SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Maternal Prenatal Felt Security and Infant Health at Birth Interact to
Predict Infant Fussing and Crying at 12 Months Postpartum
Natsumi Sawada
McGill University
Faby M. Gagné
Granite State College
Louise Séguin
Université de Montréal
Michael S. Kramer, Helen McNamara,
and Robert W. Platt
McGill University
Lise Goulet
Université de Montréal
Michael J. Meaney and John E. Lydon
McGill University
Infants born with medical problems are at risk for less optimal developmental outcomes. This may be, in part,
because neonatal medical problems are associated with maternal distress, which may adversely impact infants.
However, the reserve capacity model suggests that an individual’s bank of psychosocial resources buffers the
adverse effects of later-encountered stressors. This prospective longitudinal study examined whether preex-
isting maternal psychosocial resources, conceptualized as felt security in close relationships, moderate the
association between neonatal medical problems and infant fussing and crying 12 months postpartum. Maternal
felt security was measured by assessing its indicators in 5,092 pregnant women. At birth, infants were
classified as healthy or having a medical problem. At 12 months, experience sampling was used to assess daily
maternal reports of fussing and crying in 135 mothers of infants who were healthy or had medical problems
at birth. Confirmatory factor analyses revealed that attachment, relationship quality, self-esteem, and social
support can be conceptualized as indicators of a single felt security factor. Multiple regression analyses
revealed that prenatal maternal felt security interacts with infant health at birth to predict fussing and crying
at 12 months. Among infants born with medical problems, higher felt security predicted decreased fussing and
crying. Maternal felt security assessed before birth dampens the association between neonatal medical
problems and subsequent infant behavior. This supports the hypothesis that psychosocial resources in reserve
can be called upon in the face of a stressor to reduce its adverse effects on the self or others.
Keywords: prenatal felt security, prenatal psychosocial resources, maternal stressor, newborn health
problems, infant fussing and crying
Infants born preterm, low birth weight, or with severe medical
problems are at risk for later emotional, cognitive, and behavioral
problems (Bhutta, Cleves, Casey, Cradock, & Anand, 2002; Hack,
Klein, & Taylor, 1995; Miceli et al., 2000). This may be in part
because neonatal medical problems and stressors associated with
medical care may cause alterations in brain structure and functioning
This article was published Online First December 22, 2014.
Natsumi Sawada, Department of Psychology, McGill University; Faby
M. Gagné, Department of Behavioral Sciences, Granite State College;
Louise Séguin, Département de Médecine Sociale et Preventive, École de
Santé Publique, Université de Montréal; Michael S. Kramer, Department of
Pediatrics, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec,
Canada, Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics, and Occupational
Health, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Can-
ada; Helen McNamara, Department of Obstetrics & Gynaecology, McGill
University; Robert W. Platt, Department of Pediatrics, McGill University
Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, Department of Epidemi-
ology, Biostatistics, and Occupational Health, McGill University Faculty
of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; Lise Goulet, Département de
Médecine Sociale et Preventive, École de Santé Publique, Université de
Montréal; Michael J. Meaney, Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psy-
chobiology and Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology and Neurosur-
gery, McGill University; and John E. Lydon, Department of Psychology,
McGill University.
This research was supported by grants from the March of Dimes and
the Canadian Institutes for Health Research. Robert Platt is a chercheur-
boursier of the Fonds de la Recherche du Québec – Santé (FRQ-S), and
Drs. Platt and Kramer are members of the Research Institute of the
McGill University Health Centre, which receives core funding from
the FRQ-S. We thank Henriette Lussier, Antoinette Ghanem, Alex
McIntyre-Smith, Peggy McLean, Alisa Khan, Marie-Pierre Markon-
Lalonde, Katherine Pascuzzo, Amelie Zonato, Andrée-Ann Bouvette-
Turcot, and the student interviewers and nursing students who helped
with this study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nat-
sumi Sawada, Psychology Department, McGill University, 1205 ave-
nue, Docteur Penfield, Montreal, Quebec H3A 1B1. E-mail: natsumi
.sawada@mail.mcgill.ca
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
Health Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 34, No. 8, 811–819 0278-6133/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000152
811
(Anand & Scalzo, 2000; Stewart et al., 1999). This may also be in part
because birth of a medically vulnerable infant is a significant maternal
stressor (Blumberg, 1980; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Singer et al.,
1999). Maternal stress is associated with less sensitive behavior and
disturbances in mother–infant interactions (Crnic, Greenberg,
Ragozin, Robinson, & Basham, 1983; Muller-Nix et al., 2004). The
stressor of having a newborn with a medical problem may thereby
interfere with parents’ ability to provide infants with an environment
that promotes resilience and optimal development (Blumberg, 1980;
Darke & Goldberg, 1994; Miceli et al., 2000; Muller-Nix et al., 2004;
Singer et al., 1999), resulting in poorer developmental outcomes for
the child (Bell & Ainsworth, 1972; Browne & Talmi, 2005; Hane &
Fox, 2006).
However, psychosocial resources moderate the adverse effects
of stressors on well-being (Cohen & Wills, 1985; DeLongis,
Folkman, & Lazarus, 1988). Specifically, research suggests that
maternal psychosocial resources moderate the relationship of in-
fant medical problems or maternal stress with maternal distress
and behavior. For example, social support moderates the relation
between maternal stress and quality of mother–infant interactions
among mothers of premature and healthy infants (Crnic et al.,
1983). Perceived social support moderates the relation between
infant medical vulnerability and maternal distress (Singer, Davil-
lier, Bruening, Hawkins, & Yamashita, 1996). Secure attachment
predicts decreased maternal distress in the context of infant med-
ical problems, with the effects mediated by perceived coping
abilities (Berant, Mikulincer, & Florian, 2001). Thus, when an
infant is born with a medical problem, maternal psychosocial
resources such as social support, attachment security, and per-
ceived coping may promote better infant outcomes by dampening
the maternal distress that undermines maternal sensitive respon-
siveness associated with the development of infant affect regula-
tion (Feldman, Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999; Olafsen et al., 2008;
van den Boom, 1994). This is consistent with the finding that
prenatal social support dampens maternal distress, which, in turn,
predicts lower distress among healthy infants (Stapleton et al.,
2012).
Studies examining the effects of maternal psychosocial re-
sources in the context of infant medical problems have often
measured resources at or after diagnosis of the medical problem
(e.g., Berant et al., 2001; Crnic et al., 1983; Hopkins, Campbell, &
Marcus, 1987; Miceli et al., 2000; Singer et al., 1996). However,
having an infant with a medical problem may influence psychos-
ocial resources such as social support and self-esteem (e.g., M.
McGrath, Boukydis, & Lester, 1993). Postdiagnosis assessment of
psychosocial resources may therefore be biased. Many studies
have examined the effects of prenatal psychosocial resources and
maternal stress on birth and maternal outcomes. However, evi-
dence of a buffering effect of maternal prenatal social support has
been mixed (e.g., Adamakos et al., 1986; Bee, Hammond, Eyres,
Barnard, & Snyder, 1986; Collins, Dunkel-Schetter, Lobel, &
Scrimshaw, 1993; Goldstein, Diener, & Mangelsdorf, 1996), and
scant attention has been given to downstream effects on infants (cf.
Crnic, Greenberg, & Slough, 1986). It is therefore unclear whether
relationships among maternal psychosocial resources, infant med-
ical problems, and maternal or infant outcomes can be explained
by preexisting differences in mothers’ psychosocial resources.
Gallo and Matthews (2003) theorize that individuals’ reserve
capacity or “bank” of psychological, social, and tangible resources
buffer the effects of later-encountered stressors. This suggests that
preexisting maternal resources may moderate the effects of later-
encountered infant medical problems to promote more favorable
outcomes for infants.
The purpose of this study was to examine whether maternal
prenatal psychosocial resources moderate the effects of neonatal
medical problems, a maternal stressor, on important infant devel-
opmental outcomes: fussing and crying. Infant fussing and crying
may reflect elevated distress and negative affectivity, and may thus
predict internalizing and externalizing problems later in childhood
(Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995; Natsuaki et al.,
2010; Rende, 1993).
Existing research on the buffering effects of maternal psychos-
ocial resources has investigated how constructs such as social
support or attachment security singly moderate outcomes associ-
ated with maternal stressors. However, researchers have noted that
stress-buffering resources tend to occur in aggregate (Gallo &
Matthews, 2003). Further, a range of these resources may be
conceptually integrated as indicators of felt security. This concept
is derived from the attachment literature and is defined as a sense
of confidence and safety in the continued affection and availability
of close others that regulates interpersonal closeness and depen-
dence in times of need (Murray, Holmes, & Collins, 2006; Murray,
Holmes, & Griffin, 2000; Sroufe & Waters, 1977). Researchers
propose that felt security has, at its core, perceived positive regard
from close others, trust in their availability and responsiveness,
and perceived love by one’s partner. Felt security has thus been
conceptualized as encompassing self-esteem, attachment security,
relationship quality, and perceived social support (Collins &
Feeney, 2004; Murray et al., 2006; Murray, Holmes, Griffin,
Bellavia, & Rose, 2001; Reis, Clark, & Holmes, 2004). Further,
there is evidence of associations among these constructs in a
sample of pregnant women (Stapleton et al., 2012). Thus, although
the effects of maternal attachment security, social support, self-
esteem, and relationship quality on postpartum outcomes have
been examined separately (e.g., Berant et al., 2001; Hall, Kotch,
Browne, & Rayens, 1996), these constructs may more parsimoni-
ously reflect an overarching sense of felt security.
Attachment theory and the reserve capacity model suggest
that individuals’ reserves of inter- and intrapersonal resources,
such as attachment security, self-esteem, social support, and
relationship quality, contribute to a cognitive representation of
the self in relation to others that moderates effects of later-
encountered stressors. We therefore predicted that maternal felt
security might protect against detrimental effects of neonatal
medical problems on infant development. We therefore exam-
ined whether infant medical problems at birth predict infant
fussing and crying at 12 months and whether maternal felt
security moderates this association.
To examine whether mothers’ baseline felt security interacts
with a later-encountered infant medical stressor to predict later
fussing and crying, we assessed felt security prenatally as an
indicator of resources available at the onset of the stressor
unbiased by the experience of the stressor. Experience samp-
ling was used to assess maternal report of infant behavior
(Larson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1983). We controlled for preg-
nancy anxiety, as this might be confounded with mothers’
reports of infant fussing and crying (J. M. McGrath et al., 2008;
Mebert, 1991).
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
812 SAWADA ET AL.
No validated measure of felt security that encompasses at-
tachment, relationship quality, self-esteem, and social support
exists. Before testing the central hypothesis that maternal felt
security would interact with infant health at birth to predict
infant fussing and crying, we therefore tested the structure of
felt security using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), with
measures of attachment, romantic relationship quality, self-
esteem, and social support as indicators.
Method
Participants
Over a 5-year period, as part of the Montreal Prematurity Study,
women from a wide socioeconomic spectrum were recruited be-
tween 24 and 26 weeks gestation when they presented at one of
four Montreal hospitals for routine hospital ultrasound examina-
tions (Time 1). All women (N ϭ 5,092) were over age 17, pregnant
with a singleton fetus, and spoke English or French. Exclusion
criteria were placenta praevia, a major fetal anomaly in the current
pregnancy, an incompetent cervix diagnosis in a previous preg-
nancy, or chronic illnesses other than hypertension, asthma, or
diabetes (Kramer et al., 2009). At birth, infants were categorized as
healthy or as having a severe neonatal medical problem likely to be
a stressor for mothers (Time 2). Mothers were interviewed again at
6 months postpartum (Time 3).
A follow-up experience-sampling study was conducted in the
final year of the Montreal Prematurity Study. All mothers of
infants with a medical problem at birth who were 12 months old
when the follow-up study began were invited to participate (Time
4). For each mother of an infant with a medical problem at birth,
two mothers of infants who were healthy at birth were invited. The
latter comprised the control group. Of all mothers invited, 69%
agreed. Mothers who participated were not different from those
who did not on demographic variables, maternal felt security, or
infant medical problem versus control group classifications, ps Ͼ
.1. Participants who did not respond to felt security measures were
excluded from analyses (n ϭ 4). The final sample at Time 4
included 135 mothers (M ϭ 27.9 years). Demographic information
is in Table 1.
Procedure and Measures
Time 1. Nurses administered a psychosocial interview be-
tween 24 and 26 weeks gestation. The interview included English
or French measures assessing indicators of maternal felt security,
pregnancy anxiety, depression, sociodemographic information,
and medical history.
Maternal felt security. Indicators were social support, self-
esteem, attachment, and romantic relationship quality. A five-item
Arizona Social Support Interview (Barrera, 1981) assessed emo-
tional and instrumental social support received in the preceding 2
weeks. Three additional questions assessed (a) mothers’ expected
support postpartum, (b) the number of people currently available to
talk freely to/confide in, and (c), the number of people currently
available to help in a time of need. Self-esteem was assessed with
a four-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (␣ ϭ .73; Major, Rich-
ards, Cooper, Cozzarelli, & Zubek, 1998). Attachment was as-
sessed with a seven-item Experiences in Close Relationships Scale
(Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). As per Brennan et al. (1998),
the three items loading most highly on the anxiety dimension were
aggregated to form an anxiety score (␣ ϭ .62). The two highest-
loading fear-of-closeness items and the two highest-loading fear-
of-dependency items were aggregated to form an avoidance score
(␣ ϭ .49). Romantic relationship quality was assessed with a
three-item Quality of Marriage Index modified to include cohab-
iting relationships (␣ ϭ .78; Norton, 1983). Felt security scores
were calculated using the regression method in principal compo-
nents analysis.
Pregnancy anxiety and depressive symptoms. Pregnancy anx-
iety was measured with a four-item Pregnancy-Specific Anxiety
Scale (␣ ϭ .81; Mancuso, Schetter, Rini, Roesch, & Hobel, 2004).
Depressive symptoms were measured with a 20-item Center for
Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (␣ ϭ .89; Radloff, 1977).
Time 2. Shortly after birth, medical records were reviewed.
Mothers were categorized as having a healthy newborn or a
newborn with a medical problem defined as (a) early preterm
birth (Յ33 completed weeks of gestation) following spontane-
ous labor onset or prelabor membrane rupture, (b) neonatal
hospitalization Ն7 days, or (c) a birth complication not detected
by prenatal screening (e.g., congenital heart or urinary system
anomalies, musculoskeletal anomalies, obstetrical trauma, se-
vere fetal asphyxiation, or respiratory distress). Four medical
doctors (MDs) with expertise in perinatal epidemiology re-
viewed a list of all medical conditions in the sample. A condi-
tion was defined as a medical problem at birth if three MDs
thought it was a serious medical problem and a likely maternal
stressor throughout the first postnatal year. Mothers of healthy
infants born at Ն36 weeks gestation constituted the control
group. There were no differences in felt security, age, preg-
Table 1
Demographic Data in Percentages
Control
(n ϭ 93)
Medical problem
(n ϭ 42)
Primiparity 72 69
Marital/cohabitation status
Legally married 35.9 38.1
Cohabiting 60.9 59.5
Single 3.3 2.4
Place of birth
Province of Quebec 74.2 83.3
Other province in Canada 3.2 2.4
Other country 22.6 14.3
First language
French 74.2 88.1
English 8.6 4.8
Other 17.2 7.1
Maternal education
Secondary or less 33 23.8
College or university 67 76.2
Family income, CAD/year
Ͻ$15,000 11.8 9.8
$15,000 to Ͻ $30,000 17.6 14.6
$30,000 to Ͻ $50,000 23.5 9.8
$50,000 to Ͻ $80,000 29.4 46.3
Ն$80,000 17.6 19.5
Note. Demographic variables did not significantly differ between the
control and stressful birth groups, ␹2
Ͻ 5.34, n.s.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
813HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING
nancy anxiety, or depressive symptoms between medical-
problem-at-birth and control groups, ts Ͻ 1.48 (see Table 2).
Time 3. To measure mothers’ perceptions of infant health,
mothers were asked, “How would you rate the overall health of
your baby at birth?” and “How would you rate the overall health
of your baby at the present time?” in a 6-month postpartum
interview. Mothers responded on a 5-point scale (1 ϭ excellent,
5 ϭ bad; Idler & Benyamini, 1997).
Time 4. Mothers who participated in the 12-month postpar-
tum follow-up study provided data on infant fussing and crying
using experience sampling on personal digital assistants. Moth-
ers were paged at 10:00 a.m., 2:00 p.m., and 7:00 p.m. for 7
days and were asked to answer 11 questions about themselves
and their most recent interaction with their infant. One question
was, “In your last interaction your baby was ____.” Response
options were “happy/playful,” “sleepy,” “fussy,” “very upset/
crying,” “calm/quiet,” and “active/energetic.” These options are
similar to those used in previous diary research. For example,
previous studies have asked mothers to report when, throughout
the day, infants were asleep, awake content, awake fussy, awake
crying, or awake sucking, at a time of their convenience, using a
paper-and-pencil diary (Barr, Kramer, Boisjoly, McVey-White, &
Pless, 1988; Wells, Hinds, & Davies, 1997). Response selections of
“fussy” or “very upset/crying” were summed across days to
create an indirect indicator of frequency of infant fussing and
crying throughout the week. Reports of four or more fussy and
very upset/crying occasions were given a score of 4 to create a
fussing and crying scale of 0 to 4, to reduce the influence of
outliers and positive skew. The mean fussing and crying score
across groups was 1.42 (SD ϭ 1.23). Mothers’ perceptions of
infant health were also assessed at 12 months, with the same
procedure used at 6 months.
Statistical Analysis
Data analyses involved three steps. First, an independent
samples t test compared mothers’ ratings of infant health at
birth between the medical problem and control groups to cross-
validate participant classification by MDs.
Then, CFA was used to test whether prenatal maternal self-
esteem, attachment, quality of romantic relationship, and social
support are indicators of felt security. First, the Montreal Pre-
maturity Study sample (N ϭ 5,092) was randomly divided into
a derivation and a validation sample. Then, with the derivation
sample (n ϭ 2,549), CFA was conducted using structural equa-
tion modeling. This tested the goodness of fit of a one-factor
model in which mothers’ prenatal attachment anxiety and
avoidance, self-esteem, relationship quality, and the four social
support variables loaded on a single factor. We included cor-
related measurement errors among the four social support in-
dicators to account for the possibility that these indicators
would share unique variance not accounted for by the model.
The goodness of fit of an alternative two-factor model in which
the four social support indicators were loaded onto one factor,
and attachment anxiety and avoidance, self esteem, and rela-
tionship quality were loaded onto a second factor, was com-
pared with the goodness of fit of the one-factor model with the
Akaike information criterion (AIC; Kline, 1998), the compar-
ative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approx-
imation (RMSEA; Byrne, 2010; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).
Finally, hierarchical multiple regression was used to test the
main study hypothesis that prenatal maternal felt security in-
teracts with infant health at birth to predict infant fussing and
crying. Bootstrapping was used to obtain a more robust estimate
of standard error.
Results
Validating Infant Health at Birth
An independent samples t test revealed that, consistent with
MD’s classifications, mothers of infants with medical problems
at birth rated their infants as less healthy at birth (M ϭ 2.45,
SD ϭ 1.23) than mothers in the control condition (M ϭ 1.36,
SD ϭ .70), t(127) ϭ Ϫ5.36, p Ͻ .01. However, mothers’ ratings
of infant health at 6 months did not differ between the medical-
problem-at-birth (M ϭ 1.33, SD ϭ .53) and control groups
(M ϭ 1.3, SD ϭ .57), t Ͻ 1, because the medical-problem-at-
birth group rated health at 6 months to be significantly better
than at birth, t(41) ϭ 6.08, p Ͻ .01. Mothers’ ratings of infant
health at 12 months also did not differ between the medical-
problem-at-birth (M ϭ 1.57, SD ϭ .67) and control groups
(M ϭ 1.56, SD ϭ .74), t Ͻ 1.
Modeling Maternal Prenatal Felt Security
Comparison of the fit indices from the CFA suggested that
the one-factor model was acceptable (CFI ϭ .95, RMSEA ϭ
.06, AIC ϭ 215.31) and yielded substantially better fit than the
two-factor model (CFI ϭ .76, RMSEA ϭ .11, AIC ϭ 752.39).
The one-factor solution was therefore retained. Standardized
estimates for all eight indicators of the one-factor solution were
statistically significant and varied from Ϯ.28 to .60. The vali-
dation sample (n ϭ 2,543) cross-validated the structure of the
one-factor model (CFI ϭ .96, RMSEA ϭ .055, AIC ϭ 182.54),
confirming its validity (see Figure 1 for an illustration).
Predicting Infant Fussing and Crying
Thirteen mothers responded to the fussing and crying
experience-sampling question less than seven times and were
therefore excluded from this hierarchical multiple regression
analysis. In the first step of the regression, maternal pregnancy
anxiety was not a significant predictor of infant fussing and
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations According to Control or
Medical-Problem-at-Birth Classification
Measures
Control
(n ϭ 93)
Medical
problem
(n ϭ 42)
M SD M SD
Prenatal felt security Ϫ0.01 0.83 0.21 0.73
Age 27.70 4.60 28.70 4.72
Pregnancy anxiety 0.83 0.78 1.05 0.92
Prenatal depressive symptoms 11.12 7.06 9.76 8.74
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
814 SAWADA ET AL.
crying, B ϭ .08, SE ϭ .14, ns. In the second step, maternal felt
security and infant health at birth were also not significant
predictors, Bs ϭ Ϫ.26 and .34, SEs ϭ .15 and .24, ps ϭ .088
and .17, respectively. In the third step, the product of the
predictor variables was entered to test the interaction between
maternal prenatal felt security and infant health at birth. This
interaction term was a significant predictor of infant fussing and
crying at 12 months, B ϭ Ϫ56, SE ϭ .27, p Ͻ .05.1
The results
of these regression analyses are presented in Table 3.
The simple slopes between maternal prenatal felt security and
infant fussing and crying were analyzed according to Aiken and
West (1991). Among mothers in the control group, prenatal felt
security did not significantly predict infant fussing and crying,
B ϭ Ϫ.13, SE ϭ .16, ns. However, among mothers in the medical-
problem-at-birth group, prenatal felt security significantly pre-
dicted infant fussing and crying, B ϭ Ϫ.69, SE ϭ .23, p Ͻ .01.
Among infants born with a medical problem, those with mothers
with high prenatal felt security fussed and cried less than those
with mothers with low prenatal felt security. Further, among
infants of mothers with low prenatal felt security, those with
medical problems at birth fussed and cried more at 12 months than
infants who were healthy at birth, B ϭ .89, SE ϭ .37, p Ͻ .05.
Among infants of mothers with high prenatal felt security, health
at birth did not significantly predict fussing and crying, B ϭ Ϫ.03,
SE ϭ .28, ns.
We reran the analyses, also controlling for maternal age, pre-
natal depressive symptoms, education, and income as a stricter test
of the hypothesis. None of the covariates were significantly related
to the outcome variable, all ps Ͼ .32. The interaction remained
statistically significant, B ϭ Ϫ.64, SE ϭ .32, p ϭ .05.
The simple effects remained highly significant for infants in the
medical-problem-at-birth group, B ϭ Ϫ.69, SE ϭ .30, p Ͻ .05, and
for infants whose mothers were low in prenatal felt security, B ϭ
1.05, SE ϭ .39, p Ͻ .01.
Discussion
Infant health at birth did not predict infant fussing and crying at
12 months. However, as hypothesized, infant health at birth inter-
acted with maternal prenatal felt security to predict frequency of
infant fussing and crying at 12 months. Among infants who were
healthy at birth, maternal prenatal felt security was not related to
fussing and crying. However, among infants born with a medical
problem, maternal prenatal felt security predicted fussing and
crying such that those with mothers with low felt security fussed
and cried more than those with mothers with high felt security.
Infants born with medical problems fussed and cried more than did
infants who were healthy at birth, only if their mothers had low
prenatal felt security but not if their mothers had high prenatal felt
security. The results of this study suggest that a mother’s bank of
preexisting psychosocial resources buffers the effects of infant
medical problems at birth on infant developmental outcomes at 12
months postpartum.
We hypothesize that elevated fussing and crying among infants
born with medical problems to low felt security mothers reflects
increased negative reactivity or decreased regulatory capabilities.
This may be directly related to infants’ early medical problems or
to exposure to stressors such as hospitalization, medical proce-
dures, and pain in the neonatal period. Increased infant negative
affect or decreased regulatory abilities may result in more chal-
lenging mother–infant interactions, and may persist into childhood
to predict emotional and behavioral disorders. However, infant
affect and behavior regulation capabilities may develop through
interactions with caregivers who are able to regulate their own
emotions effectively and provide sensitive care (Braungart-Rieker,
1
We also examined whether social support, attachment security, self-
esteem, or relationship quality individually interacted with infant health at
birth to predict infant fussing and crying in four separate regression
analyses. None of these variables significantly interacted with health at
birth to predict fussing and crying; all ps Ͼ .1
Figure 1. Prenatal felt security model.
Table 3
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Infant
Fussing and Crying at 12 Months From Prenatal Maternal Felt
Security and Infant Health at Birth
Infant fussing
and crying at
12 months
(n ϭ 122)
B SE
Step 1
Pregnancy anxiety .08 .14
Step 2
Prenatal maternal felt security Ϫ.26 .15
Infant health at birth .34 .24
Step 3
Maternal felt security ϫ Infant health at birth Ϫ.56‫ء‬
.27
Simple slope – Control Ϫ.13 .16
Simple slope – Medical problem Ϫ.69‫ءء‬
.23
‫ء‬
p Ͻ .05. ‫ءء‬
p Ͻ .001.
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
815HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING
Garwood, Powers, & Notaro, 1998; Calkins, Smith, Gill, & John-
son, 1998; Olafsen et al., 2008). We theorize that prenatal felt
security may be associated with better maternal well-being and
may thus provide a foundation for maternal, affect, cognition, and
behavior that support the development of infant affect and behav-
ior regulation among infants born with health problems.
There are several mechanisms whereby maternal felt security
measured in pregnancy might moderate infant fussing and crying
at 12 months postpartum. First, mothers’ prenatal felt security is
likely an indicator of mothers’ internal sense of security in close
relationships at the onset of the infant medical stressor. Research
on attachment, social support, and self-esteem suggests that moth-
ers with higher prenatal felt security might perceive close others as
more available and responsive in times of need, and that these
secure mental representations of the self in relation to close others
might attenuate mothers’ appraisals of threat and facilitate more
adaptive coping in the context of a stressor (Alexander, Feeney,
Hohaus, & Noller, 2001; Berant et al., 2001; Cutrona & Troutman,
1986; Florian, Mikulincer, & Bucholtz, 1995; Mikulincer & Flo-
rian, 1998; Ognibene & Collins, 1998). Mothers with higher pre-
natal felt security may thus experience the birth of an infant with
medical problems as less stressful and may perceive themselves as
more capable of coping with the challenges it presents. Maternal
prenatal felt security may thus buffer against the possible adverse
effects of this event on maternal well-being. Decreased maternal
stress or increased self-efficacy may facilitate the provision of
sensitive maternal care associated with the development of infant
affect and behavior regulation capabilities among mothers with
higher prenatal felt security. Second, prenatal felt security may be
an indicator of the level of observable social support that mothers
receive. In other words, mothers with higher prenatal felt security
may have a larger bank of tangible and social resources ready at
their disposal when the infant medical stressor occurs. Mothers
with higher prenatal felt security may receive more support be-
cause they are confident in drawing upon this reserve (Ognibene &
Collins, 1998). Alternatively, mothers with higher prenatal felt
security may receive more social support without necessarily seek-
ing it out or even noticing it. This observable support may directly
benefit the mother and the medically vulnerable infant.
Conversely, for mothers with low prenatal felt security, the birth
of an infant with a medical problem may lead to elevated stress and
decreased parenting efficacy. The birth of a medically fragile
infant is a stressor in and of itself. Further, mothers with low
prenatal felt security may perceive greater threat and perceive
themselves as having fewer psychological, social, and tangible
resources to cope than mothers with higher prenatal felt security.
In addition, they may concretely have fewer psychological, social,
and tangible resources. Regardless, this perceived or actual lack of
resources may compound the effects of an infant medical stressor
on their well-being. They may experience more distress, which
may negatively impact their ability to engage in interactions that
support the development of infant affect and behavior regulation.
This may result in increased infant fussing and crying at 12 months
postpartum.
Future research might examine how mothers’ felt security re-
lates to mothers’ stress appraisal and coping, as well as observable
receipt of social and tangible resources postpartum. This may
elucidate the extent to which the effects of felt security are medi-
ated by a perceived feeling of security in close relationships and
cognitive factors versus observable social support received.
We hypothesized that preexisting, prenatal psychosocial re-
sources may play a particularly important protective role. Al-
though psychosocial resources may be mobilized postpartum, ma-
ternal distress may be immediate, whereas the mobilization of
resources may take time. Maternal distress in the early postnatal
period may set in motion a pattern of less adaptive maternal
cognitions and behavior, and mother–infant interactions that may
persist beyond the duration of the stressful experience and con-
tinue, despite the later availability of resources mobilized after
birth (Minde, Whitelaw, Brown, & Fitzhardinge, 1983). However,
the current study cannot directly test the relative importance of
preexisting versus mobilized psychosocial resources, because post-
partum assessments of felt security indicators are likely biased by
mothers’ experience of the newborn’s medical stressor. Future
research should examine the issue of the relative importance of
preexisting versus mobilized resources.
Although we hypothesized that maternal reports of infant fuss-
ing and crying reflect actual levels of fussing and crying, an
alternate possibility is that low felt security mothers of infants with
medical problems experience elevated stress, and thus perceive
their infants as having more negative affect regardless of actual
levels of fussiness or negative affect. However, this seems un-
likely, as no association between maternal prenatal felt security
and maternal report of fussing and crying was found among infants
who were healthy at birth. Further, the regression analysis con-
trolled for mothers’ pregnancy anxiety. Because we do not have a
third-party report of our dependent measure, we cannot be sure of
whether maternal reports of infant fussing and crying in this study
reflect actual levels of fussing and crying or maternal perceptions
of negative infant affect. Regardless, the findings reported here
have important implications for child outcomes. Maternal percep-
tions of difficult infant temperament are related to maternal stress,
distress, and feelings of incompetence (Cutrona & Troutman,
1986; Östberg & Hagekull, 2000). Maternal perception of in-
creased infant fussing and crying may thus predict decreased
maternal well-being and ability to provide optimal care. However,
future research might further examine this issue by obtaining
objective reports of infant fussing and crying with sound-recording
equipment, for example.
Further, although the large size of the initial cohort allowed
cross-validation of the latent variable, the wide range of measures
needed in the multidisciplinary Montreal Prematurity Study re-
quired abbreviated versions of several measures. This resulted in
low to moderate reliability coefficients for some measures, such as
the attachment scales.
Despite these limitations, these findings expand on previous
research in several ways and the study has a number of strengths.
First, this study makes a unique contribution to the literature on
infant medical vulnerability and maternal stress and psychosocial
resources by demonstrating that prenatal maternal psychosocial
resources and infant medical problems at birth interact to predict
an infant developmental outcome at 12 months postpartum. Sec-
ond, the longitudinal design ensured that maternal measures of
psychosocial resources preceded, and were therefore not influ-
enced by, infant medical problems.
Third, this study employed a more comprehensive and parsimo-
nious measure of maternal psychosocial resources than is typically
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
816 SAWADA ET AL.
used in related research. Felt security has been conceptualized in
the adult attachment literature as encompassing attachment secu-
rity, perceived regard from close others, romantic relationship
quality, and the expectation that others are available and respon-
sive in times of need (Florian et al., 1995; Murray et al., 2006;
Murray et al., 2000; Murray et al., 2001). No validated measure of
felt security that reflects this conceptualization exists. We there-
fore modeled felt security using CFA, with attachment, self-
esteem, relationship quality, and social support as indicators. Felt
security scores were then computed and used to test the primary
hypothesis that this factor interacts with infant health problems at
birth to predict infant fussing and crying. The results of this study
suggest that attachment security, self-esteem, relationship quality,
and social support are indeed indicators of felt security, thus
contributing to an improved empirical understanding of this con-
struct. Further, although maternal prenatal felt security interacted
with infant health at birth to predict infant fussing and crying, none
of the individual indicators did. This suggests that mothers’ ag-
gregate resources matter. However, it is possible that with more
reliable measures of any of the felt security indicators, one may
have significantly interacted with health at birth. It is also possible
that some measures, like social support, could have direct or
indirect moderating effects by promoting attachment security. Fu-
ture research should examine these issues. In the meantime, the
finding that maternal prenatal felt security interacts with a stressor
to predict an infant outcome lays the groundwork for future re-
search on felt security, which may aim to develop a concise
measure or examine the buffering effects of felt security in other
contexts.
Fourth, the study used experience sampling, thus minimizing
memory bias associated with post hoc questionnaires and increas-
ing ecological validity. Few studies examining interactions be-
tween infant medical problems and maternal psychosocial re-
sources have used this methodology. This study makes a
significant contribution by demonstrating that felt security and
neonatal medical problems predict infant behavior with a measure
of mothers’ real-time, daily life experience.
Lastly, mothers’ ratings of infant health at 6 and 12 months did
not differ between infants with medical problems and infants who
were healthy at birth. This suggests that infants who are born with
medical problems and have mothers with low prenatal felt security
are not fussier simply because of persistently poorer (perceived)
health. Rather, these results suggest that medical problems at birth
combined with lower maternal prenatal felt security contribute to
patterns of mother–infant interactions that are associated with
increased maternal report of infant fussing and crying that persist
12 months postpartum, despite perceived improvements in infant
health. Future studies should investigate the relationship between
neonatal medical problems, maternal psychosocial resources, and
the child’s emotional and behavioral regulation beyond infancy.
The results of this study suggest the relevance and time course
of interventions that might increase felt security among mothers of
infants with medical problems or mothers at risk of having an
infant with medical problems. Resilience-promoting interventions
that increase felt security during pregnancy may be useful for
promoting more favorable maternal and infant outcomes after
birth.
References
Adamakos, H., Ryan, K., Ullman, D. G., Pascoe, J., Diaz, R., & Chessare,
J. (1986). Maternal social support as a predictor of mother-child stress
and stimulation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 10, 463–470. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/0145-2134(86)90050-5
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Alexander, R., Feeney, J., Hohaus, L., & Noller, P. (2001). Attachment
style and coping resources as predictors of coping strategies in the
transition to parenthood. Personal Relationships, 8, 137–152. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2001.tb00032.x
Anand, K. J. S., & Scalzo, F. M. (2000). Can adverse neonatal experiences
alter brain development and subsequent behavior? Biology of the Neo-
nate, 77, 69–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000014197
Barr, R. G., Kramer, M. S., Boisjoly, C., McVey-White, L., & Pless, I. B.
(1988). Parental diary of infant cry and fuss behavior. Archives of
Disease in Childhood, 63, 380–387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.63.4
.380
Barrera, M. (1981). Social support in the adjustment of pregnant adoles-
cents: Assessment issues. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.), Social networks and
social support (pp. 69–96). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Bee, H. L., Hammond, M. A., Eyres, S. J., Barnard, K. E., & Snyder, C.
(1986). The impact of parental life change on the early development of
children. Research in Nursing & Health, 9, 65–74. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/nur.4770090111
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal
responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171–1190. http://dx.doi.org/
10.2307/1127506
Berant, E., Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2001). The association of
mothers’ attachment style and their psychological reactions to the diag-
nosis of infant’s congenital heart disease. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 20, 208–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.20.2.208
.22264
Bhutta, A. T., Cleves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., & Anand,
K. J. S. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged
children who were born preterm. JAMA: Journal of the American
Medical Association, 288, 728–737. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.288
.6.728
Blumberg, N. L. (1980). Effects of neonatal risk, maternal attitude, and
cognitive style on early postpartum adjustment. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 89, 139–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.89.2
.139
Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Notaro, P. C.
(1998). Infant affect and affect regulation during the still-face paradigm
with mothers and fathers: The role of infant characteristics and parental
sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1428–1437. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1428
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure-
ment of adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A.
Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relation-
ships (pp. 46–76). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Browne, J. V., & Talmi, A. (2005). Family-based intervention to enhance
infant–parent relationships in the neonatal intensive care unit. Journal of
Pediatric Psychology, 30, 667–677. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/
jsi053
Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic
concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Routledge.
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal
interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and
physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7,
350–369. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00072
Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1995).
Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
817HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING
age three to age fifteen. Child Development, 66, 55–68. http://dx.doi
.org/10.2307/1131190
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering
hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310
Collins, N. L., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Lobel, M., & Scrimshaw, S. C. (1993).
Social support in pregnancy: Psychosocial correlates of birth outcomes
and postpartum depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 65, 1243–1258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1243
Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment
shape perceptions of social support: Evidence from experimental and
observational studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
363–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.363
Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M., Ragozin, A., Robinson, N., & Basham, R.
(1983). Effects of stress and social support on mothers and premature
and full-term infants. Child Development, 54, 209–217. http://dx.doi
.org/10.2307/1129878
Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M., & Slough, N. (1986). Early stress and social
support influences on mothers’ and high-risk infants’ functioning in late
infancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 7, 19–33. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/1097-0355(198621)7:1Ͻ19::AID-IMHJ2280070104Ͼ3.0.CO;2-1
Cutrona, C. E., & Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social support, infant temper-
ament, and parenting self-efficacy: A mediational model of postpartum
depression. Child Development, 57, 1507–1518. http://dx.doi.org/
10.2307/1130428
Darke, P. R., & Goldberg, S. (1994). Father-infant interaction and parent
stress with healthy and medically compromised infants. Infant Be-
havior & Development, 17, 3–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0163-
6383(94)90017-5
DeLongis, L., & Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The impact of daily
stress on health and mood: Psychological and social resources as medi-
ators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 486–495.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.486
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant
affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control.
Developmental Psychology, 35, 223–231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0012-1649.35.1.223
Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Bucholtz, I. (1995). Effects of adult attach-
ment style on the perception and search for social support. The Journal
of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 129, 665–676. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1995.9914937
Gallo, L. C., & Matthews, K. (2003). Understanding the association be-
tween socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions
play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 10–51. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.10
Goldstein, L. H., Diener, M. L., & Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1996). Maternal
characteristics and social support across the transition to motherhood:
Associations with maternal behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 10,
60–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.1.60
Hack, M., Klein, N. K., & Taylor, H. G. (1995). Long-term developmental
outcomes of low birth weight infants. Future of Children, 5, 176–196.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1602514
Hall, L. A., Kotch, J. B., Browne, D., & Rayens, M. K. (1996). Self-esteem
as a mediator of the effects of stressors and social resources on depres-
sive symptoms in postpartum mothers. Nursing Research, 45, 231–238.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006199-199607000-00007
Hane, A. A., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Ordinary variations in maternal
caregiving influence human infants’ stress reactivity. Psychological
Science, 17, 550–556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006
.01742.x
Hopkins, J., Campbell, S. B., & Marcus, M. (1987). Role of infant-related
stressors in postpartum depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96,
237–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.96.3.237
Idler, E. L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A
review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 38, 21–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2955359
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation mod-
eling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kramer, M. S., Lydon, J. E., Séguin, L., Goulet, L., Kahn, S. R., McNa-
mara, H., . . . Platt, R. W. (2009). Stress pathways to spontaneous
preterm birth: The role of stressors, psychological distress, and stress
hormones. American Journal of Epidemiology, 169, 1319–1326. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwp061
Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling
method. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Sci-
ence, 15, 41–56.
Major, B., Richards, C., Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C., & Zubek, J. (1998).
Personal resilience, cognitive appraisals, and coping: An integrative
model of adjustment to abortion. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 735–752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.735
Mancuso, R. A., Schetter, C. D., Rini, C. M., Roesch, S. C., & Hobel, C. J.
(2004). Maternal prenatal anxiety and corticotropin-releasing hormone
associated with timing of delivery. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 762–
769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000138284.70670.d5
McGrath, J. M., Records, K., & Rice, M. (2008). Maternal depression and
infant temperament characteristics. Infant Behavior & Development, 31,
71–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.07.001
McGrath, M., Boukydis, C. F. Z., & Lester, B. M. (1993). Determinants of
maternal self-esteem in the neonatal period. Infant Mental Health Jour-
nal, 13, 35–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355
Mebert, C. J. (1991). Dimensions of subjectivity in parents’ ratings of
infant temperament. Child Development, 62, 352–361. http://dx.doi.org/
10.2307/1131008
Miceli, P. J., Goeke-Morey, M., Whitman, T., Kolberg, K., Miller-Loncar,
C., & White, R. (2000). Brief report: Birth status, medical complications,
and social environment: Individual differences in development of pre-
term, very low birth weight infants. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 25,
353–358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/25.5.353
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult
attachment styles and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful
events. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and
close relationships (pp. 143–165). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Minde, K., Whitelaw, A., Brown, J., & Fitzhardinge, P. (1983). Effect of
neonatal complications in premature infants on early parent-infant in-
teractions. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 25, 763–777.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1983.tb13845.x
Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini,
A., & Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother–
child interactions. Early Human Development, 79, 145–158. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J., & Collins, N. (2006). Optimizing assurance: The
risk regulation system in relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 132,
641–666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.641
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2000). Self-esteem and the
quest for felt security: How perceived regard regulates attachment pro-
cesses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 478–498.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.478
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., Griffin, D. W., Bellavia, G., & Rose, P.
(2001). The mismeasure of love: How self-doubt contaminates relation-
ship beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 423–436.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167201274004
Natsuaki, M. N., Ge, X., Leve, L. D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S.,
Conger, R. D., . . . Reiss, D. (2010). Genetic liability, environment, and
the development of fussiness in toddlers: The roles of maternal depres-
sion and parental responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1147–
1158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019659
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
818 SAWADA ET AL.
Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the depen-
dent variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141–151. http://
dx.doi.org/10.2307/351302
Ognibene, T. C., & Collins, N. L. (1998). Adult attachment styles, per-
ceived social support and coping strategies. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 15, 323–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0265407598153002
Olafsen, K. S., Kaaresen, P., Handegård, B., Ulvund, S., Dahl, L., &
Rønning, J. (2008). Maternal ratings of infant regulatory competence
from 6 to 12 months: Influence of perceived stress, birth-weight, and
intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Infant Behavior & Devel-
opment, 31, 408–421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.005
Östberg, M., & Hagekull, B. (2000). A structural modeling approach to the
understanding of parenting stress. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
29, 615–625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP2904_13
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for
research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement,
1, 385–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306
Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner
responsiveness as an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and
closeness. In D. J. Mashek and A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness
and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rende, R. D. (1993). Longitudinal relations between temperament traits
and behavioral syndromes in middle childhood. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 287–290. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1097/00004583-199303000-00008
Singer, L., Davillier, M., Bruening, P., Hawkins, S., & Yamashita, T.
(1996). Social support, psychological distress, and parenting strains in
mothers of very low birthweight infants. Family Relations: An Interdis-
ciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 45, 343–350. http://dx.doi
.org/10.2307/585507
Singer, L. T., Salvator, A., Guo, S., Collin, M., Lilien, L., & Baley, J.
(1999). Maternal psychological distress and parenting stress after the
birth of a very low-birth-weight infant. JAMA: Journal of the American
Medical Association, 281, 799–805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.281
.9.799
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational
construct. Child Development, 48, 1184–1199. http://dx.doi.org/
10.2307/1128475
Stapleton, L. R. T., Schetter, C. D., Westling, E., Rini, C., Glynn, L. M.,
Hobel, C. J., & Sandman, C. A. (2012). Perceived partner support in
pregnancy predicts lower maternal and infant distress. Journal of Family
Psychology, 26, 453–463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028332
Stewart, A. L., Rifkin, L., Amess, P. N., Kirkbride, V., Townsend, J. P.,
Miller, D. H., . . . Murray, R. M. (1999). Brain structure and neuro-
cognitive and behavioral function in adolescents who were born very
preterm. The Lancet, 353, 1653–1657. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-
6736(98)07130-X
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th
ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
van den Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering
on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensi-
tive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants.
Child Development, 65, 1457–1477. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131511
Wells, J. C. K., Hinds, A., & Davies, P. S. W. (1997). Free-living energy
expenditure and behavior in late infancy. Archives of Disease in Child-
hood, 76, 490–494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.76.6.490
Received May 8, 2013
Revision received June 23, 2014
Accepted July 19, 2014 Ⅲ
ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers.
Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly.
819HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING

More Related Content

What's hot

Sharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancy
Sharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancySharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancy
Sharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancyTherese Gratia
 
School Mental Health
School Mental HealthSchool Mental Health
School Mental HealthTara Powell
 
Treatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed Children
Treatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed ChildrenTreatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed Children
Treatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed Childrenguestcb5107
 
Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...
Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...
Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...BARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...worldwideww
 
Assessment and Treatment in Child Psychology
Assessment and Treatment in Child PsychologyAssessment and Treatment in Child Psychology
Assessment and Treatment in Child PsychologyAlexandraPerkins5
 
Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...
Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...
Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...NationalBurnsCentre2000
 
Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...
Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...
Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...AddisonWilson
 
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.orgUnderstanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.orgCassondra Turner McArthur
 
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 NiedzweckiThe Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 NiedzweckiChristian Niedzwecki
 
Exploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling Adolescents
Exploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling AdolescentsExploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling Adolescents
Exploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling AdolescentsWill Dobud
 
Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3
Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3
Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3Brenna Patterson
 
Powerpoint research project cs
Powerpoint research project csPowerpoint research project cs
Powerpoint research project csChristyCrawford7
 
Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...
Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...
Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...iosrjce
 

What's hot (17)

Sharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancy
Sharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancySharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancy
Sharon Rising Presentation on CenteringPregnancy
 
School Mental Health
School Mental HealthSchool Mental Health
School Mental Health
 
Treatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed Children
Treatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed ChildrenTreatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed Children
Treatment Of Depressed Mothers To Depressed Children
 
M8 a2 clapp_lael
M8 a2 clapp_laelM8 a2 clapp_lael
M8 a2 clapp_lael
 
Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...
Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...
Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and r...
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
 
Assessment and Treatment in Child Psychology
Assessment and Treatment in Child PsychologyAssessment and Treatment in Child Psychology
Assessment and Treatment in Child Psychology
 
Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...
Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...
Psychological assessment of burns by Suhasini Oliveira, National Burns Centre...
 
Powerpoint daft 6
Powerpoint daft 6Powerpoint daft 6
Powerpoint daft 6
 
Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...
Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...
Dadds Spence 1997 Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders JCo...
 
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.orgUnderstanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
 
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 NiedzweckiThe Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 2008 Niedzwecki
 
Exploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling Adolescents
Exploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling AdolescentsExploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling Adolescents
Exploring Adventure Therapy as an Early Intervention for Struggling Adolescents
 
Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3
Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3
Patterson_Poster Draft_OT+s2014_RB3
 
Powerpoint research project cs
Powerpoint research project csPowerpoint research project cs
Powerpoint research project cs
 
Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...
Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...
Coping Strategies Among Caregivers Of Patients With Schizophrenia: A Descript...
 
Trauma Informed Care: Theory & Pactice with Laurie Robinson
Trauma Informed Care: Theory & Pactice with Laurie RobinsonTrauma Informed Care: Theory & Pactice with Laurie Robinson
Trauma Informed Care: Theory & Pactice with Laurie Robinson
 

Similar to Health Psychology 2014

Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...hamsulbasri
 
Clinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With Breastfeeding
Clinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With BreastfeedingClinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With Breastfeeding
Clinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With BreastfeedingBiblioteca Virtual
 
Perinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASF
Perinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASFPerinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASF
Perinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASFErin Huie
 
Hardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature ReviewHardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature ReviewGabriel Hardt
 
Critique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdfCritique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdfsdfghj21
 
Maternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child DevelopmentMaternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child DevelopmentMegan Dean-Mahoney
 
Coping strategies of mothers having children with special needs
Coping strategies of mothers having children with special needsCoping strategies of mothers having children with special needs
Coping strategies of mothers having children with special needsAlexander Decker
 
Research Paper_TEN_GCypher
Research Paper_TEN_GCypherResearch Paper_TEN_GCypher
Research Paper_TEN_GCypherGabrielle Cypher
 
Critical Review of Research Evidence Part 3 FD
Critical Review of Research Evidence Part 3  FDCritical Review of Research Evidence Part 3  FD
Critical Review of Research Evidence Part 3 FDRobert Cope
 
More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11
More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11
More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11CORE Group
 
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxAdult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxdaniahendric
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective studylucacerniglia
 
Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007
Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007
Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007MedicineAndHealth
 
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...https://www.facebook.com/garmentspace
 
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...inventionjournals
 
3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx
3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx
3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docxlorainedeserre
 
Caregiver stress
Caregiver stressCaregiver stress
Caregiver stressashbrink
 
Day Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: Study
Day Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: StudyDay Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: Study
Day Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: StudyOliver_Gray
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...inventionjournals
 

Similar to Health Psychology 2014 (20)

Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
 
Clinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With Breastfeeding
Clinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With BreastfeedingClinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With Breastfeeding
Clinician Support And Psychosocial Risk Factors Associated With Breastfeeding
 
Perinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASF
Perinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASFPerinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASF
Perinatal MH Brief_Final_MHASF
 
Hardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature ReviewHardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature Review
 
Critique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdfCritique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdf
 
Maternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child DevelopmentMaternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child Development
 
Coping strategies of mothers having children with special needs
Coping strategies of mothers having children with special needsCoping strategies of mothers having children with special needs
Coping strategies of mothers having children with special needs
 
Research Paper_TEN_GCypher
Research Paper_TEN_GCypherResearch Paper_TEN_GCypher
Research Paper_TEN_GCypher
 
Critical Review of Research Evidence Part 3 FD
Critical Review of Research Evidence Part 3  FDCritical Review of Research Evidence Part 3  FD
Critical Review of Research Evidence Part 3 FD
 
More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11
More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11
More than baby blues_Senefeld, Reider, Schooley_10.13.11
 
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxAdult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective study
 
Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007
Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007
Gap Prevention Psychiatry Slides12 2007
 
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
 
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
 
3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx
3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx
3Assignment Three Purpose of the study and Research Questions.docx
 
Report edited
Report editedReport edited
Report edited
 
Caregiver stress
Caregiver stressCaregiver stress
Caregiver stress
 
Day Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: Study
Day Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: StudyDay Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: Study
Day Care May Be Beneficial to Children of Depressed Moms: Study
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
 

Health Psychology 2014

  • 1. Maternal Prenatal Felt Security and Infant Health at Birth Interact to Predict Infant Fussing and Crying at 12 Months Postpartum Natsumi Sawada McGill University Faby M. Gagné Granite State College Louise Séguin Université de Montréal Michael S. Kramer, Helen McNamara, and Robert W. Platt McGill University Lise Goulet Université de Montréal Michael J. Meaney and John E. Lydon McGill University Infants born with medical problems are at risk for less optimal developmental outcomes. This may be, in part, because neonatal medical problems are associated with maternal distress, which may adversely impact infants. However, the reserve capacity model suggests that an individual’s bank of psychosocial resources buffers the adverse effects of later-encountered stressors. This prospective longitudinal study examined whether preex- isting maternal psychosocial resources, conceptualized as felt security in close relationships, moderate the association between neonatal medical problems and infant fussing and crying 12 months postpartum. Maternal felt security was measured by assessing its indicators in 5,092 pregnant women. At birth, infants were classified as healthy or having a medical problem. At 12 months, experience sampling was used to assess daily maternal reports of fussing and crying in 135 mothers of infants who were healthy or had medical problems at birth. Confirmatory factor analyses revealed that attachment, relationship quality, self-esteem, and social support can be conceptualized as indicators of a single felt security factor. Multiple regression analyses revealed that prenatal maternal felt security interacts with infant health at birth to predict fussing and crying at 12 months. Among infants born with medical problems, higher felt security predicted decreased fussing and crying. Maternal felt security assessed before birth dampens the association between neonatal medical problems and subsequent infant behavior. This supports the hypothesis that psychosocial resources in reserve can be called upon in the face of a stressor to reduce its adverse effects on the self or others. Keywords: prenatal felt security, prenatal psychosocial resources, maternal stressor, newborn health problems, infant fussing and crying Infants born preterm, low birth weight, or with severe medical problems are at risk for later emotional, cognitive, and behavioral problems (Bhutta, Cleves, Casey, Cradock, & Anand, 2002; Hack, Klein, & Taylor, 1995; Miceli et al., 2000). This may be in part because neonatal medical problems and stressors associated with medical care may cause alterations in brain structure and functioning This article was published Online First December 22, 2014. Natsumi Sawada, Department of Psychology, McGill University; Faby M. Gagné, Department of Behavioral Sciences, Granite State College; Louise Séguin, Département de Médecine Sociale et Preventive, École de Santé Publique, Université de Montréal; Michael S. Kramer, Department of Pediatrics, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics, and Occupational Health, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Can- ada; Helen McNamara, Department of Obstetrics & Gynaecology, McGill University; Robert W. Platt, Department of Pediatrics, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, Department of Epidemi- ology, Biostatistics, and Occupational Health, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; Lise Goulet, Département de Médecine Sociale et Preventive, École de Santé Publique, Université de Montréal; Michael J. Meaney, Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psy- chobiology and Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology and Neurosur- gery, McGill University; and John E. Lydon, Department of Psychology, McGill University. This research was supported by grants from the March of Dimes and the Canadian Institutes for Health Research. Robert Platt is a chercheur- boursier of the Fonds de la Recherche du Québec – Santé (FRQ-S), and Drs. Platt and Kramer are members of the Research Institute of the McGill University Health Centre, which receives core funding from the FRQ-S. We thank Henriette Lussier, Antoinette Ghanem, Alex McIntyre-Smith, Peggy McLean, Alisa Khan, Marie-Pierre Markon- Lalonde, Katherine Pascuzzo, Amelie Zonato, Andrée-Ann Bouvette- Turcot, and the student interviewers and nursing students who helped with this study. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nat- sumi Sawada, Psychology Department, McGill University, 1205 ave- nue, Docteur Penfield, Montreal, Quebec H3A 1B1. E-mail: natsumi .sawada@mail.mcgill.ca ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. Health Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 34, No. 8, 811–819 0278-6133/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000152 811
  • 2. (Anand & Scalzo, 2000; Stewart et al., 1999). This may also be in part because birth of a medically vulnerable infant is a significant maternal stressor (Blumberg, 1980; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Singer et al., 1999). Maternal stress is associated with less sensitive behavior and disturbances in mother–infant interactions (Crnic, Greenberg, Ragozin, Robinson, & Basham, 1983; Muller-Nix et al., 2004). The stressor of having a newborn with a medical problem may thereby interfere with parents’ ability to provide infants with an environment that promotes resilience and optimal development (Blumberg, 1980; Darke & Goldberg, 1994; Miceli et al., 2000; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Singer et al., 1999), resulting in poorer developmental outcomes for the child (Bell & Ainsworth, 1972; Browne & Talmi, 2005; Hane & Fox, 2006). However, psychosocial resources moderate the adverse effects of stressors on well-being (Cohen & Wills, 1985; DeLongis, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1988). Specifically, research suggests that maternal psychosocial resources moderate the relationship of in- fant medical problems or maternal stress with maternal distress and behavior. For example, social support moderates the relation between maternal stress and quality of mother–infant interactions among mothers of premature and healthy infants (Crnic et al., 1983). Perceived social support moderates the relation between infant medical vulnerability and maternal distress (Singer, Davil- lier, Bruening, Hawkins, & Yamashita, 1996). Secure attachment predicts decreased maternal distress in the context of infant med- ical problems, with the effects mediated by perceived coping abilities (Berant, Mikulincer, & Florian, 2001). Thus, when an infant is born with a medical problem, maternal psychosocial resources such as social support, attachment security, and per- ceived coping may promote better infant outcomes by dampening the maternal distress that undermines maternal sensitive respon- siveness associated with the development of infant affect regula- tion (Feldman, Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999; Olafsen et al., 2008; van den Boom, 1994). This is consistent with the finding that prenatal social support dampens maternal distress, which, in turn, predicts lower distress among healthy infants (Stapleton et al., 2012). Studies examining the effects of maternal psychosocial re- sources in the context of infant medical problems have often measured resources at or after diagnosis of the medical problem (e.g., Berant et al., 2001; Crnic et al., 1983; Hopkins, Campbell, & Marcus, 1987; Miceli et al., 2000; Singer et al., 1996). However, having an infant with a medical problem may influence psychos- ocial resources such as social support and self-esteem (e.g., M. McGrath, Boukydis, & Lester, 1993). Postdiagnosis assessment of psychosocial resources may therefore be biased. Many studies have examined the effects of prenatal psychosocial resources and maternal stress on birth and maternal outcomes. However, evi- dence of a buffering effect of maternal prenatal social support has been mixed (e.g., Adamakos et al., 1986; Bee, Hammond, Eyres, Barnard, & Snyder, 1986; Collins, Dunkel-Schetter, Lobel, & Scrimshaw, 1993; Goldstein, Diener, & Mangelsdorf, 1996), and scant attention has been given to downstream effects on infants (cf. Crnic, Greenberg, & Slough, 1986). It is therefore unclear whether relationships among maternal psychosocial resources, infant med- ical problems, and maternal or infant outcomes can be explained by preexisting differences in mothers’ psychosocial resources. Gallo and Matthews (2003) theorize that individuals’ reserve capacity or “bank” of psychological, social, and tangible resources buffer the effects of later-encountered stressors. This suggests that preexisting maternal resources may moderate the effects of later- encountered infant medical problems to promote more favorable outcomes for infants. The purpose of this study was to examine whether maternal prenatal psychosocial resources moderate the effects of neonatal medical problems, a maternal stressor, on important infant devel- opmental outcomes: fussing and crying. Infant fussing and crying may reflect elevated distress and negative affectivity, and may thus predict internalizing and externalizing problems later in childhood (Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995; Natsuaki et al., 2010; Rende, 1993). Existing research on the buffering effects of maternal psychos- ocial resources has investigated how constructs such as social support or attachment security singly moderate outcomes associ- ated with maternal stressors. However, researchers have noted that stress-buffering resources tend to occur in aggregate (Gallo & Matthews, 2003). Further, a range of these resources may be conceptually integrated as indicators of felt security. This concept is derived from the attachment literature and is defined as a sense of confidence and safety in the continued affection and availability of close others that regulates interpersonal closeness and depen- dence in times of need (Murray, Holmes, & Collins, 2006; Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2000; Sroufe & Waters, 1977). Researchers propose that felt security has, at its core, perceived positive regard from close others, trust in their availability and responsiveness, and perceived love by one’s partner. Felt security has thus been conceptualized as encompassing self-esteem, attachment security, relationship quality, and perceived social support (Collins & Feeney, 2004; Murray et al., 2006; Murray, Holmes, Griffin, Bellavia, & Rose, 2001; Reis, Clark, & Holmes, 2004). Further, there is evidence of associations among these constructs in a sample of pregnant women (Stapleton et al., 2012). Thus, although the effects of maternal attachment security, social support, self- esteem, and relationship quality on postpartum outcomes have been examined separately (e.g., Berant et al., 2001; Hall, Kotch, Browne, & Rayens, 1996), these constructs may more parsimoni- ously reflect an overarching sense of felt security. Attachment theory and the reserve capacity model suggest that individuals’ reserves of inter- and intrapersonal resources, such as attachment security, self-esteem, social support, and relationship quality, contribute to a cognitive representation of the self in relation to others that moderates effects of later- encountered stressors. We therefore predicted that maternal felt security might protect against detrimental effects of neonatal medical problems on infant development. We therefore exam- ined whether infant medical problems at birth predict infant fussing and crying at 12 months and whether maternal felt security moderates this association. To examine whether mothers’ baseline felt security interacts with a later-encountered infant medical stressor to predict later fussing and crying, we assessed felt security prenatally as an indicator of resources available at the onset of the stressor unbiased by the experience of the stressor. Experience samp- ling was used to assess maternal report of infant behavior (Larson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1983). We controlled for preg- nancy anxiety, as this might be confounded with mothers’ reports of infant fussing and crying (J. M. McGrath et al., 2008; Mebert, 1991). ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 812 SAWADA ET AL.
  • 3. No validated measure of felt security that encompasses at- tachment, relationship quality, self-esteem, and social support exists. Before testing the central hypothesis that maternal felt security would interact with infant health at birth to predict infant fussing and crying, we therefore tested the structure of felt security using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), with measures of attachment, romantic relationship quality, self- esteem, and social support as indicators. Method Participants Over a 5-year period, as part of the Montreal Prematurity Study, women from a wide socioeconomic spectrum were recruited be- tween 24 and 26 weeks gestation when they presented at one of four Montreal hospitals for routine hospital ultrasound examina- tions (Time 1). All women (N ϭ 5,092) were over age 17, pregnant with a singleton fetus, and spoke English or French. Exclusion criteria were placenta praevia, a major fetal anomaly in the current pregnancy, an incompetent cervix diagnosis in a previous preg- nancy, or chronic illnesses other than hypertension, asthma, or diabetes (Kramer et al., 2009). At birth, infants were categorized as healthy or as having a severe neonatal medical problem likely to be a stressor for mothers (Time 2). Mothers were interviewed again at 6 months postpartum (Time 3). A follow-up experience-sampling study was conducted in the final year of the Montreal Prematurity Study. All mothers of infants with a medical problem at birth who were 12 months old when the follow-up study began were invited to participate (Time 4). For each mother of an infant with a medical problem at birth, two mothers of infants who were healthy at birth were invited. The latter comprised the control group. Of all mothers invited, 69% agreed. Mothers who participated were not different from those who did not on demographic variables, maternal felt security, or infant medical problem versus control group classifications, ps Ͼ .1. Participants who did not respond to felt security measures were excluded from analyses (n ϭ 4). The final sample at Time 4 included 135 mothers (M ϭ 27.9 years). Demographic information is in Table 1. Procedure and Measures Time 1. Nurses administered a psychosocial interview be- tween 24 and 26 weeks gestation. The interview included English or French measures assessing indicators of maternal felt security, pregnancy anxiety, depression, sociodemographic information, and medical history. Maternal felt security. Indicators were social support, self- esteem, attachment, and romantic relationship quality. A five-item Arizona Social Support Interview (Barrera, 1981) assessed emo- tional and instrumental social support received in the preceding 2 weeks. Three additional questions assessed (a) mothers’ expected support postpartum, (b) the number of people currently available to talk freely to/confide in, and (c), the number of people currently available to help in a time of need. Self-esteem was assessed with a four-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (␣ ϭ .73; Major, Rich- ards, Cooper, Cozzarelli, & Zubek, 1998). Attachment was as- sessed with a seven-item Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). As per Brennan et al. (1998), the three items loading most highly on the anxiety dimension were aggregated to form an anxiety score (␣ ϭ .62). The two highest- loading fear-of-closeness items and the two highest-loading fear- of-dependency items were aggregated to form an avoidance score (␣ ϭ .49). Romantic relationship quality was assessed with a three-item Quality of Marriage Index modified to include cohab- iting relationships (␣ ϭ .78; Norton, 1983). Felt security scores were calculated using the regression method in principal compo- nents analysis. Pregnancy anxiety and depressive symptoms. Pregnancy anx- iety was measured with a four-item Pregnancy-Specific Anxiety Scale (␣ ϭ .81; Mancuso, Schetter, Rini, Roesch, & Hobel, 2004). Depressive symptoms were measured with a 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (␣ ϭ .89; Radloff, 1977). Time 2. Shortly after birth, medical records were reviewed. Mothers were categorized as having a healthy newborn or a newborn with a medical problem defined as (a) early preterm birth (Յ33 completed weeks of gestation) following spontane- ous labor onset or prelabor membrane rupture, (b) neonatal hospitalization Ն7 days, or (c) a birth complication not detected by prenatal screening (e.g., congenital heart or urinary system anomalies, musculoskeletal anomalies, obstetrical trauma, se- vere fetal asphyxiation, or respiratory distress). Four medical doctors (MDs) with expertise in perinatal epidemiology re- viewed a list of all medical conditions in the sample. A condi- tion was defined as a medical problem at birth if three MDs thought it was a serious medical problem and a likely maternal stressor throughout the first postnatal year. Mothers of healthy infants born at Ն36 weeks gestation constituted the control group. There were no differences in felt security, age, preg- Table 1 Demographic Data in Percentages Control (n ϭ 93) Medical problem (n ϭ 42) Primiparity 72 69 Marital/cohabitation status Legally married 35.9 38.1 Cohabiting 60.9 59.5 Single 3.3 2.4 Place of birth Province of Quebec 74.2 83.3 Other province in Canada 3.2 2.4 Other country 22.6 14.3 First language French 74.2 88.1 English 8.6 4.8 Other 17.2 7.1 Maternal education Secondary or less 33 23.8 College or university 67 76.2 Family income, CAD/year Ͻ$15,000 11.8 9.8 $15,000 to Ͻ $30,000 17.6 14.6 $30,000 to Ͻ $50,000 23.5 9.8 $50,000 to Ͻ $80,000 29.4 46.3 Ն$80,000 17.6 19.5 Note. Demographic variables did not significantly differ between the control and stressful birth groups, ␹2 Ͻ 5.34, n.s. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 813HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING
  • 4. nancy anxiety, or depressive symptoms between medical- problem-at-birth and control groups, ts Ͻ 1.48 (see Table 2). Time 3. To measure mothers’ perceptions of infant health, mothers were asked, “How would you rate the overall health of your baby at birth?” and “How would you rate the overall health of your baby at the present time?” in a 6-month postpartum interview. Mothers responded on a 5-point scale (1 ϭ excellent, 5 ϭ bad; Idler & Benyamini, 1997). Time 4. Mothers who participated in the 12-month postpar- tum follow-up study provided data on infant fussing and crying using experience sampling on personal digital assistants. Moth- ers were paged at 10:00 a.m., 2:00 p.m., and 7:00 p.m. for 7 days and were asked to answer 11 questions about themselves and their most recent interaction with their infant. One question was, “In your last interaction your baby was ____.” Response options were “happy/playful,” “sleepy,” “fussy,” “very upset/ crying,” “calm/quiet,” and “active/energetic.” These options are similar to those used in previous diary research. For example, previous studies have asked mothers to report when, throughout the day, infants were asleep, awake content, awake fussy, awake crying, or awake sucking, at a time of their convenience, using a paper-and-pencil diary (Barr, Kramer, Boisjoly, McVey-White, & Pless, 1988; Wells, Hinds, & Davies, 1997). Response selections of “fussy” or “very upset/crying” were summed across days to create an indirect indicator of frequency of infant fussing and crying throughout the week. Reports of four or more fussy and very upset/crying occasions were given a score of 4 to create a fussing and crying scale of 0 to 4, to reduce the influence of outliers and positive skew. The mean fussing and crying score across groups was 1.42 (SD ϭ 1.23). Mothers’ perceptions of infant health were also assessed at 12 months, with the same procedure used at 6 months. Statistical Analysis Data analyses involved three steps. First, an independent samples t test compared mothers’ ratings of infant health at birth between the medical problem and control groups to cross- validate participant classification by MDs. Then, CFA was used to test whether prenatal maternal self- esteem, attachment, quality of romantic relationship, and social support are indicators of felt security. First, the Montreal Pre- maturity Study sample (N ϭ 5,092) was randomly divided into a derivation and a validation sample. Then, with the derivation sample (n ϭ 2,549), CFA was conducted using structural equa- tion modeling. This tested the goodness of fit of a one-factor model in which mothers’ prenatal attachment anxiety and avoidance, self-esteem, relationship quality, and the four social support variables loaded on a single factor. We included cor- related measurement errors among the four social support in- dicators to account for the possibility that these indicators would share unique variance not accounted for by the model. The goodness of fit of an alternative two-factor model in which the four social support indicators were loaded onto one factor, and attachment anxiety and avoidance, self esteem, and rela- tionship quality were loaded onto a second factor, was com- pared with the goodness of fit of the one-factor model with the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Kline, 1998), the compar- ative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approx- imation (RMSEA; Byrne, 2010; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Finally, hierarchical multiple regression was used to test the main study hypothesis that prenatal maternal felt security in- teracts with infant health at birth to predict infant fussing and crying. Bootstrapping was used to obtain a more robust estimate of standard error. Results Validating Infant Health at Birth An independent samples t test revealed that, consistent with MD’s classifications, mothers of infants with medical problems at birth rated their infants as less healthy at birth (M ϭ 2.45, SD ϭ 1.23) than mothers in the control condition (M ϭ 1.36, SD ϭ .70), t(127) ϭ Ϫ5.36, p Ͻ .01. However, mothers’ ratings of infant health at 6 months did not differ between the medical- problem-at-birth (M ϭ 1.33, SD ϭ .53) and control groups (M ϭ 1.3, SD ϭ .57), t Ͻ 1, because the medical-problem-at- birth group rated health at 6 months to be significantly better than at birth, t(41) ϭ 6.08, p Ͻ .01. Mothers’ ratings of infant health at 12 months also did not differ between the medical- problem-at-birth (M ϭ 1.57, SD ϭ .67) and control groups (M ϭ 1.56, SD ϭ .74), t Ͻ 1. Modeling Maternal Prenatal Felt Security Comparison of the fit indices from the CFA suggested that the one-factor model was acceptable (CFI ϭ .95, RMSEA ϭ .06, AIC ϭ 215.31) and yielded substantially better fit than the two-factor model (CFI ϭ .76, RMSEA ϭ .11, AIC ϭ 752.39). The one-factor solution was therefore retained. Standardized estimates for all eight indicators of the one-factor solution were statistically significant and varied from Ϯ.28 to .60. The vali- dation sample (n ϭ 2,543) cross-validated the structure of the one-factor model (CFI ϭ .96, RMSEA ϭ .055, AIC ϭ 182.54), confirming its validity (see Figure 1 for an illustration). Predicting Infant Fussing and Crying Thirteen mothers responded to the fussing and crying experience-sampling question less than seven times and were therefore excluded from this hierarchical multiple regression analysis. In the first step of the regression, maternal pregnancy anxiety was not a significant predictor of infant fussing and Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations According to Control or Medical-Problem-at-Birth Classification Measures Control (n ϭ 93) Medical problem (n ϭ 42) M SD M SD Prenatal felt security Ϫ0.01 0.83 0.21 0.73 Age 27.70 4.60 28.70 4.72 Pregnancy anxiety 0.83 0.78 1.05 0.92 Prenatal depressive symptoms 11.12 7.06 9.76 8.74 ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 814 SAWADA ET AL.
  • 5. crying, B ϭ .08, SE ϭ .14, ns. In the second step, maternal felt security and infant health at birth were also not significant predictors, Bs ϭ Ϫ.26 and .34, SEs ϭ .15 and .24, ps ϭ .088 and .17, respectively. In the third step, the product of the predictor variables was entered to test the interaction between maternal prenatal felt security and infant health at birth. This interaction term was a significant predictor of infant fussing and crying at 12 months, B ϭ Ϫ56, SE ϭ .27, p Ͻ .05.1 The results of these regression analyses are presented in Table 3. The simple slopes between maternal prenatal felt security and infant fussing and crying were analyzed according to Aiken and West (1991). Among mothers in the control group, prenatal felt security did not significantly predict infant fussing and crying, B ϭ Ϫ.13, SE ϭ .16, ns. However, among mothers in the medical- problem-at-birth group, prenatal felt security significantly pre- dicted infant fussing and crying, B ϭ Ϫ.69, SE ϭ .23, p Ͻ .01. Among infants born with a medical problem, those with mothers with high prenatal felt security fussed and cried less than those with mothers with low prenatal felt security. Further, among infants of mothers with low prenatal felt security, those with medical problems at birth fussed and cried more at 12 months than infants who were healthy at birth, B ϭ .89, SE ϭ .37, p Ͻ .05. Among infants of mothers with high prenatal felt security, health at birth did not significantly predict fussing and crying, B ϭ Ϫ.03, SE ϭ .28, ns. We reran the analyses, also controlling for maternal age, pre- natal depressive symptoms, education, and income as a stricter test of the hypothesis. None of the covariates were significantly related to the outcome variable, all ps Ͼ .32. The interaction remained statistically significant, B ϭ Ϫ.64, SE ϭ .32, p ϭ .05. The simple effects remained highly significant for infants in the medical-problem-at-birth group, B ϭ Ϫ.69, SE ϭ .30, p Ͻ .05, and for infants whose mothers were low in prenatal felt security, B ϭ 1.05, SE ϭ .39, p Ͻ .01. Discussion Infant health at birth did not predict infant fussing and crying at 12 months. However, as hypothesized, infant health at birth inter- acted with maternal prenatal felt security to predict frequency of infant fussing and crying at 12 months. Among infants who were healthy at birth, maternal prenatal felt security was not related to fussing and crying. However, among infants born with a medical problem, maternal prenatal felt security predicted fussing and crying such that those with mothers with low felt security fussed and cried more than those with mothers with high felt security. Infants born with medical problems fussed and cried more than did infants who were healthy at birth, only if their mothers had low prenatal felt security but not if their mothers had high prenatal felt security. The results of this study suggest that a mother’s bank of preexisting psychosocial resources buffers the effects of infant medical problems at birth on infant developmental outcomes at 12 months postpartum. We hypothesize that elevated fussing and crying among infants born with medical problems to low felt security mothers reflects increased negative reactivity or decreased regulatory capabilities. This may be directly related to infants’ early medical problems or to exposure to stressors such as hospitalization, medical proce- dures, and pain in the neonatal period. Increased infant negative affect or decreased regulatory abilities may result in more chal- lenging mother–infant interactions, and may persist into childhood to predict emotional and behavioral disorders. However, infant affect and behavior regulation capabilities may develop through interactions with caregivers who are able to regulate their own emotions effectively and provide sensitive care (Braungart-Rieker, 1 We also examined whether social support, attachment security, self- esteem, or relationship quality individually interacted with infant health at birth to predict infant fussing and crying in four separate regression analyses. None of these variables significantly interacted with health at birth to predict fussing and crying; all ps Ͼ .1 Figure 1. Prenatal felt security model. Table 3 Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Infant Fussing and Crying at 12 Months From Prenatal Maternal Felt Security and Infant Health at Birth Infant fussing and crying at 12 months (n ϭ 122) B SE Step 1 Pregnancy anxiety .08 .14 Step 2 Prenatal maternal felt security Ϫ.26 .15 Infant health at birth .34 .24 Step 3 Maternal felt security ϫ Infant health at birth Ϫ.56‫ء‬ .27 Simple slope – Control Ϫ.13 .16 Simple slope – Medical problem Ϫ.69‫ءء‬ .23 ‫ء‬ p Ͻ .05. ‫ءء‬ p Ͻ .001. ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 815HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING
  • 6. Garwood, Powers, & Notaro, 1998; Calkins, Smith, Gill, & John- son, 1998; Olafsen et al., 2008). We theorize that prenatal felt security may be associated with better maternal well-being and may thus provide a foundation for maternal, affect, cognition, and behavior that support the development of infant affect and behav- ior regulation among infants born with health problems. There are several mechanisms whereby maternal felt security measured in pregnancy might moderate infant fussing and crying at 12 months postpartum. First, mothers’ prenatal felt security is likely an indicator of mothers’ internal sense of security in close relationships at the onset of the infant medical stressor. Research on attachment, social support, and self-esteem suggests that moth- ers with higher prenatal felt security might perceive close others as more available and responsive in times of need, and that these secure mental representations of the self in relation to close others might attenuate mothers’ appraisals of threat and facilitate more adaptive coping in the context of a stressor (Alexander, Feeney, Hohaus, & Noller, 2001; Berant et al., 2001; Cutrona & Troutman, 1986; Florian, Mikulincer, & Bucholtz, 1995; Mikulincer & Flo- rian, 1998; Ognibene & Collins, 1998). Mothers with higher pre- natal felt security may thus experience the birth of an infant with medical problems as less stressful and may perceive themselves as more capable of coping with the challenges it presents. Maternal prenatal felt security may thus buffer against the possible adverse effects of this event on maternal well-being. Decreased maternal stress or increased self-efficacy may facilitate the provision of sensitive maternal care associated with the development of infant affect and behavior regulation capabilities among mothers with higher prenatal felt security. Second, prenatal felt security may be an indicator of the level of observable social support that mothers receive. In other words, mothers with higher prenatal felt security may have a larger bank of tangible and social resources ready at their disposal when the infant medical stressor occurs. Mothers with higher prenatal felt security may receive more support be- cause they are confident in drawing upon this reserve (Ognibene & Collins, 1998). Alternatively, mothers with higher prenatal felt security may receive more social support without necessarily seek- ing it out or even noticing it. This observable support may directly benefit the mother and the medically vulnerable infant. Conversely, for mothers with low prenatal felt security, the birth of an infant with a medical problem may lead to elevated stress and decreased parenting efficacy. The birth of a medically fragile infant is a stressor in and of itself. Further, mothers with low prenatal felt security may perceive greater threat and perceive themselves as having fewer psychological, social, and tangible resources to cope than mothers with higher prenatal felt security. In addition, they may concretely have fewer psychological, social, and tangible resources. Regardless, this perceived or actual lack of resources may compound the effects of an infant medical stressor on their well-being. They may experience more distress, which may negatively impact their ability to engage in interactions that support the development of infant affect and behavior regulation. This may result in increased infant fussing and crying at 12 months postpartum. Future research might examine how mothers’ felt security re- lates to mothers’ stress appraisal and coping, as well as observable receipt of social and tangible resources postpartum. This may elucidate the extent to which the effects of felt security are medi- ated by a perceived feeling of security in close relationships and cognitive factors versus observable social support received. We hypothesized that preexisting, prenatal psychosocial re- sources may play a particularly important protective role. Al- though psychosocial resources may be mobilized postpartum, ma- ternal distress may be immediate, whereas the mobilization of resources may take time. Maternal distress in the early postnatal period may set in motion a pattern of less adaptive maternal cognitions and behavior, and mother–infant interactions that may persist beyond the duration of the stressful experience and con- tinue, despite the later availability of resources mobilized after birth (Minde, Whitelaw, Brown, & Fitzhardinge, 1983). However, the current study cannot directly test the relative importance of preexisting versus mobilized psychosocial resources, because post- partum assessments of felt security indicators are likely biased by mothers’ experience of the newborn’s medical stressor. Future research should examine the issue of the relative importance of preexisting versus mobilized resources. Although we hypothesized that maternal reports of infant fuss- ing and crying reflect actual levels of fussing and crying, an alternate possibility is that low felt security mothers of infants with medical problems experience elevated stress, and thus perceive their infants as having more negative affect regardless of actual levels of fussiness or negative affect. However, this seems un- likely, as no association between maternal prenatal felt security and maternal report of fussing and crying was found among infants who were healthy at birth. Further, the regression analysis con- trolled for mothers’ pregnancy anxiety. Because we do not have a third-party report of our dependent measure, we cannot be sure of whether maternal reports of infant fussing and crying in this study reflect actual levels of fussing and crying or maternal perceptions of negative infant affect. Regardless, the findings reported here have important implications for child outcomes. Maternal percep- tions of difficult infant temperament are related to maternal stress, distress, and feelings of incompetence (Cutrona & Troutman, 1986; Östberg & Hagekull, 2000). Maternal perception of in- creased infant fussing and crying may thus predict decreased maternal well-being and ability to provide optimal care. However, future research might further examine this issue by obtaining objective reports of infant fussing and crying with sound-recording equipment, for example. Further, although the large size of the initial cohort allowed cross-validation of the latent variable, the wide range of measures needed in the multidisciplinary Montreal Prematurity Study re- quired abbreviated versions of several measures. This resulted in low to moderate reliability coefficients for some measures, such as the attachment scales. Despite these limitations, these findings expand on previous research in several ways and the study has a number of strengths. First, this study makes a unique contribution to the literature on infant medical vulnerability and maternal stress and psychosocial resources by demonstrating that prenatal maternal psychosocial resources and infant medical problems at birth interact to predict an infant developmental outcome at 12 months postpartum. Sec- ond, the longitudinal design ensured that maternal measures of psychosocial resources preceded, and were therefore not influ- enced by, infant medical problems. Third, this study employed a more comprehensive and parsimo- nious measure of maternal psychosocial resources than is typically ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 816 SAWADA ET AL.
  • 7. used in related research. Felt security has been conceptualized in the adult attachment literature as encompassing attachment secu- rity, perceived regard from close others, romantic relationship quality, and the expectation that others are available and respon- sive in times of need (Florian et al., 1995; Murray et al., 2006; Murray et al., 2000; Murray et al., 2001). No validated measure of felt security that reflects this conceptualization exists. We there- fore modeled felt security using CFA, with attachment, self- esteem, relationship quality, and social support as indicators. Felt security scores were then computed and used to test the primary hypothesis that this factor interacts with infant health problems at birth to predict infant fussing and crying. The results of this study suggest that attachment security, self-esteem, relationship quality, and social support are indeed indicators of felt security, thus contributing to an improved empirical understanding of this con- struct. Further, although maternal prenatal felt security interacted with infant health at birth to predict infant fussing and crying, none of the individual indicators did. This suggests that mothers’ ag- gregate resources matter. However, it is possible that with more reliable measures of any of the felt security indicators, one may have significantly interacted with health at birth. It is also possible that some measures, like social support, could have direct or indirect moderating effects by promoting attachment security. Fu- ture research should examine these issues. In the meantime, the finding that maternal prenatal felt security interacts with a stressor to predict an infant outcome lays the groundwork for future re- search on felt security, which may aim to develop a concise measure or examine the buffering effects of felt security in other contexts. Fourth, the study used experience sampling, thus minimizing memory bias associated with post hoc questionnaires and increas- ing ecological validity. Few studies examining interactions be- tween infant medical problems and maternal psychosocial re- sources have used this methodology. This study makes a significant contribution by demonstrating that felt security and neonatal medical problems predict infant behavior with a measure of mothers’ real-time, daily life experience. Lastly, mothers’ ratings of infant health at 6 and 12 months did not differ between infants with medical problems and infants who were healthy at birth. This suggests that infants who are born with medical problems and have mothers with low prenatal felt security are not fussier simply because of persistently poorer (perceived) health. Rather, these results suggest that medical problems at birth combined with lower maternal prenatal felt security contribute to patterns of mother–infant interactions that are associated with increased maternal report of infant fussing and crying that persist 12 months postpartum, despite perceived improvements in infant health. Future studies should investigate the relationship between neonatal medical problems, maternal psychosocial resources, and the child’s emotional and behavioral regulation beyond infancy. The results of this study suggest the relevance and time course of interventions that might increase felt security among mothers of infants with medical problems or mothers at risk of having an infant with medical problems. Resilience-promoting interventions that increase felt security during pregnancy may be useful for promoting more favorable maternal and infant outcomes after birth. References Adamakos, H., Ryan, K., Ullman, D. G., Pascoe, J., Diaz, R., & Chessare, J. (1986). Maternal social support as a predictor of mother-child stress and stimulation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 10, 463–470. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0145-2134(86)90050-5 Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Alexander, R., Feeney, J., Hohaus, L., & Noller, P. (2001). Attachment style and coping resources as predictors of coping strategies in the transition to parenthood. Personal Relationships, 8, 137–152. http://dx .doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2001.tb00032.x Anand, K. J. S., & Scalzo, F. M. (2000). Can adverse neonatal experiences alter brain development and subsequent behavior? Biology of the Neo- nate, 77, 69–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000014197 Barr, R. G., Kramer, M. S., Boisjoly, C., McVey-White, L., & Pless, I. B. (1988). Parental diary of infant cry and fuss behavior. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 63, 380–387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.63.4 .380 Barrera, M. (1981). Social support in the adjustment of pregnant adoles- cents: Assessment issues. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.), Social networks and social support (pp. 69–96). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Bee, H. L., Hammond, M. A., Eyres, S. J., Barnard, K. E., & Snyder, C. (1986). The impact of parental life change on the early development of children. Research in Nursing & Health, 9, 65–74. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/nur.4770090111 Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171–1190. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.2307/1127506 Berant, E., Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2001). The association of mothers’ attachment style and their psychological reactions to the diag- nosis of infant’s congenital heart disease. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 208–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.20.2.208 .22264 Bhutta, A. T., Cleves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., & Anand, K. J. S. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 728–737. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.288 .6.728 Blumberg, N. L. (1980). Effects of neonatal risk, maternal attitude, and cognitive style on early postpartum adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 139–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.89.2 .139 Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Notaro, P. C. (1998). Infant affect and affect regulation during the still-face paradigm with mothers and fathers: The role of infant characteristics and parental sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1428–1437. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1428 Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure- ment of adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relation- ships (pp. 46–76). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Browne, J. V., & Talmi, A. (2005). Family-based intervention to enhance infant–parent relationships in the neonatal intensive care unit. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 667–677. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/ jsi053 Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7, 350–369. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00072 Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 817HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING
  • 8. age three to age fifteen. Child Development, 66, 55–68. http://dx.doi .org/10.2307/1131190 Cohen, S., & Wills, T. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 Collins, N. L., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Lobel, M., & Scrimshaw, S. C. (1993). Social support in pregnancy: Psychosocial correlates of birth outcomes and postpartum depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 65, 1243–1258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1243 Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment shape perceptions of social support: Evidence from experimental and observational studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 363–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.363 Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M., Ragozin, A., Robinson, N., & Basham, R. (1983). Effects of stress and social support on mothers and premature and full-term infants. Child Development, 54, 209–217. http://dx.doi .org/10.2307/1129878 Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M., & Slough, N. (1986). Early stress and social support influences on mothers’ and high-risk infants’ functioning in late infancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 7, 19–33. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/1097-0355(198621)7:1Ͻ19::AID-IMHJ2280070104Ͼ3.0.CO;2-1 Cutrona, C. E., & Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social support, infant temper- ament, and parenting self-efficacy: A mediational model of postpartum depression. Child Development, 57, 1507–1518. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.2307/1130428 Darke, P. R., & Goldberg, S. (1994). Father-infant interaction and parent stress with healthy and medically compromised infants. Infant Be- havior & Development, 17, 3–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0163- 6383(94)90017-5 DeLongis, L., & Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The impact of daily stress on health and mood: Psychological and social resources as medi- ators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 486–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.486 Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223–231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0012-1649.35.1.223 Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Bucholtz, I. (1995). Effects of adult attach- ment style on the perception and search for social support. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 129, 665–676. http://dx .doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1995.9914937 Gallo, L. C., & Matthews, K. (2003). Understanding the association be- tween socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 10–51. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.10 Goldstein, L. H., Diener, M. L., & Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1996). Maternal characteristics and social support across the transition to motherhood: Associations with maternal behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 60–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.1.60 Hack, M., Klein, N. K., & Taylor, H. G. (1995). Long-term developmental outcomes of low birth weight infants. Future of Children, 5, 176–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1602514 Hall, L. A., Kotch, J. B., Browne, D., & Rayens, M. K. (1996). Self-esteem as a mediator of the effects of stressors and social resources on depres- sive symptoms in postpartum mothers. Nursing Research, 45, 231–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006199-199607000-00007 Hane, A. A., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving influence human infants’ stress reactivity. Psychological Science, 17, 550–556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006 .01742.x Hopkins, J., Campbell, S. B., & Marcus, M. (1987). Role of infant-related stressors in postpartum depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 237–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.96.3.237 Idler, E. L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 21–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2955359 Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation mod- eling. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kramer, M. S., Lydon, J. E., Séguin, L., Goulet, L., Kahn, S. R., McNa- mara, H., . . . Platt, R. W. (2009). Stress pathways to spontaneous preterm birth: The role of stressors, psychological distress, and stress hormones. American Journal of Epidemiology, 169, 1319–1326. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwp061 Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Sci- ence, 15, 41–56. Major, B., Richards, C., Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C., & Zubek, J. (1998). Personal resilience, cognitive appraisals, and coping: An integrative model of adjustment to abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 735–752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.735 Mancuso, R. A., Schetter, C. D., Rini, C. M., Roesch, S. C., & Hobel, C. J. (2004). Maternal prenatal anxiety and corticotropin-releasing hormone associated with timing of delivery. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 762– 769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000138284.70670.d5 McGrath, J. M., Records, K., & Rice, M. (2008). Maternal depression and infant temperament characteristics. Infant Behavior & Development, 31, 71–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.07.001 McGrath, M., Boukydis, C. F. Z., & Lester, B. M. (1993). Determinants of maternal self-esteem in the neonatal period. Infant Mental Health Jour- nal, 13, 35–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355 Mebert, C. J. (1991). Dimensions of subjectivity in parents’ ratings of infant temperament. Child Development, 62, 352–361. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.2307/1131008 Miceli, P. J., Goeke-Morey, M., Whitman, T., Kolberg, K., Miller-Loncar, C., & White, R. (2000). Brief report: Birth status, medical complications, and social environment: Individual differences in development of pre- term, very low birth weight infants. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 25, 353–358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/25.5.353 Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult attachment styles and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful events. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 143–165). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Minde, K., Whitelaw, A., Brown, J., & Fitzhardinge, P. (1983). Effect of neonatal complications in premature infants on early parent-infant in- teractions. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 25, 763–777. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1983.tb13845.x Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., & Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother– child interactions. Early Human Development, 79, 145–158. http://dx .doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002 Murray, S. L., Holmes, J., & Collins, N. (2006). Optimizing assurance: The risk regulation system in relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 641–666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.641 Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2000). Self-esteem and the quest for felt security: How perceived regard regulates attachment pro- cesses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 478–498. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.478 Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., Griffin, D. W., Bellavia, G., & Rose, P. (2001). The mismeasure of love: How self-doubt contaminates relation- ship beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 423–436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167201274004 Natsuaki, M. N., Ge, X., Leve, L. D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., Conger, R. D., . . . Reiss, D. (2010). Genetic liability, environment, and the development of fussiness in toddlers: The roles of maternal depres- sion and parental responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1147– 1158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019659 ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 818 SAWADA ET AL.
  • 9. Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the depen- dent variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141–151. http:// dx.doi.org/10.2307/351302 Ognibene, T. C., & Collins, N. L. (1998). Adult attachment styles, per- ceived social support and coping strategies. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 323–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0265407598153002 Olafsen, K. S., Kaaresen, P., Handegård, B., Ulvund, S., Dahl, L., & Rønning, J. (2008). Maternal ratings of infant regulatory competence from 6 to 12 months: Influence of perceived stress, birth-weight, and intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Infant Behavior & Devel- opment, 31, 408–421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.005 Östberg, M., & Hagekull, B. (2000). A structural modeling approach to the understanding of parenting stress. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 615–625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP2904_13 Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306 Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. J. Mashek and A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rende, R. D. (1993). Longitudinal relations between temperament traits and behavioral syndromes in middle childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 287–290. http://dx.doi .org/10.1097/00004583-199303000-00008 Singer, L., Davillier, M., Bruening, P., Hawkins, S., & Yamashita, T. (1996). Social support, psychological distress, and parenting strains in mothers of very low birthweight infants. Family Relations: An Interdis- ciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 45, 343–350. http://dx.doi .org/10.2307/585507 Singer, L. T., Salvator, A., Guo, S., Collin, M., Lilien, L., & Baley, J. (1999). Maternal psychological distress and parenting stress after the birth of a very low-birth-weight infant. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 799–805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.281 .9.799 Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development, 48, 1184–1199. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.2307/1128475 Stapleton, L. R. T., Schetter, C. D., Westling, E., Rini, C., Glynn, L. M., Hobel, C. J., & Sandman, C. A. (2012). Perceived partner support in pregnancy predicts lower maternal and infant distress. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 453–463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028332 Stewart, A. L., Rifkin, L., Amess, P. N., Kirkbride, V., Townsend, J. P., Miller, D. H., . . . Murray, R. M. (1999). Brain structure and neuro- cognitive and behavioral function in adolescents who were born very preterm. The Lancet, 353, 1653–1657. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140- 6736(98)07130-X Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education. van den Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensi- tive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457–1477. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131511 Wells, J. C. K., Hinds, A., & Davies, P. S. W. (1997). Free-living energy expenditure and behavior in late infancy. Archives of Disease in Child- hood, 76, 490–494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.76.6.490 Received May 8, 2013 Revision received June 23, 2014 Accepted July 19, 2014 Ⅲ ThisdocumentiscopyrightedbytheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationoroneofitsalliedpublishers. Thisarticleisintendedsolelyforthepersonaluseoftheindividualuserandisnottobedisseminatedbroadly. 819HEALTH AT BIRTH, FELT SECURITY, FUSSING AND CRYING