SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 121
r Academy of Management Journal
2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–1642.
https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.0795
MICROFOUNDATIONS OF CORPORATE SOCIAL
RESPONSIBILITY AND IRRESPONSIBILITY
CATHERINE T. SHEA
Carnegie Mellon University
OLGA V. HAWN
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
This study examines the importance of social perception of
corporate social responsibility
(CSR) and irresponsibility (CSI). Drawing from social
psychology literature on stereotypes,
we argue that two fundamental dimensions of social
perception—warmth and
competence—help explain the underlying processes and
conditions under which CSR
leads to specific outcomes. We propose that firms engaging in
CSR are perceived as higher
in warmth and, by default, competence; moreover, different
perceptions of the organiza-
tion’s warmth and competence can moderate CSR rewards and
CSI penalties. To dem-
onstrate this, we conduct three experiments. Experiment 1 links
CSR with perceptions of
warmth and competence, showing that warmth perceptions
mediate the relationship be-
tween CSR and important outcomes, such as purchase intentions
and reputation. Exper-
iment 2 adds information on firms’ countries of origin,
revealing that CSR rewards and CSI
penalties differ depending on the (mis)alignment of CSR
strategy with country stereotypes.
Experiment 3 replicates these findings using behavioral
paradigms. We find that firms
from high-warmth countries (the United States, Sweden,
Portugal) receive lower CSR
rewards and pay higher CSI penalties than firms from low-
warmth countries (Germany,
Pakistan) but this effect is moderated by competence. Our
micro–macro study advances
social evaluation, strategic CSR, and international management
literatures.
Corporate social responsibility (CSR) has grown
markedly in the past decade both as an important
phenomenon in practice and as a critical field in
academia (Wang, Tong, Takeuchi, & George, 2016).
We have theorized about its institutional (Campbell,
2007; Ioannou & Serafeim, 2012) and organizational
drivers (Aguilera, Rupp, Williams, & Ganapathi,
2007; McWilliams & Siegel, 2001) as well as exam-
ined its effect on firm performance (Berman, Wicks,
Kotha, & Jones, 1999; Cochran & Wood, 1984;
McGuire, Sundgren, & Schneeweis, 1988; Orlitzky,
Schmidt, & Rynes, 2003; Russo & Fouts, 1997)
and other outcomes (Berrone & Gomez-Mejia, 2009;
Flammer, 2013; Turban & Greening, 1997; Yoon,
Gürhan-Canli, & Schwarz, 2006). Yet CSR is primarily
studied at the macro level (i.e., institutional or orga-
nizational level) compared to the micro level (i.e.,
individual level) of analysis: a recent review of the
CSR literature shows that only 4% of all studies ex-
amine the individual level, while only 5% address
CSR at two or more levels of analysis (Aguinis &
Glavas, 2012). Therefore, what is lacking in CSR re-
search is deeper appreciation, at the individual level,
of how CSR makes an impact (Wang et al., 2016).
Accordingly, this paper addresses the need for micro
studies of CSR with a multilevel approach (Morgeson,
Aguinis, Waldman, & Siegel, 2013) in order to un-
derstand the underlying processes (i.e., mediating
effects) and conditions under which (i.e., moderating
effects) CSR leads to specific outcomes.
In particular, we still know very little about the
microfoundations of CSR (for the most recent review,
see Gond, El Akremi, Swaen, & Babu, 2017): how CSR
and, importantly, corporate social irresponsibility
The authors thank anonymous reviewers and confer-
ence participants at 2014 Academy of Management,
Academy of International Business, and Strategic Man-
agement Society annual meetings, as well as Sinziana
Dorobantu, Sanjay Patnaik, Elena Kulchina, Nel Dutt,
Elena Vidal, Amandine Ody-Brasier, Marlo Raveendran,
Zach Burns, and the brown-bag participants at Boston
University for comments on the early version of this paper.
An earlier version of this manuscript was a Finalist for
2014 AIB Haynes Prize for the Most Promising Scholar.
Please address all correspondence to Catherine Shea:
Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University,
5000 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, email:
[email protected]
1609
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved.
Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or
otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express
written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles
for individual use only.
https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.0795
mailto:[email protected]
(CSI) (Lange & Washburn, 2012; Surroca, Tribó, &
Zahra, 2013) are perceived by individuals, and what
effect this perception might have on the relationship
between CSR and specific outcomes at individual
and organizational levels of analysis. Forexample,we
know that CSR positively affects company reputation
(Turban & Greening, 1997)—one of our outcomes—
but why and what mediates this relationship at the
individual level is still unclear. A small but growing
literature on the micro approaches to CSR shows that
involvement in CSR positively influences employee
performance, behaviors, and attitudes (Burbano,
2016), such as employee engagement (Caligiuri,
Mencin, & Jiang, 2013), organizational citizenship
behavior (Rupp, Shao, Thornton, & Skarlicki, 2013),
identification with the firm (Farooq, Rupp, & Farooq,
2017), retention (Jones, 2010), in-role performance,
and commitment, as well as attractiveness to pro-
spective employees (Jones, Willness, & Madey, 2014;
Turban & Greening, 1997). Yet, few studies go beyond
employees (i.e., providing information on other
stakeholders) or seek to unpack the processes of CSR
evaluations by individuals more generally (Gond
et al., 2017). Our study applies to a larger number
of stakeholders—which may include employees,
customers, environmentalists, suppliers, the commu-
nity as a whole, and owners/shareholders—because it
examines general individual judgments and percep-
tions of warmth and competence.
Warmth and competence serve as universal di-
mensions of social judgment (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick,
2004; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy,
Glick, & Xu, 2002). “Warmth” is defined as percep-
tions related to intent, including friendliness, trust-
worthiness, helpfulness, sincerity, and morality,
whereas “competence” is conceptualized as percep-
tions related to ability, including skill, intelligence,
creativity, and efficacy (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008).
With the help of these two dimensions, one can por-
tray social perceptions of activities, individuals, or-
ganizations, and even countries (Cuddy, Fiske, &
Glick, 2007). Moreover, social judgment of various
degrees (i.e., high or low) of warmth and/or compe-
tence can predict a distinct emotion and behavior
toward a target; for example, being perceived as high
on warmth and/or competence is beneficial, while
being low on one of the dimensions is costly (Aaker,
Vohs, & Mogilner, 2010). Importantly, compared to
other potential mechanisms, warmth and compe-
tence help predict specific behavioral outcomes from
these social perceptions (e.g., helping those higher in
warmth; Cuddy et al., 2007), adding a more nuanced
mechanism between CSR, social perception, and
behavior toward the firm, which can ultimately affect
more macro-level outcomes (e.g., reputation, sales,
stakeholder and shareholder value).
While previous research mainly examines CSR, we
apply these two fundamental dimensions of social
perceptiontounderstand themicrofoundationsof CSR
and CSI. Naturally, our basic argument is that firms
with CSR generate a greater warmth and (by default,
without any other information) competence percep-
tion than control firms or firms engaging in CSI.
We extend this logic to argue that this perception of
warmth in turn mediates CSR and CSI effects on vari-
ous outcomes (i.e., CSR rewards and CSI penalties—
theeffectsforCSRandCSIaboveandbelowthecontrol
condition). Importantly, we then argue that different
perceptions of warmth and competence of the organi-
zation from other sources (in this paper, based on its
country of origin) moderate CSR rewards and CSI
penalties depending on the (mis)alignment of CSR
strategy with the (home country) stereotype. Using
three experiments with 774 participants and the
warmth and competence variation across five coun-
tries (i.e., the United States, Sweden, Germany, Portu-
gal, Pakistan), we show that warmth and competence
perceptions explain causal effects of CSR and CSI on
purchase intentions, price, reputation, and quality as-
sessments. Moreover, consistent with the stereotype
content model (SCM), firms from high-warmth coun-
tries pay a higher price for CSI than firms from low-
warmth countries (Germany, Pakistan), yet this effect
changes when combined with perceptions of high
competence (the United States, Sweden).
Our study has several important theoretical im-
plications for the management literature on CSR,
social cognition, and international business, as well
as practical applications. First, we develop a deeper
understanding of the microfoundations of CSR and
CSI—by examining their social perception by in-
dividuals. We extend existing individual-level CSR
research that relies on a different set of theoretical
frameworks, such as system justification theory
(Hafenbrädl & Waeger, 2016), organizational justice,
social influence, needs, and self-determination the-
ories (Aguinis & Glavas, 2012), by drawing from the
social psychology research on social perception and
stereotypes and arguing that CSR influences and is
influenced by two fundamental dimensions of social
perception—warmth and competence—which me-
diate and moderate the effects of CSR (and CSI) on
different outcomes. Our work is important because,
in comparison to objective CSR ratings, the “sub-
jective” evaluations of CSR likely matter more for
individual reactions to CSR (Rupp et al., 2013),
1610 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
which in turn lead to different outcomes (Gond et al.,
2017). For example, employees’ exposure to CSR
initiatives does not directly translate into favorable
CSR attitudes (Glavas & Godwin, 2013). Thus, it is
important to understand how the social perception
of CSR is formed more generally.
Second, we advance prior literature examining
different mediators and moderators of CSR at the
individual level, such as trust (or whether, how, and
when consumers’ perceptions of motives directly
influence consumer responses to CSR) (Vlachos,
Tsamakos, Vrechopoulos, & Avramidis, 2009), cus-
tomer satisfaction (Luo & Bhattacharya, 2006), and
organizational pride and identity (Jones, 2010; Jones
et al., 2014), by conducting a multidisciplinary
multi-experimental analysis of the role of warmth
and competence in the CSR context. If we under-
stand how the average individual perceives CSR and
CSI and how these social perceptions may coincide
(or diverge) with (from) their expectations (stereo-
types) of organizations, we can understand more
about how the value from CSR is generated more
broadly for a variety of stakeholders. In addition,
since most firms try to avoid CSI, we can understand
which firms will not suffer from CSI as much as
others and why.
Third, while most prior studies focus on only one
organizational practice, we distinguish between CSR
and CSI—crucial for moving the CSR literature for-
ward. Another important contribution to the CSR lit-
erature, prone to endogeneity and causality issues,
is empirical: by utilizing controlled experimental
methods, we are able to isolate discrete mechanisms
linking CSR/CSI to organizational outcomes. This
gives managers unequivocal insights into which “le-
vers to pull” when activating a CSR/CSI strategybased
on their current organizational context, and, impor-
tantly,thishelpsspecifyconditionsunderwhichCSR/
CSI does not reap the expected benefits/costs. Finally,
we extend the international management literature by
showing that initial social judgments about the origin
of the firm may improve or worsen outcomes based on
the choice of CSR strategy. This has important practi-
cal implications, particularly for managers of firms
that expand abroad for the first time.
THEORY AND HYPOTHESES
Social Perception
The ability to quickly judge another individual is a
fundamental evolutionary skill. It helps us to ascer-
tain whether “they” are a part of our social group and
to assess the goodness of their motives and compe-
tence to enact these good motives (see Fiske et al.,
2007, for a review). Despite common beliefs that we
need (and use) a wealth of knowledge to form judg-
ments about other individuals, research shows that
we make trait inferences spontaneously (Newman &
Uleman, 1993; Winter & Uleman, 1984; Winter,
Uleman, & Cunniff, 1985). For instance, when ob-
serving a fictional character, Donald, help an old
lady cross the street, we quickly (and somewhat
permanently) conclude that Donald is a kind and
helpful individual—despite our having only limited
information about him. Related work on “thin slic-
ing” shows that a mere 30 seconds watching a college
professor teach is sufficient to predict their end-of-
term teaching evaluations (see Ambady & Rosenthal,
1992, for a review). Thus, despite the wealth of social
information at our disposal, individuals primarily
rely on quick social judgments of others, and filter
all subsequent social information based—barring a
major behavioral change—on the first impression of
someone (Newman & Uleman, 1993; Ross, Lepper, &
Hubbard, 1975).
Many of these quick social judgments about other
individuals result from stereotypes. Stereotypes are
cognitive beliefs about the characteristics of another
group (Fiske, 1998). Fiske and colleagues (Cuddy
et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007; Fiske et al., 2002)
identified two fundamental dimensions of all stereo-
types used to evaluate people and social groups:
warmth and competence. The SCM posits that all
social groups fit into one of four quadrants based on
whether they are high or low on warmth and com-
petence. Further research (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske
et al., 2007; Fiske et al., 2002; Fiske, Xu, Cuddy, &
Glick, 1999) has demonstrated the robustness of the
warmth and competence dimensions, and the fact
that they can be used to classify not only individuals
but also social groups (e.g., elderly people, Jews,
housewives, immigrants, and the homeless) and even
national cultures.
Importantly, this work on social perception has a
behavioral analog, the “behaviors from intergroup
affect and stereotypes” (BIAS) map framework
(Cuddy et al., 2007). Based on the dimensions of
warmth and competence, the BIAS map predicts not
only cognitions derived from group stereotypes but
alsospecificbehaviorstowardthe group. Warmthand
competence evoke active and passive behaviors, re-
spectively: in particular, while high-warmth targets
are helped, low-warmth targets are harmed, and
whereas high-competence targets are passively facil-
itated, low-competence targets are neglected (Cuddy
2019 1611Shea and Hawn
et al., 2007). Furthermore, each combination of the
two trait dimensions predicts a distinct emotion or
behavior toward the target: people admire those who
are high in both competence and warmth, they feel
contempt toward those who are low competence and
low warmth, they envy those who are competent but
not warm, and they pity those who are incompetent
but warm (Fiske et al., 2002; Lee & Fiske, 2006).
Firms as Subject to Stereotypes
Although originally developed to explain personal
and social group perception, the SCM has been ex-
trapolated to non-human objects. Kervyn, Fiske,
and Malone (2012), in their “brands as intentional
agents” framework, showed that consumers perceive
brands in the same way they perceive people. Cuddy
et al. (2007) used the SCM to map social perceptions
of European Union countries. Aaker et al. (2010)
applied the SCM to organizations, explaining for-
profit and not-for-profit firms’ success and failure,
and showing that not-for-profit organizations are
associated with higher warmth and lower compe-
tence than for-profit firms.
It is not surprising that organizations are often
anthropomorphized into human beings—famously
so in the movie The Corporation and in recent de-
cisions by the U.S. Supreme Court, such as in the
Citizens United case. In management studies, orga-
nizations are often perceived to possess actions,
thoughts, opinions (Knobe & Prinz, 2008), goals,
tastes, styles, personalities (Pfeffer, 1981), and even
attention (Ocasio, 1997). In fact, they are viewed as
social actors similar to individuals precisely because
the features that distinguish humans as actors are
functionally equivalent to the features common to
organizational actors (King, Felin, & Whetten, 2010).
Yet, when compared to human beings, organizations
are thought to have equal capability for agentic
(i.e., competence) behavior (Gray & Wegner, 2010;
Haran, 2013; Knobe & Prinz, 2008) but are less likely
to be seen as experiencing emotions and feelings
(Gray & Wegner, 2010).
Nonetheless, a large literature in marketing dem-
onstrates that people perceive brands to have per-
sonalities (Aaker, 1997) and that people form
“relationships” with various brands and products
(Aaker & Fournier, 1995; Fournier, 1998). We build
off this previous literature by going beyond brands
and products and examining the perceptions of or-
ganizational practices. Specifically, in this paper, we
will compare perceptions of socially responsible
and irresponsible organizations, predicated on
social perception dimensions developed to classify
individuals and social groups. Before we discuss
how socially (ir)responsible activities may generate
social perceptions of warmth and/or competence,
we need to define CSR and CSI.
Main Effect: CSR, CSI, and Social Perception
CSR, sustainability, corporate citizenship, and
other terms are generally used to describe a portfolio
of socioeconomic activities, including environmen-
tal, social, and corporate governance actions of the
firm (Gardberg & Fombrun, 2006). Because these
voluntary actions are aimed at improving social or
ecological conditions (McWilliams & Siegel, 2001),
many observers regard CSR as an activity that bene-
fits firms, markets, and societies (Orlitzky, 2013).
We propose that CSR represents organizational
behavior that connotes warmth, and that, as such, it
should lead to perceptions of greater warmth. Why
should CSR be associated with warmth? Individuals
and social groups associated with high warmth are
associated with behaviors that are trustworthy and
moral (Fiske et al., 2007). Additionally, warmth has
both a moral dimension (e.g., good intentions) as well
as a relational component (e.g., can successfully work
with allies) (Goodwin, 2015). By definition, CSR in-
cludes a trustworthy behavior (i.e., “responsible” in
its name); furthermore, CSR involves working with
others (e.g., stakeholders; Harrison & Freeman, 1999).
Prior literature distinguishes CSR—social actions
motivatedby moral obligation—from corporate social
performance—social actions of firms—to highlight
that CSR is a moral behavior (Baron, 2009).
In turn, to distinguish between CSR and CSI, we will
follow Campbell’s (2007) threshold: if corporations
(a) knowingly do something that could harm their
stakeholders—their investors, employees, customers,
suppliers, or the local community within which they
operate—and (b) do not rectify the harm caused by
them (whenever it is discovered and brought to their
attention), the minimum behavioral standard with re-
spect to the corporation’s relationship to its stake-
holders is broken, and such corporate behavior
becomes socially irresponsible. For example, CSI be-
havior includes using child labor, sweatshops, and
polluting facilities in manufacturing operations. CSR
behavior, on the other hand, includes charity, volun-
teering, community engagement, fair labor practices,
andenvironmentallyfriendlymanufacturingfacilities.
In comparison to CSI and other organizational behav-
ior, CSR behavior resembles the same set of attributes
that social psychologists traditionally associate with
1612 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
being high in warmth. Therefore, as our baseline hy-
pothesis, we propose:
Hypothesis 1. Ceteris paribus, firms engaging in CSR
(CSI) will be perceived as having higher (lower) levels
of warmth.
Mediating Effect: Spillover and Primacy Effects of
Warmth Judgments
Warmth perceptions have two effects that are
worth discussing: halo effects (Nisbett & Wilson,
1977) and the primacy of warmth over competence.
First, let us discuss the “halo effect”—that is, when
the presence (or lack) of warmth spills over into our
judgments of competence (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske
et al., 2007; Singh & Teoh, 2000; Tausch, Kenworthy,
& Hewstone, 2007). This effect is particularly salient
when we have no information beyond that on
warmth. Thus, we automatically assume—in the
absence of further information—that an individual
possessing warmth also possesses some degree of
competence.
In our setting, this will mean that, in comparison to
CSI firms, in the absence of further information, be-
cause CSR firms are associated with higher warmth
they will also be associated with higher competence.
We argue that this will be the case because the lim-
ited information on CSR practices suggests to the
evaluator that the organization mastered at least
one skill and therefore is intelligent and competent.
Competence is conceptualized as perceptions re-
lated to ability, including skill, intelligence, crea-
tivity, and efficacy; therefore, in comparison to firms
engaging in CSI demonstrating their inability to be a
corporate citizen and behave in socially responsible
ways, the perception of competence will be greater
for firms engaging in CSR.
This prediction is consistent with recent work in
consumer research: products of companies engaged
in prosocial activities are perceived as performing
better—due to the moral undertone of the com-
pany’s motivation for engaging in socially re-
sponsible behavior; more importantly, this effect
holds even when consumers can directly observe
and experience the product and when the acts of
social goodwill are unrelated to the company’s core
business (Chernev & Blair, 2015). Another alterna-
tive explanation for why CSR engagement may be
perceived to be high in competence is that, in com-
parison with CSI, it can improve firm reputation,
performance, and other outcomes (Choi & Wang,
2009; Yoon et al., 2006) traditionally associated with
competence.
A counterargument will require a discussion of the
purpose of the firm (Friedman, 1970), market actors as
social evaluators, and more information on other or-
ganizational practices, activities, or performance. For
example,ifevaluatorsweremarketactors(particularly
in earlier years), they might have assumed that firms
engaging in CSR were less competent because CSR
investment requires diverting scarce resources from
other more strategic or core business activities of the
firm. However, in comparison to CSI, recent strategy
work shows that CSR engagement is now making or-
ganizations more competent because they perform
better even in financial markets (Cheng, Ioannou, &
Serafeim, 2013; Eccles, Ioannou, & Serafeim, 2014;
Ioannou & Serafeim, 2015). Therefore, due tothe “halo
effect” of warmth, as well as other alternative expla-
nations listed above, we propose:
Hypothesis 2. Ceteris paribus, firms engaging in CSR
(CSI) will be perceived as having higher (lower) levels
of competence.
Second, let us discuss the primacy effect of
warmth judgment. Although the SCM is predicted on
two dimensions—warmth and competence—when
it comes to forming a social perception, a large body
of research highlights the primacy of warmth
(Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1997; Kenworthy
& Tausch, 2008; Wojciszke, Bazinska, & Jaworski,
1998). That is, we anchor on perceptions of warmth
and adjust, albeit insufficiently, based on percep-
tions of competence. At the individual level, this
happens because information needed to determine
warmth (e.g., facial expressions, interpersonal skills)
is simply more readily available during the begin-
ning of social interactions than information needed
to determine competence (e.g., skills, knowledge,
and abilities). Hence, when it comes to initial social
judgments, warmth information carries more weight
than competence information (Cuddy et al., 2008:
89–92; Singh & Teoh, 2000; Tausch et al., 2007).
At the level of organizations, we argue that this
primacy effect will be important in the relationship
between CSR and organizational outcomes, partic-
ularly those that involve a single immediate evalua-
tion or a first encounter with the firm, because, just
like at the individual level, information needed to
determine warmth is simply more readily available
during the beginning of social interactions with this
social actor (King et al., 2010) than information
needed to determine competence. CSR studies have
long examined the effect of CSR on firm performance
(Cochran & Wood, 1984; Waddock & Graves, 1997)
among other outcomes (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001;
2019 1613Shea and Hawn
Turban&Greening,1997),generallyfindingasignificant
positive effect (Margolis, Elfenbein, & Walsh, 2009).
We distinguish between outcomes that involve a
single immediate evaluation (i.e., a purchase in-
tention or reputation assessment based on the first
encounter with the firm) and those involving an
ongoing evaluation (e.g., by market analysts and
investors who collect information over time, accu-
mulate more information contributing to a compe-
tence perception of the firm, and value competence
over warmth in their judgment).
We argue that, for first-time encounters, the
warmth perception that CSR generates could in fact
act as the mechanism by which CSR affects these
outcomes. In particular, we suggest that, in the ab-
sence of further information, the primacy of warmth
over competence will play a mediating role in the
relationship between CSR and the outcome of the
first encounter. We previously argued that, when
individuals evaluate CSR, they perceive greater
levels of warmth and, by default, competence than
when they evaluate CSI (Hypotheses 1 and 2). Now,
we argue that, when observers make single imme-
diate evaluations of CSR/CSI behavior, they will pay
more attention to the warmth than to the competence
perception. There are three reasons for this. First,
cognitively, people prove more sensitive to warmth
information than to competence information. Sec-
ond, they judge warmth faster than they do compe-
tence (Cuddy et al., 2008: 90). Third, higher warmth
leads to higher levels of helping behavior (Cuddy
et al., 2007); in an organizational context, help can be
conceptualized as positive evaluations and willing-
ness to purchase from the firm. Therefore:
Hypothesis 3. Warmth perceptions will mediate the
relationship between CSR/CSI and outcomes that
involve single immediate evaluation.
Moderating Effect: Stereotype Fulfillment
and Violation
While Hypotheses 1–3 posit CSR as a source of
warmth and, by default, competence perception,
and warmth as a potential mediator between CSR
and outcomes that involve single immediate eval-
uation, we next discuss how other sources of
warmth perception in an organizational context can
interact with CSR and potentially moderate the re-
lationship between CSR/CSI and organizational
outcomes. In doing so, we assume, based on prior
literature, that engaging in CSR leads to rewards
(Margolis et al., 2009) and engaging in CSI leads to
penalties for the focal organization (Salaiz, 2016).
Thus, we theorize about organizational contexts in
which the rewards/penalties to a CSR/CSI strategy
are amplified/mitigated, answering the following
question: “Is it always useful to engage in CSR and/
or avoid CSI?”
We propose that the answer to this question is
grounded in the literature on stereotype fulfillment
and violation. When we evaluate people from other
social groups or cultures, stereotypes act as cognitive
shortcuts. For instance, we meet a rule-abiding, for-
mal German and our stereotype of German people
is confirmed (i.e., high competence, low warmth;
Cuddy et al., 2007). But, what happens when we
meet a bubbly, scatterbrained German? Social judg-
ments broadly fall into two categories: assimilation
and contrast (Biernat, 2005; Newman & Uleman,
1993; Sherif & Hovland, 1961). “Assimilation” oc-
curs when we judge the target according to the held
stereotype (e.g., rule-abiding, formal German), and
“contrast” occurs when we differentiate the target
from our traditionally held stereotype (e.g., bubbly,
scatterbrained German). In the absence of contra-
dictory evidence, assimilation is relatively auto-
matic (Dijksterhuis, Spears, & Lépinasse, 2001);
otherwise, contrast takes place.
The effect of contrasting social judgments can
be both positive and negative. For instance,
professionals—perceived as highly competent—
receive differential treatment based on their gender
after having a baby (Cuddy et al., 2004). Professional
women are seen as less competent and do not expe-
rience a boost in perceived warmth from mother-
hood to make up for their newly perceived lack of
competence. Men, on the other hand, receive in-
creased perceptions of warmth and maintain their
competence, gaining a net benefit from parenthood
(Cuddy et al., 2004). Likewise, agentic women—
violating the female stereotype—experience a back-
lash when they apply for feminized jobs, whereas
prototypical women do not (Rudman & Glick, 1999,
2001). These studies suggest that additional infor-
mation on other sources of warmth and/or compe-
tence perception for the subject of interest (e.g.,
gender) can help explain the benefits and detriments
of certain behavior. The mechanism is that additional
information may strengthen the original stereotype
through assimilation, or change it through contrast.
In order to examine under what conditions CSR
and CSI behaviors generate greater (lower) out-
comes, we add another piece of information about
the firm that exogenously changes perceptions of its
warmth and competence. As discussed in Studies 2
and 3, we achieve this by adding cues on the firm’s
1614 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
origin; Cuddy and colleagues (2007) portrayed ste-
reotypes of countries on the two dimensions of
warmth and competence, finding significant varia-
tion. In this section, to further our understanding of
CSR as a warmth strategy, we mainly focus on the
warmth dimension of the firm (or its origin); this
helps to disentangle the mediating or supplemental
warmth effects, if any, of CSR. This also helps ad-
dress a potential reverse-causality issue: if firms
perceived to be high in warmth are the ones who
engage in CSR in the first place, this section of our
theory (and analysis) helps to identify the leftover
effect, if any, of CSR as a warmth strategy.
We are interested in how social perception of the
firm based on other sources of information (i.e., its
origin) moderates CSR and CSI outcomes. In par-
ticular, if firms that engage in CSR are perceived to
be higher in warmth and this warmth perception
mediates the effect of CSR on our outcomes (Hy-
potheses 1 and 3), does an exogenous increase
(decrease) in warmth of the firm help improve (at-
tenuate) these outcomes for firms engaging in CSI
(CSR)? We suggest that, because of assimilation
with the existing stereotype (i.e., a match between
low-warmth country of origin and low-warmth
practice of CSI), firms perceived to be low in
warmth will be forgiven for CSI behavior, and,
therefore, will not be punished as harshly. High-
warmth firms, on the other hand, assimilate with
CSR, not CSI, so, when they engage in socially ir-
responsible practices, contrast occurs and the pen-
alty is high (i.e., a mismatch between high-warmth
country of origin and low-warmth practice of CSI
results in greater punishment).
A similar logic applies to CSR: if a high-warmth
firm engages in CSR, this demonstrates stereotypical
behavior for this kind of a firm (i.e., assimilation
occurs); hence, the benefits are mediocre. If a low-
warmth firm engages in CSR, on the other hand, that
contradicts the stereotype and, as a result, generates
greater rewards from the surprised but positive re-
action of the firm’s observers. Importantly, stereo-
type match and violation based on the fit between the
social perception of the firm and its behavior (CSR/
CSI) help advance our understanding of the cogni-
tive mechanisms behind the efficacy of CSR in-
vestments, suggesting that not all firms engaging in
CSR/CSI reap the same benefits/costs.
Hypothesis 4. The higher the perception of warmth of
the firm, the higher the penalties for CSI.
Hypothesis 5. The higher the perception of warmth of
the firm, the lower the rewards for CSR.
Our key hypothesis is that CSR is predominantly
perceived as influencing warmth. However, individ-
uals do not only evaluate organizations on warmth
but also competence, and thus we need to consider
competence in our predictions. Prior research found
that, although not-for-profit organizations were per-
ceived to be high in warmth, consumers were less
willing to buy from them unless they perceived them
to be highly competent (Aaker et al., 2010). Therefore,
we speculate that the presence of high (low) levels
of perceived competence in addition to high (low)
warmth may affect the above relationships.
As we consider CSR and CSI, it is important to ex-
amine how the valence of these actions interacts with
both warmth and competence. While warmth has a
moral dimension (e.g., good intentions) and a re-
lational component (e.g., can successfully work with
allies) (Goodwin, 2015), competence relates directly to
ability to execute a plan, positive or negative. In the
case of CSI, organizations are behaving negatively, and
we know that negative news generates stronger ob-
serverreactionsthanpositiveeventsintheCSRcontext
(Lange & Washburn, 2012). As per Hypothesis 1, these
negative events should bear negatively on warmth,
particularly the moral aspects of warmth (Goodwin,
Piazza, & Rozin, 2014), thus canceling out the positive
effects of warmth on organizational outcomes. More-
over, negative behaviors are diagnostic of competence
(Fiske et al., 2002), or lack thereof, and, per Hypothesis
2, CSI should decrease competence evaluations. Thus,
a firm caught engaging in CSI will be perceived to be
low on both competence and warmth.
However, when additional information on warmth
and competence stemming from other sources than
organizational behavior (e.g., origin) is revealed, it
may change the observers’ reaction to CSI, and, in
fact, if the levels of both warmth and competence are
high from this additional source, it could potentially
buffer the firm against negative effects of CSI (Koh,
Qian, & Wang, 2014; Shiu & Yang, 2017). This is once
again due to the stereotype fulfillment and violation
mechanism: observers expect high-competence–
high-warmth firms to solve CSI issues much faster
than low-competence–high-/low-warmth firms, and
therefore do not punish them as harshly for CSI.
Therefore, high competence and warmth of the home
country of the firm that engages in CSI will buffer it
from the negative effects of its poor behavior when
we assess both dimensions of social perception
(compared to firms high only on warmth).
In turn, in the case of CSR, if a firm from a low-
competence–low-warmth country is found to engage
in CSR, observers may perceive it as an exemplar in
2019 1615Shea and Hawn
that country (due to the stereotype violation on both
warmthandcompetencedimensions).Asaresult,this
perception will increase their helping behaviors and
positive evaluations toward such firms because they
aredoingthe“mostgood”(namely,helpingthosewho
need it the most). Moreover, becauseCSR engagement
is costly—as it may divert scarce resources from other
more strategic or core business activities of the firm
(Friedman, 1970)—engaging in CSR when low in both
warmth and competence provides a stronger positive
signal compared to firms low only on warmth. To
summarize, we predict that the simultaneous pres-
ence of high warmth and high competence will lower
the negative impact of CSI (attenuating Hypothesis 4),
while the simultaneous presence of low warmth and
low competence will amplify the positive effect of
CSR (strengthening Hypothesis 5).
Hypothesis 6. The perception of competence of the
firm moderates the relationships in Hypotheses 4 and
5, such that (a) the higher the perception of warmth
and competence of the firm (as opposed to lower), the
lower the penalties for CSI, and (b) the lower the
perception of warmth and competence (as opposed to
higher), the higher the rewards to CSR.
METHODS
Given the plethora of empirical challenges in CSR
research, it was important to conduct experiments
to examine and establish the causal links between
CSR/CSI, social perceptions, and different out-
comes. Experiments allowed us to make causal
statements based on control of the environment
(Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002), making them
ideal for testing our theory. They helped isolate our
theoretical mechanisms and, therefore, advance
theory (Haslam & McGarty, 2004; Mook, 1983).
Moreover, as we test our hypotheses using multiple
paradigms and outcomes, multiple experiments
allow for conceptual replication and extension,
highlighting the robustness of the effects. We con-
ducted three experiments.
In Study 1, we manipulated CSR and CSI to ex-
amine perceptions of warmth and competence as
well as such outcomes as reputation and purchase
intentions. In addition, thanks to the temporal order
in our experimental design, we were able to infer
warmth mediation. In Study 2, owing to the differ-
ences in the domain of the country of origin—
organizational context—we were able to manipulate
warmth and competence of the organization, and
examine them as moderators in the relationship be-
tween CSR, CSI, and firm outcomes. Specifically, we
examined four countries representing each quadrant
ofthewarmth–competenceBIASmap(i.e.,theUnited
States, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan) and three
conditions (i.e., CSR, CSI, and control). Finally, in
Study 3, we replicated and extended these effects in a
behavioral experiment measuring helping and pur-
chasing behaviors using a within-subjects design.
STUDY 1
Study 1 examines the relationship between CSR
activities and perceptions of warmth and compe-
tence (Hypotheses 1 and 2). It also examines whether
warmth and competence serve as mediating mech-
anisms between CSR and important organizational
outcomes (Hypothesis 3), such as purchase inten-
tions (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001) and reputation
(Wagner, Lutz, & Weitz, 2009).
Sample and Procedures
Participants and design. One hundred and two
participants (66 males, meanage 5 31, SD 5 10.11)
were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk
(Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Horton, Rand,
& Zeckhauser, 2011; see also O’Reilly, Robinson,
Berdahl, & Banki, 2015, for recent management re-
search using this data source; Paolacci & Chandler,
2014) in exchange for $0.30 in Amazon credits. All
participants were based in the United States and
were employed full-time. All participants passed
attention filters embedded in the survey (i.e., “I will
choose ‘disagree’ to demonstrate that I am paying
attention”), and all participants had a unique IP ad-
dress located within the United States. We used a
one-factor design that assigned participants to either
a CSR, CSI, or control condition randomly.
Procedure. Participants were told that they would
read a brief business scenario and provide their
opinions. Participants completed the study online.
CSR manipulation. Participants read about
“Company X,” a fictitious company that engaged in
either CSR or CSI activities, involving fair/unfair
manufacturing processes overseas—the vignette had
been established in prior literature (Sen & Bhattacharya,
2001). We also included a control condition with
no information on CSR (see Appendix A for full
vignettes).
Measures
Warmth and competence. Immediately following
the CSR manipulation, participants rated whether
1616 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
they believed Company X to possess various attri-
butes. Specifically, they rated whether Company X
was tolerant, warm, good natured, and sincere (Fiske
et al., 2002), and competent, confident, independent,
competitive, and intelligent (Fiske et al., 2002). These
items were randomly embedded in other, unrelated
attribute items. Confirmatory factor analysis sup-
ported a two-factor solution that explained 72.2% of
the variance. The four warmth items loaded onto
Factor1withloadingsrangingfrom.80to.85.Thefive
competence items loaded onto Factor 2 with loadings
ranging from .77 to .84. The internal consistency co-
efficient of the warmth and competence items, mea-
sured using Cronbach’s alpha, were .93 and .86
respectively. In the control condition, warmth and
competence were not correlated (r 5 .19, p 5 .26).
Additional dependent variables. After rating
Company X on the dimensions of warmth and com-
petence, participants answered questions about two
additional dependent variables. First, participants
indicated their purchase intentions. Specifically,
they were asked: “If the products from Company X
were available for purchase, what is your likelihood
of purchasing a product from Company X?” (1 5 not
at all likely, 7 5 very likely) (see Chandon, Morwitz,
& Reinartz, 2005, for a discussion of external val-
idity). Additionally, participants rated their percep-
tions of the firm’s reputation: favorable, good,
pleasant, positive (reputation a 5 .97; Homer, 1995;
Wagner et al., 2009). Then, participants completed
another manipulation check, regarding the content
of the CSR scenario, and provided demographic data.
Results
Hypothesis 1: Warmth. A one-way analysis of
variance (ANOVA) showed that participants who
were assigned to CSR, CSI, or control manipulations
were more likely to judge the firm as possessing
significantly different levels of warmth, F(2, 99) 5
133.43, p , .000, hp
2 5 .73 (see Table 1). Results of
planned comparisons indicated that participants
assigned to the CSR condition (M 5 4.33, SD 5 0.58)
were more likely to view the firm as warm than were
participants assigned to the CSI condition (M 5 2.02,
SD 5 0.61), F(1, 67) 5 265.25, p , .000, hp
2 5 .80, d 5
3.95, 95% CI [3.74, 4.17], or to our control condition
(M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.59), F(1, 67) 5 66.71, p , .000, hp
2 5
.50,d5 1.96,95%CI[1.76,2.18].Moreover,participants
in the control condition perceived the firm as having
significantly higher levels of warmth than did those in
the CSI condition, F(1, 64) 5 64.32, p , .000, hp
2 5 .50,
d5 2.00,95%CI[1.79,2.22].Themeansaredisplayedin
Table 1 and provide support for Hypothesis 1.
Hypothesis 2: Competence. A one-way ANOVA
showed that participants who were assigned to CSR,
CSI, or control manipulations were more likely to
judge the firm as possessing significantly different
levels of competence, F(2, 99) 5 15.65, p , .000, hp
2 5
.24 (see Table 1). Results of planned comparisons in-
dicated that participants assigned to the CSR condi-
tion (M 5 4.14, SD 5 0.48) were more likely to view
the firm ascompetent thanwereparticipants assigned
to the CSI condition (M 5 3.30, SD 5 0.88), F(1, 67) 5
24.68, p , .000, hp
2 5 .27, d 5 1.21, 95% CI [1.03,
1.52],and thecontrol condition (M 5 3.93, SD5 0.50),
F(1, 67) 5 3.18, p 5 .08, hp
2 5 .05, d 5 0.44, 95% CI
[0.26, 0.61], while participants in the control condi-
tion perceived the firm as having significantly higher
levels of competence than did participants in the CSI
condition, F(1, 64) 5 12.70, p 5 .001, hp
2 5 .17, d 5
1.28, 95% CI [1.10, 1.46]. The means are displayed in
Table 1 and provide support for Hypothesis 2.
Reputation. To check the validity of our assump-
tion that CSR generates rewards and CSI generates
penalties (Margolis et al., 2009; Salaiz, 2016), we ran
the same analysis on Reputation. Participants who
were assigned to CSR, CSI, or control manipulations
TABLE 1
Summary of Results for Experiment 1
Condition
Study 1 (one-way ANOVA)
Warmth (1–5 scale) Competence (1–5 scale) Purchase Intentions
(1–7 scale) Reputation (1–5 scale)
CSR 4.33 (0.58)*** 4.14 (0.48)*** 5.61 (1.15)*** 4.57
(0.63)***
CSI 2.02 (0.61)*** 3.3 (0.88)*** 2.7 (1.38)*** 1.84 (0.73)***
Control 3.2 (0.59)*** 3.93 (0.5)* 4.82 (1.03)** 3.48 (0.72)***
Notes: Table 1 shows means with standard deviations in
brackets. CSR 5 corporate social responsibility; CSI 5 corporate
social irrespon-
sibility (see Appendix A for scenarios).
*p , .10
**p , .05
***p , .01
2019 1617Shea and Hawn
perceived significantly different levels of firm reputa-
tion, F(2, 99) 5 134.71, p , .000, hp
2 5 .73 (see Table 1).
Results of planned comparisons indicated that partici-
pants assigned to the CSR condition (M 5 4.57, SD 5
0.63) perceived the firm as having a better reputation
than did participants assigned to the CSI condition
(M5 1.84,SD5 0.73),F(1,67)5 279.94,p, .000,hp
2 5
.81, d 5 4.07, 95% CI [3.85, 4.33], or to our control
condition (M 5 3.48, SD 5 0.72), F(1, 67) 5 45.12, p ,
.000, hp
2 5 .40, d 5 1.64, 95% CI [1.41, 1.90]. Partici-
pants in the control condition perceived the firm as
having a significantly better reputation than did partic-
ipants in the CSI condition, F(1, 64) 5 83.73, p , .000,
hp
25 .57,d5 2.30,95%CI[2.04,2.56].Resultsfromthis
test support our assumption that rewards are above and
penalties are below the control condition—see Table 1.
Purchase intentions. Similarly,aone-wayANOVA
showed that participants who were assigned to CSR,
CSI, or control manipulations were more likely
to report different levels of purchase intentions,
F(2, 99) 5 52.15, p , .000, hp
2 5 .513 (see Table 1).
Results of planned comparisons indicated that partic-
ipants assigned to the CSR condition (M 5 5.61, SD 5
1.15) were more likely to have higher purchase in-
tentions than participants assigned to the CSI condi-
tion (M 5 2.70, SD 5 1.38), F(1, 67) 5 91.11, p , .000,
hp
2 5 .58, d5 2.33,95%CI[1.92, 2.82]ortoourcontrol
condition (M 5 4.82, SD 5 1.03), F(1, 67) 5 8.49, p 5
.005, hp
2 5 .11, d 5 0.74, 95% CI [0.32, 1.10]. More-
over, participantsin thecontrol condition had higher
purchase intentions than participants in the CSI
condition, F(1, 64) 5 47.57, p , .000, hp
2 5 .43, d 5
1.77, 95% CI [1.40, 2.26]. These results support our
assumption that CSR generates rewards, and CSI
penalties (i.e., rewards are above and penalties are
below the control condition—see Table 1).
Hypothesis 3: Warmth and competence as
mediators. Using Hayes’s (2013) process macros and
the bootstrap method, we ran mediational analyses,
predicting two dependent variables: firm reputation
and purchase intentions. We used the bootstrap model
of Preacher and Hayes (2004) to estimate the indi-
rect effect of warmth and competence based on 10,000
bootstrap samples. This method is preferred to the tra-
ditional method proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986),
as it does not rely on the assumption that the sampling
distribution of the mediation effect is normal. We un-
dertook multiple sets of analyses. First, we looked at
mediation of CSR versus Control, and CSI versus Con-
trol for warmth while controlling for competence1 to
predict reputation and purchase intentions.2 Next,
we looked at a sequential mediation model between
warmth, competence, reputation, and purchase in-
tentions. This allowed us to examine the distinct
effects of warmth and competence in both socially
responsible and irresponsible firms.
Results for reputation. We first constructed two
mediation models in which CSR versus control, CSI
versus control predicted reputation, with warmth as
a mediator (controlling for competence).3 We then
examined the indirect pathway comparing CSR and
control (i.e., CSR influences reputation through in-
creased warmth). The 95% CI for the bootstrap esti-
mation of the indirect pathway did not overlap with
0 when examining warmth in the CSR condition, b 5
0.91, 95% CI [0.59, 1.19], indicating mediation.4
Next, we examined the indirect pathway comparing
CSI and control (i.e., CSI influences reputation
through decreased warmth controlling for compe-
tence). The 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the
indirect pathway did not overlap with 0 when ex-
amining warmth in the CSI condition, b 5 20.95,
95% CI [21.28, 20.67].5 This indicates that warmth
mediates the relationship between CSR/CSI and firm
reputation, further substantiating Hypothesis 3.
Results for purchase intentions. We constructed
two mediation models in which CSR versus control,
CSI versus control predicted purchase intentions,
with warmth as mediator controlling for compe-
tence.6 First, we examined the indirect pathway
comparing CSR and control (i.e., CSR influences
1 We footnote results for mediations with both warmth
and competence as simultaneous mediators.
2 We code such that the control condition is the refer-
ence group in each separate analysis.
3 When examined as joint predictors with the three ex-
perimental conditions, warmth, b 5 0.75, t 5 2.63, p 5
.009, predicted reputation, while competence, b 5 0.04, t 5
0.44, p 5 .44, did not.
4 According to the same analysis where Competence
serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, a significant in-
direct effect emerges for warmth, b 5 0.83, 95% CI [0.51,
1.10], but the 95% CI for competence contains 0, b 5 0.05,
95% CI [20.02, 0.16], suggesting warmth, not competence,
mediates this relationship.
5 According to the same analysis where Competence
serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, a significant in-
direct effect emerges for warmth, b 5 20.93, 95% CI
[21.28, 20.66], but the 95% CI for competence contains 0,
b 5 20.02, 95% CI [20.15, 0.12], suggesting warmth, not
competence, mediates this relationship.
6 When examined as joint predictors with the three ex-
perimental conditions, both warmth, b 5 0.50, t 5 2.48, p 5
.01, and competence, b 5 0.73, t 5 3.99, p 5 .001, predicted
purchase intentions.
1618 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
Purchase Intentions through increased warmth
controlling for competence). The estimation of the
indirect effect did not overlap with 0, b 5 0.83, 95%
CI [0.15, 1.46], indicating mediation of purchase in-
tentions by warmth.7 Next, we completed the same
analyses for CSI versus control (i.e., CSI influences
Purchase Intentions through decreased warmth
controlling for competence). The estimation of the
indirect effect did not overlap with 0, b 5 20.98,
95% CI [21.84, 20.33], indicating mediation of
purchase intentions by warmth.8
Sequential mediation. We constructed sequential
mediation models including reputation (a cognitive
evaluation) to predict purchase intentions (a behav-
ioral intention). We examined purchase intentions
as the final dependent variable as it is the most dis-
tant variable and represents a behavioral intention.
We also tested primacy of warmth assumptions by
adding in competence as a sequential mediator fol-
lowing warmth. We examined the indirect pathway
(i.e., CSR/CSI influences purchase intentions through
warmth, competence, and reputation; PROCESS,
Model 6). Using three mediators, seven potential in-
direct effects were calculated. The 95% CI for the
bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway between
CSR → Warmth → Competence → Purchase Inten-
tions excluded 0, b 5 0.34, 95% CI [0.09, 0.64], in-
dicating sequential mediation. Likewise, the 95% CI
for the bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway
between CSR → Warmth → Reputation → Purchase
Intentions excluded 0, b 5 0.64, 95% CI [0.19, 1.11],
indicating sequential mediation as well. No other in-
direct effects excluded 0.
Next, we examined the indirect pathway with CSI.
The 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the in-
direct pathway between CSI → Warmth → Compe-
tence → Purchase Intentions excluded 0, b 5 20.49,
95%CI[20.82,20.19], indicatingsequential mediation.
Likewise, the 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of
the indirect pathway between CSI → Warmth →
Reputation → Purchase Intentions excluded 0,
b 5 20.81, 95% CI [21.42, 20.43], indicating se-
quential mediation as well. No other indirect effects
excluded 0. We ran equivalent models changing the
order of the potential mediators. Importantly, ana-
lyses in which competence preceded warmth in the
model did not produce indirect effects excluding 0,
suggesting the primacy of warmth as a mediating
mechanism in our data set. Overall, our results indi-
cate that warmth consistently mediates the relation-
ship between CSR/CSI and purchase intentions (on
its own and when predicting Purchase intentions in
conjunction with Reputation), whereas competence
does not consistently mediate it, supporting Hy-
pothesis 3.
Discussion
Study 1 provides support for Hypotheses 1 through
3. When “Company X” engaged in CSR activities, it
was judged to be significantly higher in warmth than
both control and CSI firms (supporting Hypothesis 1).
In addition, per traditional halo effects in psycholog-
ical research (Ross & Nisbett, 1991) as well as other
alternative explanations, we observed a positive re-
lationship between CSR and competence ratings
(supporting Hypothesis 2). However, although ratings
of competence differed between CSR and CSI condi-
tions, the control condition was not significantly dif-
ferent from the CSR condition, p . .08, indicating that
competence may not be directly related to the pres-
ence of CSR activities but is rather related to their
absence (i.e., CSI). This finding is consistent with
Fiske et al. (2007), which found that negative behav-
iors are more indicative of competence while positive
behaviors are more indicative of warmth. Next, we
established further evidence of CSR as a warmth
strategy in our mediational analyses. Specifically,
warmth mediates the relationship between CSR/CSI
and the control condition to predict reputation and
purchase intentions (supporting Hypothesis 3). When
examined as a sequential mediator, warmth is nec-
essary to mediate the relationship between CSR/CSI
and purchase intention while including measures of
competence and reputation. This evidence confirms
previous research on the primacy of warmth when
forming social judgments (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske
et al., 2007; Singh & Teoh, 2000; Tausch et al., 2007).
Although this study represents, to our knowledge,
one of the first empirical links between corporate
strategy and social perception, many questions re-
main. These results are about “Company X”—a fic-
titious organization used to maintain experimental
7 According to the same analysis where Competence
serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, the 95% CI for the
bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway did overlap
with 0 when examining warmth and competence together
in the CSR condition, bwarmth 5 0.43, 95% CI [20.18,
1.008]; bcompetence 5 0.20, 95% CI [20.02, 0.53].
8 According to the same analysis where Competence
serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, the 95% CI for the
bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway did overlap
with 0 when examining warmth and competence together
in the CSI condition, bwarmth 5 20.47, 95% CI [21.29,
0.07], but not for competence, bcompetence 5 20.50, 95% CI
[20.86, 20.19].
2019 1619Shea and Hawn
control. Typically, we know more about an organi-
zation when making a purchase and evaluating firm
reputation (e.g., country of origin, size, status/brand
of the firm). Much of this auxiliary information can
be related to the dimensions of warmth and compe-
tence. Our results so far suggest that, just like ste-
reotypes of organizations (Aaker et al., 2010),
stereotypes of organizational practices indeed exist,
and warmth and competence are in fact organizing
dimensions that help individuals categorize com-
panies and their strategies (i.e., CSR or CSI). More-
over, warmth perceptions mediate the relationship
between CSR/CSI and outcomes. However, if this is
the case, do warmth and competence perceptions of
organizations from other sources of evaluation color
the way in which CSR and CSI are evaluated? Could
they shift individual willingness to buy or evaluation
of firm reputation or other behavioral outcomes of
CSR and CSI? Experiments 2 and 3 were designed to
address these questions.
STUDY 2
Study 1 examined the relationship between a firm’s
CSR activities and perceptions of warmth and com-
petence, establishing CSR as a warmth strategy via
mediation. Study 2 shifts our focus to the warmth–
competence moderating effect: in particular, do
warmth and competence perceptions of the organi-
zation from other sources (i.e., country of origin)
moderate the effects of CSR (CSI) on different out-
comes? In other words, under what conditions do
firms achieve greater rewards (penalties) for CSR
(CSI) in terms of favorable (unfavorable) evalua-
tions? To test this, we manipulated the perception of
the organization by changing a firm’s country of or-
igin to reflect different levels of warmth and com-
petence (i.e., high or low). Just as the national origin
of immigrants guides majority members’ perceptions
of them (Lee & Fiske, 2006), we expect the country of
origin to affect the social perception of the firm and of
its strategic choices.
Sample and Procedures
Participants and design. Five hundred and
seventy-two participants (357 males, meanage 5
32.13, SD 5 11.12) were recruited from Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk (Buhrmester et al., 2011) in ex-
change for $0.50 in Amazon credits. To avoid the
out-of-group bias (e.g., rating a U.S. firm differently if
you were based outside the United States), all par-
ticipants were based in the United States and passed
attention filters embedded in the survey. We used a
three-factor design that randomly assigned partici-
pants to a CSR state (i.e., CSR, CSI, or control),
country-of-origin warmth (i.e., high, low), and com-
petence (i.e., high, low). We also included a pure
control condition that did not manipulate country of
origin; its inclusion did not change the significance
or pattern of our results. However, because this ex-
periment tests Hypotheses 4 through 6 using warmth
and competence perceptions of the countries of ori-
gin and the control condition does not specify any
country (and, hence, warmth or competence), we did
not include it in the final analyses.
Procedure. Participants completed the study
online. They were asked to read a brief business
scenario and to provide their opinions by rating the
warmth and competence of the firm’s country of or-
igin as well as their perceptions of the firm based on
the CSR vignette.
CSR manipulation. Participants read the same
vignettes as in Study 1 (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001),
modified to manipulate warmth and competence via
country of origin.
Warmth and competence manipulations. Based
on previous research, we chose the in-group
to represent the high-warmth, high-competence
country (in our sample, the United States, as all
participants were based in the United States) (e.g.
Cuddy et al., 2008). We chose Germany (low
warmth, high competence) and Portugal (high
warmth, low competence) as the two countries far-
thest from each other on the BIAS map (see Cuddy
et al., 2007). Finally, because there was no Euro-
pean low–low country and it has the lowest ranking
on the BIAS map, we chose Pakistan as our low-
warmth, low-competence country (see Cuddy et al.,
2007). To manipulate warmth and competence, we
changed the name of the organization in the vi-
gnette to include the country of origin: thus,
U.S. Tech Corp., Pakistan Tech Corp., German Tech
Corp., Portugal Tech Corp., and Company X for the
control condition.
Measures
Measures of warmth and competence. Partici-
pants completed the same warmth and competence
items as in Study 1 (warmth, a 5 .94; competence,
a 5 .84) (Fiske et al., 2002). In our pure control
condition (no CSR/CSI, or country of origin), we
performed a confirmatory factor analysis, which
supported a two-factor solution that explained
75.69% of the variance. The four warmth items
1620 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
loaded onto Factor 1 with loadings ranging from .73
to .97. The five competence items loaded onto Factor
2 with loadings ranging from .40 to .92. Warmth and
competence were not significantly correlated in our
control condition, r 5 .35, p 5 .12.
Additional dependent variables. After rating the
firm on the dimensions of warmth and competence,
participants answered the same questions consti-
tuting our two dependent variables as in Study 1.
Participants then completed a manipulation check
regarding the content of the CSR scenario and pro-
vided demographic information.9
Results
Data analysis strategy. To test Hypotheses 4 and
5, we analyzed the two-way interaction effects be-
tween Warmth (high, low) and CSR/CSI conditions
(vs. control), collapsing across firm competence. To
test Hypothesis 6, we conducted pairwise compari-
sons regarding whether changes in competence
moderate the effects of warmth on CSR/CSI out-
comes. Table 2 presents our results.
Reputation. Table 2a presents the means with
standard errors across all conditions predicting
reputation. Table 2b collapses across our warmth
conditions. We ran a three-way ANOVA to analyze
the effect of Condition (CSR, CSI, Control) by
Warmth (high, low) and Competence (high, low) on
Reputation. A significant main effect emerged for
CSR condition, F(2, 494) 5 613.94, p 5 .000, hp
2 5
.71. Interaction effects emerged between Warmth 3
Condition, Competence 3 Condition, and Warmth 3
Competence, Fs . 2.4, ps , .10, hp
2 . .009. These
effects were qualified by a significant three-way in-
teraction, F(2, 494) 5 4.37, p 5 .01, hp
2 5 .017. The
main effect of CSR condition once again confirms our
initial assumption about rewards to CSR and penal-
ties to CSI: firms engaging in CSR (M 5 4.27, SD 5
0.69) attained higher levels of reputation than firms
engaging in CSI (M 5 1.67, SD 5 0.79), F(1, 351) 5
1068.95, p 5 .00, hp
2 5 .75, d 5 3.52, 95% CI [3.41,
3.63], and control firms (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.69), F(1,
331) 5 83.64, p 5 .00, hp
2 5 .20, d 5 1.00, 95% CI
[0.90, 1.11). Control firms had higher reputation than
CSI firms, F(1, 326) 5 530.55, p 5 .00, hp
2 5 .62, d 5
2.58, 95% CI [2.48, 2.70] (Figure 1a and Figure 1b).
To test Hypothesis 4 (comparing CSI outcomes
across high- and low-warmth firms), a two-way
ANOVA was run to analyze the relative effect of
Condition (CSI, control) by Warmth (high, low)
on Reputation. A main effect for CSI emerged,
TABLE 2b
Mean Levels of Reputation by Warmth
(Hypotheses 4 and 5)
CSI Control CSR
High Warmth 1.56 (0.07) 3.63 (0.07) 4.25 (0.08)
Low Warmth 1.78 (0.09) 3.52 (0.09) 4.30 (0.07)
Notes: Reputation was measured on a 5-point scale. Standard
errors in brackets.
TABLE 2a
Mean Levels of Reputation by Country (Hypothesis 6)
CSI Control CSR
United States
(high, high)
1.60 (0.11) 3.54 (0.10) 4.16 (0.12)
Germany (low, high) 1.82 (0.15) 3.85 (0.08) 4.25 (0.10)
Portugal (high, low) 1.51 (0.11) 3.75 (0.10) 4.34 (0.10)
Pakistan (low, low) 1.74 (0.12) 3.20 (0.14) 4.35 (0.09)
Notes: Reputation was measured on a 5-point scale. Standard
errors in brackets.
TABLE 2
Summary of Results for Experiment 2
TABLE 2c
Mean Levels of Purchase Intentions by Country
(Hypothesis 6)
CSI Control CSR
United States
(high, high)
2.32 (0.19) 4.70 (0.13) 5.43 (0.18)
Germany (low, high) 2.41 (0.25) 4.81 (0.16) 5.40 (0.18)
Portugal (high, low) 2.32 (0.35) 4.75 (0.17) 5.36 (0.21)
Pakistan (low, low) 2.74 (0.21) 4.11 (0.18) 5.52 (0.20)
Notes: Purchase Intentions was measured on a 7-point scale.
Standard errors in brackets.
TABLE 2d
Mean Levels of Purchase Intentions by Warmth
(Hypotheses 4 and 5)
CSI Control CSR
High Warmth 2.28 (0.14) 4.73 (0.11) 5.39 (0.13)
Low Warmth 2.57 (0.16) 4.45 (0.12) 5.46 (0.14)
Notes: Purchase Intentions was measured on a 7-point scale.
Standard errors in brackets.
9 In our pure control condition, purchase intentions and
reputation were not significantly correlated, r 5 .29, p 5
21.
2019 1621Shea and Hawn
F(1, 324) 5 533.72, p 5 .000, hp
2 5 .62, and this effect
was qualified by a significant interaction between
CSI x Warmth, F(1, 324) 5 4.37, p 5 .04, hp
2 5 .01,
suggesting moderation. No significant differences
emerged in the control condition between high (M 5
3.63, SD 5 0.62) and low (M 5 3.52, SD 5 0.75)
warmth firms, F(1, 152) 5 1.12, p 5 .29, hp
2 5 .007,
d 5 0.17, 95% CI [0.03, 0.44]. However, when high-
warmth firms engaged in CSI (M 5 1.56, SD 5 0.70),
they received lower reputation evaluations than low-
warmth firms (M 5 1.78, SD 5 0.88), F(1, 172) 5 3.58,
p 5 .06, hp
2 5 .02, d 5 0.31, 95% CI [0.15, 0.45]. This
provides support for Hypothesis 4 for reputation:
high-warmth firms face a larger reputation penalty
(Δ2.07) for engaging in CSI than low-warmth
firms (Δ1.74).
To test Hypothesis 5 (comparing CSR outcomes
across high- and low-warmth firms), a two-way
ANOVA was run to analyze the relative effect of
Condition (CSR, control) by Warmth (high, low) on
Reputation. The interaction was not significant,
F(1, 328) 5 1.18, p 5 .28, hp
2 5 .004, and, thus, we
do not find support for Hypothesis 5 for reputation:
high- and low-warmth firms had equivalent out-
comes for engaging in CSR compared to control
firms.
Next, to test Hypothesis 6 (comparing CSI out-
comes for Germany–Pakistan, United States–
Portugal), a Condition (CSI, control) by Competence
(high, low) ANOVA was run within both high- and
low-warmth conditions. At low levels of country
warmth (Germany–Pakistan), a Condition (CSI,
control) by Competence (high, low) ANOVA
revealed an interaction between CSI and compe-
tence, F(1, 157) 5 5.00, p 5 .02, hp
2 5 .03. Specifi-
cally, while firms from Germany (M 5 1.83, SD 5
0.99) and Pakistan (M 5 1.74, SD 5 0.75) do not
differ when they engage in CSI, F , .18, p . .67, a
significant difference emerges in the control con-
dition, F(1, 72) 5 16.31, p 5 .000, hp
2 5 .19, d 5 0.97,
95% CI [0.82, 1.24], such that firms from Germany
(M 5 3.85, SD 5 0.47) have higher reputation than
firms from Pakistan (M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.84). This
suggests that competence may not buffer reputation
when low-warmth firms engage in CSI in terms of
the relative losses to reputation (ΔGermany 5 2.02;
ΔPakistan 5 1.46). At high levels of country warmth
(United States–Portugal), the interaction between
CSI and competence is not significant, F , .02, p .
.88; therefore, we find no support for Hypothesis 6a
regarding reputation.
Next, to test Hypothesis 6b (comparing CSR
outcomes for Germany–Pakistan, United States–
Portugal), a Condition (CSR, control) by Compe-
tence (high, low) ANOVA was run within both high
and low warmth conditions. At low levels of coun-
try warmth (Germany–Pakistan), a Condition (CSR,
control) by Competence (high, low) revealed an
interaction between CSR and competence, F(1,
159) 5 12.59, p 5 .001, hp
2 5 .07, d 5 0.98, 95% CI
[0.83, 1.13]. Specifically, while Germany (M 5 4.25,
SD 5 0.66) and Pakistan (M 5 4.35, SD 5 0.63) do
not differ when they engage in CSR, F , .18, p . .67,
the interaction is driven by the relative differences
(ΔGermany 5 0.41; ΔPakistan 5 1.14) gained from en-
gaging in CSR versus control (Germany: M 5 3.84,
SD 5 0.47; Pakistan: M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.84). When
engaging in CSR, a firm from Pakistan, a low–low
country, made higher relative gains compared to
FIGURE 1a
Results of Study 2: Average Effect of Warmth and
Condition on Reputation
1
1.5
2
2.5
3
3.5
4
4.5
5
CSR Control CSI
R
ep
u
ta
ti
o
n
High Warmth Low Warmth
FIGURE 1b
Results of Study 2: Average Effect of Warmth and
Condition on Purchase Intentions
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
CSR Control CSI
P
u
rc
h
as
e
In
te
n
ti
o
n
s
High Warmth Low Warmth
1622 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
that from Germany (low-warmth–high-competence
country), suggesting that initial competence did not
have an additive effect, but, rather, low–low firms
are seen as exemplars and get higher returns to CSR,
supporting Hypothesis 6b. At high levels of country
warmth (United States–Portugal), however, the in-
teraction between CSR and competence is not
significant, F , .02, p . .88, suggesting that the
additive benefits of competence are limited to low-
warmth firms engaging in CSR (which violates the
stereotype of low–low organizations).
Purchase intentions. Table 2c presents the means
with standard error bars across all conditions pre-
dicting purchase intentions. Table 2d includes
a separate plot collapsing across our warmth-
competence conditions. First, a three-way ANOVA
was run to analyze the effect of Condition (CSR, CSI,
Control) by Warmth (high, low) and Competence
(high, low) on Purchase Intentions. While the three-
way interaction did not reach statistical signifi-
cance, F(2, 494) 5 2.00, p 5 .14, hp
2 5 .008, we
tested our hypotheses using a series of two-way
ANOVAs reported below. A significant main effect
emerged for CSR condition, F(2, 494) 5 266.50, p 5
.000, hp
2 5 .52. Once again, the main effect of CSR
condition supported our assumption: firms engag-
ing in CSR (M 5 5.43, SD 5 1.28) attained higher
levels of purchase intentions than firms engaging in
CSI (M 5 2.43, SD 5 1.42), F(1, 351) 5 434.86, p 5
.00, hp
2 5 .55, d 5 2.23, 95% CI [2.04, 2.44], and
control firms (M 5 4.59, SD 5 1.01), F(1, 331) 5
42.82, p 5 .00, hp
2 5 .43, d 5 0.73, 95% CI [0.54,
0.88]. Control firms had greater purchase intentions
than CSI firms, F(1, 327) 5 245.99, p 5 .00, hp
2 5
.12, d 5 1.76, 95% CI [1.61, 1.97].
To test Hypothesis 4 (comparing CSI outcomes
across high and low warmth), a two-way ANOVA
was run to analyze the effect of Condition (CSI,
control) by Warmth (high, low) on Purchase In-
tentions. A main effect for CSI emerged, F(1, 324) 5
246.41, p 5 .000, hp
2 5 .432, and this effect was
qualified by a significant interaction between CSI 3
Warmth, F(1, 324) 5 4.41, p 5 .04, hp
2 5 .01. When
firms high (M 5 2.28, SD 5 1.30) or low (M 5 2.57,
SD 5 1.52) in warmth engage in CSI, purchase in-
tentions are not significantly different, F(1, 172) 5
1.93, p 5 .16, hp
2 5 .01, d 5 0.21, 95% CI [0.12,
0.48]. However, marginally significant differ-
ences emerged between high (M 5 4.73, SD 5 0.94)
and low (M 5 4.45, SD 5 1.07) warmth firms in the
control condition, F(1, 152) 5 2.95, p 5 .09, hp
2 5
.02, d 5 0.28, 95% CI [0.07, 0.52]. Thus, when high-
warmth firms shift from no strategy to CSI (Δ2.45),
compared to low-warmth firms (Δ1.88), they face a
bigger deficit in purchase intentions. This provides
support for Hypothesis 4.
To test Hypothesis 5 (comparing CSR outcomes
across high and low warmth), a two-way ANOVA
was run to analyze the effect of Condition (CSR,
control) by Warmth (high, low) on purchase in-
tentions. The interaction was not significant, F(1,
328) 5 1.84, p 5 .18, hp
2 5 .006, and does not provide
support for Hypothesis 5 for purchase intentions.
Next, to test Hypothesis 6a (comparing CSI
outcomes for Germany–Pakistan, United States–
Portugal), a condition (CSI, control) by Competence
(high, low) ANOVA was run within both high and
low warmth conditions. At low levels of country
warmth (Germany–Pakistan), an interaction be-
tween CSI and competence emerged, F(1, 157) 5
6.18, p 5 .01, hp
2 5 .04. Specifically, while Ger-
many (M 5 2.41, SD 5 1.64) and Pakistan (M 5 2.74,
SD 5 1.38) do not differ in purchase intentions
when they engage in CSI, F , 1.05, p . .30, a sig-
nificant difference emerges in the control condi-
tion, F(1, 72) 5 8.68, p 5 .004, hp
2 5 .11, d 5 0.69,
95% CI [.38, 1.05], such that Germany (M 5 4.81,
SD 5 0.95) has higher purchase intentions than
Pakistan (M 5 4.11, SD 5 1.09). When low-warmth
firms engage in CSI, competence does not buffer the
negative impact leading to greater relative decreases
in purchases intentions (ΔGermany 5 2.40; ΔPakistan 5
1.37). At high levels of warmth, the interaction be-
tween CSI and competence is not significant, F ,
.02, p . .91. Therefore, just like for reputation, we
do not find support for Hypothesis 6a for purchase
intentions.
Next, to test Hypothesis 6b (comparing CSR
outcomes for Germany–Pakistan, United States–
Portugal), a Condition (CSR, control) by Compe-
tence (high, low) ANOVA was run within both
high and low warmth conditions. At low levels of
warmth (Germany–Pakistan), an interaction be-
tween CSR and competence emerged, F(1, 159) 5
5.11, p 5 .03, hp
2 5 .03, d 5 0.56, 95% CI [0.28,
0.87]. Specifically, while Germany (M 5 5.40, SD 5
1.18) and Pakistan (M 5 5.52, SD 5 1.34) do not
differ when they engage in CSR, F , .21, p . .64, the
interaction is driven by the relative differences
gained (ΔGermany 5 .59; ΔPakistan 5 1.41) versus
control (Germany: M 5 4.81, SD 5 0.95; Pakistan:
M 5 4.11, SD 5 1.09). At high levels of warmth, the
interaction between CSR and competence is not
significant, F , .12, p . .77. Thus, our findings
suggest that low-warmth–low-competence firms
receive a larger relative benefit when engaging in
2019 1623Shea and Hawn
CSR—supporting Hypothesis 6b—even though
high-warmth firms do not see differences with the
addition of competence.
Discussion
There are several implications of Study 2, with
the main one being that we find no support for Hy-
potheses 5 and 6a for both outcomes (reputation
and purchase intentions). Hypotheses 4 and 6b
demonstrated consistent results for both outcomes
for firms from a low–low country (Pakistan), sug-
gesting that, in comparison to Hypothesis 5, which
only considers warmth perception, both high com-
petence and warmth help a low–low company
achieve higher rewards for CSR. Thus, the effec-
tiveness of a CSR strategy is contingent on the
warmth and competence perception of the organi-
zation’s country of origin (Hypothesis 6b), while
firms from high-warmth countries received harsher
evaluations for engaging in CSI (Hypothesis 4). We
theorize that these effects are driven by assimilation
(CSI) and contrast (CSR) effects from the stereo-
types associated with low-warmth countries. Im-
portantly, this study provides insight into why
some organizations fare better or worse in observer
reactions to their corporate strategies. The attri-
butes of the firm—conceptualized as country-
of-origin warmth and competence—influenced
important organizational perceptions, such as rep-
utation and purchase intentions.
In addition, Study 2 helps explore the question of
endogeneity regarding whether high-warmth orga-
nizations are more likely to engage in CSR behavior
in the first place—to earn greater benefits from it. We
find consistent marginal support for Hypothesis 4
that states that firms with higher warmth will pay a
higher cost for CSI, and no support for Hypothesis 5
about lower rewards for CSR. Likewise, we do not
find support for the argument that the costs for en-
gaging in irresponsible behavior are lower for orga-
nizations from high–high countries. In fact, firms
from Pakistan (a low-warmth and low-competence
country) experienced the greatest rewards for CSR
engagement, due to stereotype violation. To con-
clude, Study 2 demonstrates that a firm’s country of
origin (manipulating warmth and competence per-
ceptions) influences the success or failure of its CSR
strategies relative to its baseline state. Specifically,
we find that CSR can supplement for low-warmth
(and low-competence) country of origin, and that
low-warmth country of origin shields organizations
from harsher judgments of CSI.
STUDY 3
Studies 1 and 2 showed how CSR and CSI affect the
perceived warmth and competence of an organiza-
tion, andthattheseeffectsareamplifieddependingon
the firms’ country of origin. Study 3 aims to replicate
and extend these findings while addressing various
shortcomingsof Studies 1 and 2. First,while Studies 1
and 2 measure perceptions, Study 3 uses behavioral
measures (Colquitt, 2008), adding external validity to
our previous measures. Specifically, participants
used their own money to purchase products from
firms with differing social strategies and also pro-
vided help (e.g., feedback) to the organizations.
Second, to ensure that our high-warmth–high-
competence results are not an artifact of an in-group
bias, Study 3 uses Sweden as the high-warmth–
high-competence country.10 Third, we switched our
CSR manipulations to a fact-sheet format that in-
cluded information on both labor and environmental
10 Prior to running Study 3, we sought to validate previous
research on the BIAS map (Cuddy et al., 2007) with regard to
where countries fall on the warmth and competence di-
mensions.Inaddition,wesoughttoreplacetheUnitedStates
in the final study. Using a unique sample of 95 Mechanical
Turk workers, we assessed the warmth and competence of
seven countries (Germany, Iran, Pakistan, Portugal, Spain,
Sweden, and the United States) previous research had clas-
sified into one of the four quadrants on the BIAS map. We
first asked about the warmth and competence of each
country (Fiske et al., 2002) and then had participants rank
order the countries in terms of their warmth and compe-
tence. Pairwise comparisons, rank order, and correlational
evidence support the classification in our experimental
paradigm. First, ranked from highest to lowest warmth, were
Sweden, the United States, Spain, Portugal, Germany,
Pakistan, and Iran; and, second, ranked from highest to
lowest competence were Germany, the United States, Swe-
den, Spain, Portugal, Iran, and Pakistan. Looking at corre-
lations between warmth and competence, the United States,
r 5 .55, p , .000, and Sweden, r 5 .70, p , .000, are seen as
high warmth and competence. Given that our study com-
prises U.S. participants, we used Sweden to mitigate in-
group bias concerns and “Made in America” preferences in
our Study 3. Pakistan is viewed as low warmth and compe-
tence,r5 .50,p, .000,ascomparedtoIran,r5 .21,p, .000,
given the high correlation between warmth and competence
as well as low means. Germany is viewed as high compe-
tence and low warmth, r 5 .21, p 5 .04, given the smaller
correlation. Portugal is viewed as high warmth and low
competence. r 5 .48, p , .000, as compared to Spain, r 5 .63,
p , .000; although not as cleanly manipulated, we believe
that Portugal represents the better manipulation of low
competence and high warmth, given the smaller correlation
coefficient and previous research on the BIAS map.
1624 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
performance to ensure that the effects were not idio-
syncratic to our Study 1 and 2 materials. And, finally,
to provide a conservative test of our hypotheses, we
modified our design to assess these behaviors within
subjects; that is, all participants engaged with firms
from four countries within CSR and CSI conditions.
Sample and Procedures
Participants. We recruited 100 participants from
a paid student research lab at a private Midwestern
university. We excluded one participant due to ex-
pletive language in all of our written prompts, and 11
participants who failed the manipulation check. In-
cluding these participants in analyses does not alter
the direction of effects.
Design. Study 3 employed a mixed design method-
ology; in particular, 3(CSR, CSI, control condition) by2
(Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) between-subjects
factors across a four-factor country-of-origin warmth by
competence (HH 5 Sweden; HL 5 Germany; LH 5
Portugal; LL 5 Pakistan) within-subjects factor. We
randomized the order of our country of origin manip-
ulation as a repeated measure across all conditions.
Procedure. The study was advertised as a 30-min-
ute product development study. Participants received
$8inexchangefortheirparticipationandanadditional
$1 for a purchase decision. The study asked partici-
pants to test, evaluate, and make purchase decisions
about a series of pens. We chose pens as an experi-
mental stimulus because pens have been used suc-
cessfully in previous research (e.g., Shah & Wolford,
2007). We purchased four types of pens priced
$0.78–$0.83 from a wholesaler. The pens did not differ
on measures of quality or looks. Upon arrival to the lab,
each participant was seated at a computer station
where they received a plastic bag containing four
“prototype” pens to evaluate as well as $1 in coins for
the pricing task (in addition to the $8 payment). They
also received a notepad to test out each pen. Partici-
pants completed the entire study at a computer station.
Warmth–Competence manipulation. To manip-
ulate country of origin we had the pens labeled as
“Made in Sweden,” “Made in Germany,” “Made in
Portugal,” and “Made in Pakistan” in a 1-millimeter
font. We then used neutral labels on each pen to serve
aslogosandtranslatedtheword“fine”intoeachof the
four languages to put on the logo. Each participant
received and rated each of these four pens as a within-
subjects variable.
Social responsibility manipulation. Participants
were randomly assigned one of three experimental
conditions: CSR, CSI or control. We provided them
withbackgroundinformationabouteachofthefourpen
manufacturers whose products they would be testing.
Embedded within neutral performance information
was information on environmental impact and labor
practices of each of the manufacturers. In the CSR con-
dition, participants read that all four pen manufacturers
were going out of their way to promote strong environ-
mental protection and repair, as well as employee
standards. In the CSI condition, pen manufacturers
were stated to be poor in environmental standards and
not desirable in terms of their labor practices. In the
control condition, participants were provided with
neutral information. All companies were established in
their industries and in good financial standing. Each
conditionhadthesamenumberofcategoriestodescribe
the organization.11 Please see Appendix B.
11 As a manipulation check, participants rated both
the warmth and competence of the pen manufacturing
organizations using a single-item measure (“The pen
manufacturing organizations seem to be warm/compe-
tent”). A three-cell (CSR, CSI, control conditions) between-
subjects analysis of variance on perceptions of Warmth
revealed a significant effect of social responsibility condi-
tion, F(2, 81) 5 5.73, p 5 .005, hp
2 5 .12. Specifically, in-
dividuals in the CSR condition (M 5 4.56, SD 5 1.41) had
significantly higher perceptions of warmth than individ-
uals in the CSI condition (M 5 3.33, SD 5 1.42), F(1, 53) 5
10.18, p 5 .002, hp
2 5 .16, d 5 0.88, 95% CI [0.38, 1.39].
Individuals in the Control condition (M 5 3.97, SD 5 1.18)
had marginally higher perceptions of warmth than individ-
uals in the CSI condition, F(1, 57) 5 3.44, p 5 .07, hp
2 5 .06,
d 5 0.50, 95% CI [0.08, 1.01]. Individuals in the CSR con-
dition had marginally higher perceptions of warmth than
individuals in the control condition, F(1, 52) 5 2.82, p 5 .09,
hp
2 5 .05, d 5 0.46, 95% CI [20.04, 0.88]. This replicates our
previous studies that found CSR and CSI are linked with
perceptions of warmth, supporting Hypothesis 1. A three-
cell(CSR,CSI,controlconditions)between-subjectsanalysis
of variance on perceptions of Competence revealed a sig-
nificant effect of social responsibility condition, F(2, 81) 5
5.14, p5 .008, hp
2 5 .11. Specifically, individuals intheCSR
condition (M 5 5.56, SD 5 0.82) had significantly higher
perceptions of competence than individuals in the CSI
condition (M 5 4.70, SD 5 1.26), F(1, 53) 5 8.56, p 5 .005,
hp
2 5 .14, d 5 0.82, 95% CI [0.53, 1.27]. Individuals in the
Control condition (M 5 5.31, SD 5 0.93) had significantly
higher perceptions of competence than individuals in the
CSI condition, F(1, 57) 5 4.44, p 5 .04, hp
2 5 .07, d 5 0.56,
95% CI [0.23, 1.01]. Individuals in the CSR and control
conditions did not differ significantly in perceptions of
competence, F(1, 52) 5 1.08, p 5 .30, hp
2 5 .02, d 5 0.29, CI
[0.00, 0.62]. This replicates our previous studies that found
CSR and CSI are linked with perceptions of competence
while control conditions oftentimes do not differ from CSR,
supporting Hypothesis 2.
2019 1625Shea and Hawn
Measures
Participants completed a feedback task set and a
purchasing task. The two tasks were counter-
balanced.12 When relevant, we controlled for order.
Each participant completed each dependent mea-
sure across the four country of origin manipulations.
Feedback task. The first set of tasks examined the
time and effort that participants devoted to providing
feedback to each pen manufacturing organization.
Participants were told that the pens were prototypes
and that the organizations would like feedback on the
pen designs. We encouraged participants to try out
each pen in order to form an opinion of each product.
Participants were given a blank essay box to provide
feedback to the organization on their pen design. We
measured the number of words written (M 5 21.87,
SD 5 10.42) as a measure of helping (adapted from
Porath & Erez, 2007). We also measured the subjective
quality of each pen (“The pen is of high quality,” “The
pen is desirable”; as 5 .72–.76).
Purchasing task. The second task was a purchas-
ingtaskusingtheBecker–DeGroot–Marschakmethod
(Becker, DeGroot, & Marschak, 1964), which has been
used to reveal true reservation prices in a variety
of contexts (Burbano, 2016; Kahneman, Knetsch, &
Thaler, 1991; Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004).
In this task, participants were given an additional
dollar and were told that they could use this money to
purchase the pens from the experiment and to keep
whatever money they did not use. Participants were
told that the computer program would randomly se-
lect one pen and a random price at which they had the
opportunity to purchase it. If their stated price was
below the randomly generated price, they would not
have the opportunity to purchase the pen. If their
stated price was above the randomly generated price,
they would have the opportunity to purchase the pen.
Participants had to successfully recall these in-
structions prior to continuing to ensure that they
understood the experimental paradigm. This goes
above traditional willingness-to-pay measures by
forcing participants to forgo some of their bonus pay
to make a purchase. Likewise, the random lottery
nature of the task ensures that participants reveal
their true purchase prices (Becker et al., 1964). Par-
ticipants stated a purchase price for each of the four
pens on a sliding scale between $0.01 and $1.00 (M 5
$0.33, SD 5 $0.17). No pens significantly differed
from this price and no pen was consistently priced as
the “top” pen across the entire sample. After partic-
ipants stated their price for each pen, a random
payment price was generated, they took the pen (if
purchased), and left the purchase price at the com-
puterstation at the end of the study. Participantsthen
completed a manipulation check about the social
practices of the pen manufacturers, as well as de-
mographic questions.
Results
Data analysis strategy. We ran a series of re-
peated measures ANOVA across dependent mea-
sures. We first ran a CSR condition (three: CSR, CSI,
Control) by Order (two: Pricing first, Feedback first)
across the four country of origin manipulations
(separately as well as grouped together by high and
low warmth) as a repeated measures ANOVA.13 To
directly test hypotheses, we re-ran models with a
series of planned comparisons to probe statistically
significant mean differences both within and be-
tween conditions (see Table 3). Assumption of
sphericity was not violated in any of the analyses;
therefore, we report the sphericity-assumed results.
Number of words. A three-cell (CSI, CSI, control
condition) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first)
repeated measures ANOVA was run on the number of
words written for the high- and low-warmth firms, as
well as the Sweden, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan
country of origin conditions. No significant differences
emerged in the models, Fs , 2.02, ps . .11. When
examined as a between-subjects analysis, a significant
effect emerged for the social responsibility condition,
F(2,78) 5 3.46, p 5 .04, hp
2 5 .08. Specifically, in-
dividuals in the CSR condition (M 5 26.28, SD 5
14.10) wrote marginally more words than individuals
in the CSI condition (M 5 20.53, SD 5 7.22), F(1,53) 5
3.81, p 5 .05, hp
2 5 .07, d 5 0.52, 95% CI [24.52,
3.11], and individuals in the control condition (M 5
19.45, SD 5 8.48), F(1,52) 5 4.80, p 5 .03, hp
2 5 .09,
d 5 0.60, 95% CI [24.45, 3.63]. The CSI and control
conditions did not significantly differ, F , .3, p . .60.
Although we did not see variation within our country
of origin manipulation, individuals offered more help
to organizations that engaged in CSR, providing be-
havioral replication and extension of Hypothesis 1 as
12 Pre-tests indicated that licensing effects may occur
when tasks were sequential (i.e., “I did not help the orga-
nization, so I supplement by overpaying for their product,”
or vice versa).
13 We re-ran analyses excluding the control condition as
it did not produce any significant effect, nor were there any
significant interactions between the control condition and
other conditions. We report both sets of ANOVAs below.
1626 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
well as support for our initial assumption about CSR
generating rewards.
Subjective quality. To test Hypotheses 4 and 5, a
two-cell (CSI or CSI) by two (Order: Pricing first,
Feedbackfirst)repeatedmeasuresANOVAwasrunon
subjective quality in high- and low-warmth firms.14
The three-way interaction was significant, F(1, 51) 5
4.71, p 5 .04, hp
2 5 .08. When participants completed
the feedback tasks first,15 an interaction emerged, F(1,
23) 5 6.51, p 5 .02, hp
2 5 .22, suggesting moderation.
While no differences between high- and low-warmth
FIGURE 2a
Results of Study 3: Average Effect of Warmth and
Condition on Perceived Quality
1.000
1.500
2.000
2.500
3.000
3.500
4.000
CSI Control CSR
S
u
b
je
ct
iv
e
Q
u
al
it
y
Evaluation Task First
High Warmth Low Warmth
1.000
1.500
2.000
2.500
3.000
3.500
4.000
CSI Control CSR
S
u
b
je
ct
iv
e
Q
u
al
it
y
Pricing Task First
High Warmth Low Warmth
TABLE 3
Summary of Results for Experiment 3
TABLE 3c
Mean Levels of Subjective Quality by Country (Hypothesis
6)
CSI Control CSR
Sweden (high, high) 3.19 (0.22) 3.09 (0.21) 3.00 (0.26)
Germany (low, high) 2.88 (0.16) 2.94 (0.25) 3.00 (0.20)
Portugal (high, low) 2.88 (0.21) 3.03 (0.19) 2.83 (0.19)
Pakistan (low, low) 2.54 (0.18) 3.12 (0.27) 3.58 (0.25)
Notes: Subjective quality was measured on a 5-point scale.
Standard errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between-
subjects factors, while country of origin is a within-subjects
factor.
TABLE 3d
Mean Levels of Subjective Quality by Warmth (Hypothe-
ses 4 and 5)
CSI Control CSR
High Warmth 3.04 (0.20) 3.06 (0.18) 2.92 (0.21)
Low Warmth 2.71 (0.21) 3.03 (0.19) 3.29 (0.22)
Notes: Subjective quality was measured on a 5-point scale.
Standard errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between-
subjects factors, while warmth is a within-subjects factor.
TABLE 3a
Mean Levels of Purchase Price by Country (Hypothesis 6)
CSI Control CSR
Sweden (high, high) 0.43 (0.07) 0.35 (0.06) 0.31 (0.08)
Germany (low, high) 0.25 (0.03) 0.26 (0.05) 0.35 (0.06)
Portugal (high, low) 0.37 (0.08) 0.32 (0.06) 0.27 (0.06)
Pakistan (low, low) 0.31 (0.05) 0.29 (0.05) 0.37 (0.05)
Notes: Purchase Price was measured on a $1 scale. Standard
errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between-subjects
factors, while country of origin is a within-subjects factor.
TABLE 3b
Mean Levels of Purchase Price by Warmth (Hypotheses 4
and 5)
CSI Control CSR
High Warmth 0.40 (0.06) 0.33 (0.06) 0.29 (0.06)
Low Warmth 0.28 (0.05) 0.28 (0.05) 0.36 (0.06)
Notes: Purchase Price was measured on a $1 scale. Standard
errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between-subjects
factors, while warmth is a within-subjects factor.
14 Including all CSR/CSI/control conditions, the three-
way interaction is not significant, F(2, 78) 5 1.68, p 5 .19,
hp
2 5 .041.
15 When participants completed the pricing task first, a
main effect for factor emerges, F(1, 28) 5 4.45, p 5 .04,
hp
2 5 .137, such that participants had a preference for high-
warmth firms (M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.64) over low-warmth firms
(M 5 2.88, SD 5 0.70) across conditions.
2019 1627Shea and Hawn
firms emerged in the CSR condition, t(11) 5 1.62, p 5
.13, within the CSI condition, paired samples t tests
revealed that high-warmth firms (M 5 3.09, SD 5 0.45)
hadmarginallyhigherqualityratingsthanlow-warmth
firms (M 5 2.71, SD 5 0.50), t(12) 5 2.10, p 5 .06,
failing to provide support for Hypothesis 4. However,
exploring the moderation effect between conditions,
no significant differences emerged for high-warmth
firms, F , .3, p . .59, while low-warmth firms saw
significant gains between CSI (M 5 2.71, SD 5 0.50)
andCSR(M 5 3.29,SD5 0.54),F(1,23)5 7.78,p5 .01,
hp
2 5 .25,d5 1.16,95%CI[0.86,1.44].Takentogether,
the significant moderation effect suggests that, while
low-warmth firms receive equivalent outcomes for
CSR, but not CSI, they significantly increase their
outcomes as they changed strategies from CSI to CSR
(Δ 5 .58), while high-warmth firms did not (Δ 5 .12).
This provides indirect support for Hypothesis 5 that
low-warmth firms reap higher gains from CSR.
To test Hypothesis 6, a two-cell (CSI or CSI) by
two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) repeated
measures ANOVA was run on subjective quality in
the Sweden, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan
country of origin conditions.16 The three-way in-
teraction was significant, F(3, 153) 5 3.46, p 5 .02,
hp
2 5 .06. When the feedback task came first, a sig-
nificant interaction emerged between CSR/CSI and
country of origin, F(3, 69) 5 3.90, p 5 .01, hp
2 5 .25.
We report paired samples t tests across countries of
origin within the feedback task first condition.
In the CSI condition, a paired samples t test indi-
cated that Pakistan (M 5 2.54, SD 5 0.66) had a sig-
nificantly lower subjective quality than Sweden (M 5
3.19, SD 5 0.78), t(12) 5 2.85, p5 .02, d 5 0.94, 95%CI
[0.50, 1.31]. No significant effects were observed be-
tween other countries, all ts , 1.67, ps . .12. This does
not provide clear support for Hypothesis 6a.
In the CSR condition, a paired samples t test indicated
that Pakistan (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.87)17 had a significantly
higher subjective quality than Portugal (M 5 2.83, SD 5
0.65), t(11) 5 2.37, p 5 .04, and a marginally significant
higher subjective quality than Sweden (M 5 3.00, SD 5
0.90), t(11) 5 1.83, p 5 .09. No significant effects were
observed between other countries, all ts , 1.74, ps .
.11.18 This provides support for Hypothesis 6b, which
predicted higher rewards to CSR for low–low firms.
These results—obtained within subject—suggest that a
low-warmth–low-competence firm gains significantly
higher benefits for doing CSR than high-warmth firms.
Purchase price. To test Hypotheses 4 and 5, a two-
cell (CSI, CSI) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback
first) repeated measures ANOVA was run on Pur-
chase Price in the high- and low-warmth firms.19 A
marginaleffectemergedforCondition,F(1,51)5 3.08,
FIGURE 2b
Results of Study 3: Average Effect of Warmth and
Condition on Purchase Price
0.000
0.050
0.100
0.150
0.200
0.250
0.300
0.350
0.400
0.450
0.500
CSI Control CSR
P
u
rc
h
as
e
P
ri
ce
Evaluation Task First
High Warmth Low Warmth
0.000
0.100
0.200
0.300
0.400
0.500
CSI Control CSR
P
u
rc
h
as
e
P
ri
ce
Pricing Task First
High Warmth Low Warmth
16 Including all CSR/CSI/control conditions, the three-
way interaction is marginally significant, F(6, 234) 5 1.83,
p 5 .09, hp
2 5 .045.
17 Probing this interaction between conditions, a one-
factor ANOVA on Pakistan’s subjective quality revealed a
significant effect, F(1,23) 5 11.48, p 5 .003, hp
2 5 .333, d 5
1.37, 95% CI [1.06, 1.61]. Specifically, subjective quality
for the Pakistani pen was significantly higher in the CSR
condition (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.87) than in the CSI condition
(M 5 2.54, SD 5 0.66).
18 While not directly testing a hypothesis, a one-factor
ANOVA between CSR and CSI conditions on Pakistan’s
subjective quality revealed a significant effect, F(1,23) 5
11.48, p 5 .003, hp
2 5 .333. Specifically, subjective quality
for the Pakistani pen was significantly higher in the CSR
condition (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.87) than in the CSI condition
(M 5 2.54, SD 5 0.66).
19 Including all CSR/CSI/control conditions, the three-
way interaction is marginally significant, F(2, 78) 5 2.68,
p 5 .07, hp
2 5 .064.
1628 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal
p 5 .09, hp
2 5 .06. This effect was qualified by a
significant three-way interaction, F(1, 51) 5 5.69, p 5
.02, hp
2 5 .10. When participants completed the
feedback tasks first,20 an interaction emerged, F(1,
23) 5 7.04, p 5 .01, hp
2 5 .23, suggesting moderation.
Exploring these interactions, in the CSI condition,
paired samples t tests revealed that high-warmth
firms (M 5 0.40, SD 5 0.07) and low-warmth firms
(M 5 0.28, SD 5 0.03) had significantly different
purchase prices, t(12) 5 2.44, p 5 .02, with high-
warmth firms having significantly higher prices,
which does not support Hypothesis 4. However, in
the CSR condition, we observed no differences be-
tween high- and low-warmth firms, t(11) 5 1.35, p 5
.20. Looking between conditions, no significant dif-
ferences emerged for high- or low-warmth firms, F ,
1.8, p . .19. Thus, the interaction term reaches sig-
nificance, driven by the difference in prices in the
CSI condition, with the means crossover: when high-
warmth firms switch from CSI to CSR, their purchase
price decreased (Δ 5 .11), while low-warmth firm’s
price increased (Δ 5 .08). Taken together, these re-
sults suggest that low-warmth firms closed the gap as
they changed strategies from CSI to CSR, while high-
warmth firms surprisingly saw a decrease in terms of
pricing (between conditions), providing indirect
support for Hypothesis 5.
To test Hypothesis 6, a two (Condition: CSI or CSI)
by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) repeated
measures ANOVA was run on Purchase Price in the
Sweden, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan country of
origin conditions.21 The three-way interaction was
significant, F(3, 153) 5 2.61, p 5 .05, hp
2 5 .05.
Probing this interaction further within Order condi-
tions, when purchase price was determined after the
feedback task, a significant interaction emerged be-
tween CSR/CSI and country of origin, F(3, 69) 5 3.45,
p 5 .03, hp
2 5 .13.22 In the CSI condition, a paired
samples t test indicated that Sweden (M 5 0.43, SD 5
0.25) had a significantly higher price than Germany
(M 5 0.25, SD 5 0.12), t(12) 5 2.69, p 5 .02, d 5 0.94,
95% CI [0.83, 0.99]; likewise, Sweden had a higher
price than Pakistan (M 5 0.31, SD 5 0.17), t(12) 5 2.70,
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx
r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx

More Related Content

Similar to r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx

Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...
Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...
Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...inventy
 
Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...
Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...
Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...Adam Shafi Shaik PhD.
 
"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship
"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship
"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory RelationshipGuy Sack, M.A.
 
Employees’ expectations from csr the case of master students
Employees’ expectations from csr  the case of master studentsEmployees’ expectations from csr  the case of master students
Employees’ expectations from csr the case of master studentsbarizah94
 
Corporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docx
Corporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docxCorporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docx
Corporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docxfaithxdunce63732
 
E263953
E263953E263953
E263953aijbm
 
Running head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY .docx
Running head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY                            .docxRunning head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY                            .docx
Running head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY .docxSUBHI7
 
HR Practices and Internal Corporate Social Responsibility
HR Practices and Internal Corporate Social ResponsibilityHR Practices and Internal Corporate Social Responsibility
HR Practices and Internal Corporate Social Responsibilityscmsnoida5
 
Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...
Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...
Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...Alexander Decker
 
Pg academic writing using reading in your assignments
Pg academic writing   using reading in your assignmentsPg academic writing   using reading in your assignments
Pg academic writing using reading in your assignmentsRhianWynWilliams
 
5Factors of Engagement
5Factors of Engagement5Factors of Engagement
5Factors of EngagementOmar Farooq
 
2003 sharma, chrisman, chua
2003 sharma, chrisman, chua2003 sharma, chrisman, chua
2003 sharma, chrisman, chuamadabouttoole
 
The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...
The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...
The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...frank acheampong
 
Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...
Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...
Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...Alexander Decker
 
Pg using reading in your assignments worksheet
Pg using reading in your assignments worksheetPg using reading in your assignments worksheet
Pg using reading in your assignments worksheetRhianWynWilliams
 
Examining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docx
Examining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docxExamining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docx
Examining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docxSANSKAR20
 
The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...
The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...
The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...CSCJournals
 

Similar to r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx (20)

Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...
Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...
Managerial perceptions on corporate social responsibility in select companies...
 
I037054058
I037054058I037054058
I037054058
 
Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...
Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...
Corporate social responsibility institutional drivers a comparative study fro...
 
"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship
"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship
"Friend" Me: Social Media Privacy In A Supervisory Relationship
 
Employees’ expectations from csr the case of master students
Employees’ expectations from csr  the case of master studentsEmployees’ expectations from csr  the case of master students
Employees’ expectations from csr the case of master students
 
Corporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docx
Corporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docxCorporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docx
Corporate Reputation Review,Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 261–277© 2.docx
 
10120140501005
1012014050100510120140501005
10120140501005
 
E263953
E263953E263953
E263953
 
Running head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY .docx
Running head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY                            .docxRunning head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY                            .docx
Running head ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY .docx
 
HR Practices and Internal Corporate Social Responsibility
HR Practices and Internal Corporate Social ResponsibilityHR Practices and Internal Corporate Social Responsibility
HR Practices and Internal Corporate Social Responsibility
 
Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...
Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...
Perception of job performance appraisals toward turnover intention and job sa...
 
Pg academic writing using reading in your assignments
Pg academic writing   using reading in your assignmentsPg academic writing   using reading in your assignments
Pg academic writing using reading in your assignments
 
5Factors of Engagement
5Factors of Engagement5Factors of Engagement
5Factors of Engagement
 
2003 sharma, chrisman, chua
2003 sharma, chrisman, chua2003 sharma, chrisman, chua
2003 sharma, chrisman, chua
 
The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...
The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...
The Effect of CSR on Brand Equity in the IT Solutions Industry; the Case of A...
 
Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...
Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...
Corporate social disclosure quantity and quality as moderators between corpor...
 
Pg using reading in your assignments worksheet
Pg using reading in your assignments worksheetPg using reading in your assignments worksheet
Pg using reading in your assignments worksheet
 
Examining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docx
Examining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docxExamining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docx
Examining how preferences foremployer branding attributes.docx
 
The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...
The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...
The Impact of Shared Values, Corporate Cultural Characteristics, and Implemen...
 
10220140502001
1022014050200110220140502001
10220140502001
 

More from audeleypearl

Mr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docx
Mr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docxMr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docx
Mr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docxaudeleypearl
 
Movie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docx
Movie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docxMovie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docx
Movie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docxaudeleypearl
 
Motivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docx
Motivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docxMotivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docx
Motivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mother of the Year In recognition of superlative paren.docx
Mother of the Year         In recognition of superlative paren.docxMother of the Year         In recognition of superlative paren.docx
Mother of the Year In recognition of superlative paren.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docx
Mrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docxMrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docx
Mrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docx
Mr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docxMr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docx
Mr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docx
Mr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docxMr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docx
Mr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docxaudeleypearl
 
Moving members of the organization through the change process ca.docx
Moving members of the organization through the change process ca.docxMoving members of the organization through the change process ca.docx
Moving members of the organization through the change process ca.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docx
Mr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docxMr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docx
Mr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docx
Mr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docxMr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docx
Mr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docxaudeleypearl
 
Motor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docx
Motor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docxMotor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docx
Motor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docxaudeleypearl
 
Most women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docx
Most women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docxMost women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docx
Most women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docxaudeleypearl
 
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docxMost patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docxaudeleypearl
 
Most of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docx
Most of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docxMost of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docx
Most of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docxaudeleypearl
 
Most people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docx
Most people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docxMost people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docx
Most people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docxaudeleypearl
 
Most of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docx
Most of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docxMost of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docx
Most of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docxaudeleypearl
 
Most healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docx
Most healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docxMost healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docx
Most healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docxaudeleypearl
 
More work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docx
More work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docxMore work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docx
More work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docxaudeleypearl
 
Mortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docx
Mortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docxMortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docx
Mortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docxaudeleypearl
 
Moral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docx
Moral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docxMoral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docx
Moral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docxaudeleypearl
 

More from audeleypearl (20)

Mr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docx
Mr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docxMr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docx
Mr. Bush, a 45-year-old middle school teacher arrives at the emergen.docx
 
Movie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docx
Movie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docxMovie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docx
Movie Project Presentation Movie TroyInclude Architecture i.docx
 
Motivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docx
Motivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docxMotivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docx
Motivation and Retention Discuss the specific strategies you pl.docx
 
Mother of the Year In recognition of superlative paren.docx
Mother of the Year         In recognition of superlative paren.docxMother of the Year         In recognition of superlative paren.docx
Mother of the Year In recognition of superlative paren.docx
 
Mrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docx
Mrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docxMrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docx
Mrs. G, a 55 year old Hispanic female, presents to the office for he.docx
 
Mr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docx
Mr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docxMr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docx
Mr. Rivera is a 72-year-old patient with end stage COPD who is in th.docx
 
Mr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docx
Mr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docxMr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docx
Mr. B, a 40-year-old avid long-distance runner previously in goo.docx
 
Moving members of the organization through the change process ca.docx
Moving members of the organization through the change process ca.docxMoving members of the organization through the change process ca.docx
Moving members of the organization through the change process ca.docx
 
Mr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docx
Mr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docxMr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docx
Mr. Friend is acrime analystwith the SantaCruz, Califo.docx
 
Mr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docx
Mr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docxMr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docx
Mr. E is a pleasant, 70-year-old, black, maleSource Self, rel.docx
 
Motor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docx
Motor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docxMotor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docx
Motor Milestones occur in a predictable developmental progression in.docx
 
Most women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docx
Most women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docxMost women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docx
Most women experience their closest friendships with those of th.docx
 
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docxMost patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. Howev.docx
 
Most of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docx
Most of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docxMost of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docx
Most of our class readings and discussions to date have dealt wi.docx
 
Most people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docx
Most people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docxMost people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docx
Most people agree we live in stressful times. Does stress and re.docx
 
Most of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docx
Most of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docxMost of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docx
Most of the ethical prescriptions of normative moral philosophy .docx
 
Most healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docx
Most healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docxMost healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docx
Most healthcare organizations in the country are implementing qualit.docx
 
More work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docx
More work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docxMore work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docx
More work is necessary on how to efficiently model uncertainty in ML.docx
 
Mortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docx
Mortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docxMortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docx
Mortgage-Backed Securities and the Financial CrisisKelly Finn.docx
 
Moral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docx
Moral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docxMoral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docx
Moral Development  Lawrence Kohlberg developed six stages to mora.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinRaunakKeshri1
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 

r Academy of Management Journal2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–16.docx

  • 1. r Academy of Management Journal 2019, Vol. 62, No. 5, 1609–1642. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.0795 MICROFOUNDATIONS OF CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY AND IRRESPONSIBILITY CATHERINE T. SHEA Carnegie Mellon University OLGA V. HAWN University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill This study examines the importance of social perception of corporate social responsibility (CSR) and irresponsibility (CSI). Drawing from social psychology literature on stereotypes, we argue that two fundamental dimensions of social perception—warmth and competence—help explain the underlying processes and conditions under which CSR leads to specific outcomes. We propose that firms engaging in CSR are perceived as higher in warmth and, by default, competence; moreover, different perceptions of the organiza- tion’s warmth and competence can moderate CSR rewards and CSI penalties. To dem- onstrate this, we conduct three experiments. Experiment 1 links CSR with perceptions of warmth and competence, showing that warmth perceptions mediate the relationship be- tween CSR and important outcomes, such as purchase intentions
  • 2. and reputation. Exper- iment 2 adds information on firms’ countries of origin, revealing that CSR rewards and CSI penalties differ depending on the (mis)alignment of CSR strategy with country stereotypes. Experiment 3 replicates these findings using behavioral paradigms. We find that firms from high-warmth countries (the United States, Sweden, Portugal) receive lower CSR rewards and pay higher CSI penalties than firms from low- warmth countries (Germany, Pakistan) but this effect is moderated by competence. Our micro–macro study advances social evaluation, strategic CSR, and international management literatures. Corporate social responsibility (CSR) has grown markedly in the past decade both as an important phenomenon in practice and as a critical field in academia (Wang, Tong, Takeuchi, & George, 2016). We have theorized about its institutional (Campbell, 2007; Ioannou & Serafeim, 2012) and organizational drivers (Aguilera, Rupp, Williams, & Ganapathi, 2007; McWilliams & Siegel, 2001) as well as exam- ined its effect on firm performance (Berman, Wicks, Kotha, & Jones, 1999; Cochran & Wood, 1984; McGuire, Sundgren, & Schneeweis, 1988; Orlitzky, Schmidt, & Rynes, 2003; Russo & Fouts, 1997) and other outcomes (Berrone & Gomez-Mejia, 2009; Flammer, 2013; Turban & Greening, 1997; Yoon, Gürhan-Canli, & Schwarz, 2006). Yet CSR is primarily studied at the macro level (i.e., institutional or orga- nizational level) compared to the micro level (i.e., individual level) of analysis: a recent review of the CSR literature shows that only 4% of all studies ex-
  • 3. amine the individual level, while only 5% address CSR at two or more levels of analysis (Aguinis & Glavas, 2012). Therefore, what is lacking in CSR re- search is deeper appreciation, at the individual level, of how CSR makes an impact (Wang et al., 2016). Accordingly, this paper addresses the need for micro studies of CSR with a multilevel approach (Morgeson, Aguinis, Waldman, & Siegel, 2013) in order to un- derstand the underlying processes (i.e., mediating effects) and conditions under which (i.e., moderating effects) CSR leads to specific outcomes. In particular, we still know very little about the microfoundations of CSR (for the most recent review, see Gond, El Akremi, Swaen, & Babu, 2017): how CSR and, importantly, corporate social irresponsibility The authors thank anonymous reviewers and confer- ence participants at 2014 Academy of Management, Academy of International Business, and Strategic Man- agement Society annual meetings, as well as Sinziana Dorobantu, Sanjay Patnaik, Elena Kulchina, Nel Dutt, Elena Vidal, Amandine Ody-Brasier, Marlo Raveendran, Zach Burns, and the brown-bag participants at Boston University for comments on the early version of this paper. An earlier version of this manuscript was a Finalist for 2014 AIB Haynes Prize for the Most Promising Scholar. Please address all correspondence to Catherine Shea: Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, email: [email protected] 1609 Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express
  • 4. written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.0795 mailto:[email protected] (CSI) (Lange & Washburn, 2012; Surroca, Tribó, & Zahra, 2013) are perceived by individuals, and what effect this perception might have on the relationship between CSR and specific outcomes at individual and organizational levels of analysis. Forexample,we know that CSR positively affects company reputation (Turban & Greening, 1997)—one of our outcomes— but why and what mediates this relationship at the individual level is still unclear. A small but growing literature on the micro approaches to CSR shows that involvement in CSR positively influences employee performance, behaviors, and attitudes (Burbano, 2016), such as employee engagement (Caligiuri, Mencin, & Jiang, 2013), organizational citizenship behavior (Rupp, Shao, Thornton, & Skarlicki, 2013), identification with the firm (Farooq, Rupp, & Farooq, 2017), retention (Jones, 2010), in-role performance, and commitment, as well as attractiveness to pro- spective employees (Jones, Willness, & Madey, 2014; Turban & Greening, 1997). Yet, few studies go beyond employees (i.e., providing information on other stakeholders) or seek to unpack the processes of CSR evaluations by individuals more generally (Gond et al., 2017). Our study applies to a larger number of stakeholders—which may include employees, customers, environmentalists, suppliers, the commu- nity as a whole, and owners/shareholders—because it examines general individual judgments and percep- tions of warmth and competence.
  • 5. Warmth and competence serve as universal di- mensions of social judgment (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2004; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002). “Warmth” is defined as percep- tions related to intent, including friendliness, trust- worthiness, helpfulness, sincerity, and morality, whereas “competence” is conceptualized as percep- tions related to ability, including skill, intelligence, creativity, and efficacy (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008). With the help of these two dimensions, one can por- tray social perceptions of activities, individuals, or- ganizations, and even countries (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2007). Moreover, social judgment of various degrees (i.e., high or low) of warmth and/or compe- tence can predict a distinct emotion and behavior toward a target; for example, being perceived as high on warmth and/or competence is beneficial, while being low on one of the dimensions is costly (Aaker, Vohs, & Mogilner, 2010). Importantly, compared to other potential mechanisms, warmth and compe- tence help predict specific behavioral outcomes from these social perceptions (e.g., helping those higher in warmth; Cuddy et al., 2007), adding a more nuanced mechanism between CSR, social perception, and behavior toward the firm, which can ultimately affect more macro-level outcomes (e.g., reputation, sales, stakeholder and shareholder value). While previous research mainly examines CSR, we apply these two fundamental dimensions of social perceptiontounderstand themicrofoundationsof CSR and CSI. Naturally, our basic argument is that firms with CSR generate a greater warmth and (by default, without any other information) competence percep-
  • 6. tion than control firms or firms engaging in CSI. We extend this logic to argue that this perception of warmth in turn mediates CSR and CSI effects on vari- ous outcomes (i.e., CSR rewards and CSI penalties— theeffectsforCSRandCSIaboveandbelowthecontrol condition). Importantly, we then argue that different perceptions of warmth and competence of the organi- zation from other sources (in this paper, based on its country of origin) moderate CSR rewards and CSI penalties depending on the (mis)alignment of CSR strategy with the (home country) stereotype. Using three experiments with 774 participants and the warmth and competence variation across five coun- tries (i.e., the United States, Sweden, Germany, Portu- gal, Pakistan), we show that warmth and competence perceptions explain causal effects of CSR and CSI on purchase intentions, price, reputation, and quality as- sessments. Moreover, consistent with the stereotype content model (SCM), firms from high-warmth coun- tries pay a higher price for CSI than firms from low- warmth countries (Germany, Pakistan), yet this effect changes when combined with perceptions of high competence (the United States, Sweden). Our study has several important theoretical im- plications for the management literature on CSR, social cognition, and international business, as well as practical applications. First, we develop a deeper understanding of the microfoundations of CSR and CSI—by examining their social perception by in- dividuals. We extend existing individual-level CSR research that relies on a different set of theoretical frameworks, such as system justification theory (Hafenbrädl & Waeger, 2016), organizational justice, social influence, needs, and self-determination the- ories (Aguinis & Glavas, 2012), by drawing from the
  • 7. social psychology research on social perception and stereotypes and arguing that CSR influences and is influenced by two fundamental dimensions of social perception—warmth and competence—which me- diate and moderate the effects of CSR (and CSI) on different outcomes. Our work is important because, in comparison to objective CSR ratings, the “sub- jective” evaluations of CSR likely matter more for individual reactions to CSR (Rupp et al., 2013), 1610 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal which in turn lead to different outcomes (Gond et al., 2017). For example, employees’ exposure to CSR initiatives does not directly translate into favorable CSR attitudes (Glavas & Godwin, 2013). Thus, it is important to understand how the social perception of CSR is formed more generally. Second, we advance prior literature examining different mediators and moderators of CSR at the individual level, such as trust (or whether, how, and when consumers’ perceptions of motives directly influence consumer responses to CSR) (Vlachos, Tsamakos, Vrechopoulos, & Avramidis, 2009), cus- tomer satisfaction (Luo & Bhattacharya, 2006), and organizational pride and identity (Jones, 2010; Jones et al., 2014), by conducting a multidisciplinary multi-experimental analysis of the role of warmth and competence in the CSR context. If we under- stand how the average individual perceives CSR and CSI and how these social perceptions may coincide (or diverge) with (from) their expectations (stereo- types) of organizations, we can understand more
  • 8. about how the value from CSR is generated more broadly for a variety of stakeholders. In addition, since most firms try to avoid CSI, we can understand which firms will not suffer from CSI as much as others and why. Third, while most prior studies focus on only one organizational practice, we distinguish between CSR and CSI—crucial for moving the CSR literature for- ward. Another important contribution to the CSR lit- erature, prone to endogeneity and causality issues, is empirical: by utilizing controlled experimental methods, we are able to isolate discrete mechanisms linking CSR/CSI to organizational outcomes. This gives managers unequivocal insights into which “le- vers to pull” when activating a CSR/CSI strategybased on their current organizational context, and, impor- tantly,thishelpsspecifyconditionsunderwhichCSR/ CSI does not reap the expected benefits/costs. Finally, we extend the international management literature by showing that initial social judgments about the origin of the firm may improve or worsen outcomes based on the choice of CSR strategy. This has important practi- cal implications, particularly for managers of firms that expand abroad for the first time. THEORY AND HYPOTHESES Social Perception The ability to quickly judge another individual is a fundamental evolutionary skill. It helps us to ascer- tain whether “they” are a part of our social group and to assess the goodness of their motives and compe- tence to enact these good motives (see Fiske et al.,
  • 9. 2007, for a review). Despite common beliefs that we need (and use) a wealth of knowledge to form judg- ments about other individuals, research shows that we make trait inferences spontaneously (Newman & Uleman, 1993; Winter & Uleman, 1984; Winter, Uleman, & Cunniff, 1985). For instance, when ob- serving a fictional character, Donald, help an old lady cross the street, we quickly (and somewhat permanently) conclude that Donald is a kind and helpful individual—despite our having only limited information about him. Related work on “thin slic- ing” shows that a mere 30 seconds watching a college professor teach is sufficient to predict their end-of- term teaching evaluations (see Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992, for a review). Thus, despite the wealth of social information at our disposal, individuals primarily rely on quick social judgments of others, and filter all subsequent social information based—barring a major behavioral change—on the first impression of someone (Newman & Uleman, 1993; Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975). Many of these quick social judgments about other individuals result from stereotypes. Stereotypes are cognitive beliefs about the characteristics of another group (Fiske, 1998). Fiske and colleagues (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007; Fiske et al., 2002) identified two fundamental dimensions of all stereo- types used to evaluate people and social groups: warmth and competence. The SCM posits that all social groups fit into one of four quadrants based on whether they are high or low on warmth and com- petence. Further research (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007; Fiske et al., 2002; Fiske, Xu, Cuddy, & Glick, 1999) has demonstrated the robustness of the warmth and competence dimensions, and the fact
  • 10. that they can be used to classify not only individuals but also social groups (e.g., elderly people, Jews, housewives, immigrants, and the homeless) and even national cultures. Importantly, this work on social perception has a behavioral analog, the “behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes” (BIAS) map framework (Cuddy et al., 2007). Based on the dimensions of warmth and competence, the BIAS map predicts not only cognitions derived from group stereotypes but alsospecificbehaviorstowardthe group. Warmthand competence evoke active and passive behaviors, re- spectively: in particular, while high-warmth targets are helped, low-warmth targets are harmed, and whereas high-competence targets are passively facil- itated, low-competence targets are neglected (Cuddy 2019 1611Shea and Hawn et al., 2007). Furthermore, each combination of the two trait dimensions predicts a distinct emotion or behavior toward the target: people admire those who are high in both competence and warmth, they feel contempt toward those who are low competence and low warmth, they envy those who are competent but not warm, and they pity those who are incompetent but warm (Fiske et al., 2002; Lee & Fiske, 2006). Firms as Subject to Stereotypes Although originally developed to explain personal and social group perception, the SCM has been ex- trapolated to non-human objects. Kervyn, Fiske,
  • 11. and Malone (2012), in their “brands as intentional agents” framework, showed that consumers perceive brands in the same way they perceive people. Cuddy et al. (2007) used the SCM to map social perceptions of European Union countries. Aaker et al. (2010) applied the SCM to organizations, explaining for- profit and not-for-profit firms’ success and failure, and showing that not-for-profit organizations are associated with higher warmth and lower compe- tence than for-profit firms. It is not surprising that organizations are often anthropomorphized into human beings—famously so in the movie The Corporation and in recent de- cisions by the U.S. Supreme Court, such as in the Citizens United case. In management studies, orga- nizations are often perceived to possess actions, thoughts, opinions (Knobe & Prinz, 2008), goals, tastes, styles, personalities (Pfeffer, 1981), and even attention (Ocasio, 1997). In fact, they are viewed as social actors similar to individuals precisely because the features that distinguish humans as actors are functionally equivalent to the features common to organizational actors (King, Felin, & Whetten, 2010). Yet, when compared to human beings, organizations are thought to have equal capability for agentic (i.e., competence) behavior (Gray & Wegner, 2010; Haran, 2013; Knobe & Prinz, 2008) but are less likely to be seen as experiencing emotions and feelings (Gray & Wegner, 2010). Nonetheless, a large literature in marketing dem- onstrates that people perceive brands to have per- sonalities (Aaker, 1997) and that people form “relationships” with various brands and products (Aaker & Fournier, 1995; Fournier, 1998). We build
  • 12. off this previous literature by going beyond brands and products and examining the perceptions of or- ganizational practices. Specifically, in this paper, we will compare perceptions of socially responsible and irresponsible organizations, predicated on social perception dimensions developed to classify individuals and social groups. Before we discuss how socially (ir)responsible activities may generate social perceptions of warmth and/or competence, we need to define CSR and CSI. Main Effect: CSR, CSI, and Social Perception CSR, sustainability, corporate citizenship, and other terms are generally used to describe a portfolio of socioeconomic activities, including environmen- tal, social, and corporate governance actions of the firm (Gardberg & Fombrun, 2006). Because these voluntary actions are aimed at improving social or ecological conditions (McWilliams & Siegel, 2001), many observers regard CSR as an activity that bene- fits firms, markets, and societies (Orlitzky, 2013). We propose that CSR represents organizational behavior that connotes warmth, and that, as such, it should lead to perceptions of greater warmth. Why should CSR be associated with warmth? Individuals and social groups associated with high warmth are associated with behaviors that are trustworthy and moral (Fiske et al., 2007). Additionally, warmth has both a moral dimension (e.g., good intentions) as well as a relational component (e.g., can successfully work with allies) (Goodwin, 2015). By definition, CSR in- cludes a trustworthy behavior (i.e., “responsible” in its name); furthermore, CSR involves working with
  • 13. others (e.g., stakeholders; Harrison & Freeman, 1999). Prior literature distinguishes CSR—social actions motivatedby moral obligation—from corporate social performance—social actions of firms—to highlight that CSR is a moral behavior (Baron, 2009). In turn, to distinguish between CSR and CSI, we will follow Campbell’s (2007) threshold: if corporations (a) knowingly do something that could harm their stakeholders—their investors, employees, customers, suppliers, or the local community within which they operate—and (b) do not rectify the harm caused by them (whenever it is discovered and brought to their attention), the minimum behavioral standard with re- spect to the corporation’s relationship to its stake- holders is broken, and such corporate behavior becomes socially irresponsible. For example, CSI be- havior includes using child labor, sweatshops, and polluting facilities in manufacturing operations. CSR behavior, on the other hand, includes charity, volun- teering, community engagement, fair labor practices, andenvironmentallyfriendlymanufacturingfacilities. In comparison to CSI and other organizational behav- ior, CSR behavior resembles the same set of attributes that social psychologists traditionally associate with 1612 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal being high in warmth. Therefore, as our baseline hy- pothesis, we propose: Hypothesis 1. Ceteris paribus, firms engaging in CSR (CSI) will be perceived as having higher (lower) levels of warmth.
  • 14. Mediating Effect: Spillover and Primacy Effects of Warmth Judgments Warmth perceptions have two effects that are worth discussing: halo effects (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977) and the primacy of warmth over competence. First, let us discuss the “halo effect”—that is, when the presence (or lack) of warmth spills over into our judgments of competence (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007; Singh & Teoh, 2000; Tausch, Kenworthy, & Hewstone, 2007). This effect is particularly salient when we have no information beyond that on warmth. Thus, we automatically assume—in the absence of further information—that an individual possessing warmth also possesses some degree of competence. In our setting, this will mean that, in comparison to CSI firms, in the absence of further information, be- cause CSR firms are associated with higher warmth they will also be associated with higher competence. We argue that this will be the case because the lim- ited information on CSR practices suggests to the evaluator that the organization mastered at least one skill and therefore is intelligent and competent. Competence is conceptualized as perceptions re- lated to ability, including skill, intelligence, crea- tivity, and efficacy; therefore, in comparison to firms engaging in CSI demonstrating their inability to be a corporate citizen and behave in socially responsible ways, the perception of competence will be greater for firms engaging in CSR. This prediction is consistent with recent work in consumer research: products of companies engaged
  • 15. in prosocial activities are perceived as performing better—due to the moral undertone of the com- pany’s motivation for engaging in socially re- sponsible behavior; more importantly, this effect holds even when consumers can directly observe and experience the product and when the acts of social goodwill are unrelated to the company’s core business (Chernev & Blair, 2015). Another alterna- tive explanation for why CSR engagement may be perceived to be high in competence is that, in com- parison with CSI, it can improve firm reputation, performance, and other outcomes (Choi & Wang, 2009; Yoon et al., 2006) traditionally associated with competence. A counterargument will require a discussion of the purpose of the firm (Friedman, 1970), market actors as social evaluators, and more information on other or- ganizational practices, activities, or performance. For example,ifevaluatorsweremarketactors(particularly in earlier years), they might have assumed that firms engaging in CSR were less competent because CSR investment requires diverting scarce resources from other more strategic or core business activities of the firm. However, in comparison to CSI, recent strategy work shows that CSR engagement is now making or- ganizations more competent because they perform better even in financial markets (Cheng, Ioannou, & Serafeim, 2013; Eccles, Ioannou, & Serafeim, 2014; Ioannou & Serafeim, 2015). Therefore, due tothe “halo effect” of warmth, as well as other alternative expla- nations listed above, we propose: Hypothesis 2. Ceteris paribus, firms engaging in CSR (CSI) will be perceived as having higher (lower) levels of competence.
  • 16. Second, let us discuss the primacy effect of warmth judgment. Although the SCM is predicted on two dimensions—warmth and competence—when it comes to forming a social perception, a large body of research highlights the primacy of warmth (Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1997; Kenworthy & Tausch, 2008; Wojciszke, Bazinska, & Jaworski, 1998). That is, we anchor on perceptions of warmth and adjust, albeit insufficiently, based on percep- tions of competence. At the individual level, this happens because information needed to determine warmth (e.g., facial expressions, interpersonal skills) is simply more readily available during the begin- ning of social interactions than information needed to determine competence (e.g., skills, knowledge, and abilities). Hence, when it comes to initial social judgments, warmth information carries more weight than competence information (Cuddy et al., 2008: 89–92; Singh & Teoh, 2000; Tausch et al., 2007). At the level of organizations, we argue that this primacy effect will be important in the relationship between CSR and organizational outcomes, partic- ularly those that involve a single immediate evalua- tion or a first encounter with the firm, because, just like at the individual level, information needed to determine warmth is simply more readily available during the beginning of social interactions with this social actor (King et al., 2010) than information needed to determine competence. CSR studies have long examined the effect of CSR on firm performance (Cochran & Wood, 1984; Waddock & Graves, 1997) among other outcomes (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001; 2019 1613Shea and Hawn
  • 17. Turban&Greening,1997),generallyfindingasignificant positive effect (Margolis, Elfenbein, & Walsh, 2009). We distinguish between outcomes that involve a single immediate evaluation (i.e., a purchase in- tention or reputation assessment based on the first encounter with the firm) and those involving an ongoing evaluation (e.g., by market analysts and investors who collect information over time, accu- mulate more information contributing to a compe- tence perception of the firm, and value competence over warmth in their judgment). We argue that, for first-time encounters, the warmth perception that CSR generates could in fact act as the mechanism by which CSR affects these outcomes. In particular, we suggest that, in the ab- sence of further information, the primacy of warmth over competence will play a mediating role in the relationship between CSR and the outcome of the first encounter. We previously argued that, when individuals evaluate CSR, they perceive greater levels of warmth and, by default, competence than when they evaluate CSI (Hypotheses 1 and 2). Now, we argue that, when observers make single imme- diate evaluations of CSR/CSI behavior, they will pay more attention to the warmth than to the competence perception. There are three reasons for this. First, cognitively, people prove more sensitive to warmth information than to competence information. Sec- ond, they judge warmth faster than they do compe- tence (Cuddy et al., 2008: 90). Third, higher warmth leads to higher levels of helping behavior (Cuddy et al., 2007); in an organizational context, help can be
  • 18. conceptualized as positive evaluations and willing- ness to purchase from the firm. Therefore: Hypothesis 3. Warmth perceptions will mediate the relationship between CSR/CSI and outcomes that involve single immediate evaluation. Moderating Effect: Stereotype Fulfillment and Violation While Hypotheses 1–3 posit CSR as a source of warmth and, by default, competence perception, and warmth as a potential mediator between CSR and outcomes that involve single immediate eval- uation, we next discuss how other sources of warmth perception in an organizational context can interact with CSR and potentially moderate the re- lationship between CSR/CSI and organizational outcomes. In doing so, we assume, based on prior literature, that engaging in CSR leads to rewards (Margolis et al., 2009) and engaging in CSI leads to penalties for the focal organization (Salaiz, 2016). Thus, we theorize about organizational contexts in which the rewards/penalties to a CSR/CSI strategy are amplified/mitigated, answering the following question: “Is it always useful to engage in CSR and/ or avoid CSI?” We propose that the answer to this question is grounded in the literature on stereotype fulfillment and violation. When we evaluate people from other social groups or cultures, stereotypes act as cognitive shortcuts. For instance, we meet a rule-abiding, for- mal German and our stereotype of German people is confirmed (i.e., high competence, low warmth;
  • 19. Cuddy et al., 2007). But, what happens when we meet a bubbly, scatterbrained German? Social judg- ments broadly fall into two categories: assimilation and contrast (Biernat, 2005; Newman & Uleman, 1993; Sherif & Hovland, 1961). “Assimilation” oc- curs when we judge the target according to the held stereotype (e.g., rule-abiding, formal German), and “contrast” occurs when we differentiate the target from our traditionally held stereotype (e.g., bubbly, scatterbrained German). In the absence of contra- dictory evidence, assimilation is relatively auto- matic (Dijksterhuis, Spears, & Lépinasse, 2001); otherwise, contrast takes place. The effect of contrasting social judgments can be both positive and negative. For instance, professionals—perceived as highly competent— receive differential treatment based on their gender after having a baby (Cuddy et al., 2004). Professional women are seen as less competent and do not expe- rience a boost in perceived warmth from mother- hood to make up for their newly perceived lack of competence. Men, on the other hand, receive in- creased perceptions of warmth and maintain their competence, gaining a net benefit from parenthood (Cuddy et al., 2004). Likewise, agentic women— violating the female stereotype—experience a back- lash when they apply for feminized jobs, whereas prototypical women do not (Rudman & Glick, 1999, 2001). These studies suggest that additional infor- mation on other sources of warmth and/or compe- tence perception for the subject of interest (e.g., gender) can help explain the benefits and detriments of certain behavior. The mechanism is that additional information may strengthen the original stereotype through assimilation, or change it through contrast.
  • 20. In order to examine under what conditions CSR and CSI behaviors generate greater (lower) out- comes, we add another piece of information about the firm that exogenously changes perceptions of its warmth and competence. As discussed in Studies 2 and 3, we achieve this by adding cues on the firm’s 1614 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal origin; Cuddy and colleagues (2007) portrayed ste- reotypes of countries on the two dimensions of warmth and competence, finding significant varia- tion. In this section, to further our understanding of CSR as a warmth strategy, we mainly focus on the warmth dimension of the firm (or its origin); this helps to disentangle the mediating or supplemental warmth effects, if any, of CSR. This also helps ad- dress a potential reverse-causality issue: if firms perceived to be high in warmth are the ones who engage in CSR in the first place, this section of our theory (and analysis) helps to identify the leftover effect, if any, of CSR as a warmth strategy. We are interested in how social perception of the firm based on other sources of information (i.e., its origin) moderates CSR and CSI outcomes. In par- ticular, if firms that engage in CSR are perceived to be higher in warmth and this warmth perception mediates the effect of CSR on our outcomes (Hy- potheses 1 and 3), does an exogenous increase (decrease) in warmth of the firm help improve (at- tenuate) these outcomes for firms engaging in CSI (CSR)? We suggest that, because of assimilation
  • 21. with the existing stereotype (i.e., a match between low-warmth country of origin and low-warmth practice of CSI), firms perceived to be low in warmth will be forgiven for CSI behavior, and, therefore, will not be punished as harshly. High- warmth firms, on the other hand, assimilate with CSR, not CSI, so, when they engage in socially ir- responsible practices, contrast occurs and the pen- alty is high (i.e., a mismatch between high-warmth country of origin and low-warmth practice of CSI results in greater punishment). A similar logic applies to CSR: if a high-warmth firm engages in CSR, this demonstrates stereotypical behavior for this kind of a firm (i.e., assimilation occurs); hence, the benefits are mediocre. If a low- warmth firm engages in CSR, on the other hand, that contradicts the stereotype and, as a result, generates greater rewards from the surprised but positive re- action of the firm’s observers. Importantly, stereo- type match and violation based on the fit between the social perception of the firm and its behavior (CSR/ CSI) help advance our understanding of the cogni- tive mechanisms behind the efficacy of CSR in- vestments, suggesting that not all firms engaging in CSR/CSI reap the same benefits/costs. Hypothesis 4. The higher the perception of warmth of the firm, the higher the penalties for CSI. Hypothesis 5. The higher the perception of warmth of the firm, the lower the rewards for CSR. Our key hypothesis is that CSR is predominantly perceived as influencing warmth. However, individ- uals do not only evaluate organizations on warmth
  • 22. but also competence, and thus we need to consider competence in our predictions. Prior research found that, although not-for-profit organizations were per- ceived to be high in warmth, consumers were less willing to buy from them unless they perceived them to be highly competent (Aaker et al., 2010). Therefore, we speculate that the presence of high (low) levels of perceived competence in addition to high (low) warmth may affect the above relationships. As we consider CSR and CSI, it is important to ex- amine how the valence of these actions interacts with both warmth and competence. While warmth has a moral dimension (e.g., good intentions) and a re- lational component (e.g., can successfully work with allies) (Goodwin, 2015), competence relates directly to ability to execute a plan, positive or negative. In the case of CSI, organizations are behaving negatively, and we know that negative news generates stronger ob- serverreactionsthanpositiveeventsintheCSRcontext (Lange & Washburn, 2012). As per Hypothesis 1, these negative events should bear negatively on warmth, particularly the moral aspects of warmth (Goodwin, Piazza, & Rozin, 2014), thus canceling out the positive effects of warmth on organizational outcomes. More- over, negative behaviors are diagnostic of competence (Fiske et al., 2002), or lack thereof, and, per Hypothesis 2, CSI should decrease competence evaluations. Thus, a firm caught engaging in CSI will be perceived to be low on both competence and warmth. However, when additional information on warmth and competence stemming from other sources than organizational behavior (e.g., origin) is revealed, it may change the observers’ reaction to CSI, and, in fact, if the levels of both warmth and competence are
  • 23. high from this additional source, it could potentially buffer the firm against negative effects of CSI (Koh, Qian, & Wang, 2014; Shiu & Yang, 2017). This is once again due to the stereotype fulfillment and violation mechanism: observers expect high-competence– high-warmth firms to solve CSI issues much faster than low-competence–high-/low-warmth firms, and therefore do not punish them as harshly for CSI. Therefore, high competence and warmth of the home country of the firm that engages in CSI will buffer it from the negative effects of its poor behavior when we assess both dimensions of social perception (compared to firms high only on warmth). In turn, in the case of CSR, if a firm from a low- competence–low-warmth country is found to engage in CSR, observers may perceive it as an exemplar in 2019 1615Shea and Hawn that country (due to the stereotype violation on both warmthandcompetencedimensions).Asaresult,this perception will increase their helping behaviors and positive evaluations toward such firms because they aredoingthe“mostgood”(namely,helpingthosewho need it the most). Moreover, becauseCSR engagement is costly—as it may divert scarce resources from other more strategic or core business activities of the firm (Friedman, 1970)—engaging in CSR when low in both warmth and competence provides a stronger positive signal compared to firms low only on warmth. To summarize, we predict that the simultaneous pres- ence of high warmth and high competence will lower the negative impact of CSI (attenuating Hypothesis 4),
  • 24. while the simultaneous presence of low warmth and low competence will amplify the positive effect of CSR (strengthening Hypothesis 5). Hypothesis 6. The perception of competence of the firm moderates the relationships in Hypotheses 4 and 5, such that (a) the higher the perception of warmth and competence of the firm (as opposed to lower), the lower the penalties for CSI, and (b) the lower the perception of warmth and competence (as opposed to higher), the higher the rewards to CSR. METHODS Given the plethora of empirical challenges in CSR research, it was important to conduct experiments to examine and establish the causal links between CSR/CSI, social perceptions, and different out- comes. Experiments allowed us to make causal statements based on control of the environment (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002), making them ideal for testing our theory. They helped isolate our theoretical mechanisms and, therefore, advance theory (Haslam & McGarty, 2004; Mook, 1983). Moreover, as we test our hypotheses using multiple paradigms and outcomes, multiple experiments allow for conceptual replication and extension, highlighting the robustness of the effects. We con- ducted three experiments. In Study 1, we manipulated CSR and CSI to ex- amine perceptions of warmth and competence as well as such outcomes as reputation and purchase intentions. In addition, thanks to the temporal order in our experimental design, we were able to infer warmth mediation. In Study 2, owing to the differ-
  • 25. ences in the domain of the country of origin— organizational context—we were able to manipulate warmth and competence of the organization, and examine them as moderators in the relationship be- tween CSR, CSI, and firm outcomes. Specifically, we examined four countries representing each quadrant ofthewarmth–competenceBIASmap(i.e.,theUnited States, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan) and three conditions (i.e., CSR, CSI, and control). Finally, in Study 3, we replicated and extended these effects in a behavioral experiment measuring helping and pur- chasing behaviors using a within-subjects design. STUDY 1 Study 1 examines the relationship between CSR activities and perceptions of warmth and compe- tence (Hypotheses 1 and 2). It also examines whether warmth and competence serve as mediating mech- anisms between CSR and important organizational outcomes (Hypothesis 3), such as purchase inten- tions (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001) and reputation (Wagner, Lutz, & Weitz, 2009). Sample and Procedures Participants and design. One hundred and two participants (66 males, meanage 5 31, SD 5 10.11) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Horton, Rand, & Zeckhauser, 2011; see also O’Reilly, Robinson, Berdahl, & Banki, 2015, for recent management re- search using this data source; Paolacci & Chandler, 2014) in exchange for $0.30 in Amazon credits. All participants were based in the United States and
  • 26. were employed full-time. All participants passed attention filters embedded in the survey (i.e., “I will choose ‘disagree’ to demonstrate that I am paying attention”), and all participants had a unique IP ad- dress located within the United States. We used a one-factor design that assigned participants to either a CSR, CSI, or control condition randomly. Procedure. Participants were told that they would read a brief business scenario and provide their opinions. Participants completed the study online. CSR manipulation. Participants read about “Company X,” a fictitious company that engaged in either CSR or CSI activities, involving fair/unfair manufacturing processes overseas—the vignette had been established in prior literature (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001). We also included a control condition with no information on CSR (see Appendix A for full vignettes). Measures Warmth and competence. Immediately following the CSR manipulation, participants rated whether 1616 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal they believed Company X to possess various attri- butes. Specifically, they rated whether Company X was tolerant, warm, good natured, and sincere (Fiske et al., 2002), and competent, confident, independent, competitive, and intelligent (Fiske et al., 2002). These items were randomly embedded in other, unrelated
  • 27. attribute items. Confirmatory factor analysis sup- ported a two-factor solution that explained 72.2% of the variance. The four warmth items loaded onto Factor1withloadingsrangingfrom.80to.85.Thefive competence items loaded onto Factor 2 with loadings ranging from .77 to .84. The internal consistency co- efficient of the warmth and competence items, mea- sured using Cronbach’s alpha, were .93 and .86 respectively. In the control condition, warmth and competence were not correlated (r 5 .19, p 5 .26). Additional dependent variables. After rating Company X on the dimensions of warmth and com- petence, participants answered questions about two additional dependent variables. First, participants indicated their purchase intentions. Specifically, they were asked: “If the products from Company X were available for purchase, what is your likelihood of purchasing a product from Company X?” (1 5 not at all likely, 7 5 very likely) (see Chandon, Morwitz, & Reinartz, 2005, for a discussion of external val- idity). Additionally, participants rated their percep- tions of the firm’s reputation: favorable, good, pleasant, positive (reputation a 5 .97; Homer, 1995; Wagner et al., 2009). Then, participants completed another manipulation check, regarding the content of the CSR scenario, and provided demographic data. Results Hypothesis 1: Warmth. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that participants who were assigned to CSR, CSI, or control manipulations were more likely to judge the firm as possessing significantly different levels of warmth, F(2, 99) 5
  • 28. 133.43, p , .000, hp 2 5 .73 (see Table 1). Results of planned comparisons indicated that participants assigned to the CSR condition (M 5 4.33, SD 5 0.58) were more likely to view the firm as warm than were participants assigned to the CSI condition (M 5 2.02, SD 5 0.61), F(1, 67) 5 265.25, p , .000, hp 2 5 .80, d 5 3.95, 95% CI [3.74, 4.17], or to our control condition (M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.59), F(1, 67) 5 66.71, p , .000, hp 2 5 .50,d5 1.96,95%CI[1.76,2.18].Moreover,participants in the control condition perceived the firm as having significantly higher levels of warmth than did those in the CSI condition, F(1, 64) 5 64.32, p , .000, hp 2 5 .50, d5 2.00,95%CI[1.79,2.22].Themeansaredisplayedin Table 1 and provide support for Hypothesis 1. Hypothesis 2: Competence. A one-way ANOVA showed that participants who were assigned to CSR, CSI, or control manipulations were more likely to judge the firm as possessing significantly different levels of competence, F(2, 99) 5 15.65, p , .000, hp 2 5 .24 (see Table 1). Results of planned comparisons in- dicated that participants assigned to the CSR condi- tion (M 5 4.14, SD 5 0.48) were more likely to view the firm ascompetent thanwereparticipants assigned to the CSI condition (M 5 3.30, SD 5 0.88), F(1, 67) 5 24.68, p , .000, hp
  • 29. 2 5 .27, d 5 1.21, 95% CI [1.03, 1.52],and thecontrol condition (M 5 3.93, SD5 0.50), F(1, 67) 5 3.18, p 5 .08, hp 2 5 .05, d 5 0.44, 95% CI [0.26, 0.61], while participants in the control condi- tion perceived the firm as having significantly higher levels of competence than did participants in the CSI condition, F(1, 64) 5 12.70, p 5 .001, hp 2 5 .17, d 5 1.28, 95% CI [1.10, 1.46]. The means are displayed in Table 1 and provide support for Hypothesis 2. Reputation. To check the validity of our assump- tion that CSR generates rewards and CSI generates penalties (Margolis et al., 2009; Salaiz, 2016), we ran the same analysis on Reputation. Participants who were assigned to CSR, CSI, or control manipulations TABLE 1 Summary of Results for Experiment 1 Condition Study 1 (one-way ANOVA) Warmth (1–5 scale) Competence (1–5 scale) Purchase Intentions (1–7 scale) Reputation (1–5 scale) CSR 4.33 (0.58)*** 4.14 (0.48)*** 5.61 (1.15)*** 4.57 (0.63)*** CSI 2.02 (0.61)*** 3.3 (0.88)*** 2.7 (1.38)*** 1.84 (0.73)*** Control 3.2 (0.59)*** 3.93 (0.5)* 4.82 (1.03)** 3.48 (0.72)***
  • 30. Notes: Table 1 shows means with standard deviations in brackets. CSR 5 corporate social responsibility; CSI 5 corporate social irrespon- sibility (see Appendix A for scenarios). *p , .10 **p , .05 ***p , .01 2019 1617Shea and Hawn perceived significantly different levels of firm reputa- tion, F(2, 99) 5 134.71, p , .000, hp 2 5 .73 (see Table 1). Results of planned comparisons indicated that partici- pants assigned to the CSR condition (M 5 4.57, SD 5 0.63) perceived the firm as having a better reputation than did participants assigned to the CSI condition (M5 1.84,SD5 0.73),F(1,67)5 279.94,p, .000,hp 2 5 .81, d 5 4.07, 95% CI [3.85, 4.33], or to our control condition (M 5 3.48, SD 5 0.72), F(1, 67) 5 45.12, p , .000, hp 2 5 .40, d 5 1.64, 95% CI [1.41, 1.90]. Partici- pants in the control condition perceived the firm as having a significantly better reputation than did partic- ipants in the CSI condition, F(1, 64) 5 83.73, p , .000, hp 25 .57,d5 2.30,95%CI[2.04,2.56].Resultsfromthis
  • 31. test support our assumption that rewards are above and penalties are below the control condition—see Table 1. Purchase intentions. Similarly,aone-wayANOVA showed that participants who were assigned to CSR, CSI, or control manipulations were more likely to report different levels of purchase intentions, F(2, 99) 5 52.15, p , .000, hp 2 5 .513 (see Table 1). Results of planned comparisons indicated that partic- ipants assigned to the CSR condition (M 5 5.61, SD 5 1.15) were more likely to have higher purchase in- tentions than participants assigned to the CSI condi- tion (M 5 2.70, SD 5 1.38), F(1, 67) 5 91.11, p , .000, hp 2 5 .58, d5 2.33,95%CI[1.92, 2.82]ortoourcontrol condition (M 5 4.82, SD 5 1.03), F(1, 67) 5 8.49, p 5 .005, hp 2 5 .11, d 5 0.74, 95% CI [0.32, 1.10]. More- over, participantsin thecontrol condition had higher purchase intentions than participants in the CSI condition, F(1, 64) 5 47.57, p , .000, hp 2 5 .43, d 5 1.77, 95% CI [1.40, 2.26]. These results support our assumption that CSR generates rewards, and CSI penalties (i.e., rewards are above and penalties are below the control condition—see Table 1). Hypothesis 3: Warmth and competence as mediators. Using Hayes’s (2013) process macros and the bootstrap method, we ran mediational analyses, predicting two dependent variables: firm reputation
  • 32. and purchase intentions. We used the bootstrap model of Preacher and Hayes (2004) to estimate the indi- rect effect of warmth and competence based on 10,000 bootstrap samples. This method is preferred to the tra- ditional method proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986), as it does not rely on the assumption that the sampling distribution of the mediation effect is normal. We un- dertook multiple sets of analyses. First, we looked at mediation of CSR versus Control, and CSI versus Con- trol for warmth while controlling for competence1 to predict reputation and purchase intentions.2 Next, we looked at a sequential mediation model between warmth, competence, reputation, and purchase in- tentions. This allowed us to examine the distinct effects of warmth and competence in both socially responsible and irresponsible firms. Results for reputation. We first constructed two mediation models in which CSR versus control, CSI versus control predicted reputation, with warmth as a mediator (controlling for competence).3 We then examined the indirect pathway comparing CSR and control (i.e., CSR influences reputation through in- creased warmth). The 95% CI for the bootstrap esti- mation of the indirect pathway did not overlap with 0 when examining warmth in the CSR condition, b 5 0.91, 95% CI [0.59, 1.19], indicating mediation.4 Next, we examined the indirect pathway comparing CSI and control (i.e., CSI influences reputation through decreased warmth controlling for compe- tence). The 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway did not overlap with 0 when ex- amining warmth in the CSI condition, b 5 20.95, 95% CI [21.28, 20.67].5 This indicates that warmth
  • 33. mediates the relationship between CSR/CSI and firm reputation, further substantiating Hypothesis 3. Results for purchase intentions. We constructed two mediation models in which CSR versus control, CSI versus control predicted purchase intentions, with warmth as mediator controlling for compe- tence.6 First, we examined the indirect pathway comparing CSR and control (i.e., CSR influences 1 We footnote results for mediations with both warmth and competence as simultaneous mediators. 2 We code such that the control condition is the refer- ence group in each separate analysis. 3 When examined as joint predictors with the three ex- perimental conditions, warmth, b 5 0.75, t 5 2.63, p 5 .009, predicted reputation, while competence, b 5 0.04, t 5 0.44, p 5 .44, did not. 4 According to the same analysis where Competence serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, a significant in- direct effect emerges for warmth, b 5 0.83, 95% CI [0.51, 1.10], but the 95% CI for competence contains 0, b 5 0.05, 95% CI [20.02, 0.16], suggesting warmth, not competence, mediates this relationship. 5 According to the same analysis where Competence serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, a significant in- direct effect emerges for warmth, b 5 20.93, 95% CI [21.28, 20.66], but the 95% CI for competence contains 0, b 5 20.02, 95% CI [20.15, 0.12], suggesting warmth, not competence, mediates this relationship. 6 When examined as joint predictors with the three ex-
  • 34. perimental conditions, both warmth, b 5 0.50, t 5 2.48, p 5 .01, and competence, b 5 0.73, t 5 3.99, p 5 .001, predicted purchase intentions. 1618 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal Purchase Intentions through increased warmth controlling for competence). The estimation of the indirect effect did not overlap with 0, b 5 0.83, 95% CI [0.15, 1.46], indicating mediation of purchase in- tentions by warmth.7 Next, we completed the same analyses for CSI versus control (i.e., CSI influences Purchase Intentions through decreased warmth controlling for competence). The estimation of the indirect effect did not overlap with 0, b 5 20.98, 95% CI [21.84, 20.33], indicating mediation of purchase intentions by warmth.8 Sequential mediation. We constructed sequential mediation models including reputation (a cognitive evaluation) to predict purchase intentions (a behav- ioral intention). We examined purchase intentions as the final dependent variable as it is the most dis- tant variable and represents a behavioral intention. We also tested primacy of warmth assumptions by adding in competence as a sequential mediator fol- lowing warmth. We examined the indirect pathway (i.e., CSR/CSI influences purchase intentions through warmth, competence, and reputation; PROCESS, Model 6). Using three mediators, seven potential in- direct effects were calculated. The 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway between CSR → Warmth → Competence → Purchase Inten- tions excluded 0, b 5 0.34, 95% CI [0.09, 0.64], in-
  • 35. dicating sequential mediation. Likewise, the 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway between CSR → Warmth → Reputation → Purchase Intentions excluded 0, b 5 0.64, 95% CI [0.19, 1.11], indicating sequential mediation as well. No other in- direct effects excluded 0. Next, we examined the indirect pathway with CSI. The 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the in- direct pathway between CSI → Warmth → Compe- tence → Purchase Intentions excluded 0, b 5 20.49, 95%CI[20.82,20.19], indicatingsequential mediation. Likewise, the 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway between CSI → Warmth → Reputation → Purchase Intentions excluded 0, b 5 20.81, 95% CI [21.42, 20.43], indicating se- quential mediation as well. No other indirect effects excluded 0. We ran equivalent models changing the order of the potential mediators. Importantly, ana- lyses in which competence preceded warmth in the model did not produce indirect effects excluding 0, suggesting the primacy of warmth as a mediating mechanism in our data set. Overall, our results indi- cate that warmth consistently mediates the relation- ship between CSR/CSI and purchase intentions (on its own and when predicting Purchase intentions in conjunction with Reputation), whereas competence does not consistently mediate it, supporting Hy- pothesis 3. Discussion Study 1 provides support for Hypotheses 1 through 3. When “Company X” engaged in CSR activities, it was judged to be significantly higher in warmth than
  • 36. both control and CSI firms (supporting Hypothesis 1). In addition, per traditional halo effects in psycholog- ical research (Ross & Nisbett, 1991) as well as other alternative explanations, we observed a positive re- lationship between CSR and competence ratings (supporting Hypothesis 2). However, although ratings of competence differed between CSR and CSI condi- tions, the control condition was not significantly dif- ferent from the CSR condition, p . .08, indicating that competence may not be directly related to the pres- ence of CSR activities but is rather related to their absence (i.e., CSI). This finding is consistent with Fiske et al. (2007), which found that negative behav- iors are more indicative of competence while positive behaviors are more indicative of warmth. Next, we established further evidence of CSR as a warmth strategy in our mediational analyses. Specifically, warmth mediates the relationship between CSR/CSI and the control condition to predict reputation and purchase intentions (supporting Hypothesis 3). When examined as a sequential mediator, warmth is nec- essary to mediate the relationship between CSR/CSI and purchase intention while including measures of competence and reputation. This evidence confirms previous research on the primacy of warmth when forming social judgments (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007; Singh & Teoh, 2000; Tausch et al., 2007). Although this study represents, to our knowledge, one of the first empirical links between corporate strategy and social perception, many questions re- main. These results are about “Company X”—a fic- titious organization used to maintain experimental 7 According to the same analysis where Competence serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, the 95% CI for the
  • 37. bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway did overlap with 0 when examining warmth and competence together in the CSR condition, bwarmth 5 0.43, 95% CI [20.18, 1.008]; bcompetence 5 0.20, 95% CI [20.02, 0.53]. 8 According to the same analysis where Competence serves as a joint mediator with Warmth, the 95% CI for the bootstrap estimation of the indirect pathway did overlap with 0 when examining warmth and competence together in the CSI condition, bwarmth 5 20.47, 95% CI [21.29, 0.07], but not for competence, bcompetence 5 20.50, 95% CI [20.86, 20.19]. 2019 1619Shea and Hawn control. Typically, we know more about an organi- zation when making a purchase and evaluating firm reputation (e.g., country of origin, size, status/brand of the firm). Much of this auxiliary information can be related to the dimensions of warmth and compe- tence. Our results so far suggest that, just like ste- reotypes of organizations (Aaker et al., 2010), stereotypes of organizational practices indeed exist, and warmth and competence are in fact organizing dimensions that help individuals categorize com- panies and their strategies (i.e., CSR or CSI). More- over, warmth perceptions mediate the relationship between CSR/CSI and outcomes. However, if this is the case, do warmth and competence perceptions of organizations from other sources of evaluation color the way in which CSR and CSI are evaluated? Could they shift individual willingness to buy or evaluation of firm reputation or other behavioral outcomes of CSR and CSI? Experiments 2 and 3 were designed to
  • 38. address these questions. STUDY 2 Study 1 examined the relationship between a firm’s CSR activities and perceptions of warmth and com- petence, establishing CSR as a warmth strategy via mediation. Study 2 shifts our focus to the warmth– competence moderating effect: in particular, do warmth and competence perceptions of the organi- zation from other sources (i.e., country of origin) moderate the effects of CSR (CSI) on different out- comes? In other words, under what conditions do firms achieve greater rewards (penalties) for CSR (CSI) in terms of favorable (unfavorable) evalua- tions? To test this, we manipulated the perception of the organization by changing a firm’s country of or- igin to reflect different levels of warmth and com- petence (i.e., high or low). Just as the national origin of immigrants guides majority members’ perceptions of them (Lee & Fiske, 2006), we expect the country of origin to affect the social perception of the firm and of its strategic choices. Sample and Procedures Participants and design. Five hundred and seventy-two participants (357 males, meanage 5 32.13, SD 5 11.12) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (Buhrmester et al., 2011) in ex- change for $0.50 in Amazon credits. To avoid the out-of-group bias (e.g., rating a U.S. firm differently if you were based outside the United States), all par- ticipants were based in the United States and passed attention filters embedded in the survey. We used a
  • 39. three-factor design that randomly assigned partici- pants to a CSR state (i.e., CSR, CSI, or control), country-of-origin warmth (i.e., high, low), and com- petence (i.e., high, low). We also included a pure control condition that did not manipulate country of origin; its inclusion did not change the significance or pattern of our results. However, because this ex- periment tests Hypotheses 4 through 6 using warmth and competence perceptions of the countries of ori- gin and the control condition does not specify any country (and, hence, warmth or competence), we did not include it in the final analyses. Procedure. Participants completed the study online. They were asked to read a brief business scenario and to provide their opinions by rating the warmth and competence of the firm’s country of or- igin as well as their perceptions of the firm based on the CSR vignette. CSR manipulation. Participants read the same vignettes as in Study 1 (Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001), modified to manipulate warmth and competence via country of origin. Warmth and competence manipulations. Based on previous research, we chose the in-group to represent the high-warmth, high-competence country (in our sample, the United States, as all participants were based in the United States) (e.g. Cuddy et al., 2008). We chose Germany (low warmth, high competence) and Portugal (high warmth, low competence) as the two countries far- thest from each other on the BIAS map (see Cuddy et al., 2007). Finally, because there was no Euro- pean low–low country and it has the lowest ranking
  • 40. on the BIAS map, we chose Pakistan as our low- warmth, low-competence country (see Cuddy et al., 2007). To manipulate warmth and competence, we changed the name of the organization in the vi- gnette to include the country of origin: thus, U.S. Tech Corp., Pakistan Tech Corp., German Tech Corp., Portugal Tech Corp., and Company X for the control condition. Measures Measures of warmth and competence. Partici- pants completed the same warmth and competence items as in Study 1 (warmth, a 5 .94; competence, a 5 .84) (Fiske et al., 2002). In our pure control condition (no CSR/CSI, or country of origin), we performed a confirmatory factor analysis, which supported a two-factor solution that explained 75.69% of the variance. The four warmth items 1620 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal loaded onto Factor 1 with loadings ranging from .73 to .97. The five competence items loaded onto Factor 2 with loadings ranging from .40 to .92. Warmth and competence were not significantly correlated in our control condition, r 5 .35, p 5 .12. Additional dependent variables. After rating the firm on the dimensions of warmth and competence, participants answered the same questions consti- tuting our two dependent variables as in Study 1. Participants then completed a manipulation check regarding the content of the CSR scenario and pro-
  • 41. vided demographic information.9 Results Data analysis strategy. To test Hypotheses 4 and 5, we analyzed the two-way interaction effects be- tween Warmth (high, low) and CSR/CSI conditions (vs. control), collapsing across firm competence. To test Hypothesis 6, we conducted pairwise compari- sons regarding whether changes in competence moderate the effects of warmth on CSR/CSI out- comes. Table 2 presents our results. Reputation. Table 2a presents the means with standard errors across all conditions predicting reputation. Table 2b collapses across our warmth conditions. We ran a three-way ANOVA to analyze the effect of Condition (CSR, CSI, Control) by Warmth (high, low) and Competence (high, low) on Reputation. A significant main effect emerged for CSR condition, F(2, 494) 5 613.94, p 5 .000, hp 2 5 .71. Interaction effects emerged between Warmth 3 Condition, Competence 3 Condition, and Warmth 3 Competence, Fs . 2.4, ps , .10, hp 2 . .009. These effects were qualified by a significant three-way in- teraction, F(2, 494) 5 4.37, p 5 .01, hp 2 5 .017. The main effect of CSR condition once again confirms our initial assumption about rewards to CSR and penal- ties to CSI: firms engaging in CSR (M 5 4.27, SD 5 0.69) attained higher levels of reputation than firms
  • 42. engaging in CSI (M 5 1.67, SD 5 0.79), F(1, 351) 5 1068.95, p 5 .00, hp 2 5 .75, d 5 3.52, 95% CI [3.41, 3.63], and control firms (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.69), F(1, 331) 5 83.64, p 5 .00, hp 2 5 .20, d 5 1.00, 95% CI [0.90, 1.11). Control firms had higher reputation than CSI firms, F(1, 326) 5 530.55, p 5 .00, hp 2 5 .62, d 5 2.58, 95% CI [2.48, 2.70] (Figure 1a and Figure 1b). To test Hypothesis 4 (comparing CSI outcomes across high- and low-warmth firms), a two-way ANOVA was run to analyze the relative effect of Condition (CSI, control) by Warmth (high, low) on Reputation. A main effect for CSI emerged, TABLE 2b Mean Levels of Reputation by Warmth (Hypotheses 4 and 5) CSI Control CSR High Warmth 1.56 (0.07) 3.63 (0.07) 4.25 (0.08) Low Warmth 1.78 (0.09) 3.52 (0.09) 4.30 (0.07) Notes: Reputation was measured on a 5-point scale. Standard errors in brackets. TABLE 2a Mean Levels of Reputation by Country (Hypothesis 6)
  • 43. CSI Control CSR United States (high, high) 1.60 (0.11) 3.54 (0.10) 4.16 (0.12) Germany (low, high) 1.82 (0.15) 3.85 (0.08) 4.25 (0.10) Portugal (high, low) 1.51 (0.11) 3.75 (0.10) 4.34 (0.10) Pakistan (low, low) 1.74 (0.12) 3.20 (0.14) 4.35 (0.09) Notes: Reputation was measured on a 5-point scale. Standard errors in brackets. TABLE 2 Summary of Results for Experiment 2 TABLE 2c Mean Levels of Purchase Intentions by Country (Hypothesis 6) CSI Control CSR United States (high, high) 2.32 (0.19) 4.70 (0.13) 5.43 (0.18) Germany (low, high) 2.41 (0.25) 4.81 (0.16) 5.40 (0.18) Portugal (high, low) 2.32 (0.35) 4.75 (0.17) 5.36 (0.21) Pakistan (low, low) 2.74 (0.21) 4.11 (0.18) 5.52 (0.20) Notes: Purchase Intentions was measured on a 7-point scale. Standard errors in brackets.
  • 44. TABLE 2d Mean Levels of Purchase Intentions by Warmth (Hypotheses 4 and 5) CSI Control CSR High Warmth 2.28 (0.14) 4.73 (0.11) 5.39 (0.13) Low Warmth 2.57 (0.16) 4.45 (0.12) 5.46 (0.14) Notes: Purchase Intentions was measured on a 7-point scale. Standard errors in brackets. 9 In our pure control condition, purchase intentions and reputation were not significantly correlated, r 5 .29, p 5 21. 2019 1621Shea and Hawn F(1, 324) 5 533.72, p 5 .000, hp 2 5 .62, and this effect was qualified by a significant interaction between CSI x Warmth, F(1, 324) 5 4.37, p 5 .04, hp 2 5 .01, suggesting moderation. No significant differences emerged in the control condition between high (M 5 3.63, SD 5 0.62) and low (M 5 3.52, SD 5 0.75) warmth firms, F(1, 152) 5 1.12, p 5 .29, hp 2 5 .007, d 5 0.17, 95% CI [0.03, 0.44]. However, when high-
  • 45. warmth firms engaged in CSI (M 5 1.56, SD 5 0.70), they received lower reputation evaluations than low- warmth firms (M 5 1.78, SD 5 0.88), F(1, 172) 5 3.58, p 5 .06, hp 2 5 .02, d 5 0.31, 95% CI [0.15, 0.45]. This provides support for Hypothesis 4 for reputation: high-warmth firms face a larger reputation penalty (Δ2.07) for engaging in CSI than low-warmth firms (Δ1.74). To test Hypothesis 5 (comparing CSR outcomes across high- and low-warmth firms), a two-way ANOVA was run to analyze the relative effect of Condition (CSR, control) by Warmth (high, low) on Reputation. The interaction was not significant, F(1, 328) 5 1.18, p 5 .28, hp 2 5 .004, and, thus, we do not find support for Hypothesis 5 for reputation: high- and low-warmth firms had equivalent out- comes for engaging in CSR compared to control firms. Next, to test Hypothesis 6 (comparing CSI out- comes for Germany–Pakistan, United States– Portugal), a Condition (CSI, control) by Competence (high, low) ANOVA was run within both high- and low-warmth conditions. At low levels of country warmth (Germany–Pakistan), a Condition (CSI, control) by Competence (high, low) ANOVA revealed an interaction between CSI and compe- tence, F(1, 157) 5 5.00, p 5 .02, hp 2 5 .03. Specifi- cally, while firms from Germany (M 5 1.83, SD 5
  • 46. 0.99) and Pakistan (M 5 1.74, SD 5 0.75) do not differ when they engage in CSI, F , .18, p . .67, a significant difference emerges in the control con- dition, F(1, 72) 5 16.31, p 5 .000, hp 2 5 .19, d 5 0.97, 95% CI [0.82, 1.24], such that firms from Germany (M 5 3.85, SD 5 0.47) have higher reputation than firms from Pakistan (M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.84). This suggests that competence may not buffer reputation when low-warmth firms engage in CSI in terms of the relative losses to reputation (ΔGermany 5 2.02; ΔPakistan 5 1.46). At high levels of country warmth (United States–Portugal), the interaction between CSI and competence is not significant, F , .02, p . .88; therefore, we find no support for Hypothesis 6a regarding reputation. Next, to test Hypothesis 6b (comparing CSR outcomes for Germany–Pakistan, United States– Portugal), a Condition (CSR, control) by Compe- tence (high, low) ANOVA was run within both high and low warmth conditions. At low levels of coun- try warmth (Germany–Pakistan), a Condition (CSR, control) by Competence (high, low) revealed an interaction between CSR and competence, F(1, 159) 5 12.59, p 5 .001, hp 2 5 .07, d 5 0.98, 95% CI [0.83, 1.13]. Specifically, while Germany (M 5 4.25, SD 5 0.66) and Pakistan (M 5 4.35, SD 5 0.63) do not differ when they engage in CSR, F , .18, p . .67, the interaction is driven by the relative differences (ΔGermany 5 0.41; ΔPakistan 5 1.14) gained from en- gaging in CSR versus control (Germany: M 5 3.84, SD 5 0.47; Pakistan: M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.84). When
  • 47. engaging in CSR, a firm from Pakistan, a low–low country, made higher relative gains compared to FIGURE 1a Results of Study 2: Average Effect of Warmth and Condition on Reputation 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 CSR Control CSI R ep u ta ti o
  • 48. n High Warmth Low Warmth FIGURE 1b Results of Study 2: Average Effect of Warmth and Condition on Purchase Intentions 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 CSR Control CSI P u rc h as e In te
  • 49. n ti o n s High Warmth Low Warmth 1622 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal that from Germany (low-warmth–high-competence country), suggesting that initial competence did not have an additive effect, but, rather, low–low firms are seen as exemplars and get higher returns to CSR, supporting Hypothesis 6b. At high levels of country warmth (United States–Portugal), however, the in- teraction between CSR and competence is not significant, F , .02, p . .88, suggesting that the additive benefits of competence are limited to low- warmth firms engaging in CSR (which violates the stereotype of low–low organizations). Purchase intentions. Table 2c presents the means with standard error bars across all conditions pre- dicting purchase intentions. Table 2d includes a separate plot collapsing across our warmth- competence conditions. First, a three-way ANOVA was run to analyze the effect of Condition (CSR, CSI, Control) by Warmth (high, low) and Competence (high, low) on Purchase Intentions. While the three- way interaction did not reach statistical signifi-
  • 50. cance, F(2, 494) 5 2.00, p 5 .14, hp 2 5 .008, we tested our hypotheses using a series of two-way ANOVAs reported below. A significant main effect emerged for CSR condition, F(2, 494) 5 266.50, p 5 .000, hp 2 5 .52. Once again, the main effect of CSR condition supported our assumption: firms engag- ing in CSR (M 5 5.43, SD 5 1.28) attained higher levels of purchase intentions than firms engaging in CSI (M 5 2.43, SD 5 1.42), F(1, 351) 5 434.86, p 5 .00, hp 2 5 .55, d 5 2.23, 95% CI [2.04, 2.44], and control firms (M 5 4.59, SD 5 1.01), F(1, 331) 5 42.82, p 5 .00, hp 2 5 .43, d 5 0.73, 95% CI [0.54, 0.88]. Control firms had greater purchase intentions than CSI firms, F(1, 327) 5 245.99, p 5 .00, hp 2 5 .12, d 5 1.76, 95% CI [1.61, 1.97]. To test Hypothesis 4 (comparing CSI outcomes across high and low warmth), a two-way ANOVA was run to analyze the effect of Condition (CSI, control) by Warmth (high, low) on Purchase In- tentions. A main effect for CSI emerged, F(1, 324) 5 246.41, p 5 .000, hp 2 5 .432, and this effect was qualified by a significant interaction between CSI 3 Warmth, F(1, 324) 5 4.41, p 5 .04, hp
  • 51. 2 5 .01. When firms high (M 5 2.28, SD 5 1.30) or low (M 5 2.57, SD 5 1.52) in warmth engage in CSI, purchase in- tentions are not significantly different, F(1, 172) 5 1.93, p 5 .16, hp 2 5 .01, d 5 0.21, 95% CI [0.12, 0.48]. However, marginally significant differ- ences emerged between high (M 5 4.73, SD 5 0.94) and low (M 5 4.45, SD 5 1.07) warmth firms in the control condition, F(1, 152) 5 2.95, p 5 .09, hp 2 5 .02, d 5 0.28, 95% CI [0.07, 0.52]. Thus, when high- warmth firms shift from no strategy to CSI (Δ2.45), compared to low-warmth firms (Δ1.88), they face a bigger deficit in purchase intentions. This provides support for Hypothesis 4. To test Hypothesis 5 (comparing CSR outcomes across high and low warmth), a two-way ANOVA was run to analyze the effect of Condition (CSR, control) by Warmth (high, low) on purchase in- tentions. The interaction was not significant, F(1, 328) 5 1.84, p 5 .18, hp 2 5 .006, and does not provide support for Hypothesis 5 for purchase intentions. Next, to test Hypothesis 6a (comparing CSI outcomes for Germany–Pakistan, United States– Portugal), a condition (CSI, control) by Competence (high, low) ANOVA was run within both high and low warmth conditions. At low levels of country
  • 52. warmth (Germany–Pakistan), an interaction be- tween CSI and competence emerged, F(1, 157) 5 6.18, p 5 .01, hp 2 5 .04. Specifically, while Ger- many (M 5 2.41, SD 5 1.64) and Pakistan (M 5 2.74, SD 5 1.38) do not differ in purchase intentions when they engage in CSI, F , 1.05, p . .30, a sig- nificant difference emerges in the control condi- tion, F(1, 72) 5 8.68, p 5 .004, hp 2 5 .11, d 5 0.69, 95% CI [.38, 1.05], such that Germany (M 5 4.81, SD 5 0.95) has higher purchase intentions than Pakistan (M 5 4.11, SD 5 1.09). When low-warmth firms engage in CSI, competence does not buffer the negative impact leading to greater relative decreases in purchases intentions (ΔGermany 5 2.40; ΔPakistan 5 1.37). At high levels of warmth, the interaction be- tween CSI and competence is not significant, F , .02, p . .91. Therefore, just like for reputation, we do not find support for Hypothesis 6a for purchase intentions. Next, to test Hypothesis 6b (comparing CSR outcomes for Germany–Pakistan, United States– Portugal), a Condition (CSR, control) by Compe- tence (high, low) ANOVA was run within both high and low warmth conditions. At low levels of warmth (Germany–Pakistan), an interaction be- tween CSR and competence emerged, F(1, 159) 5 5.11, p 5 .03, hp 2 5 .03, d 5 0.56, 95% CI [0.28, 0.87]. Specifically, while Germany (M 5 5.40, SD 5 1.18) and Pakistan (M 5 5.52, SD 5 1.34) do not
  • 53. differ when they engage in CSR, F , .21, p . .64, the interaction is driven by the relative differences gained (ΔGermany 5 .59; ΔPakistan 5 1.41) versus control (Germany: M 5 4.81, SD 5 0.95; Pakistan: M 5 4.11, SD 5 1.09). At high levels of warmth, the interaction between CSR and competence is not significant, F , .12, p . .77. Thus, our findings suggest that low-warmth–low-competence firms receive a larger relative benefit when engaging in 2019 1623Shea and Hawn CSR—supporting Hypothesis 6b—even though high-warmth firms do not see differences with the addition of competence. Discussion There are several implications of Study 2, with the main one being that we find no support for Hy- potheses 5 and 6a for both outcomes (reputation and purchase intentions). Hypotheses 4 and 6b demonstrated consistent results for both outcomes for firms from a low–low country (Pakistan), sug- gesting that, in comparison to Hypothesis 5, which only considers warmth perception, both high com- petence and warmth help a low–low company achieve higher rewards for CSR. Thus, the effec- tiveness of a CSR strategy is contingent on the warmth and competence perception of the organi- zation’s country of origin (Hypothesis 6b), while firms from high-warmth countries received harsher evaluations for engaging in CSI (Hypothesis 4). We theorize that these effects are driven by assimilation
  • 54. (CSI) and contrast (CSR) effects from the stereo- types associated with low-warmth countries. Im- portantly, this study provides insight into why some organizations fare better or worse in observer reactions to their corporate strategies. The attri- butes of the firm—conceptualized as country- of-origin warmth and competence—influenced important organizational perceptions, such as rep- utation and purchase intentions. In addition, Study 2 helps explore the question of endogeneity regarding whether high-warmth orga- nizations are more likely to engage in CSR behavior in the first place—to earn greater benefits from it. We find consistent marginal support for Hypothesis 4 that states that firms with higher warmth will pay a higher cost for CSI, and no support for Hypothesis 5 about lower rewards for CSR. Likewise, we do not find support for the argument that the costs for en- gaging in irresponsible behavior are lower for orga- nizations from high–high countries. In fact, firms from Pakistan (a low-warmth and low-competence country) experienced the greatest rewards for CSR engagement, due to stereotype violation. To con- clude, Study 2 demonstrates that a firm’s country of origin (manipulating warmth and competence per- ceptions) influences the success or failure of its CSR strategies relative to its baseline state. Specifically, we find that CSR can supplement for low-warmth (and low-competence) country of origin, and that low-warmth country of origin shields organizations from harsher judgments of CSI. STUDY 3 Studies 1 and 2 showed how CSR and CSI affect the
  • 55. perceived warmth and competence of an organiza- tion, andthattheseeffectsareamplifieddependingon the firms’ country of origin. Study 3 aims to replicate and extend these findings while addressing various shortcomingsof Studies 1 and 2. First,while Studies 1 and 2 measure perceptions, Study 3 uses behavioral measures (Colquitt, 2008), adding external validity to our previous measures. Specifically, participants used their own money to purchase products from firms with differing social strategies and also pro- vided help (e.g., feedback) to the organizations. Second, to ensure that our high-warmth–high- competence results are not an artifact of an in-group bias, Study 3 uses Sweden as the high-warmth– high-competence country.10 Third, we switched our CSR manipulations to a fact-sheet format that in- cluded information on both labor and environmental 10 Prior to running Study 3, we sought to validate previous research on the BIAS map (Cuddy et al., 2007) with regard to where countries fall on the warmth and competence di- mensions.Inaddition,wesoughttoreplacetheUnitedStates in the final study. Using a unique sample of 95 Mechanical Turk workers, we assessed the warmth and competence of seven countries (Germany, Iran, Pakistan, Portugal, Spain, Sweden, and the United States) previous research had clas- sified into one of the four quadrants on the BIAS map. We first asked about the warmth and competence of each country (Fiske et al., 2002) and then had participants rank order the countries in terms of their warmth and compe- tence. Pairwise comparisons, rank order, and correlational evidence support the classification in our experimental paradigm. First, ranked from highest to lowest warmth, were Sweden, the United States, Spain, Portugal, Germany, Pakistan, and Iran; and, second, ranked from highest to lowest competence were Germany, the United States, Swe-
  • 56. den, Spain, Portugal, Iran, and Pakistan. Looking at corre- lations between warmth and competence, the United States, r 5 .55, p , .000, and Sweden, r 5 .70, p , .000, are seen as high warmth and competence. Given that our study com- prises U.S. participants, we used Sweden to mitigate in- group bias concerns and “Made in America” preferences in our Study 3. Pakistan is viewed as low warmth and compe- tence,r5 .50,p, .000,ascomparedtoIran,r5 .21,p, .000, given the high correlation between warmth and competence as well as low means. Germany is viewed as high compe- tence and low warmth, r 5 .21, p 5 .04, given the smaller correlation. Portugal is viewed as high warmth and low competence. r 5 .48, p , .000, as compared to Spain, r 5 .63, p , .000; although not as cleanly manipulated, we believe that Portugal represents the better manipulation of low competence and high warmth, given the smaller correlation coefficient and previous research on the BIAS map. 1624 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal performance to ensure that the effects were not idio- syncratic to our Study 1 and 2 materials. And, finally, to provide a conservative test of our hypotheses, we modified our design to assess these behaviors within subjects; that is, all participants engaged with firms from four countries within CSR and CSI conditions. Sample and Procedures Participants. We recruited 100 participants from a paid student research lab at a private Midwestern university. We excluded one participant due to ex- pletive language in all of our written prompts, and 11 participants who failed the manipulation check. In-
  • 57. cluding these participants in analyses does not alter the direction of effects. Design. Study 3 employed a mixed design method- ology; in particular, 3(CSR, CSI, control condition) by2 (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) between-subjects factors across a four-factor country-of-origin warmth by competence (HH 5 Sweden; HL 5 Germany; LH 5 Portugal; LL 5 Pakistan) within-subjects factor. We randomized the order of our country of origin manip- ulation as a repeated measure across all conditions. Procedure. The study was advertised as a 30-min- ute product development study. Participants received $8inexchangefortheirparticipationandanadditional $1 for a purchase decision. The study asked partici- pants to test, evaluate, and make purchase decisions about a series of pens. We chose pens as an experi- mental stimulus because pens have been used suc- cessfully in previous research (e.g., Shah & Wolford, 2007). We purchased four types of pens priced $0.78–$0.83 from a wholesaler. The pens did not differ on measures of quality or looks. Upon arrival to the lab, each participant was seated at a computer station where they received a plastic bag containing four “prototype” pens to evaluate as well as $1 in coins for the pricing task (in addition to the $8 payment). They also received a notepad to test out each pen. Partici- pants completed the entire study at a computer station. Warmth–Competence manipulation. To manip- ulate country of origin we had the pens labeled as “Made in Sweden,” “Made in Germany,” “Made in Portugal,” and “Made in Pakistan” in a 1-millimeter font. We then used neutral labels on each pen to serve aslogosandtranslatedtheword“fine”intoeachof the
  • 58. four languages to put on the logo. Each participant received and rated each of these four pens as a within- subjects variable. Social responsibility manipulation. Participants were randomly assigned one of three experimental conditions: CSR, CSI or control. We provided them withbackgroundinformationabouteachofthefourpen manufacturers whose products they would be testing. Embedded within neutral performance information was information on environmental impact and labor practices of each of the manufacturers. In the CSR con- dition, participants read that all four pen manufacturers were going out of their way to promote strong environ- mental protection and repair, as well as employee standards. In the CSI condition, pen manufacturers were stated to be poor in environmental standards and not desirable in terms of their labor practices. In the control condition, participants were provided with neutral information. All companies were established in their industries and in good financial standing. Each conditionhadthesamenumberofcategoriestodescribe the organization.11 Please see Appendix B. 11 As a manipulation check, participants rated both the warmth and competence of the pen manufacturing organizations using a single-item measure (“The pen manufacturing organizations seem to be warm/compe- tent”). A three-cell (CSR, CSI, control conditions) between- subjects analysis of variance on perceptions of Warmth revealed a significant effect of social responsibility condi- tion, F(2, 81) 5 5.73, p 5 .005, hp 2 5 .12. Specifically, in- dividuals in the CSR condition (M 5 4.56, SD 5 1.41) had
  • 59. significantly higher perceptions of warmth than individ- uals in the CSI condition (M 5 3.33, SD 5 1.42), F(1, 53) 5 10.18, p 5 .002, hp 2 5 .16, d 5 0.88, 95% CI [0.38, 1.39]. Individuals in the Control condition (M 5 3.97, SD 5 1.18) had marginally higher perceptions of warmth than individ- uals in the CSI condition, F(1, 57) 5 3.44, p 5 .07, hp 2 5 .06, d 5 0.50, 95% CI [0.08, 1.01]. Individuals in the CSR con- dition had marginally higher perceptions of warmth than individuals in the control condition, F(1, 52) 5 2.82, p 5 .09, hp 2 5 .05, d 5 0.46, 95% CI [20.04, 0.88]. This replicates our previous studies that found CSR and CSI are linked with perceptions of warmth, supporting Hypothesis 1. A three- cell(CSR,CSI,controlconditions)between-subjectsanalysis of variance on perceptions of Competence revealed a sig- nificant effect of social responsibility condition, F(2, 81) 5 5.14, p5 .008, hp 2 5 .11. Specifically, individuals intheCSR condition (M 5 5.56, SD 5 0.82) had significantly higher perceptions of competence than individuals in the CSI condition (M 5 4.70, SD 5 1.26), F(1, 53) 5 8.56, p 5 .005, hp 2 5 .14, d 5 0.82, 95% CI [0.53, 1.27]. Individuals in the Control condition (M 5 5.31, SD 5 0.93) had significantly higher perceptions of competence than individuals in the CSI condition, F(1, 57) 5 4.44, p 5 .04, hp 2 5 .07, d 5 0.56, 95% CI [0.23, 1.01]. Individuals in the CSR and control
  • 60. conditions did not differ significantly in perceptions of competence, F(1, 52) 5 1.08, p 5 .30, hp 2 5 .02, d 5 0.29, CI [0.00, 0.62]. This replicates our previous studies that found CSR and CSI are linked with perceptions of competence while control conditions oftentimes do not differ from CSR, supporting Hypothesis 2. 2019 1625Shea and Hawn Measures Participants completed a feedback task set and a purchasing task. The two tasks were counter- balanced.12 When relevant, we controlled for order. Each participant completed each dependent mea- sure across the four country of origin manipulations. Feedback task. The first set of tasks examined the time and effort that participants devoted to providing feedback to each pen manufacturing organization. Participants were told that the pens were prototypes and that the organizations would like feedback on the pen designs. We encouraged participants to try out each pen in order to form an opinion of each product. Participants were given a blank essay box to provide feedback to the organization on their pen design. We measured the number of words written (M 5 21.87, SD 5 10.42) as a measure of helping (adapted from Porath & Erez, 2007). We also measured the subjective quality of each pen (“The pen is of high quality,” “The pen is desirable”; as 5 .72–.76).
  • 61. Purchasing task. The second task was a purchas- ingtaskusingtheBecker–DeGroot–Marschakmethod (Becker, DeGroot, & Marschak, 1964), which has been used to reveal true reservation prices in a variety of contexts (Burbano, 2016; Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1991; Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004). In this task, participants were given an additional dollar and were told that they could use this money to purchase the pens from the experiment and to keep whatever money they did not use. Participants were told that the computer program would randomly se- lect one pen and a random price at which they had the opportunity to purchase it. If their stated price was below the randomly generated price, they would not have the opportunity to purchase the pen. If their stated price was above the randomly generated price, they would have the opportunity to purchase the pen. Participants had to successfully recall these in- structions prior to continuing to ensure that they understood the experimental paradigm. This goes above traditional willingness-to-pay measures by forcing participants to forgo some of their bonus pay to make a purchase. Likewise, the random lottery nature of the task ensures that participants reveal their true purchase prices (Becker et al., 1964). Par- ticipants stated a purchase price for each of the four pens on a sliding scale between $0.01 and $1.00 (M 5 $0.33, SD 5 $0.17). No pens significantly differed from this price and no pen was consistently priced as the “top” pen across the entire sample. After partic- ipants stated their price for each pen, a random payment price was generated, they took the pen (if purchased), and left the purchase price at the com- puterstation at the end of the study. Participantsthen
  • 62. completed a manipulation check about the social practices of the pen manufacturers, as well as de- mographic questions. Results Data analysis strategy. We ran a series of re- peated measures ANOVA across dependent mea- sures. We first ran a CSR condition (three: CSR, CSI, Control) by Order (two: Pricing first, Feedback first) across the four country of origin manipulations (separately as well as grouped together by high and low warmth) as a repeated measures ANOVA.13 To directly test hypotheses, we re-ran models with a series of planned comparisons to probe statistically significant mean differences both within and be- tween conditions (see Table 3). Assumption of sphericity was not violated in any of the analyses; therefore, we report the sphericity-assumed results. Number of words. A three-cell (CSI, CSI, control condition) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) repeated measures ANOVA was run on the number of words written for the high- and low-warmth firms, as well as the Sweden, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan country of origin conditions. No significant differences emerged in the models, Fs , 2.02, ps . .11. When examined as a between-subjects analysis, a significant effect emerged for the social responsibility condition, F(2,78) 5 3.46, p 5 .04, hp 2 5 .08. Specifically, in- dividuals in the CSR condition (M 5 26.28, SD 5 14.10) wrote marginally more words than individuals in the CSI condition (M 5 20.53, SD 5 7.22), F(1,53) 5 3.81, p 5 .05, hp
  • 63. 2 5 .07, d 5 0.52, 95% CI [24.52, 3.11], and individuals in the control condition (M 5 19.45, SD 5 8.48), F(1,52) 5 4.80, p 5 .03, hp 2 5 .09, d 5 0.60, 95% CI [24.45, 3.63]. The CSI and control conditions did not significantly differ, F , .3, p . .60. Although we did not see variation within our country of origin manipulation, individuals offered more help to organizations that engaged in CSR, providing be- havioral replication and extension of Hypothesis 1 as 12 Pre-tests indicated that licensing effects may occur when tasks were sequential (i.e., “I did not help the orga- nization, so I supplement by overpaying for their product,” or vice versa). 13 We re-ran analyses excluding the control condition as it did not produce any significant effect, nor were there any significant interactions between the control condition and other conditions. We report both sets of ANOVAs below. 1626 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal well as support for our initial assumption about CSR generating rewards. Subjective quality. To test Hypotheses 4 and 5, a two-cell (CSI or CSI) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedbackfirst)repeatedmeasuresANOVAwasrunon subjective quality in high- and low-warmth firms.14 The three-way interaction was significant, F(1, 51) 5
  • 64. 4.71, p 5 .04, hp 2 5 .08. When participants completed the feedback tasks first,15 an interaction emerged, F(1, 23) 5 6.51, p 5 .02, hp 2 5 .22, suggesting moderation. While no differences between high- and low-warmth FIGURE 2a Results of Study 3: Average Effect of Warmth and Condition on Perceived Quality 1.000 1.500 2.000 2.500 3.000 3.500 4.000 CSI Control CSR S u b je
  • 65. ct iv e Q u al it y Evaluation Task First High Warmth Low Warmth 1.000 1.500 2.000 2.500 3.000 3.500 4.000 CSI Control CSR S u b
  • 66. je ct iv e Q u al it y Pricing Task First High Warmth Low Warmth TABLE 3 Summary of Results for Experiment 3 TABLE 3c Mean Levels of Subjective Quality by Country (Hypothesis 6) CSI Control CSR Sweden (high, high) 3.19 (0.22) 3.09 (0.21) 3.00 (0.26) Germany (low, high) 2.88 (0.16) 2.94 (0.25) 3.00 (0.20) Portugal (high, low) 2.88 (0.21) 3.03 (0.19) 2.83 (0.19) Pakistan (low, low) 2.54 (0.18) 3.12 (0.27) 3.58 (0.25) Notes: Subjective quality was measured on a 5-point scale. Standard errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between- subjects factors, while country of origin is a within-subjects
  • 67. factor. TABLE 3d Mean Levels of Subjective Quality by Warmth (Hypothe- ses 4 and 5) CSI Control CSR High Warmth 3.04 (0.20) 3.06 (0.18) 2.92 (0.21) Low Warmth 2.71 (0.21) 3.03 (0.19) 3.29 (0.22) Notes: Subjective quality was measured on a 5-point scale. Standard errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between- subjects factors, while warmth is a within-subjects factor. TABLE 3a Mean Levels of Purchase Price by Country (Hypothesis 6) CSI Control CSR Sweden (high, high) 0.43 (0.07) 0.35 (0.06) 0.31 (0.08) Germany (low, high) 0.25 (0.03) 0.26 (0.05) 0.35 (0.06) Portugal (high, low) 0.37 (0.08) 0.32 (0.06) 0.27 (0.06) Pakistan (low, low) 0.31 (0.05) 0.29 (0.05) 0.37 (0.05) Notes: Purchase Price was measured on a $1 scale. Standard errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between-subjects factors, while country of origin is a within-subjects factor. TABLE 3b Mean Levels of Purchase Price by Warmth (Hypotheses 4 and 5) CSI Control CSR
  • 68. High Warmth 0.40 (0.06) 0.33 (0.06) 0.29 (0.06) Low Warmth 0.28 (0.05) 0.28 (0.05) 0.36 (0.06) Notes: Purchase Price was measured on a $1 scale. Standard errors in brackets. CSI, Control, and CSR are between-subjects factors, while warmth is a within-subjects factor. 14 Including all CSR/CSI/control conditions, the three- way interaction is not significant, F(2, 78) 5 1.68, p 5 .19, hp 2 5 .041. 15 When participants completed the pricing task first, a main effect for factor emerges, F(1, 28) 5 4.45, p 5 .04, hp 2 5 .137, such that participants had a preference for high- warmth firms (M 5 3.20, SD 5 0.64) over low-warmth firms (M 5 2.88, SD 5 0.70) across conditions. 2019 1627Shea and Hawn firms emerged in the CSR condition, t(11) 5 1.62, p 5 .13, within the CSI condition, paired samples t tests revealed that high-warmth firms (M 5 3.09, SD 5 0.45) hadmarginallyhigherqualityratingsthanlow-warmth firms (M 5 2.71, SD 5 0.50), t(12) 5 2.10, p 5 .06, failing to provide support for Hypothesis 4. However, exploring the moderation effect between conditions, no significant differences emerged for high-warmth firms, F , .3, p . .59, while low-warmth firms saw significant gains between CSI (M 5 2.71, SD 5 0.50)
  • 69. andCSR(M 5 3.29,SD5 0.54),F(1,23)5 7.78,p5 .01, hp 2 5 .25,d5 1.16,95%CI[0.86,1.44].Takentogether, the significant moderation effect suggests that, while low-warmth firms receive equivalent outcomes for CSR, but not CSI, they significantly increase their outcomes as they changed strategies from CSI to CSR (Δ 5 .58), while high-warmth firms did not (Δ 5 .12). This provides indirect support for Hypothesis 5 that low-warmth firms reap higher gains from CSR. To test Hypothesis 6, a two-cell (CSI or CSI) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) repeated measures ANOVA was run on subjective quality in the Sweden, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan country of origin conditions.16 The three-way in- teraction was significant, F(3, 153) 5 3.46, p 5 .02, hp 2 5 .06. When the feedback task came first, a sig- nificant interaction emerged between CSR/CSI and country of origin, F(3, 69) 5 3.90, p 5 .01, hp 2 5 .25. We report paired samples t tests across countries of origin within the feedback task first condition. In the CSI condition, a paired samples t test indi- cated that Pakistan (M 5 2.54, SD 5 0.66) had a sig- nificantly lower subjective quality than Sweden (M 5 3.19, SD 5 0.78), t(12) 5 2.85, p5 .02, d 5 0.94, 95%CI [0.50, 1.31]. No significant effects were observed be- tween other countries, all ts , 1.67, ps . .12. This does not provide clear support for Hypothesis 6a.
  • 70. In the CSR condition, a paired samples t test indicated that Pakistan (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.87)17 had a significantly higher subjective quality than Portugal (M 5 2.83, SD 5 0.65), t(11) 5 2.37, p 5 .04, and a marginally significant higher subjective quality than Sweden (M 5 3.00, SD 5 0.90), t(11) 5 1.83, p 5 .09. No significant effects were observed between other countries, all ts , 1.74, ps . .11.18 This provides support for Hypothesis 6b, which predicted higher rewards to CSR for low–low firms. These results—obtained within subject—suggest that a low-warmth–low-competence firm gains significantly higher benefits for doing CSR than high-warmth firms. Purchase price. To test Hypotheses 4 and 5, a two- cell (CSI, CSI) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) repeated measures ANOVA was run on Pur- chase Price in the high- and low-warmth firms.19 A marginaleffectemergedforCondition,F(1,51)5 3.08, FIGURE 2b Results of Study 3: Average Effect of Warmth and Condition on Purchase Price 0.000 0.050 0.100 0.150 0.200 0.250 0.300 0.350 0.400 0.450 0.500
  • 71. CSI Control CSR P u rc h as e P ri ce Evaluation Task First High Warmth Low Warmth 0.000 0.100 0.200 0.300 0.400 0.500 CSI Control CSR P
  • 72. u rc h as e P ri ce Pricing Task First High Warmth Low Warmth 16 Including all CSR/CSI/control conditions, the three- way interaction is marginally significant, F(6, 234) 5 1.83, p 5 .09, hp 2 5 .045. 17 Probing this interaction between conditions, a one- factor ANOVA on Pakistan’s subjective quality revealed a significant effect, F(1,23) 5 11.48, p 5 .003, hp 2 5 .333, d 5 1.37, 95% CI [1.06, 1.61]. Specifically, subjective quality for the Pakistani pen was significantly higher in the CSR condition (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.87) than in the CSI condition (M 5 2.54, SD 5 0.66). 18 While not directly testing a hypothesis, a one-factor ANOVA between CSR and CSI conditions on Pakistan’s subjective quality revealed a significant effect, F(1,23) 5
  • 73. 11.48, p 5 .003, hp 2 5 .333. Specifically, subjective quality for the Pakistani pen was significantly higher in the CSR condition (M 5 3.58, SD 5 0.87) than in the CSI condition (M 5 2.54, SD 5 0.66). 19 Including all CSR/CSI/control conditions, the three- way interaction is marginally significant, F(2, 78) 5 2.68, p 5 .07, hp 2 5 .064. 1628 OctoberAcademy of Management Journal p 5 .09, hp 2 5 .06. This effect was qualified by a significant three-way interaction, F(1, 51) 5 5.69, p 5 .02, hp 2 5 .10. When participants completed the feedback tasks first,20 an interaction emerged, F(1, 23) 5 7.04, p 5 .01, hp 2 5 .23, suggesting moderation. Exploring these interactions, in the CSI condition, paired samples t tests revealed that high-warmth firms (M 5 0.40, SD 5 0.07) and low-warmth firms (M 5 0.28, SD 5 0.03) had significantly different purchase prices, t(12) 5 2.44, p 5 .02, with high- warmth firms having significantly higher prices, which does not support Hypothesis 4. However, in the CSR condition, we observed no differences be-
  • 74. tween high- and low-warmth firms, t(11) 5 1.35, p 5 .20. Looking between conditions, no significant dif- ferences emerged for high- or low-warmth firms, F , 1.8, p . .19. Thus, the interaction term reaches sig- nificance, driven by the difference in prices in the CSI condition, with the means crossover: when high- warmth firms switch from CSI to CSR, their purchase price decreased (Δ 5 .11), while low-warmth firm’s price increased (Δ 5 .08). Taken together, these re- sults suggest that low-warmth firms closed the gap as they changed strategies from CSI to CSR, while high- warmth firms surprisingly saw a decrease in terms of pricing (between conditions), providing indirect support for Hypothesis 5. To test Hypothesis 6, a two (Condition: CSI or CSI) by two (Order: Pricing first, Feedback first) repeated measures ANOVA was run on Purchase Price in the Sweden, Germany, Portugal, and Pakistan country of origin conditions.21 The three-way interaction was significant, F(3, 153) 5 2.61, p 5 .05, hp 2 5 .05. Probing this interaction further within Order condi- tions, when purchase price was determined after the feedback task, a significant interaction emerged be- tween CSR/CSI and country of origin, F(3, 69) 5 3.45, p 5 .03, hp 2 5 .13.22 In the CSI condition, a paired samples t test indicated that Sweden (M 5 0.43, SD 5 0.25) had a significantly higher price than Germany (M 5 0.25, SD 5 0.12), t(12) 5 2.69, p 5 .02, d 5 0.94, 95% CI [0.83, 0.99]; likewise, Sweden had a higher price than Pakistan (M 5 0.31, SD 5 0.17), t(12) 5 2.70,