SlideShare a Scribd company logo
The Influence of Perceived Product Risk on Consumers’ e-Tailer 
Shopping Preference 
Pradeep A. Korgaonkar Æ Eric J. Karson 
Published online: 18 May 2007 
 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007 
Abstract Increasingly, retailers are combining Internet 
and store based operations to become ‘‘multi-channel’’ as 
they attempt to attract and retain customers. This study 
investigates how the type and level of perceived product 
risk (specifically economic and psychosocial risk) influence 
patronage preference for shopping from three types of 
e-tailers. The e-tailer formats studied are: pure play 
e-tailers (e.g., Overstock.com), value-oriented store based 
e-tailers (e.g., Wal-Mart.com), and prestigious store based 
e-tailers (e.g., Bloomingdales.com). The hypotheses, 
based upon prior research in the area of perceived product 
risk, show that type and level of risk do matter. Further, 
e-tailers linked with prestigious stores have an advantage 
over both other e-tailer types. Results also show an inter-action 
between perceived product risk and the e-tail format. 
Based on samples from the Northeast and Southeast 
USA, the results are found to be similar in these diverse 
regions, improving the generalizability of the findings. 
Keywords Risk  Internet  e-Tailing  Online shopping  
Store patronage 
Introduction 
The Internet has clearly revolutionized the way consumers 
acquire and process, and marketers disseminate, informa-tion. 
As online retail sales continue to increase at a slower 
pace than expected, practitioners, and academics alike are 
still searching for factors that influence consumer prefer-ence 
for shopping on the Internet. Although published re-search 
exists related to consumer Internet shopping, little is 
known about how consumers shop from stores that have 
added web sites to their ‘‘brick and mortar’’ retailing 
(e.g., Jarvenpaa  Todd, 1996–97; Jones  Vijayasarathy, 
1998). As technology increases consumer shopping alter-natives, 
research is needed to uncover how the web sites of 
multi-channel retailers such as Eddie Bauer compare vis-a-vis 
pure play Internet retailers such as Shopzilla.com. 
Specifically, this study attempts to provide insight into 
which products are preferred by consumers using a par-ticular 
e-tailer format. 
Research to date suggests that perceived risk is likely to 
be useful in understanding a variety of online consumer 
behaviors, including e-tailing patronage (Donthu  Garcia, 
1999; Ha, 2002). Still, little is known about how risk per-ceptions 
influence patronage among the major variants of 
Internet store formats e.g., pure play Internet retailers such 
as ShopNBC.com, value oriented discount store based 
‘‘click and mortar’’ retailers such as Target.com, or pres-tigious 
department store based ‘‘click and mortar’’ retailers 
such as Saksfifthavenue.com, henceforth called pure play, 
value CM, and prestigious CM, respectively. Although, 
past studies have investigated product categories that are 
best suited for Internet retailing in general (e.g., Cheskin 
Research and Studio Archtype/Sapient, 1999; Girard, 
Silverblatt,  Korgaonkar, 2002; Peterson, Balasubrama-nian, 
 Bronnenberg, 1997), published research on the 
topic of different types of e-tailers is scant. Taking 
advantage of the rich perceived risk paradigm literature, 
this study empirically tests whether value and prestigious 
CM e-tailers have an advantage over strictly pure play 
P. A. Korgaonkar 
College of Business, Florida Atlantic University, University 
Tower, 220 S.E. 2nd Avenue, Fort Lauderdale, FL 33301, USA 
e-mail: Korgaonk@fau.edu 
E. J. Karson () 
Department of Marketing, Villanova School of Business, 
Villanova University, Villanova, PA 19085-1678, USA 
e-mail: eric.karson@villanova.edu 
123 
J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 
DOI 10.1007/s10869-007-9044-y
56 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 
e-tailers depending on the product risks perceived by on-line 
shoppers for each e-tailer type. 
Specifically, this study first tests whether consumers’ 
overall preference for online shopping differs based on the 
perceived product risk. Second, we hypothesize that overall 
shopping preference is highest for prestigious CM 
e-tailers followed by value CM e-tailers, and the lowest 
for pure play e-tailers, regardless of the product risk. Third, 
hypotheses about the interaction effects of online e-tailer 
types and perceived product risk types (e.g., economic risk 
and psychosocial risk) on preference for shopping online 
are formed and tested. 
While we acknowledge that many factors are likely to 
influence online shopping behavior, our focus on risk ex-tends 
previous research on Internet retailing as risk is likely 
to serve as a ‘‘catch-all’’ for consumers’ reservations to-wards 
Web shopping, or their preference for one type of 
e-tailer over another. As the perceived product risk concept 
has been fruitful in explaining consumers’ choice of 
products, retailers, catalog/telephone shopping, and inter-net 
shopping in the past, we feel the extension of this 
simple and useful concept will aid in understanding and 
explaining patronage preferences between the three types 
of e-tailers as well. 
Product risk 
Past studies suggest that the usefulness of the Internet as a 
shopping medium is closely linked to the product that 
consumers intend to purchase. For example, Rosen and 
Howard (2000) propose what they term as e-potential for 
different products to be sold on the Internet. Proponents of 
the transaction cost paradigm suggest that product features 
will influence transaction costs and, as such, play a key role 
in e-tailer selection (e.g., Benjamin  Wigand, 1995). We 
emphasize the perceived risk paradigm in our study as 
the focal point of discussion as, for many consumers, 
buying from the Internet is a new way of buying. In fact, a 
May 2003 study by International Demographics, Inc., 
shows only 22.5% of US households were regular Internet 
purchasers in 2002, making over four purchases. As a result 
many consumers who buy online are often insecure and 
perceive risk. This risk has two main sources: (a) risk re-lated 
to the types of product purchased, and (b) the risk 
associated with the type of online merchant they are pur-chasing 
from. 
Since Bauer’s (1960) seminal work, several studies in 
marketing have explored the concept of perceived risk to-wards 
understanding patronage behavior. The concept of 
perceived risk has been used to explain and predict tradi-tional 
store based shopping preferences as well as in-home 
shopping behavior (e.g., Akaah  Korgaonkar, 1998; 
Spence, Engel,  Blackwell, 1970). Studies by Dowling 
and Staelin (1994), Shrimp and Bearden (1982), White and 
Truly (1989), among others, suggest that perceived risk 
toward a product category is inversely related to purchase 
intentions. The literature also strongly suggests that con-sumers 
are reluctant to patronize a retail store when they 
are uncertain of the risks associated with purchase (Prasad, 
1975). 
There are different types of risks perceived by con-sumers. 
Based on the early work of Roselius (1971) and 
Jacoby and Kaplan (1972) the literature generally identifies 
six different types of risks: financial, performance, physi-cal, 
psychological, social, and time loss. However, rarely 
are all six different types of perceived risk incorporated 
into a single study. Given that Stone and Gronhaug (1993), 
in their study of various dimensions of perceived risk, 
suggest that the financial and psychosocial dimensions of 
risks captured the majority of the overall risk perceptions 
(compared to the time, performance and physical dimen-sions 
of risks), and in line with past research (e.g., 
Korgaonkar, 1982; Prasad, 1975), we have selected the two 
types of perceived product risk most likely to influence 
behavior in an e-tail situation: economic and psychosocial. 
Each is defined as follows: Economic risk refers to how the 
choice of a product will affect the individual shopper’s 
ability to make other purchases. Thus, it varies with the 
financial considerations of price in relation to factors such 
as the shopper’s income, ability to pay, and alternative uses 
of money. Psychosocial risk relates to how the purchase 
decision will affect the opinions other people hold of the 
shopper. Thus, it varies with such factors as the social 
conspicuousness and social relevance of the product. 
In addition to the fact that economic and psychosocial 
risks are reported to be more relevant than other types, we 
maintain that the risk dimensions of time, product perfor-mance, 
and physical dimensions of the product remain 
largely invariant across the three e-tailer formats. In other 
words, regardless of the type of outlet selling a product, the 
features, performance, and physical dimensions of the 
product do not change. Similarly, given the widespread 
availability of overnight and express delivery and tracking 
systems from companies such as Federal Express, UPS, 
and USPS, except for rare situations, product acquisition 
time also remains fairly consistent. Based on past research, 
we suggest: 
H1: Consumers will prefer low risk versus high-risk 
products when shopping online. Hence, 
(a) Consumers will prefer products of low levels of 
psychosocial risk versus products of high psychoso-cial 
risk when shopping online; independent of eco-nomic 
risk. 
123
J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 57 
(b) Consumers will prefer products of low levels of 
economic risk versus high economic risk when 
shopping online; independent of psychosocial risk. 
Literature suggests that, in addition to the level of risk, 
the type of risk perceptions also influence shopping pref-erences. 
Studies such as Perry and Hamm (1969), Peter and 
Tarpey (1975), and Prasad (1975) suggest that the type of 
product risk influences purchase decisions. Derbaix (1983) 
and Mitchell (1999), studying food, cars, and TVs, show 
that economic risks play an important role in influencing 
purchasing decisions, while for clothing they suggest that 
psychosocial risks play an important role in buying deci-sions. 
Based on these past studies we hypothesize that: 
H2: Consumers patronage preferences will be influenced 
by the type of risk (economic and psychosocial) regardless 
of the level of risks. 
Online retail store type 
Although a limited number of empirical studies exist on the 
role consumer risk perceptions play in the selection of 
traditional and non-store shopping channels, little attention 
has been devoted to the role of perceived risks in online 
shopping format preference. Due to this limited research, 
the hypotheses of this study are based on the extant 
empirical findings on the level of consumers’ perceived 
risk in traditional in-store shopping channels (i.e., depart-ment, 
specialty and discount retail stores, catalog show-rooms) 
and non-in-store shopping channels (i.e., catalog 
orders by mail, telephone, or internet. Namely: Bhatnagar, 
Misra,  Rao, (2000); Cox  Rich, (1964); Festervand, 
Snyder,  Tsalikis, (1986); Hisrich, Dornoff,  
Kernan, (1972); Korgaonkar, (1982); Korgaonkar  
Moschis, (1989); Miyazaki  Fernandez, (2001); Prasad, 
(1975); Spence et al., (1970)). Results from non-online 
retail studies indicate that the ‘‘perceived risk of a product 
is transferable to the store that sells the product’’ 
(Korgaonkar, 1982, p 78). Previous findings also suggest 
that in-store shopping is perceived as less risky than tele-phone 
or mail catalog order shopping (Cox  Rich, 1964; 
Festervand et al., 1986; Spence et al., 1970). 
We propose that the work of Bettman (1973) provides a 
theoretical base for prior findings. Bettman posits that risk 
has two components: inherent risk that is endemic to a 
product class, and handled risk, a clear derivative of 
inherent risk, that varies with the amount of additional 
information available about the purchase. When looking at 
different retailer types (e.g., department stores, specialty 
stores, discount stores, and non-in store retailing) each 
certainly presents different types and amounts of infor-mation 
to consumers. In the literature reviewed, generally, 
the amount and type of information was greater for in-store 
than ‘‘at-home’’ retailers, likely explaining the drop in 
consumers’ perceptions of handled risk and increase in 
shopping preference for those stores with a physical pres-ence. 
The same is expected to hold true for e-commerce. In 
general, we hypothesize that e-tailers without a physical 
presence will be perceived as riskier places to shop com-pared 
to clicks and mortar e-tailers. 
Further, while Internet shopping does allow for 24/7 ac-cess, 
easier price comparisons, and the ability to find rare 
products, along with many other benefits, these advantages 
are offset by a number of concerns. Among these concerns 
are: privacy and security of the medium (e.g., Korgaonkar  
Wolin, 1999; Liebermann  Stashevsky, 2002), lack of 
familiarity or experience with certain online retailers, and 
generally, the risks associated with the intangible nature of 
online shopping. Patronage of a pure play e-tailer such as 
eBay poses the additional risks of getting a defective/ 
damaged product, delayed product arrival, the products not 
matching descriptions posted on the seller’s Web site, etc. 
Conversely, a store-based internet operation such as 
Sears.com allows the consumer to physically check the 
merchandise prior to purchase, or easily exchange or return 
the merchandise to the store after purchase. Additionally a 
physical presence provides a variety of available tangible cues 
such as product displays supplemented with POP material, as 
well as quality cues to help reduce perceived risk prior to and 
post-purchase. Thus the store based CM e-tailers are able to 
offer the best of both worlds and reduce the risks associated 
with shopping from a pure play e-tailer. Therefore: 
H3: Consumer’s shopping preferences will be the lowest 
for pure play e-tailers compared to store based CM 
e-tailers when shopping online, independent of product 
risk. 
Based on prior literature (Grewal, Iyer,  Levy, 2004), 
we further speculate that among store-based CM 
e-tailers, the prestige CM e-tailers, with their stronger 
brand reputations, will be perceived as less risky than value 
CM e-tailers, as their brand equity communicates a better 
selection of quality merchandise, as well as superior cus-tomer 
service versus discount stores, again reducing han-dled 
risk. Given this, our study predicts that online 
shoppers will perceive the lowest risk for shopping from a 
prestigious CM Web site, medium amounts of risk 
shopping from a value CM Web site, and the highest risk 
shopping from a pure play e-tailer. 
The following hypotheses, drawing on the increased 
levels of handled risk different e-tail formats allow, suggest 
that consumers’ online shopping preference will vary by 
the type of Internet store independent of the type of per-ceived 
product risk. Specifically, 
123
58 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 
H4: Consumers will prefer prestige CM e-tailers over 
value CM e-tailers when shopping online, independent of 
perceived product risks. 
H5: Consumers will prefer value CM e-tailers over 
pure play e-tailers when shopping online, independent of 
perceived product risks. 
Interaction effects between product risk and online 
store-type risks on online shopping preferences 
Two way interactions 
Past studies in traditional, as well as online, retailing sug-gest 
that retailer type and product type have important 
influences in determining retail patronage (e.g., Darden, 
1979; Jones  Vijayasarathy, 1998; Kling  Palmer, 1997; 
Sheth, 1983). As expected, the interaction between product 
and retailer type is supported in studies incorporating the 
congruency concept in e-tailing (De Figueiredo, 2000; 
Jahng, Jain,  Ramamurthy, 2000), as patronage should 
decrease as perceptions of risk increase. Previous research 
has suggested that perceived product risks affect prefer-ences 
not only for retail store selection, but between 
product categories as well (Bhatnagar et al., 2000; 
Miyazaki  Fernandez, 2001). 
Based on these studies, we predict that online e-tail store 
type and product risk interact as consumers choose the type 
of Internet retailer they prefer to shop from. Simply stated, 
for different levels of perceived risk in varied product cate-gories, 
consumers will prefer different types of e-tailers, with 
store type, and the information this signals, being more 
important the greater inherent risk a product class has. 
Compared to store based e-tailers, pure play e-tailers are 
likely to pose higher economic, as well as psychosocial, 
risks because of the limited information consumers can get 
about these stores through physical inspection. Because 
consumers are unable to personally experience/evaluate the 
product or service prior to purchase, products that are high 
in economic and/or psychosocial risk will be least preferred 
by shoppers on pure play sites. Stated another way, pure 
play e-tailers will have higher shopping preferences only 
when risks are perceived to be low. Formally: 
H6: Online shopping preference will be the highest for 
pure play e-tailers for products with low inherent (eco-nomic 
as well as psychosocial) risk. 
Turning to the effects of economic risk on retailer 
preference, online shopping from the Web site of value 
CM e-tailer should be preferred when it lowers economic 
risk. In other words, value CMs’ positioning helps 
‘‘handle’’ economic risk. Specifically, shoppers for high 
economic risk products will view discounter’s ‘‘value ap-peal,’’ 
a common discount/value store strategy, as lowering 
the economic risk. This is evidenced as the online suc-cesses 
of value CM Web sites such as Bestbuy.com and 
Wal-Mart.com, etc. are partly attributable to their capacity 
to offer low prices, especially for expensive products. This 
leads to the following hypothesis: 
H7: For high economic risk products, online shopping 
preference will be the highest for value oriented discount 
store e-tailers. 
Finally, the small number of research studies that have 
investigated the role of perceived product risk in the selec-tion 
of a shopping channel (Bhatnagar et al., 2000; Forsythe 
 Shi, 2003; Hisrich et al., 1972; Korgaonkar, 1982; 
Korgaonkar  Moschis, 1989; Prasad, 1975) indicate that 
when shopping for high social risk products, consumers 
perceive a lower amount of risk for department and specialty 
stores versus discount stores. We expect similar relation-ships 
in the context of online retailing. Online shopping from 
the Web site of prestigious CM e-tailers will be appealing 
to shoppers for high psychosocial risk products as the 
prestigious stores: offer more desirable brands, enable an 
authentic view of the merchandise, provide higher security, 
and are of superior graphic quality. These, and other po-tential 
information cues, should reduce handled risk over 
that of value CM e-tailers. Thus we propose that: 
H8: For high psychosocial risk products, online shopping 
preference will be the highest for prestige CM e-etailers. 
Methodology 
Pretests 
Given our interest on risk perceived across different types 
of Internet retailers, and the types of risks (economic and 
psychosocial) with different product types as well, pre-testing 
was done to establish several categories of products 
that would satisfy all four combinations of high or low 
economic and psychosocial risk. First, a group of 36 stu-dents 
in a public southeastern university were given the 
definitions of economic and psychosocial risk. They were 
given a four quadrant diagram with high and low category 
on one axis and economic and psychosocial risk on the 
other axis. Then they were asked to develop a list of 
products and/or services that would fit the four possible 
categories. That task yielded 42 unique high economic and 
high psychosocial risk products or services (henceforth 
products), 56 low economic and high psychosocial risk 
products, 77 high economic and low psychosocial 
123
J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 59 
risk products, and 96 low economic and low psychosocial 
risk products. After combining like items (e.g., ‘‘cleaning 
products,’’ ‘‘cleaning supplies,’’ and ‘‘glass cleaner’’) 34, 
40, 45, and 50 major categories of each product, respec-tively, 
remained. 
Products such as cars, personal services, and tattoos that 
appeared in more than one classification were eliminated. 
Next, the authors selected 16 products thought to best 
typify products in each of the four categories of interest. 
This list, shown in Table 1, was then cross validated by 
asking 99 respondents from a private northeastern univer-sity 
to classify each of the 16 product categories as being 
high or low on economic and psychosocial risk. These 
ratings supported the classifications of product categories 
used for the main study. 
Main study 
In order to ensure a more diverse sample, two groups of 
Internet shoppers were used, one from the southeast (SE), 
and one from the northeast (NE). The data were collected 
by having undergraduate marketing students conduct face-to- 
face survey interviews. The students were told to dis-tribute 
surveys to ‘‘persons 18 years or older who are 
regular internet users.’’ Students received extra-credit for 
distributing the surveys. Two hundred and forty completed 
surveys were gathered in the SE, while 276 surveys were 
gathered in the NE. While this is, admittedly, a conve-nience 
sample, the divergent populations and pre-selection 
of Internet users is appropriate given the objectives of the 
study. As can be seen from Table 2, the two samples were 
only statistically similar on gender. Chi-square tests reveal 
(p  .05, df = 5) that the NE sample was older (38.5% 
were over 44, while in the SE only 16.6% were), and had 
much higher income (41.1% of the NE sample recorded 
income over $100,000, while only 10.4% of the SE sample 
did). As for Internet buying, when asked if they had bought 
on the Web in the last 6 months, 85.5% of the NE sample 
had versus 74.6% of the SE sample. Further, the NE sample 
was more satisfied with their ‘‘most recent online pur-chase’’ 
reporting a mean of 4.26 (on a scale of 1–5), versus 
the SE (3.6). 
Survey instrument 
In the main study, four different survey versions were 
prepared. Not only did this allow for four different 
groupings of product categories from Table 1, but coun-terbalancing 
the ordering in which product risk categories 
were presented. Prior to answering any questions, defini-tions 
of e-tailer types in the study, and definitions of the 
types of risk, were provided. During the survey respondents 
were presented with each of the three e-tailer types one by 
one. For each e-tailer type four examples of products for 
the four risk combinations we presented and subjects were 
asked to indicate their shopping preference for each of the 
four types of products on a 1–5 scale (anchored by ‘‘may 
never buy’’ and ‘‘may prefer buying’’). The statistical 
design was a 3 (Type of e-tailer: Pure play, Value CM 
e-tailer, Prestige CM e-tailer) · 2 (Perceived Level of 
Economic Risk: Low, High) · 2 (Perceived Level of Psy-chosocial 
Risk: Low, High) within subject design. Analysis 
of variance was used to test the specific research hypoth-esis. 
Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations for 
each of the 12 cells for the combined sample, while Fig. 1 
shows the overall preferences between e-tail type. 
Table 1 Product service 
classifications used in final 
study 
Ninety nine respondents in pre-test 
to verify 
Product category Economic risk Psychosocial risk 
Accessories (friendship bracelet, watch  $40, costume jewellery) Low High 
Personal grooming (deodorant, cologne, hair care) Low High 
Apparel (fabric gloves, plastic sunglasses) Low High 
Sundries (bottled water, greeting cards, wallets) Low High 
Personal care items (soap, shampoo, toothbrush) Low Low 
Office/school supplies (pen, pencil, notebooks) Low Low 
Household products (cleaning supplies, detergent, napkins) Low Low 
Toiletries (toothpaste, chap stick) Low Low 
Home furnishings (floor covering, bedding) High Low 
Home appliances (refrigerator, washing machine) High Low 
Home entertainment (TV, stereo system, DVD player) High Low 
Electronics (digital camera, camcorder, computer) High Low 
Online services (education, health care) High High 
Formal/dress apparel (dress shirts/blouses, shoes, suit) High High 
Jewellery (diamond rings, formal wristwatch) High High 
Durables (furniture, cars, boats) High High 
123
60 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 
Analysis and results 
Manipulation checks 
To verify our manipulations for each of the four surveys, 
means were tested for all combinations of high economic 
risk (e.g., products with high economic risk and low psy-chosocial 
risk summed with products with high economic 
risk and high psychosocial risk) versus combinations of 
low economic risk, as were high psychosocial risks versus 
low psychosocial risks across the three e-tailer types. In all 
cases there were significant differences across all com-parisons 
of high and low economic and psychosocial risks 
at the p  .001 level (t-values [economic risks]: 6.439, 
8.900, 9.156, 12.352; [psychosocial risks]: 4.795, 19.399, 
6.750, 6.197, with between 120 and 140 degrees of 
freedom). These tests confirm our manipulations for the 12 
product categories used in Table 1. 
H1: shopping preference influenced by level of risk 
Looking at the significant economic risk by type of e-tailer 
(p  .001), and psychosocial risk by type of e-tailer 
(p  .05) interaction in Fig. 2, one can clearly see that 
across e-tailer type, products with low psychosocial risks 
are preferred over those of high risk (supporting H1a), and 
products of low economic risk are preferred over those of 
high economic risk, supporting H1b (for prestigious CM 
e-tailers there is no significant difference at the p  .05 
level in the difference between low and high economic risk 
products). The direction of the relationship is suggested in 
the table of means for the combined sample (Table 3). 
3.2 
3.1 
3 
2.9 
2.8 
2.7 
2.6 
2.5 
2.4 
Pure Play 
Shopping Preference 
Value CM Prestigious CM 
Fig. 1 Store preferences, combined sample 
The two samples were also analyzed independently for 
cross validation (see Tables 4 and 5). As expected, some 
differences are noted in the two regions, however, there is 
more convergence than divergence among the sample results. 
In both samples we find significant main effects for both 
psychosocial risk, although its interaction with e-tailer type is 
only significant in the NE sample, supporting H1a. On the 
other hand, themain effect of economic risk is only significant 
in SE sample (p  .05), although the NE sample is direc-tionally 
correct (mean = 2.38 low, 2.76 high). Again, the 
interaction between economic risk e-tailer type is significant 
(p  .01) in the both samples. These results support H1b. 
For H1 we are looking for the main effect of psycho-social 
risk and economic risk that occurred in both sam-ples. 
Thus, the results of the combined and separate 
samples provide support for Hypotheses 1 (level of risks). 
H2: shopping preference influenced by type of risk 
Figure 3 displays the psychosocial · economic risk inter-action. 
Coupled with the significant main effects just 
discussed, we find support for H2, that the type of risk does 
have an effect on shopping preference, with the possible 
exception of economic risk in the NE sample. 
H3–H5: difference in store preferences 
The ANOVA analysis for both the combined and regional 
data shows that the type of e-tailer is significantly related to 
shopping preference (p  .01), with the sample means also 
Table 2 Sample characteristics 
Descriptor NE SE 
Bought OL in last 6 months 85.50% 74.60% 
Satisfaction with last OL buying 
experience (1–5) 
4.26 3.6 
Gender (percent female) 50.40% 52.10% 
Household income $50–74,999 $35–49,999 
Age 25–34 20–24 
N 275 240 
Numbers in parenthesis indicate range of categorical answers 
Table 3 Shopping preference 
means (standard deviations in 
parenthesis): combined sample 
Cells report mean, standard 
deviation 
n = 511 
Perceived product risk Pure play 
e-tailer 
Value CM 
e-tailer 
Prestige CM 
e-tailer 
Row mean 
Low psychosocial, low economic 2.58 (1.351) 3.08 (1.366) 2.96 (1.398) 2.87 
High psychosocial, low economic 2.64 (1.284) 2.99 (1.358) 3.14 (1.315) 2.92 
High psychosocial, high economic 2.03 (1.215) 2.56 (1.371) 2.91 (1.355) 2.50 
Low psychosocial, high economic 2.60 (1.247) 3.12 (1.241) 3.48 (1.180) 3.07 
Column mean 2.46 2.94 3.12 2.84 
123
J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 61 
3.4 
3.2 
3 
2.8 
2.6 
2.4 
supporting Hypothesis 3 about the relative preference for 
e-tailer type. For the combined data, the overall shopping 
preference mean for pure play e-tailers is 2.46, while it is 
2.937, and 3.122 for value CM and prestige CM 
retailers respectively. Further, consumer’s shopping pref-erences 
are significantly (p  .01) lower for pure play 
e-tailers compared to clicks and mortar stores. Adjusting 
for multiple comparisons, all e-tailers have different levels 
of shopping preferences from each other (p  .05). 
The data also show support for the proposition that the 
overall preference for prestige CM e-tailers is higher than 
the overall preference for value CM e-tailers regardless 
of type of risks (H4) for the combined as well as regional 
data (p  .01). Similarly, support is found for Hypothesis 5 
stating that overall shopping preference is higher for value 
CM e-tailers than for pure play e-tailers in all three data 
sets (p  .01). Figure 1 clearly reflects the overall store 
preferences hypothesized. 
Hypothesis 6: pure plays preferred under low risk 
The hypothesis that higher preferences will be demon-strated 
for pure play e-tailers with low economic and/or 
low psychosocial risk products is not supported. As seen in 
Fig. 2 the results for the total sample show that the pref-erence 
for low economic risk products was the lowest for 
pure play (2.61), versus prestige CM e-tailers (3.05), and 
value CM e-tailers (3.10), largely mirroring overall 
preferences just reported. Similarly, for psychosocial risk 
the mean preference score for pure plays (2.59) is lowest in 
comparison to both value CM e-tailers (3.04) and 
2.2 
Pure Play 
Shopping Preference 
Lo Econ 
Hi Econ 
Lo psychosoc 
Hi psychosoc 
Value CM Prestigious CM 
Fig. 2 Risk · type of retailer interaction, combined sample 
Table 4 Analysis of variance 
(ANOVA) of patronage 
preference by type of perceived 
risk (High–Low), and type of 
e-tailer (Northeast sample) 
a p  .001 
** p  .01 
* p  .05 
Source of variance Sum of 
squares 
Degrees of 
freedom 
Mean 
square 
F 
Main effects 4 
Economic risk 4.718 1 4.719 1.346 
Psychosocial risk 68.123 1 68.123 38.817a 
Type of e-tailer 205.276 2 102.638 56.065a 
2-way effects 5 
Econ risk · Psychosocial Risk 114.198 1 114.198 63.160a 
Econ Risk · Type of e-tailer 56.330 2 28.165 32.105a 
Psychosocial Risk · Type of e-tailer 4.892 2 2.446 5.123** 
3-way effect 2 
Econ Risk · Psychosocial Risk · Type of 
e-tailer 
5.886 2 2.943 5.273** 
Table 5 Analysis of variance 
(ANOVA) of patronage 
preference by type of perceived 
risk (High–Low), and type of 
e-tailer. (Southeast sample) 
a p  .001 
** p  .01 
* p  .05 
Source of variance Sum of squares Degrees of freedom Mean square F 
Main effects 4 
Economic risk 16.336 1 16.336 4.013* 
PsychoSocial risk 34.084 1 34.084 20.761a 
Type of e-tailer 303.292 2 151.645 87.672a 
2-way Effects 5 
Econ risk · Psychosocial risk 37.211 1 37.211 21.219a 
Econ risk · Type of e-tailer 9.015 2 4.507 6.786** 
Psychosocial risk · Type of e-tailer 1.338 2 .669 1.266 
3-way effect 2 
Econ risk · Psychosocial risk · Type 
of e-tailer 
.597 2 .299 .527 
123
62 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 
3.4 
3.2 
3 
2.8 
2.6 
2.4 
prestige CM e-tailers (3.22). A pairwise t-test shows that 
the preference scores for each type of risks is significantly 
lower (p  .01) for the pure play versus the CM 
e-tailers. Similar results are found for each region. Thus, 
the results are opposite our stated hypothesis, suggesting 
that even for low risk products consumers are reluctant to 
patronize pure play e-tailers over both clicks and mortar 
formats. 
Hypothesis 7: higher preference for value CM 
e-tailers with high economic risk 
The hypothesis suggesting that value CM e-tailers will 
be most preferred for high economic risk products was 
partially supported. The mean scores of preferences for 
high economic risk products for the total sample show that 
value CM e-tailers are preferred over pure play e-tailers 
(2.78 vs. 2.31) at p  .001, but are less preferred over the 
prestige CM e-tailers (3.20) at p  .001. Similar results 
are seen for each of the two regions. In the SE, for high 
economic risk products, value CM e-tailers are preferred 
over pure plays (2.96 vs. 2.29), but less preferable to 
prestige CM e-tailers (3.06, p  .01). In the NE, the 
prestige CM e-tailers are, once again, most preferred 
(3.00) followed by value CM e-tailers (2.61) and the 
pure play (2.37, all p  .001). Thus, overall, we see that 
for high economic risk products, value CM e-tailers are 
preferred over pure plays, but not the prestige CM 
format. 
Hypothesis 8: High psychosocial risks raises shopping 
preference of prestige CM e-tailers 
Our last hypothesis states that for high psychosocial risk 
products, shopping preference will be highest for presti-gious 
CM e-tailers. The results for the total sample, as 
well as two regions, support this hypothesis. For the total 
sample we find that prestige CM e-tailers have the 
highest mean score for high psychosocial risk products, 
3.02, followed by a mean preference of 2.84 for value 
CM e-tailers, and 2.33 for pure plays. Pairwise tests show 
the prestige CM e-tailer’s preference is higher than other 
e-tailers at p  .001. Similarly, in the SE, preference for 
prestige CM e-tailers is highest at 3.13 and marginally 
higher than the preference for value CM e-tailers (3.00, 
p  .10), and higher than pure plays (2.28, p  .001). Fi-nally, 
the NE preference is significantly higher for prestige 
CM e-tailers (p  .001) than the other two with the mean 
preference scores of 3.23 for prestige, 2.69 for value, and 
2.35 for pure play, respectively. 
Discussion 
Although the number and type of firms who sell products 
online continues to increase, relatively small numbers of 
consumers have embraced the e-tailing alternative (Cheung 
 Lee, 2001). While e-commerce retail sales in the third 
quarter of 2003 reached $13.3 billion, an increase of about 
7% over the previous quarter, this still only accounted for 
1.5% of total retail sales (U.S. Census Bureau, 2003). 
While the press is enamored with the success of Ama-zon. 
com, a pure play e-tailer, many other pure plays (e.g., 
eToys.com, Pets.com, Streamline.com and Webvan) have 
met with failure and, some would say, helped fuel the 
dot.com bust of the early 2000s. 
Recognizing these difficulties, pure play e-tailers 
increasingly opt for hybrid clicks and mortar approaches in 
several product categories such as general merchandise 
(Target at Amazon), clothing (with Sears’ acquisition of 
Land’s End), travel (Marriott.com), electronics (Best Buy), 
etc. It seems that multi-channel retailing is here to stay. 
However, few published studies exist exploring which 
e-tail format is suitable for various kinds of products. Al-though 
scholars have suggested which products are best 
suited for selling on the Internet (e.g., Rosen  Howard, 
2000), little published information is available to e-tailers 
of the three formats studied here. Our results suggest that, 
overall, pure play e-tailers will continue to have a signifi-cant 
disadvantage in comparison to the clicks and mortar 
e-tailers, almost regardless of the type or level of inherent 
risk. In this study, for each of the four categories of 
products surveyed in each of two regions, the preference 
for pure play e-tailers was always the lowest. This suggests 
that pure play e-tailers have yet to fully earn the trust of 
consumers. Additionally, our findings demonstrate the 
substantial advantages that brand equity, visibility, and 
multi-channel consumer options hold for CM e-tailers 
over pure play e-tailers. 
2.2 
Low Psychosocial 
Shopping Preference 
Low Econ 
High Econ 
High Psychosocial 
Fig. 3 Psychosocial risk · economic risk, combined sample 
123
J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 63 
The literature suggests that trust is essential for the 
development of e-tailing. At the most basic level, trust 
helps address concerns over factors such as privacy and 
security essential to online transactions (Cheskin Research 
 Studio Archtype/Sapient, 1999). Also, trust can mini-mize 
feelings of risk and lack of control that are often 
characteristic of e-tailing transactions (Bhattacherjee, 
2000). Trust becomes especially pivotal in selecting 
products or services that are already perceived as risky 
(Mayer et al., 1995). As our results clearly demonstrate, 
pure-play e-tailers need to overcome these trust issues to 
reduce risk, and draw even with CM e-tailers. 
Several studies already suggest perceived risks as an 
antecedent to trust (e.g., Corbitt, Thanasankit,  Yi, 2003; 
Tan, 1999). One of the innovative ways of reducing risk 
and building trust for pure play e-tailers is by providing 
online sales help similar to live salespeople in retail envi-ronments. 
Instead of just offering chat rooms as an option 
to shoppers, a few e-tailers are monitoring Web shoppers 
on their site, looking for opportunities to open a chat 
window on the shopper’s screen to ask if they need any 
help (Higgins, 2004). Another way of reducing the risk of 
purchasing products from pure play e-tailers is to carry 
well known brand names. Brands can communicate 
valuable information to consumers, especially in online 
environments where it is harder to physically inspect 
products. Consumers may have personal experience or 
knowledge about well known brands, lowering the risk of 
purchasing them from a pure play e-tailer. A third way of 
reducing risk, if possible, is to build the brand of the 
e-tailer itself, either through heavy promotion or creation 
of a very large e-tailer, such as Amazon. Generally, con-sumers 
are less apprehensive purchasing products from 
well known and/or large organizations. Finally, a seal of 
approval from an organization such as eTrust may also go a 
long way in alleviating consumers risk perceptions of 
shopping from pure play e-tailers. 
Limitations and future research 
While statistics on Internet shopping vary widely, estimates 
are that some 60–80% of all US adults are online, with 30– 
50% of them buying online (ABC News Poll, 2003; 
Pastore, 2001). This is, of course, US adults only, and our 
sample, while diverse, does not represent all US shoppers. 
Perhaps more significantly, if one looks at statistics 
worldwide, one can see that online shopping penetration is 
much lower than in the US. EMarketer (2004) reports that 
only 16% of Internet users in the EU-15 buy online. Clearly 
more representative samples in both the US and worldwide 
are called for, with particular attention to the drastically 
lower shopping rates in other countries. 
Additionally, as this study identifies the challenges pure 
play e-tailers face, branding—of either goods or sites, as 
suggested—is likely to overcome many of these chal-lenges. 
Studies on the effect of well-known versus less-known 
brands’ ability to mitigate various risks are certainly 
needed, and should provide useful insight as to additional 
antecedents of online shopping risks. 
Finally, much as catalogers removed perceived risk with 
‘‘satisfaction guaranteed’’ pledges years ago, e-tailers must 
fully understand all the risks perceived by potential online 
shoppers, and how to address them. Once these risks, and 
their interactions, are fully understood, consumer seg-mentation 
based on online shopping risk perceptions is 
possible, as well as insight into how to overcome these risk 
perceptions. Given the potential for growth in online 
shopping those firms that most fully recognize and address 
consumers’ concerns will likely reap great benefits. 
Our results show that the prestige clicks and bricks have 
an advantage over other e-tail formats for three out of four 
product categories, while value CM e-tailers have an edge 
over the other two online formats for products with low 
economic risks. Thus, the results are largely supportive of 
our study hypotheses in two different regions of the country. 
This study shows that level and type of perceived risk 
provides a good explanation for the congruity approach, and 
the importance of handled risk provided by prestige CM 
e-tailers. This research helps suggest which types of prod-ucts 
are most suitable for the three e-tail formats. 
References 
ABC News Poll. (2003). 2003 Holiday shopping season: More 
shoppers plan to go online. Retrieved November 23, 2005, from 
abcnews.go.com/images/pdf/883a36Holiday Spending.pdf 
Akaah, I. P.,  Korgaonkar, P. K. (1988). Conjoint investigation of the 
relative importance of risk relievers in direct marketing. Journal 
of Advertising Research, 28(August/September), 38–44. 
Bauer, R. A. (1960). Consumer behavior as risk taking. In R. Hancock 
(Ed.), Dynamic marketing for a changing world: Proceedings of 
the 43rd conference (pp. 389–398). Chicago, IL: American 
Marketing Association. 
Benjamin, R.,  Wigand, W. (1995). Electronic markets and virtual 
value chains on the information superhighway. Sloan Manage-ment 
Review, 36(Winter), 62–72. 
Bettman, J. R. (1973). Perceived risk and its components: A model 
and empirical test. Journal of Marketing Research, 19(May), 
184–190. 
Bhatnagar, A., Misra, S.,  Rao, R. H. (2000). On risk, convenience, 
and internet shopping behavior. Communications of the ACM, 
43(November), 98–105. 
Bhattacherjee, A. (2000). Acceptance of e-commerce services: The 
case of electronic brokerages. IEEE Transactions on Systems, 
Man, and Cybernetics: Part A, 32(July), 411-420. 
Cheskin Research and Studio Archtype/Sapient. (1999). E-commerce 
trust study. Retrieved from http://www.Sapient.com/cheskin 
Cheung, C. M. K.,  Lee, M. K. O. (2001). Trust in internet 
shopping: Instrument development and validation through 
123
64 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 
classical and modern approaches. Journal of Global Information 
Management, 9(July–September), 23–36. 
Corbitt, B. J., Thanasanki, T.,  Yi, H. (2003). Trust and e-commerce: 
A study of consumer perceptions. Electronic Com-merce 
Research and Applications, 2(Autumn), 203–215. 
Cox, D. F.,  Rich, S. U. (1964). Perceived risk and consumer 
decision making—the case of telephone shopping. Journal of 
Marketing Research, 1(November), 32-39. 
Darden, W. R. (1979). A patronage model of consumer behavior. In 
R. Stampfl, E. C. Hirschman (Eds.), Competitive structure in 
retail markets: The department store perspective (pp. 43–52) 
Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association. 
Derbaix, C. (1983). Perceived risk and risk relievers: An empirical 
investigation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 3(March), 
19–38. 
De Figueiredo, J. M. (2000). Finding sustainable profitability in 
electronic commerce. Sloan Management Review, 41(Summer), 
41–52. 
Donthu, N.,  Garcia, A. (1999). The internet shopper. Journal of 
Advertising Research, 39(May–June), 52–58. 
Dowling, G. R.,  Staelin, R. (1994). A model of perceived risk and 
intended risk-handling activity. Journal of Consumer Research, 
21(June), 119–135. 
eMarketer. (2004). Internet users in the EU-15 who have ever 
purchased products and/or services online by country, September 
2003, (March 1), Retrieved from http://www.emarketer.com 
Festervand, T. A., Snyder, D. R.,  Tsalikis, J. D. (1986). Influence of 
catalog vs. store shopping and prior satisfaction on perceived 
risk. Academy of Marketing Science, 14(Winter), 28–36. 
Forsythe, S. M.,  Shi, B. (2003). Consumer patronage and risk 
perceptions in internet shopping. Journal of Business Research, 
55(November), 867–875. 
Girard, T. R. S.,  P. K. Korgaonkar (2002). Influence of product 
class on preference for shopping on the internet. Journal of 
Computer-Mediated Communication, 8 (October), Retrieved 
from ascusc.org/jcmc/. 
Grewal, D., Iyer, G. R.,  Levy, M. (2004). Internet retailing: 
Enablers, limiters and market consequences. Journal of Business 
Research, 57(July), 703–713. 
Ha, H. (2002). The effects of consumer risk perceptions on pre-purchase 
information in online auctions: Brand, word-of-mouth 
and customized information. Journal of Consumer Mediated 
Communication, 8(October), Retrieved from ascusc.org/jcmc/ 
Higgins, M. (2004). Pop-up sales clerks: Web sites try the hard sell. 
The Wall Street Journal, (April 15) D1–D2. 
Hisrich, R. D., Dornoff, R. J.,  Kernan, J. B. (1972). Perceived risk 
in store selection. Journal of Marketing Research, 9(November), 
453–459. 
Jahng, J., Jain, H.,  Ramamurthy, K. (2000). Effective design of 
electronic commerce environments: A proposed theory of 
congruence and an illustration. IEEE Transactions on Systems, 
Man, and Cybernetics: Part A, 30(July), 456–471. 
Jarvenpaa, S. L.,  Todd, P. A. (1996–97). Consumer reactions to 
electronic shopping on the world wide web. International 
Journal of Electronic Commerce, 1(Winter), 59–88. 
Jacoby, J.,  Kaplan, L. B. (1972). The components of perceived risk. 
In M. Venkatesan (Ed.), Proceedings, 3rd annual conference 
association for consumer research (pp. 382–393). Chicago, IL: 
Association for Consumer Research. 
Jones, J. M.,  Vijayasarathy, L. (1998). Internet consumer catalog 
shopping: Findings from an exploratory study and directions for 
future research. Journal of Internet Research: Electronic 
Networking Applications and Policy, 8(4), 322–330. 
Kling, R.,  Palmer, J. W. (1997). Electronic commerce in retailing: 
Differences across retail formats. The Information Society, 
13(January–March), 75–91. 
Korgaonkar, P. K. (1982). Consumer preferences for catalog show-rooms 
and discount stores: The moderating role of product risk. 
Journal of Retailing, 58(Fall), 76–88. 
Korgaonkar, P. K.,  Moschis, G. P. (1989). The effects of perceived 
risk and social class on consumer preferences for distribution 
outlets. In P. Bloom, R. Winer, H. H. Kassarjian, D. L. 
Scammon, B. Weitz, R. Spekman, V. Mahajan  M. Levy 
(Eds.), American marketing association educators’ proceedings: 
Enhancing knowledge development in marketing (pp. 39–43). 
Chicago: AMA. 
Korgaonkar, P. K.,  Wolin, L. D. (1999). A multivariate analysis of 
web usage. Journal of Advertising Research, 39(March/April), 
53–68. 
Liebermann, Y.,  Stashevsky, S. (2002). Perceived risk as barriers to 
internet and e-commerce usage. Qualitative Market Research, 
5(November), 291–300. 
Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H.,  Schoorman, D. F. (1995). An integrative 
model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 
20(July), 709–734. 
Mitchell, V. (1999). Consumer perceived risk: Conceptualizations 
and models. European Journal of Marketing, 33½., 163–195. 
Miyazaki, A. D.,  Fernandez, A. (2001). Consumer perceptions of 
privacy and security risks for online shopping. The Journal of 
Consumer Affairs, 35(Summer), 27–44. 
Pastore, M. (2001). Half of American adults now shop online, (April 
24), Retrieved from http://www.clickz.com/stats/market/retail-ing/ 
article.php/6061_751021 
Perry, M.,  Hamm, C. (1969). Canonical analysis of relations 
between socio-economic risk and personal influence in purchase 
decision. Journal of Marketing Research, 6(August), 351–354. 
Peter, J. P.,  Tarpey, Sr. L. X. (1975). A comparative analysis of 
three consumer strategies. Journal of Consumer Research, 
2(June), 29–38. 
Peterson, R. T., Balasubramanian, S.,  Bronnenberg, B. J. (1997). 
Exploring the implications of the internet for consumer market-ing. 
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 25(Fall), 
329–346. 
Prasad, K. V. (1975). Socioeconomic product risk and patronage 
preferences of retail shoppers. Journal of Marketing, 39(July), 
42–47. 
Roselius, R. (1971). Consumer rankings of risk reduction methods. 
Journal of Marketing, 35(January), 56–61. 
Rosen, K. T.,  Howard, A. L. (2000). E-retail: Gold rush or fool’s 
gold? California Management Review, 42(Spring), 72–100. 
Sheth, J. N. (1983). An integrative theory of patronage preference and 
behavior. In W. R. Darden  R. F. Lusch (Eds.), Patronage 
behavior and retail management (pp. 9–28). North-Holland, NY, 
NY: Elsevier Science Publicator Company. 
Shimp, T. A.,  Bearden, W. O. (1982). Warranty and other extrinsic 
cue effects on consumers’ risk perceptions. Journal of Consumer 
Research, 9(June), 938–946. 
Spence, H. E., Engle, J. F.,  Blackwell, R. D. (1970). Perceived risk 
in mail-order and retail store buying. Journal of Marketing 
Research, 8(August), 364–369. 
Stone, R. N.,  Gronhaug, K. (1993). Perceived risk: Further 
considerations for the marketing discipline. European Journal of 
Marketing, 27(3), 372–394. 
Tan, S. J. (1999). Strategies for reducing consumers’ risk aversion in 
internet shopping. The Journal of Consumer Marketing, 16(2), 
163–180. 
U.S Census Bureau, Retail 3Q. (2003). E-commerce report (Novem-ber 
21). 
White, J. D.,  Truly, E. L. (1989). Price-quality integration in 
warranty premium evaluation: A preliminary test of alternative 
models of risk assessment. Journal of Business Research, 
19(September), 109–125. 
123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

More Related Content

What's hot

The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2
The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2
The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2jessie714
 
An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...
An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...
An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...
Dr. Amarjeet Singh
 
John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics MIT 2013
John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics  MIT 2013John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics  MIT 2013
John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics MIT 2013
Barney Stacher
 
Foreign E-Commerce Transactions
Foreign E-Commerce TransactionsForeign E-Commerce Transactions
Foreign E-Commerce Transactions
Andreas Kurtz
 
Study ecommerce goods_en
Study ecommerce goods_enStudy ecommerce goods_en
Study ecommerce goods_en
soyooz
 
Determinants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury Goods
Determinants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury GoodsDeterminants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury Goods
Determinants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury Goods
International Journal of Business Marketing and Management (IJBMM)
 
Information Gaps
Information GapsInformation Gaps
Information Gaps
Hugo OGrady
 
research paper
research paperresearch paper
research paperErica Biby
 
Consumer's decision making process
Consumer's decision making processConsumer's decision making process
Consumer's decision making process
Giang Coffee
 
Influence of counterfeiting on luxury brands
Influence of counterfeiting on luxury brandsInfluence of counterfeiting on luxury brands
Influence of counterfeiting on luxury brands
FaridaBakkalla
 
FirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMiller
FirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMillerFirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMiller
FirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMillerLonnie Miller
 
Smarter strategies for_free_shipping
Smarter strategies for_free_shippingSmarter strategies for_free_shipping
Smarter strategies for_free_shipping
prestamonster
 
Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004
Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004
Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004Mercatus Center
 
US Consumer Analysis: Apparel and Footwear
US Consumer Analysis: Apparel and FootwearUS Consumer Analysis: Apparel and Footwear
US Consumer Analysis: Apparel and Footwear
Deborah Weinswig
 
Impulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumers
Impulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumersImpulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumers
Impulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumersRoger P. Dill, MBA
 
Manoj Nair LUBS
Manoj Nair LUBSManoj Nair LUBS
Manoj Nair LUBS
Manoj Nair
 
Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012
Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012
Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012
RedMasAdv
 

What's hot (20)

The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2
The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2
The influence of college students’ shopping orientations v.2
 
An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...
An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...
An Evaluation of the Effect of Consumer Characteristics on Retail Format Patr...
 
John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics MIT 2013
John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics  MIT 2013John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics  MIT 2013
John R. Hauser, Consideration-Set Heuristics MIT 2013
 
Foreign E-Commerce Transactions
Foreign E-Commerce TransactionsForeign E-Commerce Transactions
Foreign E-Commerce Transactions
 
Study ecommerce goods_en
Study ecommerce goods_enStudy ecommerce goods_en
Study ecommerce goods_en
 
Determinants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury Goods
Determinants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury GoodsDeterminants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury Goods
Determinants of Turkish Consumer Attitudes towards Counterfeit Luxury Goods
 
Information Gaps
Information GapsInformation Gaps
Information Gaps
 
research paper
research paperresearch paper
research paper
 
Ps34
Ps34Ps34
Ps34
 
Ps12
Ps12Ps12
Ps12
 
Economics of Automobiles
Economics of AutomobilesEconomics of Automobiles
Economics of Automobiles
 
Consumer's decision making process
Consumer's decision making processConsumer's decision making process
Consumer's decision making process
 
Influence of counterfeiting on luxury brands
Influence of counterfeiting on luxury brandsInfluence of counterfeiting on luxury brands
Influence of counterfeiting on luxury brands
 
FirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMiller
FirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMillerFirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMiller
FirstTimeBuyers_QUIRKS_LonnieMiller
 
Smarter strategies for_free_shipping
Smarter strategies for_free_shippingSmarter strategies for_free_shipping
Smarter strategies for_free_shipping
 
Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004
Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004
Ellig E Commerce In Wine Cos Retreat Presentation 2004
 
US Consumer Analysis: Apparel and Footwear
US Consumer Analysis: Apparel and FootwearUS Consumer Analysis: Apparel and Footwear
US Consumer Analysis: Apparel and Footwear
 
Impulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumers
Impulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumersImpulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumers
Impulse buying no longer the norm for Puerto Rican consumers
 
Manoj Nair LUBS
Manoj Nair LUBSManoj Nair LUBS
Manoj Nair LUBS
 
Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012
Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012
Mi carro nuevo - Survey 2012
 

Similar to Ps33

Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.
Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.
Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.
María del Carmen González-Sandoval Marrero
 
A Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying Behavior
A Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying BehaviorA Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying Behavior
A Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying Behavior
Steven Wallach
 
An Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of Consumers
An Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of ConsumersAn Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of Consumers
An Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of Consumers
Joe Osborn
 
Risk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in Nigeria
Risk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in NigeriaRisk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in Nigeria
Risk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in Nigeria
International Journal of Business Marketing and Management (IJBMM)
 
Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/
Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/
Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/
Assignment Help
 
RUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docx
RUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docxRUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docx
RUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docx
charisellington63520
 
Chen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketing
Chen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketingChen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketing
Chen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketing
Sijan Pokharel
 
Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...
Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...
Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...
Business, Management and Economics Research
 
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
IJRTEMJOURNAL
 
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
IJRTEMJOURNAL
 
Online shopping assignment
Online shopping assignmentOnline shopping assignment
Online shopping assignment
aim001
 
Digital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docx
Digital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docxDigital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docx
Digital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docx
jakeomoore75037
 
covid19
covid19covid19
IMS Research Paper
IMS Research PaperIMS Research Paper
IMS Research PaperAngela Chang
 
Values vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docx
Values vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docxValues vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docx
Values vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docx
tienboileau
 
1420 5621-1-pb 3
1420 5621-1-pb 31420 5621-1-pb 3
1420 5621-1-pb 3Oliver John
 
Social trust slide show
Social trust slide showSocial trust slide show
Social trust slide showSadie
 
Running head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docx
Running head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docxRunning head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docx
Running head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docx
charisellington63520
 

Similar to Ps33 (20)

Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.
Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.
Dissertation- Agents behavior at the Supermarket.
 
A Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying Behavior
A Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying BehaviorA Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying Behavior
A Review Of Factors Affecting Online Buying Behavior
 
An Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of Consumers
An Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of ConsumersAn Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of Consumers
An Analysis Of Factors Affecting On Online Shopping Behavior Of Consumers
 
Risk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in Nigeria
Risk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in NigeriaRisk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in Nigeria
Risk Perceptions and Online Shopping Intention among Internet Users in Nigeria
 
Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/
Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/
Nwom15 http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co.uk/
 
RUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docx
RUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docxRUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docx
RUnning head ONLINE1ONLINE11Online Shopping Hypo.docx
 
Chen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketing
Chen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketingChen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketing
Chen 2011 journal-of-interactive-marketing
 
Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...
Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...
Do Your Neighbors Matter to You? : Focused on Chinese Jiu Ling Hou (Post-’90s...
 
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
 
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
Impacts of Perceived Risks on Internet Purchasing Intention: In case of Mongo...
 
Online shopping assignment
Online shopping assignmentOnline shopping assignment
Online shopping assignment
 
3
33
3
 
Ps54p
Ps54pPs54p
Ps54p
 
Digital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docx
Digital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docxDigital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docx
Digital Nudging Numeric and Semantic Priming inE-Commerce.docx
 
covid19
covid19covid19
covid19
 
IMS Research Paper
IMS Research PaperIMS Research Paper
IMS Research Paper
 
Values vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docx
Values vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docxValues vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docx
Values vs. Value.  New research shows a disparity between what s.docx
 
1420 5621-1-pb 3
1420 5621-1-pb 31420 5621-1-pb 3
1420 5621-1-pb 3
 
Social trust slide show
Social trust slide showSocial trust slide show
Social trust slide show
 
Running head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docx
Running head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docxRunning head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docx
Running head ONLINE CONSUMER BEHAVIORS .docx
 

More from Can Erdem (20)

Ps55
Ps55Ps55
Ps55
 
Ps53
Ps53Ps53
Ps53
 
Ps52
Ps52Ps52
Ps52
 
Ps51
Ps51Ps51
Ps51
 
Ps50
Ps50Ps50
Ps50
 
Ps5
Ps5Ps5
Ps5
 
Ps48p
Ps48pPs48p
Ps48p
 
Ps47
Ps47Ps47
Ps47
 
Ps46p
Ps46pPs46p
Ps46p
 
Ps45
Ps45Ps45
Ps45
 
Ps44
Ps44Ps44
Ps44
 
Ps43p
Ps43pPs43p
Ps43p
 
Ps42
Ps42Ps42
Ps42
 
Ps40
Ps40Ps40
Ps40
 
Ps4
Ps4Ps4
Ps4
 
Ps39p
Ps39pPs39p
Ps39p
 
Ps38
Ps38Ps38
Ps38
 
Ps37
Ps37Ps37
Ps37
 
Ps36
Ps36Ps36
Ps36
 
Ps35
Ps35Ps35
Ps35
 

Ps33

  • 1. The Influence of Perceived Product Risk on Consumers’ e-Tailer Shopping Preference Pradeep A. Korgaonkar Æ Eric J. Karson Published online: 18 May 2007 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007 Abstract Increasingly, retailers are combining Internet and store based operations to become ‘‘multi-channel’’ as they attempt to attract and retain customers. This study investigates how the type and level of perceived product risk (specifically economic and psychosocial risk) influence patronage preference for shopping from three types of e-tailers. The e-tailer formats studied are: pure play e-tailers (e.g., Overstock.com), value-oriented store based e-tailers (e.g., Wal-Mart.com), and prestigious store based e-tailers (e.g., Bloomingdales.com). The hypotheses, based upon prior research in the area of perceived product risk, show that type and level of risk do matter. Further, e-tailers linked with prestigious stores have an advantage over both other e-tailer types. Results also show an inter-action between perceived product risk and the e-tail format. Based on samples from the Northeast and Southeast USA, the results are found to be similar in these diverse regions, improving the generalizability of the findings. Keywords Risk Internet e-Tailing Online shopping Store patronage Introduction The Internet has clearly revolutionized the way consumers acquire and process, and marketers disseminate, informa-tion. As online retail sales continue to increase at a slower pace than expected, practitioners, and academics alike are still searching for factors that influence consumer prefer-ence for shopping on the Internet. Although published re-search exists related to consumer Internet shopping, little is known about how consumers shop from stores that have added web sites to their ‘‘brick and mortar’’ retailing (e.g., Jarvenpaa Todd, 1996–97; Jones Vijayasarathy, 1998). As technology increases consumer shopping alter-natives, research is needed to uncover how the web sites of multi-channel retailers such as Eddie Bauer compare vis-a-vis pure play Internet retailers such as Shopzilla.com. Specifically, this study attempts to provide insight into which products are preferred by consumers using a par-ticular e-tailer format. Research to date suggests that perceived risk is likely to be useful in understanding a variety of online consumer behaviors, including e-tailing patronage (Donthu Garcia, 1999; Ha, 2002). Still, little is known about how risk per-ceptions influence patronage among the major variants of Internet store formats e.g., pure play Internet retailers such as ShopNBC.com, value oriented discount store based ‘‘click and mortar’’ retailers such as Target.com, or pres-tigious department store based ‘‘click and mortar’’ retailers such as Saksfifthavenue.com, henceforth called pure play, value CM, and prestigious CM, respectively. Although, past studies have investigated product categories that are best suited for Internet retailing in general (e.g., Cheskin Research and Studio Archtype/Sapient, 1999; Girard, Silverblatt, Korgaonkar, 2002; Peterson, Balasubrama-nian, Bronnenberg, 1997), published research on the topic of different types of e-tailers is scant. Taking advantage of the rich perceived risk paradigm literature, this study empirically tests whether value and prestigious CM e-tailers have an advantage over strictly pure play P. A. Korgaonkar College of Business, Florida Atlantic University, University Tower, 220 S.E. 2nd Avenue, Fort Lauderdale, FL 33301, USA e-mail: Korgaonk@fau.edu E. J. Karson () Department of Marketing, Villanova School of Business, Villanova University, Villanova, PA 19085-1678, USA e-mail: eric.karson@villanova.edu 123 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 DOI 10.1007/s10869-007-9044-y
  • 2. 56 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 e-tailers depending on the product risks perceived by on-line shoppers for each e-tailer type. Specifically, this study first tests whether consumers’ overall preference for online shopping differs based on the perceived product risk. Second, we hypothesize that overall shopping preference is highest for prestigious CM e-tailers followed by value CM e-tailers, and the lowest for pure play e-tailers, regardless of the product risk. Third, hypotheses about the interaction effects of online e-tailer types and perceived product risk types (e.g., economic risk and psychosocial risk) on preference for shopping online are formed and tested. While we acknowledge that many factors are likely to influence online shopping behavior, our focus on risk ex-tends previous research on Internet retailing as risk is likely to serve as a ‘‘catch-all’’ for consumers’ reservations to-wards Web shopping, or their preference for one type of e-tailer over another. As the perceived product risk concept has been fruitful in explaining consumers’ choice of products, retailers, catalog/telephone shopping, and inter-net shopping in the past, we feel the extension of this simple and useful concept will aid in understanding and explaining patronage preferences between the three types of e-tailers as well. Product risk Past studies suggest that the usefulness of the Internet as a shopping medium is closely linked to the product that consumers intend to purchase. For example, Rosen and Howard (2000) propose what they term as e-potential for different products to be sold on the Internet. Proponents of the transaction cost paradigm suggest that product features will influence transaction costs and, as such, play a key role in e-tailer selection (e.g., Benjamin Wigand, 1995). We emphasize the perceived risk paradigm in our study as the focal point of discussion as, for many consumers, buying from the Internet is a new way of buying. In fact, a May 2003 study by International Demographics, Inc., shows only 22.5% of US households were regular Internet purchasers in 2002, making over four purchases. As a result many consumers who buy online are often insecure and perceive risk. This risk has two main sources: (a) risk re-lated to the types of product purchased, and (b) the risk associated with the type of online merchant they are pur-chasing from. Since Bauer’s (1960) seminal work, several studies in marketing have explored the concept of perceived risk to-wards understanding patronage behavior. The concept of perceived risk has been used to explain and predict tradi-tional store based shopping preferences as well as in-home shopping behavior (e.g., Akaah Korgaonkar, 1998; Spence, Engel, Blackwell, 1970). Studies by Dowling and Staelin (1994), Shrimp and Bearden (1982), White and Truly (1989), among others, suggest that perceived risk toward a product category is inversely related to purchase intentions. The literature also strongly suggests that con-sumers are reluctant to patronize a retail store when they are uncertain of the risks associated with purchase (Prasad, 1975). There are different types of risks perceived by con-sumers. Based on the early work of Roselius (1971) and Jacoby and Kaplan (1972) the literature generally identifies six different types of risks: financial, performance, physi-cal, psychological, social, and time loss. However, rarely are all six different types of perceived risk incorporated into a single study. Given that Stone and Gronhaug (1993), in their study of various dimensions of perceived risk, suggest that the financial and psychosocial dimensions of risks captured the majority of the overall risk perceptions (compared to the time, performance and physical dimen-sions of risks), and in line with past research (e.g., Korgaonkar, 1982; Prasad, 1975), we have selected the two types of perceived product risk most likely to influence behavior in an e-tail situation: economic and psychosocial. Each is defined as follows: Economic risk refers to how the choice of a product will affect the individual shopper’s ability to make other purchases. Thus, it varies with the financial considerations of price in relation to factors such as the shopper’s income, ability to pay, and alternative uses of money. Psychosocial risk relates to how the purchase decision will affect the opinions other people hold of the shopper. Thus, it varies with such factors as the social conspicuousness and social relevance of the product. In addition to the fact that economic and psychosocial risks are reported to be more relevant than other types, we maintain that the risk dimensions of time, product perfor-mance, and physical dimensions of the product remain largely invariant across the three e-tailer formats. In other words, regardless of the type of outlet selling a product, the features, performance, and physical dimensions of the product do not change. Similarly, given the widespread availability of overnight and express delivery and tracking systems from companies such as Federal Express, UPS, and USPS, except for rare situations, product acquisition time also remains fairly consistent. Based on past research, we suggest: H1: Consumers will prefer low risk versus high-risk products when shopping online. Hence, (a) Consumers will prefer products of low levels of psychosocial risk versus products of high psychoso-cial risk when shopping online; independent of eco-nomic risk. 123
  • 3. J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 57 (b) Consumers will prefer products of low levels of economic risk versus high economic risk when shopping online; independent of psychosocial risk. Literature suggests that, in addition to the level of risk, the type of risk perceptions also influence shopping pref-erences. Studies such as Perry and Hamm (1969), Peter and Tarpey (1975), and Prasad (1975) suggest that the type of product risk influences purchase decisions. Derbaix (1983) and Mitchell (1999), studying food, cars, and TVs, show that economic risks play an important role in influencing purchasing decisions, while for clothing they suggest that psychosocial risks play an important role in buying deci-sions. Based on these past studies we hypothesize that: H2: Consumers patronage preferences will be influenced by the type of risk (economic and psychosocial) regardless of the level of risks. Online retail store type Although a limited number of empirical studies exist on the role consumer risk perceptions play in the selection of traditional and non-store shopping channels, little attention has been devoted to the role of perceived risks in online shopping format preference. Due to this limited research, the hypotheses of this study are based on the extant empirical findings on the level of consumers’ perceived risk in traditional in-store shopping channels (i.e., depart-ment, specialty and discount retail stores, catalog show-rooms) and non-in-store shopping channels (i.e., catalog orders by mail, telephone, or internet. Namely: Bhatnagar, Misra, Rao, (2000); Cox Rich, (1964); Festervand, Snyder, Tsalikis, (1986); Hisrich, Dornoff, Kernan, (1972); Korgaonkar, (1982); Korgaonkar Moschis, (1989); Miyazaki Fernandez, (2001); Prasad, (1975); Spence et al., (1970)). Results from non-online retail studies indicate that the ‘‘perceived risk of a product is transferable to the store that sells the product’’ (Korgaonkar, 1982, p 78). Previous findings also suggest that in-store shopping is perceived as less risky than tele-phone or mail catalog order shopping (Cox Rich, 1964; Festervand et al., 1986; Spence et al., 1970). We propose that the work of Bettman (1973) provides a theoretical base for prior findings. Bettman posits that risk has two components: inherent risk that is endemic to a product class, and handled risk, a clear derivative of inherent risk, that varies with the amount of additional information available about the purchase. When looking at different retailer types (e.g., department stores, specialty stores, discount stores, and non-in store retailing) each certainly presents different types and amounts of infor-mation to consumers. In the literature reviewed, generally, the amount and type of information was greater for in-store than ‘‘at-home’’ retailers, likely explaining the drop in consumers’ perceptions of handled risk and increase in shopping preference for those stores with a physical pres-ence. The same is expected to hold true for e-commerce. In general, we hypothesize that e-tailers without a physical presence will be perceived as riskier places to shop com-pared to clicks and mortar e-tailers. Further, while Internet shopping does allow for 24/7 ac-cess, easier price comparisons, and the ability to find rare products, along with many other benefits, these advantages are offset by a number of concerns. Among these concerns are: privacy and security of the medium (e.g., Korgaonkar Wolin, 1999; Liebermann Stashevsky, 2002), lack of familiarity or experience with certain online retailers, and generally, the risks associated with the intangible nature of online shopping. Patronage of a pure play e-tailer such as eBay poses the additional risks of getting a defective/ damaged product, delayed product arrival, the products not matching descriptions posted on the seller’s Web site, etc. Conversely, a store-based internet operation such as Sears.com allows the consumer to physically check the merchandise prior to purchase, or easily exchange or return the merchandise to the store after purchase. Additionally a physical presence provides a variety of available tangible cues such as product displays supplemented with POP material, as well as quality cues to help reduce perceived risk prior to and post-purchase. Thus the store based CM e-tailers are able to offer the best of both worlds and reduce the risks associated with shopping from a pure play e-tailer. Therefore: H3: Consumer’s shopping preferences will be the lowest for pure play e-tailers compared to store based CM e-tailers when shopping online, independent of product risk. Based on prior literature (Grewal, Iyer, Levy, 2004), we further speculate that among store-based CM e-tailers, the prestige CM e-tailers, with their stronger brand reputations, will be perceived as less risky than value CM e-tailers, as their brand equity communicates a better selection of quality merchandise, as well as superior cus-tomer service versus discount stores, again reducing han-dled risk. Given this, our study predicts that online shoppers will perceive the lowest risk for shopping from a prestigious CM Web site, medium amounts of risk shopping from a value CM Web site, and the highest risk shopping from a pure play e-tailer. The following hypotheses, drawing on the increased levels of handled risk different e-tail formats allow, suggest that consumers’ online shopping preference will vary by the type of Internet store independent of the type of per-ceived product risk. Specifically, 123
  • 4. 58 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 H4: Consumers will prefer prestige CM e-tailers over value CM e-tailers when shopping online, independent of perceived product risks. H5: Consumers will prefer value CM e-tailers over pure play e-tailers when shopping online, independent of perceived product risks. Interaction effects between product risk and online store-type risks on online shopping preferences Two way interactions Past studies in traditional, as well as online, retailing sug-gest that retailer type and product type have important influences in determining retail patronage (e.g., Darden, 1979; Jones Vijayasarathy, 1998; Kling Palmer, 1997; Sheth, 1983). As expected, the interaction between product and retailer type is supported in studies incorporating the congruency concept in e-tailing (De Figueiredo, 2000; Jahng, Jain, Ramamurthy, 2000), as patronage should decrease as perceptions of risk increase. Previous research has suggested that perceived product risks affect prefer-ences not only for retail store selection, but between product categories as well (Bhatnagar et al., 2000; Miyazaki Fernandez, 2001). Based on these studies, we predict that online e-tail store type and product risk interact as consumers choose the type of Internet retailer they prefer to shop from. Simply stated, for different levels of perceived risk in varied product cate-gories, consumers will prefer different types of e-tailers, with store type, and the information this signals, being more important the greater inherent risk a product class has. Compared to store based e-tailers, pure play e-tailers are likely to pose higher economic, as well as psychosocial, risks because of the limited information consumers can get about these stores through physical inspection. Because consumers are unable to personally experience/evaluate the product or service prior to purchase, products that are high in economic and/or psychosocial risk will be least preferred by shoppers on pure play sites. Stated another way, pure play e-tailers will have higher shopping preferences only when risks are perceived to be low. Formally: H6: Online shopping preference will be the highest for pure play e-tailers for products with low inherent (eco-nomic as well as psychosocial) risk. Turning to the effects of economic risk on retailer preference, online shopping from the Web site of value CM e-tailer should be preferred when it lowers economic risk. In other words, value CMs’ positioning helps ‘‘handle’’ economic risk. Specifically, shoppers for high economic risk products will view discounter’s ‘‘value ap-peal,’’ a common discount/value store strategy, as lowering the economic risk. This is evidenced as the online suc-cesses of value CM Web sites such as Bestbuy.com and Wal-Mart.com, etc. are partly attributable to their capacity to offer low prices, especially for expensive products. This leads to the following hypothesis: H7: For high economic risk products, online shopping preference will be the highest for value oriented discount store e-tailers. Finally, the small number of research studies that have investigated the role of perceived product risk in the selec-tion of a shopping channel (Bhatnagar et al., 2000; Forsythe Shi, 2003; Hisrich et al., 1972; Korgaonkar, 1982; Korgaonkar Moschis, 1989; Prasad, 1975) indicate that when shopping for high social risk products, consumers perceive a lower amount of risk for department and specialty stores versus discount stores. We expect similar relation-ships in the context of online retailing. Online shopping from the Web site of prestigious CM e-tailers will be appealing to shoppers for high psychosocial risk products as the prestigious stores: offer more desirable brands, enable an authentic view of the merchandise, provide higher security, and are of superior graphic quality. These, and other po-tential information cues, should reduce handled risk over that of value CM e-tailers. Thus we propose that: H8: For high psychosocial risk products, online shopping preference will be the highest for prestige CM e-etailers. Methodology Pretests Given our interest on risk perceived across different types of Internet retailers, and the types of risks (economic and psychosocial) with different product types as well, pre-testing was done to establish several categories of products that would satisfy all four combinations of high or low economic and psychosocial risk. First, a group of 36 stu-dents in a public southeastern university were given the definitions of economic and psychosocial risk. They were given a four quadrant diagram with high and low category on one axis and economic and psychosocial risk on the other axis. Then they were asked to develop a list of products and/or services that would fit the four possible categories. That task yielded 42 unique high economic and high psychosocial risk products or services (henceforth products), 56 low economic and high psychosocial risk products, 77 high economic and low psychosocial 123
  • 5. J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 59 risk products, and 96 low economic and low psychosocial risk products. After combining like items (e.g., ‘‘cleaning products,’’ ‘‘cleaning supplies,’’ and ‘‘glass cleaner’’) 34, 40, 45, and 50 major categories of each product, respec-tively, remained. Products such as cars, personal services, and tattoos that appeared in more than one classification were eliminated. Next, the authors selected 16 products thought to best typify products in each of the four categories of interest. This list, shown in Table 1, was then cross validated by asking 99 respondents from a private northeastern univer-sity to classify each of the 16 product categories as being high or low on economic and psychosocial risk. These ratings supported the classifications of product categories used for the main study. Main study In order to ensure a more diverse sample, two groups of Internet shoppers were used, one from the southeast (SE), and one from the northeast (NE). The data were collected by having undergraduate marketing students conduct face-to- face survey interviews. The students were told to dis-tribute surveys to ‘‘persons 18 years or older who are regular internet users.’’ Students received extra-credit for distributing the surveys. Two hundred and forty completed surveys were gathered in the SE, while 276 surveys were gathered in the NE. While this is, admittedly, a conve-nience sample, the divergent populations and pre-selection of Internet users is appropriate given the objectives of the study. As can be seen from Table 2, the two samples were only statistically similar on gender. Chi-square tests reveal (p .05, df = 5) that the NE sample was older (38.5% were over 44, while in the SE only 16.6% were), and had much higher income (41.1% of the NE sample recorded income over $100,000, while only 10.4% of the SE sample did). As for Internet buying, when asked if they had bought on the Web in the last 6 months, 85.5% of the NE sample had versus 74.6% of the SE sample. Further, the NE sample was more satisfied with their ‘‘most recent online pur-chase’’ reporting a mean of 4.26 (on a scale of 1–5), versus the SE (3.6). Survey instrument In the main study, four different survey versions were prepared. Not only did this allow for four different groupings of product categories from Table 1, but coun-terbalancing the ordering in which product risk categories were presented. Prior to answering any questions, defini-tions of e-tailer types in the study, and definitions of the types of risk, were provided. During the survey respondents were presented with each of the three e-tailer types one by one. For each e-tailer type four examples of products for the four risk combinations we presented and subjects were asked to indicate their shopping preference for each of the four types of products on a 1–5 scale (anchored by ‘‘may never buy’’ and ‘‘may prefer buying’’). The statistical design was a 3 (Type of e-tailer: Pure play, Value CM e-tailer, Prestige CM e-tailer) · 2 (Perceived Level of Economic Risk: Low, High) · 2 (Perceived Level of Psy-chosocial Risk: Low, High) within subject design. Analysis of variance was used to test the specific research hypoth-esis. Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations for each of the 12 cells for the combined sample, while Fig. 1 shows the overall preferences between e-tail type. Table 1 Product service classifications used in final study Ninety nine respondents in pre-test to verify Product category Economic risk Psychosocial risk Accessories (friendship bracelet, watch $40, costume jewellery) Low High Personal grooming (deodorant, cologne, hair care) Low High Apparel (fabric gloves, plastic sunglasses) Low High Sundries (bottled water, greeting cards, wallets) Low High Personal care items (soap, shampoo, toothbrush) Low Low Office/school supplies (pen, pencil, notebooks) Low Low Household products (cleaning supplies, detergent, napkins) Low Low Toiletries (toothpaste, chap stick) Low Low Home furnishings (floor covering, bedding) High Low Home appliances (refrigerator, washing machine) High Low Home entertainment (TV, stereo system, DVD player) High Low Electronics (digital camera, camcorder, computer) High Low Online services (education, health care) High High Formal/dress apparel (dress shirts/blouses, shoes, suit) High High Jewellery (diamond rings, formal wristwatch) High High Durables (furniture, cars, boats) High High 123
  • 6. 60 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 Analysis and results Manipulation checks To verify our manipulations for each of the four surveys, means were tested for all combinations of high economic risk (e.g., products with high economic risk and low psy-chosocial risk summed with products with high economic risk and high psychosocial risk) versus combinations of low economic risk, as were high psychosocial risks versus low psychosocial risks across the three e-tailer types. In all cases there were significant differences across all com-parisons of high and low economic and psychosocial risks at the p .001 level (t-values [economic risks]: 6.439, 8.900, 9.156, 12.352; [psychosocial risks]: 4.795, 19.399, 6.750, 6.197, with between 120 and 140 degrees of freedom). These tests confirm our manipulations for the 12 product categories used in Table 1. H1: shopping preference influenced by level of risk Looking at the significant economic risk by type of e-tailer (p .001), and psychosocial risk by type of e-tailer (p .05) interaction in Fig. 2, one can clearly see that across e-tailer type, products with low psychosocial risks are preferred over those of high risk (supporting H1a), and products of low economic risk are preferred over those of high economic risk, supporting H1b (for prestigious CM e-tailers there is no significant difference at the p .05 level in the difference between low and high economic risk products). The direction of the relationship is suggested in the table of means for the combined sample (Table 3). 3.2 3.1 3 2.9 2.8 2.7 2.6 2.5 2.4 Pure Play Shopping Preference Value CM Prestigious CM Fig. 1 Store preferences, combined sample The two samples were also analyzed independently for cross validation (see Tables 4 and 5). As expected, some differences are noted in the two regions, however, there is more convergence than divergence among the sample results. In both samples we find significant main effects for both psychosocial risk, although its interaction with e-tailer type is only significant in the NE sample, supporting H1a. On the other hand, themain effect of economic risk is only significant in SE sample (p .05), although the NE sample is direc-tionally correct (mean = 2.38 low, 2.76 high). Again, the interaction between economic risk e-tailer type is significant (p .01) in the both samples. These results support H1b. For H1 we are looking for the main effect of psycho-social risk and economic risk that occurred in both sam-ples. Thus, the results of the combined and separate samples provide support for Hypotheses 1 (level of risks). H2: shopping preference influenced by type of risk Figure 3 displays the psychosocial · economic risk inter-action. Coupled with the significant main effects just discussed, we find support for H2, that the type of risk does have an effect on shopping preference, with the possible exception of economic risk in the NE sample. H3–H5: difference in store preferences The ANOVA analysis for both the combined and regional data shows that the type of e-tailer is significantly related to shopping preference (p .01), with the sample means also Table 2 Sample characteristics Descriptor NE SE Bought OL in last 6 months 85.50% 74.60% Satisfaction with last OL buying experience (1–5) 4.26 3.6 Gender (percent female) 50.40% 52.10% Household income $50–74,999 $35–49,999 Age 25–34 20–24 N 275 240 Numbers in parenthesis indicate range of categorical answers Table 3 Shopping preference means (standard deviations in parenthesis): combined sample Cells report mean, standard deviation n = 511 Perceived product risk Pure play e-tailer Value CM e-tailer Prestige CM e-tailer Row mean Low psychosocial, low economic 2.58 (1.351) 3.08 (1.366) 2.96 (1.398) 2.87 High psychosocial, low economic 2.64 (1.284) 2.99 (1.358) 3.14 (1.315) 2.92 High psychosocial, high economic 2.03 (1.215) 2.56 (1.371) 2.91 (1.355) 2.50 Low psychosocial, high economic 2.60 (1.247) 3.12 (1.241) 3.48 (1.180) 3.07 Column mean 2.46 2.94 3.12 2.84 123
  • 7. J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 61 3.4 3.2 3 2.8 2.6 2.4 supporting Hypothesis 3 about the relative preference for e-tailer type. For the combined data, the overall shopping preference mean for pure play e-tailers is 2.46, while it is 2.937, and 3.122 for value CM and prestige CM retailers respectively. Further, consumer’s shopping pref-erences are significantly (p .01) lower for pure play e-tailers compared to clicks and mortar stores. Adjusting for multiple comparisons, all e-tailers have different levels of shopping preferences from each other (p .05). The data also show support for the proposition that the overall preference for prestige CM e-tailers is higher than the overall preference for value CM e-tailers regardless of type of risks (H4) for the combined as well as regional data (p .01). Similarly, support is found for Hypothesis 5 stating that overall shopping preference is higher for value CM e-tailers than for pure play e-tailers in all three data sets (p .01). Figure 1 clearly reflects the overall store preferences hypothesized. Hypothesis 6: pure plays preferred under low risk The hypothesis that higher preferences will be demon-strated for pure play e-tailers with low economic and/or low psychosocial risk products is not supported. As seen in Fig. 2 the results for the total sample show that the pref-erence for low economic risk products was the lowest for pure play (2.61), versus prestige CM e-tailers (3.05), and value CM e-tailers (3.10), largely mirroring overall preferences just reported. Similarly, for psychosocial risk the mean preference score for pure plays (2.59) is lowest in comparison to both value CM e-tailers (3.04) and 2.2 Pure Play Shopping Preference Lo Econ Hi Econ Lo psychosoc Hi psychosoc Value CM Prestigious CM Fig. 2 Risk · type of retailer interaction, combined sample Table 4 Analysis of variance (ANOVA) of patronage preference by type of perceived risk (High–Low), and type of e-tailer (Northeast sample) a p .001 ** p .01 * p .05 Source of variance Sum of squares Degrees of freedom Mean square F Main effects 4 Economic risk 4.718 1 4.719 1.346 Psychosocial risk 68.123 1 68.123 38.817a Type of e-tailer 205.276 2 102.638 56.065a 2-way effects 5 Econ risk · Psychosocial Risk 114.198 1 114.198 63.160a Econ Risk · Type of e-tailer 56.330 2 28.165 32.105a Psychosocial Risk · Type of e-tailer 4.892 2 2.446 5.123** 3-way effect 2 Econ Risk · Psychosocial Risk · Type of e-tailer 5.886 2 2.943 5.273** Table 5 Analysis of variance (ANOVA) of patronage preference by type of perceived risk (High–Low), and type of e-tailer. (Southeast sample) a p .001 ** p .01 * p .05 Source of variance Sum of squares Degrees of freedom Mean square F Main effects 4 Economic risk 16.336 1 16.336 4.013* PsychoSocial risk 34.084 1 34.084 20.761a Type of e-tailer 303.292 2 151.645 87.672a 2-way Effects 5 Econ risk · Psychosocial risk 37.211 1 37.211 21.219a Econ risk · Type of e-tailer 9.015 2 4.507 6.786** Psychosocial risk · Type of e-tailer 1.338 2 .669 1.266 3-way effect 2 Econ risk · Psychosocial risk · Type of e-tailer .597 2 .299 .527 123
  • 8. 62 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 3.4 3.2 3 2.8 2.6 2.4 prestige CM e-tailers (3.22). A pairwise t-test shows that the preference scores for each type of risks is significantly lower (p .01) for the pure play versus the CM e-tailers. Similar results are found for each region. Thus, the results are opposite our stated hypothesis, suggesting that even for low risk products consumers are reluctant to patronize pure play e-tailers over both clicks and mortar formats. Hypothesis 7: higher preference for value CM e-tailers with high economic risk The hypothesis suggesting that value CM e-tailers will be most preferred for high economic risk products was partially supported. The mean scores of preferences for high economic risk products for the total sample show that value CM e-tailers are preferred over pure play e-tailers (2.78 vs. 2.31) at p .001, but are less preferred over the prestige CM e-tailers (3.20) at p .001. Similar results are seen for each of the two regions. In the SE, for high economic risk products, value CM e-tailers are preferred over pure plays (2.96 vs. 2.29), but less preferable to prestige CM e-tailers (3.06, p .01). In the NE, the prestige CM e-tailers are, once again, most preferred (3.00) followed by value CM e-tailers (2.61) and the pure play (2.37, all p .001). Thus, overall, we see that for high economic risk products, value CM e-tailers are preferred over pure plays, but not the prestige CM format. Hypothesis 8: High psychosocial risks raises shopping preference of prestige CM e-tailers Our last hypothesis states that for high psychosocial risk products, shopping preference will be highest for presti-gious CM e-tailers. The results for the total sample, as well as two regions, support this hypothesis. For the total sample we find that prestige CM e-tailers have the highest mean score for high psychosocial risk products, 3.02, followed by a mean preference of 2.84 for value CM e-tailers, and 2.33 for pure plays. Pairwise tests show the prestige CM e-tailer’s preference is higher than other e-tailers at p .001. Similarly, in the SE, preference for prestige CM e-tailers is highest at 3.13 and marginally higher than the preference for value CM e-tailers (3.00, p .10), and higher than pure plays (2.28, p .001). Fi-nally, the NE preference is significantly higher for prestige CM e-tailers (p .001) than the other two with the mean preference scores of 3.23 for prestige, 2.69 for value, and 2.35 for pure play, respectively. Discussion Although the number and type of firms who sell products online continues to increase, relatively small numbers of consumers have embraced the e-tailing alternative (Cheung Lee, 2001). While e-commerce retail sales in the third quarter of 2003 reached $13.3 billion, an increase of about 7% over the previous quarter, this still only accounted for 1.5% of total retail sales (U.S. Census Bureau, 2003). While the press is enamored with the success of Ama-zon. com, a pure play e-tailer, many other pure plays (e.g., eToys.com, Pets.com, Streamline.com and Webvan) have met with failure and, some would say, helped fuel the dot.com bust of the early 2000s. Recognizing these difficulties, pure play e-tailers increasingly opt for hybrid clicks and mortar approaches in several product categories such as general merchandise (Target at Amazon), clothing (with Sears’ acquisition of Land’s End), travel (Marriott.com), electronics (Best Buy), etc. It seems that multi-channel retailing is here to stay. However, few published studies exist exploring which e-tail format is suitable for various kinds of products. Al-though scholars have suggested which products are best suited for selling on the Internet (e.g., Rosen Howard, 2000), little published information is available to e-tailers of the three formats studied here. Our results suggest that, overall, pure play e-tailers will continue to have a signifi-cant disadvantage in comparison to the clicks and mortar e-tailers, almost regardless of the type or level of inherent risk. In this study, for each of the four categories of products surveyed in each of two regions, the preference for pure play e-tailers was always the lowest. This suggests that pure play e-tailers have yet to fully earn the trust of consumers. Additionally, our findings demonstrate the substantial advantages that brand equity, visibility, and multi-channel consumer options hold for CM e-tailers over pure play e-tailers. 2.2 Low Psychosocial Shopping Preference Low Econ High Econ High Psychosocial Fig. 3 Psychosocial risk · economic risk, combined sample 123
  • 9. J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 63 The literature suggests that trust is essential for the development of e-tailing. At the most basic level, trust helps address concerns over factors such as privacy and security essential to online transactions (Cheskin Research Studio Archtype/Sapient, 1999). Also, trust can mini-mize feelings of risk and lack of control that are often characteristic of e-tailing transactions (Bhattacherjee, 2000). Trust becomes especially pivotal in selecting products or services that are already perceived as risky (Mayer et al., 1995). As our results clearly demonstrate, pure-play e-tailers need to overcome these trust issues to reduce risk, and draw even with CM e-tailers. Several studies already suggest perceived risks as an antecedent to trust (e.g., Corbitt, Thanasankit, Yi, 2003; Tan, 1999). One of the innovative ways of reducing risk and building trust for pure play e-tailers is by providing online sales help similar to live salespeople in retail envi-ronments. Instead of just offering chat rooms as an option to shoppers, a few e-tailers are monitoring Web shoppers on their site, looking for opportunities to open a chat window on the shopper’s screen to ask if they need any help (Higgins, 2004). Another way of reducing the risk of purchasing products from pure play e-tailers is to carry well known brand names. Brands can communicate valuable information to consumers, especially in online environments where it is harder to physically inspect products. Consumers may have personal experience or knowledge about well known brands, lowering the risk of purchasing them from a pure play e-tailer. A third way of reducing risk, if possible, is to build the brand of the e-tailer itself, either through heavy promotion or creation of a very large e-tailer, such as Amazon. Generally, con-sumers are less apprehensive purchasing products from well known and/or large organizations. Finally, a seal of approval from an organization such as eTrust may also go a long way in alleviating consumers risk perceptions of shopping from pure play e-tailers. Limitations and future research While statistics on Internet shopping vary widely, estimates are that some 60–80% of all US adults are online, with 30– 50% of them buying online (ABC News Poll, 2003; Pastore, 2001). This is, of course, US adults only, and our sample, while diverse, does not represent all US shoppers. Perhaps more significantly, if one looks at statistics worldwide, one can see that online shopping penetration is much lower than in the US. EMarketer (2004) reports that only 16% of Internet users in the EU-15 buy online. Clearly more representative samples in both the US and worldwide are called for, with particular attention to the drastically lower shopping rates in other countries. Additionally, as this study identifies the challenges pure play e-tailers face, branding—of either goods or sites, as suggested—is likely to overcome many of these chal-lenges. Studies on the effect of well-known versus less-known brands’ ability to mitigate various risks are certainly needed, and should provide useful insight as to additional antecedents of online shopping risks. Finally, much as catalogers removed perceived risk with ‘‘satisfaction guaranteed’’ pledges years ago, e-tailers must fully understand all the risks perceived by potential online shoppers, and how to address them. Once these risks, and their interactions, are fully understood, consumer seg-mentation based on online shopping risk perceptions is possible, as well as insight into how to overcome these risk perceptions. Given the potential for growth in online shopping those firms that most fully recognize and address consumers’ concerns will likely reap great benefits. Our results show that the prestige clicks and bricks have an advantage over other e-tail formats for three out of four product categories, while value CM e-tailers have an edge over the other two online formats for products with low economic risks. Thus, the results are largely supportive of our study hypotheses in two different regions of the country. This study shows that level and type of perceived risk provides a good explanation for the congruity approach, and the importance of handled risk provided by prestige CM e-tailers. This research helps suggest which types of prod-ucts are most suitable for the three e-tail formats. References ABC News Poll. (2003). 2003 Holiday shopping season: More shoppers plan to go online. Retrieved November 23, 2005, from abcnews.go.com/images/pdf/883a36Holiday Spending.pdf Akaah, I. P., Korgaonkar, P. K. (1988). Conjoint investigation of the relative importance of risk relievers in direct marketing. Journal of Advertising Research, 28(August/September), 38–44. Bauer, R. A. (1960). Consumer behavior as risk taking. In R. Hancock (Ed.), Dynamic marketing for a changing world: Proceedings of the 43rd conference (pp. 389–398). Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association. Benjamin, R., Wigand, W. (1995). Electronic markets and virtual value chains on the information superhighway. Sloan Manage-ment Review, 36(Winter), 62–72. Bettman, J. R. (1973). Perceived risk and its components: A model and empirical test. Journal of Marketing Research, 19(May), 184–190. Bhatnagar, A., Misra, S., Rao, R. H. (2000). On risk, convenience, and internet shopping behavior. Communications of the ACM, 43(November), 98–105. Bhattacherjee, A. (2000). Acceptance of e-commerce services: The case of electronic brokerages. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics: Part A, 32(July), 411-420. Cheskin Research and Studio Archtype/Sapient. (1999). E-commerce trust study. Retrieved from http://www.Sapient.com/cheskin Cheung, C. M. K., Lee, M. K. O. (2001). Trust in internet shopping: Instrument development and validation through 123
  • 10. 64 J Bus Psychol (2007) 22:55–64 classical and modern approaches. Journal of Global Information Management, 9(July–September), 23–36. Corbitt, B. J., Thanasanki, T., Yi, H. (2003). Trust and e-commerce: A study of consumer perceptions. Electronic Com-merce Research and Applications, 2(Autumn), 203–215. Cox, D. F., Rich, S. U. (1964). Perceived risk and consumer decision making—the case of telephone shopping. Journal of Marketing Research, 1(November), 32-39. Darden, W. R. (1979). A patronage model of consumer behavior. In R. Stampfl, E. C. Hirschman (Eds.), Competitive structure in retail markets: The department store perspective (pp. 43–52) Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association. Derbaix, C. (1983). Perceived risk and risk relievers: An empirical investigation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 3(March), 19–38. De Figueiredo, J. M. (2000). Finding sustainable profitability in electronic commerce. Sloan Management Review, 41(Summer), 41–52. Donthu, N., Garcia, A. (1999). The internet shopper. Journal of Advertising Research, 39(May–June), 52–58. Dowling, G. R., Staelin, R. (1994). A model of perceived risk and intended risk-handling activity. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(June), 119–135. eMarketer. (2004). Internet users in the EU-15 who have ever purchased products and/or services online by country, September 2003, (March 1), Retrieved from http://www.emarketer.com Festervand, T. A., Snyder, D. R., Tsalikis, J. D. (1986). Influence of catalog vs. store shopping and prior satisfaction on perceived risk. Academy of Marketing Science, 14(Winter), 28–36. Forsythe, S. M., Shi, B. (2003). Consumer patronage and risk perceptions in internet shopping. Journal of Business Research, 55(November), 867–875. Girard, T. R. S., P. K. Korgaonkar (2002). Influence of product class on preference for shopping on the internet. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 8 (October), Retrieved from ascusc.org/jcmc/. Grewal, D., Iyer, G. R., Levy, M. (2004). Internet retailing: Enablers, limiters and market consequences. Journal of Business Research, 57(July), 703–713. Ha, H. (2002). The effects of consumer risk perceptions on pre-purchase information in online auctions: Brand, word-of-mouth and customized information. Journal of Consumer Mediated Communication, 8(October), Retrieved from ascusc.org/jcmc/ Higgins, M. (2004). Pop-up sales clerks: Web sites try the hard sell. The Wall Street Journal, (April 15) D1–D2. Hisrich, R. D., Dornoff, R. J., Kernan, J. B. (1972). Perceived risk in store selection. Journal of Marketing Research, 9(November), 453–459. Jahng, J., Jain, H., Ramamurthy, K. (2000). Effective design of electronic commerce environments: A proposed theory of congruence and an illustration. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics: Part A, 30(July), 456–471. Jarvenpaa, S. L., Todd, P. A. (1996–97). Consumer reactions to electronic shopping on the world wide web. International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 1(Winter), 59–88. Jacoby, J., Kaplan, L. B. (1972). The components of perceived risk. In M. Venkatesan (Ed.), Proceedings, 3rd annual conference association for consumer research (pp. 382–393). Chicago, IL: Association for Consumer Research. Jones, J. M., Vijayasarathy, L. (1998). Internet consumer catalog shopping: Findings from an exploratory study and directions for future research. Journal of Internet Research: Electronic Networking Applications and Policy, 8(4), 322–330. Kling, R., Palmer, J. W. (1997). Electronic commerce in retailing: Differences across retail formats. The Information Society, 13(January–March), 75–91. Korgaonkar, P. K. (1982). Consumer preferences for catalog show-rooms and discount stores: The moderating role of product risk. Journal of Retailing, 58(Fall), 76–88. Korgaonkar, P. K., Moschis, G. P. (1989). The effects of perceived risk and social class on consumer preferences for distribution outlets. In P. Bloom, R. Winer, H. H. Kassarjian, D. L. Scammon, B. Weitz, R. Spekman, V. Mahajan M. Levy (Eds.), American marketing association educators’ proceedings: Enhancing knowledge development in marketing (pp. 39–43). Chicago: AMA. Korgaonkar, P. K., Wolin, L. D. (1999). A multivariate analysis of web usage. Journal of Advertising Research, 39(March/April), 53–68. Liebermann, Y., Stashevsky, S. (2002). Perceived risk as barriers to internet and e-commerce usage. Qualitative Market Research, 5(November), 291–300. Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., Schoorman, D. F. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20(July), 709–734. Mitchell, V. (1999). Consumer perceived risk: Conceptualizations and models. European Journal of Marketing, 33½., 163–195. Miyazaki, A. D., Fernandez, A. (2001). Consumer perceptions of privacy and security risks for online shopping. The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 35(Summer), 27–44. Pastore, M. (2001). Half of American adults now shop online, (April 24), Retrieved from http://www.clickz.com/stats/market/retail-ing/ article.php/6061_751021 Perry, M., Hamm, C. (1969). Canonical analysis of relations between socio-economic risk and personal influence in purchase decision. Journal of Marketing Research, 6(August), 351–354. Peter, J. P., Tarpey, Sr. L. X. (1975). A comparative analysis of three consumer strategies. Journal of Consumer Research, 2(June), 29–38. Peterson, R. T., Balasubramanian, S., Bronnenberg, B. J. (1997). Exploring the implications of the internet for consumer market-ing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 25(Fall), 329–346. Prasad, K. V. (1975). Socioeconomic product risk and patronage preferences of retail shoppers. Journal of Marketing, 39(July), 42–47. Roselius, R. (1971). Consumer rankings of risk reduction methods. Journal of Marketing, 35(January), 56–61. Rosen, K. T., Howard, A. L. (2000). E-retail: Gold rush or fool’s gold? California Management Review, 42(Spring), 72–100. Sheth, J. N. (1983). An integrative theory of patronage preference and behavior. In W. R. Darden R. F. Lusch (Eds.), Patronage behavior and retail management (pp. 9–28). North-Holland, NY, NY: Elsevier Science Publicator Company. Shimp, T. A., Bearden, W. O. (1982). Warranty and other extrinsic cue effects on consumers’ risk perceptions. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(June), 938–946. Spence, H. E., Engle, J. F., Blackwell, R. D. (1970). Perceived risk in mail-order and retail store buying. Journal of Marketing Research, 8(August), 364–369. Stone, R. N., Gronhaug, K. (1993). Perceived risk: Further considerations for the marketing discipline. European Journal of Marketing, 27(3), 372–394. Tan, S. J. (1999). Strategies for reducing consumers’ risk aversion in internet shopping. The Journal of Consumer Marketing, 16(2), 163–180. U.S Census Bureau, Retail 3Q. (2003). E-commerce report (Novem-ber 21). White, J. D., Truly, E. L. (1989). Price-quality integration in warranty premium evaluation: A preliminary test of alternative models of risk assessment. Journal of Business Research, 19(September), 109–125. 123
  • 11. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.