SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 7
Download to read offline
When Intentions Go Public
Does Social Reality Widen the Intention-Behavior Gap?
Peter M. Gollwitzer,1,2 Paschal Sheeran,3
Verena Michalski,2 and Andrea E. Seifert2
1New York University, 2UniversitdJ Konstanz, and 3University ofSheffield
ABSTRACT-Based on Lewiniangoal theory in general and
self-completion theory in particular, four experiments ex-
amined the implications of other people taking notice of
one~ identity-related behavioral intentions (e.g., the in-
tention to read law periodicals regularly to reach the
identity goal of becoming a lawyer). Identity-related be-
havioral intentions that had been noticed by other people
were translated into action less intensively than those that
had been ignored (Studies 1-3). This effect was evident in
the field (persistent striving over 1 week~time; Study 1)
and in the laboratory (jumping on opportunities to act;
Studies 2 and 3), and it held among participants with
strong but not weak commitment to the identitygoal (Study
3). Study 4 showed, in addition, that when other people
take notice of an individual~ identity-related behavioral
intention, this gives the individual a premature sense of
possessing the aspired-to identity.
Are scientists more likely to write papers if they tell colleagues
about this intention than if they keep the intention private? It is
commonly assumed that whenever people make their intentions
public, the behavioral impact of these intentions is enhanced
(e.g., Staats, Harland, & Wilke, 2004). These effects are pos-
tulated to be a consequence of multiple processes. Research on
persuasion techniques points to one of these processes (Cialdini
& Trost, 1998). It is argued that a publicly stated behavioral
intention commits the individual to a certain self-view (e.g., "I
am a productive person") with which the person then acts con-
sistently. Indeed, individuals with a higher need for consistency
show stronger public-commitment effects (Cialdini, Wosinka,
Barrett, Butner, & Gornik-Durose, 1999). The second process is
referred to as accountability (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999). Making
intentions public is said to make a person accountable to the
addressed audience, and research has shown that various ac-
Address correspondence to Peter M. Gollwitzer, New York Univer-
sity, Psychology Department, 6 Washington Place, 7th Floor, New
York, NY 10003, e-mail: peter.gollwitzer@nyu.edu.
612
countability-related features of the audience (e.g., competence,
power) and the individual (e.g., identifiability, expectations of
having to explain oneself) affect the strength of public-com-
mitment effects.
Both ofthese lines ofresearch focus on intentions in which the
specified behavior is a desired outcome in and of itself. Lewin
(1926) and his colleagues (e.g., Mahler, 1935; Ovsiankina,
1928), however, argued that people often construe behavioral
intentions in more general terms, thus allowing substitution of
means for attainment. For instance, consider a student who has
started an assigned math task with the intention to successfully
solve the required addition problems. During the process, this
student may construe the intention as being to demonstrate
mathematical skills, and this conceptually broader intention
may also be reached by solving subtraction problems (i.e., by
substitute activities). Ovsiankina and Mahler observed that a
substitute activity engenders a sense of having reached the
conceptually broader intention, given that performance of the
substitute activity has been witnessed by other people (i.e., has
become a social reality). On the basis of this line of thought-
which we explicate in the framework of self-completion theory
(SCT; Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998; Wicklund & Gollwitzer,
1982)-we propose that social recognition of an identity-rele-
vant behavioral intention may have negative effects on its en-
actment.
SCT proposes that people who are committed to identity goals
(e.g., becoming a good parent, scientist, or craftsperson) can
undertake a variety of activities to claim goal attainment. For a
scientist, such activities, or identity symbols, include engaging
in professional duties (e.g., giving lectures), making positive
self-descriptions (e.g., "I discovered a new principle!"), exerting
identity-relevant social influence (e.g., advising students), and
acquiring skills and tools that facilitate striving for the identity
goal (e.g., programming skills, computers). However, failing to
perform an identity-relevant activity or facing the lack of an
identity symbol produces a state ofincompleteness (Wicklund &
Gollwitzer, 1982). To restore completeness, the individual
makes efforts to acquire alternative identity symbols (e.g., de-
scribing oneself as having the required personality attributes:
First publ. in: Psychological Science 20 (2009), 5, pp. 612-618
Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS)
URN: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-86958
URL: http://kops.ub.uni-konstanz.de/volltexte/2009/8695/
Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; engaging in identity-relevant
activities: Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996; showing off relevant
status symbols: Harmon-Jones, Schmeichel, & Harmon-Jones,
2009). Using opportunities to affirm one's general self-integrity
or to bolster one's self-esteem is not sufficient to offset incom-
pleteness regarding an identity goal; rather, it is necessary to
acquire specific identity symbols (Ledgerwood, Liviatan, &
Carnevale, 2007).
SCT research has also shown that an individual reaches a
higher level of completeness when his or her identity-relevant
activities are noticed by a social audience (Gollwitzer, 1986).
Moreover, research has shown that incomplete individuals are
more concerned with finding an audience for their identity
strivings, compared with complete individuals (Brunstein &
Gollwitzer, 1996). Positive self-descriptions made in public
qualify as powerful identity symbols (Gollwitzer, Wicklund, &
Hilton, 1982), and having an audience for behavioral intentions
that specify the successful performance of an identity-relevant
activity should have the same symbolic impact. The implication
is that when other people take notice ofa stated identity-relevant
behavioral intention, this should engender completeness re-
garding the superordinate identity goal, and thus reaching the
identity goal by actually performing the intended behavior
should become less necessary. In other words, people should be
less likely to translate their identity-relevant behavioral inten-
tions into action when other people have taken notice of those
intentions. We conducted four experiments entailing a variety of
identity goals and behavioral intentions to test this hypothesis.
STUDY 1
In Study 1, we asked college students committed to becoming a
psychologist to form identity-relevant studying intentions. After
either taking notice of these intentions or ignoring them, we
assessed how effectively the students enacted their intentions
over the subsequent week.
Method
Forty-nine psychology students (38 women, 11 men) at a Ger-
man university were recruited after introductory lectures. Par-
ticipants were informed that they would take part in a survey
about the study intentions of first-year psychology students. A
first questionnaire assessed commitment to becoming a psy-
chologist using the following items:
• "How imp011ant is it for you to find a psychology-related job?"
• "Suppose that you could not finish your studies of psychology
successfully. How much would that bother you?"
• "How happy would you be in a job that is not related to
psychology?" (reverse-coded)
Participants responded to these items on 9-point scales ranging
from 1, not at all, to 9, very much. Responses were averaged to
form a scale (ex = .80). Next, participants were asked to write
down their two most important study intentions for the forth-
coming week (e.g., "I will take my reading assignments more
seriously," "I intend to study more statistics").
In the social-reality condition, the experimenter read through
each participant's reported intentions, presumably to ensure
that the participant had understood the instructions. In the no-
social-reality condition, participants were told that the page of
the questionnaire on which participants had written down their
behavioral intentions had been wrongly included in the study,
and that this page would be discarded (i.e., the students' in-
tentions remained unnoticed).
One week later, all participants were sent a second ques-
tionnaire via e-mail. They had to first write down the two be-
havioral intentions they had listed the previous week. Then, they
indicated on exactly which days of the past week they had acted
on each intention. Finally, participants were asked to bring th~ir
completed questionnaire to the experimenter's office, where they
received payment (€5) or course credit.
Results and Discussion
Overall, participants were highly committed to the identity goal
(range = 6-9; M = 7.32, SD = 1.64), and there was no
significant difference in commitment scores between the
social-reality condition (M = 7.21, SD = 1.64) and the no-
social-reality condition (M = 7.43, SD = 1.08), F < 1,p > .32,
d < 0.16. We analyzed the number of days participants acted on
their intentions in a 2 x 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with
social reality as the between-participants factor and the two
specified behavioral intentions as the within-participants factor.
Results showed a significant main effect of social reality, F(l,
47) = 4.38, p < .05, d = 0.60; participants whose intentions
remained private acted on their intentions on more days of the
week (M = 2.70, SD = 1.83) than did participants whose in-
tentions were noticed (M = 1.92, SD = 0.78). No other effects
were significant.
An apparent strength of Study 1 is that the observed negative
effects of having one's intentions noticed cannot easily be at-
tributed to emotions that might accrue from the experimenter's
behavior (e.g., pride). Such affective responses should vanish
quickly, whereas the effects on behavioral enactment in Study 1
were evident over a period of 1 week. An obvious weakness of
this study, however, is that enactment of intentions was assessed
via self-report.
STUDY 2
In Study 2, therefore, we observed actual enactment of inten-
tions. Participants were law students who formed the behavioral
intention to make use of identity-relevant educational oppor-
tunities. This intention was then either noticed by other people
or ignored. We then observed the degree to which participants
613
acted on their intention when such an educational opportunity
was actually provided.
Method
Law students at a German university were approached after a
lecture and asked to fill out a three-item commitment ques-
tionnaire adapted from Study 1 (7-point answer scales were used
this time). Only participants with high commitment to becoming
a successfuljurist (score 2: 5 on each item; ex = .76) were invited
to take part in the study (N = 32; 13 women, 19 men). The
students received €5 for their participation.
Participants were greeted individually and informed that the
experiment consisted of two independent pal1S. The first was
introduced as an assessment ofstudents' willingness to intensify
their study oflaw. Participants were asked to answer a four-page
questionnaire. On the first page, the following critical intention
item was presented: "I intend to make the best possible use of
educational opportunities in law." Participants responded on a
9-point scale ranging from 1, definitely not, to 9, definitely yes. In
the social-reality condition, after a participant completed the
questionnaire, the experimenter looked at this item and asked
whether the number circled on the answer scale was the one the
participant actually wanted to circle. Then the experimenter
dropped the questionnaire into a box. In the no-social-reality
condition, participants were simply asked to drop the ques-
tionnaire into a prepared box. As the questionnaire was anon-
ymous, it was clear to participants in this condition that the
experimenter would never be able to link the expressed inten-
tions to individual participants.
The experimenter then turned to the supposed second part of
the experiment, which concerned the development of a com-
puter-based study package for law students. New study mate-
rials were needed, so she had prepared 20 different criminal law
cases. Participants were asked to help her find which cases to
select for the package by trying hard to solve each case. The
students were given 45 min to work on the prepared cases (plus
the time needed to finish the case they were working on when the
time limit was reached), but they were told that they could finish
earlier if they wished. The time participants spent working on
these cases was used to assess how successfully participants
translated their intention into behavior.
Results and Discussion
Only participants who intended to make the best possible use of
educational opportunities (score > 5) were included (30 out of
the 32 original participants). Participants worked on the law
cases for less time ifthis behavioral intention was noticed than if
it was ignored by the experimenter (M = 41.52 min, SD = 4.42,
vs. M =45.65 min, SD =2.92), t(29) = 3.26,p < .01, d = 1.10.
Thus, law students-all of them highly committed to the identity
goal ofbeing ajurist-who had stated the behavioral intention to
take advantage of educational opportunities in law acted less
614
intensively on this intention when it was noticed by the exper-
imenter than when it was ignored.
STUDY 3
Studies 1 and 2 were both conducted with participants who were
highly committed to the identity goal in question (Le., psy-
chologist in Study 1, jurist in Study 2). As only individuals who
are highly committed to an identity goal can be expected to
experience self-completeness by accumulating identity symbols
(Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982), it follows that noncommitted
individuals should not show reduced intention enactment
whenever their identity-related behavioral intentions are no-
ticed by other people. Study 3 tested this hypothesis in two ways:
First, we compared students who wanted to become clinical
psychologists and those who wanted to become other types of
psychologists to determine whether they differed in how social
reality affected their enactment of behavioral intentions in the
service of the identity goal of clinical psychologist. Second, we
assessed the strength of participants' commitment to the identity
goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. We decided to assess
rather than manipulate strength ofidentity-goal commitment, as
strong identity commitments are not easily created on the spot,
but often take years to develop (Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998).
Method
Sixty-three psychology students at a German university (40
women, 23 men) participated for course credit. They were in-
formed that they would take part in two independent studies.
The first study was described as exploring students' willingness
to intensify their studies and involved answering several short
questionnaires. The first questionnaire assessed commitment to
the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. The first
item asked, "There are different fields of specialization in psy-
chology. Which one are you trying to pursue?" Response options
were "child," "industrial," "clinical," "experimental," "math-
ematical," and "undecided." Participants who answered "clin-
ical" were assumed to possess this identity goal, whereas the
others were not. We assumed that strongly committed individ-
uals would be willing to take on hardships (e.g., moving to an-
other town) en route to attaining an identity goal (Gollwitzer &
Kirchhof, 1998), and -the subsequent strength-of-commitment
item asked, "Would you switch universities in order to receive an
optimal education in your field of interest?" Participants re-
sponded to this question using a 5-point scale ranging from 1,
No, I would never do that, to 5, Yes, I would do that for sure.
Asecond questionnaire (one item per page) was introduced as
an inventory concerning ways of intensifying one's studies. The
critical behavioral-intention question was the only item on the
first page: "I intend to watch videotapes of therapy sessions to
learn more about therapeutic techniques." The response scale
for this item ranged from 1, definitely no, to 5, difinitely yes.
Upon completion of this questionnaire, participants in the so-
cial-reality condition were told that because of incomplete re-
sponses of prior paIticipants, only the question on the first page
of the questionnaire could be analyzed. The experimenter then
studied that page, tore it off, and handed the remaining pages
back to the participant. In the rw-social-reality condition, the
experimenter gave the same cover stOl:y about missing data, but
applied it to all the items on the questionnaire; then she returned
the entire questionnaire without looking at it.
Finally, the experiment,er introduced participants to the sec-
ond experimenter, who told them that she was trying to find out
the extent to which making eye contact affects the quality of an
interaction. She explained that she had prepared a video
showing a conversation between a therapist and a client. Par-
ticipants' task was to count the instances of making eye contact
and to rate the quality of interaction after each minute of the
conversation. Participants were told that the video lasted 40
min, but they should feel free to stop the video whenever they
wanted.
Results and Discussion
Thirty-one participants indicated that clinical psychology was
the field they wanted to pursue; 32 participants indicated other
fields. Because only participants with strong behavioral inten-
tions were of interest, we excluded 6 participants (2 aspiring
clinical psychologists and 4 no-goal participants) who scored 4
or less on the item "I intend to watch videotapes of therapy
sessions to learn more about therapeutic techniques." A 2
(clinical-psychologist identity goal: present vs. absent) X 2
(social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA on the amount of time
spent watching the therapy video yielded the predicted inter-
action effect, F(I, 53) = 3.95, p = .05, d = 0.95; only the
performance of aspiring clinical psychologists was affected by
social reality. As expected, aspiring clinical psychologists
whose behavioral intention to study videotaped therapy sessions
had been noticed by the experimenter invested less time in
watching the video than did aspiring clinical psychologists
whose intentions remained unnoticed (M = 29.51 min, SD =
6.72, vs. M =34.22 min, SD = 7.19), t(28) = 1.82,p =.04 (one-
tailed), d = 0.68. None ofthe other comparisons were significant
(all ts < 0.15, ps > .88, ds < 0.06).
In an additional analysis, we examined whether strength of
commitment to the identity goal, as measured by the mobility
item, moderated the effect of social reality among the aspiring
clinical psychologists (weak commitment:::; 3, strong commit-
ment ? 4). A 2 (strength of commitment: strong vs. weak) X 2
(social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA revealed a main ef-
fect ofsocial reality, F(I, 25) =5.50,p < .03, d = 0.94, that was
qualified by a significant interaction with strength of commit-
ment, F(I, 25) = 5.04,p < .04, d = 0.90. As expected (see Table
I), participants with a strong commitment to the identity goal of
becoming a clinical psychologist spent less time (more than 11
TABLE 1
Mean Time Spent Stndying the Videotaped Therapy Session (in
Minutes) in Study 3
Commitment to identity
goal of clinical psychologist
Strong
Weak
Social reality
Present Absent
27.95 (3.11)
30.68 (3.62)
39.17 (4.51)
30.90 (6.24)
Note. Standard deviations are given in parentheses.
min less) studying the videotaped therapy session if they were in
the social-reality condition than if they were in the no-social-
reality condition, t(ll) = 5.02, p < .001, d = 2.96; in contrast,
participants with weak commitment to the identity goal spent
close to the same amount oftime studying the therapy session no
matter whether they had or had not received social recognition
for their behavioral intention to study videotapes of therapy
sessions (there was only a 13.2-s difference !?etween groups; t =
0.09, p = .93, d = 0.04).
STUDY 4
seT (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982) postulates that public rec-
ognition of an identity-relevant symbol engenders a sense of
having attained the aspired-to identity goal (i.e., self-com-
pleteness). As Studies I through 3 suggest that identity-relevant
behavioral intentions do qualify as identity symbols, it follows
that social recognition of such intentions should also lead to a
heightened sense of completeness. In Study 4, we tested this
hypothesis with law students committed to becoming successful
jurists.
Method
Twenty-four first-year and second-year law students (I() women,
14 men) from a German university participated in this experi-
ment in exchange for €5. They were recruited at the end of a law
seminar.
Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants met the experi-
menter and two other students (actually confederates) who were
described as fellow law students. The experimenter explained to
each group that he was conducting a study on law students'
intentions to advance their careers and then asked participants
to complete a questionnaire. The questionnaire comprised two
items. The first item measured participants' commitment to
becoming a lawyer: "How important is a successful career in law
to you personally?" Responses were made on a 7-point scale
ranging from I, rwt at all important, to 7, very important. The
second item asked participants to write out their three most
important behavioral intentions with respect to the goal of be-
coming a successful jurist (e.g., "I will read law periodicals
regularly"). The importance of each intention also had to be
615
rated on a 7-point scale. Once participants had completed the
questionnaire, the experimenter asked them to either rate the
allractiveness of 10 pictures of landscapes (the rw-social-reality
condition) or tell him and the rest of the group what intentions
they had written down (the social-reality condition). In the so-
cial-reality condition, the participants always reported their
intentions first; the confederates then reported behavioral in-
tentions derived from prior participants.
Next, a second questionnaire was handed out. This ques-
tionnaire showed a l4,-cm line, above which were aligned five
pictures of the same member of the German Supreme Court
wearing the characteristic attire. The pictures varied in size,
ranging from small (1.5 X 1 cm) to large (3.6 x 2.4 cm). These
size gradations provided a visual analogue of the extent of
possessing the identity of being a jurist. Participants responded
to the item "How much do you feel like a jurist right now?" by
marking the respective point on the line. This self-assessment
manekin (SAM) rating procedure facilitates quick, nonreflective
self-evaluations (Bradley & Lang, 1994).
Results and Discussion
The commitment ofstudents in the social-reality condition (M =
6.00, SD = 1.49) did not differ from that of students in the no-
social-reality condition (M = 6.08, SD = 0.67), t(21) = 0.17,
P = .68, d = 0.07. Ratings of the importance of the listed in-
tentions were high and also did not differ between conditions
(M = 6.10, SD = 0.55, vs. M = 6.00, SD = 0.55), t(21) = 0.42,
p = .87, d = 0.18.
We first checked the validity of our measure of felt com-
pleteness. Indeed, more semesters of law education was asso-
ciated with stronger feelings ofcompleteness as a jurist (r = .54,
p = .005). In a hierarchical regression analysis, the social-
reality manipulation was significantly associated with feelings
of self-completeness even after number of semesters of law edu-
cation had been taken into account (ps = .29 and .45 for edu-
cation and condition, respectively, ps < .05), and inclusion of
this variable enhanced the fit of the model (/lR2
= .17, I:lF =
5.72, p < .03). As we predicted, participants felt closer to the
identity goal of becoming 'a jurist when their behavioral inten-
tions were recognized than when those intentions remained pri-
vate (Ms = 4.19 and 3.10, respectively).
CONCLUSION
When other people take notice of one's identity-relevant be-
havioral intentions, one's performance of the intended behaviors
is compromised. This effect occurs both when the intentions are
experimenter supplied and when they are self-generated, and is
observed in both immediate performance and performance
measured over a period of 1 week. It does not emerge when
people are not committed to the superordinate identity goal.
616
Other people's taking notice ofone's identity-relevant intentions
apparently engenders a premature sense of completeness re-
garding the identity goal.
Fishbein (1980) and Ajzen (1991) showed that the strength of
a behavioral intention determines how well it is translated into
behavior (Webb & Sheeran, 2006). Moreover, a substantial lit-
erature on moderators of intention-behavior relations (e.g.,
certainty, temporal stability) has developed (Cooke & Sheeran,
2004; Sheeran, 2002). Interestingly, however, previous research
has not explored what psychological processes may intervene
between the formation of a behavioral intention and its enact-
ment. The present studies indicate that the simple malleI' of
identity-relevant behavioral intentions becoming public un-
dermines the realization of those intentions.
The present research is also unique in its allempt to bring
back Lewin's work on intentions as it applies to the actual re-
alization of intentions. Most of the current research based on
Lewin's (1926) goal theory focuses on the activation level of the
mental representation of a person's intention (following Zei-
garnik, 1927). For instance, research has shown that the ac-
cessibility of goal-related constructs is increased as long as the
goal is active, that goal fulfillment inhibits accessibility of goal-
related constructs, and that these effects are proportional to the
strength of commitment to the goal (Forster, Liberman, & Hig-
gins, 2005; Goschke & Kuhl, 1993; Marsh, Hicks, & Bink,
1998).
Our findings are also important from an applied perspective.
Given that the effect is limited to committed individuals-those
who are most eager to reach their identity goals-an important
question is how these individuals might try to escape this effect.
Future research might address this question by exploring vari-
ous routes. First, might it suffice to increase the need for con-
sistency (Cialdini &'n'ost, 1998) by allending to relevant norms?
Or is it also necessary to increase perceived accountability
(Lerner & Tetlock, 1999) by considering relevant attributes of
the audience (e.g., power) or by specifying one's behavioral in-
tention in a particular way (e.g., spelling out specific frequency
or quality standards vs. stating only that one wants to do one's
best; Locke & Latham, 2002) so that the audience can more
easily check on its enactment? Second, might it also be effective
for one to furnish a behavioral intention with a plan for how to
enact it-that is, to form a corresponding implementation in-
tention (e.g., "If situation X is encountered, then I will perform
the intended behavior Y"; Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer &
Sheeran, 2006)? As such if-then plans delegate the control of a
person's behavior to situational cues, the intended behavior
should be executed when the critical cue arises, whether or not
the expression of the behavioral intention had been acknowl-
edged by other people. Third, recent research by Fishbach and
her colleagues (Fishbach & Dhar, 2005; Koo & Fishbach, 2008)
suggests that interpreting a behavioral performance in terms of
indicating commitment to a goal enhances further goal striving,
whereas conceiving of a performance in terms of progress toward
a goal reduces further goal striving. This implies that a behav-
ioral intention worded to indicate a strong commitment to the
identity goal (e.g., "1 want to write a paper to become a great
scientist") should be less negatively affected by social reality
than a behavioral intention that implies progress toward the
identity goal (e.g., "1 intend to write a paper, as is done by great
scientists").
Finally, from a goal-systems (Kruglanski et aI., 2002) or goal-
hierarchy (Vallacher & Wegner, 1987) perspective on action
control, it stands to reason that any striving for goals-and not
just identity goals-that can be attained by various behavioral
routes (means) is vulnerable to the negative effects of social
reality on the enactment of behavioral intentions. If a person is
highly committed to a superordinate goal, and if public recog-
nition ofa behavioral intention specifying the use ofone route to
the goal engenders a sense of goal attainment, then the enact-
ment ofthis very intention should be hampered. Recent research
by Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang (2006) is in line with this rea-
soning, showing that success on a subgoal (e.g., eating healthy
meals) in the service of a superordinate goal (i.e., keeping in
shape) reduces striving for alternative subgoals (e.g., going to
the gym).
REFERENCES
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes, 50,179-211.
Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-
assessment manekin and the semantic differentia!' Journal of
Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49-59.
Brunstein, J.C., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (1996). Effects of failure on sub-
sequent pelformance: The impOltance of self-defining goals.
Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 70,395-407.
Cialdini, R.B., & Trost, M.R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms,
conformity, and compliance. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G.
Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp.
151-192). Boston: McGraw-Hil!.
Cialdini, R.B., Wosinka, W., Barrell, D.W., Butner, J., & Gornik-
Durose, M. (1999). Compliance with a request in two cultures:
The differential influence of social proof and commitment/con-
sistency on collectivists and individualists. Personality and So-
cial Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1242-1253.
Cooke, R, & Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and
cognition-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of properties of
variables from the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of
Social Psychology, 43, 159-186.
Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The
liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of
Consumer Research, 32, 370-377.
Fishbach, A., Dhar, R, & Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substitutes or
complements: The role of goal accessibility. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 91, 232-242.
Fishbein, M. (1980). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications
and implications. In H. Howe & M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska sym-
posium on motivation (Vo!. 27, pp. 65-116). Lincoln: University
of Nebraska Press.
Forster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E.T. (2005). Accessibility from
active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 41,220-239.
Gollwitzer, P.M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social re-
ality of self-symbolizing. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Private selfand
public self (pp. 143-159). New York: Springer.
Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of
simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493-503.
Gollwitzer, P.M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity.
In J. Heckhausen & C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-
regulation across the life span (pp. 389-423). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Gollwitzer, P.M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions
and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes.
In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vo!. 38, pp. 69-119). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Gollwitzer, P.M., & Wicklund, RA. (1985). Self-symbolizing and the
neglect of others' perspectives. Journal ofPersonality and Social
Psychology, 48, 702-715.
Gollwitzer, P.M., Wicklund, RA., & Hilton, J.1. (1982). Admission of
failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory.
Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 43, 358-371.
Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions:
Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211-1226.
Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B.J., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009).
Symbolic self-completion in academia: Evidence from depart-
ment web pages and email signature files. European Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 311-316.
Koo, M., & Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation:
How (un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 183-195.
Kruglanski, A.W., Shah, J.Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R, Chun, W.Y.,
& Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M.E
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psyclwlogy (Vo!. 34,
pp. 331-378). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Ledgerwood, A., Liviatan, I., & Carnevale, P.J. (2007). Gl'oup-identity
completion and the symbolic value of property. Psychological
Science, 18, 873-878.
Lerner, J.S., & Tetlock, P.E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of ac-
countability. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 255-275.
Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille und Bediirfnis. Psychologische For-
schung, 7,330-385.
Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful
theory of goal selling and task motivation. American Psychologist,
57,705-717.
Mahler, W. (1933). Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen Realitiitsgrades.
Psychologische Forschung, 18, 27-89.
Marsh, RI.., Hicks, J.1., & Bink, M.1. (1998). Activation of com-
pleted, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
24, 350-361.
Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Hand-
lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302-379.
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and
empirical review. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European
review of social psychology (Vo!. 12, pp. 1-36). Chichester, En-
gland: Wiley.
Staats, H., Harland, E, & Wilke, H.A. (2004). Effecting durable
change: A team approach to improve environmental behavior in
the household. Environment and Behavior, 36, 341-367.
617
Vallacher, R.R., & Wegner, D.M. (1987). What do people think they
are doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psycho-
logical Review, 94, 3-15.
Wehh, T.L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing hehavioral
intentions engender hehavior change? A meta-analysis of the
experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249-268.
618
Wicklund, R.A., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlhaum.
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Hand-
lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1-85.

More Related Content

What's hot

Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology PaperPersonality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology PaperJennifer Burke
 
THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2
THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2
THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2ANGELA RESCHKE
 
Assessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IAT
Assessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IATAssessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IAT
Assessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IATLouis Oberdiear
 
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitNarcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitamit657720
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016Daniel Horsley
 
Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015
Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015
Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015Neel Sitaramya
 
Thinking Before You Speak Report
Thinking Before You Speak ReportThinking Before You Speak Report
Thinking Before You Speak ReportSarah Robinson
 
Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...
Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...
Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...Kayla Brown
 
Imagined interaction theory
Imagined interaction theoryImagined interaction theory
Imagined interaction theoryJames Honeycutt
 
Religiosity and depression in college students
Religiosity and depression in college studentsReligiosity and depression in college students
Religiosity and depression in college studentsDevon Berry
 

What's hot (13)

FINAL thesis 4.28
FINAL thesis 4.28FINAL thesis 4.28
FINAL thesis 4.28
 
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology PaperPersonality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
 
THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2
THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2
THESIS_Final_Revisions_2.5.2010_2
 
Assessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IAT
Assessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IATAssessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IAT
Assessing the Construct Validity of an Emotional Intelligence IAT
 
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitNarcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016
 
Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015
Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015
Lab Paper ~ Winter 2015
 
Thinking Before You Speak Report
Thinking Before You Speak ReportThinking Before You Speak Report
Thinking Before You Speak Report
 
Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...
Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...
Effects of valuing an individual’s wellbeing_ evoking empathy and motivating ...
 
Imagined interaction theory
Imagined interaction theoryImagined interaction theory
Imagined interaction theory
 
Religiosity and depression in college students
Religiosity and depression in college studentsReligiosity and depression in college students
Religiosity and depression in college students
 
I Nudge You
I Nudge YouI Nudge You
I Nudge You
 
Helping behaviour
Helping behaviourHelping behaviour
Helping behaviour
 

Similar to When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.

Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Ashley Fisher
 
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsStrengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsSandra Arveseth
 
Engagement parentingsct munno
Engagement parentingsct munnoEngagement parentingsct munno
Engagement parentingsct munnoGreg Munno
 
Standardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School SystemsStandardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School SystemsTiffany Sandoval
 
2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk
2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk
2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public TalkJohn C. Besley
 
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docxvickeryr87
 
Cross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural PsychologyCross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural PsychologyMonica Carter
 
Running head STATISTICS .docx
Running head STATISTICS                                          .docxRunning head STATISTICS                                          .docx
Running head STATISTICS .docxtoltonkendal
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxkarlhennesey
 
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docxReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docxaudeleypearl
 
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass media
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass mediaBjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass media
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass mediaRai University
 
Online assignment
Online assignmentOnline assignment
Online assignmentvarshapg
 
Tiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research ResultTiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research Resultkudozprototype
 

Similar to When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE. (20)

Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
 
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsStrengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
 
Engagement parentingsct munno
Engagement parentingsct munnoEngagement parentingsct munno
Engagement parentingsct munno
 
Standardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School SystemsStandardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School Systems
 
2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk
2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk
2021 - Communicating Astronomy with the Public Talk
 
Appleby college research
Appleby college researchAppleby college research
Appleby college research
 
O Behave! Issue 25
O Behave! Issue 25O Behave! Issue 25
O Behave! Issue 25
 
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
 
Cross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural PsychologyCross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural Psychology
 
Running head STATISTICS .docx
Running head STATISTICS                                          .docxRunning head STATISTICS                                          .docx
Running head STATISTICS .docx
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
 
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docxReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
 
Appleby College Research
Appleby College ResearchAppleby College Research
Appleby College Research
 
Is Negative Campaigning Effective?
Is Negative Campaigning Effective?Is Negative Campaigning Effective?
Is Negative Campaigning Effective?
 
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass media
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass mediaBjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass media
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii, effect of mass media
 
Proposal Neg Campaigns
Proposal Neg CampaignsProposal Neg Campaigns
Proposal Neg Campaigns
 
Previously,
Previously,Previously,
Previously,
 
Final Paper-FINAL PRODUCT
Final Paper-FINAL PRODUCTFinal Paper-FINAL PRODUCT
Final Paper-FINAL PRODUCT
 
Online assignment
Online assignmentOnline assignment
Online assignment
 
Tiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research ResultTiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research Result
 

Recently uploaded

Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh 9711199012
Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh  9711199012Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh  9711199012
Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh 9711199012sapnasaifi408
 
Ioannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdf
Ioannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdfIoannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdf
Ioannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdfjtzach
 
VIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Bhilai
VIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service BhilaiVIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Bhilai
VIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service BhilaiSuhani Kapoor
 
Preventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptx
Preventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptxPreventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptx
Preventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptxGry Tina Tinde
 
Sonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call Girls
Sonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call GirlsSonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call Girls
Sonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call GirlsNiya Khan
 
Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...
Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...
Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...Niya Khan
 
Notes of bca Question paper for exams and tests
Notes of bca Question paper for exams and testsNotes of bca Question paper for exams and tests
Notes of bca Question paper for exams and testspriyanshukumar97908
 
办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样
办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样
办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样umasea
 
VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...Suhani Kapoor
 
The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdf
The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdfThe Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdf
The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdftheknowledgereview1
 
Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...
Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...
Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...shivangimorya083
 
Internshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University Certificate
Internshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University CertificateInternshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University Certificate
Internshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University CertificateSoham Mondal
 
VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...Suhani Kapoor
 
Employee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India Research
Employee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India ResearchEmployee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India Research
Employee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India ResearchSoham Mondal
 
Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...
Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...
Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...Suhani Kapoor
 
VIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Cuttack
VIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service CuttackVIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Cuttack
VIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service CuttackSuhani Kapoor
 
NPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdf
NPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdfNPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdf
NPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdfDivyeshPatel234692
 
内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士
内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士
内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士obuhobo
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh 9711199012
Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh  9711199012Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh  9711199012
Call Girl in Low Price Delhi Punjabi Bagh 9711199012
 
Call Girls In Prashant Vihar꧁❤ 🔝 9953056974🔝❤꧂ Escort ServiCe
Call Girls In Prashant Vihar꧁❤ 🔝 9953056974🔝❤꧂ Escort ServiCeCall Girls In Prashant Vihar꧁❤ 🔝 9953056974🔝❤꧂ Escort ServiCe
Call Girls In Prashant Vihar꧁❤ 🔝 9953056974🔝❤꧂ Escort ServiCe
 
Ioannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdf
Ioannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdfIoannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdf
Ioannis Tzachristas Self-Presentation for MBA.pdf
 
VIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Bhilai
VIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service BhilaiVIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Bhilai
VIP Call Girl Bhilai Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Bhilai
 
Preventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptx
Preventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptxPreventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptx
Preventing and ending sexual harassment in the workplace.pptx
 
Sonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call Girls
Sonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call GirlsSonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call Girls
Sonam +91-9537192988-Mind-blowing skills and techniques of Ahmedabad Call Girls
 
Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...
Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...
Neha +91-9537192988-Friendly Ahmedabad Call Girls has Complete Authority for ...
 
Notes of bca Question paper for exams and tests
Notes of bca Question paper for exams and testsNotes of bca Question paper for exams and tests
Notes of bca Question paper for exams and tests
 
办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样
办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样
办理学位证(纽伦堡大学文凭证书)纽伦堡大学毕业证成绩单原版一模一样
 
VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Jamshedpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
 
The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdf
The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdfThe Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdf
The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on Education.pdf
 
Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...
Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...
Delhi Call Girls Preet Vihar 9711199171 ☎✔👌✔ Whatsapp Body to body massage wi...
 
Internshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University Certificate
Internshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University CertificateInternshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University Certificate
Internshala Student Partner 6.0 Jadavpur University Certificate
 
VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
VIP Call Girls Service Saharanpur Aishwarya 8250192130 Independent Escort Ser...
 
Employee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India Research
Employee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India ResearchEmployee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India Research
Employee of the Month - Samsung Semiconductor India Research
 
Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...
Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...
Low Rate Call Girls Gorakhpur Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Gor...
 
VIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Cuttack
VIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service CuttackVIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Cuttack
VIP Call Girl Cuttack Aashi 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Cuttack
 
NPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdf
NPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdfNPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdf
NPPE STUDY GUIDE - NOV2021_study_104040.pdf
 
内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士
内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士
内布拉斯加大学林肯分校毕业证录取书( 退学 )学位证书硕士
 
FULL ENJOY Call Girls In Gautam Nagar (Delhi) Call Us 9953056974
FULL ENJOY Call Girls In Gautam Nagar (Delhi) Call Us 9953056974FULL ENJOY Call Girls In Gautam Nagar (Delhi) Call Us 9953056974
FULL ENJOY Call Girls In Gautam Nagar (Delhi) Call Us 9953056974
 

When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.

  • 1. When Intentions Go Public Does Social Reality Widen the Intention-Behavior Gap? Peter M. Gollwitzer,1,2 Paschal Sheeran,3 Verena Michalski,2 and Andrea E. Seifert2 1New York University, 2UniversitdJ Konstanz, and 3University ofSheffield ABSTRACT-Based on Lewiniangoal theory in general and self-completion theory in particular, four experiments ex- amined the implications of other people taking notice of one~ identity-related behavioral intentions (e.g., the in- tention to read law periodicals regularly to reach the identity goal of becoming a lawyer). Identity-related be- havioral intentions that had been noticed by other people were translated into action less intensively than those that had been ignored (Studies 1-3). This effect was evident in the field (persistent striving over 1 week~time; Study 1) and in the laboratory (jumping on opportunities to act; Studies 2 and 3), and it held among participants with strong but not weak commitment to the identitygoal (Study 3). Study 4 showed, in addition, that when other people take notice of an individual~ identity-related behavioral intention, this gives the individual a premature sense of possessing the aspired-to identity. Are scientists more likely to write papers if they tell colleagues about this intention than if they keep the intention private? It is commonly assumed that whenever people make their intentions public, the behavioral impact of these intentions is enhanced (e.g., Staats, Harland, & Wilke, 2004). These effects are pos- tulated to be a consequence of multiple processes. Research on persuasion techniques points to one of these processes (Cialdini & Trost, 1998). It is argued that a publicly stated behavioral intention commits the individual to a certain self-view (e.g., "I am a productive person") with which the person then acts con- sistently. Indeed, individuals with a higher need for consistency show stronger public-commitment effects (Cialdini, Wosinka, Barrett, Butner, & Gornik-Durose, 1999). The second process is referred to as accountability (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999). Making intentions public is said to make a person accountable to the addressed audience, and research has shown that various ac- Address correspondence to Peter M. Gollwitzer, New York Univer- sity, Psychology Department, 6 Washington Place, 7th Floor, New York, NY 10003, e-mail: peter.gollwitzer@nyu.edu. 612 countability-related features of the audience (e.g., competence, power) and the individual (e.g., identifiability, expectations of having to explain oneself) affect the strength of public-com- mitment effects. Both ofthese lines ofresearch focus on intentions in which the specified behavior is a desired outcome in and of itself. Lewin (1926) and his colleagues (e.g., Mahler, 1935; Ovsiankina, 1928), however, argued that people often construe behavioral intentions in more general terms, thus allowing substitution of means for attainment. For instance, consider a student who has started an assigned math task with the intention to successfully solve the required addition problems. During the process, this student may construe the intention as being to demonstrate mathematical skills, and this conceptually broader intention may also be reached by solving subtraction problems (i.e., by substitute activities). Ovsiankina and Mahler observed that a substitute activity engenders a sense of having reached the conceptually broader intention, given that performance of the substitute activity has been witnessed by other people (i.e., has become a social reality). On the basis of this line of thought- which we explicate in the framework of self-completion theory (SCT; Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998; Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982)-we propose that social recognition of an identity-rele- vant behavioral intention may have negative effects on its en- actment. SCT proposes that people who are committed to identity goals (e.g., becoming a good parent, scientist, or craftsperson) can undertake a variety of activities to claim goal attainment. For a scientist, such activities, or identity symbols, include engaging in professional duties (e.g., giving lectures), making positive self-descriptions (e.g., "I discovered a new principle!"), exerting identity-relevant social influence (e.g., advising students), and acquiring skills and tools that facilitate striving for the identity goal (e.g., programming skills, computers). However, failing to perform an identity-relevant activity or facing the lack of an identity symbol produces a state ofincompleteness (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982). To restore completeness, the individual makes efforts to acquire alternative identity symbols (e.g., de- scribing oneself as having the required personality attributes: First publ. in: Psychological Science 20 (2009), 5, pp. 612-618 Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URN: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-86958 URL: http://kops.ub.uni-konstanz.de/volltexte/2009/8695/
  • 2. Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; engaging in identity-relevant activities: Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996; showing off relevant status symbols: Harmon-Jones, Schmeichel, & Harmon-Jones, 2009). Using opportunities to affirm one's general self-integrity or to bolster one's self-esteem is not sufficient to offset incom- pleteness regarding an identity goal; rather, it is necessary to acquire specific identity symbols (Ledgerwood, Liviatan, & Carnevale, 2007). SCT research has also shown that an individual reaches a higher level of completeness when his or her identity-relevant activities are noticed by a social audience (Gollwitzer, 1986). Moreover, research has shown that incomplete individuals are more concerned with finding an audience for their identity strivings, compared with complete individuals (Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996). Positive self-descriptions made in public qualify as powerful identity symbols (Gollwitzer, Wicklund, & Hilton, 1982), and having an audience for behavioral intentions that specify the successful performance of an identity-relevant activity should have the same symbolic impact. The implication is that when other people take notice ofa stated identity-relevant behavioral intention, this should engender completeness re- garding the superordinate identity goal, and thus reaching the identity goal by actually performing the intended behavior should become less necessary. In other words, people should be less likely to translate their identity-relevant behavioral inten- tions into action when other people have taken notice of those intentions. We conducted four experiments entailing a variety of identity goals and behavioral intentions to test this hypothesis. STUDY 1 In Study 1, we asked college students committed to becoming a psychologist to form identity-relevant studying intentions. After either taking notice of these intentions or ignoring them, we assessed how effectively the students enacted their intentions over the subsequent week. Method Forty-nine psychology students (38 women, 11 men) at a Ger- man university were recruited after introductory lectures. Par- ticipants were informed that they would take part in a survey about the study intentions of first-year psychology students. A first questionnaire assessed commitment to becoming a psy- chologist using the following items: • "How imp011ant is it for you to find a psychology-related job?" • "Suppose that you could not finish your studies of psychology successfully. How much would that bother you?" • "How happy would you be in a job that is not related to psychology?" (reverse-coded) Participants responded to these items on 9-point scales ranging from 1, not at all, to 9, very much. Responses were averaged to form a scale (ex = .80). Next, participants were asked to write down their two most important study intentions for the forth- coming week (e.g., "I will take my reading assignments more seriously," "I intend to study more statistics"). In the social-reality condition, the experimenter read through each participant's reported intentions, presumably to ensure that the participant had understood the instructions. In the no- social-reality condition, participants were told that the page of the questionnaire on which participants had written down their behavioral intentions had been wrongly included in the study, and that this page would be discarded (i.e., the students' in- tentions remained unnoticed). One week later, all participants were sent a second ques- tionnaire via e-mail. They had to first write down the two be- havioral intentions they had listed the previous week. Then, they indicated on exactly which days of the past week they had acted on each intention. Finally, participants were asked to bring th~ir completed questionnaire to the experimenter's office, where they received payment (€5) or course credit. Results and Discussion Overall, participants were highly committed to the identity goal (range = 6-9; M = 7.32, SD = 1.64), and there was no significant difference in commitment scores between the social-reality condition (M = 7.21, SD = 1.64) and the no- social-reality condition (M = 7.43, SD = 1.08), F < 1,p > .32, d < 0.16. We analyzed the number of days participants acted on their intentions in a 2 x 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with social reality as the between-participants factor and the two specified behavioral intentions as the within-participants factor. Results showed a significant main effect of social reality, F(l, 47) = 4.38, p < .05, d = 0.60; participants whose intentions remained private acted on their intentions on more days of the week (M = 2.70, SD = 1.83) than did participants whose in- tentions were noticed (M = 1.92, SD = 0.78). No other effects were significant. An apparent strength of Study 1 is that the observed negative effects of having one's intentions noticed cannot easily be at- tributed to emotions that might accrue from the experimenter's behavior (e.g., pride). Such affective responses should vanish quickly, whereas the effects on behavioral enactment in Study 1 were evident over a period of 1 week. An obvious weakness of this study, however, is that enactment of intentions was assessed via self-report. STUDY 2 In Study 2, therefore, we observed actual enactment of inten- tions. Participants were law students who formed the behavioral intention to make use of identity-relevant educational oppor- tunities. This intention was then either noticed by other people or ignored. We then observed the degree to which participants 613
  • 3. acted on their intention when such an educational opportunity was actually provided. Method Law students at a German university were approached after a lecture and asked to fill out a three-item commitment ques- tionnaire adapted from Study 1 (7-point answer scales were used this time). Only participants with high commitment to becoming a successfuljurist (score 2: 5 on each item; ex = .76) were invited to take part in the study (N = 32; 13 women, 19 men). The students received €5 for their participation. Participants were greeted individually and informed that the experiment consisted of two independent pal1S. The first was introduced as an assessment ofstudents' willingness to intensify their study oflaw. Participants were asked to answer a four-page questionnaire. On the first page, the following critical intention item was presented: "I intend to make the best possible use of educational opportunities in law." Participants responded on a 9-point scale ranging from 1, definitely not, to 9, definitely yes. In the social-reality condition, after a participant completed the questionnaire, the experimenter looked at this item and asked whether the number circled on the answer scale was the one the participant actually wanted to circle. Then the experimenter dropped the questionnaire into a box. In the no-social-reality condition, participants were simply asked to drop the ques- tionnaire into a prepared box. As the questionnaire was anon- ymous, it was clear to participants in this condition that the experimenter would never be able to link the expressed inten- tions to individual participants. The experimenter then turned to the supposed second part of the experiment, which concerned the development of a com- puter-based study package for law students. New study mate- rials were needed, so she had prepared 20 different criminal law cases. Participants were asked to help her find which cases to select for the package by trying hard to solve each case. The students were given 45 min to work on the prepared cases (plus the time needed to finish the case they were working on when the time limit was reached), but they were told that they could finish earlier if they wished. The time participants spent working on these cases was used to assess how successfully participants translated their intention into behavior. Results and Discussion Only participants who intended to make the best possible use of educational opportunities (score > 5) were included (30 out of the 32 original participants). Participants worked on the law cases for less time ifthis behavioral intention was noticed than if it was ignored by the experimenter (M = 41.52 min, SD = 4.42, vs. M =45.65 min, SD =2.92), t(29) = 3.26,p < .01, d = 1.10. Thus, law students-all of them highly committed to the identity goal ofbeing ajurist-who had stated the behavioral intention to take advantage of educational opportunities in law acted less 614 intensively on this intention when it was noticed by the exper- imenter than when it was ignored. STUDY 3 Studies 1 and 2 were both conducted with participants who were highly committed to the identity goal in question (Le., psy- chologist in Study 1, jurist in Study 2). As only individuals who are highly committed to an identity goal can be expected to experience self-completeness by accumulating identity symbols (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982), it follows that noncommitted individuals should not show reduced intention enactment whenever their identity-related behavioral intentions are no- ticed by other people. Study 3 tested this hypothesis in two ways: First, we compared students who wanted to become clinical psychologists and those who wanted to become other types of psychologists to determine whether they differed in how social reality affected their enactment of behavioral intentions in the service of the identity goal of clinical psychologist. Second, we assessed the strength of participants' commitment to the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. We decided to assess rather than manipulate strength ofidentity-goal commitment, as strong identity commitments are not easily created on the spot, but often take years to develop (Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998). Method Sixty-three psychology students at a German university (40 women, 23 men) participated for course credit. They were in- formed that they would take part in two independent studies. The first study was described as exploring students' willingness to intensify their studies and involved answering several short questionnaires. The first questionnaire assessed commitment to the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. The first item asked, "There are different fields of specialization in psy- chology. Which one are you trying to pursue?" Response options were "child," "industrial," "clinical," "experimental," "math- ematical," and "undecided." Participants who answered "clin- ical" were assumed to possess this identity goal, whereas the others were not. We assumed that strongly committed individ- uals would be willing to take on hardships (e.g., moving to an- other town) en route to attaining an identity goal (Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998), and -the subsequent strength-of-commitment item asked, "Would you switch universities in order to receive an optimal education in your field of interest?" Participants re- sponded to this question using a 5-point scale ranging from 1, No, I would never do that, to 5, Yes, I would do that for sure. Asecond questionnaire (one item per page) was introduced as an inventory concerning ways of intensifying one's studies. The critical behavioral-intention question was the only item on the first page: "I intend to watch videotapes of therapy sessions to learn more about therapeutic techniques." The response scale for this item ranged from 1, definitely no, to 5, difinitely yes.
  • 4. Upon completion of this questionnaire, participants in the so- cial-reality condition were told that because of incomplete re- sponses of prior paIticipants, only the question on the first page of the questionnaire could be analyzed. The experimenter then studied that page, tore it off, and handed the remaining pages back to the participant. In the rw-social-reality condition, the experimenter gave the same cover stOl:y about missing data, but applied it to all the items on the questionnaire; then she returned the entire questionnaire without looking at it. Finally, the experiment,er introduced participants to the sec- ond experimenter, who told them that she was trying to find out the extent to which making eye contact affects the quality of an interaction. She explained that she had prepared a video showing a conversation between a therapist and a client. Par- ticipants' task was to count the instances of making eye contact and to rate the quality of interaction after each minute of the conversation. Participants were told that the video lasted 40 min, but they should feel free to stop the video whenever they wanted. Results and Discussion Thirty-one participants indicated that clinical psychology was the field they wanted to pursue; 32 participants indicated other fields. Because only participants with strong behavioral inten- tions were of interest, we excluded 6 participants (2 aspiring clinical psychologists and 4 no-goal participants) who scored 4 or less on the item "I intend to watch videotapes of therapy sessions to learn more about therapeutic techniques." A 2 (clinical-psychologist identity goal: present vs. absent) X 2 (social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA on the amount of time spent watching the therapy video yielded the predicted inter- action effect, F(I, 53) = 3.95, p = .05, d = 0.95; only the performance of aspiring clinical psychologists was affected by social reality. As expected, aspiring clinical psychologists whose behavioral intention to study videotaped therapy sessions had been noticed by the experimenter invested less time in watching the video than did aspiring clinical psychologists whose intentions remained unnoticed (M = 29.51 min, SD = 6.72, vs. M =34.22 min, SD = 7.19), t(28) = 1.82,p =.04 (one- tailed), d = 0.68. None ofthe other comparisons were significant (all ts < 0.15, ps > .88, ds < 0.06). In an additional analysis, we examined whether strength of commitment to the identity goal, as measured by the mobility item, moderated the effect of social reality among the aspiring clinical psychologists (weak commitment:::; 3, strong commit- ment ? 4). A 2 (strength of commitment: strong vs. weak) X 2 (social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA revealed a main ef- fect ofsocial reality, F(I, 25) =5.50,p < .03, d = 0.94, that was qualified by a significant interaction with strength of commit- ment, F(I, 25) = 5.04,p < .04, d = 0.90. As expected (see Table I), participants with a strong commitment to the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist spent less time (more than 11 TABLE 1 Mean Time Spent Stndying the Videotaped Therapy Session (in Minutes) in Study 3 Commitment to identity goal of clinical psychologist Strong Weak Social reality Present Absent 27.95 (3.11) 30.68 (3.62) 39.17 (4.51) 30.90 (6.24) Note. Standard deviations are given in parentheses. min less) studying the videotaped therapy session if they were in the social-reality condition than if they were in the no-social- reality condition, t(ll) = 5.02, p < .001, d = 2.96; in contrast, participants with weak commitment to the identity goal spent close to the same amount oftime studying the therapy session no matter whether they had or had not received social recognition for their behavioral intention to study videotapes of therapy sessions (there was only a 13.2-s difference !?etween groups; t = 0.09, p = .93, d = 0.04). STUDY 4 seT (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982) postulates that public rec- ognition of an identity-relevant symbol engenders a sense of having attained the aspired-to identity goal (i.e., self-com- pleteness). As Studies I through 3 suggest that identity-relevant behavioral intentions do qualify as identity symbols, it follows that social recognition of such intentions should also lead to a heightened sense of completeness. In Study 4, we tested this hypothesis with law students committed to becoming successful jurists. Method Twenty-four first-year and second-year law students (I() women, 14 men) from a German university participated in this experi- ment in exchange for €5. They were recruited at the end of a law seminar. Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants met the experi- menter and two other students (actually confederates) who were described as fellow law students. The experimenter explained to each group that he was conducting a study on law students' intentions to advance their careers and then asked participants to complete a questionnaire. The questionnaire comprised two items. The first item measured participants' commitment to becoming a lawyer: "How important is a successful career in law to you personally?" Responses were made on a 7-point scale ranging from I, rwt at all important, to 7, very important. The second item asked participants to write out their three most important behavioral intentions with respect to the goal of be- coming a successful jurist (e.g., "I will read law periodicals regularly"). The importance of each intention also had to be 615
  • 5. rated on a 7-point scale. Once participants had completed the questionnaire, the experimenter asked them to either rate the allractiveness of 10 pictures of landscapes (the rw-social-reality condition) or tell him and the rest of the group what intentions they had written down (the social-reality condition). In the so- cial-reality condition, the participants always reported their intentions first; the confederates then reported behavioral in- tentions derived from prior participants. Next, a second questionnaire was handed out. This ques- tionnaire showed a l4,-cm line, above which were aligned five pictures of the same member of the German Supreme Court wearing the characteristic attire. The pictures varied in size, ranging from small (1.5 X 1 cm) to large (3.6 x 2.4 cm). These size gradations provided a visual analogue of the extent of possessing the identity of being a jurist. Participants responded to the item "How much do you feel like a jurist right now?" by marking the respective point on the line. This self-assessment manekin (SAM) rating procedure facilitates quick, nonreflective self-evaluations (Bradley & Lang, 1994). Results and Discussion The commitment ofstudents in the social-reality condition (M = 6.00, SD = 1.49) did not differ from that of students in the no- social-reality condition (M = 6.08, SD = 0.67), t(21) = 0.17, P = .68, d = 0.07. Ratings of the importance of the listed in- tentions were high and also did not differ between conditions (M = 6.10, SD = 0.55, vs. M = 6.00, SD = 0.55), t(21) = 0.42, p = .87, d = 0.18. We first checked the validity of our measure of felt com- pleteness. Indeed, more semesters of law education was asso- ciated with stronger feelings ofcompleteness as a jurist (r = .54, p = .005). In a hierarchical regression analysis, the social- reality manipulation was significantly associated with feelings of self-completeness even after number of semesters of law edu- cation had been taken into account (ps = .29 and .45 for edu- cation and condition, respectively, ps < .05), and inclusion of this variable enhanced the fit of the model (/lR2 = .17, I:lF = 5.72, p < .03). As we predicted, participants felt closer to the identity goal of becoming 'a jurist when their behavioral inten- tions were recognized than when those intentions remained pri- vate (Ms = 4.19 and 3.10, respectively). CONCLUSION When other people take notice of one's identity-relevant be- havioral intentions, one's performance of the intended behaviors is compromised. This effect occurs both when the intentions are experimenter supplied and when they are self-generated, and is observed in both immediate performance and performance measured over a period of 1 week. It does not emerge when people are not committed to the superordinate identity goal. 616 Other people's taking notice ofone's identity-relevant intentions apparently engenders a premature sense of completeness re- garding the identity goal. Fishbein (1980) and Ajzen (1991) showed that the strength of a behavioral intention determines how well it is translated into behavior (Webb & Sheeran, 2006). Moreover, a substantial lit- erature on moderators of intention-behavior relations (e.g., certainty, temporal stability) has developed (Cooke & Sheeran, 2004; Sheeran, 2002). Interestingly, however, previous research has not explored what psychological processes may intervene between the formation of a behavioral intention and its enact- ment. The present studies indicate that the simple malleI' of identity-relevant behavioral intentions becoming public un- dermines the realization of those intentions. The present research is also unique in its allempt to bring back Lewin's work on intentions as it applies to the actual re- alization of intentions. Most of the current research based on Lewin's (1926) goal theory focuses on the activation level of the mental representation of a person's intention (following Zei- garnik, 1927). For instance, research has shown that the ac- cessibility of goal-related constructs is increased as long as the goal is active, that goal fulfillment inhibits accessibility of goal- related constructs, and that these effects are proportional to the strength of commitment to the goal (Forster, Liberman, & Hig- gins, 2005; Goschke & Kuhl, 1993; Marsh, Hicks, & Bink, 1998). Our findings are also important from an applied perspective. Given that the effect is limited to committed individuals-those who are most eager to reach their identity goals-an important question is how these individuals might try to escape this effect. Future research might address this question by exploring vari- ous routes. First, might it suffice to increase the need for con- sistency (Cialdini &'n'ost, 1998) by allending to relevant norms? Or is it also necessary to increase perceived accountability (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999) by considering relevant attributes of the audience (e.g., power) or by specifying one's behavioral in- tention in a particular way (e.g., spelling out specific frequency or quality standards vs. stating only that one wants to do one's best; Locke & Latham, 2002) so that the audience can more easily check on its enactment? Second, might it also be effective for one to furnish a behavioral intention with a plan for how to enact it-that is, to form a corresponding implementation in- tention (e.g., "If situation X is encountered, then I will perform the intended behavior Y"; Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006)? As such if-then plans delegate the control of a person's behavior to situational cues, the intended behavior should be executed when the critical cue arises, whether or not the expression of the behavioral intention had been acknowl- edged by other people. Third, recent research by Fishbach and her colleagues (Fishbach & Dhar, 2005; Koo & Fishbach, 2008) suggests that interpreting a behavioral performance in terms of indicating commitment to a goal enhances further goal striving, whereas conceiving of a performance in terms of progress toward
  • 6. a goal reduces further goal striving. This implies that a behav- ioral intention worded to indicate a strong commitment to the identity goal (e.g., "1 want to write a paper to become a great scientist") should be less negatively affected by social reality than a behavioral intention that implies progress toward the identity goal (e.g., "1 intend to write a paper, as is done by great scientists"). Finally, from a goal-systems (Kruglanski et aI., 2002) or goal- hierarchy (Vallacher & Wegner, 1987) perspective on action control, it stands to reason that any striving for goals-and not just identity goals-that can be attained by various behavioral routes (means) is vulnerable to the negative effects of social reality on the enactment of behavioral intentions. If a person is highly committed to a superordinate goal, and if public recog- nition ofa behavioral intention specifying the use ofone route to the goal engenders a sense of goal attainment, then the enact- ment ofthis very intention should be hampered. Recent research by Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang (2006) is in line with this rea- soning, showing that success on a subgoal (e.g., eating healthy meals) in the service of a superordinate goal (i.e., keeping in shape) reduces striving for alternative subgoals (e.g., going to the gym). REFERENCES Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Be- havior and Human Decision Processes, 50,179-211. Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self- assessment manekin and the semantic differentia!' Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49-59. Brunstein, J.C., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (1996). Effects of failure on sub- sequent pelformance: The impOltance of self-defining goals. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 70,395-407. Cialdini, R.B., & Trost, M.R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity, and compliance. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 151-192). Boston: McGraw-Hil!. Cialdini, R.B., Wosinka, W., Barrell, D.W., Butner, J., & Gornik- Durose, M. (1999). Compliance with a request in two cultures: The differential influence of social proof and commitment/con- sistency on collectivists and individualists. Personality and So- cial Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1242-1253. Cooke, R, & Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 159-186. Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 370-377. Fishbach, A., Dhar, R, & Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substitutes or complements: The role of goal accessibility. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 91, 232-242. Fishbein, M. (1980). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications and implications. In H. Howe & M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska sym- posium on motivation (Vo!. 27, pp. 65-116). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Forster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E.T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- chology, 41,220-239. Gollwitzer, P.M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social re- ality of self-symbolizing. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Private selfand public self (pp. 143-159). New York: Springer. Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493-503. Gollwitzer, P.M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity. In J. Heckhausen & C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self- regulation across the life span (pp. 389-423). New York: Cam- bridge University Press. Gollwitzer, P.M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vo!. 38, pp. 69-119). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Gollwitzer, P.M., & Wicklund, RA. (1985). Self-symbolizing and the neglect of others' perspectives. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 48, 702-715. Gollwitzer, P.M., Wicklund, RA., & Hilton, J.1. (1982). Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 43, 358-371. Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211-1226. Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B.J., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Symbolic self-completion in academia: Evidence from depart- ment web pages and email signature files. European Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 311-316. Koo, M., & Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation: How (un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 183-195. Kruglanski, A.W., Shah, J.Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R, Chun, W.Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M.E Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psyclwlogy (Vo!. 34, pp. 331-378). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Ledgerwood, A., Liviatan, I., & Carnevale, P.J. (2007). Gl'oup-identity completion and the symbolic value of property. Psychological Science, 18, 873-878. Lerner, J.S., & Tetlock, P.E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of ac- countability. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 255-275. Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille und Bediirfnis. Psychologische For- schung, 7,330-385. Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal selling and task motivation. American Psychologist, 57,705-717. Mahler, W. (1933). Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen Realitiitsgrades. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 27-89. Marsh, RI.., Hicks, J.1., & Bink, M.1. (1998). Activation of com- pleted, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 350-361. Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Hand- lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302-379. Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vo!. 12, pp. 1-36). Chichester, En- gland: Wiley. Staats, H., Harland, E, & Wilke, H.A. (2004). Effecting durable change: A team approach to improve environmental behavior in the household. Environment and Behavior, 36, 341-367. 617
  • 7. Vallacher, R.R., & Wegner, D.M. (1987). What do people think they are doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psycho- logical Review, 94, 3-15. Wehh, T.L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing hehavioral intentions engender hehavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249-268. 618 Wicklund, R.A., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlhaum. Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Hand- lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1-85.