SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 6
Download to read offline
The Classroom as Clinic: Applications for a Method of Teaching Clinical Reasoning 
Maureen E. Neistadt 
Key Words: education. evaluation studies 
This study examined the efficacy ofone methodfor teaching diagnostic reasoning to occupational therapy students. During a clinical reasoning seminar in their first academic year, 80 entry-level occupational therapy master's degree students in three successive classes were given three different levels ofexposure to classroom-as-clinic or in-class evaluations ofadults with physical or psychosocial disabilities. During the following summer, most students completed their first Level II fieldwork experience. Students' grades for a second-year classroom-as-clinic experience with adults with physial disabilities were then compared across groups to determine the relative effect ofthe different seminarformats andfieldwork experiences. Students who had experienced in-class evaluations during their first academic year wrote Significantly more accurate second-year evaluations than those who had not. Students who had completed psychosocial Level II fieldwork experiences were as accurate on their evaluations as students who had had physical dYsfunction fieldwork experiences. The results suggest that in-class evaluations improve students' diagnostic reasoning skills. 
Maureen E. Neistadt, SeD. OTM, is Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy Department, University of New Hampshire, Hewitt Hall, Durham, New Hampshire 03824-3563. At the time of this study she was Assistant Professor, Tufts University-Boston School of Occupational Therapy, Medford, Massachusetts. 
This ai-tide was accepted for publica/ion April 10, 1992 
Skill in clinical reasoning is essential for effective occupational therapy practice (Fleming, 1991; Parham, 1987; Rogers, 1983; Slater & Cohn, 1991). "Clinical reasoning is a dynamic process of inquiry in action that takes place in the context of occupational therapy evaluation and treatment" (Tufts University-Boston School of Occupational Therapy [BSOT), 1990, p. 3). Schwartz (1991) recently highlighted the need for occupational therapy education programs to develop and implement teaching methodologies that encourage the development of students' clinical reasoning abilities. 
In 1987, I described a classroom-as-clinic method designed to teach occupational therapy students the clinical reasoning process associated with evaluation and treatment planning. A previous study demonstrated that this method improved occupational therapy students' abilities to accurately analyze preassessment data and formulate appropriate treatment plans (Neistadt, 1987). Students in that study engaged in classroom-based evaluations of adults with physical disabilities at the end of an occupational therapy curriculum, after they had taken all of their nonelective course work and before they had begun their Level II fieldwork experiences. One question raised by that study was whether in-class evaluations of adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities would effectively teach clinical reasoning earlier in an occupational therapy curriculum. The present study addressed this question. 
Literature Review 
Rogers and Masagatani (1982) and Rogers (1983) originally described therapists' thought processes during initial evaluations as involVing a sequence of deduction, induction, dialectical reasoning, and ethical reasoning. Therapists, said these authors, begin their evaluations with a review of clients' charts or other preassessment information that might be available or both. From this review, therapists form hypotheses about possible problems of clients through a process of deduction. Therapists evaluate their clients, then modify their preassessment hypotheses by considering the specific details of clients' cases (induction) and deciding between different interpretations for clients' behaviors (dialectical reasoning). Therapists then work with their clients to establish treatment priorities consistent with the clients' value systems (ethical reasoning). 
More recently, Rogers and Holm (1991) referred to the thought processes that occupational therapists use during initial evaluation as diagnostic reasoning. Diagnostic reasoning "is the sequence of decisions that leads to occupational therapy diagnosis" and "is one component of the clinical reasoning involved in the occupational therapy process" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). The occupational therapy diagnosis "describes the actual or potential effects of disease, trauma, developmental disor- 
September 1992, Volume 46, Number 9 814
ders, age-associated changes, environmental deprivation, and mher etiologic agents on occupational sWtus" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). This occupational therapy diagnosis becomes the foundation for collaborative treatment planning with the cliene 
According [0 Rogers and Holm (1991), the diagnostic reasoning process involves both problem sensing and problem definition. "A therapist senses a problem by framing it, that is, by deciding what will be included in the picture. The picture inside that frame is the clinical image" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). This formation of clinical images begins during the chart review stage of assessment and is influenced by the reason for occupational therapy referral; the practice setting; the experience and frames of reference of the therapist; and the client's condition, age, and sex. The severity of the client'S condition will also influence the clinical image (Rogers & Holm, 1991). This clinical image would include mental hypotheses about the client's pmential problems -hypotheses formed through deductive reasoning about the information available from the chart review and other preassessment information (Rogers, 1983; Rogers & Masagatani, 1982). 
Problem definition is a process in which the therapist concisely and precisely describes and names the client's problems. "A5 a result of this descriptive process, the therapist's clinical image of a client becomes more like the actual client encountered in the clinic" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). Therapists engage in the problemdefinition process during the initial evaluation of a c1iene Rogers and Holm presented an information-[Jrocessing perspective on problem definition that sees the therapist as a data processor and the client and the client's Jiving situation as the data field. The therapist "collects, organizes, analyzes, and synthesizes data about a client's occupational status" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1048). A5 a data processor, the therapist uses "four basic processes: cue acqUisition, hypmhesis generation, cue interpretation, and hypothesis evaluation" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, 
r. 1048). Cues are data to which therapists attend. Therapists interpret the cues gathered during initial evaluation [0 test their [Jreassessment hypotheses and [0 form and test new hypotheses. They use dialectic [Jrocess to weigh the relative merits of alternative hypotheses and ethical reasoning to consider the influence of clients' values and mmivations on rroblem definitions. 
A5 Rogers and Holm (1991) have suggested, diagnostic reasoning is only one component of occurational therarists' clinical reasoning process. Fleming (1991) has suggested that occupational therapists simultaneously use three different ways of thinking: procedural, interactive, and conditional. Therapists use procedural reasoning to focus on diagnosis and disability by following a logical medical decision-making process of problem identification, goal setting, and treatment planning that uses their medical, technical, and occupational knowledge. 
The Ilmerican journal or Occupational Therapy 
Fleming's procedural reasoning corresponds to Rogers and Holm's diagnostic reasoning. Therapists use interactive reasoning during meetings with clients to try to understand how the client makes sense of the disability or disease and how that disability or disease interferes with the roles and activities that give that person's life meaning. Therapists use conditional reasoning to think about the client's future, "given the constraints of the physical condition within the client's personal and social context" (Fleming, 1991, p. 1013). 
During chart review, therapists use primarily procedural (diagnostic) reasoning. Experienced clinicians might also use conditional reasoning at this stage to begin forming an image of the client's future, given the diagnosis, prognosis, and social and vocational history. During a client evaluation, therapists combine procedural (diagnostic), interactive, and conditional reasoning to observe, elicit, and interpret cues so they can develop a treatment plan that is meaningful [0 the client. 
Cohn (1991) stated that occupational therapy clinicians and clinical educators frequentlycompJain "that academic programs do not adequately prepare students for the uncertainties inherent in the challenges of practice" 
(p. 969). Perhaps these complaints arise because the traditional teaching and testing methodologies of higher education cannot foster the complex array of reasoning skills that occupational therapists must use in practice. More experiential teaching modes that use testing methods linked to clinical practice might teach clinical reasoning better (Schwartz, 1991). The primary purpose of the present experimental study was to see whether a modified classroom-as-clinic method in the first year of an entry-level master's degree program would improve the clinical reasoning skills of students by the second year of their rrogram, as measured by performance in a classroom- as-cJinic experience at the beginning of the second academic year. A secondary purpose was to assess the effects of Level II fieldwork experiences on these students' second-year classroom-as-c1inic performances. 
Method 
Design 
A post hoc experimental design was used to compare the second year classroom-as-clinic performances of three independent groups of students. A5 a result of ongOing curriculum development, three successive groups of students were given three different levels of exposure to classroom-as-clinic or in-class evaluations of adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities during a clinical reasoning seminar in the second semeSter of their first academic year. 
Subjects 
The subjects in this study were 80 entry-level master's degree students at Tufts University-Boston School of Oc815
cupational Therapy, Medford, Massachusetts Subjects were members of three successive groups of students attending the university between the years 1989 and 1992 (for Group 1, n = 21; Group 2, n = 31; Group 3, Jl = 28). Their ages ranged from 22 years to 40 years. Five subjects were men and 75 were women. The average preadmission grade point average was 3.1 for aU three groups of students. 
All subjects took their basic science, pathology, and introductory occupational therapy course work in their first academic year. As part of the first academic year's work, all subjects participated in clinical reasoning seminars on observation skills and interactive reasoning during their first and second semesters, respectively. Subjects also took either a psychosocial or physical dysfunction course in the second semester of their first year, to prepare them for a first summer Level II fieldwork corresponding to the dysfunction course they had taken. Some subjects elected not to do a Level II fieldwork that first summer for personal or financial reasons. In the first semester of the second year, all subjects participated in an advanced occupational therapy course that used the c1assroom-as-c1inic teaching method. The second-year course work included clinical reasoning seminars on procedural and conditional reasoning in the first and second semesters, respectively. Additional course work in pediatrics and in the major dysfunction course not taken in the first year was also offered Most subjects completed their second Level II fieldwork in the summer after the second academic year, with the remaining subjects completing their first and second Level II fieldwork at this time. 
For Group 1, the interactive reasoning seminar did not include contact with persons with physical or psychosocial disabilities. The goal in this first seminar was to improve subjects' self-awareness so that they would he able to interact as therapeutic agents with future clients. Lectures and small group exercises about interviewing, empathy, and nonverhal communication were used. Subjects expressed dissatisfaction with the lack of client contact in this seminar. Consequently, the interactive seminar for Group 2 included in-class student group interviews with persons with rhysical or psychosocial disabilities. Faculty thought that this interview experience helped students develop their interactive reasoning skills, hut that it did not force students to use interactive reasoning in conjunction with procedural and conditional reasoning, as would be required in clinical evaluations and treatment. Therefore, the interactive seminar for Group 3 included modified c1assroom-as-clinic experiences. 
Course outlines, testing methods, ancl Level I fieldwork for all courses hut the interactive seminar remained constant during the study period. Group 3 had a different instructor than Groups 1 and 2 for two psychosocial courses and one pathology course. Otherwise, course instructors remained constant throughout the study period. 
Procedure 
The format of the classroom-as-clinic experience was hased on Rogers' model ofclinical reasoning during initial evaluation (Rogers, 1983) ami has already heen described 
in detail (Neistadt, 1987). During these experiences, the suhjects were exrected to write a problem-goal-plan list after reviewing limited preassessment information (i.e., diagnosis and social situation) and to revise that list after interviewing a guest participant with a physical or psychosocial disability. In the original c1assroom-as-c1inic method which was used in the advanced occupational therapy co~rse at the beginning of the subjects' second year, subjects did nor receive any information on the diagnosis of the guest participant before the day of the in-class evaluation and were expected to write their first prohlemgoal- plan list in the 30 to 40 min immediately preceding their meeting with the guest participant. For this first second-year evaluation, the guest participants all had conditions diagnosed as central nervous system dysfunction In the modified in-class evaluation used in the firstveal' interactive reasoning seminar for subjects in Group3, subjects received preassessment information ahout the guest participants 1 week in advance and were given 1 week to work on their initial problem-goal-plan lists at home, using their books and class notes as references. For all in-class evaluations, suhjects met in small groups with one guest participant for 90 min and then wrote revised prohlem-goal-plan lists which they then handed in at the end of class. 
The grades on the problem-goal-plan lists represented the percentage of correct problems that the suhjects recorded from a list of expected problems for a given diagnosis or guest participant. The preassessment or chart review correct problems lists were derived from the Uniform Occupational Therapy Evaluation Checklist (American Occupational Therapy Association, 1981). Prohlem areas specific to particular diagnoses were selected from this list according to the occupational therapy literature and the instructor's clinical experience. The postassessment or evaluation correct problems lists were also derived from the uniform checklist and were based on the clinical experience ofthe instructor and the clinical observations of the faculty coJeaders in the guest participants' groups. The grading rrocedure, course instructor, and guest participants were the same for all three subject groups. 
Results 
Subjects' grades on the chart review and evaluation prohlem- goal-rJan lists for the first classroom-as-c1inic experience in the second year were anal)'7.ed with two-way analvses ofvariance and Tukey pairwise comparisons (Cody & Smith, 1987). Subject group during the first year and Level II fieldwork during the first summer were the indeSeplell1! 
Jel' 1992. Volume 40. Numher 9 816
pendent variables, and grades on the second-year problem- goal-pian lists were the dependent variables in these analyses. A significance level of 05 was used. 
Chart Review lists 
For the chan review lists, there was no significant effect for either subject group [F (2, 71) = 2.47, P = .0919] or type of Level II fieldwork [F (2, 71) = 0.60, p = .5495]. Tukey pairwise comparisons showed no significant differences in chart review grades among any of the three subject groups (see Tables 1 and 2). 
There was a significant group by Level 11 fieldwork interaction [F (2, 71) = 3.05, P = .0224] in the chart review analysis ofv<lri<lnce. When the sample was soned by groups, one-way analyses of variance with Level 11 fieldwork <lS the independent variable <lnd chan review grades as the dependent variable showed a significant Level II fieldwork effect only for Group 1 [F (2, 18) = 436, P = .0286J. Tukey pairwise comparisons for this group showed significant differences in ch<lrt review gr<ldes between subjects who had had physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those who had had psychosocial Level 11 fieldwork. The former scored an average of 90.7%: the latter, an average of 79.7%. 
Evaluation Lisls 
For the evaluation lists, there were significant effects for both group [F(2, 71) = 11.74, P = .0001] and Level 11 fieldwork IF(2, 71) = 4.27, P = .0177]. There W<lS no significant group by Level II fieldwork interaction [F(2, 71) = 1.12, /) = .35581. Tukey pairwise comparisons showed significant differences in evaluation list grades among all three groups (see Table 1) and between those suhjects who had had physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those vho had not done any Level II fieldwork in the preceding summer (see Table 2). Paired I-test comparisons showed that only Group :3, with the in-class evaluation expcrience, improved significantly from the chan review [0 thc evaluation list grades (see Table 2). 
Table 1 
Tukey Pairwise Comparison of Average Grades for 
Subject Groups 
Chan R<.:view Lists Evaluation Lists 
Group .VI (%) SO M(%) SO 
1 (/1 = 21) 871 94 1345 9,1 2 (/1 = 31) 904 99 909 90 3 (11 = 28) 928 65 973 49 
Nole For Group 1, the ill[cractivc r<.:asoning seminar did not lIlciude contact ,,'iLh pnsons Ivith phvsical or psychosocial cJisabiliti<.:s, Fo,' Group 2. the ill[eractiv<.: seminar lI1c1ud<.:d in-class stud<.:nt group interviews wil h p<.:rsons wirh physiGI or psvchosocial disabilities. For Group 3, th<.: interactive seminar included modifi<.:d classroom-as-c1inic <.:x[)<.:riCIlCl.:' 
S. 
lhe Alllericull .IoUrJlCI1 or OCClljJUliollal FherajJl' 
Table 2 Tukey Pairwise Comparison of Average Grades for 
Level" Fieldwork Experiences 
Chan R<:vi<.:w Lists Evaluation Lists 
Lcv<.:1 II Fieldwork !vI (%) SD !vI (%) SO 
Physical dysfunction (n = 32) 90.9 8.2 94.3 8.13 Psy<.:hosocial dysfunction (11 = 34) 90.5 9.2 90.3 9.1 None (n = 14) 89.1 10.1 877 93 
Note. Preadmission grade point average was comparable for all Leveill fi<.:ldwork groups. 
Discussion 
Results suggest that the use of in-class evaluations of adults with physical or psychosocial dysfunction during the first year of an entry-level master's program help~ students to develop their clinical reasoning skills. The general consistency of instructors, content, and teaching and testing methods for Other courses in the curriculum during the study period strongly suggests that the results are related to the in-class evaluation variable. Subjects who had experienced in-class evaluations during their fir~t academic year were significantly more accurate than those who had nor experienced in-class evaluations in writing evaluation problem-goal-plan lists for an in-class evaluation experience in the second academic year. Results for both the chan review and evaluation lists are discussed below. 
Cbart Reuiew Lists 
Subjeci groups. The lack of significant differences bctween thc three different subject groups on the second- year chart review li~ts suggest~ that ~kiJl with this pan of the evaluation process i~ not strongly affected by interaction with adults with physical or p~ychosocial disabilitie~ in the fil·~t academic year. AJthough not stati~ticallv significant, a trend emerged showing that subjects wl[h interview experience in the first year did better than those with no interview experience and that those with a modified classroom-as-cJinic experience did better than tho~e with only interview experience (see Table 1). 
In my pl-eviou~ cJassroom-a~-c1inic~tudy, I reponed that students had repeatedly said that "meeting adults wlth disabilities hel ps them to make sense ofand 'picture' the theoretic<l] information they have learned in the c1a~sroom" (Neisradt, 1987, p. 634) The interview experiences may have given subJeers ~ome beginning clinical images (Rogers & Holm, 1991) of people with rarticular disabilitie~, and r(;trieving these images may have helped them write more accurate chart review lists in the second academic vear. Subjects who were forced to contrast their preassessment Jnd postasse~sment image~ in the modified classroom-as-c1inic experience may have had more vivid and accurate clinical image~ to draw on in the second 'ear of their program. 
817
Fieldwork experiences. The lack of significant differences among the three different Level II fieldwork experiences (physical dysfunction, psychosocial dysfunction, none) on the second-year chart review lists suggests that skill with this part of the evaluation process is also n.ot 
strongly affected by interaction with adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities during fieldwork. However, there was a trend for subjects with psychosocial Level II fieldwork to do better than those without Level II fieldwork experience and for those with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork to do better than those with psychosocial experience (see Table 2). 
The preassessment list is primarily an exercise in problem sensing (Rogers & Holm, 1991). Subjects Without Level 11 fieldwork would have missed intensive practice with the problem-sensing process. The psychosocial Level II fieldwork would have given subjects practice with problem sensing for adults with psychosocial dysfunctions, whereas the physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork would have provided practice with problem sensing for adults with physical disabilities. Because the guest participants in the second-year classroom-as-c1inic examined here all had physical disabilities, one would expect the physical disability Level II fieldwork problem-sensing experience to be more applicable; however, the differences between the performances of all subjects who had done psychosocial and physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork were not significant. The significant difference in chart review accuracy between subjects with different Level II fieldwork experiences within Group 1 is most likely related to a few weak subjects in the psychosocial group whose grades lowered the entire psychosocial average. Rogers and Holm have said that "diagnostic reasoning is generic to all practice areas" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1047). These chart review results suggest that the problem-sensing part of diagnostic reasoning, in partinllar, can be generalized across treatment settings. 
Evaluation Lists 
Subject groups. The significant differences among the three subject groups on the second-year evaluation lists suggests that students' skill with this pan of the evaluation process is strongly affected by interaction with adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities in the first academic year. Subjects with interview experience in the first year did significantly better than those without interview experience, and those with a modified classroom-as-c1inic experience did significantly better than those with either no interview or only interview experience (see Table 1). 
This result suggests that the c1assroom-as-clinic experience provided subjects with practice in using the combination of interactive, procedural, and conditional reasoning that Fleming (1991) has said is essential to clinical evaluation. Practice in constantly switching from one type of reasoning to the other during a time- pressured meeting with an adult with a disability may heir students to hone their problem-definition skills (Rogers & Holm, 1991). That is, the first year in-class evaluations seemed to make subjects more proficient at 
obserVing, eliciting, and interpreting cues during an initial interview. Consequently, in their second year, subjects could describe guest participant problems more accurately than could subjects who had not experienced the classroom-as-c1inic method. The finding that only the subjects who had the classroom-as-clinic experience in their first year improved significantly from the chart review to evaluation lists in their second year further supports the notion that this teaching method improves students' reasoning abilities during the client evaluation process. 
Fieldwork experiences. The lack ofsignificant differences between the subjects with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and psychosocial Level II fieldwork experiences on the second-year evaluation lists suggests that Level II fieldwork in either practice setting provides students with generalizable experience in combining procedural, interactive, and conditional reasoning to yield accurate client-specific problem definitions. The trend for subjects with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork to do better than those with psychosocial Level II fieldwork (see Table 2) may relate, again, to practice with the population seen in the second-year c1assroom-as-clinic experience. Rogers & Holm (1991) have suggested that recency, intensity, and frequency of practice with particular populations will influence the accuracy of problem definition, even though the general process of diagnostic reasoning can be applied across practice settings. 
The significant difference between subjects with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those without Level II fieldwork on the second-year evaluation lists probably also relates to the Level II fieldwork practice the former had with the population seen in second-year c1assroom-as-clinic experience. The lack ofsignificant differences in evaluation list scores between subjects with psychosocial dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those without Level II fieldwork may relate to the lack ofLeve! II fieldwork practice for either group in evaluating a physical dysfunction population, but the trend was for the subjects with psychosocial Level II fieldwork to score better than those without Level II fieldwork. This trend probably reflects the former's Level II fieldwork practice with diagnostic and interactive reasoning. 
Conclusion 
The results of this study suggest that the use of the classroom-as-clinic teaching method in the first year of an entry-level master's program helps to improve students' clinical reasoning during the clinical evaluation process. The use of this method early in an occupational therapy curriculum may also give students an experiential base for 
September 1992, Volume 46, Number .9 818
their concurrent and suosequent didactic and theoretical learning. Belenky, C1inchy, Goldberger, and Tarule (1986) suggested that providing experience as a base for theoretical learning is an important [lal1 of what they called connected leaching The small student group interview component of the c1assroom-as-c1inic experience and the processing of the experiences in suosequent classes (Neistadt, 1987) allow for collaboration and evolution of personal knowledge through open discussion, which is another aspect of connected teaching (Schwartz, 1991). 
Research on the effect of the classroom-as-clinic 
method on Level II fieldwork and early practice performance 
would oe helpful in further validating this teaching 
method Additional research on the relative effect of different 
aspects of the method might help refine the method 
and provide guidelines on modifications needed for 
different groups of students at different points in occu[lational 
therapy curricula. 
Provision of training in clinical reasoning may be the best educational strategy for preparing clinicians to meet the complex demands of modern practiCe. This study has examined the relative efficacy of one method for providing that training. Other methods for teaching clinical reasoning need to be developed, tested, and shared so that occupational therapy can continue to evolve to meet the ongoing challenges of health care provision . .&. 
References 
American Occupational Therapy Association. (1981). Linifmm Occupational Therapy Evaluarion Checklist American Journal 0/ Occupational Therapy, 35. H17-818. 
Belenky, M. F, Clinchy, B. V" Goldberger, N. R, & Tarule,J IV!. (1986). lXIomen's ways o/knowing New York: Basic. 
Cody, R. P, & Smith,J K (1987). Appliedslatistics and the SAS prof!,ramminp, language (2nd ed.) New York: NorthHolland. 
Cohn, E. S. (1991) Nationally Speaking -Clinical reasoning: Explicating complexity. Americanjournal ofOccupational Therapr. 45, 969-971 
Fleming, M. H. (1991). The therapist wirh the three-track 
mind. American journal of Occupational Therapv, 45 
1007-1014 
Neistadt, M. E. (1987) Classroom as clinic: A nlodel for 
reaching clinical reasoning in occupational therapy education 
American journal or Occupational Therapy. 4], 631637 
Parham, D. (1987). Nationally Speaking -Toward pmfes" ionali5m: The reflecrive therarisr. American Journal orOccupatlonal Therapr. 41. 555-561 
Rogers, J. C (1983) Eleanm Clarke Slagle lecrureship1983 -Clinical reasoning: The erhics, science ancl an. American Journal of OccupationaL Therapl', 37, 601-616 
Roger's, J C, & Holm, M. B. (1991). Occupational therarY diagnosric reasoning: Acomponenr ofclinical reasoning. American jOllrnal 0/ Occupational Therapl', 45 1045-1053 
Rogers,.! C, & i'vJasagatani, G. (1982). Clinical reasoning of occupational therapists during the initial assessment of rhl'sicall disabled parients. Occupational Therapy JournaL orResearch. ], 195-219 
Schwanz, K. B. (1991). Clinical reasoning anc! new ideas on intelligence: Imrlications for' teaching and learning. American }Ollmal 0/ Occupational Tberapl', 45. 103,3-1037. 
Slarer, D. Y, & Cohn, E. S. (1991). Staff development through anah'sis of practice. AmericanJournaL o/Occupational Thempl. 45. 1038-1044 
Tufts lIniversit'-Boston School of Occupational Therary, 
(1990). ,,fclsler's degree programs in occupational therapy 
"kelford, MA: Tufrs Univcrsit'. 
Perhaps our readers have noticed that beginning with the]unc issue ofAjOT, the cover of the journal has looked or felt slightly different. This is because in the interest of the environment we have switched from catalyst (solventbased) coating to aqueous (water-based) coating for our cover scock As catalyst coating dries, it emits chemicals that harm the ozone. Aqueous coating, however, does nor harm the ozone and is easily repulped for entry into the recyclable waste stream. We are pleased that as we are informing our readers, we are also being kind to the Earth. 
lhC' ,liller/cali ./UUl'!wt of OcclljJalioliut lhclDpJ' 819

More Related Content

What's hot

ReeseUsherBowmanetetal
ReeseUsherBowmanetetalReeseUsherBowmanetetal
ReeseUsherBowmanetetalBarry Duncan
 
critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016
critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016
critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016James Nichols
 
E-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical Standards
E-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical StandardsE-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical Standards
E-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical StandardsJames Tobin, Ph.D.
 
Quantitative Research Methods
Quantitative Research MethodsQuantitative Research Methods
Quantitative Research MethodsFahrulRosyid1
 
Beyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoringBeyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoringScott Miller
 
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...Scott Miller
 
Detecting flawed meta analyses
Detecting flawed meta analysesDetecting flawed meta analyses
Detecting flawed meta analysesJames Coyne
 
Non-Experimental Research Design
Non-Experimental Research DesignNon-Experimental Research Design
Non-Experimental Research DesignAsokan R
 
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)Scott Miller
 
Research critiquing (Nursing Research) Critiques
Research critiquing (Nursing Research) CritiquesResearch critiquing (Nursing Research) Critiques
Research critiquing (Nursing Research) CritiquesTanoj Patidar
 
GEMC - Nursing Research
GEMC - Nursing ResearchGEMC - Nursing Research
GEMC - Nursing ResearchOpen.Michigan
 
Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)
Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)
Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)Barry Duncan
 
Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016Scott Miller
 

What's hot (19)

Seminar iv
Seminar ivSeminar iv
Seminar iv
 
ReeseUsherBowmanetetal
ReeseUsherBowmanetetalReeseUsherBowmanetetal
ReeseUsherBowmanetetal
 
critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016
critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016
critique osteoarthritis and cartilagefinal4182016
 
Nursing research introduction
Nursing research introductionNursing research introduction
Nursing research introduction
 
Nr 1 in 2
Nr 1 in 2Nr 1 in 2
Nr 1 in 2
 
E-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical Standards
E-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical StandardsE-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical Standards
E-Therapy: A Critical Review of Practice Characteristics and Ethical Standards
 
Quantitative Research Methods
Quantitative Research MethodsQuantitative Research Methods
Quantitative Research Methods
 
psychology
psychologypsychology
psychology
 
Edad 510 final
Edad 510 final Edad 510 final
Edad 510 final
 
Beyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoringBeyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoring
 
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
 
Detecting flawed meta analyses
Detecting flawed meta analysesDetecting flawed meta analyses
Detecting flawed meta analyses
 
Non-Experimental Research Design
Non-Experimental Research DesignNon-Experimental Research Design
Non-Experimental Research Design
 
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
 
Research critiquing (Nursing Research) Critiques
Research critiquing (Nursing Research) CritiquesResearch critiquing (Nursing Research) Critiques
Research critiquing (Nursing Research) Critiques
 
GEMC - Nursing Research
GEMC - Nursing ResearchGEMC - Nursing Research
GEMC - Nursing Research
 
Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)
Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)
Outcome Rating Scale (ORS)
 
module 6 lesson 2
module 6 lesson 2module 6 lesson 2
module 6 lesson 2
 
Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016
 

Similar to Razonamiento Clínico 1

Assessment in ME
Assessment in MEAssessment in ME
Assessment in MES A Tabish
 
Concepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docx
Concepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docxConcepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docx
Concepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docxstudywriters
 
19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse
19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse
19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and AsseAnastaciaShadelb
 
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docxEvidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docxgitagrimston
 
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docxEvidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docxSANSKAR20
 
Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)Scott Miller
 
C O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docx
C O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docxC O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docx
C O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docxclairbycraft
 
Thinking like a nurse.docx
Thinking like a nurse.docxThinking like a nurse.docx
Thinking like a nurse.docxwrite5
 
Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs Studying Propranolol In ...
Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs  Studying Propranolol In ...Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs  Studying Propranolol In ...
Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs Studying Propranolol In ...Stephen Faucher
 
PPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
PPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptxPPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
PPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptxssuser064316
 
Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
Asking Resaerch Question.pptxAsking Resaerch Question.pptx
Asking Resaerch Question.pptxssuser064316
 
A Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem Solving
A Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem SolvingA Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem Solving
A Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem SolvingBecky Gilbert
 
Anger Treatment For Adults A Meta-Analytic Review
Anger Treatment For Adults  A Meta-Analytic ReviewAnger Treatment For Adults  A Meta-Analytic Review
Anger Treatment For Adults A Meta-Analytic ReviewScott Faria
 
Training Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docx
Training Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docxTraining Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docx
Training Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docxjuliennehar
 
Bachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed unitsBachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed unitsShira Rotem
 
Master of Science in Nursing   Pra
Master of Science in Nursing                             PraMaster of Science in Nursing                             Pra
Master of Science in Nursing   PraAbramMartino96
 
Master of Science in Nursing   Pra
Master of Science in Nursing                             PraMaster of Science in Nursing                             Pra
Master of Science in Nursing   PraAbramMartino96
 
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdfMcClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdfDrog3
 
1PAGE 21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx
1PAGE  21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx1PAGE  21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx
1PAGE 21. What is the question the authors are asking .docxfelicidaddinwoodie
 

Similar to Razonamiento Clínico 1 (20)

1045.full
1045.full1045.full
1045.full
 
Assessment in ME
Assessment in MEAssessment in ME
Assessment in ME
 
Concepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docx
Concepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docxConcepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docx
Concepts For Clinical Judgment Discussion Module 3.docx
 
19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse
19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse
19DOI 10.103714048-002APA Handbook of Testing and Asse
 
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docxEvidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
 
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docxEvidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
Evidence-Based Practice in PsychologyImplications for Resea.docx
 
Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)
 
C O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docx
C O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docxC O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docx
C O N C E P T A N A L Y S I SClinical reasoning concept a.docx
 
Thinking like a nurse.docx
Thinking like a nurse.docxThinking like a nurse.docx
Thinking like a nurse.docx
 
Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs Studying Propranolol In ...
Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs  Studying Propranolol In ...Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs  Studying Propranolol In ...
Advanced Regression Methods For Single-Case Designs Studying Propranolol In ...
 
PPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
PPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptxPPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
PPT-Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
 
Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
Asking Resaerch Question.pptxAsking Resaerch Question.pptx
Asking Resaerch Question.pptx
 
A Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem Solving
A Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem SolvingA Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem Solving
A Method For The Study Of Medical Thinking And Problem Solving
 
Anger Treatment For Adults A Meta-Analytic Review
Anger Treatment For Adults  A Meta-Analytic ReviewAnger Treatment For Adults  A Meta-Analytic Review
Anger Treatment For Adults A Meta-Analytic Review
 
Training Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docx
Training Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docxTraining Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docx
Training Therapists in Evidence-Based Practice A Critical.docx
 
Bachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed unitsBachelor of OT completed units
Bachelor of OT completed units
 
Master of Science in Nursing   Pra
Master of Science in Nursing                             PraMaster of Science in Nursing                             Pra
Master of Science in Nursing   Pra
 
Master of Science in Nursing   Pra
Master of Science in Nursing                             PraMaster of Science in Nursing                             Pra
Master of Science in Nursing   Pra
 
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdfMcClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
 
1PAGE 21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx
1PAGE  21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx1PAGE  21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx
1PAGE 21. What is the question the authors are asking .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptxPSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptxPoojaSen20
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptxPSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 

Razonamiento Clínico 1

  • 1. The Classroom as Clinic: Applications for a Method of Teaching Clinical Reasoning Maureen E. Neistadt Key Words: education. evaluation studies This study examined the efficacy ofone methodfor teaching diagnostic reasoning to occupational therapy students. During a clinical reasoning seminar in their first academic year, 80 entry-level occupational therapy master's degree students in three successive classes were given three different levels ofexposure to classroom-as-clinic or in-class evaluations ofadults with physical or psychosocial disabilities. During the following summer, most students completed their first Level II fieldwork experience. Students' grades for a second-year classroom-as-clinic experience with adults with physial disabilities were then compared across groups to determine the relative effect ofthe different seminarformats andfieldwork experiences. Students who had experienced in-class evaluations during their first academic year wrote Significantly more accurate second-year evaluations than those who had not. Students who had completed psychosocial Level II fieldwork experiences were as accurate on their evaluations as students who had had physical dYsfunction fieldwork experiences. The results suggest that in-class evaluations improve students' diagnostic reasoning skills. Maureen E. Neistadt, SeD. OTM, is Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy Department, University of New Hampshire, Hewitt Hall, Durham, New Hampshire 03824-3563. At the time of this study she was Assistant Professor, Tufts University-Boston School of Occupational Therapy, Medford, Massachusetts. This ai-tide was accepted for publica/ion April 10, 1992 Skill in clinical reasoning is essential for effective occupational therapy practice (Fleming, 1991; Parham, 1987; Rogers, 1983; Slater & Cohn, 1991). "Clinical reasoning is a dynamic process of inquiry in action that takes place in the context of occupational therapy evaluation and treatment" (Tufts University-Boston School of Occupational Therapy [BSOT), 1990, p. 3). Schwartz (1991) recently highlighted the need for occupational therapy education programs to develop and implement teaching methodologies that encourage the development of students' clinical reasoning abilities. In 1987, I described a classroom-as-clinic method designed to teach occupational therapy students the clinical reasoning process associated with evaluation and treatment planning. A previous study demonstrated that this method improved occupational therapy students' abilities to accurately analyze preassessment data and formulate appropriate treatment plans (Neistadt, 1987). Students in that study engaged in classroom-based evaluations of adults with physical disabilities at the end of an occupational therapy curriculum, after they had taken all of their nonelective course work and before they had begun their Level II fieldwork experiences. One question raised by that study was whether in-class evaluations of adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities would effectively teach clinical reasoning earlier in an occupational therapy curriculum. The present study addressed this question. Literature Review Rogers and Masagatani (1982) and Rogers (1983) originally described therapists' thought processes during initial evaluations as involVing a sequence of deduction, induction, dialectical reasoning, and ethical reasoning. Therapists, said these authors, begin their evaluations with a review of clients' charts or other preassessment information that might be available or both. From this review, therapists form hypotheses about possible problems of clients through a process of deduction. Therapists evaluate their clients, then modify their preassessment hypotheses by considering the specific details of clients' cases (induction) and deciding between different interpretations for clients' behaviors (dialectical reasoning). Therapists then work with their clients to establish treatment priorities consistent with the clients' value systems (ethical reasoning). More recently, Rogers and Holm (1991) referred to the thought processes that occupational therapists use during initial evaluation as diagnostic reasoning. Diagnostic reasoning "is the sequence of decisions that leads to occupational therapy diagnosis" and "is one component of the clinical reasoning involved in the occupational therapy process" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). The occupational therapy diagnosis "describes the actual or potential effects of disease, trauma, developmental disor- September 1992, Volume 46, Number 9 814
  • 2. ders, age-associated changes, environmental deprivation, and mher etiologic agents on occupational sWtus" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). This occupational therapy diagnosis becomes the foundation for collaborative treatment planning with the cliene According [0 Rogers and Holm (1991), the diagnostic reasoning process involves both problem sensing and problem definition. "A therapist senses a problem by framing it, that is, by deciding what will be included in the picture. The picture inside that frame is the clinical image" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). This formation of clinical images begins during the chart review stage of assessment and is influenced by the reason for occupational therapy referral; the practice setting; the experience and frames of reference of the therapist; and the client's condition, age, and sex. The severity of the client'S condition will also influence the clinical image (Rogers & Holm, 1991). This clinical image would include mental hypotheses about the client's pmential problems -hypotheses formed through deductive reasoning about the information available from the chart review and other preassessment information (Rogers, 1983; Rogers & Masagatani, 1982). Problem definition is a process in which the therapist concisely and precisely describes and names the client's problems. "A5 a result of this descriptive process, the therapist's clinical image of a client becomes more like the actual client encountered in the clinic" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1045). Therapists engage in the problemdefinition process during the initial evaluation of a c1iene Rogers and Holm presented an information-[Jrocessing perspective on problem definition that sees the therapist as a data processor and the client and the client's Jiving situation as the data field. The therapist "collects, organizes, analyzes, and synthesizes data about a client's occupational status" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1048). A5 a data processor, the therapist uses "four basic processes: cue acqUisition, hypmhesis generation, cue interpretation, and hypothesis evaluation" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, r. 1048). Cues are data to which therapists attend. Therapists interpret the cues gathered during initial evaluation [0 test their [Jreassessment hypotheses and [0 form and test new hypotheses. They use dialectic [Jrocess to weigh the relative merits of alternative hypotheses and ethical reasoning to consider the influence of clients' values and mmivations on rroblem definitions. A5 Rogers and Holm (1991) have suggested, diagnostic reasoning is only one component of occurational therarists' clinical reasoning process. Fleming (1991) has suggested that occupational therapists simultaneously use three different ways of thinking: procedural, interactive, and conditional. Therapists use procedural reasoning to focus on diagnosis and disability by following a logical medical decision-making process of problem identification, goal setting, and treatment planning that uses their medical, technical, and occupational knowledge. The Ilmerican journal or Occupational Therapy Fleming's procedural reasoning corresponds to Rogers and Holm's diagnostic reasoning. Therapists use interactive reasoning during meetings with clients to try to understand how the client makes sense of the disability or disease and how that disability or disease interferes with the roles and activities that give that person's life meaning. Therapists use conditional reasoning to think about the client's future, "given the constraints of the physical condition within the client's personal and social context" (Fleming, 1991, p. 1013). During chart review, therapists use primarily procedural (diagnostic) reasoning. Experienced clinicians might also use conditional reasoning at this stage to begin forming an image of the client's future, given the diagnosis, prognosis, and social and vocational history. During a client evaluation, therapists combine procedural (diagnostic), interactive, and conditional reasoning to observe, elicit, and interpret cues so they can develop a treatment plan that is meaningful [0 the client. Cohn (1991) stated that occupational therapy clinicians and clinical educators frequentlycompJain "that academic programs do not adequately prepare students for the uncertainties inherent in the challenges of practice" (p. 969). Perhaps these complaints arise because the traditional teaching and testing methodologies of higher education cannot foster the complex array of reasoning skills that occupational therapists must use in practice. More experiential teaching modes that use testing methods linked to clinical practice might teach clinical reasoning better (Schwartz, 1991). The primary purpose of the present experimental study was to see whether a modified classroom-as-clinic method in the first year of an entry-level master's degree program would improve the clinical reasoning skills of students by the second year of their rrogram, as measured by performance in a classroom- as-cJinic experience at the beginning of the second academic year. A secondary purpose was to assess the effects of Level II fieldwork experiences on these students' second-year classroom-as-c1inic performances. Method Design A post hoc experimental design was used to compare the second year classroom-as-clinic performances of three independent groups of students. A5 a result of ongOing curriculum development, three successive groups of students were given three different levels of exposure to classroom-as-clinic or in-class evaluations of adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities during a clinical reasoning seminar in the second semeSter of their first academic year. Subjects The subjects in this study were 80 entry-level master's degree students at Tufts University-Boston School of Oc815
  • 3. cupational Therapy, Medford, Massachusetts Subjects were members of three successive groups of students attending the university between the years 1989 and 1992 (for Group 1, n = 21; Group 2, n = 31; Group 3, Jl = 28). Their ages ranged from 22 years to 40 years. Five subjects were men and 75 were women. The average preadmission grade point average was 3.1 for aU three groups of students. All subjects took their basic science, pathology, and introductory occupational therapy course work in their first academic year. As part of the first academic year's work, all subjects participated in clinical reasoning seminars on observation skills and interactive reasoning during their first and second semesters, respectively. Subjects also took either a psychosocial or physical dysfunction course in the second semester of their first year, to prepare them for a first summer Level II fieldwork corresponding to the dysfunction course they had taken. Some subjects elected not to do a Level II fieldwork that first summer for personal or financial reasons. In the first semester of the second year, all subjects participated in an advanced occupational therapy course that used the c1assroom-as-c1inic teaching method. The second-year course work included clinical reasoning seminars on procedural and conditional reasoning in the first and second semesters, respectively. Additional course work in pediatrics and in the major dysfunction course not taken in the first year was also offered Most subjects completed their second Level II fieldwork in the summer after the second academic year, with the remaining subjects completing their first and second Level II fieldwork at this time. For Group 1, the interactive reasoning seminar did not include contact with persons with physical or psychosocial disabilities. The goal in this first seminar was to improve subjects' self-awareness so that they would he able to interact as therapeutic agents with future clients. Lectures and small group exercises about interviewing, empathy, and nonverhal communication were used. Subjects expressed dissatisfaction with the lack of client contact in this seminar. Consequently, the interactive seminar for Group 2 included in-class student group interviews with persons with rhysical or psychosocial disabilities. Faculty thought that this interview experience helped students develop their interactive reasoning skills, hut that it did not force students to use interactive reasoning in conjunction with procedural and conditional reasoning, as would be required in clinical evaluations and treatment. Therefore, the interactive seminar for Group 3 included modified c1assroom-as-clinic experiences. Course outlines, testing methods, ancl Level I fieldwork for all courses hut the interactive seminar remained constant during the study period. Group 3 had a different instructor than Groups 1 and 2 for two psychosocial courses and one pathology course. Otherwise, course instructors remained constant throughout the study period. Procedure The format of the classroom-as-clinic experience was hased on Rogers' model ofclinical reasoning during initial evaluation (Rogers, 1983) ami has already heen described in detail (Neistadt, 1987). During these experiences, the suhjects were exrected to write a problem-goal-plan list after reviewing limited preassessment information (i.e., diagnosis and social situation) and to revise that list after interviewing a guest participant with a physical or psychosocial disability. In the original c1assroom-as-c1inic method which was used in the advanced occupational therapy co~rse at the beginning of the subjects' second year, subjects did nor receive any information on the diagnosis of the guest participant before the day of the in-class evaluation and were expected to write their first prohlemgoal- plan list in the 30 to 40 min immediately preceding their meeting with the guest participant. For this first second-year evaluation, the guest participants all had conditions diagnosed as central nervous system dysfunction In the modified in-class evaluation used in the firstveal' interactive reasoning seminar for subjects in Group3, subjects received preassessment information ahout the guest participants 1 week in advance and were given 1 week to work on their initial problem-goal-plan lists at home, using their books and class notes as references. For all in-class evaluations, suhjects met in small groups with one guest participant for 90 min and then wrote revised prohlem-goal-plan lists which they then handed in at the end of class. The grades on the problem-goal-plan lists represented the percentage of correct problems that the suhjects recorded from a list of expected problems for a given diagnosis or guest participant. The preassessment or chart review correct problems lists were derived from the Uniform Occupational Therapy Evaluation Checklist (American Occupational Therapy Association, 1981). Prohlem areas specific to particular diagnoses were selected from this list according to the occupational therapy literature and the instructor's clinical experience. The postassessment or evaluation correct problems lists were also derived from the uniform checklist and were based on the clinical experience ofthe instructor and the clinical observations of the faculty coJeaders in the guest participants' groups. The grading rrocedure, course instructor, and guest participants were the same for all three subject groups. Results Subjects' grades on the chart review and evaluation prohlem- goal-rJan lists for the first classroom-as-c1inic experience in the second year were anal)'7.ed with two-way analvses ofvariance and Tukey pairwise comparisons (Cody & Smith, 1987). Subject group during the first year and Level II fieldwork during the first summer were the indeSeplell1! Jel' 1992. Volume 40. Numher 9 816
  • 4. pendent variables, and grades on the second-year problem- goal-pian lists were the dependent variables in these analyses. A significance level of 05 was used. Chart Review lists For the chan review lists, there was no significant effect for either subject group [F (2, 71) = 2.47, P = .0919] or type of Level II fieldwork [F (2, 71) = 0.60, p = .5495]. Tukey pairwise comparisons showed no significant differences in chart review grades among any of the three subject groups (see Tables 1 and 2). There was a significant group by Level 11 fieldwork interaction [F (2, 71) = 3.05, P = .0224] in the chart review analysis ofv<lri<lnce. When the sample was soned by groups, one-way analyses of variance with Level 11 fieldwork <lS the independent variable <lnd chan review grades as the dependent variable showed a significant Level II fieldwork effect only for Group 1 [F (2, 18) = 436, P = .0286J. Tukey pairwise comparisons for this group showed significant differences in ch<lrt review gr<ldes between subjects who had had physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those who had had psychosocial Level 11 fieldwork. The former scored an average of 90.7%: the latter, an average of 79.7%. Evaluation Lisls For the evaluation lists, there were significant effects for both group [F(2, 71) = 11.74, P = .0001] and Level 11 fieldwork IF(2, 71) = 4.27, P = .0177]. There W<lS no significant group by Level II fieldwork interaction [F(2, 71) = 1.12, /) = .35581. Tukey pairwise comparisons showed significant differences in evaluation list grades among all three groups (see Table 1) and between those suhjects who had had physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those vho had not done any Level II fieldwork in the preceding summer (see Table 2). Paired I-test comparisons showed that only Group :3, with the in-class evaluation expcrience, improved significantly from the chan review [0 thc evaluation list grades (see Table 2). Table 1 Tukey Pairwise Comparison of Average Grades for Subject Groups Chan R<.:view Lists Evaluation Lists Group .VI (%) SO M(%) SO 1 (/1 = 21) 871 94 1345 9,1 2 (/1 = 31) 904 99 909 90 3 (11 = 28) 928 65 973 49 Nole For Group 1, the ill[cractivc r<.:asoning seminar did not lIlciude contact ,,'iLh pnsons Ivith phvsical or psychosocial cJisabiliti<.:s, Fo,' Group 2. the ill[eractiv<.: seminar lI1c1ud<.:d in-class stud<.:nt group interviews wil h p<.:rsons wirh physiGI or psvchosocial disabilities. For Group 3, th<.: interactive seminar included modifi<.:d classroom-as-c1inic <.:x[)<.:riCIlCl.:' S. lhe Alllericull .IoUrJlCI1 or OCClljJUliollal FherajJl' Table 2 Tukey Pairwise Comparison of Average Grades for Level" Fieldwork Experiences Chan R<:vi<.:w Lists Evaluation Lists Lcv<.:1 II Fieldwork !vI (%) SD !vI (%) SO Physical dysfunction (n = 32) 90.9 8.2 94.3 8.13 Psy<.:hosocial dysfunction (11 = 34) 90.5 9.2 90.3 9.1 None (n = 14) 89.1 10.1 877 93 Note. Preadmission grade point average was comparable for all Leveill fi<.:ldwork groups. Discussion Results suggest that the use of in-class evaluations of adults with physical or psychosocial dysfunction during the first year of an entry-level master's program help~ students to develop their clinical reasoning skills. The general consistency of instructors, content, and teaching and testing methods for Other courses in the curriculum during the study period strongly suggests that the results are related to the in-class evaluation variable. Subjects who had experienced in-class evaluations during their fir~t academic year were significantly more accurate than those who had nor experienced in-class evaluations in writing evaluation problem-goal-plan lists for an in-class evaluation experience in the second academic year. Results for both the chan review and evaluation lists are discussed below. Cbart Reuiew Lists Subjeci groups. The lack of significant differences bctween thc three different subject groups on the second- year chart review li~ts suggest~ that ~kiJl with this pan of the evaluation process i~ not strongly affected by interaction with adults with physical or p~ychosocial disabilitie~ in the fil·~t academic year. AJthough not stati~ticallv significant, a trend emerged showing that subjects wl[h interview experience in the first year did better than those with no interview experience and that those with a modified classroom-as-cJinic experience did better than tho~e with only interview experience (see Table 1). In my pl-eviou~ cJassroom-a~-c1inic~tudy, I reponed that students had repeatedly said that "meeting adults wlth disabilities hel ps them to make sense ofand 'picture' the theoretic<l] information they have learned in the c1a~sroom" (Neisradt, 1987, p. 634) The interview experiences may have given subJeers ~ome beginning clinical images (Rogers & Holm, 1991) of people with rarticular disabilitie~, and r(;trieving these images may have helped them write more accurate chart review lists in the second academic vear. Subjects who were forced to contrast their preassessment Jnd postasse~sment image~ in the modified classroom-as-c1inic experience may have had more vivid and accurate clinical image~ to draw on in the second 'ear of their program. 817
  • 5. Fieldwork experiences. The lack of significant differences among the three different Level II fieldwork experiences (physical dysfunction, psychosocial dysfunction, none) on the second-year chart review lists suggests that skill with this part of the evaluation process is also n.ot strongly affected by interaction with adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities during fieldwork. However, there was a trend for subjects with psychosocial Level II fieldwork to do better than those without Level II fieldwork experience and for those with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork to do better than those with psychosocial experience (see Table 2). The preassessment list is primarily an exercise in problem sensing (Rogers & Holm, 1991). Subjects Without Level 11 fieldwork would have missed intensive practice with the problem-sensing process. The psychosocial Level II fieldwork would have given subjects practice with problem sensing for adults with psychosocial dysfunctions, whereas the physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork would have provided practice with problem sensing for adults with physical disabilities. Because the guest participants in the second-year classroom-as-c1inic examined here all had physical disabilities, one would expect the physical disability Level II fieldwork problem-sensing experience to be more applicable; however, the differences between the performances of all subjects who had done psychosocial and physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork were not significant. The significant difference in chart review accuracy between subjects with different Level II fieldwork experiences within Group 1 is most likely related to a few weak subjects in the psychosocial group whose grades lowered the entire psychosocial average. Rogers and Holm have said that "diagnostic reasoning is generic to all practice areas" (Rogers & Holm, 1991, p. 1047). These chart review results suggest that the problem-sensing part of diagnostic reasoning, in partinllar, can be generalized across treatment settings. Evaluation Lists Subject groups. The significant differences among the three subject groups on the second-year evaluation lists suggests that students' skill with this pan of the evaluation process is strongly affected by interaction with adults with physical or psychosocial disabilities in the first academic year. Subjects with interview experience in the first year did significantly better than those without interview experience, and those with a modified classroom-as-c1inic experience did significantly better than those with either no interview or only interview experience (see Table 1). This result suggests that the c1assroom-as-clinic experience provided subjects with practice in using the combination of interactive, procedural, and conditional reasoning that Fleming (1991) has said is essential to clinical evaluation. Practice in constantly switching from one type of reasoning to the other during a time- pressured meeting with an adult with a disability may heir students to hone their problem-definition skills (Rogers & Holm, 1991). That is, the first year in-class evaluations seemed to make subjects more proficient at obserVing, eliciting, and interpreting cues during an initial interview. Consequently, in their second year, subjects could describe guest participant problems more accurately than could subjects who had not experienced the classroom-as-c1inic method. The finding that only the subjects who had the classroom-as-clinic experience in their first year improved significantly from the chart review to evaluation lists in their second year further supports the notion that this teaching method improves students' reasoning abilities during the client evaluation process. Fieldwork experiences. The lack ofsignificant differences between the subjects with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and psychosocial Level II fieldwork experiences on the second-year evaluation lists suggests that Level II fieldwork in either practice setting provides students with generalizable experience in combining procedural, interactive, and conditional reasoning to yield accurate client-specific problem definitions. The trend for subjects with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork to do better than those with psychosocial Level II fieldwork (see Table 2) may relate, again, to practice with the population seen in the second-year c1assroom-as-clinic experience. Rogers & Holm (1991) have suggested that recency, intensity, and frequency of practice with particular populations will influence the accuracy of problem definition, even though the general process of diagnostic reasoning can be applied across practice settings. The significant difference between subjects with physical dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those without Level II fieldwork on the second-year evaluation lists probably also relates to the Level II fieldwork practice the former had with the population seen in second-year c1assroom-as-clinic experience. The lack ofsignificant differences in evaluation list scores between subjects with psychosocial dysfunction Level II fieldwork and those without Level II fieldwork may relate to the lack ofLeve! II fieldwork practice for either group in evaluating a physical dysfunction population, but the trend was for the subjects with psychosocial Level II fieldwork to score better than those without Level II fieldwork. This trend probably reflects the former's Level II fieldwork practice with diagnostic and interactive reasoning. Conclusion The results of this study suggest that the use of the classroom-as-clinic teaching method in the first year of an entry-level master's program helps to improve students' clinical reasoning during the clinical evaluation process. The use of this method early in an occupational therapy curriculum may also give students an experiential base for September 1992, Volume 46, Number .9 818
  • 6. their concurrent and suosequent didactic and theoretical learning. Belenky, C1inchy, Goldberger, and Tarule (1986) suggested that providing experience as a base for theoretical learning is an important [lal1 of what they called connected leaching The small student group interview component of the c1assroom-as-c1inic experience and the processing of the experiences in suosequent classes (Neistadt, 1987) allow for collaboration and evolution of personal knowledge through open discussion, which is another aspect of connected teaching (Schwartz, 1991). Research on the effect of the classroom-as-clinic method on Level II fieldwork and early practice performance would oe helpful in further validating this teaching method Additional research on the relative effect of different aspects of the method might help refine the method and provide guidelines on modifications needed for different groups of students at different points in occu[lational therapy curricula. Provision of training in clinical reasoning may be the best educational strategy for preparing clinicians to meet the complex demands of modern practiCe. This study has examined the relative efficacy of one method for providing that training. Other methods for teaching clinical reasoning need to be developed, tested, and shared so that occupational therapy can continue to evolve to meet the ongoing challenges of health care provision . .&. References American Occupational Therapy Association. (1981). Linifmm Occupational Therapy Evaluarion Checklist American Journal 0/ Occupational Therapy, 35. H17-818. Belenky, M. F, Clinchy, B. V" Goldberger, N. R, & Tarule,J IV!. (1986). lXIomen's ways o/knowing New York: Basic. Cody, R. P, & Smith,J K (1987). Appliedslatistics and the SAS prof!,ramminp, language (2nd ed.) New York: NorthHolland. Cohn, E. S. (1991) Nationally Speaking -Clinical reasoning: Explicating complexity. Americanjournal ofOccupational Therapr. 45, 969-971 Fleming, M. H. (1991). The therapist wirh the three-track mind. American journal of Occupational Therapv, 45 1007-1014 Neistadt, M. E. (1987) Classroom as clinic: A nlodel for reaching clinical reasoning in occupational therapy education American journal or Occupational Therapy. 4], 631637 Parham, D. (1987). Nationally Speaking -Toward pmfes" ionali5m: The reflecrive therarisr. American Journal orOccupatlonal Therapr. 41. 555-561 Rogers, J. C (1983) Eleanm Clarke Slagle lecrureship1983 -Clinical reasoning: The erhics, science ancl an. American Journal of OccupationaL Therapl', 37, 601-616 Roger's, J C, & Holm, M. B. (1991). Occupational therarY diagnosric reasoning: Acomponenr ofclinical reasoning. American jOllrnal 0/ Occupational Therapl', 45 1045-1053 Rogers,.! C, & i'vJasagatani, G. (1982). Clinical reasoning of occupational therapists during the initial assessment of rhl'sicall disabled parients. Occupational Therapy JournaL orResearch. ], 195-219 Schwanz, K. B. (1991). Clinical reasoning anc! new ideas on intelligence: Imrlications for' teaching and learning. American }Ollmal 0/ Occupational Tberapl', 45. 103,3-1037. Slarer, D. Y, & Cohn, E. S. (1991). Staff development through anah'sis of practice. AmericanJournaL o/Occupational Thempl. 45. 1038-1044 Tufts lIniversit'-Boston School of Occupational Therary, (1990). ,,fclsler's degree programs in occupational therapy "kelford, MA: Tufrs Univcrsit'. Perhaps our readers have noticed that beginning with the]unc issue ofAjOT, the cover of the journal has looked or felt slightly different. This is because in the interest of the environment we have switched from catalyst (solventbased) coating to aqueous (water-based) coating for our cover scock As catalyst coating dries, it emits chemicals that harm the ozone. Aqueous coating, however, does nor harm the ozone and is easily repulped for entry into the recyclable waste stream. We are pleased that as we are informing our readers, we are also being kind to the Earth. lhC' ,liller/cali ./UUl'!wt of OcclljJalioliut lhclDpJ' 819