SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 11
Download to read offline
British Journal of Anaesthesia 89 (1): 156±66 (2002)




In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia and its components
                                      J. F. Antognini1* and E. Carstens2
1
 Department of Anesthesiology, TB-170, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA. 2Section
 of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior, 193 Briggs Hall, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA
                                              95616, USA
                                                  *Corresponding author




               Br J Anaesth 2002; 89: 156±66
               Keywords: anaesthesia, general; theories of anaesthetic action




One of the fundamental problems facing researchers of             ness occurred before signi®cant or complete analgesia. That
anaesthetic mechanisms is linking a particular effect on a        is, unconsciousness simply became part and parcel of
receptor to a speci®c clinical effect. Thus, to fully under-      general anaesthesia. From a practical aspect, unconscious-
stand how anaesthetics act we must approach anaesthetic           ness became important as conscious patients, even if they
mechanisms at multiple levels. Ultimately, receptor effects       had complete analgesia, would probably talk, which would
must be viewed within the context of the clinical charac-         disrupt the surgical procedure. Indeed, an unconscious and
teristics of general anaesthesia. Does a particular anaes-        immobile patient permits surgical procedures that are
thetic alter receptor function at clinically relevant             limited only by the available technology and skill of the
anaesthetic concentrations? Is there evidence (anatomical,        surgeon.
neurophysiological, pharmacological) that a speci®c recep-           Within a year of Morton's public demonstration of ether
tor is involved in a clinically relevant neurophysiological       anaesthesia, John Snow published an account of the
process, such as memory?                                          pharmacological and physiological effects of ether.86
   Anaesthesia can be de®ned in arbitrary terms, although         These effects were divided into stages, progressing from
practical considerations often govern speci®c de®nitions.         consciousness to deep coma, muscle ¯accidity and respira-
Thus, from a practical standpoint, most people would              tory paralysis.
include unconsciousness, amnesia and immobility as                   Guedel described four stages of ether anaesthesia,46
important end-points. On the other hand, reduction of stress      similar to those described by Snow. In the ®rst stage the
hormones is not necessarily an absolute, required anaes-          patient was conscious but had analgesia (Fig. 1). In the
thetic end-point. In this review we will examine the various      second stage (the delirium stage) the patient exhibited
components of general anaesthesia. That is, what are the          excessive motor activity, even to the point of violence. Eye
clinical goals we seek to achieve? What are the side-effects      movements were irregular and erratic, as was breathing. The
we hope to avoid? This latter component is important              third stage represented the surgical stage; four planes were
because an anaesthetic (or any drug) is only as good as the       originally described, with increasing anaesthetic depth as
minimization of its side-effects.                                 the patient progressed from the ®rst to the last plane.
                                                                  Respiration became progressively weaker. In the fourth
                                                                  stage of anaesthesia, respiratory paralysis occurred. These
Historical aspects                                                classic signs have broad application, but it is clear that not
Before Morton's display of ether anaesthesia, patients were       all anaesthetics cause the same progression of clinical signs.
accustomed to being conscious during surgeryÐpainfully            Newer anaesthetics, in particular i.v. anaesthetics such as
so. There was no a priori expectation that they should be         propofol, may not exhibit such signs. One wonders how
unconscious. Their hope was not so much to be unconscious         much of this difference is due to pharmacokinetic as
but rather to be pain-free. After all, few people would           opposed to pharmacodynamic reasons. Many of the newer
voluntarily give up consciousness if complete analgesia           inhaled anaesthetics have low blood-gas solubilities and
were otherwise possible. It was the characteristic of ether,      thus patients may pass from one stage to the next relatively
chloroform and subsequent anaesthetics that unconscious-          quickly. Likewise, with i.v. anaesthetics patients are


                   Ó The Board of Management and Trustees of the British Journal of Anaesthesia 2002
In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia


                                                                            remain `within normal limits'. An anaesthetist must balance
                                                                            the demands of all three.
                                                                               For practical purposes we de®ne general anaesthesia as
                                                                            the presence of unconsciousness, amnesia and immobility
                                                                            (in response to noxious stimulation). Analgesia is not always
                                                                            included in the list. Analgesia might be an important indirect
                                                                            means to help achieve all of the goals of anaesthesia, but is it
                                                                            essential? We answer `no'. Analgesics reduce or eliminate
                                                                            pain. Pain is the conscious awareness of a noxious stimulus.
                                                                            Anaesthetized patients are unconscious. Thus, they cannot
                                                                            perceive pain. Therefore analgesia is not directly relevant
                                                                            and may be excluded as a necessary component of general
                                                                            anaesthesia. However, analgesia would be desirable in the
                                                                            rare cases in which patients regain consciousness during
                                                                            surgery and remember the experience. Moreover, drugs with
                                                                            analgesic properties are often used during anaesthesia and
                                                                            can be important for patient management, i.e. to control
                                                                            haemodynamic perturbations. We also exclude lack of
                                                                            haemodynamic responses as an absolute requirement.
                                                                            Although tight haemodynamic control is desirable in some
                                                                            patients, increased heart rate and increased blood pressure
                                                                            are not, by themselves, harmful to many patients. Finally,
Fig 1 Guedel's signs for ether anaesthesia included muscle tone,
breathing pattern and eye movements. Stage 1 was marked by analgesia
                                                                            pre-emptive analgesia could be included as a desirable goal,
and consciousness. In Stage 2, the patient became unconscious, breathing    but it is not an absolute requirement for general anaesthesia.
was erratic but delirium could occur, leading to an excitement phase. In
Stage 3, surgical anaesthesia occurred, with four planes or levels
describing increasing depth until breathing became weak. Stage 4 was        Neurophysiological processes pertinent to
marked by respiratory paralysis and death. Other signs included pupil       anaesthesia
size. Newer anaesthetics and `balanced' anaesthesia have rendered some
of these signs less reliable. (Based on reference 46.)
                                                                            Memory
                                                                            Memory formation occurs at a variety of sites in the brain,
brought to deeper stages rapidly by bolus administration.                   including the hippocampus, amygdala, prefrontal cortex and
Thus, there are few data comparing these newer anaesthetics                 other cortical sensory and motor areas.37 54 Anaesthetics
with older ones, such as ether.                                             may affect any or all of these sites and thereby result in
                                                                            amnesia.
                                                                               There are two types of memory that are usually discussed
                                                                            in relation to anaesthesia: implicit and explicit recall.37 54
What de®nes anaesthesia?                                                    Explicit recall is what most people usually describe as
How do we decide what are the essential components of                       memory: they can explicitly recall a speci®c event, such as a
general anaesthesia? What are the non-essential but desir-                  football game or their wedding day. Implicit recall occurs
able goals? These decisions may be made with practical,                     when an individual cannot remember a certain event, but
scienti®c, theoretical and historical considerations.                       upon speci®c testing there is evidence that information has
   We ®rst differentiate `general anaesthesia' from a `gen-                 been retained. There are several studies that suggest that, at
eral anaesthetic'. The former de®nes a pharmacologically                    low anaesthetic concentrations, implicit memory formation
induced physiological state in which the essential goals of                 may occur while explicit recall is blocked.21 23 42 The
general anaesthesia are achieved. This may be accom-                        speci®c sites where this action occurs are not known.
plished with a single agent or with a variety of drugs. A
general anaesthetic, however, is a drug that, by itself,
achieves all of the essential goals of general anaesthesia.                 Consciousness
   What are regarded as the essential goals of general                      The physiological processes and anatomical sites that give
anaesthesia can depend on the perspective of the de®ner. For                rise to consciousness are poorly understood, and the sites
the patient, an important goal will be amnesiaÐhe or she                    where anaesthetics induce unconsciousness are understood
does not want to remember anything. A close second would                    even less. Several sites are likely to participate in con-
probably be unconsciousnessÐthe patient does not want to                    sciousness, including the cerebral cortex, thalamus and
be awake during surgery. A surgeon wants a still patient. A                 reticular formation.66 88 All of those structures are affected
cardiologist wants the patient's blood pressure and pulse to                by anaesthetics.6 22 84 Alkire and colleagues have examined


                                                                        157
Antognini and Carstens


                                                                              noxious stimulus (Fig. 2). Such re¯exes are undoubtedly
                                                                              also involved in initiating more complex behavioural
                                                                              responses to escape from or otherwise cope with a
                                                                              threatening environment. The motor consequence of a
                                                                              noxious stimulus might thus consist of a simple withdrawal
                                                                              re¯ex in which the stimulated extremity is pulled away from
                                                                              the stimulus, or of a violent escape response in which the
                                                                              animal uses all its limbs. Alternatively, the animal may
                                                                              orient towards the source of stimulation and even attack. All
                                                                              of these motor responses are eliminated by anaesthetics.
                                                                                 A noxious stimulus activates peripheral nociceptors that
                                                                              transmit impulses to second-order neurones in the dorsal
                                                                              horn of the spinal cord.96 These second-order neurones may
                                                                              synapse onto motor neurones either directly or indirectly via
                                                                              higher-order interneurones. Activation of these motor
                                                                              neurones leads to muscle contraction that, depending on
                                                                              the extent and pattern of activation, results in a nociceptive
                                                                              re¯ex (e.g. withdrawal). Nociceptive input is also likely to
                                                                              activate central pattern generators, which coordinate move-
                                                                              ment such that the animal can move ef®ciently in a walk,
                                                                              trot or gallop.
Fig 2 Noxious stimulation results in impulse transmission to the dorsal
horn, where second-order neurones might send impulses to ascending
tracts (such as the spinothalamic tract) or to motor neurones that            Neurotransmitter systems
innervate muscles that initiate a motor response, such as an escape or        A wide variety of neurotransmitter systems is likely to be
withdrawal response. The ascending transmission of these impulses
                                                                              affected by anaesthetics but it is unclear how these actions
`activates' the brain and results in increased arousal. The location of the
spinothalamic tract varies from species to species.                           translate into clinical effects.60 In addition, it is unclear if
                                                                              presynaptic or postsynaptic effects are more important. The
                                                                              GABA receptor has been the focus of intense research and
the effect of halothane, iso¯urane and propofol on cerebral
                                                                              there is ample evidence that alteration of GABAergic
metabolism [using positron emission tomography (PET)] as
                                                                              neurotransmission could alter consciousness, memory and
an indirect method of investigating sites of anaesthetic
                                                                              nociceptive responses. Other neurotransmitters are also
action.1±5 These anaesthetics depress metabolism in the
                                                                              likely to be involved in these processes.60 For example,
cortex, thalamus and reticular formation, as one would
                                                                              anaesthetics affect the physiology and pharmacology of
expect. Whether the effects at each site contribute equally to
                                                                              glutamate and nitric oxide.71 Antagonists of N-methyl-D-
unconsciousness or whether one site is more crucial than the
                                                                              aspartate (NMDA), such as MK-801, drastically reduce the
others remains to be elucidated. Fiset and colleagues
                                                                              minimum alveolar concentration (MAC),82 but is this a
investigated the effects of propofol using PET scanning
                                                                              direct and relevant anaesthetic action or does it merely
and found that propofol produced greater metabolic reduc-
                                                                              re¯ect the importance of glutamate in the initiation of
tion in the medial thalamus and other brain sites associated
                                                                              nociceptive re¯exes, such that simply by blocking glutamate
with arousal.38 A logical conclusion from their data is that
                                                                              transmission one can prevent nociceptive responses?
propofol produced unconsciousness through a preferential
                                                                              Acetylcholine and the cholinergic neurotransmitter system
effect on sites associated with arousal. Alkire and others
                                                                              are also possibly involved in anaesthesia. Physostigmine has
have suggested that anaesthetics might affect thalamocor-
                                                                              been shown to reverse partially the sedative effects of
tical loops that appear to be critical for conscious aware-
                                                                              propofol and halothane.48 65 The cerebral activation that is
ness.3 In an intriguing study, Devor and Zalkind injected
                                                                              associated with increased arousal is also associated with
pentobarbital into a discrete area of the rat mesopontine
                                                                              cortical release of acetylcholine.52 Sensory stimulation can
tegmental area bilaterally, producing unconsciousness and
                                                                              likewise cause cortical release of acetylcholine,61 and
analgesia.28 Clearly, however, considerably more work is
                                                                              iso¯urane can affect this release.83
needed to understand where anaesthetics act in the brain to
in¯uence consciousness.
                                                                              Anaesthetic goals

Nociceptive re¯exes                                                           Immobility
Nociceptive re¯exes have evolved as a protective mechan-                      Anaesthetic end-points can sometimes be described in
ism to withdraw the body (or part of the body) from a                         relation to the concentration of anaesthetic required to block


                                                                          158
In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia


                                                                            Amnesia and unconsciousness
                                                                            Amnesia and unconsciousness are among the ®rst end-
                                                                            points to be reached when an anaesthetic is administered.
                                                                            The concentration that results in a patient passing from
                                                                            wakefulness to unconsciousness is called `MAC-awake',
                                                                            ®rst described by Stoelting and colleagues.90 They meas-
                                                                            ured the concentrations of methoxy¯urane, halothane, ether
                                                                            and ¯uroxene that just permitted and prevented conscious-
                                                                            ness (as de®ned by the response to verbal stimuli); MAC-
                                                                            awake was the average of these. Two subject groups were
                                                                            used. One consisted of volunteers who did not undergo a
                                                                            surgical procedure, while the other group did. Because the
                                                                            anaesthetics studied were not equally divided between the
Fig 3 These dose±response curves for various end-points were developed      two groups, it is unclear how the presence of post-surgical
from a variety of studies. Note that the curves for memory and              pain might have altered the results, inasmuch as noxious
consciousness are close to each other. The effective dose that results in   stimulation is likely to result in cerebral activation and
50% of patients having unconsciousness is called `MAC-awake'. This
value is about 30±40% of MAC, although there are differences among
                                                                            increased probability of consciousness. The MAC-awake
anaesthetics. MAC-awake might be greater during the noxious                 for ether (as a fraction of MAC) was greater than that for the
stimulation of surgery. The anaesthetic concentration that blocks the       other anaesthetics, despite the fact that ether was studied
cardiovascular response is called `MAC-BAR'.                                only in volunteers. The MAC-awake varied between 0.5 and
                                                                            0.65 MAC. Another source of potential error is that MAC
                                                                            values were obtained from historical controls. In a critical
movementÐthe MAC (Fig. 3). This is essentially the                          editorial,95 Waud and Waud placed limits on the interpret-
median effective dose (ED50), the dose that causes a                        ation of the work of Stoelting and colleagues90 (but see also
particular effect in 50% of people or animals.                              reference 20). Speci®cally, examination of only one point
   The MAC concept was developed in 1965 as a way to                        on a curve or set of curves (e.g. the point at which 50% of
compare equipotent concentrations of anaesthetics.34 The                    patients are awake) says nothing about the overall curve
measurement of MAC depends essentially on achieving a                       and, hence, relative potency and effect. They also ques-
stable end-tidal anaesthetic concentration, applying a                      tioned whether examination of MAC-awake and other
standard noxious stimulus and observing whether move-                       variants of MAC would yield any useful information
ment occurs.34 74 Positive movement was arbitrarily de®ned                  regarding anaesthetic mechanisms. Many investigators
as `gross and purposeful', although any other movement                      now believe that anaesthetics act at different sites, and
type could have been included. Thus, a pawing motion or                     therefore knowledge regarding the anaesthetic concentra-
turning of the head towards the stimulus are usually                        tion required to achieve certain end-points (unconsciousness
considered positive, while coughing, straining, chewing                     and immobility) is directly relevant to determining anaes-
and stiffening are considered negative.74 In general, many                  thetic mechanisms.36 It is interesting to note that an
investigators also consider simple withdrawal of the stimu-                 editorial44 published nearly 30 yr after the work of
lated extremity as being negative. What is important is that                Stoelting and colleagues and Waud and Waud commented
in any individual study there is consistency as to what is                  on the effect of fentanyl on MAC-awake56 (Fig. 4), stating
positive and what is negative movement.                                     that such studies were critical to our elucidation of
   The noxious stimulus must be supramaximal. That is,                      anaesthetic mechanisms.
increasing the stimulus intensity must not result in further                   Dwyer and colleagues determined that consciousness was
increases in anaesthetic requirements. A mechanical stimu-                  present at iso¯urane concentrations (0.3 MAC) at which
lus is usually used, such as a clamp placed across the tail or a            memory formation was impaired.30 Furthermore, the
hind paw. The stimulus is applied for 1 min or until                        authors concluded that, at equipotent MAC concentrations,
movement occurs, after which the anaesthetic concentration                  iso¯urane was more potent than nitrous oxide as regards the
is changed (up or down, depending on the response). MAC                     production of unconsciousness and the reduction of explicit
is de®ned as the average of the two anaesthetic concentra-                  memory formation. The volunteers in this study were not
tions that are observed to just permit and just prevent the                 subjected to noxious stimulation, as might occur during
movement, respectively. In humans undergoing surgery, a                     surgery, and so it is unclear whether the amount of
skin incision is often used to determine the MAC, but this                  anaesthetic needed for amnesia and unconsciousness is
stimulus can be applied only once. A population MAC can                     increased during noxious stimulation; presumably it is. In an
still be obtained by adjusting the anaesthetic concentration                earlier study, however, this group of investigators showed
in subsequent patients so that about half of these patients                 that iso¯urane at 0.6 MAC or greater prevented conscious-
move and half do not move.                                                  ness and memory formation (explicit and implicit) during


                                                                        159
Antognini and Carstens


                                                                                 Interestingly, Steriade and colleagues have reported that
                                                                              cortical activity (as assessed by the EEG) is more sensitive
                                                                              to anaesthesia than subcortical (e.g. thalamic) activity.89 We
                                                                              have made similar observations (Fig. 5). The data of both
                                                                              Steriade and colleagues and ourselves were obtained at
                                                                              anaesthetic concentrations greatly exceeding those required
                                                                              to prevent memory formation and consciousness.
                                                                              Nonetheless, it is theoretically possible that anaesthetic-
                                                                              induced unconsciousness may occur primarily as the result
                                                                              of anaesthetic action within the cerebral cortex. Dwyer and
                                                                              colleagues collected EEG data in iso¯urane-anaesthetized
                                                                              patients to determine if processed EEG variables might
                                                                              correlate with clinically relevant anaesthetic end-points.32
                                                                              Spectral edge frequency, median power and total power
Fig 4 Effect of fentanyl on sevo¯urane requirements. Note that MAC-           were among the variables examined. The authors were
BAR (the minimum anaesthetic concentration required to block                  unable to make predictions regarding patient movement,
adrenergic responses) is affected the most, suggesting that analgesia is an   memory formation or consciousness.32 Indeed, human and
important tool for the control of haemodynamic responses to noxious           rat data suggest that marked EEG depression resulting from
stimulation. MAC-awake (the concentration that results in
                                                                              iso¯urane and thiopental does not correlate with movement
unconsciousness) is affected least and merges with MAC and MAC-
BAR. Thus a patient given opiates might not move or have a                    resulting from noxious stimulation,51 77 although haemo-
haemodynamic response to noxious stimulation but might be conscious.          dynamic responses to laryngoscopy and tracheal intubation
This is not unexpected, because there are numerous reports of patients        might correlate with EEG changes.79
awake during what is primarily an opiate `anaesthetic'. Based on the             There is substantial evidence that some anaesthetics
work of Katoh and colleagues.56 57
                                                                              prevent movement via a spinal cord action,10 16 75 78 but is
                                                                              there any reason to believe that other end-points (amnesia,
surgical procedures.31 Ropcke and colleagues showed that
                          È                                                   unconsciousness) might be affected by a spinal cord action?
noxious stimulation can alter anaesthetic effects on the                      We believe that there is some indirect evidence. First,
electroencephalogram (EEG).81 They studied two groups of                      epidural anaesthesia decreases the amount of anaesthetic
des¯urane-anaesthetized patients, one with and one without                    required to prevent the movement that results from noxious
noxious surgical stimulation. At equal concentrations, the                    stimulation applied above the level of the block.50 This
group with surgical stimulation demonstrated EEG acti-                        might be due to an indirect action in the brain, such as
vation, whereas the non-stimulated group did not. These                       resetting of the brain's arousal level. Secondly, spinal and
data suggest that noxious stimulation during otherwise                        epidural anaesthetics decrease the amount of sedative/
adequate anaesthesia results in a shift towards increased                     hypnotics needed to achieve a certain level of
arousal.81                                                                    sedation,33 49 92 93 presumably by blocking afferent
   Learning and memory formation during anaesthesia                           impulses, and sedation is associated with amnesia and
depends on the circumstances under which stimuli are                          unconsciousness.43 Thirdly, volatile anaesthetics and i.v.
presented. Dutton and colleagues studied conditioned                          anaesthetics (such as propofol and thiopental) block
learning in rats anaesthetized with iso¯urane.29 They                         nociceptive impulses in the spinal cord.59 67 94 Such actions
found that fear-conditioning to a tone was less sensitive to                  blunt the ascending transmission of noxious impulses to the
iso¯urane than fear-conditioning to context (e.g. the                         brain, which is likely to diminish the arousal of the brain,
surrounding environment). Approximately 0.5 MAC iso-                          thereby decreasing the probability of memory formation and
¯urane was required to prevent fear-conditioning associated                   consciousness (Fig. 6). We have evidence that iso¯urane,
with the tone compared with 0.25 MAC for conditioning                         probably by a spinal cord action, might affect brain arousal.
associated with the context.29                                                When iso¯urane is selectively removed from the torso while
   Non-immobilizers are compounds that are predicted to be                    a constant concentration is kept in the brain, a noxious
anaesthetics on the basis of their physicochemical properties                 stimulus can increase brain arousal, as indicated by
but in fact are not. Originally these compounds were called                   increased neuronal activity in the reticular formation and
non-anaesthetics but one report has shown that they can                       thalamus and EEG desynchronization.12 18 We have made
suppress learning and hence memory.55 Amnesia is a                            similar observations with propofol.15
desired anaesthetic end-point, and thus it would be impre-
cise to call these compounds non-anaesthetics. Because
these molecules do not contribute to immobility, the                          Haemodynamic and neuroendocrine control
consensus is to label them non-immobilizers. These data                       Roizen and colleagues examined the ability of halothane,
offer further evidence that anaesthetic end-points are likely                 en¯urane, opiates and spinal anaesthesia to block the
to result from anaesthetic action at different sites.87                       cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses to skin inci-


                                                                          160
In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia




Fig 5 At the top are shown the bifrontal EEGs from a goat anaesthetized with iso¯urane at 1.5, 3.5 and 5% (3 min and 10 s samples). Below are
peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) representing the corresponding activity of a midbrain reticular formation (MRF) cell. In the ®rst two PSTHs the
arrows represent application of a clamp to the goat's lip for 1 min. This MRF cell is inhibited by the noxious stimulus, but at 3.5% iso¯urane the
spontaneous activity is increased and the cell is not completely inhibited by the noxious clamp. Note that at 3.5 and 5% iso¯urane the EEG is
isoelectric (with occasional spikes) but the MRF cell is still active.




sion.80 Halothane and en¯urane were added to 60% nitrous                    mentioned above, one reason may be the presence of
oxide. They described the blocking effect in terms of the                   analgesic properties. Iso¯urane, for example, appears to
anaesthetic concentration that blocked adrenergic responses                 have little or no analgesic effect at subanaesthetic concen-
(MAC-BAR).80 A positive response was arbitrarily de®ned                     trations. Petersen-Felix and colleagues did not detect any
as an increase of 10% or more in heart rate, blood pressure                 evidence of analgesia to noxious thermal, mechanical or
or norepinephrine values. Interestingly, while halothane and                electrical stimulation at iso¯urane concentrations ranging
an opiate±nitrous oxide technique could block adrenergic                    from 0.0 to 0.26%.73 Halothane, on the other hand, does
responses at clinically relevant concentrations, en¯urane                   have analgesic properties,85 although not all studies con-
could not. Spinal anaesthesia, as long as it blocked the pain               cur.91 Anaesthetic-induced sedation and unconsciousness
of incision, also blocked the neuroendocrine and cardio-                    makes interpretation of analgesia studies dif®cult.85 The
vascular response. Could the inability of en¯urane to block                 presence of analgesic properties will blunt the nociceptive
these responses be related to its poor analgesic properties?                responses (e.g. increased blood pressure and heart rate).
While it is tempting to think that the ability of halothane to              Although most studies suggest that anaesthetics have some
block the cardiovascular responses is in part due to direct                 analgesic properties or none, there is evidence that at
cardiovascular depression, en¯urane is one of the most                      relatively low anaesthetic concentration (0.1 MAC) hyper-
potent cardiovascular depressants, and yet it was unable to                 algesia might occur. Zhang and colleagues studied four
prevent the cardiovascular responses to incisions. This                     anaesthetics on hind paw withdrawal elicited by noxious
concept of MAC-BAR was investigated further by Zbinden                      stimulation in rats.100 Iso¯urane, halothane, nitrous oxide
and colleagues, who studied patients anaesthetized with                     and diethyl ether decreased withdrawal latency, suggesting
iso¯urane.98 99 Different stimuli were used, such as                        a hyperalgesic effect. Barbiturates are also associated with
trapezius squeeze, tetanic stimulation and skin incision.                   hyperalgesia.19
They found that iso¯urane, in clinically relevant concen-                      A second way that anaesthetics might alter cardiovascular
trations, was unable to prevent the haemodynamic response                   responses is by direct action on the heart and blood vessels.
to noxious stimulation, even when iso¯urane decreased the                   In addition, we examined whether anaesthetic action in the
prestimulation value to clinically unacceptable levels. These               brain might affect cardiovascular responses to noxious
data suggest that iso¯urane by itself is not a good agent to                stimulation.8 During differential iso¯urane delivery to the
use to control a patient's blood pressure adequately within a               head, we observed that heart rate and blood pressure
normal range.                                                               increased signi®cantly during noxious stimulation and that
   Why do some anaesthetics suppress these haemodynamic                     only supraclinical iso¯urane concentrations in the brain
responses to noxious stimulation while others do not? As                    could prevent this response.8


                                                                        161
Antognini and Carstens


                                                                               and then transected, a method that minimizes or prevents
                                                                               spinal shock. It was reported that the iso¯urane MAC
                                                                               (determined when the noxious stimulus was applied below
                                                                               the level of transection) was unchanged.75
                                                                                  We have used a goat model to investigate the relative
                                                                               roles of the brain and spinal cord in anaesthetic actions.7 13 16
                                                                               In brief, because of the unique cerebral circulation of goats,
                                                                               we can differentially deliver anaesthetics to the head (brain)
                                                                               or torso (spinal cord). Within the central nervous system, the
                                                                               watershed area where the torso and head circulation mix is
                                                                               at the level of the caudal medulla or upper cervical cord, the
                                                                               exact level depending on the blood pressure difference
                                                                               between the systemic and cranial circulations.13
                                                                                  With an intact native circulation, iso¯urane MAC in these
                                                                               goats was 1.2%.16 During differential iso¯urane delivery,
                                                                               cranial iso¯urane requirements (to suppress movement)
                                                                               were »3% when torso iso¯urane concentration (and hence
                                                                               spinal cord concentration) was low (»0.2±0.3%).16 As one
                                                                               would expect, the EEG was depressed at these high
                                                                               iso¯urane concentrations in the brain. We subsequently
                                                                               examined whether halothane and thiopental had similar
                                                                               effects on movement.10 Halothane appeared to be less
                                                                               potent in the brain (compared with iso¯urane) inasmuch as
                                                                               halothane at high concentrations (>4%) delivered to the
                                                                               cranial circulation could not stop movement in some
                                                                               animals. Overall, the cranial halothane requirement in-
                                                                               creased by nearly 400%. Despite a greatly depressed or
                                                                               isoelectric EEG, these animals still moved in response to
                                                                               noxious stimulation. In contrast, the cranial requirement for
Fig 6 Differential anaesthetic delivery aids elucidation of how
anaesthetic action in the spinal cord affects brain arousal.12 18 In the top
                                                                               thiopental increased by a factor of only 2, because
panel, the anaesthetic (iso¯urane) concentration in the spinal cord is         »40 mg ml±1 was required to prevent movement during
suf®cient to effectively block the ascending transmission of nociceptive       cranial thiopental delivery whereas »20 mg ml±1 was
impulses to the midbrain reticular formation (MRF), thalamus (Thal) and        required during systemic administration.10 Interestingly,
brain; no EEG activation occurs. In one scenario (middle), decreasing the      the EEG was active during the differential thiopental
spinal concentration of anaesthesia facilitates the ascending transmission
of nociceptive impulses, but the sensitivity of the brain is unchanged
                                                                               delivery even though the animals ceased moving.
compared with the top panel. There is minimal EEG activation, owing to            We have interpreted these collective data10 16 to suggest
the added nociceptive impulses that are transmitted to the brain. In the       that anaesthetic action in the spinal cord is important for the
scenario in the lower panel, we hypothesize that the brain is more             ability of iso¯urane, halothane and thiopental to prevent
sensitive to the additional nociceptive impulses, and there is marked EEG      movement. There are important differences, however. First,
activation (desynchronization). The enhanced sensitivity might be due to
increased ascending afferent activity that `resets' the brain's arousal
                                                                               halothane and iso¯urane appear to differ in that substantially
level. By indirectly diminishing brain arousal, the spinal cord action of      more halothane is required in the brain to prevent movement
anaesthetics could affect memory and unconsciousness.                          (compared with iso¯urane). Secondly, although the relative
                                                                               increases in the cranial requirements for thiopental and
                                                                               iso¯urane were quantitatively similar, there was a distinct
                                                                               qualitative difference in that thiopental, but not iso¯urane,
Spinal cord as a site of anaesthetic action                                    permitted an active EEG when its cranial action could
For many decades, the brain has been thought to be the site                    prevent movement. This indicates that the immobilizing
of anaesthetic action, insofar as memory, consciousness and                    effect of thiopental in the brain is more potent than that of
initiation of movement occur in the brain. In the last decade                  halothane, relative to the EEG suppression effect.
several lines of evidence have emerged indicating that the                     Furthermore, it is not entirely clear that iso¯urane,
spinal cord might be an important site of anaesthetic action.                  halothane and thiopental act at common sites in the brain.
The work of Rampil and colleagues strongly suggests that                          Although we have dismissed other sites in the torso as
an action of iso¯urane on the spinal cord is important to                      regards anaesthetic action, how do we know with certainty
suppression of movement.75 76 78 The MAC was unchanged                         that, when using an experimental preparation in which we
after precollicular decerebration in rats.78 In a follow-up                    remove an anaesthetic from the torso, we can ascribe an
study, a section of the spinal cord was rendered hypothermic                   action to the spinal cord? Is it possible that anaesthetics


                                                                           162
In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia


might act at the other sites, such as the peripheral nerve?            action on the spinal motor neurone with little or no indirect
Halothane and iso¯urane do not depress peripheral                      supraspinal component.11
nociceptors; in fact, excitation is more likely.24 63 The                 The ability of anaesthetic agents to produce immobility is
peripheral nerve is not substantially affected by clinical             usually assessed by their effectiveness in blocking gross
concentrations of anaesthetics.26                                      purposeful movement. An understanding of the underlying
   To rule out a peripheral site of anaesthetic action, we             mechanism will ultimately require study of anaesthetic
performed a bypass study in dogs in which we selectively               effects on complex motor processing. The spinal cord
removed iso¯urane from the lower torso.14 Anaesthetic                  appears to be capable of generating a variety of complex,
requirements (MAC) were determined with application of                 coordinated movement patterns. Decerebrate rats are cap-
the noxious stimulus to a lower torso site in the presence or          able of normal activity, such as grooming, exploring their
absence of iso¯urane at that site. We found that MAC was               cages and eating.62 97 Frogs sectioned at an upper cervical
unchanged, suggesting that peripheral action of iso¯urane is           level still have a normal wiping re¯ex, whereby the hind
not important, at least as regards immobility.                         limb moves to wipe away a noxious chemical stimulus
   Within the spinal cord, the dorsal horn has received much           applied to the forelimb. If the forelimb is moved, the hind
attention as a site of anaesthetic action. Because the dorsal          limb is able to determine the position of the forelimb,
horn acts as a `spinal gate', anaesthetic effects on dorsal            despite the absence of any supraspinal input.41 Cats
horn neuronal responses have been investigated over several            sectioned at a mid-thoracic level still exhibit fairly normal
decades.27 67 Virtually all anaesthetics examined appeared             locomotion on a treadmill after recovery from spinal shock
to depress spontaneous ®ring and/or responses to innocuous             and with support of the body weight.39 72 Lastly, brain-dead
and noxious stimuli. There were, however, several meth-                humans can display the Lazarus phenomenon, in which they
odological differences among these studies, making direct              have spontaneous movement, including crossing of the arms
comparisons dif®cult. We were interested in the depressant             over the chest, sitting up in bed and turning of the head.25 64
effect of iso¯urane on dorsal horn neuronal responses to                   All of the complex movement patterns described above
supraspinal noxious stimulation within the narrow range                are mediated by neural circuits, including central pattern
that just permitted and just prevented movement (0.9±1.0               generators (CPGs), which are intrinsic to the spinal cord. It
MAC).9 We observed that increasing the iso¯urane con-                  is currently not known how anaesthetics affect CPGs. To
centration from 0.9 to 1.1 MAC resulted in a modest (15%)              begin to address this question, we recently investigated the
depression in evoked responses. It is unclear if a change of           effects of inhaled anaesthetic agents on the pattern of
this small order of magnitude is suf®cient to account for              coordinated limb and head movements evoked by supra-
such a signi®cant behavioural change. Furthermore, the                 maximal stimulation in rats.17 All limbs and the snout were
neurones studied were not identi®ed according to their                 attached to force transducers to measure the direction and
function, e.g. as re¯ex interneurones or ascending projection          force of movements. At sub-MAC concentrations of
neurones. It is conceivable that anaesthetics may exert                iso¯urane or halothane, the supramaximal noxious stimulus
differential effects on functionally distinct classes of spinal        elicited synchronous, rather than alternating, movements of
neurones, an issue that deserves further study.                        all four limbs. When there was head movement it was
   Over the last several years the spinal motor neurone has            almost always towards the side of the noxious stimulus. As
received signi®cant attention as a possible site of anaes-             the concentration of iso¯urane or halothane was increased,
thetic action. In a series of studies, Rampil and Zhou and             there was ®rst a reduction in the number of movements, and
their respective colleagues indirectly assessed anaesthetic            at higher concentrations, a reduction in the force of
effects on the motor neurone using the F-wave.58 76 101 102            movements. We are at an early stage in investigating
The F-wave is evoked by electrical stimulation of a                    anaesthetic action on complex movement patterns, and there
peripheral motor (or mixed) nerve, and is thought to result            are many open questions. Noxious stimuli evoke limb
from antidromic invasion of the motor neuronal cell body               withdrawal re¯exes; what is the relationship of these fairly
with recurrent orthodromic conduction back down the                    simple re¯ex pathways to CPGs involved in generating
motor nerve to evoke muscle contraction.76 The F-wave is               more complex escape responses? Does noxious stimulation
thought to re¯ect motor neurone excitability, although the             elicit synchronous (or other) patterns of coordinated limb
evidence for this is mainly indirect. The F-wave is enhanced           movements in other species? Studies addressing these and
in clinical conditions associated with increased motor                 related questions will be worthwhile in understanding better
neurone excitability, such as spasticity. Inhaled agents               how anaesthetics affect spinal CPGs that generate complex
(iso¯urane, halothane, des¯urane, sevo¯urane) and nitrous              movement patterns.
oxide have been shown to depress the F-wave.40 58 76 101 102
However, it is not clear if this depression is a causal factor in
the ability of anaesthetic agents to produce immobility or if          Undesirable effects
it is merely correlated with immobility that is primarily              Although the primary emphasis of research on anaesthetic
caused by some other anaesthetic action. In any event,                 mechanisms is to determine how anaesthetics produce their
anaesthetic depression of the F-wave appears to be a direct            desired effects, we must also determine how these powerful


                                                                   163
Antognini and Carstens


drugs produce undesired effects, such as post-operative                        other anaesthetic agents on the centripetal transmission of
nausea and vomiting (PONV) and respiratory and cardio-                         sensory information. Gen Pharmacol 1992; 23: 945±63
                                                                          7    Antognini JF, Jinks S, Buzin V, Carstens E. A method for
vascular depression. Cyclopropane and ether produced
                                                                               differential delivery of intravenous drugs to the head and torso
limited cardiovascular depressive effects,35 45 53 but were                    of the goat. Anesth Analg 1998; 87: 1450±2
associated with PONV (particularly ether). Both drugs were                8    Antognini JF, Berg K. Cardiovascular responses to noxious
eliminated from clinical use because of in¯ammability.                         stimuli during iso¯urane anesthesia are minimally affected by
Halogenation of the carbon skeleton eliminated in¯amma-                        anesthetic action in the brain. Anesth Analg 1995; 81: 843±8
bility but also introduced cardiovascular depression. Thus,               9    Antognini JF, Carstens, E. Increasing iso¯urane from 0.9 to 1.1
changing the structure of an anaesthetic might enhance                         minimum alveolar concentration minimally affects dorsal horn
                                                                               cell responses to noxious stimulation. Anesthesiology 1999; 90:
desirable characteristics but may also introduce or exacer-
                                                                               208±14
bate side-effects.                                                        10   Antognini JF, Carstens E, Atherley R. Does thiopental's
   Respiratory depression is a common side-effect of                           immobilizing effect in brain exceed that of halothane?
virtually all anaesthetics. The mechanisms for this effect                     Anesthesiology 2002; 96: 980±6
have not been fully elucidated, but there are likely to be                11   Antognini JF, Carstens E, Buzin V. Iso¯urane depresses
differences among anaesthetics. Nieuwenhuijs and col-                          motoneuron excitability by a direct spinal action: an F-wave
leagues found that propofol depressed the central chemore-                     study. Anesth Analg 1999; 88: 681±5
                                                                          12   Antognini JF, Carstens E, Sudo M, Sudo S. Iso¯urane depresses
¯ex loop but had no signi®cant effect on the peripheral
                                                                               electroencephalographic and medial thalamic responses to
chemore¯ex loop.69 Inhaled anaesthetics depress the per-                       noxious stimulation via an indirect spinal action. Anesth Analg
ipheral loop. Further work is required to elucidate mech-                      2000; 91: 1282±8
anisms of respiratory depression. Hopefully it will be                    13   Antognini JF, Kien ND. A method for preferential delivery of
possible someday to develop an anaesthetic that has little or                  volatile anesthetics to the in situ goat brain. Anesthesiology 1994;
no cardiopulmonary depression.                                                 80: 1148±54
   From a patient's perspective, PONV is one of the most                  14   Antognini JF, Kien ND. Potency (minimum alveolar anesthetic
                                                                               concentration) of iso¯urane is independent of peripheral
common distressing complications of surgery and anaes-
                                                                               anesthetic effects. Anesth Analg 1995; 81: 69±72
thesia. However, it is not a life-threatening complication.               15   Antognini JF, Saadi J, Wang XW, Carstens E, Piercy M. Propofol
There is little research examining the mechanisms of                           action in both spinal cord and brain blunts electro-
anaesthetic-induced PONV.                                                      encephalographic responses to noxious stimulation in goats.
   There is evidence that anaesthetics might in¯uence the                      Sleep 2001; 24: 26±31
immune responses that are important in recovery after                     16   Antognini JF, Schwartz K. Exaggerated anesthetic requirements
surgery.47 68 70 As surgical techniques progress and                           in the preferentially anesthetized brain. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:
                                                                               1244±9
improve, patient outcome could perhaps be tied more
                                                                          17   Antognini JF, Wang XW, Carstens E. Quantitative and qualitative
closely to these subtle effects.                                               effects of iso¯urane on movement occurring after noxious
                                                                               stimulation. Anesthesiology 1999; 91: 1064±71
                                                                          18   Antognini JF, Wang XW, Carstens, E. Iso¯urane action in the
                                                                               spinal cord blunts electroencephalographic and thalamic-
Acknowledgements                                                               reticular formation responses to noxious stimulation in goats.
The authors were supported in part by grants NIH GM57970 and GM61283.          Anesthesiology 2000; 92: 559±66
                                                                          19   Archer DP, Ewen A, Roth SH, Samanani N. Plasma, brain, and
                                                                               spinal cord concentrations of thiopental associated with
                                                                               hyperalgesia in the rat. Anesthesiology 1994; 80: 168±76
References                                                                20   Bachman L, Eger EI 2nd, Waud BE, Waud DR. MAC and
  1 Alkire MT. Quantitative EEG correlations with brain glucose                dose±response curves. Anesthesiology 1971; 34: 201±4
    metabolic rate during anesthesia in volunteers. Anesthesiology        21   Bennett HL, Davis HS, Giannini JA. Non-verbal response to
    1998; 89: 323±33                                                           intraoperative conversation. Br J Anaesth 1985; 57: 174±9
  2 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Barker SJ, et al. Cerebral metabolism during     22   Berg-Johnsen J, Langmoen IA. Iso¯urane hyperpolarizes
    propofol anesthesia in humans studied with positron emission               neurones in rat and human cerebral cortex. Acta Physiol Scand
    tomography. Anesthesiology 1995; 82: 393±403                               1987; 130: 679±85
  3 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Fallon JH. Toward a uni®ed theory of             23   Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA. Human learning
    narcosis: brain imaging evidence for a thalamocortical switch as           during general anaesthesia and surgery. Br J Anaesth 1991; 66:
    the neurophysiologic basis of anesthetic-induced unconscious-              170±8
    ness. Consciousness Cogn 2000; 9: 370±86                              24   Campbell JN, Raja SN, Meyer RA. Halothane sensitizes
  4 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Shah NK, Anderson CT. Positron emission               cutaneous nociceptors in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 1984; 52:
    tomography study of regional cerebral metabolism in humans                 762±70
    during iso¯urane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 549±57          25   Christie JM, O'Lenic TD, Cane RD. Head turning in brain death. J
  5 Alkire MT, Pomfrett CJ, Haier RJ, et al. Functional brain imaging          Clin Anesth 1996; 8: 141±3
    during anesthesia in humans: effects of halothane on global and       26   de Jong RH, Nace RA. Nerve impulse conduction and cutaneous
    regional cerebral glucose metabolism. Anesthesiology 1999; 90:             receptor responses during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology
    701±9                                                                      1967; 28: 851±5
  6 Angel A, LeBeau F. A comparison of the effects of propofol with       27   de Jong RH, Wagman IH. Block of afferent impulses in the dorsal



                                                                    164
In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia


     horn of monkey. A possible mechanism of anesthesia. Exp Neurol               sepsis following anesthesia in mice. Anesthesiology 1985; 63:
     1968; 20: 352±8                                                              267±73
28   Devor M, Zalkind V. Reversible analgesia, atonia, and loss of           48   Hill GE, Stanley TH, Sentker CR. Physostigmine reversal of post-
     consciousness on bilateral intracerebral microinjection of                   operative somnolence. Can Anaesth Soc J 1977; 24: 707±11
     pentobarbital. Pain 2001; 94: 101±2                                     49   Hodgson PS, Liu SS. Epidural lidocaine decreases sevo¯urane
29   Dutton RC, Maurer AJ, Sonner JM, Fanselow MS, Laster MJ, Eger                requirement for adequate depth of anesthesia as measured by
     EI 2nd. The concentration of iso¯urane required to suppress                  the Bispectral Indexâ monitor. Anesthesiology 2001; 94: 799±803
     learning depends on the type of learning. Anesthesiology 2001; 94:      50   Hodgson PS, Liu SS, Gras TW. Does epidural anesthesia have
     514±9                                                                        general anesthetic effects? Anesthesiology 1999; 91: 1687±92
30   Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI 2nd, Heilbron D. Effects of                51   Hung OR, Varvel JR, Shafer SL, Stanski DR. Thiopental
     iso¯urane and nitrous oxide in subanesthetic concentrations on               pharmacodynamics. II. Quantitation of clinical and
     memory and responsiveness in volunteers. Anesthesiology 1992;                electroencephalographic depth of anesthesia. Anesthesiology
     77: 888±98                                                                   1992; 77: 237±44
31   Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI 2nd, Peterson N. Iso¯urane                 52   Jones BE. The organization of central cholinergic systems and
     anesthesia prevents unconscious learning. Anesth Analg 1992; 75:             their functional importance in sleep-waking states. Prog Brain Res
     107±12                                                                       1993; 98: 61±71
32   Dwyer RC, Rampil IJ, Eger EI, Bennett HL. The                           53   Jones RE, Guldmann N, Linde HW, Dripps RD, Price HL.
     electroencephalogram does not predict depth of iso¯urane                     Cyclopropane anesthesia. III. Effects of cyclopropane on
     anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1994; 81: 403±9                                   respiration and circulation in normal man. Anesthesiology 1960;
33   Eappen S, Kissin I. Effect of subarachnoid bupivacaine block on              21: 380±93
     anesthetic requirements for thiopental in rats. Anesthesiology          54   Kandel E. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue
     1998; 88: 1036±42                                                            between genes and synapses. Science 2001; 294: 1030±8
34   Eger EI, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Minimum alveolar anesthetic         55   Kandel L, Chortkoff BS, Sonner J, Laster MJ, Eger EI 2nd.
     concentration: a standard of anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology              Nonanesthetics can suppress learning. Anesth Analg 1996; 82:
     1965; 26: 756±63                                                             321±6
35   Eger EI 2nd, Smith NT, Cullen DJ, Cullen BF, Gregory GA. A              56   Katoh T, Ikeda K. The effects of fentanyl on sevo¯urane
     comparison of the cardiovascular effects of halothane, ¯uroxene,             requirements for loss of consciousness and skin incision.
                                                 Â
     ether and cyclopropane in man: a resume. Anesthesiology 1971;                Anesthesiology 1998; 88: 18±24
     34: 25±41                                                               57   Katoh T, Kobayashi S, Suzuki A, Iwamoto T, Bito H, Ikeda K. The
36   Eger EI 2nd, Koblin DD, Harris RA, et al. Hypothesis: inhaled                effect of fentanyl on sevo¯urane requirements for somatic and
     anesthetics produce immobility and amnesia by different                      sympathetic responses to surgical incision. Anesthesiology 1999;
     mechanisms at different sites. Anesth Analg 1997; 84: 915±8                  90: 398±405.
37   Eichenbaum HB, Cahill LF, Gluck MA, et al. Learning and                 58   King BS, Rampil IJ. Anesthetic depression of spinal motor
     memory: systems analysis. In: Zigmond MJ, Bloom FE, Landis SC,               neurons may contribute to lack of movement in response to
     Roberts JL, Squire LR, eds. Fundamental Neuroscience. San Diego:             noxious stimuli. Anesthesiology 1994; 81: 1484±92
     Academic Press, 1999; 1455±86                                           59   Kitahata LM, Ghazi-Saidi K, Yamashita M, Kosaka, Bonikos C,
38   Fiset P, Paus T, Daloze T, et al. Brain mechanisms of propofol-              Taub A. The depressant effect of halothane and sodium
     induced loss of consciousness in humans: a positron emission                 thiopental on the spontaneous and evoked activity of dorsal
     tomographic study. J Neurosci 1999; 19: 5506±13                              horn cells: lamina speci®city, time course and dose dependence. J
39   Forssberg H, Grillner S, Rossignol S. Phase dependent re¯ex                  Pharmacol Exp Ther 1975; 195: 515±21
     reversal during walking in chronic spinal cats. Brain Res 1975; 85:     60   Krasowski MD, Harrison NL. General anaesthetic actions on
     103±7                                                                        ligand-gated ion channels. Cell Mol Life Sci 1999; 55: 1278±303
40   Friedman Y, King BS, Rampil I J. Nitrous oxide depresses spinal F       61   Kurosawa M, Sato A, Sato Y. Cutaneous mechanical sensory
     waves in rats. Anesthesiology 1996; 85: 135±41                               stimulation increases extracellular acetylcholine release in
41   Fukson OI, Berkinblit MB, Feldman AG. The spinal frog takes                  cerebral cortex in anesthetized rats. Neurochem Int 1992; 21:
     into account the scheme of its body during the wiping re¯ex.                 423±7
     Science 1980; 209: 1261±3                                               62   Lovick TA. The behavioural repertoire of precollicular
42   Ghoneim MM, Block RI. Learning and consciousness during                      decerebrate rats. J Physiol (Lond)1972; 226: 4P±6P
     general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992; 76: 279±305                    63   MacIver MB, Tanelian DL. Volatile anesthetics excite mammalian
43   Glass PS, Bloom M, Kearse L, Rosow C, Sebel P, Manberg P.                    nociceptor afferents recorded in vitro. Anesthesiology 1990; 72:
     Bispectral analysis measures sedation and memory effects of                  1022±30
     propofol, midazolam, iso¯urane, and alfentanil in healthy               64   Mandel S, Arenas A, Scasta D. Spinal automatism in cerebral
     volunteers. Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 836±47                                  death [letter]. N Engl J Med 1982; 307: 501±1
44   Glass PS. Anesthetic drug interactions: an insight into general         65   Meuret P, Backman SB, Bonhomme V, Plourde G, Fiset P.
     anesthesiaÐits mechanism and dosing strategies. Anesthesiology               Physostigmine reverses propofol-induced unconsciousness and
     1998; 88: 5±6                                                                attenuation of the auditory steady state response and bispectral
45   Gregory GA, Eger EI 2nd, Smith NT, Cullen BF, Cullen DJ. The                 index in human volunteers. Anesthesiology 2000; 93: 708±17
     cardiovascular effects of diethyl ether in man. Anesthesiology          66   Moruzzi G, Magoun HW. Brain stem reticular formation and
     1971; 34: 19±24                                                              activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1949;
46   Guedel AE. Third stage ether anesthesia: a sub-classi®cation                 1: 455±73
     regarding the signi®cance of the position and movements of the          67   Namiki A, Collins JG, Kitahata LM, Kikuchi H, Homma E,
     eyeball. Am J Surg (Anesth Suppl) 1920; 34: 53±7                             Thalhammer JG. Effects of halothane on spinal neuronal
47   Hansbrough JF, Zapata-Sirvent RL, Bartle EJ, et al. Alterations in           responses to graded noxious heat stimulation in the cat.
     splenic lymphocyte subpopulations and increased mortality from               Anesthesiology 1980; 53: 475±80



                                                                       165
Antognini and Carstens


68 Mikawa K, Akamatsu H, Nishina K, et al. Propofol inhibits human       86 Snow J. On the Inhalation of the Vapour of Ether in Surgical
   neutrophil functions. Anesth Analg 1998; 87: 695±700                     Operations. London: Churchill, 1847.
69 Nieuwenhuijs D, Sarton E, Teppema LJ, et al. Respiratory sites of     87 Sonner JM, Li J, Eger EI 2nd. Des¯urane and the nonimmobilizer
   action of propofol. Absence of depression of peripheral                  1,2-dichlorohexa¯uorocyclobutane suppress learning by a
   chemore¯ex loop by low-dose propofol. Anesthesiology 2001;               mechanism independent of the level of unconditioned
   95: 889±95                                                               stimulation. Anesth Analg 1998; 87: 200±5
70 Nishina K, Akamatsu H, Mikawa K, et al. The inhibitory effects of     88 Steriade M. Arousal: revisiting the reticular activating system.
   thiopental, midazolam, and ketamine on human neutrophil                  Science 1996; 272: 225±6
   functions. Anesth Analg 1998; 86: 159±65                              89 Steriade M, Amzica F, Contreras D. Cortical and thalamic
71 Pajewski TN, DiFazio CA, Moscicki JC, Johns RA. Nitric oxide             cellular correlates of electroencephalographic burst-
   synthase inhibitors, 7-nitro indazole and nitroG-L-arginine              suppression. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994; 90: 1±16
   methyl ester, dose dependently reduce the threshold for               90 Stoelting RK, Longnecker DE, Eger EI 2nd. Minimum alveolar
   iso¯urane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1996; 85: 1111±9                    concentrations in man on awakening from methoxy¯urane,
72 Pearson K. The control of walking. Sci Am 1976; 235: 72±4,               halothane, ether and ¯uroxene anesthesia: MAC awake.
   79±82, 83±6                                                              Anesthesiology 1970; 33: 5±9
73 Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bak P, et al. Analgesic effect    91 Tomi K, Mashimo T, Tashiro C, et al. Alterations in pain
   in humans of subanaesthetic iso¯urane concentrations evaluated           threshold and psychomotor response associated with
   by experimentally induced pain. Br J Anaesth 1995; 75: 55±60.            subanaesthetic concentrations of inhalation anaesthetics in
74 Quasha AL, Eger EI, Tinker JH. Determination and applications of
                                                                            humans. Br J Anaesth 1993; 70: 684±6
   MAC. Anesthesiology 1980; 53: 315±34
                                                                         92 Tverskoy M, Fleyshman G, Bachrak L, Ben-Shlomo I. Effect of
75 Rampil IJ. Anesthetic potency is not altered after hypothermic
                                                                            bupivacaine-induced spinal block on the hypnotic requirement of
   spinal cord transection in rats. Anesthesiology 1994; 80: 606±10
                                                                            propofol. Anaesthesia 1996; 51: 652±3
76 Rampil IJ, King BS. Volatile anesthetics depress spinal motor
                                                                         93 Tverskoy M, Shagal M, Finger J, Kissin I. Subarachnoid
   neurons. Anesthesiology 1996; 85: 129±34
                                                                            bupivacaine blockade decreases midazolam and thiopental
77 Rampil IJ, Laster MJ. No correlation between quantitative
                                                                            hypnotic requirements. J Clin Anesth 1994; 6: 487±90.
   electroencephalographic      measurements       and    movement
                                                                         94 Uchida H, Kishikawa K, Collins JG. Effect of propofol on spinal
   response to noxious stimuli during iso¯urane anesthesia in
   rats. Anesthesiology 1992; 77: 920±5                                     dorsal horn neurons. Comparison with lack of ketamine effects.
78 Rampil IJ, Mason P, Singh H. Anesthetic potency (MAC) is                 Anesthesiology 1995; 83: 1312±22
   independent of forebrain structures in the rat. Anesthesiology        95 Waud BE, Waud DR. On dose±response curves and anesthetics.
   1993; 78: 707±12                                                         Anesthesiology 1970; 33: 1±4
79 Rampil IJ, Matteo RS. Changes in EEG spectral edge frequency          96 Willis WD, Westlund KN. Neuroanatomy of the pain system
   correlate with the hemodynamic response to laryngoscopy and              and of the pathways that modulate pain. J Clin Neurophysiol 1997;
   intubation. Anesthesiology 1987; 67: 139±42                              14: 2±31
80 Roizen MF, Horrigan RW, Frazer BM. Anesthetic doses blocking          97 Woods JW. Behavior of chronic decerebrate rats. J Neurophysiol
   adrenergic (stress) and cardiovascular responses to incisionÐ            1964; 27: 635±44
   MAC BAR. Anesthesiology 1981; 54: 390±8                               98 Zbinden AM, Maggiorini M, Petersen-Felix S, Lauber R, Thomson
     È
81 Ropcke H, Rehberg B, Koenen-Bergmann M, Bouillon T, Bruhn J,             DA, Minder CE. Anesthetic depth de®ned using multiple noxious
   Hoeft A. Surgical stimulation shifts EEG concentration±response          stimuli during iso¯urane/oxygen anesthesia. I. Motor reactions.
   relationship of des¯urane. Anesthesiology 2001; 94: 390±9                Anesthesiology 1994; 80: 253±60
82 Scheller MS, Zornow MH, Fleischer JE, Shearman GT, Greber             99 Zbinden AM, Petersen-Felix S, Thomson DA. Anesthetic depth
   TF. The noncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor                     de®ned using multiple noxious stimuli during iso¯urane/oxygen
   antagonist MK-801 profoundly reduces volatile anesthetic                 anesthesia. II. Hemodynamic responses. Anesthesiology 1994; 80:
   requirements in rabbits. Neuropharmacology 1989; 28: 677±81              261±7
83 Shichino T, Murakawa M, Adachi T, et al. Effects of iso¯urane on     100 Zhang Y, Eger EI 2nd, Dutton RC, Sonner JM. Inhaled anesthetics
   in vivo release of acetylcholine in the rat cerebral cortex and          have hyperalgesic effects at 0.1 minimum alveolar anesthetic
   striatum. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1997; 41: 1335±40                      concentration. Anesth Analg 2000; 91: 462±6
84 Shimoji K, Fujioka H, Fukazawa T, Hashiba M, Maruyama Y.             101 Zhou HH, Jin TT, Qin B, Turndorf H. Suppression of spinal cord
   Anesthetics and excitatory/inhibitory responses of midbrain              motoneuron excitability correlates with surgical immobility
   reticular neurons. Anesthesiology 1984; 61: 151±5                        during iso¯urane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1998; 88: 955±61
85 Siker ES, Wolfson B, Ciccarelli HE, Telan RA. Effect of              102 Zhou HH, Mehta M, Leis AA. Spinal cord motoneuron
   subanesthetic concentrations of halothane and methoxy¯urane              excitability during iso¯urane and nitrous oxide anesthesia.
   on pain threshold in conscious volunteers. Anesthesiology 1967;          Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 302±7
   28: 337±42




                                                                    166

More Related Content

What's hot

General Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. Overview
General Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. OverviewGeneral Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. Overview
General Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. Overview
Anisul Mazumder
 

What's hot (20)

Awareness under anaesthesia
Awareness under anaesthesiaAwareness under anaesthesia
Awareness under anaesthesia
 
Anesthesia awareness
Anesthesia awarenessAnesthesia awareness
Anesthesia awareness
 
Anesthesiology Information
Anesthesiology InformationAnesthesiology Information
Anesthesiology Information
 
Pain management in neurosurgical patients
Pain management in neurosurgical patientsPain management in neurosurgical patients
Pain management in neurosurgical patients
 
Delayed recovery from anaesthesia.ppt
Delayed recovery from anaesthesia.pptDelayed recovery from anaesthesia.ppt
Delayed recovery from anaesthesia.ppt
 
Different types of anesthesia by john gerancher
Different types of anesthesia by john gerancherDifferent types of anesthesia by john gerancher
Different types of anesthesia by john gerancher
 
Awareness Anesthesia
Awareness AnesthesiaAwareness Anesthesia
Awareness Anesthesia
 
Conduct of general anesthesia
Conduct of general anesthesiaConduct of general anesthesia
Conduct of general anesthesia
 
Symptomatic treatment of Anxiety & Asthenia
Symptomatic treatment of Anxiety & AstheniaSymptomatic treatment of Anxiety & Asthenia
Symptomatic treatment of Anxiety & Asthenia
 
Anesthesia CPT Coding 2017
Anesthesia CPT Coding 2017Anesthesia CPT Coding 2017
Anesthesia CPT Coding 2017
 
General anesthesia & oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
General anesthesia & oral and Maxillofacial SurgeryGeneral anesthesia & oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
General anesthesia & oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
 
General anesthesia
General anesthesiaGeneral anesthesia
General anesthesia
 
Pain management in cardiac sx
Pain management in cardiac sxPain management in cardiac sx
Pain management in cardiac sx
 
Clinical pharmacy in Neurology
Clinical pharmacy in NeurologyClinical pharmacy in Neurology
Clinical pharmacy in Neurology
 
Anesthesia & Cardio-pulmonary Resuscitation
Anesthesia & Cardio-pulmonary ResuscitationAnesthesia & Cardio-pulmonary Resuscitation
Anesthesia & Cardio-pulmonary Resuscitation
 
General Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. Overview
General Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. OverviewGeneral Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. Overview
General Anaesthesia In Maxillofacial Surgery. Overview
 
Awareness during anesthesia
Awareness during anesthesiaAwareness during anesthesia
Awareness during anesthesia
 
Types of Anesthesia
Types of AnesthesiaTypes of Anesthesia
Types of Anesthesia
 
Sedation in neurocritical care unit
Sedation in neurocritical care unitSedation in neurocritical care unit
Sedation in neurocritical care unit
 
Delayed recovery of unconsciousness from anaesthesia
Delayed recovery of unconsciousness from anaesthesiaDelayed recovery of unconsciousness from anaesthesia
Delayed recovery of unconsciousness from anaesthesia
 

Similar to AnestéSicos Review

anesthesia ppt medical surgical nursing
anesthesia ppt  medical surgical nursinganesthesia ppt  medical surgical nursing
anesthesia ppt medical surgical nursing
DishaThakur53
 
Lecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptx
Lecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptxLecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptx
Lecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptx
Rida329646
 

Similar to AnestéSicos Review (20)

Chapter 1-intro duction of anesthesia 22439
Chapter  1-intro duction of anesthesia 22439Chapter  1-intro duction of anesthesia 22439
Chapter 1-intro duction of anesthesia 22439
 
Chapter 1-intro duction of anesthesia
Chapter  1-intro duction of anesthesiaChapter  1-intro duction of anesthesia
Chapter 1-intro duction of anesthesia
 
General anesthetics
General anestheticsGeneral anesthetics
General anesthetics
 
General Anesthetics
General AnestheticsGeneral Anesthetics
General Anesthetics
 
Anaesthesia power point
Anaesthesia power pointAnaesthesia power point
Anaesthesia power point
 
Introduction to Anaesthesia.pptx
Introduction to Anaesthesia.pptxIntroduction to Anaesthesia.pptx
Introduction to Anaesthesia.pptx
 
ga-170603164733.pptx
ga-170603164733.pptxga-170603164733.pptx
ga-170603164733.pptx
 
Ppt on anaesthesia
Ppt on anaesthesiaPpt on anaesthesia
Ppt on anaesthesia
 
sedation and anesthesia
sedation and anesthesiasedation and anesthesia
sedation and anesthesia
 
General anesthetics(VK)
General anesthetics(VK)General anesthetics(VK)
General anesthetics(VK)
 
anesthesia ppt medical surgical nursing
anesthesia ppt  medical surgical nursinganesthesia ppt  medical surgical nursing
anesthesia ppt medical surgical nursing
 
Anesthesia surgery.pptx
Anesthesia surgery.pptxAnesthesia surgery.pptx
Anesthesia surgery.pptx
 
Lecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptx
Lecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptxLecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptx
Lecture 2-Surgery(Anesthesia).pptx
 
General Anesthetics.pptx
General Anesthetics.pptxGeneral Anesthetics.pptx
General Anesthetics.pptx
 
monitoringdepthofanesthesia-190113032251.pdf
monitoringdepthofanesthesia-190113032251.pdfmonitoringdepthofanesthesia-190113032251.pdf
monitoringdepthofanesthesia-190113032251.pdf
 
Conscious Sedation
Conscious SedationConscious Sedation
Conscious Sedation
 
GENERAL ANESTHETICS.pptx
GENERAL ANESTHETICS.pptxGENERAL ANESTHETICS.pptx
GENERAL ANESTHETICS.pptx
 
Anesthesia & its types
Anesthesia & its typesAnesthesia & its types
Anesthesia & its types
 
General anesthetics and local anesthetics
General anesthetics and local anestheticsGeneral anesthetics and local anesthetics
General anesthetics and local anesthetics
 
general and local anesthesia
general and local anesthesia general and local anesthesia
general and local anesthesia
 

Recently uploaded

Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝 9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝  9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝  9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝 9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7
9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls * UPA...
Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls  * UPA...Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls  * UPA...
Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls * UPA...
mahaiklolahd
 
Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...
Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...
Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...
adilkhan87451
 
🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...
🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...
🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...
Call Girls In Delhi Whatsup 9873940964 Enjoy Unlimited Pleasure
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝 9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝  9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝  9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Gagan Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝 9953056974 🔝] escort service 24X7
 
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Majestic ⟟ 9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Genuine S...
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Majestic ⟟  9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Genuine S...Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Majestic ⟟  9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Genuine S...
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Majestic ⟟ 9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Genuine S...
 
(Low Rate RASHMI ) Rate Of Call Girls Jaipur ❣ 8445551418 ❣ Elite Models & Ce...
(Low Rate RASHMI ) Rate Of Call Girls Jaipur ❣ 8445551418 ❣ Elite Models & Ce...(Low Rate RASHMI ) Rate Of Call Girls Jaipur ❣ 8445551418 ❣ Elite Models & Ce...
(Low Rate RASHMI ) Rate Of Call Girls Jaipur ❣ 8445551418 ❣ Elite Models & Ce...
 
Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls * UPA...
Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls  * UPA...Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls  * UPA...
Call Girl in Indore 8827247818 {LowPrice} ❤️ (ahana) Indore Call Girls * UPA...
 
Jogeshwari ! Call Girls Service Mumbai - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 90042684...
Jogeshwari ! Call Girls Service Mumbai - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 90042684...Jogeshwari ! Call Girls Service Mumbai - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 90042684...
Jogeshwari ! Call Girls Service Mumbai - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 90042684...
 
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Richmond Circle ⟟ 9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Ge...
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Richmond Circle ⟟  9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Ge...Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Richmond Circle ⟟  9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Ge...
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Richmond Circle ⟟ 9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For Ge...
 
Russian Call Girls Service Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️PALLAVI VIP Jaipur Call Gir...
Russian Call Girls Service  Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️PALLAVI VIP Jaipur Call Gir...Russian Call Girls Service  Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️PALLAVI VIP Jaipur Call Gir...
Russian Call Girls Service Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️PALLAVI VIP Jaipur Call Gir...
 
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
Pondicherry Call Girls Book Now 9630942363 Top Class Pondicherry Escort Servi...
 
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
Premium Bangalore Call Girls Jigani Dail 6378878445 Escort Service For Hot Ma...
 
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Ramamurthy Nagar ⟟ 9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For G...
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Ramamurthy Nagar ⟟  9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For G...Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Ramamurthy Nagar ⟟  9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For G...
Top Rated Bangalore Call Girls Ramamurthy Nagar ⟟ 9332606886 ⟟ Call Me For G...
 
Call Girls Vadodara Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Vadodara Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Vadodara Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Vadodara Just Call 8617370543 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Mumbai ] (Call Girls) in Mumbai 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 98333...
Mumbai ] (Call Girls) in Mumbai 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 98333...Mumbai ] (Call Girls) in Mumbai 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 98333...
Mumbai ] (Call Girls) in Mumbai 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 98333...
 
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
Best Rate (Patna ) Call Girls Patna ⟟ 8617370543 ⟟ High Class Call Girl In 5 ...
 
Night 7k to 12k Navi Mumbai Call Girl Photo 👉 BOOK NOW 9833363713 👈 ♀️ night ...
Night 7k to 12k Navi Mumbai Call Girl Photo 👉 BOOK NOW 9833363713 👈 ♀️ night ...Night 7k to 12k Navi Mumbai Call Girl Photo 👉 BOOK NOW 9833363713 👈 ♀️ night ...
Night 7k to 12k Navi Mumbai Call Girl Photo 👉 BOOK NOW 9833363713 👈 ♀️ night ...
 
Call Girls Service Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP BHAWNA Call Girl in Jaipur Raja...
Call Girls Service Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP BHAWNA Call Girl in Jaipur Raja...Call Girls Service Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP BHAWNA Call Girl in Jaipur Raja...
Call Girls Service Jaipur {8445551418} ❤️VVIP BHAWNA Call Girl in Jaipur Raja...
 
Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...
Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...
Russian Call Girls Lucknow Just Call 👉👉7877925207 Top Class Call Girl Service...
 
💕SONAM KUMAR💕Premium Call Girls Jaipur ↘️9257276172 ↙️One Night Stand With Lo...
💕SONAM KUMAR💕Premium Call Girls Jaipur ↘️9257276172 ↙️One Night Stand With Lo...💕SONAM KUMAR💕Premium Call Girls Jaipur ↘️9257276172 ↙️One Night Stand With Lo...
💕SONAM KUMAR💕Premium Call Girls Jaipur ↘️9257276172 ↙️One Night Stand With Lo...
 
8980367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
8980367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad8980367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
8980367676 Call Girls In Ahmedabad Escort Service Available 24×7 In Ahmedabad
 
Call Girls Raipur Just Call 9630942363 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Raipur Just Call 9630942363 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Raipur Just Call 9630942363 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Raipur Just Call 9630942363 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...
🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...
🌹Attapur⬅️ Vip Call Girls Hyderabad 📱9352852248 Book Well Trand Call Girls In...
 

AnestéSicos Review

  • 1. British Journal of Anaesthesia 89 (1): 156±66 (2002) In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia and its components J. F. Antognini1* and E. Carstens2 1 Department of Anesthesiology, TB-170, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA. 2Section of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior, 193 Briggs Hall, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA *Corresponding author Br J Anaesth 2002; 89: 156±66 Keywords: anaesthesia, general; theories of anaesthetic action One of the fundamental problems facing researchers of ness occurred before signi®cant or complete analgesia. That anaesthetic mechanisms is linking a particular effect on a is, unconsciousness simply became part and parcel of receptor to a speci®c clinical effect. Thus, to fully under- general anaesthesia. From a practical aspect, unconscious- stand how anaesthetics act we must approach anaesthetic ness became important as conscious patients, even if they mechanisms at multiple levels. Ultimately, receptor effects had complete analgesia, would probably talk, which would must be viewed within the context of the clinical charac- disrupt the surgical procedure. Indeed, an unconscious and teristics of general anaesthesia. Does a particular anaes- immobile patient permits surgical procedures that are thetic alter receptor function at clinically relevant limited only by the available technology and skill of the anaesthetic concentrations? Is there evidence (anatomical, surgeon. neurophysiological, pharmacological) that a speci®c recep- Within a year of Morton's public demonstration of ether tor is involved in a clinically relevant neurophysiological anaesthesia, John Snow published an account of the process, such as memory? pharmacological and physiological effects of ether.86 Anaesthesia can be de®ned in arbitrary terms, although These effects were divided into stages, progressing from practical considerations often govern speci®c de®nitions. consciousness to deep coma, muscle ¯accidity and respira- Thus, from a practical standpoint, most people would tory paralysis. include unconsciousness, amnesia and immobility as Guedel described four stages of ether anaesthesia,46 important end-points. On the other hand, reduction of stress similar to those described by Snow. In the ®rst stage the hormones is not necessarily an absolute, required anaes- patient was conscious but had analgesia (Fig. 1). In the thetic end-point. In this review we will examine the various second stage (the delirium stage) the patient exhibited components of general anaesthesia. That is, what are the excessive motor activity, even to the point of violence. Eye clinical goals we seek to achieve? What are the side-effects movements were irregular and erratic, as was breathing. The we hope to avoid? This latter component is important third stage represented the surgical stage; four planes were because an anaesthetic (or any drug) is only as good as the originally described, with increasing anaesthetic depth as minimization of its side-effects. the patient progressed from the ®rst to the last plane. Respiration became progressively weaker. In the fourth stage of anaesthesia, respiratory paralysis occurred. These Historical aspects classic signs have broad application, but it is clear that not Before Morton's display of ether anaesthesia, patients were all anaesthetics cause the same progression of clinical signs. accustomed to being conscious during surgeryÐpainfully Newer anaesthetics, in particular i.v. anaesthetics such as so. There was no a priori expectation that they should be propofol, may not exhibit such signs. One wonders how unconscious. Their hope was not so much to be unconscious much of this difference is due to pharmacokinetic as but rather to be pain-free. After all, few people would opposed to pharmacodynamic reasons. Many of the newer voluntarily give up consciousness if complete analgesia inhaled anaesthetics have low blood-gas solubilities and were otherwise possible. It was the characteristic of ether, thus patients may pass from one stage to the next relatively chloroform and subsequent anaesthetics that unconscious- quickly. Likewise, with i.v. anaesthetics patients are Ó The Board of Management and Trustees of the British Journal of Anaesthesia 2002
  • 2. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia remain `within normal limits'. An anaesthetist must balance the demands of all three. For practical purposes we de®ne general anaesthesia as the presence of unconsciousness, amnesia and immobility (in response to noxious stimulation). Analgesia is not always included in the list. Analgesia might be an important indirect means to help achieve all of the goals of anaesthesia, but is it essential? We answer `no'. Analgesics reduce or eliminate pain. Pain is the conscious awareness of a noxious stimulus. Anaesthetized patients are unconscious. Thus, they cannot perceive pain. Therefore analgesia is not directly relevant and may be excluded as a necessary component of general anaesthesia. However, analgesia would be desirable in the rare cases in which patients regain consciousness during surgery and remember the experience. Moreover, drugs with analgesic properties are often used during anaesthesia and can be important for patient management, i.e. to control haemodynamic perturbations. We also exclude lack of haemodynamic responses as an absolute requirement. Although tight haemodynamic control is desirable in some patients, increased heart rate and increased blood pressure are not, by themselves, harmful to many patients. Finally, Fig 1 Guedel's signs for ether anaesthesia included muscle tone, breathing pattern and eye movements. Stage 1 was marked by analgesia pre-emptive analgesia could be included as a desirable goal, and consciousness. In Stage 2, the patient became unconscious, breathing but it is not an absolute requirement for general anaesthesia. was erratic but delirium could occur, leading to an excitement phase. In Stage 3, surgical anaesthesia occurred, with four planes or levels describing increasing depth until breathing became weak. Stage 4 was Neurophysiological processes pertinent to marked by respiratory paralysis and death. Other signs included pupil anaesthesia size. Newer anaesthetics and `balanced' anaesthesia have rendered some of these signs less reliable. (Based on reference 46.) Memory Memory formation occurs at a variety of sites in the brain, brought to deeper stages rapidly by bolus administration. including the hippocampus, amygdala, prefrontal cortex and Thus, there are few data comparing these newer anaesthetics other cortical sensory and motor areas.37 54 Anaesthetics with older ones, such as ether. may affect any or all of these sites and thereby result in amnesia. There are two types of memory that are usually discussed in relation to anaesthesia: implicit and explicit recall.37 54 What de®nes anaesthesia? Explicit recall is what most people usually describe as How do we decide what are the essential components of memory: they can explicitly recall a speci®c event, such as a general anaesthesia? What are the non-essential but desir- football game or their wedding day. Implicit recall occurs able goals? These decisions may be made with practical, when an individual cannot remember a certain event, but scienti®c, theoretical and historical considerations. upon speci®c testing there is evidence that information has We ®rst differentiate `general anaesthesia' from a `gen- been retained. There are several studies that suggest that, at eral anaesthetic'. The former de®nes a pharmacologically low anaesthetic concentrations, implicit memory formation induced physiological state in which the essential goals of may occur while explicit recall is blocked.21 23 42 The general anaesthesia are achieved. This may be accom- speci®c sites where this action occurs are not known. plished with a single agent or with a variety of drugs. A general anaesthetic, however, is a drug that, by itself, achieves all of the essential goals of general anaesthesia. Consciousness What are regarded as the essential goals of general The physiological processes and anatomical sites that give anaesthesia can depend on the perspective of the de®ner. For rise to consciousness are poorly understood, and the sites the patient, an important goal will be amnesiaÐhe or she where anaesthetics induce unconsciousness are understood does not want to remember anything. A close second would even less. Several sites are likely to participate in con- probably be unconsciousnessÐthe patient does not want to sciousness, including the cerebral cortex, thalamus and be awake during surgery. A surgeon wants a still patient. A reticular formation.66 88 All of those structures are affected cardiologist wants the patient's blood pressure and pulse to by anaesthetics.6 22 84 Alkire and colleagues have examined 157
  • 3. Antognini and Carstens noxious stimulus (Fig. 2). Such re¯exes are undoubtedly also involved in initiating more complex behavioural responses to escape from or otherwise cope with a threatening environment. The motor consequence of a noxious stimulus might thus consist of a simple withdrawal re¯ex in which the stimulated extremity is pulled away from the stimulus, or of a violent escape response in which the animal uses all its limbs. Alternatively, the animal may orient towards the source of stimulation and even attack. All of these motor responses are eliminated by anaesthetics. A noxious stimulus activates peripheral nociceptors that transmit impulses to second-order neurones in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord.96 These second-order neurones may synapse onto motor neurones either directly or indirectly via higher-order interneurones. Activation of these motor neurones leads to muscle contraction that, depending on the extent and pattern of activation, results in a nociceptive re¯ex (e.g. withdrawal). Nociceptive input is also likely to activate central pattern generators, which coordinate move- ment such that the animal can move ef®ciently in a walk, trot or gallop. Fig 2 Noxious stimulation results in impulse transmission to the dorsal horn, where second-order neurones might send impulses to ascending tracts (such as the spinothalamic tract) or to motor neurones that Neurotransmitter systems innervate muscles that initiate a motor response, such as an escape or A wide variety of neurotransmitter systems is likely to be withdrawal response. The ascending transmission of these impulses affected by anaesthetics but it is unclear how these actions `activates' the brain and results in increased arousal. The location of the spinothalamic tract varies from species to species. translate into clinical effects.60 In addition, it is unclear if presynaptic or postsynaptic effects are more important. The GABA receptor has been the focus of intense research and the effect of halothane, iso¯urane and propofol on cerebral there is ample evidence that alteration of GABAergic metabolism [using positron emission tomography (PET)] as neurotransmission could alter consciousness, memory and an indirect method of investigating sites of anaesthetic nociceptive responses. Other neurotransmitters are also action.1±5 These anaesthetics depress metabolism in the likely to be involved in these processes.60 For example, cortex, thalamus and reticular formation, as one would anaesthetics affect the physiology and pharmacology of expect. Whether the effects at each site contribute equally to glutamate and nitric oxide.71 Antagonists of N-methyl-D- unconsciousness or whether one site is more crucial than the aspartate (NMDA), such as MK-801, drastically reduce the others remains to be elucidated. Fiset and colleagues minimum alveolar concentration (MAC),82 but is this a investigated the effects of propofol using PET scanning direct and relevant anaesthetic action or does it merely and found that propofol produced greater metabolic reduc- re¯ect the importance of glutamate in the initiation of tion in the medial thalamus and other brain sites associated nociceptive re¯exes, such that simply by blocking glutamate with arousal.38 A logical conclusion from their data is that transmission one can prevent nociceptive responses? propofol produced unconsciousness through a preferential Acetylcholine and the cholinergic neurotransmitter system effect on sites associated with arousal. Alkire and others are also possibly involved in anaesthesia. Physostigmine has have suggested that anaesthetics might affect thalamocor- been shown to reverse partially the sedative effects of tical loops that appear to be critical for conscious aware- propofol and halothane.48 65 The cerebral activation that is ness.3 In an intriguing study, Devor and Zalkind injected associated with increased arousal is also associated with pentobarbital into a discrete area of the rat mesopontine cortical release of acetylcholine.52 Sensory stimulation can tegmental area bilaterally, producing unconsciousness and likewise cause cortical release of acetylcholine,61 and analgesia.28 Clearly, however, considerably more work is iso¯urane can affect this release.83 needed to understand where anaesthetics act in the brain to in¯uence consciousness. Anaesthetic goals Nociceptive re¯exes Immobility Nociceptive re¯exes have evolved as a protective mechan- Anaesthetic end-points can sometimes be described in ism to withdraw the body (or part of the body) from a relation to the concentration of anaesthetic required to block 158
  • 4. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia Amnesia and unconsciousness Amnesia and unconsciousness are among the ®rst end- points to be reached when an anaesthetic is administered. The concentration that results in a patient passing from wakefulness to unconsciousness is called `MAC-awake', ®rst described by Stoelting and colleagues.90 They meas- ured the concentrations of methoxy¯urane, halothane, ether and ¯uroxene that just permitted and prevented conscious- ness (as de®ned by the response to verbal stimuli); MAC- awake was the average of these. Two subject groups were used. One consisted of volunteers who did not undergo a surgical procedure, while the other group did. Because the anaesthetics studied were not equally divided between the Fig 3 These dose±response curves for various end-points were developed two groups, it is unclear how the presence of post-surgical from a variety of studies. Note that the curves for memory and pain might have altered the results, inasmuch as noxious consciousness are close to each other. The effective dose that results in stimulation is likely to result in cerebral activation and 50% of patients having unconsciousness is called `MAC-awake'. This value is about 30±40% of MAC, although there are differences among increased probability of consciousness. The MAC-awake anaesthetics. MAC-awake might be greater during the noxious for ether (as a fraction of MAC) was greater than that for the stimulation of surgery. The anaesthetic concentration that blocks the other anaesthetics, despite the fact that ether was studied cardiovascular response is called `MAC-BAR'. only in volunteers. The MAC-awake varied between 0.5 and 0.65 MAC. Another source of potential error is that MAC values were obtained from historical controls. In a critical movementÐthe MAC (Fig. 3). This is essentially the editorial,95 Waud and Waud placed limits on the interpret- median effective dose (ED50), the dose that causes a ation of the work of Stoelting and colleagues90 (but see also particular effect in 50% of people or animals. reference 20). Speci®cally, examination of only one point The MAC concept was developed in 1965 as a way to on a curve or set of curves (e.g. the point at which 50% of compare equipotent concentrations of anaesthetics.34 The patients are awake) says nothing about the overall curve measurement of MAC depends essentially on achieving a and, hence, relative potency and effect. They also ques- stable end-tidal anaesthetic concentration, applying a tioned whether examination of MAC-awake and other standard noxious stimulus and observing whether move- variants of MAC would yield any useful information ment occurs.34 74 Positive movement was arbitrarily de®ned regarding anaesthetic mechanisms. Many investigators as `gross and purposeful', although any other movement now believe that anaesthetics act at different sites, and type could have been included. Thus, a pawing motion or therefore knowledge regarding the anaesthetic concentra- turning of the head towards the stimulus are usually tion required to achieve certain end-points (unconsciousness considered positive, while coughing, straining, chewing and immobility) is directly relevant to determining anaes- and stiffening are considered negative.74 In general, many thetic mechanisms.36 It is interesting to note that an investigators also consider simple withdrawal of the stimu- editorial44 published nearly 30 yr after the work of lated extremity as being negative. What is important is that Stoelting and colleagues and Waud and Waud commented in any individual study there is consistency as to what is on the effect of fentanyl on MAC-awake56 (Fig. 4), stating positive and what is negative movement. that such studies were critical to our elucidation of The noxious stimulus must be supramaximal. That is, anaesthetic mechanisms. increasing the stimulus intensity must not result in further Dwyer and colleagues determined that consciousness was increases in anaesthetic requirements. A mechanical stimu- present at iso¯urane concentrations (0.3 MAC) at which lus is usually used, such as a clamp placed across the tail or a memory formation was impaired.30 Furthermore, the hind paw. The stimulus is applied for 1 min or until authors concluded that, at equipotent MAC concentrations, movement occurs, after which the anaesthetic concentration iso¯urane was more potent than nitrous oxide as regards the is changed (up or down, depending on the response). MAC production of unconsciousness and the reduction of explicit is de®ned as the average of the two anaesthetic concentra- memory formation. The volunteers in this study were not tions that are observed to just permit and just prevent the subjected to noxious stimulation, as might occur during movement, respectively. In humans undergoing surgery, a surgery, and so it is unclear whether the amount of skin incision is often used to determine the MAC, but this anaesthetic needed for amnesia and unconsciousness is stimulus can be applied only once. A population MAC can increased during noxious stimulation; presumably it is. In an still be obtained by adjusting the anaesthetic concentration earlier study, however, this group of investigators showed in subsequent patients so that about half of these patients that iso¯urane at 0.6 MAC or greater prevented conscious- move and half do not move. ness and memory formation (explicit and implicit) during 159
  • 5. Antognini and Carstens Interestingly, Steriade and colleagues have reported that cortical activity (as assessed by the EEG) is more sensitive to anaesthesia than subcortical (e.g. thalamic) activity.89 We have made similar observations (Fig. 5). The data of both Steriade and colleagues and ourselves were obtained at anaesthetic concentrations greatly exceeding those required to prevent memory formation and consciousness. Nonetheless, it is theoretically possible that anaesthetic- induced unconsciousness may occur primarily as the result of anaesthetic action within the cerebral cortex. Dwyer and colleagues collected EEG data in iso¯urane-anaesthetized patients to determine if processed EEG variables might correlate with clinically relevant anaesthetic end-points.32 Spectral edge frequency, median power and total power Fig 4 Effect of fentanyl on sevo¯urane requirements. Note that MAC- were among the variables examined. The authors were BAR (the minimum anaesthetic concentration required to block unable to make predictions regarding patient movement, adrenergic responses) is affected the most, suggesting that analgesia is an memory formation or consciousness.32 Indeed, human and important tool for the control of haemodynamic responses to noxious rat data suggest that marked EEG depression resulting from stimulation. MAC-awake (the concentration that results in iso¯urane and thiopental does not correlate with movement unconsciousness) is affected least and merges with MAC and MAC- BAR. Thus a patient given opiates might not move or have a resulting from noxious stimulation,51 77 although haemo- haemodynamic response to noxious stimulation but might be conscious. dynamic responses to laryngoscopy and tracheal intubation This is not unexpected, because there are numerous reports of patients might correlate with EEG changes.79 awake during what is primarily an opiate `anaesthetic'. Based on the There is substantial evidence that some anaesthetics work of Katoh and colleagues.56 57 prevent movement via a spinal cord action,10 16 75 78 but is there any reason to believe that other end-points (amnesia, surgical procedures.31 Ropcke and colleagues showed that È unconsciousness) might be affected by a spinal cord action? noxious stimulation can alter anaesthetic effects on the We believe that there is some indirect evidence. First, electroencephalogram (EEG).81 They studied two groups of epidural anaesthesia decreases the amount of anaesthetic des¯urane-anaesthetized patients, one with and one without required to prevent the movement that results from noxious noxious surgical stimulation. At equal concentrations, the stimulation applied above the level of the block.50 This group with surgical stimulation demonstrated EEG acti- might be due to an indirect action in the brain, such as vation, whereas the non-stimulated group did not. These resetting of the brain's arousal level. Secondly, spinal and data suggest that noxious stimulation during otherwise epidural anaesthetics decrease the amount of sedative/ adequate anaesthesia results in a shift towards increased hypnotics needed to achieve a certain level of arousal.81 sedation,33 49 92 93 presumably by blocking afferent Learning and memory formation during anaesthesia impulses, and sedation is associated with amnesia and depends on the circumstances under which stimuli are unconsciousness.43 Thirdly, volatile anaesthetics and i.v. presented. Dutton and colleagues studied conditioned anaesthetics (such as propofol and thiopental) block learning in rats anaesthetized with iso¯urane.29 They nociceptive impulses in the spinal cord.59 67 94 Such actions found that fear-conditioning to a tone was less sensitive to blunt the ascending transmission of noxious impulses to the iso¯urane than fear-conditioning to context (e.g. the brain, which is likely to diminish the arousal of the brain, surrounding environment). Approximately 0.5 MAC iso- thereby decreasing the probability of memory formation and ¯urane was required to prevent fear-conditioning associated consciousness (Fig. 6). We have evidence that iso¯urane, with the tone compared with 0.25 MAC for conditioning probably by a spinal cord action, might affect brain arousal. associated with the context.29 When iso¯urane is selectively removed from the torso while Non-immobilizers are compounds that are predicted to be a constant concentration is kept in the brain, a noxious anaesthetics on the basis of their physicochemical properties stimulus can increase brain arousal, as indicated by but in fact are not. Originally these compounds were called increased neuronal activity in the reticular formation and non-anaesthetics but one report has shown that they can thalamus and EEG desynchronization.12 18 We have made suppress learning and hence memory.55 Amnesia is a similar observations with propofol.15 desired anaesthetic end-point, and thus it would be impre- cise to call these compounds non-anaesthetics. Because these molecules do not contribute to immobility, the Haemodynamic and neuroendocrine control consensus is to label them non-immobilizers. These data Roizen and colleagues examined the ability of halothane, offer further evidence that anaesthetic end-points are likely en¯urane, opiates and spinal anaesthesia to block the to result from anaesthetic action at different sites.87 cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses to skin inci- 160
  • 6. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia Fig 5 At the top are shown the bifrontal EEGs from a goat anaesthetized with iso¯urane at 1.5, 3.5 and 5% (3 min and 10 s samples). Below are peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) representing the corresponding activity of a midbrain reticular formation (MRF) cell. In the ®rst two PSTHs the arrows represent application of a clamp to the goat's lip for 1 min. This MRF cell is inhibited by the noxious stimulus, but at 3.5% iso¯urane the spontaneous activity is increased and the cell is not completely inhibited by the noxious clamp. Note that at 3.5 and 5% iso¯urane the EEG is isoelectric (with occasional spikes) but the MRF cell is still active. sion.80 Halothane and en¯urane were added to 60% nitrous mentioned above, one reason may be the presence of oxide. They described the blocking effect in terms of the analgesic properties. Iso¯urane, for example, appears to anaesthetic concentration that blocked adrenergic responses have little or no analgesic effect at subanaesthetic concen- (MAC-BAR).80 A positive response was arbitrarily de®ned trations. Petersen-Felix and colleagues did not detect any as an increase of 10% or more in heart rate, blood pressure evidence of analgesia to noxious thermal, mechanical or or norepinephrine values. Interestingly, while halothane and electrical stimulation at iso¯urane concentrations ranging an opiate±nitrous oxide technique could block adrenergic from 0.0 to 0.26%.73 Halothane, on the other hand, does responses at clinically relevant concentrations, en¯urane have analgesic properties,85 although not all studies con- could not. Spinal anaesthesia, as long as it blocked the pain cur.91 Anaesthetic-induced sedation and unconsciousness of incision, also blocked the neuroendocrine and cardio- makes interpretation of analgesia studies dif®cult.85 The vascular response. Could the inability of en¯urane to block presence of analgesic properties will blunt the nociceptive these responses be related to its poor analgesic properties? responses (e.g. increased blood pressure and heart rate). While it is tempting to think that the ability of halothane to Although most studies suggest that anaesthetics have some block the cardiovascular responses is in part due to direct analgesic properties or none, there is evidence that at cardiovascular depression, en¯urane is one of the most relatively low anaesthetic concentration (0.1 MAC) hyper- potent cardiovascular depressants, and yet it was unable to algesia might occur. Zhang and colleagues studied four prevent the cardiovascular responses to incisions. This anaesthetics on hind paw withdrawal elicited by noxious concept of MAC-BAR was investigated further by Zbinden stimulation in rats.100 Iso¯urane, halothane, nitrous oxide and colleagues, who studied patients anaesthetized with and diethyl ether decreased withdrawal latency, suggesting iso¯urane.98 99 Different stimuli were used, such as a hyperalgesic effect. Barbiturates are also associated with trapezius squeeze, tetanic stimulation and skin incision. hyperalgesia.19 They found that iso¯urane, in clinically relevant concen- A second way that anaesthetics might alter cardiovascular trations, was unable to prevent the haemodynamic response responses is by direct action on the heart and blood vessels. to noxious stimulation, even when iso¯urane decreased the In addition, we examined whether anaesthetic action in the prestimulation value to clinically unacceptable levels. These brain might affect cardiovascular responses to noxious data suggest that iso¯urane by itself is not a good agent to stimulation.8 During differential iso¯urane delivery to the use to control a patient's blood pressure adequately within a head, we observed that heart rate and blood pressure normal range. increased signi®cantly during noxious stimulation and that Why do some anaesthetics suppress these haemodynamic only supraclinical iso¯urane concentrations in the brain responses to noxious stimulation while others do not? As could prevent this response.8 161
  • 7. Antognini and Carstens and then transected, a method that minimizes or prevents spinal shock. It was reported that the iso¯urane MAC (determined when the noxious stimulus was applied below the level of transection) was unchanged.75 We have used a goat model to investigate the relative roles of the brain and spinal cord in anaesthetic actions.7 13 16 In brief, because of the unique cerebral circulation of goats, we can differentially deliver anaesthetics to the head (brain) or torso (spinal cord). Within the central nervous system, the watershed area where the torso and head circulation mix is at the level of the caudal medulla or upper cervical cord, the exact level depending on the blood pressure difference between the systemic and cranial circulations.13 With an intact native circulation, iso¯urane MAC in these goats was 1.2%.16 During differential iso¯urane delivery, cranial iso¯urane requirements (to suppress movement) were »3% when torso iso¯urane concentration (and hence spinal cord concentration) was low (»0.2±0.3%).16 As one would expect, the EEG was depressed at these high iso¯urane concentrations in the brain. We subsequently examined whether halothane and thiopental had similar effects on movement.10 Halothane appeared to be less potent in the brain (compared with iso¯urane) inasmuch as halothane at high concentrations (>4%) delivered to the cranial circulation could not stop movement in some animals. Overall, the cranial halothane requirement in- creased by nearly 400%. Despite a greatly depressed or isoelectric EEG, these animals still moved in response to noxious stimulation. In contrast, the cranial requirement for Fig 6 Differential anaesthetic delivery aids elucidation of how anaesthetic action in the spinal cord affects brain arousal.12 18 In the top thiopental increased by a factor of only 2, because panel, the anaesthetic (iso¯urane) concentration in the spinal cord is »40 mg ml±1 was required to prevent movement during suf®cient to effectively block the ascending transmission of nociceptive cranial thiopental delivery whereas »20 mg ml±1 was impulses to the midbrain reticular formation (MRF), thalamus (Thal) and required during systemic administration.10 Interestingly, brain; no EEG activation occurs. In one scenario (middle), decreasing the the EEG was active during the differential thiopental spinal concentration of anaesthesia facilitates the ascending transmission of nociceptive impulses, but the sensitivity of the brain is unchanged delivery even though the animals ceased moving. compared with the top panel. There is minimal EEG activation, owing to We have interpreted these collective data10 16 to suggest the added nociceptive impulses that are transmitted to the brain. In the that anaesthetic action in the spinal cord is important for the scenario in the lower panel, we hypothesize that the brain is more ability of iso¯urane, halothane and thiopental to prevent sensitive to the additional nociceptive impulses, and there is marked EEG movement. There are important differences, however. First, activation (desynchronization). The enhanced sensitivity might be due to increased ascending afferent activity that `resets' the brain's arousal halothane and iso¯urane appear to differ in that substantially level. By indirectly diminishing brain arousal, the spinal cord action of more halothane is required in the brain to prevent movement anaesthetics could affect memory and unconsciousness. (compared with iso¯urane). Secondly, although the relative increases in the cranial requirements for thiopental and iso¯urane were quantitatively similar, there was a distinct qualitative difference in that thiopental, but not iso¯urane, Spinal cord as a site of anaesthetic action permitted an active EEG when its cranial action could For many decades, the brain has been thought to be the site prevent movement. This indicates that the immobilizing of anaesthetic action, insofar as memory, consciousness and effect of thiopental in the brain is more potent than that of initiation of movement occur in the brain. In the last decade halothane, relative to the EEG suppression effect. several lines of evidence have emerged indicating that the Furthermore, it is not entirely clear that iso¯urane, spinal cord might be an important site of anaesthetic action. halothane and thiopental act at common sites in the brain. The work of Rampil and colleagues strongly suggests that Although we have dismissed other sites in the torso as an action of iso¯urane on the spinal cord is important to regards anaesthetic action, how do we know with certainty suppression of movement.75 76 78 The MAC was unchanged that, when using an experimental preparation in which we after precollicular decerebration in rats.78 In a follow-up remove an anaesthetic from the torso, we can ascribe an study, a section of the spinal cord was rendered hypothermic action to the spinal cord? Is it possible that anaesthetics 162
  • 8. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia might act at the other sites, such as the peripheral nerve? action on the spinal motor neurone with little or no indirect Halothane and iso¯urane do not depress peripheral supraspinal component.11 nociceptors; in fact, excitation is more likely.24 63 The The ability of anaesthetic agents to produce immobility is peripheral nerve is not substantially affected by clinical usually assessed by their effectiveness in blocking gross concentrations of anaesthetics.26 purposeful movement. An understanding of the underlying To rule out a peripheral site of anaesthetic action, we mechanism will ultimately require study of anaesthetic performed a bypass study in dogs in which we selectively effects on complex motor processing. The spinal cord removed iso¯urane from the lower torso.14 Anaesthetic appears to be capable of generating a variety of complex, requirements (MAC) were determined with application of coordinated movement patterns. Decerebrate rats are cap- the noxious stimulus to a lower torso site in the presence or able of normal activity, such as grooming, exploring their absence of iso¯urane at that site. We found that MAC was cages and eating.62 97 Frogs sectioned at an upper cervical unchanged, suggesting that peripheral action of iso¯urane is level still have a normal wiping re¯ex, whereby the hind not important, at least as regards immobility. limb moves to wipe away a noxious chemical stimulus Within the spinal cord, the dorsal horn has received much applied to the forelimb. If the forelimb is moved, the hind attention as a site of anaesthetic action. Because the dorsal limb is able to determine the position of the forelimb, horn acts as a `spinal gate', anaesthetic effects on dorsal despite the absence of any supraspinal input.41 Cats horn neuronal responses have been investigated over several sectioned at a mid-thoracic level still exhibit fairly normal decades.27 67 Virtually all anaesthetics examined appeared locomotion on a treadmill after recovery from spinal shock to depress spontaneous ®ring and/or responses to innocuous and with support of the body weight.39 72 Lastly, brain-dead and noxious stimuli. There were, however, several meth- humans can display the Lazarus phenomenon, in which they odological differences among these studies, making direct have spontaneous movement, including crossing of the arms comparisons dif®cult. We were interested in the depressant over the chest, sitting up in bed and turning of the head.25 64 effect of iso¯urane on dorsal horn neuronal responses to All of the complex movement patterns described above supraspinal noxious stimulation within the narrow range are mediated by neural circuits, including central pattern that just permitted and just prevented movement (0.9±1.0 generators (CPGs), which are intrinsic to the spinal cord. It MAC).9 We observed that increasing the iso¯urane con- is currently not known how anaesthetics affect CPGs. To centration from 0.9 to 1.1 MAC resulted in a modest (15%) begin to address this question, we recently investigated the depression in evoked responses. It is unclear if a change of effects of inhaled anaesthetic agents on the pattern of this small order of magnitude is suf®cient to account for coordinated limb and head movements evoked by supra- such a signi®cant behavioural change. Furthermore, the maximal stimulation in rats.17 All limbs and the snout were neurones studied were not identi®ed according to their attached to force transducers to measure the direction and function, e.g. as re¯ex interneurones or ascending projection force of movements. At sub-MAC concentrations of neurones. It is conceivable that anaesthetics may exert iso¯urane or halothane, the supramaximal noxious stimulus differential effects on functionally distinct classes of spinal elicited synchronous, rather than alternating, movements of neurones, an issue that deserves further study. all four limbs. When there was head movement it was Over the last several years the spinal motor neurone has almost always towards the side of the noxious stimulus. As received signi®cant attention as a possible site of anaes- the concentration of iso¯urane or halothane was increased, thetic action. In a series of studies, Rampil and Zhou and there was ®rst a reduction in the number of movements, and their respective colleagues indirectly assessed anaesthetic at higher concentrations, a reduction in the force of effects on the motor neurone using the F-wave.58 76 101 102 movements. We are at an early stage in investigating The F-wave is evoked by electrical stimulation of a anaesthetic action on complex movement patterns, and there peripheral motor (or mixed) nerve, and is thought to result are many open questions. Noxious stimuli evoke limb from antidromic invasion of the motor neuronal cell body withdrawal re¯exes; what is the relationship of these fairly with recurrent orthodromic conduction back down the simple re¯ex pathways to CPGs involved in generating motor nerve to evoke muscle contraction.76 The F-wave is more complex escape responses? Does noxious stimulation thought to re¯ect motor neurone excitability, although the elicit synchronous (or other) patterns of coordinated limb evidence for this is mainly indirect. The F-wave is enhanced movements in other species? Studies addressing these and in clinical conditions associated with increased motor related questions will be worthwhile in understanding better neurone excitability, such as spasticity. Inhaled agents how anaesthetics affect spinal CPGs that generate complex (iso¯urane, halothane, des¯urane, sevo¯urane) and nitrous movement patterns. oxide have been shown to depress the F-wave.40 58 76 101 102 However, it is not clear if this depression is a causal factor in the ability of anaesthetic agents to produce immobility or if Undesirable effects it is merely correlated with immobility that is primarily Although the primary emphasis of research on anaesthetic caused by some other anaesthetic action. In any event, mechanisms is to determine how anaesthetics produce their anaesthetic depression of the F-wave appears to be a direct desired effects, we must also determine how these powerful 163
  • 9. Antognini and Carstens drugs produce undesired effects, such as post-operative other anaesthetic agents on the centripetal transmission of nausea and vomiting (PONV) and respiratory and cardio- sensory information. Gen Pharmacol 1992; 23: 945±63 7 Antognini JF, Jinks S, Buzin V, Carstens E. A method for vascular depression. Cyclopropane and ether produced differential delivery of intravenous drugs to the head and torso limited cardiovascular depressive effects,35 45 53 but were of the goat. Anesth Analg 1998; 87: 1450±2 associated with PONV (particularly ether). Both drugs were 8 Antognini JF, Berg K. Cardiovascular responses to noxious eliminated from clinical use because of in¯ammability. stimuli during iso¯urane anesthesia are minimally affected by Halogenation of the carbon skeleton eliminated in¯amma- anesthetic action in the brain. Anesth Analg 1995; 81: 843±8 bility but also introduced cardiovascular depression. Thus, 9 Antognini JF, Carstens, E. Increasing iso¯urane from 0.9 to 1.1 changing the structure of an anaesthetic might enhance minimum alveolar concentration minimally affects dorsal horn cell responses to noxious stimulation. Anesthesiology 1999; 90: desirable characteristics but may also introduce or exacer- 208±14 bate side-effects. 10 Antognini JF, Carstens E, Atherley R. Does thiopental's Respiratory depression is a common side-effect of immobilizing effect in brain exceed that of halothane? virtually all anaesthetics. The mechanisms for this effect Anesthesiology 2002; 96: 980±6 have not been fully elucidated, but there are likely to be 11 Antognini JF, Carstens E, Buzin V. Iso¯urane depresses differences among anaesthetics. Nieuwenhuijs and col- motoneuron excitability by a direct spinal action: an F-wave leagues found that propofol depressed the central chemore- study. Anesth Analg 1999; 88: 681±5 12 Antognini JF, Carstens E, Sudo M, Sudo S. Iso¯urane depresses ¯ex loop but had no signi®cant effect on the peripheral electroencephalographic and medial thalamic responses to chemore¯ex loop.69 Inhaled anaesthetics depress the per- noxious stimulation via an indirect spinal action. Anesth Analg ipheral loop. Further work is required to elucidate mech- 2000; 91: 1282±8 anisms of respiratory depression. Hopefully it will be 13 Antognini JF, Kien ND. A method for preferential delivery of possible someday to develop an anaesthetic that has little or volatile anesthetics to the in situ goat brain. Anesthesiology 1994; no cardiopulmonary depression. 80: 1148±54 From a patient's perspective, PONV is one of the most 14 Antognini JF, Kien ND. Potency (minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration) of iso¯urane is independent of peripheral common distressing complications of surgery and anaes- anesthetic effects. Anesth Analg 1995; 81: 69±72 thesia. However, it is not a life-threatening complication. 15 Antognini JF, Saadi J, Wang XW, Carstens E, Piercy M. Propofol There is little research examining the mechanisms of action in both spinal cord and brain blunts electro- anaesthetic-induced PONV. encephalographic responses to noxious stimulation in goats. There is evidence that anaesthetics might in¯uence the Sleep 2001; 24: 26±31 immune responses that are important in recovery after 16 Antognini JF, Schwartz K. Exaggerated anesthetic requirements surgery.47 68 70 As surgical techniques progress and in the preferentially anesthetized brain. Anesthesiology 1993; 79: 1244±9 improve, patient outcome could perhaps be tied more 17 Antognini JF, Wang XW, Carstens E. Quantitative and qualitative closely to these subtle effects. effects of iso¯urane on movement occurring after noxious stimulation. Anesthesiology 1999; 91: 1064±71 18 Antognini JF, Wang XW, Carstens, E. Iso¯urane action in the spinal cord blunts electroencephalographic and thalamic- Acknowledgements reticular formation responses to noxious stimulation in goats. The authors were supported in part by grants NIH GM57970 and GM61283. Anesthesiology 2000; 92: 559±66 19 Archer DP, Ewen A, Roth SH, Samanani N. Plasma, brain, and spinal cord concentrations of thiopental associated with hyperalgesia in the rat. Anesthesiology 1994; 80: 168±76 References 20 Bachman L, Eger EI 2nd, Waud BE, Waud DR. MAC and 1 Alkire MT. Quantitative EEG correlations with brain glucose dose±response curves. Anesthesiology 1971; 34: 201±4 metabolic rate during anesthesia in volunteers. Anesthesiology 21 Bennett HL, Davis HS, Giannini JA. Non-verbal response to 1998; 89: 323±33 intraoperative conversation. Br J Anaesth 1985; 57: 174±9 2 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Barker SJ, et al. Cerebral metabolism during 22 Berg-Johnsen J, Langmoen IA. Iso¯urane hyperpolarizes propofol anesthesia in humans studied with positron emission neurones in rat and human cerebral cortex. Acta Physiol Scand tomography. Anesthesiology 1995; 82: 393±403 1987; 130: 679±85 3 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Fallon JH. Toward a uni®ed theory of 23 Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA. Human learning narcosis: brain imaging evidence for a thalamocortical switch as during general anaesthesia and surgery. Br J Anaesth 1991; 66: the neurophysiologic basis of anesthetic-induced unconscious- 170±8 ness. Consciousness Cogn 2000; 9: 370±86 24 Campbell JN, Raja SN, Meyer RA. Halothane sensitizes 4 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Shah NK, Anderson CT. Positron emission cutaneous nociceptors in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 1984; 52: tomography study of regional cerebral metabolism in humans 762±70 during iso¯urane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 549±57 25 Christie JM, O'Lenic TD, Cane RD. Head turning in brain death. J 5 Alkire MT, Pomfrett CJ, Haier RJ, et al. Functional brain imaging Clin Anesth 1996; 8: 141±3 during anesthesia in humans: effects of halothane on global and 26 de Jong RH, Nace RA. Nerve impulse conduction and cutaneous regional cerebral glucose metabolism. Anesthesiology 1999; 90: receptor responses during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 701±9 1967; 28: 851±5 6 Angel A, LeBeau F. A comparison of the effects of propofol with 27 de Jong RH, Wagman IH. Block of afferent impulses in the dorsal 164
  • 10. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia horn of monkey. A possible mechanism of anesthesia. Exp Neurol sepsis following anesthesia in mice. Anesthesiology 1985; 63: 1968; 20: 352±8 267±73 28 Devor M, Zalkind V. Reversible analgesia, atonia, and loss of 48 Hill GE, Stanley TH, Sentker CR. Physostigmine reversal of post- consciousness on bilateral intracerebral microinjection of operative somnolence. Can Anaesth Soc J 1977; 24: 707±11 pentobarbital. Pain 2001; 94: 101±2 49 Hodgson PS, Liu SS. Epidural lidocaine decreases sevo¯urane 29 Dutton RC, Maurer AJ, Sonner JM, Fanselow MS, Laster MJ, Eger requirement for adequate depth of anesthesia as measured by EI 2nd. The concentration of iso¯urane required to suppress the Bispectral Indexâ monitor. Anesthesiology 2001; 94: 799±803 learning depends on the type of learning. Anesthesiology 2001; 94: 50 Hodgson PS, Liu SS, Gras TW. Does epidural anesthesia have 514±9 general anesthetic effects? Anesthesiology 1999; 91: 1687±92 30 Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI 2nd, Heilbron D. Effects of 51 Hung OR, Varvel JR, Shafer SL, Stanski DR. Thiopental iso¯urane and nitrous oxide in subanesthetic concentrations on pharmacodynamics. II. Quantitation of clinical and memory and responsiveness in volunteers. Anesthesiology 1992; electroencephalographic depth of anesthesia. Anesthesiology 77: 888±98 1992; 77: 237±44 31 Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI 2nd, Peterson N. Iso¯urane 52 Jones BE. The organization of central cholinergic systems and anesthesia prevents unconscious learning. Anesth Analg 1992; 75: their functional importance in sleep-waking states. Prog Brain Res 107±12 1993; 98: 61±71 32 Dwyer RC, Rampil IJ, Eger EI, Bennett HL. The 53 Jones RE, Guldmann N, Linde HW, Dripps RD, Price HL. electroencephalogram does not predict depth of iso¯urane Cyclopropane anesthesia. III. Effects of cyclopropane on anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1994; 81: 403±9 respiration and circulation in normal man. Anesthesiology 1960; 33 Eappen S, Kissin I. Effect of subarachnoid bupivacaine block on 21: 380±93 anesthetic requirements for thiopental in rats. Anesthesiology 54 Kandel E. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue 1998; 88: 1036±42 between genes and synapses. Science 2001; 294: 1030±8 34 Eger EI, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Minimum alveolar anesthetic 55 Kandel L, Chortkoff BS, Sonner J, Laster MJ, Eger EI 2nd. concentration: a standard of anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology Nonanesthetics can suppress learning. Anesth Analg 1996; 82: 1965; 26: 756±63 321±6 35 Eger EI 2nd, Smith NT, Cullen DJ, Cullen BF, Gregory GA. A 56 Katoh T, Ikeda K. The effects of fentanyl on sevo¯urane comparison of the cardiovascular effects of halothane, ¯uroxene, requirements for loss of consciousness and skin incision. Â ether and cyclopropane in man: a resume. Anesthesiology 1971; Anesthesiology 1998; 88: 18±24 34: 25±41 57 Katoh T, Kobayashi S, Suzuki A, Iwamoto T, Bito H, Ikeda K. The 36 Eger EI 2nd, Koblin DD, Harris RA, et al. Hypothesis: inhaled effect of fentanyl on sevo¯urane requirements for somatic and anesthetics produce immobility and amnesia by different sympathetic responses to surgical incision. Anesthesiology 1999; mechanisms at different sites. Anesth Analg 1997; 84: 915±8 90: 398±405. 37 Eichenbaum HB, Cahill LF, Gluck MA, et al. Learning and 58 King BS, Rampil IJ. Anesthetic depression of spinal motor memory: systems analysis. In: Zigmond MJ, Bloom FE, Landis SC, neurons may contribute to lack of movement in response to Roberts JL, Squire LR, eds. Fundamental Neuroscience. San Diego: noxious stimuli. Anesthesiology 1994; 81: 1484±92 Academic Press, 1999; 1455±86 59 Kitahata LM, Ghazi-Saidi K, Yamashita M, Kosaka, Bonikos C, 38 Fiset P, Paus T, Daloze T, et al. Brain mechanisms of propofol- Taub A. The depressant effect of halothane and sodium induced loss of consciousness in humans: a positron emission thiopental on the spontaneous and evoked activity of dorsal tomographic study. J Neurosci 1999; 19: 5506±13 horn cells: lamina speci®city, time course and dose dependence. J 39 Forssberg H, Grillner S, Rossignol S. Phase dependent re¯ex Pharmacol Exp Ther 1975; 195: 515±21 reversal during walking in chronic spinal cats. Brain Res 1975; 85: 60 Krasowski MD, Harrison NL. General anaesthetic actions on 103±7 ligand-gated ion channels. Cell Mol Life Sci 1999; 55: 1278±303 40 Friedman Y, King BS, Rampil I J. Nitrous oxide depresses spinal F 61 Kurosawa M, Sato A, Sato Y. Cutaneous mechanical sensory waves in rats. Anesthesiology 1996; 85: 135±41 stimulation increases extracellular acetylcholine release in 41 Fukson OI, Berkinblit MB, Feldman AG. The spinal frog takes cerebral cortex in anesthetized rats. Neurochem Int 1992; 21: into account the scheme of its body during the wiping re¯ex. 423±7 Science 1980; 209: 1261±3 62 Lovick TA. The behavioural repertoire of precollicular 42 Ghoneim MM, Block RI. Learning and consciousness during decerebrate rats. J Physiol (Lond)1972; 226: 4P±6P general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992; 76: 279±305 63 MacIver MB, Tanelian DL. Volatile anesthetics excite mammalian 43 Glass PS, Bloom M, Kearse L, Rosow C, Sebel P, Manberg P. nociceptor afferents recorded in vitro. Anesthesiology 1990; 72: Bispectral analysis measures sedation and memory effects of 1022±30 propofol, midazolam, iso¯urane, and alfentanil in healthy 64 Mandel S, Arenas A, Scasta D. Spinal automatism in cerebral volunteers. Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 836±47 death [letter]. N Engl J Med 1982; 307: 501±1 44 Glass PS. Anesthetic drug interactions: an insight into general 65 Meuret P, Backman SB, Bonhomme V, Plourde G, Fiset P. anesthesiaÐits mechanism and dosing strategies. Anesthesiology Physostigmine reverses propofol-induced unconsciousness and 1998; 88: 5±6 attenuation of the auditory steady state response and bispectral 45 Gregory GA, Eger EI 2nd, Smith NT, Cullen BF, Cullen DJ. The index in human volunteers. Anesthesiology 2000; 93: 708±17 cardiovascular effects of diethyl ether in man. Anesthesiology 66 Moruzzi G, Magoun HW. Brain stem reticular formation and 1971; 34: 19±24 activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1949; 46 Guedel AE. Third stage ether anesthesia: a sub-classi®cation 1: 455±73 regarding the signi®cance of the position and movements of the 67 Namiki A, Collins JG, Kitahata LM, Kikuchi H, Homma E, eyeball. Am J Surg (Anesth Suppl) 1920; 34: 53±7 Thalhammer JG. Effects of halothane on spinal neuronal 47 Hansbrough JF, Zapata-Sirvent RL, Bartle EJ, et al. Alterations in responses to graded noxious heat stimulation in the cat. splenic lymphocyte subpopulations and increased mortality from Anesthesiology 1980; 53: 475±80 165
  • 11. Antognini and Carstens 68 Mikawa K, Akamatsu H, Nishina K, et al. Propofol inhibits human 86 Snow J. On the Inhalation of the Vapour of Ether in Surgical neutrophil functions. Anesth Analg 1998; 87: 695±700 Operations. London: Churchill, 1847. 69 Nieuwenhuijs D, Sarton E, Teppema LJ, et al. Respiratory sites of 87 Sonner JM, Li J, Eger EI 2nd. Des¯urane and the nonimmobilizer action of propofol. Absence of depression of peripheral 1,2-dichlorohexa¯uorocyclobutane suppress learning by a chemore¯ex loop by low-dose propofol. Anesthesiology 2001; mechanism independent of the level of unconditioned 95: 889±95 stimulation. Anesth Analg 1998; 87: 200±5 70 Nishina K, Akamatsu H, Mikawa K, et al. The inhibitory effects of 88 Steriade M. Arousal: revisiting the reticular activating system. thiopental, midazolam, and ketamine on human neutrophil Science 1996; 272: 225±6 functions. Anesth Analg 1998; 86: 159±65 89 Steriade M, Amzica F, Contreras D. Cortical and thalamic 71 Pajewski TN, DiFazio CA, Moscicki JC, Johns RA. Nitric oxide cellular correlates of electroencephalographic burst- synthase inhibitors, 7-nitro indazole and nitroG-L-arginine suppression. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994; 90: 1±16 methyl ester, dose dependently reduce the threshold for 90 Stoelting RK, Longnecker DE, Eger EI 2nd. Minimum alveolar iso¯urane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1996; 85: 1111±9 concentrations in man on awakening from methoxy¯urane, 72 Pearson K. The control of walking. Sci Am 1976; 235: 72±4, halothane, ether and ¯uroxene anesthesia: MAC awake. 79±82, 83±6 Anesthesiology 1970; 33: 5±9 73 Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bak P, et al. Analgesic effect 91 Tomi K, Mashimo T, Tashiro C, et al. Alterations in pain in humans of subanaesthetic iso¯urane concentrations evaluated threshold and psychomotor response associated with by experimentally induced pain. Br J Anaesth 1995; 75: 55±60. subanaesthetic concentrations of inhalation anaesthetics in 74 Quasha AL, Eger EI, Tinker JH. Determination and applications of humans. Br J Anaesth 1993; 70: 684±6 MAC. Anesthesiology 1980; 53: 315±34 92 Tverskoy M, Fleyshman G, Bachrak L, Ben-Shlomo I. Effect of 75 Rampil IJ. Anesthetic potency is not altered after hypothermic bupivacaine-induced spinal block on the hypnotic requirement of spinal cord transection in rats. Anesthesiology 1994; 80: 606±10 propofol. Anaesthesia 1996; 51: 652±3 76 Rampil IJ, King BS. Volatile anesthetics depress spinal motor 93 Tverskoy M, Shagal M, Finger J, Kissin I. Subarachnoid neurons. Anesthesiology 1996; 85: 129±34 bupivacaine blockade decreases midazolam and thiopental 77 Rampil IJ, Laster MJ. No correlation between quantitative hypnotic requirements. J Clin Anesth 1994; 6: 487±90. electroencephalographic measurements and movement 94 Uchida H, Kishikawa K, Collins JG. Effect of propofol on spinal response to noxious stimuli during iso¯urane anesthesia in rats. Anesthesiology 1992; 77: 920±5 dorsal horn neurons. Comparison with lack of ketamine effects. 78 Rampil IJ, Mason P, Singh H. Anesthetic potency (MAC) is Anesthesiology 1995; 83: 1312±22 independent of forebrain structures in the rat. Anesthesiology 95 Waud BE, Waud DR. On dose±response curves and anesthetics. 1993; 78: 707±12 Anesthesiology 1970; 33: 1±4 79 Rampil IJ, Matteo RS. Changes in EEG spectral edge frequency 96 Willis WD, Westlund KN. Neuroanatomy of the pain system correlate with the hemodynamic response to laryngoscopy and and of the pathways that modulate pain. J Clin Neurophysiol 1997; intubation. Anesthesiology 1987; 67: 139±42 14: 2±31 80 Roizen MF, Horrigan RW, Frazer BM. Anesthetic doses blocking 97 Woods JW. Behavior of chronic decerebrate rats. J Neurophysiol adrenergic (stress) and cardiovascular responses to incisionÐ 1964; 27: 635±44 MAC BAR. Anesthesiology 1981; 54: 390±8 98 Zbinden AM, Maggiorini M, Petersen-Felix S, Lauber R, Thomson È 81 Ropcke H, Rehberg B, Koenen-Bergmann M, Bouillon T, Bruhn J, DA, Minder CE. Anesthetic depth de®ned using multiple noxious Hoeft A. Surgical stimulation shifts EEG concentration±response stimuli during iso¯urane/oxygen anesthesia. I. Motor reactions. relationship of des¯urane. Anesthesiology 2001; 94: 390±9 Anesthesiology 1994; 80: 253±60 82 Scheller MS, Zornow MH, Fleischer JE, Shearman GT, Greber 99 Zbinden AM, Petersen-Felix S, Thomson DA. Anesthetic depth TF. The noncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor de®ned using multiple noxious stimuli during iso¯urane/oxygen antagonist MK-801 profoundly reduces volatile anesthetic anesthesia. II. Hemodynamic responses. Anesthesiology 1994; 80: requirements in rabbits. Neuropharmacology 1989; 28: 677±81 261±7 83 Shichino T, Murakawa M, Adachi T, et al. Effects of iso¯urane on 100 Zhang Y, Eger EI 2nd, Dutton RC, Sonner JM. Inhaled anesthetics in vivo release of acetylcholine in the rat cerebral cortex and have hyperalgesic effects at 0.1 minimum alveolar anesthetic striatum. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1997; 41: 1335±40 concentration. Anesth Analg 2000; 91: 462±6 84 Shimoji K, Fujioka H, Fukazawa T, Hashiba M, Maruyama Y. 101 Zhou HH, Jin TT, Qin B, Turndorf H. Suppression of spinal cord Anesthetics and excitatory/inhibitory responses of midbrain motoneuron excitability correlates with surgical immobility reticular neurons. Anesthesiology 1984; 61: 151±5 during iso¯urane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1998; 88: 955±61 85 Siker ES, Wolfson B, Ciccarelli HE, Telan RA. Effect of 102 Zhou HH, Mehta M, Leis AA. Spinal cord motoneuron subanesthetic concentrations of halothane and methoxy¯urane excitability during iso¯urane and nitrous oxide anesthesia. on pain threshold in conscious volunteers. Anesthesiology 1967; Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 302±7 28: 337±42 166