Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations

1,444 views

Published on

We conducted eye tracking experiments on naturalistic stereo images presented through a haploscope, and found that fixated
luminance contrast and luminance gradient were generally higher than randomly selected luminance contrast and luminance gradient, which agrees with previous literature. However we also found that the fixated disparity contrast and disparity gradient were generally lower than randomly selected disparity contrast and disparity gradient. We discuss the implications of this remarkable
result.

0 Comments
0 Likes
Statistics
Notes
  • Be the first to comment

  • Be the first to like this

No Downloads
Views
Total views
1,444
On SlideShare
0
From Embeds
0
Number of Embeds
1
Actions
Shares
0
Downloads
5
Comments
0
Likes
0
Embeds 0
No embeds

No notes for slide

Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations

  1. 1. Natural Scene Statistics at Stereo Fixations Yang Liu Lawrence K. Cormack Alan C. Bovik Department of ECE Department of Psychology Department of ECE Univeristy of Texas at Austin University of Texas at Austin University of Texas at Austin young76@mail.utexas.edu cormack@psy.utexas.edu bovik@ece.utexas.edu Abstract the adaptation of the visual system [Liu et al. 2008]. One reason for the lack of studies on fixations in 3D space has been the dearth We conducted eye tracking experiments on naturalistic stereo of 3D ground truth natural scene data. The few databases of natu- images presented through a haploscope, and found that fixated ralistic stereo images do not adequately fulfill this need, since luminance contrast and luminance gradient were generally higher what is needed are dense disparity maps for each scene. One very than randomly selected luminance contrast and luminance gra- recent study [Jansen et al., 2009] used natural scenes with ground dient, which agrees with previous literatures. However we also truth disparity map acquired from laser scanning. They found that found that the fixated disparity contrast and disparity gradient disparity appears to be a salient feature that affects eye move- were generally lower than randomly selected disparity contrast ments. In particular, they found that the presence of disparity and disparity gradient. We discuss the implications of this re- information may affect saccade length, but not duration; that dis- markable result. parity appears not to affect the saliency of luminance features; and that subjects tend to fixate nearer objects earlier than more dis- Keywords: fixation, stereopsis, natural scene statistics tance objects. A reasonable agreement with intuition may be found in each of the results. The authors also found that in 3D noise images, subjects tended to fixate depth discontinuities more 1 Introduction / Overview frequently than smooth depth regions. One might view this result as intuitive also; as with luminance images, such locations might Many studies have shown that several low level luminance fea- be deemed interesting. tures at human fixations are significantly different from those at other areas. Reinagel and Zador [1999] found that the regions 2 Stereo Eyetracking around human fixations tend to higher spatial contrasts and spatial entropies than random fixation regions, which suggests that the human visual system may try to select image regions that help 2.1 Observers maximize the information content transmitted to the visual cortex, by minimizing the redundancy in the image representation. By Three male observers participated in this study. Their stereo abili- varying the patch sizes around the fixations, Parkhurst and Niebur ty was tested to be normal by presenting them random dot stereo- [2003] found that the largest difference between the luminance grams. Observer LKC has extensive experience in psychophysical contrast of fixated regions and that of image shuffled (pseudo- studies, and knew the purpose of the experiment. Observers JSL random) regions is observed when the patch size is 1 degree. In a and CHY were naïve. recent study [Rajashekar et al. 2007], fixated image patches were foveated using an eccentricity-based model. Higher order statis- 2.2 Stimulus tics were analyzed than in prior studies. They found that bandpass contrast showed a notably larger difference between fixations and We manually selected 48 grayscale stereoscopic outdoor scenes random patches than other higher-order statistics. Other features [Hoyer & Hyvärinen, 2000] that contained mountains, trees, wa- found to attract fixations (in decreasing order of attractiveness) ter, rocks, bushes, etc., but avoided manmade objects. We didn’t included bandpass luminance, RMS contrast, and luminance. This have the ground truth disparity data from these scenes. Instead, data was subsequently used to develop an image processing “fixa- we relied on a local correlation method which is simple yet bio- tion predictor” [Rajashekar et al. 2008]. logical inspired to solve this issue. Models of binocular complex neurons [Anzai et al. 1999; Fleet et al. 1996; Ohzawa et al. 1990; The body of literature on visual fixations on 2D luminance images Qian, 1994] commonly contain a cross correlation term in their appears to be largely consistent across the studies. However, there response function. has been very little work done on analyzing the nature or statistics of images and scenes at the point of gaze in three dimensions. The simple correspondence algorithm was defined as follows. Certainly the three dimensional attributes of the world affect the Given a pixel (xr,yr) in the right image, we defined a 161×5 way we interact with it both visually and physically. Moreover, (3.2°×0.1°) search window centered on the same pixel location the 3D statistics of the natural world have likely played a role in (preferring zero disparity) in the left image. Given a 1°×1° patch in the right image centered on the pixel (xr,yr), the algorithm com- puted the cross correlation between the right patch and a candi- date 1°×1° left patch centered on each pixel in the 161×5 search Copyright © 2010 by the Association for Computing Machinery, Inc. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or window. The left patch yielding the largest cross correlation was classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed deemed to be the matched patch. The location (xl, yl) correspond- for commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the ing the center of the patch was deemed the matched pixel for the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be right pixel (xr,yr), hence the horizontal disparity of (xr,yr) was honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. taken to be D(xr,yr) = xr-xl. Request permissions from Permissions Dept, ACM Inc., fax +1 (212) 869-0481 or e-mail permissions@acm.org. ETRA 2010, Austin, TX, March 22 – 24, 2010. © 2010 ACM 978-1-60558-994-7/10/0003 $10.00 161
  2. 2. It could be argued that, since we are only interested in local scene centers of both monitors to help the observers to fuse before the statistics, why not record the movements of both eyes, and use the presentation of the next stereo pair. disparity between the recorded left and right fixations to find the correct match? There are several reasons why this wasn’t practic- This calibration routine was repeated compulsorily every 10 im- al. First, there are fixation disparities that occur between the right ages, and a calibration test run every 5 images. This was achieved eye and the left eyes. Most people have a fixation disparity that is by requiring that the observer fixate for 500ms within a 5s time less than 6 arcmin, but can be as large as 20 arcmin with peripher- limit on a central square region (0.3◦ ×0.3◦) prior to progressing to al visual targets [Wick, 1985]. When fixation disparity occurs, the next image in the stimulus collection. If the calibration had the image of an object point that a person is trying to fixate do not drifted, the observer would be unable to satisfy this test, and the fall on exactly corresponding points. Secondly, each Purkinje eye full calibration procedure was re-run. tracker has an accuracy about 7 acrmin (the median offset from real fixations); hence the error between two eye trackers is about Observers who became uncomfortable during the experiment 14 arcmin, which corresponds to about 12 pixels. This is quite a were allowed to take a break of any duration they desired. large error, considering that the stereo images are only 800×600. Thirdly, the fixation detection algorithm (associated with eye- The ambient illumination in the experiment room was kept con- tracking) of the two eye paths can also introduce unwanted noise stant for all observers, with a minimum of 5 minutes luminance into the corresponding fixation locations; registered stereoscopic adaptation provided while the eye-tracker was calibrated. eyetracking is difficult to accomplish in practice. Lastly, since we are interested in disparity features within neighborhoods (not just points) of the fixations, a dense disparity map is required for all points in the neighborhood. Hence, local processing of the type that our disparity algorithm accomplishes would be required any- way. 2.3 Equipments Stereo images were displayed on two 17 inch, gamma calibrated Luminance Disparity monitors. The distance between the monitors and the observer was 124 cm. Each monitor’s screen resolution was set at 800×600 pixels, corresponding to about 50 pixels per degree of visual an- gle. The total spatial extent of each display was thus about 16◦ × 12◦ of visual angle. A haploscope was placed between the two monitors and the ob- servers to completely separate the displays from the left and right Luminance gradient Disparity gradient monitor. Eye movements were recorded by a SRI Generation V Dual Purkinje eye tracker. This eye tracker has an accuracy of < 10′ of arc, a response time of under 1 ms, and bandwidth of DC to > 400Hz. The output of the eye tracker (horizontal and vertical eye position signals) was low-pass filtered in the hardware and then sampled at 200 hHz by a National Instruments data acquisi- tion board in a Pentium IV host computer, where the data were stored for offline data analysis. The observers used a bite bar and Luminance contrast Disparity contrast a forehead rest to restrict their head movements. A viewing session was composed of 48 viewed stereo image Figure 1. The luminance and disparity features pairs. At the beginning of each session, a 0.3°×0.3° crosshair was displayed on the centers of both monitors to help the observers to fuse by fixating on it. When the correct binocular fixation (the crosshair was perceived single) was achieved, the observer 3 Analysis pressed a button to start the calibration. Two 3×3 calibration grids were displayed on the monitors respectively. After the observers 3.1 Computation of scene statistics visited all 9 dots, a linear interpolation was then done to establish the transformation between the output voltages of the eye tracker Denote the right image as I, and the dense disparity map as D We and the position of the subject’s gaze on each computer display. computed the luminance gradient map: The calibration also accounted for crosstalk between the horizon- tal and vertical voltage measurements. After correct calibration, a 0.3°×0.3° crosshair was displayed on the centers to force all ob- servers to start from the same center position. The stereo images were displayed on two monitors for 10 seconds during which the eye movements were recorded. Between two consecutive image and the disparity gradient map: pairs, two identical Gaussian noise images were displayed for 3 seconds on both monitors to help suppress after-images corres- ponding to the previous stereo pairs that may otherwise have at- tracted fixations. Then a 0.3°×0.3° crosshair was displayed on the 162
  3. 3. using the Matlab function gradient(X). Here we define luminance ∑ , contrast as the RMS contrast of a luminance patch: where , is the location of the fixation. ∑ , We also computed the mean patch gradient of the random loca- tions the image as: and likewise, disparity contrast as ∑ , ∑ where , is the location of the random location. which is the standard deviation of a disparity patch. All of the For each image, we then defined the fixation-to-random lumin- following analysis is based on these four scene features: lumin- ance contrast ratio / and the fixation-to-random lu- minance gradient ratio / . If 1, it means that ance contrast, disparity contrast, luminance gradient, and disparity the fixated patches generally have a larger luminance contrast gradient. Figure 1 depicts these scene features for one example than randomly selected patches on the image being considered. If stereo pair. 1, then the meaning is reversed. The same meaning applies to the luminance gradient ratio. 3.2 Random locations The same analysis method that was used on the luminance con- Suppose an observer made fixations for the image. Then, trast and luminance gradient was also applied to for the analysis the total number of fixations that the observer made during a ses- of disparity. We calculated the mean disparity contrast on the sion is ∑ . We assume that a random observer also made the fixated patches, and the same quantity on the randomly se- same number of fixations as the subject did. That is, for the lected patches. The ratio of disparity contrast between the fixated image, the random observer selected fixations uniformly distri- patches and the randomly selected patches is defined as buted on the image plane too. For each human observer, we as- / . sume that there are 100 random observers each making the same number of fixations in each image as the human observer. For The mean patch disparity gradient at the fixated patches ( ) and example, the overall fixation that subject LKC made is 486 fixa- the randomly selected patches ( was also calculated. The ratio tions in 48 images, so each random observer selected 486 random of the disparity gradient between the fixated patches and the ran- locations too. The total number of random locations is 48,600. We dom patches is defined as / . If the ratios are signif- wanted to know whether or not there is a statistically significant icantly greater than 1, then fixated patches tend to have a larger difference between image features at fixations and those at ran- disparity contrast and gradient than randomly picked locations. domly selected locations by comparing the human observer’s data and the 100 random observers’ data. 3.3 Fixation/Random ratios 1.1 For the image, we computed the mean luminance contrast at the fixations as: 1 Ratios ∑ , 0.9 where , is the location of the fixation, and is the lu- minance contrast map. 0.8 We also computed the mean luminance contrast at the random locations as: 0.7 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 ∑ , Patch size (deg) Figure 2. Mean luminance gradient ratios (red), and where , is the location of the random location. mean disparity gradient ratios (blue) of three observers. We defined patch luminance gradient as the mean gradient of the patch: We ran 100 simulations for each image, and plotted the mean ∑ , ⁄ , luminance gradient ratio with 95% confidence intervals (CI), and the mean disparity gradient ratio with 95% CIs, for all where is the luminance gradient map. Similarly, we computed subjects as shown in Figure 2. For better comparison, we plotted 1 the mean patch luminance gradient of the fixations for the as a straight horizontal line across all patch sizes. The red curves image as: show the ratios of luminance gradient, and the blue curves showed the ratios of disparity gradients. Different markers were used to represent the observers: LKC (*), CHY (o), JSL (∆). We made a 163
  4. 4. similar ensemble comparison plot for the mean luminance con- References trast ratio and mean disparity contrast ratio, as displayed in Figure 3. It is clear that all the mean ratios of luminance gradient/contrast ANZAI, A., OHZAWA, I., AND FREEMAN, R. D. 1999. Neural Me- and their 95% CIs fall well above 1, while the mean ratios of dis- chanisms for Processing Binocular Information II. Complex Cells. parity gradient/contrast fell well below 1. All of the results we J Neurophysiol, 82, 2, 909–924. obtained lead to the conclusion that humans tend to fixate at re- gions of higher luminance variation, and lower disparity variation. BACKUS, B. T., FLEET, D. J., PARKER, A. J., & HEEGER, D. J. 2001. Human Cortical Activity Correlates With Stereoscopic Depth 1.3 Perception. J Neurophysiol, 86, 4, 2054-2068. 1.2 FLEET D. J., WAGNER H., AND HEEGER D. J. 1996. Neural Encod- ing of Binocular Disparity: Energy Models, Position Shifts and Ratios Phase Shifts. Vision Research, 36, 1839-1857. 1.1 GEORGIEVA, S., PEETERS, R., KOLSTER, H., TODD, J. T., & ORBAN, 1 G. A. 2009. The Processing of Three-Dimensional Shape from Disparity in the Human Brain. J. Neurosci., 29, 3, 727-742. 0.9 HOYER, P. O., AND HYVÄRINEN, A. 2000. Independent Component Analysis Applied to Feature Extraction from Colour and Stereo 0.8 Images . Network Computation in Neural Systems, 11, 191--210. 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 JANSEN, L., ONAT, S., AND KÖNIG, P. 2009. Influence of Disparity Patch size (deg) on Fixation and Saccades in Free Viewing of Natural Scenes. Journal of Vision 9, 1, 1-19. Figure 3. Mean luminance contrast ratios (red), and mean disparity contrast ratios (blue) of three observers. LIU, Y., BOVIK, A. C., AND CORMACK, L. K. 2008. Disparity Sta- tistics in Natural Scenes. Journal of Vision 8, 11, 1-14. 4 Discussion OHZAWA, I., DEANGELIS, G., AND FREEMAN, R. 1990. Stereoscop- The most immediate explanation for this behavioral phenomenon ic Depth Discrimination in The Visual Cortex: Neurons Ideally is that the binocular vision system actively seeks to fixate scene Suited as Disparity Detectors. Science, 249, 4972, 1037-1041. points that will simplify or enhance stereoscopic perception. The nature of this enhancement is less clear. We suggest that the bino- PARKHURST, D. J., AND NIEBUR, E. 2003. Scene Content selected cular visual system, unless directed otherwise by a higher-level by active vision. Spatial Vision 16, 2, 125-154. mediator, seeks fixations that simplify the computational process of disparity and depth calculation. In particular, we propose that QIAN, N. 1994. Computing Stereo Disparity and Motion with the binocular vision system seeks to avoid, when possible, regions Known Binocular Cell Properties. Neural Computation, 6, 3, 390- where the disparity computations are complicated by missing 404. information, such as occlusions, or rapid changes in disparity, which may be harder to resolve. RAJASHEKAR, U., VAN DER LINDE, I., BOVIK, AND CORMACK, L. K.. 2007. Foveated Analysis of Image Features at Fixations. Vision Many fMRI studies have reported that the V1 activity increases Research 47, 25, 3160-3172. with disparity variation [Tootell et al. 1988; Backus et al. 2001]. Georgieva et al. [2009] showed that activity in occipital cortex RAJASHEKAR, U., VAN DER LINDE, I., BOVIK, A. C., AND CORMACK, and ventral IPS, at the edges of the V3A complex was correlated L. K. 2008. GAFFE: A Gaze-attentive Fixation Finding Engine. with the amplitude of the disparity variation that subjects per- IEEE Transactions on Image Processing 17, 4, 564-573. ceived in the stimuli. All of these studies show that the processing of large, complex disparity shapes involves the allocation of more REINAGEL, P., AND ZADOR, A. M. 1999. Natural Scene Statistics at neuronal resources and energy than do smooth disparity fields. the Center of Gaze. Networks 10, 4, 341-350. Another (related) possibility is that when viewing an object, ob- TOOTELL, R. B., HAMILTON, S. L., SILVERMAN, M. S., & SWITKES, servers tend to fixate towards the center of the object, leaving the E. 1988. Functional Anatomy of Macaque Striate Cortex. I. Ocu- object boundaries (often associated with depth discontinuities) to lar Dominance, Binocular Interactions, and Baseline Conditions. peripheral processing involving larger receptive fields both in J. Neurosci., 8, 5, 1500-1530. space and disparity. Such a strategy would allow the fovea to operate under better-posed stereo viewing conditions, and to WICK, B. 1985. Forced Vergence Fixation Disparity Curves at process 3D surface detail, while the periphery could simulta- Distance and Near in an Asymptomatic Young Adult Population. neously encode the (statistically) large disparities associated with American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 62, 9, object boundaries. 591-599. 164

×