Cross, & Daly, 1971; Malpass & Kravitz, 1969). Explained simply, the CRE is a tendency for recogni- tion accuracy to be better for same-race faces than for cross- race faces, an effect that has been shown to be surprisingly robust across numerous racial groups and research paradigms (Meissner & Brigham, 2001). Generally, two different kinds of theoretical models have been proposed to explain the CRE: perceptual-expertise models and social-categorization models. Perceptual-expertise models are perhaps the longest-standing explanations for the CRE (see Meissner & Brigham, 2001). Although there are many variations of this hypothesis (Ng & Lindsay, 1994), the core argument
  • 5. is that de facto racial segregation leads perceivers to have dif- ferential expertise in processing same-race versus cross-race faces. This differential expertise then leads to differential rec- ognition accuracy. The lesser contact with individuals of other races than with individuals of the same race yields fewer op- portunities for distinguishing between cross-race faces, mean- ing perceivers are relatively inexpert at distinguishing between cross-race faces. Although there is general consensus about why differential expertise occurs, the specific mechanism by which this differential expertise elicits differential recognition of cross-race faces is a matter of some debate (see Sporer, 2001, for a review). For example, a proposal in line with popular models of recognition memory (e.g., McClelland & Chappell, 1998) is that a lack of contact may lead to a lack of expertise with the di- mensions on which cross-race faces actually vary (see MacLin & Malpass, 2001). Alternatively, lower levels of expertise with
  • 6. cross-race faces may elicit less holistic and more feature-based processing of cross-race faces relative to same-race faces (Mi- chel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006; Rhodes, Brake, Taylor, & Tan, 1989). Mechanisms aside, this perceptual-expertise hypothesis has a history of empirical support. For example, Malpass, Lavigueur, and Weldon (1973) found that practice at perceptual discrimi- nation between same-race and cross-race faces in the laboratory can at least temporarily reduce the magnitude of the CRE (see also Elliott, Wills, & Goldstein, 1973; Goldstein & Chance, 1985). In addition, cross-race faces are perceptually discrimi- nated with less facility than are same-race faces (e.g., Byatt & Rhodes, 1998; Walker & Tanaka, 2003). More recently, San- grigoli, Pallier, Argenti, Ventureyra, and de Schonen (2005) found that lifelong training with cross-race faces can even re- verse the direction of the CRE. In their study, individuals of Korean heritage who were adopted as children by Caucasian families in Europe showed a reversal of the CRE by adulthood.
  • 7. That is, despite their Korean heritage, these adoptees who grew to maturity among Caucasian families were like their adoptive Address correspondence to Kurt Hugenberg, Department of Psy- chology, Psychology Building, Miami University, Oxford, OH 45056, e-mail: [email protected] P S Y C H O L O G I C A L S C I E N C E 706 Volume 18—Number 8Copyright r 2007 Association for Psychological Science Caucasian parents in finding Asian faces more difficult to rec- ognize than White faces. Despite this corpus of supportive empirical evidence, some recent studies have begun to call into question the extent to which differential expertise alone is sufficient to explain the CRE. For example, Hugenberg, Miller, and Claypool (2007) recently found that instructing subjects at encoding that they were likely to show a racial bias in recognition, and that they should attend closely to the individuating characteristics of the faces, was sufficient to eliminate the CRE. The sufficiency of instructions to eliminate the CRE suggests that the CRE may
  • 8. partly originate from motivational differences elicited by social categorization. In related research, MacLin and Malpass (2001, 2003) found that adding Latino- or Black-stereotypic hairstyles to racially ambiguous Latino-Black faces not only influenced race categorization, but also was sufficient to elicit the CRE. For example, ambiguous-race faces with Latino-stereotypic hair- styles were not only categorized as Latino, but were also better recognized by Latino perceivers than were faces with Black- stereotypic hairstyles. Taken together, this research falls in line with a number of re- cent models that in part explain the CRE via social-cognitive mechanisms of categorization versus individuation (Sporer, 2001). At the core of social-cognitive theory is the ubiquitous tendency of perceivers to think categorically about out-group members (e.g., Bodenhausen, Macrae, & Hugenberg, 2003). Thus, according to social-categorization models, the CRE is not due to differential
  • 9. expertise with cross-race faces per se, but rather is due to differ- ences in social cognitions typically elicited when processing in- group and out-group members (see Sporer, 2001, for a review). Indeed, a large body of research has shown that merely catego- rizing a stimulus as an in-group or an out-group member has a host of important cognitive, motivational, and behavioral sequelae (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel, Billing, Brundy, & Flament, 1971; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). As is the case with perceptual-expertise models, social-categorization models vary in the specific proposed mech- anisms underlying the CRE. For example, in Levin’s (1996, 2000) feature-selection model, thinking categorically about out-group members leads individuals to search for category-specifying features (e.g., skin tone) in cross-race faces, instead of the in- dividuating features that distinguish one face from another (see also MacLin & Malpass, 2001, 2003). Alternatively, categorizing a
  • 10. target as an out-group member might reduce processing motiva- tion, leading to weaker encoding of the individuating features of cross-race faces relative to same-race faces (Rodin, 1987). Regardless of specific mechanisms, these recent findings suggest that the CRE may have its origins, at least in part, in social categorization. If social categorization is implicated in the CRE, the ramifications are quite profound. First, the CRE should be reduced or even eliminated by factors that tend to reduce reliance on social categories (Hugenberg et al., 2007). Second, if the CRE is due at least in part to social categorization, then social categorization alone, absent any differences in ex- pertise, should be sufficient to elicit recognition differences between in-group and out-group targets. Drawing on this logic, we hypothesized that merely labeling or categorizing faces as belonging to an in-group should facilitate recognition, relative to recognition of faces believed to belong to an out-group, even when perceivers’ expertise with the stimuli is
  • 11. held constant. We designed two studies to test the hypothesis that beliefs about targets’ group membership alone, independent of race or a priori expertise or exposure, are sufficient to elicit recognition differences. In essence, we hypothesized that the CRE may be, in part, a subset of a broader cross-category effect, such that mere social categorization is sufficient to lead to dif- ferences in recognition. STUDY 1 Overview Study 1 tested the hypothesis that individuals are better able to recognize in-group than out-group faces, even when perceptual expertise with the in-group and out-group faces is equal. White subjects saw a series of White faces presented on red and green backgrounds. Subjects in the category-label condition were in- structed that faces on the red background were university in- group members (i.e., fellow Miami University students) and that faces on the green background were university out-group mem-
  • 12. bers (i.e., students at Marshall University, a perennial football rival). Subjects in the control condition were given no specific instructions regarding the background color. Subjects in the control condition were expected to show equivalent performance for faces on the red and green backgrounds. However, if merely categorizing targets as in-group and out-group members is suf- ficient to elicit cross-race-like effects (i.e., cross-category ef- fects), subjects told that the background was indicative of group affiliation would be expected to show better recognition perfor- mance for faces on red backgrounds than for faces on green backgrounds. Because all subjects and targets were of the same race, and targets were counterbalanced across backgrounds, a perceptual-expertise model would predict no differences in face recognition across backgrounds in the category-label condition. Method Subjects and Design Sixty White Miami University undergraduates (22 women) participated for course credit. A 2 (background color: red,
  • 13. green) � 2 (category label: present, not present) mixed-model experi- mental design was used, with repeated measures on the first factor. Materials Eighty gray-scale faces of White college-age males displaying neutral expressions were used as the stimuli (no stimuli were Miami or Marshall University students). Adobe Photoshop was Volume 18—Number 8 707 Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg used to resize the images to approximately 2.25 � 1.5 in., and each face was then placed on both red and green backgrounds measuring 3 � 3 in. For the control condition, the words ‘‘Red’’ and ‘‘Green’’ were inscribed in white letters at the bottom of the red and green backgrounds, respectively. For the category-label condition, the university name (‘‘Miami University’’ for red; ‘‘Marshall University’’ for green) was inscribed in white at the bottom of the background (red and green are the school
  • 14. colors for Miami and Marshall, respectively; see Fig. 1 for ex- ample stimuli). Procedure After providing informed consent, subjects were seated at computers in individual cubicles and instructed that they would complete a face recognition experiment consisting of a learning phase and a recognition phase. All instructions and stimuli were presented via computer. Subjects were instructed that during the learning phase, they would see 40 faces on the computer screen and should attend closely to these faces in order to recognize them later. Subjects in the control condition received no in- structions regarding the background colors. Subjects in the category-label condition, however, were instructed that the faces on red backgrounds were fellow Miami University students, whereas the faces on green backgrounds were Marshall Uni- versity students. Subjects then began the learning phase, during which 40 target faces (20 on the green background and 20 on the
  • 15. red background) were displayed in a randomized order. Each face was displayed for 2 s, and the interstimulus interval was 500 ms. All faces were counterbalanced across background color and for presence/absence during the learning phase on a be- tween-subjects basis, such that for each subject, each face was equally likely to be on a red or green background and was equally likely to be seen or not seen during the learning phase. Preliminary analyses found no effects of the counterbalancing; therefore, it is not discussed further. After completing a 5- to 7-min unrelated distractor task, subjects engaged in the recognition phase. They were instructed that they would see a series of faces, some of which they had seen during the learning phase (old faces) and some of which they had not seen (new faces). Subjects were instructed that as each face appeared on the screen, they should report if they had or had not seen it during the learning phase. Each face remained on the screen until a decision was rendered, at which point the next
  • 16. face appeared. The 80 faces presented during the recognition phase included the 40 faces previously seen during learning and 40 new faces (20 on the green background and 20 on the red background), displayed in a separate random order for each subject. Each face remained on the screen until a response was made, and then the next trial began. After completing all tasks, subjects were probed for suspicion, thanked, and debriefed. Results and Discussion Of interest was the extent to which the presence of the category labels influenced face recognition. Within the signal detection framework, the CRE is observed as lower sensitivity (d0) for cross-race compared with same-race faces. Thus, hit rates and false alarm rates were calculated separately for targets on red and green backgrounds, and these rates were then used to cal- culate the separate sensitivity (d0) parameters for red and green targets. 1 To test whether the mere presence of social-category labels
  • 17. influenced face recognition, we subjected sensitivity scores for red- and green-background targets to a 2 (background color) � 2 (category label) mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA), with repeated measures on the first factor. Results were in line with the social-categorization account, as the ANOVA revealed the predicted Background Color � Category Label interaction, F(1, 58) 5 5.16, p < .05, prep 5 .91, Z 2 5 .082 (see Fig. 2, top panel). When no social-category labels were present, recogni- tion performance was equivalent for the red (M 5 1.08, SD 5 0.58) and green (M 5 1.14, SD 5 0.56) backgrounds, t(27) < 1. However, when category labels were present, faces on the red background (i.e., in-group members; M 5 1.23, SD 5 0.66) were better recognized than were faces on the green background (i.e., out-group members; M 5 0.94, SD 5 0.77), t(31) 5 2.97, p < .01, prep 5 .96, d 5 0.40. Thus, when perceivers did not believe the background color was diagnostic of group membership, no recognition differences emerged. However, as predicted by the social-categorization account of the CRE, when background
  • 18. color was indicative of group status, faces on red (in-group) backgrounds were better recognized than were faces on green Fig. 1. Example of stimuli in the experimental (top) and control (bottom) conditions in Study 1. 1 For both studies, we conducted preliminary analyses on criterion, but no reliable interaction was found in either study. Therefore, criterion is not dis- cussed further. 708 Volume 18—Number 8 Cross-Category Effect (out-group) backgrounds. Because the race of the faces was held constant and the background color for each target was coun- terbalanced, subjects’ perceptual expertise with the targets was equated across conditions. Thus, the recognition differences between in-group and out-group targets seem difficult to explain using solely an expertise mechanism. Instead, the mere pres-
  • 19. ence of category labels seems sufficient to have elicited a pattern of recognition analogous to the CRE, even without manipulating race. Although the current results are consistent with social-cate- gorization accounts of the CRE, this study did rely on preex- isting groups (i.e., university affiliations), which itself could be problematic. For example, insofar as the out-group label may have been infrequently seen relative to the in-group label prior to this study, this novelty may have competed for participants’ attention during encoding. Alternatively, specific stereotype content about the out-group may have made subjects unwilling to attend to faces of that out-group. To eliminate problems that may arise because of preexisting in-group/out-group distinc- tions, we turned to the well-established minimal-group para- digm. In the social-cognitive literature, research employing this paradigm has shown strong cognitive, motivational, and be- havioral differences in responses to relatively arbitrarily con- structed in-groups and out-groups (e.g., DeSteno, Dasgupta,
  • 20. Bartlett, & Cajdric, 2004; Tajfel et al., 1971). The strong version of the social-categorization explanation for the CRE suggests that even categorizing perceivers into relatively minimalistic groups with no previous history of between-groups distinctions should be sufficient to lead to differences in face recognition. If mere categorization into in-groups and out-groups is sufficient to elicit cross-race-like effects, or cross-category effects, then similar results should be observed when subjects are separated into artificially constructed social groups as well. Therefore, we performed a second study using a variant of the minimal-group paradigm to extend the current results and to ensure that in- group/out-group categorizations alone are sufficient to elicit differences in face recognition accuracy. STUDY 2 Overview Study 2 used a design similar to that of Study 1; however, before
  • 21. completing the learning phase, all subjects completed a bogus personality test that categorized them as having either ‘‘red’’ or ‘‘green’’ personality types. No description of the personality types was provided. Subjects then saw faces of 40 people labeled as belonging to these personality types (i.e., 20 people per personality type). If social categorization is sufficient to elicit differences in face recognition, subjects would be expected to recognize members of their newly found personality type better than members of the other personality type. Method Subjects and Design Forty-three White Miami University undergraduates (39 wom- en) participated for course credit. Four subjects were removed from the analysis; 2 did not receive the group manipulation because of experimenter error, and 2 admitted not following instructions or not understanding the task. Analyses were con- ducted on data from the remaining 39 subjects. A 2 (background color: red, green) � 2 (in-group: red, green) mixed-model ex-
  • 22. perimental design was used, with repeated measures on the first factor. Materials The faces used in the previous study were used in this study, except that all faces had ‘‘Red’’ or ‘‘Green’’ category labels. A bogus personality test was used to create the minimal groups. Forty questions taken from the Big Five Personality Test (Goldberg, 1993; John & Srivastava, 1999) were presented to subjects one at a time on the computer screen. Each question remained on the screen until a response was made. Responses were given on 7-point Likert scales, with higher values indi- Fig. 2. Recognition accuracy (d0) for faces with red and green back- grounds as a function of category label (Study 1, top panel) and minimal- group manipulation (Study 2, bottom panel). Volume 18—Number 8 709 Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg cating greater agreement. The questions were not systematically
  • 23. representative of personality dimensions; therefore, these data were not analyzed. Procedure The procedure was identical to that of Study 1, except as noted. After providing informed consent, subjects were instructed that they would take a computerized personality test. After subjects completed this test, the computer ostensibly analyzed their re- sponses, and informed them that they were either a ‘‘red’’ or a ‘‘green’’ personality type. Subjects were then instructed: This personality measure has been found to be very good at pre- dicting future success both socially and monetarily. The measure itself is often used by businesses and organizations as a means of identifying strong candidates for competitive positions. Further, psychologists who study relationships often use this personality inventory to identify future success in relationships. Subjects were given no further description of the personality types. They were then given a green or red wristband to wear,
  • 24. and told it was to identify them as a member of their particular group (see DeSteno et al., 2004, for a similar procedure). Subjects were then instructed that they would view faces on the computer, and that the background color for each face would denote whether that person had the red personality type or the green personality type. As in Study 1, all faces were counterbalanced across background color and for presence/absence in the learning phase on a between-subjects basis, such that for each subject, each face was equally likely to be on a red or green background and equally likely to be seen or not seen during the learning phase. Results and Discussion As in Study 1, sensitivity scores (d0) for red and green targets were subjected to a 2 (background color) � 2 (in-group) mixed- model ANOVA, with repeated measures on the first factor. Re- sults were in line with the social-categorization model, as the ANOVA yielded the predicted Background Color � In-Group
  • 25. interaction, F(1, 37) 5 4.36, p < .05, prep 5 .89, Z 2 5 .11 (see Fig. 2, bottom panel). Critically, this study tested whether a relatively arbitrarily constructed in-group is sufficient to elicit cross-race-like effects. To test this hypothesis, we collapsed the data across in-group color, to directly compare performance for in-group and out-group faces. As predicted, in-group faces (M 5 1.55, SD 5 0.56) were better recognized than out-group faces (M 5 1.34, SD 5 0.56), t(38) 5 2.06, p < .05, prep 5 .88, d 5 0.38, replicating the pattern of recognition shown in Study 1. Thus, even when subjects have equivalent perceptual expertise with in-group and out-group targets, and the in-groups and out- groups are constructed in the experimental session itself, merely categorizing targets as in-group and out-group members is suf- ficient to elicit better face recognition for in-group targets. Moreover, insofar as this study experimentally created in- groups and out-groups, it rules out alternate explanations that involve
  • 26. properties of preexisting groups. GENERAL DISCUSSION Our results fall clearly in line with social-categorization models of the CRE. That is, we found that merely categorizing faces as belonging to an in-group facilitates their recognition, relative to faces believed to belong to an out-group. Across two studies using both real and minimal groups, faces categorized as in- group members were better recognized than those categorized as out-group members. Critically, this phenomenon occurred even though perceivers’ expertise with the stimuli was held constant. In our studies, subjects and targets were always of the same race, and in all cases, stimuli were counterbalanced to equate ex- pertise with the stimuli across conditions. Thus, perceptual- expertise models do not seem entirely adequate to explain the current findings. Instead, it appears that merely categorizing a face as a member of the in-group or out-group is sufficient to influence recognition accuracy, as predicted by social-catego- rization models of the CRE.
  • 27. Although the findings of these two studies are in line with other recent findings suggesting that the CRE may be due, in part, to social-cognitive phenomena (e.g., Hugenberg et al., 2007; Levin, 1996, 2000; MacLin & Malpass, 2001, 2003; Sporer, 2001), the current research is one of only a handful of studies to show that social categorization alone, absent differ- ences in expertise, is sufficient to elicit deficits in face recog- nition. Moreover, considering the current findings in light of the CRE may also offer parsimonious explanations for other phe- nomena previously attributed to differential expertise with same-race and cross-race faces. For example, Sangrigoli et al. (2005) showed that Korean children adopted by French Cau- casian parents had better recognition for White than Asian targets. Sangrigoli et al. interpreted their findings to indicate that immersion in a cross-race environment yields substantial practice with cross-race faces, leading to better cross-race than same-race recognition. Though we agree that the experience of
  • 28. these adoptees yielded substantial expertise with cross-race faces, this experience may also have changed the adoptees’ manner of self-categorization. Adopted Korean children living in a primarily European Caucasian environment may implicitly categorize themselves as belonging to the predominant White group in their community. If so, just as Miami University stu- dents recognized supposed Miami students better than supposed students from other universities, so too may Koreans adopted by Whites recognize White faces better than Asian faces. This same process could also explain other between-groups biases in face recognition, such as the ‘‘own-age bias’’ (Anastasi & Rhodes, 2006) and the ‘‘own-sex bias’’ (Wright & Sladden, 2003). Although the current results are difficult to explain using a pure expertise mechanism, this does not mean that perceptual 710 Volume 18—Number 8 Cross-Category Effect expertise plays no role in face-processing biases such as the
  • 29. CRE. To the contrary, we argue that perceptual expertise is certainly a necessary condition for strong recognition (Hugen- berg et al., 2007). Perceivers who lack facility with the dimen- sions on which stimuli (faces included) differ are certain to have substantial difficulties in recognizing those stimuli (Tanaka & Farah, 1993; Tanaka, Kiefer, & Bukach, 2004). Indeed, the in- group/out-group model (IOM; Sporer, 2001), a theoretical model designed to explain the CRE by integrating social-categoriza- tion and perceptual-expertise mechanisms, makes predictions quite similar to our findings. In this model, as a result of greater expertise, in-group faces are processed in a default, automatic manner, characterized by holistic processing and superior rec- ognition. When out-group cues are detected, however, social categorization disrupts default holistic processing, and may be a cue to disregard the stimulus, resulting in poor recognition. A significant implication of the IOM is that recognition will suffer not just for cross-race faces, but for out-group faces more gen-
  • 30. erally. Thus, the current findings are consistent with the IOM and other models designed to synthesize social-cognitive and perceptual-expertise explanations for the CRE. When we strip away differences in perceptual expertise, a recognition deficit still occurs for out-group faces, strongly suggesting that social categorization is at play in the CRE, as well as in similar cross- age and cross-sex effects. It is important to note that the observed advantage for in-group recognition in these studies (i.e., the cross-category effect) may involve mechanisms that are partially different from those that are typically employed to explain the CRE. Although this work extends study of face recognition biases beyond the CRE, it does not yet provide a mechanism to account for these biases. Al- though it is possible that our subjects employed greater holistic processing for in-group than for out-group faces, a plausible alternative is that they differentially attended to in-group and out-group targets during encoding. For example, perhaps per-
  • 31. ceivers attended to the category-specifying information (i.e., the category labels), rather than the individuating features of the faces, when processing out-group targets (Levin, 1996, 2000). Alternatively, perhaps faces categorized as out-group members are cognitively disregarded (Rodin, 1987), deemed as less deserving of attention than faces of in-group members, leading to worse out-group recognition. Or perhaps the evaluative pos- itivity elicited by in-group targets (Claypool, Hugenberg, Housley, & Mackie, 2007; Tajfel & Turner, 1986) facilitates deeper encoding. Such positivity itself may elicit more holistic processing (Gasper & Clore, 2002; see Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005, for a similar argument), thereby facilitating later recog- nition (Michel et al., 2006; Rhodes et al., 1989). Current work in our lab is investigating these possibilities. Although the CRE certainly involves elements of differential perceptual expertise with same-race and cross-race faces, the current research provides novel evidence that in-group and out- group social-category distinctions are sufficient to elicit differ-
  • 32. ences in face recognition, even when perceptual expertise is held constant. More research is certainly needed to show how perceptual-expertise and social-categorization mechanisms act together to elicit biases in face recognition. However, the current results, taken together with other recent findings (e.g., Hugen- berg et al., 2007; Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005), suggest that researchers should take seriously the possibility that cross- category biases in face recognition, such as the CRE, may be due in part to social categorization. REFERENCES Anastasi, J.S., & Rhodes, M.G. (2006). Evidence for an own- age bias in face recognition. North American Journal of Psychology, 8, 237–252. Anthony, T., Copper, C., & Mullen, B. (1992). Cross-racial facial iden- tification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 296–301. Bodenhausen, G.V., Macrae, C.N., & Hugenberg, K. (2003).
  • 33. Social cognition. In T. Millon & M.J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychol- ogy: Personality and social psychology (pp. 257–282). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Byatt, G., & Rhodes, G. (1998). Recognition of own-race and other- race caricatures: Implications for models of face recognition. Vision Research, 38, 2455–2468. Chance, J.E., & Goldstein, A.G. (1981). Depth of processing in response to own- and other-race faces. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 475–480. Claypool, H., Hugenberg, K., Housley, M., & Mackie, D. (2007). Us and them: Familiarity disambiguates group membership. Manu- script submitted for publication. Cross, J.F., Cross, J., & Daly, J. (1971). Sex, race, age, and beauty as factors in recognition of faces. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 393–396. DeSteno, D., Dasgupta, N., Bartlett, M.Y., & Cajdric, A. (2004). Prejudice from thin air: The effect of emotion on automatic in-
  • 34. tergroup attitudes. Psychological Science, 15, 319–324. Elliott, E.A., Wills, E.J., & Goldstein, A.G. (1973). The effects of discrimination training on the recognition of white and oriental faces. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 71–73. Gasper, K., & Clore, G.L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psy- chological Science, 13, 34–40. Goldberg, L.R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26–34. Goldstein, A.G., & Chance, J.E. (1985). Effects of training on Japanese face recognition: Reduction of the other-race effect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 211–214. Hugenberg, K., Miller, J., & Claypool, H. (2007). Categorization and individuation in the cross-race recognition deficit: Toward a so- lution to an insidious problem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 334–340. John, O.P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big-five trait taxonomy: His- tory, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. Pervin &
  • 35. O.P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press. Johnson, K.J., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2005). ‘‘We all look the same to me’’: Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face rec- ognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875–881. Volume 18—Number 8 711 Michael J. Bernstein, Steven G. Young, and Kurt Hugenberg Levin, D.T. (1996). Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem- ory, and Cognition, 22, 1364–1382. Levin, D.T. (2000). Race as a visual feature: Using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: General, 129, 559–574. MacLin, O.H., & Malpass, R.S. (2001). Racial categorization of faces: The ambiguous-race face effect. Psychology, Public Policy, and
  • 36. Law, 7, 98–118. MacLin, O.H., & Malpass, R.S. (2003). The ambiguous-race face il- lusion. Perception, 32, 249–252. Malpass, R.S., & Kravitz, J. (1969). Recognition for faces of own and other race. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 330–334. Malpass, R.S., Lavigueur, H., & Weldon, D.E. (1973). Verbal and visual training in face recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 285– 292. McClelland, J.L., & Chappell, M. (1998). Familiarity breeds differ- entiation: A subjective-likelihood approach to the effects of expe- rience in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 105, 734– 760. Meissner, C.A., & Brigham, J.C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3–35. Michel, C., Rossion, B., Han, J., Chung, C.S., & Caldara, R. (2006).
  • 37. Holistic processing is finely tuned for faces of one’s own race. Psychological Science, 17, 608–615. Ng, W., & Lindsay, R. (1994). Cross-race recognition: Failure of the contact hypothesis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 25, 217–232. Rhodes, G., Brake, S., Taylor, K., & Tan, S. (1989). Expertise and configurable coding in face recognition. British Journal of Psy- chology, 80, 313–331. Rodin, M.J. (1987). Who is memorable to whom: A study of cognitive disregard. Social Cognition, 5, 144–165. Sangrigoli, S., Pallier, C., Argenti, A.-M., Ventureyra, V.A.G., & de Schonen, S. (2005). Reversibility of the other-race effect in face recognition during childhood. Psychological Science, 16, 440– 444. Sporer, S.L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An inte- gration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 36– 97. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual
  • 38. Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39. Tajfel, H., Billing, M., Brundy, R., & Flament, C. (1971). Social cat- egorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–178. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of inter- group behavior. In S. Worchek & W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson Hall. Tanaka, J., & Farah, M. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 397–405. Tanaka, J., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C.M. (2004). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: Evidence from a cross- cultural study. Cognition, 93, 1–9. Walker, P.M., & Tanaka, J.W. (2003). An encoding advantage for own- race versus other-race faces. Perception, 32, 1117–1125. Wright, D.B., & Sladden, B. (2003). An own gender bias and the im- portance of hair in face recognition. Acta Psychologica, 114, 101– 114. (RECEIVED 10/19/06; REVISION ACCEPTED 12/8/06;
  • 39. FINAL MATERIALS RECEIVED 12/15/06) 712 Volume 18—Number 8 Cross-Category Effect