SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Police Officer’s Behaviours Associated with Successful
                  Conflict Resolution




             Helen Braithwaite, National Crime Authority




  Paper presented at the Australian Institute of Criminology Conference
             First Australasian Women Police Conference

                     Sydney, 29, 30 & 31 July 1996




                                   1
1. INTRODUCTION
...there are officers who are 'experts at turning parking tickets into riots' as well as
officers who can arrest extremely dangerous suspects and 'leave em laughing'. To
say we know both types of officers exist, however, is not to say that we know
anything systematic about their personal characteristics or methods.1
1.1 Background
Police officers are regularly called upon to deal with conflict situations. These
conflicts range from acting as a mediator in a domestic dispute, to restoring order
in a pub brawl. Individual officers have considerable discretion in the behaviours
they use to resolve these disputes. Officers will thus vary in their approach or
policing style, depending upon a number of factors such as personality and
experience. It is likely that from the wide range of styles shown by police officers
in general, some strategies will prove more successful than others. That is, some
police officers will more effectively deal with certain types of conflicts than other
officers. The aim of the present research project was to identify the specific police
officer behaviours which were successful in reducing the amount of citizen
resistance while on patrol.
Geller, W. A. (1985). Officer restraint in the use of deadly force. Journal of Police
Science and Administration, 13: 153 - 171.
Previous research has shown that the police-citizen encounter is an interaction or
process, the course of which is influenced by the behaviour of both parties.
Therefore, police not only react to the behaviour of citizens, but in turn the
behavioural tactics they employ affect the way the citizen responds to them.
Several studies have isolated effective police behaviours, and demonstrated that
certain officer behaviours increase the likelihood of non-cooperation or violence.2
In a conflict situation, effective behaviours are those that calm the situation down
and provide a solution to the conflict which is satisfactory to both parties.
Conversely, behaviours which are ineffective do not resolve the conflict, and may
even lead to an escalation of hostilities. It is reasonable to expect that, taking all
police officers, there will be a small proportion who are expert at using behavioural
tactics which are highly successful in resolving conflict. At the other end of the
scale, there will be a small proportion who use ineffective tactics, and may even
make matters worse. There will also be a large group in the middle who are about
average.
Researchers and police alike have this general notion that police differ in their
effectiveness when dealing with the public. A recent Australian study 3 examined
the amount of resistance police encountered while undertaking various taskings. It
was found that a small sub-group of officers (4% of the total sample) accounted for
21% of assaults with a weapon, and 15% of assaults experienced by the entire
group. This small group of officers was described by the authors as "resistance
prone". They did not differ from the other officers in the sample on any of the
descriptive variables such as experience or rank. Therefore, the difference must
have been due to an individual variable, such as personality or actual behaviour in
the field.




                                           2
The current research project attempted to discover to what extent an officers'
behaviour when dealing with the public was related to the amount of resistance
they encountered. It attempted to do so by directly observing police on patrol, and
collecting data on the behaviours
For example, Brent, E. E. & Sykes, R. E. (1979). A mathematical model of
symbolic interaction between police and suspects. Behavioral Science, 24: 388 -
402; Sykes, R. E. & Brent, E. E. (1980). The regulation of interaction by police: A
systems view of taking charge. Criminology, 18: 182 - 197; Wiley, M. G. & Hudik,
T. L. (1974). Police-citizen encounters: A field test of exchange theory. Social
Problems, 22: 119-129; Binder, A. & Scharf, P. (1980). The violent police-citizen
encounter. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 452:
111-121; McIver, J. P. & Parks, R. B. (1983). Evaluating police performance:
Identification of effective and ineffective police actions. In R. R. Bennett (Ed.).
Police at Work: Policy Issues and Analysis, pp 21 - 44. Sage; Bayley, D. H. &
Garafalo, J. (1989). The management of violence by police patrol officers.
Criminology, 27: 1-22. 3 Wilson, C. & Brewer, N. (1991). When do Patrol Officers
Encounter Resistance? National Police Research Unit Report displayed by both the
police and the citizens they were dealing with. By recording this behaviour in real-
time (with the aid of a computer the size of a calculator) it was possible to analyse
sequences of behaviours to determine the causal links between them. If the officer
does X, what was the citizen likely to do?
most research on police officers in general, and patrol officers in particular,
concentrates on cataloguing and explaining officer behavior...In other words, these
studies uniformly stop just short of the question of importance: what consequence
does the behavior of the police officer have on the resolution of the incident?
1.2 Measuring conflict resolution behaviours in police officers
Before observations of police and citizen interactions could take place, a system for
the coding and recording of behaviours had to be developed. A review of the
psychological literature on conflict resolution was undertaken, in order to
determine the results of previous studies. This process provided very little insight
into specific behaviours which were found to influence the outcome of conflict.
What was revealed, however, were general concepts such as "good communication
skills" and "ability to defuse the situation" which, when applied to a conflict
situation, yielded beneficial results. The current project aimed at refining these
broad categories into actual behaviours which would be effective in reducing
conflict. A secondary aim was to design an instrument for measuring this taxonomy
of behaviours.
Findings from past conflict resolution research contributed to selecting behaviours
for inclusion in the taxonomy. As an example, the psychological literature
identified concern for the other party as an important step in successfully resolving
disputes. The more empathy a person had for the position of the other party, the
more likely the conflict would reach a solution which was satisfactory to all
concerned. This lead to the behavioural category called "support", which was
designed to measure the extent to which officers make statements expressing
concern for the citizen. Examples of statements coded into this category were "look
mate, you did the right thing" or "you have every right to be upset". There was a



                                         3
total of ten behavioural categories, into which all verbal and physical behaviour of
the officers and members of the public could be coded.
McIver, J. P. & Parks, R. B. (1983). Evaluating police performance: Identification
of effective and ineffective police actions. In R. R. Bennett (Ed.). Police at Work:
Policy Issues and Analysis, pp 21 -44. Sage.
The instrument was pilot tested in several different situations, including a breath
testing station, traffic response car, and foot patrol. The primary researcher spent
considerable time training in the use of the instrument, to ensure consistent coding
of behaviours and accurate entering into the datalogger.
2. METHODOLOGY
2.1 Sample
Subjects were sixteen South Australian patrol officers from two police stations in
the Adelaide metropolitan area. Subjects were identified through the use of two
appraisal instruments, described below, and were individually approached and
asked to participate in the study. No officer declined to participate.
The sample consisted of four female officers and twelve male officers. All were of
the rank of constable. All female officers were single, while six of the male officers
were married.
Officers were selected into two groups - those identified by their peers and
supervisors as particularly skilled in their dealings with the public, and officers not
so identified. Two pencil and paper tests were used to assess group membership. A
questionnaire was given to all patrol officers located at the two stations. Officers
were asked to list six officers from all those at the station that they perceived to be
most skilled at handling conflict. Four officers at each station were identified based
on the total number of nominations they received, and placed into the 'skilled'
group. An equal number were also selected into an 'average' group on the basis of
receiving a nomination of 0 or 1 by their peers. As a secondary measure,
supervisors were asked to rate each member of their patrol team on an 8-item
questionnaire assessing the officer's skills in dealing with the public. Supervisors
rated the skilled officers more highly than the average officers, indicating that
supervisor ratings were consistent with peer perceptions.
The selection of subjects was carried out by a secondary researcher. Neither the
officers nor the primary researcher was aware of the officers' group membership.
The female officers, the focus of this paper, were all selected into the skilled group.
The analysis will therefore predominantly focus on comparing the four 'skilled
males' with the four 'skilled females', as well as looking at general trends in officer
behaviour.
2.2 Technique for observing
Observations were conducted during afternoon and night shift, to maximise the
chance of observing conflict. The primary researcher accompanied the selected
officer, and his/her partner, on general duties patrol. When the officer commenced
an interaction with a member of the public, usually through responding to a call
over the radio, data collection began. Each verbal statement was instantly coded
into one of the behavioural categories and entered into the datalogger, by


                                          4
depressing the corresponding key on the keypad. They key was depressed for the
duration of the behaviour, then the next behaviour was recorded in a similar
fashion. Physical contact between the police and public was also recorded, and the
datalogger had a facility for overlapping the recording of physical behaviour, as it
was often the case that police spoke with a citizen in conjunction with physically
handling him or her. Two separate keys were used to indicate whether the
behaviour belonged to the second officer or the citizen, otherwise the behaviour
was presumed to belong to the target officer for ease of coding.
The entire conversation was thus transformed, as it happened, into a series of
keystrokes which recorded the type of behaviour, who committed the behaviour,
the time the behaviour commenced, and the time the behaviour ceased. This data
was stored into the memory of the datalogger and later down-loaded to a PC for
analysis.
Each officer was observed for a minimum of 25 interactions each 5 . Coding of
interactions took place over a 5 month period.
2.3 Datalogger
The datalogger is a hand-held computerised device developed by Sturt Technology,
South Australia. It is about the size of a 'Nintendo Gameboy', and has a 4 line LCD
display. The datalogger has an in-built clock, so is capable of recording every
behaviour and the time that behaviour occurred, in sequence. It has 256K of RAM
to temporarily store data, before the data is downloaded to a PC. An internal NiCad
battery provides about 24 hours operation, before needing to be recharged.
Except for two officers who were observed for 20 interactions 2.4 Combating bias
Studies which employ the participant-observation methodology involve a risk of
the researcher influencing the behaviour being observed. Every effort was made to
ensure the integrity of the data in this project.
The primary researcher employed several tactics while on patrol to minimise her
presence. Body language directed attention away from herself, by remaining at a
distance from the interaction, not making eye contact with participants, and never
verbally becoming involved in an encounter. The observer was careful not to give
feedback to the officers about the progress of an interaction, or to make judgements
about their handling of any situation. The datalogger which was used to record the
data was unobtrusive enough to be held in one hand and largely go unnoticed.
It was also advantageous that the officers were aware at the outset of the extensive
time period involved in collecting the data. It was unlikely that officers would
adopt an approach of 'being on their best behaviour' for the duration of the study,
seeing as this would involve maintaining their change in behaviour for five months.
Officers were assured strict anonymity and were assigned an identifying number
for recording purposes. Subjects were also assured of the confidentiality of the
data. Officers were briefed regarding the categories used to record the behaviour,
and understood that these were broad enough to exclude much of the detail of the
conversation. The researcher also explained that the individual officer's data
contributed to group aggregates only, and that personal performance was not being
assessed.
2.5 Reliability


                                         5
It was necessary to ascertain the reliability of the data to ensure that the primary
observer was objective in the recording of behaviours, and that the procedure could
be replicated and was not subject to bias. A second observer supplied by the
National Police Research Unit was trained in the use of the datalogger and the
behavioural categories, and accompanied the primary researcher on patrol for
approximately 10% of the total number of interactions. The two observers used the
procedure outlined above to both record the interaction. A computer program was
written to analyse the two sets of data for agreement. A quantitative assessment of
reliability revealed it to be of an acceptable standard.
3. RESULTS
Results will be discussed in two sections. Firstly, the overall frequency count of
behaviours will be examined, to provide a general picture of how police spend their
time on patrol. The differences between male and female officers in the number of
times they use particular tactics will be observed. Secondly, the relationship
between officer tactics and citizen behaviours will be explored, by examining the
causal links between behaviours. This sequential analysis will indicate how male
and female officers, through their behavioural tactics, influence the course of the
interaction differently.
3.1 Frequency of behaviours
All interactions
Preliminary analyses were undertaken to determine how frequently particular
behaviours occurred. The results indicated that the most frequent behaviour
engaged in was information exchange, which involved a rapid exchange of
questions and answers between the officers and citizens. This is not surprising, as
the major goal of police of arrival at the scene is to establish what has happened.
Officers do this by asking questions, which takes control of the situation by casting
the citizen into the role of information provider.
Officers also spent a significant proportion of the interaction making statements
which were aimed at directly controlling the situation. Both the target officer and
his or her partner frequently directed the behaviour of the citizen. The citizen, on
the other hand, contributed to the interaction by making defensive statements,
which denied their role as offender or made justifications for their behaviour.
Therefore, members of the public generally answered questions asked of them by
police, but also frequently defended their actions.
To a lesser extent, officers were engaged in other behaviours which either rejected
the citizen's account, or supported the citizen in a display of empathy. Coercive
behaviours, such as threats and verbal abuse, occurred infrequently compared with
the relative frequency of all other behaviours. Physical behaviour, while not
appearing to occur very often, had an extensive duration. Unlike verbal behaviours,
which rapidly stop-started, physical behaviour was more enduring and therefore did
not feature in the frequency statistics. Approximately 11% of interactions contained
physical behaviour on the part of one or both of the officers.
Males versus females
Comparing the frequency of behaviours which occurred when the lead officer was
either male or female (both of which were identified as 'skilled'), significant


                                         6
differences appeared. Female officers in this role were more supportive towards
citizens, engaging in more information exchange and support behaviours. Males
preferred the tactics of controlling statements, threats, and physical acts. Examining
the frequency of the citizen's behaviour, male officers tended to elicit more
defensive statements and verbal abuse from the citizens than did female officers.
3.2 Sequential analysis
Frequency data alone cannot reveal the causal links between behaviours. If an
officer frequently uses threats and is also involved in a higher than average number
of physical confrontations, one cannot conclude that one caused the other. The next
stage in the analysis was to examine the sequences of behaviours, seeing as the
behaviours were stored in the order that they happened. Remembering that the
police-citizen encounter is an interactive process, the actions of both the citizen and
officer will influence the course of the interaction.
A computer program based on a statistical procedure called lag sequential analysis
was written, which could identify sequences of behaviour contained in the data.
This program allowed the researcher to nominate a behaviour of interest (a
'criterion behaviour'), and then determine what other behaviours were likely to
surround this criterion behaviour. For example, let's take a threat by an officer. The
program then computed the behaviours which were more than likely by chance to
occur immediately before the threat, or immediately after the threat. In this manner,
behavioural sequences could be established which provided insight to research
questions such as "what happened next?" or "what caused this to happen?".
All interactions
Before examining gender differences, it may be useful to consider briefly the
patterns of behaviour which described the interactions generally. The results
showed that some officer tactics lead to a positive sequence of behaviour which
was likely to minimise resistance from the public.
For example, if an officer followed questioning the citizen by supportive
statements, this was associated with a heightened probability of further information
exchange behaviour, during which the citizen cooperated in providing information.
Once an officer made a statement in support for the citizen, the sequence of
information exchange and further supportive statements was likely.
INFORMATION ® SUPPORT ® INFORMATION EXCHANGE EXCHANGE
"What did he look like?" "I wish everyone was "What else?" "Tall, long hair,
wearing as observant as you" "Jeans, black shoes" a brown jumper"
Other officer tactics resulted in increased probability of violence, by steering the
interaction away from information exchange and towards more coercive
behaviours. For example, when an officer made a controlling statement, the chance
of information exchange behaviour was reduced, and behaviours such as further
controlling statements and the citizen displaying both verbal and physical abuse,
were more likely. A rejecting statement by the officer was also likely to reduce
information exchange, and lead to a pattern of further rejecting statements and
citizen defensive behaviour. Similarly, when the officer threatened a citizen this
often lead to further threats being issued. This resulted in a heightened probability
of verbal and physical abuse from the citizen.


                                          7
Sequences of behaviour were also initiated by citizen actions. For example, when a
citizen refused to cooperate with the officer, it was more likely that verbal abuse
and physical conflict would result.
REFUSE ® VERBAL ABUSE ® PHYSICAL "There's no way I'm "Haven't you
got any Officer grabs the citizen getting in the car"real work to do, you
bastard?"and assists him into car
Males versus females
Analysis revealed that female officers used slightly different tactics when dealing
with the public than their male counterparts of equivalent skill level.
Probably the most significant difference was the ability of the females to get the
interaction "back on track" after a behaviour which threatened to divert the
interaction down a more coercive path. That is, females were more likely than
males to follow behaviours such as threats and rejecting statements with questions.
This resulted in the citizen answering the question, allowing further questions to be
asked in this mutual exchange of information.
Female Officers - Likely Scenario
REJECT ® INFORMATIONEXCHANGE
"Don't give me that rubbish."Try telling me again,"He pushed my girl friend He
just told me he saw youwhat made you hit him?"against the wall" do it"
Female officers were also more likely than males to persist with a series of
supportive statements, interspersed during the exchange of information process.
Males were also found to engage in this sequence of behaviour, but to a lesser
extent than females. Once a female officer showed empathy for the citizen and
made a statement supporting them, this 'set the tone' for future positive statements
towards the citizen.
Females used more rejecting statements than their male counterparts, and also were
more likely to repeat rejecting statements. This could indicate that this was a
preferred tactic for dealing with citizens. Alternatively, it may have resulted from
members of the public having a tendency to distort the truth to females officers in
an attempt to influence them. Perhaps offenders would 'try one on' with female
officers, resulting in them getting caught out.
Males were a lot more likely than females to repeat a threat, whereas females
sometimes asked questions directly after the threat, as well as repeating the threat
(refer to scenarios over the page). Male officers were more likely to elicit verbal
abuse or defensive behaviour from the citizen after giving a threat. This could be
due to male officer threats being in reality more threatening than females, or
perceived as more threatening my members of the public. Either way, the result
was an escalation of conflict.
Male Officers - Likely Scenario
THREAT ® THREAT ® VERBAL ABUSE
"Look mate, if you don't "You'll be in the cells "Get lost. I'd like to start telling the
truth, before you know it" see you try" I'll lock you up"
Female officers - Likely Scenario


                                           8
THREAT ® INFORMATION EXCHANGE
"If you refuse to answer, "So tell me again, "Must have been you will be arrested"
what time did you go there? around 10 o'clock"
More verbal abuse was directed towards male officers, and its probability of
occurrence was increased when male officers rejected a citizen's account of events,
threatened the citizen, or physically handled the citizen. In contrast, abusive
language was likely to be directed towards the female officers in response to her
attempts to control the situation by making a statement which directed their
behaviour. Once verbal abuse began, the likelihood of further abusive statements
was greatly increased.
4. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
In conclusion, this research project demonstrated that police-citizen encounters
were able to be studied as an interactive process, and that police officers influenced
the response they received from citizens through the behavioural tactics they used.
Male officers were found to be more coercive than their female counterparts,
preferring tactics such as threats, controlling statements, and physical behaviours.
This approach lead to males experiencing higher levels of citizen resistance while
on patrol.
Female officers, in contrast, were more adept at steering an interaction away from
conflict by directing questions at citizens, placing them in the position of
information provider and hence exerting control over the interaction. Females were
also more supportive of citizens than male officers, often giving positive feedback
to the person they were conversing with. This in turn lead to the citizen cooperating
with further questions, again resulting in empathy from the officer. Additionally,
female officers preferred a rejecting tactic, which repudiated the citizen's account
of events. Either the females used this tactic in preference to other coercive tactics,
such as threats, or members of the public more often lied to female officers,
causing them to reject their statements.
These findings call into question the myth that female police officers are a risk to
their male colleagues because they 'can't handle conflict'. It would appear from the
data that females have well developed strategies for avoiding conflict, and elicit a
more positive response from the people they speak with on patrol.




                                          9

More Related Content

What's hot

schuster critical evaluation
schuster critical evaluationschuster critical evaluation
schuster critical evaluationMichael Schuster
 
Chapter 2 Criminal Justice
Chapter 2 Criminal JusticeChapter 2 Criminal Justice
Chapter 2 Criminal JusticeKatrina Homer
 
Police Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson RichardPolice Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson RichardRichard Anderson
 
BIS 300 Research proposal
BIS 300 Research proposalBIS 300 Research proposal
BIS 300 Research proposalMelisa Le
 
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on SocietyPolice Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on SocietyAutumn Moody
 
Cja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.com
Cja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.comCja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.com
Cja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.comStephenson25
 
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in NigeriaEffectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in NigeriaGabriel Ken
 

What's hot (10)

Chapter2
Chapter2Chapter2
Chapter2
 
schuster critical evaluation
schuster critical evaluationschuster critical evaluation
schuster critical evaluation
 
Chapter 2 Criminal Justice
Chapter 2 Criminal JusticeChapter 2 Criminal Justice
Chapter 2 Criminal Justice
 
Identifying “mischievous” responders through Latent Class Analysis
Identifying “mischievous” responders through Latent Class AnalysisIdentifying “mischievous” responders through Latent Class Analysis
Identifying “mischievous” responders through Latent Class Analysis
 
Police Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson RichardPolice Pursuits_Anderson Richard
Police Pursuits_Anderson Richard
 
BIS 300 Research proposal
BIS 300 Research proposalBIS 300 Research proposal
BIS 300 Research proposal
 
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on SocietyPolice Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
Police Mistreatment and the Effect on Society
 
Cja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.com
Cja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.comCja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.com
Cja 415 Enthusiastic Study / snaptutorial.com
 
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in NigeriaEffectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
 
URC Poster 2016
URC Poster 2016URC Poster 2016
URC Poster 2016
 

Viewers also liked

Lesson 8 family
Lesson 8 familyLesson 8 family
Lesson 8 familyleihun5744
 
free jav, video jav, jav download
free jav, video jav, jav downloadfree jav, video jav, jav download
free jav, video jav, jav downloadxuan ty
 
Lesson 3 colours
Lesson 3 coloursLesson 3 colours
Lesson 3 coloursleihun5744
 
Ep intro Nov 2011
Ep intro Nov 2011Ep intro Nov 2011
Ep intro Nov 2011Badsett
 
Lesson 9 places
Lesson 9 placesLesson 9 places
Lesson 9 placesleihun5744
 
Lesson 10 school
Lesson 10 schoolLesson 10 school
Lesson 10 schoolleihun5744
 
Eakate toetamine1
Eakate toetamine1Eakate toetamine1
Eakate toetamine1kristikopli
 
Lesson 11 hobby
Lesson 11 hobbyLesson 11 hobby
Lesson 11 hobbyleihun5744
 
Lesson 12 food and drinks
Lesson 12 food and drinksLesson 12 food and drinks
Lesson 12 food and drinksleihun5744
 
Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015
Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015
Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015Logan Bartlett
 
Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)
Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)
Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)Abhi Gosavi
 

Viewers also liked (17)

Pensamiento computacionalilustrado
Pensamiento computacionalilustradoPensamiento computacionalilustrado
Pensamiento computacionalilustrado
 
Lesson 8 family
Lesson 8 familyLesson 8 family
Lesson 8 family
 
free jav, video jav, jav download
free jav, video jav, jav downloadfree jav, video jav, jav download
free jav, video jav, jav download
 
Lesson 3 colours
Lesson 3 coloursLesson 3 colours
Lesson 3 colours
 
Ep intro Nov 2011
Ep intro Nov 2011Ep intro Nov 2011
Ep intro Nov 2011
 
Lesson 9 places
Lesson 9 placesLesson 9 places
Lesson 9 places
 
Johari window
Johari windowJohari window
Johari window
 
Lesson 10 school
Lesson 10 schoolLesson 10 school
Lesson 10 school
 
Test 3
Test 3Test 3
Test 3
 
Body
BodyBody
Body
 
Eakate toetamine1
Eakate toetamine1Eakate toetamine1
Eakate toetamine1
 
Lesson 2
Lesson 2 Lesson 2
Lesson 2
 
Lesson 11 hobby
Lesson 11 hobbyLesson 11 hobby
Lesson 11 hobby
 
Lesson 12 food and drinks
Lesson 12 food and drinksLesson 12 food and drinks
Lesson 12 food and drinks
 
Idps
IdpsIdps
Idps
 
Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015
Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015
Logan Bartlett - Mattermark Webinar 4.30.2015
 
Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)
Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)
Abhishek Gosavi (Updated Resume)
 

Similar to Conductasdeloficial solucionesproblemas

Police and Communities brief.pdf
Police and Communities brief.pdfPolice and Communities brief.pdf
Police and Communities brief.pdfElizabethDay32
 
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School BetseyCalderon89
 
Running head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY .docx
Running head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY   .docxRunning head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY   .docx
Running head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY .docxwlynn1
 
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docx
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docxCopyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docx
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docxvanesaburnand
 
Exploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docx
Exploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docxExploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docx
Exploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docxlmelaine
 
Runnin head POLICE AND CRIME .docx
Runnin head POLICE AND CRIME                                   .docxRunnin head POLICE AND CRIME                                   .docx
Runnin head POLICE AND CRIME .docxSUBHI7
 
U.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docx
U.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docxU.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docx
U.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docxmarilucorr
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACASantosConleyha
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACABenitoSumpter862
 
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docxI have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docxsamirapdcosden
 
Legitimacy and Procedural Justice A New El.docx
Legitimacy and Procedural Justice  A New El.docxLegitimacy and Procedural Justice  A New El.docx
Legitimacy and Procedural Justice A New El.docxsmile790243
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017PubliAnastaciaShadelb
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017PubliKiyokoSlagleis
 
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publiUMAR48665
 
Com 212 formal lit review v. 2
Com 212 formal lit review v. 2Com 212 formal lit review v. 2
Com 212 formal lit review v. 2Geneva Mae Lewis
 
Deviant behavior police brutality
Deviant behavior police brutalityDeviant behavior police brutality
Deviant behavior police brutalitygithure eliud
 
jciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1 9-F
jciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1  9-Fjciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1  9-F
jciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1 9-FTatianaMajor22
 
1 Beyond Oversight A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform
1 Beyond  Oversight   A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform1 Beyond  Oversight   A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform
1 Beyond Oversight A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police ReformApril Knyff
 
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docxModule 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docxbunnyfinney
 
Operational report - UK Police Morale
Operational report - UK Police MoraleOperational report - UK Police Morale
Operational report - UK Police MoraleElena Collins
 

Similar to Conductasdeloficial solucionesproblemas (20)

Police and Communities brief.pdf
Police and Communities brief.pdfPolice and Communities brief.pdf
Police and Communities brief.pdf
 
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICING 1Jervaughn F. ReidHelm School
 
Running head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY .docx
Running head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY   .docxRunning head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY   .docx
Running head LESS THAN LETHAL FORCE IS THERE A BETTER WAY .docx
 
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docx
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docxCopyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docx
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd 20 62–71 (2010)DOI.docx
 
Exploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docx
Exploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docxExploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docx
Exploring the relationshipbetween foot and car patrolin .docx
 
Runnin head POLICE AND CRIME .docx
Runnin head POLICE AND CRIME                                   .docxRunnin head POLICE AND CRIME                                   .docx
Runnin head POLICE AND CRIME .docx
 
U.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docx
U.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docxU.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docx
U.S. Department of JusticeOffice of Justice ProgramsNation.docx
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
 
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docxI have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
I have to complete Module 5, 6, and 7.......Each Modulehas 3 questio.docx
 
Legitimacy and Procedural Justice A New El.docx
Legitimacy and Procedural Justice  A New El.docxLegitimacy and Procedural Justice  A New El.docx
Legitimacy and Procedural Justice A New El.docx
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
 
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi168 Public Administration Review • March  April 2017Publi
168 Public Administration Review • March April 2017Publi
 
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi168 public administration review • march  april 2017 publi
168 public administration review • march april 2017 publi
 
Com 212 formal lit review v. 2
Com 212 formal lit review v. 2Com 212 formal lit review v. 2
Com 212 formal lit review v. 2
 
Deviant behavior police brutality
Deviant behavior police brutalityDeviant behavior police brutality
Deviant behavior police brutality
 
jciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1 9-F
jciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1  9-Fjciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1  9-F
jciprod01productnHHLP10-1HLP101.txt unknown Seq 1 9-F
 
1 Beyond Oversight A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform
1 Beyond  Oversight   A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform1 Beyond  Oversight   A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform
1 Beyond Oversight A Problem-Oriented Approach To Police Reform
 
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docxModule 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
Module 5 CRJ 307 Police, Society, and Community ServiceEssays.docx
 
Operational report - UK Police Morale
Operational report - UK Police MoraleOperational report - UK Police Morale
Operational report - UK Police Morale
 

Conductasdeloficial solucionesproblemas

  • 1. Police Officer’s Behaviours Associated with Successful Conflict Resolution Helen Braithwaite, National Crime Authority Paper presented at the Australian Institute of Criminology Conference First Australasian Women Police Conference Sydney, 29, 30 & 31 July 1996 1
  • 2. 1. INTRODUCTION ...there are officers who are 'experts at turning parking tickets into riots' as well as officers who can arrest extremely dangerous suspects and 'leave em laughing'. To say we know both types of officers exist, however, is not to say that we know anything systematic about their personal characteristics or methods.1 1.1 Background Police officers are regularly called upon to deal with conflict situations. These conflicts range from acting as a mediator in a domestic dispute, to restoring order in a pub brawl. Individual officers have considerable discretion in the behaviours they use to resolve these disputes. Officers will thus vary in their approach or policing style, depending upon a number of factors such as personality and experience. It is likely that from the wide range of styles shown by police officers in general, some strategies will prove more successful than others. That is, some police officers will more effectively deal with certain types of conflicts than other officers. The aim of the present research project was to identify the specific police officer behaviours which were successful in reducing the amount of citizen resistance while on patrol. Geller, W. A. (1985). Officer restraint in the use of deadly force. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 13: 153 - 171. Previous research has shown that the police-citizen encounter is an interaction or process, the course of which is influenced by the behaviour of both parties. Therefore, police not only react to the behaviour of citizens, but in turn the behavioural tactics they employ affect the way the citizen responds to them. Several studies have isolated effective police behaviours, and demonstrated that certain officer behaviours increase the likelihood of non-cooperation or violence.2 In a conflict situation, effective behaviours are those that calm the situation down and provide a solution to the conflict which is satisfactory to both parties. Conversely, behaviours which are ineffective do not resolve the conflict, and may even lead to an escalation of hostilities. It is reasonable to expect that, taking all police officers, there will be a small proportion who are expert at using behavioural tactics which are highly successful in resolving conflict. At the other end of the scale, there will be a small proportion who use ineffective tactics, and may even make matters worse. There will also be a large group in the middle who are about average. Researchers and police alike have this general notion that police differ in their effectiveness when dealing with the public. A recent Australian study 3 examined the amount of resistance police encountered while undertaking various taskings. It was found that a small sub-group of officers (4% of the total sample) accounted for 21% of assaults with a weapon, and 15% of assaults experienced by the entire group. This small group of officers was described by the authors as "resistance prone". They did not differ from the other officers in the sample on any of the descriptive variables such as experience or rank. Therefore, the difference must have been due to an individual variable, such as personality or actual behaviour in the field. 2
  • 3. The current research project attempted to discover to what extent an officers' behaviour when dealing with the public was related to the amount of resistance they encountered. It attempted to do so by directly observing police on patrol, and collecting data on the behaviours For example, Brent, E. E. & Sykes, R. E. (1979). A mathematical model of symbolic interaction between police and suspects. Behavioral Science, 24: 388 - 402; Sykes, R. E. & Brent, E. E. (1980). The regulation of interaction by police: A systems view of taking charge. Criminology, 18: 182 - 197; Wiley, M. G. & Hudik, T. L. (1974). Police-citizen encounters: A field test of exchange theory. Social Problems, 22: 119-129; Binder, A. & Scharf, P. (1980). The violent police-citizen encounter. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 452: 111-121; McIver, J. P. & Parks, R. B. (1983). Evaluating police performance: Identification of effective and ineffective police actions. In R. R. Bennett (Ed.). Police at Work: Policy Issues and Analysis, pp 21 - 44. Sage; Bayley, D. H. & Garafalo, J. (1989). The management of violence by police patrol officers. Criminology, 27: 1-22. 3 Wilson, C. & Brewer, N. (1991). When do Patrol Officers Encounter Resistance? National Police Research Unit Report displayed by both the police and the citizens they were dealing with. By recording this behaviour in real- time (with the aid of a computer the size of a calculator) it was possible to analyse sequences of behaviours to determine the causal links between them. If the officer does X, what was the citizen likely to do? most research on police officers in general, and patrol officers in particular, concentrates on cataloguing and explaining officer behavior...In other words, these studies uniformly stop just short of the question of importance: what consequence does the behavior of the police officer have on the resolution of the incident? 1.2 Measuring conflict resolution behaviours in police officers Before observations of police and citizen interactions could take place, a system for the coding and recording of behaviours had to be developed. A review of the psychological literature on conflict resolution was undertaken, in order to determine the results of previous studies. This process provided very little insight into specific behaviours which were found to influence the outcome of conflict. What was revealed, however, were general concepts such as "good communication skills" and "ability to defuse the situation" which, when applied to a conflict situation, yielded beneficial results. The current project aimed at refining these broad categories into actual behaviours which would be effective in reducing conflict. A secondary aim was to design an instrument for measuring this taxonomy of behaviours. Findings from past conflict resolution research contributed to selecting behaviours for inclusion in the taxonomy. As an example, the psychological literature identified concern for the other party as an important step in successfully resolving disputes. The more empathy a person had for the position of the other party, the more likely the conflict would reach a solution which was satisfactory to all concerned. This lead to the behavioural category called "support", which was designed to measure the extent to which officers make statements expressing concern for the citizen. Examples of statements coded into this category were "look mate, you did the right thing" or "you have every right to be upset". There was a 3
  • 4. total of ten behavioural categories, into which all verbal and physical behaviour of the officers and members of the public could be coded. McIver, J. P. & Parks, R. B. (1983). Evaluating police performance: Identification of effective and ineffective police actions. In R. R. Bennett (Ed.). Police at Work: Policy Issues and Analysis, pp 21 -44. Sage. The instrument was pilot tested in several different situations, including a breath testing station, traffic response car, and foot patrol. The primary researcher spent considerable time training in the use of the instrument, to ensure consistent coding of behaviours and accurate entering into the datalogger. 2. METHODOLOGY 2.1 Sample Subjects were sixteen South Australian patrol officers from two police stations in the Adelaide metropolitan area. Subjects were identified through the use of two appraisal instruments, described below, and were individually approached and asked to participate in the study. No officer declined to participate. The sample consisted of four female officers and twelve male officers. All were of the rank of constable. All female officers were single, while six of the male officers were married. Officers were selected into two groups - those identified by their peers and supervisors as particularly skilled in their dealings with the public, and officers not so identified. Two pencil and paper tests were used to assess group membership. A questionnaire was given to all patrol officers located at the two stations. Officers were asked to list six officers from all those at the station that they perceived to be most skilled at handling conflict. Four officers at each station were identified based on the total number of nominations they received, and placed into the 'skilled' group. An equal number were also selected into an 'average' group on the basis of receiving a nomination of 0 or 1 by their peers. As a secondary measure, supervisors were asked to rate each member of their patrol team on an 8-item questionnaire assessing the officer's skills in dealing with the public. Supervisors rated the skilled officers more highly than the average officers, indicating that supervisor ratings were consistent with peer perceptions. The selection of subjects was carried out by a secondary researcher. Neither the officers nor the primary researcher was aware of the officers' group membership. The female officers, the focus of this paper, were all selected into the skilled group. The analysis will therefore predominantly focus on comparing the four 'skilled males' with the four 'skilled females', as well as looking at general trends in officer behaviour. 2.2 Technique for observing Observations were conducted during afternoon and night shift, to maximise the chance of observing conflict. The primary researcher accompanied the selected officer, and his/her partner, on general duties patrol. When the officer commenced an interaction with a member of the public, usually through responding to a call over the radio, data collection began. Each verbal statement was instantly coded into one of the behavioural categories and entered into the datalogger, by 4
  • 5. depressing the corresponding key on the keypad. They key was depressed for the duration of the behaviour, then the next behaviour was recorded in a similar fashion. Physical contact between the police and public was also recorded, and the datalogger had a facility for overlapping the recording of physical behaviour, as it was often the case that police spoke with a citizen in conjunction with physically handling him or her. Two separate keys were used to indicate whether the behaviour belonged to the second officer or the citizen, otherwise the behaviour was presumed to belong to the target officer for ease of coding. The entire conversation was thus transformed, as it happened, into a series of keystrokes which recorded the type of behaviour, who committed the behaviour, the time the behaviour commenced, and the time the behaviour ceased. This data was stored into the memory of the datalogger and later down-loaded to a PC for analysis. Each officer was observed for a minimum of 25 interactions each 5 . Coding of interactions took place over a 5 month period. 2.3 Datalogger The datalogger is a hand-held computerised device developed by Sturt Technology, South Australia. It is about the size of a 'Nintendo Gameboy', and has a 4 line LCD display. The datalogger has an in-built clock, so is capable of recording every behaviour and the time that behaviour occurred, in sequence. It has 256K of RAM to temporarily store data, before the data is downloaded to a PC. An internal NiCad battery provides about 24 hours operation, before needing to be recharged. Except for two officers who were observed for 20 interactions 2.4 Combating bias Studies which employ the participant-observation methodology involve a risk of the researcher influencing the behaviour being observed. Every effort was made to ensure the integrity of the data in this project. The primary researcher employed several tactics while on patrol to minimise her presence. Body language directed attention away from herself, by remaining at a distance from the interaction, not making eye contact with participants, and never verbally becoming involved in an encounter. The observer was careful not to give feedback to the officers about the progress of an interaction, or to make judgements about their handling of any situation. The datalogger which was used to record the data was unobtrusive enough to be held in one hand and largely go unnoticed. It was also advantageous that the officers were aware at the outset of the extensive time period involved in collecting the data. It was unlikely that officers would adopt an approach of 'being on their best behaviour' for the duration of the study, seeing as this would involve maintaining their change in behaviour for five months. Officers were assured strict anonymity and were assigned an identifying number for recording purposes. Subjects were also assured of the confidentiality of the data. Officers were briefed regarding the categories used to record the behaviour, and understood that these were broad enough to exclude much of the detail of the conversation. The researcher also explained that the individual officer's data contributed to group aggregates only, and that personal performance was not being assessed. 2.5 Reliability 5
  • 6. It was necessary to ascertain the reliability of the data to ensure that the primary observer was objective in the recording of behaviours, and that the procedure could be replicated and was not subject to bias. A second observer supplied by the National Police Research Unit was trained in the use of the datalogger and the behavioural categories, and accompanied the primary researcher on patrol for approximately 10% of the total number of interactions. The two observers used the procedure outlined above to both record the interaction. A computer program was written to analyse the two sets of data for agreement. A quantitative assessment of reliability revealed it to be of an acceptable standard. 3. RESULTS Results will be discussed in two sections. Firstly, the overall frequency count of behaviours will be examined, to provide a general picture of how police spend their time on patrol. The differences between male and female officers in the number of times they use particular tactics will be observed. Secondly, the relationship between officer tactics and citizen behaviours will be explored, by examining the causal links between behaviours. This sequential analysis will indicate how male and female officers, through their behavioural tactics, influence the course of the interaction differently. 3.1 Frequency of behaviours All interactions Preliminary analyses were undertaken to determine how frequently particular behaviours occurred. The results indicated that the most frequent behaviour engaged in was information exchange, which involved a rapid exchange of questions and answers between the officers and citizens. This is not surprising, as the major goal of police of arrival at the scene is to establish what has happened. Officers do this by asking questions, which takes control of the situation by casting the citizen into the role of information provider. Officers also spent a significant proportion of the interaction making statements which were aimed at directly controlling the situation. Both the target officer and his or her partner frequently directed the behaviour of the citizen. The citizen, on the other hand, contributed to the interaction by making defensive statements, which denied their role as offender or made justifications for their behaviour. Therefore, members of the public generally answered questions asked of them by police, but also frequently defended their actions. To a lesser extent, officers were engaged in other behaviours which either rejected the citizen's account, or supported the citizen in a display of empathy. Coercive behaviours, such as threats and verbal abuse, occurred infrequently compared with the relative frequency of all other behaviours. Physical behaviour, while not appearing to occur very often, had an extensive duration. Unlike verbal behaviours, which rapidly stop-started, physical behaviour was more enduring and therefore did not feature in the frequency statistics. Approximately 11% of interactions contained physical behaviour on the part of one or both of the officers. Males versus females Comparing the frequency of behaviours which occurred when the lead officer was either male or female (both of which were identified as 'skilled'), significant 6
  • 7. differences appeared. Female officers in this role were more supportive towards citizens, engaging in more information exchange and support behaviours. Males preferred the tactics of controlling statements, threats, and physical acts. Examining the frequency of the citizen's behaviour, male officers tended to elicit more defensive statements and verbal abuse from the citizens than did female officers. 3.2 Sequential analysis Frequency data alone cannot reveal the causal links between behaviours. If an officer frequently uses threats and is also involved in a higher than average number of physical confrontations, one cannot conclude that one caused the other. The next stage in the analysis was to examine the sequences of behaviours, seeing as the behaviours were stored in the order that they happened. Remembering that the police-citizen encounter is an interactive process, the actions of both the citizen and officer will influence the course of the interaction. A computer program based on a statistical procedure called lag sequential analysis was written, which could identify sequences of behaviour contained in the data. This program allowed the researcher to nominate a behaviour of interest (a 'criterion behaviour'), and then determine what other behaviours were likely to surround this criterion behaviour. For example, let's take a threat by an officer. The program then computed the behaviours which were more than likely by chance to occur immediately before the threat, or immediately after the threat. In this manner, behavioural sequences could be established which provided insight to research questions such as "what happened next?" or "what caused this to happen?". All interactions Before examining gender differences, it may be useful to consider briefly the patterns of behaviour which described the interactions generally. The results showed that some officer tactics lead to a positive sequence of behaviour which was likely to minimise resistance from the public. For example, if an officer followed questioning the citizen by supportive statements, this was associated with a heightened probability of further information exchange behaviour, during which the citizen cooperated in providing information. Once an officer made a statement in support for the citizen, the sequence of information exchange and further supportive statements was likely. INFORMATION ® SUPPORT ® INFORMATION EXCHANGE EXCHANGE "What did he look like?" "I wish everyone was "What else?" "Tall, long hair, wearing as observant as you" "Jeans, black shoes" a brown jumper" Other officer tactics resulted in increased probability of violence, by steering the interaction away from information exchange and towards more coercive behaviours. For example, when an officer made a controlling statement, the chance of information exchange behaviour was reduced, and behaviours such as further controlling statements and the citizen displaying both verbal and physical abuse, were more likely. A rejecting statement by the officer was also likely to reduce information exchange, and lead to a pattern of further rejecting statements and citizen defensive behaviour. Similarly, when the officer threatened a citizen this often lead to further threats being issued. This resulted in a heightened probability of verbal and physical abuse from the citizen. 7
  • 8. Sequences of behaviour were also initiated by citizen actions. For example, when a citizen refused to cooperate with the officer, it was more likely that verbal abuse and physical conflict would result. REFUSE ® VERBAL ABUSE ® PHYSICAL "There's no way I'm "Haven't you got any Officer grabs the citizen getting in the car"real work to do, you bastard?"and assists him into car Males versus females Analysis revealed that female officers used slightly different tactics when dealing with the public than their male counterparts of equivalent skill level. Probably the most significant difference was the ability of the females to get the interaction "back on track" after a behaviour which threatened to divert the interaction down a more coercive path. That is, females were more likely than males to follow behaviours such as threats and rejecting statements with questions. This resulted in the citizen answering the question, allowing further questions to be asked in this mutual exchange of information. Female Officers - Likely Scenario REJECT ® INFORMATIONEXCHANGE "Don't give me that rubbish."Try telling me again,"He pushed my girl friend He just told me he saw youwhat made you hit him?"against the wall" do it" Female officers were also more likely than males to persist with a series of supportive statements, interspersed during the exchange of information process. Males were also found to engage in this sequence of behaviour, but to a lesser extent than females. Once a female officer showed empathy for the citizen and made a statement supporting them, this 'set the tone' for future positive statements towards the citizen. Females used more rejecting statements than their male counterparts, and also were more likely to repeat rejecting statements. This could indicate that this was a preferred tactic for dealing with citizens. Alternatively, it may have resulted from members of the public having a tendency to distort the truth to females officers in an attempt to influence them. Perhaps offenders would 'try one on' with female officers, resulting in them getting caught out. Males were a lot more likely than females to repeat a threat, whereas females sometimes asked questions directly after the threat, as well as repeating the threat (refer to scenarios over the page). Male officers were more likely to elicit verbal abuse or defensive behaviour from the citizen after giving a threat. This could be due to male officer threats being in reality more threatening than females, or perceived as more threatening my members of the public. Either way, the result was an escalation of conflict. Male Officers - Likely Scenario THREAT ® THREAT ® VERBAL ABUSE "Look mate, if you don't "You'll be in the cells "Get lost. I'd like to start telling the truth, before you know it" see you try" I'll lock you up" Female officers - Likely Scenario 8
  • 9. THREAT ® INFORMATION EXCHANGE "If you refuse to answer, "So tell me again, "Must have been you will be arrested" what time did you go there? around 10 o'clock" More verbal abuse was directed towards male officers, and its probability of occurrence was increased when male officers rejected a citizen's account of events, threatened the citizen, or physically handled the citizen. In contrast, abusive language was likely to be directed towards the female officers in response to her attempts to control the situation by making a statement which directed their behaviour. Once verbal abuse began, the likelihood of further abusive statements was greatly increased. 4. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS In conclusion, this research project demonstrated that police-citizen encounters were able to be studied as an interactive process, and that police officers influenced the response they received from citizens through the behavioural tactics they used. Male officers were found to be more coercive than their female counterparts, preferring tactics such as threats, controlling statements, and physical behaviours. This approach lead to males experiencing higher levels of citizen resistance while on patrol. Female officers, in contrast, were more adept at steering an interaction away from conflict by directing questions at citizens, placing them in the position of information provider and hence exerting control over the interaction. Females were also more supportive of citizens than male officers, often giving positive feedback to the person they were conversing with. This in turn lead to the citizen cooperating with further questions, again resulting in empathy from the officer. Additionally, female officers preferred a rejecting tactic, which repudiated the citizen's account of events. Either the females used this tactic in preference to other coercive tactics, such as threats, or members of the public more often lied to female officers, causing them to reject their statements. These findings call into question the myth that female police officers are a risk to their male colleagues because they 'can't handle conflict'. It would appear from the data that females have well developed strategies for avoiding conflict, and elicit a more positive response from the people they speak with on patrol. 9