SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 15
Download to read offline
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
DOI 10.1007/s10882-012-9305-1 
ORIGINAL ARTICLE 
Development of a Combined Intervention to Decrease 
Problem Behavior Displayed by Siblings with Pervasive 
Developmental Disorder 
Niamh M. Doyle & Nicole M. DeRosa & 
Henry S. Roane 
Published online: 12 October 2012 
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012 
Abstract Approximately 10–20 % of children with pervasive developmental disor-der 
are likely to have a sibling with that disorder. Under such circumstances, care-givers 
may be faced with simultaneously implementing separate interventions for 
their children. Merging different treatments to decrease both children's problem 
behavior may permit for more fluid parent responses and increased procedural 
integrity. The current study describes one approach to treating problem behavior 
displayed by siblings with autism. Separate functional analyses identified a different 
maintaining reinforcer for each child's aggression. These behaviors were initially 
treated separately using functional communication training and delay fading. To 
ensure that the caregiver would be able to manage these siblings' behaviors concur-rently, 
a protocol using differential reinforcement of other behaviors was imple-mented 
that was based on the separate functions of each child's behavior. 
Keywords Parent training . Group contingency . Problembehavior 
Recent studies have indicated that caregivers of children with pervasive developmen-tal 
disorders (PDDs) often experience symptoms of anxiety and depression (Griffith 
and Richard 2010; Quintero and McIntyre 2010). Such symptoms can and often are 
exacerbated if the child also engages in problem behavior (e.g., aggression, disrup-tion, 
self-injurious behavior [SIB]). Parent training focusing on the implementation of 
behavioral interventions for problem behavior has reportedly been effective in dimin-ishing 
the negative experiences associated with caring for children with a variety of 
N. M. Doyle : N. M. DeRosa : H. S. Roane 
SUNY Upstate Medical University, The Kelberman Center, Syracuse, NY, USA 
N. M. Doyle (*) 
Department of Pediatrics, Upstate Medical University, 600 E. Genesee Street, Suite 124, Syracuse, NY 
13202, USA 
e-mail: doylen@upstate.edu
92 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
diagnoses, including autism (Farley et al. 2005; Pelham and Fabiano 2008). Several 
studies have examined the effectiveness of parent training in regard to both increasing 
children’s adaptive behavior skills, as well as decreasing problem behavior (Ben 
Chaabanne et al. 2009; Lerman et al. 2000; Meaden et al. 2009; Moes and Frea 2002; 
Smith and Lerman 1999). However, the effectiveness of parent training for caregivers 
in multiplex families (e.g., families with more than one child diagnosed with autism) 
has not been well studied. 
Approximately 10–20 % of children with autism also have a sibling diagnosed 
with the disorder (Constantine et al. 2010). Given that destructive behavior (e.g., SIB, 
aggression, or disruption) is common among children with developmental disabil-ities, 
parents of multiplex families may have to simultaneously implement separate 
behavior interventions. Additionally, group comparisons suggest that non-disabled 
siblings of children with autism may display characteristics of the disorder, as well as 
engage in severe problem behavior (Constantine et al. 2010; Petalas et al. 2009; 
Rodriguez et al. 1993; Ross and Cuskelly 2006). The implementation of separate 
interventions for more than one child may result in increased symptoms of anxiety 
and/or depression for caregivers, as well as potentially result in poor procedural 
integrity of one or both behavior interventions. 
The implementation ofmultiple treatmentsmay be similar to treating problem behavior 
that is maintained by multiple reinforcement contingencies (e.g., Borrero and Vollmer 
2006; Day et al. 1994; Ingvarsson et al. 2008; Lalli and Casey 1996; Smith et al. 
1993). More specifically, implementing multiple treatments can often be associated 
with several different procedural variables, such as different discriminative stimuli, 
communicative responses, prompting procedures, and contingencies for appropriate 
and problem behavior. Previous treatments for multiply controlled problem behavior 
have included separate interventions for each reinforcement contingency (Borrero 
and Vollmer 2006; Day et al. 1994; Smith et al. 1993) or treating both functions 
through the use of a highly preferred stimulus (e.g., food; Ingvarsson et al. 2008). 
Several authors have examined an alternative treatment approach for implement-ing 
multiple treatments simultaneously that involves merging the establishing oper-ation 
(EO) for multiple maintaining variables into a single context (Bachmeyer et al. 
2009; Call et al. 2005). An EO is an environmental event that (a) increases the 
effectiveness of a stimulus as a reinforcer and (b) increases the probability of the 
occurrence of behaviors that have produced that reinforcer in the past. 
Bachmeyer et al. evaluated individual and combined function-based extinction 
procedures for children whose inappropriate mealtime behavior was maintained 
by both positive and negative reinforcement. Attention extinction alone did not 
reduce problem behavior or increase acceptance of foods, while escape extinc-tion 
increased acceptance and reduced problem behavior, but not to clinically 
acceptable levels. Therefore, the combination of attention and escape extinction 
was evaluated and found to be effective in reducing inappropriate mealtime 
behavior, as well as maintaining high and stable acceptance. Call et al. com-pared 
single-antecedent functional analysis test conditions and combined-antecedent 
test conditions. In that study, problem behavior was only observed 
during the combined-antecedent test conditions, suggesting false-negative results 
from the single-antecedent assessment. Treatments derived from the results of the 
combined-antecedent functional analysis were effective in reducing occurrences of
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 93 
problem behavior. Both of these studies demonstrate that interventions, which targeted 
multiple reinforcement contingencies, can be effective in reducing targeted problem 
behavior when implemented by trained therapists within analog settings. However, it 
remains unknown if the effectiveness of such combined interventions maintains when 
implemented by caregivers. 
The purpose of the current study was twofold. In Study 1, we sought to evaluate 
differential treatments for siblings diagnosed with PDD whose problem behavior was 
maintained by different functional reinforcers. Second, we examined the integration 
of the two different behavioral interventions, using procedures similar to that of 
Bachmeyer et al. (2009) and Call et al. (2005), such that a combined treatment could 
be implemented for both children by their caregiver. 
General Procedures 
Participants and Settings 
Molly was an 8-year-old girl who had been previously diagnosed with a variety 
of behavior disorders, including PDD-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), 
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, and Oppositional Defiant Disorder. 
She was referred to an outpatient clinic for the treatment of aggression. She 
attended the clinic 3 days per week, for one 1.5-h appointment per day. Her 
brother Sam was 7-years old and had been previously diagnosed with PDD-NOS 
and Disruptive Behavior Disorder. He was referred to the same outpatient 
clinic for the treatment of aggression and disruption. Sam attended the clinic on 
the same schedule as Molly, though his initial treatment evaluation occurred 
following the conclusion of Molly’s admission. Both Molly and Sam partici-pated 
in Study 1 and Study 2. 
During Study 1, sessions occurred in individual treatment rooms (4 m×4 m) for 
both Molly and Sam. The rooms were equipped with a one-way observation window 
as well as various materials that depended on the condition in effect (e.g., table, 
chairs, preferred toys). During Study 2, sessions were conducted in a play area with 
both children and a therapist or the participants’ mother present. Baseline sessions in 
Study 1 were 5 min in duration, whereas treatment session durations varied according 
to individual protocols (described below). In Study 2, baseline sessions were 5 min in 
duration and treatment sessions varied based on the delay interval in place. For both 
participants, 3 to 5 sessions were conducted daily. 
Response Measurement and Reliability 
Aggression for both children was defined as contact, or attempted contact of hands, 
feet or legs against another person from a distance of 15 cm or greater; applying, or 
attempting to apply, force to a person’s body (e.g., pushing or pulling a person off 
balance or in a direction opposite of the person’s original movement); throwing an 
object at a person; pulling on another person’s hair; and pinching, scratching, or 
biting another person. Sam’s disruptive behavior was defined as throwing an object at 
a wall or a stationary object from a distance of 30 cm or greater; kicking or hitting
94 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
walls or windows; and turning or attempting to turn over furniture. The target 
communication response for Molly was a card exchange which consisted of Molly 
removing a card with the words, “Play, please” written on it, from the wall and 
placing it in the therapist’s hand. The communication response for Sam was vocally 
saying the phrase, “May I play with the ball, please.” For both participants, the 
targeted communication response was taught prior to their respective initial treatment 
analysis using the procedures described by Worsdell et al. (2000). 
Frequency data were collected for all dependent measures using laptop computers 
equipped with data-collection software. For the purpose of data analysis, all frequency 
data were converted to a response rate (responses per min; rpm). A second observer 
independently collected interobserver agreement (IOA) data during 33.9 % of all 
sessions. Exact agreement was calculated by partitioning each session into 10-s intervals 
and dividing agreements by total agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 
100. An agreement was scored when both observers recorded the occurrence of a 
response in the same 10-s interval, and a disagreement occurred when they did not. 
During Study 1, the mean IOA for Molly’s aggression and communication was 98.3 % 
(range, 83.3 % - 100 %) and 99.9 % (range, 96.7 % - 100 %), respectively. The mean 
IOA for Sam’s aggression, disruption, and communication during Study 1 was 94.3 % 
(range, 68.3 % - 100 %), 99.7 % (range, 96.7 % to 100 %), and 98.3 % (range, 88.3 % - 
100 %), respectively. During Study 2, IOA for Molly’s aggression averaged 99.3 % 
(range, 89.2 % - 100 %), IOA for Sam’s aggression averaged 99.4 % (range, 89.2 % - 
100 %), and IOA for Sam’s disruption was 100 %. 
Study 1: Individual Treatment Evaluations 
Method 
Preference Assessment Prior to each participant’s functional analysis, a paired-stimulus 
preference assessment (Fisher et al. 1992) was conducted separately for 
Molly and for Sam. During the preference assessment, a therapist presented a pair of 
items to the participant and instructed him/her to make a choice (e.g., “Molly, would 
you rather play with the ball or play on the computer?”). Following a selection, the 
participant was allowed to engage with the selected item for 30 s. The assessment 
continued until each toy was presented with every other toy one time. A hierarchy of 
preference was determined based on the percent of trials during which Molly and 
Sam selected each item. Results of this assessment were used in the subsequent 
functional analysis and treatment evaluations. 
Functional Analysis A functional analysis (FA), based on the procedures described 
by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) with modifications as described by Fisher et al. (1996), 
was conducted with each sibling. Molly and Sam were both exposed to demand, 
tangible, ignore, and toy play conditions. Additionally, Molly was exposed to a 
diverted attention condition, whereas Sam was exposed to a “standard” attention 
condition. During the demand condition, a therapist and the participant were seated at 
a table with instructional materials. The therapist instructed the participant to com-plete 
instructions using a three-step prompting hierarchy (successive verbal, gestural, 
and physical prompts) with no more than 5 s between each prompt. Praise was
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 95 
delivered contingent on compliance following either the verbal or gestural prompt. 
Contingent on a targeted response (e.g., aggression), a 20-s break from instructional 
demands occurred. Prior to each tangible session, the participant received approxi-mately 
2 min of pre-session access to his or her preferred item. Thereafter, the 
therapist removed the toy from the participant and kept it within sight but out of 
reach. The occurrence of a targeted response resulted in 20-s access to the preferred 
item. During the ignore condition, a therapist and participant were in the room with 
no materials present. The therapist did not interact with the participant throughout the 
session and no consequences were provided contingent on a targeted response. 
During the toy play condition, the therapist and participant were in the room with 
the participant’s highly preferred toy. The participant had access to the toy and the 
therapist interacted with the participant throughout the session, providing praise 
statements approximately every 30-s. No demands were delivered and no consequen-ces 
were provided contingent on a targeted response. During the diverted attention 
condition for Molly, two therapists and the participant were in the room with a low 
preferred toy. The two therapists engaged in a conversation with each other while 
Molly had access to a low preference toy. Contingent on a targeted response one 
therapist provided 20 s of attention in the form of verbal reprimands (e.g., saying, 
“Molly, stop that. We’re trying to talk.”). During Sam’s attention condition, a 
therapist was in the room with Sam, and Sam had access to a low preference toy 
while the therapist was reading. Contingent on a targeted response Sam received 20-s 
of attention in the form of verbal reprimands (e.g., saying, “Sam, it’s not nice when 
you throw something at me. You might break the window.”). 
Initial Treatment Analysis—Molly The initial treatment for Molly’s aggression was 
evaluated using a reversal (ABCBD) design, including baseline, functional commu-nication 
training (FCT), FCT+extinction (EXT), and FCT+EXT+Toys conditions. 
Baseline sessions were identical to the diverted attention condition of the FA. During 
all subsequent treatment conditions a card with the words, “Play, please”, was 
attached to the wall. Contingent on a card exchange, Molly received access to adult 
attention (e.g., saying, “Great job handing me the card, let’s play.”) and interactive 
play for 20 s on a fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule of reinforcement. During the FCT 
condition, aggression also resulted in access to attention (e.g., saying, “Molly you 
shouldn’t hit me that hurts.”) for 20 s on an FR-1 schedule. During the FCT+EXT 
condition, aggression was placed on EXT (i.e., no longer produced access to atten-tion) 
such that only card exchanges produced adult attention and interactive play. 
Aggression remained on EXT, card exchanges were reinforced with 20 s of attention 
on an FR-1 schedule, and Molly had continuous access to her most preferred toy (i.e., 
a game played on a laptop computer) throughout the FCT+EXT+Toys condition. 
During the second exposure to the FCT-only condition, delay fading for the commu-nication 
response was introduced and remained in place throughout subsequent 
sessions. Delay fading consisted of increasing the duration of time (initially up to 
40 s) before the “Play, please” card was presented (as described by Roane et al. 2004) 
and Molly was able to engage in a communicative response to gain access to 
attention. Subsequent delay fading in the FCT+EXT and FCT+EXT+Toys condi-tions 
progressed to a 346-s and an 1,800-s delay, respectively. Throughout delay 
fading, the reinforcement interval remained constant at 20 s.
96 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
Initial Treatment Analysis—Sam The treatment analysis for Sam was evaluated 
within a reversal (ABABAB) design, including baseline and FCT conditions. 
Baseline sessions were identical to the tangible condition of the FA. During the 
FCT condition, both appropriate communication and problem behavior (combined 
aggression and disruption) were reinforced on an FR-1 schedule. Reinforcement 
included 20-s access to a toy ball (Sam’s most preferred item as determined by the 
initial preference assessment). In addition, a multiple-schedule procedure (Hanley et al. 
2001) was used during the FCT condition to increase the delay to reinforcement. 
During the multiple schedule, the presentation of a red piece of construction paper 
(approximately 21 cm×27.5 cm) signaled that communication was on EXT while 
problem behavior was reinforced. After a designated period of time, the red-card 
interval ended and an identically sized green card was presented. The green card 
signaled that both communication and problem behavior were reinforced. The cards 
were alternated across sessions such that longer delays occurred prior to the green card 
being presented (i.e., communication resulting in reinforcement). During the final FCT 
phase, delays to reinforcement for successive sessions increased to variable durations 
that averaged 300 s (range within session0180 s to 420 s), 450 s (range within session0 
330 s to 570 s), and 600 s (range within session0480 s to 720 s). Throughout delay 
fading for Sam, the reinforcement interval remained constant at 20 s. 
Results 
Results of Molly’s FA indicated that her aggression was maintained by access to adult 
attention (data from the FA are available upon request). Figure 1 displays the results 
for Molly’s treatment evaluation. During baseline, Molly engaged in moderate to high 
rates of aggression (M01.1 rpm; range, 0.4–2.6 rpm). During the initial FCT phase, 
Molly continued to engage in moderate to high rates of aggression (M01.3 rpm; 
range, 0.3–3.8 rpm); thus EXT for problem behavior was implemented during the 
third phase. During FCT+EXT, rates of aggression immediately decreased to near-zero 
levels and generally occurred at relatively low to moderate rates throughout the 
remainder of the phase (M00.3 rpm; range, 0–1.6 rpm). However, per caregiver 
request a return to the FCT condition was implemented. Specifically, Molly’s mother 
stated that she would be unable to ignore Molly’s aggression. When the EXT 
component was removed, rates of aggression continued to remain low (M00.3 rpm; 
range, 0–1.3 rpm), so delay fading was initiated. Consistent reductions in aggression 
were not maintained when FCT was implemented in the absence of EXT while delay 
fading occurred (M00.6 rpm; range, 0–2.8 rpm). Therefore, FCT+EXT was re-implemented 
with inclusion of delay fading, starting with a 5-s delay, which was 
again associated an inconsistent pattern of responding (M00.5 rpm; range, 0– 
3.2 rpm). The final phase, FCT+EXT+Toys, began with a delay of 231 s. 
Aggression decreased to zero following the initial session. A return to a 346-s delay 
initially resulted in elevated rates of aggression, which decreased to zero and 
remained at near-zero rates with increases in the delay up to 1,800 s (M00.3 rpm; 
range, 0–3.5 rpm across all FCT+EXT+Toys sessions). 
Results of Sam’s FA indicated that his problem behavior (aggression and disrup-tion) 
was maintained by access to tangible items (data available upon request). The 
treatment analysis for Sam is depicted in Fig. 2. During baseline, Sam engaged in
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 97 
Fig. 1 Results of the initial treatment analysis for Molly. Arrows denote the session at which a given delay 
interval was introduced. The accompanying numbers represent the duration of each delay (in seconds) 
relatively stable rates of problem behavior (M02.0 rpm; range, 1.9–2.2 rpm). 
Implementation of FCT with a 5-s delay resulted in problem behavior immediately 
decreasing to near-zero across the first 3 sessions. Following a brief increase in 
problem behavior, low rates of aggression were observed across the remainder of 
the condition (M00.6 rpm; range, 0–2.9 rpm). Variable, but generally higher levels of 
problem behavior occurred during the reversal to baseline (M02.3 rpm; range, 0– 
8.0 rpm). When the FCT condition was introduced with delay fading, problem 
behavior immediately decreased and maintained at near-zero levels, with few excep-tions 
up to a 300-s delay interval (M00.3 rpm; range, 0–3.2 rpm). During the final 
return to baseline, problem behavior increased to moderate levels (M01.0 rpm; range, 
0.1–2.4 rpm). The final FCT phase resulted in the maintenance of near zero-rates of 
problem behavior up to a 600-s delay to reinforcement (M00.3 rpm; range, 0– 
2.3 rpm). 
Study 2: Combined Treatment Evaluation 
The FAs for both participants revealed different maintaining reinforcement contin-gencies 
for their respective problem behavior. Subsequent to the FAs, an effective 
treatment was developed for both participants. In addition, delay fading was
98 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
Fig. 2 Results of the initial treatment analysis for Sam. Arrows denote the session at which a given delay 
interval was introduced. The accompanying numbers represent the duration of each delay (in seconds) 
conducted for both participants such that there was a delay to reinforcement of at least 
600 s. Despite these outcomes, the treatments were distinct for each participant. Such 
a scenario may be impractical for caregivers (e.g., the caregiver would have to 
implement separate treatment procedures for each child, possibly at the same time). 
For that reason, the purpose of Study 2 was to merge the treatment contingencies for 
both participants such that their mother could implement an effective, integrated 
treatment. 
Method 
During baseline, we attempted to develop a condition that would combine the 
relevant EOs for each participant (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 2005). Recall 
that the EO for Molly’s aggression was attention deprivation, whereas the EO for 
Sam’s problem behavior was restriction of preferred items. Thus, in the combined 
baseline condition, we restricted access to attention for Sally and restricted access to 
the preferred item for Sam. Specifically, throughout baseline Molly had access to 
Sam’s toy but the attention of the therapist was diverted toward Sam (i.e., the adult 
engaged in a conversation with Sam while Molly played with Sam’s preferred toy). 
Contingent on problem behavior Sam received 30-s access to his preferred toy or
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 99 
Molly received 30-s of attention from the therapist. There was no other attention 
available to Molly and no alternative toys for Sam to engage with during the 
combined baseline condition. The reinforcement contingencies operated indepen-dently 
for both participants, and both reinforcers could be delivered simultaneously. 
One concern in developing a treatment in this baseline context was that both 
participants could request reinforcement at the same time, which might have resulted 
in high rates of reinforcement and caregiver engagement (which could interfere with 
the caregiver’s ability to engage in other household-related responsibilities). 
Therefore, after consultation with the participants’ mother, we evaluated a combined 
differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) contingency as a treatment. 
During the DRO condition, which was first implemented with Molly, a therapist 
informed Molly of the contingency prior to each session by stating, “I am going to set 
the timer for 10 min. If you can play for 10 min without hitting, kicking, throwing 
things, or biting, you can have the laptop computer. If you hit, kick, throw things, or 
bite I will reset the timer for 10 min.” Molly was prompted to repeat the rules and 
then a timer was set for 10 min and placed within view of both participants. If Molly 
met the DRO contingency, she received 1-min access to the laptop and attention (e.g., 
praise, interactive play) from the therapist. Contingent on the occurrence of aggres-sion 
by Molly, the therapist would reset the timer and would explain why the timer 
was being reset. Whilst the DRO contingency was in place for Molly, Sam remained 
in baseline contingencies (i.e., if Sam engaged in problem behavior he gained 30-s 
access to his preferred toy). 
Once decreased rates of aggression were observed for Molly, the DRO contingen-cy 
was applied to Sam’s behavior. The DRO contingency for Sam was identical to 
that described previously for Molly, with two exceptions. First, the pre-session rules 
were modified to include a revised description that stated if either Molly or Sam 
engaged in one of the targeted topographies of problem behavior, then the timer 
would be reset. Second, the DRO contingency addressed problem behavior displayed 
by either child. That is, contingent on problem behavior by either participant, the 
therapist reset the timer and explained to both participants why the timer was being 
reset; thus, the DRO contingency affected both participants such that problem 
behavior exhibited by one participant reset the DRO interval for both participants 
(i.e., a group DRO contingency; e.g., Kamps et al. 2011). If both children success-fully 
completed the DRO interval, Molly gained access to the laptop and adult 
attention for 1 min and Sam gained access to his preferred toy for 1 min. 
The combined treatment was evaluated within a multiple-baseline across participants 
design. Initially both participants were exposed to baseline sessions. Following success-ful 
implementation of the combined DRO procedure with a therapist, the participants’ 
mother was trained to implement the treatment contingency as described above. 
Results 
Figure 3 displays the results of the combined treatment. During baseline, Molly (top 
panel) engaged in moderate rates of aggression (M01.1 rpm; range, 0–1.8 rpm). 
During implementation of the DRO contingency for Molly, aggression decreased 
throughout the course of the treatment (M00.07 rpm; range, 0–0.2 rpm). Throughout 
baseline, Sam (bottom panel) engaged in somewhat variable, yet moderate rates of
100 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
Fig. 3 Results of the combined treatment analysis for Molly (top panel) and Sam (bottom panel) 
problem behavior (M01.5 rpm; range, 0–2.3 rpm). Upon implementation of the 
combined DRO contingency, Sam’s problem behavior immediately decreased (M0 
0.06 rpm; range, 0–0.1 rpm). 
Following decreases in problem behavior for both participants under the combined 
DRO treatment, the treatment was withdrawn and the baseline contingencies were re-implemented 
for both participants simultaneously. At this point, the participants’ 
mother implemented all sessions. During baseline, increases in problem behavior 
were observed for both Molly (M00.8 rpm; range, 0.4–1.6 rpm) and Sam (M0 
2.1 rpm; range, 0.5–6.4 rpm). Upon implementation of the combined DRO treatment 
by the mother, both participants’ problem behavior dropped to near-zero (M0 
0.08 rpm; range, 0–0.1 rpm and M00.08 rpm; range, 0–0.5 rpm for Molly and 
Sam, respectively). 
Discussion 
In the current investigation we evaluated a procedure for simultaneously implement-ing 
an intervention that addressed the function of two siblings’ problem behavior. In 
Study 1, individual treatments were evaluated for each participant based on the results
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 101 
of an FA. Although both effectively reduced the occurrence of problem behavior, 
additional concerns led to the development of a combined treatment. Thus, the 
relevant EOs for both participants were combined to develop a single baseline 
condition. A DRO procedure was then superimposed on this baseline to address the 
problem behavior of both children by affecting access to their functional reinforcers. 
Results indicated that the combined DRO effectively maintained low rates of problem 
behavior. Moreover, reductions in problem behavior maintained when the partici-pants’ 
mother served as the therapist. 
Previous studies have demonstrated that parent training focusing on interventions for 
the treatment of problem behavior can be successful across several clinical populations 
(Farley et al. 2005; Pelham and Fabiano 2008). However, the existing literature is 
limited with respect to parent-training studies that have focused on the simultaneous 
implementation of separate behavior interventions for more than one child within a 
single household (i.e.,multiplex families). The current investigation offers a preliminary 
method for addressing problematic behavior that can occur in multiplex families. 
To date, there have been several studies that have merged multiple treatments into 
a single, multi-component intervention (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 2005). 
Such studies have done so by introducing multiple EOs concurrently and developing 
a function-based treatment that addressed all relevant operant variables. However, 
these previous analyses have focused on an intervention for a single individual whose 
problem behavior was maintained by multiple environmental variables. The current 
investigation extended this work by demonstrating that multiple treatments can be 
combined into a single treatment to effectively reduce the occurrence of problem 
behavior for two individuals. Additionally, the effects of the combined treatment were 
maintained when the siblings’ mother implemented the procedures. 
Although reductions in targeted problem behavior were maintained when the 
participants’ mother implemented treatment, data on procedural integrity were not 
collected and it is therefore unknown, aside from anecdotal observation, whether or 
not the mother implemented procedures correctly. Also, all sessions were conducted 
within an analog setting and the long-term effects of the combined treatment within 
the home setting are unknown. Data on procedural integrity and generalization into 
community or home settings represent a critical advancement for future research. 
The generality of the current results is also limited by other factors. For example, 
this investigation reported results for one set of siblings. The current participants 
displayed many diagnostic and behavioral features that may have impacted the 
efficacy of the combined treatment. For example, both children were diagnosed with 
similar disorders, which reflect a similar level of impairment (in the present cases, 
relatively mild). Siblings who display more varied clinical presentations would 
presumably be difficult to treat in a combined manner (e.g., one sibling might be 
able to attend to discriminative stimuli better than another). Also, the current partic-ipants 
displayed problem behavior that was topographically similar. Thus, the rele-vant 
types of problem behavior that a caregiver would need to attend to could affect 
treatment outcomes. As an illustrative example, previous research has shown that 
observers’ record of behavior degrades as the number of target behaviors under 
observation increases (cf., Kazdin 1977). It is likely that additional topographies of 
behavior across siblings could negatively impact a caregiver’s ability to implement 
specific contingencies with a high degree of integrity.
102 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
Perhaps a more limiting concern is that, although the current participants displayed 
behavior that was sensitive to different reinforcement contingencies, both exhibited 
problem behavior that was maintained by positive reinforcement. If both negative and 
positive reinforcement were involved that could possibly cause greater difficulties in 
treatment design and implementation by a caregiver. It should be noted, however, that 
several previous investigations have developed procedures for combining EOs that 
address different reinforcement contingencies (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 
2005; Smith et al. 1993). 
There are a few other variables that impact the current results. First, the combined 
DRO procedure was implemented following successful (individual) treatment with 
FCT. It is therefore unknown if similar results would have been obtained without such 
a history. Also, the FCT and DRO procedures for Molly produced differential 
reinforcement of her functional reinforcer (attention), yet both also utilized access 
to a highly preferred reinforcer. It is possible that the use of the preferred toy alone 
would have been sufficient to decrease the occurrence of Molly’s problem behavior 
(Fischer et al. 1997; Hanley et al. 1997). Finally, the baseline and treatment con-ditions 
were implemented simultaneously for Molly and Sam when their mother 
began conducting sessions. As such, this aspect of the combined treatment analysis 
did not afford a demonstration of experimental control in and of itself. However, the 
initial effects of the combined DRO procedure were demonstrated in accordance with 
a multiple baseline across participants design, and the removal and subsequent 
reintroduction of the baseline and treatment conditions with the mother serving as 
the therapist approximated a reversal design (i.e., successive introduction and remov-al 
of the independent variable). 
In considering future directions for studies on multiplex families and the difficul-ties 
inherent in managing multiple problem behaviors, the importance of including 
caregivers in all aspects of treatment should not be underestimated. If the intention is 
to have caregivers independently manage behavior, then training them in defining 
behaviors, collecting data, identifying the functions of the behavior as well as 
implementing a function-based treatment should be the ultimate goal of clinicians. 
Previous research has demonstrated that various professional and para-professionals 
can be trained in such skills in a relatively time efficient manner (e.g., Phillips and 
Mudford 2008; Wallace et al. 2004). In addition, previous research has suggested that 
caregivers of single children with disabilities suffer high levels of anxiety and 
depression (Hastings 2003; Quintero and McIntyre 2010). Collecting data on care-giver 
mental health in multiplex families would be of great interest, particularly 
comparing pre- and post-treatment data. 
References 
Bachmeyer, M. H., Piazza, C. C., Fredrick, L. D., Reed, G. K., Rivas, K. D., & Kadey, H. J. (2009). 
Functional analysis and treatment of multiply controlled inappropriate mealtime behavior. Journal of 
Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 641–658. 
Ben Chaabanne, D. B., Alber-Morgan, S. R., & DeBar, R. M. (2009). The effects of parent-implemented 
PECS training on improvisation of mands by children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior 
Analysis, 42, 671–677.
J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 103 
Borrero, C. S. W., & Vollmer, T. R. (2006). Experimental analysis and treatment of multiply controlled 
problem behavior: a systematic replication and extension. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 
375–379. 
Call, N. A., Wacker, D. P., Ringdahl, J. E., & Boelter, E. W. (2005). Combined antecedent variables as 
motivating operations within functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 385–389. 
Constantine, J. N., Zhang, Y., Frazier, T., Abbacchi, A. M., & Law, P. (2010). Sibling recurrence and the 
genetic epidemiology of Autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 1349–1356. 
Day, H. M., Horner, R. H., & O’Neill, R. (1994). Multiple functions or problem behaviors: assessment and 
intervention. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 279–289. 
Farley, S. E., Adams, J. S., Lutton, M. E., & Scoville, C. J. (2005). What are effective treatments for 
oppositional and defiant behaviors in preadolescents? The Journal of Family Practice, 54, 162–165. 
Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Worsdell, A. S. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as 
treatment for self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 335–338. 
Fisher,W.W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., & Owens, J. C. (1992). A comparison of two 
approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of 
Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. 
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., & Chiang, C. L. (1996). Effects of equal and unequal reinforcer duration 
during functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 117–120. 
Griffith, G. M., & Richard, P. (2010). Using matched groups to explore child behavior problems and 
maternal well-being in children with Down Syndrome and Autism. Journal Autism and Developmental 
Disorders, 40, 610–619. 
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. (1997). Noncontingent presentation of attention and alternative 
stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior 
Analysis, 30, 229–237. 
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001). Reinforcement schedule thinning following 
treatment with functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38. 
Hastings, R. P. (2003). Brief Report: behavioral adjustment of siblings of children with autism. Journal of 
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 99–104. 
Ingvarsson, E. T., Kahng, S., & Hausman, N. L. (2008). Some Effects of noncontingent positive reinforce-ment 
on multiply controlled problem behavior and compliance in a demand context. Journal of Applied 
Behavior Analysis, 41, 435–440. 
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional 
analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis 
and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). 
Kamps, D., Wills, H. P., Heitzman-Powell, L., Laylin, J., Szoke, C., Petrillo, T., & Culey, A. (2011). Class-wide 
function-related intervention teams: effects of group contingency programs in urban classrooms. 
The Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13, 154–157. 
Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Artifact, bias, and complexity of assessment: the ABCs of reliability. Journal of 
Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 141–150. 
Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied 
Behavior Analysis, 29, 391–395. 
Lerman, D. C., Swiezy, N., Perkins-Parks, S., & Roane, H. S. (2000). Skill acquisition in parents of 
children with developmental disabilities: interaction between skill type and instructional format. 
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 183–196. 
Meaden, H., Ostrosky, M. M., Zaghlawan, H. Y., & SeonYeong, Y. (2009). Promoting the social and 
communicative behavior of young children with autism spectrum disorders. A review of parent-implemented 
intervention studies. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 29, 90–104. 
Moes, D. R., & Frea, W. D. (2002). Contextualized behavioral support in early intervention for children 
with autism and their families. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 519–533. 
Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for Attention-Deficit- 
Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 184–214. 
Petalas, M. A., Hastings, R. P., Nash, S., Lloyd, T., & Dowey, A. (2009). Emotional and behavioural 
adjustment in siblings of children with intellectual disability with and without autism. Autism, 13, 471– 
483. 
Phillips, K. J., & Mudford, O. C. (2008). Functional analysis skills training for residential caregivers. 
Behavioral Interventions, 23, 1–12. 
Quintero, N., & McIntyre, L. L. (2010). Sibling adjustment and maternal well-being: an examination of 
families with and without a child with an autism spectrum disorder. Focus Autism Other Developmen-tal 
Disabilities, 25, 37–46.
104 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Sgro, G. M., Falcomata, T. S., & Pabico, R. R. (2004). An alternative method 
of thinning reinforcer delivery during differential reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 
37, 213–218. 
Rodriguez, J. R., Geffken, G. R., & Morgan, S. B. (1993). Perceived competence and behavioral 
adjustment of siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 
665–674. 
Ross, P., & Cuskelly, M. (2006). Adjustment, sibling problems and coping strategies of brothers and sisters of 
children with autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 31, 77–86. 
Smith, M. R., & Lerman, D. C. (1999). A preliminary comparison of guided compliance and high-probability 
instructional sequences as treatment for noncompliance in children with developmental 
disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20, 183–195. 
Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1993). Experimental analysis and treatment of 
multiply controlled self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 183–196. 
Wallace, M. D., Doney, J. K., Mintz-Resudek, C. M., & Tarbox, R. S. F. (2004). Training educators to 
implement functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 89–92. 
Worsdell, A. S., Iwata, B. A., Hanley, G. P., Thompson, R. H., & Kahng, S. (2000). Effects of continuous 
and intermittent reinforcement for problem behavior during functional communication training. Journal of 
Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 167–179.
Copyright of Journal of Developmental & Physical Disabilities is the property of Springer Science & Business 
Media B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the 
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for 
individual use.

More Related Content

What's hot

Teacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School Children
Teacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School ChildrenTeacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School Children
Teacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School Children
iosrjce
 
Final Draft Advanced
Final Draft AdvancedFinal Draft Advanced
Final Draft Advanced
Liam Sprague
 
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Emily Kothe
 
Hardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature ReviewHardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature Review
Gabriel Hardt
 
Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15
Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15
Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15
Bethany D. Bruno
 
Power point aba
Power point abaPower point aba
Power point aba
pagonea
 
Language matrix Larsson
Language matrix LarssonLanguage matrix Larsson
Language matrix Larsson
Marta Montoro
 
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
iosrjce
 

What's hot (20)

WPA_2015_poster
WPA_2015_posterWPA_2015_poster
WPA_2015_poster
 
Teacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School Children
Teacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School ChildrenTeacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School Children
Teacher Attachment and Intention of Aggressive Behaviour among School Children
 
Final Draft Advanced
Final Draft AdvancedFinal Draft Advanced
Final Draft Advanced
 
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
 
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
Freijy - ASBHM - Do interventions based on cognitive dissonance promote healt...
 
poster session
poster sessionposter session
poster session
 
ABA Program
ABA ProgramABA Program
ABA Program
 
ABA Handout
ABA HandoutABA Handout
ABA Handout
 
Final Thesis Paper
Final Thesis PaperFinal Thesis Paper
Final Thesis Paper
 
Hardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature ReviewHardt Literature Review
Hardt Literature Review
 
Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15
Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15
Use This One-Bruno Poster 6-22-15
 
Research Symposium Poster
Research Symposium PosterResearch Symposium Poster
Research Symposium Poster
 
Power point aba
Power point abaPower point aba
Power point aba
 
Language matrix Larsson
Language matrix LarssonLanguage matrix Larsson
Language matrix Larsson
 
Adolescents coping understanding the role of gender and academic competence
Adolescents coping understanding the role of gender and academic competenceAdolescents coping understanding the role of gender and academic competence
Adolescents coping understanding the role of gender and academic competence
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective study
 
McNeill_Thesis_2016
McNeill_Thesis_2016McNeill_Thesis_2016
McNeill_Thesis_2016
 
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
Influential Determinants of Capacity Building to Cope With Stress among Unive...
 
aids and behavior
aids and behavioraids and behavior
aids and behavior
 
Applied behavior analysis
Applied behavior analysisApplied behavior analysis
Applied behavior analysis
 

Viewers also liked

Testigos De La Historia
Testigos De La HistoriaTestigos De La Historia
Testigos De La Historia
guest4d8fb49
 
Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003
Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003
Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003
alvarochristian
 
Con La Comida No Se Juega
Con La Comida No Se JuegaCon La Comida No Se Juega
Con La Comida No Se Juega
pacoresinas
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Promocion de la_salud_mental
Promocion de la_salud_mentalPromocion de la_salud_mental
Promocion de la_salud_mental
 
Diseño para la portada de una casa señorial ecijana
Diseño para la portada de una casa señorial ecijanaDiseño para la portada de una casa señorial ecijana
Diseño para la portada de una casa señorial ecijana
 
Cumple de Javi
Cumple de JaviCumple de Javi
Cumple de Javi
 
Ilusiones opticas
Ilusiones opticasIlusiones opticas
Ilusiones opticas
 
Romenia
RomeniaRomenia
Romenia
 
Semestrario
Semestrario Semestrario
Semestrario
 
Testigos De La Historia
Testigos De La HistoriaTestigos De La Historia
Testigos De La Historia
 
Los juegos del hambre, Suzanne Collins
Los juegos del hambre, Suzanne CollinsLos juegos del hambre, Suzanne Collins
Los juegos del hambre, Suzanne Collins
 
Besos Famosos
Besos  FamososBesos  Famosos
Besos Famosos
 
Quatrième projet
Quatrième projetQuatrième projet
Quatrième projet
 
Crema de patatas
Crema de patatasCrema de patatas
Crema de patatas
 
Ppt ff6 [recuperado]
Ppt ff6 [recuperado]Ppt ff6 [recuperado]
Ppt ff6 [recuperado]
 
La Mujer Es Mujer
La Mujer Es MujerLa Mujer Es Mujer
La Mujer Es Mujer
 
Mercado Flotante En Holanda
Mercado Flotante En HolandaMercado Flotante En Holanda
Mercado Flotante En Holanda
 
Prueba 03 de septiembre
Prueba 03 de septiembrePrueba 03 de septiembre
Prueba 03 de septiembre
 
La catedral de santiago
La catedral de santiago La catedral de santiago
La catedral de santiago
 
La mujer
La mujer La mujer
La mujer
 
Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003
Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003
Power point chesskids alvaro cris 2003
 
Con La Comida No Se Juega
Con La Comida No Se JuegaCon La Comida No Se Juega
Con La Comida No Se Juega
 
II Día da Ciencia en Galego
II  Día da Ciencia en GalegoII  Día da Ciencia en Galego
II Día da Ciencia en Galego
 

Similar to Behavior problems

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
croysierkathey
 
NotesTensors.pdf.docx
NotesTensors.pdf.docxNotesTensors.pdf.docx
NotesTensors.pdf.docx
henrymartin15260
 
REVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docx
REVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docxREVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docx
REVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docx
audeleypearl
 
Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper
Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research PaperLee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper
Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper
Sierra Lee
 
14Discrete Trial Training (DTT)
 14Discrete Trial Training (DTT) 14Discrete Trial Training (DTT)
14Discrete Trial Training (DTT)
MargaritoWhitt221
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docx
croysierkathey
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Marissa Schneider
 
EFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COS
EFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COSEFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COS
EFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COS
EvonCanales257
 
Running head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docx
Running head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docxRunning head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docx
Running head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docx
joellemurphey
 
ProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16 Funded mu.docx
ProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16  Funded mu.docxProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16  Funded mu.docx
ProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16 Funded mu.docx
stilliegeorgiana
 
1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main
1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main
1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main
Anne de Hulster
 
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docxEffectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
SALU18
 

Similar to Behavior problems (20)

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
 
NotesTensors.pdf.docx
NotesTensors.pdf.docxNotesTensors.pdf.docx
NotesTensors.pdf.docx
 
REVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docx
REVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docxREVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docx
REVIEW ARTICLETreatment of severe problem behaviour in chi.docx
 
Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper
Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research PaperLee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper
Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper
 
14Discrete Trial Training (DTT)
 14Discrete Trial Training (DTT) 14Discrete Trial Training (DTT)
14Discrete Trial Training (DTT)
 
Attachment-Based Interventions And Eating Related Disorders In Females
Attachment-Based Interventions And Eating Related Disorders In FemalesAttachment-Based Interventions And Eating Related Disorders In Females
Attachment-Based Interventions And Eating Related Disorders In Females
 
DahlmanResearchPaper
DahlmanResearchPaperDahlmanResearchPaper
DahlmanResearchPaper
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISSETTING GENERALITY AND.docx
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
 
EFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COS
EFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COSEFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COS
EFFICACY OF AND PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT ANDRESPONSE COS
 
Running head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docx
Running head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docxRunning head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docx
Running head Advanced behavioral analysis and its successful use .docx
 
Anger Presentation
Anger PresentationAnger Presentation
Anger Presentation
 
Infant day care.fpptx
Infant day care.fpptxInfant day care.fpptx
Infant day care.fpptx
 
ProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16 Funded mu.docx
ProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16  Funded mu.docxProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16  Funded mu.docx
ProcedureASD children92 children recruited 16 Funded mu.docx
 
1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main
1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main
1 s2.0-s0005796709000539-main
 
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docxEffectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
 
Downs
DownsDowns
Downs
 
Presentation
PresentationPresentation
Presentation
 
Emotional Behavioral Disorder
Emotional Behavioral DisorderEmotional Behavioral Disorder
Emotional Behavioral Disorder
 
PTSD POSTER
PTSD POSTERPTSD POSTER
PTSD POSTER
 

Behavior problems

  • 1. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 DOI 10.1007/s10882-012-9305-1 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Development of a Combined Intervention to Decrease Problem Behavior Displayed by Siblings with Pervasive Developmental Disorder Niamh M. Doyle & Nicole M. DeRosa & Henry S. Roane Published online: 12 October 2012 # Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012 Abstract Approximately 10–20 % of children with pervasive developmental disor-der are likely to have a sibling with that disorder. Under such circumstances, care-givers may be faced with simultaneously implementing separate interventions for their children. Merging different treatments to decrease both children's problem behavior may permit for more fluid parent responses and increased procedural integrity. The current study describes one approach to treating problem behavior displayed by siblings with autism. Separate functional analyses identified a different maintaining reinforcer for each child's aggression. These behaviors were initially treated separately using functional communication training and delay fading. To ensure that the caregiver would be able to manage these siblings' behaviors concur-rently, a protocol using differential reinforcement of other behaviors was imple-mented that was based on the separate functions of each child's behavior. Keywords Parent training . Group contingency . Problembehavior Recent studies have indicated that caregivers of children with pervasive developmen-tal disorders (PDDs) often experience symptoms of anxiety and depression (Griffith and Richard 2010; Quintero and McIntyre 2010). Such symptoms can and often are exacerbated if the child also engages in problem behavior (e.g., aggression, disrup-tion, self-injurious behavior [SIB]). Parent training focusing on the implementation of behavioral interventions for problem behavior has reportedly been effective in dimin-ishing the negative experiences associated with caring for children with a variety of N. M. Doyle : N. M. DeRosa : H. S. Roane SUNY Upstate Medical University, The Kelberman Center, Syracuse, NY, USA N. M. Doyle (*) Department of Pediatrics, Upstate Medical University, 600 E. Genesee Street, Suite 124, Syracuse, NY 13202, USA e-mail: doylen@upstate.edu
  • 2. 92 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 diagnoses, including autism (Farley et al. 2005; Pelham and Fabiano 2008). Several studies have examined the effectiveness of parent training in regard to both increasing children’s adaptive behavior skills, as well as decreasing problem behavior (Ben Chaabanne et al. 2009; Lerman et al. 2000; Meaden et al. 2009; Moes and Frea 2002; Smith and Lerman 1999). However, the effectiveness of parent training for caregivers in multiplex families (e.g., families with more than one child diagnosed with autism) has not been well studied. Approximately 10–20 % of children with autism also have a sibling diagnosed with the disorder (Constantine et al. 2010). Given that destructive behavior (e.g., SIB, aggression, or disruption) is common among children with developmental disabil-ities, parents of multiplex families may have to simultaneously implement separate behavior interventions. Additionally, group comparisons suggest that non-disabled siblings of children with autism may display characteristics of the disorder, as well as engage in severe problem behavior (Constantine et al. 2010; Petalas et al. 2009; Rodriguez et al. 1993; Ross and Cuskelly 2006). The implementation of separate interventions for more than one child may result in increased symptoms of anxiety and/or depression for caregivers, as well as potentially result in poor procedural integrity of one or both behavior interventions. The implementation ofmultiple treatmentsmay be similar to treating problem behavior that is maintained by multiple reinforcement contingencies (e.g., Borrero and Vollmer 2006; Day et al. 1994; Ingvarsson et al. 2008; Lalli and Casey 1996; Smith et al. 1993). More specifically, implementing multiple treatments can often be associated with several different procedural variables, such as different discriminative stimuli, communicative responses, prompting procedures, and contingencies for appropriate and problem behavior. Previous treatments for multiply controlled problem behavior have included separate interventions for each reinforcement contingency (Borrero and Vollmer 2006; Day et al. 1994; Smith et al. 1993) or treating both functions through the use of a highly preferred stimulus (e.g., food; Ingvarsson et al. 2008). Several authors have examined an alternative treatment approach for implement-ing multiple treatments simultaneously that involves merging the establishing oper-ation (EO) for multiple maintaining variables into a single context (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 2005). An EO is an environmental event that (a) increases the effectiveness of a stimulus as a reinforcer and (b) increases the probability of the occurrence of behaviors that have produced that reinforcer in the past. Bachmeyer et al. evaluated individual and combined function-based extinction procedures for children whose inappropriate mealtime behavior was maintained by both positive and negative reinforcement. Attention extinction alone did not reduce problem behavior or increase acceptance of foods, while escape extinc-tion increased acceptance and reduced problem behavior, but not to clinically acceptable levels. Therefore, the combination of attention and escape extinction was evaluated and found to be effective in reducing inappropriate mealtime behavior, as well as maintaining high and stable acceptance. Call et al. com-pared single-antecedent functional analysis test conditions and combined-antecedent test conditions. In that study, problem behavior was only observed during the combined-antecedent test conditions, suggesting false-negative results from the single-antecedent assessment. Treatments derived from the results of the combined-antecedent functional analysis were effective in reducing occurrences of
  • 3. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 93 problem behavior. Both of these studies demonstrate that interventions, which targeted multiple reinforcement contingencies, can be effective in reducing targeted problem behavior when implemented by trained therapists within analog settings. However, it remains unknown if the effectiveness of such combined interventions maintains when implemented by caregivers. The purpose of the current study was twofold. In Study 1, we sought to evaluate differential treatments for siblings diagnosed with PDD whose problem behavior was maintained by different functional reinforcers. Second, we examined the integration of the two different behavioral interventions, using procedures similar to that of Bachmeyer et al. (2009) and Call et al. (2005), such that a combined treatment could be implemented for both children by their caregiver. General Procedures Participants and Settings Molly was an 8-year-old girl who had been previously diagnosed with a variety of behavior disorders, including PDD-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, and Oppositional Defiant Disorder. She was referred to an outpatient clinic for the treatment of aggression. She attended the clinic 3 days per week, for one 1.5-h appointment per day. Her brother Sam was 7-years old and had been previously diagnosed with PDD-NOS and Disruptive Behavior Disorder. He was referred to the same outpatient clinic for the treatment of aggression and disruption. Sam attended the clinic on the same schedule as Molly, though his initial treatment evaluation occurred following the conclusion of Molly’s admission. Both Molly and Sam partici-pated in Study 1 and Study 2. During Study 1, sessions occurred in individual treatment rooms (4 m×4 m) for both Molly and Sam. The rooms were equipped with a one-way observation window as well as various materials that depended on the condition in effect (e.g., table, chairs, preferred toys). During Study 2, sessions were conducted in a play area with both children and a therapist or the participants’ mother present. Baseline sessions in Study 1 were 5 min in duration, whereas treatment session durations varied according to individual protocols (described below). In Study 2, baseline sessions were 5 min in duration and treatment sessions varied based on the delay interval in place. For both participants, 3 to 5 sessions were conducted daily. Response Measurement and Reliability Aggression for both children was defined as contact, or attempted contact of hands, feet or legs against another person from a distance of 15 cm or greater; applying, or attempting to apply, force to a person’s body (e.g., pushing or pulling a person off balance or in a direction opposite of the person’s original movement); throwing an object at a person; pulling on another person’s hair; and pinching, scratching, or biting another person. Sam’s disruptive behavior was defined as throwing an object at a wall or a stationary object from a distance of 30 cm or greater; kicking or hitting
  • 4. 94 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 walls or windows; and turning or attempting to turn over furniture. The target communication response for Molly was a card exchange which consisted of Molly removing a card with the words, “Play, please” written on it, from the wall and placing it in the therapist’s hand. The communication response for Sam was vocally saying the phrase, “May I play with the ball, please.” For both participants, the targeted communication response was taught prior to their respective initial treatment analysis using the procedures described by Worsdell et al. (2000). Frequency data were collected for all dependent measures using laptop computers equipped with data-collection software. For the purpose of data analysis, all frequency data were converted to a response rate (responses per min; rpm). A second observer independently collected interobserver agreement (IOA) data during 33.9 % of all sessions. Exact agreement was calculated by partitioning each session into 10-s intervals and dividing agreements by total agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100. An agreement was scored when both observers recorded the occurrence of a response in the same 10-s interval, and a disagreement occurred when they did not. During Study 1, the mean IOA for Molly’s aggression and communication was 98.3 % (range, 83.3 % - 100 %) and 99.9 % (range, 96.7 % - 100 %), respectively. The mean IOA for Sam’s aggression, disruption, and communication during Study 1 was 94.3 % (range, 68.3 % - 100 %), 99.7 % (range, 96.7 % to 100 %), and 98.3 % (range, 88.3 % - 100 %), respectively. During Study 2, IOA for Molly’s aggression averaged 99.3 % (range, 89.2 % - 100 %), IOA for Sam’s aggression averaged 99.4 % (range, 89.2 % - 100 %), and IOA for Sam’s disruption was 100 %. Study 1: Individual Treatment Evaluations Method Preference Assessment Prior to each participant’s functional analysis, a paired-stimulus preference assessment (Fisher et al. 1992) was conducted separately for Molly and for Sam. During the preference assessment, a therapist presented a pair of items to the participant and instructed him/her to make a choice (e.g., “Molly, would you rather play with the ball or play on the computer?”). Following a selection, the participant was allowed to engage with the selected item for 30 s. The assessment continued until each toy was presented with every other toy one time. A hierarchy of preference was determined based on the percent of trials during which Molly and Sam selected each item. Results of this assessment were used in the subsequent functional analysis and treatment evaluations. Functional Analysis A functional analysis (FA), based on the procedures described by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) with modifications as described by Fisher et al. (1996), was conducted with each sibling. Molly and Sam were both exposed to demand, tangible, ignore, and toy play conditions. Additionally, Molly was exposed to a diverted attention condition, whereas Sam was exposed to a “standard” attention condition. During the demand condition, a therapist and the participant were seated at a table with instructional materials. The therapist instructed the participant to com-plete instructions using a three-step prompting hierarchy (successive verbal, gestural, and physical prompts) with no more than 5 s between each prompt. Praise was
  • 5. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 95 delivered contingent on compliance following either the verbal or gestural prompt. Contingent on a targeted response (e.g., aggression), a 20-s break from instructional demands occurred. Prior to each tangible session, the participant received approxi-mately 2 min of pre-session access to his or her preferred item. Thereafter, the therapist removed the toy from the participant and kept it within sight but out of reach. The occurrence of a targeted response resulted in 20-s access to the preferred item. During the ignore condition, a therapist and participant were in the room with no materials present. The therapist did not interact with the participant throughout the session and no consequences were provided contingent on a targeted response. During the toy play condition, the therapist and participant were in the room with the participant’s highly preferred toy. The participant had access to the toy and the therapist interacted with the participant throughout the session, providing praise statements approximately every 30-s. No demands were delivered and no consequen-ces were provided contingent on a targeted response. During the diverted attention condition for Molly, two therapists and the participant were in the room with a low preferred toy. The two therapists engaged in a conversation with each other while Molly had access to a low preference toy. Contingent on a targeted response one therapist provided 20 s of attention in the form of verbal reprimands (e.g., saying, “Molly, stop that. We’re trying to talk.”). During Sam’s attention condition, a therapist was in the room with Sam, and Sam had access to a low preference toy while the therapist was reading. Contingent on a targeted response Sam received 20-s of attention in the form of verbal reprimands (e.g., saying, “Sam, it’s not nice when you throw something at me. You might break the window.”). Initial Treatment Analysis—Molly The initial treatment for Molly’s aggression was evaluated using a reversal (ABCBD) design, including baseline, functional commu-nication training (FCT), FCT+extinction (EXT), and FCT+EXT+Toys conditions. Baseline sessions were identical to the diverted attention condition of the FA. During all subsequent treatment conditions a card with the words, “Play, please”, was attached to the wall. Contingent on a card exchange, Molly received access to adult attention (e.g., saying, “Great job handing me the card, let’s play.”) and interactive play for 20 s on a fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule of reinforcement. During the FCT condition, aggression also resulted in access to attention (e.g., saying, “Molly you shouldn’t hit me that hurts.”) for 20 s on an FR-1 schedule. During the FCT+EXT condition, aggression was placed on EXT (i.e., no longer produced access to atten-tion) such that only card exchanges produced adult attention and interactive play. Aggression remained on EXT, card exchanges were reinforced with 20 s of attention on an FR-1 schedule, and Molly had continuous access to her most preferred toy (i.e., a game played on a laptop computer) throughout the FCT+EXT+Toys condition. During the second exposure to the FCT-only condition, delay fading for the commu-nication response was introduced and remained in place throughout subsequent sessions. Delay fading consisted of increasing the duration of time (initially up to 40 s) before the “Play, please” card was presented (as described by Roane et al. 2004) and Molly was able to engage in a communicative response to gain access to attention. Subsequent delay fading in the FCT+EXT and FCT+EXT+Toys condi-tions progressed to a 346-s and an 1,800-s delay, respectively. Throughout delay fading, the reinforcement interval remained constant at 20 s.
  • 6. 96 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 Initial Treatment Analysis—Sam The treatment analysis for Sam was evaluated within a reversal (ABABAB) design, including baseline and FCT conditions. Baseline sessions were identical to the tangible condition of the FA. During the FCT condition, both appropriate communication and problem behavior (combined aggression and disruption) were reinforced on an FR-1 schedule. Reinforcement included 20-s access to a toy ball (Sam’s most preferred item as determined by the initial preference assessment). In addition, a multiple-schedule procedure (Hanley et al. 2001) was used during the FCT condition to increase the delay to reinforcement. During the multiple schedule, the presentation of a red piece of construction paper (approximately 21 cm×27.5 cm) signaled that communication was on EXT while problem behavior was reinforced. After a designated period of time, the red-card interval ended and an identically sized green card was presented. The green card signaled that both communication and problem behavior were reinforced. The cards were alternated across sessions such that longer delays occurred prior to the green card being presented (i.e., communication resulting in reinforcement). During the final FCT phase, delays to reinforcement for successive sessions increased to variable durations that averaged 300 s (range within session0180 s to 420 s), 450 s (range within session0 330 s to 570 s), and 600 s (range within session0480 s to 720 s). Throughout delay fading for Sam, the reinforcement interval remained constant at 20 s. Results Results of Molly’s FA indicated that her aggression was maintained by access to adult attention (data from the FA are available upon request). Figure 1 displays the results for Molly’s treatment evaluation. During baseline, Molly engaged in moderate to high rates of aggression (M01.1 rpm; range, 0.4–2.6 rpm). During the initial FCT phase, Molly continued to engage in moderate to high rates of aggression (M01.3 rpm; range, 0.3–3.8 rpm); thus EXT for problem behavior was implemented during the third phase. During FCT+EXT, rates of aggression immediately decreased to near-zero levels and generally occurred at relatively low to moderate rates throughout the remainder of the phase (M00.3 rpm; range, 0–1.6 rpm). However, per caregiver request a return to the FCT condition was implemented. Specifically, Molly’s mother stated that she would be unable to ignore Molly’s aggression. When the EXT component was removed, rates of aggression continued to remain low (M00.3 rpm; range, 0–1.3 rpm), so delay fading was initiated. Consistent reductions in aggression were not maintained when FCT was implemented in the absence of EXT while delay fading occurred (M00.6 rpm; range, 0–2.8 rpm). Therefore, FCT+EXT was re-implemented with inclusion of delay fading, starting with a 5-s delay, which was again associated an inconsistent pattern of responding (M00.5 rpm; range, 0– 3.2 rpm). The final phase, FCT+EXT+Toys, began with a delay of 231 s. Aggression decreased to zero following the initial session. A return to a 346-s delay initially resulted in elevated rates of aggression, which decreased to zero and remained at near-zero rates with increases in the delay up to 1,800 s (M00.3 rpm; range, 0–3.5 rpm across all FCT+EXT+Toys sessions). Results of Sam’s FA indicated that his problem behavior (aggression and disrup-tion) was maintained by access to tangible items (data available upon request). The treatment analysis for Sam is depicted in Fig. 2. During baseline, Sam engaged in
  • 7. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 97 Fig. 1 Results of the initial treatment analysis for Molly. Arrows denote the session at which a given delay interval was introduced. The accompanying numbers represent the duration of each delay (in seconds) relatively stable rates of problem behavior (M02.0 rpm; range, 1.9–2.2 rpm). Implementation of FCT with a 5-s delay resulted in problem behavior immediately decreasing to near-zero across the first 3 sessions. Following a brief increase in problem behavior, low rates of aggression were observed across the remainder of the condition (M00.6 rpm; range, 0–2.9 rpm). Variable, but generally higher levels of problem behavior occurred during the reversal to baseline (M02.3 rpm; range, 0– 8.0 rpm). When the FCT condition was introduced with delay fading, problem behavior immediately decreased and maintained at near-zero levels, with few excep-tions up to a 300-s delay interval (M00.3 rpm; range, 0–3.2 rpm). During the final return to baseline, problem behavior increased to moderate levels (M01.0 rpm; range, 0.1–2.4 rpm). The final FCT phase resulted in the maintenance of near zero-rates of problem behavior up to a 600-s delay to reinforcement (M00.3 rpm; range, 0– 2.3 rpm). Study 2: Combined Treatment Evaluation The FAs for both participants revealed different maintaining reinforcement contin-gencies for their respective problem behavior. Subsequent to the FAs, an effective treatment was developed for both participants. In addition, delay fading was
  • 8. 98 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 Fig. 2 Results of the initial treatment analysis for Sam. Arrows denote the session at which a given delay interval was introduced. The accompanying numbers represent the duration of each delay (in seconds) conducted for both participants such that there was a delay to reinforcement of at least 600 s. Despite these outcomes, the treatments were distinct for each participant. Such a scenario may be impractical for caregivers (e.g., the caregiver would have to implement separate treatment procedures for each child, possibly at the same time). For that reason, the purpose of Study 2 was to merge the treatment contingencies for both participants such that their mother could implement an effective, integrated treatment. Method During baseline, we attempted to develop a condition that would combine the relevant EOs for each participant (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 2005). Recall that the EO for Molly’s aggression was attention deprivation, whereas the EO for Sam’s problem behavior was restriction of preferred items. Thus, in the combined baseline condition, we restricted access to attention for Sally and restricted access to the preferred item for Sam. Specifically, throughout baseline Molly had access to Sam’s toy but the attention of the therapist was diverted toward Sam (i.e., the adult engaged in a conversation with Sam while Molly played with Sam’s preferred toy). Contingent on problem behavior Sam received 30-s access to his preferred toy or
  • 9. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 99 Molly received 30-s of attention from the therapist. There was no other attention available to Molly and no alternative toys for Sam to engage with during the combined baseline condition. The reinforcement contingencies operated indepen-dently for both participants, and both reinforcers could be delivered simultaneously. One concern in developing a treatment in this baseline context was that both participants could request reinforcement at the same time, which might have resulted in high rates of reinforcement and caregiver engagement (which could interfere with the caregiver’s ability to engage in other household-related responsibilities). Therefore, after consultation with the participants’ mother, we evaluated a combined differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) contingency as a treatment. During the DRO condition, which was first implemented with Molly, a therapist informed Molly of the contingency prior to each session by stating, “I am going to set the timer for 10 min. If you can play for 10 min without hitting, kicking, throwing things, or biting, you can have the laptop computer. If you hit, kick, throw things, or bite I will reset the timer for 10 min.” Molly was prompted to repeat the rules and then a timer was set for 10 min and placed within view of both participants. If Molly met the DRO contingency, she received 1-min access to the laptop and attention (e.g., praise, interactive play) from the therapist. Contingent on the occurrence of aggres-sion by Molly, the therapist would reset the timer and would explain why the timer was being reset. Whilst the DRO contingency was in place for Molly, Sam remained in baseline contingencies (i.e., if Sam engaged in problem behavior he gained 30-s access to his preferred toy). Once decreased rates of aggression were observed for Molly, the DRO contingen-cy was applied to Sam’s behavior. The DRO contingency for Sam was identical to that described previously for Molly, with two exceptions. First, the pre-session rules were modified to include a revised description that stated if either Molly or Sam engaged in one of the targeted topographies of problem behavior, then the timer would be reset. Second, the DRO contingency addressed problem behavior displayed by either child. That is, contingent on problem behavior by either participant, the therapist reset the timer and explained to both participants why the timer was being reset; thus, the DRO contingency affected both participants such that problem behavior exhibited by one participant reset the DRO interval for both participants (i.e., a group DRO contingency; e.g., Kamps et al. 2011). If both children success-fully completed the DRO interval, Molly gained access to the laptop and adult attention for 1 min and Sam gained access to his preferred toy for 1 min. The combined treatment was evaluated within a multiple-baseline across participants design. Initially both participants were exposed to baseline sessions. Following success-ful implementation of the combined DRO procedure with a therapist, the participants’ mother was trained to implement the treatment contingency as described above. Results Figure 3 displays the results of the combined treatment. During baseline, Molly (top panel) engaged in moderate rates of aggression (M01.1 rpm; range, 0–1.8 rpm). During implementation of the DRO contingency for Molly, aggression decreased throughout the course of the treatment (M00.07 rpm; range, 0–0.2 rpm). Throughout baseline, Sam (bottom panel) engaged in somewhat variable, yet moderate rates of
  • 10. 100 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 Fig. 3 Results of the combined treatment analysis for Molly (top panel) and Sam (bottom panel) problem behavior (M01.5 rpm; range, 0–2.3 rpm). Upon implementation of the combined DRO contingency, Sam’s problem behavior immediately decreased (M0 0.06 rpm; range, 0–0.1 rpm). Following decreases in problem behavior for both participants under the combined DRO treatment, the treatment was withdrawn and the baseline contingencies were re-implemented for both participants simultaneously. At this point, the participants’ mother implemented all sessions. During baseline, increases in problem behavior were observed for both Molly (M00.8 rpm; range, 0.4–1.6 rpm) and Sam (M0 2.1 rpm; range, 0.5–6.4 rpm). Upon implementation of the combined DRO treatment by the mother, both participants’ problem behavior dropped to near-zero (M0 0.08 rpm; range, 0–0.1 rpm and M00.08 rpm; range, 0–0.5 rpm for Molly and Sam, respectively). Discussion In the current investigation we evaluated a procedure for simultaneously implement-ing an intervention that addressed the function of two siblings’ problem behavior. In Study 1, individual treatments were evaluated for each participant based on the results
  • 11. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 101 of an FA. Although both effectively reduced the occurrence of problem behavior, additional concerns led to the development of a combined treatment. Thus, the relevant EOs for both participants were combined to develop a single baseline condition. A DRO procedure was then superimposed on this baseline to address the problem behavior of both children by affecting access to their functional reinforcers. Results indicated that the combined DRO effectively maintained low rates of problem behavior. Moreover, reductions in problem behavior maintained when the partici-pants’ mother served as the therapist. Previous studies have demonstrated that parent training focusing on interventions for the treatment of problem behavior can be successful across several clinical populations (Farley et al. 2005; Pelham and Fabiano 2008). However, the existing literature is limited with respect to parent-training studies that have focused on the simultaneous implementation of separate behavior interventions for more than one child within a single household (i.e.,multiplex families). The current investigation offers a preliminary method for addressing problematic behavior that can occur in multiplex families. To date, there have been several studies that have merged multiple treatments into a single, multi-component intervention (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 2005). Such studies have done so by introducing multiple EOs concurrently and developing a function-based treatment that addressed all relevant operant variables. However, these previous analyses have focused on an intervention for a single individual whose problem behavior was maintained by multiple environmental variables. The current investigation extended this work by demonstrating that multiple treatments can be combined into a single treatment to effectively reduce the occurrence of problem behavior for two individuals. Additionally, the effects of the combined treatment were maintained when the siblings’ mother implemented the procedures. Although reductions in targeted problem behavior were maintained when the participants’ mother implemented treatment, data on procedural integrity were not collected and it is therefore unknown, aside from anecdotal observation, whether or not the mother implemented procedures correctly. Also, all sessions were conducted within an analog setting and the long-term effects of the combined treatment within the home setting are unknown. Data on procedural integrity and generalization into community or home settings represent a critical advancement for future research. The generality of the current results is also limited by other factors. For example, this investigation reported results for one set of siblings. The current participants displayed many diagnostic and behavioral features that may have impacted the efficacy of the combined treatment. For example, both children were diagnosed with similar disorders, which reflect a similar level of impairment (in the present cases, relatively mild). Siblings who display more varied clinical presentations would presumably be difficult to treat in a combined manner (e.g., one sibling might be able to attend to discriminative stimuli better than another). Also, the current partic-ipants displayed problem behavior that was topographically similar. Thus, the rele-vant types of problem behavior that a caregiver would need to attend to could affect treatment outcomes. As an illustrative example, previous research has shown that observers’ record of behavior degrades as the number of target behaviors under observation increases (cf., Kazdin 1977). It is likely that additional topographies of behavior across siblings could negatively impact a caregiver’s ability to implement specific contingencies with a high degree of integrity.
  • 12. 102 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 Perhaps a more limiting concern is that, although the current participants displayed behavior that was sensitive to different reinforcement contingencies, both exhibited problem behavior that was maintained by positive reinforcement. If both negative and positive reinforcement were involved that could possibly cause greater difficulties in treatment design and implementation by a caregiver. It should be noted, however, that several previous investigations have developed procedures for combining EOs that address different reinforcement contingencies (Bachmeyer et al. 2009; Call et al. 2005; Smith et al. 1993). There are a few other variables that impact the current results. First, the combined DRO procedure was implemented following successful (individual) treatment with FCT. It is therefore unknown if similar results would have been obtained without such a history. Also, the FCT and DRO procedures for Molly produced differential reinforcement of her functional reinforcer (attention), yet both also utilized access to a highly preferred reinforcer. It is possible that the use of the preferred toy alone would have been sufficient to decrease the occurrence of Molly’s problem behavior (Fischer et al. 1997; Hanley et al. 1997). Finally, the baseline and treatment con-ditions were implemented simultaneously for Molly and Sam when their mother began conducting sessions. As such, this aspect of the combined treatment analysis did not afford a demonstration of experimental control in and of itself. However, the initial effects of the combined DRO procedure were demonstrated in accordance with a multiple baseline across participants design, and the removal and subsequent reintroduction of the baseline and treatment conditions with the mother serving as the therapist approximated a reversal design (i.e., successive introduction and remov-al of the independent variable). In considering future directions for studies on multiplex families and the difficul-ties inherent in managing multiple problem behaviors, the importance of including caregivers in all aspects of treatment should not be underestimated. If the intention is to have caregivers independently manage behavior, then training them in defining behaviors, collecting data, identifying the functions of the behavior as well as implementing a function-based treatment should be the ultimate goal of clinicians. Previous research has demonstrated that various professional and para-professionals can be trained in such skills in a relatively time efficient manner (e.g., Phillips and Mudford 2008; Wallace et al. 2004). In addition, previous research has suggested that caregivers of single children with disabilities suffer high levels of anxiety and depression (Hastings 2003; Quintero and McIntyre 2010). Collecting data on care-giver mental health in multiplex families would be of great interest, particularly comparing pre- and post-treatment data. References Bachmeyer, M. H., Piazza, C. C., Fredrick, L. D., Reed, G. K., Rivas, K. D., & Kadey, H. J. (2009). Functional analysis and treatment of multiply controlled inappropriate mealtime behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 641–658. Ben Chaabanne, D. B., Alber-Morgan, S. R., & DeBar, R. M. (2009). The effects of parent-implemented PECS training on improvisation of mands by children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 671–677.
  • 13. J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 103 Borrero, C. S. W., & Vollmer, T. R. (2006). Experimental analysis and treatment of multiply controlled problem behavior: a systematic replication and extension. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 375–379. Call, N. A., Wacker, D. P., Ringdahl, J. E., & Boelter, E. W. (2005). Combined antecedent variables as motivating operations within functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 385–389. Constantine, J. N., Zhang, Y., Frazier, T., Abbacchi, A. M., & Law, P. (2010). Sibling recurrence and the genetic epidemiology of Autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 1349–1356. Day, H. M., Horner, R. H., & O’Neill, R. (1994). Multiple functions or problem behaviors: assessment and intervention. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 279–289. Farley, S. E., Adams, J. S., Lutton, M. E., & Scoville, C. J. (2005). What are effective treatments for oppositional and defiant behaviors in preadolescents? The Journal of Family Practice, 54, 162–165. Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Worsdell, A. S. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as treatment for self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 335–338. Fisher,W.W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., & Owens, J. C. (1992). A comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., & Chiang, C. L. (1996). Effects of equal and unequal reinforcer duration during functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 117–120. Griffith, G. M., & Richard, P. (2010). Using matched groups to explore child behavior problems and maternal well-being in children with Down Syndrome and Autism. Journal Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 610–619. Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. (1997). Noncontingent presentation of attention and alternative stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 229–237. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001). Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment with functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38. Hastings, R. P. (2003). Brief Report: behavioral adjustment of siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 99–104. Ingvarsson, E. T., Kahng, S., & Hausman, N. L. (2008). Some Effects of noncontingent positive reinforce-ment on multiply controlled problem behavior and compliance in a demand context. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 435–440. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). Kamps, D., Wills, H. P., Heitzman-Powell, L., Laylin, J., Szoke, C., Petrillo, T., & Culey, A. (2011). Class-wide function-related intervention teams: effects of group contingency programs in urban classrooms. The Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13, 154–157. Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Artifact, bias, and complexity of assessment: the ABCs of reliability. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 141–150. Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 391–395. Lerman, D. C., Swiezy, N., Perkins-Parks, S., & Roane, H. S. (2000). Skill acquisition in parents of children with developmental disabilities: interaction between skill type and instructional format. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 183–196. Meaden, H., Ostrosky, M. M., Zaghlawan, H. Y., & SeonYeong, Y. (2009). Promoting the social and communicative behavior of young children with autism spectrum disorders. A review of parent-implemented intervention studies. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 29, 90–104. Moes, D. R., & Frea, W. D. (2002). Contextualized behavioral support in early intervention for children with autism and their families. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 519–533. Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for Attention-Deficit- Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 184–214. Petalas, M. A., Hastings, R. P., Nash, S., Lloyd, T., & Dowey, A. (2009). Emotional and behavioural adjustment in siblings of children with intellectual disability with and without autism. Autism, 13, 471– 483. Phillips, K. J., & Mudford, O. C. (2008). Functional analysis skills training for residential caregivers. Behavioral Interventions, 23, 1–12. Quintero, N., & McIntyre, L. L. (2010). Sibling adjustment and maternal well-being: an examination of families with and without a child with an autism spectrum disorder. Focus Autism Other Developmen-tal Disabilities, 25, 37–46.
  • 14. 104 J Dev Phys Disabil (2013) 25:91–104 Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Sgro, G. M., Falcomata, T. S., & Pabico, R. R. (2004). An alternative method of thinning reinforcer delivery during differential reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 213–218. Rodriguez, J. R., Geffken, G. R., & Morgan, S. B. (1993). Perceived competence and behavioral adjustment of siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 665–674. Ross, P., & Cuskelly, M. (2006). Adjustment, sibling problems and coping strategies of brothers and sisters of children with autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 31, 77–86. Smith, M. R., & Lerman, D. C. (1999). A preliminary comparison of guided compliance and high-probability instructional sequences as treatment for noncompliance in children with developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20, 183–195. Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1993). Experimental analysis and treatment of multiply controlled self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 183–196. Wallace, M. D., Doney, J. K., Mintz-Resudek, C. M., & Tarbox, R. S. F. (2004). Training educators to implement functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 89–92. Worsdell, A. S., Iwata, B. A., Hanley, G. P., Thompson, R. H., & Kahng, S. (2000). Effects of continuous and intermittent reinforcement for problem behavior during functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 167–179.
  • 15. Copyright of Journal of Developmental & Physical Disabilities is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.