SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 17
Download to read offline
636
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 27
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE
Douglas B. Marlowe, David S. DeMatteo, Nicholas S. Patapis, and David S. Festinger
S
ubstance use is highly prevalent in forensic and correc-
tional populations. Approximately 70% to 85% of crimi-
naloffendersintheUnitedStatesmeetabroaddefinition
of substance involvement, meaning they were arrested for a
drug or alcohol-related offense, were intoxicated at the time of
their offense, reporting committing their offense to support a
drughabit,orhaveaserioushistoryofsubstanceabuse(Bureau
ofJusticeStatistics,1998,2001;NationalCenteronAddiction
& Substance Abuse [NCASA], 2010; Zhang, 2003). Rates of
diagnosable substance use disorders are lower but still substan-
tial. Approximately 65% of offenders meet diagnostic criteria
for substance abuse, and 40% to 45% meet criteria for sub-
stance dependence1
, also referred to as addiction2
(Fazel, Bains
& Doll, 2006; Karberg & James, 2005; NCASA, 2010; Zang,
2003).
A diagnosis of substance abuse indicates that the individ-
ual has repeatedly ingested illicit drugs or alcohol under cir-
cumstances that are harmful to him or herself or others
(American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000). A diagnosis
of substance dependence indicates the individual’s usage has
become compulsive and he or she is likely to have considerable
difficulty remaining abstinent without formal treatment
(APA, 2000). In other words, substance abuse is defined essen-
tially by the experience of repeated adverse consequences of
substance use, whereas substance dependence is defined by a
substantial inability to stop using the substance. The latter
symptoms of substance dependence may reflect a form of neu-
rological or neuro-chemical damage to the brain (Baler &
Volkow, 2006; Dackis & O’Brien, 2005; Goldstein, Craig,
Bechara, Garavan, Childress, Paulus, & Volkow, 2009).
The above figures do not simply reflect drug possession
offenses. In a nationally representative sample of U.S. booking
facilities, positive urine drug-screens were obtained from more
than 60% of the arrestees for most categories of offenses,
including approximately 50% of violent offenders, 50%–70%
of theft and property offenders, and 75% of drug dealers or
manufacturers (Zhang, 2003).
In the civil forensic context, substance use is similarly
estimated to be a substantial factor in approximately 40% of
1. A diagnosis of substance dependence preempts or encompasses a diagnosis of
substance abuse (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Therefore, offenders
diagnosed with substance dependence are often included within the reported
percentages of offenders diagnosed with substance abuse.
2. Some scholars use the terms addiction and dependence interchangeably,
whereas others reserve the term addiction for chronic or severe dependence on
non-legally prescribed substances.
traffic fatalities and 7% of motor vehicle crashes (National
Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2005), 60%–80% of
substantiated child abuse or neglect cases (e.g., Besinger,
Garland, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 1999; Dunn, Tarter,
Mezzich, Vanyukov, Kirisci, & Kirillova, 2002), 40%–75% of
professional malpractice actions (e.g., Elwork, 2007), and 40%
of emergency room visits (Substance Abuse and Mental
Health Services Administration, 2010).
Attorneys and forensic experts are frequently called upon
to consider what implications substance use might have for a
subject’s criminal or civil liability, and whether that individual
could be amenable to substance abuse treatment in lieu of a
more punitive or restrictive disposition. This chapter begins
by reviewing the circumstances under which evidence of acute
intoxication may serve to reduce legal culpability, and it
describes an approach to critically evaluating an intoxication
defense that considers the characteristics of the substance, of
the defendant, and of the offense. Subsequently, this chapter
addresses some of the thorny issues that are commonly con-
fronted when conducting substance abuse assessments in
forensic contexts and reviews the research literature on the
efficacy of substance abuse treatment interventions.
It is concluded from this review of the scientific literature
that drug or alcohol use rarely has the capacity to overtake
conscious intent to engage in criminal or tortious activities,
although it may increase the likelihood of intentional or reck-
less misconduct that was not deliberated or planned in
advance. In addition, the efficacy of traditional substance
abuse treatment interventions may have been substantially
overestimated for offender populations. The majority of
offenders abuse or misuse drugs or alcohol; however, lacking
an appreciable compulsion to their usage, less than half can be
characterized as dependent or addicted and thus in need of
formal substance abuse treatment services. Among those who
do require formal treatment, the base rate for success in tradi-
tional treatment programs has generally been low to modest.
Relatively few individuals remain in substance abuse treat-
ment long enough to receive a minimally adequate dosage of
services, and the effect size (ES) for many community-based
treatment programs is often too small to serve public safety or
public health objectives.
The programs that have shown reliable promise for reduc-
ing substance use and crime among offenders are those that
integrate community-based substance abuse treatment with
ongoing criminal justice supervision, and provide immediate
and consistent consequences for participants’ progress—or
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63627-Faust-Ch27.indd 636 5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 637
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 lack thereof—in treatment. This requires substantial ingenu-
ity on the part of forensic experts and attorneys to craft suit-
able dispositional plans. It is no longer defensible from a
scientific standpoint to simplistically attribute crime to the
effects of drugs or alcohol, or to refer subjects to treatment
without also clearly specifying how compliance with treat-
ment should be monitored and how infractions in treatment
should be addressed.
SUBSTANCE INTOXICATION
AND LIABILITY
In civil cases, voluntary intoxication may be considered to be
probative of liability. That is, it may be viewed as gross negli-
gence or recklessness to become voluntarily intoxicated (Mack
& Barros, 2008; e.g., Little Rock v. Cameron, 1995; Miles v.
General Motors Corp., 2001). The matter is more complicated,
however, in criminal cases where a few narrowly drawn ave-
nues may be available to reduce liability based on instances of
acute intoxication.
DIMINISHED CAPACITY
As of 1999, a national survey of U.S. states and territories
found that nearly three-quarters of jurisdictions permitted
evidence of intoxication to negate the mens rea (i.e., the requi-
site mental state) element of certain intentional offenses
(Marlowe, Lambert, & Thompson, 1999). Roughly one-third
of the jurisdictions permitted evidence of intoxication to
negate mens rea for any general intent crime, roughly 40% per-
mitted such evidence only to negate mens rea for specific
intent crimes, and a small proportion (about 5%) permitted
such evidence only to negate mens rea in first-degree murder
cases. General intent refers to any intention to perform a pro-
scribed act or closely related act, whereas specific intent
requires a purpose plus deliberation or premeditation (e.g.,
Kaplan & Weisberg, 1991). Where admissible, the effect of
intoxication evidence can be to reduce culpability to that of a
lesser included offense. For example, the specific-intent crime
of burglary might be reduced to a general-intent crime of
trespassing or breaking and entering.
Where such a defense is available, it is of little consequence
that the defendant intentionally elected to become intoxi-
cated. The controlling issue is whether the defendant had the
requisite intent to commit a further criminal act. Because this
may be viewed by some constituencies, such as victims’ rights
groups, as being too lenient on crime, a handful of states have
passed legislation barring evidence of voluntary intoxication
from forming the basis of a criminal responsibility defense. In
effect, these statutes transfer the intention to become intoxi-
cated to the intention to commit further crimes while the
offender is under the influence (e.g., Layton, 1997). The U. S.
SupremeCourtupheldonesuchstatuteinMontana(Montana
v. Egelhoff, 1996), which could open the door for more states
to prohibit evidence of voluntary intoxication from negating
mens rea in all criminal cases.
INSANITY DEFENSE
In most cases, voluntary intoxication cannot form the basis of
an insanity defense. Regardless of which insanity test a juris-
diction follows, the cognitive or volitional impairment must
be the product of a “mental disease or defect.” Mere intoxica-
tion, without more, does not satisfy the definition of a mental
disease or defect (e.g., LaFave & Scott, 1986; Melton, Petrila,
Poythress, & Slobogin, 2007).
A substantial minority of jurisdictions permit an insanity
defense to be predicated on substance abuse if chronic abuse
of drugs or alcohol resulted in a “settled insanity” (e.g., People
v. Free, 1983). As a result of long-term or serious usage, the
defendant must suffer from an independent psychiatric syn-
drome (e.g., substance-induced hallucinosis or dementia) that
predates and continues beyond the incident of intoxication
that is linked to the crime (e.g., People v. Skinner, 1985). The
traditional rule required this substance-induced insanity to be
permanent; however, some cases have held that a mental defect
may be sufficiently “fixed” to satisfy the permanence require-
ment if it is present for a substantial time, both before and
after the instance of intoxication in question, even if it eventu-
ally resolves (e.g., Porreca v. State, 1981).
An insanity defense may be easier to establish in cases of
involuntary intoxication or pathological intoxication; how-
ever, given the limited fact patterns that satisfy the criteria
for these defenses, they are infrequently raised successfully
in practice. Involuntary intoxication is intoxication that is
induced by coercion, by an innocent mistake of fact (i.e., by a
reasonable belief that the drug was a non-intoxicating lawful
substance), or by an unanticipated idiosyncratic reaction to a
prescribed medication. Pathological intoxication is intoxica-
tion that is grossly excessive in degree given the amount and
type of intoxicant that was ingested. The defendant must
lack actual knowledge about the potential effects of the sub-
stance on his or her behavior and must not have reason to
know about such effects. If the defendant had experienced
unusual reactions to the substance in the past, a defense of
involuntary intoxication or pathological intoxication would
ordinarily be unavailable. Courts are generally hesitant to
recognize a defense of pathological intoxication that was
induced by an unanticipated reaction to illicit drugs or alco-
hol, because these substances are widely known to have intoxi-
cating qualities (e.g., Commonwealth v. Henry, 1990; People v.
Matthews, 1985). An insanity defense is more likely to be
available in cases involving an idiosyncratic reaction to pre-
scription medication.
AUTOMATISM DEFENSE
In rare instances, extreme intoxication or extreme idiosyn-
cratic reactions to a substance can cause symptoms of delirium
or dissociation. Under such circumstances, the individual
might be considered to be, in essence, unconscious of his or
her actions. In these so-called automatism cases, the law may
view the individual as not having engaged in the actus reus
(requisite act) of the offense (e.g., Fulcher v. State, 1981).
Similar to the insanity defense, the automatism defense is
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63727-Faust-Ch27.indd 637 5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
638 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 unavailable if the defendant knew, or had reason to know,
about his or her hypersensitivity to the substance (e.g., Tift v.
State, 1916). The automatism defense is almost never raised
successfully in this country because of the rare fact patterns
that can justify its application; therefore, it has little practical
utility in typical forensic work (e.g., Melton et al., 2007).
Absent evidence of spasmodic or flailing motor activity, or of
a rare somnambulant fugue state, it is difficult to envision a
scenario that could support such a defense.
EVALUATING AN INTOXICATION DEFENSE
In any evaluation of criminal responsibility, it is difficult to
reconstruct a defendant’s mental state at an earlier point in
time. This may be particularly problematic in intoxication
cases, because the effects of drugs and alcohol typically wear
off in several hours, whereas the residual symptoms of psy-
chotic or affective disorders can last for days, weeks, or even
months. Researchers are often unable to measure the accuracy
of forensic evaluators’ conclusions because there is usually no
objectively measurable standard against which to validate
their findings. Typically, the only feasible approach is to con-
duct forensic substance abuse evaluations in accordance with
professionally accepted best practices (see Appendix A for a
list of best practices in forensic substance abuse evaluations),
and to include in those evaluations a consideration of the fac-
tors that are most likely to be credited by courts and juries.
These factors include the characteristics of the substance, the
characteristics of the defendant, and the characteristics of the
offense.
Characteristics of the Substance
Even severe clinical reactions to psychoactive substances
would not ordinarily be expected to interfere with an individ-
ual’s capacity to form a specific criminal intent. Acute intoxi-
cation has its most profound effects on impulse control,
executive functions (planning and sequencing of behavior),
and motor coordination (Baler & Volkow, 2006; Dackis &
O’Brien, 2005; Fishbein, 2000; Hoaken & Stewart, 2003).
Ordinarily, therefore, it could be expected to precipitate spon-
taneous behavior that is intended but not deliberated. For
example, an intoxicated person might be predisposed to lash
out aggressively against an actual or perceived insult. This
reaction would be intended, albeit sudden and ill conceived.
Consequently, intoxication evidence may be relevant to negate
specific intent, but it will infrequently make a compelling
argument for negating general intent.
Certain classes of psychoactive substances are known to
have disinhibiting and agitating effects on the central nervous
system (CNS), and they have been demonstrated to induce
aggression in controlled settings. Alcohol, in particular, has
been shown to trigger aggressive responses in the laboratory,
and alcohol is the drug most closely linked to violent crime
(De La Rosa, Lambert, & Gropper, 1990; Fagan, 1990; Mack
& Barros, 2008; Whitfield, 1990). Other substances that have
activating and disinhibiting effects on the CNS, such as
amphetamines, phencyclidine (PCP) and cocaine, can cause
agitation as well; however, research suggests that preexisting
personality factors may be as or more important than pharma-
cological factors in predicting whether the abuse of these
drugs will result in crime or violence (Hoaken & Stewart,
2003).
Although initial reports linked PCP to delirium, extreme
psychosis, and unprovoked bouts of violence, subsequent data
suggestedamore“sobering”analysisofitseffects(e.g.,Brecher,
Wang, Wong, & Morgan, 1988; Davis, 1982; Khajawall,
Erickson, & Simpson, 1982; Kinlock, 1991). Extreme reac-
tions to acute PCP ingestion do occur, but they are relatively
infrequent (Fauman & Fauman, 1982). Due, in part, to the
low purity of street-level PCP and the fact that many PCP
abusers titrate their doses over months or years, leading to sub-
stantial tolerance, acute psychotic reactions are rarer today
than they might have been when pharmaceutical-grade PCP
first became widely available in the 1970s.
Substances that have sedating and analgesic (pain reduc-
ing) properties are less likely to invoke violent or aggressive
criminal reactions during periods of acute intoxication. For
instance, when controlling for subjects’ prior criminal history,
no definitive link has been shown between aggressive criminal
behavior and intoxication on cannabis (marijuana) or opiates
such as heroin (e.g., Bennett et al., 2008; Goldstein, 1985;
Pedersen & Skardhamar, 2010). Indeed, opiate intoxication
may precipitate the “nods,” in which the individual has diffi-
culty remaining alert and perhaps even keeping his or her head
up. This would not ordinarily be conducive to intentionally
aggressive criminal conduct. To the extent that cannabis or
opiate abusers become involved in criminal activity (and many
do), this is usually attributable to their efforts to support their
drug habit, or to increased associations with other antisocial
individuals (Bennett et al., 2008; Farrington, 2010; Pedersen
& Skardhamar, 2010).
Sedatives such as barbiturates or benzodiazepines may
produce acute dysphoria, agitation, and paranoia in high
doses, particularly when they are taken over an extended binge
in which the subject may be deprived of sleep or food for a
long period of time. Ordinarily, however, these drugs cause
lethargy, euphoria, and psychomotor slowing, which tend
to be incompatible with violence or coordinated criminal
activity (e.g., Hoaken & Stewart, 2003).
Less is known about the effects of newer “designer
drugs” or “club drugs” such as “ecstasy” (3,4-methylene-
dioxymethamphetamine or MDMA), “GHB” (gamma-
hydroxybutyrate), “roofies” (flunitrazepam or Rohypnol), or
“vitamin K” (ketamine or Ketalar). Many of these drugs are
classified or partially classified as “dissociative anesthetics,”
with features similar to those of PCP (Kohrs & Durieux,
1998). The neurological effects of these drugs may include
confusion, delirium, euphoria, paranoia, and depression.
Research has not connected the use of these drugs to the com-
mission of intentional criminal or violent activity, although
more current research suggests that such a link may exist in the
case of ketamine (e.g., Sanders, Lankenau, Bloom, & Hathaz,
2009). Indeed, some studies have associated MDMA use with
a decrease in aggression among laboratory animals (Hoaken &
Stewart, 2003).
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63827-Faust-Ch27.indd 638 5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 639
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 The relationship between drug use and psychosis warrants
further discussion. A psychosis is defined as a break from real-
ity, and the clinical hallmarks of psychosis include hallucina-
tions (e.g., seeing or hearing things that other people cannot
see or hear) or delusions (i.e., persistent beliefs or ideas that do
not have a reasonable basis in fact). Psychosis can be defined in
terms of episodes (i.e., symptoms meet diagnostic criteria for a
psychotic episode) or symptoms (i.e., one or more psychotic
featuresthatdonotmeetdiagnosticcriteriaforafullpsychotic
episode).
Drug use is generally associated with psychosis in one of
three ways. First, some drugs, such as hallucinogens (e.g.,
LSD), specifically act to induce psychotic-type symptoms.
These symptoms typically include auditory or visual hallucina-
tions and delusional beliefs. However, unlike endogenous psy-
choses associated with certain psychiatric conditions, such as
schizophrenia, drug-induced psychoses are less likely to
include disorganized thinking and behavior. Second, some
drugs, such as stimulants, can cause psychotic symptoms as an
unintended side effect of their intended recreational effects
(i.e., promoting alertness or inducing euphoria). Finally, with-
drawal syndromes associated with some drugs may include
psychotic symptoms. For example, when sedatives, including
alcohol, are used in large quantities over prolonged periods of
time, the sudden discontinuation of use of those substances
can result in psychotic symptoms. The following sections pres-
ent a more detailed examination of the relationship between
certain substances of abuse and psychosis.
Alcohol
Alcohol is classified as a depressant because of its predom-
inant effect on brain functioning, but alcohol is well known
for causing a wide range of emotional and behavioral responses
across users. Typical responses to alcohol intoxication range
from sedation or euphoria to excitability, irritability, and
aggression. As with most drugs, an individual’s responses to
alcohol will vary depending on the amount ingested and the
context in which the substance is used. Large doses of alcohol
that exceed one’s tolerance lead to cognitive and motor dys-
function, and eventually to unconsciousness and possibly even
death. Alcohol is most likely to be implicated in cases of psy-
chosis during the withdrawal period after chronic use.
Although alcohol withdrawal is better known for its motor
symptoms (i.e., DTs, or delirium tremens), it may also include
transient visual and auditory hallucinations. There is no
official persisting psychotic state associated with alcohol use
(see Table 27.1); however, chronic alcohol abuse has been
associated with damage to the brain that can lead to functional
deficits and dementia.
Opioids
Synthetic opioids and naturally occurring opiates are
widely used throughout the world. Although the DSM-IV
indicates that opioid intoxication can lead to psychotic symp-
toms, particularly auditory hallucinations, the frequency of
such occurrences is infrequent. Much like benzodiazepines,
opioids exert their action at extremely localized points in the
CNS that are largely unassociated with psychotic symptoms.
Sedatives (Hypnotics/Anxiolytics)
Sedative drugs come in many classes and include anesthet-
ics, analgesics, hypnotics (sleep medications), and even anti-
histamines. This section will consider the most commonly
abused class of sedatives, the benzodiazepines (“benzos” or
“blues”), and will devote less attention to barbiturates because
of the low base rate of barbiturate abuse in recent decades.
Benzodiazepines are only available by prescription and are one
of the most commonly prescribed classes of drugs in this coun-
try. Brand names of popular benzodiazepines include Valium,
Xanax, and Ativan. In 2004, over 32 million prescriptions
were filled for just the leading benzodiazepine and its generic
equivalent.Medically,benzodiazepinesareprimarilyindicated
to reduce anxiety (anxiolytic), to induce sleep (hypnotic), and
as an adjunct to anesthesia during certain surgical procedures.
Aside from differences in their absorption rate, duration
of action, and dosage required to achieve therapeutic effects,
benzodiazepines have nearly identical pharmacodynamic pro-
files. None of the benzodiazepines carry a risk of causing psy-
chotic symptoms or episodes, although this fact does not
exclude such symptoms from co-occurring with their use. The
relatively minor risk of psychosis associated with benzodiaz-
epines occurs in cases when there is an abrupt cessation of the
drug in a physically dependent individual. However, more
common effects of abrupt termination include agitation, anxi-
ety, and seizures. As will be discussed, benzodiazepine intoxi-
cation is most likely to disrupt the formation of memories,
which may lead to “blackouts.”
The abuse of barbiturates (e.g., Seconal, Tuinal, Luminal)
is currently rare. These medications are no longer prescribed
very often due to their narrow therapeutic index (i.e., for con-
trol of brain seizures) and high risk of overdose, especially
when combined with alcohol. For the most part, the risk of
psychosisassociatedwithbarbituratesisduringthewithdrawal
period, and the barbiturate withdrawal syndrome is clinically
very similar to that of alcohol.
Stimulants
The stimulant drugs carry a substantially higher risk of
causingpsychoticsymptomsorepisodesthanmostotherdrugs
of abuse. Heavy cocaine use (including “crack”) frequently
inducesparanoidideationandhallucinations.Suchexperiences
Table 27.1 DSM-IV DRUG-INDUCED PSYCHOTIC
DISORDERS
DRUG INTOXICATION WITHDRAWAL
Alcohol + +
Amphetamines +
Cannabis +
Cocaine +
Hallucinogens + +
Opioids +
Sedative/hypnotics + +
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63927-Faust-Ch27.indd 639 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
640 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY
113
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 are even more common in intravenous cocaine users. Heavy
cocaine use may also be associated with a tactile psychotic
symptom known as formication, which is the feeling that bugs
are crawling under one’s skin. Psychotic symptoms can begin
during cocaine intoxication and persist for several hours or
days. Despite the ability of cocaine to cause acute psychotic
symptoms, it is not believed to precipitate persisting psychotic
symptoms in persons without a history of psychosis.
Amphetamines are potent CNS stimulants. Like cocaine,
amphetamines can be smoked or injected, commonly in the
formofmethamphetamine(“ice”or“crank”).However,unlike
cocaine, many commonly abused amphetamines are available
by prescription as pills (e.g., Ritalin, Dexedrine, Adderall),
which can be swallowed or crushed and then snorted nasally.
Like cocaine-induced psychosis, amphetamine-induced psy-
chosis typically consists of paranoid ideation and hallucina-
tions (with visual hallucinations being more common than
auditory hallucinations). Amphetamine-induced psychosis is
usually short lived, lasting no more than about 48–72 hours in
most cases.
Similar to the amphetamines, the designer amphetamines
(e.g., MDMA) have potent stimulant effects on the CNS.
However, unlike the other amphetamines, designer amphet-
amines have significant pharmacological differences and
intoxication profiles, sometimes including hallucinations and
perceptual disturbances. For this reason, they are discussed
below with the hallucinogens.
Hallucinogens
Some drugs predictably (as opposed to idiosyncratically)
induce psychotic states. In fact, the “traditional” hallucino-
gens, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), mescaline (peyote),
and psilocybin (“shrooms”), are used primarily for their visual
and auditory hallucinogenic effects. They can also produce
feelings of euphoria and synesthesia (i.e., a blurring of the
senses; for example, hearing colors or seeing sounds).
Cannabis is the most widely used hallucinogen and it can
have a range of effects on the CNS. Therefore, it may produce
a spectrum of reactions across users. Cannabis-induced psy-
chotic disorder, which is a diagnostic category in the DSM-IV,
can develop shortly after cannabis use and typically involves
persecutory delusions (National Institute on Drug Abuse,
2005a). Evidence suggests this relatively rare syndrome may be
the result of triggering a prodromal or sub-clinical psychotic
predisposition, especially in adolescent and young adult users
(Foti, Kotov, Guey, & Bromet, 2010; Patton, Coffey, Carlin,
Degenhardt, Lynskey & Hall, 2006). By contrast, hallucina-
tions associated with cannabis use are rare and typically result
when very high blood levels are reached or when the cannabis
has been infiltrated or contaminated with another substance
of abuse, such as PCP or cocaine. Cannabis-induced psychotic
disorder usually remits within 1 day, although instances have
been reported in which it persisted for several days before
remitting.
In addition to the acute effects of cannabis on sensation
and perception, cannabis has substantial effects on cognition
and motor skills similar to the sedative drugs. This is perhaps
why there is such a low reported incidence of cannabis-related
violence. It may also explain why cannabis is frequently impli-
cated in traffic accidents and incidents of driving under the
influence (DUI) (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2005b).
MDMA and other designer amphetamines can produce
perceptual and sensational effects similar to the traditional
hallucinogens. However, relatively little is known about these
drugs in comparison to either the amphetamines or older
hallucinogens. Nonetheless, they are believed to carry many
of the same liabilities of both the stimulants and hallucino-
gens, which can include both acute and persisting psychotic
symptoms.
As a class, hallucinogens are the drugs most closely associ-
ated with persisting psychotic symptoms. These symptoms,
which may include “flashbacks,” can outlast the known psy-
choactive period of the drugs and have been anecdotally
reported to occur years after the last reported use. This raises
the question of whether hallucinogens may be permanently
altering the chemical transmission between neurons or the
functions of neural pathways. Persisting psychotic symptoms
of hallucinogen use were first documented in the 1960s and
1970s when the use of LSD surged in popularity. The increas-
ing use of designer amphetamines, coupled with advances in
neuroimaging techniques, has brought the etiology of these
persisting symptoms to the forefront of drug abuse research.
For example, preliminary data suggest there is a possibility of
damage to the brain resulting from MDMA abuse that may
account for subsequent mood and perceptual disturbance
occurring after drug use has been discontinued.
Characteristics of the Defendant
For purposes of sentencing or disposition (which are discussed
later), it is often in the defendant’s best interest to establish
that he or she has a severe substance use history and meets
diagnostic criteria for a substance use disorder that requires
treatment. Paradoxically, however, such a history could make
it more difficult to establish a mens rea or insanity defense. On
the one hand, a serious substance abuse history could persuade
the fact finder that the defendant has a propensity for intoxi-
cation, and thus that he or she was likely to have been intoxi-
cated at the time of the offense. On the other hand, this could
also suggest that the defendant has a greater tolerance for the
substance, and thus has a greater ability to engage in deliberate
conduct despite having a high blood-level concentration of
the drug. It is also less likely that an experienced substance user
would be unaware of the psychoactive properties of a particu-
lar drug or would be unaware that he or she has a peculiar
sensitivity to that substance.
For this reason, “naïve” subjects who have a limited sub-
stance use history are often the best candidates for an intoxica-
tiondefense.Somereportssuggestthatanintoxicationdefense
is most likely to be successful for defendants who have no prior
treatment history, who have no prior arrests, and who ingested
unusually high doses of a drug they were unfamiliar with
(Senay & Wettstein, 1983).
Perhaps the best candidate for an intoxication defense is a
naïve subject who also presents with evidence of brain injury
or neuropsychological dysfunction. Idiosyncratic reactions to
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64027-Faust-Ch27.indd 640 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 641
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 drugs may be most likely to occur if there is a preexisting lesion
(damage) in brain regions controlling executive functions
(i.e., the prefrontal cortex) or affective regulation (i.e., the
limbic system) (e.g., Fishbein, 2000). Most psychoactive
substances exert their effects, at least in part, by precipitating
a surge of the neurotransmitter, dopamine, in these regions
of the brain (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2008). If there
is neurological immaturity or destruction of brain tissue in
these areas, a dopamine surge could have an unexpected and
intense impact on executive control and emotional regulation,
perhaps leading to spontaneous aggression or impulsive crimi-
nality. This process has been analogized to placing a match
near a keg of dynamite. Dopamine surges are also associated
with the onset of psychotic or schizophrenic-like symptoms,
which is why stimulant abuse has a tendency to cause psy-
chotic reactions. By contrast, no compelling argument has
been set forth that neuropsychological deficits in the areas of
language, attention, or memory (which may be quite common
among offenders) should have discernable effects on impulsive
criminality.
Characteristics of the Offense
Courts and juries are very likely to focus their attention on the
factual circumstances surrounding the offense. Commonly
mentioned factors in court decisions include (1) whether the
defendant had an apparent motive for the crime, (2) whether
the crime required a coordinated sequence of conduct over
time, and (3) whether the defendant engaged in coordinated
efforts to conceal the crime (e.g., Boettcher, 1987; LaFave &
Scott, 1986; Marlowe et al., 1999).
The most common motives for crime include the “Four
Rs”: robbery, rape, reputation, or revenge. If one or more of
these motives is present, it may be difficult to argue that there
was no intent to commit the offense. This is particularly true if
the criminal behavior was contemplated prior to the instance
of intoxication. For example, inner-city youths may ingest
drugs or alcohol prior to confronting rivals or committing a
burglary. In such instances, substance use may be part of a pre-
offense “ritual” designed to lower anxiety and foster group
cohesiveness. Rather than reducing the apparent intent for the
crime, this may actually increase the degree of premeditation
because steps are being taken to remove obstacles to the com-
mission of the offense (i.e., to remove fear or reticence).
If a motive for the offense is present, but the defendant did
not contemplate the offense prior to its commission, then this
might reduce the degree of culpability from that of a specific-
intent crime to one of a general-intent crime; rarely, however,
would it negate general intent. For example, assume a defen-
dant became intoxicated at a party, subsequently came upon a
rival, and assaulted the rival during a spontaneous exchange.
In this instance, the motive of revenge or reputation would be
present, but there would be no indication that the assault was
contemplated prior to its commission. In this situation, intox-
ication could be viewed as triggering a potentially explosive
situation. Although the defendant might still have had a gen-
eral intent to commit a simple assault, this could negate the
element of deliberation or premeditation necessary to prove a
more serious specific-intent crime, such as assault with intent
to inflict bodily harm.
It is also important to consider whether the crime was
spontaneous as opposed to planned, whether it was carried
out over a long or short period of time, and whether it involved
an ordered sequence of steps. For example, if a defendant stole
a car, drove it across town to pick up an accomplice, and then
used the car during a robbery, this would indicate planning
and sequencing over an extended period of time. If another
defendant picked up a brick from the road, threw it through a
store window, and grabbed an odd assortment of items, this
might indicate minimal planning and a rapid progression of
events. If both of these defendants had similar blood alcohol
concentrations (BACs) in the intoxicated range, the scenarios
might suggest that alcohol was not particularly influential to
the first defendant’s conduct but could have contributed sub-
stantially to the conduct of the second defendant.
Finally, it is conceivable that a defendant might commit a
crime while intoxicated, subsequently sober up, and then
engage in coordinated efforts to escape or to conceal the
offense. The longer the period of time between the offense and
the subsequent efforts at obfuscation, the more likely it is
that the defendant’s mental status may have cleared. If, for
example, the defendant took immediate pains to conceal
the crime, this could suggest that the defendant was not so
intoxicated as to be unable to engage in deliberate conduct
at the time of the offense (e.g., Commonwealth v. Gribble,
1997). On the other hand, if the defendant awakened 8 hours
later on the following morning before engaging in efforts to
cover the crime, this might suggest that he or she was too
intoxicated to take self-protective measures during or soon
after its commission.
SUBSTANCE-INDUCED AMNESIA
Many criminal defendants purport not to recall the circum-
stances surrounding an alleged offense, often attributing this
to substance-induced amnesia. Some reports have suggested
that between 25% and 65% of violent offenders claimed some
degree of amnesia for their crimes (Bradford & Smith, 1979;
Schacter, 1986). These defendants may be surprised to learn
that this is essentially irrelevant to their criminal culpability.
For an intoxication defense, the dispositive matter is whether
the defendant intended to commit the crime. There is no
requirement that the defendant also remember the crime.
Amnesia for the crime is also not dispositive of a defen-
dant’s competence to stand trial (e.g., State v. Kleypas, 2001).
Amnesia is typically treated as one factor to be considered in
making a competency determination. Courts will consider,
among other factors, the degree to which defense counsel can
reasonably reconstruct the events surrounding the crime from
extrinsic sources such as eyewitnesses or physical evidence
(e.g., Wilson v. United States, 1968).
It is possible for an individual to engage in goal-directed
activity and to appear normal to bystanders but still have no
memory for the events in question. However, barring an
unusual susceptibility to the drug, such “blackouts” have only
been definitively connected to relatively high blood-alcohol
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64127-Faust-Ch27.indd 641 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
642 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 levels (Acheson, Stein, & Swartzwelder, 1998; Goodwin,
1995; Mack & Barros, 2008; Ryan & Butters, 1983; Ryback,
1971; White, Signer, Kraus, & Swartzwelder, 2004), intrave-
nous administration of benzodiazepines (Kumar, Mac,
Gabrielli, & Goodwin, 1987), or a combination of sedatives
and alcohol (Morris & Estes, 1987). A particular benzodiaz-
epine, Triazolam (Halcion), has been anecdotally reported to
cause blackouts at therapeutic dosages (Rothschild, 1992).
No definitive data exist to support the occurrence of true
blackouts from opiates, cannabis, cocaine, PCP, or stimulants.
Although memory may be sketchy after abusing these drugs,
cued recall should be sufficient to assist the defendant to
remember salient events, particularly if those events were
paired with violence or with autonomic arousal.
Again, less is known about the effects of newer “designer
drugs.” Rohypnol and GHB have been associated with antero-
grade amnesia (an inability to form new memories) combined
with a loss of consciousness (Dyer, Roth, & Hyma, 2001;
Garrison & Mueller, 1998), which accounts for their reported
use as “date rape” drugs. Repeated laboratory administration
of MDMA has also been associated with cognitive deficits
and potentially permanent memory impairment (Broening,
Morford,Inman-Wood,Fukumura,&Vorhees,2001;Ricaurte,
McCann, Szabo, & Scheffel, 2000). These effects may be
greater with the concurrent ingestion of alcohol or sedatives
(Schwartz & Weaver, 1998). Although pharmacological evi-
dence and case law have yet to catch up with these newer
classes of drugs, there is no reason to believe legal standards
should apply differently to them. To the extent that these
drugs interfere with the formation or retrieval of new memo-
ries, it may become necessary to reconstruct the events of a
crime from extrinsic sources to move forward with a fair
trial. Similarly, to the extent they precipitate psychosis or
dissociation, their usage might support an insanity defense if
the symptoms were “fixed” over an extended time or if they
were ingested inadvertently or with a reasonable absence of
knowledge about their effects.
ASSESSMENT OF SUBSTANCE ABUSE
AND DEPENDENCE
There are literally hundreds of substance abuse assessment
instruments. The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and
Alcoholism (NIAAA) and the National Institute on Drug
Abuse (NIDA) have published compendia reviewing the psy-
chometric research on many of these instruments (Allen &
Wilson, 2003; Rounsaville, Tims, Horton, & Sowder, 1993),
although these are a bit outdated at this juncture. In addition,
more recent review articles have been published that address
specific topics in substance abuse assessment, including assess-
ment in criminal justice settings (Andrews, Bonta, &
Wormith,2006;Inciardi,1994;Peters,Greenbaum,Steinberg,
Carter, Ortiz, Fry, & Valle, 2000; Simpson & Knight, 2007),
co-occurringsubstanceabuseandpsychiatricdisorders(Carey,
2002), screening in clinical settings (Carroll & Rounsaville,
2002), screening in primary care settings (McPherson &
Hersch, 2000), and motivation for change (Carey, Purnine,
Maisto, & Carey, 1999). The reader is advised to consult these
resources for in-depth information on a particular instrument
relevant to a given case. In the limited space available, we
address some of the difficult issues that are commonly con-
fronted when conducting substance abuse assessments in
forensic or correctional contexts.
It is estimated that nearly 40% of criminal offenders who
are diverted into substance abuse treatment do not have a seri-
ous substance use disorder that requires formal treatment
(Kleiman et al., 2003). In some studies, nearly one half of mis-
demeanor drug court clients (Marlowe, Festinger, Lee, et al.,
2003; DeMatteo, Marlowe, Festinger, & Arabia, 2009),
one-third of felony drug court clients (Marlowe, Festinger, &
Lee, 2004), and two-thirds of drug-involved pretrial super-
visees (Lee et al., 2001) produced sub-threshold scores on a
structured clinical interview, similar to a community sample
of non-substance abusers. This suggests that many individuals
who may be experimenting with drugs, or who may be non-
drug-using dealers, are perhaps being diverted into these pro-
grams unnecessarily (DeMatteo, Marlowe, & Festinger, 2006).
There are several possible explanations for these false-
positive rates. (In this context, a false positive is the misidenti-
fication of an individual as being in need of substance abuse
treatment when the individual does not require formal treat-
ment.) First, many jurisdictions make these referrals simply on
the basis of whether the individual has a current drug charge,
which does not indicate whether he or she has a clinically sig-
nificant substance abuse problem (e.g., Marlowe, Patapis, &
DeMatteo, 2003). Second, many assessment instruments may
inflate the prevalence of substance use disorders either because
theyemployexcessivelybroaddiagnosticcriteria,orbecausethey
assume most addicts to be in “denial” or “pre-contemplation”
about their problem, and thus likely to be underreporting sub-
stance use. These instruments were not designed to detect over-
reporting or malingering of substance use, which might be of
greater concern in a forensic or correctional context.
DIAGNOSIS
Official diagnostic criteria for substance abuse and depen-
dence are worded so generically that they may fail to convey
clinically precise or meaningful information. Moreover, they
may blur the important distinction between substance depen-
dence and abuse. As was noted previously, the term substance
abuse conveys a repetitive pattern of substance ingestion under
dangerous or inappropriate circumstances, whereas substance
dependence conveys a pattern of compulsive use or addiction.
However, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation, 2000), a client can receive a diagnosis of substance
dependence without exhibiting the features that one would
ordinarily associate with addiction, such as withdrawal symp-
toms, compulsive use, or cravings. It is possible, for example, to
be diagnosed as drug dependent by virtue of spending a great
deal of time using drugs, developing a tolerance to the drugs,
and using more drugs than initially planned on multiple occa-
sions.Thesesymptomscanoccuratanytimeduringa12-month
period, with no requirement that all of the symptoms occur at
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64227-Faust-Ch27.indd 642 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 643
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 the same time, or that they last for an appreciable length
of time.
The failure of current diagnostic nomenclature to capture
the prototypical symptoms of dependence, or to discriminate
sufficiently between abuse and dependence, has led some
scholars to argue that the dependence diagnosis should be
reserved for individuals who suffer from severe withdrawal
symptoms when they refrain from using drugs or alcohol (e.g.,
Langenbucher et al., 2000), or who experience overwhelming
cravings or compulsions to use drugs or alcohol (e.g., O’Brien,
Childress, Ehrman, & Robbins, 1998). Anything short of that,
according to this argument, may constitute abuse or misuse,
which although dangerous and illegal may be undeserving of a
formal psychiatric diagnosis. In fact, early indications suggest
that the upcoming 5th edition of the DSM might drop the
substance abuse diagnosis altogether and instead differentiate
among degrees of severity of substance dependence (Knopf,
2010).
In practice, many criminal justice professionals may lack
the requisite time or skills to render an accurate diagnosis
using a DSM-IV-congruent structured interview. Many of
these practitioners employ brief screening instruments that
can purportedly identify substance dependence using no more
than a dozen to a few dozen self-report items. The content
domains of these instruments vary considerably depending
upon the philosophies of the test developers, and they are
often incongruent with theDSM. As a result, it may be unclear
what syndrome(s) they actually measure. As noted earlier, a
particular concern is that many of these instruments were
designed to detect substance dependence in individuals who
are in “denial.” Thus, they may be apt to increase sensitivity
(i.e., increase true positive rates for identifying addicts) at the
expense of decreasing specificity (i.e., misidentifying many
non-addicts as addicts).
For example, one of the most widely used screening instru-
mentsistheSubstanceAbuseSubtleScreeningInventory(SASSI;
Miller, Roberts, Brooks, & Lazowski, 1997). Most of the
SASSI scales contain items that do not inquire explicitly about
substance use, but have been found by the test developer to dis-
criminate empirically between groups with and without sub-
stance abuse problems. Although initial validation studies by
the test developer were impressive, with sensitivity and speci-
ficity exceeding 90% (Lazowski, Miller, Boye, & Miller, 1998),
subsequent studies have been less sanguine, with true-positive
ratesbelow70%(Pearson,2000),sensitivitybelow60%(Fuller,
Fishman, Taylor, & Wood, 1994; Svanum & McGrew, 1995),
and inadequate specificity (Peters et al., 2000). In one study
involving juvenile offenders, the SASSI misclassified nearly
two-thirds of nonusers as being substance dependent (Rogers,
Cashel, Johansen, Sewell, & Gonzalez, 1997). This suggests
that the SASSI might substantially overestimate the need for
substance abuse treatment among offenders.
MULTIDIMENSIONAL ASSESSMENT
INTERVIEWS
A number of semi-structured interviews have been developed
that provide a behavioral assessment of substance use severity,
and that can detect changes in substance use over time or as a
function of treatment. Many of these instruments are multi-
dimensional, meaning that they inquire about problems in
several life domains that are commonly affected by substance
abuse.
Perhaps the most widely used of these instruments is the
Addiction Severity Index, which is currently in its fifth edi-
tion3
(ASI-5; McLellan et al., 1992). The ASI-5 measures cur-
rent (past 30 days) and lifetime problem severity in the areas
of drug problems, alcohol problems, legal problems, medical
problems, family and social problems, employment problems,
and psychiatric problems. The ASI-5 is comprised predomi-
nantly of two types of items. One type of item inquires about
the frequency of specific events, such as the number of days
the subject used illicit drugs or committed illegal activities
during the preceding 30 days and during the individual’s
lifetime. The second type of item is on a Likert scale and
assesses how serious the subject believes his or her problems to
be in each domain, and how much the subject desires treat-
ment for those problems (from “not at all” to “extremely”).
Composite scores are calculated from a weighted combination
of both types of items and serve as global indicators of prob-
lem severity in each domain. The composite scores are based
exclusively on events occurring during the immediately pre-
ceding 30 days.
The ASI-5 and its predecessors are supported by a long
historyofrigorouspsychometricvalidationresearchpublished
in peer-reviewed scientific journals. Multiple examinations
of ASI composite scores and lifetime items have yielded
impressive evidence of interrater reliability (i.e., the extent to
which different raters agree on ASI scores), test–retest reli-
ability (i.e., the stability of ASI scores over time), concurrent
validity (i.e., the correlation of scores on the ASI with perfor-
mance on other measures at the same point in time), predic-
tive validity (i.e., the correlation of scores on the ASI with
performance on other measures at some point in the future),
and discriminative utility (i.e., the ability of the ASI to dis-
criminate between clinical and nonclinical samples) across
various groups of clients characterized by age, race, gender,
and primary drug of abuse (e.g., Alterman et al., 1998;
Cacciola, Koppenhaver, McKay, & Alterman, 1999; McLellan
et al., 1985, 1992; McLellan, Luborsky, O’Brien, & Woody,
1980).
There are several factors, however, that could be expected
to limit the utility of the ASI-5 in a forensic or correctional
context, and these factors exemplify many of the same prob-
lems that are presented by many substance abuse interviews.
First, the ASI-5 was not normed and does not yield standard-
ized scale scores. That is, it has not, to date, been administered
to a large, well-defined, and representative sample of partici-
pants that can be used as a point of reference against which to
compare the results of future test takers. Therefore, it is not
possible to make meaningful comparisons across scales. For
example, one cannot compare the severity of a client’s drug
problems with his or her alcohol problems. Clinical Factor
3. A 6th edition of the ASI is undergoing psychometric research and
development.
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64327-Faust-Ch27.indd 643 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
644 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY
115
114
113
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Scores (CFSs) have been devised for the ASI-5 which are on a
standardized t-score scale (mean = 50 and SD = 10)
(McDermott et al., 1996); however, they were only standard-
ized on a narrow sample (n = 500) of methadone maintenance
clients, raising questions about the stability and generalizabil-
ity of the scaling. Moreover, few community treatment pro-
viders may have the requisite software to compute the CFS
scoring algorithms.
Without standardized scaling, it is difficult to know
whether a particular client’s substance abuse problem should
be categorized as mild, moderate, or severe as compared with a
normative population. The most that can be inferred from the
composite scores is whether they decline over time, or whether
different groups have significantly higher or lower composite
scores within the same domain. Many treatment providers will
attempt to classify the severity of clients’ substance abuse
problems using what are called “interviewer severity ratings”
(ISRs) on the ASI-5, which are essentially derived from the
assessor’s own clinical judgment. The research evidence is clear
that the ISRs are unreliable, meaning that different raters may
cometodifferentconclusionsaboutthesameclient(Alterman,
Brown, Zaballero, & McKay, 1994). As a result, the test devel-
opers have advised against their use and have dropped them
from the upcoming sixth edition of the instrument. Thus, in a
forensic context, one would have great difficulty concluding
from the ASI-5 whether a client has a serious substance abuse
problem. The best that can be accomplished is to review
responses to specific items, such as how often the client used
drugs in the previous month, and then to make inferences of
severity from those frequency data.
Although the ASI-5 is commonly used by clinicians, it is
important to recognize that it was originally developed by
researchers to measure change during treatment–outcome
studies. The items were chosen predominantly on the basis of
their sensitivity to change over time or their ability to predict
long-term outcomes. As a result, the ASI-5 does not render
information related to clinical symptoms or diagnoses. There
is no way to reach a DSM-IV diagnosis from the ASI-5 or to
assess prototypical features of dependence, such as withdrawal
or cravings. Moreover, there is no information about how
much drugs or alcohol an individual consumed. Because sub-
jects’ reports were empirically determined to be unreliable in
terms of the amount of substances they consumed, the ASI-5
focuses instead on the frequency of use, measured in days. This
leaves many questions unanswered concerning the clinical
characteristics of a client’s substance abuse problem.
Finally, the ASI-5 is highly susceptible to the issue of “days
at risk.” Because the scales measure events occurring within a
30-day window, if a subject were in a restricted environment
such as jail or prison during that time, this could systematically
deflate the apparent severity of his or her problems. Clinicians
often attempt to compensate for this problem either by calcu-
lating the proportion of days at risk that the subject engaged in
various behaviors, or by inquiring about the 30 days immedi-
ately preceding the subject’s entry into a restrictive setting.
There are no published studies indicating whether this
approach is valid or reliable and how it might alter the assess-
ment results.
OVERREPORTING AND UNDERREPORTING
OF SUBSTANCE USE
Depending on the context, individuals may have reason either
to underreport or overreport their level of substance use. For
example, individuals on probation or parole might underre-
port or deny drug use out of an understandable concern that a
violation of the terms of their probation or parole may result
in incarceration. By contrast, it is conceivable that criminal
defendants seeking to establish an insanity defense might
overreport drug use on the belief that voluntary intoxication
could form the basis of such a defense. Although several stud-
ies have addressed the underreporting of drug use among
criminal and civil populations, we were unable to locate reli-
able research directly addressing the overreporting of drug use.
Therefore, we will limit our discussion to a review of research
regarding the underreporting of drug use.
Among criminal populations, research has typically found
that arrestees underreport their recent use of illicit drugs,
particularly cocaine (e.g., Hser, 1997; Lu, Taylor, & Riley,
2000; Mieczkowski, Barzelay, Gropper, & Wish, 1991;
Yacoubian, 2000). Harrison (1995) found that among arrest-
ees who tested positive for marijuana, cocaine, and opiates,
only half reported using any of those drugs within the past
3 days. Research also suggests that a high proportion of drunk-
driving offenders underreport their use of drugs besides alco-
hol (e.g., Lapham, C’de Baca, Chang, Hunt, & Berger, 2002).
Researchers have also found underreporting of drug use
among other high-risk, but non-arrestee, populations (e.g.,
Fendrich,Johnson,Sudman,Wislar,&Spiehler,1999;Morral,
McCaffrey, & Iguchi, 2000). Fendrich et al. (1999) collected
self-report survey data and hair specimens from a high-risk
sample of household respondents in Chicago, and results indi-
cated significant underreporting of cocaine and heroin use
during the past 30 days. In another study, Morral et al. (2000)
explored the accuracy of self-reported drug-use frequency
among 701 methadone maintenance clients and found that
use frequencies averaged 34% higher for opiates and 20%
higher for cocaine than were self-reported. In a study exam-
ining self-reported drug use and urine-testing data among
59 methadone maintenance patients, researchers found that
the subjects over- or underreported their use of cocaine and
heroin by an average of 15% (Ehrman & Robbins, 1994).
Finally, in the civil context, some commentators have sug-
gested that parents involved in child custody disputes may
routinely underreport their use of drugs and alcohol (e.g.,
Schleuderer & Campagna, 2004).
Taken together, the weight of the available evidence
appears to suggest that underreporting of drug use is a signifi-
cant concern among some offender and non-offender popula-
tions. This highlights the importance of not relying exclusively
on self-report data when conducting a substance abuse evalua-
tion or monitoring an individual’s abstinence. Best-practice
standards strongly underscore the importance of combining
self-report data with objective measures of drug use, such as
urine drug-screen results (Carver, 2004; Harrell & Kleiman,
2002; Messina, Wish, Nemes, & Wraight, 2000; Rosay,
Najaka, & Herz, 2000).
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64427-Faust-Ch27.indd 644 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 645
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 TREATMENT OF SUBSTANCE ABUSE
AND DEPENDENCE
As was noted earlier, attorneys and forensic experts are fre-
quently called upon to predict whether a subject would be
likely to succeed in substance abuse treatment in lieu of a more
punitive or restrictive disposition. As with any prediction, the
answer to this question will often depend, in large part, on the
base rate for success in the population. Unfortunately, success
in traditional substance abuse treatment programs can by no
means be confidently assumed.
ATTRITION FROM TREATMENT
Attrition from substance abuse treatment is unacceptably
high. Between approximately one-half and two-thirds of indi-
viduals who schedule an initial intake appointment for drug
abuse treatment fail to show for the first intake session
(Festinger, Lamb, Kirby, & Marlowe, 1996; Festinger, Lamb,
Kountz, Kirby, & Marlowe, 1995; Festinger, Lamb, Marlowe,
& Kirby, 2002). Of those who do attend an intake, between
40% and 80% drop out of treatment within 3 months (e.g.,
Gainey, Wells, Hawkins, & Catalano, 1993; Simpson, Joe, &
Brown, 1997; Stark, 1992) and between 80% and 90% drop
out within 12 months (e.g., Satel, 1999).
Comparablefiguresarereportedamongsubstance-abusing
criminal offenders. Only about 25% of drug-involved offend-
ers may be expected to complete a substance abuse treatment
regimen (UCLA, 2007). Between 40% and 70% of proba-
tioners and parolees drop out of treatment or attend irregu-
larly within 2–6 months (Nurco, Hanlon, & Kinlock, 1991;
Taxman, 1999a; Young, Usdane, & Torres, 1991).
EvidencefromthenationalDrugAbuseTreatmentOutcome
Study(DATOS)suggeststhat3monthsofdrugabusetreatment
may be a minimum threshold for detecting dose–response
effects for the interventions, and 6–12 months may be a thresh-
old for observing clinically meaningful reductions in substance
use (Simpson et al., 1997). After 12 months of drug abuse treat-
ment, clients have roughly a 50% probability of remaining con-
tinuously abstinent for an additional year following completion
oftreatment(McLellan,Lewis,O’Brien,&Kleber,2000).Given
that no more than about 25% of drug-abusing offenders remain
in treatment for 12 months, one might reasonably expect no
more than about 10% to 15% of the original intent-to-treat
cohort, on average, to achieve a sustained interval of sobriety.
TREATMENT EFFECTS
In community-based substance abuse treatment programs, the
magnitudes of the treatment effects are typically small to mod-
erate. A meta-analysis of 78 studies that compared standard
drug abuse treatment with a no-treatment or minimal-treat-
ment control condition reported small impacts on crime and
moderate impacts on drug use (Prendergast, Podus, Chang, &
Urada, 2002). (A meta-analysis is a quantitative method of
combining the results of several research studies so that infer-
ences can be drawn across studies.) Using a liberal definition
of“success”(i.e.,abetterthanaverageoutcomeforthesample),
the mean weighted ES for criminal activity was 0.13, equiva-
lent to about a 6 percentage-point increase in the percentage
of “successful” cases. (An ES is a way of expressing the size or
magnitude of the effects of a particular intervention). The
average weighted ES for drug use was 0.30, equivalent to about
a 15 percentage-point increase in the proportion of “success-
ful” cases. A meta-analysis of 361 controlled alcohol treatment
studies found that the most commonly administered treat-
ments in standard practice (i.e., psychoeducational group
counseling, milieu therapy, and relaxation training) yielded no
appreciable evidence of efficacy (Miller & Wilbourne, 2002).
There may be reason to anticipate lesser success within the
criminal justice system. Nationally, less than 10 percent of pro-
bationers and 20 percent of prison or jail inmates have access
to needed substance abuse treatment services on any given day
(Chandler, Fletcher, & Volkow, 2009; Friedmann, Taxman, &
Henderson, 2007; Taxman, Perdoni, & Harrison, 2007).
What little treatment is available is often not evidence-based,
lacking in a coherent focus or structure, and delivered by inad-
equately trained staff (Lutze & van Wormer, 2007; Taxman &
Bouffard, 2003). The most commonly administered “treat-
ment”incorrectionalsettingsisdrug-educationgroups,which
offer minimal benefits for addicted offenders (Pearson &
Lipton, 1999). Although psycho-education might be a useful
secondary-prevention strategy for non-addicted substance
abusers, it has almost no value for addicts.
Adding case management services to standard drug abuse
treatment for offenders has produced mixed but potentially
promising findings. Under the rubric of what was originally
named Treatment Alternatives to Street Crime (TASC)—
renamed Treatment Accountability for Safer Communities—
hundreds of case management agencies were founded across
the country to identify and refer drug-abusing offenders to a
range of needed treatment services, to monitor their progress
in treatment, and to report compliance information to appro-
priate criminal justice authorities. Early evaluations concluded
that TASC programs were effective at identifying substance
abuse problems among offendersandmaking appropriatetreat-
ment referrals (Weinman, 1990). Moreover, clients involved
with the criminal justice system tended to remain in treatment
significantly longer when they were under TASC supervision
(Hubbard, Collins, Rachal, & Cavanaugh, 1988). However, an
evaluation of five large and representative TASC programs
concludedthateffectsondruguseandcriminalrecidivismwere
mixed (Anglin, Longshore, & Turner, 1999). Drug use was sig-
nificantly lower for TASC clients in three of the five sites and
criminal activity was lower in two of the sites. Moreover, the
positive findings were small to moderate in magnitude. These
results suggest that TASC programs may be a critical compo-
nent of successful treatment for substance-involved offenders;
however, they require additional support and back-up from the
courts, probation or parole officers, and treatment agencies to
achieve significant and sustained positive effects.
QUALITY OF TREATMENT
The disappointing outcomes reviewed above should come as
no surprise to anyone who is familiar with the substance abuse
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64527-Faust-Ch27.indd 645 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
646 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY
114
113
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 treatment system in this country. A study of a nationally repre-
sentative sample of public and private substance abuse treat-
ment programs in the U.S. found that nearly 45% of the
programs were either closed or no longer providing substance
abuse services within a 16-month period (McLellan, Carise, &
Kleber, 2003). That same study found more than a 50% staff-
turnover rate among both the clinicians and administrators
within 12 months. With such gross instability in staffing, the
programs could not reasonably be expected to maintain the
skills level necessary to deliver effective services. Moreover,
given that between one-quarter and one-half of substance
abuse treatment providers, nationally, have no more than a
2-year college degree (McLellan et al., 2003; Taxman &
Bouffard, 2003), it is difficult to imagine how they could be
expected to master the complicated principles and techniques
that are necessary to treat chronic and intransigent conditions
such as crime and addiction.
One multisite observational study of counseling sessions
for drug-abusing or addicted offenders reported that only
about 60% of the sessions addressed clinically relevant mate-
rial, few counselors endorsed a cohesive or identifiable treat-
ment philosophy, there was no conceptual clarity to the
sessions, few evidence-based treatments were administered,
and the counselors failed to show for nearly 20% of the sched-
uled appointments (Taxman & Bouffard, 2003). This is con-
sistent with several earlier national studies finding that few
alcohol or drug abuse treatment programs provided evidence-
based treatments for their clients (Lamb, Greenlick, &
McCarty, 1998).
EFFECTIVE DISPOSITIONS
Given the relatively low base rate for success in standard cor-
rectional treatment programs, it should go without saying that
simply diverting offenders into treatment, without more, is
unlikely to reliably serve public-health or public-safety objec-
tives (Marlowe, 2002, 2003). This does not, however, justify a
wholesale abandonment of treatment objectives. Outcomes
from substance abuse treatment are actually better than those
derived from punitive correctional sentences, and they are
considerably less costly. For instance, the research evidence is
clear that imprisonment has virtually no impact on drug use or
crime. Over 95% of drug-abusing offenders return to drug use
within 3 years of release from prison, with roughly 85% relaps-
ing within only the first 6–12 months (Hanlon, Nurco,
Bateman, & O’Grady, 1998; Maddux & Desmond, 1981;
Martin, Butzin, Saum, & Inciardi, 1999; Nurco et al., 1991;
Pelissier, Jones, & Cadigan, 2007; Vaillant, 1973). Moreover,
within3yearsofreleasefromprison,approximatelytwo-thirds
of offenders are arrested for a new crime, and roughly one-half
are convicted of a new crime or reincarcerated for a technical
parole violation (Langan & Levin, 2002). Dozens of program
evaluations similarly revealed no impacts on drug use or crime
for so-called intermediate sanctions, such as boot camps, home
detention, or electronic monitoring (e.g., Gendreau, Goggins,
Cullen, & Andrews, 2000; Taxman, 1999b).
The programs that have produced the most consistent
evidence of success are those that blend the functions of the
criminal justice system and the substance abuse treatment
system (Marlowe, 2002, 2003). Substance abuse treatment
assumes a central role in these programs, rather than being
viewed as peripheral to punitive ends, and is provided in sub-
jects’ community of origin, where they can maintain family
and social contacts and can seek or continue in gainful educa-
tion or employment. Responsibility for ensuring subjects’
attendance in treatment and avoidance of drug use and crimi-
nal activity is not, however, delegated to treatment personnel
who may be unprepared or disinclined to deal with such mat-
ters, and who often have limited power to intervene. The crim-
inal justice system maintains substantial supervisory control
over offenders and has enhanced authority through plea agree-
ments and similar arrangements to respond rapidly and con-
sistently to infractions in the program.
Drug courts are one example of an integrated program
that has shown substantial promise for reducing drug use and
crime among pretrial defendants and probationers. Drug
courts are special criminal-court dockets that provide judi-
cially supervised substance abuse treatment in lieu of prosecu-
tion or incarceration. The core ingredients of a drug court
include regular status hearings before the judge in court,
random weekly urine drug screens, mandatory completion of
a prescribed regimen of treatment and case management ser-
vices, progressive negative sanctions for program infractions,
and positive rewards for program accomplishments.
Participants who satisfactorily complete the program may
have their criminal charges dropped, reduce their probation-
ary obligations, or receive a sentence of time served in the pro-
gram. Defendants are generally required to plead guilty or no
contest as a condition of entering a pre-adjudication drug
court; therefore, termination from the program ordinarily
results in a criminal conviction and sentencing to probation or
incarceration.
The evidence is clear that drug courts provide enhanced
supervision of drug-involved offenders and increase their
exposure to substance abuse treatment (Lindquist, Krebs,
Warner, & Lattimore, 2009). Reviews of dozens of program
evaluations concluded that an average of 60% of drug court
clients completed 1 year or more of treatment, and roughly
one-half graduated from the program (Belenko, 1998, 1999,
2001, 2002). This compares quite favorably to the usual reten-
tion rates in community-based drug abuse treatment pro-
grams, where, as was noted earlier, 75% to 90% of probationers
drop out or attend irregularly in less than 1 year.
Five meta-analyses conducted by independent scientific
teams have all concluded that adult drug courts significantly
reduced criminal recidivism (typically measured by re-arrest
rates) by an average of approximately 8 to 14 percentage points
(Aos, Miller, & Drake, 2006; Latimer, Morton-Bourgon, &
Chretien, 2006; Lowenkamp, Holsinger, & Latessa, 2005;
Shaffer, 2006; Wilson, Mitchell, & MacKenzie, 2006). In ran-
domized experimental studies, drug court participants exhib-
ited roughly a 15 percentage-point reduction in rearrest rates
at 2 years and 3 years post-admission compared to probation-
ers (Gottfredson, Kearley, Najaka, & Rocha, 2005;
Gottfredson, Najaka, & Kearley, 2003; Turner, Greenwood,
Fain, & Deschenes, 1999).
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64627-Faust-Ch27.indd 646 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 647
120
119
118
117
116
115
114
113
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 A number of dismantling studies have investigated the
critical ingredients of drug court programs. Dismantling stud-
ies use an experimental design to determine the independent
contributions of specific components of an intervention. A
series of controlled, parametric studies found that holding fre-
quent judicial status hearings improved outcomes for high-
risk drug court clients who had more severe drug-use histories
or a comorbid diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder
(Festinger, Marlowe, Lee, Kirby, Bovasso, & McLellan, 2002;
Marlowe, Festinger, & Lee, 2003, 2004; Marlowe, Festinger,
Dugosh, Lee, & Benasutti, 2007; Marlowe, Festinger, Lee,
Dugosh, & Benasutti, 2006). Two other experimental studies
found that imposing graduated sanctions for positive urine
drug screens and other infractions improved outcomes over
standardpretrialsupervisionorprobation(Harrell,Cavanagh,
& Roman, 1998; Hawken & Kleiman, 2009). Finally, at least
one study has suggested that outcomes might be improved for
the more incorrigible types of drug offenders when they can
earn tangible rewards for positive achievements in the pro-
gram (Marlowe, Festinger, Dugosh, Arabia, & Kirby, 2008).
The results of these dismantling studies indicate that the
positive impacts of drug courts cannot be attributed simply to
the effects of substance abuse treatment. The additional ele-
ments of judicial supervision, urine drug testing, and contin-
gent sanctions and rewards appear to be making significant
incremental contributions to outcomes (Marlowe, DeMatteo,
& Festinger, 2003). This suggests that forensic experts and
attorneys will need to be substantially creative when crafting
dispositional recommendations. It may not be sufficient to
simply recommend that a subject receive substance abuse
treatment, especially in light of the relatively modest base rates
for success in standard treatment programs. Particularly for
the more serious or recidivist offenders, a suitable disposition
plan will need to include provisions for ongoing monitoring
by criminal justice authorities, as well as for the immediate and
consistent imposition of sanctions and rewards contingent
upon the subject’s progress or lack of progress in treatment.
SUMMARY
In summary, the research evidence fails to support a simplistic
premise that substance use is primarily responsible for crimi-
nal activity in many cases, or that substance abuse treatment,
alone, is necessarily the most appropriate strategy for forestall-
ing future wrongdoing. Criminal and tortious activities are
frequently influenced as much by the circumstances surround-
ing a particular event, or by the personal characteristics of
the defendant, than by the pharmacological properties of a
given substance. Moreover, it appears that many assessment
instruments may overinflate the prevalence or severity of diag-
nosable substance use disorders among offenders, and there-
fore may overestimate treatment needs in this population.
Finally, difficult as it might be to accept, the research evidence
does not provide reliable or consistent support for the effec-
tiveness of traditional community-based substance abuse
treatment interventions for reducing substance abuse and
crime among offenders. Unless attorneys and forensic experts
develop dispositional plans that include careful procedures for
monitoring offenders’ compliance in treatment and respond-
ing consistently to infractions, the majority of offenders might
be expected to drop out of treatment prematurely or to fail to
achieve a sustained interval of sobriety.
REFERENCES
Acheson, S. K., Stein, R. M., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (1998). Impairment in
semantic and figural memory by acute ethanol: Age-dependent effects.
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 22, 1437–1442.
Allen, J. P., & Wilson, V. B. (Eds.) (2003). Assessing alcohol problems: A
guide for clinicians and researchers (2nd ed.) [NIH Pub. No. 03-3745].
Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse & Alcoholism,
U.S. Dept. of Health & Human Services.
Alterman, A. I., Brown, L. S., Zaballero, A., & McKay, J. R. (1994). The
interviewer severity ratings and composite scores of the ASI: A further
look. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 34, 201–209.
Alterman, A. I., McDermott, P. A., Cook, T. G., Metzger, D., Rutherford,
M. J., Cacciola, J. S., & Brown, L. S. (1998). New scales to assess change
in the Addiction Severity Index for the opioid, cocaine, and alcohol
dependent. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12, 233–246.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Wormith, J. S. (2006). The recent past and near
future of risk and/or need assessment. Crime & Delinquency, 52, 7–27.
Anglin, M. D., Longshore, D., & Turner, S. (1999). Treatment alternatives
to street crime: An evaluation of five programs. Criminal Justice and
Behavior, 26, 168–195.
Aos, S., Miller, M., & Drake, E. (2006). Evidence-based public policy options
to reduce future prison construction, criminal justice costs, and crime rates.
Olympia, WA: Washington State Institute for Public Policy.
Baler, R. D., & Volkow, N. D. (2006). Drug addiction: The neurobiology
of disrupted self-control. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 12, 559–566.
Belenko, S. (1998). Research on drug courts: A critical review. National
Drug Court Institute Review, 1, 1–42.
Belenko, S. (1999). Research on drug courts: A critical review 1999
update. National Drug Court Institute Review, 2(2), 1–58.
Belenko, S. (2001). Research on drug courts: A critical review 2001 update.
New York, NY: National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse at
Columbia University.
Belenko, S. (2002). Drug courts. In C. G. Leukefeld, F. Tims, & D. Farabee
(Eds.), Treatment of drug offenders: Policies and issues (pp. 301–318).
New York, NY: Springer.
Bennett, T., Holloway, K., & Farrington, D. (2008). The statistical associa-
tion between drug misuse and crime: A meta-analysis. Aggression &
Violent Behavior, 13, 107–118.
Besinger, B. A., Garland, A. F., Litrownik, A. J., & Landsverk, J. A. (1999).
Caregiver substance abuse among maltreated children placed in out-
of-home care. Child Welfare, 78, 221–239.
Boettcher, M. J. (1987). Voluntary intoxication: A defense to specific
intent crimes. University of Detroit Law Review, 65, 33–71.
Bradford, J. W., & Smith, S. M. (1979). Amnesia and homicide: The
Pedolacaseandastudyofthirtycases.BulletinoftheAmericanAcademy
of Psychiatry and the Law, 7, 219–231.
Brecher,M.,Wang,B.W.,Wong,H.,&Morgan,J.P.(1988).Phencyclidine
and violence: Clinical and legal issues. Journal of Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 8, 397–401.
Broening, H. W., Morford, L. L., Inman-Wood, S. L., Fukumura,
M., & Vorhees, C. V. (2001). 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine
(ecstasy)-induced learning and memory impairments depend on the
age of exposure during early development. Journal of Neuroscience, 21,
3228–3235.
Bureau of Justice Statistics. (1998). Substance abuse and treatment of adults
on probation, 1995 [NCJ No. 166611]. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Justice.
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64727-Faust-Ch27.indd 647 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
648 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY
138
137
136
135
134
133
132
131
130
129
128
127
126
125
124
123
122
121
120
119
118
117
116
115
114
113
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2001). Trends in state parole, 1990–2000.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Cacciola, J. S., Koppenhaver, J. M., McKay, J. R., & Alterman, A. I. (1999).
Test-retest reliability of the lifetime items on the Addiction Severity
Index. Psychological Assessment, 11, 86–93.
Carey, K. B. (2002). Clinically useful assessments: Substance abuse and
comorbid psychiatric disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40,
1345–1361.
Carey, K. B., Purnine, D. M., Maisto, S. A., & Carey, M. P. (1999).
Assessing readiness to change substance abuse: A critical review of
instruments. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 245–266.
Carroll, K. M., & Rounsaville, B. J. (2002). On beyond urine: Clinically
useful assessment instruments in the treatment of drug dependence.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1329–1344.
Carver, J. (2004). Drug testing: A necessary prerequisite for treatment and
for crime control. In P. Bean & T. Nemitz (Eds.), Drug treatment:
What works? (pp. 142–177). London: Routledge.
Chandler, R. K., Fletcher, B. W., & Volkow, N. D. (2009). Treating drug
abuse and addiction in the criminal justice system: Improving public
health and safety. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301,
183–190.
Dackis, C., & O’Brien, C. (2005). Neurobiology of addiction: Treatment
and public policy ramifications. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1431–1436.
Davis, B. L. (1982). The PCP epidemic: A critical appraisal. International
Journal of the Addictions, 17, 1137–1155.
De La Rosa, M., Lambert, E. Y., & Gropper, B. (1990). Exploring the
substance abuse-violence connection [NIDA Research Monograph
No. 103]. Rockville, MD: U.S. Deptartment of Health and Human
Services.
DeMatteo, D. S., Marlowe, D. B., & Festinger, D. S. (2006). Secondary
prevention services for clients who are low risk in drug court: A con-
ceptual model. Crime and Delinquency, 52, 114–134.
DeMatteo, D. S., Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., & Arabia, P. L. (2009).
Outcome trajectories in drug court: Do all participants have serious
drug problems? Criminal Justice & Behavior, 36, 354–368.
Dunn, M. G., Tarter, R. E., Mezzich, A. C., Vanyukov, M., Kirisci, L., &
Kirillova, G. (2002). Origins and consequences of child neglect in sub-
stance abuse families. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 1063–1090.
Dyer, J. E., Roth, B., & Hyma, B. A. (2001). Gamma-hydroxybutyrate
withdrawal syndrome. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 37, 147–153.
Ehrman,R.N.,&Robbins,S.J.(1994).Reliabilityandvalidityof6-month
timeline reports of cocaine and heroin use in a methadone population.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 843–850.
Elwork, A. (2007). Stress management for lawyers (3rd ed.). North Wales,
PA: Vorkell Group.
Fagan, J. (1990). Intoxication and aggression. In M. Tonry & J. Q. Wilson
(Eds.), Drugs and crime (pp. 241–320). Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Farrington,D.(2010).CommentaryonPedersenandSkardhamar(2010):
Does cannabis predict non-drug offending? Addiction, 105, 119–120.
Fauman, M. A., & Fauman, B. J. (1982). Phencyclidine abuse and crime:
A psychiatric perspective. Bulletin of the American Academy of
Psychiatry and the Law, 10, 171–176.
Fazel, S., Bains, P., & Doll, H. (2006). Substance abuse and dependence in
prisoners: A systematic review. Addiction, 101, 181–191.
Fendrich,M.,Johnson,T.P.Sudman,S.,Wislar,J.S.,&Spiehler,V.(1999).
Validity of drug use reporting in a high-risk community sample: A
comparison of cocaine and heroin survey reports with hair tests.
American Journal of Epidemiology, 149, 955–962.
Festinger, D. S., Lamb, R. J., Kirby, K. C., & Marlowe, D. B. (1996). The
accelerated intake: A method for increasing initial attendance to out-
patient cocaine treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,
387–389.
Festinger, D. S., Lamb, R. J., Kountz, M., Kirby, K. C., & Marlowe, D. B.
(1995). Pretreatment dropout as a function of treatment delay and
client variables. Addictive Behaviors, 20, 111–115.
Festinger, D. S., Lamb, R. J., Marlowe, D. B., & Kirby, K. C. (2002). From
telephone to office: Intake attendance as a function of appointment
delay. Addictive Behaviors, 27, 131–137.
Festinger, D. S., Marlowe, D. B., Lee, P. A., Kirby, K. C., Bovasso, G., &
McLellan, A. T. (2002). Status hearings in drug court: When more is
less and less is more. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 68, 151–157.
Fishbein, D. (2000). Neuropsychological function, drug abuse, and vio-
lence: A conceptual framework. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27,
139–159.
Foti, D. J., Kotov, R., Guey, L. T., & Bromet, E. J. (2010). Cannabis use and
the course of schizophrenia: 10-year follow-up after first hospitaliza-
tion. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 987–993.
Friedmann, P. D., Taxman, F. S., & Henderson, C. E. (2007). Evidence-
based treatment practices for drug-involved adults in the criminal jus-
tice system. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 32, 267–277.
Fuller, M. G., Fishman, E., Taylor, C. A., & Wood, R. B. (1994). Screening
patients with traumatic brain injuries for substance abuse. Journal of
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 6, 143–146.
Gainey, R. R., Wells, E. A., Hawkins, J. D., & Catalano, R. F. (1993).
Predicting treatment retention among cocaine users. International
Journal of the Addictions, 28, 487–505.
Garrison, G., & Mueller, P. (1998). Clinical features and outcomes after
unintentional gamma hydroxybutyrate (GHB) overdose [Abstract].
Journal of Toxicology: Clinical Toxicology, 36, 503–504.
Gendreau, P., Goggins, C., Cullen, F. T., & Andrews, D. A. (2000).
The effects of community sanctions and incarceration on recidivism.
Forum on Corrections Research, 12, 10–13.
Goldstein, P. J. (1985). The drugs-violence nexus: A tripartite conceptual
framework. Journal of Drug Issues, 15, 493–506.
Goldstein, R. Z., Craig, A. D., Bechara, A., Garavan, H., Childress, A. R.,
Paulus, M. P., & Volkow, N. D. (2009). The neurocircuitry of impaired
insight in drug addiction. Cell, 13, 372–380.
Goodwin, D. W. (1995). Alcohol amnesia. Addiction, 90, 315–317.
Gottfredson, D. C., Kearley, B. W., Najaka, S. S., & Rocha, C. M. (2005).
The Baltimore City Drug Treatment Court: 3-year outcome study.
Evaluation Review, 29, 42–64.
Gottfredson, D. C., Najaka, S. S., & Kearley, B. (2003). Effectiveness of
drug courts: Evidence from a randomized trial. Criminology and Public
Policy, 2, 171–196.
Hanlon, T. E., Nurco, D. N., Bateman, R. W., & O’Grady, K. E. (1998).
The response of drug abuser parolees to a combination of treatment
and intensive supervision. Prison Journal, 78, 31–44.
Harrell, A., Cavanagh, S., & Roman, J. (1998). Final report: Findings from
the evaluation of the D.C. superior court drug intervention program.
Washington, DC: The Urban Institute.
Harrell, A., & Kleiman, M. (2002). Drug testing in criminal justice set-
tings. In C. G. Leukefeld, F. Tims, & D. Farabee (Eds.), Treatment of
drug offenders (pp. 149–171). New York: Springer.
Harrison, L. D. (1995). The validity of self-reported data on drug use.
Journal of Drug Issues, 25, 91–111.
Hawken, A., & Kleiman, M. (2009). Managing drug involved probation-
ers with swift and certain sanctions: Evaluating Hawaii’s HOPE
[NCJRS no. 229023]. Washington DC: National Institute of Justice.
Available at http://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/229023.pdf.
Hoaken,P.N.S.,&Stewart,S.H.(2003).Drugsofabuseandtheelicitation
of human aggressive behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1533–1554.
Hser, Y. (1997). Self-reported drug use: Results of selected empirical
investigations of validity. NIDA Research Monograph, 167, 320–343.
Hubbard, R. L., Collins, J. J., Rachal, J. V., & Cavanaugh, E. R. (1988). The
criminal justice client in drug abuse treatment. In C. G. Leukefeld & F.
M. Tims (Eds.), Compulsory treatment of drug abuse: Research and
clinical practice [NIDA Research Monograph No. 86] (pp. 57–80).
Rockville, MD: NIDA.
Inciardi, J. A. (Ed.) (1994). Screening and assessment for alcohol and other
drug abuse among adults in the criminal justice system [Treatment
Improvement Protocol Series No. 7]. Rockville, MD: Center for
Substance Abuse Treatment, U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Kaplan, J., & Weisberg, R. (1991). Criminal law. Boston, MA: Little Brown.
Karberg, J. C., & James, D. J. (2005). Substance dependence, abuse, and
treatment of jail inmates, 2002 [NCJ 209588]. Washington, DC:
Bureau of Justice Statistics, U.S. Dept. of Justice.
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64827-Faust-Ch27.indd 648 5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 649
138
137
136
135
134
133
132
131
130
129
128
127
126
125
124
123
122
121
120
119
118
117
116
115
114
113
112
111
110
109
108
107
106
105
104
103
102
101
100
99
98
97
96
95
94
93
92
91
90
89
88
87
86
85
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
74
73
72
71
70
69
68
67
66
65
64
63
62
61
60
59
58
57
56
55
54
53
52
51
50
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Khajawall, A. M., Erickson, T. B., & Simpson, G. M. (1982). Chronic
phencyclidine abuse and physical assault. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 139, 1604–1606.
Kinlock, T. W. (1991). Does phencylidine (PCP) use increase violent
crime? Journal of Drug Issues, 21, 795–817.
Kleiman, M. A. R., Tran, T. H., Fishbein, P., Magula, M., Allen, W., &
Lacy, G. (2003). Opportunities and barriers in probation reform: A case
study of drug testing and sanctions. Berkeley, CA: University of
California, California Policy Research Center.
Knopf, A. (2010). Diagnostic uncertainty: Changes in the proposed
DSM-5 worry addiction counselors, research professionals. Addiction
Professional, 8(5), 8–13.
Kohrs, R., & Durieux M. E. (1998). Ketamine: Teaching an old drug new
tricks. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 87, 1186–1193.
Kumar, R., Mac, D. S., Gabrielli, W. F., & Goodwin, D. W. (1987).
Anxiolitics and memory: A comparison of lorazepam and alprazolam.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 158–160.
LaFave, W. R., & Scott, A. W. (1986). Substantive criminal law (2nd ed.).
St. Paul, MN: West Publishing Co.
Lamb, S., Greenlick, M. R., & McCarty, D. (Eds.). (1998). Bridging the
gap between practice and research: Forging partnerships with community-
based drug and alcohol treatment. Washington, DC: Institute of
Medicine, National Academy of Sciences.
Langan, P. A., & Levin, D. J. (2002). Recidivism of prisoners released in
1994. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, U.S. Department
of Justice.
Langenbucher, J., Martin, C. S., Labouvie, E., Sanjuan, P. M., Bavly, L., &
Pollock, N. K. (2000). Toward the DSM-V: The withdrawal-gate
model versus the DSM-IV in the diagnosis of alcohol abuse and depen-
dence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 799–809.
Lapham, S. C., C’de Baca, J., Chang, I., Hunt, W. C., & Berger,
L. R. (2002). Are drunk-driving offenders referred for screening
accurately reporting their drug use? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 66,
243–253.
Latimer, J., Morton-Bourgon, K., & Chretien, J. (2006). A meta-analytic
examination of drug treatment courts: Do they reduce recidivism?
Canada Dept. of Justice, Research & Statistics Division.
Layton, C. J. (1997). No more excuses: Closing the door on the voluntary
intoxication defense. John Marshall Law Review, 30, 535–565.
Lazowski, L. E., Miller, F. G., Boye, M. W., & Miller, G. A. (1998). Efficacy
of the Substance Abuse Subtle Screening Inventory-3 (SASSI-3) in
identifying substance dependence disorders in clinical settings. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 71, 114–128.
Lee, P. A., Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Cacciola, J. S., McNellis, J.,
Schepise, M. M., & McLellan, A. T. (2001). Did “Breaking the Cycle”
(BTC) clients receive appropriate services? [abstract]. Drug and
Alcohol Dependence, 63(Supp. 1), S89.
Lindquist, C. H., Krebs, C. P., Warner, T. D., & Lattimore, P. K. (2009).
An exploration of treatment and supervision intensity among drug
court and non-drug court participants. Journal of Offender Reha-
bilitation, 48, 167–193.
Lowenkamp, C. T., Holsinger, A. M., & Latessa, E. J. (2005). Are drug
courts effective? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Community
Corrections, Fall, 5–28.
Lu, N. T., Taylor, B. G., & Riley, K. J. (2000). The validity of adult arrestee
self-reports of crack cocaine. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol
Abuse, 27, 399–407.
Lutze, F. E., & van Wormer, J. G. (2007). The nexus between drug and
alcohol treatment program integrity and drug court effectiveness:
Policy recommendations for pursuing success. Criminal Justice Policy
Review, 18, 226–245.
Mack, A. H., & Barros, M. (2008). Forensic addiction psychiatry.
In M. Galanter & H. D. Kleber (Eds.), Textbook of substance abuse
treatment (pp. 689–700). Washington DC: American Psychiatric
Publishing.
Maddux, J. F., & Desmond, D. P. (1981). Careers of opiate users. New York,
NY: Praeger.
Marlowe, D. B. (2002). Effective strategies for intervening with drug abus-
ing offenders. Villanova Law Review, 47, 989–1025.
Marlowe, D. B. (2003). Integrating substance abuse treatment and crimi-
nal justice supervision. NIDA Science and Practice Perspectives, 2(1),
4–14.
Marlowe, D. B., DeMatteo, D. S., & Festinger, D. S. (2003). A sober
assessment of drug courts. Federal Sentencing Reporter, 16, 153–157.
Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Dugosh, K. L., Arabia, P. L., & Kirby, K.
C. (2008). An effectiveness trial of contingency management in a
felony pre-adjudication drug court. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 41, 565–577.
Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Dugosh, K. L., Lee, P. A., & Benasutti, K.
M. (2007). Adapting judicial supervision to the risk level of drug
offenders: Discharge and six-month outcomes from a prospective
matching study. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 88S, 4–13.
Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., & Lee, P. A. (2003). The role of judicial
status hearings in drug court. Offender Substance Abuse Report, 3, 33–46.
Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., & Lee, P. A. (2004). The judge is a key
component of drug court. Drug Court Review, 4, 1–34.
Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Lee, P. A., Dugosh, K. L., & Benasutti, K.
M. (2006). Matching judicial supervision to clients’ risk status in drug
court. Crime & Delinquency, 52, 52–76.
Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Lee, P. A., Schepise, M. M., Hazzard,
J. E. R., Merrill, J. C., Mulvaney, F. D., & McLellan, A. T. (2003). Are
judicial status hearings a key component of drug court? During-
treatment data from a randomized trial. Criminal Justice and Behavior,
30, 141–162.
Marlowe, D. B., Lambert, J. B., & Thompson, R. G. (1999). Voluntary
intoxication and criminal responsibility. Behavioral Sciences and the
Law, 17, 195–217.
Marlowe, D. B., Patapis, N. S., & DeMatteo, D. S. (2003). Amenability to
treatment of drug offenders. Federal Probation, 67, 40–46.
Martin, S. S., Butzin, C. A., Saum, S. A., & Inciardi, J. A. (1999). Three-
year outcomes of therapeutic community treatment for drug-involved
offenders in Delaware. Prison Journal, 79, 294–320.
McDermott, P. A., Alterman, A. I., Brown, L., Zaballero, A., Snider,
E. C., & McKay, J. R. (1996). Construct refinement and confirmation
for the Addiction Severity Index. Psychological Assessment, 8,
182–189.
McLellan A.T., Cacciola J., Kushner, H., Peters R., Smith I., & Pettinati,
H. (1992). The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index: Cautions,
additions and normative data. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 9,
461–480.
McLellan, A. T., Carise, D., & Kleber, H. D. (2003). Can the national
addiction treatment infrastructure support the public’s demand for
quality care? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 25, 117–121.
McLellan, A. T., Lewis, D. C., O’Brien, C. P., & Kleber, H. D. (2000).
Drug dependence, a chronic medical illness: Implications for treat-
ment, insurance, and outcomes evaluation. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 284, 1689–1695.
McLellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., Cacciola, J. C., Griffith, J., Evans, F., Barr,
H. L., & O’Brien, C. P. (1985). New data from the Addiction Severity
Index: Reliability and validity in three centers. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 173, 412–423.
McLellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., O’Brien, C. P., & Woody, G. E. (1980). An
improved diagnostic instrument for substance abuse clients: The
Addiction Severity Index. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 168,
26–33.
McPherson, T. L., & Hersch, R. K. (2000). Brief substance abuse screen-
inginstrumentsforprimarycaresettings:Areview. JournalofSubstance
Abuse Treatment, 18, 193–202.
Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (2007).
Psychological evaluations for the courts: A handbook for mental health
professionals and lawyers (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.
Messina, N. P., Wish, E. D., Nemes, S., & Wraight, B. (2000). Correlates
of underreporting of post-discharge cocaine use among therapeutic
community clients. Journal of Drug Issues, 30, 119–132.
Mieczkowski, T., Barzelay, D., Gropper, B., & Wish, E. D. (1991).
Concordance of three measures of cocaine use in an arrestee popula-
tion: Hair, urine, and self-report. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 23,
241–246.
27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64927-Faust-Ch27.indd 649 5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27
Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27
Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27

More Related Content

What's hot

Cap 5. socioeconomic issues in medicine
Cap 5.  socioeconomic issues in medicineCap 5.  socioeconomic issues in medicine
Cap 5. socioeconomic issues in medicinecharly-mau
 
'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...
'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...
'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...Julian Buchanan
 
Drugged drivingnadcp5.12
Drugged drivingnadcp5.12Drugged drivingnadcp5.12
Drugged drivingnadcp5.12CADCP
 
Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7
Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7
Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7satoriwatersfl
 
Psy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag M
Psy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag MPsy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag M
Psy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag Mzuschette
 
A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...
A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...
A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...Jim Bloyd, DrPH, MPH
 
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...Paul Coelho, MD
 
Ph 4 rannizzisi
Ph 4 rannizzisiPh 4 rannizzisi
Ph 4 rannizzisiOPUNITE
 
More young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcohol
More young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcoholMore young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcohol
More young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcoholΔρ. Γιώργος K. Κασάπης
 
Road Safety in the Era of Legal Cannabis
Road Safety in the Era of Legal CannabisRoad Safety in the Era of Legal Cannabis
Road Safety in the Era of Legal CannabisThomas Barakat
 
The National HIV Prevention Inventory Final
The National HIV Prevention Inventory FinalThe National HIV Prevention Inventory Final
The National HIV Prevention Inventory FinalNASTAD
 
Dr. Tom Frieden keynote
Dr. Tom Frieden keynoteDr. Tom Frieden keynote
Dr. Tom Frieden keynoteOPUNITE
 
Money and politics the opiate epidemic
Money and politics the opiate epidemicMoney and politics the opiate epidemic
Money and politics the opiate epidemicMrsunny4
 
Capstone power point notes
Capstone power point notesCapstone power point notes
Capstone power point notesHarold Sowards
 
Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on
Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on
Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on SAHIL781034
 
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...Paul Coelho, MD
 
Substance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, MichiganSubstance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, Michiganrecoveryrestart2
 
Medical billsin Ankasas
Medical billsin AnkasasMedical billsin Ankasas
Medical billsin AnkasasOkoth Morgan
 

What's hot (20)

Cap 5. socioeconomic issues in medicine
Cap 5.  socioeconomic issues in medicineCap 5.  socioeconomic issues in medicine
Cap 5. socioeconomic issues in medicine
 
'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...
'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...
'It's there own fault for taking drugs' Professorial Lecture Glyndwr Universi...
 
Drugged drivingnadcp5.12
Drugged drivingnadcp5.12Drugged drivingnadcp5.12
Drugged drivingnadcp5.12
 
Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7
Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7
Outlet density associated_harms_summary-3.10.2011_373894_7
 
Psy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag M
Psy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag MPsy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag M
Psy492 Communication Skills Zuschlag M
 
A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...
A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...
A 5-Year Retrospective Analysis of Legal Intervention Injuries and Mortality ...
 
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
Mortality quadrupled among opioid-driven hospitalizations notably within lowe...
 
Opioid Use Disorder
Opioid Use Disorder Opioid Use Disorder
Opioid Use Disorder
 
Ph 4 rannizzisi
Ph 4 rannizzisiPh 4 rannizzisi
Ph 4 rannizzisi
 
More young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcohol
More young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcoholMore young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcohol
More young people in Canada are visiting EDs because of drinking alcohol
 
Road Safety in the Era of Legal Cannabis
Road Safety in the Era of Legal CannabisRoad Safety in the Era of Legal Cannabis
Road Safety in the Era of Legal Cannabis
 
The National HIV Prevention Inventory Final
The National HIV Prevention Inventory FinalThe National HIV Prevention Inventory Final
The National HIV Prevention Inventory Final
 
Dr. Tom Frieden keynote
Dr. Tom Frieden keynoteDr. Tom Frieden keynote
Dr. Tom Frieden keynote
 
Mental Illness and Policing
Mental Illness and PolicingMental Illness and Policing
Mental Illness and Policing
 
Money and politics the opiate epidemic
Money and politics the opiate epidemicMoney and politics the opiate epidemic
Money and politics the opiate epidemic
 
Capstone power point notes
Capstone power point notesCapstone power point notes
Capstone power point notes
 
Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on
Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on
Point counterpoint theodore joyce,editorperspectives on
 
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
Correlation of opioid mortality with prescriptions and social determinants -a...
 
Substance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, MichiganSubstance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, Michigan
 
Medical billsin Ankasas
Medical billsin AnkasasMedical billsin Ankasas
Medical billsin Ankasas
 

Similar to Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27

Effective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing Offenders
Effective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing OffendersEffective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing Offenders
Effective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing Offenderslakatos
 
Green pt1 state-pmp
Green pt1 state-pmpGreen pt1 state-pmp
Green pt1 state-pmpWelcome40
 
Paskash Research Paper
Paskash Research PaperPaskash Research Paper
Paskash Research PaperLucas Paskash
 
Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013
Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013
Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013Karen O'Neal
 
Final Draft Article for Publication
Final Draft Article for Publication Final Draft Article for Publication
Final Draft Article for Publication Tiffany Conelly
 
Medication-Assisted Treatment
Medication-Assisted TreatmentMedication-Assisted Treatment
Medication-Assisted TreatmentBenjamin Wilbur
 
Running head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docx
Running head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docxRunning head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docx
Running head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docxtodd271
 
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docxDrugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docxjacksnathalie
 
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docxDrugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docxkanepbyrne80830
 
STA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docx
STA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docxSTA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docx
STA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docxsusanschei
 
Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...
Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...
Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...Joyce Fuller
 
Differential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce Fuller
Differential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce FullerDifferential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce Fuller
Differential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce FullerJoyce Fuller
 
I. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docx
I. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docxI. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docx
I. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docxsheronlewthwaite
 
Take a Stand
Take a StandTake a Stand
Take a StandSarah M
 
Public Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docx
Public Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docxPublic Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docx
Public Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docxwoodruffeloisa
 
FINAL RESEARCH PROPOSAL
FINAL RESEARCH PROPOSALFINAL RESEARCH PROPOSAL
FINAL RESEARCH PROPOSALErica Veno
 
The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.
The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.
The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.Paul Coelho, MD
 
Sandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on Drugs
Sandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on DrugsSandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on Drugs
Sandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on DrugsCSSDPUofT
 

Similar to Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27 (20)

Effective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing Offenders
Effective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing OffendersEffective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing Offenders
Effective Strategies For Intervening With Drug Abusing Offenders
 
Green pt1 state-pmp
Green pt1 state-pmpGreen pt1 state-pmp
Green pt1 state-pmp
 
Paskash Research Paper
Paskash Research PaperPaskash Research Paper
Paskash Research Paper
 
Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013
Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013
Drug Abuse in Juveniles-2013
 
Final Draft Article for Publication
Final Draft Article for Publication Final Draft Article for Publication
Final Draft Article for Publication
 
Medication-Assisted Treatment
Medication-Assisted TreatmentMedication-Assisted Treatment
Medication-Assisted Treatment
 
Running head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docx
Running head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docxRunning head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docx
Running head DRUG TRAFFICKING 1DRUG TRAFFICKING 2.docx
 
THESIS FOR SOC 400
THESIS FOR SOC 400THESIS FOR SOC 400
THESIS FOR SOC 400
 
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docxDrugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
 
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docxDrugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
Drugs and Crime5chapter After you have com-pleted this chapt.docx
 
STA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docx
STA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docxSTA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docx
STA 544Homework 1Work on the following problem set and show yo.docx
 
Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...
Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...
Masters thesis differential effectiveness of substance abuse treatment by j f...
 
Differential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce Fuller
Differential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce FullerDifferential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce Fuller
Differential Effectiveness of Substance Abuse Treatment by Joyce Fuller
 
I. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docx
I. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docxI. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docx
I. Drug courts that are ran effectively, can curb drug use and d.docx
 
GrazioliF_YPG_Brief
GrazioliF_YPG_BriefGrazioliF_YPG_Brief
GrazioliF_YPG_Brief
 
Take a Stand
Take a StandTake a Stand
Take a Stand
 
Public Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docx
Public Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docxPublic Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docx
Public Acceptance for Drug Treatment in Lieu of Incarceration fo.docx
 
FINAL RESEARCH PROPOSAL
FINAL RESEARCH PROPOSALFINAL RESEARCH PROPOSAL
FINAL RESEARCH PROPOSAL
 
The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.
The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.
The economic burden of prescription opioid overdose... 2013.
 
Sandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on Drugs
Sandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on DrugsSandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on Drugs
Sandra Ka Hon Chu - Panel - Alternatives to the War on Drugs
 

More from Nicholas Patapis

More from Nicholas Patapis (6)

Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27
Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27
Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27
 
EconomicBenefits_of_Drug_Trx_02.05_
EconomicBenefits_of_Drug_Trx_02.05_EconomicBenefits_of_Drug_Trx_02.05_
EconomicBenefits_of_Drug_Trx_02.05_
 
2966497
29664972966497
2966497
 
dcr_vi._ii_summer_2009_1
dcr_vi._ii_summer_2009_1dcr_vi._ii_summer_2009_1
dcr_vi._ii_summer_2009_1
 
2003augfp
2003augfp2003augfp
2003augfp
 
PCA_cvNSP0915
PCA_cvNSP0915PCA_cvNSP0915
PCA_cvNSP0915
 

Ziskin & Faust Chapter 27

  • 1. 636 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 27 SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE Douglas B. Marlowe, David S. DeMatteo, Nicholas S. Patapis, and David S. Festinger S ubstance use is highly prevalent in forensic and correc- tional populations. Approximately 70% to 85% of crimi- naloffendersintheUnitedStatesmeetabroaddefinition of substance involvement, meaning they were arrested for a drug or alcohol-related offense, were intoxicated at the time of their offense, reporting committing their offense to support a drughabit,orhaveaserioushistoryofsubstanceabuse(Bureau ofJusticeStatistics,1998,2001;NationalCenteronAddiction & Substance Abuse [NCASA], 2010; Zhang, 2003). Rates of diagnosable substance use disorders are lower but still substan- tial. Approximately 65% of offenders meet diagnostic criteria for substance abuse, and 40% to 45% meet criteria for sub- stance dependence1 , also referred to as addiction2 (Fazel, Bains & Doll, 2006; Karberg & James, 2005; NCASA, 2010; Zang, 2003). A diagnosis of substance abuse indicates that the individ- ual has repeatedly ingested illicit drugs or alcohol under cir- cumstances that are harmful to him or herself or others (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000). A diagnosis of substance dependence indicates the individual’s usage has become compulsive and he or she is likely to have considerable difficulty remaining abstinent without formal treatment (APA, 2000). In other words, substance abuse is defined essen- tially by the experience of repeated adverse consequences of substance use, whereas substance dependence is defined by a substantial inability to stop using the substance. The latter symptoms of substance dependence may reflect a form of neu- rological or neuro-chemical damage to the brain (Baler & Volkow, 2006; Dackis & O’Brien, 2005; Goldstein, Craig, Bechara, Garavan, Childress, Paulus, & Volkow, 2009). The above figures do not simply reflect drug possession offenses. In a nationally representative sample of U.S. booking facilities, positive urine drug-screens were obtained from more than 60% of the arrestees for most categories of offenses, including approximately 50% of violent offenders, 50%–70% of theft and property offenders, and 75% of drug dealers or manufacturers (Zhang, 2003). In the civil forensic context, substance use is similarly estimated to be a substantial factor in approximately 40% of 1. A diagnosis of substance dependence preempts or encompasses a diagnosis of substance abuse (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Therefore, offenders diagnosed with substance dependence are often included within the reported percentages of offenders diagnosed with substance abuse. 2. Some scholars use the terms addiction and dependence interchangeably, whereas others reserve the term addiction for chronic or severe dependence on non-legally prescribed substances. traffic fatalities and 7% of motor vehicle crashes (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2005), 60%–80% of substantiated child abuse or neglect cases (e.g., Besinger, Garland, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 1999; Dunn, Tarter, Mezzich, Vanyukov, Kirisci, & Kirillova, 2002), 40%–75% of professional malpractice actions (e.g., Elwork, 2007), and 40% of emergency room visits (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2010). Attorneys and forensic experts are frequently called upon to consider what implications substance use might have for a subject’s criminal or civil liability, and whether that individual could be amenable to substance abuse treatment in lieu of a more punitive or restrictive disposition. This chapter begins by reviewing the circumstances under which evidence of acute intoxication may serve to reduce legal culpability, and it describes an approach to critically evaluating an intoxication defense that considers the characteristics of the substance, of the defendant, and of the offense. Subsequently, this chapter addresses some of the thorny issues that are commonly con- fronted when conducting substance abuse assessments in forensic contexts and reviews the research literature on the efficacy of substance abuse treatment interventions. It is concluded from this review of the scientific literature that drug or alcohol use rarely has the capacity to overtake conscious intent to engage in criminal or tortious activities, although it may increase the likelihood of intentional or reck- less misconduct that was not deliberated or planned in advance. In addition, the efficacy of traditional substance abuse treatment interventions may have been substantially overestimated for offender populations. The majority of offenders abuse or misuse drugs or alcohol; however, lacking an appreciable compulsion to their usage, less than half can be characterized as dependent or addicted and thus in need of formal substance abuse treatment services. Among those who do require formal treatment, the base rate for success in tradi- tional treatment programs has generally been low to modest. Relatively few individuals remain in substance abuse treat- ment long enough to receive a minimally adequate dosage of services, and the effect size (ES) for many community-based treatment programs is often too small to serve public safety or public health objectives. The programs that have shown reliable promise for reduc- ing substance use and crime among offenders are those that integrate community-based substance abuse treatment with ongoing criminal justice supervision, and provide immediate and consistent consequences for participants’ progress—or 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63627-Faust-Ch27.indd 636 5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 2. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 637 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 lack thereof—in treatment. This requires substantial ingenu- ity on the part of forensic experts and attorneys to craft suit- able dispositional plans. It is no longer defensible from a scientific standpoint to simplistically attribute crime to the effects of drugs or alcohol, or to refer subjects to treatment without also clearly specifying how compliance with treat- ment should be monitored and how infractions in treatment should be addressed. SUBSTANCE INTOXICATION AND LIABILITY In civil cases, voluntary intoxication may be considered to be probative of liability. That is, it may be viewed as gross negli- gence or recklessness to become voluntarily intoxicated (Mack & Barros, 2008; e.g., Little Rock v. Cameron, 1995; Miles v. General Motors Corp., 2001). The matter is more complicated, however, in criminal cases where a few narrowly drawn ave- nues may be available to reduce liability based on instances of acute intoxication. DIMINISHED CAPACITY As of 1999, a national survey of U.S. states and territories found that nearly three-quarters of jurisdictions permitted evidence of intoxication to negate the mens rea (i.e., the requi- site mental state) element of certain intentional offenses (Marlowe, Lambert, & Thompson, 1999). Roughly one-third of the jurisdictions permitted evidence of intoxication to negate mens rea for any general intent crime, roughly 40% per- mitted such evidence only to negate mens rea for specific intent crimes, and a small proportion (about 5%) permitted such evidence only to negate mens rea in first-degree murder cases. General intent refers to any intention to perform a pro- scribed act or closely related act, whereas specific intent requires a purpose plus deliberation or premeditation (e.g., Kaplan & Weisberg, 1991). Where admissible, the effect of intoxication evidence can be to reduce culpability to that of a lesser included offense. For example, the specific-intent crime of burglary might be reduced to a general-intent crime of trespassing or breaking and entering. Where such a defense is available, it is of little consequence that the defendant intentionally elected to become intoxi- cated. The controlling issue is whether the defendant had the requisite intent to commit a further criminal act. Because this may be viewed by some constituencies, such as victims’ rights groups, as being too lenient on crime, a handful of states have passed legislation barring evidence of voluntary intoxication from forming the basis of a criminal responsibility defense. In effect, these statutes transfer the intention to become intoxi- cated to the intention to commit further crimes while the offender is under the influence (e.g., Layton, 1997). The U. S. SupremeCourtupheldonesuchstatuteinMontana(Montana v. Egelhoff, 1996), which could open the door for more states to prohibit evidence of voluntary intoxication from negating mens rea in all criminal cases. INSANITY DEFENSE In most cases, voluntary intoxication cannot form the basis of an insanity defense. Regardless of which insanity test a juris- diction follows, the cognitive or volitional impairment must be the product of a “mental disease or defect.” Mere intoxica- tion, without more, does not satisfy the definition of a mental disease or defect (e.g., LaFave & Scott, 1986; Melton, Petrila, Poythress, & Slobogin, 2007). A substantial minority of jurisdictions permit an insanity defense to be predicated on substance abuse if chronic abuse of drugs or alcohol resulted in a “settled insanity” (e.g., People v. Free, 1983). As a result of long-term or serious usage, the defendant must suffer from an independent psychiatric syn- drome (e.g., substance-induced hallucinosis or dementia) that predates and continues beyond the incident of intoxication that is linked to the crime (e.g., People v. Skinner, 1985). The traditional rule required this substance-induced insanity to be permanent; however, some cases have held that a mental defect may be sufficiently “fixed” to satisfy the permanence require- ment if it is present for a substantial time, both before and after the instance of intoxication in question, even if it eventu- ally resolves (e.g., Porreca v. State, 1981). An insanity defense may be easier to establish in cases of involuntary intoxication or pathological intoxication; how- ever, given the limited fact patterns that satisfy the criteria for these defenses, they are infrequently raised successfully in practice. Involuntary intoxication is intoxication that is induced by coercion, by an innocent mistake of fact (i.e., by a reasonable belief that the drug was a non-intoxicating lawful substance), or by an unanticipated idiosyncratic reaction to a prescribed medication. Pathological intoxication is intoxica- tion that is grossly excessive in degree given the amount and type of intoxicant that was ingested. The defendant must lack actual knowledge about the potential effects of the sub- stance on his or her behavior and must not have reason to know about such effects. If the defendant had experienced unusual reactions to the substance in the past, a defense of involuntary intoxication or pathological intoxication would ordinarily be unavailable. Courts are generally hesitant to recognize a defense of pathological intoxication that was induced by an unanticipated reaction to illicit drugs or alco- hol, because these substances are widely known to have intoxi- cating qualities (e.g., Commonwealth v. Henry, 1990; People v. Matthews, 1985). An insanity defense is more likely to be available in cases involving an idiosyncratic reaction to pre- scription medication. AUTOMATISM DEFENSE In rare instances, extreme intoxication or extreme idiosyn- cratic reactions to a substance can cause symptoms of delirium or dissociation. Under such circumstances, the individual might be considered to be, in essence, unconscious of his or her actions. In these so-called automatism cases, the law may view the individual as not having engaged in the actus reus (requisite act) of the offense (e.g., Fulcher v. State, 1981). Similar to the insanity defense, the automatism defense is 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63727-Faust-Ch27.indd 637 5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 3. 638 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 unavailable if the defendant knew, or had reason to know, about his or her hypersensitivity to the substance (e.g., Tift v. State, 1916). The automatism defense is almost never raised successfully in this country because of the rare fact patterns that can justify its application; therefore, it has little practical utility in typical forensic work (e.g., Melton et al., 2007). Absent evidence of spasmodic or flailing motor activity, or of a rare somnambulant fugue state, it is difficult to envision a scenario that could support such a defense. EVALUATING AN INTOXICATION DEFENSE In any evaluation of criminal responsibility, it is difficult to reconstruct a defendant’s mental state at an earlier point in time. This may be particularly problematic in intoxication cases, because the effects of drugs and alcohol typically wear off in several hours, whereas the residual symptoms of psy- chotic or affective disorders can last for days, weeks, or even months. Researchers are often unable to measure the accuracy of forensic evaluators’ conclusions because there is usually no objectively measurable standard against which to validate their findings. Typically, the only feasible approach is to con- duct forensic substance abuse evaluations in accordance with professionally accepted best practices (see Appendix A for a list of best practices in forensic substance abuse evaluations), and to include in those evaluations a consideration of the fac- tors that are most likely to be credited by courts and juries. These factors include the characteristics of the substance, the characteristics of the defendant, and the characteristics of the offense. Characteristics of the Substance Even severe clinical reactions to psychoactive substances would not ordinarily be expected to interfere with an individ- ual’s capacity to form a specific criminal intent. Acute intoxi- cation has its most profound effects on impulse control, executive functions (planning and sequencing of behavior), and motor coordination (Baler & Volkow, 2006; Dackis & O’Brien, 2005; Fishbein, 2000; Hoaken & Stewart, 2003). Ordinarily, therefore, it could be expected to precipitate spon- taneous behavior that is intended but not deliberated. For example, an intoxicated person might be predisposed to lash out aggressively against an actual or perceived insult. This reaction would be intended, albeit sudden and ill conceived. Consequently, intoxication evidence may be relevant to negate specific intent, but it will infrequently make a compelling argument for negating general intent. Certain classes of psychoactive substances are known to have disinhibiting and agitating effects on the central nervous system (CNS), and they have been demonstrated to induce aggression in controlled settings. Alcohol, in particular, has been shown to trigger aggressive responses in the laboratory, and alcohol is the drug most closely linked to violent crime (De La Rosa, Lambert, & Gropper, 1990; Fagan, 1990; Mack & Barros, 2008; Whitfield, 1990). Other substances that have activating and disinhibiting effects on the CNS, such as amphetamines, phencyclidine (PCP) and cocaine, can cause agitation as well; however, research suggests that preexisting personality factors may be as or more important than pharma- cological factors in predicting whether the abuse of these drugs will result in crime or violence (Hoaken & Stewart, 2003). Although initial reports linked PCP to delirium, extreme psychosis, and unprovoked bouts of violence, subsequent data suggestedamore“sobering”analysisofitseffects(e.g.,Brecher, Wang, Wong, & Morgan, 1988; Davis, 1982; Khajawall, Erickson, & Simpson, 1982; Kinlock, 1991). Extreme reac- tions to acute PCP ingestion do occur, but they are relatively infrequent (Fauman & Fauman, 1982). Due, in part, to the low purity of street-level PCP and the fact that many PCP abusers titrate their doses over months or years, leading to sub- stantial tolerance, acute psychotic reactions are rarer today than they might have been when pharmaceutical-grade PCP first became widely available in the 1970s. Substances that have sedating and analgesic (pain reduc- ing) properties are less likely to invoke violent or aggressive criminal reactions during periods of acute intoxication. For instance, when controlling for subjects’ prior criminal history, no definitive link has been shown between aggressive criminal behavior and intoxication on cannabis (marijuana) or opiates such as heroin (e.g., Bennett et al., 2008; Goldstein, 1985; Pedersen & Skardhamar, 2010). Indeed, opiate intoxication may precipitate the “nods,” in which the individual has diffi- culty remaining alert and perhaps even keeping his or her head up. This would not ordinarily be conducive to intentionally aggressive criminal conduct. To the extent that cannabis or opiate abusers become involved in criminal activity (and many do), this is usually attributable to their efforts to support their drug habit, or to increased associations with other antisocial individuals (Bennett et al., 2008; Farrington, 2010; Pedersen & Skardhamar, 2010). Sedatives such as barbiturates or benzodiazepines may produce acute dysphoria, agitation, and paranoia in high doses, particularly when they are taken over an extended binge in which the subject may be deprived of sleep or food for a long period of time. Ordinarily, however, these drugs cause lethargy, euphoria, and psychomotor slowing, which tend to be incompatible with violence or coordinated criminal activity (e.g., Hoaken & Stewart, 2003). Less is known about the effects of newer “designer drugs” or “club drugs” such as “ecstasy” (3,4-methylene- dioxymethamphetamine or MDMA), “GHB” (gamma- hydroxybutyrate), “roofies” (flunitrazepam or Rohypnol), or “vitamin K” (ketamine or Ketalar). Many of these drugs are classified or partially classified as “dissociative anesthetics,” with features similar to those of PCP (Kohrs & Durieux, 1998). The neurological effects of these drugs may include confusion, delirium, euphoria, paranoia, and depression. Research has not connected the use of these drugs to the com- mission of intentional criminal or violent activity, although more current research suggests that such a link may exist in the case of ketamine (e.g., Sanders, Lankenau, Bloom, & Hathaz, 2009). Indeed, some studies have associated MDMA use with a decrease in aggression among laboratory animals (Hoaken & Stewart, 2003). 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63827-Faust-Ch27.indd 638 5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM5/18/2011 8:23:59 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 4. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 639 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 The relationship between drug use and psychosis warrants further discussion. A psychosis is defined as a break from real- ity, and the clinical hallmarks of psychosis include hallucina- tions (e.g., seeing or hearing things that other people cannot see or hear) or delusions (i.e., persistent beliefs or ideas that do not have a reasonable basis in fact). Psychosis can be defined in terms of episodes (i.e., symptoms meet diagnostic criteria for a psychotic episode) or symptoms (i.e., one or more psychotic featuresthatdonotmeetdiagnosticcriteriaforafullpsychotic episode). Drug use is generally associated with psychosis in one of three ways. First, some drugs, such as hallucinogens (e.g., LSD), specifically act to induce psychotic-type symptoms. These symptoms typically include auditory or visual hallucina- tions and delusional beliefs. However, unlike endogenous psy- choses associated with certain psychiatric conditions, such as schizophrenia, drug-induced psychoses are less likely to include disorganized thinking and behavior. Second, some drugs, such as stimulants, can cause psychotic symptoms as an unintended side effect of their intended recreational effects (i.e., promoting alertness or inducing euphoria). Finally, with- drawal syndromes associated with some drugs may include psychotic symptoms. For example, when sedatives, including alcohol, are used in large quantities over prolonged periods of time, the sudden discontinuation of use of those substances can result in psychotic symptoms. The following sections pres- ent a more detailed examination of the relationship between certain substances of abuse and psychosis. Alcohol Alcohol is classified as a depressant because of its predom- inant effect on brain functioning, but alcohol is well known for causing a wide range of emotional and behavioral responses across users. Typical responses to alcohol intoxication range from sedation or euphoria to excitability, irritability, and aggression. As with most drugs, an individual’s responses to alcohol will vary depending on the amount ingested and the context in which the substance is used. Large doses of alcohol that exceed one’s tolerance lead to cognitive and motor dys- function, and eventually to unconsciousness and possibly even death. Alcohol is most likely to be implicated in cases of psy- chosis during the withdrawal period after chronic use. Although alcohol withdrawal is better known for its motor symptoms (i.e., DTs, or delirium tremens), it may also include transient visual and auditory hallucinations. There is no official persisting psychotic state associated with alcohol use (see Table 27.1); however, chronic alcohol abuse has been associated with damage to the brain that can lead to functional deficits and dementia. Opioids Synthetic opioids and naturally occurring opiates are widely used throughout the world. Although the DSM-IV indicates that opioid intoxication can lead to psychotic symp- toms, particularly auditory hallucinations, the frequency of such occurrences is infrequent. Much like benzodiazepines, opioids exert their action at extremely localized points in the CNS that are largely unassociated with psychotic symptoms. Sedatives (Hypnotics/Anxiolytics) Sedative drugs come in many classes and include anesthet- ics, analgesics, hypnotics (sleep medications), and even anti- histamines. This section will consider the most commonly abused class of sedatives, the benzodiazepines (“benzos” or “blues”), and will devote less attention to barbiturates because of the low base rate of barbiturate abuse in recent decades. Benzodiazepines are only available by prescription and are one of the most commonly prescribed classes of drugs in this coun- try. Brand names of popular benzodiazepines include Valium, Xanax, and Ativan. In 2004, over 32 million prescriptions were filled for just the leading benzodiazepine and its generic equivalent.Medically,benzodiazepinesareprimarilyindicated to reduce anxiety (anxiolytic), to induce sleep (hypnotic), and as an adjunct to anesthesia during certain surgical procedures. Aside from differences in their absorption rate, duration of action, and dosage required to achieve therapeutic effects, benzodiazepines have nearly identical pharmacodynamic pro- files. None of the benzodiazepines carry a risk of causing psy- chotic symptoms or episodes, although this fact does not exclude such symptoms from co-occurring with their use. The relatively minor risk of psychosis associated with benzodiaz- epines occurs in cases when there is an abrupt cessation of the drug in a physically dependent individual. However, more common effects of abrupt termination include agitation, anxi- ety, and seizures. As will be discussed, benzodiazepine intoxi- cation is most likely to disrupt the formation of memories, which may lead to “blackouts.” The abuse of barbiturates (e.g., Seconal, Tuinal, Luminal) is currently rare. These medications are no longer prescribed very often due to their narrow therapeutic index (i.e., for con- trol of brain seizures) and high risk of overdose, especially when combined with alcohol. For the most part, the risk of psychosisassociatedwithbarbituratesisduringthewithdrawal period, and the barbiturate withdrawal syndrome is clinically very similar to that of alcohol. Stimulants The stimulant drugs carry a substantially higher risk of causingpsychoticsymptomsorepisodesthanmostotherdrugs of abuse. Heavy cocaine use (including “crack”) frequently inducesparanoidideationandhallucinations.Suchexperiences Table 27.1 DSM-IV DRUG-INDUCED PSYCHOTIC DISORDERS DRUG INTOXICATION WITHDRAWAL Alcohol + + Amphetamines + Cannabis + Cocaine + Hallucinogens + + Opioids + Sedative/hypnotics + + 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 63927-Faust-Ch27.indd 639 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 5. 640 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 113 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 are even more common in intravenous cocaine users. Heavy cocaine use may also be associated with a tactile psychotic symptom known as formication, which is the feeling that bugs are crawling under one’s skin. Psychotic symptoms can begin during cocaine intoxication and persist for several hours or days. Despite the ability of cocaine to cause acute psychotic symptoms, it is not believed to precipitate persisting psychotic symptoms in persons without a history of psychosis. Amphetamines are potent CNS stimulants. Like cocaine, amphetamines can be smoked or injected, commonly in the formofmethamphetamine(“ice”or“crank”).However,unlike cocaine, many commonly abused amphetamines are available by prescription as pills (e.g., Ritalin, Dexedrine, Adderall), which can be swallowed or crushed and then snorted nasally. Like cocaine-induced psychosis, amphetamine-induced psy- chosis typically consists of paranoid ideation and hallucina- tions (with visual hallucinations being more common than auditory hallucinations). Amphetamine-induced psychosis is usually short lived, lasting no more than about 48–72 hours in most cases. Similar to the amphetamines, the designer amphetamines (e.g., MDMA) have potent stimulant effects on the CNS. However, unlike the other amphetamines, designer amphet- amines have significant pharmacological differences and intoxication profiles, sometimes including hallucinations and perceptual disturbances. For this reason, they are discussed below with the hallucinogens. Hallucinogens Some drugs predictably (as opposed to idiosyncratically) induce psychotic states. In fact, the “traditional” hallucino- gens, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), mescaline (peyote), and psilocybin (“shrooms”), are used primarily for their visual and auditory hallucinogenic effects. They can also produce feelings of euphoria and synesthesia (i.e., a blurring of the senses; for example, hearing colors or seeing sounds). Cannabis is the most widely used hallucinogen and it can have a range of effects on the CNS. Therefore, it may produce a spectrum of reactions across users. Cannabis-induced psy- chotic disorder, which is a diagnostic category in the DSM-IV, can develop shortly after cannabis use and typically involves persecutory delusions (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2005a). Evidence suggests this relatively rare syndrome may be the result of triggering a prodromal or sub-clinical psychotic predisposition, especially in adolescent and young adult users (Foti, Kotov, Guey, & Bromet, 2010; Patton, Coffey, Carlin, Degenhardt, Lynskey & Hall, 2006). By contrast, hallucina- tions associated with cannabis use are rare and typically result when very high blood levels are reached or when the cannabis has been infiltrated or contaminated with another substance of abuse, such as PCP or cocaine. Cannabis-induced psychotic disorder usually remits within 1 day, although instances have been reported in which it persisted for several days before remitting. In addition to the acute effects of cannabis on sensation and perception, cannabis has substantial effects on cognition and motor skills similar to the sedative drugs. This is perhaps why there is such a low reported incidence of cannabis-related violence. It may also explain why cannabis is frequently impli- cated in traffic accidents and incidents of driving under the influence (DUI) (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2005b). MDMA and other designer amphetamines can produce perceptual and sensational effects similar to the traditional hallucinogens. However, relatively little is known about these drugs in comparison to either the amphetamines or older hallucinogens. Nonetheless, they are believed to carry many of the same liabilities of both the stimulants and hallucino- gens, which can include both acute and persisting psychotic symptoms. As a class, hallucinogens are the drugs most closely associ- ated with persisting psychotic symptoms. These symptoms, which may include “flashbacks,” can outlast the known psy- choactive period of the drugs and have been anecdotally reported to occur years after the last reported use. This raises the question of whether hallucinogens may be permanently altering the chemical transmission between neurons or the functions of neural pathways. Persisting psychotic symptoms of hallucinogen use were first documented in the 1960s and 1970s when the use of LSD surged in popularity. The increas- ing use of designer amphetamines, coupled with advances in neuroimaging techniques, has brought the etiology of these persisting symptoms to the forefront of drug abuse research. For example, preliminary data suggest there is a possibility of damage to the brain resulting from MDMA abuse that may account for subsequent mood and perceptual disturbance occurring after drug use has been discontinued. Characteristics of the Defendant For purposes of sentencing or disposition (which are discussed later), it is often in the defendant’s best interest to establish that he or she has a severe substance use history and meets diagnostic criteria for a substance use disorder that requires treatment. Paradoxically, however, such a history could make it more difficult to establish a mens rea or insanity defense. On the one hand, a serious substance abuse history could persuade the fact finder that the defendant has a propensity for intoxi- cation, and thus that he or she was likely to have been intoxi- cated at the time of the offense. On the other hand, this could also suggest that the defendant has a greater tolerance for the substance, and thus has a greater ability to engage in deliberate conduct despite having a high blood-level concentration of the drug. It is also less likely that an experienced substance user would be unaware of the psychoactive properties of a particu- lar drug or would be unaware that he or she has a peculiar sensitivity to that substance. For this reason, “naïve” subjects who have a limited sub- stance use history are often the best candidates for an intoxica- tiondefense.Somereportssuggestthatanintoxicationdefense is most likely to be successful for defendants who have no prior treatment history, who have no prior arrests, and who ingested unusually high doses of a drug they were unfamiliar with (Senay & Wettstein, 1983). Perhaps the best candidate for an intoxication defense is a naïve subject who also presents with evidence of brain injury or neuropsychological dysfunction. Idiosyncratic reactions to 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64027-Faust-Ch27.indd 640 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 6. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 641 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 drugs may be most likely to occur if there is a preexisting lesion (damage) in brain regions controlling executive functions (i.e., the prefrontal cortex) or affective regulation (i.e., the limbic system) (e.g., Fishbein, 2000). Most psychoactive substances exert their effects, at least in part, by precipitating a surge of the neurotransmitter, dopamine, in these regions of the brain (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2008). If there is neurological immaturity or destruction of brain tissue in these areas, a dopamine surge could have an unexpected and intense impact on executive control and emotional regulation, perhaps leading to spontaneous aggression or impulsive crimi- nality. This process has been analogized to placing a match near a keg of dynamite. Dopamine surges are also associated with the onset of psychotic or schizophrenic-like symptoms, which is why stimulant abuse has a tendency to cause psy- chotic reactions. By contrast, no compelling argument has been set forth that neuropsychological deficits in the areas of language, attention, or memory (which may be quite common among offenders) should have discernable effects on impulsive criminality. Characteristics of the Offense Courts and juries are very likely to focus their attention on the factual circumstances surrounding the offense. Commonly mentioned factors in court decisions include (1) whether the defendant had an apparent motive for the crime, (2) whether the crime required a coordinated sequence of conduct over time, and (3) whether the defendant engaged in coordinated efforts to conceal the crime (e.g., Boettcher, 1987; LaFave & Scott, 1986; Marlowe et al., 1999). The most common motives for crime include the “Four Rs”: robbery, rape, reputation, or revenge. If one or more of these motives is present, it may be difficult to argue that there was no intent to commit the offense. This is particularly true if the criminal behavior was contemplated prior to the instance of intoxication. For example, inner-city youths may ingest drugs or alcohol prior to confronting rivals or committing a burglary. In such instances, substance use may be part of a pre- offense “ritual” designed to lower anxiety and foster group cohesiveness. Rather than reducing the apparent intent for the crime, this may actually increase the degree of premeditation because steps are being taken to remove obstacles to the com- mission of the offense (i.e., to remove fear or reticence). If a motive for the offense is present, but the defendant did not contemplate the offense prior to its commission, then this might reduce the degree of culpability from that of a specific- intent crime to one of a general-intent crime; rarely, however, would it negate general intent. For example, assume a defen- dant became intoxicated at a party, subsequently came upon a rival, and assaulted the rival during a spontaneous exchange. In this instance, the motive of revenge or reputation would be present, but there would be no indication that the assault was contemplated prior to its commission. In this situation, intox- ication could be viewed as triggering a potentially explosive situation. Although the defendant might still have had a gen- eral intent to commit a simple assault, this could negate the element of deliberation or premeditation necessary to prove a more serious specific-intent crime, such as assault with intent to inflict bodily harm. It is also important to consider whether the crime was spontaneous as opposed to planned, whether it was carried out over a long or short period of time, and whether it involved an ordered sequence of steps. For example, if a defendant stole a car, drove it across town to pick up an accomplice, and then used the car during a robbery, this would indicate planning and sequencing over an extended period of time. If another defendant picked up a brick from the road, threw it through a store window, and grabbed an odd assortment of items, this might indicate minimal planning and a rapid progression of events. If both of these defendants had similar blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) in the intoxicated range, the scenarios might suggest that alcohol was not particularly influential to the first defendant’s conduct but could have contributed sub- stantially to the conduct of the second defendant. Finally, it is conceivable that a defendant might commit a crime while intoxicated, subsequently sober up, and then engage in coordinated efforts to escape or to conceal the offense. The longer the period of time between the offense and the subsequent efforts at obfuscation, the more likely it is that the defendant’s mental status may have cleared. If, for example, the defendant took immediate pains to conceal the crime, this could suggest that the defendant was not so intoxicated as to be unable to engage in deliberate conduct at the time of the offense (e.g., Commonwealth v. Gribble, 1997). On the other hand, if the defendant awakened 8 hours later on the following morning before engaging in efforts to cover the crime, this might suggest that he or she was too intoxicated to take self-protective measures during or soon after its commission. SUBSTANCE-INDUCED AMNESIA Many criminal defendants purport not to recall the circum- stances surrounding an alleged offense, often attributing this to substance-induced amnesia. Some reports have suggested that between 25% and 65% of violent offenders claimed some degree of amnesia for their crimes (Bradford & Smith, 1979; Schacter, 1986). These defendants may be surprised to learn that this is essentially irrelevant to their criminal culpability. For an intoxication defense, the dispositive matter is whether the defendant intended to commit the crime. There is no requirement that the defendant also remember the crime. Amnesia for the crime is also not dispositive of a defen- dant’s competence to stand trial (e.g., State v. Kleypas, 2001). Amnesia is typically treated as one factor to be considered in making a competency determination. Courts will consider, among other factors, the degree to which defense counsel can reasonably reconstruct the events surrounding the crime from extrinsic sources such as eyewitnesses or physical evidence (e.g., Wilson v. United States, 1968). It is possible for an individual to engage in goal-directed activity and to appear normal to bystanders but still have no memory for the events in question. However, barring an unusual susceptibility to the drug, such “blackouts” have only been definitively connected to relatively high blood-alcohol 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64127-Faust-Ch27.indd 641 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 7. 642 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 levels (Acheson, Stein, & Swartzwelder, 1998; Goodwin, 1995; Mack & Barros, 2008; Ryan & Butters, 1983; Ryback, 1971; White, Signer, Kraus, & Swartzwelder, 2004), intrave- nous administration of benzodiazepines (Kumar, Mac, Gabrielli, & Goodwin, 1987), or a combination of sedatives and alcohol (Morris & Estes, 1987). A particular benzodiaz- epine, Triazolam (Halcion), has been anecdotally reported to cause blackouts at therapeutic dosages (Rothschild, 1992). No definitive data exist to support the occurrence of true blackouts from opiates, cannabis, cocaine, PCP, or stimulants. Although memory may be sketchy after abusing these drugs, cued recall should be sufficient to assist the defendant to remember salient events, particularly if those events were paired with violence or with autonomic arousal. Again, less is known about the effects of newer “designer drugs.” Rohypnol and GHB have been associated with antero- grade amnesia (an inability to form new memories) combined with a loss of consciousness (Dyer, Roth, & Hyma, 2001; Garrison & Mueller, 1998), which accounts for their reported use as “date rape” drugs. Repeated laboratory administration of MDMA has also been associated with cognitive deficits and potentially permanent memory impairment (Broening, Morford,Inman-Wood,Fukumura,&Vorhees,2001;Ricaurte, McCann, Szabo, & Scheffel, 2000). These effects may be greater with the concurrent ingestion of alcohol or sedatives (Schwartz & Weaver, 1998). Although pharmacological evi- dence and case law have yet to catch up with these newer classes of drugs, there is no reason to believe legal standards should apply differently to them. To the extent that these drugs interfere with the formation or retrieval of new memo- ries, it may become necessary to reconstruct the events of a crime from extrinsic sources to move forward with a fair trial. Similarly, to the extent they precipitate psychosis or dissociation, their usage might support an insanity defense if the symptoms were “fixed” over an extended time or if they were ingested inadvertently or with a reasonable absence of knowledge about their effects. ASSESSMENT OF SUBSTANCE ABUSE AND DEPENDENCE There are literally hundreds of substance abuse assessment instruments. The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) have published compendia reviewing the psy- chometric research on many of these instruments (Allen & Wilson, 2003; Rounsaville, Tims, Horton, & Sowder, 1993), although these are a bit outdated at this juncture. In addition, more recent review articles have been published that address specific topics in substance abuse assessment, including assess- ment in criminal justice settings (Andrews, Bonta, & Wormith,2006;Inciardi,1994;Peters,Greenbaum,Steinberg, Carter, Ortiz, Fry, & Valle, 2000; Simpson & Knight, 2007), co-occurringsubstanceabuseandpsychiatricdisorders(Carey, 2002), screening in clinical settings (Carroll & Rounsaville, 2002), screening in primary care settings (McPherson & Hersch, 2000), and motivation for change (Carey, Purnine, Maisto, & Carey, 1999). The reader is advised to consult these resources for in-depth information on a particular instrument relevant to a given case. In the limited space available, we address some of the difficult issues that are commonly con- fronted when conducting substance abuse assessments in forensic or correctional contexts. It is estimated that nearly 40% of criminal offenders who are diverted into substance abuse treatment do not have a seri- ous substance use disorder that requires formal treatment (Kleiman et al., 2003). In some studies, nearly one half of mis- demeanor drug court clients (Marlowe, Festinger, Lee, et al., 2003; DeMatteo, Marlowe, Festinger, & Arabia, 2009), one-third of felony drug court clients (Marlowe, Festinger, & Lee, 2004), and two-thirds of drug-involved pretrial super- visees (Lee et al., 2001) produced sub-threshold scores on a structured clinical interview, similar to a community sample of non-substance abusers. This suggests that many individuals who may be experimenting with drugs, or who may be non- drug-using dealers, are perhaps being diverted into these pro- grams unnecessarily (DeMatteo, Marlowe, & Festinger, 2006). There are several possible explanations for these false- positive rates. (In this context, a false positive is the misidenti- fication of an individual as being in need of substance abuse treatment when the individual does not require formal treat- ment.) First, many jurisdictions make these referrals simply on the basis of whether the individual has a current drug charge, which does not indicate whether he or she has a clinically sig- nificant substance abuse problem (e.g., Marlowe, Patapis, & DeMatteo, 2003). Second, many assessment instruments may inflate the prevalence of substance use disorders either because theyemployexcessivelybroaddiagnosticcriteria,orbecausethey assume most addicts to be in “denial” or “pre-contemplation” about their problem, and thus likely to be underreporting sub- stance use. These instruments were not designed to detect over- reporting or malingering of substance use, which might be of greater concern in a forensic or correctional context. DIAGNOSIS Official diagnostic criteria for substance abuse and depen- dence are worded so generically that they may fail to convey clinically precise or meaningful information. Moreover, they may blur the important distinction between substance depen- dence and abuse. As was noted previously, the term substance abuse conveys a repetitive pattern of substance ingestion under dangerous or inappropriate circumstances, whereas substance dependence conveys a pattern of compulsive use or addiction. However, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Asso- ciation, 2000), a client can receive a diagnosis of substance dependence without exhibiting the features that one would ordinarily associate with addiction, such as withdrawal symp- toms, compulsive use, or cravings. It is possible, for example, to be diagnosed as drug dependent by virtue of spending a great deal of time using drugs, developing a tolerance to the drugs, and using more drugs than initially planned on multiple occa- sions.Thesesymptomscanoccuratanytimeduringa12-month period, with no requirement that all of the symptoms occur at 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64227-Faust-Ch27.indd 642 5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM5/18/2011 8:24:00 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 8. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 643 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 the same time, or that they last for an appreciable length of time. The failure of current diagnostic nomenclature to capture the prototypical symptoms of dependence, or to discriminate sufficiently between abuse and dependence, has led some scholars to argue that the dependence diagnosis should be reserved for individuals who suffer from severe withdrawal symptoms when they refrain from using drugs or alcohol (e.g., Langenbucher et al., 2000), or who experience overwhelming cravings or compulsions to use drugs or alcohol (e.g., O’Brien, Childress, Ehrman, & Robbins, 1998). Anything short of that, according to this argument, may constitute abuse or misuse, which although dangerous and illegal may be undeserving of a formal psychiatric diagnosis. In fact, early indications suggest that the upcoming 5th edition of the DSM might drop the substance abuse diagnosis altogether and instead differentiate among degrees of severity of substance dependence (Knopf, 2010). In practice, many criminal justice professionals may lack the requisite time or skills to render an accurate diagnosis using a DSM-IV-congruent structured interview. Many of these practitioners employ brief screening instruments that can purportedly identify substance dependence using no more than a dozen to a few dozen self-report items. The content domains of these instruments vary considerably depending upon the philosophies of the test developers, and they are often incongruent with theDSM. As a result, it may be unclear what syndrome(s) they actually measure. As noted earlier, a particular concern is that many of these instruments were designed to detect substance dependence in individuals who are in “denial.” Thus, they may be apt to increase sensitivity (i.e., increase true positive rates for identifying addicts) at the expense of decreasing specificity (i.e., misidentifying many non-addicts as addicts). For example, one of the most widely used screening instru- mentsistheSubstanceAbuseSubtleScreeningInventory(SASSI; Miller, Roberts, Brooks, & Lazowski, 1997). Most of the SASSI scales contain items that do not inquire explicitly about substance use, but have been found by the test developer to dis- criminate empirically between groups with and without sub- stance abuse problems. Although initial validation studies by the test developer were impressive, with sensitivity and speci- ficity exceeding 90% (Lazowski, Miller, Boye, & Miller, 1998), subsequent studies have been less sanguine, with true-positive ratesbelow70%(Pearson,2000),sensitivitybelow60%(Fuller, Fishman, Taylor, & Wood, 1994; Svanum & McGrew, 1995), and inadequate specificity (Peters et al., 2000). In one study involving juvenile offenders, the SASSI misclassified nearly two-thirds of nonusers as being substance dependent (Rogers, Cashel, Johansen, Sewell, & Gonzalez, 1997). This suggests that the SASSI might substantially overestimate the need for substance abuse treatment among offenders. MULTIDIMENSIONAL ASSESSMENT INTERVIEWS A number of semi-structured interviews have been developed that provide a behavioral assessment of substance use severity, and that can detect changes in substance use over time or as a function of treatment. Many of these instruments are multi- dimensional, meaning that they inquire about problems in several life domains that are commonly affected by substance abuse. Perhaps the most widely used of these instruments is the Addiction Severity Index, which is currently in its fifth edi- tion3 (ASI-5; McLellan et al., 1992). The ASI-5 measures cur- rent (past 30 days) and lifetime problem severity in the areas of drug problems, alcohol problems, legal problems, medical problems, family and social problems, employment problems, and psychiatric problems. The ASI-5 is comprised predomi- nantly of two types of items. One type of item inquires about the frequency of specific events, such as the number of days the subject used illicit drugs or committed illegal activities during the preceding 30 days and during the individual’s lifetime. The second type of item is on a Likert scale and assesses how serious the subject believes his or her problems to be in each domain, and how much the subject desires treat- ment for those problems (from “not at all” to “extremely”). Composite scores are calculated from a weighted combination of both types of items and serve as global indicators of prob- lem severity in each domain. The composite scores are based exclusively on events occurring during the immediately pre- ceding 30 days. The ASI-5 and its predecessors are supported by a long historyofrigorouspsychometricvalidationresearchpublished in peer-reviewed scientific journals. Multiple examinations of ASI composite scores and lifetime items have yielded impressive evidence of interrater reliability (i.e., the extent to which different raters agree on ASI scores), test–retest reli- ability (i.e., the stability of ASI scores over time), concurrent validity (i.e., the correlation of scores on the ASI with perfor- mance on other measures at the same point in time), predic- tive validity (i.e., the correlation of scores on the ASI with performance on other measures at some point in the future), and discriminative utility (i.e., the ability of the ASI to dis- criminate between clinical and nonclinical samples) across various groups of clients characterized by age, race, gender, and primary drug of abuse (e.g., Alterman et al., 1998; Cacciola, Koppenhaver, McKay, & Alterman, 1999; McLellan et al., 1985, 1992; McLellan, Luborsky, O’Brien, & Woody, 1980). There are several factors, however, that could be expected to limit the utility of the ASI-5 in a forensic or correctional context, and these factors exemplify many of the same prob- lems that are presented by many substance abuse interviews. First, the ASI-5 was not normed and does not yield standard- ized scale scores. That is, it has not, to date, been administered to a large, well-defined, and representative sample of partici- pants that can be used as a point of reference against which to compare the results of future test takers. Therefore, it is not possible to make meaningful comparisons across scales. For example, one cannot compare the severity of a client’s drug problems with his or her alcohol problems. Clinical Factor 3. A 6th edition of the ASI is undergoing psychometric research and development. 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64327-Faust-Ch27.indd 643 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 9. 644 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 115 114 113 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Scores (CFSs) have been devised for the ASI-5 which are on a standardized t-score scale (mean = 50 and SD = 10) (McDermott et al., 1996); however, they were only standard- ized on a narrow sample (n = 500) of methadone maintenance clients, raising questions about the stability and generalizabil- ity of the scaling. Moreover, few community treatment pro- viders may have the requisite software to compute the CFS scoring algorithms. Without standardized scaling, it is difficult to know whether a particular client’s substance abuse problem should be categorized as mild, moderate, or severe as compared with a normative population. The most that can be inferred from the composite scores is whether they decline over time, or whether different groups have significantly higher or lower composite scores within the same domain. Many treatment providers will attempt to classify the severity of clients’ substance abuse problems using what are called “interviewer severity ratings” (ISRs) on the ASI-5, which are essentially derived from the assessor’s own clinical judgment. The research evidence is clear that the ISRs are unreliable, meaning that different raters may cometodifferentconclusionsaboutthesameclient(Alterman, Brown, Zaballero, & McKay, 1994). As a result, the test devel- opers have advised against their use and have dropped them from the upcoming sixth edition of the instrument. Thus, in a forensic context, one would have great difficulty concluding from the ASI-5 whether a client has a serious substance abuse problem. The best that can be accomplished is to review responses to specific items, such as how often the client used drugs in the previous month, and then to make inferences of severity from those frequency data. Although the ASI-5 is commonly used by clinicians, it is important to recognize that it was originally developed by researchers to measure change during treatment–outcome studies. The items were chosen predominantly on the basis of their sensitivity to change over time or their ability to predict long-term outcomes. As a result, the ASI-5 does not render information related to clinical symptoms or diagnoses. There is no way to reach a DSM-IV diagnosis from the ASI-5 or to assess prototypical features of dependence, such as withdrawal or cravings. Moreover, there is no information about how much drugs or alcohol an individual consumed. Because sub- jects’ reports were empirically determined to be unreliable in terms of the amount of substances they consumed, the ASI-5 focuses instead on the frequency of use, measured in days. This leaves many questions unanswered concerning the clinical characteristics of a client’s substance abuse problem. Finally, the ASI-5 is highly susceptible to the issue of “days at risk.” Because the scales measure events occurring within a 30-day window, if a subject were in a restricted environment such as jail or prison during that time, this could systematically deflate the apparent severity of his or her problems. Clinicians often attempt to compensate for this problem either by calcu- lating the proportion of days at risk that the subject engaged in various behaviors, or by inquiring about the 30 days immedi- ately preceding the subject’s entry into a restrictive setting. There are no published studies indicating whether this approach is valid or reliable and how it might alter the assess- ment results. OVERREPORTING AND UNDERREPORTING OF SUBSTANCE USE Depending on the context, individuals may have reason either to underreport or overreport their level of substance use. For example, individuals on probation or parole might underre- port or deny drug use out of an understandable concern that a violation of the terms of their probation or parole may result in incarceration. By contrast, it is conceivable that criminal defendants seeking to establish an insanity defense might overreport drug use on the belief that voluntary intoxication could form the basis of such a defense. Although several stud- ies have addressed the underreporting of drug use among criminal and civil populations, we were unable to locate reli- able research directly addressing the overreporting of drug use. Therefore, we will limit our discussion to a review of research regarding the underreporting of drug use. Among criminal populations, research has typically found that arrestees underreport their recent use of illicit drugs, particularly cocaine (e.g., Hser, 1997; Lu, Taylor, & Riley, 2000; Mieczkowski, Barzelay, Gropper, & Wish, 1991; Yacoubian, 2000). Harrison (1995) found that among arrest- ees who tested positive for marijuana, cocaine, and opiates, only half reported using any of those drugs within the past 3 days. Research also suggests that a high proportion of drunk- driving offenders underreport their use of drugs besides alco- hol (e.g., Lapham, C’de Baca, Chang, Hunt, & Berger, 2002). Researchers have also found underreporting of drug use among other high-risk, but non-arrestee, populations (e.g., Fendrich,Johnson,Sudman,Wislar,&Spiehler,1999;Morral, McCaffrey, & Iguchi, 2000). Fendrich et al. (1999) collected self-report survey data and hair specimens from a high-risk sample of household respondents in Chicago, and results indi- cated significant underreporting of cocaine and heroin use during the past 30 days. In another study, Morral et al. (2000) explored the accuracy of self-reported drug-use frequency among 701 methadone maintenance clients and found that use frequencies averaged 34% higher for opiates and 20% higher for cocaine than were self-reported. In a study exam- ining self-reported drug use and urine-testing data among 59 methadone maintenance patients, researchers found that the subjects over- or underreported their use of cocaine and heroin by an average of 15% (Ehrman & Robbins, 1994). Finally, in the civil context, some commentators have sug- gested that parents involved in child custody disputes may routinely underreport their use of drugs and alcohol (e.g., Schleuderer & Campagna, 2004). Taken together, the weight of the available evidence appears to suggest that underreporting of drug use is a signifi- cant concern among some offender and non-offender popula- tions. This highlights the importance of not relying exclusively on self-report data when conducting a substance abuse evalua- tion or monitoring an individual’s abstinence. Best-practice standards strongly underscore the importance of combining self-report data with objective measures of drug use, such as urine drug-screen results (Carver, 2004; Harrell & Kleiman, 2002; Messina, Wish, Nemes, & Wraight, 2000; Rosay, Najaka, & Herz, 2000). 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64427-Faust-Ch27.indd 644 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 10. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 645 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 TREATMENT OF SUBSTANCE ABUSE AND DEPENDENCE As was noted earlier, attorneys and forensic experts are fre- quently called upon to predict whether a subject would be likely to succeed in substance abuse treatment in lieu of a more punitive or restrictive disposition. As with any prediction, the answer to this question will often depend, in large part, on the base rate for success in the population. Unfortunately, success in traditional substance abuse treatment programs can by no means be confidently assumed. ATTRITION FROM TREATMENT Attrition from substance abuse treatment is unacceptably high. Between approximately one-half and two-thirds of indi- viduals who schedule an initial intake appointment for drug abuse treatment fail to show for the first intake session (Festinger, Lamb, Kirby, & Marlowe, 1996; Festinger, Lamb, Kountz, Kirby, & Marlowe, 1995; Festinger, Lamb, Marlowe, & Kirby, 2002). Of those who do attend an intake, between 40% and 80% drop out of treatment within 3 months (e.g., Gainey, Wells, Hawkins, & Catalano, 1993; Simpson, Joe, & Brown, 1997; Stark, 1992) and between 80% and 90% drop out within 12 months (e.g., Satel, 1999). Comparablefiguresarereportedamongsubstance-abusing criminal offenders. Only about 25% of drug-involved offend- ers may be expected to complete a substance abuse treatment regimen (UCLA, 2007). Between 40% and 70% of proba- tioners and parolees drop out of treatment or attend irregu- larly within 2–6 months (Nurco, Hanlon, & Kinlock, 1991; Taxman, 1999a; Young, Usdane, & Torres, 1991). EvidencefromthenationalDrugAbuseTreatmentOutcome Study(DATOS)suggeststhat3monthsofdrugabusetreatment may be a minimum threshold for detecting dose–response effects for the interventions, and 6–12 months may be a thresh- old for observing clinically meaningful reductions in substance use (Simpson et al., 1997). After 12 months of drug abuse treat- ment, clients have roughly a 50% probability of remaining con- tinuously abstinent for an additional year following completion oftreatment(McLellan,Lewis,O’Brien,&Kleber,2000).Given that no more than about 25% of drug-abusing offenders remain in treatment for 12 months, one might reasonably expect no more than about 10% to 15% of the original intent-to-treat cohort, on average, to achieve a sustained interval of sobriety. TREATMENT EFFECTS In community-based substance abuse treatment programs, the magnitudes of the treatment effects are typically small to mod- erate. A meta-analysis of 78 studies that compared standard drug abuse treatment with a no-treatment or minimal-treat- ment control condition reported small impacts on crime and moderate impacts on drug use (Prendergast, Podus, Chang, & Urada, 2002). (A meta-analysis is a quantitative method of combining the results of several research studies so that infer- ences can be drawn across studies.) Using a liberal definition of“success”(i.e.,abetterthanaverageoutcomeforthesample), the mean weighted ES for criminal activity was 0.13, equiva- lent to about a 6 percentage-point increase in the percentage of “successful” cases. (An ES is a way of expressing the size or magnitude of the effects of a particular intervention). The average weighted ES for drug use was 0.30, equivalent to about a 15 percentage-point increase in the proportion of “success- ful” cases. A meta-analysis of 361 controlled alcohol treatment studies found that the most commonly administered treat- ments in standard practice (i.e., psychoeducational group counseling, milieu therapy, and relaxation training) yielded no appreciable evidence of efficacy (Miller & Wilbourne, 2002). There may be reason to anticipate lesser success within the criminal justice system. Nationally, less than 10 percent of pro- bationers and 20 percent of prison or jail inmates have access to needed substance abuse treatment services on any given day (Chandler, Fletcher, & Volkow, 2009; Friedmann, Taxman, & Henderson, 2007; Taxman, Perdoni, & Harrison, 2007). What little treatment is available is often not evidence-based, lacking in a coherent focus or structure, and delivered by inad- equately trained staff (Lutze & van Wormer, 2007; Taxman & Bouffard, 2003). The most commonly administered “treat- ment”incorrectionalsettingsisdrug-educationgroups,which offer minimal benefits for addicted offenders (Pearson & Lipton, 1999). Although psycho-education might be a useful secondary-prevention strategy for non-addicted substance abusers, it has almost no value for addicts. Adding case management services to standard drug abuse treatment for offenders has produced mixed but potentially promising findings. Under the rubric of what was originally named Treatment Alternatives to Street Crime (TASC)— renamed Treatment Accountability for Safer Communities— hundreds of case management agencies were founded across the country to identify and refer drug-abusing offenders to a range of needed treatment services, to monitor their progress in treatment, and to report compliance information to appro- priate criminal justice authorities. Early evaluations concluded that TASC programs were effective at identifying substance abuse problems among offendersandmaking appropriatetreat- ment referrals (Weinman, 1990). Moreover, clients involved with the criminal justice system tended to remain in treatment significantly longer when they were under TASC supervision (Hubbard, Collins, Rachal, & Cavanaugh, 1988). However, an evaluation of five large and representative TASC programs concludedthateffectsondruguseandcriminalrecidivismwere mixed (Anglin, Longshore, & Turner, 1999). Drug use was sig- nificantly lower for TASC clients in three of the five sites and criminal activity was lower in two of the sites. Moreover, the positive findings were small to moderate in magnitude. These results suggest that TASC programs may be a critical compo- nent of successful treatment for substance-involved offenders; however, they require additional support and back-up from the courts, probation or parole officers, and treatment agencies to achieve significant and sustained positive effects. QUALITY OF TREATMENT The disappointing outcomes reviewed above should come as no surprise to anyone who is familiar with the substance abuse 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64527-Faust-Ch27.indd 645 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 11. 646 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 114 113 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 treatment system in this country. A study of a nationally repre- sentative sample of public and private substance abuse treat- ment programs in the U.S. found that nearly 45% of the programs were either closed or no longer providing substance abuse services within a 16-month period (McLellan, Carise, & Kleber, 2003). That same study found more than a 50% staff- turnover rate among both the clinicians and administrators within 12 months. With such gross instability in staffing, the programs could not reasonably be expected to maintain the skills level necessary to deliver effective services. Moreover, given that between one-quarter and one-half of substance abuse treatment providers, nationally, have no more than a 2-year college degree (McLellan et al., 2003; Taxman & Bouffard, 2003), it is difficult to imagine how they could be expected to master the complicated principles and techniques that are necessary to treat chronic and intransigent conditions such as crime and addiction. One multisite observational study of counseling sessions for drug-abusing or addicted offenders reported that only about 60% of the sessions addressed clinically relevant mate- rial, few counselors endorsed a cohesive or identifiable treat- ment philosophy, there was no conceptual clarity to the sessions, few evidence-based treatments were administered, and the counselors failed to show for nearly 20% of the sched- uled appointments (Taxman & Bouffard, 2003). This is con- sistent with several earlier national studies finding that few alcohol or drug abuse treatment programs provided evidence- based treatments for their clients (Lamb, Greenlick, & McCarty, 1998). EFFECTIVE DISPOSITIONS Given the relatively low base rate for success in standard cor- rectional treatment programs, it should go without saying that simply diverting offenders into treatment, without more, is unlikely to reliably serve public-health or public-safety objec- tives (Marlowe, 2002, 2003). This does not, however, justify a wholesale abandonment of treatment objectives. Outcomes from substance abuse treatment are actually better than those derived from punitive correctional sentences, and they are considerably less costly. For instance, the research evidence is clear that imprisonment has virtually no impact on drug use or crime. Over 95% of drug-abusing offenders return to drug use within 3 years of release from prison, with roughly 85% relaps- ing within only the first 6–12 months (Hanlon, Nurco, Bateman, & O’Grady, 1998; Maddux & Desmond, 1981; Martin, Butzin, Saum, & Inciardi, 1999; Nurco et al., 1991; Pelissier, Jones, & Cadigan, 2007; Vaillant, 1973). Moreover, within3yearsofreleasefromprison,approximatelytwo-thirds of offenders are arrested for a new crime, and roughly one-half are convicted of a new crime or reincarcerated for a technical parole violation (Langan & Levin, 2002). Dozens of program evaluations similarly revealed no impacts on drug use or crime for so-called intermediate sanctions, such as boot camps, home detention, or electronic monitoring (e.g., Gendreau, Goggins, Cullen, & Andrews, 2000; Taxman, 1999b). The programs that have produced the most consistent evidence of success are those that blend the functions of the criminal justice system and the substance abuse treatment system (Marlowe, 2002, 2003). Substance abuse treatment assumes a central role in these programs, rather than being viewed as peripheral to punitive ends, and is provided in sub- jects’ community of origin, where they can maintain family and social contacts and can seek or continue in gainful educa- tion or employment. Responsibility for ensuring subjects’ attendance in treatment and avoidance of drug use and crimi- nal activity is not, however, delegated to treatment personnel who may be unprepared or disinclined to deal with such mat- ters, and who often have limited power to intervene. The crim- inal justice system maintains substantial supervisory control over offenders and has enhanced authority through plea agree- ments and similar arrangements to respond rapidly and con- sistently to infractions in the program. Drug courts are one example of an integrated program that has shown substantial promise for reducing drug use and crime among pretrial defendants and probationers. Drug courts are special criminal-court dockets that provide judi- cially supervised substance abuse treatment in lieu of prosecu- tion or incarceration. The core ingredients of a drug court include regular status hearings before the judge in court, random weekly urine drug screens, mandatory completion of a prescribed regimen of treatment and case management ser- vices, progressive negative sanctions for program infractions, and positive rewards for program accomplishments. Participants who satisfactorily complete the program may have their criminal charges dropped, reduce their probation- ary obligations, or receive a sentence of time served in the pro- gram. Defendants are generally required to plead guilty or no contest as a condition of entering a pre-adjudication drug court; therefore, termination from the program ordinarily results in a criminal conviction and sentencing to probation or incarceration. The evidence is clear that drug courts provide enhanced supervision of drug-involved offenders and increase their exposure to substance abuse treatment (Lindquist, Krebs, Warner, & Lattimore, 2009). Reviews of dozens of program evaluations concluded that an average of 60% of drug court clients completed 1 year or more of treatment, and roughly one-half graduated from the program (Belenko, 1998, 1999, 2001, 2002). This compares quite favorably to the usual reten- tion rates in community-based drug abuse treatment pro- grams, where, as was noted earlier, 75% to 90% of probationers drop out or attend irregularly in less than 1 year. Five meta-analyses conducted by independent scientific teams have all concluded that adult drug courts significantly reduced criminal recidivism (typically measured by re-arrest rates) by an average of approximately 8 to 14 percentage points (Aos, Miller, & Drake, 2006; Latimer, Morton-Bourgon, & Chretien, 2006; Lowenkamp, Holsinger, & Latessa, 2005; Shaffer, 2006; Wilson, Mitchell, & MacKenzie, 2006). In ran- domized experimental studies, drug court participants exhib- ited roughly a 15 percentage-point reduction in rearrest rates at 2 years and 3 years post-admission compared to probation- ers (Gottfredson, Kearley, Najaka, & Rocha, 2005; Gottfredson, Najaka, & Kearley, 2003; Turner, Greenwood, Fain, & Deschenes, 1999). 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64627-Faust-Ch27.indd 646 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 12. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 647 120 119 118 117 116 115 114 113 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 A number of dismantling studies have investigated the critical ingredients of drug court programs. Dismantling stud- ies use an experimental design to determine the independent contributions of specific components of an intervention. A series of controlled, parametric studies found that holding fre- quent judicial status hearings improved outcomes for high- risk drug court clients who had more severe drug-use histories or a comorbid diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder (Festinger, Marlowe, Lee, Kirby, Bovasso, & McLellan, 2002; Marlowe, Festinger, & Lee, 2003, 2004; Marlowe, Festinger, Dugosh, Lee, & Benasutti, 2007; Marlowe, Festinger, Lee, Dugosh, & Benasutti, 2006). Two other experimental studies found that imposing graduated sanctions for positive urine drug screens and other infractions improved outcomes over standardpretrialsupervisionorprobation(Harrell,Cavanagh, & Roman, 1998; Hawken & Kleiman, 2009). Finally, at least one study has suggested that outcomes might be improved for the more incorrigible types of drug offenders when they can earn tangible rewards for positive achievements in the pro- gram (Marlowe, Festinger, Dugosh, Arabia, & Kirby, 2008). The results of these dismantling studies indicate that the positive impacts of drug courts cannot be attributed simply to the effects of substance abuse treatment. The additional ele- ments of judicial supervision, urine drug testing, and contin- gent sanctions and rewards appear to be making significant incremental contributions to outcomes (Marlowe, DeMatteo, & Festinger, 2003). This suggests that forensic experts and attorneys will need to be substantially creative when crafting dispositional recommendations. It may not be sufficient to simply recommend that a subject receive substance abuse treatment, especially in light of the relatively modest base rates for success in standard treatment programs. Particularly for the more serious or recidivist offenders, a suitable disposition plan will need to include provisions for ongoing monitoring by criminal justice authorities, as well as for the immediate and consistent imposition of sanctions and rewards contingent upon the subject’s progress or lack of progress in treatment. SUMMARY In summary, the research evidence fails to support a simplistic premise that substance use is primarily responsible for crimi- nal activity in many cases, or that substance abuse treatment, alone, is necessarily the most appropriate strategy for forestall- ing future wrongdoing. Criminal and tortious activities are frequently influenced as much by the circumstances surround- ing a particular event, or by the personal characteristics of the defendant, than by the pharmacological properties of a given substance. Moreover, it appears that many assessment instruments may overinflate the prevalence or severity of diag- nosable substance use disorders among offenders, and there- fore may overestimate treatment needs in this population. Finally, difficult as it might be to accept, the research evidence does not provide reliable or consistent support for the effec- tiveness of traditional community-based substance abuse treatment interventions for reducing substance abuse and crime among offenders. Unless attorneys and forensic experts develop dispositional plans that include careful procedures for monitoring offenders’ compliance in treatment and respond- ing consistently to infractions, the majority of offenders might be expected to drop out of treatment prematurely or to fail to achieve a sustained interval of sobriety. REFERENCES Acheson, S. K., Stein, R. M., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (1998). Impairment in semantic and figural memory by acute ethanol: Age-dependent effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 22, 1437–1442. Allen, J. P., & Wilson, V. B. (Eds.) (2003). Assessing alcohol problems: A guide for clinicians and researchers (2nd ed.) [NIH Pub. No. 03-3745]. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse & Alcoholism, U.S. Dept. of Health & Human Services. Alterman, A. I., Brown, L. S., Zaballero, A., & McKay, J. R. (1994). The interviewer severity ratings and composite scores of the ASI: A further look. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 34, 201–209. Alterman, A. I., McDermott, P. A., Cook, T. G., Metzger, D., Rutherford, M. J., Cacciola, J. S., & Brown, L. S. (1998). New scales to assess change in the Addiction Severity Index for the opioid, cocaine, and alcohol dependent. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12, 233–246. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press. Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Wormith, J. S. (2006). The recent past and near future of risk and/or need assessment. Crime & Delinquency, 52, 7–27. Anglin, M. D., Longshore, D., & Turner, S. (1999). Treatment alternatives to street crime: An evaluation of five programs. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 168–195. Aos, S., Miller, M., & Drake, E. (2006). Evidence-based public policy options to reduce future prison construction, criminal justice costs, and crime rates. Olympia, WA: Washington State Institute for Public Policy. Baler, R. D., & Volkow, N. D. (2006). Drug addiction: The neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 12, 559–566. Belenko, S. (1998). Research on drug courts: A critical review. National Drug Court Institute Review, 1, 1–42. Belenko, S. (1999). Research on drug courts: A critical review 1999 update. National Drug Court Institute Review, 2(2), 1–58. Belenko, S. (2001). Research on drug courts: A critical review 2001 update. New York, NY: National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse at Columbia University. Belenko, S. (2002). Drug courts. In C. G. Leukefeld, F. Tims, & D. Farabee (Eds.), Treatment of drug offenders: Policies and issues (pp. 301–318). New York, NY: Springer. Bennett, T., Holloway, K., & Farrington, D. (2008). The statistical associa- tion between drug misuse and crime: A meta-analysis. Aggression & Violent Behavior, 13, 107–118. Besinger, B. A., Garland, A. F., Litrownik, A. J., & Landsverk, J. A. (1999). Caregiver substance abuse among maltreated children placed in out- of-home care. Child Welfare, 78, 221–239. Boettcher, M. J. (1987). Voluntary intoxication: A defense to specific intent crimes. University of Detroit Law Review, 65, 33–71. Bradford, J. W., & Smith, S. M. (1979). Amnesia and homicide: The Pedolacaseandastudyofthirtycases.BulletinoftheAmericanAcademy of Psychiatry and the Law, 7, 219–231. Brecher,M.,Wang,B.W.,Wong,H.,&Morgan,J.P.(1988).Phencyclidine and violence: Clinical and legal issues. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 397–401. Broening, H. W., Morford, L. L., Inman-Wood, S. L., Fukumura, M., & Vorhees, C. V. (2001). 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy)-induced learning and memory impairments depend on the age of exposure during early development. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 3228–3235. Bureau of Justice Statistics. (1998). Substance abuse and treatment of adults on probation, 1995 [NCJ No. 166611]. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64727-Faust-Ch27.indd 647 5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM5/18/2011 8:24:01 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 13. 648 • SCIENTIFIC STATUS OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 138 137 136 135 134 133 132 131 130 129 128 127 126 125 124 123 122 121 120 119 118 117 116 115 114 113 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2001). Trends in state parole, 1990–2000. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Cacciola, J. S., Koppenhaver, J. M., McKay, J. R., & Alterman, A. I. (1999). Test-retest reliability of the lifetime items on the Addiction Severity Index. Psychological Assessment, 11, 86–93. Carey, K. B. (2002). Clinically useful assessments: Substance abuse and comorbid psychiatric disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1345–1361. Carey, K. B., Purnine, D. M., Maisto, S. A., & Carey, M. P. (1999). Assessing readiness to change substance abuse: A critical review of instruments. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 245–266. Carroll, K. M., & Rounsaville, B. J. (2002). On beyond urine: Clinically useful assessment instruments in the treatment of drug dependence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1329–1344. Carver, J. (2004). Drug testing: A necessary prerequisite for treatment and for crime control. In P. Bean & T. Nemitz (Eds.), Drug treatment: What works? (pp. 142–177). London: Routledge. Chandler, R. K., Fletcher, B. W., & Volkow, N. D. (2009). Treating drug abuse and addiction in the criminal justice system: Improving public health and safety. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301, 183–190. Dackis, C., & O’Brien, C. (2005). Neurobiology of addiction: Treatment and public policy ramifications. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1431–1436. Davis, B. L. (1982). The PCP epidemic: A critical appraisal. International Journal of the Addictions, 17, 1137–1155. De La Rosa, M., Lambert, E. Y., & Gropper, B. (1990). Exploring the substance abuse-violence connection [NIDA Research Monograph No. 103]. Rockville, MD: U.S. Deptartment of Health and Human Services. DeMatteo, D. S., Marlowe, D. B., & Festinger, D. S. (2006). Secondary prevention services for clients who are low risk in drug court: A con- ceptual model. Crime and Delinquency, 52, 114–134. DeMatteo, D. S., Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., & Arabia, P. L. (2009). Outcome trajectories in drug court: Do all participants have serious drug problems? Criminal Justice & Behavior, 36, 354–368. Dunn, M. G., Tarter, R. E., Mezzich, A. C., Vanyukov, M., Kirisci, L., & Kirillova, G. (2002). Origins and consequences of child neglect in sub- stance abuse families. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 1063–1090. Dyer, J. E., Roth, B., & Hyma, B. A. (2001). Gamma-hydroxybutyrate withdrawal syndrome. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 37, 147–153. Ehrman,R.N.,&Robbins,S.J.(1994).Reliabilityandvalidityof6-month timeline reports of cocaine and heroin use in a methadone population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 843–850. Elwork, A. (2007). Stress management for lawyers (3rd ed.). North Wales, PA: Vorkell Group. Fagan, J. (1990). Intoxication and aggression. In M. Tonry & J. Q. Wilson (Eds.), Drugs and crime (pp. 241–320). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Farrington,D.(2010).CommentaryonPedersenandSkardhamar(2010): Does cannabis predict non-drug offending? Addiction, 105, 119–120. Fauman, M. A., & Fauman, B. J. (1982). Phencyclidine abuse and crime: A psychiatric perspective. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 10, 171–176. Fazel, S., Bains, P., & Doll, H. (2006). Substance abuse and dependence in prisoners: A systematic review. Addiction, 101, 181–191. Fendrich,M.,Johnson,T.P.Sudman,S.,Wislar,J.S.,&Spiehler,V.(1999). Validity of drug use reporting in a high-risk community sample: A comparison of cocaine and heroin survey reports with hair tests. American Journal of Epidemiology, 149, 955–962. Festinger, D. S., Lamb, R. J., Kirby, K. C., & Marlowe, D. B. (1996). The accelerated intake: A method for increasing initial attendance to out- patient cocaine treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 387–389. Festinger, D. S., Lamb, R. J., Kountz, M., Kirby, K. C., & Marlowe, D. B. (1995). Pretreatment dropout as a function of treatment delay and client variables. Addictive Behaviors, 20, 111–115. Festinger, D. S., Lamb, R. J., Marlowe, D. B., & Kirby, K. C. (2002). From telephone to office: Intake attendance as a function of appointment delay. Addictive Behaviors, 27, 131–137. Festinger, D. S., Marlowe, D. B., Lee, P. A., Kirby, K. C., Bovasso, G., & McLellan, A. T. (2002). Status hearings in drug court: When more is less and less is more. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 68, 151–157. Fishbein, D. (2000). Neuropsychological function, drug abuse, and vio- lence: A conceptual framework. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 139–159. Foti, D. J., Kotov, R., Guey, L. T., & Bromet, E. J. (2010). Cannabis use and the course of schizophrenia: 10-year follow-up after first hospitaliza- tion. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 987–993. Friedmann, P. D., Taxman, F. S., & Henderson, C. E. (2007). Evidence- based treatment practices for drug-involved adults in the criminal jus- tice system. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 32, 267–277. Fuller, M. G., Fishman, E., Taylor, C. A., & Wood, R. B. (1994). Screening patients with traumatic brain injuries for substance abuse. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 6, 143–146. Gainey, R. R., Wells, E. A., Hawkins, J. D., & Catalano, R. F. (1993). Predicting treatment retention among cocaine users. International Journal of the Addictions, 28, 487–505. Garrison, G., & Mueller, P. (1998). Clinical features and outcomes after unintentional gamma hydroxybutyrate (GHB) overdose [Abstract]. Journal of Toxicology: Clinical Toxicology, 36, 503–504. Gendreau, P., Goggins, C., Cullen, F. T., & Andrews, D. A. (2000). The effects of community sanctions and incarceration on recidivism. Forum on Corrections Research, 12, 10–13. Goldstein, P. J. (1985). The drugs-violence nexus: A tripartite conceptual framework. Journal of Drug Issues, 15, 493–506. Goldstein, R. Z., Craig, A. D., Bechara, A., Garavan, H., Childress, A. R., Paulus, M. P., & Volkow, N. D. (2009). The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Cell, 13, 372–380. Goodwin, D. W. (1995). Alcohol amnesia. Addiction, 90, 315–317. Gottfredson, D. C., Kearley, B. W., Najaka, S. S., & Rocha, C. M. (2005). The Baltimore City Drug Treatment Court: 3-year outcome study. Evaluation Review, 29, 42–64. Gottfredson, D. C., Najaka, S. S., & Kearley, B. (2003). Effectiveness of drug courts: Evidence from a randomized trial. Criminology and Public Policy, 2, 171–196. Hanlon, T. E., Nurco, D. N., Bateman, R. W., & O’Grady, K. E. (1998). The response of drug abuser parolees to a combination of treatment and intensive supervision. Prison Journal, 78, 31–44. Harrell, A., Cavanagh, S., & Roman, J. (1998). Final report: Findings from the evaluation of the D.C. superior court drug intervention program. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute. Harrell, A., & Kleiman, M. (2002). Drug testing in criminal justice set- tings. In C. G. Leukefeld, F. Tims, & D. Farabee (Eds.), Treatment of drug offenders (pp. 149–171). New York: Springer. Harrison, L. D. (1995). The validity of self-reported data on drug use. Journal of Drug Issues, 25, 91–111. Hawken, A., & Kleiman, M. (2009). Managing drug involved probation- ers with swift and certain sanctions: Evaluating Hawaii’s HOPE [NCJRS no. 229023]. Washington DC: National Institute of Justice. Available at http://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/229023.pdf. Hoaken,P.N.S.,&Stewart,S.H.(2003).Drugsofabuseandtheelicitation of human aggressive behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1533–1554. Hser, Y. (1997). Self-reported drug use: Results of selected empirical investigations of validity. NIDA Research Monograph, 167, 320–343. Hubbard, R. L., Collins, J. J., Rachal, J. V., & Cavanaugh, E. R. (1988). The criminal justice client in drug abuse treatment. In C. G. Leukefeld & F. M. Tims (Eds.), Compulsory treatment of drug abuse: Research and clinical practice [NIDA Research Monograph No. 86] (pp. 57–80). Rockville, MD: NIDA. Inciardi, J. A. (Ed.) (1994). Screening and assessment for alcohol and other drug abuse among adults in the criminal justice system [Treatment Improvement Protocol Series No. 7]. Rockville, MD: Center for Substance Abuse Treatment, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Kaplan, J., & Weisberg, R. (1991). Criminal law. Boston, MA: Little Brown. Karberg, J. C., & James, D. J. (2005). Substance dependence, abuse, and treatment of jail inmates, 2002 [NCJ 209588]. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, U.S. Dept. of Justice. 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64827-Faust-Ch27.indd 648 5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH
  • 14. SUBSTANCE USE, ABUSE, AND DEPENDENCE • 649 138 137 136 135 134 133 132 131 130 129 128 127 126 125 124 123 122 121 120 119 118 117 116 115 114 113 112 111 110 109 108 107 106 105 104 103 102 101 100 99 98 97 96 95 94 93 92 91 90 89 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 60 59 58 57 56 55 54 53 52 51 50 49 48 47 46 45 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Khajawall, A. M., Erickson, T. B., & Simpson, G. M. (1982). Chronic phencyclidine abuse and physical assault. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1604–1606. Kinlock, T. W. (1991). Does phencylidine (PCP) use increase violent crime? Journal of Drug Issues, 21, 795–817. Kleiman, M. A. R., Tran, T. H., Fishbein, P., Magula, M., Allen, W., & Lacy, G. (2003). Opportunities and barriers in probation reform: A case study of drug testing and sanctions. Berkeley, CA: University of California, California Policy Research Center. Knopf, A. (2010). Diagnostic uncertainty: Changes in the proposed DSM-5 worry addiction counselors, research professionals. Addiction Professional, 8(5), 8–13. Kohrs, R., & Durieux M. E. (1998). Ketamine: Teaching an old drug new tricks. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 87, 1186–1193. Kumar, R., Mac, D. S., Gabrielli, W. F., & Goodwin, D. W. (1987). Anxiolitics and memory: A comparison of lorazepam and alprazolam. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 158–160. LaFave, W. R., & Scott, A. W. (1986). Substantive criminal law (2nd ed.). St. Paul, MN: West Publishing Co. Lamb, S., Greenlick, M. R., & McCarty, D. (Eds.). (1998). Bridging the gap between practice and research: Forging partnerships with community- based drug and alcohol treatment. Washington, DC: Institute of Medicine, National Academy of Sciences. Langan, P. A., & Levin, D. J. (2002). Recidivism of prisoners released in 1994. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, U.S. Department of Justice. Langenbucher, J., Martin, C. S., Labouvie, E., Sanjuan, P. M., Bavly, L., & Pollock, N. K. (2000). Toward the DSM-V: The withdrawal-gate model versus the DSM-IV in the diagnosis of alcohol abuse and depen- dence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 799–809. Lapham, S. C., C’de Baca, J., Chang, I., Hunt, W. C., & Berger, L. R. (2002). Are drunk-driving offenders referred for screening accurately reporting their drug use? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 66, 243–253. Latimer, J., Morton-Bourgon, K., & Chretien, J. (2006). A meta-analytic examination of drug treatment courts: Do they reduce recidivism? Canada Dept. of Justice, Research & Statistics Division. Layton, C. J. (1997). No more excuses: Closing the door on the voluntary intoxication defense. John Marshall Law Review, 30, 535–565. Lazowski, L. E., Miller, F. G., Boye, M. W., & Miller, G. A. (1998). Efficacy of the Substance Abuse Subtle Screening Inventory-3 (SASSI-3) in identifying substance dependence disorders in clinical settings. Journal of Personality Assessment, 71, 114–128. Lee, P. A., Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Cacciola, J. S., McNellis, J., Schepise, M. M., & McLellan, A. T. (2001). Did “Breaking the Cycle” (BTC) clients receive appropriate services? [abstract]. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63(Supp. 1), S89. Lindquist, C. H., Krebs, C. P., Warner, T. D., & Lattimore, P. K. (2009). An exploration of treatment and supervision intensity among drug court and non-drug court participants. Journal of Offender Reha- bilitation, 48, 167–193. Lowenkamp, C. T., Holsinger, A. M., & Latessa, E. J. (2005). Are drug courts effective? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Community Corrections, Fall, 5–28. Lu, N. T., Taylor, B. G., & Riley, K. J. (2000). The validity of adult arrestee self-reports of crack cocaine. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 27, 399–407. Lutze, F. E., & van Wormer, J. G. (2007). The nexus between drug and alcohol treatment program integrity and drug court effectiveness: Policy recommendations for pursuing success. Criminal Justice Policy Review, 18, 226–245. Mack, A. H., & Barros, M. (2008). Forensic addiction psychiatry. In M. Galanter & H. D. Kleber (Eds.), Textbook of substance abuse treatment (pp. 689–700). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. Maddux, J. F., & Desmond, D. P. (1981). Careers of opiate users. New York, NY: Praeger. Marlowe, D. B. (2002). Effective strategies for intervening with drug abus- ing offenders. Villanova Law Review, 47, 989–1025. Marlowe, D. B. (2003). Integrating substance abuse treatment and crimi- nal justice supervision. NIDA Science and Practice Perspectives, 2(1), 4–14. Marlowe, D. B., DeMatteo, D. S., & Festinger, D. S. (2003). A sober assessment of drug courts. Federal Sentencing Reporter, 16, 153–157. Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Dugosh, K. L., Arabia, P. L., & Kirby, K. C. (2008). An effectiveness trial of contingency management in a felony pre-adjudication drug court. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 565–577. Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Dugosh, K. L., Lee, P. A., & Benasutti, K. M. (2007). Adapting judicial supervision to the risk level of drug offenders: Discharge and six-month outcomes from a prospective matching study. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 88S, 4–13. Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., & Lee, P. A. (2003). The role of judicial status hearings in drug court. Offender Substance Abuse Report, 3, 33–46. Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., & Lee, P. A. (2004). The judge is a key component of drug court. Drug Court Review, 4, 1–34. Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Lee, P. A., Dugosh, K. L., & Benasutti, K. M. (2006). Matching judicial supervision to clients’ risk status in drug court. Crime & Delinquency, 52, 52–76. Marlowe, D. B., Festinger, D. S., Lee, P. A., Schepise, M. M., Hazzard, J. E. R., Merrill, J. C., Mulvaney, F. D., & McLellan, A. T. (2003). Are judicial status hearings a key component of drug court? During- treatment data from a randomized trial. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 141–162. Marlowe, D. B., Lambert, J. B., & Thompson, R. G. (1999). Voluntary intoxication and criminal responsibility. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 17, 195–217. Marlowe, D. B., Patapis, N. S., & DeMatteo, D. S. (2003). Amenability to treatment of drug offenders. Federal Probation, 67, 40–46. Martin, S. S., Butzin, C. A., Saum, S. A., & Inciardi, J. A. (1999). Three- year outcomes of therapeutic community treatment for drug-involved offenders in Delaware. Prison Journal, 79, 294–320. McDermott, P. A., Alterman, A. I., Brown, L., Zaballero, A., Snider, E. C., & McKay, J. R. (1996). Construct refinement and confirmation for the Addiction Severity Index. Psychological Assessment, 8, 182–189. McLellan A.T., Cacciola J., Kushner, H., Peters R., Smith I., & Pettinati, H. (1992). The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index: Cautions, additions and normative data. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 9, 461–480. McLellan, A. T., Carise, D., & Kleber, H. D. (2003). Can the national addiction treatment infrastructure support the public’s demand for quality care? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 25, 117–121. McLellan, A. T., Lewis, D. C., O’Brien, C. P., & Kleber, H. D. (2000). Drug dependence, a chronic medical illness: Implications for treat- ment, insurance, and outcomes evaluation. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 1689–1695. McLellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., Cacciola, J. C., Griffith, J., Evans, F., Barr, H. L., & O’Brien, C. P. (1985). New data from the Addiction Severity Index: Reliability and validity in three centers. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 412–423. McLellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., O’Brien, C. P., & Woody, G. E. (1980). An improved diagnostic instrument for substance abuse clients: The Addiction Severity Index. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 168, 26–33. McPherson, T. L., & Hersch, R. K. (2000). Brief substance abuse screen- inginstrumentsforprimarycaresettings:Areview. JournalofSubstance Abuse Treatment, 18, 193–202. Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (2007). Psychological evaluations for the courts: A handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. Messina, N. P., Wish, E. D., Nemes, S., & Wraight, B. (2000). Correlates of underreporting of post-discharge cocaine use among therapeutic community clients. Journal of Drug Issues, 30, 119–132. Mieczkowski, T., Barzelay, D., Gropper, B., & Wish, E. D. (1991). Concordance of three measures of cocaine use in an arrestee popula- tion: Hair, urine, and self-report. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 23, 241–246. 27-Faust-Ch27.indd 64927-Faust-Ch27.indd 649 5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM5/18/2011 8:24:02 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF, 19/05/2011, GLYPH