SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 16
Download to read offline
Spirituality in Clinical Practice
God Image Narrative Therapy: A Mixed-Methods
Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual
Intervention
Trevor Olson, Theresa Clement Tisdale, Edward B. Davis, Elizabeth A. Park, Jiyun Nam, Glendon
L. Moriarty, Don E. Davis, Michael J. Thomas, Andrew D. Cuthbert, and Lance W. Hays
Online First Publication, May 12, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096
CITATION
Olson, T., Tisdale, T. C., Davis, E. B., Park, E. A., Nam, J., Moriarty, G. L., Davis, D. E., Thomas, M.
J., Cuthbert, A. D., & Hays, L. W. (2016, May 12). God Image Narrative Therapy: A Mixed-
Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention. Spirituality in Clinical
Practice. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096
God Image Narrative Therapy: A Mixed-Methods Investigation of a
Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention
Trevor Olson
His Mansion Ministries, Hillsboro, New Hampshire
Theresa Clement Tisdale
Azusa Pacific University
Edward B. Davis
Wheaton College
Elizabeth A. Park
Children’s Hospital Los Angeles,
Los Angeles, California
Jiyun Nam
Azusa Pacific University
Glendon L. Moriarty
Regent University
Don E. Davis
Georgia State University
Michael J. Thomas
Eastern University
Andrew D. Cuthbert and Lance W. Hays
Wheaton College
We report findings from a controlled, manualized 10-week group-based spiritual
intervention designed to improve God images, attachment to God, and narrative
identity, using primarily narrative and experiential interventions. Participants were 61
Christian adults (n ϭ 32 intervention, n ϭ 29 matched controls) from the student
population of 2 faith-based universities. Quantitative results (including data from
self-report measures and quantified data from God-representation figure drawings)
yielded nonsignificant findings. However, in posttest journal entries and during de-
briefing interviews, intervention participants reported experiencing positive changes in
God images, God attachment, and narrative identity. These discrepant results are
Trevor Olson, His Mansion Ministries, Hillsboro, New
Hampshire; Theresa Clement Tisdale, Department of Grad-
uate Psychology, Azusa Pacific University; Edward B.
Davis, Department of Psychology, Wheaton College; Eliz-
abeth A. Park, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los An-
geles, California; Jiyun Nam, Department of Graduate Psy-
chology, Azusa Pacific University; Glendon L. Moriarty,
School of Psychology and Counseling, Regent University;
Don E. Davis, Psychology Department, Georgia State Uni-
versity; Michael J. Thomas, Department of Psychology,
Eastern University; Andrew D. Cuthbert, and Lance W.
Hays, Department of Psychology, Wheaton College.
This research was the focus of Trevor Olson’s dissertation
at Regent University. The study was funded in part by a grant
to Theresa Clement Tisdale from the Faculty Research Coun-
cil at Azusa Pacific University. This publication was also
made possible through the support of a grant from the John
Templeton Foundation (Grant 44040) to Edward B. Davis
and Don E. Davis. The opinions expressed in this publication
are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the
views of the John Templeton Foundation.
We thank Ken Pargament for his review and comments
on an earlier version of this article. The authors would also
like to thank the research assistants (Hershey Desai, Zy-
anya Mendoza, and Elizabeth Park) and group facilitators
for the intervention condition (Neal Collins, Lauren De-
Silva, Lacey Edwards, Christina Girlando, Cormack Mc-
Kinney, Cory McKinsey, Vanessa Myers, Chelsae Roby
Pistello, Kristin Shier, Tiffany Spooner, Bethany Vaudrey,
Ashley Wickliffe, and Joshua Ziesel). The authors would
also like to thank the research assistant (Carl Waitz) and
raters who served on the validation project for the Integra-
tive Scoring System for God Representation Figure Draw-
ings (Rebekah Bartholomew, Hughes Choy, Rebecca
Diercks, Lacey Edwards, Sabrina Ferris, Irene Kao, Jin
Kim, Ronilleen Maglian, Jacqueline Orozco, Rachel Sills,
Allison Vrieze, and Brigette Worthen).
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Theresa Clement Tisdale, Department of Grad-
uate Psychology, Azusa Pacific University, 701 East Foot-
hill Boulevard, Azusa, CA 91702. E-mail: tctisdale@apu
.edu
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
Spirituality in Clinical Practice © 2016 American Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 3, No. 2, 000 2326-4500/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096
1
discussed in terms of the existing literature, with a focus on measurement issues. We
also discuss the potential of using qualitative and mixed-methods research to study God
images and God attachment, particularly (a) in the context of outcome research and (b)
if non-self-report methods (e.g., projective measures, narrative methods) are utilized
more heavily than self-report methods.
Keywords: God images, attachment, narrative, relational spirituality, outcome research
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096.supp
Over 80% of the world’s population self-
identifies with a religious group, and within the
United States, 60% of adults report believing
that God is a person with whom one can have a
relationship (Pew Research Center, 2008,
2012). In fact, within the United States, most
religious/spiritual persons view their perceived
relationship to God as central to what it means
to be religious or spiritual (Gallup & Jones,
1989; Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, 2008; Moriarty
& Davis, 2012).
There is increasing scientific interest in un-
derstanding how people conceptually view and
experientially relate to God—that is, people’s
God concepts and images (Davis et al., 2016;
Davis, Moriarty, & Mauch, 2013). However,
apart from a few exceptions (e.g., Cheston,
Piedmont, Eanes, & Lavin, 2003; Murray-
Swank & Pargament, 2005; Thomas, Moriarty,
Davis, & Anderson, 2011; Tisdale et al., 1997),
little research has studied psychotherapeutically
promoted God-image change. Recent theoreti-
cal developments in the God image literature
(e.g., Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty &
Davis, 2012) suggest narrative-experiential ap-
proaches may be effective in promoting positive
God-image change (e.g., more benevolent God
images), but as of yet, no peer-reviewed studies
have examined this possibility. Thus, we report
findings from a controlled and manualized 10-
week group-based spiritual intervention de-
signed to promote adaptive God image change,
using primarily narrative and experiential inter-
ventions.
Definitions and Theoretical Framework
God Representations (God Concepts
and Images)
God representations refer to mental (e.g.,
cognitive structures) or neural (e.g., neural fir-
ing pattern) representations of God (Davis et al.,
2013). It has been postulated (Rizzuto, 1979)
that there are two main types of God represen-
tations: God concepts and God images. God
concepts are propositional religious cognitions
(e.g., affect-light, doctrinal, head knowledge)
that mentally/neurally underlie the ways people
conceptually view God. That is, God concepts
are the conceptual God representations that un-
derlie one’s theological understanding of God.
In contrast, God images are implicational reli-
gious cognitions (e.g., affect-laden, experien-
tial, heart knowledge) that underlie the ways
people experientially relate to God. That is, God
images refer to the relational and emotional God
representations that underlie one’s embodied,
emotional experience in perceived relationship
to God (Davis et al., 2013, 2016; Hall & Fu-
jikawa, 2013; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; Rizzuto,
1979). Whereas God concepts tend to be more
belief-oriented (i.e., affect-light, cortically dom-
inant), God images tend to be more emotion-
oriented (i.e., affect-laden, subcortically domi-
nant). See Davis et al. (2013, 2016) for
elaboration on these definitions.
Healthy Relational Spirituality and
Adaptive Head–Heart Congruence
Pargament (2007, 2013) has argued that a
healthy spirituality is characterized by a high
degree of integration among its ingredients
(e.g., one’s beliefs, practices, and experiences),
whereas an unhealthy spirituality is character-
ized by a low degree of integration among its
ingredients. Building on this conceptualization,
a healthy relational spirituality can be defined
as the degree to which a person’s head (doctri-
nal) and heart (experiential) knowledge of God
is congruent, well-integrated, and contextually
adaptive across time and situations. Stated dif-
ferently, a healthy relational spirituality is char-
acterized by a consistently high degree of adap-
tive head–heart congruence—that is, resonant
2 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
correspondence between one’s head and heart
knowledge of God, if that resonant head and
heart knowledge is (a) contextually adaptive
(e.g., associated with positively valenced psy-
chological and spiritual outcomes within the
context of one’s life [situational context] and
culture [sociocultural context]) and (b) consis-
tently evident across time and situations (cf.
Moriarty & Davis, 2012; Pargament, 2007,
2013; Zahl & Gibson, 2012; Zahl, Sharp, &
Gibson, 2013). Increasing adaptive head–heart
congruence is a vital goal of psychotherapeuti-
cally promoted God-image change (Moriarty &
Davis, 2012).
Narratives, Narrative Identity, and
God-Image Narrative Identity
Narratives refer to “the linear telling of a
sequence of events [and they] entail a focus on
action and on the mental states of the individu-
als of the story, including the self or narrator”
(Siegel, 2012b, p. 478). More specifically, nar-
rative identity refers to “an individual’s inter-
nalized, evolving, and integrative story of the
self” (McAdams, 2008, p. 242), and God-image
narrative identity refers to one’s narrative iden-
tity in perceived relationship to God (Davis &
Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012).
Theoretical Framework
Relational Spirituality
In the current study, our intervention proto-
col’s (Davis, 2009) conceptualization of God
image development and dynamics was based on
Hall’s (2004) relational-spirituality theory (cf.
Hall, 2007; Hall, Fujikawa, Halcrow, Hill, &
Delaney, 2009). Therefore, it was undergirded
by that theory’s central organizing principles, as
described in Hall (2004). For example, it was
based on the following two assumptions:
Implicit relational representations are repetitions of
relational experiences, sharing a common affective
core, that are conceptually encoded in the mind as
nonpropositional meaning structures. They are the
memory basis for implicit relational [knowing]; that is,
our “gut level” sense of how significant relationships
work. (Hall, 2004, p. 71)
Implicit relational representations, formed particularly
from experiences [in] early relationships with caregiv-
ers, shape the emotional appraisal of meaning and
subsequent patterns of relationship. (Hall, 2004, p. 72)
Implicit Relational Knowing Correspondence
Because we adopted Hall and colleagues’
(Hall, 2004; Hall et al., 2009) relational-
spirituality framework, our conceptualization of
God image development and dynamics assumed
implicit-relational-knowing correspondence. In
other words, we assumed people’s implicit re-
lational knowing about how to perceive, expe-
rience, and interact with a divine attachment
figure (e.g., God) corresponds to their implicit
relational knowing about how to perceive, ex-
perience, and interact with human attachment
figures (Davis et al., 2013; Hall, 2004; Hall et
al., 2009; Moriarty & Davis, 2012).
Narrative Identity
Additionally, we adopted McAdams’s
(1993, 2008) narrative identity framework,
particularly the understanding that a healthy
narrative identity is reflected in personal nar-
ratives exhibiting high degrees of coherence,
openness, credibility, differentiation, recon-
ciliation, and generative integration. Specifi-
cally, a healthy narrative identity emerges
from personal narratives typifying the follow-
ing six narrative standards:
• Coherence: The narrative’s components
(e.g., events, themes, characters, settings)
logically relate to and make sense in light
of each other, without excessive contradic-
tion.
• Openness: The narrative is flexible, adap-
tive, and resilient, and it is welcoming of
adaptive change and growth.
• Credibility: The narrative is based on facts
and on reality-based perceptions of events,
without gross or fantasy-based distortions.
• Differentiation: The narrative is rich with
depth, texture, and complexity, including
many layers of nuanced meaning.
• Reconciliation: The narrative’s differenti-
ated elements and conflicting tensions cul-
minate in harmonious resolution.
• Generative Integration: The narrative’s
protagonist is well-integrated into the soci-
ety in which he or she is embedded and has
become dedicated to bettering the lives of
others, particularly the next generation
(McAdams, 1993).
3GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
Interventions for Promoting Adaptive
God-Image Change
Clinical Trials
A few clinical trials have examined the pro-
motion of adaptive God-image change through
psychotherapy. For example, Tisdale et al.
(1997) found spiritually integrated multimodal
psychotherapy in an inpatient setting was effec-
tive in promoting improvements in God images,
object relations development, and self-esteem,
and all three types of improvements were main-
tained over a 12-month follow-up period. In a
similar study, Cheston et al. (2003) found that
traditional individual psychotherapy in an out-
patient setting was also effective in promoting
positive God-image change, but only for people
who evidenced significant corresponding emo-
tional and spiritual change (as per clinician rat-
ings). Taken together, these studies suggest psy-
chotherapeutically promoted God-image
change is closely connected to other forms of
emotional and spiritual change (e.g., improve-
ments in self-esteem and object relations devel-
opment). As Moriarty and Davis (2012) have
argued, it may be that healthy human attach-
ments, healthy self-esteem, and healthy rela-
tional spirituality (as defined above) are inti-
mately related and reciprocally interactive, such
that improvements in one of these areas leads to
improvements in the others. For instance, as
people experience greater attachment security
in their human relationships, they may also de-
velop higher self-esteem and become more psy-
chologically able to experience love, security,
acceptance, and closeness in their perceived re-
lationship to God (cf. Mikulincer & Shaver,
2004).
Manualized Interventions
Two other studies have examined the promo-
tion of God-image change through participation
in a manualized psychotherapeutic intervention.
In a small-scale study with two adult female
survivors of sexual abuse, Murray-Swank and
Pargament (2005) found a brief, manualized,
spiritually integrated, individual psychotherapy
intervention was effective in promoting positive
God-image change. Similarly, in a noncon-
trolled pilot study with 26 Christian adults,
Thomas et al. (2011) found a brief, manualized,
spiritually integrated, group psychotherapy in-
tervention was effective in promoting positive
shifts in God images and God attachment, as
well as increased adaptive head–heart congru-
ence.
In sum, outcome research has provided pre-
liminary evidence that God-image change can
be effected through a variety of therapy modal-
ities and formats (e.g., inpatient or outpatient;
multimodal, individual, or group; broad-based
or targeted; manualized or nonmanualized).
However, each of these studies had notable
methodological limitations (e.g., no control
group; sole use of quantitative measures; non-
manualized intervention), as Thomas et al.
(2011) detailed.
Narrative-Experiential Approaches to
Promoting Adaptive God-Image Change
Recently, scholars have discussed the prom-
ise of using narrative-experiential approaches to
promote God-image change (Davis & Bade-
noch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012). Indeed,
Cozolino (2010); Hall (2007), and Siegel
(2012a, 2012b) have described the myriad sci-
entifically grounded reasons why coherent nar-
ratives offer an optimal mechanism for promot-
ing psychotherapeutic change. For example, by
their very nature, coherent narratives link input
from several layers of mental processing (e.g.,
sensations, images, feelings, and thoughts; left-
and right-mode processes; implicit and explicit
mental processes) and several types of neural
circuits (e.g., cortical and subcortical circuits,
sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous sys-
tems). Through this linking, coherent narratives
promote health and well-being (a) by facilitat-
ing various types of mental and neural integra-
tion and (b) by supporting affect regulation and
maintaining homeostasis, both in the present
and across time (Cozolino, 2010; Siegel,
2012a).
When using narrative-experiential interventions
to transform God images, the focus is primarily on
promoting narrative integration through facilitat-
ing new experiences and new understandings (cf.
Levenson, 2010). In particular, God-image narra-
tive therapy (GINT) has a three-fold treatment
goal: the integration of a client’s spiritual narra-
tive, the integration of a client’s life narrative, and
the integration of a client’s spiritual narrative with
his or her life narrative. Toward these ends, in
4 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
GINT the psychotherapist uses traditional narra-
tive therapy techniques, such as deconstructing
problem-saturated narratives, asking questions
that help link events across time and explore iden-
tity-relevant themes, and coconstructing alterna-
tive growth-promoting storylines (Davis & Bade-
noch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; cf. Madigan,
2011).
GINT is a spiritually oriented, narrative-
experiential intervention that is based on the
recognition that attachment is a central human
need and motivation. Thus, it is based on the
assumption that affect-involving interpersonal
engagement is the primary medium and mech-
anism for facilitating new experiences and new
understandings. Specifically, GINT utilizes the
interpersonal nature of the therapy relationship
(individual therapy format) and the group con-
text (group therapy format) to help clients ar-
ticulate and link various components of their
spiritual and life narratives, including those nar-
ratives’ salient chapters and settings, characters,
plots and subplots, themes and tones, images
and metaphors, milestones, and conflicts (Da-
vis, 2009; Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty
& Davis, 2012; cf. Levenson, 2010; McAdams,
1993).
To help clients articulate these different nar-
rative components, narrative and experiential
interventions are used during and between ses-
sions. For example, between sessions, clients
use artistic, reflective, and expressive writing
exercises to articulate aspects of their narra-
tives. Then during sessions clients are asked to
share these aspects of their narratives and ver-
bally process the meanings they are ascribing to
their life experiences. Through reflection, artic-
ulation, and interactive engagement, partici-
pants bring to the surface the emotions and
physiological sensations that are associated
with their implicit relational knowing of self,
others, and God. Verbal and written reflection is
used to attach words, metaphors, and images to
these emotions and sensations, thereby promot-
ing narratives that are coherent (e.g., both log-
ical and emotionally meaningful). The last
phase of this treatment method is dedicated to
(a) coconstructing anticipated future storylines
that are likely to foster continued growth and
well-being and (b) empowering clients to live
out those new storylines (Davis, 2009; Davis &
Badenoch, 2010; Hall, 2007; Moriarty & Davis,
2012; cf. Bucci, 1997; Cozolino, 2010). Nota-
bly, as research on experiential psychotherapy
has reliably shown, for clients who participate
in experiential psychotherapies, much of the
change they experience may take place after the
treatment has ended, particularly when the treat-
ment is brief (see Levenson, 2010; Shedler,
2010, for reviews).
Within the context of individual or group-
based GINT, the interpersonal nature of narra-
tive sharing is a central component of the
change process. Interpersonal discourse helps
clients explicate and integrate the memories,
thoughts, feelings, sensations, meanings, and
perceptions that are embedded within their life
and spiritual narratives. Interpersonal sharing
also affords opportunities for clients to explore
alternative (and often more adaptive and posi-
tively valenced) understandings of their life ex-
periences, thereby providing ample chances for
growth-promoting meaning making (cf. Madi-
gan, 2011; Shin & Steger, 2014).
The Current Study
To date, no published studies have evaluated
the efficacy of using narrative-experiential in-
terventions to promote God-image change. Be-
cause similar types of interventions have dem-
onstrated efficacy in promoting positive therapy
outcomes (e.g., see Elliott, Greenberg, Watson,
Timulak, & Freire, 2013; Levenson, 2010; Shin
& Steger, 2014), we hypothesized this narra-
tive-experiential GINT intervention would also
demonstrate efficacy. Specifically, we sought to
examine the efficacy of a 10-week, manualized,
narrative-experiential group psychotherapy pro-
tocol focusing on promoting positive changes in
God images, God attachment, and narrative
identity. We predicted that, relative to control
participants, intervention participants would re-
port greater change in (a) both positively and
negatively valenced God images (increase and
decrease, respectively), (b) both God attach-
ment anxiety and God attachment avoidance
(decrease for both), and (c) self-reported head–
heart congruence (increase).
We hypothesized these changes would occur
because intervention participants would have an
interpersonal context within which to have new
experiences and develop new understandings of
their spiritual and life narratives. In particular,
we surmised participants would experience in-
creased attachment security in their perceived
5GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
relationship to God (lower God attachment anx-
iety and avoidance) as they recognized God’s
presence and role in their spiritual and life nar-
ratives. In so doing, they would report experi-
encing positively valenced God-image change
and increased head–heart congruence. Further-
more, given both the empathic, growth-oriented
psychotherapeutic context and the narrative fo-
cus of the intervention, we expected participants
would discover more adaptive, hopeful mean-
ings embedded within their spiritual and life
narratives, while concurrently achieving greater
coherence in those narratives. In this study, we
sought to improve upon three aforementioned
limitations of prior research on God-image
change by (a) including a control group, (b)
employing a mixed-methods design (collecting
quantitative and qualitative data), and (c) utiliz-
ing a manualized intervention.
Method
Participants
All participants were adult undergraduate and
graduate students at two faith-based universi-
ties. Participants were recruited via posted fly-
ers, campus-wide e-mails, university chapel an-
nouncements, or word-of-mouth (e.g., clinician
and campus-chaplain referrals). They were in-
vited to participate in a research study about
their spiritual life, relationships, and experience
of God, and they were given two options for
participating: (a) completing a 10-week inter-
vention group (1.5-hr sessions) or (b) complet-
ing questionnaires only (at two time points).
Participants self-selected to participate in the
intervention condition or the measures-only
condition, based on their level of interest and
their availability. Thirty-two participants com-
pleted the intervention condition (84% reten-
tion), and 29 participants completed the mea-
sures-only condition (100% retention).
Intervention condition. Thirty-eight pro-
spective participants completed a face-to-face
screening session, and they all met inclusionary
criteria: at least 18 years old; self-identified
Christian; currently nonsuicidal; interested in
participating in a group intervention; interested
in exploring their spiritual life, relationships,
and experience of God; and willing to commit
to a 10-week group. In particular, these partic-
ipants sought spiritual growth experiences that
would help them nurture positive spiritual
change in their spiritual life and their perceived
relationship to God.
Thirty-two participants completed the 10-
week intervention condition. Of those partici-
pants, there were 4 males (13%) and 28 females
(87%), and the mean age was 21.97 (SD ϭ 7.11,
range: 18–52 years old). Thirteen self-identified
as White (41%), 8 as Black (25%), 6 as Asian
(19%), 2 as Latino/a (6%), and 3 as multiracial
(9%). All participants were currently either an
undergraduate student (n ϭ 28, 87%) or grad-
uate student (n ϭ 4, 13%). In terms of religious
affiliation, 30 participants self-identified as
Protestant Christian (94%), whereas 2 self-
identified as Catholic Christian (6%). Partici-
pants reported having been a Christian for an
average of 9.41 years (SD ϭ 7.59), and every-
one endorsed that religion/spirituality was ei-
ther a somewhat important (n ϭ 2, 6%) or very
important (n ϭ 30, 94%) part of their lives.
Control condition. The 29 control partici-
pants were recruited via the same methods and
were matched as closely as possible with inter-
vention participants, based on sex, age, and
education. There were 5 males (17%) and 24
females (83%), and the mean age was 22.07
(SD ϭ 7.28, range: 18–56). Twenty self-
identified as White (69%), 2 as Black (7%), 2 as
Asian (7%), 3 as Latino/a (10%), 1 as Native
American (3%), and 1 as multiracial (3%). All
participants were currently either an undergrad-
uate student (n ϭ 26, 90%) or graduate student
(n ϭ 3, 10%). All 29 self-identified as Protes-
tant Christian in affiliation. Participants re-
ported they had been Christian for an average of
11.00 years (SD ϭ 4.16), and everyone en-
dorsed that religion/spirituality was either a
somewhat important (n ϭ 2, 7%) or very im-
portant (n ϭ 27, 93%) part of their lives.
Group Facilitators
For the intervention condition, there in total
were 14 group facilitators (males ϭ 5, fe-
males ϭ 9; White ϭ 12, Latino/a ϭ 1, Asian ϭ
1), and each person facilitated only one group.
Each of the seven groups was facilitated by two
doctoral psychology trainees. Five groups had a
male and a female facilitator; two groups had
two female facilitators. All 14 facilitators were
in their mid-to-late 20s. Twelve identified as
6 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
Protestant Christian, and two identified as Cath-
olic Christian.
All facilitators were in their second, third, or
fourth year of a doctoral clinical psychology
program. Everyone had completed doctoral-
level coursework in individual and group psy-
chotherapy, and they all had at least some clin-
ical experience conducting psychotherapy.
Facilitators participated in a 4-hr session to
train them in how to facilitate their group in
adherence to the manualized protocol. To en-
sure appropriate clinical care and to monitor
compliance with intervention protocol, the sec-
ond and sixth authors (licensed psychologists
TCT and GLM) clinically supervised their re-
spective institution’s facilitators. Each dyad re-
ceived 1 hour of weekly supervision, starting 2
weeks before their group began and lasting until
2 weeks after their group ended. Facilitators
participated in an all-facilitator debriefing ses-
sion as well.
Procedure
Prospective participants self-selected to ei-
ther the intervention or measures-only control
condition. At an in-person screening meeting,
prospective control participants were read the
study’s cover letter and then signed the in-
formed consent form, after any questions were
addressed. Prospective intervention participants
also completed a one-on-one screening session,
involving an interview to determine whether
they met inclusionary criteria. Because all in-
tervention participants met these criteria, they
were either notified of their group’s day/time or
they signed up for a group based on compati-
bility with their schedule. Then they were pro-
vided: a copy of the manualized GINT protocol;
the contact information for their group cofacili-
tators and faculty supervisor; and details regard-
ing the day, time, and location of their group.
In the intervention condition, each group con-
sisted of 3 to 6 members and met for ten 1.5-
hour sessions, with a homework assignment be-
tween sessions. Group members were strongly
discouraged from missing sessions and were
informed that missing three or more sessions
would disqualify them from continued partici-
pation.
Intervention participants completed the pre-
test measures between weeks 1 and 2 of the
group and the posttest measures immediately
following completion of the 10-week protocol.
Control participants completed the posttest
measures 10 weeks after they completed the
pretest measures. Neither the intervention nor
control participants were given any information
about the measures; they were only asked to
complete the measures and either return them to
their group facilitators (intervention group) or
drop them off at an assigned location (control
group).
Following the conclusion of the 10-week
group, intervention participants had an individ-
ual 15-minute debriefing session, during which
they were asked for feedback about the experi-
ence, thanked for their participation, and given
the study stipend ($40 cash) and debriefing
form. Control participants did not complete a
debriefing session but were given their stipend
($10 cash) in person and thanked for their par-
ticipation. All interactions with participants
were conducted in compliance with the Ameri-
can Psychological Association ethics code and
the National Institutes of Health standards for
research involving human subjects.
Materials
Measures. The pre- and posttest measure
set included the God Adjective Checklist (Zahl
& Gibson, 2012), Attachment to God Inventory
(Beck & McDonald, 2004), and a single-item
measure of head–heart congruence (cf. Thomas
et al., 2011). At pretest and posttest, interven-
tion participants also completed three God rep-
resentation figure drawings (Davis et al., 2014a,
2014b), including a brief written narrative de-
scribing each drawing. Also, at posttest inter-
vention participants completed a written journal
entry about their overall experience in the
group.
God Adjective Checklist. The God Adjec-
tive Checklist (GAC) is a 30-item self-report
measure comparing individuals’ God concepts
and images (Zahl & Gibson, 2012). Using a
7-point Likert scale (1 ϭ very undescriptive to
7 ϭ very descriptive), respondents rate each of
the 30 trait words. Fifteen trait words measure
positively valenced God representations (e.g.,
loving, patient, kind, and reliable) and 15 mea-
sure negatively valenced God representations
(e.g., critical, distant, inconsistent, and unfor-
giving). For each adjective, the respondent is
first asked to rate each word according to “what
7GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
I should believe God is like” and then to rate
each word according to “my personal experi-
ence of what God is like.” Therefore, the GAC
asks respondents to differentiate between their
“head” (doctrinal) and “heart” (experiential)
knowledge of God. It includes four subscales—
two for positively valenced God representations
(positively valenced God concepts and images)
and two for negatively valenced God represen-
tations (negatively valenced God concepts and
images). In the current study, given our research
questions, we only concentrated on the two God
image subscales. The Cronbach’s alphas (at pre-
test) were .95 for the positively valenced God
images subscale and .86 for the negatively va-
lenced God images subscale.
Attachment to God Inventory. The At-
tachment to God Inventory (AGI; Beck & Mc-
Donald, 2004) is a 28-item self-report measure
of God attachment. It uses a 7-point Likert scale
(1 ϭ disagree strongly to 7 ϭ agree strongly)
and has two subscales, assessing God attach-
ment anxiety (i.e., anxiety about abandonment
by God; e.g., “I worry a lot about my relation-
ship with God”) and God attachment avoidance
(i.e., avoidance of intimacy with God; e.g., “I
am uncomfortable being emotional in my com-
munication with God”). In the current study,
Cronbach’s alphas (at pretest) were .88 for the
AGI anxiety subscale and .87 for the AGI
avoidance subscale.
Head– heart congruence. To measure
head–heart congruence, we utilized a similar
single-item measure to the one used in Thomas
et al. (2011). Specifically, participants were
asked to use a 7-point Likert scale (1 ϭ highly
dissimilar to 7 ϭ highly similar) to rate the
following statement: “At present, my head
knowledge of God and heart knowledge of God
are ________.”
God representation figure drawings and
integrative scoring system. God representa-
tion figure drawings (Davis et al., 2014a,
2014b) are based on Rizzuto’s (1979) qualita-
tive research and consist of having respondents
draw pictures of God and oneself. We utilized
three prompts, based on Moriarty and Davis’s
(2012) recommendations: (1) “Draw a picture
of you and God”; (2) “Draw a picture of how
you FEEL you and God look when you do
something wrong. Draw what you feel, not what
you think”; and (3) “Draw a picture of how you
would like to feel you and God look when you
do something wrong.” We used a prompt about
doing something wrong because such times are
modally characterized by a rupture in one’s
perceived relationship to God, at least tempo-
rarily leading to decreased head–heart congru-
ence.
Given the narrative-experiential and creative
nature of our manualized intervention, we in-
cluded these God representation figure draw-
ings in the pretest and posttest measure set for
the intervention participants only, because we
believed drawings would potentially provide
rich insights into these participants’ God-
representation dynamics and would thus foster
emotionally meaningful reflection (e.g., grist for
personal and group process). Also, building on
prior scholarship, we believed that, relative to
self-report measures, these drawings may be
more sensitive to changes in God images (Gib-
son, 2007; Moriarty & Davis, 2012) and more
revealing of internal working models, which
often operate outside conscious awareness (Da-
vis et al., 2013; Zahl et al., 2013). At both time
periods, because of the experiential nature of
the intervention, we asked participants to use
colored pencils to complete the drawings, to
stimulate right-mode processing and to evoke
emotions and emotion-laden meaning-making
(cf. Mihura, Meyer, Dumitrascu, & Bombel,
2013).
To facilitate more rigorous analysis of this
visual qualitative data, we developed an inte-
grative scoring system (Davis et al., 2014b; see
Figure S1) to quantify data from participants’
God representation figure drawings. This scor-
ing system consists of 11 items, divided into
three subscales: God concepts/images (rating
indicators of God as benevolent, accessible, re-
sponsive, and authoritarian), God attachment
(rating indicators of God as a secure base and
safe haven, as well as indicators of God attach-
ment anxiety, avoidance, and disorganization),
and self-functioning (rating indicators of self-
esteem and psychological well-being).
We also wanted to ensure the scoring system
demonstrated strong interrater reliability. Be-
cause we hope this system will be utilized in
applied settings (e.g., settings in which clini-
cians often do not have time or money to par-
ticipate in extensive training on a given instru-
ment), we tested whether there would be a
difference in interrater reliability between un-
trained raters with less clinical experience (N ϭ
8 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
6; e.g., first- and second-year graduate psychol-
ogy students) and more clinically experienced
raters who had been trained on the measure
(N ϭ 6; e.g., third- through fifth-year graduate
psychology students). Each of the 12 raters was
blinded to the study’s hypotheses and rated
drawings in counterbalanced order (to control
for order effects).
Replicating what prior research has shown in
behavioral ratings of marital interactions (Bau-
com, Baucom, & Christensen, 2012; Waldinger,
Schulz, Hauser, Allen, & Crowell, 2004), the
group of 6 untrained and less clinically experi-
enced graduate students demonstrated statisti-
cally equivalent interrater reliability to the
group of 6 trained and more clinically experi-
enced raters. For all three drawings, the aver-
age-measures intraclass correlation coefficient
was in the excellent range for both groups (i.e.,
from the upper .80s to the mid .90s). Also, when
we combined the ratings for each rater and
examined each drawing, the scoring system’s
total scale and three subscales each demon-
strated excellent internal consistency: .91 to .95
for the total scale, .86 to .91 for the God con-
cepts/images subscale, .83 to .93 for the God
attachment subscale, and .83 to .90 for the self
functioning subscale (see Tables S1 and S2 in
the online supplemental material).
Manualized protocol. With the interven-
tion participants, we used an adapted version of
the GINT manual developed by Davis (2009).
The original manual was developed for use in
an individual psychotherapy format. However,
for the current study, the first author (Trevor
Olson) adapted the original manual so it could
be used in a group psychotherapy format, and
the second author (Theresa Clement Tisdale)
developed guidance notes for group facilitators.
In both versions of the manual, the GINT pro-
tocol reflects an assimilative-integrationist ap-
proach to treating God image difficulties. That
is, it incorporates interventions from various
psychotherapeutic approaches (e.g., dynamic-
interpersonal and cognitive–behavioral), but it
is generally guided by a relational-spirituality
conceptualization and chiefly utilizes narrative-
experiential interventions, such as (a) expres-
sive-writing exercises to facilitate articulation
and deconstruction of one’s problem-saturated
spiritual and life narratives (e.g., chapters, char-
acters, plots, themes, etc.); (b) mindful-
awareness practices (e.g., contemplation and
guided reflection) and interpersonal discourse to
help link events across time and explore iden-
tity-relevant themes; (c) God-image figure
drawings and automatic thought records to fos-
ter insight into implicit relational knowing of
self, God, and others; and (d) narrative inter-
ventions to coconstruct alternative growth-
promoting storylines (Davis & Badenoch, 2010;
Moriarty & Davis, 2012). See Table S3 for a
detailed overview of the protocol (see Davis,
2009, for a copy of the full GINT protocol).
Results
Analysis of Quantitative Data From
Self-Report Measures
Data were screened to see whether assump-
tions were met for conducting a 2 (Intervention
vs. Control) ϫ 2 (Pretest, Posttest) mixed-
model repeated measures multivariate analysis
of variance (MANOVA) with five dependent
variables (positively and negatively valenced
God images; God attachment anxiety and
avoidance; head–heart congruence). Three par-
ticipants (1 intervention and 2 controls) had not
completed all items on one pre- or posttest
measure, and those participants’ scores thus
were not calculated for the affected measure.
The pre- or posttest GAC subscale scores had
a few univariate outliers (1 to 3), which were
recoded to the nearest nonextreme value.
Also, the pre- and posttest positively valenced
God image GAC scores were substantially
skewed (Ϫ2.94 and Ϫ1.29, respectively), but
transformations did not improve the normal-
ity of these variables. For the other variables,
the pre- and posttest skew values indicated
acceptable normality: negatively valenced
God images (0.94 and 0.72), God attachment
anxiety (Ϫ0.21 and 0.21), God attachment
avoidance (0.46 and 0.50), and head–heart
congruence (Ϫ0.43 and Ϫ0.78), respectively.
Levene’s tests were nonsignificant, indicating
equality of error variances for each variable.
Finally, we conducted the mixed-model re-
peated measures MANOVA, which was non-
significant, Wilks’ ⌳(5,111) ϭ 0.97, p ϭ
.681. Each variable’s descriptive statistics
and between-subjects condition-by-time in-
teraction effect is presented in Table 1.
In sum, we predicted that, relative to control
participants, intervention participants would re-
9GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
port greater change in (1) both positively and
negatively valenced God images (increase and
decrease, respectively), (2) both God attach-
ment anxiety and God attachment avoidance
(decrease for both), and (3) self-reported head–
heart congruence (increase). However, none of
these hypotheses was supported.
Analysis of Qualitative Data from Figure
Drawings and Posttest Journal Entries
We also evaluated whether intervention par-
ticipants’ coded figure drawings evidenced pre-
test–posttest changes (see Figures S2 and S3 in
online supplemental material, for examples of
participants’ figure drawings). To do that, we
aggregated all 12 raters’ ratings (6 untrained, 6
trained) to provide an average (mean) rating for
each participant, each drawing (prompts 1, 2,
and 3), and each time period (pretest and post-
test). Paired-samples t tests revealed no signif-
icant pretest–posttest differences.
Our research team was extremely puzzled,
however, because intervention participants
often had reported experiencing significant
spiritual and psychological growth. For exam-
ple, in their posttest journal entries and debrief-
ing interviews, a large number of participants
reported experiencing increased honesty, close-
ness, security, satisfaction, and head–heart con-
gruence in their perceived relationship to God.
Similarly, participants commonly discussed ex-
periencing increased self-insight and self-
acceptance as a result of their group participa-
tion. In short, participants frequently reported
experiencing positive changes in their God im-
ages, God attachment, head–heart congruence,
God-image narrative identity, and general nar-
rative identity.
Therefore, for exploratory purposes, we con-
ducted a text analysis of the intervention partic-
ipants’ posttest journal entries, using the well-
utilized and well-validated Linguistic Inquiry
and Word Count (LIWC) 2007 software pro-
gram (Pennebaker, Booth, & Francis, 2007).
The LIWC 2007 program analyzes written text
samples, evaluating the percentage of words
falling into different psychologically meaning-
ful categories (e.g., social, affective, or cogni-
tive processes). Text analysis of participants’
posttest journal entries revealed that partici-
pants wrote 203.88 words on average (SD ϭ
274.81, range: 66–1471) and mainly discussed
cognitive processes (M ϭ 22.09% of the text,
SD ϭ 4.22%), social processes (M ϭ 6.87%,
SD ϭ 3.06%), and affective processes (M ϭ
5.27%, SD ϭ 2.30%). With regard to cognitive
processes, participants primarily used words in-
dicating insight (e.g., think, know; M ϭ 5.43%,
SD ϭ 2.45%) and inclusiveness (e.g., and, with;
M ϭ 5.37%, SD ϭ 2.04%). Regarding social
processes, they chiefly discussed their perceived
relationship to God. Last, regarding affective
processes, they discussed significantly more
positive emotions (e.g., love, kindness; M ϭ
Table 1
Pretest and Posttest Descriptive Statistics and Between-Subjects Condition-by-Time Interaction Effects for
Study Variables
Intervention (n ϭ 32) Control (n ϭ 29)
Variable
Pretest
M (SD)
Posttest
M (SD)
Pretest
M (SD)
Posttest
M (SD) C ϫ T p
God Adjective Checklist (GAC)
Positively valenced God imagesa
95.25 (10.31) 97.35 (9.15) 93.93 (10.42) 93.69 (11.53) .38 .540
Negatively valenced God images 35.63 (13.34) 35.23 (14.74) 32.37 (12.17) 33.14 (14.57) .05 .818
Attachment to God Inventory (AGI)
God attachment anxiety 53.63 (13.09) 51.42 (11.56) 46.07 (15.15) 47.62 (14.58) .57 .454
God attachment avoidance 40.38 (11.28) 40.74 (12.06) 42.56 (14.81) 42.79 (15.59) .00 .979
Single-item head–heart congruence 4.69 (1.28) 5.00 (1.44) 5.29 (.94) 4.93 (1.46) 2.01 .159
Note. C ϭ condition; T ϭ time. Possible values for the subscales of the God Adjective Checklist (Zahl & Gibson, 2012)
range from 15 to 105, possible values for the two subscales of the Attachment to God Inventory (Beck & McDonald, 2004)
range from 14 to 98, and possible values for the single-item head–heart congruence measure (cf. Thomas et al., 2011) range
from 1 to 7. For each variable, higher scores indicate higher levels of the measured construct.
a
This variable’s scores were substantially skewed both at pretest and posttest (Ϫ2.94 and Ϫ1.29, respectively), but
transformations did not improve this variable’s normality.
10 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
3.67%, SD ϭ 1.83%) than negative emotions
(e.g., anxiety, sadness; M ϭ 1.31%, SD ϭ
1.47%), t(25) ϭ 5.09, p Ͻ .001.
Discussion
Taken together, neither our quantitative data
from self-report measures nor our quantified
qualitative data from figure drawings supported
our hypotheses. Needless to say, this is disap-
pointing news for any researcher. However, text
analysis of intervention participants’ posttest
journal entries (as well as intervention partici-
pants’ spontaneous comments during debriefing
interviews) suggested that intervention partici-
pants experienced an increase in adaptive, pos-
itively valenced religious/spiritual meaning
making, including increases in insight and pos-
itive emotions. This mixture of findings is puz-
zling indeed.
There are several possibilities for explaining
our largely null results. It is possible our manu-
alized GINT intervention was not effective in
promoting the majority of changes we hypoth-
esized. However, such a possibility seems un-
likely, given that in their posttest journal entries
and debriefing interviews, most intervention
participants reported experiencing spiritual and
psychological growth. Another possibility is
that our mostly null findings were artifacts of
several previously identified measurement is-
sues (e.g., lack of sufficient psychometric ro-
bustness; ceiling and floor effects). For exam-
ple, self-report measures of God representations
and God attachment may be highly susceptible
to socially desirable responding (along with as-
sociated ceiling and floor effects; Gibson, 2007;
Moriarty & Davis, 2012), particularly among
highly religious persons (see Sedikides & Ge-
bauer, 2010). Thus, for our intervention partic-
ipants, it is possible their pretest responses were
largely influenced by socially desirable re-
sponding (e.g., reflecting positive impression
management or self-deception), whereas their
posttest responses represented more authentic
self-descriptions (e.g., representing actual gains
achieved through intervention participation).
Also, as numerous scholars have noted, self-
report measures of God representations and God
attachment likely tend to assess consciously ac-
cessible aspects of these constructs, even
though God representations (particularly God
images) and God attachment may be largely
processed outside conscious awareness (Davis
et al., 2013, 2016; Hall & Fujikawa, 2013; Mo-
riarty & Davis, 2012; Zahl & Gibson, 2012;
Zahl et al., 2013).
In sum, there is a need for advances in the
measurement of God representations and God
attachment. In particular, there is a need for God
representation and God attachment measures to
(a) have inbuilt validity scales; (b) have varying
levels of item nuance, sophistication, and diffi-
culty (to lessen the likelihood of ceiling and
floor effects); (c) have reliable and valid ways to
measure implicit God images and God attach-
ment; and (d) be sensitive enough to track
changes over time (e.g., over the course of an
intervention; Davis et al., 2016; Hall & Fu-
jikawa, 2013; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; Zahl et
al., 2013). This last suggestion may be espe-
cially challenging when interventions are brief
and when reliable change actually may occur
largely after treatment has ended (e.g., during a
follow-up period; cf. Levenson, 2010; Shedler,
2010).
Moreover, there is a need for increased reli-
ance on non-self-report methods for measuring
God representations (e.g., God concepts and
images) and related constructs (e.g., head–heart
congruence and God attachment). Because
these constructs are highly nuanced, complex,
and context-dependent—and because these con-
structs may largely operate outside conscious
awareness (e.g., Davis et al., 2013, 2016; Zahl
et al., 2013)—it may be that existing self-report
measures of these constructs do not have the
sophistication, sensitivity, or psychometric ro-
bustness to assess their underlying constructs
reliably and validly. Instead, non-self-report
measures may be a better alternative. In partic-
ular, the use of projective measures (e.g., God
representation figure drawings) and narrative
methods (e.g., narrative inquiry, text analysis)
may prove to be especially effective and eluci-
dating ways to measure God images and God
attachment (Davis et al., 2016; Hall, 2007; Mo-
riarty & Davis, 2012). Toward that end, we
encourage religious cognition scholars to uti-
lize, evaluate, and refine our preliminarily val-
idated Integrative Scoring System for God Rep-
resentation Figure Drawings (Davis et al.,
2014b). We also encourage the development
and validation of narrative-based God image
and God attachment measures, such as inter-
view and scoring systems similar to that of the
11GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
well-utilized and well-validated Adult Attach-
ment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main,
1996; Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, 2013). Last, we
encourage the use of text-analysis programs
such as LIWC (Pennebaker et al., 2007) to
analyze text from written or transcribed narra-
tives (e.g., journal entries or transcribed inter-
views about one’s perceived relationship to
God; Davis et al., 2016).
Limitations
Our study had several limitations. Most no-
tably, our sample size was quite small (N ϭ 61;
thus our study was underpowered), and we did
not utilize random assignment (thus there was
self-selection bias in sampling). With regard to
sample size, a statistical power analysis using
G‫ء‬
Power 3.0.10 indicated that, to use a mixed-
model repeated measures MANOVA to detect a
medium-sized effect (f ϭ .25) with two groups
and two time periods, a total sample size of 210
(110 per group) would have been required. In
terms of random assignment, we designed the
study to give participants a dichotomous choice
between the intervention or measures-only
(control) group, based on their level of interest
and availability. Also, due to logistical con-
straints, we had to conduct the study within one
Spring semester (15 weeks), before all partici-
pants and facilitators left for the summer,
thereby precluding a waitlist-control group.
The generalizability of our findings is also
limited, because our sample was (a) undergrad-
uate and graduate students, (b) Christian (pre-
dominantly Protestant Christian), (c) highly re-
ligious, and (d) predominantly female. In
addition, we relied heavily on self-report mea-
sures, some of which demonstrated poor psy-
chometric properties (e.g., the GAC demon-
strated substantial skewness, seemingly due to
ceiling effects), whereas other measures may
not have been sensitive enough to detect
changes that did occur. Furthermore, we as-
sessed only spiritual outcomes. In hindsight,
assessing psychological outcomes (e.g.,
changes in self-esteem, depressive and anxious
symptoms, or perceived social support) would
have been useful too. Also, we only assessed
dispositional spiritual outcomes (e.g., God rep-
resentations and attachment), but assessing con-
textual/situational spiritual outcomes (e.g., reli-
gious/spiritual struggles) would have been
interesting as well. In fact, doing so may have
been more likely to yield significant quantita-
tive results, given the greater likelihood of psy-
chotherapeutically promoting change in situa-
tional than dispositional outcomes, especially
when it comes to brief psychotherapeutic inter-
ventions such as ours. Moreover, we did not
assess participant personality characteristics
that might have moderated the intervention’s
effects. For instance, we perhaps should have
measured trait extraversion, given that previous
research suggests extraverts are more likely to
benefit from group psychotherapy than intro-
verts (Ogrodniczuk et al., 2003).
Last, the study design did not include fol-
low-up assessment (e.g., at 3-, 6-, and/or 12-
months postintervention). It is possible a narra-
tive-experiential approach to promoting God-
image change may lead to beneficial effects that
emerge largely in the weeks and months follow-
ing intervention (i.e., the so-called “incubator
effect”). As noted earlier, with insight-oriented
and experiential approaches such as the one we
utilized in this brief intervention, reliable
change often emerges after the intervention con-
cludes (Levenson, 2010; Shedler, 2010). There-
fore, future outcome research in this area should
include follow-up assessment as well.
Conclusions and Suggestions for
Future Research
Consistent with what prior researchers have
proposed (Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Hall,
2007; Moriarty & Davis, 2012), some of our
qualitative data (e.g., posttest journal entries
and debriefing interviews) suggest narrative-
experiential interventions (e.g., GINT) may pro-
mote positive spiritual and psychological out-
comes, including changes in God images, God
attachment, head–heart congruence, God-image
narrative identity, and general narrative iden-
tity. However, our quantitative data did not cor-
roborate these hypothesized outcomes. Hence,
much more research on GINT is needed, and
there are several measurement issues that need
to be addressed so that quantitative data, qual-
itative data, and quantified projective data may
yield more valid and reliable results. Because of
the nuance and complexity of these constructs,
mixed-methods designs perhaps hold the most
promise for yielding robust and clinically mean-
12 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
ingful findings (Davis et al., 2016). More spir-
itually diverse samples are needed as well.
We agree with scholars who have recom-
mended the more frequent use of qualitative and
mixed methods to study God images and God
attachment (Davis et al., 2016; Gibson, 2007;
Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, 2013; Hall & Fu-
jikawa, 2013). We also agree that religious cog-
nition researchers need to rely increasingly on
non-self-report methods (e.g., projective mea-
sures, narrative methods) for assessing God im-
ages and God attachment. As Zahl and Gibson
(2012) have noted, self-report measures neces-
sarily reflect information that is consciously
available and filtered through a respondent’s
reflection and state of mind, making such mea-
sures subject to the constraints of what can be
expressed propositionally in words. Addition-
ally, we concur there is a particular need for
more outcome research (with follow-up assess-
ment) on psychotherapeutically mediated
changes in God images and God attachment
(Hall & Fujikawa, 2013; Thomas et al., 2011;
Tisdale et al., 1997). In particular, we encourage
researchers to explore the interconnections
among healthy human attachments, self-esteem,
and relational spirituality (as defined above),
especially the possibility that psychotherapeuti-
cally promoted changes in one of those areas
will lead to corresponding improvements in the
others (cf. Mikulincer & Shaver, 2004; Mori-
arty & Davis, 2012). More broadly, we encour-
age researchers to explore the interconnections
among mental/neural representations of God,
self, and others. Ultimately, we eagerly antici-
pate what future research on relational spiritu-
ality will discover as researchers seek to illumi-
nate the complexities of how people view and
relate to God and how they deconstruct and
reauthor their spiritual and life narratives.
References
Baucom, K. J., Baucom, B. R., & Christensen, A.
(2012). Do the naïve know best? The predictive
power of naïve ratings of couple interactions. Psy-
chological Assessment, 24, 983–994. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0028680
Beck, R., & McDonald, A. (2004). Attachment to
God: The Attachment to God Inventory, tests of
working model correspondence, and an explora-
tion of faith group differences. Journal of Psychol-
ogy and Theology, 3, 92–103.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive sci-
ence: A multiple code theory. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Cheston, S. E., Piedmont, R. L., Eanes, B., & Lavin,
L. P. (2003). Changes in clients’ image of God
over the course of outpatient therapy. Counseling
and Values, 47, 96–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
j.2161-007X.2003.tb00227.x
Cozolino, L. (2010). The neuroscience of psychother-
apy (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Norton.
Davis, E. B. (2009). God image narrative therapy: A
treatment manual, Unpublished manuscript. Vir-
ginia Beach, VA: Regent University. Retrieved
from http://www.wheaton.edu/Academics/Faculty/
D/Ward-Davis
Davis, E. B., & Badenoch, B. (2010). Storying God
images: Bringing narrative integration to our ex-
perience of the divine. GAINS Quarterly, Winter
2009, 14–24. Retrieved from http://www.wheaton
.edu/Academics/Faculty/D/Ward-Davis
Davis, E. B., Cuthbert, A. D., Hays, L. W., Aten,
J. D., Van Tongeren, D. R., Hook, J. N., . . . Boan,
D. (2016). Using qualitative and mixed methods to
study relational spirituality. Psychology of Reli-
gion and Spirituality, 8, 92–98. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/rel0000046
Davis, E. B., Moriarty, G. L., & Mauch, J. C. (2013).
God images and God concepts: Definitions, devel-
opment, and dynamics. Psychology of Religion
and Spirituality, 5, 51–60. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0029289
Davis, E. B., Tisdale, T. C., Olson, T., Park, E., Nam,
J., Moriarty, G. L., & Cuthbert, A. D. (2014a). God
Representation Figure Drawings (GRFD): Perfor-
mance-based measure of God representations. Un-
published instrument. Retrieved from http://www
.wheaton.edu/Academics/Faculty/D/Ward-Davis
Davis, E. B., Tisdale, T. C., Olson, T., Park, E., Nam,
J., Moriarty, G. L., & Cuthbert, A. D. (2014b).
Integrative Scoring System for God Representation
Figure Drawings (ISSGRFD): Rubric for scoring
God representation figure drawings. Unpublished
instrument. Retrieved from http://www.wheaton
.edu/Academics/Faculty/D/Ward-Davis
Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., Watson, J. C., Timulak,
L., & Freire, E. (2013). Research on humanistic-
experiential psychotherapies. In M. J. Lambert
(Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change (6th ed., pp. 495–538). New York, NY:
Wiley.
Gallup, G., Jr., & Jones, S. (1989). One hundred
questions and answers: Religion in America.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton Religious Research Cen-
ter.
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult
Attachment Interview protocol (3rd ed.). Unpub-
lished manuscript, University of California at
Berkeley.
13GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
Gibson, N. J. S. (2007). Measurement issues in God
image research and practice. In G. L. Moriarty &
L. Hoffman (Eds.), God image handbook for spir-
itual counseling and psychotherapy (pp. 227–246).
Binghamton, NY: Haworth/Routledge.
Granqvist, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2008). Attach-
ment and religious representations and behavior.
In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical appli-
cations (2nd ed., pp. 906–933). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Granqvist, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2013). Religion,
spirituality, and attachment. In K. I. Pargament
(Ed.), APA handbook of psychology, religion, and
spirituality (Vol. 1, pp. 139–155). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Hall, T. W. (2004). Christian spirituality and mental
health: A relational spirituality framework for em-
pirical research. Journal of Psychology and Chris-
tianity, 23, 66–81.
Hall, T. W. (2007). Psychoanalysis, attachment, and
spirituality part II: The spiritual stories we live by.
Journal of Psychology and Theology, 35, 29–42.
Hall, T. W., & Fujikawa, A. M. (2013). God image
and the sacred. In K. I. Pargament (Ed.), APA
handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality
(Vol. 1, pp. 277–292). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Hall, T. W., Fujikawa, A., Halcrow, S. R., Hill, P. C.,
& Delaney, H. (2009). Attachment to God and
implicit spirituality: Clarifying correspondence
and compensation models. Journal of Psychology
and Theology, 37, 227–242.
Levenson, H. (2010). Brief dynamic therapy. Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Madigan, S. (2011). Narrative therapy. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
McAdams, D. P. (1993). The stories we live by. New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
McAdams, D. P. (2008). Personal narratives and the
life story. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A.
Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality (3rd ed.,
pp. 242–262). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Mihura, J. L., Meyer, G. J., Dumitrascu, N., &
Bombel, G. (2013). The validity of individual Ror-
schach variables: Systematic reviews and meta-
analyses of the comprehensive system. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 139, 548–605. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0029406
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Security-
based self-representations in adulthood: Contents
and processes. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson
(Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical implications (pp. 159–195). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Moriarty, G. L., & Davis, E. B. (2012). Client God
images: Theory, research, and clinical practice. In
J. Aten, K. O’Grady, & E. Worthington, Jr., (Eds.),
The psychology of religion and spirituality for
clinicians (pp. 131–160). New York, NY: Rout-
ledge.
Murray-Swank, N. A., & Pargament, K. I. (2005).
God, where are you?: Evaluating a spiritually-
integrated intervention for sexual abuse. Mental
Health, Religion & Culture, 8, 191–203. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1080/13694670500138866
Ogrodniczuk, J. S., Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., Mc-
Callum, M., & Rosie, J. S. (2003). NEO-five factor
personality traits as predictors of response to two
forms of group psychotherapy. International Jour-
nal of Group Psychotherapy, 53, 417–442. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1521/ijgp.53.4.417.42832
Pargament, K. I. (2007). Spiritually-integrated psy-
chotherapy: Understanding and addressing the sa-
cred. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Pargament, K. I. (2013). Searching for the sacred:
Toward a nonreductionistic theory of spirituality.
In K. I. Pargament (Ed.), APA handbook of psy-
chology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1, pp.
257–273). Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
ical Association.
Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E.
(2007). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC
2001 Operator’s manual. Austin, TX: LIWC.net.
Pew Research Center. (2008). U.S. Religious Land-
scape Survey, Pt. 2: Religious beliefs and prac-
tices. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from
http://religions.pewforum.org/
Pew Research Center. (2012). The global religious
landscape: A report on the size and distribution of
the world’s major religious groups as of 2010.
Washington, DC: Author.
Rizzuto, A. (1979). The birth of the living God.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Sedikides, C., & Gebauer, J. E. (2010). Religiosity as
self-enhancement: A meta-analysis of the relation
between socially desirable responding and religi-
osity. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
14, 17–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/10888
68309351002
Shedler, J. (2010). The efficacy of psychodynamic
psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 65, 98–
109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018378
Shin, J. Y., & Steger, M. F. (2014). Promoting mean-
ing and purpose in life. In A. C. Parks & S. M.
Schueller (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell Handbook
of Positive Psychological Interventions (pp. 90–
110). Malden, MA: Wiley Blackwell. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1002/9781118315927.ch5
Siegel, D. J. (2012a). The developing mind (2nd ed.).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Siegel, D. J. (2012b). Pocket guide to interpersonal
neurobiology. New York, NY: Norton.
Thomas, M., Moriarty, G., Davis, E., & Anderson, L.
(2011). The effects of a manualized group-
14 OLSON ET AL.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
psychotherapy intervention on client God images
and attachment to God: A pilot study. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 39, 44–58.
Tisdale, T. C., Key, T. L., Kemperman, S. R., Cloud,
H., Townsend, J., & Okamoto, T. (1997). Impact
of treatment on God image and personal adjust-
ment, and correlations of God image to personal
adjustment and object relations development.
Journal of Psychology and Theology, 25, 227–239.
Waldinger, R. J., Schulz, M. S., Hauser, S. T., Allen,
J. P., & Crowell, J. A. (2004). Reading others
emotions: The role of intuitive judgments in pre-
dicting marital satisfaction, quality, and stability.
Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 58–71. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.58
Zahl, B. P., & Gibson, N. J. S. (2012). God repre-
sentations, attachment to God, and satisfaction
with life: A comparison of doctrinal and experien-
tial representations of God in Christian young
adults. International Journal for the Psychology of
Religion, 22, 216–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10508619.2012.670027
Zahl, B. P., Sharp, C. A., & Gibson, N. J. S. (2013).
Empirical measures of the religious heart. In F. N.
Watts & G. Dumbreck (Eds.), Head and heart:
Perspectives from religion and psychology (pp.
97–124). West Conshohocken, PA: John Temple-
ton Press.
Received August 31, 2015
Revision received December 22, 2015
Accepted January 14, 2016 Ⅲ
15GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.

More Related Content

Similar to Olson et al., 2016 (God Image Narrative Therapy - A Mixed-Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention)

Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011
Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011
Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011Michael Thomas
 
People with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docx
People with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docxPeople with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docx
People with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docxdanhaley45372
 
Psychotherapy - The myths and realities in Pakistan
Psychotherapy - The myths and realities in PakistanPsychotherapy - The myths and realities in Pakistan
Psychotherapy - The myths and realities in PakistanNasar Khan
 
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docx
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docxJournal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docx
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docxcroysierkathey
 
Adler ASU
Adler ASUAdler ASU
Adler ASUsmccoy3
 
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceCallaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceOmar Gonzalez-Valentino
 
Kallimel dissertation proposal
Kallimel dissertation proposalKallimel dissertation proposal
Kallimel dissertation proposalgrackal
 
Journal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docx
Journal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docxJournal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docx
Journal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docxpriestmanmable
 
ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx
ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docxArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx
ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docxdavezstarr61655
 
Psychology and Spirituality I
Psychology and Spirituality IPsychology and Spirituality I
Psychology and Spirituality Iruffing
 
Positive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing World
Positive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing WorldPositive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing World
Positive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing WorldMindful Summits
 
Spirituality in nursing
Spirituality in nursingSpirituality in nursing
Spirituality in nursingVasudevan BK
 
Kuhtreiber Mindfulness Poster
Kuhtreiber Mindfulness PosterKuhtreiber Mindfulness Poster
Kuhtreiber Mindfulness PosterKara Kuhtreiber
 
Running head PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docx
Running head  PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docxRunning head  PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docx
Running head PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docxtoddr4
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...inventionjournals
 
MFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, Travis
MFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, TravisMFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, Travis
MFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, TravisXander Schmidtz
 
Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine...
Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine... Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine...
Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine... dyutirajagiri
 

Similar to Olson et al., 2016 (God Image Narrative Therapy - A Mixed-Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention) (20)

Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011
Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011
Thomas, Moriarty, Davis and Anderson, 2011
 
People with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docx
People with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docxPeople with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docx
People with Disabilities (PWD) Part 1 The best and most be.docx
 
Psychotherapy - The myths and realities in Pakistan
Psychotherapy - The myths and realities in PakistanPsychotherapy - The myths and realities in Pakistan
Psychotherapy - The myths and realities in Pakistan
 
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docx
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docxJournal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docx
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 15107–122, 2013Co.docx
 
Adler ASU
Adler ASUAdler ASU
Adler ASU
 
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceCallaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
 
Kallimel dissertation proposal
Kallimel dissertation proposalKallimel dissertation proposal
Kallimel dissertation proposal
 
Journal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docx
Journal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docxJournal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docx
Journal of Traumatic StressFebruary 2015, 28, 57–64Spiri.docx
 
ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx
ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docxArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx
ArticleSocial Identity Reduces Depression byFostering Po.docx
 
Psychology and Spirituality I
Psychology and Spirituality IPsychology and Spirituality I
Psychology and Spirituality I
 
An Expert Look At Ebook
An Expert Look At EbookAn Expert Look At Ebook
An Expert Look At Ebook
 
Positive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing World
Positive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing WorldPositive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing World
Positive Neuroplasticity: How to Grow Resilient Well-Being In a Changing World
 
Spirituality in nursing
Spirituality in nursingSpirituality in nursing
Spirituality in nursing
 
Kuhtreiber Mindfulness Poster
Kuhtreiber Mindfulness PosterKuhtreiber Mindfulness Poster
Kuhtreiber Mindfulness Poster
 
The Impact of Spirituality on Counseling
The Impact of Spirituality on CounselingThe Impact of Spirituality on Counseling
The Impact of Spirituality on Counseling
 
Running head PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docx
Running head  PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docxRunning head  PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docx
Running head PERSPECTIVE ON INTEGRATION BETWEEN CHRISTIAN FAITH .docx
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
 
Introduction to spirituality
Introduction to spiritualityIntroduction to spirituality
Introduction to spirituality
 
MFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, Travis
MFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, TravisMFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, Travis
MFQ Poster 2 - Emily, Xander, Travis
 
Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine...
Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine... Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine...
Spirituality in Family....Dr. catherine...
 

Olson et al., 2016 (God Image Narrative Therapy - A Mixed-Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention)

  • 1. Spirituality in Clinical Practice God Image Narrative Therapy: A Mixed-Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention Trevor Olson, Theresa Clement Tisdale, Edward B. Davis, Elizabeth A. Park, Jiyun Nam, Glendon L. Moriarty, Don E. Davis, Michael J. Thomas, Andrew D. Cuthbert, and Lance W. Hays Online First Publication, May 12, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096 CITATION Olson, T., Tisdale, T. C., Davis, E. B., Park, E. A., Nam, J., Moriarty, G. L., Davis, D. E., Thomas, M. J., Cuthbert, A. D., & Hays, L. W. (2016, May 12). God Image Narrative Therapy: A Mixed- Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention. Spirituality in Clinical Practice. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096
  • 2. God Image Narrative Therapy: A Mixed-Methods Investigation of a Controlled Group-Based Spiritual Intervention Trevor Olson His Mansion Ministries, Hillsboro, New Hampshire Theresa Clement Tisdale Azusa Pacific University Edward B. Davis Wheaton College Elizabeth A. Park Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California Jiyun Nam Azusa Pacific University Glendon L. Moriarty Regent University Don E. Davis Georgia State University Michael J. Thomas Eastern University Andrew D. Cuthbert and Lance W. Hays Wheaton College We report findings from a controlled, manualized 10-week group-based spiritual intervention designed to improve God images, attachment to God, and narrative identity, using primarily narrative and experiential interventions. Participants were 61 Christian adults (n ϭ 32 intervention, n ϭ 29 matched controls) from the student population of 2 faith-based universities. Quantitative results (including data from self-report measures and quantified data from God-representation figure drawings) yielded nonsignificant findings. However, in posttest journal entries and during de- briefing interviews, intervention participants reported experiencing positive changes in God images, God attachment, and narrative identity. These discrepant results are Trevor Olson, His Mansion Ministries, Hillsboro, New Hampshire; Theresa Clement Tisdale, Department of Grad- uate Psychology, Azusa Pacific University; Edward B. Davis, Department of Psychology, Wheaton College; Eliz- abeth A. Park, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los An- geles, California; Jiyun Nam, Department of Graduate Psy- chology, Azusa Pacific University; Glendon L. Moriarty, School of Psychology and Counseling, Regent University; Don E. Davis, Psychology Department, Georgia State Uni- versity; Michael J. Thomas, Department of Psychology, Eastern University; Andrew D. Cuthbert, and Lance W. Hays, Department of Psychology, Wheaton College. This research was the focus of Trevor Olson’s dissertation at Regent University. The study was funded in part by a grant to Theresa Clement Tisdale from the Faculty Research Coun- cil at Azusa Pacific University. This publication was also made possible through the support of a grant from the John Templeton Foundation (Grant 44040) to Edward B. Davis and Don E. Davis. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the John Templeton Foundation. We thank Ken Pargament for his review and comments on an earlier version of this article. The authors would also like to thank the research assistants (Hershey Desai, Zy- anya Mendoza, and Elizabeth Park) and group facilitators for the intervention condition (Neal Collins, Lauren De- Silva, Lacey Edwards, Christina Girlando, Cormack Mc- Kinney, Cory McKinsey, Vanessa Myers, Chelsae Roby Pistello, Kristin Shier, Tiffany Spooner, Bethany Vaudrey, Ashley Wickliffe, and Joshua Ziesel). The authors would also like to thank the research assistant (Carl Waitz) and raters who served on the validation project for the Integra- tive Scoring System for God Representation Figure Draw- ings (Rebekah Bartholomew, Hughes Choy, Rebecca Diercks, Lacey Edwards, Sabrina Ferris, Irene Kao, Jin Kim, Ronilleen Maglian, Jacqueline Orozco, Rachel Sills, Allison Vrieze, and Brigette Worthen). Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- dressed to Theresa Clement Tisdale, Department of Grad- uate Psychology, Azusa Pacific University, 701 East Foot- hill Boulevard, Azusa, CA 91702. E-mail: tctisdale@apu .edu ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. Spirituality in Clinical Practice © 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 3, No. 2, 000 2326-4500/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096 1
  • 3. discussed in terms of the existing literature, with a focus on measurement issues. We also discuss the potential of using qualitative and mixed-methods research to study God images and God attachment, particularly (a) in the context of outcome research and (b) if non-self-report methods (e.g., projective measures, narrative methods) are utilized more heavily than self-report methods. Keywords: God images, attachment, narrative, relational spirituality, outcome research Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/scp0000096.supp Over 80% of the world’s population self- identifies with a religious group, and within the United States, 60% of adults report believing that God is a person with whom one can have a relationship (Pew Research Center, 2008, 2012). In fact, within the United States, most religious/spiritual persons view their perceived relationship to God as central to what it means to be religious or spiritual (Gallup & Jones, 1989; Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, 2008; Moriarty & Davis, 2012). There is increasing scientific interest in un- derstanding how people conceptually view and experientially relate to God—that is, people’s God concepts and images (Davis et al., 2016; Davis, Moriarty, & Mauch, 2013). However, apart from a few exceptions (e.g., Cheston, Piedmont, Eanes, & Lavin, 2003; Murray- Swank & Pargament, 2005; Thomas, Moriarty, Davis, & Anderson, 2011; Tisdale et al., 1997), little research has studied psychotherapeutically promoted God-image change. Recent theoreti- cal developments in the God image literature (e.g., Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012) suggest narrative-experiential ap- proaches may be effective in promoting positive God-image change (e.g., more benevolent God images), but as of yet, no peer-reviewed studies have examined this possibility. Thus, we report findings from a controlled and manualized 10- week group-based spiritual intervention de- signed to promote adaptive God image change, using primarily narrative and experiential inter- ventions. Definitions and Theoretical Framework God Representations (God Concepts and Images) God representations refer to mental (e.g., cognitive structures) or neural (e.g., neural fir- ing pattern) representations of God (Davis et al., 2013). It has been postulated (Rizzuto, 1979) that there are two main types of God represen- tations: God concepts and God images. God concepts are propositional religious cognitions (e.g., affect-light, doctrinal, head knowledge) that mentally/neurally underlie the ways people conceptually view God. That is, God concepts are the conceptual God representations that un- derlie one’s theological understanding of God. In contrast, God images are implicational reli- gious cognitions (e.g., affect-laden, experien- tial, heart knowledge) that underlie the ways people experientially relate to God. That is, God images refer to the relational and emotional God representations that underlie one’s embodied, emotional experience in perceived relationship to God (Davis et al., 2013, 2016; Hall & Fu- jikawa, 2013; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; Rizzuto, 1979). Whereas God concepts tend to be more belief-oriented (i.e., affect-light, cortically dom- inant), God images tend to be more emotion- oriented (i.e., affect-laden, subcortically domi- nant). See Davis et al. (2013, 2016) for elaboration on these definitions. Healthy Relational Spirituality and Adaptive Head–Heart Congruence Pargament (2007, 2013) has argued that a healthy spirituality is characterized by a high degree of integration among its ingredients (e.g., one’s beliefs, practices, and experiences), whereas an unhealthy spirituality is character- ized by a low degree of integration among its ingredients. Building on this conceptualization, a healthy relational spirituality can be defined as the degree to which a person’s head (doctri- nal) and heart (experiential) knowledge of God is congruent, well-integrated, and contextually adaptive across time and situations. Stated dif- ferently, a healthy relational spirituality is char- acterized by a consistently high degree of adap- tive head–heart congruence—that is, resonant 2 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 4. correspondence between one’s head and heart knowledge of God, if that resonant head and heart knowledge is (a) contextually adaptive (e.g., associated with positively valenced psy- chological and spiritual outcomes within the context of one’s life [situational context] and culture [sociocultural context]) and (b) consis- tently evident across time and situations (cf. Moriarty & Davis, 2012; Pargament, 2007, 2013; Zahl & Gibson, 2012; Zahl, Sharp, & Gibson, 2013). Increasing adaptive head–heart congruence is a vital goal of psychotherapeuti- cally promoted God-image change (Moriarty & Davis, 2012). Narratives, Narrative Identity, and God-Image Narrative Identity Narratives refer to “the linear telling of a sequence of events [and they] entail a focus on action and on the mental states of the individu- als of the story, including the self or narrator” (Siegel, 2012b, p. 478). More specifically, nar- rative identity refers to “an individual’s inter- nalized, evolving, and integrative story of the self” (McAdams, 2008, p. 242), and God-image narrative identity refers to one’s narrative iden- tity in perceived relationship to God (Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012). Theoretical Framework Relational Spirituality In the current study, our intervention proto- col’s (Davis, 2009) conceptualization of God image development and dynamics was based on Hall’s (2004) relational-spirituality theory (cf. Hall, 2007; Hall, Fujikawa, Halcrow, Hill, & Delaney, 2009). Therefore, it was undergirded by that theory’s central organizing principles, as described in Hall (2004). For example, it was based on the following two assumptions: Implicit relational representations are repetitions of relational experiences, sharing a common affective core, that are conceptually encoded in the mind as nonpropositional meaning structures. They are the memory basis for implicit relational [knowing]; that is, our “gut level” sense of how significant relationships work. (Hall, 2004, p. 71) Implicit relational representations, formed particularly from experiences [in] early relationships with caregiv- ers, shape the emotional appraisal of meaning and subsequent patterns of relationship. (Hall, 2004, p. 72) Implicit Relational Knowing Correspondence Because we adopted Hall and colleagues’ (Hall, 2004; Hall et al., 2009) relational- spirituality framework, our conceptualization of God image development and dynamics assumed implicit-relational-knowing correspondence. In other words, we assumed people’s implicit re- lational knowing about how to perceive, expe- rience, and interact with a divine attachment figure (e.g., God) corresponds to their implicit relational knowing about how to perceive, ex- perience, and interact with human attachment figures (Davis et al., 2013; Hall, 2004; Hall et al., 2009; Moriarty & Davis, 2012). Narrative Identity Additionally, we adopted McAdams’s (1993, 2008) narrative identity framework, particularly the understanding that a healthy narrative identity is reflected in personal nar- ratives exhibiting high degrees of coherence, openness, credibility, differentiation, recon- ciliation, and generative integration. Specifi- cally, a healthy narrative identity emerges from personal narratives typifying the follow- ing six narrative standards: • Coherence: The narrative’s components (e.g., events, themes, characters, settings) logically relate to and make sense in light of each other, without excessive contradic- tion. • Openness: The narrative is flexible, adap- tive, and resilient, and it is welcoming of adaptive change and growth. • Credibility: The narrative is based on facts and on reality-based perceptions of events, without gross or fantasy-based distortions. • Differentiation: The narrative is rich with depth, texture, and complexity, including many layers of nuanced meaning. • Reconciliation: The narrative’s differenti- ated elements and conflicting tensions cul- minate in harmonious resolution. • Generative Integration: The narrative’s protagonist is well-integrated into the soci- ety in which he or she is embedded and has become dedicated to bettering the lives of others, particularly the next generation (McAdams, 1993). 3GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 5. Interventions for Promoting Adaptive God-Image Change Clinical Trials A few clinical trials have examined the pro- motion of adaptive God-image change through psychotherapy. For example, Tisdale et al. (1997) found spiritually integrated multimodal psychotherapy in an inpatient setting was effec- tive in promoting improvements in God images, object relations development, and self-esteem, and all three types of improvements were main- tained over a 12-month follow-up period. In a similar study, Cheston et al. (2003) found that traditional individual psychotherapy in an out- patient setting was also effective in promoting positive God-image change, but only for people who evidenced significant corresponding emo- tional and spiritual change (as per clinician rat- ings). Taken together, these studies suggest psy- chotherapeutically promoted God-image change is closely connected to other forms of emotional and spiritual change (e.g., improve- ments in self-esteem and object relations devel- opment). As Moriarty and Davis (2012) have argued, it may be that healthy human attach- ments, healthy self-esteem, and healthy rela- tional spirituality (as defined above) are inti- mately related and reciprocally interactive, such that improvements in one of these areas leads to improvements in the others. For instance, as people experience greater attachment security in their human relationships, they may also de- velop higher self-esteem and become more psy- chologically able to experience love, security, acceptance, and closeness in their perceived re- lationship to God (cf. Mikulincer & Shaver, 2004). Manualized Interventions Two other studies have examined the promo- tion of God-image change through participation in a manualized psychotherapeutic intervention. In a small-scale study with two adult female survivors of sexual abuse, Murray-Swank and Pargament (2005) found a brief, manualized, spiritually integrated, individual psychotherapy intervention was effective in promoting positive God-image change. Similarly, in a noncon- trolled pilot study with 26 Christian adults, Thomas et al. (2011) found a brief, manualized, spiritually integrated, group psychotherapy in- tervention was effective in promoting positive shifts in God images and God attachment, as well as increased adaptive head–heart congru- ence. In sum, outcome research has provided pre- liminary evidence that God-image change can be effected through a variety of therapy modal- ities and formats (e.g., inpatient or outpatient; multimodal, individual, or group; broad-based or targeted; manualized or nonmanualized). However, each of these studies had notable methodological limitations (e.g., no control group; sole use of quantitative measures; non- manualized intervention), as Thomas et al. (2011) detailed. Narrative-Experiential Approaches to Promoting Adaptive God-Image Change Recently, scholars have discussed the prom- ise of using narrative-experiential approaches to promote God-image change (Davis & Bade- noch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012). Indeed, Cozolino (2010); Hall (2007), and Siegel (2012a, 2012b) have described the myriad sci- entifically grounded reasons why coherent nar- ratives offer an optimal mechanism for promot- ing psychotherapeutic change. For example, by their very nature, coherent narratives link input from several layers of mental processing (e.g., sensations, images, feelings, and thoughts; left- and right-mode processes; implicit and explicit mental processes) and several types of neural circuits (e.g., cortical and subcortical circuits, sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous sys- tems). Through this linking, coherent narratives promote health and well-being (a) by facilitat- ing various types of mental and neural integra- tion and (b) by supporting affect regulation and maintaining homeostasis, both in the present and across time (Cozolino, 2010; Siegel, 2012a). When using narrative-experiential interventions to transform God images, the focus is primarily on promoting narrative integration through facilitat- ing new experiences and new understandings (cf. Levenson, 2010). In particular, God-image narra- tive therapy (GINT) has a three-fold treatment goal: the integration of a client’s spiritual narra- tive, the integration of a client’s life narrative, and the integration of a client’s spiritual narrative with his or her life narrative. Toward these ends, in 4 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 6. GINT the psychotherapist uses traditional narra- tive therapy techniques, such as deconstructing problem-saturated narratives, asking questions that help link events across time and explore iden- tity-relevant themes, and coconstructing alterna- tive growth-promoting storylines (Davis & Bade- noch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; cf. Madigan, 2011). GINT is a spiritually oriented, narrative- experiential intervention that is based on the recognition that attachment is a central human need and motivation. Thus, it is based on the assumption that affect-involving interpersonal engagement is the primary medium and mech- anism for facilitating new experiences and new understandings. Specifically, GINT utilizes the interpersonal nature of the therapy relationship (individual therapy format) and the group con- text (group therapy format) to help clients ar- ticulate and link various components of their spiritual and life narratives, including those nar- ratives’ salient chapters and settings, characters, plots and subplots, themes and tones, images and metaphors, milestones, and conflicts (Da- vis, 2009; Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; cf. Levenson, 2010; McAdams, 1993). To help clients articulate these different nar- rative components, narrative and experiential interventions are used during and between ses- sions. For example, between sessions, clients use artistic, reflective, and expressive writing exercises to articulate aspects of their narra- tives. Then during sessions clients are asked to share these aspects of their narratives and ver- bally process the meanings they are ascribing to their life experiences. Through reflection, artic- ulation, and interactive engagement, partici- pants bring to the surface the emotions and physiological sensations that are associated with their implicit relational knowing of self, others, and God. Verbal and written reflection is used to attach words, metaphors, and images to these emotions and sensations, thereby promot- ing narratives that are coherent (e.g., both log- ical and emotionally meaningful). The last phase of this treatment method is dedicated to (a) coconstructing anticipated future storylines that are likely to foster continued growth and well-being and (b) empowering clients to live out those new storylines (Davis, 2009; Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Hall, 2007; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; cf. Bucci, 1997; Cozolino, 2010). Nota- bly, as research on experiential psychotherapy has reliably shown, for clients who participate in experiential psychotherapies, much of the change they experience may take place after the treatment has ended, particularly when the treat- ment is brief (see Levenson, 2010; Shedler, 2010, for reviews). Within the context of individual or group- based GINT, the interpersonal nature of narra- tive sharing is a central component of the change process. Interpersonal discourse helps clients explicate and integrate the memories, thoughts, feelings, sensations, meanings, and perceptions that are embedded within their life and spiritual narratives. Interpersonal sharing also affords opportunities for clients to explore alternative (and often more adaptive and posi- tively valenced) understandings of their life ex- periences, thereby providing ample chances for growth-promoting meaning making (cf. Madi- gan, 2011; Shin & Steger, 2014). The Current Study To date, no published studies have evaluated the efficacy of using narrative-experiential in- terventions to promote God-image change. Be- cause similar types of interventions have dem- onstrated efficacy in promoting positive therapy outcomes (e.g., see Elliott, Greenberg, Watson, Timulak, & Freire, 2013; Levenson, 2010; Shin & Steger, 2014), we hypothesized this narra- tive-experiential GINT intervention would also demonstrate efficacy. Specifically, we sought to examine the efficacy of a 10-week, manualized, narrative-experiential group psychotherapy pro- tocol focusing on promoting positive changes in God images, God attachment, and narrative identity. We predicted that, relative to control participants, intervention participants would re- port greater change in (a) both positively and negatively valenced God images (increase and decrease, respectively), (b) both God attach- ment anxiety and God attachment avoidance (decrease for both), and (c) self-reported head– heart congruence (increase). We hypothesized these changes would occur because intervention participants would have an interpersonal context within which to have new experiences and develop new understandings of their spiritual and life narratives. In particular, we surmised participants would experience in- creased attachment security in their perceived 5GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 7. relationship to God (lower God attachment anx- iety and avoidance) as they recognized God’s presence and role in their spiritual and life nar- ratives. In so doing, they would report experi- encing positively valenced God-image change and increased head–heart congruence. Further- more, given both the empathic, growth-oriented psychotherapeutic context and the narrative fo- cus of the intervention, we expected participants would discover more adaptive, hopeful mean- ings embedded within their spiritual and life narratives, while concurrently achieving greater coherence in those narratives. In this study, we sought to improve upon three aforementioned limitations of prior research on God-image change by (a) including a control group, (b) employing a mixed-methods design (collecting quantitative and qualitative data), and (c) utiliz- ing a manualized intervention. Method Participants All participants were adult undergraduate and graduate students at two faith-based universi- ties. Participants were recruited via posted fly- ers, campus-wide e-mails, university chapel an- nouncements, or word-of-mouth (e.g., clinician and campus-chaplain referrals). They were in- vited to participate in a research study about their spiritual life, relationships, and experience of God, and they were given two options for participating: (a) completing a 10-week inter- vention group (1.5-hr sessions) or (b) complet- ing questionnaires only (at two time points). Participants self-selected to participate in the intervention condition or the measures-only condition, based on their level of interest and their availability. Thirty-two participants com- pleted the intervention condition (84% reten- tion), and 29 participants completed the mea- sures-only condition (100% retention). Intervention condition. Thirty-eight pro- spective participants completed a face-to-face screening session, and they all met inclusionary criteria: at least 18 years old; self-identified Christian; currently nonsuicidal; interested in participating in a group intervention; interested in exploring their spiritual life, relationships, and experience of God; and willing to commit to a 10-week group. In particular, these partic- ipants sought spiritual growth experiences that would help them nurture positive spiritual change in their spiritual life and their perceived relationship to God. Thirty-two participants completed the 10- week intervention condition. Of those partici- pants, there were 4 males (13%) and 28 females (87%), and the mean age was 21.97 (SD ϭ 7.11, range: 18–52 years old). Thirteen self-identified as White (41%), 8 as Black (25%), 6 as Asian (19%), 2 as Latino/a (6%), and 3 as multiracial (9%). All participants were currently either an undergraduate student (n ϭ 28, 87%) or grad- uate student (n ϭ 4, 13%). In terms of religious affiliation, 30 participants self-identified as Protestant Christian (94%), whereas 2 self- identified as Catholic Christian (6%). Partici- pants reported having been a Christian for an average of 9.41 years (SD ϭ 7.59), and every- one endorsed that religion/spirituality was ei- ther a somewhat important (n ϭ 2, 6%) or very important (n ϭ 30, 94%) part of their lives. Control condition. The 29 control partici- pants were recruited via the same methods and were matched as closely as possible with inter- vention participants, based on sex, age, and education. There were 5 males (17%) and 24 females (83%), and the mean age was 22.07 (SD ϭ 7.28, range: 18–56). Twenty self- identified as White (69%), 2 as Black (7%), 2 as Asian (7%), 3 as Latino/a (10%), 1 as Native American (3%), and 1 as multiracial (3%). All participants were currently either an undergrad- uate student (n ϭ 26, 90%) or graduate student (n ϭ 3, 10%). All 29 self-identified as Protes- tant Christian in affiliation. Participants re- ported they had been Christian for an average of 11.00 years (SD ϭ 4.16), and everyone en- dorsed that religion/spirituality was either a somewhat important (n ϭ 2, 7%) or very im- portant (n ϭ 27, 93%) part of their lives. Group Facilitators For the intervention condition, there in total were 14 group facilitators (males ϭ 5, fe- males ϭ 9; White ϭ 12, Latino/a ϭ 1, Asian ϭ 1), and each person facilitated only one group. Each of the seven groups was facilitated by two doctoral psychology trainees. Five groups had a male and a female facilitator; two groups had two female facilitators. All 14 facilitators were in their mid-to-late 20s. Twelve identified as 6 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 8. Protestant Christian, and two identified as Cath- olic Christian. All facilitators were in their second, third, or fourth year of a doctoral clinical psychology program. Everyone had completed doctoral- level coursework in individual and group psy- chotherapy, and they all had at least some clin- ical experience conducting psychotherapy. Facilitators participated in a 4-hr session to train them in how to facilitate their group in adherence to the manualized protocol. To en- sure appropriate clinical care and to monitor compliance with intervention protocol, the sec- ond and sixth authors (licensed psychologists TCT and GLM) clinically supervised their re- spective institution’s facilitators. Each dyad re- ceived 1 hour of weekly supervision, starting 2 weeks before their group began and lasting until 2 weeks after their group ended. Facilitators participated in an all-facilitator debriefing ses- sion as well. Procedure Prospective participants self-selected to ei- ther the intervention or measures-only control condition. At an in-person screening meeting, prospective control participants were read the study’s cover letter and then signed the in- formed consent form, after any questions were addressed. Prospective intervention participants also completed a one-on-one screening session, involving an interview to determine whether they met inclusionary criteria. Because all in- tervention participants met these criteria, they were either notified of their group’s day/time or they signed up for a group based on compati- bility with their schedule. Then they were pro- vided: a copy of the manualized GINT protocol; the contact information for their group cofacili- tators and faculty supervisor; and details regard- ing the day, time, and location of their group. In the intervention condition, each group con- sisted of 3 to 6 members and met for ten 1.5- hour sessions, with a homework assignment be- tween sessions. Group members were strongly discouraged from missing sessions and were informed that missing three or more sessions would disqualify them from continued partici- pation. Intervention participants completed the pre- test measures between weeks 1 and 2 of the group and the posttest measures immediately following completion of the 10-week protocol. Control participants completed the posttest measures 10 weeks after they completed the pretest measures. Neither the intervention nor control participants were given any information about the measures; they were only asked to complete the measures and either return them to their group facilitators (intervention group) or drop them off at an assigned location (control group). Following the conclusion of the 10-week group, intervention participants had an individ- ual 15-minute debriefing session, during which they were asked for feedback about the experi- ence, thanked for their participation, and given the study stipend ($40 cash) and debriefing form. Control participants did not complete a debriefing session but were given their stipend ($10 cash) in person and thanked for their par- ticipation. All interactions with participants were conducted in compliance with the Ameri- can Psychological Association ethics code and the National Institutes of Health standards for research involving human subjects. Materials Measures. The pre- and posttest measure set included the God Adjective Checklist (Zahl & Gibson, 2012), Attachment to God Inventory (Beck & McDonald, 2004), and a single-item measure of head–heart congruence (cf. Thomas et al., 2011). At pretest and posttest, interven- tion participants also completed three God rep- resentation figure drawings (Davis et al., 2014a, 2014b), including a brief written narrative de- scribing each drawing. Also, at posttest inter- vention participants completed a written journal entry about their overall experience in the group. God Adjective Checklist. The God Adjec- tive Checklist (GAC) is a 30-item self-report measure comparing individuals’ God concepts and images (Zahl & Gibson, 2012). Using a 7-point Likert scale (1 ϭ very undescriptive to 7 ϭ very descriptive), respondents rate each of the 30 trait words. Fifteen trait words measure positively valenced God representations (e.g., loving, patient, kind, and reliable) and 15 mea- sure negatively valenced God representations (e.g., critical, distant, inconsistent, and unfor- giving). For each adjective, the respondent is first asked to rate each word according to “what 7GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 9. I should believe God is like” and then to rate each word according to “my personal experi- ence of what God is like.” Therefore, the GAC asks respondents to differentiate between their “head” (doctrinal) and “heart” (experiential) knowledge of God. It includes four subscales— two for positively valenced God representations (positively valenced God concepts and images) and two for negatively valenced God represen- tations (negatively valenced God concepts and images). In the current study, given our research questions, we only concentrated on the two God image subscales. The Cronbach’s alphas (at pre- test) were .95 for the positively valenced God images subscale and .86 for the negatively va- lenced God images subscale. Attachment to God Inventory. The At- tachment to God Inventory (AGI; Beck & Mc- Donald, 2004) is a 28-item self-report measure of God attachment. It uses a 7-point Likert scale (1 ϭ disagree strongly to 7 ϭ agree strongly) and has two subscales, assessing God attach- ment anxiety (i.e., anxiety about abandonment by God; e.g., “I worry a lot about my relation- ship with God”) and God attachment avoidance (i.e., avoidance of intimacy with God; e.g., “I am uncomfortable being emotional in my com- munication with God”). In the current study, Cronbach’s alphas (at pretest) were .88 for the AGI anxiety subscale and .87 for the AGI avoidance subscale. Head– heart congruence. To measure head–heart congruence, we utilized a similar single-item measure to the one used in Thomas et al. (2011). Specifically, participants were asked to use a 7-point Likert scale (1 ϭ highly dissimilar to 7 ϭ highly similar) to rate the following statement: “At present, my head knowledge of God and heart knowledge of God are ________.” God representation figure drawings and integrative scoring system. God representa- tion figure drawings (Davis et al., 2014a, 2014b) are based on Rizzuto’s (1979) qualita- tive research and consist of having respondents draw pictures of God and oneself. We utilized three prompts, based on Moriarty and Davis’s (2012) recommendations: (1) “Draw a picture of you and God”; (2) “Draw a picture of how you FEEL you and God look when you do something wrong. Draw what you feel, not what you think”; and (3) “Draw a picture of how you would like to feel you and God look when you do something wrong.” We used a prompt about doing something wrong because such times are modally characterized by a rupture in one’s perceived relationship to God, at least tempo- rarily leading to decreased head–heart congru- ence. Given the narrative-experiential and creative nature of our manualized intervention, we in- cluded these God representation figure draw- ings in the pretest and posttest measure set for the intervention participants only, because we believed drawings would potentially provide rich insights into these participants’ God- representation dynamics and would thus foster emotionally meaningful reflection (e.g., grist for personal and group process). Also, building on prior scholarship, we believed that, relative to self-report measures, these drawings may be more sensitive to changes in God images (Gib- son, 2007; Moriarty & Davis, 2012) and more revealing of internal working models, which often operate outside conscious awareness (Da- vis et al., 2013; Zahl et al., 2013). At both time periods, because of the experiential nature of the intervention, we asked participants to use colored pencils to complete the drawings, to stimulate right-mode processing and to evoke emotions and emotion-laden meaning-making (cf. Mihura, Meyer, Dumitrascu, & Bombel, 2013). To facilitate more rigorous analysis of this visual qualitative data, we developed an inte- grative scoring system (Davis et al., 2014b; see Figure S1) to quantify data from participants’ God representation figure drawings. This scor- ing system consists of 11 items, divided into three subscales: God concepts/images (rating indicators of God as benevolent, accessible, re- sponsive, and authoritarian), God attachment (rating indicators of God as a secure base and safe haven, as well as indicators of God attach- ment anxiety, avoidance, and disorganization), and self-functioning (rating indicators of self- esteem and psychological well-being). We also wanted to ensure the scoring system demonstrated strong interrater reliability. Be- cause we hope this system will be utilized in applied settings (e.g., settings in which clini- cians often do not have time or money to par- ticipate in extensive training on a given instru- ment), we tested whether there would be a difference in interrater reliability between un- trained raters with less clinical experience (N ϭ 8 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 10. 6; e.g., first- and second-year graduate psychol- ogy students) and more clinically experienced raters who had been trained on the measure (N ϭ 6; e.g., third- through fifth-year graduate psychology students). Each of the 12 raters was blinded to the study’s hypotheses and rated drawings in counterbalanced order (to control for order effects). Replicating what prior research has shown in behavioral ratings of marital interactions (Bau- com, Baucom, & Christensen, 2012; Waldinger, Schulz, Hauser, Allen, & Crowell, 2004), the group of 6 untrained and less clinically experi- enced graduate students demonstrated statisti- cally equivalent interrater reliability to the group of 6 trained and more clinically experi- enced raters. For all three drawings, the aver- age-measures intraclass correlation coefficient was in the excellent range for both groups (i.e., from the upper .80s to the mid .90s). Also, when we combined the ratings for each rater and examined each drawing, the scoring system’s total scale and three subscales each demon- strated excellent internal consistency: .91 to .95 for the total scale, .86 to .91 for the God con- cepts/images subscale, .83 to .93 for the God attachment subscale, and .83 to .90 for the self functioning subscale (see Tables S1 and S2 in the online supplemental material). Manualized protocol. With the interven- tion participants, we used an adapted version of the GINT manual developed by Davis (2009). The original manual was developed for use in an individual psychotherapy format. However, for the current study, the first author (Trevor Olson) adapted the original manual so it could be used in a group psychotherapy format, and the second author (Theresa Clement Tisdale) developed guidance notes for group facilitators. In both versions of the manual, the GINT pro- tocol reflects an assimilative-integrationist ap- proach to treating God image difficulties. That is, it incorporates interventions from various psychotherapeutic approaches (e.g., dynamic- interpersonal and cognitive–behavioral), but it is generally guided by a relational-spirituality conceptualization and chiefly utilizes narrative- experiential interventions, such as (a) expres- sive-writing exercises to facilitate articulation and deconstruction of one’s problem-saturated spiritual and life narratives (e.g., chapters, char- acters, plots, themes, etc.); (b) mindful- awareness practices (e.g., contemplation and guided reflection) and interpersonal discourse to help link events across time and explore iden- tity-relevant themes; (c) God-image figure drawings and automatic thought records to fos- ter insight into implicit relational knowing of self, God, and others; and (d) narrative inter- ventions to coconstruct alternative growth- promoting storylines (Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, 2012). See Table S3 for a detailed overview of the protocol (see Davis, 2009, for a copy of the full GINT protocol). Results Analysis of Quantitative Data From Self-Report Measures Data were screened to see whether assump- tions were met for conducting a 2 (Intervention vs. Control) ϫ 2 (Pretest, Posttest) mixed- model repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with five dependent variables (positively and negatively valenced God images; God attachment anxiety and avoidance; head–heart congruence). Three par- ticipants (1 intervention and 2 controls) had not completed all items on one pre- or posttest measure, and those participants’ scores thus were not calculated for the affected measure. The pre- or posttest GAC subscale scores had a few univariate outliers (1 to 3), which were recoded to the nearest nonextreme value. Also, the pre- and posttest positively valenced God image GAC scores were substantially skewed (Ϫ2.94 and Ϫ1.29, respectively), but transformations did not improve the normal- ity of these variables. For the other variables, the pre- and posttest skew values indicated acceptable normality: negatively valenced God images (0.94 and 0.72), God attachment anxiety (Ϫ0.21 and 0.21), God attachment avoidance (0.46 and 0.50), and head–heart congruence (Ϫ0.43 and Ϫ0.78), respectively. Levene’s tests were nonsignificant, indicating equality of error variances for each variable. Finally, we conducted the mixed-model re- peated measures MANOVA, which was non- significant, Wilks’ ⌳(5,111) ϭ 0.97, p ϭ .681. Each variable’s descriptive statistics and between-subjects condition-by-time in- teraction effect is presented in Table 1. In sum, we predicted that, relative to control participants, intervention participants would re- 9GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 11. port greater change in (1) both positively and negatively valenced God images (increase and decrease, respectively), (2) both God attach- ment anxiety and God attachment avoidance (decrease for both), and (3) self-reported head– heart congruence (increase). However, none of these hypotheses was supported. Analysis of Qualitative Data from Figure Drawings and Posttest Journal Entries We also evaluated whether intervention par- ticipants’ coded figure drawings evidenced pre- test–posttest changes (see Figures S2 and S3 in online supplemental material, for examples of participants’ figure drawings). To do that, we aggregated all 12 raters’ ratings (6 untrained, 6 trained) to provide an average (mean) rating for each participant, each drawing (prompts 1, 2, and 3), and each time period (pretest and post- test). Paired-samples t tests revealed no signif- icant pretest–posttest differences. Our research team was extremely puzzled, however, because intervention participants often had reported experiencing significant spiritual and psychological growth. For exam- ple, in their posttest journal entries and debrief- ing interviews, a large number of participants reported experiencing increased honesty, close- ness, security, satisfaction, and head–heart con- gruence in their perceived relationship to God. Similarly, participants commonly discussed ex- periencing increased self-insight and self- acceptance as a result of their group participa- tion. In short, participants frequently reported experiencing positive changes in their God im- ages, God attachment, head–heart congruence, God-image narrative identity, and general nar- rative identity. Therefore, for exploratory purposes, we con- ducted a text analysis of the intervention partic- ipants’ posttest journal entries, using the well- utilized and well-validated Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) 2007 software pro- gram (Pennebaker, Booth, & Francis, 2007). The LIWC 2007 program analyzes written text samples, evaluating the percentage of words falling into different psychologically meaning- ful categories (e.g., social, affective, or cogni- tive processes). Text analysis of participants’ posttest journal entries revealed that partici- pants wrote 203.88 words on average (SD ϭ 274.81, range: 66–1471) and mainly discussed cognitive processes (M ϭ 22.09% of the text, SD ϭ 4.22%), social processes (M ϭ 6.87%, SD ϭ 3.06%), and affective processes (M ϭ 5.27%, SD ϭ 2.30%). With regard to cognitive processes, participants primarily used words in- dicating insight (e.g., think, know; M ϭ 5.43%, SD ϭ 2.45%) and inclusiveness (e.g., and, with; M ϭ 5.37%, SD ϭ 2.04%). Regarding social processes, they chiefly discussed their perceived relationship to God. Last, regarding affective processes, they discussed significantly more positive emotions (e.g., love, kindness; M ϭ Table 1 Pretest and Posttest Descriptive Statistics and Between-Subjects Condition-by-Time Interaction Effects for Study Variables Intervention (n ϭ 32) Control (n ϭ 29) Variable Pretest M (SD) Posttest M (SD) Pretest M (SD) Posttest M (SD) C ϫ T p God Adjective Checklist (GAC) Positively valenced God imagesa 95.25 (10.31) 97.35 (9.15) 93.93 (10.42) 93.69 (11.53) .38 .540 Negatively valenced God images 35.63 (13.34) 35.23 (14.74) 32.37 (12.17) 33.14 (14.57) .05 .818 Attachment to God Inventory (AGI) God attachment anxiety 53.63 (13.09) 51.42 (11.56) 46.07 (15.15) 47.62 (14.58) .57 .454 God attachment avoidance 40.38 (11.28) 40.74 (12.06) 42.56 (14.81) 42.79 (15.59) .00 .979 Single-item head–heart congruence 4.69 (1.28) 5.00 (1.44) 5.29 (.94) 4.93 (1.46) 2.01 .159 Note. C ϭ condition; T ϭ time. Possible values for the subscales of the God Adjective Checklist (Zahl & Gibson, 2012) range from 15 to 105, possible values for the two subscales of the Attachment to God Inventory (Beck & McDonald, 2004) range from 14 to 98, and possible values for the single-item head–heart congruence measure (cf. Thomas et al., 2011) range from 1 to 7. For each variable, higher scores indicate higher levels of the measured construct. a This variable’s scores were substantially skewed both at pretest and posttest (Ϫ2.94 and Ϫ1.29, respectively), but transformations did not improve this variable’s normality. 10 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 12. 3.67%, SD ϭ 1.83%) than negative emotions (e.g., anxiety, sadness; M ϭ 1.31%, SD ϭ 1.47%), t(25) ϭ 5.09, p Ͻ .001. Discussion Taken together, neither our quantitative data from self-report measures nor our quantified qualitative data from figure drawings supported our hypotheses. Needless to say, this is disap- pointing news for any researcher. However, text analysis of intervention participants’ posttest journal entries (as well as intervention partici- pants’ spontaneous comments during debriefing interviews) suggested that intervention partici- pants experienced an increase in adaptive, pos- itively valenced religious/spiritual meaning making, including increases in insight and pos- itive emotions. This mixture of findings is puz- zling indeed. There are several possibilities for explaining our largely null results. It is possible our manu- alized GINT intervention was not effective in promoting the majority of changes we hypoth- esized. However, such a possibility seems un- likely, given that in their posttest journal entries and debriefing interviews, most intervention participants reported experiencing spiritual and psychological growth. Another possibility is that our mostly null findings were artifacts of several previously identified measurement is- sues (e.g., lack of sufficient psychometric ro- bustness; ceiling and floor effects). For exam- ple, self-report measures of God representations and God attachment may be highly susceptible to socially desirable responding (along with as- sociated ceiling and floor effects; Gibson, 2007; Moriarty & Davis, 2012), particularly among highly religious persons (see Sedikides & Ge- bauer, 2010). Thus, for our intervention partic- ipants, it is possible their pretest responses were largely influenced by socially desirable re- sponding (e.g., reflecting positive impression management or self-deception), whereas their posttest responses represented more authentic self-descriptions (e.g., representing actual gains achieved through intervention participation). Also, as numerous scholars have noted, self- report measures of God representations and God attachment likely tend to assess consciously ac- cessible aspects of these constructs, even though God representations (particularly God images) and God attachment may be largely processed outside conscious awareness (Davis et al., 2013, 2016; Hall & Fujikawa, 2013; Mo- riarty & Davis, 2012; Zahl & Gibson, 2012; Zahl et al., 2013). In sum, there is a need for advances in the measurement of God representations and God attachment. In particular, there is a need for God representation and God attachment measures to (a) have inbuilt validity scales; (b) have varying levels of item nuance, sophistication, and diffi- culty (to lessen the likelihood of ceiling and floor effects); (c) have reliable and valid ways to measure implicit God images and God attach- ment; and (d) be sensitive enough to track changes over time (e.g., over the course of an intervention; Davis et al., 2016; Hall & Fu- jikawa, 2013; Moriarty & Davis, 2012; Zahl et al., 2013). This last suggestion may be espe- cially challenging when interventions are brief and when reliable change actually may occur largely after treatment has ended (e.g., during a follow-up period; cf. Levenson, 2010; Shedler, 2010). Moreover, there is a need for increased reli- ance on non-self-report methods for measuring God representations (e.g., God concepts and images) and related constructs (e.g., head–heart congruence and God attachment). Because these constructs are highly nuanced, complex, and context-dependent—and because these con- structs may largely operate outside conscious awareness (e.g., Davis et al., 2013, 2016; Zahl et al., 2013)—it may be that existing self-report measures of these constructs do not have the sophistication, sensitivity, or psychometric ro- bustness to assess their underlying constructs reliably and validly. Instead, non-self-report measures may be a better alternative. In partic- ular, the use of projective measures (e.g., God representation figure drawings) and narrative methods (e.g., narrative inquiry, text analysis) may prove to be especially effective and eluci- dating ways to measure God images and God attachment (Davis et al., 2016; Hall, 2007; Mo- riarty & Davis, 2012). Toward that end, we encourage religious cognition scholars to uti- lize, evaluate, and refine our preliminarily val- idated Integrative Scoring System for God Rep- resentation Figure Drawings (Davis et al., 2014b). We also encourage the development and validation of narrative-based God image and God attachment measures, such as inter- view and scoring systems similar to that of the 11GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 13. well-utilized and well-validated Adult Attach- ment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996; Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, 2013). Last, we encourage the use of text-analysis programs such as LIWC (Pennebaker et al., 2007) to analyze text from written or transcribed narra- tives (e.g., journal entries or transcribed inter- views about one’s perceived relationship to God; Davis et al., 2016). Limitations Our study had several limitations. Most no- tably, our sample size was quite small (N ϭ 61; thus our study was underpowered), and we did not utilize random assignment (thus there was self-selection bias in sampling). With regard to sample size, a statistical power analysis using G‫ء‬ Power 3.0.10 indicated that, to use a mixed- model repeated measures MANOVA to detect a medium-sized effect (f ϭ .25) with two groups and two time periods, a total sample size of 210 (110 per group) would have been required. In terms of random assignment, we designed the study to give participants a dichotomous choice between the intervention or measures-only (control) group, based on their level of interest and availability. Also, due to logistical con- straints, we had to conduct the study within one Spring semester (15 weeks), before all partici- pants and facilitators left for the summer, thereby precluding a waitlist-control group. The generalizability of our findings is also limited, because our sample was (a) undergrad- uate and graduate students, (b) Christian (pre- dominantly Protestant Christian), (c) highly re- ligious, and (d) predominantly female. In addition, we relied heavily on self-report mea- sures, some of which demonstrated poor psy- chometric properties (e.g., the GAC demon- strated substantial skewness, seemingly due to ceiling effects), whereas other measures may not have been sensitive enough to detect changes that did occur. Furthermore, we as- sessed only spiritual outcomes. In hindsight, assessing psychological outcomes (e.g., changes in self-esteem, depressive and anxious symptoms, or perceived social support) would have been useful too. Also, we only assessed dispositional spiritual outcomes (e.g., God rep- resentations and attachment), but assessing con- textual/situational spiritual outcomes (e.g., reli- gious/spiritual struggles) would have been interesting as well. In fact, doing so may have been more likely to yield significant quantita- tive results, given the greater likelihood of psy- chotherapeutically promoting change in situa- tional than dispositional outcomes, especially when it comes to brief psychotherapeutic inter- ventions such as ours. Moreover, we did not assess participant personality characteristics that might have moderated the intervention’s effects. For instance, we perhaps should have measured trait extraversion, given that previous research suggests extraverts are more likely to benefit from group psychotherapy than intro- verts (Ogrodniczuk et al., 2003). Last, the study design did not include fol- low-up assessment (e.g., at 3-, 6-, and/or 12- months postintervention). It is possible a narra- tive-experiential approach to promoting God- image change may lead to beneficial effects that emerge largely in the weeks and months follow- ing intervention (i.e., the so-called “incubator effect”). As noted earlier, with insight-oriented and experiential approaches such as the one we utilized in this brief intervention, reliable change often emerges after the intervention con- cludes (Levenson, 2010; Shedler, 2010). There- fore, future outcome research in this area should include follow-up assessment as well. Conclusions and Suggestions for Future Research Consistent with what prior researchers have proposed (Davis & Badenoch, 2010; Hall, 2007; Moriarty & Davis, 2012), some of our qualitative data (e.g., posttest journal entries and debriefing interviews) suggest narrative- experiential interventions (e.g., GINT) may pro- mote positive spiritual and psychological out- comes, including changes in God images, God attachment, head–heart congruence, God-image narrative identity, and general narrative iden- tity. However, our quantitative data did not cor- roborate these hypothesized outcomes. Hence, much more research on GINT is needed, and there are several measurement issues that need to be addressed so that quantitative data, qual- itative data, and quantified projective data may yield more valid and reliable results. Because of the nuance and complexity of these constructs, mixed-methods designs perhaps hold the most promise for yielding robust and clinically mean- 12 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 14. ingful findings (Davis et al., 2016). More spir- itually diverse samples are needed as well. We agree with scholars who have recom- mended the more frequent use of qualitative and mixed methods to study God images and God attachment (Davis et al., 2016; Gibson, 2007; Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, 2013; Hall & Fu- jikawa, 2013). We also agree that religious cog- nition researchers need to rely increasingly on non-self-report methods (e.g., projective mea- sures, narrative methods) for assessing God im- ages and God attachment. As Zahl and Gibson (2012) have noted, self-report measures neces- sarily reflect information that is consciously available and filtered through a respondent’s reflection and state of mind, making such mea- sures subject to the constraints of what can be expressed propositionally in words. Addition- ally, we concur there is a particular need for more outcome research (with follow-up assess- ment) on psychotherapeutically mediated changes in God images and God attachment (Hall & Fujikawa, 2013; Thomas et al., 2011; Tisdale et al., 1997). In particular, we encourage researchers to explore the interconnections among healthy human attachments, self-esteem, and relational spirituality (as defined above), especially the possibility that psychotherapeuti- cally promoted changes in one of those areas will lead to corresponding improvements in the others (cf. Mikulincer & Shaver, 2004; Mori- arty & Davis, 2012). More broadly, we encour- age researchers to explore the interconnections among mental/neural representations of God, self, and others. Ultimately, we eagerly antici- pate what future research on relational spiritu- ality will discover as researchers seek to illumi- nate the complexities of how people view and relate to God and how they deconstruct and reauthor their spiritual and life narratives. References Baucom, K. J., Baucom, B. R., & Christensen, A. (2012). Do the naïve know best? The predictive power of naïve ratings of couple interactions. Psy- chological Assessment, 24, 983–994. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/a0028680 Beck, R., & McDonald, A. (2004). Attachment to God: The Attachment to God Inventory, tests of working model correspondence, and an explora- tion of faith group differences. Journal of Psychol- ogy and Theology, 3, 92–103. Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive sci- ence: A multiple code theory. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Cheston, S. E., Piedmont, R. L., Eanes, B., & Lavin, L. P. (2003). Changes in clients’ image of God over the course of outpatient therapy. Counseling and Values, 47, 96–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ j.2161-007X.2003.tb00227.x Cozolino, L. (2010). The neuroscience of psychother- apy (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Norton. Davis, E. B. (2009). God image narrative therapy: A treatment manual, Unpublished manuscript. Vir- ginia Beach, VA: Regent University. Retrieved from http://www.wheaton.edu/Academics/Faculty/ D/Ward-Davis Davis, E. B., & Badenoch, B. (2010). Storying God images: Bringing narrative integration to our ex- perience of the divine. GAINS Quarterly, Winter 2009, 14–24. Retrieved from http://www.wheaton .edu/Academics/Faculty/D/Ward-Davis Davis, E. B., Cuthbert, A. D., Hays, L. W., Aten, J. D., Van Tongeren, D. R., Hook, J. N., . . . Boan, D. (2016). Using qualitative and mixed methods to study relational spirituality. Psychology of Reli- gion and Spirituality, 8, 92–98. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/rel0000046 Davis, E. B., Moriarty, G. L., & Mauch, J. C. (2013). God images and God concepts: Definitions, devel- opment, and dynamics. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 5, 51–60. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/a0029289 Davis, E. B., Tisdale, T. C., Olson, T., Park, E., Nam, J., Moriarty, G. L., & Cuthbert, A. D. (2014a). God Representation Figure Drawings (GRFD): Perfor- mance-based measure of God representations. Un- published instrument. Retrieved from http://www .wheaton.edu/Academics/Faculty/D/Ward-Davis Davis, E. B., Tisdale, T. C., Olson, T., Park, E., Nam, J., Moriarty, G. L., & Cuthbert, A. D. (2014b). Integrative Scoring System for God Representation Figure Drawings (ISSGRFD): Rubric for scoring God representation figure drawings. Unpublished instrument. Retrieved from http://www.wheaton .edu/Academics/Faculty/D/Ward-Davis Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., Watson, J. C., Timulak, L., & Freire, E. (2013). Research on humanistic- experiential psychotherapies. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp. 495–538). New York, NY: Wiley. Gallup, G., Jr., & Jones, S. (1989). One hundred questions and answers: Religion in America. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Religious Research Cen- ter. George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult Attachment Interview protocol (3rd ed.). Unpub- lished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley. 13GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 15. Gibson, N. J. S. (2007). Measurement issues in God image research and practice. In G. L. Moriarty & L. Hoffman (Eds.), God image handbook for spir- itual counseling and psychotherapy (pp. 227–246). Binghamton, NY: Haworth/Routledge. Granqvist, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2008). Attach- ment and religious representations and behavior. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical appli- cations (2nd ed., pp. 906–933). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Granqvist, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2013). Religion, spirituality, and attachment. In K. I. Pargament (Ed.), APA handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1, pp. 139–155). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hall, T. W. (2004). Christian spirituality and mental health: A relational spirituality framework for em- pirical research. Journal of Psychology and Chris- tianity, 23, 66–81. Hall, T. W. (2007). Psychoanalysis, attachment, and spirituality part II: The spiritual stories we live by. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 35, 29–42. Hall, T. W., & Fujikawa, A. M. (2013). God image and the sacred. In K. I. Pargament (Ed.), APA handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1, pp. 277–292). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hall, T. W., Fujikawa, A., Halcrow, S. R., Hill, P. C., & Delaney, H. (2009). Attachment to God and implicit spirituality: Clarifying correspondence and compensation models. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 37, 227–242. Levenson, H. (2010). Brief dynamic therapy. Wash- ington, DC: American Psychological Association. Madigan, S. (2011). Narrative therapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McAdams, D. P. (1993). The stories we live by. New York, NY: Guilford Press. McAdams, D. P. (2008). Personal narratives and the life story. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality (3rd ed., pp. 242–262). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Mihura, J. L., Meyer, G. J., Dumitrascu, N., & Bombel, G. (2013). The validity of individual Ror- schach variables: Systematic reviews and meta- analyses of the comprehensive system. Psycholog- ical Bulletin, 139, 548–605. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/a0029406 Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Security- based self-representations in adulthood: Contents and processes. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 159–195). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Moriarty, G. L., & Davis, E. B. (2012). Client God images: Theory, research, and clinical practice. In J. Aten, K. O’Grady, & E. Worthington, Jr., (Eds.), The psychology of religion and spirituality for clinicians (pp. 131–160). New York, NY: Rout- ledge. Murray-Swank, N. A., & Pargament, K. I. (2005). God, where are you?: Evaluating a spiritually- integrated intervention for sexual abuse. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 8, 191–203. http://dx .doi.org/10.1080/13694670500138866 Ogrodniczuk, J. S., Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., Mc- Callum, M., & Rosie, J. S. (2003). NEO-five factor personality traits as predictors of response to two forms of group psychotherapy. International Jour- nal of Group Psychotherapy, 53, 417–442. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1521/ijgp.53.4.417.42832 Pargament, K. I. (2007). Spiritually-integrated psy- chotherapy: Understanding and addressing the sa- cred. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Pargament, K. I. (2013). Searching for the sacred: Toward a nonreductionistic theory of spirituality. In K. I. Pargament (Ed.), APA handbook of psy- chology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1, pp. 257–273). Washington, DC: American Psycholog- ical Association. Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC 2001 Operator’s manual. Austin, TX: LIWC.net. Pew Research Center. (2008). U.S. Religious Land- scape Survey, Pt. 2: Religious beliefs and prac- tices. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from http://religions.pewforum.org/ Pew Research Center. (2012). The global religious landscape: A report on the size and distribution of the world’s major religious groups as of 2010. Washington, DC: Author. Rizzuto, A. (1979). The birth of the living God. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Sedikides, C., & Gebauer, J. E. (2010). Religiosity as self-enhancement: A meta-analysis of the relation between socially desirable responding and religi- osity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 17–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/10888 68309351002 Shedler, J. (2010). The efficacy of psychodynamic psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 65, 98– 109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018378 Shin, J. Y., & Steger, M. F. (2014). Promoting mean- ing and purpose in life. In A. C. Parks & S. M. Schueller (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Positive Psychological Interventions (pp. 90– 110). Malden, MA: Wiley Blackwell. http://dx.doi .org/10.1002/9781118315927.ch5 Siegel, D. J. (2012a). The developing mind (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Siegel, D. J. (2012b). Pocket guide to interpersonal neurobiology. New York, NY: Norton. Thomas, M., Moriarty, G., Davis, E., & Anderson, L. (2011). The effects of a manualized group- 14 OLSON ET AL. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
  • 16. psychotherapy intervention on client God images and attachment to God: A pilot study. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 39, 44–58. Tisdale, T. C., Key, T. L., Kemperman, S. R., Cloud, H., Townsend, J., & Okamoto, T. (1997). Impact of treatment on God image and personal adjust- ment, and correlations of God image to personal adjustment and object relations development. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 25, 227–239. Waldinger, R. J., Schulz, M. S., Hauser, S. T., Allen, J. P., & Crowell, J. A. (2004). Reading others emotions: The role of intuitive judgments in pre- dicting marital satisfaction, quality, and stability. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 58–71. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.58 Zahl, B. P., & Gibson, N. J. S. (2012). God repre- sentations, attachment to God, and satisfaction with life: A comparison of doctrinal and experien- tial representations of God in Christian young adults. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 22, 216–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 10508619.2012.670027 Zahl, B. P., Sharp, C. A., & Gibson, N. J. S. (2013). Empirical measures of the religious heart. In F. N. Watts & G. Dumbreck (Eds.), Head and heart: Perspectives from religion and psychology (pp. 97–124). West Conshohocken, PA: John Temple- ton Press. Received August 31, 2015 Revision received December 22, 2015 Accepted January 14, 2016 Ⅲ 15GOD IMAGE NARRATIVE THERAPY ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.