SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 3
Download to read offline
People Really Do Look Like Their Dogs, And Here's Why 
Dog owners look like their dogs. Or so people say. 
Take Huffington Post sports columnist Jordan Schultz (below), who says he and his wife, Breanna, 
are often told they're the spitting image of their pups Lexi and Zepplin. "People always say that," he 
said. "And I love it, huge compliment for sure." 
Whether or not you think the Schultzes look like their pets, it seems science is on their side on this 
one. 
Sadahiko Nakajima is a psychologist whose research on dog-owner resemblance has enjoyed a 
resurgence of interest in recent weeks thanks to coverage in Slate, the U.K.'s Daily Mail and other 
news outlets. Nakajima, a researcher at Japan's Kwansei Gakuin University, says there's evidence to 
support not just the notion that humans and their pet dogs look alike, but also why that's so. 
It's all in the eyes, he claims.
An experiment Nakajima conducted in 2009 showed that people were, at a rate significantly higher 
than chance, able to match dogs and their owners simply by looking at photographs of their faces. 
His findings were similar, he says, to those of previous studies. Taken together, he told The 
Huffington Post in an email, the evidence from his and other scientists' research shows that the 
popular belief in dog-owner physical resemblance is empirically valid. 
But the question as to why this was the case spurred Nakajima to conduct another experiment -- the 
results of which were published last year in the journal Anthrozoös -- that sought to determine if 
the pet-human resemblance could be traced to a specific facial feature. 
For the experiment, more than 500 people were shown two sets of photographs. One set showed 
pictures of real dog-owner pairs, while the other set had random pairings of people and dogs. The 
participants were randomly assigned to one of five different "masking" photo conditions, pictured 
below: no-mask (in which the human's and the dog's faces were unobstructed), eye-mask (the 
human's eyes were blacked out), mouth-mask (the human's mouth was blacked out), dog-eye-mask 
(the dog's eyes were blacked out), and eye-only (where just the eyes of the human and the dog could 
be seen). 
The participants were then asked to select the dog-owner pairs that physically resembled each 
other. 
The results of the experiment were fascinating. 
As in Nakajima's 2009 experiment, participants who were shown the unobstructed photos correctly 
identified the dog-owner pairs most of the time. (In this case, their accuracy rate was up to 80 
percent.) When the owners' mouths were concealed, participants were correct 73 percent of the 
time. 
But when the eyes of either the humans or the dogs were blacked out, the participants' accuracy fell
to the level of random chance -- around 50 percent either when the human's eyes were blacked out, 
or when the dog's were. 
When participants were shown only the eyes of the dog and the human, their accuracy rose to 74 
percent. 
It seems the eyes really do have it! 
The results "clearly show that individuals make decisions on dog-owner resemblance primarily by 
comparing features of the eye region between dogs and owners," Nakajima concluded in his paper. 
But why do people wind up looking like their dogs? 
Nakajima told HuffPost that "a major reason of the dog-owner 
facial resemblance is the so-called 'mere exposure 
effect,'" or the idea that a person might choose to get a dog 
who looks similar to themselves because of a preference 
for the familiar.

More Related Content

Viewers also liked

Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3
Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3
Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3LibreItalia
 
Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016
Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016
Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016LibreItalia
 
LibreOffice Writer Base
LibreOffice Writer BaseLibreOffice Writer Base
LibreOffice Writer BaseLibreItalia
 
Migrazione della ULSS 5 Veneto
Migrazione della ULSS 5 VenetoMigrazione della ULSS 5 Veneto
Migrazione della ULSS 5 VenetoLibreItalia
 
Perché scegliere LibreOffice
Perché scegliere LibreOfficePerché scegliere LibreOffice
Perché scegliere LibreOfficeLibreItalia
 
Internet e licenze copyleft per bambini
Internet e licenze copyleft per bambiniInternet e licenze copyleft per bambini
Internet e licenze copyleft per bambiniLibreItalia
 
Mobile payments - Short overview
Mobile payments - Short overviewMobile payments - Short overview
Mobile payments - Short overviewVolgamatics
 
Software Libero e LibreOffice
Software Libero e LibreOfficeSoftware Libero e LibreOffice
Software Libero e LibreOfficeLibreItalia
 
Pesticide application
Pesticide application Pesticide application
Pesticide application manisasatman
 
Cotton hybrid seed production
Cotton hybrid seed productionCotton hybrid seed production
Cotton hybrid seed productionmanisasatman
 

Viewers also liked (11)

Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3
Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3
Corso di LibreOffice per ECDL - Writer lezione 3
 
Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016
Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016
Italo Vignoli, LibreOffice e LibreItalia nel 2016
 
LibreOffice Writer Base
LibreOffice Writer BaseLibreOffice Writer Base
LibreOffice Writer Base
 
Migrazione della ULSS 5 Veneto
Migrazione della ULSS 5 VenetoMigrazione della ULSS 5 Veneto
Migrazione della ULSS 5 Veneto
 
Paul's CV
Paul's CVPaul's CV
Paul's CV
 
Perché scegliere LibreOffice
Perché scegliere LibreOfficePerché scegliere LibreOffice
Perché scegliere LibreOffice
 
Internet e licenze copyleft per bambini
Internet e licenze copyleft per bambiniInternet e licenze copyleft per bambini
Internet e licenze copyleft per bambini
 
Mobile payments - Short overview
Mobile payments - Short overviewMobile payments - Short overview
Mobile payments - Short overview
 
Software Libero e LibreOffice
Software Libero e LibreOfficeSoftware Libero e LibreOffice
Software Libero e LibreOffice
 
Pesticide application
Pesticide application Pesticide application
Pesticide application
 
Cotton hybrid seed production
Cotton hybrid seed productionCotton hybrid seed production
Cotton hybrid seed production
 

People Really Do Look Like Their Dogs, And Here's Why

  • 1. People Really Do Look Like Their Dogs, And Here's Why Dog owners look like their dogs. Or so people say. Take Huffington Post sports columnist Jordan Schultz (below), who says he and his wife, Breanna, are often told they're the spitting image of their pups Lexi and Zepplin. "People always say that," he said. "And I love it, huge compliment for sure." Whether or not you think the Schultzes look like their pets, it seems science is on their side on this one. Sadahiko Nakajima is a psychologist whose research on dog-owner resemblance has enjoyed a resurgence of interest in recent weeks thanks to coverage in Slate, the U.K.'s Daily Mail and other news outlets. Nakajima, a researcher at Japan's Kwansei Gakuin University, says there's evidence to support not just the notion that humans and their pet dogs look alike, but also why that's so. It's all in the eyes, he claims.
  • 2. An experiment Nakajima conducted in 2009 showed that people were, at a rate significantly higher than chance, able to match dogs and their owners simply by looking at photographs of their faces. His findings were similar, he says, to those of previous studies. Taken together, he told The Huffington Post in an email, the evidence from his and other scientists' research shows that the popular belief in dog-owner physical resemblance is empirically valid. But the question as to why this was the case spurred Nakajima to conduct another experiment -- the results of which were published last year in the journal Anthrozoös -- that sought to determine if the pet-human resemblance could be traced to a specific facial feature. For the experiment, more than 500 people were shown two sets of photographs. One set showed pictures of real dog-owner pairs, while the other set had random pairings of people and dogs. The participants were randomly assigned to one of five different "masking" photo conditions, pictured below: no-mask (in which the human's and the dog's faces were unobstructed), eye-mask (the human's eyes were blacked out), mouth-mask (the human's mouth was blacked out), dog-eye-mask (the dog's eyes were blacked out), and eye-only (where just the eyes of the human and the dog could be seen). The participants were then asked to select the dog-owner pairs that physically resembled each other. The results of the experiment were fascinating. As in Nakajima's 2009 experiment, participants who were shown the unobstructed photos correctly identified the dog-owner pairs most of the time. (In this case, their accuracy rate was up to 80 percent.) When the owners' mouths were concealed, participants were correct 73 percent of the time. But when the eyes of either the humans or the dogs were blacked out, the participants' accuracy fell
  • 3. to the level of random chance -- around 50 percent either when the human's eyes were blacked out, or when the dog's were. When participants were shown only the eyes of the dog and the human, their accuracy rose to 74 percent. It seems the eyes really do have it! The results "clearly show that individuals make decisions on dog-owner resemblance primarily by comparing features of the eye region between dogs and owners," Nakajima concluded in his paper. But why do people wind up looking like their dogs? Nakajima told HuffPost that "a major reason of the dog-owner facial resemblance is the so-called 'mere exposure effect,'" or the idea that a person might choose to get a dog who looks similar to themselves because of a preference for the familiar.