SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 29
Download to read offline
Effect of D1 receptor agonist within the mPFC during an appetitive trace
conditioning task; the role of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortices
The effect of a D1 receptor agonist,
SKF81297, on learning of temporally
separated stimuli was tested in an
appetitive trace conditioning task.
Wherein, a time interval existed, either two
or 10-seconds, between the conditioned
stimulus (CS) (noise) before the delivery
of the unconditioned stimulus (UCS)
(food). SKF81297 was administered via
intra-fusion into two medial prefrontal
cortex sub-regions, the infralimbic and
prelimbic cortices. The results showed
SKF81297 (10 ul) impaired learning of the
trace conditioning task under the two-
second interval. Whereas responding was
at baseline for both the drug conditions
and a saline condition under the 10-second
interval. The findings did not demonstrate
any functional differentiation between the
infralimbic and prelimbic cortices. In
conjunction with the literature, it is
suggested that the D1 receptor had an
inversed U-function. The baseline
responding under all conditions under the
10-second interval also suggests the D1
receptor is possibly not involved under
certain time intervals.
Keywords
appetitive conditioning, dopamine receptor
1, medial prefrontal cortex, infralimbic,
prelimbic, SKF81297, trace conditioning
1. Introduction
Working memory is documented to decline with increasing age (Moore, Killiany, Herndon,
Rosene, & Moss, 2003; Nieoullon, 2002), reflecting neuronal loss, particularly in subcortical
and prefrontal dopaminergic neurons (Collins, Wilkinson, Everitt, Robbins, & Roberts, 2000;
Harada, Nishiyama, Satoh, Fukumoto, Kakiuchi, & Tsukada, 2002; Henby & Trojanowski,
2003). The prefrontal cortex is involved in learning the temporal context of events and
stimuli (Fuster, Bodner, & Kroger, 2000). Indeed, damage to this area prevents the learning
of stimuli which are temporally separated (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1997). The pivotal role
of dopaminergic neurons in the age driven pathology is demonstrated by the efficacy of
dopamine receptor 1 (D1) agonists in restoring working memory (Arnsten, Cai, Murphy, &
Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Cai & Arnsten, 1997). Whilst the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) to
be implicated in working memory, the sub-regions of responsible for associating temporally
distant events and stimuli have yet to be elucidated.
The mPFC may be anatomically differentiated by the prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IF)
cortices (Balleine & O‟Doherty, 2010) although how these sub-regions are functionally
distinctive has yet to be fully determined. To explore this role of the PL and IL a trace
conditioning procedure may be utilised. This procedure requires a subject to learn the
association between a conditioned stimulus (CS) which is temporally separate, by a specified
amount of time, from the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) (Pavlov, 1927). Previous studies
have identified the role of mPFC in trace conditioning procedures (Kronforst-Collins &
Disterhoft, 1998; McLaughlin, Skaggs, Churchwell, & Powell, 2002). Of particular note is
the Vidal-Gonzalez, Vidal-Gonzalez, Rauch, & Quirk (2006) study which adopted a trace
conditioning procedure to demonstrate the involvement of the PL in learning fear between
temporally distant events.
Previous research illustrates the efficacy of trace conditioning in examining the effects of
aging on memory (Lopez-Ramos et al., 2012; Moyer & Brown, 2006), validating this
procedure as a model of memory. Applying this model in the examination of mPFC
dopaminergic mechanisms may identify the role of the PL and IL in temporally associative
memory. Evidence exists for dopaminergic modulation of trace conditioning, wherein Nelson
et al. (2011b) investigated the functional role of the nucleus accumbens core. Moreover, the
activity within the PL and IL display changes in accordance with time intervals between
stimuli (Gilmartin & McEchron, 2005). In considering trace condition procedures and mPFC
activity it is of importance to ensure a distinction between fear and appetitive trace
conditioning, as these two variants invoke distinctive functional activity.
Whilst dopaminergic mechanisms are implicated in both aversive (Feenstra, Vogel,
Botterblom, Joosten, & de Bruin, 2001) and appetitive (Dalley, Chudasama, Theobald,
Pettifer, Fletcher, & Robbins, 2002) conditioning, the PL and IL may be differentiated in
their functional role during these variants of trace conditioning (Balleine & O‟Doherty, 2010)
In regard to appetitive conditioning, studies (Killcross & Coutureau, 2003; Ostlund &
Balleine, 2005) have demonstrated distinctive functional roles of the PL and IL via lesioning
which caused sub-region specific differentiated appetitive behavioural responses. Of interest,
inactivation of the IL caused a significant delay in reward collection (Burgos-Robles et al.,
2013; Murphy, Fernando, Urcelay, Robinson, Mar, Theobald, Dalley, & Robbins, 2012),
suggesting IL mechanisms are implicated in appetitive behavioural responses (Coutureau E,
Killcross, 2003; Killcross & Coutureau, 2003). On the other hand Burgos-Robles, Bravo-
Rivera, & Quirk (2013) displayed PL inactivation to produce no noticeable effect upon
appetitive behaviour. However, this study incorporated an instrumental design, trace
conditioning procedures being a classical conditioning design. Although the surrounding
literature implicates the mPFC sub-regions in appetitive behaviour the functional role of PL
and IL in trace conditioning learning has yet to be deduced. Furthermore, Waelti, Dickinson,
& Schultz (2001) reported that activation of mPFC associated dopaminergic mechanisms are
not exclusive to presentation of the UCS, but also in the prediction of the UCS. The length of
delay between the CS and UCS may then exert an effect upon learning. As such, the
following study will incorporate two different trace intervals in order to elucidate how
interval length affects D1 activation. The aims of the following study is as follows; Identify
the role of D1 receptors in trace learning, and if there is a functional differentiation between
the prelimbic and infralimbic cortices.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Subjects
On arrival in the laboratory, rats were caged in pairs on a 12:12 h light/dark cycle and given
free access to food and water. They were handled daily for 2 weeks. The rats‟ weights on
arrival were in the range 150–175 g and they were on free food until they reached 300 g in
body weight. The amount of food provided was subsequently adjusted in order to maintain
weights as close to 300 g as possible so that the rats were all operated at about the same size.
Rats were weighed daily during the first two post-operative weeks, and weekly thereafter. 52
naıve male Wistar rats (Charles Rivers, UK), of mean weight 300 g (range 280–350 g)
underwent surgery.
Of the 52 rats, 11 were lost due to varying causes ranging from meningitis to the implanted
cannula becoming loose during the course of the experiment. The remaining rats were
randomly allocated. Twenty-one were placed in the two-second trace interval group and
twenty in the 10-second trace interval group. Within the Saline and PL conditions there were
a total of 14, with 13 in the IL condition on.
2.2. Surgery
The surgical co-ordinates for implanting the cannula into the mPFC sub-regions are as
follows;
IF = Anterior-Posterior + 3, Lateral-Medial; Left + 0.6 Right - 0.6, Ventral -5.0.
PL = Anterior-Posterior + 3, Lateral-Medial; Left + 0.6 Right - 0.6, Ventral -4.0.
For further details on the surgical procedure see Cassaday, Horsley, & Norman (2005).
2.3. Drugs
There were two treatment conditions to test the effects of the D1 agonist SKF81297 (Sigma,
Poole, UK). Microinfusions were administered (10ul) 10 min before each trace conditioning
session. This variant of SKF was employed as it is found to be highly selective for the D1
subtype of dopamine receptors (Andersen & Jansen, 1990). SKF was infused before the task
as storage and consolidation of delayed working memory is mediated by dorsal hippocampal
DA-dependent mechanisms (Packard, 1999), whereas the PFC is involved in retrieval of
memory (Floresco, Braaksma, & Phillips, 1999).
2.4. Apparatus
For details on apparatus implemented within the study see Kantini, Norman, & Cassaday
(2004).
2.5. Behavioural procedure
2.5.1 Pre-experimental
On the first day, each rat was placed in its allocated conditioning box with access to food
pellets in the magazine and shaped to nose poke. On the second and third day, rats were
placed singly in the boxes and allowed to nose poke for 15 unsignalled rewards, delivered on
a variable interval schedule over a 15-min session over four days.
2.5.2 Conditioning
Rats were conditioned with 30 signalled rewards (UCS) presented on a variable interval over
a 60-min session. Conditioning took place over eight days. The target CS was in each case a
five second auditory stimulus (pure tone set at two kHz and at 70 dB including background).
During this time, a continuous flashing light stimulus was presented in the background. The
two-second trace group was exposed to a delay of two seconds between CS offset and UCS
delivery, whereas the ten-second group experienced a ten second delay.
Assessing effects on conditioning could be confounded by the role of the dopamine system in
motivational (reward-related) or motor (nose poking) responses. However, drug effects on
levels of responding to the CS were compared with responding during the intervals
immediately before CS presentation and the equivalent time period after UCS delivery in
each case. Specifically, we recorded the number of nose pokes in the following five response
bins: (i) Background which is any response outside of the other four response windows; (ii)
„Pre-stimulus in the 5 s before CS presentation; (iii) „Stimulus in the 5 s of CS presentation;
(iv) „Trace‟ during the two or 10 second inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) between CS and UCS;
and (v) „Post-Stimulus in the 5 s immediately following CS delivery. To further examine the
pattern of anticipatory responding within the trace interval, responses were collected in 2-s
bins. One bin for the two-second trace interval group and five for the 10-second trace interval
group.
2.6. Design and analysis
The experiment was run in 3×2 factorial design for later analysis of variance (ANOVA).
Between subjects factors were drug (SKF or saline) and trace (at levels two and 10 seconds).
ANOVA used an alpha level of 0.05. The following statistical analyse were conducted.
Multivariate analysis to compare responding between response windows. Response windows
were the dependent variables with „Trace’ and „Condition’ as the fixed factors. Three
repeated measures were conducted in order to examine responding during across the 30 trials,
the ISI of the 10-trace interval group, and the two response windows „Stimulus’ and „Trace’
across the four days of experimentation. A univariate analysis was also performed,
comparing the first ISI of the trace intervals groups to identify if the length of the interval
affects responding.
3. Results
The results below detail report responding in the trace conditioning task, comparing two and
10-second trace interval groups which each contain three conditions. The conditions being
two drug groups; PL and IL cortices which receive the D1 agonist SKF, and a Saline group.
The results are sub-segregated by the day (total of four days) of experimentation in order to
display increments in responding across days.
3.1. Responding during response windows of trace conditioning task
During a session each rat may respond within five response windows; background noise, pre-
stimulus, during stimulus, within the trace interval, and post the CS. As follows are the
results displaying mean differences for between-condition responding within these five
response windows.
3.1.1. Day 1
No significant effects were found between the conditions and response windows of
responding on Day One.
Table 1 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows
Background
(mean ± SEM)
Pre-stimulus
(mean ±
SEM)
Stimulus
(mean ± SEM)
Trace interval
(mean ±
SEM)
Post
(mean ± SEM)
Day
1
IL 123.798 ±
18.704
4.500 ±
1.394
17.369 ±
3.591
5.917 ±
1.662
31.476 ±
2.490
PL 141.792 ±
18.157
6.271 ±
1.354
18.583 ±
3.486
9.292 ±
1.613
29.313 ±
2.417
Saline 140.357 ±
17.971
7.429 ±
1.340
20.214 ±
3.450
1.597±
1.597
31.857 ±
2.392
3.1.2. Day 2
On Day Two of responding only nose poking within the „Pre-stimulus’ response window
displayed significance between conditions, [F(2,33) = 3.606, p < 0.05], with the Saline
condition responding at the highest rate followed by PL (Table 2).
3.1.3. Day 3
Similarly there was no significance observed on Day Three of responding between
conditions, Table 3 illustrates mean differences between the conditions.
Table 3 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows
Background
(mean ± SEM)
Pre-stimulus
(mean ±
SEM)
Stimulus
(mean ± SEM)
Trace interval
(mean ± SEM)
Post
(mean ± SEM)
Day
3
IL 97.750 ±
22.040
4.083 ±
0.959
29.417 ±
3.386
12.667 ±
2.273
36.083 ±
2.410
PL 118.050 ±
21.763
4.950 ±
0.947
29.125 ±
3.343
14.975 ±
2.273
36.250 ±
2.380
Saline 151.310 ±
21.238
5.917 ±
0.924
36.750 ±
3.263
18.357±
2.218
35.821 ±
2.323
Table 2 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows
Background
(mean ± SEM)
Pre-stimulus
(mean ±
SEM)
Stimulus
(mean ± SEM)
Trace interval
(mean ± SEM)
Post
(mean ± SEM)
Day
2
IL 117.833 ±
24.868
3.417 ±
1.096
27.333 ±
3.902
10.667 ±
2.026
33.833 ±
2.164
PL 134.950 ±
24.555
5.600 ±
1.082
26.050 ±
3.853
9.225 ±
2.001
35.700 ±
2.137
Saline 160.214 ±
23.023
7.429 ±
1.015
28.714 ±
3.613
11.643 ±
1.876
37.286 ±
2.004
3.1.4. Day 4
Responding within the „Pre-stimulus’ response window was identified as significant between
conditions, [F(2,32) = 3.383, p < 0.05]. Similar to Day 2 the Saline condition responded at
the highest rate followed by PL (Table 4).
Table 4 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows
Background
(mean ± SEM)
Pre-stimulus
(mean ±
SEM)
Stimulus
(mean ± SEM)
Trace interval
(mean ± SEM)
Post
(mean ± SEM)
Day
4
IL 87.083 ±
17.489
2.917 ±
0.869
28.250 ±
3.960
13.833 ±
2.723
34.417 ±
2.828
PL 103.150 ±
17.269
5.325 ±
0.858
34.213 ±
3.910
17.450 ±
2.689
37.588 ±
2.792
Saline 117.238 ±
16.852
5.893 ±
0.837
41.524 ±
3.816
18.167±
2.624
34.214 ±
2.725
3.2. Rate of responding over 30 trials
Examining responding in greater detail is possible via analysis the 30 trials (segregated into
six response windows of five trials) which composed each session of the trace conditioning
task.
3.2.1. Day 1
Figure 1 illustrates the rate of responding over six response windows for both two and 10
(respectively) trace intervals. A significant difference, [F(1,35) = 4.425, p < 0.05], in
responding between rats within the two-second trace interval group to those in the 10-second
trace interval group is present. Between conditions within the two-second trace interval group
there is no significant difference of responding, [F(2,18) = 0.162, p = 0.851], nor for the 10-
second trace interval group, [F(2,17) = 0.122, p = 0.886].
Figure 1 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively)
trace interval groups
3.2.2. Day 2
The data shown in Figure 2 suggests a difference of responding overall between the two and
10-second groups. Indeed, a significant difference, [F(1,33) = 26.655, p < 0.001], is reported.
Whereas responding between conditions did not illustrate a significant difference within the
two-second trace interval group, [F(2,18) = 0.609, p = 0.555], nor the 10-second trace
interval group, [F(2,14) = 0.748, p = 0.491].
Figure 2 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively)
trace interval groups
3.2.3. Day 3
Rats within the two-second trace interval group display significantly higher levels of
responding, [F(1,32) = 43.289, p < 0.001]. Responding between the conditions demonstrated
a significant difference for the two-second trace interval group, [F(2,18) = 6.431, p < 0.05].
Figure 3 displays the higher rate of responding of the Saline condition within the two second
trace interval group. The 10-second trace interval group conditions were not significant
different, [F(2,14) = 0.748, p = 0.491].
Figure 3 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively)
trace interval groups
3.2.4. Day 4
A significant difference in responding was identified between the trace interval groups.
[F(1,32) = 67.300, p < 0.001], with Saline responses being highest (Figure 4) A significant
difference was reported between the conditions within the two-second trace interval group,
[F(2,18) = 3.680, p < 0.05], however, no difference was reported for the 10-second trace
interval group [F(2,14) = 1.278, p = 0.309].
Figure 4 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively)
trace interval groups
3.3. Responding over 10-second trace interval
For the 10-second trace interval between the stimulus and presentation of the US five bins
were collected, each being composed of two seconds from the trace interval. Each two second
bin holds data on the number of nose poke responses within the trace response window.
Responding within the trace response window was not found to be significant across any of
the four days (see section 3.1.) However, analysing the responding within each bin of the
trace response window illustrates how responding was spread throughout the interval, and if
the condition affected this distribution of responding.
3.3.1. Day 1
Although no significance was observed between conditions of rats within the 10-second trace
interval responding within the trace response window [F(2,17) = 1.830, p = 0.191]. The fifth
bin displays twice the amount of responding in the PL condition (see Table 5).
Table 5 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group
Bin 1 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 2 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 3 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 4 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 5 (mean ±
SEM)
Day
1
IL 0.048 ± 0.020 0.043 ± 0.013 0.028 ± 0.026 0.024 ± 0.022 0.057 ± 0.027
PL 0.072 ± 0.021 0.056 ± 0.014 0.083 ± 0.028 0.095 ± 0.023 0.106 ± 0.030
Saline 0.047 ± 0.020 0.019 ± 0.013 0.048 ± 0.026 0.048 ± 0.022 0.052 ± 0.027
3.3.2. Day 2
No significance was reported between conditions during the bins [F(2,15) = 0.129, p =
0.880], mean differences are shown in Table 6.
Table 6 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group
Bin 1 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 2 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 3 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 4 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 5 (mean ±
SEM)
Day
2
IL 0.053 ± 0.015 0.047 ± 0.023 0.039 ± 0.023 0.045 ± 0.024 0.059 ± 0.020
PL 0.073 ± 0.016 0.027 ± 0.025 0.027 ± 0.025 0.067 ± 0.026 0.047 ± 0.022
Saline 0.038 ± 0.014 0.067 ± 0.021 0.076 ± 0.021 0.048 ± 0.022 0.062 ± 0.018
3.3.3. Day 3
Responding was not found to significantly differ across conditions, [F(2,14) = 0.529, p =
0.601], however, responding within the fifth bin is noticeably higher in the PL condition (see
Table 7).
Table 7 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group
Bin 1 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 2 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 3 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 4 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 5 (mean ±
SEM)
Day
3
IL 0.072 ± 0.042 0.039 ± 0.017 0.034 ± 0.025 0.055 ± 0.036 0.028 ± 0.029
PL 0.133 ± 0.046 0.040 ± 0.019 0.040 ± 0.028 0.080 ± 0.039 0.113 ± 0.032
Saline 0.095 ± 0.042 0.033 ± 0.017 0.089 ± 0.025 0.061 ± 0.036 0.056 ± 0.029
3.3.4. Day 4
A significant difference between the conditions was not observed, [F(2,14) = 0.272, p =
0.766], mean differences are displayed in Table 8.
Table 8 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group
Bin 1 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 2 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 3 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 4 (mean ±
SEM)
Bin 5 (mean ±
SEM)
Day
4
IL 0.056 ± 0.038 0.039 ± 0.028 0.039 ± 0.037 0.022 ± 0.018 0.022 ± 0.031
PL 0.067 ± 0.042 0.060 ± 0.031 0.080 ± 0.041 0.060 ± 0.020 0.047 ± 0.034
Saline 0.067 ± 0.038 0.045 ± 0.028 0.050 ± 0.037 0.039 ± 0.018 0.078 ± 0.031
3.4. Responding within first bin between two and 10-second trace interval groups
Whereas the 10-second trace interval contains five two-second bins, the two-second trace
interval constitutes only a single bin. Comparison of this single bin with the first of the 10-
second trace interval group may identify if rate of responding is affected by trace
conditioning of different intervals.
3.4.1. Day One
On Day One rats within the two-second trace interval group responded (mean = 0.195 ±
0.028) at a significant higher rate, [F(1,39) = 11.014, p = 0.001], than those within the 10-
second trace interval group (mean = 0.055 ± 0.028) during the first bin.
3.4.2. Day Two
Responding within the two-second trace interval group (mean = 0.436 ± 0.041) was observed
to be significant higher rate, [F(1,37) = 41.177, p < 0.001], than the 10-second trace interval
group (mean = 0.053 ± 0.044).
3.4.3. Day Three
A significance difference was illustrated, rats within the two-second trace interval group
responding (mean = 0.698 ± 0.046) at a higher rate, [F(1,36) = 75.197, p < 0.001], the 10-
second trace interval group (mean = 0.098 ± 0.051).
3.4.4. Day Four
Data from Day Four displayed similar findings as the two-second trace interval group
responded (mean = 0.848 ± 0.051) at a significant higher rate, [F(1,36) = 105.577, p < 0.001],
compared to the 10-second trace interval group (mean = 0.063 ± 0.057).
3.5. Rate of responding across Days within response windows
How the rate of responding changed across days between the trace interval groups and
conditions allows examination of how each variable affected incremental learning.
3.5.1. Responding within the ‘Stimulus’ response windows over four days
Evident in Figure 5 the two-second trace interval group responding significantly increased
across days, [F(1,32) = 42.097, p < 0.001], whilst the 10-second trace interval group
remained at base levels of responding, [F(2,37) = 1.378, p = 0.267].
Figure 5 displaying nose pokes over four days in the ‘Stimulus’ response windows for the two and 10-second
(respectively) trace interval groups
3.5.2. Responding within the ‘Trace’ response windows over four days
As illustrated in Figure 6 the two-second trace interval group responding significantly
increased inclemently across days, [F(1,32) = 16.126, p < 0.001], whereas the 10-second
trace interval group remained at base levels of responding, [F(2,37) = 0.592, p = 0.559].
Figure 6 displaying nose pokes over four days in the ‘Trace response windows for the two and 10-second
(respectively) trace interval groups
4. Discussion
A role of D1 activity within the mPFC for working memory is supported by the literature
(Cai & Arnsten, 1997; Henby & Trojanowski, 2003). Moreover the degeneration of D1
receptors is linked to deficits in working memory, including the temporal association of
events (McLaughlin et al., 2002) which may be ameliorated with D1 agonists such as SKF
(for a review see Mizoguchia, Yuzurihara, Nagata, Ishige, Sasaki, & Tabira, 2002). Such a
role is not suggested from the findings of the present study, they do not convey that SKF
augmented learning but instead inhibited learning. Two trace intervals were utilised in this
study, a two and 10-second interval group. The saline condition within the former group
displayed a similar pattern of responding as the IL and PL conditions (whom received SKF).
However, over the course of the study the saline condition noticeably superseded the
responding of these two drug conditions. From these findings it is clear SKF inhibited
learning the US-CS contingency under the time interval of two seconds. These findings on
the inhibiting effects of SKF may appear to contradict the literature on the ameliorative
effects of mPFC-D1 agonism in aging, and other forms of pathological neurodegeneration
and dysfunction (Deutch, 1993; Dolan et al., 1994; Fibiger, 1995).
However, D1 activity have been reported be constitute an inverse U-function, wherein hypo
or hyper-activation of these receptors causing a dysfunction, thereby impairing associated D1
mechanisms (Arnsten, 1998; Zahrt, Taylor, Mathew, & Arnsten, 1997). In accord, mPFC
dysfunction, such as in the case of Parkinson‟s disease, is treated via D1 agonism (Zahrt et
al., 1997). Futhermore, chronic stress causes hyper-activation of D1 which impairs working
memory, with administration of D1 antagonists reversing the effect (Zahrt et al., 1997).
Taken together, mPFC-D1 activity may be responsible for the modulation of working
memory. The present study also focused on the functional differentiation of the IL and PL
cortices of the mPFC. These two sub-regions appear to be functionally distinct from the
literature (Balleine & O‟Doherty, 2010). However, due to the theorised hyper-activation
produced by SKF any functional distinction is indistinguishable. A theory supported by the
lack of any significant difference between the two drug conditions in both trace interval
groups. It is of importance to emphasize that SKF has shown to have a beneficial effect on
working memory, administered via either systemically or intra-mPFC infusion (Granon,
Passetti, Thomas, Dalley, Everitt, & Robbins, 2000), in subjects which have an underlying
degenerative pathology, such as in aging (Cai & Arnsten, 1997) or Parkinson‟s disease
(Lange, Robbins, Marsden, James, Owen, & Paul, 1992).
Interestingly the saline and both drug conditions did not display a difference of responding
under the 10-second trace interval group. Across 30 trials within each day and across the four
days the rate of responding remained at base levels entirely for all three conditions. It is then
reasonable to conclude that D1 activity, at normal or hyper-activated levels in healthy
subjects, bears no effect upon trace learning over certain time intervals. Examining the five
two-second bins of the 10-second trace interval supports this conclusion. If trace learning of a
10 second interval between the US and CS were successful, responding within the five bins
would be expected to polarise at the last two bins. The literature on the learning of fixed time
intervals, known as a fixed interval schedule, reports a „scallop‟ pattern of responding
(Pavlov, 1928; Skinner, 1938). The temporal expectation of a US delivery from the CS onset
is driven by dopaminergic mechanisms (Schultz, 1998). The pattern of responding is a result
of successfully learning that a CS/reward will only be delivered after a fixed amount of time
and as such responding will is only reinforced near this point of time. However, within the
present study responding was evenly distributed throughout the five bins. Particularly
illustrative of this is the PL group which on Day 3 responding most within the first and fifth
bin.
Noteworthy is how responding was measured as the behaviour of nose poking, detected by a
rat pushing open a flap to access the food dispenser. It is possible that a rat may remain
stationed at the food dispenser, keeping the flap open indefinitely and as such preventing
measurement of behaviour. The number of nose poking responses signifies learning of the
US-CS contingency. Although if a rat has learnt this contingency but remains at the food
dispenser resulting in less nose pokes, the data will suggest a lower rate of responding. As
such it is possible that rats within a particular condition exhibited a higher rate of responding
but, due to the limitation of the apparatus, this difference is not represented. Another possible
confounding element of the study is the duration of SKF effects. Surrounding research which
implemented SKF has documented the D1 agonistic effect to last up to 30 minutes
(Mizoguchi, Yuzurihara, Ishige, Sasaki, Chui, & Tabira, 2000; Sorg et al., 2001; Zahrt et al.,
1997). Within this study rats were within the task for up to 65 minutes, it is unknown if the
effects of SKF lasted the entire duration of the task.
In conclusion, the functional role of PL and IL in learning and memory of a temporally
distant US-CS were not distinguished. However, as both the PL and IL conditions appeared
to be inhibited by SKF. It may then be inferred that the two sub-regions are involved to in the
temporal learning aspect of working memory, albeit to an extent as all three conditions
responded at baseline levels under the 10-second interval group. The lack of learning within
the 10-second trace interval group may signify that the length of this trace interval is too
great. In a similar study, Floresco & Phillips (2001) focused on significantly longer time
intervals, they reported SKF to augment learning between learning a task and repeating the
task after a 12 hour delay. Moreover, the Floresco & Phillips (2001) study found that at
delays of 30 minutes SKF inhibited repetition of the task. In converging the discussed
findings and surrounding literature it would appear the mechanisms of D1 activity are
sensitive to either hypo or hyper-activation and also the length of the time interval.
5. References
Andersen, P. H., & Jansen, J. A. (1990). Dopamine receptor agonists: selectivity and D1
receptor efficacy. European Journal of Pharmacology, 188, 335–347.
Arnsten, A. F. T. (1998). Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function.
Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 436-447.
Arnsten, A. F. T., Cai, J. X., Murphy, B. L., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Dopamine D1
receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys.
Psychopharmacology, 116, 143-151.
Balleine, B. W., & O‟Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control:
corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology
35, 48–69.
Burgos-Robles, A., Bravo-Rivera, H., & Quirk, G. J. (2013). Prelimbic and Infralimbic
Neurons Signal Distinct Aspects of Appetitive Instrumental Behavior. PLOS ONE, 8,
Cai, J. X., & Arnsten, A. F. T. (1997). Dose-dependent effects of the dopamine D1 receptor
agonists A77636 or SKF81297 on spatial working memory in aged monkeys. Journal of
Pharmacology and Experimental Therapy, 283, 183–189.
Cassaday, H. J., Horsley, R. R., & Norman, C. (2005). Electrolytic lesions to nucleus
accumbens core and shell have dissociable effects on conditioning to discrete and contextual
cues in aversive and appetitive procedures respectively. Behavioural Brain Research, 160,
222–235.
Collins, P., Wilkinson, L. S., Everitt, B. J., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (2000). The
effect of dopamine depletion from the caudate nucleus of the common marmoset (Callithrix
jacchus) on tests of prefrontal cognitive function. Behavioural Neuroscience, 114, 3-17.
Coutureau, E., & Killcross, S. (2003) Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex
reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behaviour and Brain Research, 146,
167–174.
Dalley, J. W., Chudasama, Y., Theobald, D. E., Pettifer, C. L., Fletcher, C. M., & Robbins, T.
W. (2002) Nucleus accumbens dopamine and discriminated approach learning: interactive
effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions and systemic apomorphine administration.
Psychopharmacology 161, 425–433.
Deutch, A. Y. (1993). Prefrontal cortical dopamine systems and the elaboration of functional
corticostriatal circuits: implications for schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease. Journal of
Neural Transmission, 91, 197–221.
Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997) Dissociable forms of inhibitory control
within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin card sort test: restriction to novel
situations and independence from “on-line” processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 9285-
9297.
Dolan, R. J., Bench, C. J., Brown, R. G., Scott, L. C., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1994).
Neuropsychological dysfunction in depression: the relationship to regional cerebral blood
flow. Psychological Medicine, 24, 849-857.
Feenstra, M. G. P., Vogel, M., Botterblom, M. H. A., Joosten, R. N. J. M. A., & de Bruin, J.
P. C. (2001). Dopamine and noradrenaline efflux in the rat prefrontal cortex after classical
aversive conditioning to an auditory cue. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1051–1054.
Floresco, S. B., Braaksma, D. N., & Phillips, A. G. (1999). Thalamic-cortical-striatal circuitry
subserves working memory during delayed responding on a radial arm maze. Journal of
Neuroscience, 19, 11061-11071.
Floresco, S. B., & Phillips, A. G. (2001). Delay-dependent modulation of memory retrieval
by infusion of a dopamine D1 agonist into the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 115, 934–993.
Fibiger, H. C. (1995). Neurobiology of depression: focus on dopamine. Advances in
Biochemical Psychopharmacology, 49, 1–17.
Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M., & Kroger, J. (2000). Cross-modal and cross-temporal association
in neurons of frontal cortex. Nature, 405, 347-351.
Granon, S., Passetti, F., Thomas, K. L., Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2000).
Enhanced and impaired attentional performance after infusion of D1 dopaminergic receptor
agents into rat prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1208-1215.
Harada, N., Nishiyama, S., Satoh, K., Fukumoto, D., Kakiuchi, T., & Tsukada, H. (2002).
Age-related changes in the striatal dopamine system in the living brain: a multiparametric
study in conscious monkeys. Synapse, 45, 38–45.
Kantini, E., Norman, C., & Cassaday, H. J. (2004). Amphetamine decreases the expression
and acquisition of appetitive conditioning but increases the acquisition of anticipatory
responding over a trace interval. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 18, 516–526.
Killcross, S., & Coutureau, E. (2003). Coordination of actions and habits in the medial
prefrontal cortex of rats. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 400–408.
Kronforst-Collins, M A., & Disterhoft, J. F. (1998). Lesions of the caudal area of rabbit
medial prefrontal cortex impair trace eyeblink conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 69, 147-162.
Lange, K. W., Robbins, T. W., Marsden, C. D., James, M., Owen, A. M., & Paul, G.
M. (1992) L-dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive
performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychopharmacology, 107, 394–
404.
McLaughlin, J., Skaggs, H., Churchwell, J., & Powell, D. A. (2002). Medial prefrontal cortex
and Pavlovian conditioning: trace versus delay conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience,
116, 37-47.
Mizoguchia, K., Yuzurihara, M., Ishige, A., Sasaki, H., Chui, D. H., & Tabira, T. (2000).
Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1568-1578.
Mizoguchia, K., Yuzurihara, M., Nagata, M., Ishige, A., Sasaki, H., & Tabira, T. (2002).
Dopamine-receptor stimulation in the prefrontal cortex ameliorates stress-induced rotarod
impairment. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 72, 723-728.
Moore, T. L., Killiany, R. J., Herndon, J. G., Rosene, D. L., & Moss, M. B. (2003).
Impairment in abstraction and set shifting in aged hesus monkeys. Neurobiology of Aging, 24,
125–134.
Murphy, E. R., Fernando, A. B., Urcelay, G. P., Robinson, E. S., Mar, A. C., Theobald, D. E.
H., Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2012). Impulsive behaviour induced by both NMDA
receptor antagonism and GABAA receptor activation in rat ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
Psychopharmacology, 219, 401–410.
Nieoullon, A. (2002). Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Progressive
Neurobiology, 67, 53–83.
Ostlund, S. B., & Balleine, B. W. (2005). Lesions of medial prefrontal cortex disrupt the
acquisition but not the expression of goal-directed learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 25,
7763–7770.
Packard, M. G. (1999). Dissociation of multiple memory systems by posttraining
intracerebral injections of glutamate. Psychobiology, 127, 40-50.
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the
cerebral cortex. London: Oxford UP.
Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes: The higher nervous activity of animals.
London: Lawrence & Wishart.
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 80, 1–27.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Sorg, B. A., Li, N., & Wu, W. R. (2001). Dopamine D1 receptor activation in the medial
prefrontal cortex prevents the expression of cocaine sensitization. Journal of Pharmacology
and Experimental Therapy, 297, 501-508.
Vidal-Gonzalez, I., Vidal-Gonzalez, B., Rauch, S. L., & Quirk, G. J. (2006) Microstimulation
reveals opposing influences of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex on the expression of
conditioned fear. Learning & Memory, 13, 728–733.
Waelti, P., Dickinson, A., & Schultz, W. (2001). Dopamine responses comply with basic
assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature, 412, 43–48.
Zahrt, J., Taylor, J. R., Mathew, R. G., & Arnsten, A. F. T. (1997). Supranormal stimulation
of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs working memory
performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 8528-8535.

More Related Content

What's hot

20160524123048653
2016052412304865320160524123048653
20160524123048653Yang Cai
 
SfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_Progranulin
SfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_ProgranulinSfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_Progranulin
SfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_ProgranulinPierre Zwiegers
 
An inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortex
An inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortexAn inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortex
An inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortexTaruna Ikrar
 
fnbeh-06-00059
fnbeh-06-00059fnbeh-06-00059
fnbeh-06-00059Emily Lenk
 
Spatiotemporal profile of Map2
Spatiotemporal profile of Map2Spatiotemporal profile of Map2
Spatiotemporal profile of Map2Sarah Butts
 
New microsoft office word document
New microsoft office word documentNew microsoft office word document
New microsoft office word documentharsha979797
 

What's hot (8)

20160524123048653
2016052412304865320160524123048653
20160524123048653
 
SfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_Progranulin
SfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_ProgranulinSfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_Progranulin
SfN 2008_Tabata_CruzAguado_Progranulin
 
An inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortex
An inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortexAn inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortex
An inhibitory pull–push circuit in frontal cortex
 
fnbeh-06-00059
fnbeh-06-00059fnbeh-06-00059
fnbeh-06-00059
 
Smith_PNAS_2016
Smith_PNAS_2016Smith_PNAS_2016
Smith_PNAS_2016
 
nihms462541
nihms462541nihms462541
nihms462541
 
Spatiotemporal profile of Map2
Spatiotemporal profile of Map2Spatiotemporal profile of Map2
Spatiotemporal profile of Map2
 
New microsoft office word document
New microsoft office word documentNew microsoft office word document
New microsoft office word document
 

Viewers also liked

Soria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogo
Soria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogoSoria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogo
Soria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogoProfeAragon
 
Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)
Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)
Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)cmiglesias
 
Luis lopez (1)
Luis lopez (1)Luis lopez (1)
Luis lopez (1)Omar Lopez
 
Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...
Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...
Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...DML Srl
 
2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report
2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report
2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National ReportJen Lee
 

Viewers also liked (11)

Metonimias
MetonimiasMetonimias
Metonimias
 
Todd White Resume 010915
Todd White Resume 010915Todd White Resume 010915
Todd White Resume 010915
 
Harper's Globe
Harper's GlobeHarper's Globe
Harper's Globe
 
Soria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogo
Soria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogoSoria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogo
Soria, sayago y fernandez 3°c decalogo
 
Naturaleza
NaturalezaNaturaleza
Naturaleza
 
Trabajo
TrabajoTrabajo
Trabajo
 
iDoc - L'archivio on line: esempio MEDIA-PUBLISHING DOSSIER
iDoc - L'archivio on line: esempio MEDIA-PUBLISHING DOSSIERiDoc - L'archivio on line: esempio MEDIA-PUBLISHING DOSSIER
iDoc - L'archivio on line: esempio MEDIA-PUBLISHING DOSSIER
 
Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)
Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)
Como dibujar una forma fluida (4)
 
Luis lopez (1)
Luis lopez (1)Luis lopez (1)
Luis lopez (1)
 
Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...
Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...
Le sfide per i Marketer di oggi...
 
2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report
2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report
2014.10.14_Improving the Road to ACA Coverage_National Report
 

Similar to Effect of D1 receptor agonist within the mPFC

Cason Written Comps
Cason Written CompsCason Written Comps
Cason Written CompsJacob Cason
 
Senior thesis(2015)_Ganesh
Senior thesis(2015)_GaneshSenior thesis(2015)_Ganesh
Senior thesis(2015)_GaneshArjun Ganesh
 
BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)
BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)
BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)Christine Heisler
 
Pnas 2013 combined pdf
Pnas 2013 combined pdfPnas 2013 combined pdf
Pnas 2013 combined pdfSaraAton
 
NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010
NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010
NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010lrt5000
 
Independent Study Paper
Independent Study PaperIndependent Study Paper
Independent Study PaperJade Dunstan
 
JofNeuroscience2000
JofNeuroscience2000JofNeuroscience2000
JofNeuroscience2000Laura Murphy
 
Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003Jacob Sheu
 
RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1
RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1
RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1Meghann Shorrock
 
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...Taruna Ikrar
 
C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!
C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!
C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!Kian Bagheri
 
Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015
Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015
Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015Allison Nash
 
mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...
mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...
mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...Lauren Klosinski
 
Long-term potentiation.ppt
Long-term potentiation.pptLong-term potentiation.ppt
Long-term potentiation.pptAbdullahAlJalil
 
Albu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_Research
Albu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_ResearchAlbu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_Research
Albu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_ResearchStefana Albu
 
ABRCMSPosterRachelFINAL
ABRCMSPosterRachelFINALABRCMSPosterRachelFINAL
ABRCMSPosterRachelFINALRachel Clein
 

Similar to Effect of D1 receptor agonist within the mPFC (20)

Cason Written Comps
Cason Written CompsCason Written Comps
Cason Written Comps
 
Senior thesis(2015)_Ganesh
Senior thesis(2015)_GaneshSenior thesis(2015)_Ganesh
Senior thesis(2015)_Ganesh
 
SynapticPlasticity
SynapticPlasticitySynapticPlasticity
SynapticPlasticity
 
TBI feature
TBI featureTBI feature
TBI feature
 
BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)
BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)
BioE Poster Template (30w by 21h)
 
Pnas 2013 combined pdf
Pnas 2013 combined pdfPnas 2013 combined pdf
Pnas 2013 combined pdf
 
NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010
NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010
NKCC1 Poster for SIRS 2010
 
Independent Study Paper
Independent Study PaperIndependent Study Paper
Independent Study Paper
 
JofNeuroscience2000
JofNeuroscience2000JofNeuroscience2000
JofNeuroscience2000
 
Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003
 
RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1
RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1
RotationProposal_ShorrockM_D1
 
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
 
C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!
C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!
C. elegans Lab Report FINAL!!!!
 
Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015
Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015
Allison Nash SURF proposal Melibe 2015
 
mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...
mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...
mapping functional brain activation using iodoantipyrine in male serotonin tr...
 
Long-term potentiation.ppt
Long-term potentiation.pptLong-term potentiation.ppt
Long-term potentiation.ppt
 
Albu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_Research
Albu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_ResearchAlbu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_Research
Albu_et_al-2014-Journal_of_Sleep_Research
 
ABRCMSPosterRachelFINAL
ABRCMSPosterRachelFINALABRCMSPosterRachelFINAL
ABRCMSPosterRachelFINAL
 
Developmental neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of pain
Developmental neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of painDevelopmental neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of pain
Developmental neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of pain
 
Science_2005
Science_2005Science_2005
Science_2005
 

Effect of D1 receptor agonist within the mPFC

  • 1. Effect of D1 receptor agonist within the mPFC during an appetitive trace conditioning task; the role of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortices The effect of a D1 receptor agonist, SKF81297, on learning of temporally separated stimuli was tested in an appetitive trace conditioning task. Wherein, a time interval existed, either two or 10-seconds, between the conditioned stimulus (CS) (noise) before the delivery of the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) (food). SKF81297 was administered via intra-fusion into two medial prefrontal cortex sub-regions, the infralimbic and prelimbic cortices. The results showed SKF81297 (10 ul) impaired learning of the trace conditioning task under the two- second interval. Whereas responding was at baseline for both the drug conditions and a saline condition under the 10-second interval. The findings did not demonstrate any functional differentiation between the infralimbic and prelimbic cortices. In conjunction with the literature, it is suggested that the D1 receptor had an inversed U-function. The baseline responding under all conditions under the 10-second interval also suggests the D1 receptor is possibly not involved under certain time intervals. Keywords appetitive conditioning, dopamine receptor 1, medial prefrontal cortex, infralimbic, prelimbic, SKF81297, trace conditioning
  • 2. 1. Introduction Working memory is documented to decline with increasing age (Moore, Killiany, Herndon, Rosene, & Moss, 2003; Nieoullon, 2002), reflecting neuronal loss, particularly in subcortical and prefrontal dopaminergic neurons (Collins, Wilkinson, Everitt, Robbins, & Roberts, 2000; Harada, Nishiyama, Satoh, Fukumoto, Kakiuchi, & Tsukada, 2002; Henby & Trojanowski, 2003). The prefrontal cortex is involved in learning the temporal context of events and stimuli (Fuster, Bodner, & Kroger, 2000). Indeed, damage to this area prevents the learning of stimuli which are temporally separated (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1997). The pivotal role of dopaminergic neurons in the age driven pathology is demonstrated by the efficacy of dopamine receptor 1 (D1) agonists in restoring working memory (Arnsten, Cai, Murphy, & Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Cai & Arnsten, 1997). Whilst the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) to be implicated in working memory, the sub-regions of responsible for associating temporally distant events and stimuli have yet to be elucidated. The mPFC may be anatomically differentiated by the prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IF) cortices (Balleine & O‟Doherty, 2010) although how these sub-regions are functionally distinctive has yet to be fully determined. To explore this role of the PL and IL a trace conditioning procedure may be utilised. This procedure requires a subject to learn the association between a conditioned stimulus (CS) which is temporally separate, by a specified amount of time, from the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) (Pavlov, 1927). Previous studies have identified the role of mPFC in trace conditioning procedures (Kronforst-Collins & Disterhoft, 1998; McLaughlin, Skaggs, Churchwell, & Powell, 2002). Of particular note is the Vidal-Gonzalez, Vidal-Gonzalez, Rauch, & Quirk (2006) study which adopted a trace conditioning procedure to demonstrate the involvement of the PL in learning fear between temporally distant events.
  • 3. Previous research illustrates the efficacy of trace conditioning in examining the effects of aging on memory (Lopez-Ramos et al., 2012; Moyer & Brown, 2006), validating this procedure as a model of memory. Applying this model in the examination of mPFC dopaminergic mechanisms may identify the role of the PL and IL in temporally associative memory. Evidence exists for dopaminergic modulation of trace conditioning, wherein Nelson et al. (2011b) investigated the functional role of the nucleus accumbens core. Moreover, the activity within the PL and IL display changes in accordance with time intervals between stimuli (Gilmartin & McEchron, 2005). In considering trace condition procedures and mPFC activity it is of importance to ensure a distinction between fear and appetitive trace conditioning, as these two variants invoke distinctive functional activity. Whilst dopaminergic mechanisms are implicated in both aversive (Feenstra, Vogel, Botterblom, Joosten, & de Bruin, 2001) and appetitive (Dalley, Chudasama, Theobald, Pettifer, Fletcher, & Robbins, 2002) conditioning, the PL and IL may be differentiated in their functional role during these variants of trace conditioning (Balleine & O‟Doherty, 2010) In regard to appetitive conditioning, studies (Killcross & Coutureau, 2003; Ostlund & Balleine, 2005) have demonstrated distinctive functional roles of the PL and IL via lesioning which caused sub-region specific differentiated appetitive behavioural responses. Of interest, inactivation of the IL caused a significant delay in reward collection (Burgos-Robles et al., 2013; Murphy, Fernando, Urcelay, Robinson, Mar, Theobald, Dalley, & Robbins, 2012), suggesting IL mechanisms are implicated in appetitive behavioural responses (Coutureau E, Killcross, 2003; Killcross & Coutureau, 2003). On the other hand Burgos-Robles, Bravo- Rivera, & Quirk (2013) displayed PL inactivation to produce no noticeable effect upon appetitive behaviour. However, this study incorporated an instrumental design, trace conditioning procedures being a classical conditioning design. Although the surrounding
  • 4. literature implicates the mPFC sub-regions in appetitive behaviour the functional role of PL and IL in trace conditioning learning has yet to be deduced. Furthermore, Waelti, Dickinson, & Schultz (2001) reported that activation of mPFC associated dopaminergic mechanisms are not exclusive to presentation of the UCS, but also in the prediction of the UCS. The length of delay between the CS and UCS may then exert an effect upon learning. As such, the following study will incorporate two different trace intervals in order to elucidate how interval length affects D1 activation. The aims of the following study is as follows; Identify the role of D1 receptors in trace learning, and if there is a functional differentiation between the prelimbic and infralimbic cortices.
  • 5. 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Subjects On arrival in the laboratory, rats were caged in pairs on a 12:12 h light/dark cycle and given free access to food and water. They were handled daily for 2 weeks. The rats‟ weights on arrival were in the range 150–175 g and they were on free food until they reached 300 g in body weight. The amount of food provided was subsequently adjusted in order to maintain weights as close to 300 g as possible so that the rats were all operated at about the same size. Rats were weighed daily during the first two post-operative weeks, and weekly thereafter. 52 naıve male Wistar rats (Charles Rivers, UK), of mean weight 300 g (range 280–350 g) underwent surgery. Of the 52 rats, 11 were lost due to varying causes ranging from meningitis to the implanted cannula becoming loose during the course of the experiment. The remaining rats were randomly allocated. Twenty-one were placed in the two-second trace interval group and twenty in the 10-second trace interval group. Within the Saline and PL conditions there were a total of 14, with 13 in the IL condition on. 2.2. Surgery The surgical co-ordinates for implanting the cannula into the mPFC sub-regions are as follows; IF = Anterior-Posterior + 3, Lateral-Medial; Left + 0.6 Right - 0.6, Ventral -5.0. PL = Anterior-Posterior + 3, Lateral-Medial; Left + 0.6 Right - 0.6, Ventral -4.0. For further details on the surgical procedure see Cassaday, Horsley, & Norman (2005).
  • 6. 2.3. Drugs There were two treatment conditions to test the effects of the D1 agonist SKF81297 (Sigma, Poole, UK). Microinfusions were administered (10ul) 10 min before each trace conditioning session. This variant of SKF was employed as it is found to be highly selective for the D1 subtype of dopamine receptors (Andersen & Jansen, 1990). SKF was infused before the task as storage and consolidation of delayed working memory is mediated by dorsal hippocampal DA-dependent mechanisms (Packard, 1999), whereas the PFC is involved in retrieval of memory (Floresco, Braaksma, & Phillips, 1999). 2.4. Apparatus For details on apparatus implemented within the study see Kantini, Norman, & Cassaday (2004). 2.5. Behavioural procedure 2.5.1 Pre-experimental On the first day, each rat was placed in its allocated conditioning box with access to food pellets in the magazine and shaped to nose poke. On the second and third day, rats were placed singly in the boxes and allowed to nose poke for 15 unsignalled rewards, delivered on a variable interval schedule over a 15-min session over four days. 2.5.2 Conditioning Rats were conditioned with 30 signalled rewards (UCS) presented on a variable interval over a 60-min session. Conditioning took place over eight days. The target CS was in each case a five second auditory stimulus (pure tone set at two kHz and at 70 dB including background).
  • 7. During this time, a continuous flashing light stimulus was presented in the background. The two-second trace group was exposed to a delay of two seconds between CS offset and UCS delivery, whereas the ten-second group experienced a ten second delay. Assessing effects on conditioning could be confounded by the role of the dopamine system in motivational (reward-related) or motor (nose poking) responses. However, drug effects on levels of responding to the CS were compared with responding during the intervals immediately before CS presentation and the equivalent time period after UCS delivery in each case. Specifically, we recorded the number of nose pokes in the following five response bins: (i) Background which is any response outside of the other four response windows; (ii) „Pre-stimulus in the 5 s before CS presentation; (iii) „Stimulus in the 5 s of CS presentation; (iv) „Trace‟ during the two or 10 second inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) between CS and UCS; and (v) „Post-Stimulus in the 5 s immediately following CS delivery. To further examine the pattern of anticipatory responding within the trace interval, responses were collected in 2-s bins. One bin for the two-second trace interval group and five for the 10-second trace interval group. 2.6. Design and analysis The experiment was run in 3×2 factorial design for later analysis of variance (ANOVA). Between subjects factors were drug (SKF or saline) and trace (at levels two and 10 seconds). ANOVA used an alpha level of 0.05. The following statistical analyse were conducted. Multivariate analysis to compare responding between response windows. Response windows were the dependent variables with „Trace’ and „Condition’ as the fixed factors. Three repeated measures were conducted in order to examine responding during across the 30 trials,
  • 8. the ISI of the 10-trace interval group, and the two response windows „Stimulus’ and „Trace’ across the four days of experimentation. A univariate analysis was also performed, comparing the first ISI of the trace intervals groups to identify if the length of the interval affects responding.
  • 9. 3. Results The results below detail report responding in the trace conditioning task, comparing two and 10-second trace interval groups which each contain three conditions. The conditions being two drug groups; PL and IL cortices which receive the D1 agonist SKF, and a Saline group. The results are sub-segregated by the day (total of four days) of experimentation in order to display increments in responding across days. 3.1. Responding during response windows of trace conditioning task During a session each rat may respond within five response windows; background noise, pre- stimulus, during stimulus, within the trace interval, and post the CS. As follows are the results displaying mean differences for between-condition responding within these five response windows. 3.1.1. Day 1 No significant effects were found between the conditions and response windows of responding on Day One. Table 1 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows Background (mean ± SEM) Pre-stimulus (mean ± SEM) Stimulus (mean ± SEM) Trace interval (mean ± SEM) Post (mean ± SEM) Day 1 IL 123.798 ± 18.704 4.500 ± 1.394 17.369 ± 3.591 5.917 ± 1.662 31.476 ± 2.490 PL 141.792 ± 18.157 6.271 ± 1.354 18.583 ± 3.486 9.292 ± 1.613 29.313 ± 2.417 Saline 140.357 ± 17.971 7.429 ± 1.340 20.214 ± 3.450 1.597± 1.597 31.857 ± 2.392
  • 10. 3.1.2. Day 2 On Day Two of responding only nose poking within the „Pre-stimulus’ response window displayed significance between conditions, [F(2,33) = 3.606, p < 0.05], with the Saline condition responding at the highest rate followed by PL (Table 2). 3.1.3. Day 3 Similarly there was no significance observed on Day Three of responding between conditions, Table 3 illustrates mean differences between the conditions. Table 3 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows Background (mean ± SEM) Pre-stimulus (mean ± SEM) Stimulus (mean ± SEM) Trace interval (mean ± SEM) Post (mean ± SEM) Day 3 IL 97.750 ± 22.040 4.083 ± 0.959 29.417 ± 3.386 12.667 ± 2.273 36.083 ± 2.410 PL 118.050 ± 21.763 4.950 ± 0.947 29.125 ± 3.343 14.975 ± 2.273 36.250 ± 2.380 Saline 151.310 ± 21.238 5.917 ± 0.924 36.750 ± 3.263 18.357± 2.218 35.821 ± 2.323 Table 2 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows Background (mean ± SEM) Pre-stimulus (mean ± SEM) Stimulus (mean ± SEM) Trace interval (mean ± SEM) Post (mean ± SEM) Day 2 IL 117.833 ± 24.868 3.417 ± 1.096 27.333 ± 3.902 10.667 ± 2.026 33.833 ± 2.164 PL 134.950 ± 24.555 5.600 ± 1.082 26.050 ± 3.853 9.225 ± 2.001 35.700 ± 2.137 Saline 160.214 ± 23.023 7.429 ± 1.015 28.714 ± 3.613 11.643 ± 1.876 37.286 ± 2.004
  • 11. 3.1.4. Day 4 Responding within the „Pre-stimulus’ response window was identified as significant between conditions, [F(2,32) = 3.383, p < 0.05]. Similar to Day 2 the Saline condition responded at the highest rate followed by PL (Table 4). Table 4 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five response windows Background (mean ± SEM) Pre-stimulus (mean ± SEM) Stimulus (mean ± SEM) Trace interval (mean ± SEM) Post (mean ± SEM) Day 4 IL 87.083 ± 17.489 2.917 ± 0.869 28.250 ± 3.960 13.833 ± 2.723 34.417 ± 2.828 PL 103.150 ± 17.269 5.325 ± 0.858 34.213 ± 3.910 17.450 ± 2.689 37.588 ± 2.792 Saline 117.238 ± 16.852 5.893 ± 0.837 41.524 ± 3.816 18.167± 2.624 34.214 ± 2.725 3.2. Rate of responding over 30 trials Examining responding in greater detail is possible via analysis the 30 trials (segregated into six response windows of five trials) which composed each session of the trace conditioning task. 3.2.1. Day 1 Figure 1 illustrates the rate of responding over six response windows for both two and 10 (respectively) trace intervals. A significant difference, [F(1,35) = 4.425, p < 0.05], in responding between rats within the two-second trace interval group to those in the 10-second trace interval group is present. Between conditions within the two-second trace interval group there is no significant difference of responding, [F(2,18) = 0.162, p = 0.851], nor for the 10- second trace interval group, [F(2,17) = 0.122, p = 0.886].
  • 12. Figure 1 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively) trace interval groups 3.2.2. Day 2 The data shown in Figure 2 suggests a difference of responding overall between the two and 10-second groups. Indeed, a significant difference, [F(1,33) = 26.655, p < 0.001], is reported. Whereas responding between conditions did not illustrate a significant difference within the two-second trace interval group, [F(2,18) = 0.609, p = 0.555], nor the 10-second trace interval group, [F(2,14) = 0.748, p = 0.491]. Figure 2 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively) trace interval groups
  • 13. 3.2.3. Day 3 Rats within the two-second trace interval group display significantly higher levels of responding, [F(1,32) = 43.289, p < 0.001]. Responding between the conditions demonstrated a significant difference for the two-second trace interval group, [F(2,18) = 6.431, p < 0.05]. Figure 3 displays the higher rate of responding of the Saline condition within the two second trace interval group. The 10-second trace interval group conditions were not significant different, [F(2,14) = 0.748, p = 0.491]. Figure 3 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively) trace interval groups 3.2.4. Day 4 A significant difference in responding was identified between the trace interval groups. [F(1,32) = 67.300, p < 0.001], with Saline responses being highest (Figure 4) A significant difference was reported between the conditions within the two-second trace interval group, [F(2,18) = 3.680, p < 0.05], however, no difference was reported for the 10-second trace interval group [F(2,14) = 1.278, p = 0.309].
  • 14. Figure 4 displaying nose pokes over six response windows of five trials for the two and 10-second (respectively) trace interval groups 3.3. Responding over 10-second trace interval For the 10-second trace interval between the stimulus and presentation of the US five bins were collected, each being composed of two seconds from the trace interval. Each two second bin holds data on the number of nose poke responses within the trace response window. Responding within the trace response window was not found to be significant across any of the four days (see section 3.1.) However, analysing the responding within each bin of the trace response window illustrates how responding was spread throughout the interval, and if the condition affected this distribution of responding. 3.3.1. Day 1 Although no significance was observed between conditions of rats within the 10-second trace interval responding within the trace response window [F(2,17) = 1.830, p = 0.191]. The fifth bin displays twice the amount of responding in the PL condition (see Table 5).
  • 15. Table 5 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group Bin 1 (mean ± SEM) Bin 2 (mean ± SEM) Bin 3 (mean ± SEM) Bin 4 (mean ± SEM) Bin 5 (mean ± SEM) Day 1 IL 0.048 ± 0.020 0.043 ± 0.013 0.028 ± 0.026 0.024 ± 0.022 0.057 ± 0.027 PL 0.072 ± 0.021 0.056 ± 0.014 0.083 ± 0.028 0.095 ± 0.023 0.106 ± 0.030 Saline 0.047 ± 0.020 0.019 ± 0.013 0.048 ± 0.026 0.048 ± 0.022 0.052 ± 0.027 3.3.2. Day 2 No significance was reported between conditions during the bins [F(2,15) = 0.129, p = 0.880], mean differences are shown in Table 6. Table 6 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group Bin 1 (mean ± SEM) Bin 2 (mean ± SEM) Bin 3 (mean ± SEM) Bin 4 (mean ± SEM) Bin 5 (mean ± SEM) Day 2 IL 0.053 ± 0.015 0.047 ± 0.023 0.039 ± 0.023 0.045 ± 0.024 0.059 ± 0.020 PL 0.073 ± 0.016 0.027 ± 0.025 0.027 ± 0.025 0.067 ± 0.026 0.047 ± 0.022 Saline 0.038 ± 0.014 0.067 ± 0.021 0.076 ± 0.021 0.048 ± 0.022 0.062 ± 0.018 3.3.3. Day 3 Responding was not found to significantly differ across conditions, [F(2,14) = 0.529, p = 0.601], however, responding within the fifth bin is noticeably higher in the PL condition (see Table 7).
  • 16. Table 7 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group Bin 1 (mean ± SEM) Bin 2 (mean ± SEM) Bin 3 (mean ± SEM) Bin 4 (mean ± SEM) Bin 5 (mean ± SEM) Day 3 IL 0.072 ± 0.042 0.039 ± 0.017 0.034 ± 0.025 0.055 ± 0.036 0.028 ± 0.029 PL 0.133 ± 0.046 0.040 ± 0.019 0.040 ± 0.028 0.080 ± 0.039 0.113 ± 0.032 Saline 0.095 ± 0.042 0.033 ± 0.017 0.089 ± 0.025 0.061 ± 0.036 0.056 ± 0.029 3.3.4. Day 4 A significant difference between the conditions was not observed, [F(2,14) = 0.272, p = 0.766], mean differences are displayed in Table 8. Table 8 displaying Mean ± SEM nose poke responding across the five bins of the 10-second trace interval group Bin 1 (mean ± SEM) Bin 2 (mean ± SEM) Bin 3 (mean ± SEM) Bin 4 (mean ± SEM) Bin 5 (mean ± SEM) Day 4 IL 0.056 ± 0.038 0.039 ± 0.028 0.039 ± 0.037 0.022 ± 0.018 0.022 ± 0.031 PL 0.067 ± 0.042 0.060 ± 0.031 0.080 ± 0.041 0.060 ± 0.020 0.047 ± 0.034 Saline 0.067 ± 0.038 0.045 ± 0.028 0.050 ± 0.037 0.039 ± 0.018 0.078 ± 0.031 3.4. Responding within first bin between two and 10-second trace interval groups Whereas the 10-second trace interval contains five two-second bins, the two-second trace interval constitutes only a single bin. Comparison of this single bin with the first of the 10- second trace interval group may identify if rate of responding is affected by trace conditioning of different intervals.
  • 17. 3.4.1. Day One On Day One rats within the two-second trace interval group responded (mean = 0.195 ± 0.028) at a significant higher rate, [F(1,39) = 11.014, p = 0.001], than those within the 10- second trace interval group (mean = 0.055 ± 0.028) during the first bin. 3.4.2. Day Two Responding within the two-second trace interval group (mean = 0.436 ± 0.041) was observed to be significant higher rate, [F(1,37) = 41.177, p < 0.001], than the 10-second trace interval group (mean = 0.053 ± 0.044). 3.4.3. Day Three A significance difference was illustrated, rats within the two-second trace interval group responding (mean = 0.698 ± 0.046) at a higher rate, [F(1,36) = 75.197, p < 0.001], the 10- second trace interval group (mean = 0.098 ± 0.051). 3.4.4. Day Four Data from Day Four displayed similar findings as the two-second trace interval group responded (mean = 0.848 ± 0.051) at a significant higher rate, [F(1,36) = 105.577, p < 0.001], compared to the 10-second trace interval group (mean = 0.063 ± 0.057). 3.5. Rate of responding across Days within response windows How the rate of responding changed across days between the trace interval groups and conditions allows examination of how each variable affected incremental learning.
  • 18. 3.5.1. Responding within the ‘Stimulus’ response windows over four days Evident in Figure 5 the two-second trace interval group responding significantly increased across days, [F(1,32) = 42.097, p < 0.001], whilst the 10-second trace interval group remained at base levels of responding, [F(2,37) = 1.378, p = 0.267]. Figure 5 displaying nose pokes over four days in the ‘Stimulus’ response windows for the two and 10-second (respectively) trace interval groups 3.5.2. Responding within the ‘Trace’ response windows over four days As illustrated in Figure 6 the two-second trace interval group responding significantly increased inclemently across days, [F(1,32) = 16.126, p < 0.001], whereas the 10-second trace interval group remained at base levels of responding, [F(2,37) = 0.592, p = 0.559]. Figure 6 displaying nose pokes over four days in the ‘Trace response windows for the two and 10-second (respectively) trace interval groups
  • 19. 4. Discussion A role of D1 activity within the mPFC for working memory is supported by the literature (Cai & Arnsten, 1997; Henby & Trojanowski, 2003). Moreover the degeneration of D1 receptors is linked to deficits in working memory, including the temporal association of events (McLaughlin et al., 2002) which may be ameliorated with D1 agonists such as SKF (for a review see Mizoguchia, Yuzurihara, Nagata, Ishige, Sasaki, & Tabira, 2002). Such a role is not suggested from the findings of the present study, they do not convey that SKF augmented learning but instead inhibited learning. Two trace intervals were utilised in this study, a two and 10-second interval group. The saline condition within the former group displayed a similar pattern of responding as the IL and PL conditions (whom received SKF). However, over the course of the study the saline condition noticeably superseded the responding of these two drug conditions. From these findings it is clear SKF inhibited learning the US-CS contingency under the time interval of two seconds. These findings on the inhibiting effects of SKF may appear to contradict the literature on the ameliorative effects of mPFC-D1 agonism in aging, and other forms of pathological neurodegeneration and dysfunction (Deutch, 1993; Dolan et al., 1994; Fibiger, 1995). However, D1 activity have been reported be constitute an inverse U-function, wherein hypo or hyper-activation of these receptors causing a dysfunction, thereby impairing associated D1 mechanisms (Arnsten, 1998; Zahrt, Taylor, Mathew, & Arnsten, 1997). In accord, mPFC dysfunction, such as in the case of Parkinson‟s disease, is treated via D1 agonism (Zahrt et al., 1997). Futhermore, chronic stress causes hyper-activation of D1 which impairs working memory, with administration of D1 antagonists reversing the effect (Zahrt et al., 1997). Taken together, mPFC-D1 activity may be responsible for the modulation of working
  • 20. memory. The present study also focused on the functional differentiation of the IL and PL cortices of the mPFC. These two sub-regions appear to be functionally distinct from the literature (Balleine & O‟Doherty, 2010). However, due to the theorised hyper-activation produced by SKF any functional distinction is indistinguishable. A theory supported by the lack of any significant difference between the two drug conditions in both trace interval groups. It is of importance to emphasize that SKF has shown to have a beneficial effect on working memory, administered via either systemically or intra-mPFC infusion (Granon, Passetti, Thomas, Dalley, Everitt, & Robbins, 2000), in subjects which have an underlying degenerative pathology, such as in aging (Cai & Arnsten, 1997) or Parkinson‟s disease (Lange, Robbins, Marsden, James, Owen, & Paul, 1992). Interestingly the saline and both drug conditions did not display a difference of responding under the 10-second trace interval group. Across 30 trials within each day and across the four days the rate of responding remained at base levels entirely for all three conditions. It is then reasonable to conclude that D1 activity, at normal or hyper-activated levels in healthy subjects, bears no effect upon trace learning over certain time intervals. Examining the five two-second bins of the 10-second trace interval supports this conclusion. If trace learning of a 10 second interval between the US and CS were successful, responding within the five bins would be expected to polarise at the last two bins. The literature on the learning of fixed time intervals, known as a fixed interval schedule, reports a „scallop‟ pattern of responding (Pavlov, 1928; Skinner, 1938). The temporal expectation of a US delivery from the CS onset is driven by dopaminergic mechanisms (Schultz, 1998). The pattern of responding is a result of successfully learning that a CS/reward will only be delivered after a fixed amount of time and as such responding will is only reinforced near this point of time. However, within the present study responding was evenly distributed throughout the five bins. Particularly
  • 21. illustrative of this is the PL group which on Day 3 responding most within the first and fifth bin. Noteworthy is how responding was measured as the behaviour of nose poking, detected by a rat pushing open a flap to access the food dispenser. It is possible that a rat may remain stationed at the food dispenser, keeping the flap open indefinitely and as such preventing measurement of behaviour. The number of nose poking responses signifies learning of the US-CS contingency. Although if a rat has learnt this contingency but remains at the food dispenser resulting in less nose pokes, the data will suggest a lower rate of responding. As such it is possible that rats within a particular condition exhibited a higher rate of responding but, due to the limitation of the apparatus, this difference is not represented. Another possible confounding element of the study is the duration of SKF effects. Surrounding research which implemented SKF has documented the D1 agonistic effect to last up to 30 minutes (Mizoguchi, Yuzurihara, Ishige, Sasaki, Chui, & Tabira, 2000; Sorg et al., 2001; Zahrt et al., 1997). Within this study rats were within the task for up to 65 minutes, it is unknown if the effects of SKF lasted the entire duration of the task. In conclusion, the functional role of PL and IL in learning and memory of a temporally distant US-CS were not distinguished. However, as both the PL and IL conditions appeared to be inhibited by SKF. It may then be inferred that the two sub-regions are involved to in the temporal learning aspect of working memory, albeit to an extent as all three conditions responded at baseline levels under the 10-second interval group. The lack of learning within the 10-second trace interval group may signify that the length of this trace interval is too great. In a similar study, Floresco & Phillips (2001) focused on significantly longer time
  • 22. intervals, they reported SKF to augment learning between learning a task and repeating the task after a 12 hour delay. Moreover, the Floresco & Phillips (2001) study found that at delays of 30 minutes SKF inhibited repetition of the task. In converging the discussed findings and surrounding literature it would appear the mechanisms of D1 activity are sensitive to either hypo or hyper-activation and also the length of the time interval.
  • 23. 5. References Andersen, P. H., & Jansen, J. A. (1990). Dopamine receptor agonists: selectivity and D1 receptor efficacy. European Journal of Pharmacology, 188, 335–347. Arnsten, A. F. T. (1998). Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 436-447. Arnsten, A. F. T., Cai, J. X., Murphy, B. L., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology, 116, 143-151. Balleine, B. W., & O‟Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 48–69. Burgos-Robles, A., Bravo-Rivera, H., & Quirk, G. J. (2013). Prelimbic and Infralimbic Neurons Signal Distinct Aspects of Appetitive Instrumental Behavior. PLOS ONE, 8, Cai, J. X., & Arnsten, A. F. T. (1997). Dose-dependent effects of the dopamine D1 receptor agonists A77636 or SKF81297 on spatial working memory in aged monkeys. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapy, 283, 183–189.
  • 24. Cassaday, H. J., Horsley, R. R., & Norman, C. (2005). Electrolytic lesions to nucleus accumbens core and shell have dissociable effects on conditioning to discrete and contextual cues in aversive and appetitive procedures respectively. Behavioural Brain Research, 160, 222–235. Collins, P., Wilkinson, L. S., Everitt, B. J., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (2000). The effect of dopamine depletion from the caudate nucleus of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) on tests of prefrontal cognitive function. Behavioural Neuroscience, 114, 3-17. Coutureau, E., & Killcross, S. (2003) Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behaviour and Brain Research, 146, 167–174. Dalley, J. W., Chudasama, Y., Theobald, D. E., Pettifer, C. L., Fletcher, C. M., & Robbins, T. W. (2002) Nucleus accumbens dopamine and discriminated approach learning: interactive effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions and systemic apomorphine administration. Psychopharmacology 161, 425–433. Deutch, A. Y. (1993). Prefrontal cortical dopamine systems and the elaboration of functional corticostriatal circuits: implications for schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neural Transmission, 91, 197–221.
  • 25. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997) Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin card sort test: restriction to novel situations and independence from “on-line” processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 9285- 9297. Dolan, R. J., Bench, C. J., Brown, R. G., Scott, L. C., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1994). Neuropsychological dysfunction in depression: the relationship to regional cerebral blood flow. Psychological Medicine, 24, 849-857. Feenstra, M. G. P., Vogel, M., Botterblom, M. H. A., Joosten, R. N. J. M. A., & de Bruin, J. P. C. (2001). Dopamine and noradrenaline efflux in the rat prefrontal cortex after classical aversive conditioning to an auditory cue. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1051–1054. Floresco, S. B., Braaksma, D. N., & Phillips, A. G. (1999). Thalamic-cortical-striatal circuitry subserves working memory during delayed responding on a radial arm maze. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 11061-11071. Floresco, S. B., & Phillips, A. G. (2001). Delay-dependent modulation of memory retrieval by infusion of a dopamine D1 agonist into the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 934–993. Fibiger, H. C. (1995). Neurobiology of depression: focus on dopamine. Advances in Biochemical Psychopharmacology, 49, 1–17.
  • 26. Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M., & Kroger, J. (2000). Cross-modal and cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex. Nature, 405, 347-351. Granon, S., Passetti, F., Thomas, K. L., Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Enhanced and impaired attentional performance after infusion of D1 dopaminergic receptor agents into rat prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1208-1215. Harada, N., Nishiyama, S., Satoh, K., Fukumoto, D., Kakiuchi, T., & Tsukada, H. (2002). Age-related changes in the striatal dopamine system in the living brain: a multiparametric study in conscious monkeys. Synapse, 45, 38–45. Kantini, E., Norman, C., & Cassaday, H. J. (2004). Amphetamine decreases the expression and acquisition of appetitive conditioning but increases the acquisition of anticipatory responding over a trace interval. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 18, 516–526. Killcross, S., & Coutureau, E. (2003). Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 400–408. Kronforst-Collins, M A., & Disterhoft, J. F. (1998). Lesions of the caudal area of rabbit medial prefrontal cortex impair trace eyeblink conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 69, 147-162.
  • 27. Lange, K. W., Robbins, T. W., Marsden, C. D., James, M., Owen, A. M., & Paul, G. M. (1992) L-dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychopharmacology, 107, 394– 404. McLaughlin, J., Skaggs, H., Churchwell, J., & Powell, D. A. (2002). Medial prefrontal cortex and Pavlovian conditioning: trace versus delay conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 37-47. Mizoguchia, K., Yuzurihara, M., Ishige, A., Sasaki, H., Chui, D. H., & Tabira, T. (2000). Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1568-1578. Mizoguchia, K., Yuzurihara, M., Nagata, M., Ishige, A., Sasaki, H., & Tabira, T. (2002). Dopamine-receptor stimulation in the prefrontal cortex ameliorates stress-induced rotarod impairment. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 72, 723-728. Moore, T. L., Killiany, R. J., Herndon, J. G., Rosene, D. L., & Moss, M. B. (2003). Impairment in abstraction and set shifting in aged hesus monkeys. Neurobiology of Aging, 24, 125–134.
  • 28. Murphy, E. R., Fernando, A. B., Urcelay, G. P., Robinson, E. S., Mar, A. C., Theobald, D. E. H., Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2012). Impulsive behaviour induced by both NMDA receptor antagonism and GABAA receptor activation in rat ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology, 219, 401–410. Nieoullon, A. (2002). Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Progressive Neurobiology, 67, 53–83. Ostlund, S. B., & Balleine, B. W. (2005). Lesions of medial prefrontal cortex disrupt the acquisition but not the expression of goal-directed learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 7763–7770. Packard, M. G. (1999). Dissociation of multiple memory systems by posttraining intracerebral injections of glutamate. Psychobiology, 127, 40-50. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Oxford UP. Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes: The higher nervous activity of animals. London: Lawrence & Wishart. Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 1–27.
  • 29. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Sorg, B. A., Li, N., & Wu, W. R. (2001). Dopamine D1 receptor activation in the medial prefrontal cortex prevents the expression of cocaine sensitization. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapy, 297, 501-508. Vidal-Gonzalez, I., Vidal-Gonzalez, B., Rauch, S. L., & Quirk, G. J. (2006) Microstimulation reveals opposing influences of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex on the expression of conditioned fear. Learning & Memory, 13, 728–733. Waelti, P., Dickinson, A., & Schultz, W. (2001). Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature, 412, 43–48. Zahrt, J., Taylor, J. R., Mathew, R. G., & Arnsten, A. F. T. (1997). Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs working memory performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 8528-8535.