SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 51
Download to read offline
THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION AND PROCESSING ON EXPLICIT MEMORY

                           IN OLDER ADULTS




                                  A Thesis



          Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology

                  California State University, Sacramento



                     Submitted in partial satisfaction of
                     the requirements for the degree of


                            MASTER OF ARTS

                                      in

                                Psychology



                                     by

                         Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza




                                  SPRING
                                   2012
THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION AND PROCESSING ON EXPLICIT MEMORY

                            IN OLDER ADULTS



                                  A Thesis

                                     by

                          Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza



Approved by:

__________________________________, Committee Chair
 Kelly Cotter, Ph.D.

__________________________________, Second Reader
John Schaeuble, Ph.D.

__________________________________, Third Reader
Emily Wickelgren, Ph.D.


____________________________
Date




                                     ii
Student: Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza




I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University

format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to

be awarded for the thesis.




__________________________, Graduate Coordinator               ___________________
Jianjian Qin, Ph. D.                                           Date


Department of Psychology




                                              iii
Abstract

                                             of

THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION AND PROCESSING ON EXPLICIT MEMORY

                                   IN OLDER ADULTS



                                             by

                                Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza

Forty eight older adults, sampled from two senior communities, watched a series of

words on a computer monitor and then took a written exam that tested their explicit word

recall. Results revealed a main effect for presentation, such that participants correctly

recalled more words when those words were presented for 2 seconds than when those

words were presented for .04 seconds. Results also revealed an interaction between

presentation and processing, where the supraliminal presentation yielded more correctly

recalled words in thematic conditions than in non-thematic conditions. These results

suggest that older adults exhibit higher explicit memory performance when words

belonging to a theme or category are presented within conscious awareness than outside

awareness, likely because they are able to process the information more deeply.

_______________________, Committee Chair
Kelly Cotter, Ph.D.

_______________________
Date




                                             iv
DEDICATION



       I would like to dedicate this thesis to my deceased maternal grandparents, Maria

de la Luz Razo and Salvador Gomez who raised me and served as the inspiration for my

topic on cognition and aging.




                                           v
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS



          First and foremost, I would like to thank my parents for everything they have

done for me. Since preschool, my parents have impressed upon me the value of education

and importance of using one’s intellect to make a significant impact on the world;

however small or big that may be. They gave me the greatest gift that any parent could

bestow on their child, individuation, by giving me the freedom to explore my academic

pursuits. My thesis topic availed me this golden opportunity to spread my wings and fly.

Special thanks to my mother’s extended family for their undying support of my

educational endeavors and contributions to my personal development throughout

graduate school. During graduate school, my grandmother passed away which was a

major setback for our family, for she was the anchor that held our family together.

Amidst those trying times, my mom’s family taught me to rise above hardships and to

find my inner strength within to finish my degree. I would like to thank Dr. Kelly Cotter

for her patience, immense wisdom, alacrity, and generosity of time and effort towards

helping me finish my thesis. I would also like to thank Dr. John Schaeuble for helping me

arrive at my thesis topic and for assisting me with narrowing my thesis topic down to two

variables. I would like to thank Dr. Wickelgren for helping me with the Latin Square

Experimental Design of my video presentations to eliminate bias. I would like to thank

Debbie Kircher for pacing me through the program and helping me see my own beauty

within.


                                              vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Dedication ............................................................................................................................v

Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................ vi

List of Tables ..................................................................................................................... ix

List of Figures ......................................................................................................................x

Chapter

 1. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................1

           Memory ....................................................................................................................1

           Supraliminal and Subliminal Memory Processes ....................................................3

           Levels of Processing ................................................................................................4

           Interaction of Levels of Processing and Timing of Presentation .............................5

           The Present Study ....................................................................................................6

 2. METHOD .....................................................................................................................8

           Materials ................................................................................................................10

           Participants .............................................................................................................13

           Procedure ...............................................................................................................13

 3. RESULTS ...................................................................................................................16

           Hits .........................................................................................................................17

           False Alarms ..........................................................................................................19

 4. DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................20

           Strengths and Limitations ......................................................................................23
                                                                   vii
Future Directions ...................................................................................................28

         Conclusion .............................................................................................................29

Appendix          Final List of Forty Words Used in the Four Video Conditions ...................30

References ........................................................................................................................31




                                                               viii
LIST OF TABLES

Tables                                                                                                 Page

1. Descriptive Statistics for Hits and False Alarms ....................................................... 16




                                                   ix
LIST OF FIGURES

Figures                                                                                                Page

1. The Interaction of Presentation and Relatedness for Hits ..............................................18

2. The Main Effect of Presentation for False Alarms ........................................................19




                                                      x
1


                                        Chapter 1

                                    INTRODUCTION



       Dementia is a degenerative disease of the aging brain that impairs cognitive

functioning by attacking the central nervous system at the vascular and cellular levels

(Whitehouse, Price, Strubie, Clark, Coyle, & Delon, 1982). A reputable occupational

health and medical disability advisory website, MDGuidelines, provides the article

“Dementia definition” (2009) stating that although dementia impairs cognitive

functioning in many ways, profound and irreversible memory loss is its primary effect.

Because dementia is an increasingly prevalent age-related neural disease within senior

populations, developing cognitive strategies for preventing memory loss is necessary for

maintenance of quality of life and positive life orientations (Santacruz & Swagerty,

2001). I focus on two potential memory-enhancing strategies in the present study:

presentation of information and processing of information. I examine these strategies as

employed by older adults who do not suffer from cognitive impairment, in hopes that

what is learned in normal aging populations can be beneficial for those at risk of

developing dementia.

                                         Memory

       Memory, as defined in The American Heritage Dictionary (1985), is the act of

encoding, storing, and recalling a past event or item of factual knowledge. Memory can

be divided into two forms: implicit and explicit (Graf & Schacter, 1985), both of which

are affected by the aging process (Light & Singh, 1987). Implicit memory depends on
2


cognitive processes occurring outside of one’s conscious awareness (Schacter, 1987).

These automatic processes can include stimuli-associate conditioning, social modeling,

relearning and practice effects, and priming (Schacter, 1987). Roediger and McDermott

(1993) and Schacter (1987) described implicit memory tasks as indirect, where there was

no reference to past exposure to information.

       In contrast, explicit memory tasks require the conscious recollection of an item

directly referenced by past experience (Graf & Schacter, 1985). When one performs well

on an explicit memory test, he or she has brought factual knowledge into awareness

(Challis, 1996; Joyce, Paller, McIsaac, & Kutas, 1998). Roediger and McDermott (1993)

and Schacter (1987) characterized explicit memory tests as tasks performed with

intention, where participants are instructed to recall or recognize stimuli they had

previously seen.

       I chose to analyze free recall test performance as a measure of explicit memory

processes in the current study. Performance on a free recall exam is a direct measurement

of the conscious recollection of previously presented stimuli. This type of memory task

relies on processing capacity, revealing the influence of working memory and processing

speed on retrieval (Craik, Govoni, Naveh-Benjamin, & Anderson, 1996; Whiting &

Smith, 1997). Free recall tests typically depend on effortful cognitive control and bottom

up retrieval processes.

       In cross-sectional and longitudinal studies examining age-related memory, older

participants tend to score lower than younger participants on explicit memory tasks

(Anooshian, 1997; Carroll, Byrne, & Kirsner, 1985; Ellis, Ellis, & Hosey, 1993;
3


Greenbaum & Graf, 1989; Lorsbach & Morris, 1991; Lorsbach & Worman, 1990; Naito,

1990; Parkin & Streete, 1988). However, older cohorts show equal performance to young

adults on perceptual implicit tasks (Light & Singh, 1987) shedding light on potential

compensatory mechanisms used by older adults to preserve explicit memory. To

investigate the older cohorts’ adaptive use of cognitive tools further, I analyzed the

influence of two associative memory processes on explicit memory test performance in

older adults: level of processing (shallow versus deep) and awareness of presentation

(subliminal versus supraliminal), described in detail below.

                   Supraliminal and Subliminal Memory Processes

       As described above, explicit memory tests often involve asking participants to

recall or recognize information presented to them in a laboratory setting. The information

can be presented supraliminally (within one's conscious awareness) or subliminally

(outside one's ability to consciously perceive presented stimuli) in the study/encoding

session. Supraliminal memory processes are driven by strong feelings of event

recollection that enhance one’s ability to visualize the scene (Aggleton & Brown, 1999;

Gardiner, 1988; Gardiner & Java, 1990; Gardiner & Java, 1991; Gardiner, Java &

Richardson-Klavehn, 1996; Jacoby, 1991; Rajaram, 1993; Tulving, 1985; Wagner,

Verfaeille, & Gabrieli, 1997; Yonelinas, 2002). Subliminal processes depend upon

feelings of familiarity that lack a concrete experience of an actual event (Khilstrom,

1990; Khilstrom, Barnhardt, & Tataryn, 1992a; Khilstrom, Shames, & Dorfman, 1996;

Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996; Toth, 1996). The priming
4


task is a classic methodology for studying subliminal processes (Kiefer, 2007). Age has

little effect on priming (Light & Singh, 1987).

                                   Levels of Processing

       Ritchey, Bessette-Symons, Hayes, and Cabeza (2011) showed that semantic

elaboration can enhance neural processing in the medial prefrontal cortices of the human

brain. Deep encoding requires the utilization of meaning-based encoding to activate

associative memory network connections, while shallow processing refers to the

presentation of stimuli encoded at a rudimentary or physical level without the use of

semantic-level memory access (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). For example, deep processing

is applied during exposure to a word list consisting of items belonging to the same

category, such as Zoo Animals, because people form associations between the words. On

the other hand, shallow processing is utilized during exposure to randomly chosen words

because there is no categorical link between the words. Craik and Lockhart (1972)

showed that deep or semantically-processed stimuli had a higher chance of being recalled

than shallow or physically-processed stimuli. This effect was found even when

participants did not expect subsequent recall or recognition tests.

        Bradshaw and Anderson (1982) also showed the effect of elaboration and

thematic-relatedness on memory processes: Participants in their study demonstrated

higher memory performance in the thematic conditions, which required the elaborate

integration of memory traces, and lower memory performance in the conditions with

minimal thematic-relatedness. Similarly, levels of processing theory stipulates that

performance is facilitated by elaborately encoded or “deeply” processed stimuli and
5


degraded by low-level encoding or “shallow” processing of stimuli (Craik & Lockhart,

1972). Hence, Bradshaw and Anderson’s (1982) study draws parallels between thematic-

relatedness and levels of processing theory, shedding light on the role of neural networks

in the creation of interconnected explicit memory traces. In the current study, I adapted

the methodology that used thematic-relatedness in order to operationally define levels of

processing.

              Interaction of Levels of Processing and Timing of Presentation

       Effortful processing may enhance memory performance in older cohorts on

priming tests. Englekamp and Wippich (1995) demonstrate an age effect for conceptual

priming, such that younger cohorts generally show higher memory on implicit tests than

older cohorts when presented with atypical stimuli at study. Englekamp and Wippich

showed that younger cohorts tend to perform better than older cohorts in random stimuli

and subliminal presentation conditions while performing worse in moderate-relatedness

conditions.

       The distinction between the cognitive resources that drive explicit and implicit

memory processes begin to blur (Srinivas & Roediger, 1990) as category exemplar tasks

capture both conceptual subliminal processes and perceptual subliminal processes. While

unraveling the effects of conscious and unconscious processes on memory, Monti et al.

(1996) investigated how category exemplar tasks rely on relatedness to create meaning-

based or deeply processed memory traces. Although category exemplar tasks are

classically referred to as “implicit” memory tasks, their conceptual properties are

sensitive to different levels of processing at the supraliminal level (Monti et al., 1996).
6


Hence, category exemplar tests may have more conceptually-driven effects on memory if

coupled with deep levels of processing conditions. Therefore, among the memory tasks

available to researchers, the category exemplar test is one of the most effective priming

tools for studying the interaction of levels of processing and exposure to subliminal and

supraliminal stimuli (Mitchell & Bruss, 2003). Thus, in the present study I applied the

category exemplar test by presenting target exemplars from taxonomic groups of animals

and vegetables to the participants and by asking participants to recall those targets later.

         Despite the potential interaction between processing and presentation, there

remains a paucity of research that examines the moderated effects of semantically

processed stimuli on category exemplar task performance in older adults. Thus, in

contrast to recent studies examining the relationship between relatedness, elaboration,

and explicit memory (Bradshaw & Anderson, 1982), I analyzed the effect of presentation

timing and thematic-relatedness on the explicit memory of older adults in the current

study.

                                    The Present Study

         In the present study, I examined how levels of processing (deep versus shallow)

affects explicit memory task performance when moderated by presentation timing

(subliminal versus supraliminal). I operationally defined deep processing as the

presentation of thematically-related words from two thematic categories (Zoo Animals

and Vegetables). I defined shallow processing as the presentation of randomly generated,

non-thematically-related words. I defined supraliminal presentation as seeing a series of
7


words for 2 seconds each. Subliminal presentation was defined as seeing a series of

words for .04 seconds each.

       I expected to find main effects for both presentation and processing of stimuli. For

the main effect of presentation, I expected to see a higher number of supraliminally-

presented words recalled than subliminally-presented words. For the main effect of

relatedness, I proposed that memory for thematically-related words would be higher than

memory for non-thematically-related words. Specifically, I expected that thematically-

related words would provide a semantic (deep) form of processing that would facilitate

memory performance across both supraliminal and subliminal presentation conditions.

       I also hypothesized that participants would have higher memory performance in

supraliminal-deep levels of processing conditions and lower performance in supraliminal-

shallow, subliminal-deep, and subliminal-shallow conditions. I expected to see this

interaction because meaningful, related words shown for a longer amount of time on a

computer screen tend to produce more durable memory traces than random words or

words presented for a shorter amount of time. The focusing of one’s mind on semantic

content should yield a compound effect on explicit memory performance because

supraliminal processes require the effortful application of cognitive resources that spark

neural network activation and parallel processing stream distribution.
8


                                         Chapter 2

                                         METHOD



       With a 2x2x4 mixed repeated-measures factorial design, described in detail

below, I examined the nature of semantic facilitation on explicit memory for words

presented at the conscious and the unconscious perceptual level. I showed each

participant a series of forty words flashed on a computer screen and later asked

participants to recall the words. The two within-subjects independent variables were

thematic association of words and presentation of stimuli. The between-groups

independent variable was order of presentation. Explicit memory recall test performance

was my dependent variable.

       The first within-subjects independent variable was thematic association/level of

processing. Thematic association referred to how easily participants could form

associations with presented words (thus affecting their memory for those words), and was

comprised of two levels: a thematically-related word condition (deep processing) and a

non-thematic word condition (shallow processing). The thematically-related word

condition was a list of ten words related to the same category, either Zoo Animals (e.g.,

zebra, kangaroo) or Vegetables (e.g., broccoli, potato). The non-thematic word condition

was a list of ten randomly generated words that had no relation to each other (e.g., skirt,

motorcycle).

       The second within-subjects independent variable, presentation of stimuli, referred

to how long participants saw words flashed on the computer screen. This variable had
9


two levels: the supraliminal condition and the subliminal condition. In the supraliminal

condition, participants were exposed to words for 2 seconds each. This presentation time

was long enough for participants to read and understand the word, thus processing it

completely (Moore, 1982; Whiting, 1997). Words were presented for .04 seconds each in

the subliminal condition. This presentation time was long enough for participants to

detect a word, but not long enough to consciously process the word (Khilstrom, 1987;

Moore, 1982).

       Video presentation, consisting of four levels, was the only between-subjects

variable used in this study. It was used in order to control for potential order effects. I

used a partial Latin-Square to counterbalance the sequence of phases within each video

presentation. This method of counterbalancing ensured that the thematic and non-

thematic words were presented in every possible order (first, second, third, fourth). For

example, order one consisted of supraliminal thematic words presented in phase 1,

subliminal non-thematic words presented in phase 2, supraliminal non-thematic words

presented in phase 3, and subliminal thematic words presented in phase 4. In order two,

by contrast, supraliminal non-thematic words appeared in phase 1, subliminal thematic

words appeared in phase 2, supraliminal thematic words appeared in phase 3, and

subliminal non-thematic words appeared in phase 4. (Each video started with a

supraliminal phase in order to prevent the participant from thinking that there were no

words presented in the video.) Thus, the presentation conditions were counterbalanced

across the four different videos. A total of forty words were presented in each video.
10


                                          Materials

Word Generation

       To create the vegetable word list I randomly selected 41 Vegetable words from

the following website: http://www.gardenology.org/wiki/List_of_vegetables. To create

the zoo animal word list I randomly selected 26 Zoo Animal words from the following

website: http://www.catalandictionary.org/wordnets/eng/ZooAnimalList.htm. To create

the non-thematic word list I randomly generated 24 words from the following website:

http://coyotecult.com/tools/randomwordgenerator.php. Next, I trimmed each list to 10

words following the procedures described below.

Inter-rater Reliability

        The relatedness of a word to a category could affect the speed and accuracy by

which a participant could retrieve the word from memory, thus assisting in the formation

of cognitive associations (Anderson, 1980). Therefore, I wanted to make sure that one

word list did not contain words that were more related to their respective theme than the

other words to that list’s respective theme. To check the equivalence of thematic-

relatedness of the words in the two thematic word lists, I collected inter-rater reliability

data on the degree of relatedness of each word to its specific category.

       Ten friends and family members below the age of 55 participated as raters. They

were asked to rate how much each word from the vegetable list was related to the theme

of vegetables, and how much each word from the zoo animals list was related to the

theme of zoo animals. Relatedness ratings were collected only for thematic words.
11


       I also collected inter-rater reliability data on the commonness of each word in

everyday usage because the commonness of a word could also increase the accuracy of

recall (Deese, 1960; Gregg, Montgomery, & Castano, 1980; Hall, 1954; Matthews, 1966;

May, Cuddy, & Norton, 1979; May & Tryk, 1970; Postman, 1970; Sumby, 1963; Tulving

& Patkau, 1962; Whiting & Smith, 1997). The commonness inter-rater questionnaire

asked raters to rate how commonly words are used in everyday speech. Commonness

ratings were collected for thematic and non-thematic words. Upon completion of the

word rating procedure, I rewarded the raters with $5.00 Target gift cards.

Final Word List Creation

        To create lists of equivalent length (10 words) for each processing condition

(thematically-related and non-thematically-related), I entered the participants’ ratings into

a spreadsheet. Next, I eliminated some words from the original lists by sorting the word

lists (Zoo Animals, Vegetables, non-thematic) from the highest to lowest mean

commonness rating. Then I chose every other two words descending from the highest to

lowest mean commonness rating until I obtained 20 words for each thematic list (Zoo

Animals, Vegetables) and 20 words for the non-thematic word lists. The final list of 40

words was divided into four lists of ten words each using the ABBA sorting sequence.

Refer to Appendix for the list of words used in the video conditions.

       After the final word lists were created, I calculated the variability between the two

thematic word lists on relatedness and the variability between the two thematic and the

two non-thematic lists on commonness. T-tests revealed no significant differences

between Zoo Animal words (M = 4.09, SD = .54) and Vegetable words (M = 3.71, SD =
12


.76) on relatedness to their respective thematic category; t (38) = 1.73, p = .09. In other

words, Zoo Animal words and Vegetable words had the same degree of relatedness to

their respective theme. T-tests also showed no significant differences between thematic

(M = 3.49, SD = .63) and non-thematic words (M = 3.34, SD = 1.03) on commonness; t

(38) = .70, p = .48. Thus, both thematic and non-thematic words were considered

equivalent on commonness.

       Upon completion of the word list generation procedure I had four lists of 10

words. These lists were capped at 10 words each because the average older adult has a

working memory capacity of 4 (+/- 2) items (Bo, Borza & Seidler, 2009). I chose to

increase the size of the word list from four words to ten words because I wanted to avoid

any potential ceiling effects (Miller, 1956).

       Finally, to randomly sort the words within the four lists for the video presentation

conditions, I used the following website: http://textmechanic.com/Sort-Text-Lines.html,

which generated random orders for each 10-word list.

Computer

        Words were presented to participants on an ASUS gaming laptop computer

running Windows Live Movie Maker for Windows 7 Home Premium operating system.

Demographic Questionnaire

       Before administering the computerized portion of the procedure I collected the

following demographic information: Gender, Highest Education Achieved, Occupation,

Ethnicity, and Religion. Age information was not collected because my entire participant
13


pool consisted of older adults and I did not want the sensitivity of the age question to

deter potential subjects from participating in my study.

                                       Participants

       I sent letters to fourteen different agencies within the Sacramento Valley region

and called ten agencies. I recruited forty-eight older adults over the age of 55 from the

following agencies: Senior Center at Elk Grove, Mission Oaks/Swanston Community

Center in Carmichael, and Ethel Hart Senior Center. Of the twelve participants who

answered the question on “Gender,” 3 participants were “Male” and 9 participants were

“Female.” Four out of 48 participants “Completed High School,” 11 “Went to college but

did not finish,” 12 participants “Finished some graduate school or higher,” and 2 “Earned

a doctorate degree.” (The questionnaire did not include an option for those who

completed college but did not attend graduate school.) Almost half of the participants

were “Caucasian” (n = 23), 4 were “Hispanic,” 1 participant was of “Asian American”

descent, 2 were “African American,” and 18 did not indicate their ethnicity. Out of 45

participants who responded to the Religion question, 12 were “Catholic,” 13 were

“Christian,” 5 were “Spiritual,” 1 was “Mormon,” 1 was “Episcopalian,” 3 participants

were “Atheist,” 3 were “New Age,” 2 were “Baptist,” and 5 participants reported “Other”

to signify that they were of a religious affiliation other than what was asked on the

questionnaire. Three participants out of 48 did not respond to the religion question.

                                        Procedure

       I received approval at minimal risk to recruit participants from the Human

Subjects Committee in the Department of Psychology in Spring 2011. Next, I pilot-tested
14


the procedure with four individuals who did not meet the study’s criteria for participation

to make sure the testing protocols ran smoothly. There were no problems with the

procedure.

         To prepare for the experiment, I made arrangements with the activity coordinators

from each agency listed above to secure a room and a specific time to run my study.

Before the day of the experiment, I randomly assigned the participants to one of four

video conditions. On test day, I greeted each participant individually, showed the

participant to his or her seat in front of the laptop, and led the participant through the

informed consent procedure. Next, I handed the participant a consent form to sign and

return back to me. I then provided the participant with a demographic questionnaire that

asked the participant to reveal his or her gender, education status, occupation, religion,

and ethnicity.

         Before starting the video presentation, I instructed the participant to not touch any

key on the keyboard or touchpad because the screen ran by itself. Next, I started the video

assigned to the participant. Within each video, the supraliminal condition had one

thematic phase and one non-thematic phase. The subliminal condition also had one

thematic phase and one non-thematic phase. The order was counterbalanced, as described

above.

         The video presentation lasted for 3.99 minutes. In the supraliminal condition, a

blank screen appeared first for 1 second. The orienting stimulus, "_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ ",

flashed for .5 seconds before each word appeared on the screen. The masking stimulus,

"XXXXXXXXXX", followed each word and remained on the screen for .5 seconds. A
15


blank screen appeared for 1 second before the orienting stimulus for the next word. In the

subliminal condition the blank screen was shown for 2 seconds before the appearance of

the next word. The orienting stimulus, "_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _", flashed for .5 seconds before

each word and the masking stimulus, "XXXXXXXXXX", appeared on the screen for a

duration of .5 seconds after the flashing of each word. A blank screen appeared for 2

seconds before the orienting stimulus for the next word. The difference in timing of the

presentation of blank screens was potentially problematic but unequal durations for the

four videos would have compromised the internal validity of my repeated measures

design. To ensure that each video condition lasted for an equivalent amount of time, I

prioritized consistency of video presentation length across my video conditions over

equal time spacing between orienting stimuli, test words, and masking stimuli.

       After the video was complete, I administered the free recall exam by giving the

participant a pen and piece of lined 8.5 x11 white printer paper with the following

instructions at the top of the page: “Please write as many words as you can remember from

the presentation you just saw. You have as much time as you want to complete this form.”

Next, I told the participant that he or she could take as much time as needed to complete

the test. After I read the instructions to the participant, I quietly left the room. After the

exam was complete, I debriefed the participant and gave the participant a $5 gift card to

Walmart.
16


                                                    Chapter 3

                                                   RESULTS



         Using two mixed repeated measures ANOVAs, I examined differences between

the thematic and presentation conditions on word recall (see Table 1).


Table 1

Descriptive Statistics for Hits and False Alarms (N = 48)

                                            Hits                            False Alarms

Source                         M     SD        F(1, 44)       p       M     SD     F(1, 44)    p


Relatedness                                        100.37   < .001                   .19      .67

   Thematic                   2.13   0.13                            0.10   0.04

   Non-thematic               0.57   0.11                            0.13   0.05

Presentation                                       220.90   < .001                  10.05     .003

  Supraliminal                2.63   0.17                            0.22   0.06


  Subliminal                  0.07   0.05                            0.01   0.01


 Relatedness x Presentation                        109.13   < .001                  0.00      1.00

  Thematic Supraliminal       4.10   0.23                            0.21   0.07


  Thematic Subliminal         0.15   0.09                            0.00   0.00


  Nonthematic Supraliminal    1.15   0.21                            0.23   0.09


  Nonthematic Subliminal      0.00   0.00                            0.02   0.02




          The first dependent variable, the number of “hits,” represented the number of

words the participant wrote down on the recall task that were presented in the video

presentation (i.e., correctly-recalled words). The second dependent variable, number of
17


false alarms, was the number of words that the participant wrote down on the recall task

that were not presented in the video presentation. I associated each false alarm with the

condition that contained a structurally similar word. For example, if one condition

contained the word ”torchlight” and the participant wrote the word “headlight,” I placed

the false alarm score in the condition that contained “torchlight.”

                                             Hits

       The between subjects variable, presentation order, did not show a significant main

effect for hits, F (3, 44) = 2.18, p = .10, indicating that order of presentation had no effect

on correct recall for presented words. Additionally, there were no significant two-way or

three-way interactions with presentation order for hits. The first within-subjects variable,

timing of presentation, showed a significant main effect for hits, F (1, 44) = 220.90, p <

.001, semi-partial η2 = .46, such that supraliminal presentation conditions yielded a higher

number of hits than subliminal presentation conditions, as predicted (see Table 1).

However, the presence of an interaction modified the nature of this main effect. The

second independent variable, thematic-relatedness, also showed a significant main effect

for hits, F (1, 44) = 100.37, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .17, such that thematic conditions

produced significantly more hits than non-thematic conditions. However, the interaction

showed that this effect did not exist for the subliminal condition.

       Both main effects had to be viewed in the context of a significant interaction, F

(1, 44) = 109.13, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .14. Pairwise comparisons were used to test

for simple effects of the thematic conditions. For supraliminal presentation conditions

there was a significant mean difference between thematic and non-thematic conditions
18


such that the number of hits in thematic conditions was significantly higher than the

number of hits in non-thematic conditions, Mean difference = 2.96, p < .001. However,

there was no difference between the two thematic conditions in the subliminal condition,

Mean difference = .15, p =.11.

                 6.00

                 5.00

                 4.00          4.10
   Performance




                                                               Presentation
                 3.00
                                                                       Supraliminal
                 2.00                                                  Subliminal

                 1.00                            1.15

                        0.15
                 0.00                            0.00
                        Thematic        Non Thematic
                                Relatedness

 Figure 1. The interaction of presentation and relatedness for hits.
19


                                       False Alarms

       The between subjects variable, presentation order, did not show a significant main

 effect for false alarms, F (3, 44) = .60, p = .62, revealing no effect of video order on the

 number of written responses that were not on the word list but resembled words on the

 list. Moreover, there were no significant two-way or three way interactions with

 presentation order for false alarms. Presentation showed a significant main effect for

 false alarms, F (1, 44) = 10.05, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .10, such that there were

 more false alarms produced in supraliminal presentation conditions than in subliminal

 presentation conditions (see Table 1). However, thematic-relatedness did not show a

 main effect for false alarms, F (1, 44) = .19, p = .67. Similarly, there was no interaction

 of presentation and thematic-relatedness, F (1, 44) = .00, p = 1.00, for false alarms. See

 Figure 2 for the main effect of presentation time on false alarms.




                       0.30
         Performance




                       0.25
                                                                     0.23
                       0.20                                            Presentation
                                      0.21
                       0.15                                                 Supraliminal
                       0.10                                                 Subliminal
                       0.05                                   0.02
                                 0
                       0.00
                              Thematic                   Non Thematic
                                        Relatedness

Figure 2. The main effect of presentation for false alarms.
20


                                         Chapter 4

                                      DISCUSSION



       The results of the present study confirmed my hypothesis that older adults would

exhibit higher explicit memory performance when words were presented to conscious

awareness than when words were not presented within conscious awareness, but this

difference was much larger for words belonging to a theme or category than for words

not related to each other in any obvious way. Furthermore, the results supported my

prediction that words belonging to a theme or category would be more easily

remembered than words that were not related to each other in any obvious way, but this

was true only when words were presented within conscious awareness.

       As I mentioned above, performance on the recall test was higher in supraliminal

conditions than in subliminal conditions. This result is consistent with research on the

facilitation of visual recall through the engagement of conscious processes (Aggleton &

Brown, 1999; Gardiner, 1988; Gardiner & Java, 1990; Gardiner & Java, 1991; Gardiner,

Java & Richardson-Klavehn, 1996; Jacoby, 1991; Rajaram, 1993; Tulving, 1985;

Wagner, Verfaeille, & Gabrieli, 1997; Yonelinas, 2002).

       The deep processing phenomenon is evidenced in my study, which shows higher

explicit memory performance on the free recall exam in thematic conditions than in non-

thematic conditions. This finding resembled the results of Craik and Lockhart (1972),

showing the positive effect of deep processing on explicit memory.
21


       Past research indicates that older adults are more likely to use semantic

associations than younger adults when performing serial recall tasks (Golomb, Peele,

Addis, Kahana, & Wingfield, 2008). Moreover, Englekamp and Wippich (1995) found

that, for older adults, word-relatedness enhances memory of subliminally presented

words. Conversely, younger adults remember more words that are not related to each

other in priming tasks than do older adults (Englekamp & Wippich, 1995). Younger

adults may not use categories to facilitate memory performance during the completion of

implicit memory tasks such as priming.

       In contrast, older adults’ dependence on semantic content for memory

consolidation amplifies the effects of consciously perceived stimuli on explicit memory

test performance. By depicting the relationship between presentation and levels of

processing, my study demonstrated the importance of supraliminal effortful processes on

semantic facilitation of explicit memory. This claim is further supported by evidence

from Monti et al. (1996), who showed how meaning-based encoding processes through

the engagement of supraliminal memory processes enhance conceptually-driven aspects

of memory tasks. Additionally, Bradshaw and Anderson (1982) made advances in

conceptual priming and processing research by revealing the compound effect of

supraliminally-presented stimuli and thematic relatedness on category exemplar test

performance. Conclusively, the results from my study and previous research exemplified

the pivotal role that semantic associations play on conscious processes during explicit

memory tasks.
22


         Notably, older adults falsely-remembered more words in the video presentation of

supraliminal stimuli than of subliminal stimuli. In other words, there were more false

alarms in the supraliminal than in the subliminal conditions. Supraliminality and the

application of cognitive effort seemed to have a stronger effect on incorrect guessing than

the effect of meaning-based memory processes, such as levels of processing, on memory.

Possibly, deep processing produces more accurate automatic memory trace formations in

older cohorts as a result of more efficient neural activation (Hinojosa, Martin-Loeches,

Munoz, Casado, & Pozo, 2004; Karni et al., 1995) whereas increased exertion of

resources help younger cohorts with situations that require the generation of novel ideas,

such as college or graduate school (Cabeza, Anderson, Houle, Mangels, & Nyberg,

1997).

         Older adults who experience age-related memory declines due to the natural

degradation of interdependent cognitive systems could benefit from research findings on

how linguistic, semantic, and presentation tools might enhance memory (Luczsz &

Bryan, 1999). Researchers have relied on two principal theories to identify the sources of

aging phenomenon: Slowed central nervous system processing speed (Salthouse, 1980;

Salthouse, 1982; Salthouse, 1985; Salthouse, 1996) and diminished use of working

memory executive processes (Dempster, 1992; Parkin & Walter, 1992; Parkin, 1997;

Woodruff-Pak, 1997). By identifying the possible sources of age-related cognitive

declines, one can apply the findings from the present study to develop memory

preservation techniques for the aging mind, thus attempting to prevent the onset of the

debilitating disease of dementia.
23


       Although the term “dementia” encompasses a variety of cognitive deficits, the

patient with dementia undergoes a series of phases before reaching the stage of clinical

diagnosis. Mild cognitive impairment diagnoses are given to individuals who experience

more serious cognitive decline than is expected for their age group or education level;

however, this class of impairment does not affect daily functioning (Gauthier et al.,

2006). The diagnoses of mild cognitive impairments and early onset of dementia-related

disorders tend to overlap (Santacruz & Swagerty, 2001). Preventative medicine could

provide memory-enhancing strategies to individuals with a genetic history of dementia.

The dual cognitive effects of meaningful, persistent stimuli could help reduce the risk of

the onset of dementia in that population.

                               Strengths and Limitations

       The study’s design strengths bolstered its internal validity. For example, the recall

test was administered immediately after the study condition to reduce the time gap

between study and test. This measure increased the accuracy of word recall because

visual persistence of stimuli declines as the time gap increases between study and test

(Coltheart, 1980). Furthermore, by including two thematic word categories, I ensured that

the test measurement protocol was truly assessing the effect of relatedness on memory:

Using two thematic categories instead of one eliminated the possibility that recall could

be affected by category type instead of the intended independent variable, relatedness.

       Equal variances among the two thematic word categories also protected the

internal validity of the study by ensuring that one category did not contain words of

higher relatedness than the other category. In other words, the equal variances among the
24


words within each thematic category buffered the study against possible biases of two

videos showing words from the same category with differing degrees of relatedness.

       To accomplish the task of using two different categories, I used the partial

counterbalancing measure to present the four relatedness conditions in every possible

order, thus eliminating the possible confounding effect of a previous phase influencing

memory for words presented in a subsequent phase. However, the Latin-Square partial

counterbalancing measure did not eliminate all bias because the supraliminal condition

was always the first phase of each video. While a complete counterbalancing measure

would have removed this bias, it would have required that half of the videos present the

subliminal condition as the first phase, thus creating confusion for half of the participants

who saw the first set of words outside of their conscious awareness. In sum, I used a

partial counterbalancing measure to minimize this potential anxiety during the test phase.

       Additionally, my decision to study individuals over 55 years of age was based on

aging literature revealing that 55 heralds a 16 year cognitive decline affecting list and text

recall in older adults. This cutoff is one of few widely accepted norms for investigating

age related decline in memory (Powell & Whitla, 1994). Using this age bracket to

conduct a longitudinal study, Zelinski and Burnight (1997) investigated normative

changes in memory and compared performance on intelligence tests of younger and older

adult cohorts. In their study, all age cohorts over 55 showed reliable decreases in

cognitive ability. Their findings confirm that normative age-related change in cognition

typically starts to appear at age 55, thus providing a solid age baseline for my study of

cognitive aging.
25


       Furthermore, if I were to replicate this experiment, I would use the same time

limit methodology I used in the current study and not set a time limit for the testing

condition because older adults have slower processing speed than younger adults (Bryan

& Luszcz, 1996; Bryan & Luszcz, 2001). The rate of cognitive processes, such as

memory performance, varies widely from one participant to another and setting a time

limit would favor those with faster rates of cognitive processes. Thus, a time limit would

create a disadvantage for those with slower processing speeds.

       In contrast to these strengths, various methodological limitations surfaced during

the development of my study’s experimental design. The small sample size of forty-eight

participants increased the risk of Type II error, which is the tendency to accept the null

hypothesis when it is false. My null hypothesis was that there was no difference in recall

between conditions. The small sample size may have underestimated the number of

significant memory score differences between the levels of the two independent

variables: relatedness and presentation. Although the heightened risk of committing a

Type II error was potentially problematic for my study, my hypotheses were still

supported by the results.

       A small sample size also hindered my ability to draw generalizeable conclusions

about the entire senior population. Because I collected data from a convenience sample in

the community, the personalities and motivations of those who decided to volunteer for

my study could have differed from those who did not volunteer for my study. This

disparity could have biased my results to reflect positive attitudes toward academia or

community service. Furthermore, taking a convenience sample from a select group of
26


senior communities may not have adequately captured the true population mean with a

high degree of confidence. If I were to conduct this study again, I would allot more time

to participant recruitment in hopes of increasing my sample size to at least 100

participants.

       My decision to draw a convenience sample instead of a random sample limited

the representativeness of my subject pool. Because I did not collect a random sample, the

economic, educational, and health characteristics unique to the senior communities

sampled may have biased the results in favor of individuals with high levels of self-

esteem and active lifestyles. Because I used a convenience sampling method for my data

collection, my distribution of mean scores may have deviated from the normal

distribution of population test scores. To adequately capture the complete constellation of

characteristics within my target senior population I should have used a random sample

because it would have fairly represented each characteristic without bias, effects of

outliers, and confounding covariates.

       In addition, my method for recording false alarms exposed my study to

experimenter bias because it included a degree of subjectivity. I decided that an answer

was a false alarm if it shared structural similarity to the words in the video phases. The

subjectivity I used to determine whether the word was a false alarm may have positively

skewed the distribution of false alarm scores in my study. Furthermore, testing after each

phase would have been a better approach than testing after the video ended because

exposure to words from previous phases could have rendered ambiguous false alarm

assignments.
27


       The sample also consisted of a disproportionate number of females compared to

males. Research shows that females in general have higher rates of depression than males

(Kessler et al., 1994; Regier et al., 1988; Robins et al., 1984). Royall, Palmer, Chiodo,

and Polk (2011) show that depression attenuates executive motor functions. Bo, Borza

and Seidler (2009) show that diminishing executive processes are related to cognitive

declines during aging. Therefore, depression could hasten the onset of dementia. Because

depression, a disease more prevalent in women than in men, is likely to produce

deleterious effects on aging memory, the disproportionate number of women in my

sample may have created negatively biased memory performance results.

       Additionally, some participants experienced mild to moderate degrees of

frustration while watching the video, which escalated when they realized that a memory

test would immediately follow the video. My overreliance on the proctoring script and

the participants' uneasiness about taking a memory exam may have increased the

ambiguity in some of their responses and reduced the accuracy of scoring their answers.

Their anxiety may have hindered their ability to remember the exact structure of certain

words, producing a somewhat higher number of false alarms in supraliminal conditions.

       Finally, to obtain a more accurate picture of the baseline cognitive functioning

status of the participant pool, I would use the Telephone Interview Questionnaire for

Cognitive Status, a cognitive assessment tool developed by Brandt, Spencer, and Folstein

(1988). An assessment of cognitive status could help me to screen individuals and

remove those who have stages of mild cognitive impairment or physical disabilities such

as blindness that would prevent them from participating in my study. Although my study
28


did not include individuals with these types of impairments, this screening tool would

ensure that I capture only individuals with high cognitive functioning.

                                    Future Directions

        Assisting cognitive aging through modern technology and educational media

creates a dynamic transmission of knowledge throughout younger and older generations.

To further analyze the relationship between relatedness and presentation, one might want

to incorporate elements from human-computer interface psychology literature to produce

websites that will facilitate the engagement of older adults. Websites designed with age-

relevant meaning-based contextual cues can assist older adults’ recall by activating

associative frameworks within their cognitive substrates. The results of such a study may

have important applications in the design of educational curricula in senior centers. For

example, teachers could use computers to present seniors with supraliminally-timed

reading exercises that consist of related material sparking consolidation of short-term

memories and association formations within cognitive networks.

       My study of cognitive aging offers many benefits from a holistic health

perspective paving the way for further analysis of the interaction of many socio-

demographic variables that affect memory. Future studies analyzing the relationship

between age, cognitive functioning, risk for dementia, and test performance would create

a portrait of the dynamic relationship between these four variables and their significant

implications for gerontological health science research. These studies would chronicle the

progression of dementia and age-related cognitive impairments. Advances from this line
29


of research would not only impact science at an epidemiological level but also at an ionic

and cellular level.

       The results from my study may assist those older adults with a genetic history of

Alzheimer’s disease because knowing how to facilitate recall could provide older adults

with a buffer against memory loss. My research findings that processing and timing of

presentation affect explicit memory in senior populations could mobilize future research

endeavors to study consciousness and its correlates with memory. Creative uses of deep

processing and semantic memory enhancing strategies could apply not only to senior

citizens but to other populations who would benefit from effortful retrieval and controlled

allocation of cognitive resources such as dyslexic or autistic children who might use

semantic networks for reading comprehension. Comparing healthy cognitive aging to

anomalous cognitive aging in later studies would impact neuropathological research

along the lines of preventative gerontological medicine and gerontological education.

                                       Conclusion

       Gerontology research throughout the past decade has chronicled the debilitating

effects of the gradual breakdown of cognitive systems in older adult populations. Despite

cognitive decline’s deleterious impact on memory capacity, compensatory memory

systems such as semantic associations and processing time can enhance memory

consolidation within complex neural networks. The activation of these networks unveils

the profound effect of meaning and consciousness on human experience and our reliance

on memory for survival.
30


                                                         Appendix

                           Final List of Forty Words Used in the Four Video Conditions

                           Video 1                                                     Video 2
Them-Zoo    Non-them (A)   Non-them (B)    Them-Veg       Non-them (B)   Them-Zoo            Veg       Non-them (A)
 Phase I      Phase II        Phase III     Phase IV         Phase I      Phase II        Phase III      Phase IV
  Sup.          Sub.            Sup.          Sub.            Sup.          Sub.             Sup.          Sub.
elephant     grandchild       unfasten     cauliflower      bandanna       rhino            onion           test
 hippo       torchlight      bandanna       broccoli        exhilarate      tiger         cucumber       torchlight
 leopard     compactor       incongruity     lettuce        unfasten      panther          pepper       thumbprint
  lion          test         rottweiler      potato         rottweiler     chimp            carrot        notion
orangutan    thumbprint         skirt         pea             drive        panda            celery      bankruptcy
crocodile      notion          buddy        spinach           skirt       penguin            corn       middlebrow
 gorilla    middlebrow       motorcycle      peanut        motorcycle     kangaroo        asparagus     grandchild
 giraffe       groin         exhilarate     avocado           baron        snake            garlic         groin
  bear       technician         drive       pumpkin          buddy         zebra            cactus      technician
alligator    bankruptcy        baron         squash        incongruity     gazelle           beet       compactor
                           Video 3                                                     Video 4



Them-Veg Non-Them (A)      Non-Them (B)    Them-Zoo       Non-Them (B)   Them-Veg         Them-Zoo     Non-Them (A)
 Phase I      Phase II        Phase III     Phase IV         Phase I      Phase II        Phase III      Phase IV
  Sup.          Sub.            Sup.          Sub.            Sup.          Sub.             Sup.          Sub.
  corn         groin           buddy         chimp           buddy       cauliflower      elephant        notion
 pepper        notion        exhilarate     penguin        incongruity    broccoli         leopard      middlebrow
 onion          test         motorcycle     kangaroo        bandanna      spinach          giraffe      technician
asparagus    technician       unfasten       snake            skirt        potato            lion       compactor
  beet       bankruptcy      bandanna       panther         exhilarate     lettuce        crocodile      torchlight
 celery      thumbprint         skirt         tiger         unfasten      avocado         orangutan     bankruptcy
cucumber     torchlight      rottweiler      panda         motorcycle     pumpkin            bear           test
 carrot      grandchild         drive        rhino            drive        peanut          alligator       groin
 garlic     middlebrow         baron         zebra          rottweiler     squash          gorilla      thumbprint
 cactus      compactor       incongruity     gazelle          baron         pea             hippo       grandchild
31


                                       References

Aggleton, J. P., & Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the

       hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 425-444.

       doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002034

Anderson, J. (1980). Concepts, propositions, and schemata: What are the cognitive units?

       Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 28, 121-162.

Anooshian, L. J. (1997). Distinctions between implicit and explicit memory: Significance

       for understanding cognitive development. International Journal of Behavioral

       Development, 21(3), 453-478. doi: 10.1080/016502597384749

Bo, J., Borza V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Sequence learning memory correlate with

       deficits in explicit motor age-related declines in visuospatial working memory.

       Journal of Physiology, 102, 2744-2754. doi: 10.1152/jn.00393.2009

Bradshaw, G. L., & Anderson, J. R. (1982). Elaborative encoding as an explanation of

       levels of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 165-

       174. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90531-X

Brandt, J., Spencer, M., & Folstein, M. (1988). The Telephone Interview for Cognitive

       Status. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 1(2), 111-

       117.

Bryan, J. & Luszcz, M. A. (1996). Speed of information processing as a mediator

       between age and free-recall performance. Psychology and Aging, 11(1), 3-9.

Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2001). Adult age differences in self-oriented pointing tasks

       performance: Contributions from working memory, executive function and speed
32


       of processing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(5),

       608-619. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.11.1.3

Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Houle, S., Mangels, J. A., & Nyberg, L. (1997). Age-

       related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: A

       positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 391–400. doi:

       10.1162/089892900561832

Carroll, M., Byrne, B., & Kirsner, K. (1985). Autobiographical memory and perceptual

       learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and

       perceptual identification. Memory & Cognition, 13, 273-279. doi:

       10.3758/BF03197690

Challis, B. H. (1996). Implicit memory research in 1996: Introductory remarks. Canadian

       Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 1-4. doi: 10.1037/h0084961

Coltheart, M. (1980). Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception and

       Psychophysics, 27(3), 183-228. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.159

Craik, F. I. M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, N. D. K. (1996). The

       effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human

       memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 159-180. doi:

       10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.159

Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory

       research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684. doi:

       10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X
33


Deese, J. (1960). Frequency of usage and number of words in free recall: The role of

       association. Psychological Report, 7, 337-344. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1960.7.2.337

Dementia definition. (n.d.). In MDGuidelines. Retrieved from

       http://www.mdguidelines.com/dementia/definition

Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified

       theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45–75.

       doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90003-K

Ellis, H. D., Ellis, D. M., & Hosey, J. A. (1993). Priming effects in children’s face

       recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 84, 101-110. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-

       8295.1993.tb02465.x

Engelkamp, J., & Wippich, W. (1995). Current issues in implicit and explicit memory.

       Psychological Research, 57, 143-155. doi: 10.1007/BF00431276

Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Recognition failures and free-recall failures: Implications for the

       relation between recall and recognition. Memory and Cognition, 16, 446-451. doi:

       10.3758/BF03214225

Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1990). Recollective experience in word and nonword

       recognition. Memory and Cognition, 18, 23-30. doi: 10.1037/0033-

       295X.103.3.403

Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1991). Forgetting in recognition memory with and without

       recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 19, 617-623. doi: 10.1111/1467-

       8721.ep10769858
34


Gardiner, J. M., Java, R. I., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1996). How level of processing

       really influences awareness in recognition memory. Canadian Journal of

       Experimental Psychology, 50(1), 114-122. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.50.1.114

Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Ritchie, K., Broich, K., . . .

       Winblad, B. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet, 367, 1262-1270. doi:

       10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68542-5

Golomb, J. D., Peele, J. E., Addis, K. M., Kahana, M. J., & Wingfield, A. (2008). Effects

       of adult aging on utilization of temporal and semantic associations during free and

       serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 36(5), 947-956. doi: 10.2307/3203339

Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in

       normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

       Memory & Cognition, 11, 501-518. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501

Greenbaum, J. L., & Graf, P. (1989). Preschool period development of implicit and

       explicit remembering. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 417-420.

Gregg, V., Montgomery, D., & Castano, D. (1980). Recall of common and uncommon

       words from pure and mixed lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal

       Behavior, 19, 240-245. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90202-9

Hall, J. F. (1954). Learning as a function of word frequency. American Journal of

       Psychology, 67, 138-140. doi: 10.2307/1418080

Hinojosa, J. A., Martin-Loeches, M., Munoz, F., Casado, P., & Pozo, M. A. (2004).

       Electrophysiological evidence of automatic early semantic processing. Brain and

       Language, 88, 39-46. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00158-5
35


Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from

       intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 513-541.

       doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F

Joyce, C. A., Paller, K. A., McIsaac, H. K., & Kutas, M. (1998). Memory changes with

       normal aging: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Psychophysiology,

       35, 669-678. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3560669

Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G.

       (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult-motor cortex plasticity during motor-

       skill learning. Nature, 377 (6545), 155–158.

Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., . . ,

       Kendler, K. S. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM- III-R

       psychiatric disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8-

       19.

Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of unconscious ‘automatic’ processes:

       A gating framework. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 289–306. doi:

       10.2478/v10053-008-0031-2

Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237(4821), 1445-1452. doi:

       10.1126/science.3629249

Kihlstrom, J. F. (1990). The psychological unconscious. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook

       of personality: Theory and research (pp. 445-464). New York, NY: Guilford.
36


Kihilstrom, J. F., Bernhardt, T. M., & Tataryn, D. J. (1992). The psychological

       unconscious: Found, lost, and regained. American Psychologist, 47(6), 788-791.

       doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.6.788

Kihlstrom, J. F., Shames, V. A., & Dorfman, J. (1996). Intimations of memory and

       thought. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit Memory and Metacognition (pp. 1-23).

       Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Light, L. L, & Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults.

       Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13(4),

       531-541. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.4.531

Lorsbach, T. C., & Morris, A. K. (1991). Direct and indirect testing of picture memory in

       second and sixth grade children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 16, 18-

       27. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(91)90003-4

Lorsbach, T. C., & Worman, L. J. (1990). Episodic priming in children with learning

       disabilities. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15, 93-102. doi:

       10.1016/0361-476X(90)90009-P

Luszcz, M. A., & Bryan, J. (1999). Toward understanding age-related memory loss in

       late adulthood. Gerontology, 45, 2-9. doi: 10.1159/000022048

Matthews, W. A. (1966). Continued word associations and free recall. Quarterly Journal

       of Experimental Psychology, 17, 31-38. doi: 10.1080/14640746608400004

May, R. B., Cuddy, L. J., & Norton, J. M. (1979). Temporal contrast and the word

       frequency

       effect. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 141-147. doi: 10.1037/h0081712
37


May, R. B., & Tryk, H. E. (1970). Word sequence, word frequency and free recall.

       Canadian Journal of Psychology, 24, 299-304. doi: 10.1080/016502597384749

Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our

       capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. doi:

       10.1037/h0043158

Mitchell, D. B., & Bruss, P. J. (2003). Age differences in implicit memory: Conceptual,

       perceptual, or methodological? Psychology and Aging, 18, 807-822. doi:

       10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.807

Monti, L. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Reminger, S. L., Rinaldi, J. A., Wilson, R. S., &

       Fleischman, D. A. (1996). Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease on

       conceptual implicit and explicit memory. Neuropsychology, 10(1), 101-112. doi:

       10.1037/0894-4105.10.1.101

Moore, T. E. (1982). Subliminal advertising: What you see is what you get. Journal of

       Marketing, 48, 38-47. doi: 10.2307/3203339

Naito, M. (1990). Repetition priming in children and adults: Age related dissociation

       between implicit and explicit memory. Journal of Experimental Child

       Psychology, 50, 462-484. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90081-I

Parkin, A. J. (1997). Normal age-related memory loss and its relation to frontal lobe

       dysfunction In P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive function.

       East Sussex, France: Taylor & Francis.

Parkin, A. J., & Streete, S. (1988). Implicit memory in young children and adults. British

       Journal of Psychology, 79, 361-369. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02295.x
38


Parkin, A. J., & Walter, B. M. (1992). Recollective experience, normal aging, and frontal

       dysfunction. Psychology of Aging, 2, 290–298. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.2.290

Pickett, J. E. (Ed.). (1985). American heritage dictionary of the English language. Boston,

       MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Postman, L. (1970). Effects of word frequency on acquisition and retention under

       conditions of free-recall learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,

       22, 185-195. doi: 10.1080/00335557043000113

Powell, D. H., & Whitla, D. K. (1994). Normal cognitive aging: Toward empirical

       perspectives. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(1), 27-31. doi:

       10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769858

Rajaram, S. (1993). Remembering and knowing: Two means of access to the personal

       past. Memory & Cognition, 21, 89-102. doi: 10.3758/BF03211168

Regier, D. A., Boyd, J. H., Burke, J. H., Jr., Rae, D. S., Myers, J. K., Kramer, M., . . .

       Locke, B. Z. (1988). One month prevalence of mental disorders in the United

       States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 977-986.

Ritchey, M., Bessette-Symons, B., Hayes, S. M., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Emotion

       processing in the brain is modulated by semantic elaboration. Neuropsychologia,

       49, 640-650. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.009

Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Weissman, M. M., Orvaschel, H., Gruenberg, E., Burke, J.

       D., Jr., & Regier, D. A. (1984). Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric

       disorders in three sites. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 949-956.
39


Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human

       subjects. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol 8,

       pp. 63-131). Amsterdam, Denmark: Elsevier.

Royall, D. R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., & Polk, M. J. (2011). Depressive symptoms

       predict longitudinal change in executive control but not memory. International

       Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 89-96. doi: 10.1002/gps.2697

Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hillsdale, KS:

       Erlbaum.

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in

       cognition. Psychological Review, 103(2), 403-428. doi: 10.1037/0033-

       295X.103.3.403

Salthouse, T. A. (1980). Age and memory: Strategies for localizing the loss. In L. W.

       Poon, J. L. Fozard, L. Cermak, D. Arenberg, & L. W. Thompson (Eds.), (pp 47–

       65). New directions in memory and aging. Hillsdale, KS: Erlbaum.

Salthouse, T. A. (1982). Adult cognition: An experimental psychology of human aging.

       NewYork, NY: Springer.

Salthouse, T. A. (1985). A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam, Denmark.

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). General and specific speed mediation of adult age differences in

       memory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 51, 30–42. doi:

       10.1093/geronb/51B.1.P30

Santacruz, K. S., & Swagerty, D. (2001). Early diagnosis of dementia. American Family

       Physician, 63(4), 703-713.
40


Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of

       Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 501-518.

       doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501

Snirvas, K., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1990). Classifying implicit memory tests: Category

       association and anagram solution. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 389-

       412. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90063-6

Sumby, W. H. (1963). Word frequency and serial position effects. Journal of Verbal

       Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 443-450. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(63)80030-4

Szymanski, K. F., & MacLeod, C. M. (1996). Manipulation of attention at study affects

       an explicit but not an implicit test of memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 5,

       165-175. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1996.0010

Toth, J. P. (1996). Conceptual automaticity in recognition memory: Levels of processing

       effects on familiarity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50(1), 123-

       128. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.50.1.123

Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40(4),

       385-395. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.40.4.385

Tulving, E., & Patkau, J. E. (1962). Concurrent effect of contextual constraint and word

       frequency on immediate recall and learning of verbal material. Canadian Journal

       of Psychology, 16, 83-95. doi: 10.1037/h0083231

Wagner, A. D., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Verfaellie, M. (1997). Dissociations between

       familiarity processes in explicit recognition and implicit perceptual memory.
41


       Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23(2),

       305-323. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.23.2.305

Whitehouse, P. J., Price, D. L., Strubie, R. G., Clark, A. W., Coyle, J. T., & Delon, M. R.

       (1982). Alzheimer's disease and senile dementia: Loss of neurons in the basal

       forebrain. Science, 5(4537), 1237-1239. doi: 10.1126/science.7058341

Whiting, W. L., & Smith, A. D. (1997). The differential age related processing

       limitations in recall and recognition tasks. Psychology and Aging, 12(2), 216-224.

       doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.2.216

Woodruff-Pak, D. D. (1997). The neuropsychology of aging. Oxford, England:

       Blackwell.

Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years

       of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441-517. doi:

       10.1006/jmla.2002.2864

Zelinski, E. M., & Burnight, K. P. (1997). Sixteen year longitudinal and time lag changes

       in memory and cognition in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 12(3), 503-543.

       doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.3.503

More Related Content

Viewers also liked

Quality of Consumer Experience in the Future Internet
Quality of Consumer Experience in the Future InternetQuality of Consumer Experience in the Future Internet
Quality of Consumer Experience in the Future InternetVali Lalioti
 
Victor Alavardo Geometry Project
Victor Alavardo Geometry ProjectVictor Alavardo Geometry Project
Victor Alavardo Geometry ProjectVictorAlvarado
 
El costo de la anticipación
El costo de la anticipaciónEl costo de la anticipación
El costo de la anticipaciónUNAH CUROC
 
What is art?
What is art?What is art?
What is art?mertxita
 
Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...
Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...
Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...Pavel Melnikov
 
Social Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working Space
Social Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working SpaceSocial Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working Space
Social Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working SpaceWorking Space
 
Part 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countries
Part 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countriesPart 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countries
Part 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countriesPavel Melnikov
 
õPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItel
õPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItelõPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItel
õPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItelMaret Mõis
 
Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012
Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012
Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012Pavel Melnikov
 
Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014
Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014
Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014Pavel Melnikov
 
Why and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentations
Why and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentationsWhy and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentations
Why and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentationsFormation 3.0
 
Sec16 greedy algorithm no2
Sec16 greedy algorithm no2Sec16 greedy algorithm no2
Sec16 greedy algorithm no2Keisuke OTAKI
 
51st Winter Nationals Inauguration
51st Winter Nationals Inauguration51st Winter Nationals Inauguration
51st Winter Nationals Inaugurationguestdcd72
 
How to Make a Halloween Mobile
How to Make a Halloween MobileHow to Make a Halloween Mobile
How to Make a Halloween Mobilemertxita
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Argegna
ArgegnaArgegna
Argegna
 
Quality of Consumer Experience in the Future Internet
Quality of Consumer Experience in the Future InternetQuality of Consumer Experience in the Future Internet
Quality of Consumer Experience in the Future Internet
 
Victor Alavardo Geometry Project
Victor Alavardo Geometry ProjectVictor Alavardo Geometry Project
Victor Alavardo Geometry Project
 
DondeEsta
DondeEstaDondeEsta
DondeEsta
 
El costo de la anticipación
El costo de la anticipaciónEl costo de la anticipación
El costo de la anticipación
 
What is art?
What is art?What is art?
What is art?
 
Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...
Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...
Cаммари исследования Grayling восприятия последствий вступления в ВТО бизнесо...
 
Social Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working Space
Social Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working SpaceSocial Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working Space
Social Media Revolution_Luis Ongil_Working Space
 
Shoba
ShobaShoba
Shoba
 
Part 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countries
Part 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countriesPart 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countries
Part 3 - Perfect overview on economic sanctions - US, EU, other countries
 
Social Media Basics
Social Media BasicsSocial Media Basics
Social Media Basics
 
Dance Project English
Dance Project EnglishDance Project English
Dance Project English
 
õPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItel
õPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItelõPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItel
õPpeinfosüSteemi üHildamine E õPpe Keskkondadega üHe üLikooli õI Si NäItel
 
Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012
Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012
Grayling new rus government think piece may 2012
 
Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014
Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014
Natalia Zubarevich - Russian regions - September 2014
 
Why and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentations
Why and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentationsWhy and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentations
Why and how to use graphs and infographics in your presentations
 
Sec16 greedy algorithm no2
Sec16 greedy algorithm no2Sec16 greedy algorithm no2
Sec16 greedy algorithm no2
 
51st Winter Nationals Inauguration
51st Winter Nationals Inauguration51st Winter Nationals Inauguration
51st Winter Nationals Inauguration
 
How to Make a Halloween Mobile
How to Make a Halloween MobileHow to Make a Halloween Mobile
How to Make a Halloween Mobile
 
Orca
OrcaOrca
Orca
 

Similar to Felicia oropeza thesis_final_draft_ready_to_go_qin

Essay On Tourism
Essay On TourismEssay On Tourism
Essay On TourismWanda Buck
 
1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium
1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium
1st Ed - OADI Research CompendiumTremayne Waller
 
Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...
Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...
Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...William Kritsonis
 
An Essay About My School
An Essay About My SchoolAn Essay About My School
An Essay About My SchoolVickie Miller
 
LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2
LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2
LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2Luciano Renteria
 
After High School A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...
After High School  A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...After High School  A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...
After High School A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...Sara Alvarez
 
Essays On Sports.pdf
Essays On Sports.pdfEssays On Sports.pdf
Essays On Sports.pdfJill Johnson
 
Collaborative – Critical Thinking Section
Collaborative – Critical Thinking SectionCollaborative – Critical Thinking Section
Collaborative – Critical Thinking SectionMs. Beka
 
Journal Entry 2
Journal Entry 2Journal Entry 2
Journal Entry 2Ms. Beka
 

Similar to Felicia oropeza thesis_final_draft_ready_to_go_qin (20)

FIRE AND ICE
FIRE AND ICEFIRE AND ICE
FIRE AND ICE
 
Nofi paper on sa
 Nofi paper on sa Nofi paper on sa
Nofi paper on sa
 
scsantuc_capstone (1)
scsantuc_capstone (1)scsantuc_capstone (1)
scsantuc_capstone (1)
 
Essay On Tourism
Essay On TourismEssay On Tourism
Essay On Tourism
 
Hssttx1
Hssttx1Hssttx1
Hssttx1
 
self-efficacy, and self-esteem
self-efficacy, and self-esteemself-efficacy, and self-esteem
self-efficacy, and self-esteem
 
Essay Term
Essay TermEssay Term
Essay Term
 
1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium
1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium
1st Ed - OADI Research Compendium
 
Future of coaching Kim Stephenson
Future of coaching Kim StephensonFuture of coaching Kim Stephenson
Future of coaching Kim Stephenson
 
Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...
Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...
Nasrin Nazemzadeh, DissertationTitle page, Abstract, and Table of Contents, D...
 
Metacognition_2000
Metacognition_2000Metacognition_2000
Metacognition_2000
 
An Essay About My School
An Essay About My SchoolAn Essay About My School
An Essay About My School
 
LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2
LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2
LENGUA ADICIONAL AL ESPAÑOL 2
 
After High School A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...
After High School  A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...After High School  A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...
After High School A First Look At The Postschool Experiences Of Youth With D...
 
Essays On Sports.pdf
Essays On Sports.pdfEssays On Sports.pdf
Essays On Sports.pdf
 
Collaborative – Critical Thinking Section
Collaborative – Critical Thinking SectionCollaborative – Critical Thinking Section
Collaborative – Critical Thinking Section
 
Graulty_Thesis
Graulty_ThesisGraulty_Thesis
Graulty_Thesis
 
Changed My Life Essay.pdf
Changed My Life Essay.pdfChanged My Life Essay.pdf
Changed My Life Essay.pdf
 
Journal Entry 2
Journal Entry 2Journal Entry 2
Journal Entry 2
 
Nursing Thesis Proposal Format
Nursing Thesis Proposal FormatNursing Thesis Proposal Format
Nursing Thesis Proposal Format
 

Felicia oropeza thesis_final_draft_ready_to_go_qin

  • 1. THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION AND PROCESSING ON EXPLICIT MEMORY IN OLDER ADULTS A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology by Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza SPRING 2012
  • 2. THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION AND PROCESSING ON EXPLICIT MEMORY IN OLDER ADULTS A Thesis by Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Kelly Cotter, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader John Schaeuble, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Emily Wickelgren, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date ii
  • 3. Student: Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator ___________________ Jianjian Qin, Ph. D. Date Department of Psychology iii
  • 4. Abstract of THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION AND PROCESSING ON EXPLICIT MEMORY IN OLDER ADULTS by Felicia Elena Luz Oropeza Forty eight older adults, sampled from two senior communities, watched a series of words on a computer monitor and then took a written exam that tested their explicit word recall. Results revealed a main effect for presentation, such that participants correctly recalled more words when those words were presented for 2 seconds than when those words were presented for .04 seconds. Results also revealed an interaction between presentation and processing, where the supraliminal presentation yielded more correctly recalled words in thematic conditions than in non-thematic conditions. These results suggest that older adults exhibit higher explicit memory performance when words belonging to a theme or category are presented within conscious awareness than outside awareness, likely because they are able to process the information more deeply. _______________________, Committee Chair Kelly Cotter, Ph.D. _______________________ Date iv
  • 5. DEDICATION I would like to dedicate this thesis to my deceased maternal grandparents, Maria de la Luz Razo and Salvador Gomez who raised me and served as the inspiration for my topic on cognition and aging. v
  • 6. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, I would like to thank my parents for everything they have done for me. Since preschool, my parents have impressed upon me the value of education and importance of using one’s intellect to make a significant impact on the world; however small or big that may be. They gave me the greatest gift that any parent could bestow on their child, individuation, by giving me the freedom to explore my academic pursuits. My thesis topic availed me this golden opportunity to spread my wings and fly. Special thanks to my mother’s extended family for their undying support of my educational endeavors and contributions to my personal development throughout graduate school. During graduate school, my grandmother passed away which was a major setback for our family, for she was the anchor that held our family together. Amidst those trying times, my mom’s family taught me to rise above hardships and to find my inner strength within to finish my degree. I would like to thank Dr. Kelly Cotter for her patience, immense wisdom, alacrity, and generosity of time and effort towards helping me finish my thesis. I would also like to thank Dr. John Schaeuble for helping me arrive at my thesis topic and for assisting me with narrowing my thesis topic down to two variables. I would like to thank Dr. Wickelgren for helping me with the Latin Square Experimental Design of my video presentations to eliminate bias. I would like to thank Debbie Kircher for pacing me through the program and helping me see my own beauty within. vi
  • 7. TABLE OF CONTENTS Dedication ............................................................................................................................v Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................ vi List of Tables ..................................................................................................................... ix List of Figures ......................................................................................................................x Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................1 Memory ....................................................................................................................1 Supraliminal and Subliminal Memory Processes ....................................................3 Levels of Processing ................................................................................................4 Interaction of Levels of Processing and Timing of Presentation .............................5 The Present Study ....................................................................................................6 2. METHOD .....................................................................................................................8 Materials ................................................................................................................10 Participants .............................................................................................................13 Procedure ...............................................................................................................13 3. RESULTS ...................................................................................................................16 Hits .........................................................................................................................17 False Alarms ..........................................................................................................19 4. DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................20 Strengths and Limitations ......................................................................................23 vii
  • 8. Future Directions ...................................................................................................28 Conclusion .............................................................................................................29 Appendix Final List of Forty Words Used in the Four Video Conditions ...................30 References ........................................................................................................................31 viii
  • 9. LIST OF TABLES Tables Page 1. Descriptive Statistics for Hits and False Alarms ....................................................... 16 ix
  • 10. LIST OF FIGURES Figures Page 1. The Interaction of Presentation and Relatedness for Hits ..............................................18 2. The Main Effect of Presentation for False Alarms ........................................................19 x
  • 11. 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Dementia is a degenerative disease of the aging brain that impairs cognitive functioning by attacking the central nervous system at the vascular and cellular levels (Whitehouse, Price, Strubie, Clark, Coyle, & Delon, 1982). A reputable occupational health and medical disability advisory website, MDGuidelines, provides the article “Dementia definition” (2009) stating that although dementia impairs cognitive functioning in many ways, profound and irreversible memory loss is its primary effect. Because dementia is an increasingly prevalent age-related neural disease within senior populations, developing cognitive strategies for preventing memory loss is necessary for maintenance of quality of life and positive life orientations (Santacruz & Swagerty, 2001). I focus on two potential memory-enhancing strategies in the present study: presentation of information and processing of information. I examine these strategies as employed by older adults who do not suffer from cognitive impairment, in hopes that what is learned in normal aging populations can be beneficial for those at risk of developing dementia. Memory Memory, as defined in The American Heritage Dictionary (1985), is the act of encoding, storing, and recalling a past event or item of factual knowledge. Memory can be divided into two forms: implicit and explicit (Graf & Schacter, 1985), both of which are affected by the aging process (Light & Singh, 1987). Implicit memory depends on
  • 12. 2 cognitive processes occurring outside of one’s conscious awareness (Schacter, 1987). These automatic processes can include stimuli-associate conditioning, social modeling, relearning and practice effects, and priming (Schacter, 1987). Roediger and McDermott (1993) and Schacter (1987) described implicit memory tasks as indirect, where there was no reference to past exposure to information. In contrast, explicit memory tasks require the conscious recollection of an item directly referenced by past experience (Graf & Schacter, 1985). When one performs well on an explicit memory test, he or she has brought factual knowledge into awareness (Challis, 1996; Joyce, Paller, McIsaac, & Kutas, 1998). Roediger and McDermott (1993) and Schacter (1987) characterized explicit memory tests as tasks performed with intention, where participants are instructed to recall or recognize stimuli they had previously seen. I chose to analyze free recall test performance as a measure of explicit memory processes in the current study. Performance on a free recall exam is a direct measurement of the conscious recollection of previously presented stimuli. This type of memory task relies on processing capacity, revealing the influence of working memory and processing speed on retrieval (Craik, Govoni, Naveh-Benjamin, & Anderson, 1996; Whiting & Smith, 1997). Free recall tests typically depend on effortful cognitive control and bottom up retrieval processes. In cross-sectional and longitudinal studies examining age-related memory, older participants tend to score lower than younger participants on explicit memory tasks (Anooshian, 1997; Carroll, Byrne, & Kirsner, 1985; Ellis, Ellis, & Hosey, 1993;
  • 13. 3 Greenbaum & Graf, 1989; Lorsbach & Morris, 1991; Lorsbach & Worman, 1990; Naito, 1990; Parkin & Streete, 1988). However, older cohorts show equal performance to young adults on perceptual implicit tasks (Light & Singh, 1987) shedding light on potential compensatory mechanisms used by older adults to preserve explicit memory. To investigate the older cohorts’ adaptive use of cognitive tools further, I analyzed the influence of two associative memory processes on explicit memory test performance in older adults: level of processing (shallow versus deep) and awareness of presentation (subliminal versus supraliminal), described in detail below. Supraliminal and Subliminal Memory Processes As described above, explicit memory tests often involve asking participants to recall or recognize information presented to them in a laboratory setting. The information can be presented supraliminally (within one's conscious awareness) or subliminally (outside one's ability to consciously perceive presented stimuli) in the study/encoding session. Supraliminal memory processes are driven by strong feelings of event recollection that enhance one’s ability to visualize the scene (Aggleton & Brown, 1999; Gardiner, 1988; Gardiner & Java, 1990; Gardiner & Java, 1991; Gardiner, Java & Richardson-Klavehn, 1996; Jacoby, 1991; Rajaram, 1993; Tulving, 1985; Wagner, Verfaeille, & Gabrieli, 1997; Yonelinas, 2002). Subliminal processes depend upon feelings of familiarity that lack a concrete experience of an actual event (Khilstrom, 1990; Khilstrom, Barnhardt, & Tataryn, 1992a; Khilstrom, Shames, & Dorfman, 1996; Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Szymanski & MacLeod, 1996; Toth, 1996). The priming
  • 14. 4 task is a classic methodology for studying subliminal processes (Kiefer, 2007). Age has little effect on priming (Light & Singh, 1987). Levels of Processing Ritchey, Bessette-Symons, Hayes, and Cabeza (2011) showed that semantic elaboration can enhance neural processing in the medial prefrontal cortices of the human brain. Deep encoding requires the utilization of meaning-based encoding to activate associative memory network connections, while shallow processing refers to the presentation of stimuli encoded at a rudimentary or physical level without the use of semantic-level memory access (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). For example, deep processing is applied during exposure to a word list consisting of items belonging to the same category, such as Zoo Animals, because people form associations between the words. On the other hand, shallow processing is utilized during exposure to randomly chosen words because there is no categorical link between the words. Craik and Lockhart (1972) showed that deep or semantically-processed stimuli had a higher chance of being recalled than shallow or physically-processed stimuli. This effect was found even when participants did not expect subsequent recall or recognition tests. Bradshaw and Anderson (1982) also showed the effect of elaboration and thematic-relatedness on memory processes: Participants in their study demonstrated higher memory performance in the thematic conditions, which required the elaborate integration of memory traces, and lower memory performance in the conditions with minimal thematic-relatedness. Similarly, levels of processing theory stipulates that performance is facilitated by elaborately encoded or “deeply” processed stimuli and
  • 15. 5 degraded by low-level encoding or “shallow” processing of stimuli (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). Hence, Bradshaw and Anderson’s (1982) study draws parallels between thematic- relatedness and levels of processing theory, shedding light on the role of neural networks in the creation of interconnected explicit memory traces. In the current study, I adapted the methodology that used thematic-relatedness in order to operationally define levels of processing. Interaction of Levels of Processing and Timing of Presentation Effortful processing may enhance memory performance in older cohorts on priming tests. Englekamp and Wippich (1995) demonstrate an age effect for conceptual priming, such that younger cohorts generally show higher memory on implicit tests than older cohorts when presented with atypical stimuli at study. Englekamp and Wippich showed that younger cohorts tend to perform better than older cohorts in random stimuli and subliminal presentation conditions while performing worse in moderate-relatedness conditions. The distinction between the cognitive resources that drive explicit and implicit memory processes begin to blur (Srinivas & Roediger, 1990) as category exemplar tasks capture both conceptual subliminal processes and perceptual subliminal processes. While unraveling the effects of conscious and unconscious processes on memory, Monti et al. (1996) investigated how category exemplar tasks rely on relatedness to create meaning- based or deeply processed memory traces. Although category exemplar tasks are classically referred to as “implicit” memory tasks, their conceptual properties are sensitive to different levels of processing at the supraliminal level (Monti et al., 1996).
  • 16. 6 Hence, category exemplar tests may have more conceptually-driven effects on memory if coupled with deep levels of processing conditions. Therefore, among the memory tasks available to researchers, the category exemplar test is one of the most effective priming tools for studying the interaction of levels of processing and exposure to subliminal and supraliminal stimuli (Mitchell & Bruss, 2003). Thus, in the present study I applied the category exemplar test by presenting target exemplars from taxonomic groups of animals and vegetables to the participants and by asking participants to recall those targets later. Despite the potential interaction between processing and presentation, there remains a paucity of research that examines the moderated effects of semantically processed stimuli on category exemplar task performance in older adults. Thus, in contrast to recent studies examining the relationship between relatedness, elaboration, and explicit memory (Bradshaw & Anderson, 1982), I analyzed the effect of presentation timing and thematic-relatedness on the explicit memory of older adults in the current study. The Present Study In the present study, I examined how levels of processing (deep versus shallow) affects explicit memory task performance when moderated by presentation timing (subliminal versus supraliminal). I operationally defined deep processing as the presentation of thematically-related words from two thematic categories (Zoo Animals and Vegetables). I defined shallow processing as the presentation of randomly generated, non-thematically-related words. I defined supraliminal presentation as seeing a series of
  • 17. 7 words for 2 seconds each. Subliminal presentation was defined as seeing a series of words for .04 seconds each. I expected to find main effects for both presentation and processing of stimuli. For the main effect of presentation, I expected to see a higher number of supraliminally- presented words recalled than subliminally-presented words. For the main effect of relatedness, I proposed that memory for thematically-related words would be higher than memory for non-thematically-related words. Specifically, I expected that thematically- related words would provide a semantic (deep) form of processing that would facilitate memory performance across both supraliminal and subliminal presentation conditions. I also hypothesized that participants would have higher memory performance in supraliminal-deep levels of processing conditions and lower performance in supraliminal- shallow, subliminal-deep, and subliminal-shallow conditions. I expected to see this interaction because meaningful, related words shown for a longer amount of time on a computer screen tend to produce more durable memory traces than random words or words presented for a shorter amount of time. The focusing of one’s mind on semantic content should yield a compound effect on explicit memory performance because supraliminal processes require the effortful application of cognitive resources that spark neural network activation and parallel processing stream distribution.
  • 18. 8 Chapter 2 METHOD With a 2x2x4 mixed repeated-measures factorial design, described in detail below, I examined the nature of semantic facilitation on explicit memory for words presented at the conscious and the unconscious perceptual level. I showed each participant a series of forty words flashed on a computer screen and later asked participants to recall the words. The two within-subjects independent variables were thematic association of words and presentation of stimuli. The between-groups independent variable was order of presentation. Explicit memory recall test performance was my dependent variable. The first within-subjects independent variable was thematic association/level of processing. Thematic association referred to how easily participants could form associations with presented words (thus affecting their memory for those words), and was comprised of two levels: a thematically-related word condition (deep processing) and a non-thematic word condition (shallow processing). The thematically-related word condition was a list of ten words related to the same category, either Zoo Animals (e.g., zebra, kangaroo) or Vegetables (e.g., broccoli, potato). The non-thematic word condition was a list of ten randomly generated words that had no relation to each other (e.g., skirt, motorcycle). The second within-subjects independent variable, presentation of stimuli, referred to how long participants saw words flashed on the computer screen. This variable had
  • 19. 9 two levels: the supraliminal condition and the subliminal condition. In the supraliminal condition, participants were exposed to words for 2 seconds each. This presentation time was long enough for participants to read and understand the word, thus processing it completely (Moore, 1982; Whiting, 1997). Words were presented for .04 seconds each in the subliminal condition. This presentation time was long enough for participants to detect a word, but not long enough to consciously process the word (Khilstrom, 1987; Moore, 1982). Video presentation, consisting of four levels, was the only between-subjects variable used in this study. It was used in order to control for potential order effects. I used a partial Latin-Square to counterbalance the sequence of phases within each video presentation. This method of counterbalancing ensured that the thematic and non- thematic words were presented in every possible order (first, second, third, fourth). For example, order one consisted of supraliminal thematic words presented in phase 1, subliminal non-thematic words presented in phase 2, supraliminal non-thematic words presented in phase 3, and subliminal thematic words presented in phase 4. In order two, by contrast, supraliminal non-thematic words appeared in phase 1, subliminal thematic words appeared in phase 2, supraliminal thematic words appeared in phase 3, and subliminal non-thematic words appeared in phase 4. (Each video started with a supraliminal phase in order to prevent the participant from thinking that there were no words presented in the video.) Thus, the presentation conditions were counterbalanced across the four different videos. A total of forty words were presented in each video.
  • 20. 10 Materials Word Generation To create the vegetable word list I randomly selected 41 Vegetable words from the following website: http://www.gardenology.org/wiki/List_of_vegetables. To create the zoo animal word list I randomly selected 26 Zoo Animal words from the following website: http://www.catalandictionary.org/wordnets/eng/ZooAnimalList.htm. To create the non-thematic word list I randomly generated 24 words from the following website: http://coyotecult.com/tools/randomwordgenerator.php. Next, I trimmed each list to 10 words following the procedures described below. Inter-rater Reliability The relatedness of a word to a category could affect the speed and accuracy by which a participant could retrieve the word from memory, thus assisting in the formation of cognitive associations (Anderson, 1980). Therefore, I wanted to make sure that one word list did not contain words that were more related to their respective theme than the other words to that list’s respective theme. To check the equivalence of thematic- relatedness of the words in the two thematic word lists, I collected inter-rater reliability data on the degree of relatedness of each word to its specific category. Ten friends and family members below the age of 55 participated as raters. They were asked to rate how much each word from the vegetable list was related to the theme of vegetables, and how much each word from the zoo animals list was related to the theme of zoo animals. Relatedness ratings were collected only for thematic words.
  • 21. 11 I also collected inter-rater reliability data on the commonness of each word in everyday usage because the commonness of a word could also increase the accuracy of recall (Deese, 1960; Gregg, Montgomery, & Castano, 1980; Hall, 1954; Matthews, 1966; May, Cuddy, & Norton, 1979; May & Tryk, 1970; Postman, 1970; Sumby, 1963; Tulving & Patkau, 1962; Whiting & Smith, 1997). The commonness inter-rater questionnaire asked raters to rate how commonly words are used in everyday speech. Commonness ratings were collected for thematic and non-thematic words. Upon completion of the word rating procedure, I rewarded the raters with $5.00 Target gift cards. Final Word List Creation To create lists of equivalent length (10 words) for each processing condition (thematically-related and non-thematically-related), I entered the participants’ ratings into a spreadsheet. Next, I eliminated some words from the original lists by sorting the word lists (Zoo Animals, Vegetables, non-thematic) from the highest to lowest mean commonness rating. Then I chose every other two words descending from the highest to lowest mean commonness rating until I obtained 20 words for each thematic list (Zoo Animals, Vegetables) and 20 words for the non-thematic word lists. The final list of 40 words was divided into four lists of ten words each using the ABBA sorting sequence. Refer to Appendix for the list of words used in the video conditions. After the final word lists were created, I calculated the variability between the two thematic word lists on relatedness and the variability between the two thematic and the two non-thematic lists on commonness. T-tests revealed no significant differences between Zoo Animal words (M = 4.09, SD = .54) and Vegetable words (M = 3.71, SD =
  • 22. 12 .76) on relatedness to their respective thematic category; t (38) = 1.73, p = .09. In other words, Zoo Animal words and Vegetable words had the same degree of relatedness to their respective theme. T-tests also showed no significant differences between thematic (M = 3.49, SD = .63) and non-thematic words (M = 3.34, SD = 1.03) on commonness; t (38) = .70, p = .48. Thus, both thematic and non-thematic words were considered equivalent on commonness. Upon completion of the word list generation procedure I had four lists of 10 words. These lists were capped at 10 words each because the average older adult has a working memory capacity of 4 (+/- 2) items (Bo, Borza & Seidler, 2009). I chose to increase the size of the word list from four words to ten words because I wanted to avoid any potential ceiling effects (Miller, 1956). Finally, to randomly sort the words within the four lists for the video presentation conditions, I used the following website: http://textmechanic.com/Sort-Text-Lines.html, which generated random orders for each 10-word list. Computer Words were presented to participants on an ASUS gaming laptop computer running Windows Live Movie Maker for Windows 7 Home Premium operating system. Demographic Questionnaire Before administering the computerized portion of the procedure I collected the following demographic information: Gender, Highest Education Achieved, Occupation, Ethnicity, and Religion. Age information was not collected because my entire participant
  • 23. 13 pool consisted of older adults and I did not want the sensitivity of the age question to deter potential subjects from participating in my study. Participants I sent letters to fourteen different agencies within the Sacramento Valley region and called ten agencies. I recruited forty-eight older adults over the age of 55 from the following agencies: Senior Center at Elk Grove, Mission Oaks/Swanston Community Center in Carmichael, and Ethel Hart Senior Center. Of the twelve participants who answered the question on “Gender,” 3 participants were “Male” and 9 participants were “Female.” Four out of 48 participants “Completed High School,” 11 “Went to college but did not finish,” 12 participants “Finished some graduate school or higher,” and 2 “Earned a doctorate degree.” (The questionnaire did not include an option for those who completed college but did not attend graduate school.) Almost half of the participants were “Caucasian” (n = 23), 4 were “Hispanic,” 1 participant was of “Asian American” descent, 2 were “African American,” and 18 did not indicate their ethnicity. Out of 45 participants who responded to the Religion question, 12 were “Catholic,” 13 were “Christian,” 5 were “Spiritual,” 1 was “Mormon,” 1 was “Episcopalian,” 3 participants were “Atheist,” 3 were “New Age,” 2 were “Baptist,” and 5 participants reported “Other” to signify that they were of a religious affiliation other than what was asked on the questionnaire. Three participants out of 48 did not respond to the religion question. Procedure I received approval at minimal risk to recruit participants from the Human Subjects Committee in the Department of Psychology in Spring 2011. Next, I pilot-tested
  • 24. 14 the procedure with four individuals who did not meet the study’s criteria for participation to make sure the testing protocols ran smoothly. There were no problems with the procedure. To prepare for the experiment, I made arrangements with the activity coordinators from each agency listed above to secure a room and a specific time to run my study. Before the day of the experiment, I randomly assigned the participants to one of four video conditions. On test day, I greeted each participant individually, showed the participant to his or her seat in front of the laptop, and led the participant through the informed consent procedure. Next, I handed the participant a consent form to sign and return back to me. I then provided the participant with a demographic questionnaire that asked the participant to reveal his or her gender, education status, occupation, religion, and ethnicity. Before starting the video presentation, I instructed the participant to not touch any key on the keyboard or touchpad because the screen ran by itself. Next, I started the video assigned to the participant. Within each video, the supraliminal condition had one thematic phase and one non-thematic phase. The subliminal condition also had one thematic phase and one non-thematic phase. The order was counterbalanced, as described above. The video presentation lasted for 3.99 minutes. In the supraliminal condition, a blank screen appeared first for 1 second. The orienting stimulus, "_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ ", flashed for .5 seconds before each word appeared on the screen. The masking stimulus, "XXXXXXXXXX", followed each word and remained on the screen for .5 seconds. A
  • 25. 15 blank screen appeared for 1 second before the orienting stimulus for the next word. In the subliminal condition the blank screen was shown for 2 seconds before the appearance of the next word. The orienting stimulus, "_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _", flashed for .5 seconds before each word and the masking stimulus, "XXXXXXXXXX", appeared on the screen for a duration of .5 seconds after the flashing of each word. A blank screen appeared for 2 seconds before the orienting stimulus for the next word. The difference in timing of the presentation of blank screens was potentially problematic but unequal durations for the four videos would have compromised the internal validity of my repeated measures design. To ensure that each video condition lasted for an equivalent amount of time, I prioritized consistency of video presentation length across my video conditions over equal time spacing between orienting stimuli, test words, and masking stimuli. After the video was complete, I administered the free recall exam by giving the participant a pen and piece of lined 8.5 x11 white printer paper with the following instructions at the top of the page: “Please write as many words as you can remember from the presentation you just saw. You have as much time as you want to complete this form.” Next, I told the participant that he or she could take as much time as needed to complete the test. After I read the instructions to the participant, I quietly left the room. After the exam was complete, I debriefed the participant and gave the participant a $5 gift card to Walmart.
  • 26. 16 Chapter 3 RESULTS Using two mixed repeated measures ANOVAs, I examined differences between the thematic and presentation conditions on word recall (see Table 1). Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for Hits and False Alarms (N = 48) Hits False Alarms Source M SD F(1, 44) p M SD F(1, 44) p Relatedness 100.37 < .001 .19 .67 Thematic 2.13 0.13 0.10 0.04 Non-thematic 0.57 0.11 0.13 0.05 Presentation 220.90 < .001 10.05 .003 Supraliminal 2.63 0.17 0.22 0.06 Subliminal 0.07 0.05 0.01 0.01 Relatedness x Presentation 109.13 < .001 0.00 1.00 Thematic Supraliminal 4.10 0.23 0.21 0.07 Thematic Subliminal 0.15 0.09 0.00 0.00 Nonthematic Supraliminal 1.15 0.21 0.23 0.09 Nonthematic Subliminal 0.00 0.00 0.02 0.02 The first dependent variable, the number of “hits,” represented the number of words the participant wrote down on the recall task that were presented in the video presentation (i.e., correctly-recalled words). The second dependent variable, number of
  • 27. 17 false alarms, was the number of words that the participant wrote down on the recall task that were not presented in the video presentation. I associated each false alarm with the condition that contained a structurally similar word. For example, if one condition contained the word ”torchlight” and the participant wrote the word “headlight,” I placed the false alarm score in the condition that contained “torchlight.” Hits The between subjects variable, presentation order, did not show a significant main effect for hits, F (3, 44) = 2.18, p = .10, indicating that order of presentation had no effect on correct recall for presented words. Additionally, there were no significant two-way or three-way interactions with presentation order for hits. The first within-subjects variable, timing of presentation, showed a significant main effect for hits, F (1, 44) = 220.90, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .46, such that supraliminal presentation conditions yielded a higher number of hits than subliminal presentation conditions, as predicted (see Table 1). However, the presence of an interaction modified the nature of this main effect. The second independent variable, thematic-relatedness, also showed a significant main effect for hits, F (1, 44) = 100.37, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .17, such that thematic conditions produced significantly more hits than non-thematic conditions. However, the interaction showed that this effect did not exist for the subliminal condition. Both main effects had to be viewed in the context of a significant interaction, F (1, 44) = 109.13, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .14. Pairwise comparisons were used to test for simple effects of the thematic conditions. For supraliminal presentation conditions there was a significant mean difference between thematic and non-thematic conditions
  • 28. 18 such that the number of hits in thematic conditions was significantly higher than the number of hits in non-thematic conditions, Mean difference = 2.96, p < .001. However, there was no difference between the two thematic conditions in the subliminal condition, Mean difference = .15, p =.11. 6.00 5.00 4.00 4.10 Performance Presentation 3.00 Supraliminal 2.00 Subliminal 1.00 1.15 0.15 0.00 0.00 Thematic Non Thematic Relatedness Figure 1. The interaction of presentation and relatedness for hits.
  • 29. 19 False Alarms The between subjects variable, presentation order, did not show a significant main effect for false alarms, F (3, 44) = .60, p = .62, revealing no effect of video order on the number of written responses that were not on the word list but resembled words on the list. Moreover, there were no significant two-way or three way interactions with presentation order for false alarms. Presentation showed a significant main effect for false alarms, F (1, 44) = 10.05, p < .001, semi-partial η2 = .10, such that there were more false alarms produced in supraliminal presentation conditions than in subliminal presentation conditions (see Table 1). However, thematic-relatedness did not show a main effect for false alarms, F (1, 44) = .19, p = .67. Similarly, there was no interaction of presentation and thematic-relatedness, F (1, 44) = .00, p = 1.00, for false alarms. See Figure 2 for the main effect of presentation time on false alarms. 0.30 Performance 0.25 0.23 0.20 Presentation 0.21 0.15 Supraliminal 0.10 Subliminal 0.05 0.02 0 0.00 Thematic Non Thematic Relatedness Figure 2. The main effect of presentation for false alarms.
  • 30. 20 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION The results of the present study confirmed my hypothesis that older adults would exhibit higher explicit memory performance when words were presented to conscious awareness than when words were not presented within conscious awareness, but this difference was much larger for words belonging to a theme or category than for words not related to each other in any obvious way. Furthermore, the results supported my prediction that words belonging to a theme or category would be more easily remembered than words that were not related to each other in any obvious way, but this was true only when words were presented within conscious awareness. As I mentioned above, performance on the recall test was higher in supraliminal conditions than in subliminal conditions. This result is consistent with research on the facilitation of visual recall through the engagement of conscious processes (Aggleton & Brown, 1999; Gardiner, 1988; Gardiner & Java, 1990; Gardiner & Java, 1991; Gardiner, Java & Richardson-Klavehn, 1996; Jacoby, 1991; Rajaram, 1993; Tulving, 1985; Wagner, Verfaeille, & Gabrieli, 1997; Yonelinas, 2002). The deep processing phenomenon is evidenced in my study, which shows higher explicit memory performance on the free recall exam in thematic conditions than in non- thematic conditions. This finding resembled the results of Craik and Lockhart (1972), showing the positive effect of deep processing on explicit memory.
  • 31. 21 Past research indicates that older adults are more likely to use semantic associations than younger adults when performing serial recall tasks (Golomb, Peele, Addis, Kahana, & Wingfield, 2008). Moreover, Englekamp and Wippich (1995) found that, for older adults, word-relatedness enhances memory of subliminally presented words. Conversely, younger adults remember more words that are not related to each other in priming tasks than do older adults (Englekamp & Wippich, 1995). Younger adults may not use categories to facilitate memory performance during the completion of implicit memory tasks such as priming. In contrast, older adults’ dependence on semantic content for memory consolidation amplifies the effects of consciously perceived stimuli on explicit memory test performance. By depicting the relationship between presentation and levels of processing, my study demonstrated the importance of supraliminal effortful processes on semantic facilitation of explicit memory. This claim is further supported by evidence from Monti et al. (1996), who showed how meaning-based encoding processes through the engagement of supraliminal memory processes enhance conceptually-driven aspects of memory tasks. Additionally, Bradshaw and Anderson (1982) made advances in conceptual priming and processing research by revealing the compound effect of supraliminally-presented stimuli and thematic relatedness on category exemplar test performance. Conclusively, the results from my study and previous research exemplified the pivotal role that semantic associations play on conscious processes during explicit memory tasks.
  • 32. 22 Notably, older adults falsely-remembered more words in the video presentation of supraliminal stimuli than of subliminal stimuli. In other words, there were more false alarms in the supraliminal than in the subliminal conditions. Supraliminality and the application of cognitive effort seemed to have a stronger effect on incorrect guessing than the effect of meaning-based memory processes, such as levels of processing, on memory. Possibly, deep processing produces more accurate automatic memory trace formations in older cohorts as a result of more efficient neural activation (Hinojosa, Martin-Loeches, Munoz, Casado, & Pozo, 2004; Karni et al., 1995) whereas increased exertion of resources help younger cohorts with situations that require the generation of novel ideas, such as college or graduate school (Cabeza, Anderson, Houle, Mangels, & Nyberg, 1997). Older adults who experience age-related memory declines due to the natural degradation of interdependent cognitive systems could benefit from research findings on how linguistic, semantic, and presentation tools might enhance memory (Luczsz & Bryan, 1999). Researchers have relied on two principal theories to identify the sources of aging phenomenon: Slowed central nervous system processing speed (Salthouse, 1980; Salthouse, 1982; Salthouse, 1985; Salthouse, 1996) and diminished use of working memory executive processes (Dempster, 1992; Parkin & Walter, 1992; Parkin, 1997; Woodruff-Pak, 1997). By identifying the possible sources of age-related cognitive declines, one can apply the findings from the present study to develop memory preservation techniques for the aging mind, thus attempting to prevent the onset of the debilitating disease of dementia.
  • 33. 23 Although the term “dementia” encompasses a variety of cognitive deficits, the patient with dementia undergoes a series of phases before reaching the stage of clinical diagnosis. Mild cognitive impairment diagnoses are given to individuals who experience more serious cognitive decline than is expected for their age group or education level; however, this class of impairment does not affect daily functioning (Gauthier et al., 2006). The diagnoses of mild cognitive impairments and early onset of dementia-related disorders tend to overlap (Santacruz & Swagerty, 2001). Preventative medicine could provide memory-enhancing strategies to individuals with a genetic history of dementia. The dual cognitive effects of meaningful, persistent stimuli could help reduce the risk of the onset of dementia in that population. Strengths and Limitations The study’s design strengths bolstered its internal validity. For example, the recall test was administered immediately after the study condition to reduce the time gap between study and test. This measure increased the accuracy of word recall because visual persistence of stimuli declines as the time gap increases between study and test (Coltheart, 1980). Furthermore, by including two thematic word categories, I ensured that the test measurement protocol was truly assessing the effect of relatedness on memory: Using two thematic categories instead of one eliminated the possibility that recall could be affected by category type instead of the intended independent variable, relatedness. Equal variances among the two thematic word categories also protected the internal validity of the study by ensuring that one category did not contain words of higher relatedness than the other category. In other words, the equal variances among the
  • 34. 24 words within each thematic category buffered the study against possible biases of two videos showing words from the same category with differing degrees of relatedness. To accomplish the task of using two different categories, I used the partial counterbalancing measure to present the four relatedness conditions in every possible order, thus eliminating the possible confounding effect of a previous phase influencing memory for words presented in a subsequent phase. However, the Latin-Square partial counterbalancing measure did not eliminate all bias because the supraliminal condition was always the first phase of each video. While a complete counterbalancing measure would have removed this bias, it would have required that half of the videos present the subliminal condition as the first phase, thus creating confusion for half of the participants who saw the first set of words outside of their conscious awareness. In sum, I used a partial counterbalancing measure to minimize this potential anxiety during the test phase. Additionally, my decision to study individuals over 55 years of age was based on aging literature revealing that 55 heralds a 16 year cognitive decline affecting list and text recall in older adults. This cutoff is one of few widely accepted norms for investigating age related decline in memory (Powell & Whitla, 1994). Using this age bracket to conduct a longitudinal study, Zelinski and Burnight (1997) investigated normative changes in memory and compared performance on intelligence tests of younger and older adult cohorts. In their study, all age cohorts over 55 showed reliable decreases in cognitive ability. Their findings confirm that normative age-related change in cognition typically starts to appear at age 55, thus providing a solid age baseline for my study of cognitive aging.
  • 35. 25 Furthermore, if I were to replicate this experiment, I would use the same time limit methodology I used in the current study and not set a time limit for the testing condition because older adults have slower processing speed than younger adults (Bryan & Luszcz, 1996; Bryan & Luszcz, 2001). The rate of cognitive processes, such as memory performance, varies widely from one participant to another and setting a time limit would favor those with faster rates of cognitive processes. Thus, a time limit would create a disadvantage for those with slower processing speeds. In contrast to these strengths, various methodological limitations surfaced during the development of my study’s experimental design. The small sample size of forty-eight participants increased the risk of Type II error, which is the tendency to accept the null hypothesis when it is false. My null hypothesis was that there was no difference in recall between conditions. The small sample size may have underestimated the number of significant memory score differences between the levels of the two independent variables: relatedness and presentation. Although the heightened risk of committing a Type II error was potentially problematic for my study, my hypotheses were still supported by the results. A small sample size also hindered my ability to draw generalizeable conclusions about the entire senior population. Because I collected data from a convenience sample in the community, the personalities and motivations of those who decided to volunteer for my study could have differed from those who did not volunteer for my study. This disparity could have biased my results to reflect positive attitudes toward academia or community service. Furthermore, taking a convenience sample from a select group of
  • 36. 26 senior communities may not have adequately captured the true population mean with a high degree of confidence. If I were to conduct this study again, I would allot more time to participant recruitment in hopes of increasing my sample size to at least 100 participants. My decision to draw a convenience sample instead of a random sample limited the representativeness of my subject pool. Because I did not collect a random sample, the economic, educational, and health characteristics unique to the senior communities sampled may have biased the results in favor of individuals with high levels of self- esteem and active lifestyles. Because I used a convenience sampling method for my data collection, my distribution of mean scores may have deviated from the normal distribution of population test scores. To adequately capture the complete constellation of characteristics within my target senior population I should have used a random sample because it would have fairly represented each characteristic without bias, effects of outliers, and confounding covariates. In addition, my method for recording false alarms exposed my study to experimenter bias because it included a degree of subjectivity. I decided that an answer was a false alarm if it shared structural similarity to the words in the video phases. The subjectivity I used to determine whether the word was a false alarm may have positively skewed the distribution of false alarm scores in my study. Furthermore, testing after each phase would have been a better approach than testing after the video ended because exposure to words from previous phases could have rendered ambiguous false alarm assignments.
  • 37. 27 The sample also consisted of a disproportionate number of females compared to males. Research shows that females in general have higher rates of depression than males (Kessler et al., 1994; Regier et al., 1988; Robins et al., 1984). Royall, Palmer, Chiodo, and Polk (2011) show that depression attenuates executive motor functions. Bo, Borza and Seidler (2009) show that diminishing executive processes are related to cognitive declines during aging. Therefore, depression could hasten the onset of dementia. Because depression, a disease more prevalent in women than in men, is likely to produce deleterious effects on aging memory, the disproportionate number of women in my sample may have created negatively biased memory performance results. Additionally, some participants experienced mild to moderate degrees of frustration while watching the video, which escalated when they realized that a memory test would immediately follow the video. My overreliance on the proctoring script and the participants' uneasiness about taking a memory exam may have increased the ambiguity in some of their responses and reduced the accuracy of scoring their answers. Their anxiety may have hindered their ability to remember the exact structure of certain words, producing a somewhat higher number of false alarms in supraliminal conditions. Finally, to obtain a more accurate picture of the baseline cognitive functioning status of the participant pool, I would use the Telephone Interview Questionnaire for Cognitive Status, a cognitive assessment tool developed by Brandt, Spencer, and Folstein (1988). An assessment of cognitive status could help me to screen individuals and remove those who have stages of mild cognitive impairment or physical disabilities such as blindness that would prevent them from participating in my study. Although my study
  • 38. 28 did not include individuals with these types of impairments, this screening tool would ensure that I capture only individuals with high cognitive functioning. Future Directions Assisting cognitive aging through modern technology and educational media creates a dynamic transmission of knowledge throughout younger and older generations. To further analyze the relationship between relatedness and presentation, one might want to incorporate elements from human-computer interface psychology literature to produce websites that will facilitate the engagement of older adults. Websites designed with age- relevant meaning-based contextual cues can assist older adults’ recall by activating associative frameworks within their cognitive substrates. The results of such a study may have important applications in the design of educational curricula in senior centers. For example, teachers could use computers to present seniors with supraliminally-timed reading exercises that consist of related material sparking consolidation of short-term memories and association formations within cognitive networks. My study of cognitive aging offers many benefits from a holistic health perspective paving the way for further analysis of the interaction of many socio- demographic variables that affect memory. Future studies analyzing the relationship between age, cognitive functioning, risk for dementia, and test performance would create a portrait of the dynamic relationship between these four variables and their significant implications for gerontological health science research. These studies would chronicle the progression of dementia and age-related cognitive impairments. Advances from this line
  • 39. 29 of research would not only impact science at an epidemiological level but also at an ionic and cellular level. The results from my study may assist those older adults with a genetic history of Alzheimer’s disease because knowing how to facilitate recall could provide older adults with a buffer against memory loss. My research findings that processing and timing of presentation affect explicit memory in senior populations could mobilize future research endeavors to study consciousness and its correlates with memory. Creative uses of deep processing and semantic memory enhancing strategies could apply not only to senior citizens but to other populations who would benefit from effortful retrieval and controlled allocation of cognitive resources such as dyslexic or autistic children who might use semantic networks for reading comprehension. Comparing healthy cognitive aging to anomalous cognitive aging in later studies would impact neuropathological research along the lines of preventative gerontological medicine and gerontological education. Conclusion Gerontology research throughout the past decade has chronicled the debilitating effects of the gradual breakdown of cognitive systems in older adult populations. Despite cognitive decline’s deleterious impact on memory capacity, compensatory memory systems such as semantic associations and processing time can enhance memory consolidation within complex neural networks. The activation of these networks unveils the profound effect of meaning and consciousness on human experience and our reliance on memory for survival.
  • 40. 30 Appendix Final List of Forty Words Used in the Four Video Conditions Video 1 Video 2 Them-Zoo Non-them (A) Non-them (B) Them-Veg Non-them (B) Them-Zoo Veg Non-them (A) Phase I Phase II Phase III Phase IV Phase I Phase II Phase III Phase IV Sup. Sub. Sup. Sub. Sup. Sub. Sup. Sub. elephant grandchild unfasten cauliflower bandanna rhino onion test hippo torchlight bandanna broccoli exhilarate tiger cucumber torchlight leopard compactor incongruity lettuce unfasten panther pepper thumbprint lion test rottweiler potato rottweiler chimp carrot notion orangutan thumbprint skirt pea drive panda celery bankruptcy crocodile notion buddy spinach skirt penguin corn middlebrow gorilla middlebrow motorcycle peanut motorcycle kangaroo asparagus grandchild giraffe groin exhilarate avocado baron snake garlic groin bear technician drive pumpkin buddy zebra cactus technician alligator bankruptcy baron squash incongruity gazelle beet compactor Video 3 Video 4 Them-Veg Non-Them (A) Non-Them (B) Them-Zoo Non-Them (B) Them-Veg Them-Zoo Non-Them (A) Phase I Phase II Phase III Phase IV Phase I Phase II Phase III Phase IV Sup. Sub. Sup. Sub. Sup. Sub. Sup. Sub. corn groin buddy chimp buddy cauliflower elephant notion pepper notion exhilarate penguin incongruity broccoli leopard middlebrow onion test motorcycle kangaroo bandanna spinach giraffe technician asparagus technician unfasten snake skirt potato lion compactor beet bankruptcy bandanna panther exhilarate lettuce crocodile torchlight celery thumbprint skirt tiger unfasten avocado orangutan bankruptcy cucumber torchlight rottweiler panda motorcycle pumpkin bear test carrot grandchild drive rhino drive peanut alligator groin garlic middlebrow baron zebra rottweiler squash gorilla thumbprint cactus compactor incongruity gazelle baron pea hippo grandchild
  • 41. 31 References Aggleton, J. P., & Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 425-444. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002034 Anderson, J. (1980). Concepts, propositions, and schemata: What are the cognitive units? Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 28, 121-162. Anooshian, L. J. (1997). Distinctions between implicit and explicit memory: Significance for understanding cognitive development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21(3), 453-478. doi: 10.1080/016502597384749 Bo, J., Borza V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Sequence learning memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor age-related declines in visuospatial working memory. Journal of Physiology, 102, 2744-2754. doi: 10.1152/jn.00393.2009 Bradshaw, G. L., & Anderson, J. R. (1982). Elaborative encoding as an explanation of levels of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 165- 174. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90531-X Brandt, J., Spencer, M., & Folstein, M. (1988). The Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 1(2), 111- 117. Bryan, J. & Luszcz, M. A. (1996). Speed of information processing as a mediator between age and free-recall performance. Psychology and Aging, 11(1), 3-9. Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2001). Adult age differences in self-oriented pointing tasks performance: Contributions from working memory, executive function and speed
  • 42. 32 of processing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(5), 608-619. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.11.1.3 Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Houle, S., Mangels, J. A., & Nyberg, L. (1997). Age- related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 391–400. doi: 10.1162/089892900561832 Carroll, M., Byrne, B., & Kirsner, K. (1985). Autobiographical memory and perceptual learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and perceptual identification. Memory & Cognition, 13, 273-279. doi: 10.3758/BF03197690 Challis, B. H. (1996). Implicit memory research in 1996: Introductory remarks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 1-4. doi: 10.1037/h0084961 Coltheart, M. (1980). Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception and Psychophysics, 27(3), 183-228. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.159 Craik, F. I. M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, N. D. K. (1996). The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 159-180. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.159 Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X
  • 43. 33 Deese, J. (1960). Frequency of usage and number of words in free recall: The role of association. Psychological Report, 7, 337-344. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1960.7.2.337 Dementia definition. (n.d.). In MDGuidelines. Retrieved from http://www.mdguidelines.com/dementia/definition Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45–75. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90003-K Ellis, H. D., Ellis, D. M., & Hosey, J. A. (1993). Priming effects in children’s face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 84, 101-110. doi: 10.1111/j.2044- 8295.1993.tb02465.x Engelkamp, J., & Wippich, W. (1995). Current issues in implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Research, 57, 143-155. doi: 10.1007/BF00431276 Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Recognition failures and free-recall failures: Implications for the relation between recall and recognition. Memory and Cognition, 16, 446-451. doi: 10.3758/BF03214225 Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1990). Recollective experience in word and nonword recognition. Memory and Cognition, 18, 23-30. doi: 10.1037/0033- 295X.103.3.403 Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1991). Forgetting in recognition memory with and without recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 19, 617-623. doi: 10.1111/1467- 8721.ep10769858
  • 44. 34 Gardiner, J. M., Java, R. I., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1996). How level of processing really influences awareness in recognition memory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50(1), 114-122. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.50.1.114 Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Ritchie, K., Broich, K., . . . Winblad, B. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet, 367, 1262-1270. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68542-5 Golomb, J. D., Peele, J. E., Addis, K. M., Kahana, M. J., & Wingfield, A. (2008). Effects of adult aging on utilization of temporal and semantic associations during free and serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 36(5), 947-956. doi: 10.2307/3203339 Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 11, 501-518. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501 Greenbaum, J. L., & Graf, P. (1989). Preschool period development of implicit and explicit remembering. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 417-420. Gregg, V., Montgomery, D., & Castano, D. (1980). Recall of common and uncommon words from pure and mixed lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 240-245. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90202-9 Hall, J. F. (1954). Learning as a function of word frequency. American Journal of Psychology, 67, 138-140. doi: 10.2307/1418080 Hinojosa, J. A., Martin-Loeches, M., Munoz, F., Casado, P., & Pozo, M. A. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence of automatic early semantic processing. Brain and Language, 88, 39-46. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00158-5
  • 45. 35 Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 513-541. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F Joyce, C. A., Paller, K. A., McIsaac, H. K., & Kutas, M. (1998). Memory changes with normal aging: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Psychophysiology, 35, 669-678. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3560669 Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult-motor cortex plasticity during motor- skill learning. Nature, 377 (6545), 155–158. Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., . . , Kendler, K. S. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM- III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8- 19. Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of unconscious ‘automatic’ processes: A gating framework. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 289–306. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0031-2 Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237(4821), 1445-1452. doi: 10.1126/science.3629249 Kihlstrom, J. F. (1990). The psychological unconscious. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 445-464). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • 46. 36 Kihilstrom, J. F., Bernhardt, T. M., & Tataryn, D. J. (1992). The psychological unconscious: Found, lost, and regained. American Psychologist, 47(6), 788-791. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.6.788 Kihlstrom, J. F., Shames, V. A., & Dorfman, J. (1996). Intimations of memory and thought. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit Memory and Metacognition (pp. 1-23). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Light, L. L, & Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13(4), 531-541. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.4.531 Lorsbach, T. C., & Morris, A. K. (1991). Direct and indirect testing of picture memory in second and sixth grade children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 16, 18- 27. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(91)90003-4 Lorsbach, T. C., & Worman, L. J. (1990). Episodic priming in children with learning disabilities. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15, 93-102. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(90)90009-P Luszcz, M. A., & Bryan, J. (1999). Toward understanding age-related memory loss in late adulthood. Gerontology, 45, 2-9. doi: 10.1159/000022048 Matthews, W. A. (1966). Continued word associations and free recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 31-38. doi: 10.1080/14640746608400004 May, R. B., Cuddy, L. J., & Norton, J. M. (1979). Temporal contrast and the word frequency effect. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 141-147. doi: 10.1037/h0081712
  • 47. 37 May, R. B., & Tryk, H. E. (1970). Word sequence, word frequency and free recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 24, 299-304. doi: 10.1080/016502597384749 Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. doi: 10.1037/h0043158 Mitchell, D. B., & Bruss, P. J. (2003). Age differences in implicit memory: Conceptual, perceptual, or methodological? Psychology and Aging, 18, 807-822. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.807 Monti, L. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Reminger, S. L., Rinaldi, J. A., Wilson, R. S., & Fleischman, D. A. (1996). Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease on conceptual implicit and explicit memory. Neuropsychology, 10(1), 101-112. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.10.1.101 Moore, T. E. (1982). Subliminal advertising: What you see is what you get. Journal of Marketing, 48, 38-47. doi: 10.2307/3203339 Naito, M. (1990). Repetition priming in children and adults: Age related dissociation between implicit and explicit memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 462-484. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90081-I Parkin, A. J. (1997). Normal age-related memory loss and its relation to frontal lobe dysfunction In P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive function. East Sussex, France: Taylor & Francis. Parkin, A. J., & Streete, S. (1988). Implicit memory in young children and adults. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 361-369. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02295.x
  • 48. 38 Parkin, A. J., & Walter, B. M. (1992). Recollective experience, normal aging, and frontal dysfunction. Psychology of Aging, 2, 290–298. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.2.290 Pickett, J. E. (Ed.). (1985). American heritage dictionary of the English language. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Postman, L. (1970). Effects of word frequency on acquisition and retention under conditions of free-recall learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 185-195. doi: 10.1080/00335557043000113 Powell, D. H., & Whitla, D. K. (1994). Normal cognitive aging: Toward empirical perspectives. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(1), 27-31. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769858 Rajaram, S. (1993). Remembering and knowing: Two means of access to the personal past. Memory & Cognition, 21, 89-102. doi: 10.3758/BF03211168 Regier, D. A., Boyd, J. H., Burke, J. H., Jr., Rae, D. S., Myers, J. K., Kramer, M., . . . Locke, B. Z. (1988). One month prevalence of mental disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 977-986. Ritchey, M., Bessette-Symons, B., Hayes, S. M., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Emotion processing in the brain is modulated by semantic elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 49, 640-650. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.009 Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Weissman, M. M., Orvaschel, H., Gruenberg, E., Burke, J. D., Jr., & Regier, D. A. (1984). Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric disorders in three sites. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 949-956.
  • 49. 39 Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol 8, pp. 63-131). Amsterdam, Denmark: Elsevier. Royall, D. R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., & Polk, M. J. (2011). Depressive symptoms predict longitudinal change in executive control but not memory. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 89-96. doi: 10.1002/gps.2697 Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hillsdale, KS: Erlbaum. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(2), 403-428. doi: 10.1037/0033- 295X.103.3.403 Salthouse, T. A. (1980). Age and memory: Strategies for localizing the loss. In L. W. Poon, J. L. Fozard, L. Cermak, D. Arenberg, & L. W. Thompson (Eds.), (pp 47– 65). New directions in memory and aging. Hillsdale, KS: Erlbaum. Salthouse, T. A. (1982). Adult cognition: An experimental psychology of human aging. NewYork, NY: Springer. Salthouse, T. A. (1985). A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam, Denmark. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). General and specific speed mediation of adult age differences in memory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 51, 30–42. doi: 10.1093/geronb/51B.1.P30 Santacruz, K. S., & Swagerty, D. (2001). Early diagnosis of dementia. American Family Physician, 63(4), 703-713.
  • 50. 40 Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 501-518. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501 Snirvas, K., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1990). Classifying implicit memory tests: Category association and anagram solution. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 389- 412. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90063-6 Sumby, W. H. (1963). Word frequency and serial position effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 443-450. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(63)80030-4 Szymanski, K. F., & MacLeod, C. M. (1996). Manipulation of attention at study affects an explicit but not an implicit test of memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 165-175. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1996.0010 Toth, J. P. (1996). Conceptual automaticity in recognition memory: Levels of processing effects on familiarity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50(1), 123- 128. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.50.1.123 Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40(4), 385-395. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.40.4.385 Tulving, E., & Patkau, J. E. (1962). Concurrent effect of contextual constraint and word frequency on immediate recall and learning of verbal material. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 16, 83-95. doi: 10.1037/h0083231 Wagner, A. D., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Verfaellie, M. (1997). Dissociations between familiarity processes in explicit recognition and implicit perceptual memory.
  • 51. 41 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23(2), 305-323. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.23.2.305 Whitehouse, P. J., Price, D. L., Strubie, R. G., Clark, A. W., Coyle, J. T., & Delon, M. R. (1982). Alzheimer's disease and senile dementia: Loss of neurons in the basal forebrain. Science, 5(4537), 1237-1239. doi: 10.1126/science.7058341 Whiting, W. L., & Smith, A. D. (1997). The differential age related processing limitations in recall and recognition tasks. Psychology and Aging, 12(2), 216-224. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.2.216 Woodruff-Pak, D. D. (1997). The neuropsychology of aging. Oxford, England: Blackwell. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441-517. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2002.2864 Zelinski, E. M., & Burnight, K. P. (1997). Sixteen year longitudinal and time lag changes in memory and cognition in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 12(3), 503-543. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.3.503