SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 67
Article
Social Identity Reduces Depression by
Fostering Positive Attributions
Tegan Cruwys1, Erica I. South1, Katharine H. Greenaway1,
and S. Alexander Haslam1
Abstract
Social identities are generally associated with better health and
in particular lower levels of depression. However, there has
been
limited investigation of why social identities protect against
depression. The current research suggests that social identities
reduce
depression in part because they attenuate the depressive
attribution style (internal, stable, and global; e.g., ‘‘I failed
because I’m
stupid’’). These relationships are first investigated in a survey
(Study 1, N ¼ 139) and then followed up in an experiment that
manipulates social identity salience (Study 2, N ¼ 88). In both
cases, people with stronger social identities were less likely to
attribute negative events to internal, stable, or global causes and
subsequently reported lower levels of depression. These studies
thus indicate that social identities can protect and enhance
mental health by facilitating positive interpretations of stress
and
failure. Implications for clinical theory and practice are
discussed.
Keywords
depression, social identity, multiple group membership,
attribution, failure, mental health
We cannot live only for ourselves.
A thousand fibers connect us with our fellow men.
—Herman Melville, Moby Dick
Humans have an innate need for social connections that are vital
for
health and happiness in life (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Cohen
&
Wills, 1985). When this need is not met—when the ‘‘thousand
fibers’’ in Melville’s (1851) quote are reduced to a few or
none—people are at risk of reduced well-being and even mental
ill-
ness. In fact, depression—the leading cause of disability
worldwide
(World Health Organization, 2012)—commonly arises when a
per-
son lacks social connections (Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Thisted,
2010; Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2006).
Although extensive prior work has documented the fact that
social connectedness and social identities are critical to mental
health and reduced rates of depression, it remains unclear why
this is the case (Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, & Jetten,
2014; Jetten, Haslam, Haslam, Dingle, & Jones, 2014). We
address this research gap in the present work. What exactly do
social identities do, psychologically, that makes them so protec-
tive for mental health? In answering this question, we propose a
novel mechanism through which social identities can protect
peo-
ple against depression, that is, reduced depressive attributions.
Social Identity and Depression
A growing body of literature demonstrates that social identities
are a key psychological resource that is protective for health
generally (Haslam, Jetten, Postmes, & Haslam, 2009; Jetten,
Haslam, & Haslam, 2012) and against depression in particular
(Cruwys et al., 2013; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, et al,
2014; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014). Social
identity refers to that part of the self-concept that reflects the
internalization of group memberships (Tajfel & Turner,
1979). This means that when the self is defined by a given
social identity (e.g., ‘‘us Catholics,’’ ‘‘us psychologists,’’ and
‘‘us Australians’’), we see other members of that group not as
‘‘other’’ but as part of who and what we are. Moreover, when
groups are internalized in this way, they provide us with a sense
of belonging, purpose, and direction and therefore have the
capacity to enrich our lives—in particular, by providing a basis
for bonding, support, companionship, and security (Haslam
et al., 2009).
The psychological resources that social identity provides
in turn have positive consequences for health and well-being
(Jetten et al., 2012). Indeed, growing evidence suggests that
social identities have an important role in preventing and resol-
ving depression. For example, in a longitudinal study with a
nationally representative sample of over 5,000 older adults,
1
University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, Australia
Erica I. South and Tegan Cruwys are joint first authors.
Corresponding Author:
Tegan Cruwys, School of Psychology, The University of
Queensland, St Lucia,
Queensland, 4072, Australia.
Email: [email protected]
Social Psychological and
Personality Science
2015, Vol. 6(1) 65-74
ª The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1948550614543309
spps.sagepub.com
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
http://spps.sagepub.com
Cruwys et al. (2013) found that possessing multiple group
memberships protected against the development of depression,
improved the likelihood of recovering from depression, and
prevented depression relapse. Indeed, evidence suggested that
each new social group an individual joined reduced their risk
of relapse 4 years later by 24%. It is also worth noting that sev-
eral studies provide evidence that it is primarily social isolation
that leads to depression, with only limited evidence for the
opposite causal pathway (Cacioppo et al., 2010; Iyer, Jetten,
Tsivrikos, Postmes, & Haslam, 2009).
One clue that social identification is at the heart of this pro-
cess is that groups need to be psychologically important to an
individual in order to reduce depression symptoms (Cruwys,
Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, et al, 2014; Wakefield, Bickley, &
Sani, 2013). For instance, high social identification with the
army was associated with lower depression among soldiers
(Sani, Herrera, Wakefield, Boroch, & Gulyas, 2012), and stu-
dents who report high identification with an educational insti-
tution also report lower levels of depression (Bizumic,
Reynolds,
Turner, Bromhead, & Subasic, 2009; Brook, Garcia, & Fleming,
2008; Iyer et al., 2009). Similar effects were found in an inter-
vention study that encouraged people with depression to join
social groups: reductions in depression symptoms were most
marked for those individuals with high (rather than low) lev-
els of social identification (Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten,
et al., 2014).
Accumulating evidence thus points to the role of social iden-
tity as an active antidote to depression—both that having more
social identities and that identifying more strongly with any
particular social group protect against the condition. Yet while
there is strong evidence for such effects, it nonetheless remains
unclear exactly why joining social groups has this positive pro-
tective effect. Accordingly, there is clearly a need to explore
mechanisms through which social identity might reduce
depression.
Speaking to this issue, previous research has suggested
that the relationship between social identity and depression
might be mediated by social support (Haslam, O’Brien, Jetten,
Vormedal, & Penna, 2005; Jetten et al., 2014; Sani, 2012),
since a sense of shared social identity has been shown to be a
basis for both the provision of help and its positive construal
(e.g., Haslam, Reicher, & Levine, 2012; Levine, Prosser,
Evans, & Reicher, 2005). However, given that just thinking
about one’s social identities has the capacity to improve
well-being and resilience (e.g., Jones & Jetten, 2011), it would
seem likely that candidate mediators will also be psychological
and not (just) material. In this regard too, it is important to note
that both theoretical and empirical works speak to the capacity
for social identity to fundamentally restructure cognition (e.g.,
Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994). For example, stud-
ies have found that when individuals define themselves in
terms of shared social identity (rather than as separate individ-
uals; Turner, 1982), they are less paranoid, more empathic, and
more cognitively engaged (Branscombe & Miron, 2004;
Haslam et al., 2014; Reicher & Haslam, 2006). Other research
has shown that social identity has a profound impact on the way
people interpret and explain the social world—as reflected in
stereotypic attributions (Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1994;
Oakes, Turner, & Haslam, 1991) and in cognitive processing
more generally (e.g., McGarty, Yzerbyt, & Spears, 2002). A
key question, then, is whether such cognitive restructuring
might have implications for depression.
Social Identity and Attribution Style
One hallmark of depression is a negative attribution style
when generating causal explanations for events (Peterson &
Seligman, 1984; Sweeney, Anderson, & Bailey, 1986; Weiner,
Nierenberg, & Goldstein, 1976). Specifically, individuals with
a depressive attribution style are more likely to attribute nega-
tive events to causes that are internal, stable across time, and
influence many areas of their life (Abramson, Seligman, &
Teasdale, 1978). Positive events are explained in the opposite
way—as externally caused, transient, and situation specific.
This pattern of thinking leads people to blame themselves for
failure while denying credit for success (Sweeney et al., 1986).
A large body of evidence suggests that this depressive attri-
bution style is causally related to depression (Chan, 2012;
Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Sweeney et al., 1986). Therefore,
if a person’s depressive attribution style is altered so as to
become more positive (i.e., external, transient, and specific),
then depression symptoms should be reduced (Seligman
et al., 1988). In line with other work showing that social iden-
tities serve to structure attributions (e.g., Oakes et al., 1991), it
is therefore relevant to ask whether social identity might reduce
depression by attenuating depressive attribution style.
There are several reasons for hypothesizing that this might
be the case. First, a large body of research has shown that cau-
sal explanations can be altered by social factors. In particular,
attribution style has been found to develop in response to social
influences, such as children modeling their parents’ explana-
tory style (Haines, Metalsky, Cardamone, & Joiner, 1999; Lau,
Belli, Gregory, Napolitano, & Eley, 2012; Seligman et al.,
1984). Moreover, once a depressive attribution style develops,
it can still be modified, even in adults (Seligman et al., 1988).
Speaking to this possibility, Klein, Fencil-Morse, and Seligman
(1976) had depressed and nondepressed students complete
unsolvable problems. Attributions for failure were manipulated
by telling participants that most people succeeded on the task
(inducing an internal attribution for personal failure) or that
most people failed on the task (inducing an external attribution
for personal failure). After experiencing failure, depressed indi-
viduals tend to perform poorly on subsequent tasks. However,
inducing an external attribution of failure reduced the number
of depressed individuals who adopted a depressive attribution
style. More importantly, this external attribution eliminated
subsequent poor performance among depressed individuals.
In other words, simply being made aware that personal beha-
vior was similar to that of other group members produced more
positive attributions, reducing symptoms of depression and
associated poor performance.
66 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
One way in which social identity could change such attribu-
tions is by shifting attentional focus. Depressed individuals
have previously been found to have a self-focusing style such
that they tend to focus internally—that is on their personal
selves—following failure (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Burling,
& Tibbs, 1992). This focus on personal shortcomings leads to
internal attributions for failure, causing depression (Greenberg
et al., 1992; Romens, MacCoon, Abramson, & Pollak, 2011). In
contrast, thinking about one’s social identity shifts attention
away from the self as an individual (Hogg & Williams, 2000;
Turner & Onorato, 1999) and toward (generally positive) group
memberships (Turner, 1982). Thus, when social identity is
made salient, one’s shortcomings as an individual may be less
salient, reducing the chance of internal attributions for failure.
In addition to reducing internal attributions for failure,
social identity may also facilitate internal attributions for suc-
cess. This is important because, as a corollary to the patterns
discussed earlier, depressed individuals have been found to
lack the common (personal) self-serving attribution style in
which credit is taken for personal success and blame is denied
for personal failure (Greenberg et al., 1992; Seidel et al., 2012).
Yet while depressed individuals typically fail to exhibit this
form of self-serving bias, there is some evidence they
still engage in a group-serving attribution bias (Dietz-Uhler
& Murrell, 1998). For example, Schlenker and Britt (1996)
found that depressed individuals had a depressive attribution
style when explaining their own and strangers’ experiences but
that when attributing the same events for their close friends,
they made positive attributions—apportioning credit for their
successes and minimizing blame for their failures. This sug-
gests that depressed individuals are capable of making positive
attributions and that social identities may provide a cognitive
platform for them to do so.
The Present Research
The goal of the present research was to explore the interrela-
tionships between social identity, depressive attributions, and
depression. In line with the above-mentioned reasoning, we
tested the following four core hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1: social identity would be associated with
reduced depression,
Hypothesis 2: social identity would be associated with pos-
itive attribution styles,
Hypothesis 3: positive attribution styles would be associated
with reduced depression, and
Hypothesis 4: a decrease in depressive attributions will
mediate the protective effect of social identity on depression.
Importantly, while Hypothesis 1 is supported by previous
social identity research (e.g., Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam,
et al., 2014; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014;
Reicher & Haslam, 2006; Sani et al., 2012) and Hypothesis 3
by a large body of prior work on depression (e.g., Peterson &
Seligman, 1984; Sweeney et al., 1986; Weiner et al., 1976),
to our knowledge this is the first research to propose and test
Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 4 and to explore the cognitive
processes that mediate the relationship between social identity
and mental health more generally.
For this purpose, we conducted two studies. The first was a
survey study designed to explore the various relationships pos-
tulated in the above-mentioned hypotheses. The second was an
experiment in which we manipulated social identity salience
with a view to establishing its causal impact on attribution style
and depression.
Study 1
Study 1 surveyed final-year university students in the process of
completing a major research thesis in psychology. This sample
is
at high risk of depression (Murphy, Gray, Sterling, Reeves, &
DuCette, 2009; Stallman, 2010) and therefore particularly
appro-
priate for investigating our hypotheses. Furthermore, question-
naires were completed during the week that students were
submitting their research thesis, and for this reason the sample
was expected to be under considerable acute stress. Social iden-
tity was operationalized in this study as multiple group member-
ships, in line with previous research suggesting that these act as
a
psychological resource that protects against depression (Cruwys
et al., 2013; Iyer et al., 2009).
Method
Participants and Design. An online questionnaire was adminis-
tered to 139 final-year psychology students (Mage ¼ 23.93,
SD ¼ 5.07; 115 female) from four universities. The question-
naire included measures of multiple group memberships,
depressive attribution style, and depression symptoms. A range
of other variables related to personality and mental health were
also measured but do not relate to our hypothesis and will not
be discussed further.
Materials
Multiple group memberships. The Exeter Identity Transition
Scale (Haslam et al., 2008) was used to gauge participants’
involvement in multiple groups. The scale comprised 7 items
(e.g., ‘‘I am active in lots of different groups’’). Participants
rated their agreement with each item on a scale from 1 (not
at all) to 7 (completely), a ¼ .95.
Depressive attribution style. The Depressive Attributions
Questionnaire (DAQ; Kleim, Gonzalo, & Ehlers, 2011) was
used to measure depressive attribution style. This scale com-
prised 16 items across four related attribution areas (internal,
stable, global, and perceived helplessness) such as ‘‘when bad
things happen, I think it is my fault’’ on a 4-point scale from 0
(not at all) to 4 (very strongly), a ¼ .91.
Depression symptoms. The Centre for Epidemiological Stud-
ies Depression Scale (Radloff, 1977) was used to assess current
levels of depression. Participants responded to 20 statements
that asked how often they had experienced symptoms of
Cruwys et al. 67
depression over the last week. Response options varied from
1 (rarely or none of the time) to 4 (most or all of the time),
a ¼ .93.
Results
Descriptives Statistics and Analytic Strategy. The average level
of
depression was high (M ¼ 18.43; SD ¼ 12.31), exceeding the
diagnostic cutoff of 16. Average depressive attribution style
was also higher than previously reported norms (M ¼ 31.01;
SD ¼ 8.58, compared to M ¼ 20.52 in three studies by Kleim
et al., 2011). Students reported a moderate level of involvement
in groups (M ¼ 4.02; SD ¼ 1.60). This is similar to previous
research with students transitioning to university (Iyer et al.,
2009). Regression analyses were conducted to assess Hypoth-
eses 1, 2, and 3.
Hypothesis 1: Multiple group memberships protect against
depression. Results supported Hypothesis 1, with multiple
group memberships significantly predicting depression
scores, R
2 ¼ .07, F(1, 137) ¼ 11.66, b ¼�.28, p ¼ .001. Par-
ticipants who reported having more group memberships
tended to have lower levels of depression symptoms. Among
those with an above average number of group memberships,
38% were above the cutoff for depression; however, among
those with a below average number of groups, 65% met cri-
teria for depression.
Hypothesis 2: Multiple group memberships promote positive
attributions. Analyses also supported Hypothesis 2, R2 ¼ .05,
F(1, 137) ¼ 6.47, b¼�.21, p ¼ .012. Participants with multiple
group memberships had a more positive attribution style. Those
with higher levels of multiple group memberships (1 SD above
the mean) scored 4 points lower on the DAQ than those with
fewer group memberships (1 SD below the mean).
Hypotheses 3 and 4: Positive attributions mediate the protective
effect of group memberships. Regression analysis also
supported
Hypothesis 3. When entered in a second block following mul-
tiple group memberships, depressive attribution style signifi-
cantly predicted depression symptoms, R
2
D ¼ .24, F(1, 137)
¼ 48.23, b ¼ .50, p < .001. Among respondents who had a
more positive attribution style (i.e., below the sample mean),
32% exceeded the cutoff score for depression, whereas among
those with a more negative attribution style (above the mean),
70% met this cutoff. The impact of multiple group member-
ships also became less pronounced when depressive attribution
style was entered in the model, b ¼ �.17, p ¼ .018.
In order to test Hypothesis 4, a mediation analysis with
10,000 bootstrap samples was conducted (Hayes, 2013, model
4). Multiple group memberships were included as the predictor,
with depression symptoms as the outcome variable. Depressive
attribution style was entered as the mediator. The indirect
effect (IE) of multiple group memberships was significant
(IE ¼ �.11, standard error [SE] ¼ .04, 95% confidence inter-
val, CI [�.20, �.02]). In line with Hypothesis 4, a decrease
in depressive attribution style partially mediated the effect of
multiple group memberships on lower levels of depression
symptoms. The model is displayed in Figure 1.
We also tested the most plausible alternative mediation
model, in which depressive attributions predicted reduced mul-
tiple group memberships via increased depression symptoms.
This model did not provide a good fit to the data, as it explained
only 8% of the variance (compared to 32% of the variance in
the hypothesized model) and attribution style was not a signif-
icant direct predictor of multiple group memberships (b ¼
�.09, p ¼ .377). However, the IE of attribution style on multi-
ple group memberships was significant (IE ¼�.13, SE ¼ .06,
95% CI [�.25, �.02]).
Discussion
As hypothesized, participants with more group memberships
had lower levels of depression (Hypothesis 1) and were less
likely to make depressive attributions (Hypothesis 2). More-
over, depressive attributions were associated with higher
depressive symptoms (Hypothesis 3) and partially mediated the
relationship between multiple group membership and depres-
sion (Hypothesis 4). This study provides preliminary support
for the proposition that social identity can reduce depression
by attenuating depressive attribution style. In a sample of
highly stressed students, the availability of multiple social
identities protected against depression by encouraging a more
positive attribution style.
Although an alternative model predicting lower group mem-
berships through depression was also significant, this model
was less powerful than our hypothesized model. Of course,
with correlational data it is impossible to definitively determine
causal ordering of variables. To address this limitation, we
therefore conducted an experiment in which we manipulated
social identity salience.
Study 2
Study 2 investigated whether an experimental manipulation of
social identity salience could reduce depressive attributions
and negative mood following failure on a task. This design
would allow us to more confidently infer the causal role of
social identity in shaping depressive attributions. Negative
Multiple Group
Memberships
Depressive Attribution
Style
Depression
Symptoms
-.21*
-.28** (-.17*)
.50**
Figure 1. Depressive attribution style partially mediates the
relationship between multiple group memberships and
depression
symptoms. All numbers are standardized coefficients.
Note. N ¼ 139. *p < .05. **p < .001.
68 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
mood was used as an analogue of depression because it was not
feasible to alter an individual’s depression symptoms in an
experimental context (for similar logic, see Klein et al.,
1976; Spielberger, Ritterband, Reheiser, & Brunner, 2003).
Social identities were made salient by asking participants in
three independent conditions to reflect on no groups (control),
one group, or three groups that they belong to. Rather than
investigating attributions during a period of high stress (as in
Study 1), all participants in Study 2 experienced failure on a
problem-solving task—a context in which, theoretically,
depressive attributions are most problematic.
The study tested the same four hypotheses as in the previous
study. More specifically, we anticipated that participants in the
social identity conditions (one group or three groups) would
have reduced negative mood (Hypothesis 1) and make more
positive attributions (Hypothesis 2) than those in the control
condition and that positive attributions would reduce negative
mood (Hypothesis 3) and mediate the relationship between
social identity salience and negative mood (Hypothesis 4).
Method
Participants and Design. Participants were 88 undergraduate
psy-
chology students (Mage ¼ 19.72, SD ¼ 3.86; 55 females) who
received partial course credit for their participation. Partici-
pants were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions
(none vs. one vs. three groups) and identity salience was
manipulated between subjects. After the manipulation, all par-
ticipants completed a problem-solving task and received failure
feedback before completing the dependent measures.
Materials and Measures
Identity manipulation. In the control condition, participants
did not complete a written reflection task. In the two other con-
ditions, participants first read a short paragraph explaining
what constitutes a social identity (adapted from Haslam, Oakes,
Reynolds, & Turner, 1999). Participants in the one-group con-
dition then listed one group they belonged to and wrote about
‘‘why this group is an important part of who you are.’’ Partici-
pants in the three-group condition completed the same exer-
cise, listing three groups (Jones & Jetten, 2011). Most
participants took less than 5 min to complete the task.
Failure paradigm. The failure paradigm was presented to par-
ticipants as a four-question problem-solving task (following
Carver & Scheier, 1982; Klein et al., 1976; Welch & Huston,
1982). The task involved four unsolvable questions developed
in
accordance with previous attribution research (e.g., Mikulincer,
1989; Koole, Smeets, vanKnippenberg, & Dijksterhuis,
1999). The test was collected from participants after 5 minutes.
All participants received a score of 0 out of 4, provided in writ-
ing after a brief delay ostensibly to allow time for marking.
Failure manipulation check. Participants were asked to rate
their performance on the problem-solving task, on a scale rang-
ing from 1 (very poor) to 7 (excellent).
Current mood. The Positive and Negative Affect Scale (Wat-
son, Clark, & Tellegen, 1998) was used to measure current
mood. Participants were given a list of emotions and asked to
rate ‘‘the extent to which you feel this way right now, that is,
at the present moment’’ on scales ranging from 1 (very slightly
or not at all) to 5 (extremely), a ¼ .88. The 10-item negative
subscale was of central interest.
History of depression. Individuals with a history of depression
are more likely to display depressive attributions (Garber &
Flynn, 1998; Giles, Etzel, & Biggs, 1989). To explore whether
this moderated the hypothesized model, participants’ history of
depression was measured by asking them to respond ‘‘yes’’ or
‘‘no’’ to the item ‘‘Have you ever been bothered for most of
two
weeks either by feeling down, depressed or hopeless, or by lit-
tle interest or pleasure in doing things?’’ (Wulsin, 2012).
Depressive attribution style. As in Study 1, this was measured
by means of the DAQ (Kleim et al., 2011, a ¼ .93).
Identity manipulation check. The item ‘‘I see myself in terms
of my group memberships’’ was used to assess the impact of
the identity manipulation. Response options ranged from 1
(do not agree at all) to 7 (agree completely). Participants in the
social identity conditions (one group and three groups) were
expected to give higher responses than those in the control
condition.
A final open-ended question asked what participants
thought the experiment was about. This question was included
as a potential exclusion criterion.
Results
Four participants were excluded because they indicated strong
suspicion regarding the veracity of their failure feedback. The
final sample included 88 participants (control, n ¼ 31; one
group, n ¼ 29; and three groups, n ¼ 28). A high number of
participants (59.1%) responded ‘‘yes’’ to the question assessing
previous depression history.
As can be seen in Figure 2, for all variables of interest the
one- and three-group conditions together differed significantly
from the control but were statistically no different from one
another. As mediation requires that categorical predictor vari-
ables have no more than two levels in each analysis (Hayes,
2013), we therefore collapsed the two social identity conditions
in the analyses that follow.
Manipulation Checks. On average, participants rated their
perfor-
mance on the problem-solving task as 1.27 (SD ¼ 0.81) on a 7-
point scale, suggesting that the failure paradigm was successful.
The social identity manipulation check was also successful. Par-
ticipants in the combined social conditions reported thinking
about themselves in terms of group memberships (M1 group ¼
3.48; M3 groups ¼ 3.64) more than participants in the control
condition (Mcontrol ¼ 2.81), t(86) ¼�2.09, p ¼ .039. The
social
identity conditions did not differ from one another, t(55)
¼�0.36,
p ¼ .723.
Cruwys et al. 69
Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2: Social Identity and Positive
Attributions Protect Against Depression. Supporting Hypothesis
1, negative mood was significantly lower in the combined social
identity conditions (M1 group ¼ 15.73; M3 groups ¼ 16.36)
than in
the control condition (Mcontrol ¼ 19.35), t(86) ¼ 2.13, p ¼
.036.
The social identity conditions did not differ from one another,
t(55) ¼�.36, p ¼ .722.
Supporting Hypothesis 2, depressive attributions were sig-
nificantly lower in the combined social identity conditions
(M1 group ¼ 20.00; M3 groups ¼ 21.79) than in the control
con-
dition (Mcontrol ¼ 26.77), t(86) ¼ 2.14, p ¼ .036. Depressive
attributions were no different across the two social identity
conditions, t(55) ¼�.54, p ¼ .592.
Hypothesis 3 and Hypothesis 4: Positive Attributions Mediate
the
Protective Effect of Social Identities. Confirming Hypothesis 3,
depressive attributions significantly predicted negative mood
when controlling for social identity salience, b ¼ .45, p <
.001. The collapsed social identity conditions no longer signif-
icantly predicted negative mood with attributions in the model,
b ¼ �.12, p ¼ .206. A mediation analysis with 10,000 boot-
strap samples was conducted to test Hypothesis 4 (Hayes,
2013, model 4), as presented in Figure 3. Supporting Hypoth-
esis 4, the IE was significant (IE ¼ �.11, SE ¼ .06, 95% CI
[�.25, �.02]), indicating that depressive attribution style fully
mediated the effect of social identity on negative mood.
To investigate whether history of depression moderated the
effect of social identity on depressive attribution style, a sensi-
tivity analysis was conducted in which history of depression
was included as a moderator (Hayes, 2013, model 7). The
moderation path was nonsignificant (b ¼�.12, p ¼ .246), sug-
gesting that social identity salience was protective against
depressive attributions both for participants with and without
a history of depression.
Discussion
As hypothesized, an experimental manipulation of social iden-
tity salience reduced negative mood (Hypothesis 1) and depres-
sive attributions (Hypothesis 2). Depressive attribution style
predicted negative mood (Hypothesis 3), and this fully
mediated the protective benefits of social identity salience
(Hypothesis 4). These findings point to the causal role of social
identity in promoting a more positive attribution style and thus
protecting mental health.
Interestingly, there was no difference between the one-
group and three-group salience conditions in terms of their
capacity to buffer depressive attributions in the face of failure.
It is also worth noting that the effect sizes in the meditational
models are highly similar to those found in Study 1, despite the
different measures and methodology used in the two studies.
Finally, having a history of depression did not moderate the
effect of social identity on depressive attributions. This sug-
gests that social identification reduces depressive attributions
regardless of whether or not an individual has experienced
depression in the past.
General Discussion
Previous research has established a reliable link between social
identity and reduced depression, yet up to this point there has
been no clear evidence of the process by which this effect
might occur. To address this, we explored the possibility that
a novel mechanism is implicated in this so-called ‘‘social cure’’
effect—testing the idea that social identity acts to reduce
depression by promoting a more positive attribution style in
which blame for failure is minimized.
Correlational and experimental studies provided support for
our hypotheses. In Study 1, having multiple group member-
ships was associated with reduced depression. In Study 2, the
manipulated salience of social identity (one or three groups
vs. a control) served to reduce negative mood. In both cases, the
effects of social identity were mediated by reduced depressive
attributions. Although Study 1 showed multiple group member-
ships were associated with reduced depression, Study 2 showed
a
a
a
b
b
b
b
b
b
15
17
19
21
23
25
27
“I see myself in terms of
my group memberships”
Depressive attributions Negative mood
Social identity
salience condition:
Control
1 group
3 group
Figure 2. Mean effects of social identity salience condition on
the
social identity manipulation check, depressive attributions, and
negative mood.
Note. Columns with ‘‘a’’ are significantly different from
combined ‘‘b’’
within each variable at p < .05. Responses to ‘‘I see myself in
terms of
my group memberships’’ were scaled (multiplied by 7) so that
all three
variables could be presented in the same graph.
Social Identity
Conditions
(vs. Control)
Depressive Attribution
Style
Negative Mood
-.23*
-.24* (-.13)
.47**
Figure 3. Depressive attribution style mediates the relationship
between social identity salience and negative mood. All
numbers are
standardized coefficients.
Note. N ¼ 88. *p < .05. **p < .01.
70 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
that the salience of even one social identity was enough to have
this effect.
At a theoretical level, these findings confirm the capacity for
social identity to structure both cognition and perception
(Turner et al., 1994). The social identity approach, and in par-
ticular self-categorization theory, is a cognitive model that
posits that as individuals’ sense of self shifts to the collective
level, their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are filtered
through this collective lens (Turner & Oakes, 1997). However,
the focus of past research has predominantly been on the affec-
tive and behavioral consequences of social identity, rather than
on the cognitive and perceptual consequences (with some nota-
ble exceptions, e.g., Abrams, Wetherell, Cochrane, Hogg, &
Turner, 1990; Haslam et al., 1999; Oakes et al., 1994). There-
fore, the present research is important not only because it elu-
cidates a mechanism for the protective benefits of social
identity but also because it provides evidence for the impact
of social identity on the way people perceive and understand
the world. Specifically, when individuals self-define in terms
of their social identity (or identities), they are more robust in
the face of stress (Study 1) and failure (Study 2), partly because
they are less likely to believe that the cause of this negative
event is something internal, stable, and global (e.g., ‘‘I’m just
not good enough’’). This research adds to the growing body
of work elucidating the positive benefits of social identity, par-
ticularly for mental health, which acts as a counterpoint to the
traditional focus of social identity research on negative conse-
quences such as discrimination and prejudice.
In addition, these findings have implications for clinical
psychology. Although attributions are a central concept in our
understanding of the development of depression (Gladstone &
Kaslow, 1995; Sweeney et al., 1986), very few studies have
investigated the origins and development of attribution styles.
Our findings demonstrate that attribution styles are not a ‘‘set
in stone’’ individual difference but are responsive to even quite
subtle experimental manipulations. This dovetails with other
findings that simple interventions, such as affirmations, can
modify attributions and subsequent mental health outcomes
(Peden, Rayens, Hall, & Beebe, 2001). This was true not only
for low-risk participants but also for those who reported a his-
tory of depression. As the success of many therapeutic
approaches hinges on the malleability of attributions (e.g.,
cognitive-behavior therapy; Beck, 2011; Kovacs & Beck,
1978), our findings are thus encoukraging for psychotherapists.
Conclusion
The present studies suggest that social identity ameliorates
depression not only because belonging to groups makes people
feel good. Instead they support the claim that social identity has
this impact because it restructures the way people understand
the world and, in particular, the way they interpret failure. This
has important implications for clinical practice, as it suggests
not only that attribution styles can be changed, but also that
they can be modified through social, not just clinical, means.
Indeed, rather than focusing clinical energies on trying to
alter depressive attributions in the abstract, these findings sug-
gest that fostering meaningful social connections may instead
be a more fruitful means of improving a person’s explanatory
style and mental health. A growing body of research
supports such an approach (Cruwys et al., 2013; Cruwys,
Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014), and it is also apparent that
efforts to boost the availability of social identities could prove
less expensive and time consuming than traditional forms of
psychotherapy. In short, as observed by Captain Ahab in Moby
Dick, it appears there is much to be gained from focusing less
on living life ‘‘only for ourselves,’’ and more on strengthening
the social fibers that serve to make that life meaningful and
purposive.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
This research was partially funded by an Australian Research
Council
Laureate Grant no. FL110100199 (see http://www.arc.gov.au/).
The
funders had no role in study design, data collection and
analysis, deci-
sion to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
Abrams, D., Wetherell, M. S., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A., &
Turner, J.
C. (1990). Knowing what to think by knowing who you are:
Self-
categorization and the nature of norm formation, conformity and
group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29,
97–119.
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978).
Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.
Jour-
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74. doi:10.1037/0021-
843X.
87.1.49
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong:
Desire
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human
motivation.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.
117.3.497
Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics and
beyond
(2nd ed., Vol. 8809). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Bizumic, B., Reynolds, K. J., Turner, J. C., Bromhead, D., &
Subasic,
E. (2009). The role of group in individual functioning: School
identification and the psychological well-being of staff and stu-
dents. Applied Psychology, 58, 171–192. doi:10.1111/j.1464-
0597.2008.00387.x
Branscombe, N. R., & Miron, A. M. (2004). Interpreting the
ingroup’s
negative actions toward another group: Emotional reactions to
appraised harm. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach (Eds.), The
social
life of emotions. Studies in emotion and social interaction (pp.
314–335). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
Brook, A. T., Garcia, J., & Fleming, M. A. (2008). The effects
of
multiple identities on psychological well-being. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1588–1600. doi:10.1177/
0146167208324629
Cruwys et al. 71
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010).
Perceived
social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of
loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago
health,
aging and social relations study. Psychology and Aging, 25,
453–463. doi:10.1037/a0017216
Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., &
Thisted,
R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive
symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses.
Psychology
and Aging, 21, 140–151. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Outcome expectancy,
locus
of attribution for expectancy, and self-directed attention as
determinants of evaluations and performance. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 184–200. doi:10.1016/
0022-1031(82)90049-X
Chan, S. M. (2012). Early adolescent depressive mood: Direct
and
indirect effects of attributional styles and coping. Child
Psychiatry
and Human Development, 43, 455–470. doi:10.1007/s10578-
011-
0275-9
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the
buf-
fering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.
Retrieved
from http://ukpmc.ac.uk/abstract/MED/3901065
Cruwys, T., Dingle, G. A., Haslam, S. A., Haslam, C., Jetten, J.,
&
Morton, T. A. (2013). Social group memberships protect against
future depression, alleviate depression symptoms and prevent
depression relapse. Social Science & Medicine, 98, 179–186.
doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.013
Cruwys, T., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G. A., Haslam, C., & Jetten,
J.
(2014). Depression and social identity: An integrative review.
Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 215–238. doi:10.
1177/1088868314523839
Cruwys, T., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G. A., Jetten, J., Hornsey,
M. J.,
Chong, E. M. D., & Oei, T. P. S. (2014). Feeling connected
again:
Interventions that increase social identification reduce
depression
symptoms in community and clinical settings. Journal of
Affective
Disorders, 159, 139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.019
Dietz-Uhler, B., & Murrell, A. (1998). Effects of social identity
and
threat on self-esteem and group attributions. Group Dynamics:
The-
ory, Research and Practice, 2, 24–35. doi:10.1037/1089-
2699.2.1.24
Garber, J., & Flynn, C. (1998). Origins of the depressive cogni-
tive style. In D. K. Routh (Ed.), The science of clinical psy-
chology: Accomplishments and future directions (pp. 53–93).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:
10.1037/10280-003
Giles, D. E., Etzel, B. A., & Biggs, M. M. (1989). Long-term
effects of
unipolar depression on cognitions. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
30,
225–230. doi:10.1016/0010-440X(89)90042-4
Gladstone, T. R. G., & Kaslow, N. J. (1995). Depression and
attribu-
tions in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review.
Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 597–606. doi:10.1007/
BF01447664
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Burling, J., & Tibbs, K. (1992).
Depression, self-focused attention, and the self-serving attribu-
tional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 959–
965.
doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90129-D
Haines, B. A., Metalsky, G. I., Cardamone, A. L., & Joiner, T.
(1999).
Interpersonal and cognitive pathways into the origins of
attributional
style: A developmental perspective. In T. Joiner & J. C. Coyne
(Eds.),
The interactional nature of depression: Advances in
interpersonal
approaches (pp. 65–92). doi:10.1037/10311-003
Haslam, C., Haslam, S. A., Knight, C., Gleibs, I., Ysseldyk, R.,
&
McCloskey, L.-G. (2014). We can work it out: Group decision-
making builds social identity and enhances the cognitive perfor-
mance of care home residents. British Journal of Psychology,
105, 17–34. doi:10.1111/bjop.12012
Haslam, C., Holme, A., Haslam, A., Iyer, A., Jetten, J., &
Williams, H. W.
(2008). Maintaining group memberships: Social identity conti-
nuity predicts well-being after stroke. Neuropsychological
Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 18, 671–691. doi:10.
1080/09602010701643449
Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., Postmes, T., & Haslam, C. (2009).
Social iden-
tity, health and well-being: An emerging agenda for applied
psy-
chology. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 58, 1–
23.
Haslam, S. A., Oakes, P. J., Reynolds, K., & Turner, J. C.
(1999).
Social identity salience and the emergence of stereotype
consen-
sus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 809–818.
doi:10.1177/0146167299025007004
Haslam, S. A., O’Brien, A., Jetten, J., Vormedal, K., & Penna,
S.
(2005). Taking the strain: Social identity, social support, and
the
experience of stress. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44,
355–370. doi:10.1348/014466605X37468
Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., & Levine, M. (2012). When other
peo-
ple are heaven, when other people are hell: How social identity
determines the nature and impact of social support. In J. Jetten,
C. Haslam, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social cure: Identity,
health
and well-being (pp. 157–174). New York: Psychology Press.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and
con-
ditional process analysis: A regression-based approach.
Retrieved
from http://afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderation-
and-conditional-process-analysis.html
Hogg, M. A., & Williams, K. D. (2000). From I to we: Social
identity
and the collective self. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
Practice, 4, 81–97. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.4.1.81
Iyer, A., Jetten, J., Tsivrikos, D., Postmes, T., & Haslam, S. A.
(2009).
The more (and the more compatible) the merrier: Multiple group
memberships and identity compatibility as predictors of adjust-
ment after life transitions. British Journal of Social Psychology,
48, 707–733. doi:10.1348/014466608X397628
Jetten, J., Haslam, C., & Haslam, S. A. (2012). The social cure:
Iden-
tity, health and well-being. New York: Psychology Press.
Jetten, J., Haslam, C., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G., & Jones, J. M.
(2014). How groups affect our health and well-being: The path
from theory to policy. Social Issues and Policy Review, 8,
103–139. doi:10.1111/sipr.12003
Jones, J. M., & Jetten, J. (2011). Recovering from strain and
enduring
pain: Multiple group memberships promote resilience in the
face of
physical challenges. Social Psychological and Personality Sci-
ence, 2, 239–244. doi:10.1177/1948550610386806
Kleim, B., Gonzalo, D., & Ehlers, A. (2011). The depressive
attribu-
tions questionnaire (DAQ): Development of a short self-report
mea-
sure of depressogenic attributions. Journal of Psychopathology
and
Behavioral Assessment, 33, 375–385. doi:10.1007/s10862-011-
9234-9
72 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
http://ukpmc.ac.uk/abstract/MED/3901065
http://afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderation-and-
conditional-process-analysis.html
http://afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderation-and-
conditional-process-analysis.html
Klein, D. C., Fencil-Morse, E., & Seligman, E. P. (1976).
Learned
helplessness, depression and attribution of failure. Journal of
Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 33, 508–516.
Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis,
A.
(1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation.
Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111–125. doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.77.1.111
Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Maladaptive cognitive
structures in
depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 525–533.
Lau, J. Y. F., Belli, S. D., Gregory, A. M., Napolitano, M., &
Eley, T.
C. (2012). The role of children’s negative attributions on
depres-
sive symptoms: An inherited characteristic or a product of the
early
environment? Developmental Science, 15, 569–578.
doi:10.1111/j.
1467-7687.2012.01152.x
Levine, M., Prosser, A., Evans, D., & Reicher, S. (2005).
Identity and
emergency intervention: How social group membership and
inclu-
siveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior.
Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 443–453. doi:10.1177/
0146167204271651
McGarty, C., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Spears, R. (Eds.). (2002).
Stereotypes
as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about
social
groups. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Melville, H. (1851). Moby dick; or the whale. New York, NY:
Penguin
Books.
Mikulincer, M. (1989). Learned helplessness and egotism:
Effects of
internal/external attribution on performance following
unsolvable
problems. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 17–29. doi:
10.1111/j.2044-8309.1989.tb00841.x
Murphy, R. J., Gray, S. A., Sterling, G., Reeves, K., & DuCette,
J.
(2009). A comparative study of professional student stress.
Journal
of Dental Education, 73, 328–337. Retrieved from http://www.
ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19289722
Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Cognition
and the
group: Social identity and self-categorization. In P. J. Oakes, S.
A.
Haslam, & J. C. Turner (Eds.), Stereotyping and social reality
(pp.
80–103). London, UK: Blackwell.
Oakes, P. J., Turner, J. C., & Haslam, S. A. (1991). Perceiving
people
as group members: The role of fit in the salience of social
categor-
izations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 125–144.
Peden, A. R., Rayens, M. K., Hall, L. A., & Beebe, L. H.
(2001). Pre-
venting depression in high-risk college women: A report of an
18-
month follow-up. Journal of American College Health, 49,
299–306. doi:10.1080/07448480109596316
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations
as a
risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological
Review, 91, 347–374. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.347
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report
depression scale
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological
Mea-
surement, 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306
Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2006). Rethinking the psychology
of tyr-
anny: The BBC prison study. British Journal of Social
Psychology,
45, 1–40; discussion 47–53. doi:10.1348/014466605X48998
Romens, S. E., MacCoon, D. G., Abramson, L. Y., & Pollak, S.
D.
(2011). Cognitive style moderates attention to attribution-
relevant stimuli. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 134–141.
doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9345-8
Sani, F. (2012). Group identification, social relationships, and
health.
In J. Jetten, C. Haslam, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social cure:
Identity, health and well-being (pp. 21–37). New York, NY:
Psy-
chology Press.
Sani, F., Herrera, M., Wakefield, J. R. H., Boroch, O., &
Gulyas, C.
(2012). Comparing social contact and group identification as
pre-
dictors of mental health. British Journal of Social Psychology,
51,
781–790. doi:10.1111/j.2044-B309.2012.02101
Schlenker, B. R., & Britt, T. W. (1996). Depression and the
explana-
tion of events that happen to self, close others and strangers.
Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 180–192. doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.71.1.180
Seidel, E.-M., Satterthwaite, T. D., Eickhoff, S. B., Schneider,
F.,
Gur, R. C., Wolf, D. H., . . . Derntl, B. (2012). Neural
correlates
of depressive realism–an fMRI study on causal attribution in
depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 138, 268–276. doi:
10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.041
Seligman, M. E. P., Castellon, C., Cacciola, J., Schulman, P.,
Luborsky, L., Ollove, M., & Downing, R. (1988). Explanatory
style change during cognitive therapy for unipolar depression.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 13–18. doi:10.1037/0021-
843X.97.1.13
Seligman, M. E. P., Kaslow, N. D., Alloy, L. B., Peterson, C.,
Tanembaum, R. L., & Abramson, L. Y. (1984). Attributional
style
and depressive symptoms among children. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 93, 235–238. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.93.2.235
Spielberger, C. D., Ritterband, L. M., Reheiser, E. C., &
Brunner, T.
M. (2003). The nature and measurement of depression. Interna-
tional Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 3, 209–234.
Stallman, H. M. (2010). Psychological distress in university
students:
A comparison with general population data. Australian
Psycholo-
gist, 45, 249–257. doi:10.1080/00050067.2010.482109
Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986).
Attributional style
in depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology, 50, 974–991.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of
intergroup
conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worehel (Eds.), The social psy-
chology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey: Brooks/
Cole.
Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the
social
group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup
relations
(pp. 15–40). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Turner, J. C., & Oakes, P. J. (1997). The socially structured
mind. In
C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social
psychol-
ogy (pp. 355–373). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C.
(1994). Self
and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463.
Turner, J. C., & Onorato, R. S. (1999). Social identity,
personality and
the self-concept: A self-categorization perspective. In T.
Postmes &
N. R. Branscombe (Eds.), Rediscovering social identity (pp.
315–339). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Wakefield, J. R. H., Bickley, S., & Sani, F. (2013). The effects
of iden-
tification with a support group on the mental health of people
with
multiple sclerosis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 74,
420–426. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.02.002
Cruwys et al. 73
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19289722
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19289722
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development
and
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Weiner, B., Nierenberg, R., & Goldstein, M. (1976). Social
learning
(locus of control) versus attributional (causal stability)
interpreta-
tions of expectancy of success. Journal of Personality, 44,
52–68. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00583.x
Welch, R. L., & Huston, A. C. (1982). Effects of induced
success/fail-
ure and attributions on the problem-solving behavior of
psycholo-
gically androgynous and feminine women. Journal of
Personality,
50, 81–97. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1982.tb00747.x
World Health Organization. (2012). Depression: A global public
health concern. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/mental_
health/management/depression/who_paper_depression_wfmh_
2012.pdf
Wulsin, L. (2012). Comparison of two depression measures for
pre-
dicting stroke outcomes. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
72,
175–179. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.11.015
Author Biographies
Tegan Cruwys, PhD, Australian National University, is a
lecturer and
clinical psychologist at the University of Queensland. Her
research is
at the intersection of social and clinical psychology, with a
focus on
social identity processes in mental health.
Erica I. South graduated with first class honors in psychology in
2013
from the University of Queensland.
Katharine H. Greenaway, PhD, University of Queensland, is a
glo-
bal scholar with the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research,
based
at the University of Queensland, Australia. Her research centres
on
issues of control, including perceived life control and the
exertion of
self-control.
S. Alexander Haslam, PhD, Macquarie University, is a professor
of
psychology and Australian Research Council Laureate Fellow at
the
University of Queensland, Australia. His research interests are
in the
social psychology of health and well-being, stereotyping and
preju-
dice, and organizational behavior.
74 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who
_paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf
http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who
_paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf
http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who
_paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf
<<
/ASCII85EncodePages false
/AllowTransparency false
/AutoPositionEPSFiles true
/AutoRotatePages /None
/Binding /Left
/CalGrayProfile (Gray Gamma 2.2)
/CalRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/CalCMYKProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051 v2)
/sRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/CannotEmbedFontPolicy /Warning
/CompatibilityLevel 1.3
/CompressObjects /Off
/CompressPages true
/ConvertImagesToIndexed true
/PassThroughJPEGImages false
/CreateJDFFile false
/CreateJobTicket false
/DefaultRenderingIntent /Default
/DetectBlends true
/DetectCurves 0.1000
/ColorConversionStrategy /LeaveColorUnchanged
/DoThumbnails false
/EmbedAllFonts true
/EmbedOpenType false
/ParseICCProfilesInComments true
/EmbedJobOptions true
/DSCReportingLevel 0
/EmitDSCWarnings false
/EndPage -1
/ImageMemory 1048576
/LockDistillerParams true
/MaxSubsetPct 100
/Optimize true
/OPM 1
/ParseDSCComments true
/ParseDSCCommentsForDocInfo true
/PreserveCopyPage true
/PreserveDICMYKValues true
/PreserveEPSInfo true
/PreserveFlatness false
/PreserveHalftoneInfo false
/PreserveOPIComments false
/PreserveOverprintSettings true
/StartPage 1
/SubsetFonts true
/TransferFunctionInfo /Apply
/UCRandBGInfo /Remove
/UsePrologue false
/ColorSettingsFile ()
/AlwaysEmbed [ true
]
/NeverEmbed [ true
]
/AntiAliasColorImages false
/CropColorImages false
/ColorImageMinResolution 266
/ColorImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleColorImages true
/ColorImageDownsampleType /Average
/ColorImageResolution 175
/ColorImageDepth -1
/ColorImageMinDownsampleDepth 1
/ColorImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286
/EncodeColorImages true
/ColorImageFilter /DCTEncode
/AutoFilterColorImages true
/ColorImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG
/ColorACSImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.40
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/ColorImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.76
/HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2]
>>
/JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/JPEG2000ColorImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/AntiAliasGrayImages false
/CropGrayImages false
/GrayImageMinResolution 266
/GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleGrayImages true
/GrayImageDownsampleType /Average
/GrayImageResolution 175
/GrayImageDepth -1
/GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2
/GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286
/EncodeGrayImages true
/GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode
/AutoFilterGrayImages true
/GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG
/GrayACSImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.40
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/GrayImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.76
/HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2]
>>
/JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/JPEG2000GrayImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/AntiAliasMonoImages false
/CropMonoImages false
/MonoImageMinResolution 900
/MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleMonoImages true
/MonoImageDownsampleType /Average
/MonoImageResolution 175
/MonoImageDepth -1
/MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286
/EncodeMonoImages true
/MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode
/MonoImageDict <<
/K -1
>>
/AllowPSXObjects false
/CheckCompliance [
/None
]
/PDFX1aCheck false
/PDFX3Check false
/PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false
/PDFXNoTrimBoxError true
/PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
]
/PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox false
/PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
]
/PDFXOutputIntentProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051
v2)
/PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier (CGATS TR 001)
/PDFXOutputCondition ()
/PDFXRegistryName (http://www.color.org)
/PDFXTrapped /Unknown
/Description <<
/ENU
<FEFF00550073006500200074006800650073006500200053006
1006700650020007300740061006e006400610072006400200073
0065007400740069006e0067007300200066006f0072002000630
0720065006100740069006e006700200077006500620020005000
440046002000660069006c00650073002e0020005400680065007
30065002000730065007400740069006e0067007300200063006f
006e006600690067007500720065006400200066006f007200200
04100630072006f006200610074002000760037002e0030002e00
20004300720065006100740065006400200062007900200054007
2006f00790020004f007400730020006100740020005300610067
00650020005500530020006f006e002000310031002f003100300
02f0032003000300036002e000d000d0032003000300050005000
49002f003600300030005000500049002f004a005000450047002
0004d0065006400690075006d002f004300430049005400540020
00470072006f0075007000200034>
>>
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(Common)
(1.0)
]
/OtherNamespaces [
<<
/AsReaderSpreads false
/CropImagesToFrames true
/ErrorControl /WarnAndContinue
/FlattenerIgnoreSpreadOverrides false
/IncludeGuidesGrids false
/IncludeNonPrinting false
/IncludeSlug false
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(InDesign)
(4.0)
]
/OmitPlacedBitmaps false
/OmitPlacedEPS false
/OmitPlacedPDF false
/SimulateOverprint /Legacy
>>
<<
/AllowImageBreaks true
/AllowTableBreaks true
/ExpandPage false
/HonorBaseURL true
/HonorRolloverEffect false
/IgnoreHTMLPageBreaks false
/IncludeHeaderFooter false
/MarginOffset [
0
0
0
0
]
/MetadataAuthor ()
/MetadataKeywords ()
/MetadataSubject ()
/MetadataTitle ()
/MetricPageSize [
0
0
]
/MetricUnit /inch
/MobileCompatible 0
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(GoLive)
(8.0)
]
/OpenZoomToHTMLFontSize false
/PageOrientation /Portrait
/RemoveBackground false
/ShrinkContent true
/TreatColorsAs /MainMonitorColors
/UseEmbeddedProfiles false
/UseHTMLTitleAsMetadata true
>>
<<
/AddBleedMarks false
/AddColorBars false
/AddCropMarks false
/AddPageInfo false
/AddRegMarks false
/BleedOffset [
9
9
9
9
]
/ConvertColors /ConvertToRGB
/DestinationProfileName (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/DestinationProfileSelector /UseName
/Downsample16BitImages true
/FlattenerPreset <<
/ClipComplexRegions true
/ConvertStrokesToOutlines false
/ConvertTextToOutlines false
/GradientResolution 300
/LineArtTextResolution 1200
/PresetName ([High Resolution])
/PresetSelector /HighResolution
/RasterVectorBalance 1
>>
/FormElements true
/GenerateStructure false
/IncludeBookmarks false
/IncludeHyperlinks false
/IncludeInteractive false
/IncludeLayers false
/IncludeProfiles true
/MarksOffset 9
/MarksWeight 0.125000
/MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(CreativeSuite)
(2.0)
]
/PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK
/PageMarksFile /RomanDefault
/PreserveEditing true
/UntaggedCMYKHandling /UseDocumentProfile
/UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile
/UseDocumentBleed false
>>
]
/SyntheticBoldness 1.000000
>> setdistillerparams
<<
/HWResolution [288 288]
/PageSize [612.000 792.000]
>> setpagedevice

More Related Content

Similar to ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx

Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceCallaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceOmar Gonzalez-Valentino
 
Reply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docx
Reply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docxReply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docx
Reply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docxsodhi3
 
Over the past several decades
Over the past several decadesOver the past several decades
Over the past several decadeschirag patel
 
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BChronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BSarah M
 
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...Andrzej Pankalla
 
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...Andrzej Pankalla
 
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docx
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docxARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docx
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docxfestockton
 
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and copARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and copBetseyCalderon89
 
Compassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature ReviewCompassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature ReviewFatima Shaikh
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...inventionjournals
 
Relationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College Students
Relationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College StudentsRelationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College Students
Relationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College StudentsDanielle Hoyt
 
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...Victor Sojo Monzon
 
The Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death Essay
The Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death EssayThe Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death Essay
The Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death EssayAngie Lee
 
2011 Portfolio
2011 Portfolio2011 Portfolio
2011 Portfoliosherapate
 
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxAdvanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxDemetriosPeratsakis
 
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxAdvanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxDemetriosPeratsakis
 
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxAdvanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxDemetriosPeratsakis
 
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdfBrown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdftytrete
 

Similar to ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx (20)

Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceCallaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
 
Reply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docx
Reply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docxReply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docx
Reply Reply to 2 other classmates by offering 1 new piece of info.docx
 
Over the past several decades
Over the past several decadesOver the past several decades
Over the past several decades
 
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BChronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
 
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
 
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
 
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docx
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docxARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docx
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop.docx
 
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and copARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop
ARTICLEDepersonalization, adversity, emotionality, and cop
 
Compassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature ReviewCompassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature Review
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
 
N46103110.pdf
N46103110.pdfN46103110.pdf
N46103110.pdf
 
Relationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College Students
Relationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College StudentsRelationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College Students
Relationship Between Volunteering and Subjective Happiness in College Students
 
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
Mediated moderation or moderated mediation relationship between length of une...
 
The Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death Essay
The Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death EssayThe Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death Essay
The Presentation Of Stress, Grief, And Death Essay
 
2011 Portfolio
2011 Portfolio2011 Portfolio
2011 Portfolio
 
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxAdvanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
 
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxAdvanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
 
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptxAdvanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
Advanced Methods in Counseling & Psychotherapy July 2023.pptx
 
Compassion
CompassionCompassion
Compassion
 
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdfBrown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
 

More from davezstarr61655

you must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docx
you must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docxyou must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docx
you must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docxYou must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docx
You must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docxYou must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docx
You must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docxdavezstarr61655
 
you must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docx
you must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docxyou must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docx
you must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docxYou must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docxYou must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docx
You must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docxYou must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docx
You must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docx
You must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docxYou must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docx
You must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docx
You must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docxYou must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docx
You must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docx
You must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docxYou must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docx
You must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docx
You must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docxYou must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docx
You must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docx
You must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docxYou must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docx
You must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docx
You must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docxYou must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docx
You must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docx
You must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docxYou must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docx
You must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docx
You must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docxYou must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docx
You must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docx
You must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docxYou must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docx
You must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docxYou must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docxYou must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docx
You must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docxYou must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docx
You must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docxdavezstarr61655
 
You must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docx
You must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docxYou must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docx
You must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docxdavezstarr61655
 

More from davezstarr61655 (20)

you must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docx
you must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docxyou must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docx
you must read two articles which are from the field of Human Resou.docx
 
You must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docxYou must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 5 pages paper. You must use a minimu.docx
 
You must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docx
You must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docxYou must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docx
You must provide a references for entire posting. Please use APA for.docx
 
you must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docx
you must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docxyou must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docx
you must present your findings to the IT supervisor before the s.docx
 
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docxYou must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a m.docx
 
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docxYou must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docx
You must produce a minimum of a 10 pages paper. You must use a minim.docx
 
You must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docx
You must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docxYou must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docx
You must include the resources you used if any.. THese papers are op.docx
 
You must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docx
You must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docxYou must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docx
You must include the Textbook definition and a picture f.docx
 
You must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docx
You must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docxYou must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docx
You must include 6 references, two that must come peer-reviewed .docx
 
You must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docx
You must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docxYou must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docx
You must have the Project Libre to create this! Develop a chart .docx
 
You must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docx
You must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docxYou must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docx
You must have experience doing PhD proposal , so the topic of th.docx
 
You must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docx
You must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docxYou must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docx
You must have at least 3 sources. Sources should be cited within you.docx
 
You must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docx
You must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docxYou must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docx
You must have access to the book needed for the Case Study part. I w.docx
 
You must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docx
You must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docxYou must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docx
You must give the Source(s) of your answers (textbook - WITH SPECIFI.docx
 
You must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docx
You must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docxYou must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docx
You must create a court system for the newly created state of Puerto.docx
 
You must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docx
You must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docxYou must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docx
You must conduct an informational interview as part of this course. .docx
 
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docxYou must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Culture.docx
 
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docxYou must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docx
You must complete an Electronic Presentation on Black Cu.docx
 
You must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docx
You must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docxYou must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docx
You must choose from the approved list below1. Angels .docx
 
You must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docx
You must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docxYou must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docx
You must be proficient in all MS office. I am looking for someon.docx
 

Recently uploaded

भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,Virag Sontakke
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfUjwalaBharambe
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxAvyJaneVismanos
 

Recently uploaded (20)

भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
 

ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx

  • 1. Article Social Identity Reduces Depression by Fostering Positive Attributions Tegan Cruwys1, Erica I. South1, Katharine H. Greenaway1, and S. Alexander Haslam1 Abstract Social identities are generally associated with better health and in particular lower levels of depression. However, there has been limited investigation of why social identities protect against depression. The current research suggests that social identities reduce depression in part because they attenuate the depressive attribution style (internal, stable, and global; e.g., ‘‘I failed because I’m stupid’’). These relationships are first investigated in a survey (Study 1, N ¼ 139) and then followed up in an experiment that manipulates social identity salience (Study 2, N ¼ 88). In both cases, people with stronger social identities were less likely to attribute negative events to internal, stable, or global causes and subsequently reported lower levels of depression. These studies thus indicate that social identities can protect and enhance mental health by facilitating positive interpretations of stress and failure. Implications for clinical theory and practice are discussed. Keywords
  • 2. depression, social identity, multiple group membership, attribution, failure, mental health We cannot live only for ourselves. A thousand fibers connect us with our fellow men. —Herman Melville, Moby Dick Humans have an innate need for social connections that are vital for health and happiness in life (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Cohen & Wills, 1985). When this need is not met—when the ‘‘thousand fibers’’ in Melville’s (1851) quote are reduced to a few or none—people are at risk of reduced well-being and even mental ill- ness. In fact, depression—the leading cause of disability worldwide (World Health Organization, 2012)—commonly arises when a per- son lacks social connections (Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2010; Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2006). Although extensive prior work has documented the fact that social connectedness and social identities are critical to mental
  • 3. health and reduced rates of depression, it remains unclear why this is the case (Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, & Jetten, 2014; Jetten, Haslam, Haslam, Dingle, & Jones, 2014). We address this research gap in the present work. What exactly do social identities do, psychologically, that makes them so protec- tive for mental health? In answering this question, we propose a novel mechanism through which social identities can protect peo- ple against depression, that is, reduced depressive attributions. Social Identity and Depression A growing body of literature demonstrates that social identities are a key psychological resource that is protective for health generally (Haslam, Jetten, Postmes, & Haslam, 2009; Jetten, Haslam, & Haslam, 2012) and against depression in particular (Cruwys et al., 2013; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, et al, 2014; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014). Social identity refers to that part of the self-concept that reflects the internalization of group memberships (Tajfel & Turner,
  • 4. 1979). This means that when the self is defined by a given social identity (e.g., ‘‘us Catholics,’’ ‘‘us psychologists,’’ and ‘‘us Australians’’), we see other members of that group not as ‘‘other’’ but as part of who and what we are. Moreover, when groups are internalized in this way, they provide us with a sense of belonging, purpose, and direction and therefore have the capacity to enrich our lives—in particular, by providing a basis for bonding, support, companionship, and security (Haslam et al., 2009). The psychological resources that social identity provides in turn have positive consequences for health and well-being (Jetten et al., 2012). Indeed, growing evidence suggests that social identities have an important role in preventing and resol- ving depression. For example, in a longitudinal study with a nationally representative sample of over 5,000 older adults, 1 University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, Australia Erica I. South and Tegan Cruwys are joint first authors. Corresponding Author:
  • 5. Tegan Cruwys, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, 4072, Australia. Email: [email protected] Social Psychological and Personality Science 2015, Vol. 6(1) 65-74 ª The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1948550614543309 spps.sagepub.com http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://spps.sagepub.com Cruwys et al. (2013) found that possessing multiple group memberships protected against the development of depression, improved the likelihood of recovering from depression, and prevented depression relapse. Indeed, evidence suggested that each new social group an individual joined reduced their risk of relapse 4 years later by 24%. It is also worth noting that sev- eral studies provide evidence that it is primarily social isolation that leads to depression, with only limited evidence for the opposite causal pathway (Cacioppo et al., 2010; Iyer, Jetten,
  • 6. Tsivrikos, Postmes, & Haslam, 2009). One clue that social identification is at the heart of this pro- cess is that groups need to be psychologically important to an individual in order to reduce depression symptoms (Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, et al, 2014; Wakefield, Bickley, & Sani, 2013). For instance, high social identification with the army was associated with lower depression among soldiers (Sani, Herrera, Wakefield, Boroch, & Gulyas, 2012), and stu- dents who report high identification with an educational insti- tution also report lower levels of depression (Bizumic, Reynolds, Turner, Bromhead, & Subasic, 2009; Brook, Garcia, & Fleming, 2008; Iyer et al., 2009). Similar effects were found in an inter- vention study that encouraged people with depression to join social groups: reductions in depression symptoms were most marked for those individuals with high (rather than low) lev- els of social identification (Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014).
  • 7. Accumulating evidence thus points to the role of social iden- tity as an active antidote to depression—both that having more social identities and that identifying more strongly with any particular social group protect against the condition. Yet while there is strong evidence for such effects, it nonetheless remains unclear exactly why joining social groups has this positive pro- tective effect. Accordingly, there is clearly a need to explore mechanisms through which social identity might reduce depression. Speaking to this issue, previous research has suggested that the relationship between social identity and depression might be mediated by social support (Haslam, O’Brien, Jetten, Vormedal, & Penna, 2005; Jetten et al., 2014; Sani, 2012), since a sense of shared social identity has been shown to be a basis for both the provision of help and its positive construal (e.g., Haslam, Reicher, & Levine, 2012; Levine, Prosser, Evans, & Reicher, 2005). However, given that just thinking about one’s social identities has the capacity to improve
  • 8. well-being and resilience (e.g., Jones & Jetten, 2011), it would seem likely that candidate mediators will also be psychological and not (just) material. In this regard too, it is important to note that both theoretical and empirical works speak to the capacity for social identity to fundamentally restructure cognition (e.g., Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994). For example, stud- ies have found that when individuals define themselves in terms of shared social identity (rather than as separate individ- uals; Turner, 1982), they are less paranoid, more empathic, and more cognitively engaged (Branscombe & Miron, 2004; Haslam et al., 2014; Reicher & Haslam, 2006). Other research has shown that social identity has a profound impact on the way people interpret and explain the social world—as reflected in stereotypic attributions (Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1994; Oakes, Turner, & Haslam, 1991) and in cognitive processing more generally (e.g., McGarty, Yzerbyt, & Spears, 2002). A key question, then, is whether such cognitive restructuring might have implications for depression.
  • 9. Social Identity and Attribution Style One hallmark of depression is a negative attribution style when generating causal explanations for events (Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Sweeney, Anderson, & Bailey, 1986; Weiner, Nierenberg, & Goldstein, 1976). Specifically, individuals with a depressive attribution style are more likely to attribute nega- tive events to causes that are internal, stable across time, and influence many areas of their life (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). Positive events are explained in the opposite way—as externally caused, transient, and situation specific. This pattern of thinking leads people to blame themselves for failure while denying credit for success (Sweeney et al., 1986). A large body of evidence suggests that this depressive attri- bution style is causally related to depression (Chan, 2012; Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Sweeney et al., 1986). Therefore, if a person’s depressive attribution style is altered so as to become more positive (i.e., external, transient, and specific), then depression symptoms should be reduced (Seligman
  • 10. et al., 1988). In line with other work showing that social iden- tities serve to structure attributions (e.g., Oakes et al., 1991), it is therefore relevant to ask whether social identity might reduce depression by attenuating depressive attribution style. There are several reasons for hypothesizing that this might be the case. First, a large body of research has shown that cau- sal explanations can be altered by social factors. In particular, attribution style has been found to develop in response to social influences, such as children modeling their parents’ explana- tory style (Haines, Metalsky, Cardamone, & Joiner, 1999; Lau, Belli, Gregory, Napolitano, & Eley, 2012; Seligman et al., 1984). Moreover, once a depressive attribution style develops, it can still be modified, even in adults (Seligman et al., 1988). Speaking to this possibility, Klein, Fencil-Morse, and Seligman (1976) had depressed and nondepressed students complete unsolvable problems. Attributions for failure were manipulated by telling participants that most people succeeded on the task (inducing an internal attribution for personal failure) or that
  • 11. most people failed on the task (inducing an external attribution for personal failure). After experiencing failure, depressed indi- viduals tend to perform poorly on subsequent tasks. However, inducing an external attribution of failure reduced the number of depressed individuals who adopted a depressive attribution style. More importantly, this external attribution eliminated subsequent poor performance among depressed individuals. In other words, simply being made aware that personal beha- vior was similar to that of other group members produced more positive attributions, reducing symptoms of depression and associated poor performance. 66 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1) One way in which social identity could change such attribu- tions is by shifting attentional focus. Depressed individuals have previously been found to have a self-focusing style such that they tend to focus internally—that is on their personal selves—following failure (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Burling,
  • 12. & Tibbs, 1992). This focus on personal shortcomings leads to internal attributions for failure, causing depression (Greenberg et al., 1992; Romens, MacCoon, Abramson, & Pollak, 2011). In contrast, thinking about one’s social identity shifts attention away from the self as an individual (Hogg & Williams, 2000; Turner & Onorato, 1999) and toward (generally positive) group memberships (Turner, 1982). Thus, when social identity is made salient, one’s shortcomings as an individual may be less salient, reducing the chance of internal attributions for failure. In addition to reducing internal attributions for failure, social identity may also facilitate internal attributions for suc- cess. This is important because, as a corollary to the patterns discussed earlier, depressed individuals have been found to lack the common (personal) self-serving attribution style in which credit is taken for personal success and blame is denied for personal failure (Greenberg et al., 1992; Seidel et al., 2012). Yet while depressed individuals typically fail to exhibit this form of self-serving bias, there is some evidence they
  • 13. still engage in a group-serving attribution bias (Dietz-Uhler & Murrell, 1998). For example, Schlenker and Britt (1996) found that depressed individuals had a depressive attribution style when explaining their own and strangers’ experiences but that when attributing the same events for their close friends, they made positive attributions—apportioning credit for their successes and minimizing blame for their failures. This sug- gests that depressed individuals are capable of making positive attributions and that social identities may provide a cognitive platform for them to do so. The Present Research The goal of the present research was to explore the interrela- tionships between social identity, depressive attributions, and depression. In line with the above-mentioned reasoning, we tested the following four core hypotheses: Hypothesis 1: social identity would be associated with reduced depression, Hypothesis 2: social identity would be associated with pos-
  • 14. itive attribution styles, Hypothesis 3: positive attribution styles would be associated with reduced depression, and Hypothesis 4: a decrease in depressive attributions will mediate the protective effect of social identity on depression. Importantly, while Hypothesis 1 is supported by previous social identity research (e.g., Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam, et al., 2014; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014; Reicher & Haslam, 2006; Sani et al., 2012) and Hypothesis 3 by a large body of prior work on depression (e.g., Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Sweeney et al., 1986; Weiner et al., 1976), to our knowledge this is the first research to propose and test Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 4 and to explore the cognitive processes that mediate the relationship between social identity and mental health more generally. For this purpose, we conducted two studies. The first was a survey study designed to explore the various relationships pos- tulated in the above-mentioned hypotheses. The second was an
  • 15. experiment in which we manipulated social identity salience with a view to establishing its causal impact on attribution style and depression. Study 1 Study 1 surveyed final-year university students in the process of completing a major research thesis in psychology. This sample is at high risk of depression (Murphy, Gray, Sterling, Reeves, & DuCette, 2009; Stallman, 2010) and therefore particularly appro- priate for investigating our hypotheses. Furthermore, question- naires were completed during the week that students were submitting their research thesis, and for this reason the sample was expected to be under considerable acute stress. Social iden- tity was operationalized in this study as multiple group member- ships, in line with previous research suggesting that these act as a psychological resource that protects against depression (Cruwys et al., 2013; Iyer et al., 2009). Method
  • 16. Participants and Design. An online questionnaire was adminis- tered to 139 final-year psychology students (Mage ¼ 23.93, SD ¼ 5.07; 115 female) from four universities. The question- naire included measures of multiple group memberships, depressive attribution style, and depression symptoms. A range of other variables related to personality and mental health were also measured but do not relate to our hypothesis and will not be discussed further. Materials Multiple group memberships. The Exeter Identity Transition Scale (Haslam et al., 2008) was used to gauge participants’ involvement in multiple groups. The scale comprised 7 items (e.g., ‘‘I am active in lots of different groups’’). Participants rated their agreement with each item on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely), a ¼ .95. Depressive attribution style. The Depressive Attributions Questionnaire (DAQ; Kleim, Gonzalo, & Ehlers, 2011) was used to measure depressive attribution style. This scale com- prised 16 items across four related attribution areas (internal, stable, global, and perceived helplessness) such as ‘‘when bad
  • 17. things happen, I think it is my fault’’ on a 4-point scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very strongly), a ¼ .91. Depression symptoms. The Centre for Epidemiological Stud- ies Depression Scale (Radloff, 1977) was used to assess current levels of depression. Participants responded to 20 statements that asked how often they had experienced symptoms of Cruwys et al. 67 depression over the last week. Response options varied from 1 (rarely or none of the time) to 4 (most or all of the time), a ¼ .93. Results Descriptives Statistics and Analytic Strategy. The average level of depression was high (M ¼ 18.43; SD ¼ 12.31), exceeding the diagnostic cutoff of 16. Average depressive attribution style was also higher than previously reported norms (M ¼ 31.01; SD ¼ 8.58, compared to M ¼ 20.52 in three studies by Kleim et al., 2011). Students reported a moderate level of involvement in groups (M ¼ 4.02; SD ¼ 1.60). This is similar to previous research with students transitioning to university (Iyer et al., 2009). Regression analyses were conducted to assess Hypoth-
  • 18. eses 1, 2, and 3. Hypothesis 1: Multiple group memberships protect against depression. Results supported Hypothesis 1, with multiple group memberships significantly predicting depression scores, R 2 ¼ .07, F(1, 137) ¼ 11.66, b ¼�.28, p ¼ .001. Par- ticipants who reported having more group memberships tended to have lower levels of depression symptoms. Among those with an above average number of group memberships, 38% were above the cutoff for depression; however, among those with a below average number of groups, 65% met cri- teria for depression. Hypothesis 2: Multiple group memberships promote positive attributions. Analyses also supported Hypothesis 2, R2 ¼ .05, F(1, 137) ¼ 6.47, b¼�.21, p ¼ .012. Participants with multiple group memberships had a more positive attribution style. Those with higher levels of multiple group memberships (1 SD above the mean) scored 4 points lower on the DAQ than those with fewer group memberships (1 SD below the mean). Hypotheses 3 and 4: Positive attributions mediate the protective effect of group memberships. Regression analysis also supported Hypothesis 3. When entered in a second block following mul-
  • 19. tiple group memberships, depressive attribution style signifi- cantly predicted depression symptoms, R 2 D ¼ .24, F(1, 137) ¼ 48.23, b ¼ .50, p < .001. Among respondents who had a more positive attribution style (i.e., below the sample mean), 32% exceeded the cutoff score for depression, whereas among those with a more negative attribution style (above the mean), 70% met this cutoff. The impact of multiple group member- ships also became less pronounced when depressive attribution style was entered in the model, b ¼ �.17, p ¼ .018. In order to test Hypothesis 4, a mediation analysis with 10,000 bootstrap samples was conducted (Hayes, 2013, model 4). Multiple group memberships were included as the predictor, with depression symptoms as the outcome variable. Depressive attribution style was entered as the mediator. The indirect effect (IE) of multiple group memberships was significant (IE ¼ �.11, standard error [SE] ¼ .04, 95% confidence inter- val, CI [�.20, �.02]). In line with Hypothesis 4, a decrease in depressive attribution style partially mediated the effect of multiple group memberships on lower levels of depression symptoms. The model is displayed in Figure 1.
  • 20. We also tested the most plausible alternative mediation model, in which depressive attributions predicted reduced mul- tiple group memberships via increased depression symptoms. This model did not provide a good fit to the data, as it explained only 8% of the variance (compared to 32% of the variance in the hypothesized model) and attribution style was not a signif- icant direct predictor of multiple group memberships (b ¼ �.09, p ¼ .377). However, the IE of attribution style on multi- ple group memberships was significant (IE ¼�.13, SE ¼ .06, 95% CI [�.25, �.02]). Discussion As hypothesized, participants with more group memberships had lower levels of depression (Hypothesis 1) and were less likely to make depressive attributions (Hypothesis 2). More- over, depressive attributions were associated with higher depressive symptoms (Hypothesis 3) and partially mediated the relationship between multiple group membership and depres- sion (Hypothesis 4). This study provides preliminary support for the proposition that social identity can reduce depression by attenuating depressive attribution style. In a sample of
  • 21. highly stressed students, the availability of multiple social identities protected against depression by encouraging a more positive attribution style. Although an alternative model predicting lower group mem- berships through depression was also significant, this model was less powerful than our hypothesized model. Of course, with correlational data it is impossible to definitively determine causal ordering of variables. To address this limitation, we therefore conducted an experiment in which we manipulated social identity salience. Study 2 Study 2 investigated whether an experimental manipulation of social identity salience could reduce depressive attributions and negative mood following failure on a task. This design would allow us to more confidently infer the causal role of social identity in shaping depressive attributions. Negative Multiple Group Memberships
  • 22. Depressive Attribution Style Depression Symptoms -.21* -.28** (-.17*) .50** Figure 1. Depressive attribution style partially mediates the relationship between multiple group memberships and depression symptoms. All numbers are standardized coefficients. Note. N ¼ 139. *p < .05. **p < .001. 68 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1) mood was used as an analogue of depression because it was not feasible to alter an individual’s depression symptoms in an experimental context (for similar logic, see Klein et al., 1976; Spielberger, Ritterband, Reheiser, & Brunner, 2003). Social identities were made salient by asking participants in three independent conditions to reflect on no groups (control), one group, or three groups that they belong to. Rather than
  • 23. investigating attributions during a period of high stress (as in Study 1), all participants in Study 2 experienced failure on a problem-solving task—a context in which, theoretically, depressive attributions are most problematic. The study tested the same four hypotheses as in the previous study. More specifically, we anticipated that participants in the social identity conditions (one group or three groups) would have reduced negative mood (Hypothesis 1) and make more positive attributions (Hypothesis 2) than those in the control condition and that positive attributions would reduce negative mood (Hypothesis 3) and mediate the relationship between social identity salience and negative mood (Hypothesis 4). Method Participants and Design. Participants were 88 undergraduate psy- chology students (Mage ¼ 19.72, SD ¼ 3.86; 55 females) who received partial course credit for their participation. Partici- pants were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions (none vs. one vs. three groups) and identity salience was manipulated between subjects. After the manipulation, all par-
  • 24. ticipants completed a problem-solving task and received failure feedback before completing the dependent measures. Materials and Measures Identity manipulation. In the control condition, participants did not complete a written reflection task. In the two other con- ditions, participants first read a short paragraph explaining what constitutes a social identity (adapted from Haslam, Oakes, Reynolds, & Turner, 1999). Participants in the one-group con- dition then listed one group they belonged to and wrote about ‘‘why this group is an important part of who you are.’’ Partici- pants in the three-group condition completed the same exer- cise, listing three groups (Jones & Jetten, 2011). Most participants took less than 5 min to complete the task. Failure paradigm. The failure paradigm was presented to par- ticipants as a four-question problem-solving task (following Carver & Scheier, 1982; Klein et al., 1976; Welch & Huston, 1982). The task involved four unsolvable questions developed in accordance with previous attribution research (e.g., Mikulincer,
  • 25. 1989; Koole, Smeets, vanKnippenberg, & Dijksterhuis, 1999). The test was collected from participants after 5 minutes. All participants received a score of 0 out of 4, provided in writ- ing after a brief delay ostensibly to allow time for marking. Failure manipulation check. Participants were asked to rate their performance on the problem-solving task, on a scale rang- ing from 1 (very poor) to 7 (excellent). Current mood. The Positive and Negative Affect Scale (Wat- son, Clark, & Tellegen, 1998) was used to measure current mood. Participants were given a list of emotions and asked to rate ‘‘the extent to which you feel this way right now, that is, at the present moment’’ on scales ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely), a ¼ .88. The 10-item negative subscale was of central interest. History of depression. Individuals with a history of depression are more likely to display depressive attributions (Garber & Flynn, 1998; Giles, Etzel, & Biggs, 1989). To explore whether this moderated the hypothesized model, participants’ history of depression was measured by asking them to respond ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ to the item ‘‘Have you ever been bothered for most of two
  • 26. weeks either by feeling down, depressed or hopeless, or by lit- tle interest or pleasure in doing things?’’ (Wulsin, 2012). Depressive attribution style. As in Study 1, this was measured by means of the DAQ (Kleim et al., 2011, a ¼ .93). Identity manipulation check. The item ‘‘I see myself in terms of my group memberships’’ was used to assess the impact of the identity manipulation. Response options ranged from 1 (do not agree at all) to 7 (agree completely). Participants in the social identity conditions (one group and three groups) were expected to give higher responses than those in the control condition. A final open-ended question asked what participants thought the experiment was about. This question was included as a potential exclusion criterion. Results Four participants were excluded because they indicated strong suspicion regarding the veracity of their failure feedback. The final sample included 88 participants (control, n ¼ 31; one group, n ¼ 29; and three groups, n ¼ 28). A high number of participants (59.1%) responded ‘‘yes’’ to the question assessing
  • 27. previous depression history. As can be seen in Figure 2, for all variables of interest the one- and three-group conditions together differed significantly from the control but were statistically no different from one another. As mediation requires that categorical predictor vari- ables have no more than two levels in each analysis (Hayes, 2013), we therefore collapsed the two social identity conditions in the analyses that follow. Manipulation Checks. On average, participants rated their perfor- mance on the problem-solving task as 1.27 (SD ¼ 0.81) on a 7- point scale, suggesting that the failure paradigm was successful. The social identity manipulation check was also successful. Par- ticipants in the combined social conditions reported thinking about themselves in terms of group memberships (M1 group ¼ 3.48; M3 groups ¼ 3.64) more than participants in the control condition (Mcontrol ¼ 2.81), t(86) ¼�2.09, p ¼ .039. The social identity conditions did not differ from one another, t(55) ¼�0.36, p ¼ .723. Cruwys et al. 69
  • 28. Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2: Social Identity and Positive Attributions Protect Against Depression. Supporting Hypothesis 1, negative mood was significantly lower in the combined social identity conditions (M1 group ¼ 15.73; M3 groups ¼ 16.36) than in the control condition (Mcontrol ¼ 19.35), t(86) ¼ 2.13, p ¼ .036. The social identity conditions did not differ from one another, t(55) ¼�.36, p ¼ .722. Supporting Hypothesis 2, depressive attributions were sig- nificantly lower in the combined social identity conditions (M1 group ¼ 20.00; M3 groups ¼ 21.79) than in the control con- dition (Mcontrol ¼ 26.77), t(86) ¼ 2.14, p ¼ .036. Depressive attributions were no different across the two social identity conditions, t(55) ¼�.54, p ¼ .592. Hypothesis 3 and Hypothesis 4: Positive Attributions Mediate the Protective Effect of Social Identities. Confirming Hypothesis 3, depressive attributions significantly predicted negative mood when controlling for social identity salience, b ¼ .45, p < .001. The collapsed social identity conditions no longer signif- icantly predicted negative mood with attributions in the model, b ¼ �.12, p ¼ .206. A mediation analysis with 10,000 boot- strap samples was conducted to test Hypothesis 4 (Hayes,
  • 29. 2013, model 4), as presented in Figure 3. Supporting Hypoth- esis 4, the IE was significant (IE ¼ �.11, SE ¼ .06, 95% CI [�.25, �.02]), indicating that depressive attribution style fully mediated the effect of social identity on negative mood. To investigate whether history of depression moderated the effect of social identity on depressive attribution style, a sensi- tivity analysis was conducted in which history of depression was included as a moderator (Hayes, 2013, model 7). The moderation path was nonsignificant (b ¼�.12, p ¼ .246), sug- gesting that social identity salience was protective against depressive attributions both for participants with and without a history of depression. Discussion As hypothesized, an experimental manipulation of social iden- tity salience reduced negative mood (Hypothesis 1) and depres- sive attributions (Hypothesis 2). Depressive attribution style predicted negative mood (Hypothesis 3), and this fully mediated the protective benefits of social identity salience (Hypothesis 4). These findings point to the causal role of social identity in promoting a more positive attribution style and thus
  • 30. protecting mental health. Interestingly, there was no difference between the one- group and three-group salience conditions in terms of their capacity to buffer depressive attributions in the face of failure. It is also worth noting that the effect sizes in the meditational models are highly similar to those found in Study 1, despite the different measures and methodology used in the two studies. Finally, having a history of depression did not moderate the effect of social identity on depressive attributions. This sug- gests that social identification reduces depressive attributions regardless of whether or not an individual has experienced depression in the past. General Discussion Previous research has established a reliable link between social identity and reduced depression, yet up to this point there has been no clear evidence of the process by which this effect might occur. To address this, we explored the possibility that a novel mechanism is implicated in this so-called ‘‘social cure’’
  • 31. effect—testing the idea that social identity acts to reduce depression by promoting a more positive attribution style in which blame for failure is minimized. Correlational and experimental studies provided support for our hypotheses. In Study 1, having multiple group member- ships was associated with reduced depression. In Study 2, the manipulated salience of social identity (one or three groups vs. a control) served to reduce negative mood. In both cases, the effects of social identity were mediated by reduced depressive attributions. Although Study 1 showed multiple group member- ships were associated with reduced depression, Study 2 showed a a a b b b b
  • 32. b b 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 “I see myself in terms of my group memberships” Depressive attributions Negative mood Social identity salience condition: Control 1 group 3 group Figure 2. Mean effects of social identity salience condition on the
  • 33. social identity manipulation check, depressive attributions, and negative mood. Note. Columns with ‘‘a’’ are significantly different from combined ‘‘b’’ within each variable at p < .05. Responses to ‘‘I see myself in terms of my group memberships’’ were scaled (multiplied by 7) so that all three variables could be presented in the same graph. Social Identity Conditions (vs. Control) Depressive Attribution Style Negative Mood -.23* -.24* (-.13) .47** Figure 3. Depressive attribution style mediates the relationship between social identity salience and negative mood. All numbers are standardized coefficients. Note. N ¼ 88. *p < .05. **p < .01. 70 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
  • 34. that the salience of even one social identity was enough to have this effect. At a theoretical level, these findings confirm the capacity for social identity to structure both cognition and perception (Turner et al., 1994). The social identity approach, and in par- ticular self-categorization theory, is a cognitive model that posits that as individuals’ sense of self shifts to the collective level, their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are filtered through this collective lens (Turner & Oakes, 1997). However, the focus of past research has predominantly been on the affec- tive and behavioral consequences of social identity, rather than on the cognitive and perceptual consequences (with some nota- ble exceptions, e.g., Abrams, Wetherell, Cochrane, Hogg, & Turner, 1990; Haslam et al., 1999; Oakes et al., 1994). There- fore, the present research is important not only because it elu- cidates a mechanism for the protective benefits of social identity but also because it provides evidence for the impact of social identity on the way people perceive and understand
  • 35. the world. Specifically, when individuals self-define in terms of their social identity (or identities), they are more robust in the face of stress (Study 1) and failure (Study 2), partly because they are less likely to believe that the cause of this negative event is something internal, stable, and global (e.g., ‘‘I’m just not good enough’’). This research adds to the growing body of work elucidating the positive benefits of social identity, par- ticularly for mental health, which acts as a counterpoint to the traditional focus of social identity research on negative conse- quences such as discrimination and prejudice. In addition, these findings have implications for clinical psychology. Although attributions are a central concept in our understanding of the development of depression (Gladstone & Kaslow, 1995; Sweeney et al., 1986), very few studies have investigated the origins and development of attribution styles. Our findings demonstrate that attribution styles are not a ‘‘set in stone’’ individual difference but are responsive to even quite subtle experimental manipulations. This dovetails with other
  • 36. findings that simple interventions, such as affirmations, can modify attributions and subsequent mental health outcomes (Peden, Rayens, Hall, & Beebe, 2001). This was true not only for low-risk participants but also for those who reported a his- tory of depression. As the success of many therapeutic approaches hinges on the malleability of attributions (e.g., cognitive-behavior therapy; Beck, 2011; Kovacs & Beck, 1978), our findings are thus encoukraging for psychotherapists. Conclusion The present studies suggest that social identity ameliorates depression not only because belonging to groups makes people feel good. Instead they support the claim that social identity has this impact because it restructures the way people understand the world and, in particular, the way they interpret failure. This has important implications for clinical practice, as it suggests not only that attribution styles can be changed, but also that they can be modified through social, not just clinical, means. Indeed, rather than focusing clinical energies on trying to
  • 37. alter depressive attributions in the abstract, these findings sug- gest that fostering meaningful social connections may instead be a more fruitful means of improving a person’s explanatory style and mental health. A growing body of research supports such an approach (Cruwys et al., 2013; Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Jetten, et al., 2014), and it is also apparent that efforts to boost the availability of social identities could prove less expensive and time consuming than traditional forms of psychotherapy. In short, as observed by Captain Ahab in Moby Dick, it appears there is much to be gained from focusing less on living life ‘‘only for ourselves,’’ and more on strengthening the social fibers that serve to make that life meaningful and purposive. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding This research was partially funded by an Australian Research
  • 38. Council Laureate Grant no. FL110100199 (see http://www.arc.gov.au/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, deci- sion to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. References Abrams, D., Wetherell, M. S., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1990). Knowing what to think by knowing who you are: Self- categorization and the nature of norm formation, conformity and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 97–119. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Jour- nal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74. doi:10.1037/0021- 843X. 87.1.49 Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
  • 39. for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909. 117.3.497 Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics and beyond (2nd ed., Vol. 8809). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Bizumic, B., Reynolds, K. J., Turner, J. C., Bromhead, D., & Subasic, E. (2009). The role of group in individual functioning: School identification and the psychological well-being of staff and stu- dents. Applied Psychology, 58, 171–192. doi:10.1111/j.1464- 0597.2008.00387.x Branscombe, N. R., & Miron, A. M. (2004). Interpreting the ingroup’s negative actions toward another group: Emotional reactions to appraised harm. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach (Eds.), The social life of emotions. Studies in emotion and social interaction (pp. 314–335). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
  • 40. Brook, A. T., Garcia, J., & Fleming, M. A. (2008). The effects of multiple identities on psychological well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1588–1600. doi:10.1177/ 0146167208324629 Cruwys et al. 71 Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago health, aging and social relations study. Psychology and Aging, 25, 453–463. doi:10.1037/a0017216 Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and Aging, 21, 140–151. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140 Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Outcome expectancy,
  • 41. locus of attribution for expectancy, and self-directed attention as determinants of evaluations and performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 184–200. doi:10.1016/ 0022-1031(82)90049-X Chan, S. M. (2012). Early adolescent depressive mood: Direct and indirect effects of attributional styles and coping. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43, 455–470. doi:10.1007/s10578- 011- 0275-9 Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buf- fering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357. Retrieved from http://ukpmc.ac.uk/abstract/MED/3901065 Cruwys, T., Dingle, G. A., Haslam, S. A., Haslam, C., Jetten, J., & Morton, T. A. (2013). Social group memberships protect against future depression, alleviate depression symptoms and prevent
  • 42. depression relapse. Social Science & Medicine, 98, 179–186. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.013 Cruwys, T., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G. A., Haslam, C., & Jetten, J. (2014). Depression and social identity: An integrative review. Per- sonality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 215–238. doi:10. 1177/1088868314523839 Cruwys, T., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G. A., Jetten, J., Hornsey, M. J., Chong, E. M. D., & Oei, T. P. S. (2014). Feeling connected again: Interventions that increase social identification reduce depression symptoms in community and clinical settings. Journal of Affective Disorders, 159, 139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.019 Dietz-Uhler, B., & Murrell, A. (1998). Effects of social identity and threat on self-esteem and group attributions. Group Dynamics: The- ory, Research and Practice, 2, 24–35. doi:10.1037/1089- 2699.2.1.24
  • 43. Garber, J., & Flynn, C. (1998). Origins of the depressive cogni- tive style. In D. K. Routh (Ed.), The science of clinical psy- chology: Accomplishments and future directions (pp. 53–93). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/10280-003 Giles, D. E., Etzel, B. A., & Biggs, M. M. (1989). Long-term effects of unipolar depression on cognitions. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30, 225–230. doi:10.1016/0010-440X(89)90042-4 Gladstone, T. R. G., & Kaslow, N. J. (1995). Depression and attribu- tions in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 597–606. doi:10.1007/ BF01447664 Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Burling, J., & Tibbs, K. (1992). Depression, self-focused attention, and the self-serving attribu- tional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 959– 965.
  • 44. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90129-D Haines, B. A., Metalsky, G. I., Cardamone, A. L., & Joiner, T. (1999). Interpersonal and cognitive pathways into the origins of attributional style: A developmental perspective. In T. Joiner & J. C. Coyne (Eds.), The interactional nature of depression: Advances in interpersonal approaches (pp. 65–92). doi:10.1037/10311-003 Haslam, C., Haslam, S. A., Knight, C., Gleibs, I., Ysseldyk, R., & McCloskey, L.-G. (2014). We can work it out: Group decision- making builds social identity and enhances the cognitive perfor- mance of care home residents. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 17–34. doi:10.1111/bjop.12012 Haslam, C., Holme, A., Haslam, A., Iyer, A., Jetten, J., & Williams, H. W. (2008). Maintaining group memberships: Social identity conti- nuity predicts well-being after stroke. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 18, 671–691. doi:10.
  • 45. 1080/09602010701643449 Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., Postmes, T., & Haslam, C. (2009). Social iden- tity, health and well-being: An emerging agenda for applied psy- chology. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 58, 1– 23. Haslam, S. A., Oakes, P. J., Reynolds, K., & Turner, J. C. (1999). Social identity salience and the emergence of stereotype consen- sus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 809–818. doi:10.1177/0146167299025007004 Haslam, S. A., O’Brien, A., Jetten, J., Vormedal, K., & Penna, S. (2005). Taking the strain: Social identity, social support, and the experience of stress. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 355–370. doi:10.1348/014466605X37468 Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., & Levine, M. (2012). When other peo- ple are heaven, when other people are hell: How social identity
  • 46. determines the nature and impact of social support. In J. Jetten, C. Haslam, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social cure: Identity, health and well-being (pp. 157–174). New York: Psychology Press. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and con- ditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Retrieved from http://afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderation- and-conditional-process-analysis.html Hogg, M. A., & Williams, K. D. (2000). From I to we: Social identity and the collective self. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 4, 81–97. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.4.1.81 Iyer, A., Jetten, J., Tsivrikos, D., Postmes, T., & Haslam, S. A. (2009). The more (and the more compatible) the merrier: Multiple group memberships and identity compatibility as predictors of adjust- ment after life transitions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 707–733. doi:10.1348/014466608X397628 Jetten, J., Haslam, C., & Haslam, S. A. (2012). The social cure:
  • 47. Iden- tity, health and well-being. New York: Psychology Press. Jetten, J., Haslam, C., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G., & Jones, J. M. (2014). How groups affect our health and well-being: The path from theory to policy. Social Issues and Policy Review, 8, 103–139. doi:10.1111/sipr.12003 Jones, J. M., & Jetten, J. (2011). Recovering from strain and enduring pain: Multiple group memberships promote resilience in the face of physical challenges. Social Psychological and Personality Sci- ence, 2, 239–244. doi:10.1177/1948550610386806 Kleim, B., Gonzalo, D., & Ehlers, A. (2011). The depressive attribu- tions questionnaire (DAQ): Development of a short self-report mea- sure of depressogenic attributions. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 375–385. doi:10.1007/s10862-011- 9234-9 72 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1)
  • 48. http://ukpmc.ac.uk/abstract/MED/3901065 http://afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderation-and- conditional-process-analysis.html http://afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderation-and- conditional-process-analysis.html Klein, D. C., Fencil-Morse, E., & Seligman, E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness, depression and attribution of failure. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 33, 508–516. Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111–125. doi:10. 1037/0022-3514.77.1.111 Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Maladaptive cognitive structures in depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 525–533. Lau, J. Y. F., Belli, S. D., Gregory, A. M., Napolitano, M., & Eley, T. C. (2012). The role of children’s negative attributions on depres-
  • 49. sive symptoms: An inherited characteristic or a product of the early environment? Developmental Science, 15, 569–578. doi:10.1111/j. 1467-7687.2012.01152.x Levine, M., Prosser, A., Evans, D., & Reicher, S. (2005). Identity and emergency intervention: How social group membership and inclu- siveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 443–453. doi:10.1177/ 0146167204271651 McGarty, C., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Spears, R. (Eds.). (2002). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Melville, H. (1851). Moby dick; or the whale. New York, NY: Penguin Books. Mikulincer, M. (1989). Learned helplessness and egotism:
  • 50. Effects of internal/external attribution on performance following unsolvable problems. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 17–29. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1989.tb00841.x Murphy, R. J., Gray, S. A., Sterling, G., Reeves, K., & DuCette, J. (2009). A comparative study of professional student stress. Journal of Dental Education, 73, 328–337. Retrieved from http://www. ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19289722 Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Cognition and the group: Social identity and self-categorization. In P. J. Oakes, S. A. Haslam, & J. C. Turner (Eds.), Stereotyping and social reality (pp. 80–103). London, UK: Blackwell. Oakes, P. J., Turner, J. C., & Haslam, S. A. (1991). Perceiving people as group members: The role of fit in the salience of social categor-
  • 51. izations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 125–144. Peden, A. R., Rayens, M. K., Hall, L. A., & Beebe, L. H. (2001). Pre- venting depression in high-risk college women: A report of an 18- month follow-up. Journal of American College Health, 49, 299–306. doi:10.1080/07448480109596316 Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.347 Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Mea- surement, 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306 Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2006). Rethinking the psychology of tyr- anny: The BBC prison study. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 1–40; discussion 47–53. doi:10.1348/014466605X48998 Romens, S. E., MacCoon, D. G., Abramson, L. Y., & Pollak, S.
  • 52. D. (2011). Cognitive style moderates attention to attribution- relevant stimuli. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 134–141. doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9345-8 Sani, F. (2012). Group identification, social relationships, and health. In J. Jetten, C. Haslam, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social cure: Identity, health and well-being (pp. 21–37). New York, NY: Psy- chology Press. Sani, F., Herrera, M., Wakefield, J. R. H., Boroch, O., & Gulyas, C. (2012). Comparing social contact and group identification as pre- dictors of mental health. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 781–790. doi:10.1111/j.2044-B309.2012.02101 Schlenker, B. R., & Britt, T. W. (1996). Depression and the explana- tion of events that happen to self, close others and strangers. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 180–192. doi:10.
  • 53. 1037/0022-3514.71.1.180 Seidel, E.-M., Satterthwaite, T. D., Eickhoff, S. B., Schneider, F., Gur, R. C., Wolf, D. H., . . . Derntl, B. (2012). Neural correlates of depressive realism–an fMRI study on causal attribution in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 138, 268–276. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.041 Seligman, M. E. P., Castellon, C., Cacciola, J., Schulman, P., Luborsky, L., Ollove, M., & Downing, R. (1988). Explanatory style change during cognitive therapy for unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 13–18. doi:10.1037/0021- 843X.97.1.13 Seligman, M. E. P., Kaslow, N. D., Alloy, L. B., Peterson, C., Tanembaum, R. L., & Abramson, L. Y. (1984). Attributional style and depressive symptoms among children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 235–238. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.93.2.235 Spielberger, C. D., Ritterband, L. M., Reheiser, E. C., & Brunner, T.
  • 54. M. (2003). The nature and measurement of depression. Interna- tional Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 3, 209–234. Stallman, H. M. (2010). Psychological distress in university students: A comparison with general population data. Australian Psycholo- gist, 45, 249–257. doi:10.1080/00050067.2010.482109 Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional style in depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 974–991. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worehel (Eds.), The social psy- chology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey: Brooks/ Cole. Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup relations
  • 55. (pp. 15–40). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Turner, J. C., & Oakes, P. J. (1997). The socially structured mind. In C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psychol- ogy (pp. 355–373). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. (1994). Self and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463. Turner, J. C., & Onorato, R. S. (1999). Social identity, personality and the self-concept: A self-categorization perspective. In T. Postmes & N. R. Branscombe (Eds.), Rediscovering social identity (pp. 315–339). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Wakefield, J. R. H., Bickley, S., & Sani, F. (2013). The effects of iden- tification with a support group on the mental health of people with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 74, 420–426. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.02.002
  • 56. Cruwys et al. 73 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19289722 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19289722 Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 Weiner, B., Nierenberg, R., & Goldstein, M. (1976). Social learning (locus of control) versus attributional (causal stability) interpreta- tions of expectancy of success. Journal of Personality, 44, 52–68. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00583.x Welch, R. L., & Huston, A. C. (1982). Effects of induced success/fail- ure and attributions on the problem-solving behavior of psycholo- gically androgynous and feminine women. Journal of Personality, 50, 81–97. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1982.tb00747.x
  • 57. World Health Organization. (2012). Depression: A global public health concern. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/mental_ health/management/depression/who_paper_depression_wfmh_ 2012.pdf Wulsin, L. (2012). Comparison of two depression measures for pre- dicting stroke outcomes. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 72, 175–179. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.11.015 Author Biographies Tegan Cruwys, PhD, Australian National University, is a lecturer and clinical psychologist at the University of Queensland. Her research is at the intersection of social and clinical psychology, with a focus on social identity processes in mental health. Erica I. South graduated with first class honors in psychology in 2013 from the University of Queensland. Katharine H. Greenaway, PhD, University of Queensland, is a
  • 58. glo- bal scholar with the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research, based at the University of Queensland, Australia. Her research centres on issues of control, including perceived life control and the exertion of self-control. S. Alexander Haslam, PhD, Macquarie University, is a professor of psychology and Australian Research Council Laureate Fellow at the University of Queensland, Australia. His research interests are in the social psychology of health and well-being, stereotyping and preju- dice, and organizational behavior. 74 Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1) http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who _paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who _paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who _paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf <<
  • 59. /ASCII85EncodePages false /AllowTransparency false /AutoPositionEPSFiles true /AutoRotatePages /None /Binding /Left /CalGrayProfile (Gray Gamma 2.2) /CalRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /CalCMYKProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051 v2) /sRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /CannotEmbedFontPolicy /Warning /CompatibilityLevel 1.3 /CompressObjects /Off /CompressPages true /ConvertImagesToIndexed true /PassThroughJPEGImages false /CreateJDFFile false /CreateJobTicket false /DefaultRenderingIntent /Default /DetectBlends true /DetectCurves 0.1000 /ColorConversionStrategy /LeaveColorUnchanged /DoThumbnails false /EmbedAllFonts true /EmbedOpenType false /ParseICCProfilesInComments true /EmbedJobOptions true /DSCReportingLevel 0 /EmitDSCWarnings false /EndPage -1 /ImageMemory 1048576 /LockDistillerParams true /MaxSubsetPct 100 /Optimize true /OPM 1 /ParseDSCComments true /ParseDSCCommentsForDocInfo true
  • 60. /PreserveCopyPage true /PreserveDICMYKValues true /PreserveEPSInfo true /PreserveFlatness false /PreserveHalftoneInfo false /PreserveOPIComments false /PreserveOverprintSettings true /StartPage 1 /SubsetFonts true /TransferFunctionInfo /Apply /UCRandBGInfo /Remove /UsePrologue false /ColorSettingsFile () /AlwaysEmbed [ true ] /NeverEmbed [ true ] /AntiAliasColorImages false /CropColorImages false /ColorImageMinResolution 266 /ColorImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleColorImages true /ColorImageDownsampleType /Average /ColorImageResolution 175 /ColorImageDepth -1 /ColorImageMinDownsampleDepth 1 /ColorImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286 /EncodeColorImages true /ColorImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterColorImages true /ColorImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /ColorACSImageDict << /QFactor 0.40 /HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1] >> /ColorImageDict <<
  • 61. /QFactor 0.76 /HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2] >> /JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /JPEG2000ColorImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /AntiAliasGrayImages false /CropGrayImages false /GrayImageMinResolution 266 /GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleGrayImages true /GrayImageDownsampleType /Average /GrayImageResolution 175 /GrayImageDepth -1 /GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2 /GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286 /EncodeGrayImages true /GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterGrayImages true /GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /GrayACSImageDict << /QFactor 0.40 /HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1] >> /GrayImageDict << /QFactor 0.76 /HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2] >> /JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict <<
  • 62. /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /JPEG2000GrayImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /AntiAliasMonoImages false /CropMonoImages false /MonoImageMinResolution 900 /MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleMonoImages true /MonoImageDownsampleType /Average /MonoImageResolution 175 /MonoImageDepth -1 /MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286 /EncodeMonoImages true /MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode /MonoImageDict << /K -1 >> /AllowPSXObjects false /CheckCompliance [ /None ] /PDFX1aCheck false /PDFX3Check false /PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false /PDFXNoTrimBoxError true /PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000
  • 63. ] /PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox false /PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051 v2) /PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier (CGATS TR 001) /PDFXOutputCondition () /PDFXRegistryName (http://www.color.org) /PDFXTrapped /Unknown /Description << /ENU <FEFF00550073006500200074006800650073006500200053006 1006700650020007300740061006e006400610072006400200073 0065007400740069006e0067007300200066006f0072002000630 0720065006100740069006e006700200077006500620020005000 440046002000660069006c00650073002e0020005400680065007 30065002000730065007400740069006e0067007300200063006f 006e006600690067007500720065006400200066006f007200200 04100630072006f006200610074002000760037002e0030002e00 20004300720065006100740065006400200062007900200054007 2006f00790020004f007400730020006100740020005300610067 00650020005500530020006f006e002000310031002f003100300 02f0032003000300036002e000d000d0032003000300050005000 49002f003600300030005000500049002f004a005000450047002 0004d0065006400690075006d002f004300430049005400540020 00470072006f0075007000200034> >> /Namespace [ (Adobe) (Common)
  • 64. (1.0) ] /OtherNamespaces [ << /AsReaderSpreads false /CropImagesToFrames true /ErrorControl /WarnAndContinue /FlattenerIgnoreSpreadOverrides false /IncludeGuidesGrids false /IncludeNonPrinting false /IncludeSlug false /Namespace [ (Adobe) (InDesign) (4.0) ] /OmitPlacedBitmaps false /OmitPlacedEPS false /OmitPlacedPDF false /SimulateOverprint /Legacy >> << /AllowImageBreaks true /AllowTableBreaks true /ExpandPage false /HonorBaseURL true /HonorRolloverEffect false /IgnoreHTMLPageBreaks false /IncludeHeaderFooter false /MarginOffset [ 0 0 0 0 ] /MetadataAuthor ()
  • 65. /MetadataKeywords () /MetadataSubject () /MetadataTitle () /MetricPageSize [ 0 0 ] /MetricUnit /inch /MobileCompatible 0 /Namespace [ (Adobe) (GoLive) (8.0) ] /OpenZoomToHTMLFontSize false /PageOrientation /Portrait /RemoveBackground false /ShrinkContent true /TreatColorsAs /MainMonitorColors /UseEmbeddedProfiles false /UseHTMLTitleAsMetadata true >> << /AddBleedMarks false /AddColorBars false /AddCropMarks false /AddPageInfo false /AddRegMarks false /BleedOffset [ 9 9 9 9 ] /ConvertColors /ConvertToRGB /DestinationProfileName (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
  • 66. /DestinationProfileSelector /UseName /Downsample16BitImages true /FlattenerPreset << /ClipComplexRegions true /ConvertStrokesToOutlines false /ConvertTextToOutlines false /GradientResolution 300 /LineArtTextResolution 1200 /PresetName ([High Resolution]) /PresetSelector /HighResolution /RasterVectorBalance 1 >> /FormElements true /GenerateStructure false /IncludeBookmarks false /IncludeHyperlinks false /IncludeInteractive false /IncludeLayers false /IncludeProfiles true /MarksOffset 9 /MarksWeight 0.125000 /MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings /Namespace [ (Adobe) (CreativeSuite) (2.0) ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK /PageMarksFile /RomanDefault /PreserveEditing true /UntaggedCMYKHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UseDocumentBleed false >> ] /SyntheticBoldness 1.000000
  • 67. >> setdistillerparams << /HWResolution [288 288] /PageSize [612.000 792.000] >> setpagedevice