SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 18
Download to read offline
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Toxicology Letters
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/toxlet
A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and
postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure
B. González-Alzagaa
, M. Lacasañaa,b,∗
, C. Aguilar-Garduñoc
, M. Rodríguez-Barrancoa,b
,
F. Ballesterb,c,d
, M. Rebagliatob,e
, A.F. Hernándezf
a
Andalusian School of Public Health, Granada, Spain
b
CIBER of Epidemiology and Public Health (CIBERESP), Spain
c
Centre for Public Health Research (CSISP-FISABIO), Valencia, Spain
d
University of Valencia, Spain
e
Department of Medicine, University Jaume I, Castelló de la Plana, Spain
f
Department of Legal Medicine and Toxicology, University of Granada School of Medicine, Granada, Spain
h i g h l i g h t s
• This review studied the effects of OP pesticide exposure on child neurodevelopment.
• Prenatal exposure was associated with negative effects on child mental development.
• Evidence on postnatal exposure effects on child neurodevelopment is less consistent.
• The high variability of methodologies across studies made comparisons difficult.
• Standardised methodologies are needed to perform quantitative meta-analysis.
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Received 1 July 2013
Received in revised form 5 November 2013
Accepted 18 November 2013
Available online xxx
Keywords:
Organophosphorus pesticides
Prenatal exposure
Postnatal exposure
Neurodevelopment
a b s t r a c t
Agricultural and residential use of organophosphate (OP) pesticides has increased in recent decades after
banning some persistent pesticides. Although there is evidence of the effects of OPs on neurodevelopment
and behaviour in adults, limited information is available about their effects in children, who might be more
vulnerable to neurotoxic compounds. This paper was aimed at analysing the scientific evidence published
to date on potential neurodevelopmental and behavioural effects of prenatal and postnatal exposure to
OPs. A systematic review was undertaken to identify original articles published up to December 2012
evaluating prenatal or postnatal exposure to OPs in children and effects on neurodevelopment and/or
behaviour. Articles were critically compared, focusing on the methodology used to assess exposure and
adverse effects, as well as potential contributing factors that may modify both exposure and outcomes,
such as genetic susceptibility to certain enzymes involved in OPs metabolisation (e.g. paraoxonase-1) and
gender differences. Twenty articles met the inclusion criteria, 7 of which evaluated prenatal exposure to
OPs, 8 postnatal exposure and 5 both pre- and postnatal exposure. Most of the studies evaluating prenatal
exposure observed a negative effect on mental development and an increase in attention problems in
preschool and school children. The evidence on postnatal exposure is less consistent, although 2 studies
found an increase in reaction time in schoolchildren. Some paraoxonase-1 polymorphisms could enhance
the association between OPs exposure and mental and psychomotor development. A large variability in
epidemiological designs and methodologies used for assessing exposure and outcome was observed
across the different studies, which made comparisons difficult. Prenatal and to a lesser extent postnatal
exposure to OPs may contribute to neurodevelopmental and behavioural deficits in preschool and school
children. Standardised methodologies are needed to allow results to be better compared and to perform
a quantitative meta-analysis before drawing any final conclusions.
© 2013 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
∗ Corresponding author at: Escuela Andaluza de Salud Pública, Campus Universi-
tario de Cartuja, c/Cuesta del Observatorio 4, 18180 Granada, Spain.
Tel.: +34 958 027556.
E-mail address: marina.lacasana.easp@juntadeandalucia.es (M. Lacasaña).
1. Introduction
Earlier restrictions on the use of some persistent pesticides have
led to the use of non-persistent alternatives such as organophos-
phates (OPs), carbamates and pyrethroids, which are very useful
for controlling pests in both agricultural and residential settings.
0378-4274/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
2 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
Intensive use of these compounds is posing a significant risk to
public health because of their potential adverse effects. Exposure
to pesticides does not only affect those who use them occupation-
ally, as the general population is also exposed to low concentrations
through foodstuffs and the environment throughout their life-
time. There is scientific evidence of the carcinogenic, neurological,
reproductive, immunological and genotoxic effects associated with
exposure to non-persistent pesticides in adults (Koureas et al.,
2012). However, little information is available about the effects
in children, although researchers have observed a higher risk of
adverse reproductive effects (Eskenazi et al., 2004; Lacasaña et al.,
2006; Rauch et al., 2012) and changes in the nervous system and
in neurobehavioural development (Bouchard et al., 2010; Engel
et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al.,
2012). This means that the neurotoxic effects of these compounds
on children’s central nervous systems could be causing a series of
subclinical neurodevelopmental disorders. This has been termed a
‘silent pandemic’ and could have major health, economic and social
impact (Grandjean and Landrigan, 2006).
Toxicological studies in animals have provided information
about the neurotoxicity mechanisms and adverse effects of non-
persistent pesticides. For example, Maurissen (2000) and Rice and
Barone (2000) observed changes in the nervous system develop-
ment and sensory, motor and cognitive cerebral function of rodents
associated with prenatal and early postnatal exposure to chlorpyri-
fos. Prenatal exposure even to low concentrations of chlorpyrifos
also affects rodents’ organogenesis (Tian et al., 2005) and leads to
behavioural changes such as hyperactivity and working and refer-
ence memory deficit.
Factors such as age, sex, nutritional status, lifestyle and genetic
variability can modify the effects of non-persistent pesticides in
children, who are particularly susceptible to these compounds. The
health risks derived from exposure to toxic agents are strongly
influenced by genetics as a result of the variability of the genes
that code for metabolising enzymes (Costa et al., 2003; Eaton
et al., 1998; Furlong, 2007). The different possible combinations
of these polymorphisms can determine favourable or unfavourable
metabolic configurations, either facilitating the breakdown of some
neurotoxic compounds, or bioactivating initially inactive com-
pounds or delaying the metabolic breakdown of active compounds
(Costa et al., 2005a,b; Guo et al., 2012). Recent studies have ana-
lysed the effect of different paraoxonase-1 (PON1) polymorphisms
on neurodevelopment in children. Certain genotypes (PON1–108TT,
PON1192QR and PON1192RR) may be associated with reduced men-
tal and motor development in children exposed to OP pesticides
(Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2010).
The limited number of epidemiological studies available and the
huge variability of the methodologies used to assess exposure to OP
pesticides and its effects on neurodevelopment in children make
it difficult to compare their results. The aim of this review is to
carry out a detailed analysis of the evidence gathered in the stud-
ies performed to date, taking into account some of the factors that
can modify the effects of these pesticides, such as sex and gen-
der differences, genetic variability and epigenetic factors. This will
provide a better overview of the relationship between exposure to
OP pesticides and neurodevelopment and behaviour in children.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Search strategy
A systematic review of articles in the PubMed, Scopus,
Embase and Lilacs databases was carried out using the follow-
ing key words or text word combinations: “organophosphates”
OR “organophosphorus”, “child” OR “infant”, “neurodevelopment”,
“neurobehavioral” OR “neurobehavioural” (for more details see
Supplemental Material, annex I).
2.2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria
The articles selected for the review met the following inclu-
sion criteria: (a) original articles; (b) published before or during
December 2012; (c) written in Spanish, English, French or Por-
tuguese; (d) carried out in children and adolescents up to 16 years
of age; (e) evaluating prenatal and/or postnatal exposure to OP
pesticides; (f) using general intelligence tests or specific tests to
assess changes in mental and motor development or behaviour in
children.
Case series studies were not included and literature reviews
were only used to identify other original articles which had not
been found using the search syntax used.
2.3. Data analysis
Studies that met the inclusion criteria were sorted into cat-
egories based on exposure type (prenatal or postnatal) and
compared using the following criteria: (a) study design; (b) sam-
ple size; (c) age of participants; (d) exposure assessment; (e) levels
of exposure to compounds; (f) neurodevelopment tests used; (g)
effects observed; (h) adjustment variables. Table 1 provides a sum-
mary of the neuropsychological tests used in the studies included
in this review, categorised based on age range and the areas (men-
tal, motor and behavioural development) and/or specific functions
evaluated.
The level of exposure to OP pesticides is usually measured by
determining the total dialkylphosphate (DAPs) levels in the urine.
DAPs usually include the following 6 metabolites: dimethylphos-
phate (DMP), dimethylthiophosphate (DMTP), dimethyldithio-
phosphate (DMDTP), diethylphosphate (DEP), diethylthiophos-
phate (DETP) and diethyldithiophosphate (DEDTP). In these studies,
the results are expressed in nanomoles per litre (nmol/L) once all
the figures expressed in ␮g/L have been converted.
2.3.1. Assessment of methodological quality of the articles
The methodological quality of the studies included in the review
was assessed using the ‘Strengthening the Reporting of Observa-
tional Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) - Statement’ checklist (von
Elm et al., 2008). This tool was designed to evaluate the clarity of the
results of observational studies, but it has been used in other sys-
tematic reviews for the same purposes because no other tools are
available (Olmos et al., 2008; Ricci-Cabello et al., 2010; Rodríguez-
Barranco et al., 2013).
The checklist is made up of 22 items in 6 sections (title, introduc-
tion, methods, results, discussion and other information). Each item
specifies the aspects that should be included in the study and can
be used to assess its quality. For this review, the 9 items included
in the methods section were used to give a score for the method-
ological quality of each study. The studies were categorized as low
quality (0–3 of the 9 items), moderate quality (4–6 items) and high
quality (7–9 items) (see Supplementary Material; annex II). The
methodological quality scores are summarised in Tables 2 and 3.
3. Results
134 articles were identified using the search strategy described
above. Twenty of those met the inclusion criteria, 7 of which
analysed prenatal exposure to OP pesticides, 8 analysed postna-
tal exposure, and 5 analysed both pre- and postnatal exposure.
Cohort studies were the most frequent design (n = 10), followed by
cross-sectional (n = 9) and case-control (n = 1) studies. The method-
ological quality of studies on prenatal exposure was high (n = 9) or
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
3
Table 1
Summary of tests applied in the reviewed studies to assess functions of mental development (MD), psychomotor development (PD) and behavior (B).
Agea
Test MD PD B Reference
Since birth VABS Communication Fine and gross motor Daily living skills, socialization,
maladaptive behavior
Ruckart et al., 2004
Newborn–2 months BNBAS Habituation, orientation, motor
performance, range of state, regulation
of state, autonomic stability and
reflexes
Engel et al., 2007; Young et al., 2005
1 month–12 years PSI Adaptability, acceptability,
demandingness, mood,
distractability/hyperactivity and
reinforces parent
Ruckart et al., 2004
2 months–6 years GDS Adaptive behavior, language, personal
and social behavior
Motor behavior (locomotion, reaching,
balance, comprehension, drawing and
hand control)
Guodong et al., 2012
3 months–5 years A&S Q Communication, problem solving and
personal-social
Gross motor and fine motor Handal et al., 2008
6 months–3 years BSID Language (receptive and expressive)
and cognitive development
Fine motor and gross motor Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007;
Rauh et al., 2006
2–7 years WPPSI-III Verbal comprehension, perceptual
organization and processing speed
abilities
Engel et al., 2011
2–18 years CBCL Social withdrawal, somatic complaints,
anxiety and depression, destructive
behavior, social problems, thought
problems, attention problems,
aggressive behavior and delinquent
behaviors
Eskenazi et al., 2007; Lizardi et al.,
2008; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al.,
2006
3–16 years NEPSY Attention, executive functions,
language, communication, learning,
memory and social perception
Sensor-motor and Visuospatial Attention Kofman et al., 2006; Marks et al., 2010
4–5 years K-CPT ADHD Harari et al., 2010; Marks et al., 2010
>4 years BARS Attention, memory, visual memory,
learning, motivation, response speed
Coordination Sustained attention Rohlman et al., 2005
K-Bit General intelligence, verbal ability and
nonverbal reasoning
Ruckart et al., 2004
>5 years PP Visual-motor coordination, manual
dexterity and motor speed
Ruckart et al., 2004
RCPM General intelligence Harari et al., 2010
M&L Test Memory Ruckart et al., 2004
5–19 years PIC Cognitive impairment Family Dysfunction and Psychological
Discomfort. Impulsivity and
Distractibility, Reality Distortion, Social
Withdrawal, Delinquency, Somatic
Concern and Social Skill Deficits.
Ruckart et al., 2004
6–16 years WISC-III Verbal comprehension, perceptual
organization, processing speed and
freedom from distractibility
Lizardi et al., 2008
WISC-IV Verbal comprehension, perceptive
reasoning, working memory and
processing speed
Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al.,
2011; Horton et al., 2012; Rauh et al.,
2011
WISC-R Verbal comprehension, perceptual
organization and freedom from
distractibility
Grandjean et al., 2006; Kofman et al.,
2006; Harari et al., 2010
6–18 years DISC-IV ADHD Bouchard et al., 2010
6–8 yearsb
Santa Ana Form
Board
Motor coordination Grandjean et al., 2006; Harari et al.,
2010
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
4 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
Table
1
(Continued)
Age
a
Test
MD
PD
B
Reference
6–8
years
b
CTSB
Visuospatial
and
visuoconstructional
functions
Grandjean
et
al.,
2006;
Harari
et
al.,
2010
6–8
years
b
FTT
Motor
speed
and
dexterity
Harari
et
al.,
2010
7
years
b
Catsys
force
plate
Postural
stability
Grandjean
et
al.,
2006
>7
years
WCST
Cognitive
skills
ADHD
Lizardi
et
al.,
2008
>8
years
BVRT
Visual
perception
and
visual
memory
Abdel
Rasoul
et
al.,
2008
>9
years
Trail
making
A&B
Visual
perception,
sequential
skills
and
symbol
recognition
Motor
speed
Abdel
Rasoul
et
al.,
2008;
Lizardi
et
al.,
2008;
Ruckart
et
al.,
2004
>10
years
WAIS
Verbal
comprehension,
working
memory,
perceptual
organization
and
processing
speed
Abdel
Rasoul
et
al.,
2008
ADHD:
Attention
deficit
and
hyperactivity
disorder.
Tests
abbreviations:
A&S
Q:
Ages
and
Stages
Questionnaire;
BARS:
Behavioral
Assessment
and
Research
System;
BNBAS:
Brazelton
Neonatal
Behavioral
Assessment
Scale;
BSID:
Bayley
Scales
for
Infant
Development;
BVRT:
Benton
Visual
Retention
Test;
CBCL:
Child
Behavior
Checklist;
CTSB:
Copying
test
of
the
Stanford-Binet;
DISC-IV:
Diagnostic
Interview
Schedule
for
Children
IV;
FTT:
Finger
Tapping
Test;
GDS:
Gesell
Developmental
Schedules;
K-Bit:
Kaufman
Brief
Intelligence
test;
K-CPT:
Conner’
Kiddie
Continuous
Performance;
M&L
Test:
Memory
and
learning
test;
NEPSY:
A
Developmental
Neuropsychological
Assessment;
PIC:
Personality
Inventory
for
Children;
PSI:
Parenting
Stress
Index;
PP:
Purdue
Pegboard;
RCPM:
Raven’
Colored
Progressive
Matrices;
VABS:
Vineland
Adaptive
Behavior
Scales;
WAIS:
Wechsler
Adult
Intelligence
Scale;
WCST:
Wisconsin
Card
Sorting
Test;
WISC-III:
Wechsler
Intelligence
Scale
for
Children-III;
WISC-IV:
Wechsler
Intelligence
Scale
for
Children-IV;
WPPSI-III:
Wechsler
Preschool
and
Primary
Scale
of
Intelligence-III;
WISC-R:
Wechsler
Intelligence
Scale
for
Children-Revised.
a
Age
range
in
which
tests
are
usually
applied.
b
Age
range
of
children
included
in
the
study.
moderate (n = 3), while that of studies on postnatal exposure was
high (n = 7), moderate (n = 4) and low (n = 2).
3.1. Exposure assessment
Two study types were identified based on the methodol-
ogy used to assess exposure to OP pesticides. Firstly, studies
that analyse exposure based on biological samples: (a) lev-
els of parent compounds (chlorpyrifos); (b) levels of specific
metabolites of OP pesticides (3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridinol–TCPy–as
a metabolite of chlorpyrifos; p-nitrophenol as a metabolite of
methyl-parathion); (c) levels of non-specific metabolites of OP pes-
ticides (DAPs). These compounds were detected and measured
using gas chromatography–tandem mass spectrometry (Bouchard
et al., 2010; Bouchard et al., 2011; Grandjean et al., 2006; Engel
et al., 2007; Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Harari et al.,
2010; Marks et al., 2010; Young et al., 2005), gas chromatography
with flame photometric detection (Guodong et al., 2012; Lizardi
et al., 2008), gas chromatography-high-resolution mass spectrom-
etry (Horton et al., 2012; Rauh et al., 2006; Rauh et al., 2011)
and liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (Lu et al.,
2009). Secondly, other studies assessed exposure using question-
naires. Prenatal exposure was evaluated based on the mother’s
employment during pregnancy, especially in agricultural work
(Grandjean et al., 2006; Handal et al., 2008; Harari et al., 2010),
and postnatal exposure was assessed based on proximity to farm-
land (Rohlman et al., 2005), the children’s medical history of OP
pesticide poisoning (Kofman et al., 2006) and the agricultural work
carried out by the children (Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008).
3.2. Prenatal Exposure
The effects associated with prenatal exposure observed in the
different studies are summarised in Tables 2 and 4.
The effects in the first stages of children’s lives were analysed
in two birth cohort studies, one in a multi-ethnic population in
New York (Engel et al., 2007) and another in a Hispanic population
living in farming communities in California (CHAMACOS Project)
(Young et al., 2005). Both studies found that an increase in urinary
DAPs levels during pregnancy was associated with an increase in
the frequency of abnormal reflexes in children up to 2 months of
age, and this increase was statistically significant in the study by
Young et al. (2005). However, neither of the two studies found a link
between prenatal exposure to OP and other neonatal behaviours
such as habituation, orientation, motor, range of state, regulation
of state or autonomic stability.
In the CHAMACOS cohort, urinary DAPs concentrations during
pregnancy were inversely and significantly associated with the
child’s mental development index (MDI) at 24 months, but not
at 6 or 12 months (Eskenazi et al., 2007). Similarly, in the New
York birth cohort there was an inverse, but not significant, associa-
tion between maternal urinary DAPs concentrations and the MDI of
black and Hispanic children at 12 months (Engel et al., 2011). How-
ever, in white children the levels of these metabolites were not
significantly associated with a higher MDI. Neither of the two stud-
ies found significant associations between maternal DAPs levels
and the children’s psychomotor development index (PDI).
There is also evidence of the effects of prenatal exposure to OP
pesticides in children of school age. In the CHAMACOS cohort, an
increase in the average levels of maternal urinary DAPs during preg-
nancy was associated with a significant decrease in the Perceptual
Reasoning (PR), Working Memory (WM), Verbal Comprehension
(VC) and Processing Speed (PS) scores and full scale intelligence
quotient (FSIQ) of children aged 7 (Bouchard et al., 2011). The same
trend was observed by Engel et al. (2011) but the decrease was not
statistically significant. Furthermore, a significant link was found
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
5
Table 2
Studies on prenatal exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopmental and behavioural outcomes in children.
Prenatal exposure
Author, year Country Study
design
n Age Biological
samples/timing
Analyte assayed
Median (95%CI)
Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ
Engel et al., 2007 USA P 311 Newborns (before
hospital discharge)
Maternal urine
during pregnancy
(mean gestational
age: 31,2 weeks;
SD = ±3,7)
DAPs (nmol/L) 82
(35.2–194.7)a
BNBAS Increase of
abnormal reflexes
Examiner,
anesthesia during
delivery, PON1
enzyme tertiles,
urinary creatinine
and overdispersion.
H
Young et al., 2005 USA P 381 Up to 2 months Maternal urine
before and after
delivery (up to 1
week)
DAPs (nmol/L)
Average
pregnancy: 132
(4,17–12)a
;
Post-delivery: 222
(7–21.87)a
BNBAS Significant
increment of
abnormal reflexes
among 3 days old
infants. No
significant
differences in
Habituation,
Orientation, Motor
performance,
Range of state,
Regulation of state
and Autonomic
stability.
Age at BNBAS,
maternal age at
delivery, smoking,
vitamin use, BNBAS
interviewer, and
mean diastolic and
systolic blood
pressure.
M
Handal et al., 2008 Ecuador C-S 121 3–21 months Questionnaire Pesticides AS Q (all ages).
Tests of prehension
and Visual acuity
test (only 9–18
months)
Mother working in
the floriculture
during pregnancy ↓
scores on
communication,
motor skills and
visual acuity.
Child’s age,
mother’s education
(communication).
Anemia, stunting,
housing
construction,
pesticide use at
home during
pregnancy (for
gross motor).
Anemia,
stimulation at
home, housing
construction (for
fine motor).
M
Eskenazi et al., 2007 USA P 396 6–24 months Maternal urine
during pregnancy
(1st: 14–26 weeks;
2nd: shortly after
delivery)
DAPs (nmol/L)
114.9b
(105.7–125.0) TCPy
(␮g/L) = 3.54
BSID-II CBCL Significant ↓ MDI at
24 months. No
association at 6 or
12 months. No
effect on PDI at 6,
24 or 36 months.
TCPy not associated
with MDI or PDI.
Psychometrician,
location, exact age
at assessment, sex,
breast-feeding
duration, HOME
score, household
income above
poverty threshold,
parity and
maternal PPVT.
H
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
6
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
Table 2 (Continued)
Prenatal exposure
Author, year Country Study
design
n Age Biological
samples/timing
Analyte assayed
Median (95%CI)
Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ
Engel et al., 2011 USA P 200 12 months Maternal urine
during pregnancy
(26–28 weeks)
DAPs (nmol/L)
81.3 (31.7–198.1)a
BSID-II ↓ MDI among Black
and Hispanic
children. ↑ MDI
among White
children.
Maternal age at
enrolment, child
sex, examiner,
maternal PON1
enzyme activity,
season of urine
collection,
laboratory batch,
HOME score,
alcohol
consumption
during pregnancy,
urinary creatinine.
H
Rauh et al., 2006 USA P 254 12–36 months Umbilical cord
blood. Maternal
blood (2 days after
delivery)
Chlorpyrifos (pg/g)
High exposure
6.17, Low
exposure ≤6.17
BSID-II CBCL High exposure
(6.17 pg/g)
significantly
associated with ↓
MDI and PDI at 36
months. No effects
on MDI and PDI
Scores at 12 and 24
months.
Race, gender,
maternal
education,
maternal IQ,
gestational age,
prenatal ETS
exposure.
H
Marks et al., 2010 USA P 331 3.5–5 años Maternal urine
(Mean gestation
age. 1st sample: 14
week, 2nd sample:
26 week)
DAPs (nmol/L)
109b
(99.4–119.6)
NEPSY-II (3.5
years) K-CPT (5
years) CBCL (3.5
and 5 years)
Significant increase
of Attention
problems and
ADHD at 5 years.
Psychometrician,
age at assessment,
sex, child care,
breast-feeding,
maternal
education,
depressive
symptoms and
PPVT.
H
Harari et al., 2010 Ecuador C-S 84 6–8 years Questionnaire Pesticides WISC-R (WM) FTT,
Santa Ana Form
Board, K-CPT, CTSB
and RCPM.
Maternal exposure
significantly
associated with
↓scores in FTT,
Santa Ana Form
Board and CTSB.
Sex, age, BMI,
number of daily
meals (only for
current exposure),
stunting,
hematrocrit, school
grade, having
repeated one
grade, maternal
educational level,
family living in a
traditional house,
drinking water
supply, paternal
education and
employment.
H
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
7
Table 2 (Continued)
Prenatal exposure
Author, year Country Study
design
n Age Biological
samples/timing
Analyte assayed
Median (95%CI)
Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ
Engel et al., 2011 USA P 210 6–9 years Maternal urine
during pregnancy
(26–28 weeks)
DAPs (nmol/L)
81.3 (31.7–198.1)a
WISC-IV (7–9
years): PR
(BD/PC/MR), VC
(Sim/Voc/Com),
WM (DS/LNS), PS
(SS), FSIQ
WPPSI-III (6 years):
PR (BD/MR/PC), VC
(Inf/Voc/WR), PS
(SS/Cod), FSIQ
Slight decrement in
FSIQ, PR, PS and
WM (WISC-IV) and
in FSIQ, PR, VC and
PS (WPPSI-III)
Sex, race/ethnicity,
maternal
education,
language in the
home, maternal
PON1 enzymatic
activity, alcohol
use in pregnancy,
batch season of
urine collection,
urinary creatinine.
H
Horton et al., 2012 USA P 335 7 years Umbilical cord
blood plasma
Chlorpyrifos (ng/g)
0.36 (0.25–32.1)c
WISC-IV (WM,VC,
PR, PS, FSIQ)
Borderline
significant
interaction
between prenatal
exposure to
chlopyrifos and
child sex: ↓ WM
score among males.
Total HOME score,
Parental
nurturance and
environmental
stimulation.
M
Bouchard et al., 2011 USA P 329 7 years Maternal urine
during pregnancy
(≤20 weeks and
20 weeks)
DAPs
(nm/L) = 128
WISC-IV: PR
(BD/MR), VC
(Voc/Sim), WM
(DS/LNS), PS
(Cod/SS), FSIQ
Signficant
decrement in FSIQ,
WM, PS, VC and PR.
HOME score at 6
months and
maternal education
and intelligence.
VC and FSIQ
adjusted for
language
assessment.
Creatinine-
adjusted
urine.
H
Rauh et al., 2011 USA P 265 7 years Umbilical cord
blood plasma
Chlorpyrifos (pg/g)
3.17d
(0.09–32)c
WISC-IV (WM, VC,
PR, PS, FSIQ)
Significant
decrement in WM
and FSIQ.
Child sex,
race/ethnicity/maternal
IQ/maternal
education income,
child age at testing,
ETS and PAH.
H
Grandjean et al., 2006 Ecuador C-S 72 7 years Questionnaire Pesticides WISC-R: WM (DS).
Santa Ana
Pegborad, CTSB and
Catsys force plate
Prenatal exposure
significantly
associated with ↓
scores in CTSB.
Age, sex, trauma,
other injury and
meningitis in the
past medical
history, maternal
education and
current nutrition.
H
Study design abbreviations. C-S: Cross-sectional; P: Prospective; MQ: Methodology Quality; H: High; M: Medium. Tests and subtests abbreviations. AS Q: Ages and Stages Questionnaire; BD: Block Design; BNBAS: Brazelton
Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale; BSID-II: Bayley Scales for Infant Development; CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; Cod: Coding; Com: Comprehension; CTSB: Copying test of the Stanford-Binet; DS: Digit Span; FSIQ: Full
Scale Intelligence Quotient; FTT: Finger Tapping Test; Inf: Information; K-CPT: Conner’ Kiddie Continuous Performance; LNS: Letter-Number Sequence; MR: Matrix Reasoning; NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological
Assessment; PC: Picture Concepts; PR: Perceptual Reasoning; PS: Processing Speed; RCPM: Raven’ Colored Progressive Matrices; Sim: Similarities; SS: Symbol Search; VC: Verbal Comprehension; Voc: Vocabulary; WISC-IV:
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV; WISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WM: Working Memory; WPPSI-III: Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III; WR: Word Reasoning.
a
Interquartile range.
b
Geometric mean.
c
Range.
d
Mean are reported when median or 95% CI were not available.
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
8
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
Table 3
Studies on postnatal exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopmental and behavioural outcomes in children.
Postnatal exposure
Author, year Country Study
design
n Age Biological
samples/timing
Analyte assayed
median (95%CI)
Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ
Eskenazi et al., 2007 USA P 396 6–24 months Child urine (6,12
and 24 months)
DAPs (nmol/L) 6
months 45.5
(39.6–52.3); 12
months 59.5
(51.7–68.5); 24
months 70.9
(61.4–81.9)
BSDI, CBCL Significant ↑MDI at
24 months. No
association at 6 or
12 months. No
effects on PDI at 6,
12 or 24 months.
Psychometrician,
location, exact age at
assessment, sex,
breast-feeding
duration, HOME score,
household income
above poverty
threshold, parity and
maternal PPVT.
H
Guodong et al., 2012 China C-S 301 23–25 months Child urine DAPs (nmol/L)
DMP 20.16
(LOD-1495.9)a
;
DMTP 11.06
(LOD-228.3)a
;
DEP 11.64
(LOD-210.39)a
;
DETP 18.82
(LOD-317.8)a
;
DEDTP n.c.
(LOD-20.54)a
GDS No effects
observed.
Child sex, maternal
education level,
household income.
Creatinine-adjusted
urine.
M
Rohlman et al., 2005 USA C-S
(with
follow
up)
78 48–71 months Questionnaire OP BARS Exposed children
performed poorer
on Finger tapping
and Match to
Sample compared
to Non-exposed
children.
Age, mother’s
education.
M
Marks et al., 2010 USA P 331 3.5–5 years Child urine at 3.5
and 5 years.
DAPs (nmol/L)
3.5 years = 77.5
(65.4–91.9); 5
years = 92.6
(78.6–109)
NEPSY-II (3.5
years); K-CPT (5
years); CBCL (3.5
and 5 years)
Limited evidence of
association
between child
DAPs and attention.
Psychometrician, age at
assessment, sex, child
care, breast-feeding,
maternal education,
depressive symptoms
and PPVT.
H
Lu et al., 2009 Costa Rica C-S 35 17 + 18 4–10 years Two spot urine
samples of
children: evening
day 1 and first
morning urine day
2
TCPy (␮g/L); La
Amistad = 0d
(0–3.31)a
; Las
Mellizas = 0d
(0–6.71)a
BARS TCPy levels and
BARS scores not
associated.
Age, gender,
conventional or
organic group,
handedness and grade.
L
Harari et al., 2010 Ecuador C-S 84 6–8 years Child urine DAPs (levels not
available)
WISC-R: WM (DS);
FTT, Santa Ana
Form Board, K-CPT,
CTSB and RCPM.
Current exposure
associated with ↑
Reaction time
(with a borderline
statistical
significance)
Sex, age, BMI, number
of daily meals (only for
current exposure),
stunting, hematrocrit,
school grade, having
repeated one grade,
maternal educational
level, family living in a
traditional house,
drinking water supply,
paternal education and
employment.
Creatinine-adjusted
urine.
H
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
9
Table 3 (Continued)
Postnatal exposure
Author, year Country Study
design
n Age Biological
samples/timing
Analyte assayed
median (95%CI)
Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ
Kofman et al., 2006 Israel C-C
(age-
sex
match)
18 6–12 years Acute intoxication
by OP before age 3
OP WISC-R: WM (DS)
NEPSY
Significant deficit
in motor control
among children
exposed to OP.
Age M
Ruckart et al., 2004 USA P 652 6-aged
children or
younger at the
moment of the
spray with MP
Wipe samples
Urine of family
members
Methyl Parathion
(MP) in wipe
samples
Exposed:
≥150 ␮g/cm2
(Mississippi) and
≥132.9 ␮g/cm2
(Ohio)
p-nitrphenol (PNP)
in urine (ppb)
Exposed: PNP ≥
100 ppb for at least
one person in the
household
Non-exposed: no
one in the
household with
PNP 100 ppb
K-Bit, Purdue
Pegboard, ML
Test, Trail Making
AB, PSI, PIC, VABS.
No differences
between exposed
and non exposed
children.
Income, race, ethnicity,
mother’ use of
chemical at work, child
had lead or mercury
poisoning and mother
had one or more of
these conditionsc
H
Bouchard et al., 2011 USA P 329 7 years Child urine (6
months, 1, 2, 3.5
and 5 years)
DAPs (levels not
available)
WISC-IV: VC
(Voc/Sim), PR
(BD/MR), WM
(DS/LNS), PS
(Cod/SS), FSIQ
Significant ↑ scores
in VC and FSIQ at
12 months.
Exposure no
consistently
associated with
poorer scores in
FSIQ, WM, PS, VC
and PR at 6 months,
2, 3.5 and 5 years.
HOME score at 6
months and maternal
education and
intelligence. VC and
FSIQ adjusted for
language assessment.
Creatinine-adjusted
urine.
H
Lizardi et al., 2008 USA C-S 48 7 years Child urine DAPs (nmol/L)
Exposed Group:
118.28b
(95.7–149.5)
Non-exposed
Group = 52.68b
(38.7–67.74)
WISC-III,WCST,
Trail Making Test A
B, CBCL
No significant
association
between levels of
OP metabolites and
performance on
WISC-III.
– L
Please
cite
this
article
in
press
as:
González-Alzaga,
B.,
et
al.,
A
systematic
review
of
neurodevelopmental
effects
of
prenatal
and
postnatal
organophosphate
pesticide
exposure.
Toxicol.
Lett.
(2013),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE
IN
PRESS
G
Model
TOXLET-8548;
No.
of
Pages
18
10
B.
González-Alzaga
et
al.
/
Toxicology
Letters
xxx
(2013)
xxx–
xxx
Table 3 (Continued)
Postnatal exposure
Author, year Country Study
design
n Age Biological
samples/timing
Analyte assayed
median (95%CI)
Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ
Grandjean et al., 2006 Ecuador C-S 72 7 years Child urine DMP (nmol/kg
per day)
Prenatally
exposed = 0.2d
(max 6.94);
Non-prenatally
exposed = 0.42d
(max 6.53)
DEP (nmol/kg per
day)
Prenatally
exposed = 0.27d
(max 14.62)
Non-prenatally
exposed = 0.43d
(max 2.49)
WISC-R: WM (DS)
Santa Ana
Pegboard, CTSB and
Catsys force plate
Current exposure
significantly
associated with ↑
reaction time.
Age, sex, trauma other
injury and meningitis
in the past medical
history, maternal
education and current
nutrition.
Creatinine-adjusted
urine.
H
Bouchard et al., 2010 USA C-S 1139 8–15 years Child urine DAPs (nmol/L)
68.3 (6-10195)a
DISC-IV Significant risk of
ADHD diagnostic.
Gender, age,
race/ethnicity, PIR and
fasting duration.
H
Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008 Egypt C-S 60 9–15 years Questionnaire.
AChE levels in child
blood
Pesticides WAIS: VC (Inf/Sim),
WM (Ar), PR (BD)
BVRT, DS and Trail
Making AB
Pesticides
applicators
significantly ↓
scores on all the
measures
compared to the
control group.
Effects of age and
education (13-year-old
individual with 6 years
of education and a
body mass index (BMI)
of 21.7).
M
Study design abbreviations: C-S: Cross-sectional; P: Prospective; C-C: Case-Control. MQ: Methodology Quality; H: High; M: Medium; L: Low. Tests and subtests abbreviations. Ar: Arithmetic; BARS: Behavioral Assessment and
Research System; BSID: Bayley Scales for Infant Development; BD: Block Design; BVRT: Benton Visual Retention Test; CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; Cod: Coding; CTSB: Copying test of the Stanford-Binet; DISC-IV: Diagnostic
Interview Schedule for Children IV; DS: Digit Span; FSIQ: Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; FTT: Finger Tapping Test; GDS: Gesell Developmental Schedules; Inf: Information; K-Bit: Kaufman Brief Intelligence test; K-CPT: Conner’
Kiddie Continuous Performance; LNS: Letter-Number Sequence; ML Test: Memory and learning test; MR: Matrix Reasoning; NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PIC: Personality Inventory for Children;
PR: Perceptual Reasoning; BD: Block Design; PS: Processing Speed; PSI: Parenting Stress Index; RCPM: Raven’ Colored Progressive Matrices; Sim: Similarities; SS: Symbol Search; VABS: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales; VC:
Verbal Comprehension; Voc: Vocabulary; WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WCST Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III; WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV;
WISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WM: Working Memory.
a
Range.
b
Mean.
c
Diabetes or epilepsy/seizures before pregnancy, hospitalized or confined to bed during pregnancy, fever, X rays, or vaginal bleeding during pregnancy.
d
Median were reported when geometric mean or 95% CI were not available.
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 11
Table 4
Summary of effects of prenatal exposure to OP pesticides on mental development, psychomotor development, attention deficit hiperactivity disorder (ADHD) and pervasive
development disorder (PDD).
Compounds Age Findings Reference
Mental development Psychomotor development Attention problems ADHD PDD
DMP
↑ DMP Newborns ↓*
Engel et al., 2007
2 months ↓*
Young et al., 2005
6 months ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↓black/hispanic ↑white ↓black/hispanic ↑white Engel et al., 2011
↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↓ ↑ Engel et al., 2011
↓*
↓*
↑ ↑ ↑*
Eskenazi et al., 2007
3.5 years ↑*
↑*
Marks et al., 2010
5 years ↑*
↑*
Marks et al., 2010
6–9 years ↓ Engel et al., 2011
7 years ↓*
Bouchard et al., 2011
DEP
↑ DEP Newborns ↓ Engel et al., 2007
2 months ↓*
Young et al., 2005
6 months ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↓black/hispanic ↑white ↓black/hispanic ↑white Engel et al., 2011
↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↓ ↑ Engel et al., 2011
↓ ↓ ↑ ↑ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
3.5 years – ↓ Marks et al., 2010
5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010
6–9 years ↓ Engel et al., 2011
7 years ↓*
Bouchard et al., 2011
DAPs
↑ DAPs Newborns ↓*
Engel et al., 2007
2 months ↓*
Young et al., 2005
6 months ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↓black/hispanic ↑white ↓black/hispanic ↑white Engel et al., 2011
↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↓ ↑ Engel et al., 2011
↓*
↓ ↑ ↑ ↑*
Eskenazi et al., 2007
3.5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010
5 years ↑*
↑*
Marks et al., 2010
6–9 years ↓ Engel et al., 2011
7 years ↓*
Bouchard et al., 2011
Chlorpyrifos
↑ Chlorpyrifos 12 months ↓ ↓*
Rauh et al., 2006
24 months ↓ ↑ Rauh et al., 2006
36 months ↓*
↓*
↑*
↑*
↑*
Rauh et al., 2006
7 years ↓*
Horton et al., 2012
7 years ↓*
Rauh et al., 2011
↑ TCPy 6 months ↑ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↓ ↓ – – – Eskenazi et al., 2007
Pesticides (questionnaire) 3–21 months ↓ ↓ Handal et al., 2008
6–8 years ↓*
↓*
Harari et al., 2010
7 years ↓*
– Grandjean et al., 2006
(–) no effect observed; blank cells indicate effects that were not measured.
*
p  0.1.
between occupational exposure to OP pesticides during pregnancy
in farming communities in Ecuador and a decrease in the visual-
spatial function of children aged 6 -8 years (Grandjean et al., 2006;
Harari et al., 2010), and in their visual memory and motor coordi-
nation and speed (Harari et al., 2010).
The evidence of a link between prenatal exposure to OP pesti-
cides and behavioural disorders is limited because few studies have
been carried out. These effects were assessed in the CHAMACOS
cohort when the children were 2, 3.5 and 5 years of age (Eskenazi
et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010). Average urinary DAPs levels during
pregnancy were associated with a significantly higher risk of perva-
sive developmental disorder (PDD) at 2 years of age, but not with
other attention problems or attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
der (ADHD) (Eskenazi et al., 2007). However, an increased risk of
ADHD was observed at 3.5 years, and a significantly higher risk was
observed at the 5-year point (Marks et al., 2010).
With regard to the effects of exposure to chlorpyrifos on neu-
rodevelopment and behaviour in children, those with  6.17 pg/g
of chlorpyrifos in umbilical cord blood had significantly lower MDI
and PDT scores at 36 months (Rauh et al., 2006). This trend was also
observed at the 7-year point, and it has been estimated that for each
standard deviation increase in chlorpyrifos exposure (4.61 pg/g
in cord blood), full-scale intelligence quotient (IQ) declined by
1.4% and working memory declined by 2.8% (Horton et al., 2012;
Rauh et al., 2011). However, Eskenazi et al. (2007) found no link
between maternal urinary levels of TCPy, the specific metabo-
lite of chlorpyrifos, and children’s MDI or PDI scores at 6 and 24
months.
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
12 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
3.3. Postnatal Exposure
Thirteen studies analysed the association between postnatal
exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopmental and/or neu-
robehavioural effects in children. Overall, no clear trend was
observed during the first few years of life. The results of the different
studies are summarised in Tables 3 and 5.
An increase in urinary DAPs levels in children aged 6-8 was asso-
ciated with a significantly higher reaction time in the studies by
Grandjean et al. (2006) and Harari et al. (2010).
Other studies assessed postnatal exposure to OP pesticides indi-
rectly based on children’s agricultural work (Abdel Rasoul et al.,
2008), the proximity of their homes to farmland, their parents
working in farming (Rohlman et al., 2005) and the children’s his-
tory of OP poisoning (Kofman et al., 2006). These studies found
significant decreases in the children’s verbal comprehension (VC),
working memory (WM) and perceptual reasoning (PR) scores
(Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008), and changes in their response times and
latency (Rohlman et al., 2005) and motor control (Kofman et al.,
2006).
However, other studies found no association between postnatal
exposure to OP pesticides and effects on children’s motor or cogni-
tive function (Bouchard et al., 2011; Guodong et al., 2012; Lizardi
et al., 2008; Lu et al., 2009; Ruckart et al., 2004). Some studies even
reached the opposite conclusion, as a significant link was found
between an increase in urinary DAPs levels and increased MDI in
children aged 12-24 months (Eskenazi et al., 2007).
In terms of behavioural effects, high levels of urinary DAPs
have been linked with a significant increase in attention problems
(Harari et al., 2010) and ADHD (Lizardi et al., 2008; Bouchard et al.,
2010) in children aged 6-15, and PDD in children aged 2 (Eskenazi
et al., 2007).
4. Discussion
4.1. Effects potentially associated with prenatal and postnatal
exposure to OPs
The results of the aforementioned studies suggest that prena-
tal exposure to OP pesticides may affect neurodevelopment and
behaviour in infancy and childhood as well as children’s cognitive
and motor function.
The average maternal urinary DAPs in the studies reviewed
ranged from 81.3 nmol/L (Engel et al., 2007) to 132 nmol/L (Young
et al., 2005). These levels are slightly higher than those found
in the pregnant women who took part in the National Health
and Nutrition Survey 1999–2000 (NHANES), where they ranged
from 70.5 nmol/L to 72.0 nmol/L (Bradman et al., 2005). However,
25% of pregnant women in the NHANES study had DAPs levels of
246.2 nm/L (percentile 75), higher than those found in the stud-
ies where neurodevelopmental and/or behavioural changes were
observed (Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2011; Marks et al.,
2010). It is therefore essential to assess exposure to OP pesticides
not just in populations with a higher probability of exposure, but
also in the general population, in order to identify subgroups in
young children with higher risk of developing neurodevelopmental
changes.
All of the studies that determined the urinary DAPs levels of
pregnant women collected two urine samples over the course of
the pregnancy, except Engel et al. (2007, 2011), who collected only
one sample in the third trimester. The collection of several urine
samples enable to assess if prenatal exposure to OP pesticides has
been more or less constant throughout the pregnancy or if there
have been periods of higher or lower exposure. This information is
very important because there are critical windows associated with
a higher vulnerability, as occurs during organogenesis or the devel-
opment of the central nervous system (Rice and Barone, 2000). The
knowledge of the time-point of exposure to OP pesticides during
pregnancy is more informative to predict potential impacts on the
child’s motor and cognitive function.
Although there is some evidence of these effects in newborns,
the most consistent evidence has been found in children aged 24
months and over (Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh
et al., 2006). This delay between the time of exposure and the
appearance of the effect is one of the least-understood aspects in
this area. According to Rice and Barone (2000), exposure to a neuro-
toxic compound during a critical window of development may only
manifest later in time because exposure occurs at a time when cas-
cading development processes are taking place. These processes
begin during gestation and continue during the first stages of
infancy, and this could explain why these effects on cognitive func-
tion are observed at this point and not at an earlier age. It is also
difficult to detect these changes until they are quite advanced, and
some neuropsychological tests are not very sensitive. This could
account for by the delay between exposure and effect.
It is difficult to draw conclusions about the possible effects
of postnatal exposure to OP pesticides on neurodevelopment and
behaviour in children, because in some studies the level of signif-
icance of the association decreased when postnatal exposure was
analysed instead of prenatal exposure (Marks et al., 2010), or the
results for postnatal exposure were not consistent with those for
prenatal exposure (Eskenazi et al., 2007).
These differences between the effects associated with pre- and
postnatal exposure could be linked to neuroplasticity. Some animal
studies suggest that the nervous system can be adapted to expo-
sure to environmental toxins and compensate for their effects on
the organism (Rice and Barone, 2000; Selemon, 2013). This neuro-
plasticity has also been studied in children and adolescents where
a continuous development of the structures responsible for motor
function and oral communication has been observed in subjects
aged 4–17 (Paus et al., 1999). However, further studies are needed
to evaluate the effects of pre- and postnatal exposure to OP pesti-
cides using a common methodology to obtain more evidence and
evaluate the consistency of findings.
4.2. Variability in the methodologies used to evaluate exposure to
OP pesticides and effects on neurodevelopment
The wide variety of different techniques used to evaluate expo-
sure to OP pesticides and its effects on neurodevelopment and
behaviour is a major limitation when the available results are to
be compared.
The possibility of carrying out a meta-analysis of the studies
included in this systematic review was considered, distinguish-
ing between prenatal and postnatal exposure, in order to estimate
the overall effects of exposure to OP pesticides on neurodevelop-
ment and/or behaviour in children. However, this was not possible
because of the large variability in: (a) methods used to evaluate
exposure to OP pesticides (biomarkers vs. questionnaires, use of
different biomarkers); (b) windows of exposure; (c) neuropsychol-
ogical tests used to assess children’s mental and motor functions;
(d) age of the children; and (e) statistical methods used to evaluate
links between exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopment in
children.
Study design is another of the aspects that must be considered
when comparing the results of different studies. Cross-sectional
studies provide information about levels of exposure to a particular
compound, while prospective studies provide information about
exposure at different time-points (e.g., during pregnancy and in
the first few years of life in the case of birth cohorts). This makes it
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 13
Table 5
Summary of effects of postnatal exposure to OP pesticides on mental development, psychomotor development, attention deficit hiperactivity disorder (ADHD) and pervasive
development disorder (PDD).
Compounds Age Findings Reference
Mental development Psychomotor development Attention problems ADHD PDD
DMP
↑ DMP 6 months ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↑ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↑*
↑ ↑ ↑ ↑*
Eskenazi et al., 2007
23–25 months ↑ ↓ Guodong et al., 2012
3.5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010
5 years ↓ – Marks et al., 2010
7 years ↓*
↓*
Grandjean et al., 2006
8–15 years ↑*
Bouchard et al., 2010
DEP
↑ DEP 6 months ↑ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↑*
↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↑ ↑ ↑ ↑ ↑*
Eskenazi et al., 2007
23–25 months ↓ ↑ Guodong et al., 2012
3.5 years ↑*
↑ Marks et al., 2010
5 years – ↑ Marks et al., 2010
7 years ↓*
↓*
Grandjean et al., 2006
8–15 years ↑ Bouchard et al., 2010
DAPs
↑ DAPs 6 months ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
12 months ↑*
↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007
24 months ↑*
↑ ↑ ↑ ↑*
Eskenazi et al., 2007
23–25 months ↑ ↑ Guodong et al., 2012
3.5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010
5 years – – Marks et al., 2010
6–8 years ↓ ↓ ↑*
Harari et al., 2010
↑ Bouchard et al., 2011
7 years – ↑*
Lizardi et al., 2008
8–15 years ↑*
Bouchard et al., 2010
↑ Methyl Parathion 6 years ↑ ↑ ↑ Ruckart et al., 2004
↑ TCPy 4–10 years – – Lu et al., 2009
OP Pesticides (questionnaire) 48–71 months ↓*
↓*
Rohlman et al., 2005
6–12 years – ↓*
Kofman et al., 2006
9–15 years ↓*
↓*
Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008
(–) no effect observed; blank cells indicate effects that were not measured.
*
p  0.1.
possible to determine if exposure to these toxins over a particular
period of time has been constant or sporadic.
Most of the studies (77%) that analysed prenatal exposure to
OP pesticides used a longitudinal design (Bouchard et al., 2011;
Engel et al., 2007; Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Horton
et al., 2012; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006; Rauh et al.,
2011; Young et al., 2005). However, studies evaluating postnatal
exposure mainly used the cross-sectional approach (61.5%) (Abdel
Rasoul et al., 2008; Bouchard et al., 2010; Grandjean et al., 2006;
Guodong et al., 2012; Harari et al., 2010; Lizardi et al., 2008; Lu et al.,
2009; Rohlman et al., 2005), followed by the longitudinal approach
(30.7%) (Bouchard et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al.,
2010; Ruckart et al., 2004) or a case-control design (7.8%) (Kofman
et al., 2006).
Prospective studies provide better information about pre- and
postnatal exposure to OP pesticides and the effects observed in
each window of exposure. They can therefore be used to identify
critical stages in the development of a child’s nervous system and
provide more evidence of how these effects develop in each stage.
The availability of several measurements of exposure also provides
information about intraindividual variability in terms of exposure
to OP pesticides, as a single measurement of these non-persistent
pesticides is not a good indicator of an individual’s usual exposure.
Griffith et al. (2011) measured children’s urinary DAPs levels over
a period of 21 months and found that the individual DAPs levels
varied by 3–7 folds depending on the metabolite analysed, and
day-to-day variability was much greater than the between-child
variability. Inter- and intraindividual variability is a major limi-
tation of cross-sectional studies, as reported Sudakin and Stone
(2011).
The use of biomonitoring techniques to evaluate exposure to
environmental pollutants is becoming more common. In fact, 9 of
the 12 studies that evaluated prenatal exposure to OP pesticides
and 10 of the 13 studies that evaluated postnatal exposure deter-
mined the levels of the biomarkers as a measure of exposure to
these compounds in the urine or blood.
Most of the studies that analysed prenatal exposure by mea-
suring specific or non-specific (DAPs) biomarkers for OP observed
significant or near significant effects on neurodevelopment and
behaviour in children (Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2007;
Eskenazi et al., 2007; Horton et al., 2012; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh
et al., 2006; Rauh et al., 2011; Young et al., 2005). The same
occurred in studies that estimated exposure by using question-
naires (Grandjean et al., 2006; Harari et al., 2010). However, the
percentage of studies on postnatal exposure where evidence of
the effects was observed was lower when biomonitoring tech-
niques were used (3 out of 10) as compared to the utilisation of
a questionnaire (3 out of 3). Since questionnaires may examine the
accumulated exposure, to a compound, this would explain why the
effects observed in these studies were greater (Abdel Rasoul et al.,
2008; Kofman et al., 2006; Rohlman et al., 2005).
Biomonitoring techniques can be used to accurately evaluate
current exposure to non-persistent pesticides by measuring the
amount of the parent compound or one of its metabolites in urine
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
14 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
or blood samples, or to analyse accumulated exposure using hair
or meconium samples. Biomarkers of exposure to OP pesticides
in the mother or child’s urine and, to a lesser extent, in umbili-
cal cord blood samples were more often measured in the studies
included in this review. Because of their chemical structure, OP
pesticides are quickly metabolised and eliminated 24–72 h after
exposure (Fourth National Report on Human Exposure to Environ-
mental Chemicals, 2009; Maroni et al., 2000; WHO, 1996). This
means that analysing their presence in urine (OP metabolites) or
blood samples (original compounds) provides information about
recent exposure instead of accumulated exposure, and this makes
it difficult to estimate representative exposure. In the case of prena-
tal exposure, meconium samples could provide information about
the levels of exposure of the foetus from the second trimester until
birth (Bearer et al., 1999; Ostrea, 1999). Some studies that mea-
sured DAPs levels in meconium samples highlighted the usefulness
of this biological matrix for the evaluation of prenatal exposure to
OP pesticides (Tsatsakis et al., 2009; Whyatt and Barr, 2001). Recent
studies have also discussed the importance of hair as an indica-
tor of chronic exposure to these compounds (Tsatsakis et al., 2008,
2010). Measuring DAPs levels in hair could provide information
about exposure over a longer period (months), representative of
the child’s accumulated exposure (medium/long-term exposure)
during the postnatal period (Tsatsakis et al., 2008).
Analyses of prenatal and/or postnatal exposure to OP pesti-
cides have generally been carried out based on urinary DAPs levels.
However, because DAPs are non-specific metabolites it is not pos-
sible to determine the exact compound to which the subjects have
been exposed to, as these metabolites are common to several OPs
and a single OP can produce several types of DAPs (Barr et al.,
2004). Moreover, the population may be directly exposed to these
metabolites present in foodstuffs and in the atmosphere (Sudakin
and Stone, 2011) as a result of the spontaneous breakdown of the
original pesticides. There is evidence that this type of exposure con-
tributes to total urinary DAPs levels in children (Curl et al., 2002,
2003), as diet is one of the major sources of exposure to OP pesti-
cides amongst this population group (Aprea et al., 2000; Lu et al.,
2008). Despite these limitations, DAPs levels are used frequently
in epidemiological studies as occurred in almost all of the studies
included in this review. Only 5 of the 20 studies measured the con-
centrations of parent compounds such as chlorpyrifos (Horton et al.,
2012; Rauh et al., 2011; Rauh et al., 2006) or a specific metabolite
such as 3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridinol (TCPy) (Eskenazi et al., 2007)
or p-nitrophenol (PNP) (Ruckart et al., 2004), which represent the
leaving groups of chlorpyrifos and parathion or methyl-parathion,
respectively.
Distinguishing between residential and dietary exposure is a key
part of the process of characterising children’s exposure to OP pesti-
cides. However, the epidemiological studies analysing exposure to
these compounds provide little information about the role of diet
(Jurewicz and Hanke, 2008). Only 3 of the 20 studies included in
this review assessed the influence of diet on levels of exposure and
the effects studied (Grandjean et al., 2006; Guodong et al., 2012;
Harari et al., 2010).
There is scant research on the contribution of the diet to the chil-
dren’ total exposure to OP pesticides. Several studies have assessed
the contribution of the diet to the total exposure to OP pesticides
in children. Children on organic diet showed lower urinary DAPs
levels than those on conventional diet and the full substitution of a
conventional diet by an organic diet was associated with a decrease
in urinary DAPs levels (Curl et al., 2003; Lu et al., 2006). Therefore,
dietary information should be considered in future studies to clar-
ify the contribution of different food items to children’ exposure
as compared to other sources of exposure. This would represent an
important step-forward in the search and in the implementation of
feasible measures to reduce children’ exposure to these chemicals.
Social factors such as socioeconomic level, family environment,
parents’ mental health and intelligence quotient are directly linked
with a child’s upbringing, influencing aspects such as diet, exposure
to environmental toxins, the child’s level of stimulation and devel-
opment, amongst others (Forns et al., 2012). More recent studies
have started to incorporate these variables into their designs to
assess their effect on exposure to environmental pollutants and
children’s level of development and behaviour. Almost all of the
studies included in this review included one or several social indi-
cators as confounding variables to adjust their statistical models.
The most frequent variables were those related to the family’s eco-
nomic status (family income, poverty income ratio), race/ethnicity,
paternal education (level of education of the mother and/or father,
years at school, occupation), the mother’s intelligence and mental
health, and the child’s living conditions (HOME score, living in a
traditional house). Because of the limited number of studies (n = 4)
that did not include any of these confounding variables and the vari-
ability of the designs used (age of the study population, exposure
biomarkers studied, neurodevelopment tests used), it was not pos-
sible to assess the differences between the results of those studies
and the ones that did include those confounding variables.
With regard to the tests used to assess children’s neurobe-
havioural development, many of the studies used the different
Wechsler intelligence scales (WISC III, WISC-IV, WPPSI-III, WISC-R,
WAIS) and the Bayley scale (BSID-II), depending on the age range of
the participants and the date on which the population was recruited
in the studies reviewed. Both scales provide more thorough infor-
mation on children’ neurodevelopment than those tests assessing
only specific functions, such as visual memory, dexterity, motor
speed and coordination, among others. All tests can be used to
assess children’ neurodevelopment and provide valuable informa-
tion. However, the selection of a particular test responds to the aim
of the investigation and takes into account the age range of the par-
ticipants and the date when the study was performed. For a better
comparability of the results of epidemiological studies it is impor-
tant to harmonize the neurodevelopmental tests available (White
et al., 2009). Table 1 shows detailed information on the tests used
in the reviewed studies is shown.
The evaluation of effects on neurodevelopment using IQ tests is
gradually being complemented by other indexes measuring differ-
ent areas such as memory, verbal comprehension, processing speed
and perceptual reasoning, amongst others (Flanagan and Kaufman,
2004). Using specific tests would be better than simply using gen-
eral intelligence tests, as previous studies suggest that exposure to
environmental toxins could cause specific types of damage to the
central nervous system which would not be detected based on the
child’s IQ alone (White et al., 2009; Forns et al., 2012; Grandjean
et al., 2006).
Some of the studies included in this review also evaluated
behavioural disorders and attention problems in children (Eskenazi
et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010). The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)
for parents was the most frequently used method of identifying this
type of disorder along with other tests and questionnaires (Table 1).
There is a specific CBCL questionnaire for each age range that allow
the identification of behavioural changes in children, such as hyper-
activity, violent behaviour, anxiety and depression, amongst others
(Achenbach and Rescorla, 2000).
4.3. Limitations associated with the study design
An accurate estimation of the number of participants accord-
ing to the research purposes is crucial since a small sample size
could underestimate the magnitude of the associations. Since the
sample size is determined by the levels of OP exposure, the lack of
information on exposure may hamper the estimation of the sample
size in the study population. Moreover, a reduced sample size may
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 15
also prevent from observing neurodevelopmental or behavioural
effects associated with pre- or postnatal exposure to OP pesticides
(Guodong et al., 2012). Other studies, despite admitting having a
limited sample size, observed significant or near-significant effects
(Engel et al., 2011; Grandjean et al., 2006; Handal et al., 2008;
Harari et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006; Rohlman et al., 2005). Loss
of recruited children during the follow-up could introduce a selec-
tion bias that might affect the results of the study. The reviewed
studies highlighted the limitations associated with the sample size
as regards exposure assessment to OP pesticides (Bouchard et al.,
2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2011),
very likely because of the low persistence of these compounds and
the use of non-specific biomarkers, such as DAPs.
In addition to sample size limitations, the influence of statistical
analysis is also remarkable. Most of the studies reviewed were lon-
gitudinal in design, however cross-sectional analyses were made.
This could limit the advantages of cohort studies since they did not
evaluate longitudinally the changes in children’ performance over
time. Likewise, to our best knowledge no study has evaluated the
potential reversibility of effects associated either with decreased
exposure or with social, educational and/or biological factors.
4.4. Differences linked to sex/gender
Few studies have analysed the link between sex and gender
and exposure to environmental pollutants and its effects on health,
especially in children. Sex differences are due to biological factors
and affect the metabolism and exposure to xenobiotics, as well as
susceptibility and health throughout lifetime (Clougherty, 2010).
Gender, on the other hand, is a social construct, influenced by cul-
tural norms, roles and behaviour due to the relationships between
men and women and between boys and girls (Clougherty, 2010).
There are also differences in the foods that girls and boys eat and
the activities they take part in (games, sport) (Molinero et al., 2010).
Social environment during pregnancy and childhood also have a
significant influence on exposure to environmental pollutants and
neurodevelopment (Jedrychowski et al., 2009), so these factors
should be included in epidemiological studies.
A study carried out by van Wendel et al. (2011, personal
communication, unpublished) in a banana farming community in
Costa Rica found a link between postnatal exposure to chlorpyrifos
and a reduction in short-term memory in children aged 6–9, and the
effect was greater in males than in females. The reasons for these
differences between sexes are not clearly identifiable, although
they may not only be due to biological factors (hormonal differ-
ences, differences in pesticide metabolism or in the repair processes
of the damage caused by OP pesticides in cholinergic pathways)
(Arbuckle, 2006; Slotkin et al., 2008), but also to social factors, such
as different lifestyles, behaviour and occupations. It is also impos-
sible to rule out the possibility that differences between sexes vary
by age, so sex could play a greater or lesser role in children than in
adults or vice versa.
Overall, the studies reviewed have included sex as a confound-
ing variable when adjusting multivariate models, either because
they found a link in the univariate analysis or because of assumed
differences between the sexes in terms of nervous system devel-
opment or exposure to OP pesticides. Very few studies conducted
stratified analysis by sex (Bouchard et al., 2011; Horton et al., 2012;
Marks et al., 2010; Rohlman et al., 2005) to disaggregate biological
and social differences between boys and girls.
Some studies on prenatal exposure to OP pesticides found a
decrease in working memory (Horton et al., 2011) or an increase
in attention problems and ADHD (Marks et al., 2010) in boys but
not in girls. One study on postnatal exposure to OP pesticides also
found a significant decrease in response time in boys but not in
girls (Rohlman et al., 2005). However, another study that stratified
the analysis by sex did not find any difference in the association
between urinary DAPs and effects on neurodevelopment between
boys and girls (Bouchard et al., 2011).
None of these articles carried out an analysis of gender differ-
ences to separate social differences from biological characteristics,
which would have allowed the identification of differences in expo-
sure patterns in children linked to the different way in which they
interact with the environment. Beamer et al. (2008) observed major
differences between the behaviour of boys and girls aged 6–27
months in a farming community in California, where the boys came
into contact with the ground and toys more frequently, while the
girls came into more frequent contact with clothes, towels, toys
and other objects. These differences in activities could lead to dif-
ferences in their exposure to certain environmental toxins.
Future studies should examine the role that sex and gender
play in exposure to environmental contaminants and its effects on
health in children. This would help to identify effect modification
factors, as well as pathways and sources of exposure, making it
possible to design more effective interventions.
4.5. Toxicity mechanisms of organophosphate pesticides and
neurodevelopment
The association between neurodevelopmental disorders and OP
pesticide exposure could be linked to its inhibiting effect on the
enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE), which plays an important role
in the nervous system function. When AChE is inhibited by OP,
the excess acetylcholine that accumulates in the synaptic terminals
overstimulates the nicotinic and muscarinic receptors (Ecobichon,
2001). This toxic action could interfere the human neurological
development process from the early stages of development (Rice
and Barone, 2000).
However, recent studies have identified toxicity mechanisms
other than AChE inhibition that could be involved in neurodevel-
opmental disorders (Androutsopoulos et al., 2013). Recent in vitro
studies have found a decrease in the number of neurites associ-
ated with diazoxon (active metabolite of diazinon), DNA synthesis
inhibition and alterations in the differentiation of C6 glioma cells
associated with chlorpyrifos oxon (Flaskos, 2012). These effects
occur at low OP concentrations which do not inhibit AChE so that
cholinergic transmission is unaffected (Pope et al., 2005). Chlor-
pyrifos oxon is capable of phosphorylating (and therefore activate)
transcription factors involved in brain development (e.g. cAMP
response element-binding protein, CREB) at much lower concen-
trations (nanomolar) than those required to inhibit AChE, thus
affecting key processes in neural development and the develop-
ment of cognitive functions (Schuh et al., 2002).
4.6. Genetic susceptibility
Genetic variability plays an important role in determining the
individual susceptibility to environmental pollutants. This means
that genetic variations in the enzymes involved in the detoxifi-
cation of these chemicals could make an individual more or less
susceptible to their toxic effects (Costa et al., 2003).
An individual’s capacity to hydrolyse OP pesticides can also be
affected by their plasma PON1 levels, and is 3–4 folds lower in chil-
dren than in adults (Cole et al., 2012; Furlong et al., 2006; Huen
et al., 2009) making them more susceptible to the effects of these
compounds (Padilla et al., 2000). Other factors such as diet, alcohol
consumption or drug use, as well as psychological or physiological
factors, can also affect PON1 activity (Costa et al., 2005a,b).
Although some enzymes have been considered as biomarkers
of genetic susceptibility related to OP pesticide metabolism (Costa
et al., 2005a,b), only PON1 genetic polymorphisms have been eval-
uated so far in epidemiological studies (Furlong, 2007).
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
16 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
Eskenazi et al. (2010) evaluated the interaction effects between
prenatal exposure to OP pesticides and PON1192 and PON1-108
polymorphisms on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children.
There was a significant association between maternal urinary DAPs
and a lower mental development index in 2-year-olds carrying
the PON1-108T allele, as well as a lower psychomotor develop-
ment index, although in the latter case the differences were not
statistically significant. This gene-environment interaction did not
occur with the PON1192 polymorphism. The PON1192 and PON1-108
polymorphisms had no significant effect on pervasive development
disorder (PDD) in the same age group.
Engel et al. (2011) observed that increased maternal urinary
DAPs were associated with a significant decrease in the MDI of
children aged 12–24 months whose mothers carried the PON1192R
allele. In 6 to 9-year-olds, the children of mothers with the
PON1192QQ genotype scored considerably lower on the Wech-
sler perceptual reasoning scale than the children of mothers with
the PON1192R allele, although the difference was not statistically
significant. However, the other polymorphisms studied (PON155,
PON1-108) were not found to have an effect on neurodevelopment.
4.7. Epigenetics and neurodevelopment
A number of illnesses and functional changes in organ systems
have been linked with epigenetic mechanisms, including cogni-
tive dysfunction and developmental neuropsychological disorders
(Martínez-Frías, 2010; Martino et al., 2009; Weinhold, 2006). Epi-
genetics is the study of heritable changes in gene expression or
phenotype occurring without changes in DNA sequence (Martínez-
Frías, 2010; Perera and Herbstman, 2011; Rodríguez-Milord et al.,
1990). Epigenetic mechanisms that regulate gene expression, such
as DNA methylation, take place in a carbon cycle where one methyl
group, from S-adenosylmethionine, is covalently added to the car-
bon 5 of the DNA cytosine ring (CpG dinucleotide). This process is
highly dependent on the folate levels (Bird, 2002; Costello and Plass,
2001; Lee et al., 2009; Martínez-Frías, 2010). There is evidence
that the process also involves chromatin remodelling and vari-
ous complex histone modifications including acetylation for gene
expression, and phosphorylation for gene activation and repression
(mainly through different families of methyltransferases, acetyl-
transferases and deacetylases) (Cheung and Lau, 2005; Shahbazian
and Grunstein, 2007; Varga-Weisz and Becker, 2006). Less is known
about other mechanisms such as protein modifications (e.g. ubi-
quitination, sumoylation). Other epigenetic processes involve small
non-coding RNAs, known as microRNAs (miRNAs), which are the
main regulators of gene function and expression during prena-
tal development and during germ line formation in both sexes
(Filipowicz et al., 2008; Hayashi et al., 2008; Martínez-Frías, 2010;
Tam et al., 2008).
Some authors have discussed how exposure to environmental
factors, including xenobiotics, can lead to epigenetic alterations
(Perera and Herbstman, 2011; Venerosi et al., 2012). There is
a growing amount of evidence that exposure to environmen-
tal chemicals during early development may lead to epigenetic
changes that could determine susceptibility to diseases in later life
or may be transmitted from one generation to another (Weinhold,
2006).
The effect of three OP pesticides (fonofos, parathion and ter-
bufos) on human DNA was recently studied ‘in vitro’. The three
pesticides caused methylation changes in the promoter regions of
the same 712 genes, as well as exhibited their own OP-specific
methylation alterations (Zhang et al., 2012b). Epigenetic mecha-
nisms may also contribute to OP-induced carcinogenesis (Zhang
et al., 2012a,b) and possibly to other complex disorders. How-
ever, few animal studies have examined other potential epigenetic
implications of OP pesticides in neurodevelopmental disorders
(Martínez-Frías, 2010; Persico and Bourgeron, 2006; Venerosi et al.,
2012). New, innovative and multidisciplinary studies on gene-gene
and gene-environment interactions and gene susceptibility are
essential to prevent effects associated with epigenetic mechanisms.
5. Conclusions
The studies included in this review used a wide variety of dif-
ferent designs and methods to evaluate the effects of OP pesticide
exposure on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children, and this
makes it difficult to compare their results. The studies reviewed
suggest that exposure during pregnancy may have a negative effect
on the child’s mental and motor development and behaviour during
the first stages of childhood. The effects associated with postnatal
exposure are less consistent, although may also affect the child’s
cognitive and motor function, as well as increase the risk of atten-
tion problems. In order to draw conclusions, further studies need
to be carried out evaluating exposure to OP pesticides at differ-
ent stages of pregnancy and monitoring the possible effects on
neurodevelopment after birth. Studies analysing both pre- and
postnatal exposure (current and accumulated) to these compounds
are also required. It is therefore important to identify the most
suitable methods for evaluating exposure to OP pesticides and its
effects on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children so that a
standardised method can be established. This would make it eas-
ier to compare results and estimate the overall effects of exposure
to these compounds in children. It is also important to incorporate
differences associated with gender and sex into epidemiological
designs, as well as information about the children’s diet, fam-
ily environment and lifestyle. This is key to identifying the main
determining factors of exposure to OP pesticides and its possible
health effects in children. By using existing evidence as well as
future research that will broaden knowledge of the mechanisms
involved in OP pesticide metabolism, and studying the role that
an individual’s genetic characteristics play when exposed to these
compounds, it will be possible to design measures to reduce levels
of exposure and its possible health effects in children.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.
Acknowledgements
This study was partially supported by grants from the Council
of Innovation of the Andalusian Government (reference number
P08-CTS-04313, FEDER funds), the Institute of Health Carlos III
(reference numbers PI10/01101 and FIS-FEDER 11/02038) and the
European Union (EU FP7-ENV-2011 DENAMIC 2cod 28957).
The content of this article is part of the PhD thesis of Beat-
riz Gonzalez-Alzaga which was conducted at the University of
Granada under the doctoral programme “Clinical Medicine and
Public Health”.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found,
in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.
11.019.
References
Abdel Rasoul, G.M., Abou Salem, M.E., Mechael, A.A., Hendy, O.M., Rohlman, D.S.,
Ismail, A.A., 2008. Effects of occupational pesticide exposure on children apply-
ing pesticides. Neurotoxicology 29, 833–838.
Achenbach, T.M., Rescorla, L.A., 2000. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and
Profiles. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, Burlington, VT.
Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal
organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
ARTICLE IN PRESS
GModel
TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18
B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 17
Androutsopoulos, V.P., Hernandez, A.F., Liesivuori, J., Tsatsakis, A.M., 2013. A mech-
anistic overview of health associated effects of low levels of organochlorine and
organophosphorous pesticides. Toxicology 307, 89–94.
Aprea, C., Strambi, M., Novelli, M.T., Lunghini, L., Bozzi, N., 2000. Biologic monitoring
of exposure to organophosphorus pesticides in 195 italian children. Environ.
Health Perspect. 108, 521–525.
Arbuckle, T.E., 2006. Are there sex and gender differences in acute exposure to
chemicals in the same setting. Environ. Res. 101, 195–204.
Barr, D.B., Bravo, R., Weerasekera, G., Caltabiano, L.M., Whitehead Jr., R.D., Ols-
son, A.O., et al., 2004. Concentrations of dialkyl phosphate metabolites of
organophosphorus pesticides in the U.S. population. Environ. Health Perspect.
112, 186–200.
Beamer, P., Key, M.E., Ferguson, A.C., Canales, R.A., Auyeung, W., Leckie, J.O., 2008.
Quantified activity pattern data from 6 to 27-month-old farmworker children
for use in exposure assessment. Environ. Res. 108, 239–246.
Bearer, C.F., Lee, S., Salvator, A.E., Minnes, S., Swick, A., Yamashita, T., et al., 1999.
Ethyl linoleate in meconium: a biomarker of prenatal ethanol exposure. Alcohol
Clin. Exp. Res. 23, 487–493.
Bird, A., 2002. DNA methylation patterns and epigenetic memory. Genes Dev. 16,
6–21.
Bouchard, M.F., Bellinger, D.C., Wright, R.O., Weisskopf, M.G., 2010. Attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder and urinary metabolites of organophosphate
pesticides. Pediatrics 125, 1270–1277.
Bouchard, M.F., Chevrier, J., Harley, K.G., Kogut, K., Vedar, M., Calderon, N., Trujillo,
C., Johnson, C., Bradman, A., Barr, D.B., Eskenazi, B., 2011. Prenatal exposure
to organophosphate pesticides and IQ in 7-year-old children. Environ. Health
Perspect. 119, 1189–1195.
Bradman, A., Eskenazi, B., Barr, D.B., Bravo, R., Castorina, R., Chevrier, J., Kogut, K.,
Harnly, M.E., McKone, T.E., 2005. Organophosphate urinary metabolite levels
during pregnancy and after delivery in women living in an agricultural commu-
nity. Environ. Health Perspect. 113, 1802–1807.
Cheung, P., Lau, P., 2005. Epigenetic regulation by histone methylation and histone
variants. Mol. Endocrinol. 19, 563–573.
Clougherty, J.E., 2010. A growing role for gender analysis in air pollution epidemi-
ology. Environ. Health Perspect. 118, 167–176.
Cole, T.B., Fisher, J.C., Burbacher, T.M., Costa, L.G., Furlong, C.E., 2012. Neuro-
behavioral assessment of mice following repeated postnatal exposure to
chlorpyrifos-oxon. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 34, 311–322.
Costa, L.G., Cole, T.B., Vitalone, A., Furlong, C.E., 2005a. Measurement of paraoxonase
(PON1) status as a potential biomarker of susceptibility to organophosphate
toxicity. Clin. Chim. Acta 352, 37–47.
Costa, L.G., Richter, R.J., Li, W.-F., Cole, T., Guizzetti, M., Furlong, C.E., 2003. Para-
oxonase (PON 1) as a biomarker of susceptibility for organophosphate toxicity.
Biomarkers 8, 1–12.
Costa, L.G., Vitalone, A., Cole, T.B., Furlong, C.E., 2005b. Modulation of paraoxonase
(PON1) activity. Biochem. Pharmacol. 69, 541–550.
Costello, J.F., Plass, C., 2001. Methylation matters. J. Med. Genet. 38, 285–303.
Curl, C.L., Fenske, R.A., Kissel, J.C., Shirai, J.H., Moate, T.F., Griffith, W., Coronado,
G., Thompson, B., 2002. Evaluation of take-home organophosphorus pesticide
exposure among agricultural workers and their children. Environ. Health Per-
spect. 110, 787–792.
Curl, C.L., Fenske, R.A., Elgethun, K., 2003. Organophosphorus pesticide exposure of
urban and suburban preschool children with organic and conventional diets.
Environ. Health Perspect. 111, 377–382.
Eaton, D.L., Farin, F., Omiecinski, C.J., Omenn, G.S., 1998. Genetic susceptibility. In:
Rom, W.N. (Ed.), Environmental and Occupational Medicine. Lippincott-Raven,
Philadelphia, pp. 209–221.
Ecobichon, D.J., 2001. Pesticide use in developing countries. Toxicology 160, 27–33.
Engel, S.M., Berkowitz, G.S., Barr, D.B., Teitelbaum, S.L., Siskind, J., Meisel, S.J.,
et al., 2007. Prenatal organophosphate metabolite and organochlorine levels
and performance on the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale in a
multiethnic pregnancy cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 165, 1397–1404.
Engel, S.M., Wetmur, J., Chen, J., Zhu, C., Barr, D.B., Canfield, R.L., et al., 2011. Prenatal
exposure to organophosphates, paraoxonase 1, and cognitive development in
childhood. Environ. Health Perspect. 119, 1182–1188.
Eskenazi, B., Harley, K., Bradman, A., Weltzien, E., Jewell, N.P., Barr, D.B., et al., 2004.
Association of in utero organophosphate pesticide exposure and fetal growth
and length of gestation in an agricultural population. Environ. Health Perspect.
112, 1116–1124.
Eskenazi, B., Huen, K., Marks, A., Harley, K.G., Bradman, A., Barr, D.B., et al.,
2010. PON1 and neurodevelopment in children from the CHAMACOS study
exposed to organophosphate pesticides in utero. Environ. Health Perspect. 118,
1775–1781.
Eskenazi, B., Marks, A.R., Bradman, A., Harley, K., Barr, D.B., Johnson, C., et al.,
2007. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young
Mexican-American children. Environ. Health Perspect. 115, 792–798.
Filipowicz, W., Bhattacharyya, S.N., Sonenberg, N., 2008. Mechanisms of post-
transcriptional regulation by microRNAs: are the answers in sight. Nat. Rev.
Genet. 9, 102–114.
Flanagan, D.P., Kaufman, A.S., 2004. Essentials of WISC-IV Assessment. John
WileySons, New Jersey, USA.
Flaskos, J., 2012. The developmental neurotoxicity of organophosphorus insecti-
cides: a direct role for the oxon metabolites. Toxicol. Lett. 209, 86–93.
Forns, J., Aranbarri a, Grellier, J., Julvez, J., Vrijheid, M., Sunyer, J., 2012. A conceptual
framework in the study of neuropsychological development in epidemiological
studies. Neuroepidemiology 38, 203–208.
Furlong, C.E., Holland, N., Richter, R.J., Bradman, A., Ho, A., Eskenazi, B., 2006. PON1
status of farmworker mothers and children asa predictor of organophosphate
sensitivity. Pharmacogenet. Genom. 16, 183–190.
Furlong, C.E., 2007. Genetic variability in the cytochrome P450-paraoxonase 1
(PON1) pathway for detoxication of organophosphorus compounds. J. Biochem.
Mol. Toxicol. 21, 197–205.
Grandjean, P., Harari, R., Barr, D.B., Debes, F., 2006. Pesticide exposure and stunt-
ing as independent predictors of neurobehavioral deficits in Ecuadorian school
children. Pediatrics 117, e546–e556.
Grandjean, P., Landrigan, P.J., 2006. Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chem-
icals. Lancet 368, 2167–2178.
Griffith, W., Curl, C.L., Fenske, R.A., Lu, C.A., Vigoren, E.M., Faustman, E.M., 2011.
Organophosphate pesticide metabolite levels in pre-school children in an agri-
cultural community: within- and between-child variability in a longitudinal
study. Environ. Res. 111, 751–756.
Guo, Y., Zhang, J., Yu, R., Zhu, K.Y., Guo, Y., Ma, E., 2012. Identification of two new
cytochrome P450 genes and RNA interference to evaluate their roles in detoxi-
fication of commonly used insecticides in Locusta migratoria. Chemosphere 87,
709–717.
Guodong, D., Pei, W., Ying, T., Jun, Z., Yu, G., Xiaojin, W., et al., 2012. Organophosphate
pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young Shanghai children. Environ.
Sci. Technol. 46, 2911–2917.
Handal, A.J., Harlow, S.D., Breilh, J., Lozoff, B., 2008. Occupational exposure to
pesticides during pregnancy and neurobehavioral development of infants and
toddlers. Epidemiology 19, 851–859.
Harari, R., Julvez, J., Murata, K., Barr, D., Bellinger, D.C., Debes, F., et al., 2010. Neurobe-
havioral deficits and increased blood pressure in school-age children prenatally
exposed to pesticides. Environ. Health Perspect. 118, 890–896.
Hayashi, K., Chuva de Sousa Lopes, S.M., Kaneda, M., Tang, F., Hajkova, P., Lao, K.,
O’Carroll, D., Das, P.P., Tarakhovsky, A., Miska, E.A., Surani, M.A., 2008. MicroRNA
biogenesis is required for mouse primordial germ cell development and sper-
matogenesis. PLoS One 3, e1738.
Horton, M.K., Kahn, L.G., Perera, F., Barr, D.B., Rauh, V., 2012. Does the home environ-
ment and the sex of the child modify the adverse effects of prenatal exposure to
chlorpyrifos on child working memory? Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 34, 534–541.
Huen, K., Harley, K., Brooks, J., Hubbard, A., Bradman, A., Eskenazi, B., Holland, N.,
2009. Developmental changes in PON1 enzyme activity in young children and
effects of PON1polymorphisms. Environ. Health Perspect. 117, 1632–1638.
Jedrychowski, W., Perera, F., Jankowski, J., Mrozek-Budzyn, D., Mroz, E., Flak, E.,
et al., 2009. Gender specific differences in neurodevelopmental effects of prena-
tal exposure to very low-lead levels: the prospective cohort study in three-year
olds. Early Hum. Dev. 85, 503–510.
Jurewicz, J., Hanke, W., 2008. Prenatal and childhood exposure to pesticides and
neurobehavioral development: review of epidemiological studies. Int. J. Occup.
Med. Environ. Health 21, 121–132.
Kofman, O., Berger, A., Massarwa, A., Friedman, A., Jaffar, A.A., 2006. Motor inhibi-
tion and learning impairments in school-aged children following exposure to
organophosphate pesticides in infancy. Pediatric Res. 60, 88–92.
Koureas, M., Tsakalof, A., Tsatsakis, A., Hadjichristodoulou, C., 2012. Systematic
review of biomonitoring studies to determine the association between exposure
to organophosphorus and pyrethroid insecticides and human health outcomes.
Toxicol. Lett. 210, 155–168.
Lacasaña, M., Vázquez-Grameix, H., Borja-Aburto, V.H., Blanco-Muñoz, J., Romieu,
I., Aguilar-Garduño, C., García, A.M., 2006. Maternal and paternal occupational
exposure to agricultural work and the risk of anencephaly. Occup. Environ. Med.
63, 649–656.
Lizardi, P.S., O’Rourke, M.K., Morris, R.J., 2008. The effects of organophosphate pes-
ticide exposure on Hispanic children’s cognitive and behavioral functioning. J.
Pediatr. Psycholol. 33, 91–101.
Lee, D.H., Jacobs, D.R., Porta, M., 2009. Hypothesis: a unifying mechanism for nutri-
tion and chemicals as lifelong modulators of DNA hypomethylation. Environ.
Health Perspect. 117, 1799–1802.
Lu, C., Essig, C., Root, C., Rohlman, D.S., McDonald, T., Sulzbacher, S., 2009. Assessing
the association between pesticide exposure and cognitive development in rural
Costa Rican children living in organic and conventional coffee farms. Int. J. Ado-
lesc. Med. Health 21, 609–621.
Lu, C., Barr, D.B., Pearson, M.A., Waller, L.A., 2008. Dietary intake and its contribu-
tion to longitudinal organophosphorus pesticide exposure in urban/suburban
children. Environ. Health Perspect. 116, 537–542.
Lu, C., Toepel, K., Irish, R., Fenske, R.A., Barr, D.B., Bravo, R., 2006. Organic diets sig-
nificantly lower children’s dietary exposure to organophosphorus pesticides.
Environ. Health Perspect. 114, 260–263.
Marks, A.R., Harley, K., Bradman, A., Kogut, K., Barr, D.B., Johnson, C., et al.,
2010. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and attention in young Mexican-
American children: the CHAMACOS study. Environ. Health Perspect. 118,
1768–1774.
Maroni, M., Colosio, C., Ferioli, A., Fait, A., 2000. Biological monitoring of pesticide
exposure: a review. Introduction. Toxicology 143, 1–118.
Martínez-Frías, M.L., 2010. Can our understanding of epigenetics assist with primary
prevention of congenital defects. J. Med. Genet. 47, 73–80.
Martino, S., Girolamo, I.di, Orlacchio, A., Datti, A., Orlacchio, A., 2009. MicroRNA
implications across neurodevelopment and neuropathology. J. Biomed. Bio-
technol. 2009, 654346.
Maurissen, J.P., Hoberman a, M., Garman, R.H., Hanley, T.R., 2000. Lack of selec-
tive developmental neurotoxicity in rat pups from dams treated by gavage with
chlorpyrifos. Toxicol. Sci. 57, 250–263.
10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf

More Related Content

Similar to 10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf

Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...
Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...
Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...
Biblioteca Virtual
 
EnvChemExposure_Presentation
EnvChemExposure_PresentationEnvChemExposure_Presentation
EnvChemExposure_Presentation
Lila Rubenstein
 
330 www.thelancet.comneurology Vol 13 March 2014Revie.docx
330 www.thelancet.comneurology   Vol 13   March 2014Revie.docx330 www.thelancet.comneurology   Vol 13   March 2014Revie.docx
330 www.thelancet.comneurology Vol 13 March 2014Revie.docx
tamicawaysmith
 
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
M. Luisetto Pharm.D.Spec. Pharmacology
 
Edc april june2012_overview
Edc april june2012_overviewEdc april june2012_overview
Edc april june2012_overview
Hoa Hoàng
 

Similar to 10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf (20)

Autism spectrum disorders
Autism spectrum disordersAutism spectrum disorders
Autism spectrum disorders
 
2013 ets ce course materials (stresa italy)
2013 ets ce course materials (stresa italy)2013 ets ce course materials (stresa italy)
2013 ets ce course materials (stresa italy)
 
Preconception Counseling - A Critical Window for Health Promotion
Preconception Counseling - A Critical Window for Health Promotion Preconception Counseling - A Critical Window for Health Promotion
Preconception Counseling - A Critical Window for Health Promotion
 
Austin Public Health
Austin Public HealthAustin Public Health
Austin Public Health
 
Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...
Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...
Maternal And Paternal Risk Factors For Cryptorchidism And Hypospadias A Case–...
 
paediatrics
paediatricspaediatrics
paediatrics
 
Polyphenols and pregnancy (1)
Polyphenols and pregnancy (1)Polyphenols and pregnancy (1)
Polyphenols and pregnancy (1)
 
Journal.pbio.2006497
Journal.pbio.2006497Journal.pbio.2006497
Journal.pbio.2006497
 
2 epigenetics studies of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: where are we now?, ...
2 epigenetics studies of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: where are we now?, ...2 epigenetics studies of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: where are we now?, ...
2 epigenetics studies of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: where are we now?, ...
 
EnvChemExposure_Presentation
EnvChemExposure_PresentationEnvChemExposure_Presentation
EnvChemExposure_Presentation
 
Child Development & Environmental Toxins
Child Development & Environmental Toxins Child Development & Environmental Toxins
Child Development & Environmental Toxins
 
330 www.thelancet.comneurology Vol 13 March 2014Revie.docx
330 www.thelancet.comneurology   Vol 13   March 2014Revie.docx330 www.thelancet.comneurology   Vol 13   March 2014Revie.docx
330 www.thelancet.comneurology Vol 13 March 2014Revie.docx
 
(Cayzer & Stotler, 2019) Psychological risk and protective factors / Prenatal...
(Cayzer & Stotler, 2019) Psychological risk and protective factors / Prenatal...(Cayzer & Stotler, 2019) Psychological risk and protective factors / Prenatal...
(Cayzer & Stotler, 2019) Psychological risk and protective factors / Prenatal...
 
Postnatal Evaluation in Developmental and Juvenile Toxicity Studies
Postnatal Evaluation in Developmental and Juvenile Toxicity StudiesPostnatal Evaluation in Developmental and Juvenile Toxicity Studies
Postnatal Evaluation in Developmental and Juvenile Toxicity Studies
 
Lifelong impacts of moderate prenatal alcohol exposure on neuroimmune function
Lifelong impacts of moderate prenatal alcohol exposure on neuroimmune functionLifelong impacts of moderate prenatal alcohol exposure on neuroimmune function
Lifelong impacts of moderate prenatal alcohol exposure on neuroimmune function
 
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
 
Pesticides in Our Homes and Schools
Pesticides in Our Homes and SchoolsPesticides in Our Homes and Schools
Pesticides in Our Homes and Schools
 
Adolescent Depression Aetiology A Systematic Review
Adolescent Depression Aetiology  A Systematic ReviewAdolescent Depression Aetiology  A Systematic Review
Adolescent Depression Aetiology A Systematic Review
 
Edc april june2012_overview
Edc april june2012_overviewEdc april june2012_overview
Edc april june2012_overview
 
Session 5: How Environmental Toxins are Linked to Metabolic Disorders and Chr...
Session 5: How Environmental Toxins are Linked to Metabolic Disorders and Chr...Session 5: How Environmental Toxins are Linked to Metabolic Disorders and Chr...
Session 5: How Environmental Toxins are Linked to Metabolic Disorders and Chr...
 

More from azhari80

Antidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdf
Antidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdfAntidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdf
Antidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdf
azhari80
 
Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...
Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...
Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...
azhari80
 
151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf
151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf
151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf
azhari80
 
PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...
PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...
PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...
azhari80
 

More from azhari80 (11)

Antidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdf
Antidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdfAntidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdf
Antidotal or protective effects of Curcuma longa (turmeric) and its active.pdf
 
Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...
Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...
Ameliorative effects of curcumin against renal injuries mediated by inducible...
 
72. Revisi.143.Puspitasari_4.pdf
72. Revisi.143.Puspitasari_4.pdf72. Revisi.143.Puspitasari_4.pdf
72. Revisi.143.Puspitasari_4.pdf
 
151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf
151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf
151106-ID-euthanasia-pada-hewan-percobaan.pdf
 
Adverse effects of exposure to low.pdf
Adverse effects of exposure to low.pdfAdverse effects of exposure to low.pdf
Adverse effects of exposure to low.pdf
 
EnvironmentalResearch.pdf
EnvironmentalResearch.pdfEnvironmentalResearch.pdf
EnvironmentalResearch.pdf
 
10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf
10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf
10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf
 
10885044.pdf
10885044.pdf10885044.pdf
10885044.pdf
 
12889_2015_Article_2076.pdf
12889_2015_Article_2076.pdf12889_2015_Article_2076.pdf
12889_2015_Article_2076.pdf
 
PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...
PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...
PUBLIC POLICY (Public Administration and public policy 149) Understanding res...
 
Research_Methods_Handbook.pdf
Research_Methods_Handbook.pdfResearch_Methods_Handbook.pdf
Research_Methods_Handbook.pdf
 

Recently uploaded

The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
heathfieldcps1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptxInterdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
 
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptxPlant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
 
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptxGoogle Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxHow to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
 

10.1016@j.toxlet.2013.11.019.pdf

  • 1. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Toxicology Letters journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/toxlet A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure B. González-Alzagaa , M. Lacasañaa,b,∗ , C. Aguilar-Garduñoc , M. Rodríguez-Barrancoa,b , F. Ballesterb,c,d , M. Rebagliatob,e , A.F. Hernándezf a Andalusian School of Public Health, Granada, Spain b CIBER of Epidemiology and Public Health (CIBERESP), Spain c Centre for Public Health Research (CSISP-FISABIO), Valencia, Spain d University of Valencia, Spain e Department of Medicine, University Jaume I, Castelló de la Plana, Spain f Department of Legal Medicine and Toxicology, University of Granada School of Medicine, Granada, Spain h i g h l i g h t s • This review studied the effects of OP pesticide exposure on child neurodevelopment. • Prenatal exposure was associated with negative effects on child mental development. • Evidence on postnatal exposure effects on child neurodevelopment is less consistent. • The high variability of methodologies across studies made comparisons difficult. • Standardised methodologies are needed to perform quantitative meta-analysis. a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 1 July 2013 Received in revised form 5 November 2013 Accepted 18 November 2013 Available online xxx Keywords: Organophosphorus pesticides Prenatal exposure Postnatal exposure Neurodevelopment a b s t r a c t Agricultural and residential use of organophosphate (OP) pesticides has increased in recent decades after banning some persistent pesticides. Although there is evidence of the effects of OPs on neurodevelopment and behaviour in adults, limited information is available about their effects in children, who might be more vulnerable to neurotoxic compounds. This paper was aimed at analysing the scientific evidence published to date on potential neurodevelopmental and behavioural effects of prenatal and postnatal exposure to OPs. A systematic review was undertaken to identify original articles published up to December 2012 evaluating prenatal or postnatal exposure to OPs in children and effects on neurodevelopment and/or behaviour. Articles were critically compared, focusing on the methodology used to assess exposure and adverse effects, as well as potential contributing factors that may modify both exposure and outcomes, such as genetic susceptibility to certain enzymes involved in OPs metabolisation (e.g. paraoxonase-1) and gender differences. Twenty articles met the inclusion criteria, 7 of which evaluated prenatal exposure to OPs, 8 postnatal exposure and 5 both pre- and postnatal exposure. Most of the studies evaluating prenatal exposure observed a negative effect on mental development and an increase in attention problems in preschool and school children. The evidence on postnatal exposure is less consistent, although 2 studies found an increase in reaction time in schoolchildren. Some paraoxonase-1 polymorphisms could enhance the association between OPs exposure and mental and psychomotor development. A large variability in epidemiological designs and methodologies used for assessing exposure and outcome was observed across the different studies, which made comparisons difficult. Prenatal and to a lesser extent postnatal exposure to OPs may contribute to neurodevelopmental and behavioural deficits in preschool and school children. Standardised methodologies are needed to allow results to be better compared and to perform a quantitative meta-analysis before drawing any final conclusions. © 2013 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. ∗ Corresponding author at: Escuela Andaluza de Salud Pública, Campus Universi- tario de Cartuja, c/Cuesta del Observatorio 4, 18180 Granada, Spain. Tel.: +34 958 027556. E-mail address: marina.lacasana.easp@juntadeandalucia.es (M. Lacasaña). 1. Introduction Earlier restrictions on the use of some persistent pesticides have led to the use of non-persistent alternatives such as organophos- phates (OPs), carbamates and pyrethroids, which are very useful for controlling pests in both agricultural and residential settings. 0378-4274/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019
  • 2. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 2 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx Intensive use of these compounds is posing a significant risk to public health because of their potential adverse effects. Exposure to pesticides does not only affect those who use them occupation- ally, as the general population is also exposed to low concentrations through foodstuffs and the environment throughout their life- time. There is scientific evidence of the carcinogenic, neurological, reproductive, immunological and genotoxic effects associated with exposure to non-persistent pesticides in adults (Koureas et al., 2012). However, little information is available about the effects in children, although researchers have observed a higher risk of adverse reproductive effects (Eskenazi et al., 2004; Lacasaña et al., 2006; Rauch et al., 2012) and changes in the nervous system and in neurobehavioural development (Bouchard et al., 2010; Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2012). This means that the neurotoxic effects of these compounds on children’s central nervous systems could be causing a series of subclinical neurodevelopmental disorders. This has been termed a ‘silent pandemic’ and could have major health, economic and social impact (Grandjean and Landrigan, 2006). Toxicological studies in animals have provided information about the neurotoxicity mechanisms and adverse effects of non- persistent pesticides. For example, Maurissen (2000) and Rice and Barone (2000) observed changes in the nervous system develop- ment and sensory, motor and cognitive cerebral function of rodents associated with prenatal and early postnatal exposure to chlorpyri- fos. Prenatal exposure even to low concentrations of chlorpyrifos also affects rodents’ organogenesis (Tian et al., 2005) and leads to behavioural changes such as hyperactivity and working and refer- ence memory deficit. Factors such as age, sex, nutritional status, lifestyle and genetic variability can modify the effects of non-persistent pesticides in children, who are particularly susceptible to these compounds. The health risks derived from exposure to toxic agents are strongly influenced by genetics as a result of the variability of the genes that code for metabolising enzymes (Costa et al., 2003; Eaton et al., 1998; Furlong, 2007). The different possible combinations of these polymorphisms can determine favourable or unfavourable metabolic configurations, either facilitating the breakdown of some neurotoxic compounds, or bioactivating initially inactive com- pounds or delaying the metabolic breakdown of active compounds (Costa et al., 2005a,b; Guo et al., 2012). Recent studies have ana- lysed the effect of different paraoxonase-1 (PON1) polymorphisms on neurodevelopment in children. Certain genotypes (PON1–108TT, PON1192QR and PON1192RR) may be associated with reduced men- tal and motor development in children exposed to OP pesticides (Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2010). The limited number of epidemiological studies available and the huge variability of the methodologies used to assess exposure to OP pesticides and its effects on neurodevelopment in children make it difficult to compare their results. The aim of this review is to carry out a detailed analysis of the evidence gathered in the stud- ies performed to date, taking into account some of the factors that can modify the effects of these pesticides, such as sex and gen- der differences, genetic variability and epigenetic factors. This will provide a better overview of the relationship between exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopment and behaviour in children. 2. Material and methods 2.1. Search strategy A systematic review of articles in the PubMed, Scopus, Embase and Lilacs databases was carried out using the follow- ing key words or text word combinations: “organophosphates” OR “organophosphorus”, “child” OR “infant”, “neurodevelopment”, “neurobehavioral” OR “neurobehavioural” (for more details see Supplemental Material, annex I). 2.2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria The articles selected for the review met the following inclu- sion criteria: (a) original articles; (b) published before or during December 2012; (c) written in Spanish, English, French or Por- tuguese; (d) carried out in children and adolescents up to 16 years of age; (e) evaluating prenatal and/or postnatal exposure to OP pesticides; (f) using general intelligence tests or specific tests to assess changes in mental and motor development or behaviour in children. Case series studies were not included and literature reviews were only used to identify other original articles which had not been found using the search syntax used. 2.3. Data analysis Studies that met the inclusion criteria were sorted into cat- egories based on exposure type (prenatal or postnatal) and compared using the following criteria: (a) study design; (b) sam- ple size; (c) age of participants; (d) exposure assessment; (e) levels of exposure to compounds; (f) neurodevelopment tests used; (g) effects observed; (h) adjustment variables. Table 1 provides a sum- mary of the neuropsychological tests used in the studies included in this review, categorised based on age range and the areas (men- tal, motor and behavioural development) and/or specific functions evaluated. The level of exposure to OP pesticides is usually measured by determining the total dialkylphosphate (DAPs) levels in the urine. DAPs usually include the following 6 metabolites: dimethylphos- phate (DMP), dimethylthiophosphate (DMTP), dimethyldithio- phosphate (DMDTP), diethylphosphate (DEP), diethylthiophos- phate (DETP) and diethyldithiophosphate (DEDTP). In these studies, the results are expressed in nanomoles per litre (nmol/L) once all the figures expressed in ␮g/L have been converted. 2.3.1. Assessment of methodological quality of the articles The methodological quality of the studies included in the review was assessed using the ‘Strengthening the Reporting of Observa- tional Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) - Statement’ checklist (von Elm et al., 2008). This tool was designed to evaluate the clarity of the results of observational studies, but it has been used in other sys- tematic reviews for the same purposes because no other tools are available (Olmos et al., 2008; Ricci-Cabello et al., 2010; Rodríguez- Barranco et al., 2013). The checklist is made up of 22 items in 6 sections (title, introduc- tion, methods, results, discussion and other information). Each item specifies the aspects that should be included in the study and can be used to assess its quality. For this review, the 9 items included in the methods section were used to give a score for the method- ological quality of each study. The studies were categorized as low quality (0–3 of the 9 items), moderate quality (4–6 items) and high quality (7–9 items) (see Supplementary Material; annex II). The methodological quality scores are summarised in Tables 2 and 3. 3. Results 134 articles were identified using the search strategy described above. Twenty of those met the inclusion criteria, 7 of which analysed prenatal exposure to OP pesticides, 8 analysed postna- tal exposure, and 5 analysed both pre- and postnatal exposure. Cohort studies were the most frequent design (n = 10), followed by cross-sectional (n = 9) and case-control (n = 1) studies. The method- ological quality of studies on prenatal exposure was high (n = 9) or
  • 3. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 3 Table 1 Summary of tests applied in the reviewed studies to assess functions of mental development (MD), psychomotor development (PD) and behavior (B). Agea Test MD PD B Reference Since birth VABS Communication Fine and gross motor Daily living skills, socialization, maladaptive behavior Ruckart et al., 2004 Newborn–2 months BNBAS Habituation, orientation, motor performance, range of state, regulation of state, autonomic stability and reflexes Engel et al., 2007; Young et al., 2005 1 month–12 years PSI Adaptability, acceptability, demandingness, mood, distractability/hyperactivity and reinforces parent Ruckart et al., 2004 2 months–6 years GDS Adaptive behavior, language, personal and social behavior Motor behavior (locomotion, reaching, balance, comprehension, drawing and hand control) Guodong et al., 2012 3 months–5 years A&S Q Communication, problem solving and personal-social Gross motor and fine motor Handal et al., 2008 6 months–3 years BSID Language (receptive and expressive) and cognitive development Fine motor and gross motor Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Rauh et al., 2006 2–7 years WPPSI-III Verbal comprehension, perceptual organization and processing speed abilities Engel et al., 2011 2–18 years CBCL Social withdrawal, somatic complaints, anxiety and depression, destructive behavior, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, aggressive behavior and delinquent behaviors Eskenazi et al., 2007; Lizardi et al., 2008; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006 3–16 years NEPSY Attention, executive functions, language, communication, learning, memory and social perception Sensor-motor and Visuospatial Attention Kofman et al., 2006; Marks et al., 2010 4–5 years K-CPT ADHD Harari et al., 2010; Marks et al., 2010 >4 years BARS Attention, memory, visual memory, learning, motivation, response speed Coordination Sustained attention Rohlman et al., 2005 K-Bit General intelligence, verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning Ruckart et al., 2004 >5 years PP Visual-motor coordination, manual dexterity and motor speed Ruckart et al., 2004 RCPM General intelligence Harari et al., 2010 M&L Test Memory Ruckart et al., 2004 5–19 years PIC Cognitive impairment Family Dysfunction and Psychological Discomfort. Impulsivity and Distractibility, Reality Distortion, Social Withdrawal, Delinquency, Somatic Concern and Social Skill Deficits. Ruckart et al., 2004 6–16 years WISC-III Verbal comprehension, perceptual organization, processing speed and freedom from distractibility Lizardi et al., 2008 WISC-IV Verbal comprehension, perceptive reasoning, working memory and processing speed Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2011; Horton et al., 2012; Rauh et al., 2011 WISC-R Verbal comprehension, perceptual organization and freedom from distractibility Grandjean et al., 2006; Kofman et al., 2006; Harari et al., 2010 6–18 years DISC-IV ADHD Bouchard et al., 2010 6–8 yearsb Santa Ana Form Board Motor coordination Grandjean et al., 2006; Harari et al., 2010
  • 4. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 4 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx Table 1 (Continued) Age a Test MD PD B Reference 6–8 years b CTSB Visuospatial and visuoconstructional functions Grandjean et al., 2006; Harari et al., 2010 6–8 years b FTT Motor speed and dexterity Harari et al., 2010 7 years b Catsys force plate Postural stability Grandjean et al., 2006 >7 years WCST Cognitive skills ADHD Lizardi et al., 2008 >8 years BVRT Visual perception and visual memory Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008 >9 years Trail making A&B Visual perception, sequential skills and symbol recognition Motor speed Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008; Lizardi et al., 2008; Ruckart et al., 2004 >10 years WAIS Verbal comprehension, working memory, perceptual organization and processing speed Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008 ADHD: Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Tests abbreviations: A&S Q: Ages and Stages Questionnaire; BARS: Behavioral Assessment and Research System; BNBAS: Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale; BSID: Bayley Scales for Infant Development; BVRT: Benton Visual Retention Test; CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; CTSB: Copying test of the Stanford-Binet; DISC-IV: Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children IV; FTT: Finger Tapping Test; GDS: Gesell Developmental Schedules; K-Bit: Kaufman Brief Intelligence test; K-CPT: Conner’ Kiddie Continuous Performance; M&L Test: Memory and learning test; NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PIC: Personality Inventory for Children; PSI: Parenting Stress Index; PP: Purdue Pegboard; RCPM: Raven’ Colored Progressive Matrices; VABS: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales; WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WISC-III: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III; WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV; WPPSI-III: Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III; WISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. a Age range in which tests are usually applied. b Age range of children included in the study. moderate (n = 3), while that of studies on postnatal exposure was high (n = 7), moderate (n = 4) and low (n = 2). 3.1. Exposure assessment Two study types were identified based on the methodol- ogy used to assess exposure to OP pesticides. Firstly, studies that analyse exposure based on biological samples: (a) lev- els of parent compounds (chlorpyrifos); (b) levels of specific metabolites of OP pesticides (3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridinol–TCPy–as a metabolite of chlorpyrifos; p-nitrophenol as a metabolite of methyl-parathion); (c) levels of non-specific metabolites of OP pes- ticides (DAPs). These compounds were detected and measured using gas chromatography–tandem mass spectrometry (Bouchard et al., 2010; Bouchard et al., 2011; Grandjean et al., 2006; Engel et al., 2007; Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Harari et al., 2010; Marks et al., 2010; Young et al., 2005), gas chromatography with flame photometric detection (Guodong et al., 2012; Lizardi et al., 2008), gas chromatography-high-resolution mass spectrom- etry (Horton et al., 2012; Rauh et al., 2006; Rauh et al., 2011) and liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (Lu et al., 2009). Secondly, other studies assessed exposure using question- naires. Prenatal exposure was evaluated based on the mother’s employment during pregnancy, especially in agricultural work (Grandjean et al., 2006; Handal et al., 2008; Harari et al., 2010), and postnatal exposure was assessed based on proximity to farm- land (Rohlman et al., 2005), the children’s medical history of OP pesticide poisoning (Kofman et al., 2006) and the agricultural work carried out by the children (Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008). 3.2. Prenatal Exposure The effects associated with prenatal exposure observed in the different studies are summarised in Tables 2 and 4. The effects in the first stages of children’s lives were analysed in two birth cohort studies, one in a multi-ethnic population in New York (Engel et al., 2007) and another in a Hispanic population living in farming communities in California (CHAMACOS Project) (Young et al., 2005). Both studies found that an increase in urinary DAPs levels during pregnancy was associated with an increase in the frequency of abnormal reflexes in children up to 2 months of age, and this increase was statistically significant in the study by Young et al. (2005). However, neither of the two studies found a link between prenatal exposure to OP and other neonatal behaviours such as habituation, orientation, motor, range of state, regulation of state or autonomic stability. In the CHAMACOS cohort, urinary DAPs concentrations during pregnancy were inversely and significantly associated with the child’s mental development index (MDI) at 24 months, but not at 6 or 12 months (Eskenazi et al., 2007). Similarly, in the New York birth cohort there was an inverse, but not significant, associa- tion between maternal urinary DAPs concentrations and the MDI of black and Hispanic children at 12 months (Engel et al., 2011). How- ever, in white children the levels of these metabolites were not significantly associated with a higher MDI. Neither of the two stud- ies found significant associations between maternal DAPs levels and the children’s psychomotor development index (PDI). There is also evidence of the effects of prenatal exposure to OP pesticides in children of school age. In the CHAMACOS cohort, an increase in the average levels of maternal urinary DAPs during preg- nancy was associated with a significant decrease in the Perceptual Reasoning (PR), Working Memory (WM), Verbal Comprehension (VC) and Processing Speed (PS) scores and full scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) of children aged 7 (Bouchard et al., 2011). The same trend was observed by Engel et al. (2011) but the decrease was not statistically significant. Furthermore, a significant link was found
  • 5. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 5 Table 2 Studies on prenatal exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopmental and behavioural outcomes in children. Prenatal exposure Author, year Country Study design n Age Biological samples/timing Analyte assayed Median (95%CI) Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ Engel et al., 2007 USA P 311 Newborns (before hospital discharge) Maternal urine during pregnancy (mean gestational age: 31,2 weeks; SD = ±3,7) DAPs (nmol/L) 82 (35.2–194.7)a BNBAS Increase of abnormal reflexes Examiner, anesthesia during delivery, PON1 enzyme tertiles, urinary creatinine and overdispersion. H Young et al., 2005 USA P 381 Up to 2 months Maternal urine before and after delivery (up to 1 week) DAPs (nmol/L) Average pregnancy: 132 (4,17–12)a ; Post-delivery: 222 (7–21.87)a BNBAS Significant increment of abnormal reflexes among 3 days old infants. No significant differences in Habituation, Orientation, Motor performance, Range of state, Regulation of state and Autonomic stability. Age at BNBAS, maternal age at delivery, smoking, vitamin use, BNBAS interviewer, and mean diastolic and systolic blood pressure. M Handal et al., 2008 Ecuador C-S 121 3–21 months Questionnaire Pesticides AS Q (all ages). Tests of prehension and Visual acuity test (only 9–18 months) Mother working in the floriculture during pregnancy ↓ scores on communication, motor skills and visual acuity. Child’s age, mother’s education (communication). Anemia, stunting, housing construction, pesticide use at home during pregnancy (for gross motor). Anemia, stimulation at home, housing construction (for fine motor). M Eskenazi et al., 2007 USA P 396 6–24 months Maternal urine during pregnancy (1st: 14–26 weeks; 2nd: shortly after delivery) DAPs (nmol/L) 114.9b (105.7–125.0) TCPy (␮g/L) = 3.54 BSID-II CBCL Significant ↓ MDI at 24 months. No association at 6 or 12 months. No effect on PDI at 6, 24 or 36 months. TCPy not associated with MDI or PDI. Psychometrician, location, exact age at assessment, sex, breast-feeding duration, HOME score, household income above poverty threshold, parity and maternal PPVT. H
  • 6. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 6 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx Table 2 (Continued) Prenatal exposure Author, year Country Study design n Age Biological samples/timing Analyte assayed Median (95%CI) Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ Engel et al., 2011 USA P 200 12 months Maternal urine during pregnancy (26–28 weeks) DAPs (nmol/L) 81.3 (31.7–198.1)a BSID-II ↓ MDI among Black and Hispanic children. ↑ MDI among White children. Maternal age at enrolment, child sex, examiner, maternal PON1 enzyme activity, season of urine collection, laboratory batch, HOME score, alcohol consumption during pregnancy, urinary creatinine. H Rauh et al., 2006 USA P 254 12–36 months Umbilical cord blood. Maternal blood (2 days after delivery) Chlorpyrifos (pg/g) High exposure 6.17, Low exposure ≤6.17 BSID-II CBCL High exposure (6.17 pg/g) significantly associated with ↓ MDI and PDI at 36 months. No effects on MDI and PDI Scores at 12 and 24 months. Race, gender, maternal education, maternal IQ, gestational age, prenatal ETS exposure. H Marks et al., 2010 USA P 331 3.5–5 años Maternal urine (Mean gestation age. 1st sample: 14 week, 2nd sample: 26 week) DAPs (nmol/L) 109b (99.4–119.6) NEPSY-II (3.5 years) K-CPT (5 years) CBCL (3.5 and 5 years) Significant increase of Attention problems and ADHD at 5 years. Psychometrician, age at assessment, sex, child care, breast-feeding, maternal education, depressive symptoms and PPVT. H Harari et al., 2010 Ecuador C-S 84 6–8 years Questionnaire Pesticides WISC-R (WM) FTT, Santa Ana Form Board, K-CPT, CTSB and RCPM. Maternal exposure significantly associated with ↓scores in FTT, Santa Ana Form Board and CTSB. Sex, age, BMI, number of daily meals (only for current exposure), stunting, hematrocrit, school grade, having repeated one grade, maternal educational level, family living in a traditional house, drinking water supply, paternal education and employment. H
  • 7. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 7 Table 2 (Continued) Prenatal exposure Author, year Country Study design n Age Biological samples/timing Analyte assayed Median (95%CI) Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ Engel et al., 2011 USA P 210 6–9 years Maternal urine during pregnancy (26–28 weeks) DAPs (nmol/L) 81.3 (31.7–198.1)a WISC-IV (7–9 years): PR (BD/PC/MR), VC (Sim/Voc/Com), WM (DS/LNS), PS (SS), FSIQ WPPSI-III (6 years): PR (BD/MR/PC), VC (Inf/Voc/WR), PS (SS/Cod), FSIQ Slight decrement in FSIQ, PR, PS and WM (WISC-IV) and in FSIQ, PR, VC and PS (WPPSI-III) Sex, race/ethnicity, maternal education, language in the home, maternal PON1 enzymatic activity, alcohol use in pregnancy, batch season of urine collection, urinary creatinine. H Horton et al., 2012 USA P 335 7 years Umbilical cord blood plasma Chlorpyrifos (ng/g) 0.36 (0.25–32.1)c WISC-IV (WM,VC, PR, PS, FSIQ) Borderline significant interaction between prenatal exposure to chlopyrifos and child sex: ↓ WM score among males. Total HOME score, Parental nurturance and environmental stimulation. M Bouchard et al., 2011 USA P 329 7 years Maternal urine during pregnancy (≤20 weeks and 20 weeks) DAPs (nm/L) = 128 WISC-IV: PR (BD/MR), VC (Voc/Sim), WM (DS/LNS), PS (Cod/SS), FSIQ Signficant decrement in FSIQ, WM, PS, VC and PR. HOME score at 6 months and maternal education and intelligence. VC and FSIQ adjusted for language assessment. Creatinine- adjusted urine. H Rauh et al., 2011 USA P 265 7 years Umbilical cord blood plasma Chlorpyrifos (pg/g) 3.17d (0.09–32)c WISC-IV (WM, VC, PR, PS, FSIQ) Significant decrement in WM and FSIQ. Child sex, race/ethnicity/maternal IQ/maternal education income, child age at testing, ETS and PAH. H Grandjean et al., 2006 Ecuador C-S 72 7 years Questionnaire Pesticides WISC-R: WM (DS). Santa Ana Pegborad, CTSB and Catsys force plate Prenatal exposure significantly associated with ↓ scores in CTSB. Age, sex, trauma, other injury and meningitis in the past medical history, maternal education and current nutrition. H Study design abbreviations. C-S: Cross-sectional; P: Prospective; MQ: Methodology Quality; H: High; M: Medium. Tests and subtests abbreviations. AS Q: Ages and Stages Questionnaire; BD: Block Design; BNBAS: Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale; BSID-II: Bayley Scales for Infant Development; CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; Cod: Coding; Com: Comprehension; CTSB: Copying test of the Stanford-Binet; DS: Digit Span; FSIQ: Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; FTT: Finger Tapping Test; Inf: Information; K-CPT: Conner’ Kiddie Continuous Performance; LNS: Letter-Number Sequence; MR: Matrix Reasoning; NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PC: Picture Concepts; PR: Perceptual Reasoning; PS: Processing Speed; RCPM: Raven’ Colored Progressive Matrices; Sim: Similarities; SS: Symbol Search; VC: Verbal Comprehension; Voc: Vocabulary; WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV; WISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WM: Working Memory; WPPSI-III: Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III; WR: Word Reasoning. a Interquartile range. b Geometric mean. c Range. d Mean are reported when median or 95% CI were not available.
  • 8. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 8 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx Table 3 Studies on postnatal exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopmental and behavioural outcomes in children. Postnatal exposure Author, year Country Study design n Age Biological samples/timing Analyte assayed median (95%CI) Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ Eskenazi et al., 2007 USA P 396 6–24 months Child urine (6,12 and 24 months) DAPs (nmol/L) 6 months 45.5 (39.6–52.3); 12 months 59.5 (51.7–68.5); 24 months 70.9 (61.4–81.9) BSDI, CBCL Significant ↑MDI at 24 months. No association at 6 or 12 months. No effects on PDI at 6, 12 or 24 months. Psychometrician, location, exact age at assessment, sex, breast-feeding duration, HOME score, household income above poverty threshold, parity and maternal PPVT. H Guodong et al., 2012 China C-S 301 23–25 months Child urine DAPs (nmol/L) DMP 20.16 (LOD-1495.9)a ; DMTP 11.06 (LOD-228.3)a ; DEP 11.64 (LOD-210.39)a ; DETP 18.82 (LOD-317.8)a ; DEDTP n.c. (LOD-20.54)a GDS No effects observed. Child sex, maternal education level, household income. Creatinine-adjusted urine. M Rohlman et al., 2005 USA C-S (with follow up) 78 48–71 months Questionnaire OP BARS Exposed children performed poorer on Finger tapping and Match to Sample compared to Non-exposed children. Age, mother’s education. M Marks et al., 2010 USA P 331 3.5–5 years Child urine at 3.5 and 5 years. DAPs (nmol/L) 3.5 years = 77.5 (65.4–91.9); 5 years = 92.6 (78.6–109) NEPSY-II (3.5 years); K-CPT (5 years); CBCL (3.5 and 5 years) Limited evidence of association between child DAPs and attention. Psychometrician, age at assessment, sex, child care, breast-feeding, maternal education, depressive symptoms and PPVT. H Lu et al., 2009 Costa Rica C-S 35 17 + 18 4–10 years Two spot urine samples of children: evening day 1 and first morning urine day 2 TCPy (␮g/L); La Amistad = 0d (0–3.31)a ; Las Mellizas = 0d (0–6.71)a BARS TCPy levels and BARS scores not associated. Age, gender, conventional or organic group, handedness and grade. L Harari et al., 2010 Ecuador C-S 84 6–8 years Child urine DAPs (levels not available) WISC-R: WM (DS); FTT, Santa Ana Form Board, K-CPT, CTSB and RCPM. Current exposure associated with ↑ Reaction time (with a borderline statistical significance) Sex, age, BMI, number of daily meals (only for current exposure), stunting, hematrocrit, school grade, having repeated one grade, maternal educational level, family living in a traditional house, drinking water supply, paternal education and employment. Creatinine-adjusted urine. H
  • 9. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 9 Table 3 (Continued) Postnatal exposure Author, year Country Study design n Age Biological samples/timing Analyte assayed median (95%CI) Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ Kofman et al., 2006 Israel C-C (age- sex match) 18 6–12 years Acute intoxication by OP before age 3 OP WISC-R: WM (DS) NEPSY Significant deficit in motor control among children exposed to OP. Age M Ruckart et al., 2004 USA P 652 6-aged children or younger at the moment of the spray with MP Wipe samples Urine of family members Methyl Parathion (MP) in wipe samples Exposed: ≥150 ␮g/cm2 (Mississippi) and ≥132.9 ␮g/cm2 (Ohio) p-nitrphenol (PNP) in urine (ppb) Exposed: PNP ≥ 100 ppb for at least one person in the household Non-exposed: no one in the household with PNP 100 ppb K-Bit, Purdue Pegboard, ML Test, Trail Making AB, PSI, PIC, VABS. No differences between exposed and non exposed children. Income, race, ethnicity, mother’ use of chemical at work, child had lead or mercury poisoning and mother had one or more of these conditionsc H Bouchard et al., 2011 USA P 329 7 years Child urine (6 months, 1, 2, 3.5 and 5 years) DAPs (levels not available) WISC-IV: VC (Voc/Sim), PR (BD/MR), WM (DS/LNS), PS (Cod/SS), FSIQ Significant ↑ scores in VC and FSIQ at 12 months. Exposure no consistently associated with poorer scores in FSIQ, WM, PS, VC and PR at 6 months, 2, 3.5 and 5 years. HOME score at 6 months and maternal education and intelligence. VC and FSIQ adjusted for language assessment. Creatinine-adjusted urine. H Lizardi et al., 2008 USA C-S 48 7 years Child urine DAPs (nmol/L) Exposed Group: 118.28b (95.7–149.5) Non-exposed Group = 52.68b (38.7–67.74) WISC-III,WCST, Trail Making Test A B, CBCL No significant association between levels of OP metabolites and performance on WISC-III. – L
  • 10. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model TOXLET-8548; No. of Pages 18 10 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx– xxx Table 3 (Continued) Postnatal exposure Author, year Country Study design n Age Biological samples/timing Analyte assayed median (95%CI) Test Findings Adjusted variables MQ Grandjean et al., 2006 Ecuador C-S 72 7 years Child urine DMP (nmol/kg per day) Prenatally exposed = 0.2d (max 6.94); Non-prenatally exposed = 0.42d (max 6.53) DEP (nmol/kg per day) Prenatally exposed = 0.27d (max 14.62) Non-prenatally exposed = 0.43d (max 2.49) WISC-R: WM (DS) Santa Ana Pegboard, CTSB and Catsys force plate Current exposure significantly associated with ↑ reaction time. Age, sex, trauma other injury and meningitis in the past medical history, maternal education and current nutrition. Creatinine-adjusted urine. H Bouchard et al., 2010 USA C-S 1139 8–15 years Child urine DAPs (nmol/L) 68.3 (6-10195)a DISC-IV Significant risk of ADHD diagnostic. Gender, age, race/ethnicity, PIR and fasting duration. H Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008 Egypt C-S 60 9–15 years Questionnaire. AChE levels in child blood Pesticides WAIS: VC (Inf/Sim), WM (Ar), PR (BD) BVRT, DS and Trail Making AB Pesticides applicators significantly ↓ scores on all the measures compared to the control group. Effects of age and education (13-year-old individual with 6 years of education and a body mass index (BMI) of 21.7). M Study design abbreviations: C-S: Cross-sectional; P: Prospective; C-C: Case-Control. MQ: Methodology Quality; H: High; M: Medium; L: Low. Tests and subtests abbreviations. Ar: Arithmetic; BARS: Behavioral Assessment and Research System; BSID: Bayley Scales for Infant Development; BD: Block Design; BVRT: Benton Visual Retention Test; CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; Cod: Coding; CTSB: Copying test of the Stanford-Binet; DISC-IV: Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children IV; DS: Digit Span; FSIQ: Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; FTT: Finger Tapping Test; GDS: Gesell Developmental Schedules; Inf: Information; K-Bit: Kaufman Brief Intelligence test; K-CPT: Conner’ Kiddie Continuous Performance; LNS: Letter-Number Sequence; ML Test: Memory and learning test; MR: Matrix Reasoning; NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PIC: Personality Inventory for Children; PR: Perceptual Reasoning; BD: Block Design; PS: Processing Speed; PSI: Parenting Stress Index; RCPM: Raven’ Colored Progressive Matrices; Sim: Similarities; SS: Symbol Search; VABS: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales; VC: Verbal Comprehension; Voc: Vocabulary; WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WCST Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III; WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV; WISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WM: Working Memory. a Range. b Mean. c Diabetes or epilepsy/seizures before pregnancy, hospitalized or confined to bed during pregnancy, fever, X rays, or vaginal bleeding during pregnancy. d Median were reported when geometric mean or 95% CI were not available.
  • 11. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 11 Table 4 Summary of effects of prenatal exposure to OP pesticides on mental development, psychomotor development, attention deficit hiperactivity disorder (ADHD) and pervasive development disorder (PDD). Compounds Age Findings Reference Mental development Psychomotor development Attention problems ADHD PDD DMP ↑ DMP Newborns ↓* Engel et al., 2007 2 months ↓* Young et al., 2005 6 months ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↓black/hispanic ↑white ↓black/hispanic ↑white Engel et al., 2011 ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↓ ↑ Engel et al., 2011 ↓* ↓* ↑ ↑ ↑* Eskenazi et al., 2007 3.5 years ↑* ↑* Marks et al., 2010 5 years ↑* ↑* Marks et al., 2010 6–9 years ↓ Engel et al., 2011 7 years ↓* Bouchard et al., 2011 DEP ↑ DEP Newborns ↓ Engel et al., 2007 2 months ↓* Young et al., 2005 6 months ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↓black/hispanic ↑white ↓black/hispanic ↑white Engel et al., 2011 ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↓ ↑ Engel et al., 2011 ↓ ↓ ↑ ↑ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 3.5 years – ↓ Marks et al., 2010 5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010 6–9 years ↓ Engel et al., 2011 7 years ↓* Bouchard et al., 2011 DAPs ↑ DAPs Newborns ↓* Engel et al., 2007 2 months ↓* Young et al., 2005 6 months ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↓black/hispanic ↑white ↓black/hispanic ↑white Engel et al., 2011 ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↓ ↑ Engel et al., 2011 ↓* ↓ ↑ ↑ ↑* Eskenazi et al., 2007 3.5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010 5 years ↑* ↑* Marks et al., 2010 6–9 years ↓ Engel et al., 2011 7 years ↓* Bouchard et al., 2011 Chlorpyrifos ↑ Chlorpyrifos 12 months ↓ ↓* Rauh et al., 2006 24 months ↓ ↑ Rauh et al., 2006 36 months ↓* ↓* ↑* ↑* ↑* Rauh et al., 2006 7 years ↓* Horton et al., 2012 7 years ↓* Rauh et al., 2011 ↑ TCPy 6 months ↑ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↓ ↓ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↓ ↓ – – – Eskenazi et al., 2007 Pesticides (questionnaire) 3–21 months ↓ ↓ Handal et al., 2008 6–8 years ↓* ↓* Harari et al., 2010 7 years ↓* – Grandjean et al., 2006 (–) no effect observed; blank cells indicate effects that were not measured. * p 0.1. between occupational exposure to OP pesticides during pregnancy in farming communities in Ecuador and a decrease in the visual- spatial function of children aged 6 -8 years (Grandjean et al., 2006; Harari et al., 2010), and in their visual memory and motor coordi- nation and speed (Harari et al., 2010). The evidence of a link between prenatal exposure to OP pesti- cides and behavioural disorders is limited because few studies have been carried out. These effects were assessed in the CHAMACOS cohort when the children were 2, 3.5 and 5 years of age (Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010). Average urinary DAPs levels during pregnancy were associated with a significantly higher risk of perva- sive developmental disorder (PDD) at 2 years of age, but not with other attention problems or attention deficit hyperactivity disor- der (ADHD) (Eskenazi et al., 2007). However, an increased risk of ADHD was observed at 3.5 years, and a significantly higher risk was observed at the 5-year point (Marks et al., 2010). With regard to the effects of exposure to chlorpyrifos on neu- rodevelopment and behaviour in children, those with 6.17 pg/g of chlorpyrifos in umbilical cord blood had significantly lower MDI and PDT scores at 36 months (Rauh et al., 2006). This trend was also observed at the 7-year point, and it has been estimated that for each standard deviation increase in chlorpyrifos exposure (4.61 pg/g in cord blood), full-scale intelligence quotient (IQ) declined by 1.4% and working memory declined by 2.8% (Horton et al., 2012; Rauh et al., 2011). However, Eskenazi et al. (2007) found no link between maternal urinary levels of TCPy, the specific metabo- lite of chlorpyrifos, and children’s MDI or PDI scores at 6 and 24 months.
  • 12. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 12 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 3.3. Postnatal Exposure Thirteen studies analysed the association between postnatal exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopmental and/or neu- robehavioural effects in children. Overall, no clear trend was observed during the first few years of life. The results of the different studies are summarised in Tables 3 and 5. An increase in urinary DAPs levels in children aged 6-8 was asso- ciated with a significantly higher reaction time in the studies by Grandjean et al. (2006) and Harari et al. (2010). Other studies assessed postnatal exposure to OP pesticides indi- rectly based on children’s agricultural work (Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008), the proximity of their homes to farmland, their parents working in farming (Rohlman et al., 2005) and the children’s his- tory of OP poisoning (Kofman et al., 2006). These studies found significant decreases in the children’s verbal comprehension (VC), working memory (WM) and perceptual reasoning (PR) scores (Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008), and changes in their response times and latency (Rohlman et al., 2005) and motor control (Kofman et al., 2006). However, other studies found no association between postnatal exposure to OP pesticides and effects on children’s motor or cogni- tive function (Bouchard et al., 2011; Guodong et al., 2012; Lizardi et al., 2008; Lu et al., 2009; Ruckart et al., 2004). Some studies even reached the opposite conclusion, as a significant link was found between an increase in urinary DAPs levels and increased MDI in children aged 12-24 months (Eskenazi et al., 2007). In terms of behavioural effects, high levels of urinary DAPs have been linked with a significant increase in attention problems (Harari et al., 2010) and ADHD (Lizardi et al., 2008; Bouchard et al., 2010) in children aged 6-15, and PDD in children aged 2 (Eskenazi et al., 2007). 4. Discussion 4.1. Effects potentially associated with prenatal and postnatal exposure to OPs The results of the aforementioned studies suggest that prena- tal exposure to OP pesticides may affect neurodevelopment and behaviour in infancy and childhood as well as children’s cognitive and motor function. The average maternal urinary DAPs in the studies reviewed ranged from 81.3 nmol/L (Engel et al., 2007) to 132 nmol/L (Young et al., 2005). These levels are slightly higher than those found in the pregnant women who took part in the National Health and Nutrition Survey 1999–2000 (NHANES), where they ranged from 70.5 nmol/L to 72.0 nmol/L (Bradman et al., 2005). However, 25% of pregnant women in the NHANES study had DAPs levels of 246.2 nm/L (percentile 75), higher than those found in the stud- ies where neurodevelopmental and/or behavioural changes were observed (Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2011; Marks et al., 2010). It is therefore essential to assess exposure to OP pesticides not just in populations with a higher probability of exposure, but also in the general population, in order to identify subgroups in young children with higher risk of developing neurodevelopmental changes. All of the studies that determined the urinary DAPs levels of pregnant women collected two urine samples over the course of the pregnancy, except Engel et al. (2007, 2011), who collected only one sample in the third trimester. The collection of several urine samples enable to assess if prenatal exposure to OP pesticides has been more or less constant throughout the pregnancy or if there have been periods of higher or lower exposure. This information is very important because there are critical windows associated with a higher vulnerability, as occurs during organogenesis or the devel- opment of the central nervous system (Rice and Barone, 2000). The knowledge of the time-point of exposure to OP pesticides during pregnancy is more informative to predict potential impacts on the child’s motor and cognitive function. Although there is some evidence of these effects in newborns, the most consistent evidence has been found in children aged 24 months and over (Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006). This delay between the time of exposure and the appearance of the effect is one of the least-understood aspects in this area. According to Rice and Barone (2000), exposure to a neuro- toxic compound during a critical window of development may only manifest later in time because exposure occurs at a time when cas- cading development processes are taking place. These processes begin during gestation and continue during the first stages of infancy, and this could explain why these effects on cognitive func- tion are observed at this point and not at an earlier age. It is also difficult to detect these changes until they are quite advanced, and some neuropsychological tests are not very sensitive. This could account for by the delay between exposure and effect. It is difficult to draw conclusions about the possible effects of postnatal exposure to OP pesticides on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children, because in some studies the level of signif- icance of the association decreased when postnatal exposure was analysed instead of prenatal exposure (Marks et al., 2010), or the results for postnatal exposure were not consistent with those for prenatal exposure (Eskenazi et al., 2007). These differences between the effects associated with pre- and postnatal exposure could be linked to neuroplasticity. Some animal studies suggest that the nervous system can be adapted to expo- sure to environmental toxins and compensate for their effects on the organism (Rice and Barone, 2000; Selemon, 2013). This neuro- plasticity has also been studied in children and adolescents where a continuous development of the structures responsible for motor function and oral communication has been observed in subjects aged 4–17 (Paus et al., 1999). However, further studies are needed to evaluate the effects of pre- and postnatal exposure to OP pesti- cides using a common methodology to obtain more evidence and evaluate the consistency of findings. 4.2. Variability in the methodologies used to evaluate exposure to OP pesticides and effects on neurodevelopment The wide variety of different techniques used to evaluate expo- sure to OP pesticides and its effects on neurodevelopment and behaviour is a major limitation when the available results are to be compared. The possibility of carrying out a meta-analysis of the studies included in this systematic review was considered, distinguish- ing between prenatal and postnatal exposure, in order to estimate the overall effects of exposure to OP pesticides on neurodevelop- ment and/or behaviour in children. However, this was not possible because of the large variability in: (a) methods used to evaluate exposure to OP pesticides (biomarkers vs. questionnaires, use of different biomarkers); (b) windows of exposure; (c) neuropsychol- ogical tests used to assess children’s mental and motor functions; (d) age of the children; and (e) statistical methods used to evaluate links between exposure to OP pesticides and neurodevelopment in children. Study design is another of the aspects that must be considered when comparing the results of different studies. Cross-sectional studies provide information about levels of exposure to a particular compound, while prospective studies provide information about exposure at different time-points (e.g., during pregnancy and in the first few years of life in the case of birth cohorts). This makes it
  • 13. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 13 Table 5 Summary of effects of postnatal exposure to OP pesticides on mental development, psychomotor development, attention deficit hiperactivity disorder (ADHD) and pervasive development disorder (PDD). Compounds Age Findings Reference Mental development Psychomotor development Attention problems ADHD PDD DMP ↑ DMP 6 months ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↑ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↑* ↑ ↑ ↑ ↑* Eskenazi et al., 2007 23–25 months ↑ ↓ Guodong et al., 2012 3.5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010 5 years ↓ – Marks et al., 2010 7 years ↓* ↓* Grandjean et al., 2006 8–15 years ↑* Bouchard et al., 2010 DEP ↑ DEP 6 months ↑ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↑* ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↑ ↑ ↑ ↑ ↑* Eskenazi et al., 2007 23–25 months ↓ ↑ Guodong et al., 2012 3.5 years ↑* ↑ Marks et al., 2010 5 years – ↑ Marks et al., 2010 7 years ↓* ↓* Grandjean et al., 2006 8–15 years ↑ Bouchard et al., 2010 DAPs ↑ DAPs 6 months ↓ ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 12 months ↑* ↑ Eskenazi et al., 2007 24 months ↑* ↑ ↑ ↑ ↑* Eskenazi et al., 2007 23–25 months ↑ ↑ Guodong et al., 2012 3.5 years ↑ ↑ Marks et al., 2010 5 years – – Marks et al., 2010 6–8 years ↓ ↓ ↑* Harari et al., 2010 ↑ Bouchard et al., 2011 7 years – ↑* Lizardi et al., 2008 8–15 years ↑* Bouchard et al., 2010 ↑ Methyl Parathion 6 years ↑ ↑ ↑ Ruckart et al., 2004 ↑ TCPy 4–10 years – – Lu et al., 2009 OP Pesticides (questionnaire) 48–71 months ↓* ↓* Rohlman et al., 2005 6–12 years – ↓* Kofman et al., 2006 9–15 years ↓* ↓* Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008 (–) no effect observed; blank cells indicate effects that were not measured. * p 0.1. possible to determine if exposure to these toxins over a particular period of time has been constant or sporadic. Most of the studies (77%) that analysed prenatal exposure to OP pesticides used a longitudinal design (Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2007; Engel et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Horton et al., 2012; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006; Rauh et al., 2011; Young et al., 2005). However, studies evaluating postnatal exposure mainly used the cross-sectional approach (61.5%) (Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008; Bouchard et al., 2010; Grandjean et al., 2006; Guodong et al., 2012; Harari et al., 2010; Lizardi et al., 2008; Lu et al., 2009; Rohlman et al., 2005), followed by the longitudinal approach (30.7%) (Bouchard et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Ruckart et al., 2004) or a case-control design (7.8%) (Kofman et al., 2006). Prospective studies provide better information about pre- and postnatal exposure to OP pesticides and the effects observed in each window of exposure. They can therefore be used to identify critical stages in the development of a child’s nervous system and provide more evidence of how these effects develop in each stage. The availability of several measurements of exposure also provides information about intraindividual variability in terms of exposure to OP pesticides, as a single measurement of these non-persistent pesticides is not a good indicator of an individual’s usual exposure. Griffith et al. (2011) measured children’s urinary DAPs levels over a period of 21 months and found that the individual DAPs levels varied by 3–7 folds depending on the metabolite analysed, and day-to-day variability was much greater than the between-child variability. Inter- and intraindividual variability is a major limi- tation of cross-sectional studies, as reported Sudakin and Stone (2011). The use of biomonitoring techniques to evaluate exposure to environmental pollutants is becoming more common. In fact, 9 of the 12 studies that evaluated prenatal exposure to OP pesticides and 10 of the 13 studies that evaluated postnatal exposure deter- mined the levels of the biomarkers as a measure of exposure to these compounds in the urine or blood. Most of the studies that analysed prenatal exposure by mea- suring specific or non-specific (DAPs) biomarkers for OP observed significant or near significant effects on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children (Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2007; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Horton et al., 2012; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006; Rauh et al., 2011; Young et al., 2005). The same occurred in studies that estimated exposure by using question- naires (Grandjean et al., 2006; Harari et al., 2010). However, the percentage of studies on postnatal exposure where evidence of the effects was observed was lower when biomonitoring tech- niques were used (3 out of 10) as compared to the utilisation of a questionnaire (3 out of 3). Since questionnaires may examine the accumulated exposure, to a compound, this would explain why the effects observed in these studies were greater (Abdel Rasoul et al., 2008; Kofman et al., 2006; Rohlman et al., 2005). Biomonitoring techniques can be used to accurately evaluate current exposure to non-persistent pesticides by measuring the amount of the parent compound or one of its metabolites in urine
  • 14. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 14 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx or blood samples, or to analyse accumulated exposure using hair or meconium samples. Biomarkers of exposure to OP pesticides in the mother or child’s urine and, to a lesser extent, in umbili- cal cord blood samples were more often measured in the studies included in this review. Because of their chemical structure, OP pesticides are quickly metabolised and eliminated 24–72 h after exposure (Fourth National Report on Human Exposure to Environ- mental Chemicals, 2009; Maroni et al., 2000; WHO, 1996). This means that analysing their presence in urine (OP metabolites) or blood samples (original compounds) provides information about recent exposure instead of accumulated exposure, and this makes it difficult to estimate representative exposure. In the case of prena- tal exposure, meconium samples could provide information about the levels of exposure of the foetus from the second trimester until birth (Bearer et al., 1999; Ostrea, 1999). Some studies that mea- sured DAPs levels in meconium samples highlighted the usefulness of this biological matrix for the evaluation of prenatal exposure to OP pesticides (Tsatsakis et al., 2009; Whyatt and Barr, 2001). Recent studies have also discussed the importance of hair as an indica- tor of chronic exposure to these compounds (Tsatsakis et al., 2008, 2010). Measuring DAPs levels in hair could provide information about exposure over a longer period (months), representative of the child’s accumulated exposure (medium/long-term exposure) during the postnatal period (Tsatsakis et al., 2008). Analyses of prenatal and/or postnatal exposure to OP pesti- cides have generally been carried out based on urinary DAPs levels. However, because DAPs are non-specific metabolites it is not pos- sible to determine the exact compound to which the subjects have been exposed to, as these metabolites are common to several OPs and a single OP can produce several types of DAPs (Barr et al., 2004). Moreover, the population may be directly exposed to these metabolites present in foodstuffs and in the atmosphere (Sudakin and Stone, 2011) as a result of the spontaneous breakdown of the original pesticides. There is evidence that this type of exposure con- tributes to total urinary DAPs levels in children (Curl et al., 2002, 2003), as diet is one of the major sources of exposure to OP pesti- cides amongst this population group (Aprea et al., 2000; Lu et al., 2008). Despite these limitations, DAPs levels are used frequently in epidemiological studies as occurred in almost all of the studies included in this review. Only 5 of the 20 studies measured the con- centrations of parent compounds such as chlorpyrifos (Horton et al., 2012; Rauh et al., 2011; Rauh et al., 2006) or a specific metabolite such as 3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridinol (TCPy) (Eskenazi et al., 2007) or p-nitrophenol (PNP) (Ruckart et al., 2004), which represent the leaving groups of chlorpyrifos and parathion or methyl-parathion, respectively. Distinguishing between residential and dietary exposure is a key part of the process of characterising children’s exposure to OP pesti- cides. However, the epidemiological studies analysing exposure to these compounds provide little information about the role of diet (Jurewicz and Hanke, 2008). Only 3 of the 20 studies included in this review assessed the influence of diet on levels of exposure and the effects studied (Grandjean et al., 2006; Guodong et al., 2012; Harari et al., 2010). There is scant research on the contribution of the diet to the chil- dren’ total exposure to OP pesticides. Several studies have assessed the contribution of the diet to the total exposure to OP pesticides in children. Children on organic diet showed lower urinary DAPs levels than those on conventional diet and the full substitution of a conventional diet by an organic diet was associated with a decrease in urinary DAPs levels (Curl et al., 2003; Lu et al., 2006). Therefore, dietary information should be considered in future studies to clar- ify the contribution of different food items to children’ exposure as compared to other sources of exposure. This would represent an important step-forward in the search and in the implementation of feasible measures to reduce children’ exposure to these chemicals. Social factors such as socioeconomic level, family environment, parents’ mental health and intelligence quotient are directly linked with a child’s upbringing, influencing aspects such as diet, exposure to environmental toxins, the child’s level of stimulation and devel- opment, amongst others (Forns et al., 2012). More recent studies have started to incorporate these variables into their designs to assess their effect on exposure to environmental pollutants and children’s level of development and behaviour. Almost all of the studies included in this review included one or several social indi- cators as confounding variables to adjust their statistical models. The most frequent variables were those related to the family’s eco- nomic status (family income, poverty income ratio), race/ethnicity, paternal education (level of education of the mother and/or father, years at school, occupation), the mother’s intelligence and mental health, and the child’s living conditions (HOME score, living in a traditional house). Because of the limited number of studies (n = 4) that did not include any of these confounding variables and the vari- ability of the designs used (age of the study population, exposure biomarkers studied, neurodevelopment tests used), it was not pos- sible to assess the differences between the results of those studies and the ones that did include those confounding variables. With regard to the tests used to assess children’s neurobe- havioural development, many of the studies used the different Wechsler intelligence scales (WISC III, WISC-IV, WPPSI-III, WISC-R, WAIS) and the Bayley scale (BSID-II), depending on the age range of the participants and the date on which the population was recruited in the studies reviewed. Both scales provide more thorough infor- mation on children’ neurodevelopment than those tests assessing only specific functions, such as visual memory, dexterity, motor speed and coordination, among others. All tests can be used to assess children’ neurodevelopment and provide valuable informa- tion. However, the selection of a particular test responds to the aim of the investigation and takes into account the age range of the par- ticipants and the date when the study was performed. For a better comparability of the results of epidemiological studies it is impor- tant to harmonize the neurodevelopmental tests available (White et al., 2009). Table 1 shows detailed information on the tests used in the reviewed studies is shown. The evaluation of effects on neurodevelopment using IQ tests is gradually being complemented by other indexes measuring differ- ent areas such as memory, verbal comprehension, processing speed and perceptual reasoning, amongst others (Flanagan and Kaufman, 2004). Using specific tests would be better than simply using gen- eral intelligence tests, as previous studies suggest that exposure to environmental toxins could cause specific types of damage to the central nervous system which would not be detected based on the child’s IQ alone (White et al., 2009; Forns et al., 2012; Grandjean et al., 2006). Some of the studies included in this review also evaluated behavioural disorders and attention problems in children (Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010). The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) for parents was the most frequently used method of identifying this type of disorder along with other tests and questionnaires (Table 1). There is a specific CBCL questionnaire for each age range that allow the identification of behavioural changes in children, such as hyper- activity, violent behaviour, anxiety and depression, amongst others (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2000). 4.3. Limitations associated with the study design An accurate estimation of the number of participants accord- ing to the research purposes is crucial since a small sample size could underestimate the magnitude of the associations. Since the sample size is determined by the levels of OP exposure, the lack of information on exposure may hamper the estimation of the sample size in the study population. Moreover, a reduced sample size may
  • 15. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 15 also prevent from observing neurodevelopmental or behavioural effects associated with pre- or postnatal exposure to OP pesticides (Guodong et al., 2012). Other studies, despite admitting having a limited sample size, observed significant or near-significant effects (Engel et al., 2011; Grandjean et al., 2006; Handal et al., 2008; Harari et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2006; Rohlman et al., 2005). Loss of recruited children during the follow-up could introduce a selec- tion bias that might affect the results of the study. The reviewed studies highlighted the limitations associated with the sample size as regards exposure assessment to OP pesticides (Bouchard et al., 2011; Eskenazi et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2010; Rauh et al., 2011), very likely because of the low persistence of these compounds and the use of non-specific biomarkers, such as DAPs. In addition to sample size limitations, the influence of statistical analysis is also remarkable. Most of the studies reviewed were lon- gitudinal in design, however cross-sectional analyses were made. This could limit the advantages of cohort studies since they did not evaluate longitudinally the changes in children’ performance over time. Likewise, to our best knowledge no study has evaluated the potential reversibility of effects associated either with decreased exposure or with social, educational and/or biological factors. 4.4. Differences linked to sex/gender Few studies have analysed the link between sex and gender and exposure to environmental pollutants and its effects on health, especially in children. Sex differences are due to biological factors and affect the metabolism and exposure to xenobiotics, as well as susceptibility and health throughout lifetime (Clougherty, 2010). Gender, on the other hand, is a social construct, influenced by cul- tural norms, roles and behaviour due to the relationships between men and women and between boys and girls (Clougherty, 2010). There are also differences in the foods that girls and boys eat and the activities they take part in (games, sport) (Molinero et al., 2010). Social environment during pregnancy and childhood also have a significant influence on exposure to environmental pollutants and neurodevelopment (Jedrychowski et al., 2009), so these factors should be included in epidemiological studies. A study carried out by van Wendel et al. (2011, personal communication, unpublished) in a banana farming community in Costa Rica found a link between postnatal exposure to chlorpyrifos and a reduction in short-term memory in children aged 6–9, and the effect was greater in males than in females. The reasons for these differences between sexes are not clearly identifiable, although they may not only be due to biological factors (hormonal differ- ences, differences in pesticide metabolism or in the repair processes of the damage caused by OP pesticides in cholinergic pathways) (Arbuckle, 2006; Slotkin et al., 2008), but also to social factors, such as different lifestyles, behaviour and occupations. It is also impos- sible to rule out the possibility that differences between sexes vary by age, so sex could play a greater or lesser role in children than in adults or vice versa. Overall, the studies reviewed have included sex as a confound- ing variable when adjusting multivariate models, either because they found a link in the univariate analysis or because of assumed differences between the sexes in terms of nervous system devel- opment or exposure to OP pesticides. Very few studies conducted stratified analysis by sex (Bouchard et al., 2011; Horton et al., 2012; Marks et al., 2010; Rohlman et al., 2005) to disaggregate biological and social differences between boys and girls. Some studies on prenatal exposure to OP pesticides found a decrease in working memory (Horton et al., 2011) or an increase in attention problems and ADHD (Marks et al., 2010) in boys but not in girls. One study on postnatal exposure to OP pesticides also found a significant decrease in response time in boys but not in girls (Rohlman et al., 2005). However, another study that stratified the analysis by sex did not find any difference in the association between urinary DAPs and effects on neurodevelopment between boys and girls (Bouchard et al., 2011). None of these articles carried out an analysis of gender differ- ences to separate social differences from biological characteristics, which would have allowed the identification of differences in expo- sure patterns in children linked to the different way in which they interact with the environment. Beamer et al. (2008) observed major differences between the behaviour of boys and girls aged 6–27 months in a farming community in California, where the boys came into contact with the ground and toys more frequently, while the girls came into more frequent contact with clothes, towels, toys and other objects. These differences in activities could lead to dif- ferences in their exposure to certain environmental toxins. Future studies should examine the role that sex and gender play in exposure to environmental contaminants and its effects on health in children. This would help to identify effect modification factors, as well as pathways and sources of exposure, making it possible to design more effective interventions. 4.5. Toxicity mechanisms of organophosphate pesticides and neurodevelopment The association between neurodevelopmental disorders and OP pesticide exposure could be linked to its inhibiting effect on the enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE), which plays an important role in the nervous system function. When AChE is inhibited by OP, the excess acetylcholine that accumulates in the synaptic terminals overstimulates the nicotinic and muscarinic receptors (Ecobichon, 2001). This toxic action could interfere the human neurological development process from the early stages of development (Rice and Barone, 2000). However, recent studies have identified toxicity mechanisms other than AChE inhibition that could be involved in neurodevel- opmental disorders (Androutsopoulos et al., 2013). Recent in vitro studies have found a decrease in the number of neurites associ- ated with diazoxon (active metabolite of diazinon), DNA synthesis inhibition and alterations in the differentiation of C6 glioma cells associated with chlorpyrifos oxon (Flaskos, 2012). These effects occur at low OP concentrations which do not inhibit AChE so that cholinergic transmission is unaffected (Pope et al., 2005). Chlor- pyrifos oxon is capable of phosphorylating (and therefore activate) transcription factors involved in brain development (e.g. cAMP response element-binding protein, CREB) at much lower concen- trations (nanomolar) than those required to inhibit AChE, thus affecting key processes in neural development and the develop- ment of cognitive functions (Schuh et al., 2002). 4.6. Genetic susceptibility Genetic variability plays an important role in determining the individual susceptibility to environmental pollutants. This means that genetic variations in the enzymes involved in the detoxifi- cation of these chemicals could make an individual more or less susceptible to their toxic effects (Costa et al., 2003). An individual’s capacity to hydrolyse OP pesticides can also be affected by their plasma PON1 levels, and is 3–4 folds lower in chil- dren than in adults (Cole et al., 2012; Furlong et al., 2006; Huen et al., 2009) making them more susceptible to the effects of these compounds (Padilla et al., 2000). Other factors such as diet, alcohol consumption or drug use, as well as psychological or physiological factors, can also affect PON1 activity (Costa et al., 2005a,b). Although some enzymes have been considered as biomarkers of genetic susceptibility related to OP pesticide metabolism (Costa et al., 2005a,b), only PON1 genetic polymorphisms have been eval- uated so far in epidemiological studies (Furlong, 2007).
  • 16. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 16 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx Eskenazi et al. (2010) evaluated the interaction effects between prenatal exposure to OP pesticides and PON1192 and PON1-108 polymorphisms on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children. There was a significant association between maternal urinary DAPs and a lower mental development index in 2-year-olds carrying the PON1-108T allele, as well as a lower psychomotor develop- ment index, although in the latter case the differences were not statistically significant. This gene-environment interaction did not occur with the PON1192 polymorphism. The PON1192 and PON1-108 polymorphisms had no significant effect on pervasive development disorder (PDD) in the same age group. Engel et al. (2011) observed that increased maternal urinary DAPs were associated with a significant decrease in the MDI of children aged 12–24 months whose mothers carried the PON1192R allele. In 6 to 9-year-olds, the children of mothers with the PON1192QQ genotype scored considerably lower on the Wech- sler perceptual reasoning scale than the children of mothers with the PON1192R allele, although the difference was not statistically significant. However, the other polymorphisms studied (PON155, PON1-108) were not found to have an effect on neurodevelopment. 4.7. Epigenetics and neurodevelopment A number of illnesses and functional changes in organ systems have been linked with epigenetic mechanisms, including cogni- tive dysfunction and developmental neuropsychological disorders (Martínez-Frías, 2010; Martino et al., 2009; Weinhold, 2006). Epi- genetics is the study of heritable changes in gene expression or phenotype occurring without changes in DNA sequence (Martínez- Frías, 2010; Perera and Herbstman, 2011; Rodríguez-Milord et al., 1990). Epigenetic mechanisms that regulate gene expression, such as DNA methylation, take place in a carbon cycle where one methyl group, from S-adenosylmethionine, is covalently added to the car- bon 5 of the DNA cytosine ring (CpG dinucleotide). This process is highly dependent on the folate levels (Bird, 2002; Costello and Plass, 2001; Lee et al., 2009; Martínez-Frías, 2010). There is evidence that the process also involves chromatin remodelling and vari- ous complex histone modifications including acetylation for gene expression, and phosphorylation for gene activation and repression (mainly through different families of methyltransferases, acetyl- transferases and deacetylases) (Cheung and Lau, 2005; Shahbazian and Grunstein, 2007; Varga-Weisz and Becker, 2006). Less is known about other mechanisms such as protein modifications (e.g. ubi- quitination, sumoylation). Other epigenetic processes involve small non-coding RNAs, known as microRNAs (miRNAs), which are the main regulators of gene function and expression during prena- tal development and during germ line formation in both sexes (Filipowicz et al., 2008; Hayashi et al., 2008; Martínez-Frías, 2010; Tam et al., 2008). Some authors have discussed how exposure to environmental factors, including xenobiotics, can lead to epigenetic alterations (Perera and Herbstman, 2011; Venerosi et al., 2012). There is a growing amount of evidence that exposure to environmen- tal chemicals during early development may lead to epigenetic changes that could determine susceptibility to diseases in later life or may be transmitted from one generation to another (Weinhold, 2006). The effect of three OP pesticides (fonofos, parathion and ter- bufos) on human DNA was recently studied ‘in vitro’. The three pesticides caused methylation changes in the promoter regions of the same 712 genes, as well as exhibited their own OP-specific methylation alterations (Zhang et al., 2012b). Epigenetic mecha- nisms may also contribute to OP-induced carcinogenesis (Zhang et al., 2012a,b) and possibly to other complex disorders. How- ever, few animal studies have examined other potential epigenetic implications of OP pesticides in neurodevelopmental disorders (Martínez-Frías, 2010; Persico and Bourgeron, 2006; Venerosi et al., 2012). New, innovative and multidisciplinary studies on gene-gene and gene-environment interactions and gene susceptibility are essential to prevent effects associated with epigenetic mechanisms. 5. Conclusions The studies included in this review used a wide variety of dif- ferent designs and methods to evaluate the effects of OP pesticide exposure on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children, and this makes it difficult to compare their results. The studies reviewed suggest that exposure during pregnancy may have a negative effect on the child’s mental and motor development and behaviour during the first stages of childhood. The effects associated with postnatal exposure are less consistent, although may also affect the child’s cognitive and motor function, as well as increase the risk of atten- tion problems. In order to draw conclusions, further studies need to be carried out evaluating exposure to OP pesticides at differ- ent stages of pregnancy and monitoring the possible effects on neurodevelopment after birth. Studies analysing both pre- and postnatal exposure (current and accumulated) to these compounds are also required. It is therefore important to identify the most suitable methods for evaluating exposure to OP pesticides and its effects on neurodevelopment and behaviour in children so that a standardised method can be established. This would make it eas- ier to compare results and estimate the overall effects of exposure to these compounds in children. It is also important to incorporate differences associated with gender and sex into epidemiological designs, as well as information about the children’s diet, fam- ily environment and lifestyle. This is key to identifying the main determining factors of exposure to OP pesticides and its possible health effects in children. By using existing evidence as well as future research that will broaden knowledge of the mechanisms involved in OP pesticide metabolism, and studying the role that an individual’s genetic characteristics play when exposed to these compounds, it will be possible to design measures to reduce levels of exposure and its possible health effects in children. Conflict of interest The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest. Acknowledgements This study was partially supported by grants from the Council of Innovation of the Andalusian Government (reference number P08-CTS-04313, FEDER funds), the Institute of Health Carlos III (reference numbers PI10/01101 and FIS-FEDER 11/02038) and the European Union (EU FP7-ENV-2011 DENAMIC 2cod 28957). The content of this article is part of the PhD thesis of Beat- riz Gonzalez-Alzaga which was conducted at the University of Granada under the doctoral programme “Clinical Medicine and Public Health”. Appendix A. Supplementary data Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013. 11.019. References Abdel Rasoul, G.M., Abou Salem, M.E., Mechael, A.A., Hendy, O.M., Rohlman, D.S., Ismail, A.A., 2008. Effects of occupational pesticide exposure on children apply- ing pesticides. Neurotoxicology 29, 833–838. Achenbach, T.M., Rescorla, L.A., 2000. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, Burlington, VT.
  • 17. Please cite this article in press as: González-Alzaga, B., et al., A systematic review of neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal and postnatal organophosphate pesticide exposure. Toxicol. Lett. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.toxlet.2013.11.019 ARTICLE IN PRESS GModel TOXLET-8548; No.of Pages18 B. González-Alzaga et al. / Toxicology Letters xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 17 Androutsopoulos, V.P., Hernandez, A.F., Liesivuori, J., Tsatsakis, A.M., 2013. A mech- anistic overview of health associated effects of low levels of organochlorine and organophosphorous pesticides. Toxicology 307, 89–94. Aprea, C., Strambi, M., Novelli, M.T., Lunghini, L., Bozzi, N., 2000. Biologic monitoring of exposure to organophosphorus pesticides in 195 italian children. Environ. Health Perspect. 108, 521–525. Arbuckle, T.E., 2006. Are there sex and gender differences in acute exposure to chemicals in the same setting. Environ. Res. 101, 195–204. Barr, D.B., Bravo, R., Weerasekera, G., Caltabiano, L.M., Whitehead Jr., R.D., Ols- son, A.O., et al., 2004. Concentrations of dialkyl phosphate metabolites of organophosphorus pesticides in the U.S. population. Environ. Health Perspect. 112, 186–200. Beamer, P., Key, M.E., Ferguson, A.C., Canales, R.A., Auyeung, W., Leckie, J.O., 2008. Quantified activity pattern data from 6 to 27-month-old farmworker children for use in exposure assessment. Environ. Res. 108, 239–246. Bearer, C.F., Lee, S., Salvator, A.E., Minnes, S., Swick, A., Yamashita, T., et al., 1999. Ethyl linoleate in meconium: a biomarker of prenatal ethanol exposure. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 23, 487–493. Bird, A., 2002. DNA methylation patterns and epigenetic memory. Genes Dev. 16, 6–21. Bouchard, M.F., Bellinger, D.C., Wright, R.O., Weisskopf, M.G., 2010. Attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder and urinary metabolites of organophosphate pesticides. Pediatrics 125, 1270–1277. Bouchard, M.F., Chevrier, J., Harley, K.G., Kogut, K., Vedar, M., Calderon, N., Trujillo, C., Johnson, C., Bradman, A., Barr, D.B., Eskenazi, B., 2011. Prenatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides and IQ in 7-year-old children. Environ. Health Perspect. 119, 1189–1195. Bradman, A., Eskenazi, B., Barr, D.B., Bravo, R., Castorina, R., Chevrier, J., Kogut, K., Harnly, M.E., McKone, T.E., 2005. Organophosphate urinary metabolite levels during pregnancy and after delivery in women living in an agricultural commu- nity. Environ. Health Perspect. 113, 1802–1807. Cheung, P., Lau, P., 2005. Epigenetic regulation by histone methylation and histone variants. Mol. Endocrinol. 19, 563–573. Clougherty, J.E., 2010. A growing role for gender analysis in air pollution epidemi- ology. Environ. Health Perspect. 118, 167–176. Cole, T.B., Fisher, J.C., Burbacher, T.M., Costa, L.G., Furlong, C.E., 2012. Neuro- behavioral assessment of mice following repeated postnatal exposure to chlorpyrifos-oxon. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 34, 311–322. Costa, L.G., Cole, T.B., Vitalone, A., Furlong, C.E., 2005a. Measurement of paraoxonase (PON1) status as a potential biomarker of susceptibility to organophosphate toxicity. Clin. Chim. Acta 352, 37–47. Costa, L.G., Richter, R.J., Li, W.-F., Cole, T., Guizzetti, M., Furlong, C.E., 2003. Para- oxonase (PON 1) as a biomarker of susceptibility for organophosphate toxicity. Biomarkers 8, 1–12. Costa, L.G., Vitalone, A., Cole, T.B., Furlong, C.E., 2005b. Modulation of paraoxonase (PON1) activity. Biochem. Pharmacol. 69, 541–550. Costello, J.F., Plass, C., 2001. Methylation matters. J. Med. Genet. 38, 285–303. Curl, C.L., Fenske, R.A., Kissel, J.C., Shirai, J.H., Moate, T.F., Griffith, W., Coronado, G., Thompson, B., 2002. Evaluation of take-home organophosphorus pesticide exposure among agricultural workers and their children. Environ. Health Per- spect. 110, 787–792. Curl, C.L., Fenske, R.A., Elgethun, K., 2003. Organophosphorus pesticide exposure of urban and suburban preschool children with organic and conventional diets. Environ. Health Perspect. 111, 377–382. Eaton, D.L., Farin, F., Omiecinski, C.J., Omenn, G.S., 1998. Genetic susceptibility. In: Rom, W.N. (Ed.), Environmental and Occupational Medicine. Lippincott-Raven, Philadelphia, pp. 209–221. Ecobichon, D.J., 2001. Pesticide use in developing countries. Toxicology 160, 27–33. Engel, S.M., Berkowitz, G.S., Barr, D.B., Teitelbaum, S.L., Siskind, J., Meisel, S.J., et al., 2007. Prenatal organophosphate metabolite and organochlorine levels and performance on the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale in a multiethnic pregnancy cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 165, 1397–1404. Engel, S.M., Wetmur, J., Chen, J., Zhu, C., Barr, D.B., Canfield, R.L., et al., 2011. Prenatal exposure to organophosphates, paraoxonase 1, and cognitive development in childhood. Environ. Health Perspect. 119, 1182–1188. Eskenazi, B., Harley, K., Bradman, A., Weltzien, E., Jewell, N.P., Barr, D.B., et al., 2004. Association of in utero organophosphate pesticide exposure and fetal growth and length of gestation in an agricultural population. Environ. Health Perspect. 112, 1116–1124. Eskenazi, B., Huen, K., Marks, A., Harley, K.G., Bradman, A., Barr, D.B., et al., 2010. PON1 and neurodevelopment in children from the CHAMACOS study exposed to organophosphate pesticides in utero. Environ. Health Perspect. 118, 1775–1781. Eskenazi, B., Marks, A.R., Bradman, A., Harley, K., Barr, D.B., Johnson, C., et al., 2007. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young Mexican-American children. Environ. Health Perspect. 115, 792–798. Filipowicz, W., Bhattacharyya, S.N., Sonenberg, N., 2008. Mechanisms of post- transcriptional regulation by microRNAs: are the answers in sight. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 102–114. Flanagan, D.P., Kaufman, A.S., 2004. Essentials of WISC-IV Assessment. John WileySons, New Jersey, USA. Flaskos, J., 2012. The developmental neurotoxicity of organophosphorus insecti- cides: a direct role for the oxon metabolites. Toxicol. Lett. 209, 86–93. Forns, J., Aranbarri a, Grellier, J., Julvez, J., Vrijheid, M., Sunyer, J., 2012. A conceptual framework in the study of neuropsychological development in epidemiological studies. Neuroepidemiology 38, 203–208. Furlong, C.E., Holland, N., Richter, R.J., Bradman, A., Ho, A., Eskenazi, B., 2006. PON1 status of farmworker mothers and children asa predictor of organophosphate sensitivity. Pharmacogenet. Genom. 16, 183–190. Furlong, C.E., 2007. Genetic variability in the cytochrome P450-paraoxonase 1 (PON1) pathway for detoxication of organophosphorus compounds. J. Biochem. Mol. Toxicol. 21, 197–205. Grandjean, P., Harari, R., Barr, D.B., Debes, F., 2006. Pesticide exposure and stunt- ing as independent predictors of neurobehavioral deficits in Ecuadorian school children. Pediatrics 117, e546–e556. Grandjean, P., Landrigan, P.J., 2006. Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chem- icals. Lancet 368, 2167–2178. Griffith, W., Curl, C.L., Fenske, R.A., Lu, C.A., Vigoren, E.M., Faustman, E.M., 2011. Organophosphate pesticide metabolite levels in pre-school children in an agri- cultural community: within- and between-child variability in a longitudinal study. Environ. Res. 111, 751–756. Guo, Y., Zhang, J., Yu, R., Zhu, K.Y., Guo, Y., Ma, E., 2012. Identification of two new cytochrome P450 genes and RNA interference to evaluate their roles in detoxi- fication of commonly used insecticides in Locusta migratoria. Chemosphere 87, 709–717. Guodong, D., Pei, W., Ying, T., Jun, Z., Yu, G., Xiaojin, W., et al., 2012. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young Shanghai children. Environ. Sci. Technol. 46, 2911–2917. Handal, A.J., Harlow, S.D., Breilh, J., Lozoff, B., 2008. Occupational exposure to pesticides during pregnancy and neurobehavioral development of infants and toddlers. Epidemiology 19, 851–859. Harari, R., Julvez, J., Murata, K., Barr, D., Bellinger, D.C., Debes, F., et al., 2010. Neurobe- havioral deficits and increased blood pressure in school-age children prenatally exposed to pesticides. Environ. Health Perspect. 118, 890–896. Hayashi, K., Chuva de Sousa Lopes, S.M., Kaneda, M., Tang, F., Hajkova, P., Lao, K., O’Carroll, D., Das, P.P., Tarakhovsky, A., Miska, E.A., Surani, M.A., 2008. MicroRNA biogenesis is required for mouse primordial germ cell development and sper- matogenesis. PLoS One 3, e1738. Horton, M.K., Kahn, L.G., Perera, F., Barr, D.B., Rauh, V., 2012. Does the home environ- ment and the sex of the child modify the adverse effects of prenatal exposure to chlorpyrifos on child working memory? Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 34, 534–541. Huen, K., Harley, K., Brooks, J., Hubbard, A., Bradman, A., Eskenazi, B., Holland, N., 2009. Developmental changes in PON1 enzyme activity in young children and effects of PON1polymorphisms. Environ. Health Perspect. 117, 1632–1638. Jedrychowski, W., Perera, F., Jankowski, J., Mrozek-Budzyn, D., Mroz, E., Flak, E., et al., 2009. Gender specific differences in neurodevelopmental effects of prena- tal exposure to very low-lead levels: the prospective cohort study in three-year olds. Early Hum. Dev. 85, 503–510. Jurewicz, J., Hanke, W., 2008. Prenatal and childhood exposure to pesticides and neurobehavioral development: review of epidemiological studies. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 21, 121–132. Kofman, O., Berger, A., Massarwa, A., Friedman, A., Jaffar, A.A., 2006. Motor inhibi- tion and learning impairments in school-aged children following exposure to organophosphate pesticides in infancy. Pediatric Res. 60, 88–92. Koureas, M., Tsakalof, A., Tsatsakis, A., Hadjichristodoulou, C., 2012. Systematic review of biomonitoring studies to determine the association between exposure to organophosphorus and pyrethroid insecticides and human health outcomes. Toxicol. Lett. 210, 155–168. Lacasaña, M., Vázquez-Grameix, H., Borja-Aburto, V.H., Blanco-Muñoz, J., Romieu, I., Aguilar-Garduño, C., García, A.M., 2006. Maternal and paternal occupational exposure to agricultural work and the risk of anencephaly. Occup. Environ. Med. 63, 649–656. Lizardi, P.S., O’Rourke, M.K., Morris, R.J., 2008. The effects of organophosphate pes- ticide exposure on Hispanic children’s cognitive and behavioral functioning. J. Pediatr. Psycholol. 33, 91–101. Lee, D.H., Jacobs, D.R., Porta, M., 2009. Hypothesis: a unifying mechanism for nutri- tion and chemicals as lifelong modulators of DNA hypomethylation. Environ. Health Perspect. 117, 1799–1802. Lu, C., Essig, C., Root, C., Rohlman, D.S., McDonald, T., Sulzbacher, S., 2009. Assessing the association between pesticide exposure and cognitive development in rural Costa Rican children living in organic and conventional coffee farms. Int. J. Ado- lesc. Med. Health 21, 609–621. Lu, C., Barr, D.B., Pearson, M.A., Waller, L.A., 2008. Dietary intake and its contribu- tion to longitudinal organophosphorus pesticide exposure in urban/suburban children. Environ. Health Perspect. 116, 537–542. Lu, C., Toepel, K., Irish, R., Fenske, R.A., Barr, D.B., Bravo, R., 2006. Organic diets sig- nificantly lower children’s dietary exposure to organophosphorus pesticides. Environ. Health Perspect. 114, 260–263. Marks, A.R., Harley, K., Bradman, A., Kogut, K., Barr, D.B., Johnson, C., et al., 2010. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and attention in young Mexican- American children: the CHAMACOS study. Environ. Health Perspect. 118, 1768–1774. Maroni, M., Colosio, C., Ferioli, A., Fait, A., 2000. Biological monitoring of pesticide exposure: a review. Introduction. Toxicology 143, 1–118. Martínez-Frías, M.L., 2010. Can our understanding of epigenetics assist with primary prevention of congenital defects. J. Med. Genet. 47, 73–80. Martino, S., Girolamo, I.di, Orlacchio, A., Datti, A., Orlacchio, A., 2009. MicroRNA implications across neurodevelopment and neuropathology. J. Biomed. Bio- technol. 2009, 654346. Maurissen, J.P., Hoberman a, M., Garman, R.H., Hanley, T.R., 2000. Lack of selec- tive developmental neurotoxicity in rat pups from dams treated by gavage with chlorpyrifos. Toxicol. Sci. 57, 250–263.