SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 15
Download to read offline
Willem Verbeke & Richard P. Bagozzi

Sales Call Anxiety: Exploring What It
Means When Fear Rules a Sales
Encounter
The goal of this study is to develop and test a conceptualization of sales call anxiety (SCA) on the basis of current
insights from the cognitive approach to social anxiety. Sales call anxiety is an irrepressible fear of being negatively
evaluated and rejected by a customer, and it is coupled with a desire to avoid undertaking specific functional actions
in selling situations. The authors present and test a model of SCA in two selling situations known to have threatening consequences for salespeople: canvasing and closing. The authors find that SCA consists of four components: negative self-evaluations, negative evaluations from customers, awareness of physiological symptoms (e.g.,
a queasy stomach, shaky voice, blushing), and protective actions (e.g., avoiding eye contact, fiddling with the
hands, shunning self-disclosures). The authors show that these dimensions are functions of negative affectivity and
anxiety-provoking contextual cues and that they negatively influenced the performance of 189 mortgage salespeople.

We have more fears than we need and it seems that our
utterly efficient fear conditioning system, combined with
an extremely powerful ability to think about our fears and
inability to control them, is probably at fault.
—Ttie Emotionat Brain, Joseph LeDoux

1 reluctance" or the fear of making contact
with the customer has long been recognized by
salespeople as a problem. However, no
researchers have investigated the nature of this frequently
debilitating symptom (Ingram, LaForge, and Schwepker
1997; Weitz, Castleberry, and Tanner 1992). Part of the problem is that call reluctance and fear of making a contact have
largely been left undefined. To provide a basis for the
research described in this article, we define sates catt anxiety
(SCA) as the fear of being negatively evaluated and rejected
by customers, which is accompanied by urges to avoid contact with customers or, when contact is made, to refrain from
interacting effectively and asking for a commitment.
It is estimated that SCA is prevalent in selling situations
and will reach intense levels for up to 40% of salespeople at
some points in their careers (Ray 1995). A need exists to
understand better what SCA is, when it occurs, and how to
manage it.
The goal of this article is to develop a conceptualization
of SCA that is grounded in current insights from the cognitive approach to social anxiety. Specifically, we focus on
how SCA unfolds in two selling situations (canvasing and

Willem Verbeke is SMA Chair Professor in Sales and Account Management, Erasmus University. Richard P Bagozzi is J. Hugh Liedtke Professor
of Management and Professor of Psychology, Rice University, and Dwight
F. Benton Professor of Behavioral Science in Management, Business
School, University of Michigan. The authors express their gratitude to the
anonymous J M reviewers for the many constructive recommendations
they made during the review process and to Miriam Houben for her
Insightful suggestions.

closing) and how it affects the salesperson's ability to interact with customers. In addition, we propose an operationalization of SCA and investigate how SCA relates to personality traits, anxiety-provoking contextual cues, and
performance. Our conceptualization and empirical investigation is designed to provide managers with a way to recognize and understand why salespeople seek to avoid undertaking appropriate actions before and during a sales call. We
also discuss how SCA can be controlled to the benefit of the
salesperson and firm.
The article proceeds as follows. First, we explore the concept of SCA on the basis of recent conceptualizations of social
anxiety developed by Clark and Wells (1995) and others (e.g..
Barlow 1988; Wells and Matthews 1994). We discuss the situation specificity of SCA, as well as its antecedents and consequences. Then, using a sample of 189 salespeople, we
investigate how SCA manifests itself during cold canvasing
and closing a sale, two prototypical selling situations known
to cause fear in many salespeople. We then propose managerial implications and future research questions.

Memory Processes and Selling
In the field of sales management, there is agreement that
salespeople's use of declarative and procedural memory systems during customer interaction depends on the amount of
attention resources that a salesperson has available in a
given situation (e.g., Szymansky 1988). In addition, some
researchers have pointed out that metaknowledge (e.g.,
working smarter or the willingness to learn) affects the way
salespeople use selling situation categories and the ways
they apply scripts during the sales interaction (Sujan, Weitz,
and Kumar 1994). However, only a few studies have investigated the role of memory processes with regard to emotional behavior in making a sale (e.g., Badovick, Hadaway,
and Kaminski 1992; Brown, Cron, and Slocum 1997). For
example, on the basis of research by Bagozzi, Baumgartner,
Journat of tVtarketing

88 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000

Vol. 64 (July 2000), 88-101
and Pieters (1998), Brown, Cron, and Slocum (1997) show
that selling can be regarded as a goal-directed activity and
that, in the pursuit of the sales goal, anticipated emotional
reactions to success or failure influence the actions that the
salesperson takes. In other words, goal pursuit and subsequent performance can be regarded as under self-control,
but cognitive and emotional processes also initiate and
guide goal pursuit (Bagozzi 1992).
Implicit in the process of goal pursuit, but not studied to
date, is how instrumental acts—such as selling tactics during customer interactions—are maintained or abandoned at
least temporarily, after they are initiated, and then possibly
reactivated. Simon (1967) was one of the first theorists to
suggest that the cognitive control of purposive action is
facilitated by what he termed "interrupt mechanisms" or
what psychologists now call "cognitive interference" or
"attentional disruption" (Sarason and Sarason 1986). By
this, Simon meant that ongoing goal-directed behaviors are
often evaluated, modified, and even forsaken before a goal
is reached, but purely cognitive mechanisms cannot account
for such occurrences. Simon (1967) speculated that human
emotions interrupt the cognitive control of action, so that
new assessments of progress and goal commitment can be
made and changes instituted as required. However, he never
identified the emotional processes or the way they operate.

Social Anxiety and Cognitive
Interference
Social anxiety is a strong emotion that functions as an important interrupt mechanism in goal-directed sales behaviors.
How might social anxiety manifest itself in a sales situation?
A salesperson may experience anxiety in anticipation of criticism from a supervisor, rejection by coworkers, ridicule by
peers, conflicts between work and family pressures, or conIlicts between company policies and moral standards. But
probably the most salient causes of SCA for salespeople are
the fear of failure and the disapproval and repudiation by
customers. Indeed, research in sales management suggests
that when salespeople fail, it negatively affects their feelings
of self-esteem (Goodwin, Mayo, and Hill 1997) and attributions used to explain failure, which in turn affect motivation
(Johnston and Kim 1994) and emotions negatively
(Badovick, Hadaway, and Kaminski 1992). Thus, compared
with many other occupations, personal selling seems to contain inordinate opportunities for social anxiety because of its
very nature, which includes the possibility of multiple rejections and failures on a daily basis and the effects of this
potential on feelings and thoughts about the self.
To understand how SCA might occur in different situations, we focus especially on cognitive theories of social
anxiety, as they have attracted considerable attention
recently in psychology and lead to practical remedies for
people suffering from social anxiety (Wells 1998).
Early research by Ellis (1962) and Beck (1976) suggests
that anxious people possess hypervalent cognitive schemas
(called "memory biases") that continuously present a view
of reality as dangerous and the self as vulnerable. To the
extent that anxious people possess these schemata, they will
more likely appraise or perceive a myriad of situations as

threatening, which in turn reduces their belief that they can
handle the situations or cope with them properly. Both
appraisals, which interfere with memory processes during
conversations, are thought to occur automatically and represent an ongoing integrated process. Although Beck has
updated his theory (e.g.. Beck and Freeman 1990), other
researchers have refined schema theory to describe and treat
specific types of anxiety disorders, such as social anxiety.
The most well-known theories in this regard include Barlow's (1988) anxious apprehensioh theory, Clark and
Wells's (1995) cognitive approach to social anxiety, and
Rachman's (1998) anxiety model. In contrast to previous
approaches that viewed social anxiety as a pathology, the
newer theories regard many anxiety-related problems as
common errors of normal functioning rather than as signs of
deep-rooted abnormalities (Rachman 1998, p. 64). We term
these theories "attention-based cognitive anxiety models"
(see the next section). Attention-based cognitive anxiety
models differ from the pathological conceptions of anxiety
in the following ways:
1. Early theories focused on how vigilant schemata contribute to
anxiety responses, whereas researchers now focus on attention
biases and cognitive processes that are produced in specific
situations that contain threat-related stimuli. As we discuss
subsequently, the attention of people with anxiety is focused
on extemal signals as well as intemally produced signals, such
as physiological symptoms that typify anxiety. As Mineka and
Nugent (1995, p. 187) suggest, anxiety is associated with continuous monitoring of a situation for signals of potential threat
and requires a cognitive system that scans for such threats.
2. The persistency of social anxiety is reinforced by characteristics of the anxiety response itself That is, the behavioral
responses that emerge from dysfunctional appraisals are
often involved in the maintenance of danger appraisals and
beliefs about situations, as well as people's beliefs about
themselves in those situations. In other words, if anxious
people avoid or tlee from threatening situations, they never
verify whether the situations were truly dangerous and thus
maintain anxiety and thwart learning. This perspective has
promoted contemporary remedies rooted in learning how to
control attentional processes and beliefs (e.g.. Barlow 1988;
Rachman 1998; Wells et al. 1995).
3. Some research shows that people with certain personality
traits develop situation-specific feelings of anxiety (e.g..
Barlow 1988; Rachman 1998). Among these traits are negative affectivity (NA) and anxiety sensitivity. Although
some people are prone to developing social anxieties in one
or more social situations, they do not do so necessarily in all
social situations, which suggests the need to specify
anxiety-provoking cues in any particular context.

Attention-Based Cognitive Anxiety
Models and the Components of SCA
People suffering from social anxiety typically have a strong
desire to convey a particular favorable impression of themselves to others, yet they have a marked insecurity about
their ability to do so (Clark and Wells 1995). In particular,
when they enter threatening social situations, they believe
they are in danger of behaving in an inept and unacceptable
fashion, and they think that such behaviors will have negative consequences such as of loss of self worth, status, and
rejection (see Clark and Wells 1995, p. 69). Researchers

Sales Call Anxiety / 89
stress that these thoughts enter people's minds only in difficult or amhiguous situations, in which the self-doubt erupts
more or less automatically. Clark and Wells (1995) and Barlow (1988) conceive of these anxious thoughts as "anxiety
programs," which are complex constellations of cognitive,
somatic, affective, and behavioral changes that are designed
to protect a person from harm in objectively dangerous environments. When they are activated by cues in the situation,
these anxiety programs amplify the perceived danger in
those situations. Attention to threatening or ambiguous
aspects of the situation takes priority over other cues. This
narrowed focus of attention heightens the arousal of anxiety
that subsequently becomes the focus of attention, an effect
that causes people to find further evidence for perceived
danger. As a consequence, attention overload sometimes
occurs, which negatively affects people's ability to handle
social interactions and the quality of interpersonal communication. In a selling situation, this may elicit negative reactions from customers, which anxious salespeople in turn
perceive as proof of their inability to handle similar situations. Finally, some of the self-generated symptoms might
produce further symptoms (e.g., talking too fast is often
accompanied by hyperventilation and increased heart rate).
These self-amplifying or paniclike behaviors have been
observed by psychologists (Clark and Wells 1995) as well as
by such applied scholars as Dudley and Goodson (1992, p.
77), who have found that "doomsayers experience panic
attacks" in personal selling contexts.'
Two more characteristics must be added to this self-reinforcing anxiety process. First, people high in social anxiety
are motivated to search for information that gives them
clues about how others think about them. But when people
suspect that others negatively evaluate them, they avoid
examining the full extent of the negative evaluations,
because they are afraid that people might reject them if they
ask for feedback. Second, after experiencing negative social
events, people suffering from social anxiety tend to ruminate excessively about what went wrong. This rumination
reinforces their fears of the situation and reduces their feelings of self-efficacy for handling similar situations in the
future.
Dimensions of SCA
Sales call anxiety can be considered a special case of social
anxiety (Rachman 1998). On the basis of the work of Clark
and Wells (1995), who investigate general social anxiety, we
propose the following:
H|: Sales call anxiety comprises four dimensions: negative
self-evaluations, perceived negative evaluations from customers, imagined physiological symptoms, and urges to
perform protective actions.
The four dimensions of SCA are thought to occur during,
and even before, encounters with customers.
The first dimension of SCA involves negative evaluations of the self that arise in anticipation of a specific
'Doomsayers, according to Dudley and Goodson (1992), are
salespeople who believe beforehand that they will fail. This promotes a type of call reluctance.

90 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000

encounter with a customer (e.g., in cold canvasing or closing a sale). These negative expectations entail ruminations about past failures, negative images of the self in
previous as well as anticipated sales encounters, and
exaggerated considerations of what can go wrong and
how the salesperson might fail. Examples might include
"I will not remember all the selling points and will have
trouble getting the words out" or "I will be nervous and
will lose control and look foolish." In summary, one characteristic of SCA is salespeople's insecurity about their
ability to perform well and convey a positive impression
to customers.
The second dimension of SCA is imagined negative
evaluations from customers. People who have high social
anxiety have a strong desire to convey favorable impressions of themselves to others, which is coupled with a strong
need to know what others think about them (Clark and Wells
1995; Pozo et al. 1991). Yet their insecurity about their ability to perform and communicate a positive image, which is
exacerbated by excessive rumination about past failures and
images of future failure, creates strong anticipatory social
anxiety. Research suggests further that people high in social
anxiety tend to set unrealistically high standards for social
performance (Clark and Wells 1995). Examples might
include "I must get every customer to like me" or "I must
not ever show signs that I do not know my product or do not
believe in my product."
A third dimension of SCA is salespeople's physiological
symptoms, which they use to infer customers' reactions to
themselves and to appraise how well they are doing overall.
Common reactions to SCA, which are largely uncontrollable at least at the outset, are a pounding heart, an unsteady
voice, blushing, sweating, and shaky hands. Physiological
symptoms reflect the fact that social anxiety is mediated
primarily by the sympathetic nervous system. However, the
parasympathetic nervous system sometimes comes into
play as well and is manifested by a drop in blood pressure
and accompanying feelings of dizziness or weakness. Moreover, it has been found that people who have high social
anxiety tend to overestimate how negatively others evaluate
them in interpersonal exchanges (Rapee and Lim 1992;
Stopa and Clark 1993), which amplifies the communicators' perceptions of their own physiological symptoms and
further increases social anxiety. A preoccupation with physiological symptoms causes salespeople to see themselves
from the point of view of the customer, and this adds to
their negative self-evaluation (e.g., "The customer sees that
I am nervous and sweating profusely and will think I am
incompetent").
The final dimension of SCA involves urges to perform
protective actions. These are similar to what clinical psychologists term "safety-seeking behaviors" (e.g., Clark and
Wells 1995; Wells et al. 1995), or what psychologists call
emotion-regulatory processes or coping responses (Barret
1995; Lazarus and Folkman 1984; Thoits 1990). Psychologists have found that one of the symptoms or concomitants
of social anxiety is an automatic reaction that reduces or
relieves the negative effects of anxiety. Protective actions
encompass a wide range of coping responses, many of
which are involuntary but some of which can be learned or
unlearned to a certain extent as personal policies. Examples
of the more involuntary protective actions are gazing away
and avoiding eye contact, speaking quickly, and fiddling
with the hands. Protective actions more subject to self-control entail avoiding self-disclosures, saying less, controlling
the breathing (e.g., taking deep breaths, breathing slowly),
or standing still. It also entails salespeople focusing on their
own and the customer's voice and feet or holding onto
objects. Withdrawal from contact and avoidance of future
contact are extreme and usually dysfunctional protective
actions.
In accordance with Clark and Wells (1995), we believe
it is fruitful to conceive of SCA as functioning in two ways.
First, SCA manifests itself along a continuum from no social
anxiety to severe, debilitating social anxiety; second, SCA
manifests itself in a broad range of situations known to provoke social anxiety. More concretely, one salesperson might
experience intense SCA during cold canvasing but only
moderate levels of SCA during the finalizing stages in sales
conversation, or vice versa, whereas others might experience high SCA in both situations (e.g., Dudley and Goodson
1992). Similarly, Singh and Rhoads (1991) suggest that a
salesperson might experience role ambiguity related to colleagues but not to customers, and vice versa. When SCA is
examined as a situation-specific enigma, it appears less
pathological and therefore perhaps more manageable if we
focus on the self-attentive processes that cause anxiety in
those situations. This situation-specific perspective is at the
center of discussion among specialists working on social
phobia (e.g., Heckelman and Schneier 1995; Rachman
1998). Therefore,
H2: Salespeople undergo SCA as a matter of degree that ranges
from low to high levels of intensity. Two common contexts
in which salespeople face SCA are canvasing (cold calling) and closing the sale.
Antecedents and Consequences of SCA
Personatity as an antecedent of SCA. Although the focus
of our research is situational aspects of SCA, we also wish to
investigate one antecedent to social anxiety identified in psychology and organizational behavior research. Negative affectivity is a disposition of mood and indicates the general vulnerability of people to experiencing aversive emotional states:
"High-NA individuals tend to be distressed and upset and have
a negative view of self, whereas those low in the dimension are
relatively content and secure and satisfied with themselves"
(Watson and Clark 1984, p. 465). We think of NA as a type of
trait social anxiety, a personality characteristic that endows or
predisposes people to react emotionally in a particular way
across many situations. Watson and Clark (1984, p. 465) note
that NA is a unidimensional construct, and people scoring high
in NA possess chronic subjective feelings of nervousness, tension, and worry. Furthermore, Watson and Clark (1984, p. 466,
emphasis in original) posit that NA manifests itself even in the
absence of any overt stress and people high in NA are, "in any
given situation, more titcety to experience a significant level of
distress." In summary, NA is a disposition that reflects individual differences in the general propensity to experience negative emotions and have a negative view of the self.

Some research supports the effects of NA on job stress
and job strain for professional and managerial personnel in
an insurance company (Brief et al. 1988) and for employees
across a wide range of companies (Chen and Spector 1991;
see also Watson and Pennebaker 1989). Thus, we propose
the following:
H3: Negative affectivity will positively influence SCA.
That is, we expect NA to increase the salesperson's SCA in
particular situations. Support for this hypothesis can be
found in Watson and Clark's (1984, p. 466) review of the literature, in which they document that high-trait anxious people react more strongly to stressful situations but, in the
absence of stress, tend to be no more anxious than those low
in NA. Likewise, given the tendency of high-NA people to
fixate on and magnify mistakes, frustrations, disappointments, and threats (Watson and Clark 1984, p. 466), we
expect that NA should also accentuate SCA. In addition to
main effects for NA, we explore whether NA interacts with
anxiety-provoking cues to infiuence SCA.
Dependence of SCA on situationat cues. Canvasing and
closing are distinct global settings in which selling occurs,
but the real determinants of SCA are particular anxietyprovoking cues that function within these settings. Different
anxiety-provoking cues can be identified for canvasing and
closing. This interpretation is consistent with modern definitions of social anxiety that regard it as "a persistent fear of
one or more social or performance situations in which a person is exposed" (American Psychiatric Association 1994,
pp. 416-17; see also Wells and Mathews 1994).
For canvasing, one source of anxiety arises when salespeople must initiate contacts with prospects who are of a
higher rank or social status. The salespeople are put in an
awkward position because of differences in expectations and
customs that might arise as well as the uncertainty whether
deference must be shown, and if so, how, when, and how
much. A second source of anxiety in canvasing settings is the
near-universal reaction people have to meeting new people.
Without prior knowledge of the needs, style, and other characteristics of a prospect, the salesperson is asked to face a set
of unknowns that could thwart the initial encounter. A third
source of anxiety stems from the need to create a favorable
first impression and convey specific positive qualities of the
selling organization and its offerings. In summary, canvasing
presents a vague and ambiguous situation in which things can
go wrong and the salesperson is vulnerable to rejection and
failure. The anticipation of concerns about the unknown activates SCA programs. In summary, we suggest the following:
H4: Sales call anxiety will be provoked to the extent that salespeople are sensitive to difficulties in dealing with new customers, higher status customers, and the conveyance of
positive qualities of their firm and its products, three facets
of canvasing.
For closing, the strongest source of anxiety is the anticipation of asking for a commitment and being rejected. So
much rides on obtaining a commitment that the dread of
failure is a real and feared consequential outcome. Anticipatory anxiety here can lead to cycles of approach-avoidance,
procrastination, and strong apprehension. Another source of

Sales Call Anxiety / 91
anxiety is the thought of admitting failure to a supervisor
and explaining why a failure occurred. Therefore, we submit
the following:
H5: Sales call anxiety will be provoked to the extent that
salespeople are sensitive to difficulties associated with
asking for a commitment and discussing their performance
in this regard with their supervisor, two aspects of closing
sales.

Performance-related consequences of SCA. Sales call
anxiety intrudes into goal-directed activities and potentially
infiuences performance negatively. How might this happen?
One way that performance might be negatively affected is
through the interference that social anxiety produces in
customer-seller exchanges. Parallel to psychological
researchers, we expect social anxiety to drain or limit salespeople's attentional resources and so interfere with their
ability to process social cues (Clark and Wells 1995; Rachman 1998). Related to this is the general tendency in people
high in social anxiety not to "check out what is really happening" in interpersonal encounters (Clark and Wells 1995,
p. 72). People who have high social anxiety "are not data
driven but function rather like an automatic program which
is activated in a social situation," and this leads them "to run
through a repertoire of negative thoughts without really paying attention to what is happening in the situation" (Stopa
and Clark 1993, p. 264). This behavior is dysfunctional for
personal selling, in which complex communications of
needs and offers and counteroffers are necessary for success.
But not only failures in attention occur as a consequence
of social anxiety in interpersonal communication. Excessive
self-focus, negative self-evaluations, and overestimation of
how negatively others view the self can lead to protective
actions that make the salesperson high in SCA come across
as unfriendly and unresponsive. This, in turn, can elicit similar reactions from customers. The result can be a breakdown in communication and lowered performance.
Likewise, when social anxiety induces or is accompanied by negative self-evaluations by salespeople, they may
become discouraged and avoid certain interactions. Even if
they do not give up completely, salespeople high in SCA
may become distracted by their feelings and physiological
symptoms, which makes them work nervously and display
less attention to the task at hand. Disruptive social anxiety
may also interfere with working smart (Sujan, Weitz, and
Kumar 1994). All these reactions can depress performance.
As a consequence, we propose the following:
Hg! The greater the perceived SCA, the poorer is the performance in terms of communication effectiveness and sales
volume.

Method
Salespeople and Procedure
A total of 500 questionnaires was sent to salespeople from a
subdivision of a large Dutch bank. The salespeople sell
mortgages to consumers. Of these salespeople, 196 returned
the questionnaires. After eliminating incomplete questionnaires, we ended up with a final sample of 189 salespeople.

92 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000

for a 38% response rate. On the basis of comparisons of
sociodemographic characteristics and length of tenure, no
significant differences were found between respondents and
the entire sales force.
Mortgage sellers are ideal subjects for a study of SCA
because they engage in extensive cold canvasing, face many
rejections in the course of a day, work in a competitive environment, are compensated on the basis of commission, and
undergo constant pressure to sell to customers across a wide
spectrum of status and other differences. The sample can be
described as follows: A majority of the salespeople were
men (approximately 60%), most were younger than 35 years
of age (approximately 52%), and a few salespeople (approximately 9%) were more than 50 years of age. With respect to
experience, approximately 10% of the sample had been with
the organization less than 2 years, and almost 55% had been
with the company between 6 and 20 years. Finally, most
salespeople had finished basic and advanced vocational
studies, but only 1 % had a university or college degree. This
profile is typical of mortgage salespeople across Europe.

Questionnaire Design
To construct a pool of items for measuring each of the variables under study, focus group interviews were organized,
and several tests were administered in stages, as DeVellis
suggests (1991). Managers were asked to identify situations
in which their salespeople encounter apprehension problems
when selling. The managers identified canvasing and closing as the most anxiety-provoking contexts for selling mortgage-related products. The identification of canvasing and
closing as general anxiety-provoking contexts is consistent
with basic research in psychology on anxiety. Indeed, a key
defining element of social anxiety is that it occurs in situations that imply initial encounters (e.g., new customers),
consist of people who are regarded as having power or status, or place the person in a conspicuous position that is difficult to escape physically (Beatty 1988, Buss 1980; Leary
and Kowalski 1995). Thus, analogous to when a person
stands before an audience to give a speech, canvasing and
closing demand that the salesperson make actual contact
with a customer and provide specific information and
requests. To avoid doing this or to flee from such encounters, which is an urge salespeople with SCA frequently feel,
would be dysfunctional for the salesperson and the firm.
Canvasing and closing are also two activities that occupy
much of the time of mortgage salespeople in the setting at
hand and are among the most important tasks they perform.
Therefore, questions presented to subsequent focus
groups were tailored to address these two prototypical contexts to generate items for the final questionnaire. First,
items that refiect the four dimensions of SCA typical for
canvasing and closing were generated for presentation in
focus groups (as proposed by Clark and Wells 1995; Leary
1991; Patterson and Ritts 1997; Salkovskis 1991; Trower
and Gilbert 1989). Second, for each of the two situations, we
generated specific anxiety-provoking cues by asking salespeople to specify particular events and experiences that created the most anxiety for them. Five anxiety-provoking cues
were identified: three for cold canvasing (i.e., dealing with
higher-status customers; preparing for and communicating
with new, unknown customers; and conveying positive characteristics of the bank and its offerings) and two for closing
(i.e., asking for a commitment and discussing performance
with the sales manager). All items contributed to the initial
item pool were evaluated for relevance and understandability by salespeople and sales managers. It should be noted
that the salespeople who participated in focus group interviews were not part of the final sampling.
Questions presented on the final questionnaire followed
standard practice in the literature. The actual questionnaire
was administered in Dutch, but a rough English translation
is presented here (for a list of all items discussed subsequently, see the Appendix). To avoid carryover effects, the
questionnaires were printed in three different versions
wherein different sections of the questionnaire were counterbalanced (Bickart 1993). The SCA items were introduced
with an instruction that attempted to put the salesperson in
the position of anticipating or engaging in a sales interaction. Specifically, the items were introduced with the following question for cold canvasing: "You are having a first
conversation with a new and unknown customer. You will
have to ask many personal and significant questions about
the person's income, financial situation, and family situation.... A successful conversation is important to you. What
is going on in your mind?"
These instructions were followed by items designed to
measure the four dimensions of SCA in canvasing situations. For negative self-evaluations, six items were used
(e.g., "I hope I will not shut down/clam up"). For negative
evaluations from customers, ten items were used (e.g., "The
customer will think that I do not come across convincingly"). Physiological symptoms were measured by seven
items (e.g., "I am nervous," "I cannot sit still during periods
of salience"). Finally, protective actions were measured with
six items (e.g., "I adopt a passive attitude"). Responses for
these SCA measures were recorded on seven-point, "completely disagree" to "completely agree" scales.
For sales closing, the SCA items were introduced with
the following instructions:
After the orienting discussion with customers, you engage
the customer in conversation. You want to make tlnal tiie
sale. During this phase, you need to communicate some
details about the offer before the mortgage contract can be
signed. You have communicated to your manager that you
will attempt to make final the sale, and now you realize
that obtaining the offer is important to you. What is going
on in your mind?
Similar to those for cold canvasing, the four dimensions of
SCA were measured separately for closing. Specifically, for
negative self-evaluations, six items were used (e.g., "I hope
I will not rattle off my words"). For negative evaluations
from customers, eight items were used (e.g., "He will laugh
at me afterward"). Physiological symptoms were measured
by seven items (e.g., "I become panicky"). Finally, protective actions were measured with five items (e.g., "I offer the
customer too many extras").
To measure cues within the four contexts that provoke
SCA, we asked salespeople to respond to 24 items introduced by the following instructions: "In the following situations, you might experience a certain amount of tension

and uneasy feelings. Please indicate the extent to which you
experience tension." Then the following items were presented: 4 for meeting with customers of a higher status, 5 for
preparing for and communicating with new prospects, 4 for
being assertive about themselves and the organization, 7 for
approaching and asking for a commitment from customers,
and 4 for discussing performance with a manager. Responses were recorded on seven-point, "never experience
tension" to "always experience tension" scales.
Performance was measured with a scale based on
research by Behrman and Perreault (1984). One component
of performance referred to sales volume and was measured
with five items (e.g., "exceeding all sales targets and objectives," "generating sales of new company products"). The
second component of performance addressed communication quality in the sales interaction and was measured with
four items (e.g., "convincing customers that you understand
their unique problems and concerns," "working out solutions to customers' questions or objections"). Finally, NA
was measured with a scale from Tellegen (1982). Twenty
items were used.

Scale Development
Exploratory factor analyses were performed on the items for
each scale. Maximum likelihood estimation with oblimin
rotation was done with SPSS (1993). The different dimensions of scales were analyzed, and items not satisfying the
following criteria were deleted: (1) dominant loadings
greater than .40 and (2) cross-loadings less than .25. Criteria for accepting factors relied on inspection of the scree plot
(Briggs and Cheek 1986; DeVellis 1991). The reliabilities
for items of dimensions of each scale were also assessed by
means of Cronbach's alpha.
In Table 1, we summarize the results of factor analyses
for each scale: Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin of all the factor analyses are substantial. In addition, the factor loadings are generally high, and the lowest loading is equal to .41. Note
especially that two factors corresponding to cold canvasing
and closing were found for negative self-evaluations and for
perceived negative evaluations from others. The items measuring physiological responses and protective actions each
loaded on one factor. Not surprisingly, physiological
responses and protective actions reflect reactions that are
governed by the autonomic nervous system and thus tend to
be invariant across different anxiety-provoking situations.
We also present the reliabilities of each scale in Table 1. The
reliabilities are high and exceed the minimum of .70 recommended by Nunnally (1978). Finally, means and standard
deviations of measures are provided in Table 1.

Regression models
To take measurement error into account formally and avoid
problems of multicollinearity, we used LISREL (Joreskog
and Sorbom 1996) to perform regressions. One set of regressions tested the effects of anxiety-provoking situations and
NA on SCA: SCAj = f(anxiety provoking cuesj, NA); four
regressions were run, one for each dimension of SCA (i.e.,
negative self-evaluation, negative evaluation from customers, physiological symptoms, and protective actions).
Three anxiety-provoking cues (i.e., dealing with higher-

Sales Call Anxiety / 93
CJ>

CO

00

o
(0

if

1
00

o
o

o
CJ)

oq oq

ay

cvi

in

r

o

(0

U
(0

in

o~

ra
u

oJ
O
CM

i

(0

CO
CM
CM

00
CO

CM

o

in

CM
CO

CJ

Oi

CO
CO

cvi

CM
Cvi

CO

U

o
ra

in

I
Q

o
O)

•E

ra

•o

o

c
n

V)

CO
CJ>

00

o

CO

00
00

O5

CO

00

05

U

U

•a
ra

t

to"

00

CVj
CO

t
CD

oo

CO
in

CO

o>

00

to

ra
o>

C .E c
O

ra
a.
lU <

0)

00

CO

d

'^

CO
CO

(J)

CJ)

CM CO
0 5 CO

cd

in

g

in

^

00

UJ

(0

2
o

CM
cd

CO

go,
o
a>
O)

iu

CO

00

00

CO

I?

CM

05

o
o

cu
00

in

CO

iri

CO
CM

o

o

in

oo

CO

U

(A
O)

CO

in
CJ)

i

c

T5

Toq

CO

o

O
r~.

lAci

X
UJ

co

o

ot

in

in

.a
E

CO

if

CM

CO

CO

C
O

U

CO

c

ro
_3

gative
rom c

ro

o >
> <
u

B (0
LUO

g

d)

94 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000

o

o
a.

I
CO

E

g

c
g

ro

ro
o
o

il.

O

CO
CO

CJ

®

.. 2 E

o

a.

E
1_

CO CO
^

^

ororo
•tcow
0.

O)
0)

*5

ro

C

o

licati

Ui

4
in

CO

og
(U

I

Oi

CM
CO

new
ners

2.
Q

CO
CC

o
oq

in to

oq

ovol
high
tus cus

a

O)

in

oq

cq

CO

0)
o>

r

4

in

CO

?2
d) U

c
O

O

c

•£

g

CD

o ™

o "
.o ?
ro o

'

D) c
O

"co

<

^

§1
o

a>

C Q.
O

b
status customers, meeting new customers, and conveying
qualities of the organization) plus NA were entered as independent variables for canvasing, and two anxiety-provoking
cues (i.e., asking for commitment and anticipating discussion of performance with managers) plus NA were entered
as independent variables for closing sales. The second set of
regressions tested the effects of SCA on performance:
performance^ - fCSCAj); two regressions were run, one for
each dimension of performance (i.e., communication performance and volume performance).
To test for interactions between NA and anxietyprovoking cues on SCA, we performed two analyses. First,
we used classical regression analysis in which the main
effects of NA and anxiety-provoking cues, as well as their
interaction, were entered. This approach uses full information but does not take into account measurement error. Second, to take into account measurement error, we split the
sample at the median based on SCA and used structural
equation models to do the regressions. This approach does
not use full information in the sense of classical regression
analysis. Thus, there is a trade-off between the two procedures. Unfortunately, because of sample size restrictions and
the need to represent four interactions and five main effects,
structural equation models with latent variables cannot be
used both to take into account measurement error and to use
full information underlying the interactions.

Results
In Table 2, we summarize the findings for the regression
analyses. In the top panel, low to moderately high amounts
of variance in SCA are explained, and all anxiety-provoking
cues influence the four dimensions of SCA during both canvasing and closing. Nevertheless, several interesting patterns of results can be pointed out. During both canvasing
and closing, the effects of anxiety-provoking stimuli are
greatest on physiological symptoms and protective actions.
Yet strong effects are found on negative self-evaluations and
negative evaluations from customers for most anxietyprovoking stimuli. For each dimension of SCA, the effects
of the anxiety-provoking stimuli are roughly equal during
both canvasing and closing. When closing and canvasing are
compared, the effects of anxiety-provoking stimuli on SCA
are greater for the former. This occurs as well in the higher
levels of explained variance for the dimensions of SCA during closing compared with canvasing. Finally, NA influenced all fourdimensions of SCA during both canvasing and
closing. The effects of NA were generally higher on physiological symptoms and protective actions than on negative
self-evaluations and negative evaluations from customers,
and higher coefficients occurred for closing than canvasing.
In the bottom panel of Table 2, relatively small but significant proportions of variance are explained in performance by
SCA. For both canvasing and closing, more variance is
explained in volume than communication performance. Physiological symptoms and protective actions have the largest
effects on performance, yet the effects of negative self-evaluations and negative evaluations from customers are substantial.
The tests for interaction revealed that NA does not moderate effects of anxiety-provoking cues on SCA. No signifi-

cant interactions were found under either canvasing or closing. Therefore, the results for the direct effects of NA on
anxiety-provoking cues can be interpreted as main effects.

Discussion
Sales call anxiety can be conceived as an intrusive and persistent apprehension about dealing with customers in particular selling situations and is triggered by specific cues in
those situations. Rather than being a simple global fear
response, however, SCA was found to consist of four components: negative self-evaluations, imagined negative evaluations from customers, physiological responses, and protective actions. The experience of SCA, which is believed to
happen largely automatically and unconsciously, was found
to differ between cold canvasing and closing for negative
self-evaluations and imagined negative evaluations from
customers. Anxiety-provoking cues had larger effects on
negative self-evaluation and negative evaluation from customers for closing than canvasing. In contrast, SCA with
regard to physiological responses and protective actions was
invariant across the two types of selling situations (i.e., similar physiological responses and protective actions, as a
function of anxiety-provoking cues, occurred during canvasing and closing).
Negative self-evaluations involve insecurities with
respect to situational appearances or characteristics, actions
in sales encounters, or outcomes thereof. These are especially manifested in ruminations about past failures or shortcomings and images of difficulties and failure in anticipated
sales encounters. The danger of a preoccupation with negative self-evaluations is that it can discourage and immobilize
the salesperson, limit attentional resources needed to remain
alert as well as be adaptive, and disrupt the processing of
social cues and other aspects of interpersonal communication when customers are engaged. An important characteristic of SCA is that salespeople generate anxiety programs
automatically. The data in this study suggest that SCA
potentially makes salespeople vulnerable to dysfunctional
social anxiety across a variety of selling situations, and this
in turn negatively correlates with the salesperson's performance. Many salespeople who do not suffer from strong
SCA may be able to hide their anxiety, because they choose
to focus on less threatening selling activities or consciously
or unconsciously avoid situations that provoke anxiety.
Consequently, they may be relatively unaware that they
experience anxiety in a detrimental way.
Sales managers should learn to detect signs of SCA in
their salespeople. In many cases, sales managers will notice
second-order effects of anxiety when sales force strategies
change or when salespeople change accounts. For example,
imagine a change from a defender strategy to a prospector
strategy, in which salespeople are instructed to increase their
quota of new customer contacts (Miles and Snow 1978).
Such a change may be difficult for salespeople who feel
anxious and threatened by such environmental and task disruptions. Innovations in automated selling and management
might be able to incorporate ways to monitor SCA here.
Current decision support systems enable managers to detect
useful ratios (e.g., sales from new customers versus existing

Sales Call Anxiety / 95
00

1
CO

CM

CO

CO

in
0)
> M

11
t
CM

CO
CNJ

oo
CO

CM
CO

(0

o

o>
o
o

00

r

CO

CO

CO

CM

r

(0 C
>. >>

o>
c

in

CVl

•2 c

in

.CO)

a.

o
(0
0)
3

I.

o o
atB

U
O)

l

Il

C

CO

r

CO

i

§
2

1 .-2
CO

LO
CO

CO
CO

CO
CM

CO

o

CM (0

o

CD '•§

CM

> (fl

'.? c

U O

O)

CM

CO

•22
•S ?•
in E
> >

O)

O

.£ So.
s

CM
LO

u

lO
CM

Q
-

_ ifl

.III

O)
CO

CO
CM

CO
CO
CO

c»

c
(0
E

o
t:
a
>
Q.

96 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000

>

"5
ffecti

iluat
mer
o o

rotec

al

_

•^

r

3

UJ

r

o

u
(0

n

UJ

hysio
ympt

0) (0

ri
« «

<
o

"o
c

3

>

egati
from

•S «

Ition

c
.2 «
(u O

00
CO

0)
O

Of

CO
3

r

CM

.^5

LO

r

§,§5

C
O
tn

•o i i
a> CD

II

Is
i
i
i
o

E

5§
w" V
customers) that might indicate salespeople's preferences for
dealing with existing as opposed to new customers. Imbalances resulting in too many sales from existing customers
might signal a problem in SCA for management to address.
Sales managers should be careful not to jump to the conclusion that a salesperson in this case necessarily suffers from
SCA. It is possible that the salesperson lacks certain selling
skills and this deficit causes avoidance of new account
prospecting.
When a sales manager detects symptoms of SCA in one
or more salespeople, the next task is to make the salespeople aware that their performance suffers from SCA. Managers then should inquire why their salespeople, for example, find it difficult to achieve closure during interactions
with customers. By doing so, the salespeople and their managers make the SCA programs explicit (e.g., " I now realize
that I do not visit new customers because I fear negative
consequences of canvasing"). When they are made explicit,
such disclosures can help both salespeople and sales managers start joint problem solving to overcome SCA. Thus,
the sales manager should take a coaching attitude when
salespeople are prone to develop anxiety. One coaching suggestion for sales managers is that they should avoid any
stigmatization about SCA. This can be done by suggesting
to salespeople that anxiety is contingent on situational cues
that can be anticipated and coped with and that everybody
will experience SCA to a certain degree now and then. In
other words, SCA is not necessarily a pathological problem
(Rachman 1998). When salespeople recognize that they are
vulnerable to anxiety in certain circumstances, this sets the
stage for various corrective efforts by management and selfregulation by salespeople.
We propose that one or more of six different programs
can be used to address SCA:
Freeing up memory space ttirough disconnected niindfutness. One approach to counteracting negative self-evaluations is to help salespeople reengineer their anxiety programs. This might involve helping the salesperson
disconfirm counterproductive thoughts and negative selfappraisals during a sales visit. Constructive self-analyses
can be promoted by training the salesperson to ask such
questions as "What evidence do you use to infer that the customer might not like you?" and "What signals do you look
for when visiting customers?" Such questions might help
salespeople develop self- and task-related metaknowledge
(knowledge about their self-knowledge of SCA) that
enables them to break through the cycle of escalating anticipatory fears consciously (e.g., Clark and Wells 1995). By
teaching salespeople to focus on cues that reflect positive
information and avoid cues that normally trigger overwhelming fears, which tend to occupy the salespeople's
memory and prevent the activation of adaptive resources,
the salespeople can learn to free up attentional resources.
Key adaptive resources to emphasize include the matching
of customer needs to the seller's offerings and matching the
selling strategy and specific appeals to customer type. This
entails the adaptive selling tactics of "knowing what" and
"knowing how" (Weitz 1978, 1981). Only by managing
anxiety will it be possible for salespeople to practice adap-

tive selling behavior and work smarter (e.g., Spiro and Weitz
1989; Sujan, Weitz, and Kumar 1994).
Reduction of approval seetcing. Imagined negative evaluations from customers will be intensified to the extent that
salespeople have an excessive need for approval but are
afraid of asking for it (Baumeister and Leary 1995; Strickland 1977). Therefore, it is advisable to (1) help salespeople
better understand what their self-worth is based on, so as not
to overemphasize the need for approval (or to not be disapproved) for its own sake, and (2) recruit salespeople who are
not obsessive in the need for approval and help them
develop impression management techniques that focus more
on customer and product knowledge than on approval seeking and the need to belong, per se. This is not to suggest,
however, that though the sales job is inherently stressful,
sales managers should not seek to reduce, for example, role
stress. Role stress has been known to evoke feelings of job
tension and dysphoric cognitions (Singh 1993; Kramer
1994), and those levels of job tension and dysphoric cognitions might make salespeople vulnerable to rejection by the
customer and so bring about the development of SCA.
Relaxation. Sales call anxiety is also exhibited through
physiological symptoms. Excessive self-focus on shaky
hands, sweating, a quiver in the voice, or an upset stomach
can aggravate social anxiety and interfere with interpersonal
communication. One way to offset the effects of physiological symptoms is to concentrate on relaxing the muscles during a sales interaction. Indeed, through practice, relaxation
techniques can have generalized, long-term effects on reducing anxiety. In more extreme cases of SCA, such as before
closing a big deal that has been worked on for months, pharmacological agents such as beta-blockers may be called for
(Liebowitz et al. 1988). Encouragement to engage in sports
or recreation outside work might promote relaxation and
renewal (Zillman, Johnson, and Day 1974). Some firms have
in-house athletic facilities and counseling services and sponsor periodic educational and recreational programs designed
to relieve tension and social anxiety. Finally, with practice in
selling and the development of interpersonal skills and product and customer knowledge, the negative effects of physiological symptoms should become easier to control.
Regutatory tactics. Protective actions, the fourth component of SCA, are potentially the most damaging in two
senses. At the point of salesperson-customer interactions,
such protective actions as averting eye contact, speaking
quickly, avoiding self-disclosures, or prematurely withdrawing in an effort to escape embarrassment can break down
trust and lead customers to question the competence of the
salesperson. Even before salespeople engage with customers,
such protective actions as putting off calling a customer or
other forms of procrastination, refusing to answer a telephone call from a customer, or planning to avoid certain topics in an anticipated exchange can lead to lost sales. Probably the most important remedy for overcoming dysfunctional
protective actions is to develop self-regulatory tactics. To the
extent that salespeople can develop scripts to activate when
customers raise questions or objections or when the salespeople feel an urge to avoid encounters or discuss issues in
an exchange, they will be better able to handle SCA and

Sales Call Anxiety / 97
function effectively. For example, when salespeople experience setbacks after a customer asks a product-related question to which they have no answer, they can respond by saying something to the effect of "I cannot answer your question
fully now but will call you back this afternoon after I investigate our policies more completely." Other self-management
tactics can be developed to select alternative ways to make a
sale, monitor goal striving, maintain commitment, and overcome impediments to goal achievement.
Professional treatment. On occasion, SCA may become
overwhelming, and professional help may be required. In
such cases, it is likely that salespeople will display anxiety
across a broad range of selling situations, which makes their
successful functioning in the sales force more doubtful. For
some salespeople, several therapies are available for coping
with social anxiety (Heimberg et al. 1995), and various selfhelp programs may be useful (e.g.. Cheek 1989; Markaway
etal. 1992).
Selection criteria. The data in this study show that the
personality trait NA is correlated with the development of
SCA. The NA scale may thus be used as an additional criterion in the selection of salespeople and perhaps as a check
later in a salesperson's career to verify whether cynicism has
developed over time. Research shows that other personality
traits, such as fear of negative evaluation and anxiety sensitivity for which scales exist, may be associated with social
anxiety (Peterson and Reiss 1987; Rachman 1998).

Further Research
A topic not considered in the present study but worthy of
further research is the possibility that, at least for some
salespeople, anxiety can spur them on to greater achieve-

ments. Some athletes and performers arouse themselves in
preparation for a performance through anxiety-provoking
thoughts to energize their performances (e.g.. Martens
1977). It is possible that SCA can be functional for some
salespeople under certain conditions. To uncover these possibilities, it may be necessary to model the relationships of
SCA to its correlates in nonlinear patterns. For example,
SCA might initially stimulate salespeople, but when a
threshold is reached, it will not have an effect. Yet some
point later, under intense SCA, performance may be negatively affected. This will be difficult to study in the field and
may require special laboratory testing with salespeople
under controlled conditions.
It is most likely that SCA emerges during the sales
career. Johnston and Kim (1994, p. 78), for example, suggest
that "Many new salespeople quit their job within a few
months because they are not suited for it after a few
instances of failure." As sales become more automated and
selling patterns more easily detected, the sales environment
is a good opportunity to study causal relationships between
failure and the onset of SCA.
In our study, we focus on anxiety in salespeople that
stems from relationships with customers (only one dimension—talking with a manager about performance—did not
directly reflect relationships with customers). Anxiety might
also be expected to result from interactions with other salespeople, sales managers, friends, and family members. These
possibilities deserve further study.
Finally, it would be informative to investigate other
forms of self-conscious emotions in personal selling.
Shame, embarrassment, guilt, and pride are likely candidates for study, but they have not been studied systematically in personal selling to date.

APPENDIX
Scales and Items Used in the Study
I. Negative self-evaluation
A. Cold canvasing
1. I hope this conversation does not flop.
2. I hope I will not shut down/clam up.
3. I hope I will not come across as an unfriendly
person to the customer.
4. I hope I will not get the idea again that I am not
able to have a constructive conversation.
5. I hope it will not turn out that advisors from other
banks will be able to convince the customer better than I will.
6. I hope the customer will not turn away from me
when I keep asking questions.
B. Closing
1. I hope I can listen carefully to what the customer
says.
2. I hope I will not start stuttering.
3. I hope I will not fumble for words.
4. I hope I will be alert.
5. I hope I will not give in to the customer.
6. I hope I will not waffle my words.

98 / Journal of IVIarketing, July 2000

II. Perceived negative evaluation from customers
A. Cold canvasing
1. The customer will think the following of me:
a. there you have again another "salesperson."
b. that I do not come across as a credible salesperson.
c. that I do not get across convincingly.
d. that I have doubts.
e. that I adopt a kind attitude to prevent refusal.
f. that bad experiences lie ahead for him with
this salesperson.
2. I will think of the customer:
a. that he only smiles to get rid of me.
b. that he does not like me.
c. that he will tell other people that I am not a
competent person.
d. that he will tell other people that I am a very
accommodating person.
B. Closing
1. The customer will think the following of me
a. that I do not have authority.
APPENDIX
Continued
b.
c.
d.
e.
f.

that I am not professional.
that I am an insecure person.
that I am not able to sell.
that I am not reliable.
that advisors from other banks are better
salespeople than I.
2. I will think of the customer:
a. that he will laugh at me afterward.
b. that he will not do any more business with me
at a later stage.
III. Physiological symptoms
A. I am nervous.
B. I talk less quickly.
C. I repeat myself.
D. My thoughts are wandering off.
E. I suddenly start to stutter.
F. I look away from the customer.
G. I set out to talk louder now.
H. I become panicky.
I. I am already getting tired now.
J. I cannot observe a silence.
K. I dare not look the customer in the eyes.
L. My hands are trembling.
M. I am going to say too much.
N. I am losing control over the conversation.
IV. Protective actions
A. I avoid direct questions like "Please will you sign the
offer?"
B. I quickly change the subject.
C. I adopt a passive attitude.
D. I think "I should not ask this.... I find this topic
irrelevant."
E. I offer the customer too many extras.
F. I say, "You do not have to decide now; you may think
it over."
G. I say to the customer, "You need not answer all
questions."
H. I often apologize quickly to the customer.
I. I talk less persuasively to the customer.
J. I dare not argue with the customer.
V. Cues in situations that provoke SCA
A. Meeting customers with higher social status
1. I talk to a customer with a higher income.
2. I talk to someone who has a higher professional
status than I have (e.g., the director of a firm).
3. I talk to a well-known Dutchman.
4. I talk to a customer with an impressive appearance and self-assured manner.
B. Preparing for and communicating with new
prospects
1. I prepare for a sales conversation with a new and
unknown prospect in my sales office.
2. I prepare for a conversation with an unknown
prospect.
3. Unexpectedly, I have to meet an unknown
prospect because my colleague is excused.
4. I prepare myself for a sales conversation with an
unknown prospect at his home.
5. Unexpectedly, I have to meet a prospect who
walked into the sales office.

C. Being assertive about self and organization
1. I have to tell the customers, "We are the best
bank."
2. I have to explain to the customers what we have
to offer to them compared with other competing
banks.
3. I have to explain to the customers what they
might expect from me during and after the conversation.
4. I have to explain to the customers what I now
expect from them during the sales conversation.
D. Discussing performance with the sales manager
1. I have to share my sales results with my manager.
2. I have to talk with my manager about my sales
results.
3. I have to discuss my sales results with my manager, and it appears I did not achieve my sales
quotas.
4. I have to tell my sales manager how I go about
selling mortgages to customers.
E. Approaching and asking for a commitment from the
customer
1. The customer tells me that my offer is too expensive, and I have to refute this.
2. The customer shows me the offer from a competitor, and I have to reply to this.
3. I ask customers whether they know someone
else who would be interested in buying mortgages from my bank.
4. I ask customers if they are interested In buying a
mortgage from my bank.
5. Beyond my expectations, customers appear to be
well informed about mortgage offers from competitors.
6. I ask customers whether they will sign the mortgage contract.
7. Customers speak to me about something that I
do not do well (e.g., I did not show up at an
appointment).
VI. Performance
A. Sales volume
1. Producing a high market share for your company
in your territory.
2. Generating high levels of dollar sales.
3. Quickly generating sales of new company products.
4. Identifying and selling to major accounts in your
territory.
5. Exceeding all sales targets and objectives for
your territory during the year.
B. Sales interaction
1. Listening attentively to identify and understand
the real concerns of your customer.
2. Convincing customers that you understand their
unique problems and concerns.
3. Working out solutions to a customer's questions
or objections.
4. Undertaking actions when customers have problems.

Notes: The items presented here are loose translations from the original Dutch versions used in the study.

Sales Call Anxiety/99
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised, 4th ed. Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Badovick, Gordon J., Farrand J. Hadaway, and Peter F. Kaminski
(1992), "Attributions and Emotions: The Effect on Salesperson
Motivation After Successful vs. Unsuccessful Quota Performance," Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 12
(3), 1-11.
Bagozzi, Richard P (1992), "The Self-Regulation of Attitudes,
Intentions, and Behavior," Social Psychology Quarterly, 55
(March), 178-204.
, Hans Baumgartner, and Rik Pieters (1998), "GoalDirected Emotions," Cognition and Emotion, 12, 1-26.
Barlow, David H. (1988), Anxiety and Its Disorders: The Nature
and Treatment of Anxiety and Panic. New York: Guilford Press.
Barret, Karen C. (1995), "A Functionalist Approach to Shame and
Guilt," in Self-Conscious Emotions, June P Tangney and Kurt
W. Fischer, eds. New York: Guilford Press, 25-63.
Baumeister, Roy F and Mark R. Leary (1995), "The Need to Belong:
Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human
Motivation," Psychological Bulletin, 117 (May), 497-529.
Beatty, Michael J. (1988), "Situation and Predispositional Correlates of Public Speaking Anxiety," Communication Education,
37 (January), 28-39.
Beck, Aron T (1976), Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: International University Press.
and Arthur Freeman (1990), Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Behrman, Douglas N. and William D. Perreault (1984), "Measuring the Performance of Salespersons," Journal of Business
Research, 10 (September), 355-70.
Bickart, Barbara A. (1993), "Carryover and Backfire Effects in
Marketing Research," Journal ofMarlceling Research, 30 (February), 52-62.
Brief, Arthur P., Michael J. Burke, Jennifer M. George, Brian S.
Robinson, and Jane Webster (1988), "Should Negative Affectivity Remain an Unmeasured Variable in the Study of Job
Stress?" Journal of Applied Psychology, 73 (2), 193-98.
Briggs, Stephen R. and Jonathan M. Cheek (1986), "The Role of
Factor Analysis in the Development and Evaluation of Personality Scales," Journal of Personality, 54, 106-148.
Brown, Steven P, William L. Cron, and John W. Slocum Jr. (1997),
"Effects of Goal-Directed Emotions on Salesperson Volitions,
Behavior, and Performance: A Longitudinal Study," Journal of
Marketing, 61 (January), 39-50.
Buss, Arnold H. (1980), Self-Consciousness and Social Anxiety.
San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.
Cheek, Jonathan M. (1989), Conquering Shyness. New York: Dell.
Chen, Peter Y. and Paul E. Spector (1991), "Negative Affectivity
as the Underlying Cause of Correlations Between Stressors
and Strains," Journal of Applied Psychology, 76 (3),
398-407.
Clark, David M. and Adrian Wells (1995), "A Cognitive Model of
Social Phobia," in Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment and
Treatment, Richard G. Heimberg, Michael Liebowitz, Debra A.
Hope, and Franklin Schneier, eds. New York: Guilford Press,
69-93.
DeVellis, Robert (1991), Scale Development: Theories and Application. Newbury Park, NJ: Sage Publications.
Dudley, George W. and Shannon Goodson (1992), Earning What
You 're Worth: The Psychology of Sales Call Reluctance. Dallas:
Behavioral Sciences Research Press.
Ellis, Albert (1962), Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. New
York: Lyle Stuart.

100 / Joumal of Marketing, July 2000

Goodwin, Cathy, Michael Mayo, and Ronald P Hill (1997), "Sales
Response to Loss of a Major Account: A Qualitative Analysis,"
Journal of Business Research, 40, 167-80.
Heckelman, Leora R. and Franklin R. Schneier (1995), "Diagnostic Issues," in Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment, Richard G. Heimberg, Michael Liebowitz, Debra A.
Hope, and Franklin Schneier, eds. New York: Guilford, 3-20.
Heimberg, Richard G., Michael Liebowitz, Debra A. Hope, and
Franklin Schneier (1995), Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment. New York: Guilford.
Ingram, Thomas N., Raymond W. LaForge, and Charles H.
Schwepker (1997), Sales Management. Fort Worth, TX: The
Dryden Press.
Johnston, Wesley J. and Keysuk Kim (1994), "Performance, Attribution, and Expectancy Linkages in Personal Selling," Journal
of Marketing, 58 (October), 68-81.
Joreskog, Karl G. and Dag Sorbom (1996), LtSREL^ User's Reference Guide. Chicago: Scientific Software.
Kramer, Roderick M. (1994), "The Sinister Attribution Error,"
Motivation and Emotion, 18, 199-231.
Lazarus, Richard S. and Susan Folkman (1984), Stress, Appraisal,
and Coping. New York: Springer.
Leary, Mark R. (1991), "Social Anxiety, Shyness, and Related
Constructs," in Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes, John Robinson, Phillip Shaver, and Lawrence
Wrightsman, eds. New York: Academic Press, 161-94.
and Robin M. Kowalski (1995), Social Anxiety. New York:
Guilford Press.
LeDoux, Joseph L. (1996), The Emotional Brain. New York:
Simon and Schuster.
Liebowitz, Michael R., J.M. Gorman, A.J. Fyer, R. Campeas, A.P.
Levin, D. Sandberg, E. Hollander, L. Papp, and D. Goetz
(1988), "Pharmacotherapy of Social Phobia: An Interim Report
of a Placebo-Controlled Comparison of Phenelzine and
Atenolol," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 49, 252-57.
Markaway, Barbara G., Cheryl N. Carmen, Alec C. Pollard, and
Teresa Flynn (1992), Dying of Embarrassment. Oakland, CA:
New Harbinger.
Martens, Rainer (1977), Sport Competition Anxiety Test. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Miles, Raymond E. and Charles C. Snow (1978), Organizational
Strategy. Structure, and Process. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Mineka, Susan and Kathleen Nugent (1995), "Mood Congruent
Memory Biases in Anxiety and Depression," in Memory Distortion, Daniel L. Schacter, ed. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 172-93.
Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), Psychometric Theory. New York:
McGraw Hill.
Patterson, Miles L. and Vicki Ritts (1997), "Social and Communicative Anxiety: A Review and Meta-Analysis," in Communication Yearbook 20, Brant R. Burleson, ed. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage Publications, 263-304.
Peterson, R.A. and S. Reiss (1987), Test Manual for the Anxiety Sensitivity tndex. Orland Park, IL: Intemational Diagnostics Systems.
Pozo, C , Charles C. Carver, A.R. Wellens, and Michael F. Scheier
(1991), "Social Anxiety and Social Perception: Construing Others' Reactions to the Self," Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 1, 355-62.
Rachman, Stanley (998), Anxiety. London: Psychology Press.
Rapee, Ronald M. and Lina Lim (1992), "Discrepancy Between
Self- and Observer Ratings of Performance in Sociai Phobics,"
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 728-31.
Ray, Dana (1995), "Confront Call Reluctance," Personal Selling
Power, (September), 46-51.
Salkovskis, Paul M. (1991), "The Importance of Behaviour in the
Maintenance of Anxiety and Panic: A Cognitive Account,"
Behavioural Psychotherapy, 19, 6-19.
Sarason, Irwin G. and Barbara R. Sarason (1986), "Anxiety and
Interfering Thoughts: Their Effect on Social Interaction," in
Shyness: Perspectives on Research and Treatment, Warren H.
Jones, Jonathan M. Cheek, and Stephen R. Briggs, eds. New
York: Plenum Press, 253-64.
Simon, Herbert A. (1967), "Motivational and Emotional Controls
of Cognition," Psychological Review, 74, 29-39.
Singh, Jagdip (1993), "Boundary Role Ambiguity: Facets, Determinants, and Impact," Journal of Marketing, 57 (April), 11-31.
and Gary Rhoads (1991), "Boundary Role Ambiguity in
Marketing Oriented Positions: A Multidimensional, Multifaceted, Operationalization," Journal of Marketing Research, 28
(August), 328-38.
Spiro, Rosan L. and Barton A. Weitz (1989), "Adaptive Selling:
Conceptualization, Measurement, and Nomological Validity,"
Journal of Marketing Research, 11 (February), 61-69.
SPSS (1993), SPSS For Windows Professional Statistics, Release
6.0. Chicago: SPSS.
Stopa, Lusia and David M. Clark (1993), "Cognitive Processes in
Social Phobia," Behaviour Research and Therapv, 31 (3),
255-67.
Strickland, B. (1977), "Achievement Motivation," in Personality
Variables and Social Behavior, T Blass, ed. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Eribaum Associates, 315-56.
Sujan, Harish, Barton Weitz, and Nirmalya Kumar (1994), "Learning Orientation, Working Smart, and Effective Selling," Journal of Marketing, 58 (July), 39-52.
Szymansky, David (1988), "Determinants of Selling Effectiveness:
The Importance of Declarative Knowledge to the Personal Selling Concept," Journal of Marketing, 52 (January). 64-77.

Tellegen, Auke (1982), "Brief Manual for the Differential Personality Questionnaire," unpublished manuscript. University of
Minnesota.
Thoits, Peggy A. (1990), "Emotion Deviance: Research Agendas,"
in Research Agendas in the Sociology of Emotions, Theodore D.
Kemper, ed. Albany: State University of New York Press,
180-206.
Trower, Peter and Paul Gilbert (1989), "New Theoretical Conceptions of Social Anxiety and Social Phobia," Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 19-35.
Watson, David and Lee Anna Clark (1984), "Negative Affectivity:
The Disposition to Experience Aversive Emotional States,"
Psychological Bulletin, 96 (3), 465-90.
and James.W. Pennebaker (1989), "Health Complaints,
Stress, and Distress: Exploring the Central Role of Negative
Affectivity," Psychological Review, 96, 234—54.
Weitz, Bart A. (1978), "Relationship Between Salesperson Performance and Understanding of Customer Decision Making,"
Journal of Marketing Research, 15 (November), 501-16.
(1981), "Effectiveness in Sales Interactions: A Contingency Framework," Journal of Marketing, 45 (Winter),
85-103.
-, Stephen B. Castleberry, and John F. Tanner (1992), Selling: Building Partnerships. Homewood, IL: Richard D. Irwin.
Wells, Adrian (1998), Cognitive Therapy of Aivciety Disorders.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
, David M. Clark, Paul Salkovskis, John Ludgate, Ann
Hackmann, and Michael Gelder (1995), "Social Phobia: The
Role of In-Situation Safety Behaviors in Maintaining Anxiety
and Negative Beliefs," Behavior Therapy, 26, 153-61.
and Gerald Matthews (1994), Attention and Emotion.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates.
Zillman, Dolf, R.C. Johnson, and K.D. Day (1974), "Provoked and
Unprovoked Aggressiveness in Athletes." Journal of Research
in Personality, 8, 139-52.

^ Leads

Mailing Usts
For a FREE Catalog & Personalized Service Call:

Business Lists:
Over 11 Million Businesses
Select by Type of Business, Employee Size & Sales
Volume for any ZIP Code, County, City or State.

(800) 336-8349
or Fax Us at: (402) 331-1505
Plus, FREE Directory Assistance & National
Yellow Pages on Our Web Site.

WWW. inf oUSA. com
infoVSA-A Public Company. Nasdaq: lUSA
5711 S. 86th Circle • Omaha, NE 68127
E-Mail: lists@infoLISA.com

Consumer Lists:

Over 200 Miiiion Consumers

Select by Age, Income, Household Value & More.

(800) 661-5478
or Fax Us at: (402) 331-0991

Business Credit Reports
only ^5.00 each!
Also Available,

Montiiiy Updates!
Media Code: 01-JMK
Sales Call Anxiety/101
How star performers manage the sales call anxiety | Professional Capital

More Related Content

What's hot

Communications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales Work
Communications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales WorkCommunications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales Work
Communications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales WorkCris Ong
 
CEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert Heath
CEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert HeathCEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert Heath
CEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert HeathIpsosASI_NL
 
Ecpa 2013-Assessment Of Salespeople
Ecpa 2013-Assessment Of SalespeopleEcpa 2013-Assessment Of Salespeople
Ecpa 2013-Assessment Of SalespeopleAssessment Systems
 
A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...
A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...
A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...IAEME Publication
 
Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...
Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...
Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...Diane Hamilton, Ph.D.
 
Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5
Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5
Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5Chris Hennessy
 
Content server (2)
Content server (2)Content server (2)
Content server (2)msgro
 
Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...
Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...
Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...Prof Niamh M. Brennan
 

What's hot (11)

Branding from below
Branding from belowBranding from below
Branding from below
 
Communications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales Work
Communications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales WorkCommunications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales Work
Communications in Organization - Institutional Talk in Sales Work
 
CEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert Heath
CEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert HeathCEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert Heath
CEP article on emotional persuasion in advertising research by Robert Heath
 
Ecpa 2013-Assessment Of Salespeople
Ecpa 2013-Assessment Of SalespeopleEcpa 2013-Assessment Of Salespeople
Ecpa 2013-Assessment Of Salespeople
 
A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...
A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...
A study on effectiveness of cause related marketing [crm] as a strategic phil...
 
EQ 360 Case Study
EQ 360 Case Study EQ 360 Case Study
EQ 360 Case Study
 
Emotional intellegence
Emotional intellegenceEmotional intellegence
Emotional intellegence
 
Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...
Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...
Establishing best Practices for Including Emotional Intelligence in Real Esta...
 
Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5
Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5
Pearman_Brochure_Final_R5
 
Content server (2)
Content server (2)Content server (2)
Content server (2)
 
Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...
Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...
Merkl-Davies, Doris M. and Brennan, Niamh M. [2007] Discretionary Disclosure ...
 

Similar to How star performers manage the sales call anxiety | Professional Capital

Consumer behavior
Consumer behaviorConsumer behavior
Consumer behaviorsmumbahelp
 
25 Keys To Sales & Marketing 2
25 Keys To Sales & Marketing 225 Keys To Sales & Marketing 2
25 Keys To Sales & Marketing 2Saurabh Aggarwal
 
A framework for personal selling to organizations.pdf
A framework for personal selling to organizations.pdfA framework for personal selling to organizations.pdf
A framework for personal selling to organizations.pdfKimberly Pulley
 
Consumer Loyalty and the Decision Making Process
Consumer Loyalty and the Decision Making ProcessConsumer Loyalty and the Decision Making Process
Consumer Loyalty and the Decision Making ProcessDr. Herbert Thweatt
 
Report IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the cour
Report IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the courReport IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the cour
Report IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the courfelipaser7p
 
Advertising - Strong vs. Weak
Advertising - Strong vs. WeakAdvertising - Strong vs. Weak
Advertising - Strong vs. WeakDeniz Kurugöllü
 
Argumentative Essay High School
Argumentative Essay High SchoolArgumentative Essay High School
Argumentative Essay High SchoolBeth Mack
 
Developing sales force effectiveness: an exploratory study
Developing sales force effectiveness: an exploratory studyDeveloping sales force effectiveness: an exploratory study
Developing sales force effectiveness: an exploratory studyFundação Dom Cabral - FDC
 
Consumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviors
Consumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviorsConsumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviors
Consumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviorsLeslie showalter
 
Hierarchy of effects_briefing_paper
Hierarchy of effects_briefing_paperHierarchy of effects_briefing_paper
Hierarchy of effects_briefing_paperchhikaraboy
 
OVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docx
OVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docxOVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docx
OVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docxkarlacauq0
 
17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx
17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx
17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docxfelicidaddinwoodie
 
From this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docx
From this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docxFrom this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docx
From this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docxbudbarber38650
 
Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)
Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)
Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)Tony Mattson
 
Theory Research paper copy
Theory Research paper copyTheory Research paper copy
Theory Research paper copyRachel Meyer
 

Similar to How star performers manage the sales call anxiety | Professional Capital (20)

Consumer behavior
Consumer behaviorConsumer behavior
Consumer behavior
 
Introduction
IntroductionIntroduction
Introduction
 
25 Keys To Sales & Marketing 2
25 Keys To Sales & Marketing 225 Keys To Sales & Marketing 2
25 Keys To Sales & Marketing 2
 
A framework for personal selling to organizations.pdf
A framework for personal selling to organizations.pdfA framework for personal selling to organizations.pdf
A framework for personal selling to organizations.pdf
 
Consumer Loyalty and the Decision Making Process
Consumer Loyalty and the Decision Making ProcessConsumer Loyalty and the Decision Making Process
Consumer Loyalty and the Decision Making Process
 
Report IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the cour
Report IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the courReport IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the cour
Report IssueLearning ObjectivesUpon completion of the cour
 
On Risk Management
On Risk ManagementOn Risk Management
On Risk Management
 
Advertising - Strong vs. Weak
Advertising - Strong vs. WeakAdvertising - Strong vs. Weak
Advertising - Strong vs. Weak
 
Advertising Theories
Advertising TheoriesAdvertising Theories
Advertising Theories
 
Argumentative Essay High School
Argumentative Essay High SchoolArgumentative Essay High School
Argumentative Essay High School
 
Final Paper_550A_Davtian
Final Paper_550A_DavtianFinal Paper_550A_Davtian
Final Paper_550A_Davtian
 
Developing sales force effectiveness: an exploratory study
Developing sales force effectiveness: an exploratory studyDeveloping sales force effectiveness: an exploratory study
Developing sales force effectiveness: an exploratory study
 
Consumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviors
Consumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviorsConsumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviors
Consumers evaluation of unethical marketing behaviors
 
Bachlor Thesis
Bachlor ThesisBachlor Thesis
Bachlor Thesis
 
Hierarchy of effects_briefing_paper
Hierarchy of effects_briefing_paperHierarchy of effects_briefing_paper
Hierarchy of effects_briefing_paper
 
OVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docx
OVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docxOVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docx
OVERVIEWWrite a 3–4-page assessment in which you examine how adver.docx
 
17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx
17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx
17 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL FOR MANAGED CULTURE CHANGEIn Chapt.docx
 
From this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docx
From this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docxFrom this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docx
From this article Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011.docx
 
Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)
Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)
Spending (on brand) through a recessionary climate (2020)
 
Theory Research paper copy
Theory Research paper copyTheory Research paper copy
Theory Research paper copy
 

More from Professional Capital

Het biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoper
Het biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoperHet biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoper
Het biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoperProfessional Capital
 
Networking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance Implications
Networking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance ImplicationsNetworking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance Implications
Networking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance ImplicationsProfessional Capital
 
10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia
10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia
10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asiaProfessional Capital
 
Why knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional Capital
Why knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional CapitalWhy knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional Capital
Why knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional CapitalProfessional Capital
 
How successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional Capital
How successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional CapitalHow successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional Capital
How successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional CapitalProfessional Capital
 
Consequences of pride in personal selling | Professional Capital
Consequences of pride in personal selling | Professional CapitalConsequences of pride in personal selling | Professional Capital
Consequences of pride in personal selling | Professional CapitalProfessional Capital
 
What successfull account managers do with social capital.
What successfull account managers do with social capital.What successfull account managers do with social capital.
What successfull account managers do with social capital.Professional Capital
 
4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke
4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke
4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem VerbekeProfessional Capital
 

More from Professional Capital (8)

Het biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoper
Het biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoperHet biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoper
Het biologisch paspoort van een succesvolle verkoper
 
Networking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance Implications
Networking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance ImplicationsNetworking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance Implications
Networking | Social Circle Memberships and Sales Performance Implications
 
10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia
10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia
10 etiquettes you must know to sell in asia
 
Why knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional Capital
Why knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional CapitalWhy knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional Capital
Why knowledge brokers are ready for shaping sales | Professional Capital
 
How successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional Capital
How successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional CapitalHow successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional Capital
How successful sales professionals make ethical decisions | Professional Capital
 
Consequences of pride in personal selling | Professional Capital
Consequences of pride in personal selling | Professional CapitalConsequences of pride in personal selling | Professional Capital
Consequences of pride in personal selling | Professional Capital
 
What successfull account managers do with social capital.
What successfull account managers do with social capital.What successfull account managers do with social capital.
What successfull account managers do with social capital.
 
4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke
4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke
4 Sales Lessons from Evolution | Professional Capital | Prof. Willem Verbeke
 

Recently uploaded

Insurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usage
Insurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usageInsurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usage
Insurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usageMatteo Carbone
 
Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...
Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...
Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...Roland Driesen
 
Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023
Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023
Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023Neil Kimberley
 
Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...
Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...
Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...lizamodels9
 
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdfRenandantas16
 
Value Proposition canvas- Customer needs and pains
Value Proposition canvas- Customer needs and painsValue Proposition canvas- Customer needs and pains
Value Proposition canvas- Customer needs and painsP&CO
 
Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...
Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...
Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...lizamodels9
 
How to Get Started in Social Media for Art League City
How to Get Started in Social Media for Art League CityHow to Get Started in Social Media for Art League City
How to Get Started in Social Media for Art League CityEric T. Tung
 
Phases of negotiation .pptx
 Phases of negotiation .pptx Phases of negotiation .pptx
Phases of negotiation .pptxnandhinijagan9867
 
Uneak White's Personal Brand Exploration Presentation
Uneak White's Personal Brand Exploration PresentationUneak White's Personal Brand Exploration Presentation
Uneak White's Personal Brand Exploration Presentationuneakwhite
 
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine ServiceCall Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Serviceritikaroy0888
 
B.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptx
B.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptxB.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptx
B.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptxpriyanshujha201
 
Organizational Transformation Lead with Culture
Organizational Transformation Lead with CultureOrganizational Transformation Lead with Culture
Organizational Transformation Lead with CultureSeta Wicaksana
 
Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...
Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...
Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...Sheetaleventcompany
 
Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...
Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...
Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...amitlee9823
 
Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876
Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876
Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876dlhescort
 
Falcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to Prosperity
Falcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to ProsperityFalcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to Prosperity
Falcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to Prosperityhemanthkumar470700
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Insurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usage
Insurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usageInsurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usage
Insurers' journeys to build a mastery in the IoT usage
 
Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...
Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...
Ensure the security of your HCL environment by applying the Zero Trust princi...
 
VVVIP Call Girls In Greater Kailash ➡️ Delhi ➡️ 9999965857 🚀 No Advance 24HRS...
VVVIP Call Girls In Greater Kailash ➡️ Delhi ➡️ 9999965857 🚀 No Advance 24HRS...VVVIP Call Girls In Greater Kailash ➡️ Delhi ➡️ 9999965857 🚀 No Advance 24HRS...
VVVIP Call Girls In Greater Kailash ➡️ Delhi ➡️ 9999965857 🚀 No Advance 24HRS...
 
Falcon Invoice Discounting platform in india
Falcon Invoice Discounting platform in indiaFalcon Invoice Discounting platform in india
Falcon Invoice Discounting platform in india
 
Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023
Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023
Mondelez State of Snacking and Future Trends 2023
 
Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...
Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...
Call Girls From Pari Chowk Greater Noida ❤️8448577510 ⊹Best Escorts Service I...
 
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
 
Value Proposition canvas- Customer needs and pains
Value Proposition canvas- Customer needs and painsValue Proposition canvas- Customer needs and pains
Value Proposition canvas- Customer needs and pains
 
Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...
Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...
Call Girls In DLf Gurgaon ➥99902@11544 ( Best price)100% Genuine Escort In 24...
 
How to Get Started in Social Media for Art League City
How to Get Started in Social Media for Art League CityHow to Get Started in Social Media for Art League City
How to Get Started in Social Media for Art League City
 
unwanted pregnancy Kit [+918133066128] Abortion Pills IN Dubai UAE Abudhabi
unwanted pregnancy Kit [+918133066128] Abortion Pills IN Dubai UAE Abudhabiunwanted pregnancy Kit [+918133066128] Abortion Pills IN Dubai UAE Abudhabi
unwanted pregnancy Kit [+918133066128] Abortion Pills IN Dubai UAE Abudhabi
 
Phases of negotiation .pptx
 Phases of negotiation .pptx Phases of negotiation .pptx
Phases of negotiation .pptx
 
Uneak White's Personal Brand Exploration Presentation
Uneak White's Personal Brand Exploration PresentationUneak White's Personal Brand Exploration Presentation
Uneak White's Personal Brand Exploration Presentation
 
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine ServiceCall Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
 
B.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptx
B.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptxB.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptx
B.COM Unit – 4 ( CORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY ( CSR ).pptx
 
Organizational Transformation Lead with Culture
Organizational Transformation Lead with CultureOrganizational Transformation Lead with Culture
Organizational Transformation Lead with Culture
 
Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...
Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...
Chandigarh Escorts Service 📞8868886958📞 Just📲 Call Nihal Chandigarh Call Girl...
 
Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...
Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...
Call Girls Electronic City Just Call 👗 7737669865 👗 Top Class Call Girl Servi...
 
Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876
Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876
Call Girls in Delhi, Escort Service Available 24x7 in Delhi 959961-/-3876
 
Falcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to Prosperity
Falcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to ProsperityFalcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to Prosperity
Falcon's Invoice Discounting: Your Path to Prosperity
 

How star performers manage the sales call anxiety | Professional Capital

  • 1. Willem Verbeke & Richard P. Bagozzi Sales Call Anxiety: Exploring What It Means When Fear Rules a Sales Encounter The goal of this study is to develop and test a conceptualization of sales call anxiety (SCA) on the basis of current insights from the cognitive approach to social anxiety. Sales call anxiety is an irrepressible fear of being negatively evaluated and rejected by a customer, and it is coupled with a desire to avoid undertaking specific functional actions in selling situations. The authors present and test a model of SCA in two selling situations known to have threatening consequences for salespeople: canvasing and closing. The authors find that SCA consists of four components: negative self-evaluations, negative evaluations from customers, awareness of physiological symptoms (e.g., a queasy stomach, shaky voice, blushing), and protective actions (e.g., avoiding eye contact, fiddling with the hands, shunning self-disclosures). The authors show that these dimensions are functions of negative affectivity and anxiety-provoking contextual cues and that they negatively influenced the performance of 189 mortgage salespeople. We have more fears than we need and it seems that our utterly efficient fear conditioning system, combined with an extremely powerful ability to think about our fears and inability to control them, is probably at fault. —Ttie Emotionat Brain, Joseph LeDoux 1 reluctance" or the fear of making contact with the customer has long been recognized by salespeople as a problem. However, no researchers have investigated the nature of this frequently debilitating symptom (Ingram, LaForge, and Schwepker 1997; Weitz, Castleberry, and Tanner 1992). Part of the problem is that call reluctance and fear of making a contact have largely been left undefined. To provide a basis for the research described in this article, we define sates catt anxiety (SCA) as the fear of being negatively evaluated and rejected by customers, which is accompanied by urges to avoid contact with customers or, when contact is made, to refrain from interacting effectively and asking for a commitment. It is estimated that SCA is prevalent in selling situations and will reach intense levels for up to 40% of salespeople at some points in their careers (Ray 1995). A need exists to understand better what SCA is, when it occurs, and how to manage it. The goal of this article is to develop a conceptualization of SCA that is grounded in current insights from the cognitive approach to social anxiety. Specifically, we focus on how SCA unfolds in two selling situations (canvasing and Willem Verbeke is SMA Chair Professor in Sales and Account Management, Erasmus University. Richard P Bagozzi is J. Hugh Liedtke Professor of Management and Professor of Psychology, Rice University, and Dwight F. Benton Professor of Behavioral Science in Management, Business School, University of Michigan. The authors express their gratitude to the anonymous J M reviewers for the many constructive recommendations they made during the review process and to Miriam Houben for her Insightful suggestions. closing) and how it affects the salesperson's ability to interact with customers. In addition, we propose an operationalization of SCA and investigate how SCA relates to personality traits, anxiety-provoking contextual cues, and performance. Our conceptualization and empirical investigation is designed to provide managers with a way to recognize and understand why salespeople seek to avoid undertaking appropriate actions before and during a sales call. We also discuss how SCA can be controlled to the benefit of the salesperson and firm. The article proceeds as follows. First, we explore the concept of SCA on the basis of recent conceptualizations of social anxiety developed by Clark and Wells (1995) and others (e.g.. Barlow 1988; Wells and Matthews 1994). We discuss the situation specificity of SCA, as well as its antecedents and consequences. Then, using a sample of 189 salespeople, we investigate how SCA manifests itself during cold canvasing and closing a sale, two prototypical selling situations known to cause fear in many salespeople. We then propose managerial implications and future research questions. Memory Processes and Selling In the field of sales management, there is agreement that salespeople's use of declarative and procedural memory systems during customer interaction depends on the amount of attention resources that a salesperson has available in a given situation (e.g., Szymansky 1988). In addition, some researchers have pointed out that metaknowledge (e.g., working smarter or the willingness to learn) affects the way salespeople use selling situation categories and the ways they apply scripts during the sales interaction (Sujan, Weitz, and Kumar 1994). However, only a few studies have investigated the role of memory processes with regard to emotional behavior in making a sale (e.g., Badovick, Hadaway, and Kaminski 1992; Brown, Cron, and Slocum 1997). For example, on the basis of research by Bagozzi, Baumgartner, Journat of tVtarketing 88 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000 Vol. 64 (July 2000), 88-101
  • 2. and Pieters (1998), Brown, Cron, and Slocum (1997) show that selling can be regarded as a goal-directed activity and that, in the pursuit of the sales goal, anticipated emotional reactions to success or failure influence the actions that the salesperson takes. In other words, goal pursuit and subsequent performance can be regarded as under self-control, but cognitive and emotional processes also initiate and guide goal pursuit (Bagozzi 1992). Implicit in the process of goal pursuit, but not studied to date, is how instrumental acts—such as selling tactics during customer interactions—are maintained or abandoned at least temporarily, after they are initiated, and then possibly reactivated. Simon (1967) was one of the first theorists to suggest that the cognitive control of purposive action is facilitated by what he termed "interrupt mechanisms" or what psychologists now call "cognitive interference" or "attentional disruption" (Sarason and Sarason 1986). By this, Simon meant that ongoing goal-directed behaviors are often evaluated, modified, and even forsaken before a goal is reached, but purely cognitive mechanisms cannot account for such occurrences. Simon (1967) speculated that human emotions interrupt the cognitive control of action, so that new assessments of progress and goal commitment can be made and changes instituted as required. However, he never identified the emotional processes or the way they operate. Social Anxiety and Cognitive Interference Social anxiety is a strong emotion that functions as an important interrupt mechanism in goal-directed sales behaviors. How might social anxiety manifest itself in a sales situation? A salesperson may experience anxiety in anticipation of criticism from a supervisor, rejection by coworkers, ridicule by peers, conflicts between work and family pressures, or conIlicts between company policies and moral standards. But probably the most salient causes of SCA for salespeople are the fear of failure and the disapproval and repudiation by customers. Indeed, research in sales management suggests that when salespeople fail, it negatively affects their feelings of self-esteem (Goodwin, Mayo, and Hill 1997) and attributions used to explain failure, which in turn affect motivation (Johnston and Kim 1994) and emotions negatively (Badovick, Hadaway, and Kaminski 1992). Thus, compared with many other occupations, personal selling seems to contain inordinate opportunities for social anxiety because of its very nature, which includes the possibility of multiple rejections and failures on a daily basis and the effects of this potential on feelings and thoughts about the self. To understand how SCA might occur in different situations, we focus especially on cognitive theories of social anxiety, as they have attracted considerable attention recently in psychology and lead to practical remedies for people suffering from social anxiety (Wells 1998). Early research by Ellis (1962) and Beck (1976) suggests that anxious people possess hypervalent cognitive schemas (called "memory biases") that continuously present a view of reality as dangerous and the self as vulnerable. To the extent that anxious people possess these schemata, they will more likely appraise or perceive a myriad of situations as threatening, which in turn reduces their belief that they can handle the situations or cope with them properly. Both appraisals, which interfere with memory processes during conversations, are thought to occur automatically and represent an ongoing integrated process. Although Beck has updated his theory (e.g.. Beck and Freeman 1990), other researchers have refined schema theory to describe and treat specific types of anxiety disorders, such as social anxiety. The most well-known theories in this regard include Barlow's (1988) anxious apprehensioh theory, Clark and Wells's (1995) cognitive approach to social anxiety, and Rachman's (1998) anxiety model. In contrast to previous approaches that viewed social anxiety as a pathology, the newer theories regard many anxiety-related problems as common errors of normal functioning rather than as signs of deep-rooted abnormalities (Rachman 1998, p. 64). We term these theories "attention-based cognitive anxiety models" (see the next section). Attention-based cognitive anxiety models differ from the pathological conceptions of anxiety in the following ways: 1. Early theories focused on how vigilant schemata contribute to anxiety responses, whereas researchers now focus on attention biases and cognitive processes that are produced in specific situations that contain threat-related stimuli. As we discuss subsequently, the attention of people with anxiety is focused on extemal signals as well as intemally produced signals, such as physiological symptoms that typify anxiety. As Mineka and Nugent (1995, p. 187) suggest, anxiety is associated with continuous monitoring of a situation for signals of potential threat and requires a cognitive system that scans for such threats. 2. The persistency of social anxiety is reinforced by characteristics of the anxiety response itself That is, the behavioral responses that emerge from dysfunctional appraisals are often involved in the maintenance of danger appraisals and beliefs about situations, as well as people's beliefs about themselves in those situations. In other words, if anxious people avoid or tlee from threatening situations, they never verify whether the situations were truly dangerous and thus maintain anxiety and thwart learning. This perspective has promoted contemporary remedies rooted in learning how to control attentional processes and beliefs (e.g.. Barlow 1988; Rachman 1998; Wells et al. 1995). 3. Some research shows that people with certain personality traits develop situation-specific feelings of anxiety (e.g.. Barlow 1988; Rachman 1998). Among these traits are negative affectivity (NA) and anxiety sensitivity. Although some people are prone to developing social anxieties in one or more social situations, they do not do so necessarily in all social situations, which suggests the need to specify anxiety-provoking cues in any particular context. Attention-Based Cognitive Anxiety Models and the Components of SCA People suffering from social anxiety typically have a strong desire to convey a particular favorable impression of themselves to others, yet they have a marked insecurity about their ability to do so (Clark and Wells 1995). In particular, when they enter threatening social situations, they believe they are in danger of behaving in an inept and unacceptable fashion, and they think that such behaviors will have negative consequences such as of loss of self worth, status, and rejection (see Clark and Wells 1995, p. 69). Researchers Sales Call Anxiety / 89
  • 3. stress that these thoughts enter people's minds only in difficult or amhiguous situations, in which the self-doubt erupts more or less automatically. Clark and Wells (1995) and Barlow (1988) conceive of these anxious thoughts as "anxiety programs," which are complex constellations of cognitive, somatic, affective, and behavioral changes that are designed to protect a person from harm in objectively dangerous environments. When they are activated by cues in the situation, these anxiety programs amplify the perceived danger in those situations. Attention to threatening or ambiguous aspects of the situation takes priority over other cues. This narrowed focus of attention heightens the arousal of anxiety that subsequently becomes the focus of attention, an effect that causes people to find further evidence for perceived danger. As a consequence, attention overload sometimes occurs, which negatively affects people's ability to handle social interactions and the quality of interpersonal communication. In a selling situation, this may elicit negative reactions from customers, which anxious salespeople in turn perceive as proof of their inability to handle similar situations. Finally, some of the self-generated symptoms might produce further symptoms (e.g., talking too fast is often accompanied by hyperventilation and increased heart rate). These self-amplifying or paniclike behaviors have been observed by psychologists (Clark and Wells 1995) as well as by such applied scholars as Dudley and Goodson (1992, p. 77), who have found that "doomsayers experience panic attacks" in personal selling contexts.' Two more characteristics must be added to this self-reinforcing anxiety process. First, people high in social anxiety are motivated to search for information that gives them clues about how others think about them. But when people suspect that others negatively evaluate them, they avoid examining the full extent of the negative evaluations, because they are afraid that people might reject them if they ask for feedback. Second, after experiencing negative social events, people suffering from social anxiety tend to ruminate excessively about what went wrong. This rumination reinforces their fears of the situation and reduces their feelings of self-efficacy for handling similar situations in the future. Dimensions of SCA Sales call anxiety can be considered a special case of social anxiety (Rachman 1998). On the basis of the work of Clark and Wells (1995), who investigate general social anxiety, we propose the following: H|: Sales call anxiety comprises four dimensions: negative self-evaluations, perceived negative evaluations from customers, imagined physiological symptoms, and urges to perform protective actions. The four dimensions of SCA are thought to occur during, and even before, encounters with customers. The first dimension of SCA involves negative evaluations of the self that arise in anticipation of a specific 'Doomsayers, according to Dudley and Goodson (1992), are salespeople who believe beforehand that they will fail. This promotes a type of call reluctance. 90 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000 encounter with a customer (e.g., in cold canvasing or closing a sale). These negative expectations entail ruminations about past failures, negative images of the self in previous as well as anticipated sales encounters, and exaggerated considerations of what can go wrong and how the salesperson might fail. Examples might include "I will not remember all the selling points and will have trouble getting the words out" or "I will be nervous and will lose control and look foolish." In summary, one characteristic of SCA is salespeople's insecurity about their ability to perform well and convey a positive impression to customers. The second dimension of SCA is imagined negative evaluations from customers. People who have high social anxiety have a strong desire to convey favorable impressions of themselves to others, which is coupled with a strong need to know what others think about them (Clark and Wells 1995; Pozo et al. 1991). Yet their insecurity about their ability to perform and communicate a positive image, which is exacerbated by excessive rumination about past failures and images of future failure, creates strong anticipatory social anxiety. Research suggests further that people high in social anxiety tend to set unrealistically high standards for social performance (Clark and Wells 1995). Examples might include "I must get every customer to like me" or "I must not ever show signs that I do not know my product or do not believe in my product." A third dimension of SCA is salespeople's physiological symptoms, which they use to infer customers' reactions to themselves and to appraise how well they are doing overall. Common reactions to SCA, which are largely uncontrollable at least at the outset, are a pounding heart, an unsteady voice, blushing, sweating, and shaky hands. Physiological symptoms reflect the fact that social anxiety is mediated primarily by the sympathetic nervous system. However, the parasympathetic nervous system sometimes comes into play as well and is manifested by a drop in blood pressure and accompanying feelings of dizziness or weakness. Moreover, it has been found that people who have high social anxiety tend to overestimate how negatively others evaluate them in interpersonal exchanges (Rapee and Lim 1992; Stopa and Clark 1993), which amplifies the communicators' perceptions of their own physiological symptoms and further increases social anxiety. A preoccupation with physiological symptoms causes salespeople to see themselves from the point of view of the customer, and this adds to their negative self-evaluation (e.g., "The customer sees that I am nervous and sweating profusely and will think I am incompetent"). The final dimension of SCA involves urges to perform protective actions. These are similar to what clinical psychologists term "safety-seeking behaviors" (e.g., Clark and Wells 1995; Wells et al. 1995), or what psychologists call emotion-regulatory processes or coping responses (Barret 1995; Lazarus and Folkman 1984; Thoits 1990). Psychologists have found that one of the symptoms or concomitants of social anxiety is an automatic reaction that reduces or relieves the negative effects of anxiety. Protective actions encompass a wide range of coping responses, many of which are involuntary but some of which can be learned or
  • 4. unlearned to a certain extent as personal policies. Examples of the more involuntary protective actions are gazing away and avoiding eye contact, speaking quickly, and fiddling with the hands. Protective actions more subject to self-control entail avoiding self-disclosures, saying less, controlling the breathing (e.g., taking deep breaths, breathing slowly), or standing still. It also entails salespeople focusing on their own and the customer's voice and feet or holding onto objects. Withdrawal from contact and avoidance of future contact are extreme and usually dysfunctional protective actions. In accordance with Clark and Wells (1995), we believe it is fruitful to conceive of SCA as functioning in two ways. First, SCA manifests itself along a continuum from no social anxiety to severe, debilitating social anxiety; second, SCA manifests itself in a broad range of situations known to provoke social anxiety. More concretely, one salesperson might experience intense SCA during cold canvasing but only moderate levels of SCA during the finalizing stages in sales conversation, or vice versa, whereas others might experience high SCA in both situations (e.g., Dudley and Goodson 1992). Similarly, Singh and Rhoads (1991) suggest that a salesperson might experience role ambiguity related to colleagues but not to customers, and vice versa. When SCA is examined as a situation-specific enigma, it appears less pathological and therefore perhaps more manageable if we focus on the self-attentive processes that cause anxiety in those situations. This situation-specific perspective is at the center of discussion among specialists working on social phobia (e.g., Heckelman and Schneier 1995; Rachman 1998). Therefore, H2: Salespeople undergo SCA as a matter of degree that ranges from low to high levels of intensity. Two common contexts in which salespeople face SCA are canvasing (cold calling) and closing the sale. Antecedents and Consequences of SCA Personatity as an antecedent of SCA. Although the focus of our research is situational aspects of SCA, we also wish to investigate one antecedent to social anxiety identified in psychology and organizational behavior research. Negative affectivity is a disposition of mood and indicates the general vulnerability of people to experiencing aversive emotional states: "High-NA individuals tend to be distressed and upset and have a negative view of self, whereas those low in the dimension are relatively content and secure and satisfied with themselves" (Watson and Clark 1984, p. 465). We think of NA as a type of trait social anxiety, a personality characteristic that endows or predisposes people to react emotionally in a particular way across many situations. Watson and Clark (1984, p. 465) note that NA is a unidimensional construct, and people scoring high in NA possess chronic subjective feelings of nervousness, tension, and worry. Furthermore, Watson and Clark (1984, p. 466, emphasis in original) posit that NA manifests itself even in the absence of any overt stress and people high in NA are, "in any given situation, more titcety to experience a significant level of distress." In summary, NA is a disposition that reflects individual differences in the general propensity to experience negative emotions and have a negative view of the self. Some research supports the effects of NA on job stress and job strain for professional and managerial personnel in an insurance company (Brief et al. 1988) and for employees across a wide range of companies (Chen and Spector 1991; see also Watson and Pennebaker 1989). Thus, we propose the following: H3: Negative affectivity will positively influence SCA. That is, we expect NA to increase the salesperson's SCA in particular situations. Support for this hypothesis can be found in Watson and Clark's (1984, p. 466) review of the literature, in which they document that high-trait anxious people react more strongly to stressful situations but, in the absence of stress, tend to be no more anxious than those low in NA. Likewise, given the tendency of high-NA people to fixate on and magnify mistakes, frustrations, disappointments, and threats (Watson and Clark 1984, p. 466), we expect that NA should also accentuate SCA. In addition to main effects for NA, we explore whether NA interacts with anxiety-provoking cues to infiuence SCA. Dependence of SCA on situationat cues. Canvasing and closing are distinct global settings in which selling occurs, but the real determinants of SCA are particular anxietyprovoking cues that function within these settings. Different anxiety-provoking cues can be identified for canvasing and closing. This interpretation is consistent with modern definitions of social anxiety that regard it as "a persistent fear of one or more social or performance situations in which a person is exposed" (American Psychiatric Association 1994, pp. 416-17; see also Wells and Mathews 1994). For canvasing, one source of anxiety arises when salespeople must initiate contacts with prospects who are of a higher rank or social status. The salespeople are put in an awkward position because of differences in expectations and customs that might arise as well as the uncertainty whether deference must be shown, and if so, how, when, and how much. A second source of anxiety in canvasing settings is the near-universal reaction people have to meeting new people. Without prior knowledge of the needs, style, and other characteristics of a prospect, the salesperson is asked to face a set of unknowns that could thwart the initial encounter. A third source of anxiety stems from the need to create a favorable first impression and convey specific positive qualities of the selling organization and its offerings. In summary, canvasing presents a vague and ambiguous situation in which things can go wrong and the salesperson is vulnerable to rejection and failure. The anticipation of concerns about the unknown activates SCA programs. In summary, we suggest the following: H4: Sales call anxiety will be provoked to the extent that salespeople are sensitive to difficulties in dealing with new customers, higher status customers, and the conveyance of positive qualities of their firm and its products, three facets of canvasing. For closing, the strongest source of anxiety is the anticipation of asking for a commitment and being rejected. So much rides on obtaining a commitment that the dread of failure is a real and feared consequential outcome. Anticipatory anxiety here can lead to cycles of approach-avoidance, procrastination, and strong apprehension. Another source of Sales Call Anxiety / 91
  • 5. anxiety is the thought of admitting failure to a supervisor and explaining why a failure occurred. Therefore, we submit the following: H5: Sales call anxiety will be provoked to the extent that salespeople are sensitive to difficulties associated with asking for a commitment and discussing their performance in this regard with their supervisor, two aspects of closing sales. Performance-related consequences of SCA. Sales call anxiety intrudes into goal-directed activities and potentially infiuences performance negatively. How might this happen? One way that performance might be negatively affected is through the interference that social anxiety produces in customer-seller exchanges. Parallel to psychological researchers, we expect social anxiety to drain or limit salespeople's attentional resources and so interfere with their ability to process social cues (Clark and Wells 1995; Rachman 1998). Related to this is the general tendency in people high in social anxiety not to "check out what is really happening" in interpersonal encounters (Clark and Wells 1995, p. 72). People who have high social anxiety "are not data driven but function rather like an automatic program which is activated in a social situation," and this leads them "to run through a repertoire of negative thoughts without really paying attention to what is happening in the situation" (Stopa and Clark 1993, p. 264). This behavior is dysfunctional for personal selling, in which complex communications of needs and offers and counteroffers are necessary for success. But not only failures in attention occur as a consequence of social anxiety in interpersonal communication. Excessive self-focus, negative self-evaluations, and overestimation of how negatively others view the self can lead to protective actions that make the salesperson high in SCA come across as unfriendly and unresponsive. This, in turn, can elicit similar reactions from customers. The result can be a breakdown in communication and lowered performance. Likewise, when social anxiety induces or is accompanied by negative self-evaluations by salespeople, they may become discouraged and avoid certain interactions. Even if they do not give up completely, salespeople high in SCA may become distracted by their feelings and physiological symptoms, which makes them work nervously and display less attention to the task at hand. Disruptive social anxiety may also interfere with working smart (Sujan, Weitz, and Kumar 1994). All these reactions can depress performance. As a consequence, we propose the following: Hg! The greater the perceived SCA, the poorer is the performance in terms of communication effectiveness and sales volume. Method Salespeople and Procedure A total of 500 questionnaires was sent to salespeople from a subdivision of a large Dutch bank. The salespeople sell mortgages to consumers. Of these salespeople, 196 returned the questionnaires. After eliminating incomplete questionnaires, we ended up with a final sample of 189 salespeople. 92 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000 for a 38% response rate. On the basis of comparisons of sociodemographic characteristics and length of tenure, no significant differences were found between respondents and the entire sales force. Mortgage sellers are ideal subjects for a study of SCA because they engage in extensive cold canvasing, face many rejections in the course of a day, work in a competitive environment, are compensated on the basis of commission, and undergo constant pressure to sell to customers across a wide spectrum of status and other differences. The sample can be described as follows: A majority of the salespeople were men (approximately 60%), most were younger than 35 years of age (approximately 52%), and a few salespeople (approximately 9%) were more than 50 years of age. With respect to experience, approximately 10% of the sample had been with the organization less than 2 years, and almost 55% had been with the company between 6 and 20 years. Finally, most salespeople had finished basic and advanced vocational studies, but only 1 % had a university or college degree. This profile is typical of mortgage salespeople across Europe. Questionnaire Design To construct a pool of items for measuring each of the variables under study, focus group interviews were organized, and several tests were administered in stages, as DeVellis suggests (1991). Managers were asked to identify situations in which their salespeople encounter apprehension problems when selling. The managers identified canvasing and closing as the most anxiety-provoking contexts for selling mortgage-related products. The identification of canvasing and closing as general anxiety-provoking contexts is consistent with basic research in psychology on anxiety. Indeed, a key defining element of social anxiety is that it occurs in situations that imply initial encounters (e.g., new customers), consist of people who are regarded as having power or status, or place the person in a conspicuous position that is difficult to escape physically (Beatty 1988, Buss 1980; Leary and Kowalski 1995). Thus, analogous to when a person stands before an audience to give a speech, canvasing and closing demand that the salesperson make actual contact with a customer and provide specific information and requests. To avoid doing this or to flee from such encounters, which is an urge salespeople with SCA frequently feel, would be dysfunctional for the salesperson and the firm. Canvasing and closing are also two activities that occupy much of the time of mortgage salespeople in the setting at hand and are among the most important tasks they perform. Therefore, questions presented to subsequent focus groups were tailored to address these two prototypical contexts to generate items for the final questionnaire. First, items that refiect the four dimensions of SCA typical for canvasing and closing were generated for presentation in focus groups (as proposed by Clark and Wells 1995; Leary 1991; Patterson and Ritts 1997; Salkovskis 1991; Trower and Gilbert 1989). Second, for each of the two situations, we generated specific anxiety-provoking cues by asking salespeople to specify particular events and experiences that created the most anxiety for them. Five anxiety-provoking cues were identified: three for cold canvasing (i.e., dealing with higher-status customers; preparing for and communicating
  • 6. with new, unknown customers; and conveying positive characteristics of the bank and its offerings) and two for closing (i.e., asking for a commitment and discussing performance with the sales manager). All items contributed to the initial item pool were evaluated for relevance and understandability by salespeople and sales managers. It should be noted that the salespeople who participated in focus group interviews were not part of the final sampling. Questions presented on the final questionnaire followed standard practice in the literature. The actual questionnaire was administered in Dutch, but a rough English translation is presented here (for a list of all items discussed subsequently, see the Appendix). To avoid carryover effects, the questionnaires were printed in three different versions wherein different sections of the questionnaire were counterbalanced (Bickart 1993). The SCA items were introduced with an instruction that attempted to put the salesperson in the position of anticipating or engaging in a sales interaction. Specifically, the items were introduced with the following question for cold canvasing: "You are having a first conversation with a new and unknown customer. You will have to ask many personal and significant questions about the person's income, financial situation, and family situation.... A successful conversation is important to you. What is going on in your mind?" These instructions were followed by items designed to measure the four dimensions of SCA in canvasing situations. For negative self-evaluations, six items were used (e.g., "I hope I will not shut down/clam up"). For negative evaluations from customers, ten items were used (e.g., "The customer will think that I do not come across convincingly"). Physiological symptoms were measured by seven items (e.g., "I am nervous," "I cannot sit still during periods of salience"). Finally, protective actions were measured with six items (e.g., "I adopt a passive attitude"). Responses for these SCA measures were recorded on seven-point, "completely disagree" to "completely agree" scales. For sales closing, the SCA items were introduced with the following instructions: After the orienting discussion with customers, you engage the customer in conversation. You want to make tlnal tiie sale. During this phase, you need to communicate some details about the offer before the mortgage contract can be signed. You have communicated to your manager that you will attempt to make final the sale, and now you realize that obtaining the offer is important to you. What is going on in your mind? Similar to those for cold canvasing, the four dimensions of SCA were measured separately for closing. Specifically, for negative self-evaluations, six items were used (e.g., "I hope I will not rattle off my words"). For negative evaluations from customers, eight items were used (e.g., "He will laugh at me afterward"). Physiological symptoms were measured by seven items (e.g., "I become panicky"). Finally, protective actions were measured with five items (e.g., "I offer the customer too many extras"). To measure cues within the four contexts that provoke SCA, we asked salespeople to respond to 24 items introduced by the following instructions: "In the following situations, you might experience a certain amount of tension and uneasy feelings. Please indicate the extent to which you experience tension." Then the following items were presented: 4 for meeting with customers of a higher status, 5 for preparing for and communicating with new prospects, 4 for being assertive about themselves and the organization, 7 for approaching and asking for a commitment from customers, and 4 for discussing performance with a manager. Responses were recorded on seven-point, "never experience tension" to "always experience tension" scales. Performance was measured with a scale based on research by Behrman and Perreault (1984). One component of performance referred to sales volume and was measured with five items (e.g., "exceeding all sales targets and objectives," "generating sales of new company products"). The second component of performance addressed communication quality in the sales interaction and was measured with four items (e.g., "convincing customers that you understand their unique problems and concerns," "working out solutions to customers' questions or objections"). Finally, NA was measured with a scale from Tellegen (1982). Twenty items were used. Scale Development Exploratory factor analyses were performed on the items for each scale. Maximum likelihood estimation with oblimin rotation was done with SPSS (1993). The different dimensions of scales were analyzed, and items not satisfying the following criteria were deleted: (1) dominant loadings greater than .40 and (2) cross-loadings less than .25. Criteria for accepting factors relied on inspection of the scree plot (Briggs and Cheek 1986; DeVellis 1991). The reliabilities for items of dimensions of each scale were also assessed by means of Cronbach's alpha. In Table 1, we summarize the results of factor analyses for each scale: Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin of all the factor analyses are substantial. In addition, the factor loadings are generally high, and the lowest loading is equal to .41. Note especially that two factors corresponding to cold canvasing and closing were found for negative self-evaluations and for perceived negative evaluations from others. The items measuring physiological responses and protective actions each loaded on one factor. Not surprisingly, physiological responses and protective actions reflect reactions that are governed by the autonomic nervous system and thus tend to be invariant across different anxiety-provoking situations. We also present the reliabilities of each scale in Table 1. The reliabilities are high and exceed the minimum of .70 recommended by Nunnally (1978). Finally, means and standard deviations of measures are provided in Table 1. Regression models To take measurement error into account formally and avoid problems of multicollinearity, we used LISREL (Joreskog and Sorbom 1996) to perform regressions. One set of regressions tested the effects of anxiety-provoking situations and NA on SCA: SCAj = f(anxiety provoking cuesj, NA); four regressions were run, one for each dimension of SCA (i.e., negative self-evaluation, negative evaluation from customers, physiological symptoms, and protective actions). Three anxiety-provoking cues (i.e., dealing with higher- Sales Call Anxiety / 93
  • 7. CJ> CO 00 o (0 if 1 00 o o o CJ) oq oq ay cvi in r o (0 U (0 in o~ ra u oJ O CM i (0 CO CM CM 00 CO CM o in CM CO CJ Oi CO CO cvi CM Cvi CO U o ra in I Q o O) •E ra •o o c n V) CO CJ> 00 o CO 00 00 O5 CO 00 05 U U •a ra t to" 00 CVj CO t CD oo CO in CO o> 00 to ra o> C .E c O ra a. lU < 0) 00 CO d '^ CO CO (J) CJ) CM CO 0 5 CO cd in g in ^ 00 UJ (0 2 o CM cd CO go, o a> O) iu CO 00 00 CO I? CM 05 o o cu 00 in CO iri CO CM o o in oo CO U (A O) CO in CJ) i c T5 Toq CO o O r~. lAci X UJ co o ot in in .a E CO if CM CO CO C O U CO c ro _3 gative rom c ro o > > < u B (0 LUO g d) 94 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000 o o a. I CO E g c g ro ro o o il. O CO CO CJ ® .. 2 E o a. E 1_ CO CO ^ ^ ororo •tcow 0. O) 0) *5 ro C o licati Ui 4 in CO og (U I Oi CM CO new ners 2. Q CO CC o oq in to oq ovol high tus cus a O) in oq cq CO 0) o> r 4 in CO ?2 d) U c O O c •£ g CD o ™ o " .o ? ro o ' D) c O "co < ^ §1 o a> C Q. O b
  • 8. status customers, meeting new customers, and conveying qualities of the organization) plus NA were entered as independent variables for canvasing, and two anxiety-provoking cues (i.e., asking for commitment and anticipating discussion of performance with managers) plus NA were entered as independent variables for closing sales. The second set of regressions tested the effects of SCA on performance: performance^ - fCSCAj); two regressions were run, one for each dimension of performance (i.e., communication performance and volume performance). To test for interactions between NA and anxietyprovoking cues on SCA, we performed two analyses. First, we used classical regression analysis in which the main effects of NA and anxiety-provoking cues, as well as their interaction, were entered. This approach uses full information but does not take into account measurement error. Second, to take into account measurement error, we split the sample at the median based on SCA and used structural equation models to do the regressions. This approach does not use full information in the sense of classical regression analysis. Thus, there is a trade-off between the two procedures. Unfortunately, because of sample size restrictions and the need to represent four interactions and five main effects, structural equation models with latent variables cannot be used both to take into account measurement error and to use full information underlying the interactions. Results In Table 2, we summarize the findings for the regression analyses. In the top panel, low to moderately high amounts of variance in SCA are explained, and all anxiety-provoking cues influence the four dimensions of SCA during both canvasing and closing. Nevertheless, several interesting patterns of results can be pointed out. During both canvasing and closing, the effects of anxiety-provoking stimuli are greatest on physiological symptoms and protective actions. Yet strong effects are found on negative self-evaluations and negative evaluations from customers for most anxietyprovoking stimuli. For each dimension of SCA, the effects of the anxiety-provoking stimuli are roughly equal during both canvasing and closing. When closing and canvasing are compared, the effects of anxiety-provoking stimuli on SCA are greater for the former. This occurs as well in the higher levels of explained variance for the dimensions of SCA during closing compared with canvasing. Finally, NA influenced all fourdimensions of SCA during both canvasing and closing. The effects of NA were generally higher on physiological symptoms and protective actions than on negative self-evaluations and negative evaluations from customers, and higher coefficients occurred for closing than canvasing. In the bottom panel of Table 2, relatively small but significant proportions of variance are explained in performance by SCA. For both canvasing and closing, more variance is explained in volume than communication performance. Physiological symptoms and protective actions have the largest effects on performance, yet the effects of negative self-evaluations and negative evaluations from customers are substantial. The tests for interaction revealed that NA does not moderate effects of anxiety-provoking cues on SCA. No signifi- cant interactions were found under either canvasing or closing. Therefore, the results for the direct effects of NA on anxiety-provoking cues can be interpreted as main effects. Discussion Sales call anxiety can be conceived as an intrusive and persistent apprehension about dealing with customers in particular selling situations and is triggered by specific cues in those situations. Rather than being a simple global fear response, however, SCA was found to consist of four components: negative self-evaluations, imagined negative evaluations from customers, physiological responses, and protective actions. The experience of SCA, which is believed to happen largely automatically and unconsciously, was found to differ between cold canvasing and closing for negative self-evaluations and imagined negative evaluations from customers. Anxiety-provoking cues had larger effects on negative self-evaluation and negative evaluation from customers for closing than canvasing. In contrast, SCA with regard to physiological responses and protective actions was invariant across the two types of selling situations (i.e., similar physiological responses and protective actions, as a function of anxiety-provoking cues, occurred during canvasing and closing). Negative self-evaluations involve insecurities with respect to situational appearances or characteristics, actions in sales encounters, or outcomes thereof. These are especially manifested in ruminations about past failures or shortcomings and images of difficulties and failure in anticipated sales encounters. The danger of a preoccupation with negative self-evaluations is that it can discourage and immobilize the salesperson, limit attentional resources needed to remain alert as well as be adaptive, and disrupt the processing of social cues and other aspects of interpersonal communication when customers are engaged. An important characteristic of SCA is that salespeople generate anxiety programs automatically. The data in this study suggest that SCA potentially makes salespeople vulnerable to dysfunctional social anxiety across a variety of selling situations, and this in turn negatively correlates with the salesperson's performance. Many salespeople who do not suffer from strong SCA may be able to hide their anxiety, because they choose to focus on less threatening selling activities or consciously or unconsciously avoid situations that provoke anxiety. Consequently, they may be relatively unaware that they experience anxiety in a detrimental way. Sales managers should learn to detect signs of SCA in their salespeople. In many cases, sales managers will notice second-order effects of anxiety when sales force strategies change or when salespeople change accounts. For example, imagine a change from a defender strategy to a prospector strategy, in which salespeople are instructed to increase their quota of new customer contacts (Miles and Snow 1978). Such a change may be difficult for salespeople who feel anxious and threatened by such environmental and task disruptions. Innovations in automated selling and management might be able to incorporate ways to monitor SCA here. Current decision support systems enable managers to detect useful ratios (e.g., sales from new customers versus existing Sales Call Anxiety / 95
  • 9. 00 1 CO CM CO CO in 0) > M 11 t CM CO CNJ oo CO CM CO (0 o o> o o 00 r CO CO CO CM r (0 C >. >> o> c in CVl •2 c in .CO) a. o (0 0) 3 I. o o atB U O) l Il C CO r CO i § 2 1 .-2 CO LO CO CO CO CO CM CO o CM (0 o CD '•§ CM > (fl '.? c U O O) CM CO •22 •S ?• in E > > O) O .£ So. s CM LO u lO CM Q - _ ifl .III O) CO CO CM CO CO CO c» c (0 E o t: a > Q. 96 / Journal of Marketing, July 2000 > "5 ffecti iluat mer o o rotec al _ •^ r 3 UJ r o u (0 n UJ hysio ympt 0) (0 ri « « < o "o c 3 > egati from •S « Ition c .2 « (u O 00 CO 0) O Of CO 3 r CM .^5 LO r §,§5 C O tn •o i i a> CD II Is i i i o E 5§ w" V
  • 10. customers) that might indicate salespeople's preferences for dealing with existing as opposed to new customers. Imbalances resulting in too many sales from existing customers might signal a problem in SCA for management to address. Sales managers should be careful not to jump to the conclusion that a salesperson in this case necessarily suffers from SCA. It is possible that the salesperson lacks certain selling skills and this deficit causes avoidance of new account prospecting. When a sales manager detects symptoms of SCA in one or more salespeople, the next task is to make the salespeople aware that their performance suffers from SCA. Managers then should inquire why their salespeople, for example, find it difficult to achieve closure during interactions with customers. By doing so, the salespeople and their managers make the SCA programs explicit (e.g., " I now realize that I do not visit new customers because I fear negative consequences of canvasing"). When they are made explicit, such disclosures can help both salespeople and sales managers start joint problem solving to overcome SCA. Thus, the sales manager should take a coaching attitude when salespeople are prone to develop anxiety. One coaching suggestion for sales managers is that they should avoid any stigmatization about SCA. This can be done by suggesting to salespeople that anxiety is contingent on situational cues that can be anticipated and coped with and that everybody will experience SCA to a certain degree now and then. In other words, SCA is not necessarily a pathological problem (Rachman 1998). When salespeople recognize that they are vulnerable to anxiety in certain circumstances, this sets the stage for various corrective efforts by management and selfregulation by salespeople. We propose that one or more of six different programs can be used to address SCA: Freeing up memory space ttirough disconnected niindfutness. One approach to counteracting negative self-evaluations is to help salespeople reengineer their anxiety programs. This might involve helping the salesperson disconfirm counterproductive thoughts and negative selfappraisals during a sales visit. Constructive self-analyses can be promoted by training the salesperson to ask such questions as "What evidence do you use to infer that the customer might not like you?" and "What signals do you look for when visiting customers?" Such questions might help salespeople develop self- and task-related metaknowledge (knowledge about their self-knowledge of SCA) that enables them to break through the cycle of escalating anticipatory fears consciously (e.g., Clark and Wells 1995). By teaching salespeople to focus on cues that reflect positive information and avoid cues that normally trigger overwhelming fears, which tend to occupy the salespeople's memory and prevent the activation of adaptive resources, the salespeople can learn to free up attentional resources. Key adaptive resources to emphasize include the matching of customer needs to the seller's offerings and matching the selling strategy and specific appeals to customer type. This entails the adaptive selling tactics of "knowing what" and "knowing how" (Weitz 1978, 1981). Only by managing anxiety will it be possible for salespeople to practice adap- tive selling behavior and work smarter (e.g., Spiro and Weitz 1989; Sujan, Weitz, and Kumar 1994). Reduction of approval seetcing. Imagined negative evaluations from customers will be intensified to the extent that salespeople have an excessive need for approval but are afraid of asking for it (Baumeister and Leary 1995; Strickland 1977). Therefore, it is advisable to (1) help salespeople better understand what their self-worth is based on, so as not to overemphasize the need for approval (or to not be disapproved) for its own sake, and (2) recruit salespeople who are not obsessive in the need for approval and help them develop impression management techniques that focus more on customer and product knowledge than on approval seeking and the need to belong, per se. This is not to suggest, however, that though the sales job is inherently stressful, sales managers should not seek to reduce, for example, role stress. Role stress has been known to evoke feelings of job tension and dysphoric cognitions (Singh 1993; Kramer 1994), and those levels of job tension and dysphoric cognitions might make salespeople vulnerable to rejection by the customer and so bring about the development of SCA. Relaxation. Sales call anxiety is also exhibited through physiological symptoms. Excessive self-focus on shaky hands, sweating, a quiver in the voice, or an upset stomach can aggravate social anxiety and interfere with interpersonal communication. One way to offset the effects of physiological symptoms is to concentrate on relaxing the muscles during a sales interaction. Indeed, through practice, relaxation techniques can have generalized, long-term effects on reducing anxiety. In more extreme cases of SCA, such as before closing a big deal that has been worked on for months, pharmacological agents such as beta-blockers may be called for (Liebowitz et al. 1988). Encouragement to engage in sports or recreation outside work might promote relaxation and renewal (Zillman, Johnson, and Day 1974). Some firms have in-house athletic facilities and counseling services and sponsor periodic educational and recreational programs designed to relieve tension and social anxiety. Finally, with practice in selling and the development of interpersonal skills and product and customer knowledge, the negative effects of physiological symptoms should become easier to control. Regutatory tactics. Protective actions, the fourth component of SCA, are potentially the most damaging in two senses. At the point of salesperson-customer interactions, such protective actions as averting eye contact, speaking quickly, avoiding self-disclosures, or prematurely withdrawing in an effort to escape embarrassment can break down trust and lead customers to question the competence of the salesperson. Even before salespeople engage with customers, such protective actions as putting off calling a customer or other forms of procrastination, refusing to answer a telephone call from a customer, or planning to avoid certain topics in an anticipated exchange can lead to lost sales. Probably the most important remedy for overcoming dysfunctional protective actions is to develop self-regulatory tactics. To the extent that salespeople can develop scripts to activate when customers raise questions or objections or when the salespeople feel an urge to avoid encounters or discuss issues in an exchange, they will be better able to handle SCA and Sales Call Anxiety / 97
  • 11. function effectively. For example, when salespeople experience setbacks after a customer asks a product-related question to which they have no answer, they can respond by saying something to the effect of "I cannot answer your question fully now but will call you back this afternoon after I investigate our policies more completely." Other self-management tactics can be developed to select alternative ways to make a sale, monitor goal striving, maintain commitment, and overcome impediments to goal achievement. Professional treatment. On occasion, SCA may become overwhelming, and professional help may be required. In such cases, it is likely that salespeople will display anxiety across a broad range of selling situations, which makes their successful functioning in the sales force more doubtful. For some salespeople, several therapies are available for coping with social anxiety (Heimberg et al. 1995), and various selfhelp programs may be useful (e.g.. Cheek 1989; Markaway etal. 1992). Selection criteria. The data in this study show that the personality trait NA is correlated with the development of SCA. The NA scale may thus be used as an additional criterion in the selection of salespeople and perhaps as a check later in a salesperson's career to verify whether cynicism has developed over time. Research shows that other personality traits, such as fear of negative evaluation and anxiety sensitivity for which scales exist, may be associated with social anxiety (Peterson and Reiss 1987; Rachman 1998). Further Research A topic not considered in the present study but worthy of further research is the possibility that, at least for some salespeople, anxiety can spur them on to greater achieve- ments. Some athletes and performers arouse themselves in preparation for a performance through anxiety-provoking thoughts to energize their performances (e.g.. Martens 1977). It is possible that SCA can be functional for some salespeople under certain conditions. To uncover these possibilities, it may be necessary to model the relationships of SCA to its correlates in nonlinear patterns. For example, SCA might initially stimulate salespeople, but when a threshold is reached, it will not have an effect. Yet some point later, under intense SCA, performance may be negatively affected. This will be difficult to study in the field and may require special laboratory testing with salespeople under controlled conditions. It is most likely that SCA emerges during the sales career. Johnston and Kim (1994, p. 78), for example, suggest that "Many new salespeople quit their job within a few months because they are not suited for it after a few instances of failure." As sales become more automated and selling patterns more easily detected, the sales environment is a good opportunity to study causal relationships between failure and the onset of SCA. In our study, we focus on anxiety in salespeople that stems from relationships with customers (only one dimension—talking with a manager about performance—did not directly reflect relationships with customers). Anxiety might also be expected to result from interactions with other salespeople, sales managers, friends, and family members. These possibilities deserve further study. Finally, it would be informative to investigate other forms of self-conscious emotions in personal selling. Shame, embarrassment, guilt, and pride are likely candidates for study, but they have not been studied systematically in personal selling to date. APPENDIX Scales and Items Used in the Study I. Negative self-evaluation A. Cold canvasing 1. I hope this conversation does not flop. 2. I hope I will not shut down/clam up. 3. I hope I will not come across as an unfriendly person to the customer. 4. I hope I will not get the idea again that I am not able to have a constructive conversation. 5. I hope it will not turn out that advisors from other banks will be able to convince the customer better than I will. 6. I hope the customer will not turn away from me when I keep asking questions. B. Closing 1. I hope I can listen carefully to what the customer says. 2. I hope I will not start stuttering. 3. I hope I will not fumble for words. 4. I hope I will be alert. 5. I hope I will not give in to the customer. 6. I hope I will not waffle my words. 98 / Journal of IVIarketing, July 2000 II. Perceived negative evaluation from customers A. Cold canvasing 1. The customer will think the following of me: a. there you have again another "salesperson." b. that I do not come across as a credible salesperson. c. that I do not get across convincingly. d. that I have doubts. e. that I adopt a kind attitude to prevent refusal. f. that bad experiences lie ahead for him with this salesperson. 2. I will think of the customer: a. that he only smiles to get rid of me. b. that he does not like me. c. that he will tell other people that I am not a competent person. d. that he will tell other people that I am a very accommodating person. B. Closing 1. The customer will think the following of me a. that I do not have authority.
  • 12. APPENDIX Continued b. c. d. e. f. that I am not professional. that I am an insecure person. that I am not able to sell. that I am not reliable. that advisors from other banks are better salespeople than I. 2. I will think of the customer: a. that he will laugh at me afterward. b. that he will not do any more business with me at a later stage. III. Physiological symptoms A. I am nervous. B. I talk less quickly. C. I repeat myself. D. My thoughts are wandering off. E. I suddenly start to stutter. F. I look away from the customer. G. I set out to talk louder now. H. I become panicky. I. I am already getting tired now. J. I cannot observe a silence. K. I dare not look the customer in the eyes. L. My hands are trembling. M. I am going to say too much. N. I am losing control over the conversation. IV. Protective actions A. I avoid direct questions like "Please will you sign the offer?" B. I quickly change the subject. C. I adopt a passive attitude. D. I think "I should not ask this.... I find this topic irrelevant." E. I offer the customer too many extras. F. I say, "You do not have to decide now; you may think it over." G. I say to the customer, "You need not answer all questions." H. I often apologize quickly to the customer. I. I talk less persuasively to the customer. J. I dare not argue with the customer. V. Cues in situations that provoke SCA A. Meeting customers with higher social status 1. I talk to a customer with a higher income. 2. I talk to someone who has a higher professional status than I have (e.g., the director of a firm). 3. I talk to a well-known Dutchman. 4. I talk to a customer with an impressive appearance and self-assured manner. B. Preparing for and communicating with new prospects 1. I prepare for a sales conversation with a new and unknown prospect in my sales office. 2. I prepare for a conversation with an unknown prospect. 3. Unexpectedly, I have to meet an unknown prospect because my colleague is excused. 4. I prepare myself for a sales conversation with an unknown prospect at his home. 5. Unexpectedly, I have to meet a prospect who walked into the sales office. C. Being assertive about self and organization 1. I have to tell the customers, "We are the best bank." 2. I have to explain to the customers what we have to offer to them compared with other competing banks. 3. I have to explain to the customers what they might expect from me during and after the conversation. 4. I have to explain to the customers what I now expect from them during the sales conversation. D. Discussing performance with the sales manager 1. I have to share my sales results with my manager. 2. I have to talk with my manager about my sales results. 3. I have to discuss my sales results with my manager, and it appears I did not achieve my sales quotas. 4. I have to tell my sales manager how I go about selling mortgages to customers. E. Approaching and asking for a commitment from the customer 1. The customer tells me that my offer is too expensive, and I have to refute this. 2. The customer shows me the offer from a competitor, and I have to reply to this. 3. I ask customers whether they know someone else who would be interested in buying mortgages from my bank. 4. I ask customers if they are interested In buying a mortgage from my bank. 5. Beyond my expectations, customers appear to be well informed about mortgage offers from competitors. 6. I ask customers whether they will sign the mortgage contract. 7. Customers speak to me about something that I do not do well (e.g., I did not show up at an appointment). VI. Performance A. Sales volume 1. Producing a high market share for your company in your territory. 2. Generating high levels of dollar sales. 3. Quickly generating sales of new company products. 4. Identifying and selling to major accounts in your territory. 5. Exceeding all sales targets and objectives for your territory during the year. B. Sales interaction 1. Listening attentively to identify and understand the real concerns of your customer. 2. Convincing customers that you understand their unique problems and concerns. 3. Working out solutions to a customer's questions or objections. 4. Undertaking actions when customers have problems. Notes: The items presented here are loose translations from the original Dutch versions used in the study. Sales Call Anxiety/99
  • 13. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Badovick, Gordon J., Farrand J. Hadaway, and Peter F. Kaminski (1992), "Attributions and Emotions: The Effect on Salesperson Motivation After Successful vs. Unsuccessful Quota Performance," Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 12 (3), 1-11. Bagozzi, Richard P (1992), "The Self-Regulation of Attitudes, Intentions, and Behavior," Social Psychology Quarterly, 55 (March), 178-204. , Hans Baumgartner, and Rik Pieters (1998), "GoalDirected Emotions," Cognition and Emotion, 12, 1-26. Barlow, David H. (1988), Anxiety and Its Disorders: The Nature and Treatment of Anxiety and Panic. New York: Guilford Press. Barret, Karen C. (1995), "A Functionalist Approach to Shame and Guilt," in Self-Conscious Emotions, June P Tangney and Kurt W. Fischer, eds. New York: Guilford Press, 25-63. Baumeister, Roy F and Mark R. Leary (1995), "The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human Motivation," Psychological Bulletin, 117 (May), 497-529. Beatty, Michael J. (1988), "Situation and Predispositional Correlates of Public Speaking Anxiety," Communication Education, 37 (January), 28-39. Beck, Aron T (1976), Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: International University Press. and Arthur Freeman (1990), Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders. New York: Guilford Press. Behrman, Douglas N. and William D. Perreault (1984), "Measuring the Performance of Salespersons," Journal of Business Research, 10 (September), 355-70. Bickart, Barbara A. (1993), "Carryover and Backfire Effects in Marketing Research," Journal ofMarlceling Research, 30 (February), 52-62. Brief, Arthur P., Michael J. Burke, Jennifer M. George, Brian S. Robinson, and Jane Webster (1988), "Should Negative Affectivity Remain an Unmeasured Variable in the Study of Job Stress?" Journal of Applied Psychology, 73 (2), 193-98. Briggs, Stephen R. and Jonathan M. Cheek (1986), "The Role of Factor Analysis in the Development and Evaluation of Personality Scales," Journal of Personality, 54, 106-148. Brown, Steven P, William L. Cron, and John W. Slocum Jr. (1997), "Effects of Goal-Directed Emotions on Salesperson Volitions, Behavior, and Performance: A Longitudinal Study," Journal of Marketing, 61 (January), 39-50. Buss, Arnold H. (1980), Self-Consciousness and Social Anxiety. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman. Cheek, Jonathan M. (1989), Conquering Shyness. New York: Dell. Chen, Peter Y. and Paul E. Spector (1991), "Negative Affectivity as the Underlying Cause of Correlations Between Stressors and Strains," Journal of Applied Psychology, 76 (3), 398-407. Clark, David M. and Adrian Wells (1995), "A Cognitive Model of Social Phobia," in Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Treatment, Richard G. Heimberg, Michael Liebowitz, Debra A. Hope, and Franklin Schneier, eds. New York: Guilford Press, 69-93. DeVellis, Robert (1991), Scale Development: Theories and Application. Newbury Park, NJ: Sage Publications. Dudley, George W. and Shannon Goodson (1992), Earning What You 're Worth: The Psychology of Sales Call Reluctance. Dallas: Behavioral Sciences Research Press. Ellis, Albert (1962), Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart. 100 / Joumal of Marketing, July 2000 Goodwin, Cathy, Michael Mayo, and Ronald P Hill (1997), "Sales Response to Loss of a Major Account: A Qualitative Analysis," Journal of Business Research, 40, 167-80. Heckelman, Leora R. and Franklin R. Schneier (1995), "Diagnostic Issues," in Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment, Richard G. Heimberg, Michael Liebowitz, Debra A. Hope, and Franklin Schneier, eds. New York: Guilford, 3-20. Heimberg, Richard G., Michael Liebowitz, Debra A. Hope, and Franklin Schneier (1995), Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment. New York: Guilford. Ingram, Thomas N., Raymond W. LaForge, and Charles H. Schwepker (1997), Sales Management. Fort Worth, TX: The Dryden Press. Johnston, Wesley J. and Keysuk Kim (1994), "Performance, Attribution, and Expectancy Linkages in Personal Selling," Journal of Marketing, 58 (October), 68-81. Joreskog, Karl G. and Dag Sorbom (1996), LtSREL^ User's Reference Guide. Chicago: Scientific Software. Kramer, Roderick M. (1994), "The Sinister Attribution Error," Motivation and Emotion, 18, 199-231. Lazarus, Richard S. and Susan Folkman (1984), Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer. Leary, Mark R. (1991), "Social Anxiety, Shyness, and Related Constructs," in Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes, John Robinson, Phillip Shaver, and Lawrence Wrightsman, eds. New York: Academic Press, 161-94. and Robin M. Kowalski (1995), Social Anxiety. New York: Guilford Press. LeDoux, Joseph L. (1996), The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon and Schuster. Liebowitz, Michael R., J.M. Gorman, A.J. Fyer, R. Campeas, A.P. Levin, D. Sandberg, E. Hollander, L. Papp, and D. Goetz (1988), "Pharmacotherapy of Social Phobia: An Interim Report of a Placebo-Controlled Comparison of Phenelzine and Atenolol," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 49, 252-57. Markaway, Barbara G., Cheryl N. Carmen, Alec C. Pollard, and Teresa Flynn (1992), Dying of Embarrassment. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. Martens, Rainer (1977), Sport Competition Anxiety Test. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Miles, Raymond E. and Charles C. Snow (1978), Organizational Strategy. Structure, and Process. New York: McGraw-Hill. Mineka, Susan and Kathleen Nugent (1995), "Mood Congruent Memory Biases in Anxiety and Depression," in Memory Distortion, Daniel L. Schacter, ed. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 172-93. Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw Hill. Patterson, Miles L. and Vicki Ritts (1997), "Social and Communicative Anxiety: A Review and Meta-Analysis," in Communication Yearbook 20, Brant R. Burleson, ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, 263-304. Peterson, R.A. and S. Reiss (1987), Test Manual for the Anxiety Sensitivity tndex. Orland Park, IL: Intemational Diagnostics Systems. Pozo, C , Charles C. Carver, A.R. Wellens, and Michael F. Scheier (1991), "Social Anxiety and Social Perception: Construing Others' Reactions to the Self," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, 355-62. Rachman, Stanley (998), Anxiety. London: Psychology Press. Rapee, Ronald M. and Lina Lim (1992), "Discrepancy Between Self- and Observer Ratings of Performance in Sociai Phobics," Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 728-31. Ray, Dana (1995), "Confront Call Reluctance," Personal Selling Power, (September), 46-51.
  • 14. Salkovskis, Paul M. (1991), "The Importance of Behaviour in the Maintenance of Anxiety and Panic: A Cognitive Account," Behavioural Psychotherapy, 19, 6-19. Sarason, Irwin G. and Barbara R. Sarason (1986), "Anxiety and Interfering Thoughts: Their Effect on Social Interaction," in Shyness: Perspectives on Research and Treatment, Warren H. Jones, Jonathan M. Cheek, and Stephen R. Briggs, eds. New York: Plenum Press, 253-64. Simon, Herbert A. (1967), "Motivational and Emotional Controls of Cognition," Psychological Review, 74, 29-39. Singh, Jagdip (1993), "Boundary Role Ambiguity: Facets, Determinants, and Impact," Journal of Marketing, 57 (April), 11-31. and Gary Rhoads (1991), "Boundary Role Ambiguity in Marketing Oriented Positions: A Multidimensional, Multifaceted, Operationalization," Journal of Marketing Research, 28 (August), 328-38. Spiro, Rosan L. and Barton A. Weitz (1989), "Adaptive Selling: Conceptualization, Measurement, and Nomological Validity," Journal of Marketing Research, 11 (February), 61-69. SPSS (1993), SPSS For Windows Professional Statistics, Release 6.0. Chicago: SPSS. Stopa, Lusia and David M. Clark (1993), "Cognitive Processes in Social Phobia," Behaviour Research and Therapv, 31 (3), 255-67. Strickland, B. (1977), "Achievement Motivation," in Personality Variables and Social Behavior, T Blass, ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates, 315-56. Sujan, Harish, Barton Weitz, and Nirmalya Kumar (1994), "Learning Orientation, Working Smart, and Effective Selling," Journal of Marketing, 58 (July), 39-52. Szymansky, David (1988), "Determinants of Selling Effectiveness: The Importance of Declarative Knowledge to the Personal Selling Concept," Journal of Marketing, 52 (January). 64-77. Tellegen, Auke (1982), "Brief Manual for the Differential Personality Questionnaire," unpublished manuscript. University of Minnesota. Thoits, Peggy A. (1990), "Emotion Deviance: Research Agendas," in Research Agendas in the Sociology of Emotions, Theodore D. Kemper, ed. Albany: State University of New York Press, 180-206. Trower, Peter and Paul Gilbert (1989), "New Theoretical Conceptions of Social Anxiety and Social Phobia," Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 19-35. Watson, David and Lee Anna Clark (1984), "Negative Affectivity: The Disposition to Experience Aversive Emotional States," Psychological Bulletin, 96 (3), 465-90. and James.W. Pennebaker (1989), "Health Complaints, Stress, and Distress: Exploring the Central Role of Negative Affectivity," Psychological Review, 96, 234—54. Weitz, Bart A. (1978), "Relationship Between Salesperson Performance and Understanding of Customer Decision Making," Journal of Marketing Research, 15 (November), 501-16. (1981), "Effectiveness in Sales Interactions: A Contingency Framework," Journal of Marketing, 45 (Winter), 85-103. -, Stephen B. Castleberry, and John F. Tanner (1992), Selling: Building Partnerships. Homewood, IL: Richard D. Irwin. Wells, Adrian (1998), Cognitive Therapy of Aivciety Disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons. , David M. Clark, Paul Salkovskis, John Ludgate, Ann Hackmann, and Michael Gelder (1995), "Social Phobia: The Role of In-Situation Safety Behaviors in Maintaining Anxiety and Negative Beliefs," Behavior Therapy, 26, 153-61. and Gerald Matthews (1994), Attention and Emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates. Zillman, Dolf, R.C. Johnson, and K.D. Day (1974), "Provoked and Unprovoked Aggressiveness in Athletes." Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 139-52. ^ Leads Mailing Usts For a FREE Catalog & Personalized Service Call: Business Lists: Over 11 Million Businesses Select by Type of Business, Employee Size & Sales Volume for any ZIP Code, County, City or State. (800) 336-8349 or Fax Us at: (402) 331-1505 Plus, FREE Directory Assistance & National Yellow Pages on Our Web Site. WWW. inf oUSA. com infoVSA-A Public Company. Nasdaq: lUSA 5711 S. 86th Circle • Omaha, NE 68127 E-Mail: lists@infoLISA.com Consumer Lists: Over 200 Miiiion Consumers Select by Age, Income, Household Value & More. (800) 661-5478 or Fax Us at: (402) 331-0991 Business Credit Reports only ^5.00 each! Also Available, Montiiiy Updates! Media Code: 01-JMK Sales Call Anxiety/101