SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 58
Mechanisms of Mindfulness in
Communication Training
Daniel C. Huston, Eric L. Garland & Norman A.S. Farb
Mindfulness, an ancient spiritual practice, is becoming an
increasingly popular
component of communication courses, training individuals to
reserve judgment in their
dealings with others. However, the effects of mindfulness in
communication courses are
not well researched. We compared students taking an
introductory communication
course that included a mindfulness component (N �20) against
a control group of
students taking an equivalent course without mindfulness
content (N �24). Both groups
improved in their positive reappraisal tendencies following
communication training;
however, the groups appeared to differ in how they positively
reappraised situations.
Only the mindfulness group demonstrated improved mindfulness
scores following
training, accounting for that group’s increases in positive
reappraisal, and providing
evidence for mindfulness training as one mechanism for
reducing negative reactivity in
communication.
Keywords: Mindfulness; Positive Reappraisal; Blame; Mindful
Coping Model; Spirituality
Mindfulness meditation is an ancient spiritual practice
introduced over 2,500 years
ago as a means of calming the mind and gaining insight into the
impermanent and
interdependent nature of the self. Over the last few decades,
scholars and clinicians in
the West have begun to explore secular applications of
mindfulness, and have noted
the practical benefits of observing thoughts, impulses, and
emotions. This ability,
nurtured through the practice of mindfulness meditation,
appears to help people lead
happier, more productive, and fulfilling lives through the
process of coming to know
Daniel C. Huston is a Professor in the Departme nt of English,
Fine Arts, and Foreign Languages at NHTI,
Concord’s Community College, Eric L. Garland is an Assistant
Professor in the College of Social Work at Florida
State University and a Research Affiliate for Trinity Institute
for the Addictions, Norman A.S. Farb is a
postdoctoral fellow at the Rotman Research Institute. The
authors would like to thank Beth Blankenstein,
Susanne O’Brien, Diana Levine, members of the NHTI Institute
Leadership Team, and the students
who voluntarily participated in the study for their contributions.
Correspondence to: Daniel C. Huston,
Grappone Hall, NHTI, Concord’s Community College, 31
College Drive, Concord, NH 03301, USA. E-mail:
[email protected]
ISSN 0090-9882 (print)/ISSN 1479-5752 (online) # 2011
National Communication Association
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00909882.2011.608696
Journal of Applied Communication Research
Vol. 39, No. 4, November 2011, pp. 406�421
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00909882.2011.608696
themselves and realizing they are more than the self-talk they
experience, more than
the habitual patterns of behavior they have formed over the
years. They come to
realize they can ‘‘let go’’ of that ‘‘chatter’’ and those ‘‘habits’’
and open to a richer,
more complete experience of themselves and the world around
them. As a result, they
transcend the narrow perspective that had been defining how
they perceived
themselves and others; they connect to other human beings and
more fully
experience the world in which they live. This transformation,
which some might
describe as a spiritual experience, is thought to influence how
people communicate:
improving accurate expression, increasing understanding, and
reducing conflict.
King and Sawyer (1998) advocated the inclusion of mindfulness
instruction in the
teaching of communication. Since then, interest in mindfulness
in education has
increased with the development of organizations such as the
Association for
Contemplative Mind in Higher Education; the Mindfulness in
Education Network;
and the Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional
Learning. However, to
date few quantitative studies have examined the impact of
mindfulness content in
communication training.
Mindfulness in the field of communication has mainly been
considered in terms of
how consciously people plan their approach to a communication
exchange or the
extent to which they identify and respond to relevant or
irrelevant information in a
given situation (Burgoon, Berger, & Waldron, 2000; Folkes,
1985; Langer, Blank, &
Chanowitz, 1978). Stroud (2010) describes these early studies
as equating mindful
communication with ‘‘effortful, cognitive processing,’’ which
differs substantially
from the operationalization of the construct of mindfulness
within the fields of
psychology and medicine. Scholars in these fields highlight
elements of mindfulness
that have been passed down from eastern traditions such as
Buddhist meditation
practices, emphasizing for instance, the cultivation of an open
awareness to present-
moment experience without interpretation or attachment to a
particular outcome
(Kabat-Zinn, 1982). The view of mindfulness that has emerged
in these fields
recognizes mindfulness as a means of expanding one’s
experience of each moment by
nurturing qualities such as acceptance and patience (Shapiro &
Schwartz, 2000) that
allow for observation ‘‘of what is occurring both internally and
externally’’ (Brown &
Ryan, 2003) and therefore results in increased emotional
awareness and increased
self-regulation abilities (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Shapiro &
Schwartz, 2000; Shapiro,
Carlson, & Astin, 2006). Communication scholars are beginning
to use the term
mindfulness to refer to this kind of expanded awareness and its
function in adaptive
communication (e.g., Adelman, 2010; Chinn Swartz, 2008;
Stroud, 2010; Ucok,
2007). In the present study, we directly tested the hypothesis
that this open, non-
evaluative form of mindfulness may promote cognitive
strategies associated with
adaptive communication.
While ‘‘adaptive communication’’ may be a broad construct,
communication
efficacy can be measured by the types of cognitive strategies
employed by individuals in
their communication efforts. One adaptive strategy is positive
reappraisal, which is the
cognitive process through which stressful events are re-
construed as benign, beneficial,
and/or meaningful. This strategy (alternately conceptualized as
benefit-finding) is
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 407
associated with decreased distress and enhanced mental health
(Helgeson, Reynolds, &
Tomich, 2006), and also appears to modulate physiological
parameters associated
with stress (Bower, Low, Moskowitz, Sepah, & Epel, 2008;
Tugade & Fredrickson,
2004). Positive reappraisal is an active, emotion-focused coping
strategy (Folkman,
1997) that is often the first step towards a productive
reengagement with a stressful
situation. For example, in a conflictual interaction, one may
first appraise a given
communication as a personal attack stemming from disrespect,
and then positively
reappraise that communication as a brusque expression of
concern and care. In
reappraising the communication this way, the dyad may be more
willing to engage in
constructive dialogue around how best to give and receive
feedback. Positive
reappraisal has been associated with increased communication
satisfaction (Corbeil,
Quayhagen, & Quayhagen, 1999), and employing a positive
reappraisal strategy to cope
with an interpersonal offense decreased negative emotion and
physiological arousal
while exerting salutary effects on heart rate variability and
increasing positively-toned
communication (Witvliet, Knoll, Hinman, & DeYoung, 2010).
A second candidate measure of communication efficacy is a
person’s ability to
refrain from blaming others for communication difficulty, a
form of negative external
attribution that has negative impacts on communication (Burr,
1990; Cleaver, 1987;
Ford & Ford, 1995). Furthermore, blaming others is a
maladaptive strategy that has
deleterious effects on mood: compared to participants high in
positive reappraisal,
participants who frequently blamed others for communication
problems demon-
strated greater dysphoric symptoms (Schroevers, Kraaij, &
Garnefski, 2007). It is
theorized that blame often gives rise to anger and interferes
with awareness of
personal needs during moments of conflict, resulting in
communication that is
unlikely to help one meet his or her needs (Rosenberg, 2003).
Consequently,
developing an increased awareness of difficult emotions during
interpersonal conflict
is key to productive communication. As Goldstein (1993)
asserts, the increased
awareness afforded by mindfulness makes it possible not only to
‘‘initiate effective
communication,’’ but to do so ‘‘without getting caught in
reactive judgments’’
(p. 152). This emphasis is consistent with a form of
communication training called
Insight Dialogue (Kramer, 2007) that integrates Buddhist
mindfulness practices into
interpersonal communication. Key to this training is the notion
that human beings
are plagued by distorted, automatic thoughts that filter the way
we interpret
information, often leading to blame, misunderstanding, and
suffering.
The present study investigated whether mindfulness strategies
acquired through a
communication class could account for training-related changes
in positive
reappraisal and blaming others. According to the mindful
coping model (Garland,
Gaylord, & Park, 2009; Garland et al., 2010), positive
reappraisal can occur when one
disengages from automatic negative appraisal (e.g., blaming
another for a conflictual
interaction) into the state of mindfulness, a state of broadened,
metacognitive
awareness wherein evaluations of the interaction are suspended.
On the other hand,
negative emotions induced by relational conflict often linger
during a ‘‘refractory
period’’ (Ekman, 2003), a period of time during which one is
biased towards mood-
congruent information and making emotion-consistent
appraisals. Mindfulness may
408 D. C. Huston et al.
suspend blame-laden appraisals of challenging situations,
allowing individuals the
cognitive flexibility to more easily attend to the benign or
benevolent features of the
relationship and the ability to reappraise the interaction as
meaningful or even
beneficial. Repeated engagement of the state of mindfulness
may result in the
establishment of mindful dispositionality, which, in turn, could
lead to a heightened
propensity toward making positive reappraisals of interpersonal
communication.
Indeed, a recent study found that the stress-reductive effects of
increases in
dispositional mindfulness were mediated by growth in positive
reappraisal over an
eight week course of mindfulness training (Garland, Gaylord, &
Fredrickson, 2011).
Further evidence for the model may be drawn from social
psychological research to
broaden and build theory (Fredrickson, 2004), which has
identified reciprocal
relationships between broadened cognition and positive
emotions (Burns et al., 2008;
Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). Insofar as mindfulness practice
augments positive
reappraisal, it may prevent communication difficulties that arise
as a result of
unconscious, maladaptive patterns of behavior.
Identifying habitual patterns of thought and reactivity is
particularly challenging
given the extent to which individuals appear to be influenced by
stimuli unconsciously
and react to them habitually (Motley, 1986a, b, 1990). At least
one study suggests that
mindfulness reduces the automatic allocation of attention,
reducing automatic
response tendencies (Wenk-Sormaz, 2005). The curriculum
being examined in this
study is designed to help students recognize and modify
unproductive, habitual
patterns of behavior through the study of mindfulness and
communication theory.
(For a detailed description of this curriculum see Huston,
2010a, b.) Students learn to
‘‘wake up’’ to the present moment and, as a result, notice how
communication
concepts such as self-talk and nonverbal behavior act as internal
and external
influences on their thoughts and emotional reactions. With this
awareness, they can
then make informed decisions about how to appraise and
respond to challenging
situations. Through mindfulness students learn to ‘‘reenter’’
difficult situations ‘‘from
a gathered, deliberate, more spacious and less self-centered
perspective’’ (Williams,
Teasdale, Segal, & Kabat-Zinn, 2007, p. 197) that may help
them to choose ways of
communicating and interacting which allow growth and learning
to materialize
(Bstan-dzin-rgya-mtsho & Cutler, 1998).
Hence, both Eastern traditions and contemporary secular
applications of mind-
fulness emphasize the role nonjudgment plays in opening one’s
awareness and letting
go of the initial tendency to blame others that often arises
during difficult interactions.
Recognizing emotional reactions and positively reappraising the
conflict appear to be
central to choosing a productive, satisfying response.
Mindfulness training may foster
awareness of the emotional impulse to react and blame others,
promote a positive
reappraisal of the situation, and ultimately result in more
effective communication.
The aim of the present study is to examine the effects of
teaching mindfulness on
positive reappraisal among students in an introductory
communication course. We
hypothesize that, compared to a standard communication
curriculum, mindful
communication training would result in increased dispositional
mindfulness, which
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 409
would account for increased use of positive reappraisal and
decreased use of blaming
strategies in daily communication.
Method
Participants and Study Design
Students enrolled in five sections of a college communication
course offered in
the spring semester of 2010 were invited to participate in the
study at NHTI,
a comprehensive community college in Concord New
Hampshire. The study
obtained prior review and approval by the Institutional
Leadership Team, the IRB
for the college, in accordance with the Human Subject Research
Protocols established
by the college and the Community College System of New
Hampshire. Class sections
selected to be a part of the study were taught by different
instructors. The comparison
group for this study was comprised of students from two class
sections who received
a traditional communication curriculum without mindfulness
elements, while the
experimental group consisted of students enrolled in three class
sections that
incorporated mindfulness concepts and practices. From these
classes we obtained
complete participation from 24 students in the comparison
group and 20 students in
the mindfulness group. The mindfulness and comparison groups
did not differ with
respect to age (18.891.0 years vs. 19.692.2 years respectively),
gender (12 female vs.
10 female), ethnicity, or years of post-secondary education
(1.490.9 years vs.
1.291.1 years).
The data consisted of three brief paper assessments which were
administered
during the first half hour of the first day of class and the last
scheduled class
session of the semester. In an effort to reduce the potential
problem of demand
characteristics, i.e., students attempting to predict what was
being measured and
deliberately skew the results, these instruments were
administered by personnel of the
Office of Institutional Research and Grants, Academic Affairs,
distancing the study
from the professors and consequently the course material itself.
Students were sent a
letter from the associate vice president of academic affairs in
advance of the start of
the semester, inviting them to participate in the study without
mentioning what was
specifically being measured, other than that the results of the
study may be used to
improve teaching methodology in college communication
classes. Students were
assured that their participation was voluntary and confidential,
and the human
subject research protocols of the college were followed.
Participants were advised that
their participation would have no impact on their grading
performance for the
course. Furthermore, students took the pre-test before having
met their professors in
order to limit their influence. During the management of the
post-test, participating
professors were careful to treat it as they would any other study
being done at the
college. It is not uncommon, for instance, for students to
complete studies in a class
that have nothing to do with the course itself, e.g., studies that
measure recreational
drug use, use of library resources, or other general-information.
The assessment
instruments administered at the beginning of the semester
included a general
410 D. C. Huston et al.
information form which assessed prior meditation experiences
and other relevant
demographic information, the Five Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire, and relevant
items selected from the Cognitive Emotion Regulation
Questionnaire. At the
conclusion of the semester, the latter two instruments were
administered.
Communication Curricula
Course work in both the mindfulness and comparison groups
consisted mainly of
writing assignments, speeches, and small group work. The
number of assignments
was nearly identical in both groups.
The mindfulness curriculum includes seven units focused on
introducing students
to particular communication concepts, guiding them through
mindfulness medita-
tion exercises, and assigning Application Journals that ask
students to reflect on
communication concepts in their lives. The order in which the
mindfulness exercises
and communication concepts are introduced is designed to
scaffold students’
understanding and application of key ideas.
Students in the mindfulness group were led through these units
each week for seven
weeks during the 15-week term and were encouraged to think of
the related
communication concepts as internal and external influences on
their behavior (e.g.,
‘‘self-talk’’ is an internal influence; the amount of eye contact
someone is or isn’t giving
is an external influence). Students were encouraged to
participate in the in-class
guided meditations, and the teachers of these classes
recommended that students
practice these same meditations, which were available online, as
much as possible,
ideally daily. No effort was made to determine how often
students meditated on their
own.
Students in the mindfulness classes were also encouraged to
apply the abilities
mentioned above to their public speaking and group work, e.g.,
thinking about
symptoms of nervousness (such as blushing cheeks or sweaty
palms) as an indication
that they may be reacting emotionally to the situation at hand in
a way that is not
necessarily accurate or productive. Students could then use the
recognition of such
physiological symptoms as an opportunity to reappraise the
situation before they
reacted in a manner that might lead to poor communication or
performance in the
class activity.
Measures
Mindfulness. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire
(FFMQ; in this sample, total
a�.81), comprised of 39 Likert-type items rated on a five-point
scale (1 �never or
very rarely true, 5 �very often or always true), was used to
measure trait mindfulness.
The FFMQ yields a total score (computed by summing
responses across all 39 items)
and scores for five internally consistent mindfulness factors
each with their own
convergent and predictive validity: nonreactivity to inner
experience (tapped by items
such as ‘‘I watch my feelings without getting lost in them’’; 7
items, subscale a �.76),
observing and attending to experience (‘‘I pay attention to
sensations, such as the wind
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 411
in my hair or the sun on my face’’; 8 items, subscale a�.83),
describing and
discriminating emotional experiences (‘‘I’m good at finding
words to describe my
feelings’’; 7 items, subscale a�.87), nonjudging of experience
(reverse coded item:
‘‘I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way that I am feeling’’;
8 items, subscale
a�.83), and acting with awareness (reverse coded item: ‘‘I find
myself doing things
without paying attention’’; 7 items, subscale a�.91) (Baer,
Smith, Hopkins,
Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006).
Cognitive coping strategies. The positive reappraisal, refocus on
planning,
catastrophizing, and blame others subscales of the Cognitive
Emotion Regulation
Questionnaire (CERQ; Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven, 2001)
were administered to
evaluate cognitive coping. The full CERQ consists of 36 Likert-
type items that assess
how often certain cognitive strategies are employed to cope
with stressful life events.
The positive reappraisal subscale (4 items, subscale a�.80)
includes items such as
‘‘I think I can learn somethi ng from the situation,’’ and ‘‘I
think I can become a
stronger person as a result of what happened.’’ The refocus on
planning subscale
(4 items, subscale a�.73) is comprised of items such as, ‘‘I
think about a plan of
what I can do best’’ and ‘‘I think about how to change the
situation.’’ Items assessing
catastrophizing (4 items, subscale a �.77) include ‘‘I keep
thinking how terrible it is
what I have experienced’’ and ‘‘I often think what I have
experienced is the worst that
can happen to a person.’’ The blame others subscale (4 items,
subscale a�.73) includes
items like ‘‘I feel that others are responsible for what has
happened’’ and ‘‘I feel that
basically the cause lies with others.’’ The CERQ has been
shown to have good internal
consistency and convergent validity with subscales of the SCL-
90 (Garnefski & Kraaij,
2007; Garnefski et al., 2001).
Analytic Strategy
Descriptive statistics, paired t-tests, and repeated measures
analyses of variance
(ANOVA) were used to assess between- and within-groups
differences. Pearson
correlations were utilized to examine associations between pre -
post changes in
dispositional mindfulness, positive reappraisal, refocus on
planning, catastrophizing,
and blaming others.
Path analysis via structural equation modeling software (AMOS
17.0) was used to
test a hypothetical model in which participation in mindful
communication training
could lead to change in dispositional mindfulness which in turn
could exert direct
effects on change in blaming others or indirect effects through
increases in positive
reappraisal coping. The overall model fit was assessed by
examining the chi-square
statistic and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), as
well as the Root Mean
Squared Error of Approximation (RMSEA) Index (Hu &
Bentler, 1998). According to
statistical convention (Bentler, 1990; Hu & Bentler, 1998), the
CFI has typical values
between 0 and 1, with a value close to 1 indicating a good
model fit, and RMSEA
scores closer to 0 indicate a better model fit. Maximum
likelihood estimation (MLE)
was used to handle missing data in structural equation models.
412 D. C. Huston et al.
Results
Main Effect of Training
An initial analysis of within-group training effects for each of
the subscales was
performed (see Table 1). Both groups reported significant
increases in positive
reappraisal. In contrast, while the comparison group
demonstrated a significant
increase in refocusing, the mindfulness group evidenced
significant increases in total
mindfulness and the observing subscale of the FFMQ.
To look for differential effects of training group on training
effects, all subscales and
the FFMQ total scores were subjected to 2 (group) �2 (pre- and
post-training)
repeated-measures ANOVA. Main effects of training were found
for the refocusing
(F(1,42) �8.94, p �.005) and positive reappraisal (F(1,42)
�23.50, p B.001) subscales of
the CERQ, as well as for the observe subscale of the FFMQ
(F(1,43) �8.287, pB.01).
However, only the mindfulness group demonstrated significant
increases in total
FFMQ scores, as evidenced by a significant group by training
interaction
(F(1,42) �6.885, pB.01). The mindfulness group also reported a
marginally significant
increase in acting with awareness compared to the control
group’s marginal decrease
(F(1,42) �3.92, p �.054).
Individual Difference Analysis
To examine whether changes on the CERQ and FFMQ subscales
were related, we
performed an individual difference analysis on the change
scores (post � pre training)
for each of the subscales and total FFMQ with Pearson
correlations. Correlations
in FFMQ and the CERQ change scores are presented for each of
the two groups in
Table 2.
Table 1 Changes in mindfulness and communication-related
variables pre- and post-
participation in a mindful communication curricula and standard
communication course
(comparison group)
Comparison group (N �24) Mindfulness group (N �20)
Pre Post Pre Post
Refocus 13.8 (0.5) 15.3 (0.6)* 13.7 (0.6) 15.1 (0.6)
Reappraise 13.9 (0.5) 15.9 (0.5)* 14.1 (0.7) 16.2 (0.6)*
Catastrophize 7.0 (0.5) 7.7 (0.6) 7.2 (0.5) 6.9 (0.5)
Blame 6.5 (0.4) 6.8 (0.4) 6.8 (0.4) 6.9 (0.4)
FFMQ total 127.3 (2.8) 127.3 (2.9) 126.9 (3.9) 134.0 (3.5)*
Nonreacting 22 (0.6) 21.5 (0.8) 20.9 (0.8) 22.2 (1.0)
Describing 25.7 (1.2) 27 (1.2) 28.9 (1.2) 29.9 (0.9)
Observing 23.1 (1.1) 24.2 (1.0) 25.0 (1.3) 28.5 (1.1)*
Nonjudging 25.6 (1.0) 24.8 (0.9) 23.1 (1.1) 23.7 (1.2)
Act with awareness 27 (1.1) 25.9 (1.1) 25.5 (1.6) 26.2 (1.3)
Note: For each scale, means are presented with standard errors
in parentheses.
*pB.05.
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 413
Correlations observed among comparison-group participants. In
the comparison group,
the FFMQ observing subscale (e.g., ‘‘I pay attention to how my
emotions affect my
thoughts and behavior’’) was positively associated with CERQ
reappraisal and
negatively associated with blaming others, suggesting that the
tendency to observe
present-moment experience is associated with cognitive
strategies characteristic of
adaptive communication. Also in the control group, nonjudging
(a reverse scored
subscale, e.g.,‘‘I tend to evaluate whether my perceptions are
right or wrong’’) was
negatively correlated with reappraisal but positively associated
with blaming others.
This pattern suggests that comparison-group participants who
decreased their
evaluative tendencies may have simply defaulted to blaming
others for events rather
than attempting to reappraise their interpretation of these
events. In other words,
rather than be self-critical of their own maladaptive
communication tendencies,
participants in the comparison group who became less
judgmental of their own
thoughts and feelings may have simply allowed themselves to
negatively appraise
interactions and place blame on others.
Correlations observed among mindfulness-group participants.
The mindfulness group,
who showed a specific increase in FFMQ total scores,
demonstrated a strong
correlation between such FFMQ increases and CERQ refocus
and reappraisal. Among
mindfulness-group participants, the strongest associations
observed between FFMQ
and CERQ subscales were non-reacting (e.g., ‘‘I perceive my
feelings and emotions
without having to react to them’’) and describing (e.g., ‘‘I’m
good at finding the
words to describe my feelings’’) rather than the observing
subscale found in controls,
suggesting that one mechanism for the mindfulness training
effect may lie in
refraining from reacting and articulating one’s feelings instead
rather than just
Table 2 Individual difference analysis of associations between
changes in mindfulness
facets and communication-related variables observed among
students participating in a
mindful communication curricula and standard communication
course (comparison
group)
DRefocus DReappraise DCatastrophize DBlame-Others
Comparison group (N �24)
DFFMQ total �.01 .01 �.03 .00
DNonreacting .09 .26 �.19 �.20
DDescribing �.05 .00 �.17 �.02
DObserving .03 .52* �.33 �.46*
DNonjudging �.14 �.53* .32 .56*
DAct w/Awareness .06 �.10 .28 .03
Mindfulness group (N �20)
DFFMQ total .52* .50* .33 .34
DNonreacting .54* .52* �.22 .04
DDescribing .61* .51* .16 .16
DObserving .21 .29 .20 .02
DNonjudging �.14 .06 .27 .14
DAct with Awareness .13 �.04 .48* .54*
*pB.05.
414 D. C. Huston et al.
observing emotional reactions. Unexpectedly, increases in
acting with awareness
(a reverse-scored subscale, e.g., ‘‘I rush through activities
without being really
attentive to them’’) was associated with higher catastrophizing
and blaming others in
the mindfulness group, suggesting that as participants became
more mindful, they
became increasingly aware of their faults and weaknesses,
including catastrophizing
and blaming others.
A Fisher’s Z transformation was used to determine between-
groups differences in
the association between reappraisal and nonjudgment, r �.59,
pB.05. This finding
suggests that the pattern of high nonjudgment predicting low
reappraisal found in
controls was not apparent in the mindfulness group.
Path Analysis Results
Next, we conducted a multivariate path analysis of the role of
cognitive strategies in
mindful communication, where participation in the mindful
communication course
predicted change in dispositional mindfulness which in turn
could exert both direct
and indirect effects on change in blaming others through change
in positive
reappraisal coping. This model exhibited good fit: x2/df �1.34,
p �.26; RMSEA �.05
(.00, .20), CFI �.99, AIC � 26.68. Results indicated that,
relative to the comparison
group, mindful communication participants experienced
significantly larger increases
in dispositional mindfulness over the course, and such increases
in dispositional
mindfulness were associated with increases in positive
reappraisal, which were, in
turn, associated with decreases in blaming others (see Figure 1).
Overall, the model
explained 41% of the variance in changes in positive reappraisal
and 57% of the
variance in changes in blaming others.
Discussion
We examined whether the promotion of mindfulness as a
foundational component of
communication curriculum would improve cognitive strategies
associated with
adaptive communication. We compared two groups of students
participating in
communication skills courses: a mindfulness group, who
received instruction in
mindfulness techniques as explicit means of changing
communication strategies, and
Figure 1. Path analysis of training-related changes in cognitive
strategies implicated in
mindful communication. *pB05; ***pB001.
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 415
a comparison group, who received conventional communication
skills training.
Results indicate that incorporating mindfulness into
communication instruction
significantly increases students’ dispositional mindfulness.
Furthermore, training-
related increases in dispositional mindfulness are associated
with improved use of
positive reappraisal that precludes concurrent increases in
blaming others.
To the best of our knowledge, the finding of a significant
correlation between
dispositional mindfulness and positive reappraisal is one of the
first to be presented
in the literature (cf. Garland et al., 2011). Given that the
wording of the FFMQ items
does not imply making positive construals of stressful
situations, nor does it
implicate positive affective processes in general, this finding
may help to dispel
misconceptions about mindfulness as a blank or neutral state of
‘‘nondoing’’ and
relaxation. While mindfulness does appear to nurture what is
often referred to as
‘‘beginner’s mind,’’ by virtue of the reciprocal links between
broadened cognition and
positive emotion as outlined in the broaden and build theory
(Fredrickson, 2004),
the state of mindfulness appears to exert a positively-valenced
effect on how we
interpret and respond to events in our lives (Garland et al.,
2009). Furthermore, the
positive appraisal experienced by students in the mindfulness
group appears to differ
significantly from that of the controls. The mindfulness group’s
ability for increased
positive reappraisal was associated with the nonreacting and
describing subscales of
the FFMQ, suggesting that mindfulness training nurtures
positive reappraisal based
on increased awareness of emotional impulses which reduces
the tendency toward
reactivity and an increased ability to describe one’s emotions.
Such associations were
not found in the comparison group, suggesting that mindful
communication training
may be unique in its integration of nonreactivity and emotional
awareness skills into
reappraisal strategies.
While both groups demonstrated enhanced reappraisal
tendencies consequent to
communication training, participants in the comparison group
who evidenced
increases in reappraisal seemed to judge their thoughts and
feelings more harshly.
The comparison group may have learned to reappraise situations
by scrutinizing their
own reactions, rather than constructively focusing on the
positive features of the
interaction in order to reappraise its meaning. Indeed,
judgmental reappraisal may
serve to be less productive as a communication strategy, for it
appears to illustrate
what amounts to a shift in blame from self to others, rather than
resulting in
increased engagement with others during interactions.
In contrast, participants in the mindfulness group who
increasingly engaged in
positive reappraisal did so without turning to judgment or
blame. The strong fit of
the path analysis of training-related changes in factors
implicated in mindful
communication suggests that mindfulness promotes an adaptive
tradeoff between
cognitive strategies, promoting positive reappraisal while
reducing the tendency to
engage in blaming others. While students in both types of
communication courses
demonstrated improved positive reappraisal tendencies, it seems
as though increased
mindfulness is one powerful explanatory source of this change.
These findings are
consistent with Ekman’s (2003) emphasis on the importance of
observing impulses to
react prior to the emotional refractory period so that adaptive
reappraisals of stressful
416 D. C. Huston et al.
interactions are possible. Our data suggest that mindfulness can
be useful not only in
observing the impulse to react, but also in reappraising the
situation that gave rise to
the emotional reactivity. Similarly, the results of our path
analysis are congruent with
findings from recent path analytic investigations of the role of
positive reappraisal as
a mediator of the stress-reductive effects of increasing
dispositional mindfulness
(Garland et al., 2011), and lend further support to the mindful
coping model
(Garland et al., 2009, 2010).
While the associated changes mentioned above were observed
across the fifteen
weeks of a mindful communication course, hypothetically these
same relationships
may hold within micro-analytic analyses of the unfolding of
communication
processes within a single interaction. For example, a student
who took the mindful
communication course wrote an Application Journal entry
(direct quotations from
this entry are quoted below) that exemplifies many of the
cognitive strategies
described above. She had loaned money to a friend during
spring break. After a
month had gone by, her friend had only repaid a portion of the
debt, yet the student
observed her friend spending money on herself while claiming
she was broke. On a
later occasion when the student observed her friend wearing
some new clothes, she
noticed herself becoming upset by an increasing awareness of
muscular tension in her
body. Subsequently, she felt a strong urge to lash out at her
friend in accusatory
manner; however, realizing that ‘‘a situation is more than the
emotions you
experience first when confronted with a stimulus,’’ she was able
to remain nonreactive
as she ‘‘looked past the anger.’’ In so doing, she was able to
refrain from blaming her
friend for the problem at hand. The student realized that she had
never spoken with
her friend about how her previous purchases had upset her, and
she considered the
possibility that her friend was unaware of how much it bothered
her to see her buying
clothes for herself before repaying the debt. Once she was able
to positively reappraise
the situation in this manner, she chose to respond assertively
rather than aggressively
by ‘‘speaking [her] emotions instead of bursting out in anger.’’
This example
illustrates one role mindfulness can play in adaptive
communication as evidenced by
the results of our study.
Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research
There are several limitations to this study. First, the relatively
small sample size may
have resulted in a lack of statistical power to detect between-
groups differences of
smaller magnitude effect sizes. Nonetheless, significant effects
of training were
identified. Second, the lack of random assignment to mindful
communication and
standard communication curricula leaves the present findings
vulnerable to selection
threats to internal validity. Although participants did not
significantly differ at baseline
on dispositional mindfulness and the other cognitive strategy
variables assessed in the
present investigation, it is possible that there were systematic
differences on
unmeasured variables that may have led to the observed
between-groups differences.
Hence, future studies aimed to evaluate mindfulness as a
mechanism for improved
communication skills in communication curricula should
employ randomized
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 417
controlled designs with larger sample sizes. Furthermore, future
studies should
examine what other factors may be involved in the facilitation
of positive reappraisal,
in light of the fact that the comparison group evidenced
significant improvements in
this cognitive strategy. An additional limitation is that the
present investigation did not
attempt to measure communication skills directly. However,
given that the pedagogy of
the mindful communication course being studied defines
mindful communicators as
those who communicate productively as a result of recognizing
and responding to the
uniqueness of each situation such that responses that are
unproductive in one instance
may be precisely what is needed in another, designing an
instrument that would
measure such an ability is a difficult task. A more meaningful
assessment methodology
may be to observe mindful communication as it occurs (Ucok,
2007). One approach
for conducting such analysis may be to measure students’
cognitive, emotional, and
psychophysiological responses during a challenging interaction.
Using micro-analytic
research methodologies to probe the unfolding of online
emotion regulatory processes
over time (for an example of such methods, see Sheppes &
Meiran, 2007), randomized
experiments could examine the effects of mindfulness practice
relative to placebo
control on the sequential generation of mindful states and
positive reappraisals in
coping with communication challenges to offer a fuller test of
the mindful coping
model. The present investigation, therefore, paves the way for
controlled, experimental
research.
Practical Applications
The present research suggests that implementing mindfulness
training into curricula
that is geared toward the advance of practical communication
skills may be especially
fruitful, particularly for the development of those skills that
will help students
productively negotiate emotionally charged situations. Given
the stress and demands
often present in higher education, these communication skills
may serve college
students particularly well during their interactions with
teachers, peers, adminis-
trators, and staff as they work toward successful completion of
their degree programs.
The ability to identify and express emotions exhibited by the
mindfulness group in
this study is linked with what some researchers refer to as
emotional intelligence
(Mayer & Salovey, 1997), which has been associated with
increased positive affect,
decreased levels of depression, and reduced anxiety (Brown &
Ryan, 2003), all of
which may be useful for achieving satisfying and productive
communication
exchanges. Furthermore, it is important for communication
professors to examine
the extent to which traditional classroom activities and
coursework may promote a
tendency toward students becoming increasingly judgmental of
others and/or
themselves. The results of this study suggest that such
judgments are linked to
decreased positive reappraisal, which can increase conflict and
interfere with
collaboration. Conversely, training in mindful communication
may foster learning
environments characterized by teamwork and cooperation. In
sum, the concept of
mindfulness that is beginning to make its way from the health
and psychology fields
into the study of communication may be particularly useful as a
tool for increasing
418 D. C. Huston et al.
students’ awareness of emotional reactions to interactions,
reappraising those
interactions as growth or learning opportunities, and regulating
verbal interchange
in a constructive manner.
References
Adelman, M.B. (2010, November). Critical issues for
communication: Solitude and distraction.
Presented at the annual meeting of the NCA 96th annual
convention, San Francisco, CA.
Baer, R.A., Smith, G.T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney,
L. (2006). Using self-report
assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness.
Assessment, 13(1), 27�45.
Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural
models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,
238�246.
Bower, J., Low, C., Moskowitz, J., Sepah, S., & Epel, E.
(2008). Benefit finding and physical health:
Positive psychological changes and enhanced allostasis. Social
and Personality Psychology
Compass, 2(1), 223�244.
Brown, K.W., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being
present: Mindfulness and its role in
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84(4), 822�848.
Bstan-dzin-rgya-mtsho, & Cutler, H.C. (1998). The art of
happiness: A handbook for living. New
York, NY: Riverhead Books.
Burgoon, J.K., Berger, C.R., & Waldron, V.R. (2000).
Mindfulness and interpersonal communica-
tion. Journal of Social Issues, 56(1), 105�127.
Burns, A.B., Brown, J.S., Sachs-Ericsson, N., Plant, E.A.,
Curtis, J.T., Fredrickson, B.L., et al. (2008).
Upward spirals of positive emotion and coping: Replication,
extension, and initial
exploration of neurochemical substrates. Personality and
Individual Differences, 44, 360�370.
Burr, W.R. (1990). Beyond I-statements in family
communication. Family Relations, 39(3), 266�
273.
Chinn Swartz, B. (2008, November). Manifesting mindfulness:
Compassion, yogic philosophy, and
flow in the classroom. Presented at the annual meeting of the
NCA 94th annual convention,
San Diego, CA.
Cleaver, G. (1987). Marriage enrichment by means of a
structred communication programme.
Family Relations, 36, 49�54.
Corbeil, R.R., Quayhagen, M.P., & Quayhagen, M. (1999).
Intervention effects on dementia
caregiving interaction: A stress-adaptation modeling approach.
Journal of Aging & Health,
11(1), 79�95.
Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Recognizing faces and
feelings to improve communication and
emotional life. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.
Folkes, V. (1985). Mindlessness or mindfulness: A partial
replication and extension of Lange, Blank,
& Chanowitz. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48,
600�604.
Folkman, S. (1997). Positive psychological states and coping
with severe stress. Social Science &
Medicine, 45(8), 1207�1221.
Ford, J., & Ford, L. (1995). The role of conversations in
producing intentional change in
organizations. Academy of Management Review, 20(3),
541�570.
Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of
positive emotions. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological
Sciences, 359(1449), 1367�1378.
Fredrickson, B.L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions
trigger upward spirals toward emotional
well-being. Psychological Science, 13(2), 172�175.
Garland, E.L., Gaylord, S., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2011). Positive
reappraisal mediates the stress-
reductive effects of mindfulness. Mindfulness, 2(1), 59�67.
Garland, E., Gaylord, S., & Park, J. (2009). The role of
mindfulness in positive reappraisal. Explore:
The Journal of Science and Healing, 5, 37�44.
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 419
Garland, E.L., Fredrickson, B.L., Kring, A.M., Johnson, D.P.,
Meyer, P.S., & Penn, D.L. (2010).
Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals
of negativity: Insights from
the broaden-and-build theory and affective neuroscience on the
treatment of emotion
dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology. Clinical
Psychology Review, 30, 849�864.
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The Cognitive Emotion
Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric
features and prospective relationships with depression and
anxiety in adults. European
Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 141�149.
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life
events, cognitive emotion regulation,
and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences,
30, 1311�1327.
Goldstein, J. (1993). Insight meditation: The practice of
freedom. Boston, MA: Shambhala
Publications.
Helgeson, V.S., Reynolds, K.A., & Tomich, P.L. (2006). A
meta-analytic review of benefit finding and
growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(5),
797�816.
Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P.M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance
structure modeling: Sensitivity to
underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological
Methods, 3, 424�453.
Huston, D. (2010a). Communicating mindfully: Mindfulness-
based communication and emotional
intelligence (5th ed.). Mason, OH: Cengage Learning.
Huston, D. (2010b). Waking up to ourselves: The use of
mindfulness meditation and emotional
intelligence in the teaching of communications. New Directions
for Community Colleges:
Contemplative Teaching and Learning, 151, 39�50.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral
medicine for chronic pain based on the
practice of mindfulness meditation. General Hospital
Psychiatry, 4, 33�47.
King, P.E., & Sawyer, C.R. (1998). Mindfulness, mindlessness
and communication instruction.
Communication Education, 47, 326�336.
Kramer, G. (2007). Insight dialogue: The interpersonal path to
freedom. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Langer, E., Blank, A., & Chanowitz, B. (1978). The
mindlessness of ostensibly thoughtful action.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 635�642.
Mayer, J.D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional
intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.),
Emotional development and emotional intelligence:
Implications for educators (pp. 3�31).
New York, NY: Basic Books.
Motley, M.T. (1986a). Consciousness and intention in
communication: A preliminary model and
methodological approaches. Western Journal of Speech
Communication, 50, 3�23.
Motley, M.T. (1986b). The production of verbal slips and
double entendres as clues to the efficiency
of normal speech production. Journal of Language and Social
Psychology, 4, 275�293.
Motley, M.T. (1990). On whether one can(not) not
communicate: An examination via traditional
communication postulates. Western Journal of Speech
Communication, 54, 1�20.
Rosenberg, M.B. (2003). Nonviolent communication: A
language of life (2nd ed.). Encinitas, CA:
Puddle Dancer.
Schroevers, M., Kraaij, V., & Garnefski, N. (2007). Goal
disturbance, cognitive coping strategies, and
psychological adjustment to different types of stressful life
event. Personality and Individual
Differences, 43, 413�423.
Shapiro, S.L., & Schwartz, G.E. (2000). Intentional systemic
mindfulness: An integrative model for
self-regulation and health. Advances in Mind�Body Medicine,
16(2), 128�134.
Shapiro, S.L., Carlson, L.E., & Astin, J.A. (2006). Mechanisms
of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 62(3), 373�386.
Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2007). Better late than never? On
the dynamics of online regulation of
sadness using distraction and cognitive reappraisal. Personality
& Social Psychology Bulletin,
33(11), 1518�1532.
Stroud, S.R. (2010). Toward a Deweyan theory of
communicative mindfulness, Imagination.
Cognition and Personality, 30(1), 57�75.
420 D. C. Huston et al.
Tugade, M.M., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). Resilient
individuals use positive emotions to bounce
back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2),
320�333.
Ucok, I.O. (2007, November). ‘‘Dropping into being’’:
Exploring mindfulness as lived experience.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the NCA 93rd annual
convention, Chicago, IL.
Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual
responding. Advances in Mind�Body
Medicine, 21(3/4), 33�49.
Williams, M., Teasdale, J., Segal, Z., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2007).
The mindful way through depression:
Freeing yourself from chronic unhappiness. New York, NY: The
Guilford Press.
Witvliet, C.V., Knoll, R.W., Hinman, N.G., & DeYoung, P.A.
(2010). Compassion-focused
reappraisal, benefit-focused reappraisal, and rumination after an
interpersonal offense:
Emotion-regulation implications for subjective emotion,
linguistic responses, and physiol-
ogy. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(3), 226�242.
Mechanisms of Mindfulness 421
Copyright of Journal of Applied Communication Research is the
property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted
to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.
Discussion 1: Mindfulness and Perception Checking
It takes a little bit of mindfulness and a little bit of attention to
others to be a good listener, which helps cultivate emotional
nurturing and engagement.
—Deepak Chopra, Indian-American author, alternative medicine
practitioner, physician, and public speaker
Throughout the day, people experience interactions with others
that may cause misperceptions. Often people react to a situation
before taking the time to clarify misunderstandings. Reacting to
a situation before being fully aware of the meaning behind
another’s behavior or words often leads to additional confusion
and escalating negative feedbacks.
In his quote, Deepak Chopra advocates practicing mindfulness
to create successful interpersonal communication environments.
What does mindfulness mean? How can it help you build your
communication competence? How can you train yourself to be
aware of interaction as it occurs and respond appropriately to
achieve positive results? How can you be sure your perception
of an individual’s behavior is accurate?
In this discussion, consider an interpersonal communication
scenario involving confusion on the part of both participants.
You examine how mindfulness and perception checking can
change interpersonal communication outcomes
To prepare for responding to two (2) colleagues discussion, pay
particular attention to the following learning resources
Review this week’s Learning Resources, especially:
· Huston, D. C., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. S. (2011).
Mechanisms of mindfulness in communication training. Journal
of Applied Communication Research, 39(4), 406–421.
· TED. (Producer). (2012). Amy Cuddy: Your body language
may shape who you are [Video file]. Retrieved from
https://www.ted.com/talks/amy_cuddy_your_ body_language_sh
apes_who_you_are
Assignment
Respond to at least two of your peers' postings in one or more
of the following ways:
· Discuss how your interpretation of the interpersonal
communication demonstrated within the video may differ from
that of your peers.
· Share an insight about what you learned from having read your
peer’s post and discuss how and why your peer’s posting
resonated with you professionally and/or personally or how it
will impact your own mindfulness with regards to interpersonal
communication.
· Offer an example, from your experience or observation that
validates what your peer presented.
· Offer specific suggestions that will help your peer build upon
his or her own mindfulness in interpersonal communication and
explain why you think your suggestions will be of benefit based
on what you learned this week and your own experiences.
· Share how something your peer discussed changed the way
you view misperceptions and how you will improve your own
mindfulness.
· 3 – 4 paragraph response per each colleagues
· No plagiarism
· APA citing
Bottom of Form
1st Colleague – Natasha
Discussion 1 - Week 2
Top of Form
Natasha Mills
Mindfulness and Perception Checking
Top of Form
Alberto and Kathy’s interpersonal behaviors were significantly
the result of previous experiences witnessed by Alberto, as well
as Kathy’s verbal and non-verbal expressions. After ending the
call, Kathy says that she cannot take it anymore, a statement
that is accompanied by an exasperated face. On the other hand,
in an attempt to comfort Kathy, Alberto states that it is a rite of
passage in the organization to be pressured by Mr. Gregowski
(Laureate Education Producer, 2014). Therefore, factors of
previous experiences on Alberto’s part and verbal and nonverbal
cues on Kathy’s part are evident in the communication behavior
of the two.
Communication is a complex process consisting of various
elements in play every time people communicate (Quintanilla &
Wahl, 2020). For effectiveness and excellence during
communication, individuals ought to consider how these
elements interact when they communicate. In Alberto and
Kathy’s situation, these elements are displayed as personal
variables that significantly dictate their interpersonal
communication behaviors. For instance, the personal variables
in Kathy’s case include being new to the job, being female, as
Alberto puts it, and complementing Mr. Gregowski’s power
pose by adopting a less powerful pose (TED Producer, 2012).
The personal variables displayed by Alberto include being
perceptive about situations and the lack of mindfulness during
communication.
One precise instance of misperception in the video is when
Alberto tells Kathy that she should let another guy take over the
account because she is a woman, making her sensitive and
unable to handle a guy like Mr. Gregowski (Laureate Education
Producer, 2014). This is a misperception because Alberto relies
on previous occurrences to make this judgment without giving
Kathy a chance to prove whether or not she can handle Mr.
Gregowski. Therefore, Alberto’s perception is based on Kathy’s
gender, leading him to pass judgment on Kathy’s abilities.
Simply, the misperception crippled Alberto’s objectivity during
the communication process with Kathy.
Mindfulness could have had a significant positive impact on the
verbal and nonverbal communication between Alberto and
Kathy. This is because mindfulness causes people to reserve
judgment when communicating with others, thereby influencing
the outcomes of interactions (Huston et al., 2011). Hence,
mindfulness would have caused Alberto to reserve his
judgments about Kathy until he has fully understood the
situation. “Mindfulness in the field of communication has
mainly been considered in terms of how consciously people plan
their approach to a communication exchange or the extent to
which they identify and respond to relevant or irrelevant
information in a given situation” (Huston et al., 2011, 407).
I once observed an interaction between my colleagues during
the compilation of an annual report. The employees of the
company always experienced a lot of pressure during the
completion of this task due to the processes involved, but more
specifically, the tracing and compilation of records. I happened
to be in the breakroom when I heard three colleagues inquiring
about each other’s progress. When one stated that he was still a
bit behind with respect to the deadline, one of the other
colleagues asked him what it was this time that he would
present as an excuse for not finishing on time. However, the
other colleague asked why he was lurking behind, as well as
how he could be of help.
Mindfulness towards the employee that was still behind in
finishing up the annual report was only exercised by one
colleague. The other colleague rushed to pass judgment instead
of assessing to what extent he should respond to the information
that the colleague provided about finishing the report (Huston et
al., 2011). As a result, the colleague felt small, incompetent,
and less powerful, as was displayed by his nonverbal cues (TED
Producer, 2012). The application of mindfulness in this
situation by this one colleague would have reduced the negative
reaction that he had towards the colleague that was experiencing
a challenge in completing his annual report. The outcome of
mindfulness would have been a positive reappraisal of the
situation (Huston et al., 2011).
Huston, D. C., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. S. (2011).
Mechanisms of mindfulness in communication training. Journal
of Applied Communication Research, 39(4), 406–421.
Laureate Education (Producer). (2014). Interpersonal
communication case study: Interpersonal conflict [Video file].
Retrieved from https://class.waldenu.edu. (SEE ATTACHMENT
FOR TRANSCRIPT)
Quintanilla, K. M., & Wahl, S. T. (2020). Business and
professional communication: KEYS for workplace
excellence (4th ed.) Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
TED. (Producer). (2012). Amy Cuddy: Your body language may
shape who you are [Video file]. Retrieved
from https://www.ted.com/talks/amy_cuddy_your_body_languag
e_shapes_who_you_are.
Bottom of Form
Bottom of Form
Bottom of Form
Top of Form
Bottom of Form
2nd Colleague – Douglas Hayes
Discussion 1 - Week 2Top of Form
Top of Form
Douglas Hayes
Top of Form
The interpersonal communication between Kathy and Alberto
was abysmal. First, Alberto calling her "sweete" in a
professional environment was insulting and demeaning.
Secondly, telling her that she needs to give the account over to
another man was also demining. She is already stressed, and
this only compounded the issue. Thirdly, putting her hands on
anyone without their permission is never a good idea and could
be a sexual harassment issue.
Alberto putting his hands on Kathy's shoulders was a significant
problem that can affect their communication. This can have a
detrimental ripple effect on their communication, not only for
this instance but in future communication. As chapter six
details, "in order to develop professional excellence in the
workplace, you must have clear understanding of the line
between personal and professional relationships, as well as what
constitutes professional versus personal communication"
(Quintanilla, Pg. 133). It does continue that having a personal
relationship with coworkers is essential, but Alberto crossed the
line with his advances toward Kathy.
What Alberto should have done was to be mindful of Kathy's
feelings toward what was going on. Mindfulness teaches us that
if we use positive reappraisal, "which is the cognitive process
through which stressful events are re-construed as benign,
beneficial, and/or meaningful." (Huston, 2011) We can initially
take a breath before reacting to the situation impulsively and
flip a stressful situation into a conversation. If Alberto were
using this, he would have asked if Kathy needed to talk about it
and then (using her name) talked to Kathy from a chair or stood
across the room.
I have seen lots of these interactions where one party is
dominant and demeaning over another in my career. It is not
always male dominance over females, but it always ends badly.
I was in a situation like this where a supervisor was doing this
to an employee. The was a female supervisor doing this to a
male employee, but the situation was very similar. They were
having a hard time with a situation, and she was telling him that
he should just quit if it were too hard. They would call him late
at night to do reports and other things. The man eventually did
quit because the stress got too much. All the rest of us could see
how the job was affecting him physically. He started to drink to
cope with the stress, and it was not long after that he quit. It
was more critical for them to get the job done than to see what
they were doing.
Mindfulness is "a means of expanding one's experience of each
moment by nurturing qualities such as acceptance and patience
that allow for observation "of what is occurring both internally
and externally." (Huston, 2011) If the supervisors had used
mindfulness, they would have seen the damage they were doing
to this person. I would have suggested that they evaluate their
times to work after hours. They should evaluate how they are
communicating with people and what time is appropriate to do
that. Sending emails at 1 am and asking questions is
inappropriate, and they should also not call after hours to
demand data. The supervisors should use mindfulness
techniques to understand that it is appropriate to communicate
and use patience in all situations.
Refference:
Huston, D. C., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. S. (2011).
Mechanisms of Mindfulness in Communication
Training. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 39(4),
406–421.
Quintanilla, K. M., & Wahl, S. T. (2020). Business and
Professional Communication (4th ed.). Sage publishing Ltd.
Bottom of Form
Bottom of Form
Bottom of Form

More Related Content

Similar to Mechanisms of Mindfulness inCommunication TrainingDaniel C

Compassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature ReviewCompassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature Review
Fatima Shaikh
 
RUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE .docx
RUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE      .docxRUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE      .docx
RUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE .docx
agnesdcarey33086
 
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docx
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docxBRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docx
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docx
AASTHA76
 
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
lanagore871
 
Correlação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vida
Correlação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vidaCorrelação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vida
Correlação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vida
Cátia Rodrigues
 
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docxModel of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
helzerpatrina
 
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docxModel of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
roushhsiu
 
Priručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbt
Priručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbtPriručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbt
Priručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbt
Ola Karola
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016
Daniel Horsley
 
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for socialLittle research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
MerrileeDelvalle969
 
Introduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docx
Introduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docxIntroduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docx
Introduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docx
doetphipgoattuynh
 

Similar to Mechanisms of Mindfulness inCommunication TrainingDaniel C (20)

Mindfulness: An effective coaching tool for improving physical and mental health
Mindfulness: An effective coaching tool for improving physical and mental healthMindfulness: An effective coaching tool for improving physical and mental health
Mindfulness: An effective coaching tool for improving physical and mental health
 
A Complex Systems Investigation Of Group Work Dynamics In L2 Interactive Tasks
A Complex Systems Investigation Of Group Work Dynamics In L2 Interactive TasksA Complex Systems Investigation Of Group Work Dynamics In L2 Interactive Tasks
A Complex Systems Investigation Of Group Work Dynamics In L2 Interactive Tasks
 
Active Listening in Peer Interviews The Influence of Message Paraphrasing on...
Active Listening in Peer Interviews  The Influence of Message Paraphrasing on...Active Listening in Peer Interviews  The Influence of Message Paraphrasing on...
Active Listening in Peer Interviews The Influence of Message Paraphrasing on...
 
Compassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature ReviewCompassion Literature Review
Compassion Literature Review
 
RUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE .docx
RUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE      .docxRUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE      .docx
RUNNING HEAD THE ART OF LISTENING as a THERAPUTIC TECHNIQUE .docx
 
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docx
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docxBRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docx
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONInformational, Interpersonal, and Intra.docx
 
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
 
URI Poster Aug 30
URI Poster Aug 30URI Poster Aug 30
URI Poster Aug 30
 
Correlação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vida
Correlação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vidaCorrelação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vida
Correlação da inteligencia emocional com o mindfulness e satisfação com a vida
 
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docxModel of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
 
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docxModel of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
Model of TreatmentEducation and its EvaluationProblem.docx
 
Dickerson Final Paper
Dickerson Final PaperDickerson Final Paper
Dickerson Final Paper
 
Contextual Therapy
Contextual TherapyContextual Therapy
Contextual Therapy
 
Research Paper
Research PaperResearch Paper
Research Paper
 
Priručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbt
Priručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbtPriručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbt
Priručnik za rad s depresivnim djetetom cbt
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016
 
Age Differences In Problem Solving Strategies The Mediating Role Of Future T...
Age Differences In Problem Solving Strategies  The Mediating Role Of Future T...Age Differences In Problem Solving Strategies  The Mediating Role Of Future T...
Age Differences In Problem Solving Strategies The Mediating Role Of Future T...
 
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for socialLittle research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
Little research has been done on the benefit of empathy for social
 
E564956.pdf
E564956.pdfE564956.pdf
E564956.pdf
 
Introduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docx
Introduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docxIntroduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docx
Introduction to Leadership Concepts and Practice.docx
 

More from AbramMartino96

Homeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docx
Homeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docxHomeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docx
Homeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docx
AbramMartino96
 
Homework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docx
Homework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docxHomework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docx
Homework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docx
AbramMartino96
 
Homework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docx
Homework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docxHomework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docx
Homework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docx
AbramMartino96
 
Homework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docx
Homework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docxHomework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docx
Homework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docx
AbramMartino96
 
History and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docx
History and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docxHistory and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docx
History and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docx
AbramMartino96
 
Historical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docx
Historical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docxHistorical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docx
Historical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docx
AbramMartino96
 

More from AbramMartino96 (20)

Homework assignmentPlease annotate one artwork you like from this.docx
Homework assignmentPlease annotate one artwork you like from this.docxHomework assignmentPlease annotate one artwork you like from this.docx
Homework assignmentPlease annotate one artwork you like from this.docx
 
Homeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docx
Homeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docxHomeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docx
Homeland Security efforts are ably reinforced by Homeland Defense an.docx
 
Homecoming is an annual tradition in the United States. In this repo.docx
Homecoming is an annual tradition in the United States. In this repo.docxHomecoming is an annual tradition in the United States. In this repo.docx
Homecoming is an annual tradition in the United States. In this repo.docx
 
HomerAssignmentIIReadthreeofthebooksfromTheOdyss.docx
HomerAssignmentIIReadthreeofthebooksfromTheOdyss.docxHomerAssignmentIIReadthreeofthebooksfromTheOdyss.docx
HomerAssignmentIIReadthreeofthebooksfromTheOdyss.docx
 
Homelessness in America has been a problem since the settlement of t.docx
Homelessness in America has been a problem since the settlement of t.docxHomelessness in America has been a problem since the settlement of t.docx
Homelessness in America has been a problem since the settlement of t.docx
 
Homework Assignments One pagewhat the functional currency .docx
Homework Assignments One pagewhat the functional currency .docxHomework Assignments One pagewhat the functional currency .docx
Homework Assignments One pagewhat the functional currency .docx
 
Homework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docx
Homework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docxHomework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docx
Homework Assignment Company Research  This assignment req.docx
 
Homework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docx
Homework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docxHomework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docx
Homework Assignment #1Directions Please answer each of the foll.docx
 
Homework Assignment 9Due in week 10 and worth 30 pointsSuppose t.docx
Homework Assignment 9Due in week 10 and worth 30 pointsSuppose t.docxHomework Assignment 9Due in week 10 and worth 30 pointsSuppose t.docx
Homework Assignment 9Due in week 10 and worth 30 pointsSuppose t.docx
 
Homework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docx
Homework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docxHomework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docx
Homework Assignment 4 Guidelines1. Write the paper in Microsoft Wo.docx
 
Hi we are a group doing a research and we split up the work ev.docx
Hi we are a group doing a research and we split up the work ev.docxHi we are a group doing a research and we split up the work ev.docx
Hi we are a group doing a research and we split up the work ev.docx
 
hi I need research paper  about any topics in Manufacturing Proc.docx
hi I need research paper  about any topics in Manufacturing Proc.docxhi I need research paper  about any topics in Manufacturing Proc.docx
hi I need research paper  about any topics in Manufacturing Proc.docx
 
HMIS Standards  Please respond to the followingFrom the e-A.docx
HMIS Standards  Please respond to the followingFrom the e-A.docxHMIS Standards  Please respond to the followingFrom the e-A.docx
HMIS Standards  Please respond to the followingFrom the e-A.docx
 
Hi i need a paper about (Head On )German film ( Turkey part)3 to.docx
Hi i need a paper about (Head On )German film ( Turkey part)3 to.docxHi i need a paper about (Head On )German film ( Turkey part)3 to.docx
Hi i need a paper about (Head On )German film ( Turkey part)3 to.docx
 
Hi i have new work can you do it, its due in 6 hours Boyd, Ga.docx
Hi i have new work can you do it, its due in 6 hours Boyd, Ga.docxHi i have new work can you do it, its due in 6 hours Boyd, Ga.docx
Hi i have new work can you do it, its due in 6 hours Boyd, Ga.docx
 
HIT Management and Implementation  Please respond to the followi.docx
HIT Management and Implementation  Please respond to the followi.docxHIT Management and Implementation  Please respond to the followi.docx
HIT Management and Implementation  Please respond to the followi.docx
 
History and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docx
History and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docxHistory and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docx
History and TheoryConsiderthe eras, life histories.docx
 
History of an argument Are there too many people There h.docx
History of an argument Are there too many people There h.docxHistory of an argument Are there too many people There h.docx
History of an argument Are there too many people There h.docx
 
history essays- 1000 words each essay- mla and 2 works cited. every .docx
history essays- 1000 words each essay- mla and 2 works cited. every .docxhistory essays- 1000 words each essay- mla and 2 works cited. every .docx
history essays- 1000 words each essay- mla and 2 works cited. every .docx
 
Historical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docx
Historical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docxHistorical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docx
Historical Background of Housing PolicyHousing is one of the requi.docx
 

Recently uploaded

The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
heathfieldcps1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
 
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptxPlant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
Plant propagation: Sexual and Asexual propapagation.pptx
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptxGoogle Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxHow to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 

Mechanisms of Mindfulness inCommunication TrainingDaniel C

  • 1. Mechanisms of Mindfulness in Communication Training Daniel C. Huston, Eric L. Garland & Norman A.S. Farb Mindfulness, an ancient spiritual practice, is becoming an increasingly popular component of communication courses, training individuals to reserve judgment in their dealings with others. However, the effects of mindfulness in communication courses are not well researched. We compared students taking an introductory communication course that included a mindfulness component (N �20) against a control group of students taking an equivalent course without mindfulness content (N �24). Both groups improved in their positive reappraisal tendencies following communication training; however, the groups appeared to differ in how they positively reappraised situations. Only the mindfulness group demonstrated improved mindfulness scores following training, accounting for that group’s increases in positive reappraisal, and providing
  • 2. evidence for mindfulness training as one mechanism for reducing negative reactivity in communication. Keywords: Mindfulness; Positive Reappraisal; Blame; Mindful Coping Model; Spirituality Mindfulness meditation is an ancient spiritual practice introduced over 2,500 years ago as a means of calming the mind and gaining insight into the impermanent and interdependent nature of the self. Over the last few decades, scholars and clinicians in the West have begun to explore secular applications of mindfulness, and have noted the practical benefits of observing thoughts, impulses, and emotions. This ability, nurtured through the practice of mindfulness meditation, appears to help people lead happier, more productive, and fulfilling lives through the process of coming to know Daniel C. Huston is a Professor in the Departme nt of English, Fine Arts, and Foreign Languages at NHTI, Concord’s Community College, Eric L. Garland is an Assistant Professor in the College of Social Work at Florida State University and a Research Affiliate for Trinity Institute
  • 3. for the Addictions, Norman A.S. Farb is a postdoctoral fellow at the Rotman Research Institute. The authors would like to thank Beth Blankenstein, Susanne O’Brien, Diana Levine, members of the NHTI Institute Leadership Team, and the students who voluntarily participated in the study for their contributions. Correspondence to: Daniel C. Huston, Grappone Hall, NHTI, Concord’s Community College, 31 College Drive, Concord, NH 03301, USA. E-mail: [email protected] ISSN 0090-9882 (print)/ISSN 1479-5752 (online) # 2011 National Communication Association http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00909882.2011.608696 Journal of Applied Communication Research Vol. 39, No. 4, November 2011, pp. 406�421 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00909882.2011.608696 themselves and realizing they are more than the self-talk they experience, more than the habitual patterns of behavior they have formed over the years. They come to realize they can ‘‘let go’’ of that ‘‘chatter’’ and those ‘‘habits’’ and open to a richer,
  • 4. more complete experience of themselves and the world around them. As a result, they transcend the narrow perspective that had been defining how they perceived themselves and others; they connect to other human beings and more fully experience the world in which they live. This transformation, which some might describe as a spiritual experience, is thought to influence how people communicate: improving accurate expression, increasing understanding, and reducing conflict. King and Sawyer (1998) advocated the inclusion of mindfulness instruction in the teaching of communication. Since then, interest in mindfulness in education has increased with the development of organizations such as the Association for Contemplative Mind in Higher Education; the Mindfulness in Education Network; and the Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning. However, to date few quantitative studies have examined the impact of mindfulness content in
  • 5. communication training. Mindfulness in the field of communication has mainly been considered in terms of how consciously people plan their approach to a communication exchange or the extent to which they identify and respond to relevant or irrelevant information in a given situation (Burgoon, Berger, & Waldron, 2000; Folkes, 1985; Langer, Blank, & Chanowitz, 1978). Stroud (2010) describes these early studies as equating mindful communication with ‘‘effortful, cognitive processing,’’ which differs substantially from the operationalization of the construct of mindfulness within the fields of psychology and medicine. Scholars in these fields highlight elements of mindfulness that have been passed down from eastern traditions such as Buddhist meditation practices, emphasizing for instance, the cultivation of an open awareness to present- moment experience without interpretation or attachment to a particular outcome (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). The view of mindfulness that has emerged
  • 6. in these fields recognizes mindfulness as a means of expanding one’s experience of each moment by nurturing qualities such as acceptance and patience (Shapiro & Schwartz, 2000) that allow for observation ‘‘of what is occurring both internally and externally’’ (Brown & Ryan, 2003) and therefore results in increased emotional awareness and increased self-regulation abilities (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Shapiro & Schwartz, 2000; Shapiro, Carlson, & Astin, 2006). Communication scholars are beginning to use the term mindfulness to refer to this kind of expanded awareness and its function in adaptive communication (e.g., Adelman, 2010; Chinn Swartz, 2008; Stroud, 2010; Ucok, 2007). In the present study, we directly tested the hypothesis that this open, non- evaluative form of mindfulness may promote cognitive strategies associated with adaptive communication. While ‘‘adaptive communication’’ may be a broad construct, communication
  • 7. efficacy can be measured by the types of cognitive strategies employed by individuals in their communication efforts. One adaptive strategy is positive reappraisal, which is the cognitive process through which stressful events are re- construed as benign, beneficial, and/or meaningful. This strategy (alternately conceptualized as benefit-finding) is Mechanisms of Mindfulness 407 associated with decreased distress and enhanced mental health (Helgeson, Reynolds, & Tomich, 2006), and also appears to modulate physiological parameters associated with stress (Bower, Low, Moskowitz, Sepah, & Epel, 2008; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004). Positive reappraisal is an active, emotion-focused coping strategy (Folkman, 1997) that is often the first step towards a productive reengagement with a stressful situation. For example, in a conflictual interaction, one may first appraise a given communication as a personal attack stemming from disrespect,
  • 8. and then positively reappraise that communication as a brusque expression of concern and care. In reappraising the communication this way, the dyad may be more willing to engage in constructive dialogue around how best to give and receive feedback. Positive reappraisal has been associated with increased communication satisfaction (Corbeil, Quayhagen, & Quayhagen, 1999), and employing a positive reappraisal strategy to cope with an interpersonal offense decreased negative emotion and physiological arousal while exerting salutary effects on heart rate variability and increasing positively-toned communication (Witvliet, Knoll, Hinman, & DeYoung, 2010). A second candidate measure of communication efficacy is a person’s ability to refrain from blaming others for communication difficulty, a form of negative external attribution that has negative impacts on communication (Burr, 1990; Cleaver, 1987; Ford & Ford, 1995). Furthermore, blaming others is a maladaptive strategy that has
  • 9. deleterious effects on mood: compared to participants high in positive reappraisal, participants who frequently blamed others for communication problems demon- strated greater dysphoric symptoms (Schroevers, Kraaij, & Garnefski, 2007). It is theorized that blame often gives rise to anger and interferes with awareness of personal needs during moments of conflict, resulting in communication that is unlikely to help one meet his or her needs (Rosenberg, 2003). Consequently, developing an increased awareness of difficult emotions during interpersonal conflict is key to productive communication. As Goldstein (1993) asserts, the increased awareness afforded by mindfulness makes it possible not only to ‘‘initiate effective communication,’’ but to do so ‘‘without getting caught in reactive judgments’’ (p. 152). This emphasis is consistent with a form of communication training called Insight Dialogue (Kramer, 2007) that integrates Buddhist mindfulness practices into
  • 10. interpersonal communication. Key to this training is the notion that human beings are plagued by distorted, automatic thoughts that filter the way we interpret information, often leading to blame, misunderstanding, and suffering. The present study investigated whether mindfulness strategies acquired through a communication class could account for training-related changes in positive reappraisal and blaming others. According to the mindful coping model (Garland, Gaylord, & Park, 2009; Garland et al., 2010), positive reappraisal can occur when one disengages from automatic negative appraisal (e.g., blaming another for a conflictual interaction) into the state of mindfulness, a state of broadened, metacognitive awareness wherein evaluations of the interaction are suspended. On the other hand, negative emotions induced by relational conflict often linger during a ‘‘refractory period’’ (Ekman, 2003), a period of time during which one is biased towards mood-
  • 11. congruent information and making emotion-consistent appraisals. Mindfulness may 408 D. C. Huston et al. suspend blame-laden appraisals of challenging situations, allowing individuals the cognitive flexibility to more easily attend to the benign or benevolent features of the relationship and the ability to reappraise the interaction as meaningful or even beneficial. Repeated engagement of the state of mindfulness may result in the establishment of mindful dispositionality, which, in turn, could lead to a heightened propensity toward making positive reappraisals of interpersonal communication. Indeed, a recent study found that the stress-reductive effects of increases in dispositional mindfulness were mediated by growth in positive reappraisal over an eight week course of mindfulness training (Garland, Gaylord, & Fredrickson, 2011). Further evidence for the model may be drawn from social
  • 12. psychological research to broaden and build theory (Fredrickson, 2004), which has identified reciprocal relationships between broadened cognition and positive emotions (Burns et al., 2008; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). Insofar as mindfulness practice augments positive reappraisal, it may prevent communication difficulties that arise as a result of unconscious, maladaptive patterns of behavior. Identifying habitual patterns of thought and reactivity is particularly challenging given the extent to which individuals appear to be influenced by stimuli unconsciously and react to them habitually (Motley, 1986a, b, 1990). At least one study suggests that mindfulness reduces the automatic allocation of attention, reducing automatic response tendencies (Wenk-Sormaz, 2005). The curriculum being examined in this study is designed to help students recognize and modify unproductive, habitual patterns of behavior through the study of mindfulness and communication theory.
  • 13. (For a detailed description of this curriculum see Huston, 2010a, b.) Students learn to ‘‘wake up’’ to the present moment and, as a result, notice how communication concepts such as self-talk and nonverbal behavior act as internal and external influences on their thoughts and emotional reactions. With this awareness, they can then make informed decisions about how to appraise and respond to challenging situations. Through mindfulness students learn to ‘‘reenter’’ difficult situations ‘‘from a gathered, deliberate, more spacious and less self-centered perspective’’ (Williams, Teasdale, Segal, & Kabat-Zinn, 2007, p. 197) that may help them to choose ways of communicating and interacting which allow growth and learning to materialize (Bstan-dzin-rgya-mtsho & Cutler, 1998). Hence, both Eastern traditions and contemporary secular applications of mind- fulness emphasize the role nonjudgment plays in opening one’s awareness and letting
  • 14. go of the initial tendency to blame others that often arises during difficult interactions. Recognizing emotional reactions and positively reappraising the conflict appear to be central to choosing a productive, satisfying response. Mindfulness training may foster awareness of the emotional impulse to react and blame others, promote a positive reappraisal of the situation, and ultimately result in more effective communication. The aim of the present study is to examine the effects of teaching mindfulness on positive reappraisal among students in an introductory communication course. We hypothesize that, compared to a standard communication curriculum, mindful communication training would result in increased dispositional mindfulness, which Mechanisms of Mindfulness 409 would account for increased use of positive reappraisal and decreased use of blaming strategies in daily communication.
  • 15. Method Participants and Study Design Students enrolled in five sections of a college communication course offered in the spring semester of 2010 were invited to participate in the study at NHTI, a comprehensive community college in Concord New Hampshire. The study obtained prior review and approval by the Institutional Leadership Team, the IRB for the college, in accordance with the Human Subject Research Protocols established by the college and the Community College System of New Hampshire. Class sections selected to be a part of the study were taught by different instructors. The comparison group for this study was comprised of students from two class sections who received a traditional communication curriculum without mindfulness elements, while the experimental group consisted of students enrolled in three class sections that incorporated mindfulness concepts and practices. From these classes we obtained
  • 16. complete participation from 24 students in the comparison group and 20 students in the mindfulness group. The mindfulness and comparison groups did not differ with respect to age (18.891.0 years vs. 19.692.2 years respectively), gender (12 female vs. 10 female), ethnicity, or years of post-secondary education (1.490.9 years vs. 1.291.1 years). The data consisted of three brief paper assessments which were administered during the first half hour of the first day of class and the last scheduled class session of the semester. In an effort to reduce the potential problem of demand characteristics, i.e., students attempting to predict what was being measured and deliberately skew the results, these instruments were administered by personnel of the Office of Institutional Research and Grants, Academic Affairs, distancing the study from the professors and consequently the course material itself. Students were sent a
  • 17. letter from the associate vice president of academic affairs in advance of the start of the semester, inviting them to participate in the study without mentioning what was specifically being measured, other than that the results of the study may be used to improve teaching methodology in college communication classes. Students were assured that their participation was voluntary and confidential, and the human subject research protocols of the college were followed. Participants were advised that their participation would have no impact on their grading performance for the course. Furthermore, students took the pre-test before having met their professors in order to limit their influence. During the management of the post-test, participating professors were careful to treat it as they would any other study being done at the college. It is not uncommon, for instance, for students to complete studies in a class that have nothing to do with the course itself, e.g., studies that measure recreational
  • 18. drug use, use of library resources, or other general-information. The assessment instruments administered at the beginning of the semester included a general 410 D. C. Huston et al. information form which assessed prior meditation experiences and other relevant demographic information, the Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire, and relevant items selected from the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. At the conclusion of the semester, the latter two instruments were administered. Communication Curricula Course work in both the mindfulness and comparison groups consisted mainly of writing assignments, speeches, and small group work. The number of assignments was nearly identical in both groups. The mindfulness curriculum includes seven units focused on introducing students to particular communication concepts, guiding them through
  • 19. mindfulness medita- tion exercises, and assigning Application Journals that ask students to reflect on communication concepts in their lives. The order in which the mindfulness exercises and communication concepts are introduced is designed to scaffold students’ understanding and application of key ideas. Students in the mindfulness group were led through these units each week for seven weeks during the 15-week term and were encouraged to think of the related communication concepts as internal and external influences on their behavior (e.g., ‘‘self-talk’’ is an internal influence; the amount of eye contact someone is or isn’t giving is an external influence). Students were encouraged to participate in the in-class guided meditations, and the teachers of these classes recommended that students practice these same meditations, which were available online, as much as possible, ideally daily. No effort was made to determine how often students meditated on their
  • 20. own. Students in the mindfulness classes were also encouraged to apply the abilities mentioned above to their public speaking and group work, e.g., thinking about symptoms of nervousness (such as blushing cheeks or sweaty palms) as an indication that they may be reacting emotionally to the situation at hand in a way that is not necessarily accurate or productive. Students could then use the recognition of such physiological symptoms as an opportunity to reappraise the situation before they reacted in a manner that might lead to poor communication or performance in the class activity. Measures Mindfulness. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; in this sample, total a�.81), comprised of 39 Likert-type items rated on a five-point scale (1 �never or very rarely true, 5 �very often or always true), was used to measure trait mindfulness. The FFMQ yields a total score (computed by summing
  • 21. responses across all 39 items) and scores for five internally consistent mindfulness factors each with their own convergent and predictive validity: nonreactivity to inner experience (tapped by items such as ‘‘I watch my feelings without getting lost in them’’; 7 items, subscale a �.76), observing and attending to experience (‘‘I pay attention to sensations, such as the wind Mechanisms of Mindfulness 411 in my hair or the sun on my face’’; 8 items, subscale a�.83), describing and discriminating emotional experiences (‘‘I’m good at finding words to describe my feelings’’; 7 items, subscale a�.87), nonjudging of experience (reverse coded item: ‘‘I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way that I am feeling’’; 8 items, subscale a�.83), and acting with awareness (reverse coded item: ‘‘I find myself doing things without paying attention’’; 7 items, subscale a�.91) (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006). Cognitive coping strategies. The positive reappraisal, refocus on planning,
  • 22. catastrophizing, and blame others subscales of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven, 2001) were administered to evaluate cognitive coping. The full CERQ consists of 36 Likert- type items that assess how often certain cognitive strategies are employed to cope with stressful life events. The positive reappraisal subscale (4 items, subscale a�.80) includes items such as ‘‘I think I can learn somethi ng from the situation,’’ and ‘‘I think I can become a stronger person as a result of what happened.’’ The refocus on planning subscale (4 items, subscale a�.73) is comprised of items such as, ‘‘I think about a plan of what I can do best’’ and ‘‘I think about how to change the situation.’’ Items assessing catastrophizing (4 items, subscale a �.77) include ‘‘I keep thinking how terrible it is what I have experienced’’ and ‘‘I often think what I have experienced is the worst that can happen to a person.’’ The blame others subscale (4 items, subscale a�.73) includes items like ‘‘I feel that others are responsible for what has happened’’ and ‘‘I feel that basically the cause lies with others.’’ The CERQ has been
  • 23. shown to have good internal consistency and convergent validity with subscales of the SCL- 90 (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007; Garnefski et al., 2001). Analytic Strategy Descriptive statistics, paired t-tests, and repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to assess between- and within-groups differences. Pearson correlations were utilized to examine associations between pre - post changes in dispositional mindfulness, positive reappraisal, refocus on planning, catastrophizing, and blaming others. Path analysis via structural equation modeling software (AMOS 17.0) was used to test a hypothetical model in which participation in mindful communication training could lead to change in dispositional mindfulness which in turn could exert direct effects on change in blaming others or indirect effects through increases in positive reappraisal coping. The overall model fit was assessed by
  • 24. examining the chi-square statistic and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), as well as the Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (RMSEA) Index (Hu & Bentler, 1998). According to statistical convention (Bentler, 1990; Hu & Bentler, 1998), the CFI has typical values between 0 and 1, with a value close to 1 indicating a good model fit, and RMSEA scores closer to 0 indicate a better model fit. Maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) was used to handle missing data in structural equation models. 412 D. C. Huston et al. Results Main Effect of Training An initial analysis of within-group training effects for each of the subscales was performed (see Table 1). Both groups reported significant increases in positive reappraisal. In contrast, while the comparison group demonstrated a significant
  • 25. increase in refocusing, the mindfulness group evidenced significant increases in total mindfulness and the observing subscale of the FFMQ. To look for differential effects of training group on training effects, all subscales and the FFMQ total scores were subjected to 2 (group) �2 (pre- and post-training) repeated-measures ANOVA. Main effects of training were found for the refocusing (F(1,42) �8.94, p �.005) and positive reappraisal (F(1,42) �23.50, p B.001) subscales of the CERQ, as well as for the observe subscale of the FFMQ (F(1,43) �8.287, pB.01). However, only the mindfulness group demonstrated significant increases in total FFMQ scores, as evidenced by a significant group by training interaction (F(1,42) �6.885, pB.01). The mindfulness group also reported a marginally significant increase in acting with awareness compared to the control group’s marginal decrease (F(1,42) �3.92, p �.054). Individual Difference Analysis To examine whether changes on the CERQ and FFMQ subscales were related, we performed an individual difference analysis on the change
  • 26. scores (post � pre training) for each of the subscales and total FFMQ with Pearson correlations. Correlations in FFMQ and the CERQ change scores are presented for each of the two groups in Table 2. Table 1 Changes in mindfulness and communication-related variables pre- and post- participation in a mindful communication curricula and standard communication course (comparison group) Comparison group (N �24) Mindfulness group (N �20) Pre Post Pre Post Refocus 13.8 (0.5) 15.3 (0.6)* 13.7 (0.6) 15.1 (0.6) Reappraise 13.9 (0.5) 15.9 (0.5)* 14.1 (0.7) 16.2 (0.6)* Catastrophize 7.0 (0.5) 7.7 (0.6) 7.2 (0.5) 6.9 (0.5) Blame 6.5 (0.4) 6.8 (0.4) 6.8 (0.4) 6.9 (0.4) FFMQ total 127.3 (2.8) 127.3 (2.9) 126.9 (3.9) 134.0 (3.5)* Nonreacting 22 (0.6) 21.5 (0.8) 20.9 (0.8) 22.2 (1.0) Describing 25.7 (1.2) 27 (1.2) 28.9 (1.2) 29.9 (0.9) Observing 23.1 (1.1) 24.2 (1.0) 25.0 (1.3) 28.5 (1.1)* Nonjudging 25.6 (1.0) 24.8 (0.9) 23.1 (1.1) 23.7 (1.2) Act with awareness 27 (1.1) 25.9 (1.1) 25.5 (1.6) 26.2 (1.3) Note: For each scale, means are presented with standard errors in parentheses. *pB.05.
  • 27. Mechanisms of Mindfulness 413 Correlations observed among comparison-group participants. In the comparison group, the FFMQ observing subscale (e.g., ‘‘I pay attention to how my emotions affect my thoughts and behavior’’) was positively associated with CERQ reappraisal and negatively associated with blaming others, suggesting that the tendency to observe present-moment experience is associated with cognitive strategies characteristic of adaptive communication. Also in the control group, nonjudging (a reverse scored subscale, e.g.,‘‘I tend to evaluate whether my perceptions are right or wrong’’) was negatively correlated with reappraisal but positively associated with blaming others. This pattern suggests that comparison-group participants who decreased their evaluative tendencies may have simply defaulted to blaming others for events rather than attempting to reappraise their interpretation of these events. In other words,
  • 28. rather than be self-critical of their own maladaptive communication tendencies, participants in the comparison group who became less judgmental of their own thoughts and feelings may have simply allowed themselves to negatively appraise interactions and place blame on others. Correlations observed among mindfulness-group participants. The mindfulness group, who showed a specific increase in FFMQ total scores, demonstrated a strong correlation between such FFMQ increases and CERQ refocus and reappraisal. Among mindfulness-group participants, the strongest associations observed between FFMQ and CERQ subscales were non-reacting (e.g., ‘‘I perceive my feelings and emotions without having to react to them’’) and describing (e.g., ‘‘I’m good at finding the words to describe my feelings’’) rather than the observing subscale found in controls, suggesting that one mechanism for the mindfulness training effect may lie in
  • 29. refraining from reacting and articulating one’s feelings instead rather than just Table 2 Individual difference analysis of associations between changes in mindfulness facets and communication-related variables observed among students participating in a mindful communication curricula and standard communication course (comparison group) DRefocus DReappraise DCatastrophize DBlame-Others Comparison group (N �24) DFFMQ total �.01 .01 �.03 .00 DNonreacting .09 .26 �.19 �.20 DDescribing �.05 .00 �.17 �.02 DObserving .03 .52* �.33 �.46* DNonjudging �.14 �.53* .32 .56* DAct w/Awareness .06 �.10 .28 .03 Mindfulness group (N �20) DFFMQ total .52* .50* .33 .34 DNonreacting .54* .52* �.22 .04 DDescribing .61* .51* .16 .16 DObserving .21 .29 .20 .02 DNonjudging �.14 .06 .27 .14 DAct with Awareness .13 �.04 .48* .54* *pB.05. 414 D. C. Huston et al.
  • 30. observing emotional reactions. Unexpectedly, increases in acting with awareness (a reverse-scored subscale, e.g., ‘‘I rush through activities without being really attentive to them’’) was associated with higher catastrophizing and blaming others in the mindfulness group, suggesting that as participants became more mindful, they became increasingly aware of their faults and weaknesses, including catastrophizing and blaming others. A Fisher’s Z transformation was used to determine between- groups differences in the association between reappraisal and nonjudgment, r �.59, pB.05. This finding suggests that the pattern of high nonjudgment predicting low reappraisal found in controls was not apparent in the mindfulness group. Path Analysis Results Next, we conducted a multivariate path analysis of the role of cognitive strategies in mindful communication, where participation in the mindful communication course
  • 31. predicted change in dispositional mindfulness which in turn could exert both direct and indirect effects on change in blaming others through change in positive reappraisal coping. This model exhibited good fit: x2/df �1.34, p �.26; RMSEA �.05 (.00, .20), CFI �.99, AIC � 26.68. Results indicated that, relative to the comparison group, mindful communication participants experienced significantly larger increases in dispositional mindfulness over the course, and such increases in dispositional mindfulness were associated with increases in positive reappraisal, which were, in turn, associated with decreases in blaming others (see Figure 1). Overall, the model explained 41% of the variance in changes in positive reappraisal and 57% of the variance in changes in blaming others. Discussion We examined whether the promotion of mindfulness as a foundational component of communication curriculum would improve cognitive strategies associated with
  • 32. adaptive communication. We compared two groups of students participating in communication skills courses: a mindfulness group, who received instruction in mindfulness techniques as explicit means of changing communication strategies, and Figure 1. Path analysis of training-related changes in cognitive strategies implicated in mindful communication. *pB05; ***pB001. Mechanisms of Mindfulness 415 a comparison group, who received conventional communication skills training. Results indicate that incorporating mindfulness into communication instruction significantly increases students’ dispositional mindfulness. Furthermore, training- related increases in dispositional mindfulness are associated with improved use of positive reappraisal that precludes concurrent increases in blaming others. To the best of our knowledge, the finding of a significant correlation between
  • 33. dispositional mindfulness and positive reappraisal is one of the first to be presented in the literature (cf. Garland et al., 2011). Given that the wording of the FFMQ items does not imply making positive construals of stressful situations, nor does it implicate positive affective processes in general, this finding may help to dispel misconceptions about mindfulness as a blank or neutral state of ‘‘nondoing’’ and relaxation. While mindfulness does appear to nurture what is often referred to as ‘‘beginner’s mind,’’ by virtue of the reciprocal links between broadened cognition and positive emotion as outlined in the broaden and build theory (Fredrickson, 2004), the state of mindfulness appears to exert a positively-valenced effect on how we interpret and respond to events in our lives (Garland et al., 2009). Furthermore, the positive appraisal experienced by students in the mindfulness group appears to differ significantly from that of the controls. The mindfulness group’s ability for increased
  • 34. positive reappraisal was associated with the nonreacting and describing subscales of the FFMQ, suggesting that mindfulness training nurtures positive reappraisal based on increased awareness of emotional impulses which reduces the tendency toward reactivity and an increased ability to describe one’s emotions. Such associations were not found in the comparison group, suggesting that mindful communication training may be unique in its integration of nonreactivity and emotional awareness skills into reappraisal strategies. While both groups demonstrated enhanced reappraisal tendencies consequent to communication training, participants in the comparison group who evidenced increases in reappraisal seemed to judge their thoughts and feelings more harshly. The comparison group may have learned to reappraise situations by scrutinizing their own reactions, rather than constructively focusing on the positive features of the interaction in order to reappraise its meaning. Indeed,
  • 35. judgmental reappraisal may serve to be less productive as a communication strategy, for it appears to illustrate what amounts to a shift in blame from self to others, rather than resulting in increased engagement with others during interactions. In contrast, participants in the mindfulness group who increasingly engaged in positive reappraisal did so without turning to judgment or blame. The strong fit of the path analysis of training-related changes in factors implicated in mindful communication suggests that mindfulness promotes an adaptive tradeoff between cognitive strategies, promoting positive reappraisal while reducing the tendency to engage in blaming others. While students in both types of communication courses demonstrated improved positive reappraisal tendencies, it seems as though increased mindfulness is one powerful explanatory source of this change. These findings are consistent with Ekman’s (2003) emphasis on the importance of observing impulses to
  • 36. react prior to the emotional refractory period so that adaptive reappraisals of stressful 416 D. C. Huston et al. interactions are possible. Our data suggest that mindfulness can be useful not only in observing the impulse to react, but also in reappraising the situation that gave rise to the emotional reactivity. Similarly, the results of our path analysis are congruent with findings from recent path analytic investigations of the role of positive reappraisal as a mediator of the stress-reductive effects of increasing dispositional mindfulness (Garland et al., 2011), and lend further support to the mindful coping model (Garland et al., 2009, 2010). While the associated changes mentioned above were observed across the fifteen weeks of a mindful communication course, hypothetically these same relationships may hold within micro-analytic analyses of the unfolding of communication
  • 37. processes within a single interaction. For example, a student who took the mindful communication course wrote an Application Journal entry (direct quotations from this entry are quoted below) that exemplifies many of the cognitive strategies described above. She had loaned money to a friend during spring break. After a month had gone by, her friend had only repaid a portion of the debt, yet the student observed her friend spending money on herself while claiming she was broke. On a later occasion when the student observed her friend wearing some new clothes, she noticed herself becoming upset by an increasing awareness of muscular tension in her body. Subsequently, she felt a strong urge to lash out at her friend in accusatory manner; however, realizing that ‘‘a situation is more than the emotions you experience first when confronted with a stimulus,’’ she was able to remain nonreactive as she ‘‘looked past the anger.’’ In so doing, she was able to refrain from blaming her
  • 38. friend for the problem at hand. The student realized that she had never spoken with her friend about how her previous purchases had upset her, and she considered the possibility that her friend was unaware of how much it bothered her to see her buying clothes for herself before repaying the debt. Once she was able to positively reappraise the situation in this manner, she chose to respond assertively rather than aggressively by ‘‘speaking [her] emotions instead of bursting out in anger.’’ This example illustrates one role mindfulness can play in adaptive communication as evidenced by the results of our study. Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research There are several limitations to this study. First, the relatively small sample size may have resulted in a lack of statistical power to detect between- groups differences of smaller magnitude effect sizes. Nonetheless, significant effects of training were identified. Second, the lack of random assignment to mindful
  • 39. communication and standard communication curricula leaves the present findings vulnerable to selection threats to internal validity. Although participants did not significantly differ at baseline on dispositional mindfulness and the other cognitive strategy variables assessed in the present investigation, it is possible that there were systematic differences on unmeasured variables that may have led to the observed between-groups differences. Hence, future studies aimed to evaluate mindfulness as a mechanism for improved communication skills in communication curricula should employ randomized Mechanisms of Mindfulness 417 controlled designs with larger sample sizes. Furthermore, future studies should examine what other factors may be involved in the facilitation of positive reappraisal, in light of the fact that the comparison group evidenced significant improvements in
  • 40. this cognitive strategy. An additional limitation is that the present investigation did not attempt to measure communication skills directly. However, given that the pedagogy of the mindful communication course being studied defines mindful communicators as those who communicate productively as a result of recognizing and responding to the uniqueness of each situation such that responses that are unproductive in one instance may be precisely what is needed in another, designing an instrument that would measure such an ability is a difficult task. A more meaningful assessment methodology may be to observe mindful communication as it occurs (Ucok, 2007). One approach for conducting such analysis may be to measure students’ cognitive, emotional, and psychophysiological responses during a challenging interaction. Using micro-analytic research methodologies to probe the unfolding of online emotion regulatory processes over time (for an example of such methods, see Sheppes & Meiran, 2007), randomized
  • 41. experiments could examine the effects of mindfulness practice relative to placebo control on the sequential generation of mindful states and positive reappraisals in coping with communication challenges to offer a fuller test of the mindful coping model. The present investigation, therefore, paves the way for controlled, experimental research. Practical Applications The present research suggests that implementing mindfulness training into curricula that is geared toward the advance of practical communication skills may be especially fruitful, particularly for the development of those skills that will help students productively negotiate emotionally charged situations. Given the stress and demands often present in higher education, these communication skills may serve college students particularly well during their interactions with teachers, peers, adminis- trators, and staff as they work toward successful completion of their degree programs.
  • 42. The ability to identify and express emotions exhibited by the mindfulness group in this study is linked with what some researchers refer to as emotional intelligence (Mayer & Salovey, 1997), which has been associated with increased positive affect, decreased levels of depression, and reduced anxiety (Brown & Ryan, 2003), all of which may be useful for achieving satisfying and productive communication exchanges. Furthermore, it is important for communication professors to examine the extent to which traditional classroom activities and coursework may promote a tendency toward students becoming increasingly judgmental of others and/or themselves. The results of this study suggest that such judgments are linked to decreased positive reappraisal, which can increase conflict and interfere with collaboration. Conversely, training in mindful communication may foster learning environments characterized by teamwork and cooperation. In sum, the concept of
  • 43. mindfulness that is beginning to make its way from the health and psychology fields into the study of communication may be particularly useful as a tool for increasing 418 D. C. Huston et al. students’ awareness of emotional reactions to interactions, reappraising those interactions as growth or learning opportunities, and regulating verbal interchange in a constructive manner. References Adelman, M.B. (2010, November). Critical issues for communication: Solitude and distraction. Presented at the annual meeting of the NCA 96th annual convention, San Francisco, CA. Baer, R.A., Smith, G.T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27�45. Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238�246.
  • 44. Bower, J., Low, C., Moskowitz, J., Sepah, S., & Epel, E. (2008). Benefit finding and physical health: Positive psychological changes and enhanced allostasis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 223�244. Brown, K.W., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822�848. Bstan-dzin-rgya-mtsho, & Cutler, H.C. (1998). The art of happiness: A handbook for living. New York, NY: Riverhead Books. Burgoon, J.K., Berger, C.R., & Waldron, V.R. (2000). Mindfulness and interpersonal communica- tion. Journal of Social Issues, 56(1), 105�127. Burns, A.B., Brown, J.S., Sachs-Ericsson, N., Plant, E.A., Curtis, J.T., Fredrickson, B.L., et al. (2008). Upward spirals of positive emotion and coping: Replication, extension, and initial exploration of neurochemical substrates. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 360�370. Burr, W.R. (1990). Beyond I-statements in family communication. Family Relations, 39(3), 266� 273. Chinn Swartz, B. (2008, November). Manifesting mindfulness: Compassion, yogic philosophy, and
  • 45. flow in the classroom. Presented at the annual meeting of the NCA 94th annual convention, San Diego, CA. Cleaver, G. (1987). Marriage enrichment by means of a structred communication programme. Family Relations, 36, 49�54. Corbeil, R.R., Quayhagen, M.P., & Quayhagen, M. (1999). Intervention effects on dementia caregiving interaction: A stress-adaptation modeling approach. Journal of Aging & Health, 11(1), 79�95. Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication and emotional life. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company. Folkes, V. (1985). Mindlessness or mindfulness: A partial replication and extension of Lange, Blank, & Chanowitz. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 600�604. Folkman, S. (1997). Positive psychological states and coping with severe stress. Social Science & Medicine, 45(8), 1207�1221. Ford, J., & Ford, L. (1995). The role of conversations in producing intentional change in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 541�570.
  • 46. Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1367�1378. Fredrickson, B.L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13(2), 172�175. Garland, E.L., Gaylord, S., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2011). Positive reappraisal mediates the stress- reductive effects of mindfulness. Mindfulness, 2(1), 59�67. Garland, E., Gaylord, S., & Park, J. (2009). The role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing, 5, 37�44. Mechanisms of Mindfulness 419 Garland, E.L., Fredrickson, B.L., Kring, A.M., Johnson, D.P., Meyer, P.S., & Penn, D.L. (2010). Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: Insights from the broaden-and-build theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 849�864. Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric
  • 47. features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 141�149. Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation, and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 1311�1327. Goldstein, J. (1993). Insight meditation: The practice of freedom. Boston, MA: Shambhala Publications. Helgeson, V.S., Reynolds, K.A., & Tomich, P.L. (2006). A meta-analytic review of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(5), 797�816. Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P.M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424�453. Huston, D. (2010a). Communicating mindfully: Mindfulness- based communication and emotional intelligence (5th ed.). Mason, OH: Cengage Learning. Huston, D. (2010b). Waking up to ourselves: The use of mindfulness meditation and emotional intelligence in the teaching of communications. New Directions for Community Colleges: Contemplative Teaching and Learning, 151, 39�50.
  • 48. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain based on the practice of mindfulness meditation. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33�47. King, P.E., & Sawyer, C.R. (1998). Mindfulness, mindlessness and communication instruction. Communication Education, 47, 326�336. Kramer, G. (2007). Insight dialogue: The interpersonal path to freedom. Boston, MA: Shambhala. Langer, E., Blank, A., & Chanowitz, B. (1978). The mindlessness of ostensibly thoughtful action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 635�642. Mayer, J.D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators (pp. 3�31). New York, NY: Basic Books. Motley, M.T. (1986a). Consciousness and intention in communication: A preliminary model and methodological approaches. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 50, 3�23. Motley, M.T. (1986b). The production of verbal slips and double entendres as clues to the efficiency of normal speech production. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, 275�293. Motley, M.T. (1990). On whether one can(not) not communicate: An examination via traditional
  • 49. communication postulates. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 54, 1�20. Rosenberg, M.B. (2003). Nonviolent communication: A language of life (2nd ed.). Encinitas, CA: Puddle Dancer. Schroevers, M., Kraaij, V., & Garnefski, N. (2007). Goal disturbance, cognitive coping strategies, and psychological adjustment to different types of stressful life event. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 413�423. Shapiro, S.L., & Schwartz, G.E. (2000). Intentional systemic mindfulness: An integrative model for self-regulation and health. Advances in Mind�Body Medicine, 16(2), 128�134. Shapiro, S.L., Carlson, L.E., & Astin, J.A. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373�386. Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2007). Better late than never? On the dynamics of online regulation of sadness using distraction and cognitive reappraisal. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(11), 1518�1532. Stroud, S.R. (2010). Toward a Deweyan theory of communicative mindfulness, Imagination. Cognition and Personality, 30(1), 57�75. 420 D. C. Huston et al.
  • 50. Tugade, M.M., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 320�333. Ucok, I.O. (2007, November). ‘‘Dropping into being’’: Exploring mindfulness as lived experience. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the NCA 93rd annual convention, Chicago, IL. Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Advances in Mind�Body Medicine, 21(3/4), 33�49. Williams, M., Teasdale, J., Segal, Z., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2007). The mindful way through depression: Freeing yourself from chronic unhappiness. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Witvliet, C.V., Knoll, R.W., Hinman, N.G., & DeYoung, P.A. (2010). Compassion-focused reappraisal, benefit-focused reappraisal, and rumination after an interpersonal offense: Emotion-regulation implications for subjective emotion, linguistic responses, and physiol- ogy. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(3), 226�242.
  • 51. Mechanisms of Mindfulness 421 Copyright of Journal of Applied Communication Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Discussion 1: Mindfulness and Perception Checking It takes a little bit of mindfulness and a little bit of attention to others to be a good listener, which helps cultivate emotional nurturing and engagement. —Deepak Chopra, Indian-American author, alternative medicine practitioner, physician, and public speaker Throughout the day, people experience interactions with others that may cause misperceptions. Often people react to a situation before taking the time to clarify misunderstandings. Reacting to a situation before being fully aware of the meaning behind another’s behavior or words often leads to additional confusion and escalating negative feedbacks. In his quote, Deepak Chopra advocates practicing mindfulness to create successful interpersonal communication environments. What does mindfulness mean? How can it help you build your communication competence? How can you train yourself to be aware of interaction as it occurs and respond appropriately to achieve positive results? How can you be sure your perception of an individual’s behavior is accurate? In this discussion, consider an interpersonal communication
  • 52. scenario involving confusion on the part of both participants. You examine how mindfulness and perception checking can change interpersonal communication outcomes To prepare for responding to two (2) colleagues discussion, pay particular attention to the following learning resources Review this week’s Learning Resources, especially: · Huston, D. C., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. S. (2011). Mechanisms of mindfulness in communication training. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 39(4), 406–421. · TED. (Producer). (2012). Amy Cuddy: Your body language may shape who you are [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.ted.com/talks/amy_cuddy_your_ body_language_sh apes_who_you_are Assignment Respond to at least two of your peers' postings in one or more of the following ways: · Discuss how your interpretation of the interpersonal communication demonstrated within the video may differ from that of your peers. · Share an insight about what you learned from having read your peer’s post and discuss how and why your peer’s posting resonated with you professionally and/or personally or how it will impact your own mindfulness with regards to interpersonal communication. · Offer an example, from your experience or observation that validates what your peer presented. · Offer specific suggestions that will help your peer build upon his or her own mindfulness in interpersonal communication and explain why you think your suggestions will be of benefit based on what you learned this week and your own experiences. · Share how something your peer discussed changed the way you view misperceptions and how you will improve your own
  • 53. mindfulness. · 3 – 4 paragraph response per each colleagues · No plagiarism · APA citing Bottom of Form 1st Colleague – Natasha Discussion 1 - Week 2 Top of Form Natasha Mills Mindfulness and Perception Checking Top of Form Alberto and Kathy’s interpersonal behaviors were significantly the result of previous experiences witnessed by Alberto, as well as Kathy’s verbal and non-verbal expressions. After ending the call, Kathy says that she cannot take it anymore, a statement that is accompanied by an exasperated face. On the other hand, in an attempt to comfort Kathy, Alberto states that it is a rite of passage in the organization to be pressured by Mr. Gregowski (Laureate Education Producer, 2014). Therefore, factors of previous experiences on Alberto’s part and verbal and nonverbal cues on Kathy’s part are evident in the communication behavior of the two. Communication is a complex process consisting of various elements in play every time people communicate (Quintanilla & Wahl, 2020). For effectiveness and excellence during communication, individuals ought to consider how these
  • 54. elements interact when they communicate. In Alberto and Kathy’s situation, these elements are displayed as personal variables that significantly dictate their interpersonal communication behaviors. For instance, the personal variables in Kathy’s case include being new to the job, being female, as Alberto puts it, and complementing Mr. Gregowski’s power pose by adopting a less powerful pose (TED Producer, 2012). The personal variables displayed by Alberto include being perceptive about situations and the lack of mindfulness during communication. One precise instance of misperception in the video is when Alberto tells Kathy that she should let another guy take over the account because she is a woman, making her sensitive and unable to handle a guy like Mr. Gregowski (Laureate Education Producer, 2014). This is a misperception because Alberto relies on previous occurrences to make this judgment without giving Kathy a chance to prove whether or not she can handle Mr. Gregowski. Therefore, Alberto’s perception is based on Kathy’s gender, leading him to pass judgment on Kathy’s abilities. Simply, the misperception crippled Alberto’s objectivity during the communication process with Kathy. Mindfulness could have had a significant positive impact on the verbal and nonverbal communication between Alberto and Kathy. This is because mindfulness causes people to reserve judgment when communicating with others, thereby influencing the outcomes of interactions (Huston et al., 2011). Hence, mindfulness would have caused Alberto to reserve his judgments about Kathy until he has fully understood the situation. “Mindfulness in the field of communication has mainly been considered in terms of how consciously people plan their approach to a communication exchange or the extent to which they identify and respond to relevant or irrelevant information in a given situation” (Huston et al., 2011, 407). I once observed an interaction between my colleagues during
  • 55. the compilation of an annual report. The employees of the company always experienced a lot of pressure during the completion of this task due to the processes involved, but more specifically, the tracing and compilation of records. I happened to be in the breakroom when I heard three colleagues inquiring about each other’s progress. When one stated that he was still a bit behind with respect to the deadline, one of the other colleagues asked him what it was this time that he would present as an excuse for not finishing on time. However, the other colleague asked why he was lurking behind, as well as how he could be of help. Mindfulness towards the employee that was still behind in finishing up the annual report was only exercised by one colleague. The other colleague rushed to pass judgment instead of assessing to what extent he should respond to the information that the colleague provided about finishing the report (Huston et al., 2011). As a result, the colleague felt small, incompetent, and less powerful, as was displayed by his nonverbal cues (TED Producer, 2012). The application of mindfulness in this situation by this one colleague would have reduced the negative reaction that he had towards the colleague that was experiencing a challenge in completing his annual report. The outcome of mindfulness would have been a positive reappraisal of the situation (Huston et al., 2011). Huston, D. C., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. S. (2011). Mechanisms of mindfulness in communication training. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 39(4), 406–421. Laureate Education (Producer). (2014). Interpersonal communication case study: Interpersonal conflict [Video file]. Retrieved from https://class.waldenu.edu. (SEE ATTACHMENT FOR TRANSCRIPT) Quintanilla, K. M., & Wahl, S. T. (2020). Business and professional communication: KEYS for workplace excellence (4th ed.) Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
  • 56. TED. (Producer). (2012). Amy Cuddy: Your body language may shape who you are [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.ted.com/talks/amy_cuddy_your_body_languag e_shapes_who_you_are. Bottom of Form Bottom of Form Bottom of Form Top of Form Bottom of Form 2nd Colleague – Douglas Hayes Discussion 1 - Week 2Top of Form Top of Form Douglas Hayes Top of Form The interpersonal communication between Kathy and Alberto was abysmal. First, Alberto calling her "sweete" in a professional environment was insulting and demeaning. Secondly, telling her that she needs to give the account over to another man was also demining. She is already stressed, and this only compounded the issue. Thirdly, putting her hands on anyone without their permission is never a good idea and could be a sexual harassment issue. Alberto putting his hands on Kathy's shoulders was a significant problem that can affect their communication. This can have a detrimental ripple effect on their communication, not only for this instance but in future communication. As chapter six details, "in order to develop professional excellence in the workplace, you must have clear understanding of the line between personal and professional relationships, as well as what constitutes professional versus personal communication" (Quintanilla, Pg. 133). It does continue that having a personal relationship with coworkers is essential, but Alberto crossed the
  • 57. line with his advances toward Kathy. What Alberto should have done was to be mindful of Kathy's feelings toward what was going on. Mindfulness teaches us that if we use positive reappraisal, "which is the cognitive process through which stressful events are re-construed as benign, beneficial, and/or meaningful." (Huston, 2011) We can initially take a breath before reacting to the situation impulsively and flip a stressful situation into a conversation. If Alberto were using this, he would have asked if Kathy needed to talk about it and then (using her name) talked to Kathy from a chair or stood across the room. I have seen lots of these interactions where one party is dominant and demeaning over another in my career. It is not always male dominance over females, but it always ends badly. I was in a situation like this where a supervisor was doing this to an employee. The was a female supervisor doing this to a male employee, but the situation was very similar. They were having a hard time with a situation, and she was telling him that he should just quit if it were too hard. They would call him late at night to do reports and other things. The man eventually did quit because the stress got too much. All the rest of us could see how the job was affecting him physically. He started to drink to cope with the stress, and it was not long after that he quit. It was more critical for them to get the job done than to see what they were doing. Mindfulness is "a means of expanding one's experience of each moment by nurturing qualities such as acceptance and patience that allow for observation "of what is occurring both internally and externally." (Huston, 2011) If the supervisors had used mindfulness, they would have seen the damage they were doing to this person. I would have suggested that they evaluate their times to work after hours. They should evaluate how they are communicating with people and what time is appropriate to do that. Sending emails at 1 am and asking questions is inappropriate, and they should also not call after hours to demand data. The supervisors should use mindfulness
  • 58. techniques to understand that it is appropriate to communicate and use patience in all situations. Refference: Huston, D. C., Garland, E. L., & Farb, N. A. S. (2011). Mechanisms of Mindfulness in Communication Training. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 39(4), 406–421. Quintanilla, K. M., & Wahl, S. T. (2020). Business and Professional Communication (4th ed.). Sage publishing Ltd. Bottom of Form Bottom of Form Bottom of Form