SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 115
Perception of shapes targeting local and global
processes in autism spectrum disorders
Emma J. Grinter,1 Murray T. Maybery,1 Elizabeth Pellicano,1,3
Johanna C.
Badcock2 and David R. Badcock1
1School of Psychology, University of Western Australia;
2Centre for Clinical Research in Neuropsychiatry/Graylands
Hospital, School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences,
University of Western Australia; 3Department of
Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, UK
Background: Several researchers have found evidence for
impaired global processing in the dorsal
visual stream in individuals with autism spectrum disorders
(ASDs). However, support for a similar
pattern of visual processing in the ventral visual stream is less
consistent. Critical to resolving the
inconsistency is the assessment of local and global form
processing ability. Methods: Within the visual
domain, radial frequency (RF) patterns – shapes formed by
sinusoidally varying the radius of a circle to
add ‘bumps’ of a certain number to a circle – can be used to
examine local and global form perception.
Typically developing children and children with an ASD
discriminated between circles and RF patterns
that are processed either locally (RF24) or globally (RF3).
Results: Children with an ASD required
greater shape deformation to identify RF3 shapes compared to
typically developing children, consistent
with difficulty in global processing in the ventral stream. No
group difference was observed for RF24
shapes, suggesting intact local ventral-stream processing.
Conclusions: These outcomes support the
position that a deficit in global visual processing is present in
ASDs, consistent with the notion of Weak
Central Coherence. Keywords: Autism, local processing, global
processing, ventral visual pathway,
radial frequency patterns. Abbreviations: ASD, autism spectrum
disorder; TD, typically developing;
WCC, Weak Central Coherence; EPF, Enhanced Perceptual
Functioning; RF, radial frequency; ADI-R,
Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised.
Over the past three decades, several research groups
have proposed that the cognitive profile in autism
spectrum disorders (ASDs) is characterised by diffi-
culties in complex information processing (Bertone,
Mottron, Jelenic, & Faubert, 2005; Frith, 1989;
Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegal, 1997). In particular,
Weak Central Coherence (WCC) theory suggests that
individuals with an ASD demonstrate a relative fail-
ure to extract overall meaning, resulting in a reduced
awareness of the global aspects of stimuli in con-
junction with a relatively heightened awareness of
the details or parts of stimuli (Frith, 1989; Happé,
1999). Several studies have shown, however, that
integration abilities might be intact in ASDs (Mot-
tron, Burack, Stauder, & Robaey, 1999; Ozonoff,
Strayer, McMahon, & Filloux, 1994; Plaisted, Swet-
tenham, & Rees, 1999). To account for these data,
others have proposed, amongst several other
hypotheses, that individuals with an ASD show
‘Enhanced Perceptual Functioning’ (EPF; Mottron,
Dawson, Souliéres, Hubert, & Burack, 2006) in
which the salience of local features is enhanced
without corresponding deficits in integrative capa-
bilities. Research assessing visual capabilities is
uniquely positioned to clarify which of these
accounts best explains atypical processing in ASDs
since processes known to engage global integration
can be examined (Bell & Badcock, 2008; Loffler,
2008).
At the earliest stages of visual perception, neurons
in primary visual cortex (V1) extract information
about local characteristics of stimuli to provide a
spatially limited signal for perception (DeValois &
DeValois, 1988). Because the classical receptive
fields are small, however, V1 information must be
integrated to enable global perception at later stages
of both the dorsal (Movshon, 1990) and ventral
(Loffler, 2008) visual streams. There has been con-
siderable interest in visual processing in autism in
recent years, and much research has investigated
local and global processing in the dorsal and ventral
visual pathways (see Kaiser & Shiffrar, in press;
Simmons et al., 2009, for reviews). Several
researchers have found higher thresholds in
children with autism when compared to typically
developing (TD) children on tasks targeting global
dorsal stream processing that require the identifi-
cation of direction of motion or the presence of
coherent motion in a field of moving dots (e.g., Milne
et al., 2002; Pellicano, Gibson, Maybery, Durkin, &
Badcock, 2005; Spencer et al., 2000; Spencer &
O’Brien, 2006; Tsermentseli et al., 2008), in con-
junction with evidence of intact local dorsal stream
processing (e.g., Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic, & Fau-
bert, 2003; Pellicano et al., 2005). While this has
been interpreted as evidence for a disturbance in
higher-level global processing in the dorsal pathway
in ASDs (e.g., Bertone et al., 2003; Pellicano et al.,
2005), the data for a similar pattern of visual
processing in the ventral visual stream is lessConflict of interest
statement: No conflicts declared.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51:6 (2010), pp
717–724 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02203.x
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road,
Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148,
USA
consistent, with some studies reporting equivalent
(e.g., Blake, Turner, Smoski, Pozdol, & Stone, 2003;
Milne et al., 2006; Spencer et al., 2000) and others
impaired thresholds (e.g., Spencer & O’Brien, 2006;
Tsermentseli et al., 2008) on measures of ventral
stream global processing. Importantly, many of
these studies failed to examine both local and global
processing within a specified pathway or use similar
stimulus characteristics to assess the two forms of
processing (but see Bertone et al., 2003, 2005),
making it difficult to draw firm conclusions about
local and global processing in ASD.
The present study assessed both types of visual
functioning in the ventral stream using shapes in
which differences between the local and global
stimuli were minimised. Radial frequency (RF; Wil-
kinson, Wilson, & Habak, 1998) patterns are closed-
contour shapes created by deforming a circle. The
deformation is produced by sinusoidally varying the
radius as a function of polar angle. The number of
cycles of modulation in 360� corresponds to the RF
number and when the amplitude of the modulating
function is set to zero, a circle is produced (Fig-
ure 1a). Three cycles of appropriate amplitude create
a shape that looks like a triangle with rounded cor-
ners (Figure 1b), and 24 cycles result in a circle with
24 ‘bumps’ (Figure 1c). For RF patterns of high fre-
quency (e.g., RF24), performance for discriminating
the whole shape from a circle is better than when
only part of the closed shape is deformed (Loffler,
Wilson, & Wilkinson, 2003), but only by an amount
that can be explained by probability summation of
the detection of independent local features. For this
reason, the discrimination of high RF shapes is
thought to be achieved by the local orientation-tuned
cells in V1 (Wilkinson et al., 2003). In contrast, there
is evidence that curvature and position information
is pooled along the entire circumference of the pat-
tern for low radial frequencies (Bell & Badcock,
2008; Bell, Badcock, Wilson, & Wilkinson, 2007),
consistent with global signal integration in shapes
with up to about ten cycles of modulation (Bell &
Badcock, 2009; Loffler, 2008). FMRI data is consis-
tent with global pooling of orientation information to
extract global shape information further along the
ventral pathway in V4 for RF3 patterns (Wilkinson
et al., 2000).
Here we report the first study to use these stimuli
with an ASD population. We used RF3 and RF24
patterns to assess global and local ventral stream
processing, respectively. Loffler et al.’s (2003)
examination of RF shapes showed that RF3 shapes
evoke active global pooling of local curvature esti-
mates, whereas RF24 shapes involve probability
summation of local curvature estimates. Consistent
with the idea that there is a potential difference in
how RF shapes are processed, Bell, Wilkinson,
Wilson, Loffler, and Badcock (2009) showed that
discrimination of low RF patterns is underpinned by
multiple narrow-band contour shape channels. In
selecting the RF shapes for this study, we were
careful to choose clear examples for which global
processing of local curvature estimates was (RF3) or
was not (RF24) selectively activated. This allowed
determination of separate local and global contri-
butions to shape processing.
The WCC account can be used to predict that,
relative to a neurotypical comparison group, indi-
viduals with an ASD should show elevated thresh-
olds on the RF3 task (i.e., poor global processing
in the ventral stream), accompanied by either
equivalent or lower thresholds on the RF24 task (i.e.,
intact/superior local processing in the ventral
stream). Alternatively, EPF theory can be used to
predict that individuals with an ASD should dem-
onstrate equivalent or lower thresholds on the RF24
task (i.e., enhanced local processing), but, critically,
equivalent thresholds on the RF3 task (i.e., intact
global processing) relative to TD individuals.
Method
Group comparisons
Brock, Jarrold, Farran, Laws, and Riby (2007; see also
Jarrold & Brock, 2004) demonstrated that substantial
problems can be introduced when using conventional
methods to match or statistically control for psycho-
metric variables, such as verbal and non-verbal ability,
on which children with a developmental disorder and
TD children differ systematically. The analytic
approach they advocate is to regress each experimental
variable onto the relevant psychometric variables for a
large and diverse group of TD children, and then use the
regression function to generate expected scores for the
(a) (b) (c)
Figure 1 Examples of (a) a circle (b) an RF3 stimulus and (c) an
RF24 stimulus
718 Emma J. Grinter et al.
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
children in the clinical group, against which their actual
scores are then compared. Thomas et al. (2009) argued
that this approach allows meaningful group compari-
sons to be made. Accordingly, we adopted Brock et al.’s
innovative approach in our group comparisons of RF
pattern performance.
Participants
Children with an ASD were recruited through an autism
register, speech pathologists and participation in pre-
vious research projects at the University of Western
Australia. The 38 8–16-year-old children (32 males) in
the ASD sample had received an independent clinical
diagnosis from a multidisciplinary team of either
autistic disorder (N = 30), Asperger’s disorder (N = 2) or
pervasive developmental disorder – not otherwise
specified (N = 6), according to DSM-IV (American Psy-
chiatric Association, 1994) criteria. Also, each ASD
child either met full criteria for autism (N = 34) or
scored above the cut-off in two of the three symptom
domains on the Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised
(ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) (social
interaction domain: M = 20.16, SD = 6.36; communi-
cation domain: M = 15.94, SD = 5.04; repetitive
behaviours domain: M = 6.51, SD = 2.65). Children
were excluded from the ASD sample if they had a
diagnosis of any medical condition (e.g., epilepsy) or
other developmental disorder (e.g., ADHD), hearing
or visual problems, or were taking medication known to
impact on perception or cognition. TD children
(N = 132, 8–16 years old, 70 males) recruited from a
metropolitan school participated after parents com-
pleted a brief screening questionnaire ensuring the
children had no medical, hearing or visual problems or
history of developmental difficulties. Written informed
consent was obtained from the parents of all children
prior to participation in accordance with the policies of
the University of Western Australia’s Ethics Committee.
Children wore their optical corrections for visual tasks
when required but were otherwise unscreened. How-
ever, since both RF3 and RF24 patterns are composed
of the same spatial frequency content, any group dif-
ferences in acuity would impact on both patterns
equally.
The groups were well matched for chronological age,
t(168) = .96, p = .34, and non-verbal ability, t(168) =
.31, p = .76, as measured by the Matrix Reasoning
subscale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for
Children – Version IV (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003; see
Table 1). The ASD group had significantly lower verbal
ability, as measured by the Vocabulary subscale of the
WISC-IV, than the TD group, t(168) = 5.46, p < .01,
consistent with communication difficulties being a core
characteristic of autism. All children were considered
high-functioning and were attending mainstream
schools. The TD group contained more females than the
ASD group, v2(1) = 14.12, p < .01.
Stimuli
RF patterns were presented on an LG L1730SF touch
screen driven by a Sony Vaio VGNSZ34GP laptop
computer. The 1024 · 768 pixel screen had a refresh
rate of 75Hz and a mean luminance of 30 cd/m2. RF
patterns were created following Bell et al. (2007). The
RF patterns were formed according to the following
equation:
rðhÞ¼ rmeanð1þ A sinðxh þ uÞ ð1Þ
where r and h (in radians) are the polar coordinates,
rmean is the pattern’s mean radius, and A, x and u are
the amplitude of modulation of the radius, radial
frequency (RF), and phase of the pattern, respectively.
RF patterns were always presented with random
phases, rendering it impossible for the observer to
predict the exact location of the lobes from trial to trial.
All RF patterns had a mean radius of 1.5� with a centre-
to-centre separation of 3.75�. The luminance profile of a
radial cross section approximated a fourth derivative of
a Gaussian set at 99% contrast and had a peak spatial
frequency of 8c/�. Radial frequencies (x) of 3 and 24
were employed (Figure 1).
Procedure
The WISC Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subscales
were given first, followed by the two RF tasks in
randomised order, and forming part of a larger test
battery. Each RF task began with 35 practice trials that
were administered under the same conditions as the
test trials to ensure that children could meet the task
demands. For the psychophysical tasks, testing was
conducted in a darkened room at a viewing distance of
.75 m. Viewing was binocular and auditory feedback
(a computer-generated tone) was provided after each
practice and test trial.
Two shapes were presented simultaneously for
200 ms, one a circle and the other a RF3 or RF24
pattern depending on the task. There was no time limit
for response, and a 1 s delay separated the child’s touch
response and the presentation of the next stimulus.
Children were told: ‘In this task you will see two shapes
come up on the screen. On one side the shape will be a
perfect circle. On the other, the shape will not be a
perfect circle, it will ‘‘have lots of little bumps around it’’
[for RF24 patterns] or ‘‘look like a squashed egg’’ [for
RF3 patterns]. What you have to do is work out which
side of the screen ‘‘has the shape that looks like a
squashed egg’’ or ‘‘has the bumpy shape’’ – the one
that’s not a perfect circle – and then touch that side of
the screen.’ To ensure they understood the instructions,
Table 1 Participant characteristics
Measures
Children with
ASD (N = 38)
Typically developing
children (N = 132)
Age (years)
Mean 11.88 12.15
SD 2.54 1.94
Range 8.17–16.92 8.83–15.83
WISC Vocabulary (scaled score)
Mean 8.95 11.47
SD 2.75 2.40
Range 4–15 6–19
WISC Matrix Reasoning (scaled score)
Mean 10.34 10.06
SD 2.91 2.55
Range 3-16 2-18
Shape perception in autism 719
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
children were then shown two example cards that
resembled the images in Figure 1 and asked which ones
they would choose.
The method of constant stimuli was used to control
stimulus presentation (7 stimulus levels, 15 trials per
level, taking approximately 6 minutes) and thresholds
were obtained from the psychometric function by fitting
the equation:
Y ¼ 7:5þ
7:5
1þexpðthreshold�xr Þ
ð2Þ
where threshold yields the 75% correct level, exp is the
exponential function, r is a scalar determining the slope
of the psychometric function, Y is the number correct
out of 15, and x is the amplitude level. At least 60% of
variance had to be accounted for by the psychometric
function for the threshold for that observer to be
included in the analyses. Each child was given two
opportunities to meet this criterion if required (RF3: 6%
in ASD group, 9% in TD group; RF24: 0% in ASD group,
3% in TD group). Children who did not meet the
criterion on either trial run were excluded from the
results for that task (RF3: 5% in ASD group, 7% in TD
group, RF24: 5% in ASD group, 3% in TD group).
Results
Data for each group were screened for normality, and
outliers (>3 SDs from the mean) were excluded (6%
for RF3 task; 2% for RF24 task). The final sample
size and the descriptive statistics for each task are
presented in Table 2.
Regression analyses
Following Brock et al. (2007), gender, age, verbal
ability and non-verbal ability were used as predictors
to conduct simple linear regression on the dependent
variables for the TD children. The resulting regres-
sion equations were then used to generate predicted
scores for the ASD children. Next, residuals were
calculated by subtracting the expected score from
the observed score for each child with an ASD. These
residuals were then standardised by dividing by the
standard error of the regression estimate. If ability
on each of the tasks developed in line with these
predictors, then the mean standardised scores for
the ASD children should be zero.
The mean standardised threshold for children with
an ASD for RF3 patterns was .00088 (SD = .0014),
which is significantly above zero, t(31) = 3.68,
p < .01, Cohen’s d = .65, consistent with them
requiring a greater amplitude of distortion to dis-
criminate an RF3 from a circle than TD children
(Figure 2a). The mean standardised threshold for the
ASD group for RF24 patterns did not differ signifi-
cantly from zero, t(33) = .59, p = .56, Cohen’s
d = ).06, indicating that there was no significant
difference between the two groups on this task (Fig-
ure 2b).1
To examine whether the higher thresholds for the
ASD group on the RF3 task were the result of a
developmental delay in the processing of these
shapes, rate of change in RF3 thresholds as a func-
tion of age was compared for the two groups. There
was no significant group difference in slopes as a
function of age for both the RF24, t(156) = .51,
p = .61 (TD mean slope = )4.42 · 10)5, ASD mean
slope = )1.05 · 10)4) and RF3, t(142) = .19, p = .85
(TD mean slope = )2.81 · 10)4, ASD mean slope =
)4.07 · 10)4) tasks.
Discussion
The aim of the current study was to assess local and
global ventral stream functioning in children with an
ASD and TD children. Importantly, we found that,
relative to TD children, children with an ASD
obtained significantly higher RF3 shape discrimina-
tion thresholds; they required a larger distortion of
the RF3 pattern to be able to discern a difference
between the RF shape and the circle, indicating a
difficulty with global processing within the ventral
visual stream. In contrast, there was no group dif-
ference for thresholds on the RF24 task, indicating
intact local processing in the ventral visual stream.
Poor performance on the RF3 task cannot be attrib-
uted to overall poor performance on psychophysical
tasks, or to uniformly weak inputs from local pro-
cesses to global processes. Rather, these data sug-
gest that the early stages of visual form perception
are intact for individuals with an ASD, but the
mechanisms required to combine local signals into a
global form percept function less effectively for this
group. This interpretation is consistent with WCC
theory which asserts that global processing is
anomalous in this population (Frith, 1989; Happé &
Booth, 2008).
Table 2 RF pattern modulation (A in eqn 1) thresholds for the
ASD and TD groups
Task Children with ASD Typically developing
RF3 N 32 114
Mean .0137 .0111
SD .0044 .0033
Range .0068–.0240 .0036–.0210
RF24 N 34 126
Mean .0029 .0026
SD .0009 .0006
Range .0018–.0046 .0014–.0034
1
These outcomes mirror those from more traditional
analytic methods which showed that ASD children had
significantly higher thresholds on the RF3 task, F(1,
141) = 7.14, p < .01, Np
2 = .05, and did not differ from
TD children on the RF24 task, F(1, 154) = .13, p = .72,
Np
2 = .001, when the groups (which are matched for age
and non-verbal ability) were compared using ANCOVA
with gender and verbal ability as covariates.
720 Emma J. Grinter et al.
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
In addition to impaired global processing, WCC
theory predicts superior, or at least intact, local
processing abilities. In the current study, the ASD
group demonstrated no difference in ability to dis-
criminate an RF24 pattern from a circle compared to
the TD group. At first glance, these results appear to
conflict with outcomes from a recent study which
assessed sensitivity to first- and second-order
defined patterns in children with an ASD and
IQ-matched TD children (Bertone et al., 2005).
Bertone et al. reported that their ASD group
displayed superior ability to identify orientation in
first-order defined patterns, in conjunction with
impaired ability on second-order defined patterns,
relative to a comparison group. The first-order task
used by Bertone et al. measured the minimally
detectable contrast threshold required to identify
orientation. Conversely, in our RF24 task, contrast is
at supra-threshold levels and the threshold assesses
the minimum amplitude required to perceive the
curvature of the local elements that comprise the RF
shape. Given that these two tasks are assessing
different capabilities at the local level, patterns of
performance could differ. It would, however, be
informative for future research to compare the same
samples of ASD and TD individuals on both tasks.
Importantly, both studies report impaired process-
ing on visual tasks that require further processing
beyond the V1 visual area, despite second-order
processing potentially occurring earlier in the hier-
archy (V2, Baker, 1999) than the global shape per-
ception required for RF3 patterns (V4, Wilkinson
et al., 2000). These findings are consistent with the
idea that individuals with an ASD experience diffi-
culty in the processing of ‘complex’ information
(Bertone et al., 2005; Minshew et al., 1997) in that
both second-order orientation identification and RF3
shape perception invoke more complex perceptual
networks to integrate multiple stimulus elements
than do tasks typically used to target the local pro-
cessing capabilities of V1.
Other studies assessing global processing in the
ventral visual stream have not produced clear group
differences when comparing individuals with an ASD
to matched TD control groups. Milne et al. (2006)
and Spencer et al. (2000) reported no differences in
form coherence thresholds using a task that required
detecting the presence of a global pattern revealed by
giving small line segments an orientation appropri-
ate for the global pattern. In contrast, Spencer and
O’Brien (2006) and Tsermentseli, O’Brien, and
Spencer (2008) reported higher thresholds for
children with autism compared to TD controls (but
only when children with Asperger’s disorder were
excluded from analyses). The task they employed
required detecting global form in Glass patterns
(Glass, 1969) composed of aligned dot triplets as
opposed to line segments. There are many differ-
ences between these studies which might have an
impact. Importantly, the response of cells in V1 are
facilitated by horizontal connections when adjacent
cells are firing (known as collinear facilitation; see
Loffler, 2008, for a review). Li and Gilbert (2002, see
also Field & Hayes, 2004) demonstrated similar
processes occur when elements are combined in
contour detection. Therefore, one possible factor
contributing to the differences between these studies
is that the solid lines in the line segment stimuli may
enhance processing in these collinear facilitation
networks, whereas the weaker collinear facilitation
produced by having three aligned dots may be less
effective in activating these networks. Consequently,
it is possible that the contours in line segment
stimuli are facilitated by lower-level processing,
whereas Glass patterns specifically target high-level
integrative processing in the ventral stream (Wilson
& Wilkinson, 1998). If this is the case, then the
results of these combined studies are consistent with
those presently reported using RF patterns. Specifi-
cally, no difference between ASD and TD groups is
apparent on tasks assessing lower-level ventral
stream processing, whereas tasks such as RF3 and
Glass pattern tasks are sensitive to the global pro-
cessing difficulties experienced by individuals with
an ASD. This potential explanation for the different
results for the studies requires further investigation
directly comparing the different stimulus types.
While there are reports of nonsignificant group dif-
ferences for each level of processing in the literature,
(a)
(b)
0.016
T
h
re
sh
o
ld
(
A
)
T
h
re
sh
o
ld
(
A
)
RF3
0.014
0.012
0.010
0.0035 RF24
0.0030
0.0025
TD ASD
TD ASD
Figure 2 Graphs showing mean thresholds on (a) the
RF3 task and (b) the RF24 task for the typically devel-
oping and ASD groups (lines show 95% confidence
intervals). The 95% confidence intervals are smaller in
the TD group owing to the substantially larger sample
size
Shape perception in autism 721
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
when significant differences have been reported, for
local processing the differences have invariably
been in the direction of superior performance for
the ASD sample relative to neurotypical comparison
groups (Mottron et al., 2006). Conversely, for global
processing the differences have invariably been in
the direction of impaired performance for the ASD
sample relative to neurotypical comparison groups
(Happé & Booth, 2008). Thus, on balance, the
emerging empirical literature on visual processing
in ASDs favours the WCC theory given its capacity
to account for both global impairment and local
superiority. While the normal performance for the
ASD sample on the RF24 task in the current study
is compatible with the predictions derived from EPF
theory, the finding of impaired global processing for
the ASD group on the RF3 task cannot be
accounted for by EPF theory. Thus, the patterns of
findings from the current study also favour the
WCC theory. The EPF account has led to valuable
research showing superiority of children with ASDs
in local processing in several areas, including
graphic construction, visual search and the
perception of hierarchical figures (see Happé &
Booth, 2008, for a summary). However, many of
these tasks may be differentially influenced by local
and global processing, and the trade-off between
the two is often unclear. For example, local and
global processing on the Navon (1977) hierarchical
figures are associated with spatial frequency
differences (Badcock, Whitworth, Badcock, &
Lovegrove, 1990), resulting in the relative contri-
bution of local and global processing to task
performance being unclear (see also Dakin & Frith,
2005, for a discussion). The RF shapes used in the
current study are not subject to this limitation in
that they have identical spatial frequencies at the
local and global levels. However, performance on
low RF patterns (e.g., RF3) can still be attributed to
global pooling, whereas performance on high RF
patterns (e.g., RF24) can be accounted for simply
with reference to probability summation of infor-
mation from local elements (Bell et al., 2007).
An alternative explanation for the current finding
of lower sensitivity to RF3 shapes in ASDs might be
that global pooling in higher cortical regions devel-
ops later than local processing mechanisms. As a
result, the ASD group may be developmentally
delayed in their ability to process global contours
compared to the TD group. If this were the case,
then it would be expected that the TD group would
show a greater improvement with age in RF3
thresholds than the ASD children. However, the
results from the current study indicated that the
ASD children developed at the same rate as the TD
children on both the RF3 and RF24 tasks. Never-
theless, it will be important for future research to
investigate whether, in adulthood, ASD samples
eventually attain thresholds for low RF tasks that
are comparable to those of TD groups. Additionally,
with respect to the RF stimuli, it is becoming clear
that global processing occurs in shapes up to
approximately an RF of 10 whereas in shapes of
higher RF, local processing appears to be the pre-
dominant mechanism (Bell et al., 2009; Loffler
et al., 1998). In typical observers, the transfer from
global to local processing with increasing RF occurs
quite rapidly. Given the evidence suggesting a bias
for local processing in ASDs, it is possible that their
transition point is different. It will therefore be
important for future research to compare ASD and
TD groups on a continuum of RF shapes with
increasing RF numbers.
To summarise, using a novel application of RF
stimuli, we have demonstrated that global pro-
cessing is impaired in the ventral visual stream in a
reasonably large sample of individuals with an ASD.
This adds substantially to the position that a deficit
in global visual processing is present in this popu-
lation. The results are consistent with more general
problems in neural integration in ASDs, as evi-
denced by the findings from EEG (Pei et al., In
Press) and fMRI (e.g., Just, Cherkassky, Keller, &
Minshew, 2004) studies. Finally, the application of
regression analyses to determine the difference
between predicted and observed scores in a clinical
population provided an innovative means to avoid
the issues usually associated with matching for age,
verbal and non-verbal ability. These data provide an
insight into the experience of visual perception in
children with ASDs. Individuals with autism often
describe exceptional visual perceptual abilities that
allow them to identify changes or anomalies in their
environment (e.g., Grandin, 1992; Stehli, 1991) not
usually noticeable to TD individuals. According to
Kanner (1943), this ‘inability to experience wholes
without full attention to the constituent parts’
(p. 246) is a core component of autism, but these
abilities may be maladaptive in so far as they may
lead to distress at small changes in the environment
(Happé & Frith, 2006). Additionally, a disturbance
in the ability to combine visual information in eye
gaze, facial expressions and face perception has
been hypothesised to contribute to impairments in
social communication seen in ASD (see Dawson,
Webb, & McPartland, 2005, for a review). Thus,
fundamental, early-emerging difficulties in inte-
grating information to form a coherent, global per-
cept, as demonstrated using the RF patterns, could
account for some of the major behavioural mani-
festations of ASDs.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by NH&MRC Project
Grant 403942 awarded to M. T. Maybery, D. R.
Badcock, J. C. Badcock and E. Pellicano.
We are grateful to Judith Cullity for her assistance
in programming, Rachelle Fox, Lynsey Harborow,
722 Emma J. Grinter et al.
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
Dana Sidoruk and Kelly Scaramozzino for their
assistance in data collection, and two anonymous
reviewers who commented on an earlier draft of the
manuscript. Finally, we are extremely indebted to all
the children and families who gave their time to
participate in this research.
Correspondence to
Emma Grinter, School of Psychology, University of
Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley
6009, Perth, WA, Australia; Tel: +618 64882479;
Email: [email protected]
Key points
• The study introduces a new and simple shape stimulus, radial
frequency (RF) patterns, to investigate
visual functioning in children with autism spectrum disorders
(ASDs)
• RF patterns can be manipulated in precise ways to probe local
and global mechanisms involved in pro-
cessing visual form.
• Children with ASDs require more shape modulation for RF
patterns that target global form processing
compared to typically developing children.
• Children with ASDs show intact ability to process RF patterns
targeting local form processing.
• RF patterns may provide a new, readily accepted clinical tool
to examine visual function in ASDs.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV, 4th edn).
Washington DC: APA.
Badcock, J.C., Whitworth, F.A., Badcock, D.R., & Love-
grove, W.J. (1990). Low-frequency filtering and process-
ing global-local stimuli. Perception, 19, 617–629.
Baker, C.L. (1999). Central neural mechanisms for detect-
ing second-order motion. Current Opinion in Neurobiol-
ogy, 9, 461–466.
Bell, J., & Badcock, D.R. (2008). Luminance and contrast
cues are integrated in global shape detection with
contours. Vision Research, 48, 2336–2344.
Bell, J., & Badcock, D.R. (2009). Narrow-band radial
frequency shape channels revealed by sub-threshold
summation. Vision Research, 49, 843–850.
Bell, J., Badcock, D.R., Wilson, H., & Wilkinson, F. (2007).
Detection of shape in radial frequency contours: Inde-
pendence of local and global form information. Vision
Research, 47, 1518–1522.
Bell, J., Wilkinson, F., Wilson, H.R., Loffler, G., & Badcock,
D.R. (2009). Radial frequency adaptation reveals inter-
acting contour shape channels. Vision Research. Epub
ahead of print: doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.022
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003).
Motion perception in autism: A ‘complex issue’. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225.
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005).
Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information
processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity.
Brain, 128, 2430–2441.
Blake, R., Turner, L.M., Smoski, M.J., Pozdol, S.L., &
Stone, W. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion
impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science,
14, 151–157.
Brock, J., Jarrold, C., Farran, E.K., Laws, G., & Riby, D.M.
(2007). Do children with Williams syndrome really have
good vocabulary knowledge? Methods for comparing
cognitive and linguistic abilities in developmental disor-
ders. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 673–688.
Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception
in autism. Neuron, 48, 497–507.
Dawson, M., Webb, S.J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Under-
standing the nature of face processing impairment in
autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiologi-
cal studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 403–
424.
DeValois, R.L., & DeValois, K.K. (1988). Spatial vision. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Field, D.J., & Hayes, A. (2004). Contour integration and
lateral connections of V1 neurons. In L.M. Chalupa, &
J.S. Werner (Eds.), The visual neurosciences (Vol. 2, pp.
1069–1079). London: MIT Press.
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford:
Basil Blackwell.
Glass, L. (1969). Moire effect from random dots. Nature,
223, 578–580.
Grandin, T. (1992). An inside view of autism. In E.
Schopler, & G.B. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning indi-
viduals with autism (pp. 105–126). New York: Plenum
Press.
Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive
style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.
Happé, F., & Booth, R.D.L. (2008). The power of the
positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum
disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
61, 50–63.
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account:
Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disor-
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
36, 5–25.
Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match?
Methodological issues in autism-related research.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34,
81–86.
Just, M.A., Cherkassky, V.L., Keller, T.A., & Minshew, N.J.
(2004). Cortical activation and synchronisation during
sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism:
Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821.
Kaiser, M., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of
motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A
review and synthesis. Psychonomic Bulletin, 16, 761–
777.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con-
tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Li, W., & Gilbert, C.D. (2002). Global contour saliency and
local colinear interactions. Journal of Neurophysiology,
88, 2846–2856.
Loffler, G. (2008). Perception of contours and shapes: Low
and intermediate stage mechanisms. Vision Research,
48, 2106–2127.
Shape perception in autism 723
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
Loffler, G., Wilson, H., & Wilkinson, F. (2003). Local and
global contributions to shape discrimination. Vision
Research, 43, 519–530.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism
Diagnostic Interview – Revised. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R.,
Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence
thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 255–263.
Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Han-
sen, P., & Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence
detection in autism: Relationships to motor control and
2:4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 36, 225–237.
Minshew, N.J., Goldstein, G., & Siegal, D.J. (1997). Neu-
ropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex
information processing disorder. Journal of the Interna-
tional Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.
Mottron, L., Burack, J.A., Stauder, J., & Robaey, P. (1999).
Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons
with Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
40, 203–211.
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Souliéres, I., Hubert, B., &
Burack, J.A. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning
in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic
perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
ders, 36, 27–43.
Movshon, J.A. (1990). Visual processing of moving images.
In M. Weston-Smith, H.B. Barlow, & C. Blakemore
(Eds.), Images and understanding. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of
global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychol-
ogy, 9, 353–383.
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D.L., McMahon, W.M., & Filloux, F.
(1994). Executive function abilities in autism and Tou-
rette syndrome: An information processing approach.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015–
1032.
Pei, F., Baldassi, S., Procida, G., Igliozzi, R., Tancredi, R.,
Muratori, F., & Cioni, G. (in press). Neural correlates of
texture and contour integration in children with autism
spectrum disorders. Vision Research.
Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., &
Badcock, D.R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along
the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mech-
anise for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsycholo-
gia, 43, 1044–1053.
Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children
with autism show local precedence in a divided attention
task and global precedence in a selective attention task.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–
742.
Simmons, D.R., Robertson, A.E., McKay, L., Toal, E.,
McAleer, P., & Pollick, F.E. (2009). Vision in autism
spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739.
Spencer, J., & O’Brien, J. (2006). Visual form processing
deficits in autism. Perception, 35, 1047–1055.
Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson,
J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in
autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Cogni-
tive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 11, 2765–2767.
Stehli, A. (1991). The sound of a miracle. New York:
Doubleday.
Thomas, M.S.C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Scerif, G., Jarrold,
C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental
trajectories to understand developmental disorders.
Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing, 52, 336–358.
Tsermentseli, S., O’Brien, J., & Spencer, J. (2008). Com-
parison of form and motion coherence processing in
autistic spectrum disorders and dyslexia. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1201–1210.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for Children (4th edn).
Wilkinson, F., James, T.W., Wilson, H., Gati, J.S., Menon,
R.S., & Goodale, M.A. (2000). An fMRI study of the
selective activation of human extrastriate form vision
areas by radial and concentric gratings. Current Biology,
10, 1455–1458.
Wilkinson, F., Wilson, H., & Habak, C. (1998). Detection
and recognition of radial frequency patterns. Vision
Research, 38, 3555–3568.
Wilson, H., & Wilkinson, F. (1998). Detection of global
structure in Glass patterns: Implications for form vision.
Vision Research, 38, 2933–2947.
Manuscript accepted 30 October 2009
724 Emma J. Grinter et al.
� 2010 The Authors
Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
Adolescent Mental Health.
Copyright of Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry is the
property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
Object recognition with severe spatial deficits
in Williams syndrome: sparing and breakdown
Barbara Landau
a,*, James E. Hoffman
b
, Nicole Kurz
b
a
Department of Cognitive Science, Krieger Hall, Johns Hopkins
University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
b
University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
Received 31 March 2002; revised 2 September 2004; accepted
23 June 2005
Abstract
Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic disorder that results
in severe visual-spatial cognitive
deficits coupled with relative sparing in language, face
recognition, and certain aspects of motion
processing. Here, we look for evidence for sparing or
impairment in another cognitive system—
object recognition. Children with WS, normal mental-age (MA)
and chronological age-matched
(CA) children, and normal adults viewed pictures of a large
range of objects briefly presented under
various conditions of degradation, including canonical and
unusual orientations, and clear or blurred
contours. Objects were shown as either full-color views
(Experiment 1) or line drawings
(Experiment 2). Across both experiments, WS and MA children
performed similarly in all conditions
while CA children performed better than both WS group and
MA groups with unusual views. This
advantage, however, was eliminated when images were also
blurred. The error types and relative
difficulty of different objects were similar across all participant
groups. The results indicate selective
sparing of basic mechanisms of object recognition in WS,
together with developmental delay or
arrest in recognition of objects from unusual viewpoints. These
findings are consistent with the
growing literature on brain abnormalities in WS which points to
selective impairment in the parietal
areas of the brain. As a whole, the results lend further support
to the growing literature on the
functional separability of object recognition mechanisms from
other spatial functions, and raise
intriguing questions about the link between genetic deficits and
cognition.
q 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Ventral Stream; Object Recognition; Williams
syndrome
Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510
www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT
0022-2860/$ - see front matter q 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights
reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.005
* Corresponding author. Tel.: C1 410 516 5255.
E-mail address: [email protected] (B. Landau).
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510484
In this paper, we report evidence that some mechanisms of
object recognition can
develop without impairment even while other aspects of spatial
representation are severely
impaired. Our evidence comes from people with Williams
syndrome, a genetic disorder
which gives rise to an unusual cognitive profile including
severe spatial deficits together
with relatively spared language
1
. The striking imbalance between two major cognitive
systems has suggested to some that genetic defects can have
specific cognitive targets
during development (Bellugi, Marks, Bihrle & Sabo, 1988;
Jordan, Reiss, Hoffman &
Landau, 2002; see also, Frith, 1992). Such targeting has been
attributed to modular
organization of cognition (Bellugi et al., 1988; Clahsen &
Almazan, 1998) or
specialization in streams of processing in the mind and brain
(e.g. Atkinson, Braddick,
Anker, Curran, Andrew and Wattam-Bell, 2003; Dilks, Landau
& Hoffman, 2005). The
present studies provide support for specific targeting in a
genetic deficit, as they show that
basic mechanisms of object recognition can be spared even
though other aspects of spatial
representation are severely impaired. We show that, while
children with Williams
syndrome cannot reproduce the spatial organization of even
moderately complex figures
(see Fig. 1), they can recognize a wide range of familiar objects
under varying conditions
of degradation.
Testing the possibility that a specific cognitive system is spared
in a case of genetic
impairment requires several steps. First, it requires evidence
that the cognitive system in
question is specialized, that is, different from other knowledge
domains or functions on
computational, neural, and/or psychological grounds. Such
evidence is abundant in object
recognition, and we discuss it below. Second, it assumes that
genetic deficits can, in
principal, target certain cognitive systems while leaving others
either partially or fully
spared. As we discuss below, this assumption is complex and
currently under debate.
Ultimately, the debate can only be resolved by empirical study,
which we offer here.
Finally, any empirical test requires sufficient fine-grained detail
to examine key aspects of
normal cognitive architecture. If patterns of performance are
both quantitatively and
qualitatively similar to normal groups, it is reasonable to
conclude that basic aspects of the
architecture are spared. Alternatively, detailed patterns of
difference can shed light on
which aspects of the architecture are robust and which are
vulnerable in genetic deficit.
The experiments we present will test key aspects of the object
recognition system in order
to determine how, if at all, performance differs from normal.
1. Object recognition as a specialized system
Thinking of human object recognition as a specialized system is
not a new idea. At least
since the work of Marr (1982), it has been acknowledged that
one of the central
computational goals of the visual system is to rapidly recognize
objects under a wide
range of viewing conditions. The object recognition system is
designed to represent
1
We attach no technical meaning to the term ‘severe’, but rather,
use it to describe the hallmark pattern of
Williams syndrome, in which children, adolescents, and even
adults carry out visual-spatial constructive tasks
such as block construction at the level of four-year-old normal
children.
Fig. 1. Sample copies of model figures (row 1) drawn by two
children with Williams syndrome (rows 2 and 3) and
one normally developing child matched for mental age (row 4).
KBIT scores represent IQ, not raw scores used for
matching.
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 485
objects—primarily by their shapes—in a way that renders them
recognizable over changes
in irrelevant spatial properties such as size, translation, and
viewpoint. Thus, object
recognition is inherently spatial. The specific computational
problems involved in visual
object recognition include segmenting the image from
background, parsing it into edges
and/or parts, and representing the overall configuration, usually
determined by the spatial
relationships among parts. The resulting representation must
support matching to an
incoming image over changes in the object’s lighting, size,
viewpoint, etc. (Marr, 1982;
Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004).
Arguments and evidence suggest that the computational
solutions to these problems are
unique to objects. For example, researchers have suggested that
object shape is
represented by the distributed activity of a population of
neurons tuned to specific
viewpoints (Logothetis & Sheinberg, 1996; Palmeri & Gauthier,
2004). Recent data from
brain imaging suggests that the computations involved in
recognizing objects are carried
out in distributed, but constrained areas of the brain that are
distinct from areas that process
face recognition (Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004; Pietrini, Furey,
Ricciardi, Gobbini, Wu and
Cohen, 2004). Presumably, this specialized computational
system has evolved to allow us
to rapidly and efficiently recognize objects, perhaps even as
soon as we know that an
object is there (Grill-Spector & Kanwisher, 2005).
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510486
A particularly difficult problem for the visual system is
recognition of objects from
partial, sometimes degraded images that result from viewing
objects from different
viewpoints, which often occlude and distort important features.
Some theories suggest
that the visual system constructs a single unified viewpoint-
invariant representation (e.g.
Biederman, 1987; Marr, 1982). However the bulk of empirical
evidence indicates that
we recognize objects by using viewpoint-dependent
representations and interpolating
new viewpoints (Bulthoff, Edelman & Tarr 1995; Tarr & Pinker,
1990; Logothetis,
Pauls, Bulthoff & Poggio 1994; see Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004).
The mechanisms of
computing new viewpoints are not well understood, but the
capacity to do so is
acknowledged to be complex, and is assumed to be a hallmark
of our object recognition
system.
Considerations of cognitive function and brain localization also
support the idea that
visual object recognition is specialized. Abundant evidence
shows that the human visual
system is composed of separate streams which carry out
different kinds of spatial
processing that serve different functions. For example, beyond
the primary visual cortex,
the ventral stream processes information about objects and
faces. Neurons in
inferotemporal (IT) cortex of monkeys respond to the same
object under a variety of
equivalence conditions (Booth & Rolls, 1998; Logothetis et al.,
1994) and to neighboring
views of the same object (Tanaka, 1996). In humans, imaging
studies show that
homologous areas of cortex are activated during object
recognition, suggesting a specific
neural substrate (Kourtzi, Erb, Grodd & Bulthoff, 2003; Palmeri
& Gauthier, 2004).
In contrast to the ventral stream areas and their focus on
objects, the dorsal stream is
thought to be responsible for processing different kinds of
spatial information, including
some kinds of motion and location. For example, the perception
of motion coherence—
which requires computing the global direction of individual
motion elements—appears to
be processed in area V5/MT (Newsome & Pare, 1988). Still
other spatial properties and
functions appear to be governed by other areas of the dorsal
stream, especially those
properties pertinent for action (Bridgeman, Gemmer, Forsman &
Huemer, 2000; Colby &
Goldberg, 1999; Livingstone & Hubel, 1988; Milner & Goodale,
1995; Ungerleider &
Mishkin, 1982).
Finally, the psychological representation of objects has been
shown to be dissociable
from other kinds of representation that depend on spatial
organization, even those within
the ventral stream. For example, Moscovitch, Winocur &
Behrmann (1997) reported a
brain-damaged patient with severely impaired object recognition
who could nevertheless
recognize faces very well. Duchaine and Nakayama (2005)
reported the reverse pattern,
developmental prosopagnosics who could not recognize faces,
but could recognize
categories of objects such as tools and cars quite well. Milner
and Goodale (1995) reported
a patient who was severely impaired in judging a line’s
orientation, but could use her
perception of the line to guide action (e.g. posting a letter
through a slot). Importantly,
patients who lose the ability to recognize objects often have
damage to the ventral stream,
specifically areas of inferotemporal and occipital cortex. These
people often perform
worse under degraded conditions such as varying illumination
or novel viewpoints (see
Farah, 2000), suggesting that they have lost the hallmark
capacity to recognize object
equivalences over such varying conditions.
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 487
As a whole, this evidence strongly suggests that the object
recognition is specialized in
its computational mechanisms, neural substrate, and
psychological function.
2. Genetic deficits and cognitive specialization
Can a genetic deficit target certain cognitive systems while
sparing others? This issue is
controversial. One aspect of the controversy concerns whether
or not developmental (i.e.
genetic) cases can be used to make the same kinds of inferences
about cognitive structure
as adult cases of brain damage. Arguments in favor of selective
sparing have often come
from studies of brain-damaged adults, who represent the case of
a mature system that
sustains damage. Evidence from brain-damaged adults who can
perceive faces but not
objects suggests some type of specialized organization in which
systems have been
differentially targeted by the damage (e.g. Moscovitch et al.,
1997). In contrast, genetic
deficits have effects even prior to birth, and this raises the
question of whether such a
deficit will reflect the same kinds of division of labor as
revealed by cases of damage to the
mature brain. A second controversial issue concerns the
relationship between genes and
cognition. Genetic changes to an organism will inevitably result
in local changes to the
physiology of the brain. The crucial issue is whether such local
changes will inevitably
affect cognitive structure.
Views on these two aspects of the controversy lead to very
different sets of predictions
about the likely cognitive outcome of any genetic deficit. In one
view, it is possible for a
genetic defect to selectively target particular cognitive
systems—either because of distinct
computational requirements of the system, or because the defect
selectively impairs
streams of processing in the brain that normally support that
system. In this view, the local
effects that genes have on brain physiology interact with
constraints on knowledge
domains that overdetermine cognitive outcomes. Thus, cognitive
domains could be
expected to have normal structure. This possibility is consistent
with the idea that different
cognitive domains are computationally distinct, that they have
different neural substrates,
and that the internal constraints of the system guide
development (e.g. Frith, 1992;
Gallistel, Brown, Carey, Gelman & Keil, 1991; Spelke &
Newport, 1998; Tager-Flusberg,
Plesa-Skwerer & Faja, 2003).
Several recent studies in the domain of faces are consistent with
this view. For example,
Tager-Flusberg et al. reported that people with Williams
syndrome show the face
inversion effect usually observed in normal individuals,
suggesting that the face
processing mechanisms operate under normal constraints. In
addition, recent reports of
congenital prosopagnosia suggest it is possible to have a
specific deficit in face perception
(Behrmann & Avidan, 2005); additional evidence hints that
there may be a genetic basis to
this disorder. In this general view, one would predict that
mechanisms of object
recognition could be spared even in the context of severe
deficits in other areas of visual-
spatial cognition.
In a different view, genetic deficits are also assumed to have
widespread local effects on
the development of the brain, but these effects are thought to
propogate up to cognitive
structure, as follows. The genetic deficit is assumed to result in
impaired cognitive
mechanisms. Interactions between these impaired mechanisms
and particular knowledge
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510488
domains will yield differences in cognitive structure. Moreover,
the cognitive structures
that are produced will not necessarily (or even probably) show
breakdown along the lines
of a normal, mature architecture (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998).
Although one might observe
performance that appears similar to a normal profile, closer
inspection will likely show
meaningful differences in the underlying cognitive structures.
For example, Karmiloff-
Smith, Thomas, Annaz, Humphreys, Ewing and Brace (2004)
argue that face processing in
people with Williams syndrome is not normal, but rather, good
performance is
accomplished via atypical mechanisms; specifically, they argue
that configural processing
of faces in WS people is impaired. This general position is
consistent with the idea that,
despite the distinct computational requirements of different
cognitive domains, genetic
deficits will likely result in subtle cognitive impairments,
including the recognition of
objects.
These strong positions can guide our thinking, but only
empirical data can decide
between them. Perhaps more importantly, empirical data can
provide us with the
foundation for developing more nuanced theoretical positions.
3. Williams syndrome and object recognition
The case of object recognition in Williams syndrome provides
an empirical forum for
doing both of these. Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic
disorder (1:20,000 live
births) which is caused by a hemizygous submicroscopic
deletion on chromosome
7q11.23. Diagnosis is made on the basis of a unique phenotypic
pattern that includes a
characteristic facial profile, disorders of the heart, and
anomalies of the viscera. It can also
be verified by a screening technique (fluoride in situ
hybridization, FISH) that isolates the
key region of gene deletion (Ewart, Morris, Atkinson, Jin,
Sternes and Spallone, 1993;
Frangiskakis, Ewart, Morris, Mervis, Bertrand and Robinson,
1996; Morris, Ewart,
Sternes, Spallone, Stock and Leppert, 1994).
Of most importance to us, however, is the distinctive cognitive
profile of individuals with
Williams syndrome, who show severely impaired spatial
cognition together with relatively
spared language (Bellugi et al., 1988; Mervis, Morris, Bertrand
& Robinson, 1999).
Although WS individuals are also moderately mentally retarded
(Mean Composite IQZ55–
60, Mervis, et al., 1999), their unique pattern of spatial deficit
and linguistic strength sets
them apart from other groups with comparable retardation, such
as Down syndrome (Mervis
et al., 1999). The striking cognitive profile has also motivated
some of the strongest
hypotheses of developmental modularity in the field (Bellugi et
al., 1988; Pinker, 1994).
Selective sparing in cognitive systems can also be tested by
examining differential
breakdown within the broad domain of spatial representation.
Growing evidence shows
that spatial representation is not one monolithic system, but
rather, is composed of
different sub-systems that are specialized in their computational
goals, their functional
properties, and their neural substrates. One example is the
contrast between perception of
space and representation of space for action, as described
previously. Other functionally
distinct systems include the representation of space for
navigation (Aguirre, Zarahn &
D’Esposito, 1998; Gallistel, 1990; Hermer & Spelke, 1996;
Newcombe & Huttenlocher,
2000), the multiple representations of space that encode
location in different reference
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 489
systems (Andersen, Snyder, Bradley & Xing 1997; Colby &
Goldberg, 1999), and the
representation of different kinds of motion such as motion
coherence, biological motion,
and form from motion (Schenk & Zihl, 1997; Vaina, LeMay,
Bienfrang, Choi &
Nakayama, 1990).
Although the nature of spatial impairment in WS is not well
understood at present, there
are indications that it has ‘peaks and valleys’ (cf. Bellugi et al.,
1988), raising the
possibility of selective targeting. The most widely observed
hallmark of the spatial deficit
appears in so-called ‘visual-spatial construction’ tasks, in which
people are asked to
replicate an overall pattern by drawing or assembling parts.
Individuals with WS perform
extremely poorly in such tasks, with adolescents performing in
the 1st percentile—roughly
at the level of a normal four-year-old (Bellugi et al., 1988;
Mervis et al., 1999; see Fig. 1
for some examples). In these tasks, the deficient performance of
WS children can be traced
to faulty spatial representations of the individual blocks and
their relations rather than
faulty executive processes (Hoffman, Landau & Pagani, 2003).
In particular, children with
WS have trouble discriminating the ‘handedness’ of the
individual blocks which are often
split in half by color, and have distractors that are mirror-
images. Additionally, WS
children make errors in the spatial arrangement of blocks, often
erring on left-right
locations within the model. This raises the possibility that
object representation more
generally—and not just mirror-image structures—might be
impaired.
On the other hand, growing evidence suggests differential
breakdown across other
domains that involve spatial organization of elements. For
example, the perception of
biological motion and motion coherence is spared in WS
children (Jordan et al., 2002;
Reiss, Hoffman & Landau, 2005), even though perception of
form from motion is impaired
(Atkinson, King, Braddick, Nokes, Anker & Braddick 1997;
Reiss et al., 2005). Aspects of
visual-manual action appear to be impaired, even relative to
mental age matched children
(Atkinson, King, Braddick, Nokes, Anker and Braddick, 1997;
Dilks, Landau & Hoffman,
2005). Some mechanisms of global spatial perception are
unimpaired (Pani, Mervis &
Robinson, 1999) and there is evidence that face perception is
spared, even relative to
chronological age matches (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2003). As a
whole, these findings are
consistent with the idea that a genetic defect can result in
targeted—rather than omnibus—
spatial breakdown.
4. The current experiments
The case of object recognition affords a further test of this
possibility. First, it is a
natural domain that presents a unique computational problem:
Using patterns of light
striking the retina, the brain must construct a representation that
will enable the perceiver
to later recognize the same object, despite changes in viewpoint
and lighting that occur in
natural viewing conditions. This is a task that is easily solved
by human observers—even
infants—but cannot yet be solved by machines. Second,
evidence from cases of brain
damage in adults already suggests specialization of function:
People may lose the ability to
perceive objects, while still being able to act on them or reason
about them. Third, the
object recognition system seems to be vulnerable to a variety of
effects of degradation
under brain damage. For example, poor lighting or line
drawings produces special
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510490
difficulties for agnosics (Farah, 2000), and highly unusual
viewpoints produce difficulties
in patients with damage to the right parietal lobe (Warrington &
Taylor, 1973). This well-
documented pattern of vulnerability within the object
recognition system suggests a way
to examine in detail the degree to which object recognition is
spared or impaired.
Recognition of objects under degraded conditions, such as poor
lighting, unusual
viewpoint, or line drawings would provide a strong test of
sparing, but it is also possible
that some conditions might prove substantially more difficult
than others, providing
evidence for both sparing and breakdown.
Therefore, in our first experiment, we examined the capacity of
children with WS to
identify objects under degraded conditions. We used two critical
manipulations:
Presentation of clear vs. blurred images, and presentation of
objects from canonical vs.
unusual viewpoints. These two methods of image degradation
can be used to drive down
performance of all groups—including normal children and
adults—thereby allowing us to
examine whether patterns of failure are similar or different
between WS and normal
individuals. But the manipulations are also of more particular
interest with respect to
Williams syndrome.
First, blurring the image is of interest because initial
characterizations of the spatial
deficit in WS suggested that they are ‘local processors’ (Bihrle,
Bellugi, Delis & Marks,
1989; Deruelle, Mancini, Livel, Casse-Perot & de Schoon,
1999), i.e. they correctly
perceive the features of an object but are deficient in grasping
the global configuration of
those features. More recent work (Farran, Jarrold & Gathercole,
2003; Pani, Mervis &
Robinson, 1999), however, indicates that people with WS do
correctly perceive
configurations and actually have trouble focusing attention on
parts that are members of
larger configurations (Hoffman, Landau, and Pagani, 2003).
Blurring an image would
primarily affect the visibility of constituent parts (Hughes,
Nozawa, and Kitterle, 1996;
Morrison and Schyns, 2001) while leaving the global shape
relatively unaffected.
Therefore, to the extent that people with WS utilize global
shape to recognize objects, they
might be less affected by blur than control children who may
use both local and global
information for identification.
The second, and more crucial manipulation is canonical vs.
highly unusual viewpoints.
Recognizing objects from highly unusual viewpoints might
present a special problem for
people with Williams syndrome. Perrett, Oram & Ashbridge
(1998) suggest that viewpoint
effects on recognition can be understood in terms of a multiple
view theory of object
recognition in which the current view is matched to multiple
views that are stored in
inferotemporal cortex that have been laid down by previous
experiences with that object.
Novel views do not have a matching representation and must be
recognized by partial
activation of nearest neighbor views, which will be slower than
matches for canonical
views.
This theory, however, does not directly account for findings
with highly unusual views.
Unusual views are taken from a perspective that foreshortens
the principal axis of the
object and often occludes many of its salient features
(Humphreys & Riddoch, 1984). This
precludes the kind of automatic object recognition that seems to
occur with more
canonical views and instead, appears to involve a ‘problem
solving or executive
component’ (Farah, 2000) in which the observer searches the
image for parts that may
provide cues to its identity (Perrett et al., 1998). Given the role
of the parietal lobe in
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 491
directing spatial attention (Belmonte & Yurgelun-Todd, 2003),
it is perhaps not surprising
that patients with damage to parietal areas are selectively
impaired in recognizing these
views (Warrington & Taylor, 1973). The claim that recognition
of unusual views requires
executive control is also consistent with the finding that in
dual-task experiments, a task
requiring ‘central executive resources’ (random number
generation) produced more
interference with unusual views than canonical views
(Baragwanath & Turnbull, 2002).
Finally, recognition of unusual views has been found to activate
both prefrontal (Kosslyn,
Alpert, Thompson, Chabris, Rauch and Anderson, 1994) and
parietal areas (Sugio, Inui,
Matsuo, Matsuzawa, Glover and Nakai, 1999) in fMRI scans,
suggesting top-down control
of parietal areas involved with spatial attention.
The role of the parietal lobe in recognition of unusual views
suggests that WS subjects
may find such views particularly challenging. Damage to the
same parietal areas activated
in recognition of unusual views often results in impairments in
tasks such as drawing and
block construction (Benton, 1967; Turnbull, Denis, Mellet,
Ghaem & Carey, 2001)—just
those tasks that WS people typically fail. In addition, recent
neuroimaging of people with
WS during performance of these spatial tasks shows a reduced
activation of these parietal
areas (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2004), and other analyses show
than WS people have
smaller than normal volume in superior parietal areas (Eckert,
Hu, Eliez, Bellugi,
Galaburda and Korenberg, 2005). These observations are
consistent with the suggestion by
Atkinson et al. (2003) that WS (and possibly other syndromes)
is primarily a dorsal stream
deficit. People with WS might therefore be expected to have a
selective deficit in
identifying objects from highly unusual viewpoints, even if
their identification from
canonical viewpoints is not deficient.
5. Experiment 1
5.1. Participants
Twelve children with Williams syndrome (mean age 11;0, range
7;4 to 15;3 years), 12
normally developing children who were mental-age matches for
the WS group (Mean Age
5;8, Range 4;1 to 7;1 years), 12 normally developing children
who were chronological
age-matches for the WS group (Mean Age 11;11, Range 10;6 to
14;3 years) and 12
undergraduates participated. Children with WS were identified
through the National WS
Association, and had been positively diagnosed by a geneticist;
all but one had also been
diagnosed by the FISH test (the remaining person did not
undergo the test). Normal
children in the MA group were matched individually to children
with WS, using the
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990),
which yields a verbal and
non-verbal (Matrices) score. The latter does not have many
spatial items, and hence does
not unfairly penalize WS children for their spatial impairment.
The mean scores for the
WS children were VerbalZ31.7 (SEZ2.32), MatricesZ18.58
(SEZ1.11); corresponding
scores for the MA-matched controls were 29.33 (SEZ2.33) and
18.25 (SEZ1.52). Scores
for the CA group were VerbalZ59.6 (SEZ1.46) and MatricesZ38
(SEZ0.99). The mean
IQ scores for the three groups were 71.42 (SEZ4.35) for the WS
children, 116.2 (SEZ
4.11) for the MA group, and 123 (SEZ2.50) for the CA controls.
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510492
In addition, the WS children and ten of the MA controls were
tested on the Pattern
Construction Sub-test of the Differential Abilities Scales
(Elliot, 1990), which requires
children to replicate a design using individual component
blocks, and is the hallmark test
used to diagnose the WS spatial impairment. The scores for the
WS children were MZ
81.83, percentileZ1.83, SEZ5.82; scores for the MA controls
were, MZ109.2,
percentileZ58.1, SEZ6.96. The WS scores are in the range
reported by other
investigators (see Mervis et al., 1999). All but two WS children
fell into the 1st percentile
of performance, thus conforming to the reported pattern of
severe spatial deficit. Note that,
although the WS children were matched to normally developing
children on verbal and
non-verbal scores (the KBIT), they performed much worse than
their MA matches on the
Pattern Construction task, as would be expected. All
participants signed informed consent
forms.
5.2. Design, stimuli, and procedures
Participants were asked to name each of a set of 80 full color
pictures of objects which
were presented on a computer screen for 500 ms. The objects
were drawn randomly from a
set of 320 images consisting of 80 objects (listed in Table 1) in
each of four conditions:
(a) canonical view, clear image, (b) canonical view, blurred
image, (c) unusual view, clear
image, and (d) unusual view, blurred image. Canonical views
were deemed those that
exposed all of each object’s relevant parts; many were full-face
views of the object.
Unusual views varied in their orientation, and included
foreshortening along the primary
axis, and selection of views from above or below the object.
Blurred images were created
by editing the clear images using the Gaussian Blur tool (radius
10) in Photoshop. Objects
were drawn from two sources: The ‘Object DataBank’ available
from Michael Tarr (http://
www.cog.brown.edu/wtarr/stimuli.html) and the model set
which accompanied Ray
Dream Studio 5.5. Examples of objects in each of the four
conditions are shown in Fig. 2
and the complete set of images can be viewed at
http://hoffman.psych.udel.edu/
ObjectPicturesForWeb.pps.
Each participant saw 80 different objects with an equal number
of objects presented in
each of the four conditions. All objects were randomly ordered
over eight lists, and
individual WS subjects were assigned the same list as their
controls (both MA and CA).
Across lists, each object was represented equally often in each
of the four presentation
modes. Responses were recorded verbatim. If the participant
was uncertain, the
experimenter encouraged him or her to ‘give your best guess’.
Responses were coded by a person who was blind to participant
group. Each response
was presented individually on a computer screen along with the
target name (i.e. the name
that was used in creating the objects; see Table 1) and the image
of the object that the
participant had viewed when producing the name. The rater used
seven categories,
including correct name, correct definition or use, related
member of same basic category,
correct superordinate category, similar shape, incorrect name,
or do not know. For
example, if a picture of a sunflower was shown, responses
would be coded as follows:
‘sunflower’ (correct name), ‘goes in a vase’ (correct use),
‘daisy’ (related member of same
category), ‘plant’ (correct superordinate), ‘clock’ (similar
shape), ‘truck’ (incorrect name),
http://www.cog.brown.edu/~tarr/stimuli.html
http://www.cog.brown.edu/~tarr/stimuli.html
http://hoffman.psych.udel.edu/ObjectPicturesForWeb.pps
http://hoffman.psych.udel.edu/ObjectPicturesForWeb.pps
Table 1
List of objects used in Experiments 1 and 2
Anchor Jet plane
Banana Key
Barn Kite
Basket Motorcycle
Baseball bat Mug
Bed Pad lock
Bee Pen
Belt Piano
Bike Pitcher
Binoculars Plant
Blender Pliers
Bottle Pot
Bow (archery) Pretzel
Bridge Pumpkin
Brush Refrigerator
Bulb (light bulb) Ring
Cannon Rollerblades
Carrot Ruler
Cassette tape Scissors
Castle Screwdriver
Chain Shovel
Chair Sink
Clock Sports car
Couch Stool
Crayon Stove
Cup Sunflower
Desk Table
Dresser Tank
Drums Tennis racket
Egg (fried) Toothpaste tube
Eye glasses Trash can
Electric fan Truck
Fork Trumpet
Frying pan Turkey
Glass Umbrella
Grill Violin
Guitar Wagon
Hamburger anger Watch
Hair dryer Windmill
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 493
or ‘do not know’. A second rater coded 20% of the responses,
and reliability was 90%.
Where there was disagreement, the first rater’s data were used.
5.3. Results and discussion
An initial analysis examined the accuracy of participants’
labels. Responses in the first
four categories were considered to be correct. Any other
response was scored as incorrect.
Naming accuracy (percent correct) as a function of image
condition and group is shown in
Fig. 2. Examples of objects used in each condition of
Experiment 1
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510494
Fig. 3 (shown as a line graph to emphasize patterns of
interaction and additivity between
groups and viewpoint). Fig. 3a shows data for the clear image
condition and Fig. 3b shows
data for the blurred image condition.
First, it is apparent that we were effective in our manipulation
of the difficulty of
identifying the objects. For both clear and blurred images,
participants were more accurate
in naming objects shown in a canonical than in an unusual
orientation. Clear images were
identified more accurately than blurred ones. These effects of
image quality were
confirmed by significant main effects of Orientation,
F(1,44)Z344.7, P!0.001 and
Clarity, F(1,44)Z384.5, P!0.001. In addition, there was an
Orientation by Clarity
interaction, F(1,44)Z84.7, P!0.001 indicating that the effects of
combining the two
distortions was greater than the additive combination of each
distortion in isolation.
Fig. 3. (a) Experiment 1: Clear Images. Mean percent correct
(S.E.) over condition and group (Ad: Adults; CA:
chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome;
MA:mental age matches). (b) Experiment 1: Blurred
Images. Mean percent correct (S.E.) over condition and group
(Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age matches; WS:
Williams syndrome; MA: mental age matches).
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 495
Across these data, the main effect of Group was significant,
F(3,44)Z8.8, P!001, but
this was due to the adults’ superior performance. Tukey post-
hoc tests showed no
significant differences among the three children’s groups (WS
vs. MA, PZ0.72, WS vs.
CA, PZ0.62, and MA vs. CA, PZ0.12). The adult group,
however was more accurate
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510496
than both the WS group (P!0.002) and MA controls (P!0.001),
and marginally better
than the CA controls (P!0.056). In addition, there was a Clarity
X Group interaction (F(3,
44)Z5.66, P!0.002. To isolate this interaction, we analyzed the
difference between clear
and blurred images as a function of group. Tukey tests revealed
that the WS group showed
smaller effects of blur than either the MA or CA controls
(P!0.01 and 0.03, respectively)
and were indistinguishable from adults (PO0.98).
The data in Fig. 3 also suggest that the effects of viewpoint and
image clarity depended
on group. In order to evaluate the effects of viewpoint, we
analyzed the data separately for
clear and blurred images. For clear images (Fig. 3a), there is a
striking similarity between
the CA and adult groups as well as between the MA and WS
groups. The adults and CA
controls were more accurate across the two viewpoints
(MsZ0.96, 0.93) than the other
two groups (MsZ0.86, 0.83) and also showed a smaller effect of
viewpoint. An analysis of
variance on these data revealed a main effect of Group (F(3,
44)Z6.61. Tukey tests on the
Group effect revealed that the WS group was less accurate than
both the CA and adult
groups (P!0.05 and 0.001, respectively) but did not differ from
MA children (PZ0.87).
The MA group only differed from the adults (P!0.01).
There was also a significant Group X Viewpoint interaction
(F(3,44)Z5.62, P!0.002)
. Planned comparisons of the three children’s groups showed
that the effect of orientation
was larger for WS compared to CA, t(22)Z2.38, P!0.02 and MA
compared to CA,
t(22)Z2.37, P!0.02; The WS and MA groups did not differ,
t(22)Z0.18, PO0.85. It is
possible that the smaller effect of view in the CA group is due
to their performance being at
ceiling in the Canonical condition. However, this seems
unlikely because performance of
all three groups is quite good in this condition, with all means
above 90% (MsZ0.99, 0.96,
0.94, 0.92 for adults, CA, MA, and WS groups). A separate
ANOVA on these data
revealed no significant difference among groups, F(2,33)Z1.41,
PO0.25.
Blurring the image produced a different pattern of results (Fig.
3b). The WS subjects
show a relative improvement in their standing compared to the
other groups, and this
appears to be mainly attributed to the larger impact of blur on
the CA controls. In fact,
the WS group is now indistinguishable from the CA group. In
addition, blurring resulted
in similar effects of orientation for all groups. There were main
effects of Group
(F(3,44)Z9.07, P!0.001 and Orientation (F(1,44)Z280.41,
P!0.001 but no
interaction (F!1). Tukey post hoc tests showed that the main
effect of group was due
to the adults performing more accurately than all three groups
of children (P’s!0.05)
which were not different from each other. (WS vs. MA, PZ0.12,
WS vs. CA, PZ0.99,
and MA vs. CA, PZ0.20).
These results show two main patterns. First, the WS children
have very high
performance for canonical views and clear images and their
performance drops for
unusual views. Their pattern of accuracy across all conditions is
remarkably similar to MA
controls and the performance of both of these groups is worse
than that of normal adults.
The CA group appears to occupy an intermediate position. For
canonical views of clear
images, they are no different from WS children; for unusual
views, they show less impact
of viewpoint, with performance better than both WS children
and MA controls.
Second, once images were blurred, the WS children perform as
well as the CA children.
In fact, all three groups of children showed comparable
accuracy and all groups, including
adults, showed the same relative decline in performance with
unusual views.
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 497
Why the different effects of blur? Showing clear objects from
unusual viewpoints
impaired performance in all of our groups, and in this case, the
CA and adult controls were
less affected by unusual views than the WS and MA groups.
This advantage disappeared
when images were blurred, however, suggesting that a key
ingredient in deciphering
unusual views may be related to analysis of internal features
which is weakened by
blurring. This result is consistent with results reported by
Lawson & Humphreys (1999)
who studied the effects of foreshortening of the main axis of an
object presented as either a
line drawing or a silhouette. Both line drawings and silhouettes
preserve the ‘occluding
contour’ of the object and therefore allow the observer to
extract the object’s main axis of
elongation. Silhouettes, however, eliminate the possibility of
using internal details and
features for identification. Their results showed that extreme
foreshortening, similar to the
unusual views in Experiment 1, reduced identification accuracy
for both kinds of objects
but particularly for the silhouettes, suggesting that internal
features become particularly
important clues to object identity when axis information is no
longer available.
The greater effect of unusual views on MA and WS subjects
compared to CA controls
in the clear condition suggests that the latter group is better at
utilizing the internal details
of objects to identify them from unusual viewpoints. We might
expect then that if these
features were more difficult to perceive—as in the blur
condition—the CA’s advantage
would disappear and that is what we observed. When objects
were blurred, all three groups
of children were comparable. Interestingly, of the three groups
of children, the WS group
was the least affected by image blur suggesting that they may
rely on the occluding
contour of the object more than control children, even for clear
images. This is consistent
with other evidence suggesting that people with WS may have
trouble focusing attention
on subparts of a larger pattern. For example, Hoffman et al.
(2003) reported that in a
variation of the block construction task, WS children were
unable to ignore nearby blocks
when they were cued to attend to a single block in the model.
The foregoing evidence relies on measures of accuracy, and thus
might miss some fine-
grained qualitative differences in the way that WS children
label objects. Accordingly, we
analyzed the distribution of responses across the seven response
categories for the four
groups (see Fig. 4). The number of responses in each response
category for each subject
was entered into a c
2
test (SPSS Crosstabs). The only significant interaction of Group
with
a Response Category was for Correct Definition or Use (X
2
(15)Z28.9, P!0.02),
reflecting a slightly greater tendency for MA controls to label
pictures in terms of their
definition or use. Otherwise, the distribution of responses
across the various categories was
similar for all four groups (c
2
s p values ranged from 0.11 for the ‘Do not Know’ category
to 0.68 for the ‘Same Category’ response).
A final set of analyses examined whether the four groups
differed in which objects they
found difficult to identify. We computed the average accuracy
for each of the 80 objects,
collapsed across the 4 image quality conditions, for each group.
We then computed the
correlation between all pairs of groups for accuracy on the 80
objects. The correlations
were significantly greater than zero (P!0.001 for all 6
correlations) and ranged from 0.67
(WS vs. adult groups) to 0.76 (WS vs. MA groups). None of the
pairs of correlations
showed reliable differences (all PsO0.17), suggesting that there
was a high degree of
similarity among all four groups in terms of which objects were
easy or difficult to
identify. We also asked whether object identification accuracy
depended on object
Fig. 4. Experiment 1: Distribution of response types (M percent,
S.E.) over group (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological
age matches; WS: Williams syndrome; MA:mental age
matches).
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510498
complexity. Two independent raters rank ordered the entire set
of 80 objects in terms of
their complexity (correlation between ratersZ0.83). These
complexity ratings were not
reliably correlated with average accuracy of each object for any
of the groups
(correlationsZ0.09, 0.10, 0.21, 0.17 for the WS, MA, CA, and
adults, respectively,
alphaZ0.05, two-tailed test). Both of these analyses suggest
similarity across groups in
the relative difficulty of individual objects.
As a whole, the results suggest that object recognition is
surprisingly good in children
with WS, certainly much better than might be expected on the
basis of their performance in
other areas of spatial cognition such as block construction.
Indeed, on the basis of their
standardized verbal scores, they were exactly where one would
predict they should be—
comparable to MA controls in every condition. From this
perspective, it is surprising that
they were no different from CA controls in identifying objects
presented as clear canonical
images, or those presented as blurred images in either canonical
or unusual viewpoints.
The WS performance is particularly striking, in light of the fact
that recognizing objects
under these circumstances requires that they match an image
that they likely never
encountered with a stored representation of the object. This
matching likely involves
comparing overlap between two shapes, even though the
unusual orientations obscure
aspects of the objects’ shapes.
It would be tempting to conclude that WS children are capable
of representing and
extracting an object’s part structure from partial information.
However, because the
stimuli were full color renditions of objects, it is always
possible that object recognition in
WS depends more on surface features (such as color and
texture) than the object’s spatial
structure (e.g. Tanaka & Presnell, 1999; Tanaka, Weiskopf &
Williams, 2001). Moreover,
the fact that recognizing objects in unusual orientations can
sometimes activate more
parietal areas (Sugio et al., 1999) raises the question of whether
a more difficult version of
the task might reveal additional differences between WS
children and the normal groups.
In order to test this possibility, we carried out a second
experiment, using line drawings of
objects. In these cases, objects can only be recognized by their
shapes, not by surface
information such as color or texture.
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 499
6. Experiment 2
6.1. Participants
Children were the same as those who participated in Experiment
1, with the exception
of one MA control child, who replaced a child who could no
longer participate in our
studies. The new mental age-matched group was still well
matched by KBIT verbal (WS
MeanZ33, S.E.Z2.08, control MeanZ33, S.E.Z2.26) and
Matrices scores (WS MeanZ
18, S.E.Z1.06, control MeanZ19, S.E.Z1.30). The DAS scores of
the WS children had a
Mean in the 1st percentile, while the MA matched controls’
Mean was in the 55th
percentile. All participants were tested after Experiment 1.
6.2. Design, stimuli, and procedures
These were identical to Experiment 1, except that the objects
were converted to black and
white line drawings (see Fig. 5). Only the clear versions were
shown, resulting in a total of
Fig. 5. Examples of objects used in both conditions of
Experiment 2.
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510500
160 stimuli (80 objects X 2 views). Each subject saw all 80
objects with the view (canonical
or unusual) chosen randomly under the constraint that there was
an equal number of
canonical and unusual views. Eight lists of objects in a random
order were created and
subjects were randomly assigned a list, matching WS children
and their controls on the same
list. Across the lists, each object was shown equally often from
the two viewpoints.
6.3. Results and discussion
As in Experiment 1, responses were scored as correct if they
were in any of the first four
response categories (correct name, correct definition or use,
related category member, or
correct superordinate category); all other responses were scored
as incorrect. Fig. 6 shows
the percent correct for the four groups as a function of
orientation (canonical or unusual).
Consistent with what we observed in Experiment 1 using full
color solid object images,
identification was severely reduced when objects were shown in
a unusual orientation,
F(1,44)Z524.7, P!0.001. In addition, there was a main effect of
Group, F(3,44)Z20.9,
P!0.001) as well as an Orientation X Group interaction,
F(3,44)Z7.3, P!0.001 that
reflects a similar pattern to the one we observed in Experiment
1 for the clear image
condition. Post-hoc Tukey tests revealed two homogeneous
subsets of the Group variable.
WS and MA matches formed one set while the other set
consisted of CA matches and
Adults. Members of one subset differed significantly from each
member of the other set,
(P!0.001) but not from each other. (WS vs. MA, PZ0.81; CA vs.
AD, PZ0.78).
Separate analyses were also carried out for the canonical and
unusual view conditions,
with a similar result. For both conditions, the WS and MA
matches formed one subgroup
and the CA and AD subjects formed another. For canonical
views, all groups were above
Fig. 6. Experiment 2: M percent correct (S.E.) over condition
and group. (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age
matches; WS: Williams syndrome; MA:mental age matches).
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 501
90% accuracy (MsZ0.92, 0.92, 0.98, 0.97 for WS, MA, CA and
adults respective). The
WS and MA groups did not differ (PZ0.97) nor did CA and AD
groups (PZ0.92).
Members of each of the subgroups, however, differed from
members of the other group (all
P’s!0.003). For unusual views, WS and MA children were
comparable, (MsZ0.54, 0.58,
respectively, PZ0.65) as were the CA and adult groups
(MsZ0.71, 0.75, respectively,
PZ0.65). Members of each of the subgroups differed from
members of the other groups
(all P’s!0.001).
The Orientation X Group interaction was further analyzed using
the difference between
canonical and unusual views for each group. Post-hoc Tukey
tests revealed that the WS
group differed from the CA (P!0.02) and AD (P!0.001) groups
but not from MA
controls (PZ0.59) and the MA group only differed from the
Adults (P!0.03). One might
worry that the larger effect of orientation on WS subjects
compared to CA controls is due
to the CA controls being close to ceiling in the canonical view
condition. However, the
WS group was above 90% accuracy in this condition, as in the
canonical clear condition of
Experiment 1. Although the interaction should be viewed with
caution, the findings here of
the larger orientation effect among WS children than CA
controls replicates the findings of
Experiment 1. This suggests there is a real relative weakness for
unusual orientations
among WS children relative to CA, but not MA matched
children.
These results show that when subjects must rely purely on
‘shape’ information to label
objects, WS children are remarkably good at identifying
objects—over 90% with
canonical viewpoints. They are remarkably similar to the MA
controls in identification
accuracy, and this holds even when objects are shown in
unusual views that produce
substantial deficits in the performance of normal adults. These
results are similar to the
clear image condition of Experiment 1 which showed that CA
subjects were more accurate
than WS subjects and less affected by unusual views.
Once again, we analyzed the distribution of different error types
to determine whether
the four groups differed in the kinds of errors they made. These
data are shown in Fig. 7
Fig. 7. Experiment 2: Distribution of responses types (M
percent, S.E.) over group (Ad: Adults; CA:
chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome;
MA:mental age matches).
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510502
and suggest that all four groups have roughly similar profiles
across the different response
categories. The number of responses in each response category
for each subject was
entered into a c
2
test (SPSS Crosstabs) which resulted in significant interactions
between
Group and the following categories: Correct name (Pearson X
2
(81)Z105.6, P!0.05),
Correct Definition or Use (X
2
(15)Z31.3, P!0.01), Related Category member (X2 (27)Z
55.9, P!0.01). The interaction with the ‘Do not Know’ category
was marginally
significant (X
2
(48)Z64.2, P!0.06). This pattern of significance reflects the
higher use of
correct names for objects in the CA and Adult groups compared
to WS and MA children
and may partly reflect more sophisticated vocabularies (as
shown by their KBIT scores) or
relatively less difficulty in retrieving the correct names. When
this analysis was restricted
to just the WS and MA groups, none of the interactions
approached significance (all P’sO
0.14), suggesting that these two groups has a similar profile of
responses across the various
categories.
A final analysis examined whether groups differed in terms of
which objects they found
difficult to identify. Here we were only able to compare the WS
and MA groups because
accuracy for many individual objects was at 100% for the CA
and AD groups. We
computed the average accuracy for each of the 80 objects,
collapsed across the 2
orientation conditions. The correlation between the WS and MA
groups across the 80
objects was 0.74, which is significantly different from 0,
(t(78)Z9.76, P!0.001),
suggesting that WS children and their MA controls were similar,
not only in their overall
accuracy of identification but also in terms of which objects
they found easy or difficult to
identify. We also examined whether accuracy depended on the
rated complexity of the
objects for these two groups. For WS children, the correlation
was 0.13 and for the MA
group, it was 0.14, neither of which was significant (WS:
t(78)Z1.16, PO0.05 and MA:
t(78)Z1.25, PO0.05).
It is important to note that the WS and MA groups, who were
relatively less accurate
than the CA and adult groups, were nonetheless quite accurate
in this task in an absolute
sense. On average, they correctly recognized approximately
74% of the line drawings
compared to 85% for the CA and AD groups. Even in the most
difficult condition, in which
objects were shown in unusual orientations, WS and MA
children correctly recognized 54
and 58% of the line drawings, respectively. This is quite high,
given that the random
probability of guessing the correct object name is far lower than
1 in 40, which would be
chance in the extremely unlikely event that the subject knew the
database for the objects.
Some have conjectured that identifying objects under highly
unusual viewpoints may
require a form of cognitive problem-solving, as the visual
system may not be designed to
automatically compute these extreme situations (Farah, 2000).
Under the circumstances of
severe degradation (line drawing and unusual orientation), this
requirement may have
been accentuated. If so, the difference between groups is
understandable.
Comparison with the results of Experiment 1 shows that
removing color and texture
cues impaired subjects’ performance, with the largest decrement
seen when objects were
presented in an unusual view. This is consistent with the notion
that multiple mechanisms
may be at work in recognition of unusual views and that other
cues to object identity, such
as color or texture, become particularly important when shape
information becomes
sufficiently degraded (Farah, 2000). In order to see whether the
decrement from unusual
views was larger for line drawings than for full-color objects,
an additional analysis of
B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 503
variance was conducted comparing the corresponding conditions
across the two
experiments (i.e. the data from Experiment 2 vs. those from the
clear image condition
of Experiment 1). In this analysis, the Group X Experiment X
View interaction was not
significant (F(3,44)!1), suggesting that the performance
decrement associated with
unusual views for each group was similar for solid full color
objects and line drawings.
7. General discussion and conclusions
The present experiments used full color pictures and line
drawings of common objects
to examine object identification in children with WS compared
to normally developing
children of the same mental (MA) and chronological age (CA).
The difficulty of object
identification was manipulated by blurring objects (full color
objects) and portraying them
in unusual orientations (color objects and line drawings).
Overall, WS and MA children
were remarkably similar under all conditions, both in terms of
accuracy and in various
fine-grained details of performance such as which objects they
found easy or difficult to
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx
Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx

More Related Content

Similar to Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx

Musical training shapes structural brain development
Musical training shapes structural brain developmentMusical training shapes structural brain development
Musical training shapes structural brain developmentpacojavierradio
 
Science Article by Murakawa
Science Article by MurakawaScience Article by Murakawa
Science Article by MurakawaAdonai Cruz
 
Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...
Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...
Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...Dr Brendan O'Sullivan
 
Duong_H_2008b
Duong_H_2008bDuong_H_2008b
Duong_H_2008bHao Duong
 
The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...
The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...
The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...rockbam
 
Fluid reasoning and the developing brain
Fluid reasoning and the developing brainFluid reasoning and the developing brain
Fluid reasoning and the developing braincarlafig
 
Adolescent brain 2
Adolescent brain 2Adolescent brain 2
Adolescent brain 2ashit
 
The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docx
The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docxThe Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docx
The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docxtodd701
 
Cerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain Phenotype
Cerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain PhenotypeCerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain Phenotype
Cerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain PhenotypeMiguel E. Rentería, PhD
 
TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...
TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...
TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...Patricia Cedeño
 
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...James Allen
 
ענת ברנע נוירופידבק
ענת ברנע   נוירופידבקענת ברנע   נוירופידבק
ענת ברנע נוירופידבקBioFeedbackIsrael
 
Heraud And Tobin S C R Editorial Paper
Heraud And  Tobin  S C R Editorial PaperHeraud And  Tobin  S C R Editorial Paper
Heraud And Tobin S C R Editorial Paperuvperson
 
Attention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move Faster
Attention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move FasterAttention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move Faster
Attention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move FasterAmy Roman
 
Morphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxies
Morphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxiesMorphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxies
Morphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxiesSérgio Sacani
 

Similar to Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx (20)

Musical training shapes structural brain development
Musical training shapes structural brain developmentMusical training shapes structural brain development
Musical training shapes structural brain development
 
Science Article by Murakawa
Science Article by MurakawaScience Article by Murakawa
Science Article by Murakawa
 
Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...
Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...
Visuomotor Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Study by Ryuta Kawashima,...
 
Duong_H_2008b
Duong_H_2008bDuong_H_2008b
Duong_H_2008b
 
The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...
The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...
The Impact of Neurodegeneration on Network Connectivity: A Study of Change De...
 
Fluid reasoning and the developing brain
Fluid reasoning and the developing brainFluid reasoning and the developing brain
Fluid reasoning and the developing brain
 
Adolescent brain 2
Adolescent brain 2Adolescent brain 2
Adolescent brain 2
 
Emsa1
Emsa1Emsa1
Emsa1
 
Rich club
Rich clubRich club
Rich club
 
The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docx
The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docxThe Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docx
The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Item and AssociativeReco.docx
 
Cerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain Phenotype
Cerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain PhenotypeCerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain Phenotype
Cerebral Asymmetry: A Quantitative, Multifactorial and Plastic Brain Phenotype
 
TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...
TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...
TRANSCRANIAL LASER PHOTOBIOMODULATION THERAPY IN PATIENTS WITH COMMUNICATION ...
 
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
 
Dimosfismo-sexual-flounder.pdf
Dimosfismo-sexual-flounder.pdfDimosfismo-sexual-flounder.pdf
Dimosfismo-sexual-flounder.pdf
 
ענת ברנע נוירופידבק
ענת ברנע   נוירופידבקענת ברנע   נוירופידבק
ענת ברנע נוירופידבק
 
Heraud And Tobin S C R Editorial Paper
Heraud And  Tobin  S C R Editorial PaperHeraud And  Tobin  S C R Editorial Paper
Heraud And Tobin S C R Editorial Paper
 
Current Projects Summary
Current Projects SummaryCurrent Projects Summary
Current Projects Summary
 
2010_JAS_Database
2010_JAS_Database2010_JAS_Database
2010_JAS_Database
 
Attention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move Faster
Attention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move FasterAttention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move Faster
Attention Makes Moving Objects Be Perceived To Move Faster
 
Morphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxies
Morphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxiesMorphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxies
Morphologies of mid-IR variability-selected AGN host galaxies
 

More from danhaley45372

Your initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docx
Your initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docxYour initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docx
Your initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docxYour initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docx
Your initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docxYour initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docx
Your initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docxYour initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docx
Your initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docxYour initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docx
Your initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docx
Your essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docxYour essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docx
Your essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docx
Your initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docxYour initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docx
Your initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docx
Your individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docxYour individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docx
Your individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docx
Your HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docxYour HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docx
Your HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docx
Your Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docxYour Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docx
Your Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docx
Your country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docxYour country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docx
Your country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docxdanhaley45372
 
Your have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docx
Your have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docxYour have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docx
Your have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your group presentationWhat you need to do.docx
Your group presentationWhat you need to do.docxYour group presentationWhat you need to do.docx
Your group presentationWhat you need to do.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docx
Your contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docxYour contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docx
Your contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docxYour good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docxYour good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docx
Your goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docxYour goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docx
Your goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docx
Your essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docxYour essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docx
Your essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docxdanhaley45372
 
Your future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docx
Your future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docxYour future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docx
Your future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docxdanhaley45372
 
Your friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docx
Your friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docxYour friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docx
Your friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docxdanhaley45372
 

More from danhaley45372 (20)

Your initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docx
Your initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docxYour initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docx
Your initial post should be 2-3 paragraphs in length.Inclu.docx
 
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docxYour initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2,  January 21st at 4.docx
 
Your initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docx
Your initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docxYour initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docx
Your initial post should be at least 450+ words and in APA forma.docx
 
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docxYour initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docx
Your initial post should be made during Unit 2, january 21st at 4.docx
 
Your initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docx
Your initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docxYour initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docx
Your initial post should be made during, Submissions after this time.docx
 
Your essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docx
Your essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docxYour essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docx
Your essay should address the following.(a) How  is the biologic.docx
 
Your initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docx
Your initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docxYour initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docx
Your initial post is due by midnight (1159 PM) on Thursday. You mus.docx
 
Your individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docx
Your individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docxYour individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docx
Your individual sub-topic written (MIN of 1, MAX 3 pages)You.docx
 
Your HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docx
Your HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docxYour HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docx
Your HR project to develop a centralized model of deliveri.docx
 
Your Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docx
Your Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docxYour Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docx
Your Immersion Project for this course is essentially ethnographic r.docx
 
Your country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docx
Your country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docxYour country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docx
Your country just overthrew its dictator, and you are the newly .docx
 
Your have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docx
Your have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docxYour have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docx
Your have been contracted by HealthFirst Hospital Foundation (HHF),.docx
 
Your group presentationWhat you need to do.docx
Your group presentationWhat you need to do.docxYour group presentationWhat you need to do.docx
Your group presentationWhat you need to do.docx
 
Your contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docx
Your contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docxYour contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docx
Your contribution(s) must add significant information to the dis.docx
 
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docxYour good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At th.docx
 
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docxYour good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docx
Your good friends have just adopted a four-year-old child. At this p.docx
 
Your goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docx
Your goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docxYour goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docx
Your goals as the IT architect and IT security specialist are to.docx
 
Your essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docx
Your essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docxYour essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docx
Your essay should address the following problem.(a) What is .docx
 
Your future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docx
Your future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docxYour future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docx
Your future financial needs will be based on the income you can reas.docx
 
Your friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docx
Your friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docxYour friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docx
Your friend Lydia is having difficulty taking in the informati.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.arsicmarija21
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Jisc
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 

Perception of shapes targeting local and globalprocesses in .docx

  • 1. Perception of shapes targeting local and global processes in autism spectrum disorders Emma J. Grinter,1 Murray T. Maybery,1 Elizabeth Pellicano,1,3 Johanna C. Badcock2 and David R. Badcock1 1School of Psychology, University of Western Australia; 2Centre for Clinical Research in Neuropsychiatry/Graylands Hospital, School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, University of Western Australia; 3Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, UK Background: Several researchers have found evidence for impaired global processing in the dorsal visual stream in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). However, support for a similar pattern of visual processing in the ventral visual stream is less consistent. Critical to resolving the inconsistency is the assessment of local and global form processing ability. Methods: Within the visual domain, radial frequency (RF) patterns – shapes formed by sinusoidally varying the radius of a circle to add ‘bumps’ of a certain number to a circle – can be used to examine local and global form perception. Typically developing children and children with an ASD discriminated between circles and RF patterns that are processed either locally (RF24) or globally (RF3). Results: Children with an ASD required greater shape deformation to identify RF3 shapes compared to typically developing children, consistent
  • 2. with difficulty in global processing in the ventral stream. No group difference was observed for RF24 shapes, suggesting intact local ventral-stream processing. Conclusions: These outcomes support the position that a deficit in global visual processing is present in ASDs, consistent with the notion of Weak Central Coherence. Keywords: Autism, local processing, global processing, ventral visual pathway, radial frequency patterns. Abbreviations: ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TD, typically developing; WCC, Weak Central Coherence; EPF, Enhanced Perceptual Functioning; RF, radial frequency; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised. Over the past three decades, several research groups have proposed that the cognitive profile in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) is characterised by diffi- culties in complex information processing (Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic, & Faubert, 2005; Frith, 1989; Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegal, 1997). In particular, Weak Central Coherence (WCC) theory suggests that individuals with an ASD demonstrate a relative fail- ure to extract overall meaning, resulting in a reduced awareness of the global aspects of stimuli in con- junction with a relatively heightened awareness of the details or parts of stimuli (Frith, 1989; Happé, 1999). Several studies have shown, however, that integration abilities might be intact in ASDs (Mot- tron, Burack, Stauder, & Robaey, 1999; Ozonoff, Strayer, McMahon, & Filloux, 1994; Plaisted, Swet- tenham, & Rees, 1999). To account for these data, others have proposed, amongst several other hypotheses, that individuals with an ASD show ‘Enhanced Perceptual Functioning’ (EPF; Mottron, Dawson, Souliéres, Hubert, & Burack, 2006) in which the salience of local features is enhanced
  • 3. without corresponding deficits in integrative capa- bilities. Research assessing visual capabilities is uniquely positioned to clarify which of these accounts best explains atypical processing in ASDs since processes known to engage global integration can be examined (Bell & Badcock, 2008; Loffler, 2008). At the earliest stages of visual perception, neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) extract information about local characteristics of stimuli to provide a spatially limited signal for perception (DeValois & DeValois, 1988). Because the classical receptive fields are small, however, V1 information must be integrated to enable global perception at later stages of both the dorsal (Movshon, 1990) and ventral (Loffler, 2008) visual streams. There has been con- siderable interest in visual processing in autism in recent years, and much research has investigated local and global processing in the dorsal and ventral visual pathways (see Kaiser & Shiffrar, in press; Simmons et al., 2009, for reviews). Several researchers have found higher thresholds in children with autism when compared to typically developing (TD) children on tasks targeting global dorsal stream processing that require the identifi- cation of direction of motion or the presence of coherent motion in a field of moving dots (e.g., Milne et al., 2002; Pellicano, Gibson, Maybery, Durkin, & Badcock, 2005; Spencer et al., 2000; Spencer & O’Brien, 2006; Tsermentseli et al., 2008), in con- junction with evidence of intact local dorsal stream processing (e.g., Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic, & Fau- bert, 2003; Pellicano et al., 2005). While this has been interpreted as evidence for a disturbance in higher-level global processing in the dorsal pathway
  • 4. in ASDs (e.g., Bertone et al., 2003; Pellicano et al., 2005), the data for a similar pattern of visual processing in the ventral visual stream is lessConflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51:6 (2010), pp 717–724 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02203.x � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA consistent, with some studies reporting equivalent (e.g., Blake, Turner, Smoski, Pozdol, & Stone, 2003; Milne et al., 2006; Spencer et al., 2000) and others impaired thresholds (e.g., Spencer & O’Brien, 2006; Tsermentseli et al., 2008) on measures of ventral stream global processing. Importantly, many of these studies failed to examine both local and global processing within a specified pathway or use similar stimulus characteristics to assess the two forms of processing (but see Bertone et al., 2003, 2005), making it difficult to draw firm conclusions about local and global processing in ASD. The present study assessed both types of visual functioning in the ventral stream using shapes in which differences between the local and global stimuli were minimised. Radial frequency (RF; Wil- kinson, Wilson, & Habak, 1998) patterns are closed- contour shapes created by deforming a circle. The
  • 5. deformation is produced by sinusoidally varying the radius as a function of polar angle. The number of cycles of modulation in 360� corresponds to the RF number and when the amplitude of the modulating function is set to zero, a circle is produced (Fig- ure 1a). Three cycles of appropriate amplitude create a shape that looks like a triangle with rounded cor- ners (Figure 1b), and 24 cycles result in a circle with 24 ‘bumps’ (Figure 1c). For RF patterns of high fre- quency (e.g., RF24), performance for discriminating the whole shape from a circle is better than when only part of the closed shape is deformed (Loffler, Wilson, & Wilkinson, 2003), but only by an amount that can be explained by probability summation of the detection of independent local features. For this reason, the discrimination of high RF shapes is thought to be achieved by the local orientation-tuned cells in V1 (Wilkinson et al., 2003). In contrast, there is evidence that curvature and position information is pooled along the entire circumference of the pat- tern for low radial frequencies (Bell & Badcock, 2008; Bell, Badcock, Wilson, & Wilkinson, 2007), consistent with global signal integration in shapes with up to about ten cycles of modulation (Bell & Badcock, 2009; Loffler, 2008). FMRI data is consis- tent with global pooling of orientation information to extract global shape information further along the ventral pathway in V4 for RF3 patterns (Wilkinson et al., 2000). Here we report the first study to use these stimuli with an ASD population. We used RF3 and RF24 patterns to assess global and local ventral stream processing, respectively. Loffler et al.’s (2003) examination of RF shapes showed that RF3 shapes evoke active global pooling of local curvature esti-
  • 6. mates, whereas RF24 shapes involve probability summation of local curvature estimates. Consistent with the idea that there is a potential difference in how RF shapes are processed, Bell, Wilkinson, Wilson, Loffler, and Badcock (2009) showed that discrimination of low RF patterns is underpinned by multiple narrow-band contour shape channels. In selecting the RF shapes for this study, we were careful to choose clear examples for which global processing of local curvature estimates was (RF3) or was not (RF24) selectively activated. This allowed determination of separate local and global contri- butions to shape processing. The WCC account can be used to predict that, relative to a neurotypical comparison group, indi- viduals with an ASD should show elevated thresh- olds on the RF3 task (i.e., poor global processing in the ventral stream), accompanied by either equivalent or lower thresholds on the RF24 task (i.e., intact/superior local processing in the ventral stream). Alternatively, EPF theory can be used to predict that individuals with an ASD should dem- onstrate equivalent or lower thresholds on the RF24 task (i.e., enhanced local processing), but, critically, equivalent thresholds on the RF3 task (i.e., intact global processing) relative to TD individuals. Method Group comparisons Brock, Jarrold, Farran, Laws, and Riby (2007; see also Jarrold & Brock, 2004) demonstrated that substantial problems can be introduced when using conventional methods to match or statistically control for psycho-
  • 7. metric variables, such as verbal and non-verbal ability, on which children with a developmental disorder and TD children differ systematically. The analytic approach they advocate is to regress each experimental variable onto the relevant psychometric variables for a large and diverse group of TD children, and then use the regression function to generate expected scores for the (a) (b) (c) Figure 1 Examples of (a) a circle (b) an RF3 stimulus and (c) an RF24 stimulus 718 Emma J. Grinter et al. � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. children in the clinical group, against which their actual scores are then compared. Thomas et al. (2009) argued that this approach allows meaningful group compari- sons to be made. Accordingly, we adopted Brock et al.’s innovative approach in our group comparisons of RF pattern performance. Participants Children with an ASD were recruited through an autism register, speech pathologists and participation in pre- vious research projects at the University of Western Australia. The 38 8–16-year-old children (32 males) in the ASD sample had received an independent clinical diagnosis from a multidisciplinary team of either
  • 8. autistic disorder (N = 30), Asperger’s disorder (N = 2) or pervasive developmental disorder – not otherwise specified (N = 6), according to DSM-IV (American Psy- chiatric Association, 1994) criteria. Also, each ASD child either met full criteria for autism (N = 34) or scored above the cut-off in two of the three symptom domains on the Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) (social interaction domain: M = 20.16, SD = 6.36; communi- cation domain: M = 15.94, SD = 5.04; repetitive behaviours domain: M = 6.51, SD = 2.65). Children were excluded from the ASD sample if they had a diagnosis of any medical condition (e.g., epilepsy) or other developmental disorder (e.g., ADHD), hearing or visual problems, or were taking medication known to impact on perception or cognition. TD children (N = 132, 8–16 years old, 70 males) recruited from a metropolitan school participated after parents com- pleted a brief screening questionnaire ensuring the children had no medical, hearing or visual problems or history of developmental difficulties. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all children prior to participation in accordance with the policies of the University of Western Australia’s Ethics Committee. Children wore their optical corrections for visual tasks when required but were otherwise unscreened. How- ever, since both RF3 and RF24 patterns are composed of the same spatial frequency content, any group dif- ferences in acuity would impact on both patterns equally. The groups were well matched for chronological age, t(168) = .96, p = .34, and non-verbal ability, t(168) = .31, p = .76, as measured by the Matrix Reasoning subscale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children – Version IV (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003; see
  • 9. Table 1). The ASD group had significantly lower verbal ability, as measured by the Vocabulary subscale of the WISC-IV, than the TD group, t(168) = 5.46, p < .01, consistent with communication difficulties being a core characteristic of autism. All children were considered high-functioning and were attending mainstream schools. The TD group contained more females than the ASD group, v2(1) = 14.12, p < .01. Stimuli RF patterns were presented on an LG L1730SF touch screen driven by a Sony Vaio VGNSZ34GP laptop computer. The 1024 · 768 pixel screen had a refresh rate of 75Hz and a mean luminance of 30 cd/m2. RF patterns were created following Bell et al. (2007). The RF patterns were formed according to the following equation: rðhÞ¼ rmeanð1þ A sinðxh þ uÞ ð1Þ where r and h (in radians) are the polar coordinates, rmean is the pattern’s mean radius, and A, x and u are the amplitude of modulation of the radius, radial frequency (RF), and phase of the pattern, respectively. RF patterns were always presented with random phases, rendering it impossible for the observer to predict the exact location of the lobes from trial to trial. All RF patterns had a mean radius of 1.5� with a centre- to-centre separation of 3.75�. The luminance profile of a radial cross section approximated a fourth derivative of a Gaussian set at 99% contrast and had a peak spatial frequency of 8c/�. Radial frequencies (x) of 3 and 24 were employed (Figure 1).
  • 10. Procedure The WISC Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subscales were given first, followed by the two RF tasks in randomised order, and forming part of a larger test battery. Each RF task began with 35 practice trials that were administered under the same conditions as the test trials to ensure that children could meet the task demands. For the psychophysical tasks, testing was conducted in a darkened room at a viewing distance of .75 m. Viewing was binocular and auditory feedback (a computer-generated tone) was provided after each practice and test trial. Two shapes were presented simultaneously for 200 ms, one a circle and the other a RF3 or RF24 pattern depending on the task. There was no time limit for response, and a 1 s delay separated the child’s touch response and the presentation of the next stimulus. Children were told: ‘In this task you will see two shapes come up on the screen. On one side the shape will be a perfect circle. On the other, the shape will not be a perfect circle, it will ‘‘have lots of little bumps around it’’ [for RF24 patterns] or ‘‘look like a squashed egg’’ [for RF3 patterns]. What you have to do is work out which side of the screen ‘‘has the shape that looks like a squashed egg’’ or ‘‘has the bumpy shape’’ – the one that’s not a perfect circle – and then touch that side of the screen.’ To ensure they understood the instructions, Table 1 Participant characteristics Measures Children with ASD (N = 38)
  • 11. Typically developing children (N = 132) Age (years) Mean 11.88 12.15 SD 2.54 1.94 Range 8.17–16.92 8.83–15.83 WISC Vocabulary (scaled score) Mean 8.95 11.47 SD 2.75 2.40 Range 4–15 6–19 WISC Matrix Reasoning (scaled score) Mean 10.34 10.06 SD 2.91 2.55 Range 3-16 2-18 Shape perception in autism 719 � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. children were then shown two example cards that resembled the images in Figure 1 and asked which ones they would choose. The method of constant stimuli was used to control stimulus presentation (7 stimulus levels, 15 trials per level, taking approximately 6 minutes) and thresholds were obtained from the psychometric function by fitting the equation:
  • 12. Y ¼ 7:5þ 7:5 1þexpðthreshold�xr Þ ð2Þ where threshold yields the 75% correct level, exp is the exponential function, r is a scalar determining the slope of the psychometric function, Y is the number correct out of 15, and x is the amplitude level. At least 60% of variance had to be accounted for by the psychometric function for the threshold for that observer to be included in the analyses. Each child was given two opportunities to meet this criterion if required (RF3: 6% in ASD group, 9% in TD group; RF24: 0% in ASD group, 3% in TD group). Children who did not meet the criterion on either trial run were excluded from the results for that task (RF3: 5% in ASD group, 7% in TD group, RF24: 5% in ASD group, 3% in TD group). Results Data for each group were screened for normality, and outliers (>3 SDs from the mean) were excluded (6% for RF3 task; 2% for RF24 task). The final sample size and the descriptive statistics for each task are presented in Table 2. Regression analyses Following Brock et al. (2007), gender, age, verbal ability and non-verbal ability were used as predictors to conduct simple linear regression on the dependent variables for the TD children. The resulting regres- sion equations were then used to generate predicted scores for the ASD children. Next, residuals were
  • 13. calculated by subtracting the expected score from the observed score for each child with an ASD. These residuals were then standardised by dividing by the standard error of the regression estimate. If ability on each of the tasks developed in line with these predictors, then the mean standardised scores for the ASD children should be zero. The mean standardised threshold for children with an ASD for RF3 patterns was .00088 (SD = .0014), which is significantly above zero, t(31) = 3.68, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .65, consistent with them requiring a greater amplitude of distortion to dis- criminate an RF3 from a circle than TD children (Figure 2a). The mean standardised threshold for the ASD group for RF24 patterns did not differ signifi- cantly from zero, t(33) = .59, p = .56, Cohen’s d = ).06, indicating that there was no significant difference between the two groups on this task (Fig- ure 2b).1 To examine whether the higher thresholds for the ASD group on the RF3 task were the result of a developmental delay in the processing of these shapes, rate of change in RF3 thresholds as a func- tion of age was compared for the two groups. There was no significant group difference in slopes as a function of age for both the RF24, t(156) = .51, p = .61 (TD mean slope = )4.42 · 10)5, ASD mean slope = )1.05 · 10)4) and RF3, t(142) = .19, p = .85 (TD mean slope = )2.81 · 10)4, ASD mean slope = )4.07 · 10)4) tasks. Discussion The aim of the current study was to assess local and
  • 14. global ventral stream functioning in children with an ASD and TD children. Importantly, we found that, relative to TD children, children with an ASD obtained significantly higher RF3 shape discrimina- tion thresholds; they required a larger distortion of the RF3 pattern to be able to discern a difference between the RF shape and the circle, indicating a difficulty with global processing within the ventral visual stream. In contrast, there was no group dif- ference for thresholds on the RF24 task, indicating intact local processing in the ventral visual stream. Poor performance on the RF3 task cannot be attrib- uted to overall poor performance on psychophysical tasks, or to uniformly weak inputs from local pro- cesses to global processes. Rather, these data sug- gest that the early stages of visual form perception are intact for individuals with an ASD, but the mechanisms required to combine local signals into a global form percept function less effectively for this group. This interpretation is consistent with WCC theory which asserts that global processing is anomalous in this population (Frith, 1989; Happé & Booth, 2008). Table 2 RF pattern modulation (A in eqn 1) thresholds for the ASD and TD groups Task Children with ASD Typically developing RF3 N 32 114 Mean .0137 .0111 SD .0044 .0033 Range .0068–.0240 .0036–.0210 RF24 N 34 126 Mean .0029 .0026
  • 15. SD .0009 .0006 Range .0018–.0046 .0014–.0034 1 These outcomes mirror those from more traditional analytic methods which showed that ASD children had significantly higher thresholds on the RF3 task, F(1, 141) = 7.14, p < .01, Np 2 = .05, and did not differ from TD children on the RF24 task, F(1, 154) = .13, p = .72, Np 2 = .001, when the groups (which are matched for age and non-verbal ability) were compared using ANCOVA with gender and verbal ability as covariates. 720 Emma J. Grinter et al. � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. In addition to impaired global processing, WCC theory predicts superior, or at least intact, local processing abilities. In the current study, the ASD group demonstrated no difference in ability to dis- criminate an RF24 pattern from a circle compared to the TD group. At first glance, these results appear to conflict with outcomes from a recent study which assessed sensitivity to first- and second-order defined patterns in children with an ASD and IQ-matched TD children (Bertone et al., 2005).
  • 16. Bertone et al. reported that their ASD group displayed superior ability to identify orientation in first-order defined patterns, in conjunction with impaired ability on second-order defined patterns, relative to a comparison group. The first-order task used by Bertone et al. measured the minimally detectable contrast threshold required to identify orientation. Conversely, in our RF24 task, contrast is at supra-threshold levels and the threshold assesses the minimum amplitude required to perceive the curvature of the local elements that comprise the RF shape. Given that these two tasks are assessing different capabilities at the local level, patterns of performance could differ. It would, however, be informative for future research to compare the same samples of ASD and TD individuals on both tasks. Importantly, both studies report impaired process- ing on visual tasks that require further processing beyond the V1 visual area, despite second-order processing potentially occurring earlier in the hier- archy (V2, Baker, 1999) than the global shape per- ception required for RF3 patterns (V4, Wilkinson et al., 2000). These findings are consistent with the idea that individuals with an ASD experience diffi- culty in the processing of ‘complex’ information (Bertone et al., 2005; Minshew et al., 1997) in that both second-order orientation identification and RF3 shape perception invoke more complex perceptual networks to integrate multiple stimulus elements than do tasks typically used to target the local pro- cessing capabilities of V1. Other studies assessing global processing in the ventral visual stream have not produced clear group differences when comparing individuals with an ASD
  • 17. to matched TD control groups. Milne et al. (2006) and Spencer et al. (2000) reported no differences in form coherence thresholds using a task that required detecting the presence of a global pattern revealed by giving small line segments an orientation appropri- ate for the global pattern. In contrast, Spencer and O’Brien (2006) and Tsermentseli, O’Brien, and Spencer (2008) reported higher thresholds for children with autism compared to TD controls (but only when children with Asperger’s disorder were excluded from analyses). The task they employed required detecting global form in Glass patterns (Glass, 1969) composed of aligned dot triplets as opposed to line segments. There are many differ- ences between these studies which might have an impact. Importantly, the response of cells in V1 are facilitated by horizontal connections when adjacent cells are firing (known as collinear facilitation; see Loffler, 2008, for a review). Li and Gilbert (2002, see also Field & Hayes, 2004) demonstrated similar processes occur when elements are combined in contour detection. Therefore, one possible factor contributing to the differences between these studies is that the solid lines in the line segment stimuli may enhance processing in these collinear facilitation networks, whereas the weaker collinear facilitation produced by having three aligned dots may be less effective in activating these networks. Consequently, it is possible that the contours in line segment stimuli are facilitated by lower-level processing, whereas Glass patterns specifically target high-level integrative processing in the ventral stream (Wilson & Wilkinson, 1998). If this is the case, then the results of these combined studies are consistent with those presently reported using RF patterns. Specifi- cally, no difference between ASD and TD groups is
  • 18. apparent on tasks assessing lower-level ventral stream processing, whereas tasks such as RF3 and Glass pattern tasks are sensitive to the global pro- cessing difficulties experienced by individuals with an ASD. This potential explanation for the different results for the studies requires further investigation directly comparing the different stimulus types. While there are reports of nonsignificant group dif- ferences for each level of processing in the literature, (a) (b) 0.016 T h re sh o ld ( A ) T h re sh
  • 19. o ld ( A ) RF3 0.014 0.012 0.010 0.0035 RF24 0.0030 0.0025 TD ASD TD ASD Figure 2 Graphs showing mean thresholds on (a) the RF3 task and (b) the RF24 task for the typically devel- oping and ASD groups (lines show 95% confidence intervals). The 95% confidence intervals are smaller in the TD group owing to the substantially larger sample size Shape perception in autism 721 � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and
  • 20. Adolescent Mental Health. when significant differences have been reported, for local processing the differences have invariably been in the direction of superior performance for the ASD sample relative to neurotypical comparison groups (Mottron et al., 2006). Conversely, for global processing the differences have invariably been in the direction of impaired performance for the ASD sample relative to neurotypical comparison groups (Happé & Booth, 2008). Thus, on balance, the emerging empirical literature on visual processing in ASDs favours the WCC theory given its capacity to account for both global impairment and local superiority. While the normal performance for the ASD sample on the RF24 task in the current study is compatible with the predictions derived from EPF theory, the finding of impaired global processing for the ASD group on the RF3 task cannot be accounted for by EPF theory. Thus, the patterns of findings from the current study also favour the WCC theory. The EPF account has led to valuable research showing superiority of children with ASDs in local processing in several areas, including graphic construction, visual search and the perception of hierarchical figures (see Happé & Booth, 2008, for a summary). However, many of these tasks may be differentially influenced by local and global processing, and the trade-off between the two is often unclear. For example, local and global processing on the Navon (1977) hierarchical figures are associated with spatial frequency differences (Badcock, Whitworth, Badcock, & Lovegrove, 1990), resulting in the relative contri-
  • 21. bution of local and global processing to task performance being unclear (see also Dakin & Frith, 2005, for a discussion). The RF shapes used in the current study are not subject to this limitation in that they have identical spatial frequencies at the local and global levels. However, performance on low RF patterns (e.g., RF3) can still be attributed to global pooling, whereas performance on high RF patterns (e.g., RF24) can be accounted for simply with reference to probability summation of infor- mation from local elements (Bell et al., 2007). An alternative explanation for the current finding of lower sensitivity to RF3 shapes in ASDs might be that global pooling in higher cortical regions devel- ops later than local processing mechanisms. As a result, the ASD group may be developmentally delayed in their ability to process global contours compared to the TD group. If this were the case, then it would be expected that the TD group would show a greater improvement with age in RF3 thresholds than the ASD children. However, the results from the current study indicated that the ASD children developed at the same rate as the TD children on both the RF3 and RF24 tasks. Never- theless, it will be important for future research to investigate whether, in adulthood, ASD samples eventually attain thresholds for low RF tasks that are comparable to those of TD groups. Additionally, with respect to the RF stimuli, it is becoming clear that global processing occurs in shapes up to approximately an RF of 10 whereas in shapes of higher RF, local processing appears to be the pre- dominant mechanism (Bell et al., 2009; Loffler et al., 1998). In typical observers, the transfer from
  • 22. global to local processing with increasing RF occurs quite rapidly. Given the evidence suggesting a bias for local processing in ASDs, it is possible that their transition point is different. It will therefore be important for future research to compare ASD and TD groups on a continuum of RF shapes with increasing RF numbers. To summarise, using a novel application of RF stimuli, we have demonstrated that global pro- cessing is impaired in the ventral visual stream in a reasonably large sample of individuals with an ASD. This adds substantially to the position that a deficit in global visual processing is present in this popu- lation. The results are consistent with more general problems in neural integration in ASDs, as evi- denced by the findings from EEG (Pei et al., In Press) and fMRI (e.g., Just, Cherkassky, Keller, & Minshew, 2004) studies. Finally, the application of regression analyses to determine the difference between predicted and observed scores in a clinical population provided an innovative means to avoid the issues usually associated with matching for age, verbal and non-verbal ability. These data provide an insight into the experience of visual perception in children with ASDs. Individuals with autism often describe exceptional visual perceptual abilities that allow them to identify changes or anomalies in their environment (e.g., Grandin, 1992; Stehli, 1991) not usually noticeable to TD individuals. According to Kanner (1943), this ‘inability to experience wholes without full attention to the constituent parts’ (p. 246) is a core component of autism, but these abilities may be maladaptive in so far as they may lead to distress at small changes in the environment (Happé & Frith, 2006). Additionally, a disturbance
  • 23. in the ability to combine visual information in eye gaze, facial expressions and face perception has been hypothesised to contribute to impairments in social communication seen in ASD (see Dawson, Webb, & McPartland, 2005, for a review). Thus, fundamental, early-emerging difficulties in inte- grating information to form a coherent, global per- cept, as demonstrated using the RF patterns, could account for some of the major behavioural mani- festations of ASDs. Acknowledgements This research was supported by NH&MRC Project Grant 403942 awarded to M. T. Maybery, D. R. Badcock, J. C. Badcock and E. Pellicano. We are grateful to Judith Cullity for her assistance in programming, Rachelle Fox, Lynsey Harborow, 722 Emma J. Grinter et al. � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. Dana Sidoruk and Kelly Scaramozzino for their assistance in data collection, and two anonymous reviewers who commented on an earlier draft of the manuscript. Finally, we are extremely indebted to all the children and families who gave their time to participate in this research. Correspondence to
  • 24. Emma Grinter, School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley 6009, Perth, WA, Australia; Tel: +618 64882479; Email: [email protected] Key points • The study introduces a new and simple shape stimulus, radial frequency (RF) patterns, to investigate visual functioning in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) • RF patterns can be manipulated in precise ways to probe local and global mechanisms involved in pro- cessing visual form. • Children with ASDs require more shape modulation for RF patterns that target global form processing compared to typically developing children. • Children with ASDs show intact ability to process RF patterns targeting local form processing. • RF patterns may provide a new, readily accepted clinical tool to examine visual function in ASDs. References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV, 4th edn). Washington DC: APA. Badcock, J.C., Whitworth, F.A., Badcock, D.R., & Love- grove, W.J. (1990). Low-frequency filtering and process- ing global-local stimuli. Perception, 19, 617–629. Baker, C.L. (1999). Central neural mechanisms for detect-
  • 25. ing second-order motion. Current Opinion in Neurobiol- ogy, 9, 461–466. Bell, J., & Badcock, D.R. (2008). Luminance and contrast cues are integrated in global shape detection with contours. Vision Research, 48, 2336–2344. Bell, J., & Badcock, D.R. (2009). Narrow-band radial frequency shape channels revealed by sub-threshold summation. Vision Research, 49, 843–850. Bell, J., Badcock, D.R., Wilson, H., & Wilkinson, F. (2007). Detection of shape in radial frequency contours: Inde- pendence of local and global form information. Vision Research, 47, 1518–1522. Bell, J., Wilkinson, F., Wilson, H.R., Loffler, G., & Badcock, D.R. (2009). Radial frequency adaptation reveals inter- acting contour shape channels. Vision Research. Epub ahead of print: doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.022 Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A ‘complex issue’. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128, 2430–2441. Blake, R., Turner, L.M., Smoski, M.J., Pozdol, S.L., & Stone, W. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157. Brock, J., Jarrold, C., Farran, E.K., Laws, G., & Riby, D.M.
  • 26. (2007). Do children with Williams syndrome really have good vocabulary knowledge? Methods for comparing cognitive and linguistic abilities in developmental disor- ders. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 673–688. Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48, 497–507. Dawson, M., Webb, S.J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Under- standing the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiologi- cal studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 403– 424. DeValois, R.L., & DeValois, K.K. (1988). Spatial vision. New York: Oxford University Press. Field, D.J., & Hayes, A. (2004). Contour integration and lateral connections of V1 neurons. In L.M. Chalupa, & J.S. Werner (Eds.), The visual neurosciences (Vol. 2, pp. 1069–1079). London: MIT Press. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Glass, L. (1969). Moire effect from random dots. Nature, 223, 578–580. Grandin, T. (1992). An inside view of autism. In E. Schopler, & G.B. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning indi- viduals with autism (pp. 105–126). New York: Plenum Press. Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.
  • 27. Happé, F., & Booth, R.D.L. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disor- ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25. Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 81–86. Just, M.A., Cherkassky, V.L., Keller, T.A., & Minshew, N.J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronisation during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821. Kaiser, M., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A review and synthesis. Psychonomic Bulletin, 16, 761– 777. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con- tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Li, W., & Gilbert, C.D. (2002). Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 2846–2856. Loffler, G. (2008). Perception of contours and shapes: Low and intermediate stage mechanisms. Vision Research, 48, 2106–2127.
  • 28. Shape perception in autism 723 � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. Loffler, G., Wilson, H., & Wilkinson, F. (2003). Local and global contributions to shape discrimination. Vision Research, 43, 519–530. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 255–263. Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Han- sen, P., & Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence detection in autism: Relationships to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 225–237. Minshew, N.J., Goldstein, G., & Siegal, D.J. (1997). Neu- ropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the Interna- tional Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316. Mottron, L., Burack, J.A., Stauder, J., & Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
  • 29. 40, 203–211. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Souliéres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J.A. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ders, 36, 27–43. Movshon, J.A. (1990). Visual processing of moving images. In M. Weston-Smith, H.B. Barlow, & C. Blakemore (Eds.), Images and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychol- ogy, 9, 353–383. Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D.L., McMahon, W.M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and Tou- rette syndrome: An information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015– 1032. Pei, F., Baldassi, S., Procida, G., Igliozzi, R., Tancredi, R., Muratori, F., & Cioni, G. (in press). Neural correlates of texture and contour integration in children with autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research. Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D.R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mech- anise for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsycholo- gia, 43, 1044–1053. Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention
  • 30. task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733– 742. Simmons, D.R., Robertson, A.E., McKay, L., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F.E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739. Spencer, J., & O’Brien, J. (2006). Visual form processing deficits in autism. Perception, 35, 1047–1055. Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Cogni- tive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 11, 2765–2767. Stehli, A. (1991). The sound of a miracle. New York: Doubleday. Thomas, M.S.C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Scerif, G., Jarrold, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing, 52, 336–358. Tsermentseli, S., O’Brien, J., & Spencer, J. (2008). Com- parison of form and motion coherence processing in autistic spectrum disorders and dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1201–1210. Wechsler, D. (2003). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th edn). Wilkinson, F., James, T.W., Wilson, H., Gati, J.S., Menon, R.S., & Goodale, M.A. (2000). An fMRI study of the selective activation of human extrastriate form vision areas by radial and concentric gratings. Current Biology,
  • 31. 10, 1455–1458. Wilkinson, F., Wilson, H., & Habak, C. (1998). Detection and recognition of radial frequency patterns. Vision Research, 38, 3555–3568. Wilson, H., & Wilkinson, F. (1998). Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: Implications for form vision. Vision Research, 38, 2933–2947. Manuscript accepted 30 October 2009 724 Emma J. Grinter et al. � 2010 The Authors Journal compilation � 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. Copyright of Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Object recognition with severe spatial deficits in Williams syndrome: sparing and breakdown
  • 32. Barbara Landau a,*, James E. Hoffman b , Nicole Kurz b a Department of Cognitive Science, Krieger Hall, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA b University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA Received 31 March 2002; revised 2 September 2004; accepted 23 June 2005 Abstract Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic disorder that results in severe visual-spatial cognitive deficits coupled with relative sparing in language, face recognition, and certain aspects of motion processing. Here, we look for evidence for sparing or impairment in another cognitive system— object recognition. Children with WS, normal mental-age (MA) and chronological age-matched (CA) children, and normal adults viewed pictures of a large range of objects briefly presented under various conditions of degradation, including canonical and
  • 33. unusual orientations, and clear or blurred contours. Objects were shown as either full-color views (Experiment 1) or line drawings (Experiment 2). Across both experiments, WS and MA children performed similarly in all conditions while CA children performed better than both WS group and MA groups with unusual views. This advantage, however, was eliminated when images were also blurred. The error types and relative difficulty of different objects were similar across all participant groups. The results indicate selective sparing of basic mechanisms of object recognition in WS, together with developmental delay or arrest in recognition of objects from unusual viewpoints. These findings are consistent with the growing literature on brain abnormalities in WS which points to selective impairment in the parietal areas of the brain. As a whole, the results lend further support to the growing literature on the functional separability of object recognition mechanisms from other spatial functions, and raise intriguing questions about the link between genetic deficits and cognition. q 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
  • 34. Keywords: Ventral Stream; Object Recognition; Williams syndrome Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT 0022-2860/$ - see front matter q 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.005 * Corresponding author. Tel.: C1 410 516 5255. E-mail address: [email protected] (B. Landau). http://www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510484 In this paper, we report evidence that some mechanisms of object recognition can develop without impairment even while other aspects of spatial representation are severely impaired. Our evidence comes from people with Williams syndrome, a genetic disorder which gives rise to an unusual cognitive profile including severe spatial deficits together with relatively spared language 1 . The striking imbalance between two major cognitive systems has suggested to some that genetic defects can have specific cognitive targets
  • 35. during development (Bellugi, Marks, Bihrle & Sabo, 1988; Jordan, Reiss, Hoffman & Landau, 2002; see also, Frith, 1992). Such targeting has been attributed to modular organization of cognition (Bellugi et al., 1988; Clahsen & Almazan, 1998) or specialization in streams of processing in the mind and brain (e.g. Atkinson, Braddick, Anker, Curran, Andrew and Wattam-Bell, 2003; Dilks, Landau & Hoffman, 2005). The present studies provide support for specific targeting in a genetic deficit, as they show that basic mechanisms of object recognition can be spared even though other aspects of spatial representation are severely impaired. We show that, while children with Williams syndrome cannot reproduce the spatial organization of even moderately complex figures (see Fig. 1), they can recognize a wide range of familiar objects under varying conditions of degradation. Testing the possibility that a specific cognitive system is spared in a case of genetic
  • 36. impairment requires several steps. First, it requires evidence that the cognitive system in question is specialized, that is, different from other knowledge domains or functions on computational, neural, and/or psychological grounds. Such evidence is abundant in object recognition, and we discuss it below. Second, it assumes that genetic deficits can, in principal, target certain cognitive systems while leaving others either partially or fully spared. As we discuss below, this assumption is complex and currently under debate. Ultimately, the debate can only be resolved by empirical study, which we offer here. Finally, any empirical test requires sufficient fine-grained detail to examine key aspects of normal cognitive architecture. If patterns of performance are both quantitatively and qualitatively similar to normal groups, it is reasonable to conclude that basic aspects of the architecture are spared. Alternatively, detailed patterns of difference can shed light on which aspects of the architecture are robust and which are vulnerable in genetic deficit.
  • 37. The experiments we present will test key aspects of the object recognition system in order to determine how, if at all, performance differs from normal. 1. Object recognition as a specialized system Thinking of human object recognition as a specialized system is not a new idea. At least since the work of Marr (1982), it has been acknowledged that one of the central computational goals of the visual system is to rapidly recognize objects under a wide range of viewing conditions. The object recognition system is designed to represent 1 We attach no technical meaning to the term ‘severe’, but rather, use it to describe the hallmark pattern of Williams syndrome, in which children, adolescents, and even adults carry out visual-spatial constructive tasks such as block construction at the level of four-year-old normal children. Fig. 1. Sample copies of model figures (row 1) drawn by two children with Williams syndrome (rows 2 and 3) and one normally developing child matched for mental age (row 4). KBIT scores represent IQ, not raw scores used for matching.
  • 38. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 485 objects—primarily by their shapes—in a way that renders them recognizable over changes in irrelevant spatial properties such as size, translation, and viewpoint. Thus, object recognition is inherently spatial. The specific computational problems involved in visual object recognition include segmenting the image from background, parsing it into edges and/or parts, and representing the overall configuration, usually determined by the spatial relationships among parts. The resulting representation must support matching to an incoming image over changes in the object’s lighting, size, viewpoint, etc. (Marr, 1982; Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004). Arguments and evidence suggest that the computational solutions to these problems are unique to objects. For example, researchers have suggested that object shape is represented by the distributed activity of a population of neurons tuned to specific viewpoints (Logothetis & Sheinberg, 1996; Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004). Recent data from
  • 39. brain imaging suggests that the computations involved in recognizing objects are carried out in distributed, but constrained areas of the brain that are distinct from areas that process face recognition (Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004; Pietrini, Furey, Ricciardi, Gobbini, Wu and Cohen, 2004). Presumably, this specialized computational system has evolved to allow us to rapidly and efficiently recognize objects, perhaps even as soon as we know that an object is there (Grill-Spector & Kanwisher, 2005). B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510486 A particularly difficult problem for the visual system is recognition of objects from partial, sometimes degraded images that result from viewing objects from different viewpoints, which often occlude and distort important features. Some theories suggest that the visual system constructs a single unified viewpoint- invariant representation (e.g. Biederman, 1987; Marr, 1982). However the bulk of empirical evidence indicates that
  • 40. we recognize objects by using viewpoint-dependent representations and interpolating new viewpoints (Bulthoff, Edelman & Tarr 1995; Tarr & Pinker, 1990; Logothetis, Pauls, Bulthoff & Poggio 1994; see Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004). The mechanisms of computing new viewpoints are not well understood, but the capacity to do so is acknowledged to be complex, and is assumed to be a hallmark of our object recognition system. Considerations of cognitive function and brain localization also support the idea that visual object recognition is specialized. Abundant evidence shows that the human visual system is composed of separate streams which carry out different kinds of spatial processing that serve different functions. For example, beyond the primary visual cortex, the ventral stream processes information about objects and faces. Neurons in inferotemporal (IT) cortex of monkeys respond to the same object under a variety of equivalence conditions (Booth & Rolls, 1998; Logothetis et al.,
  • 41. 1994) and to neighboring views of the same object (Tanaka, 1996). In humans, imaging studies show that homologous areas of cortex are activated during object recognition, suggesting a specific neural substrate (Kourtzi, Erb, Grodd & Bulthoff, 2003; Palmeri & Gauthier, 2004). In contrast to the ventral stream areas and their focus on objects, the dorsal stream is thought to be responsible for processing different kinds of spatial information, including some kinds of motion and location. For example, the perception of motion coherence— which requires computing the global direction of individual motion elements—appears to be processed in area V5/MT (Newsome & Pare, 1988). Still other spatial properties and functions appear to be governed by other areas of the dorsal stream, especially those properties pertinent for action (Bridgeman, Gemmer, Forsman & Huemer, 2000; Colby & Goldberg, 1999; Livingstone & Hubel, 1988; Milner & Goodale, 1995; Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982).
  • 42. Finally, the psychological representation of objects has been shown to be dissociable from other kinds of representation that depend on spatial organization, even those within the ventral stream. For example, Moscovitch, Winocur & Behrmann (1997) reported a brain-damaged patient with severely impaired object recognition who could nevertheless recognize faces very well. Duchaine and Nakayama (2005) reported the reverse pattern, developmental prosopagnosics who could not recognize faces, but could recognize categories of objects such as tools and cars quite well. Milner and Goodale (1995) reported a patient who was severely impaired in judging a line’s orientation, but could use her perception of the line to guide action (e.g. posting a letter through a slot). Importantly, patients who lose the ability to recognize objects often have damage to the ventral stream, specifically areas of inferotemporal and occipital cortex. These people often perform worse under degraded conditions such as varying illumination or novel viewpoints (see
  • 43. Farah, 2000), suggesting that they have lost the hallmark capacity to recognize object equivalences over such varying conditions. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 487 As a whole, this evidence strongly suggests that the object recognition is specialized in its computational mechanisms, neural substrate, and psychological function. 2. Genetic deficits and cognitive specialization Can a genetic deficit target certain cognitive systems while sparing others? This issue is controversial. One aspect of the controversy concerns whether or not developmental (i.e. genetic) cases can be used to make the same kinds of inferences about cognitive structure as adult cases of brain damage. Arguments in favor of selective sparing have often come from studies of brain-damaged adults, who represent the case of a mature system that sustains damage. Evidence from brain-damaged adults who can perceive faces but not objects suggests some type of specialized organization in which systems have been
  • 44. differentially targeted by the damage (e.g. Moscovitch et al., 1997). In contrast, genetic deficits have effects even prior to birth, and this raises the question of whether such a deficit will reflect the same kinds of division of labor as revealed by cases of damage to the mature brain. A second controversial issue concerns the relationship between genes and cognition. Genetic changes to an organism will inevitably result in local changes to the physiology of the brain. The crucial issue is whether such local changes will inevitably affect cognitive structure. Views on these two aspects of the controversy lead to very different sets of predictions about the likely cognitive outcome of any genetic deficit. In one view, it is possible for a genetic defect to selectively target particular cognitive systems—either because of distinct computational requirements of the system, or because the defect selectively impairs streams of processing in the brain that normally support that system. In this view, the local
  • 45. effects that genes have on brain physiology interact with constraints on knowledge domains that overdetermine cognitive outcomes. Thus, cognitive domains could be expected to have normal structure. This possibility is consistent with the idea that different cognitive domains are computationally distinct, that they have different neural substrates, and that the internal constraints of the system guide development (e.g. Frith, 1992; Gallistel, Brown, Carey, Gelman & Keil, 1991; Spelke & Newport, 1998; Tager-Flusberg, Plesa-Skwerer & Faja, 2003). Several recent studies in the domain of faces are consistent with this view. For example, Tager-Flusberg et al. reported that people with Williams syndrome show the face inversion effect usually observed in normal individuals, suggesting that the face processing mechanisms operate under normal constraints. In addition, recent reports of congenital prosopagnosia suggest it is possible to have a specific deficit in face perception (Behrmann & Avidan, 2005); additional evidence hints that
  • 46. there may be a genetic basis to this disorder. In this general view, one would predict that mechanisms of object recognition could be spared even in the context of severe deficits in other areas of visual- spatial cognition. In a different view, genetic deficits are also assumed to have widespread local effects on the development of the brain, but these effects are thought to propogate up to cognitive structure, as follows. The genetic deficit is assumed to result in impaired cognitive mechanisms. Interactions between these impaired mechanisms and particular knowledge B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510488 domains will yield differences in cognitive structure. Moreover, the cognitive structures that are produced will not necessarily (or even probably) show breakdown along the lines of a normal, mature architecture (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998). Although one might observe performance that appears similar to a normal profile, closer inspection will likely show
  • 47. meaningful differences in the underlying cognitive structures. For example, Karmiloff- Smith, Thomas, Annaz, Humphreys, Ewing and Brace (2004) argue that face processing in people with Williams syndrome is not normal, but rather, good performance is accomplished via atypical mechanisms; specifically, they argue that configural processing of faces in WS people is impaired. This general position is consistent with the idea that, despite the distinct computational requirements of different cognitive domains, genetic deficits will likely result in subtle cognitive impairments, including the recognition of objects. These strong positions can guide our thinking, but only empirical data can decide between them. Perhaps more importantly, empirical data can provide us with the foundation for developing more nuanced theoretical positions. 3. Williams syndrome and object recognition The case of object recognition in Williams syndrome provides an empirical forum for
  • 48. doing both of these. Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic disorder (1:20,000 live births) which is caused by a hemizygous submicroscopic deletion on chromosome 7q11.23. Diagnosis is made on the basis of a unique phenotypic pattern that includes a characteristic facial profile, disorders of the heart, and anomalies of the viscera. It can also be verified by a screening technique (fluoride in situ hybridization, FISH) that isolates the key region of gene deletion (Ewart, Morris, Atkinson, Jin, Sternes and Spallone, 1993; Frangiskakis, Ewart, Morris, Mervis, Bertrand and Robinson, 1996; Morris, Ewart, Sternes, Spallone, Stock and Leppert, 1994). Of most importance to us, however, is the distinctive cognitive profile of individuals with Williams syndrome, who show severely impaired spatial cognition together with relatively spared language (Bellugi et al., 1988; Mervis, Morris, Bertrand & Robinson, 1999). Although WS individuals are also moderately mentally retarded (Mean Composite IQZ55– 60, Mervis, et al., 1999), their unique pattern of spatial deficit and linguistic strength sets
  • 49. them apart from other groups with comparable retardation, such as Down syndrome (Mervis et al., 1999). The striking cognitive profile has also motivated some of the strongest hypotheses of developmental modularity in the field (Bellugi et al., 1988; Pinker, 1994). Selective sparing in cognitive systems can also be tested by examining differential breakdown within the broad domain of spatial representation. Growing evidence shows that spatial representation is not one monolithic system, but rather, is composed of different sub-systems that are specialized in their computational goals, their functional properties, and their neural substrates. One example is the contrast between perception of space and representation of space for action, as described previously. Other functionally distinct systems include the representation of space for navigation (Aguirre, Zarahn & D’Esposito, 1998; Gallistel, 1990; Hermer & Spelke, 1996; Newcombe & Huttenlocher, 2000), the multiple representations of space that encode location in different reference
  • 50. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 489 systems (Andersen, Snyder, Bradley & Xing 1997; Colby & Goldberg, 1999), and the representation of different kinds of motion such as motion coherence, biological motion, and form from motion (Schenk & Zihl, 1997; Vaina, LeMay, Bienfrang, Choi & Nakayama, 1990). Although the nature of spatial impairment in WS is not well understood at present, there are indications that it has ‘peaks and valleys’ (cf. Bellugi et al., 1988), raising the possibility of selective targeting. The most widely observed hallmark of the spatial deficit appears in so-called ‘visual-spatial construction’ tasks, in which people are asked to replicate an overall pattern by drawing or assembling parts. Individuals with WS perform extremely poorly in such tasks, with adolescents performing in the 1st percentile—roughly at the level of a normal four-year-old (Bellugi et al., 1988; Mervis et al., 1999; see Fig. 1
  • 51. for some examples). In these tasks, the deficient performance of WS children can be traced to faulty spatial representations of the individual blocks and their relations rather than faulty executive processes (Hoffman, Landau & Pagani, 2003). In particular, children with WS have trouble discriminating the ‘handedness’ of the individual blocks which are often split in half by color, and have distractors that are mirror- images. Additionally, WS children make errors in the spatial arrangement of blocks, often erring on left-right locations within the model. This raises the possibility that object representation more generally—and not just mirror-image structures—might be impaired. On the other hand, growing evidence suggests differential breakdown across other domains that involve spatial organization of elements. For example, the perception of biological motion and motion coherence is spared in WS children (Jordan et al., 2002; Reiss, Hoffman & Landau, 2005), even though perception of form from motion is impaired
  • 52. (Atkinson, King, Braddick, Nokes, Anker & Braddick 1997; Reiss et al., 2005). Aspects of visual-manual action appear to be impaired, even relative to mental age matched children (Atkinson, King, Braddick, Nokes, Anker and Braddick, 1997; Dilks, Landau & Hoffman, 2005). Some mechanisms of global spatial perception are unimpaired (Pani, Mervis & Robinson, 1999) and there is evidence that face perception is spared, even relative to chronological age matches (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2003). As a whole, these findings are consistent with the idea that a genetic defect can result in targeted—rather than omnibus— spatial breakdown. 4. The current experiments The case of object recognition affords a further test of this possibility. First, it is a natural domain that presents a unique computational problem: Using patterns of light striking the retina, the brain must construct a representation that will enable the perceiver to later recognize the same object, despite changes in viewpoint and lighting that occur in
  • 53. natural viewing conditions. This is a task that is easily solved by human observers—even infants—but cannot yet be solved by machines. Second, evidence from cases of brain damage in adults already suggests specialization of function: People may lose the ability to perceive objects, while still being able to act on them or reason about them. Third, the object recognition system seems to be vulnerable to a variety of effects of degradation under brain damage. For example, poor lighting or line drawings produces special B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510490 difficulties for agnosics (Farah, 2000), and highly unusual viewpoints produce difficulties in patients with damage to the right parietal lobe (Warrington & Taylor, 1973). This well- documented pattern of vulnerability within the object recognition system suggests a way to examine in detail the degree to which object recognition is spared or impaired. Recognition of objects under degraded conditions, such as poor lighting, unusual
  • 54. viewpoint, or line drawings would provide a strong test of sparing, but it is also possible that some conditions might prove substantially more difficult than others, providing evidence for both sparing and breakdown. Therefore, in our first experiment, we examined the capacity of children with WS to identify objects under degraded conditions. We used two critical manipulations: Presentation of clear vs. blurred images, and presentation of objects from canonical vs. unusual viewpoints. These two methods of image degradation can be used to drive down performance of all groups—including normal children and adults—thereby allowing us to examine whether patterns of failure are similar or different between WS and normal individuals. But the manipulations are also of more particular interest with respect to Williams syndrome. First, blurring the image is of interest because initial characterizations of the spatial deficit in WS suggested that they are ‘local processors’ (Bihrle, Bellugi, Delis & Marks,
  • 55. 1989; Deruelle, Mancini, Livel, Casse-Perot & de Schoon, 1999), i.e. they correctly perceive the features of an object but are deficient in grasping the global configuration of those features. More recent work (Farran, Jarrold & Gathercole, 2003; Pani, Mervis & Robinson, 1999), however, indicates that people with WS do correctly perceive configurations and actually have trouble focusing attention on parts that are members of larger configurations (Hoffman, Landau, and Pagani, 2003). Blurring an image would primarily affect the visibility of constituent parts (Hughes, Nozawa, and Kitterle, 1996; Morrison and Schyns, 2001) while leaving the global shape relatively unaffected. Therefore, to the extent that people with WS utilize global shape to recognize objects, they might be less affected by blur than control children who may use both local and global information for identification. The second, and more crucial manipulation is canonical vs. highly unusual viewpoints.
  • 56. Recognizing objects from highly unusual viewpoints might present a special problem for people with Williams syndrome. Perrett, Oram & Ashbridge (1998) suggest that viewpoint effects on recognition can be understood in terms of a multiple view theory of object recognition in which the current view is matched to multiple views that are stored in inferotemporal cortex that have been laid down by previous experiences with that object. Novel views do not have a matching representation and must be recognized by partial activation of nearest neighbor views, which will be slower than matches for canonical views. This theory, however, does not directly account for findings with highly unusual views. Unusual views are taken from a perspective that foreshortens the principal axis of the object and often occludes many of its salient features (Humphreys & Riddoch, 1984). This precludes the kind of automatic object recognition that seems to occur with more canonical views and instead, appears to involve a ‘problem
  • 57. solving or executive component’ (Farah, 2000) in which the observer searches the image for parts that may provide cues to its identity (Perrett et al., 1998). Given the role of the parietal lobe in B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 491 directing spatial attention (Belmonte & Yurgelun-Todd, 2003), it is perhaps not surprising that patients with damage to parietal areas are selectively impaired in recognizing these views (Warrington & Taylor, 1973). The claim that recognition of unusual views requires executive control is also consistent with the finding that in dual-task experiments, a task requiring ‘central executive resources’ (random number generation) produced more interference with unusual views than canonical views (Baragwanath & Turnbull, 2002). Finally, recognition of unusual views has been found to activate both prefrontal (Kosslyn, Alpert, Thompson, Chabris, Rauch and Anderson, 1994) and parietal areas (Sugio, Inui, Matsuo, Matsuzawa, Glover and Nakai, 1999) in fMRI scans,
  • 58. suggesting top-down control of parietal areas involved with spatial attention. The role of the parietal lobe in recognition of unusual views suggests that WS subjects may find such views particularly challenging. Damage to the same parietal areas activated in recognition of unusual views often results in impairments in tasks such as drawing and block construction (Benton, 1967; Turnbull, Denis, Mellet, Ghaem & Carey, 2001)—just those tasks that WS people typically fail. In addition, recent neuroimaging of people with WS during performance of these spatial tasks shows a reduced activation of these parietal areas (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2004), and other analyses show than WS people have smaller than normal volume in superior parietal areas (Eckert, Hu, Eliez, Bellugi, Galaburda and Korenberg, 2005). These observations are consistent with the suggestion by Atkinson et al. (2003) that WS (and possibly other syndromes) is primarily a dorsal stream deficit. People with WS might therefore be expected to have a selective deficit in
  • 59. identifying objects from highly unusual viewpoints, even if their identification from canonical viewpoints is not deficient. 5. Experiment 1 5.1. Participants Twelve children with Williams syndrome (mean age 11;0, range 7;4 to 15;3 years), 12 normally developing children who were mental-age matches for the WS group (Mean Age 5;8, Range 4;1 to 7;1 years), 12 normally developing children who were chronological age-matches for the WS group (Mean Age 11;11, Range 10;6 to 14;3 years) and 12 undergraduates participated. Children with WS were identified through the National WS Association, and had been positively diagnosed by a geneticist; all but one had also been diagnosed by the FISH test (the remaining person did not undergo the test). Normal children in the MA group were matched individually to children with WS, using the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990), which yields a verbal and
  • 60. non-verbal (Matrices) score. The latter does not have many spatial items, and hence does not unfairly penalize WS children for their spatial impairment. The mean scores for the WS children were VerbalZ31.7 (SEZ2.32), MatricesZ18.58 (SEZ1.11); corresponding scores for the MA-matched controls were 29.33 (SEZ2.33) and 18.25 (SEZ1.52). Scores for the CA group were VerbalZ59.6 (SEZ1.46) and MatricesZ38 (SEZ0.99). The mean IQ scores for the three groups were 71.42 (SEZ4.35) for the WS children, 116.2 (SEZ 4.11) for the MA group, and 123 (SEZ2.50) for the CA controls. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510492 In addition, the WS children and ten of the MA controls were tested on the Pattern Construction Sub-test of the Differential Abilities Scales (Elliot, 1990), which requires children to replicate a design using individual component blocks, and is the hallmark test used to diagnose the WS spatial impairment. The scores for the WS children were MZ 81.83, percentileZ1.83, SEZ5.82; scores for the MA controls were, MZ109.2, percentileZ58.1, SEZ6.96. The WS scores are in the range reported by other investigators (see Mervis et al., 1999). All but two WS children fell into the 1st percentile
  • 61. of performance, thus conforming to the reported pattern of severe spatial deficit. Note that, although the WS children were matched to normally developing children on verbal and non-verbal scores (the KBIT), they performed much worse than their MA matches on the Pattern Construction task, as would be expected. All participants signed informed consent forms. 5.2. Design, stimuli, and procedures Participants were asked to name each of a set of 80 full color pictures of objects which were presented on a computer screen for 500 ms. The objects were drawn randomly from a set of 320 images consisting of 80 objects (listed in Table 1) in each of four conditions: (a) canonical view, clear image, (b) canonical view, blurred image, (c) unusual view, clear image, and (d) unusual view, blurred image. Canonical views were deemed those that exposed all of each object’s relevant parts; many were full-face views of the object. Unusual views varied in their orientation, and included foreshortening along the primary
  • 62. axis, and selection of views from above or below the object. Blurred images were created by editing the clear images using the Gaussian Blur tool (radius 10) in Photoshop. Objects were drawn from two sources: The ‘Object DataBank’ available from Michael Tarr (http:// www.cog.brown.edu/wtarr/stimuli.html) and the model set which accompanied Ray Dream Studio 5.5. Examples of objects in each of the four conditions are shown in Fig. 2 and the complete set of images can be viewed at http://hoffman.psych.udel.edu/ ObjectPicturesForWeb.pps. Each participant saw 80 different objects with an equal number of objects presented in each of the four conditions. All objects were randomly ordered over eight lists, and individual WS subjects were assigned the same list as their controls (both MA and CA). Across lists, each object was represented equally often in each of the four presentation modes. Responses were recorded verbatim. If the participant was uncertain, the experimenter encouraged him or her to ‘give your best guess’.
  • 63. Responses were coded by a person who was blind to participant group. Each response was presented individually on a computer screen along with the target name (i.e. the name that was used in creating the objects; see Table 1) and the image of the object that the participant had viewed when producing the name. The rater used seven categories, including correct name, correct definition or use, related member of same basic category, correct superordinate category, similar shape, incorrect name, or do not know. For example, if a picture of a sunflower was shown, responses would be coded as follows: ‘sunflower’ (correct name), ‘goes in a vase’ (correct use), ‘daisy’ (related member of same category), ‘plant’ (correct superordinate), ‘clock’ (similar shape), ‘truck’ (incorrect name), http://www.cog.brown.edu/~tarr/stimuli.html http://www.cog.brown.edu/~tarr/stimuli.html http://hoffman.psych.udel.edu/ObjectPicturesForWeb.pps http://hoffman.psych.udel.edu/ObjectPicturesForWeb.pps Table 1
  • 64. List of objects used in Experiments 1 and 2 Anchor Jet plane Banana Key Barn Kite Basket Motorcycle Baseball bat Mug Bed Pad lock Bee Pen Belt Piano Bike Pitcher Binoculars Plant Blender Pliers Bottle Pot Bow (archery) Pretzel Bridge Pumpkin Brush Refrigerator Bulb (light bulb) Ring Cannon Rollerblades
  • 65. Carrot Ruler Cassette tape Scissors Castle Screwdriver Chain Shovel Chair Sink Clock Sports car Couch Stool Crayon Stove Cup Sunflower Desk Table Dresser Tank Drums Tennis racket Egg (fried) Toothpaste tube Eye glasses Trash can Electric fan Truck Fork Trumpet Frying pan Turkey Glass Umbrella
  • 66. Grill Violin Guitar Wagon Hamburger anger Watch Hair dryer Windmill B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 493 or ‘do not know’. A second rater coded 20% of the responses, and reliability was 90%. Where there was disagreement, the first rater’s data were used. 5.3. Results and discussion An initial analysis examined the accuracy of participants’ labels. Responses in the first four categories were considered to be correct. Any other response was scored as incorrect. Naming accuracy (percent correct) as a function of image condition and group is shown in Fig. 2. Examples of objects used in each condition of Experiment 1 B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510494 Fig. 3 (shown as a line graph to emphasize patterns of interaction and additivity between groups and viewpoint). Fig. 3a shows data for the clear image condition and Fig. 3b shows
  • 67. data for the blurred image condition. First, it is apparent that we were effective in our manipulation of the difficulty of identifying the objects. For both clear and blurred images, participants were more accurate in naming objects shown in a canonical than in an unusual orientation. Clear images were identified more accurately than blurred ones. These effects of image quality were confirmed by significant main effects of Orientation, F(1,44)Z344.7, P!0.001 and Clarity, F(1,44)Z384.5, P!0.001. In addition, there was an Orientation by Clarity interaction, F(1,44)Z84.7, P!0.001 indicating that the effects of combining the two distortions was greater than the additive combination of each distortion in isolation. Fig. 3. (a) Experiment 1: Clear Images. Mean percent correct (S.E.) over condition and group (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome; MA:mental age matches). (b) Experiment 1: Blurred Images. Mean percent correct (S.E.) over condition and group (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome; MA: mental age matches).
  • 68. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 495 Across these data, the main effect of Group was significant, F(3,44)Z8.8, P!001, but this was due to the adults’ superior performance. Tukey post- hoc tests showed no significant differences among the three children’s groups (WS vs. MA, PZ0.72, WS vs. CA, PZ0.62, and MA vs. CA, PZ0.12). The adult group, however was more accurate B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510496 than both the WS group (P!0.002) and MA controls (P!0.001), and marginally better than the CA controls (P!0.056). In addition, there was a Clarity X Group interaction (F(3, 44)Z5.66, P!0.002. To isolate this interaction, we analyzed the difference between clear and blurred images as a function of group. Tukey tests revealed that the WS group showed smaller effects of blur than either the MA or CA controls (P!0.01 and 0.03, respectively) and were indistinguishable from adults (PO0.98). The data in Fig. 3 also suggest that the effects of viewpoint and image clarity depended on group. In order to evaluate the effects of viewpoint, we analyzed the data separately for clear and blurred images. For clear images (Fig. 3a), there is a striking similarity between
  • 69. the CA and adult groups as well as between the MA and WS groups. The adults and CA controls were more accurate across the two viewpoints (MsZ0.96, 0.93) than the other two groups (MsZ0.86, 0.83) and also showed a smaller effect of viewpoint. An analysis of variance on these data revealed a main effect of Group (F(3, 44)Z6.61. Tukey tests on the Group effect revealed that the WS group was less accurate than both the CA and adult groups (P!0.05 and 0.001, respectively) but did not differ from MA children (PZ0.87). The MA group only differed from the adults (P!0.01). There was also a significant Group X Viewpoint interaction (F(3,44)Z5.62, P!0.002) . Planned comparisons of the three children’s groups showed that the effect of orientation was larger for WS compared to CA, t(22)Z2.38, P!0.02 and MA compared to CA, t(22)Z2.37, P!0.02; The WS and MA groups did not differ, t(22)Z0.18, PO0.85. It is possible that the smaller effect of view in the CA group is due to their performance being at ceiling in the Canonical condition. However, this seems unlikely because performance of all three groups is quite good in this condition, with all means above 90% (MsZ0.99, 0.96, 0.94, 0.92 for adults, CA, MA, and WS groups). A separate ANOVA on these data
  • 70. revealed no significant difference among groups, F(2,33)Z1.41, PO0.25. Blurring the image produced a different pattern of results (Fig. 3b). The WS subjects show a relative improvement in their standing compared to the other groups, and this appears to be mainly attributed to the larger impact of blur on the CA controls. In fact, the WS group is now indistinguishable from the CA group. In addition, blurring resulted in similar effects of orientation for all groups. There were main effects of Group (F(3,44)Z9.07, P!0.001 and Orientation (F(1,44)Z280.41, P!0.001 but no interaction (F!1). Tukey post hoc tests showed that the main effect of group was due to the adults performing more accurately than all three groups of children (P’s!0.05) which were not different from each other. (WS vs. MA, PZ0.12, WS vs. CA, PZ0.99, and MA vs. CA, PZ0.20). These results show two main patterns. First, the WS children have very high performance for canonical views and clear images and their performance drops for unusual views. Their pattern of accuracy across all conditions is remarkably similar to MA
  • 71. controls and the performance of both of these groups is worse than that of normal adults. The CA group appears to occupy an intermediate position. For canonical views of clear images, they are no different from WS children; for unusual views, they show less impact of viewpoint, with performance better than both WS children and MA controls. Second, once images were blurred, the WS children perform as well as the CA children. In fact, all three groups of children showed comparable accuracy and all groups, including adults, showed the same relative decline in performance with unusual views. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 497 Why the different effects of blur? Showing clear objects from unusual viewpoints impaired performance in all of our groups, and in this case, the CA and adult controls were less affected by unusual views than the WS and MA groups. This advantage disappeared when images were blurred, however, suggesting that a key ingredient in deciphering
  • 72. unusual views may be related to analysis of internal features which is weakened by blurring. This result is consistent with results reported by Lawson & Humphreys (1999) who studied the effects of foreshortening of the main axis of an object presented as either a line drawing or a silhouette. Both line drawings and silhouettes preserve the ‘occluding contour’ of the object and therefore allow the observer to extract the object’s main axis of elongation. Silhouettes, however, eliminate the possibility of using internal details and features for identification. Their results showed that extreme foreshortening, similar to the unusual views in Experiment 1, reduced identification accuracy for both kinds of objects but particularly for the silhouettes, suggesting that internal features become particularly important clues to object identity when axis information is no longer available. The greater effect of unusual views on MA and WS subjects compared to CA controls in the clear condition suggests that the latter group is better at utilizing the internal details
  • 73. of objects to identify them from unusual viewpoints. We might expect then that if these features were more difficult to perceive—as in the blur condition—the CA’s advantage would disappear and that is what we observed. When objects were blurred, all three groups of children were comparable. Interestingly, of the three groups of children, the WS group was the least affected by image blur suggesting that they may rely on the occluding contour of the object more than control children, even for clear images. This is consistent with other evidence suggesting that people with WS may have trouble focusing attention on subparts of a larger pattern. For example, Hoffman et al. (2003) reported that in a variation of the block construction task, WS children were unable to ignore nearby blocks when they were cued to attend to a single block in the model. The foregoing evidence relies on measures of accuracy, and thus might miss some fine- grained qualitative differences in the way that WS children label objects. Accordingly, we
  • 74. analyzed the distribution of responses across the seven response categories for the four groups (see Fig. 4). The number of responses in each response category for each subject was entered into a c 2 test (SPSS Crosstabs). The only significant interaction of Group with a Response Category was for Correct Definition or Use (X 2 (15)Z28.9, P!0.02), reflecting a slightly greater tendency for MA controls to label pictures in terms of their definition or use. Otherwise, the distribution of responses across the various categories was similar for all four groups (c 2 s p values ranged from 0.11 for the ‘Do not Know’ category to 0.68 for the ‘Same Category’ response). A final set of analyses examined whether the four groups differed in which objects they found difficult to identify. We computed the average accuracy for each of the 80 objects, collapsed across the 4 image quality conditions, for each group. We then computed the
  • 75. correlation between all pairs of groups for accuracy on the 80 objects. The correlations were significantly greater than zero (P!0.001 for all 6 correlations) and ranged from 0.67 (WS vs. adult groups) to 0.76 (WS vs. MA groups). None of the pairs of correlations showed reliable differences (all PsO0.17), suggesting that there was a high degree of similarity among all four groups in terms of which objects were easy or difficult to identify. We also asked whether object identification accuracy depended on object Fig. 4. Experiment 1: Distribution of response types (M percent, S.E.) over group (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome; MA:mental age matches). B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510498 complexity. Two independent raters rank ordered the entire set of 80 objects in terms of their complexity (correlation between ratersZ0.83). These complexity ratings were not reliably correlated with average accuracy of each object for any of the groups (correlationsZ0.09, 0.10, 0.21, 0.17 for the WS, MA, CA, and adults, respectively, alphaZ0.05, two-tailed test). Both of these analyses suggest
  • 76. similarity across groups in the relative difficulty of individual objects. As a whole, the results suggest that object recognition is surprisingly good in children with WS, certainly much better than might be expected on the basis of their performance in other areas of spatial cognition such as block construction. Indeed, on the basis of their standardized verbal scores, they were exactly where one would predict they should be— comparable to MA controls in every condition. From this perspective, it is surprising that they were no different from CA controls in identifying objects presented as clear canonical images, or those presented as blurred images in either canonical or unusual viewpoints. The WS performance is particularly striking, in light of the fact that recognizing objects under these circumstances requires that they match an image that they likely never encountered with a stored representation of the object. This matching likely involves comparing overlap between two shapes, even though the unusual orientations obscure
  • 77. aspects of the objects’ shapes. It would be tempting to conclude that WS children are capable of representing and extracting an object’s part structure from partial information. However, because the stimuli were full color renditions of objects, it is always possible that object recognition in WS depends more on surface features (such as color and texture) than the object’s spatial structure (e.g. Tanaka & Presnell, 1999; Tanaka, Weiskopf & Williams, 2001). Moreover, the fact that recognizing objects in unusual orientations can sometimes activate more parietal areas (Sugio et al., 1999) raises the question of whether a more difficult version of the task might reveal additional differences between WS children and the normal groups. In order to test this possibility, we carried out a second experiment, using line drawings of objects. In these cases, objects can only be recognized by their shapes, not by surface information such as color or texture.
  • 78. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 499 6. Experiment 2 6.1. Participants Children were the same as those who participated in Experiment 1, with the exception of one MA control child, who replaced a child who could no longer participate in our studies. The new mental age-matched group was still well matched by KBIT verbal (WS MeanZ33, S.E.Z2.08, control MeanZ33, S.E.Z2.26) and Matrices scores (WS MeanZ 18, S.E.Z1.06, control MeanZ19, S.E.Z1.30). The DAS scores of the WS children had a Mean in the 1st percentile, while the MA matched controls’ Mean was in the 55th percentile. All participants were tested after Experiment 1. 6.2. Design, stimuli, and procedures These were identical to Experiment 1, except that the objects were converted to black and white line drawings (see Fig. 5). Only the clear versions were shown, resulting in a total of Fig. 5. Examples of objects used in both conditions of Experiment 2. B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510500
  • 79. 160 stimuli (80 objects X 2 views). Each subject saw all 80 objects with the view (canonical or unusual) chosen randomly under the constraint that there was an equal number of canonical and unusual views. Eight lists of objects in a random order were created and subjects were randomly assigned a list, matching WS children and their controls on the same list. Across the lists, each object was shown equally often from the two viewpoints. 6.3. Results and discussion As in Experiment 1, responses were scored as correct if they were in any of the first four response categories (correct name, correct definition or use, related category member, or correct superordinate category); all other responses were scored as incorrect. Fig. 6 shows the percent correct for the four groups as a function of orientation (canonical or unusual). Consistent with what we observed in Experiment 1 using full color solid object images, identification was severely reduced when objects were shown in a unusual orientation, F(1,44)Z524.7, P!0.001. In addition, there was a main effect of
  • 80. Group, F(3,44)Z20.9, P!0.001) as well as an Orientation X Group interaction, F(3,44)Z7.3, P!0.001 that reflects a similar pattern to the one we observed in Experiment 1 for the clear image condition. Post-hoc Tukey tests revealed two homogeneous subsets of the Group variable. WS and MA matches formed one set while the other set consisted of CA matches and Adults. Members of one subset differed significantly from each member of the other set, (P!0.001) but not from each other. (WS vs. MA, PZ0.81; CA vs. AD, PZ0.78). Separate analyses were also carried out for the canonical and unusual view conditions, with a similar result. For both conditions, the WS and MA matches formed one subgroup and the CA and AD subjects formed another. For canonical views, all groups were above Fig. 6. Experiment 2: M percent correct (S.E.) over condition and group. (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome; MA:mental age matches). B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 501 90% accuracy (MsZ0.92, 0.92, 0.98, 0.97 for WS, MA, CA and adults respective). The WS and MA groups did not differ (PZ0.97) nor did CA and AD
  • 81. groups (PZ0.92). Members of each of the subgroups, however, differed from members of the other group (all P’s!0.003). For unusual views, WS and MA children were comparable, (MsZ0.54, 0.58, respectively, PZ0.65) as were the CA and adult groups (MsZ0.71, 0.75, respectively, PZ0.65). Members of each of the subgroups differed from members of the other groups (all P’s!0.001). The Orientation X Group interaction was further analyzed using the difference between canonical and unusual views for each group. Post-hoc Tukey tests revealed that the WS group differed from the CA (P!0.02) and AD (P!0.001) groups but not from MA controls (PZ0.59) and the MA group only differed from the Adults (P!0.03). One might worry that the larger effect of orientation on WS subjects compared to CA controls is due to the CA controls being close to ceiling in the canonical view condition. However, the WS group was above 90% accuracy in this condition, as in the canonical clear condition of Experiment 1. Although the interaction should be viewed with caution, the findings here of the larger orientation effect among WS children than CA controls replicates the findings of
  • 82. Experiment 1. This suggests there is a real relative weakness for unusual orientations among WS children relative to CA, but not MA matched children. These results show that when subjects must rely purely on ‘shape’ information to label objects, WS children are remarkably good at identifying objects—over 90% with canonical viewpoints. They are remarkably similar to the MA controls in identification accuracy, and this holds even when objects are shown in unusual views that produce substantial deficits in the performance of normal adults. These results are similar to the clear image condition of Experiment 1 which showed that CA subjects were more accurate than WS subjects and less affected by unusual views. Once again, we analyzed the distribution of different error types to determine whether the four groups differed in the kinds of errors they made. These data are shown in Fig. 7 Fig. 7. Experiment 2: Distribution of responses types (M percent, S.E.) over group (Ad: Adults; CA: chronological age matches; WS: Williams syndrome;
  • 83. MA:mental age matches). B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510502 and suggest that all four groups have roughly similar profiles across the different response categories. The number of responses in each response category for each subject was entered into a c 2 test (SPSS Crosstabs) which resulted in significant interactions between Group and the following categories: Correct name (Pearson X 2 (81)Z105.6, P!0.05), Correct Definition or Use (X 2 (15)Z31.3, P!0.01), Related Category member (X2 (27)Z 55.9, P!0.01). The interaction with the ‘Do not Know’ category was marginally significant (X 2 (48)Z64.2, P!0.06). This pattern of significance reflects the higher use of correct names for objects in the CA and Adult groups compared to WS and MA children and may partly reflect more sophisticated vocabularies (as
  • 84. shown by their KBIT scores) or relatively less difficulty in retrieving the correct names. When this analysis was restricted to just the WS and MA groups, none of the interactions approached significance (all P’sO 0.14), suggesting that these two groups has a similar profile of responses across the various categories. A final analysis examined whether groups differed in terms of which objects they found difficult to identify. Here we were only able to compare the WS and MA groups because accuracy for many individual objects was at 100% for the CA and AD groups. We computed the average accuracy for each of the 80 objects, collapsed across the 2 orientation conditions. The correlation between the WS and MA groups across the 80 objects was 0.74, which is significantly different from 0, (t(78)Z9.76, P!0.001), suggesting that WS children and their MA controls were similar, not only in their overall accuracy of identification but also in terms of which objects they found easy or difficult to identify. We also examined whether accuracy depended on the
  • 85. rated complexity of the objects for these two groups. For WS children, the correlation was 0.13 and for the MA group, it was 0.14, neither of which was significant (WS: t(78)Z1.16, PO0.05 and MA: t(78)Z1.25, PO0.05). It is important to note that the WS and MA groups, who were relatively less accurate than the CA and adult groups, were nonetheless quite accurate in this task in an absolute sense. On average, they correctly recognized approximately 74% of the line drawings compared to 85% for the CA and AD groups. Even in the most difficult condition, in which objects were shown in unusual orientations, WS and MA children correctly recognized 54 and 58% of the line drawings, respectively. This is quite high, given that the random probability of guessing the correct object name is far lower than 1 in 40, which would be chance in the extremely unlikely event that the subject knew the database for the objects. Some have conjectured that identifying objects under highly unusual viewpoints may
  • 86. require a form of cognitive problem-solving, as the visual system may not be designed to automatically compute these extreme situations (Farah, 2000). Under the circumstances of severe degradation (line drawing and unusual orientation), this requirement may have been accentuated. If so, the difference between groups is understandable. Comparison with the results of Experiment 1 shows that removing color and texture cues impaired subjects’ performance, with the largest decrement seen when objects were presented in an unusual view. This is consistent with the notion that multiple mechanisms may be at work in recognition of unusual views and that other cues to object identity, such as color or texture, become particularly important when shape information becomes sufficiently degraded (Farah, 2000). In order to see whether the decrement from unusual views was larger for line drawings than for full-color objects, an additional analysis of B. Landau et al. / Cognition 100 (2006) 483–510 503
  • 87. variance was conducted comparing the corresponding conditions across the two experiments (i.e. the data from Experiment 2 vs. those from the clear image condition of Experiment 1). In this analysis, the Group X Experiment X View interaction was not significant (F(3,44)!1), suggesting that the performance decrement associated with unusual views for each group was similar for solid full color objects and line drawings. 7. General discussion and conclusions The present experiments used full color pictures and line drawings of common objects to examine object identification in children with WS compared to normally developing children of the same mental (MA) and chronological age (CA). The difficulty of object identification was manipulated by blurring objects (full color objects) and portraying them in unusual orientations (color objects and line drawings). Overall, WS and MA children were remarkably similar under all conditions, both in terms of accuracy and in various fine-grained details of performance such as which objects they found easy or difficult to