SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 8
Download to read offline
Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389                   brill.nl/jocc




    The Zeus Problem: Why Representational Content
          Biases Cannot Explain Faith in Gods


                           Will M. Gervais* and Joseph Henrich
                  Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,
                     2136 West Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T1Z4
                     * Corresponding author, e-mail: will@psych.ubc.ca



Abstract
In a recent article, Barrett (2008) argued that a collection of five representational content features
can explain both why people believe in God and why people do not believe in Santa Claus or
Mickey Mouse. In this model – and within the cognitive science of religion as a whole – it is
argued that representational content biases are central to belief. In the present paper, we challenge
the notion that representational content biases can explain the epidemiology of belief. Instead,
we propose that representational content biases might explain why some concepts become
widespread, but that context biases in cultural transmission are necessary to explain why people
come to believe in some counterintuitive agents rather than others. Many supernatural agents,
including those worshipped by other cultural groups, meet Barrett’s criteria. Nevertheless, people
do not come to believe in the gods of their neighbors. This raises a new challenge for the cognitive
science of religion: the Zeus Problem. Zeus contains all of the features of successful gods, and
was once a target for widespread belief, worship, and commitment. But Zeus is no longer a target
for widespread belief and commitment, despite having the requisite content to fulfill Barrett’s
criteria. We analyze Santa Claus, God, and Zeus with both content and context biases, finding
that context – not content – explains belief. We argue that a successful cognitive science of
religious belief needs to move beyond simplistic notions of cultural evolution that only include
representational content biases.

Keywords
Religious beliefs, cultural transmission, cognitive science of religion, god concepts



The canonical approach to the cognitive science of religion has illuminated
many of the features that make religious concepts prevalent across cultures
(Boyer, 2001; Atran and Norenzayan, 2004; Barrett, 2004). In particular,
researchers have focused on supernatural concepts that systematically violate
ontological intuitions. These minimally counterintuitive concepts enjoy
enhanced memorability – a bias that could have important effects when
applied over successive generations (Barrett and Nyhof, 2001; Boyer and

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2010                                DOI: 10.1163/156853710X531249
384   W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389

Ramble, 2001; Norenzayan et al., 2006). The Mickey Mouse problem (we
have not cited anyone for this handy label because when we systematically
pursued its origins, we found contradictory perspectives from trusted sources)
challenges the relative importance of minimal counterintuitiveness to the
explanation of religion because there may be many minimally counterintuitive
concepts in our cultural worlds, such as Mickey Mouse and Santa Claus, that
do not inspire the faith and deep commitments associated with religious rep-
resentations.
   Addressing this challenge, Barrett (2008) outlines five features that – he
argues – combine to produce ideal representations for achieving godly status.
First, successful god candidates attract attention because they violate a few
routine ontological assumptions. That is, they are minimally counterintuitive.
Second, they represent intentional agents. Minimally counterintuitive agents
(such as talking potatoes) may be more inferentially potent than are similar
non-agent concepts (such as invisible potatoes). Third, these agents possess
strategic information (Boyer, 2001) that makes them relevant to peoples’ lives.
Fourth, successful god representations are described as having detectable inter-
actions with our world. Finally, successful god candidates have representa-
tional content that motivates ritual practice that bolsters belief (Henrich,
2009). Barrett analyzed Santa Claus according to this representational content
bias framework and argued that although Santa is a more suitable candidate
for belief than many other counterintuitive agents, Santa ultimately fails
because he lacks some of the content crucial to recruit the belief and commit-
ment that is accorded to gods.
   Two of Barrett’s criteria that purportedly produce belief apparently presup-
pose some degree of belief before they could operate on the epidemiology of
representations. To our knowledge, there is no evidence that people who do
not believe an agent actually exists increase their degree of belief when they
learn that the agent is portrayed as holding strategic information, or as detect-
ably interacting with the world. Even children show a sophisticated under-
standing of the reality-fiction distinction (Morison and Gardener, 1978) and
can reason about the properties, thoughts, and abilities of fictional agents
(e.g., Taylor, 1999; Skolnick and Bloom, 2006), without coming to believe in
their actual existence. Believers might get even more interested in their favored
counterintuitive agents when they learn that these agents hold strategic infor-
mation and impact the world, but if Christians learn that Zeus knows about
upcoming storms (strategic information), or throws lightning bolts (detect-
able action), will their Zeus belief increase? This claim awaits support. We also
note that many religions possess distant high gods that do not interact in the
world (Swanson, 1960), so “detectable interactions” fail to describe some gods
that actually recruit belief.
W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 385

   We present a new challenge to this framework, the Zeus Problem, and pro-
pose that the cognitive science of religion needs to take seriously an existing
framework that looks more broadly at the cognitive and evolutionary founda-
tions of culture, and integrate beyond explanations based purely on represen-
tational content. The Zeus Problem implies that explanations invoking only
representational content cannot explain why people place faith in some god
concepts but not others, such as the gods of neighboring or intermixed cul-
tures. In closing, we apply our framework to patterns of belief in three super-
natural agents: Santa Claus, God and Zeus.
   Barrett (2008) meets the challenge of Santa and Mickey by arguing that a
cognitive “sweet spot” exists for candidate god concepts that lies at the inter-
section of counterintuition, agency, strategic information, verifiable activity,
and action motivation. This confluence of representational content features
may partially explain what sets culturally successful gods apart from Mickey
Mouse and Santa Claus, but we wonder how Barrett could explain why people
do not believe in other peoples’ gods? Depending on your background, these
“other gods” might include Zeus, Yahweh, Baal, Thor, Ganesha, Papa Gede, or
ancestor spirits. These counterintuitive agents must hit Barrett’s sweet spot,
because they all held thousands of adherents for centuries and were gloried in
rituals and art. Many of them still do. If they miss the sweet spot, Barrett has
a bigger problem because then his explanation would be confined to a particu-
lar subset of the deities that actually inspire belief. Our question is: Why
doesn’t the representational content of these gods instill faith in those who
hold the representational content, outside of their respective eras and cultural
milieus?
   Barrett’s “Cognitive science of religion” fails to recognize that there is more
to cultural evolution than merely representational content. In contrast, evolu-
tionary approaches to cognition and culture have long emphasized both
content-based learning and context-based learning (Boyd and Richerson,
1985; Henrich and McElreath, 2003). Content-based mechanisms or biases
arise from the interaction of mental representations with our cognitive machin-
ery. Such interactions may affect a representation’s memorability, transmissi-
bility, believability, or intra-psychic transformations (Henrich, forthcoming).
Counterintuitiveness is an example of a content-based memory bias (Atran
and Norenzayan, 2004).
   In contrast, context biases deal with the sources of mental representations
and their integration. Evolutionary approaches suggest that learners should
attend to cues of prestige, success, skill, age, sex, ethnic membership, health,
and self-similarity in figuring out whom to learn from, or how to weight cul-
tural information from diverse sources (Henrich and Gil-White, 2001). Learn-
ers should also weight the frequency of different beliefs or behaviors using
386   W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389

conformist transmission (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Henrich and Boyd,
1998). Learners should be especially sensitive to actions diagnostic of a mod-
el’s degree of commitment to expressed beliefs, termed credibility enhancing
displays. Rituals, for example, may have culturally evolved, in part, to exploit
our evolved context biases for cultural learning to more effectively display
commitment (Henrich, forthcoming).
    Content biases may explain why both religious beliefs and folk tales involve
similar content, but context biases may be required to determine why people
believe the former rather than the latter, or to determine which candidate god
concepts are believed to exist in a given cultural context. Context biases might
explain why a child raised in a Catholic village probably believes in the Trinity,
but not in the Trident of Poseidon. We think context-based cultural learning
better explains why people (from some societies) are more likely to have faith
in God than in Zeus or Santa.
    This illuminates an issue that the “Cognitive science of religion” has not
paid sufficient attention to: the difference between memory and recall on the
one hand and belief, faith, or commitment on the other hand. The experi-
ments so often pointed to as evidence for the importance of counterintuitive-
ness only reveal memory and recall effects (e.g., Barrett and Nyhof, 2001;
Norenzayan et al., 2006), which tell us nothing about belief. You can recall
something without believing in it or placing deep faith in it. So, do the con-
tent-based mechanisms of the cognitive science of religion tell us anything
about the origin of faith?
    Our evolutionary approach suggests that humans ought to have psycho-
logical mechanisms that (1) make counterintuitive representations more
memorable (i.e., take note of unusual stuff) and (2) create some immunity
(skepticism) against believing such representations without input from other
cultural learning mechanism (Henrich, forthcoming). That is, concepts that
systematically deviate from intuitive expectations may actually be less believ-
able than are more intuitive concepts. In one experiment (Harris et al., 2006),
children were asked whether a variety of different entities exist. The children
reported that a variety of empirically non-verifiable scientific entities such as
germs exist, and asserted the existence of endorsed beings like Santa Claus.
Although this appears to indicate that the children came to believe in counter-
intuitive agents like Santa Claus, children were more confident that scientific
entities exist than that endorsed beings exist. These authors argue that children
might in part be more skeptical of the endorsed beings simply because these
beings violate intuitive expectations, leading the children to “conclude that
the existence of special beings such as God or Santa Claus is more dubious
than that of scientific entities” (p. 92). Bloom and Weisberg (2007) further
W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 387

argue that skepticism of concepts that violate intuitive assumptions has pro-
found effects for our understanding of the world as adults; science education
in part is successful if it helps people override some of their intuitions. It is
unclear how similar processes would produce both (1) belief in religious agents
that violate intuitions, and (2) resistance to scientific concepts that violate
intuitions. We think a more likely explanation is that people adhere to those
non-intuitive beliefs expressed and demonstrated by members of their cultural
milieu, and express skepticism in non-intuitive beliefs that stand in opposition
to those they have adopted via context-based cultural learning.


Santa, God and Zeus: A Preliminary Examination of Context Biases

A cognitive and evolutionary approach to religious belief needs to explain why
so many adults believe in God (today), but so few believe in Zeus and Santa
Claus. Barrett (2008) argues that content biases adequately explain disbelief in
Santa Claus. It is puzzling, however, that belief in Santa Claus wanes during
late childhood even though Santa’s described content does not change. Per-
haps context biases can resolve this dilemma. During early childhood, con-
formist biases may reinforce belief in Santa Claus, as a child will find that his
or her peers report similar experiences with Santa Claus. Prestigious individu-
als in the child’s environment also evince belief in Santa Claus. Mom and dad
leave out cookies and milk as a credibility enhancing display of their own
Santa belief. At school, children are bombarded with testimonials about
Santa.
   As children age, their peers and parents likely cease their credibility enhanc-
ing displays. They might tell a child outright that Santa Claus is a fabrication.
A change in content does not precipitate skepticism regarding Santa Claus;
instead, belief in Santa Claus declines in concert with the decline of contextual
cues that others believe in Santa Claus. Bolstered by the appropriate context
biases, Santa Claus is a successful god among children, but a popular folktale
for the rest of us. Furthermore, we do not think that Santa is much different
along Barrett’s five dimensions than many a demi- or ancestor god in small-
scale societies.
   What, then, of God and Zeus? Both are successful god candidates in
Barrett’s (2008) content bias framework. Both even inspired widespread belief
and commitment in their own eras. Why is belief in God a powerful force in
the world today, and discussion of Zeus is relegated to mythology classes? In
ancient Greece, conformist biases, prestige biases and credibility enhancing
displays supported belief in Zeus, which was both widespread and popular
388    W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389

among the elites. Ritual practices (such as animal sacrifice) and monumental
architecture (such as the Temple of Zeus at Olympia) would have served as
credibility enhancing displays of cultural models’ genuine commitment to and
belief in Zeus, further bolstering belief among cultural learners. These factors
do not persist today for Zeus, although they do for God.
   Supernatural concepts with the right representational content become cul-
turally widespread and persistent because they recruit attention and are mem-
orable, but a core assumption of the canonical version of the cognitive science
of religion is that these concepts, or at least some subset of them, have content
that recruits belief and commitment. We know of no support for this claim.
Representational content biases may describe why some concepts become
widespread, but do not adequately explain commitment.


Conclusion
We propose that any model of religious belief needs to solve both the Mickey
Mouse problem (why are some counterintuitive concepts believed in?) and the
Zeus Problem (why do people not believe in other peoples’ gods?). Represen-
tational content biases are potentially powerful forces in cultural transmission,
and may explain why some concepts become widespread and culturally recur-
rent. However, it is far from clear how any combination of such content-based
mechanisms can explain the epidemiology of commitment. It is our hope that
cognitive science of religion will begin to draw more broadly on the available
theoretical tools of integrative evolutionary approaches to culture.


References
Atran, S. and Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, com-
  mitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27, 713-770.
Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God? AltaMira Press, Walnut Creek, CA.
——. (2008). Why Santa Claus is not a god. Journal of Cognition and Culture 8, 149-161.
Barrett, J. L. and Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading nonnatural concepts: The role of intuitive
  conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition
  and Culture 1, 69-100.
Bloom, P. and Weisberg, D. S. (2007). Childhood origins of adult resistance to science. Science
  316, 996-997.
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago
  Press, Chicago, IL.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books,
  New York, NY.
Boyer, P. and Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural
  evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science 25, 535-564.
W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 389

Harris, P. L., Pasquini, E. S., Duke, S., Asscher, J. J. and Pons, F. (2006). Germs and angels: The
  role of testimony in young children’s ontology. Developmental Science 9, 76-96.
Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Credibility
  enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human
  Behavior 30, 244-260.
Henrich, J. and Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence
  of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior 19, 215-242.
Henrich, J. and Gil-White, F. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a
  mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behav-
  ior 22, 165-196.
Henrich, J., and McElreath, R. (2003). The Evolution of Cultural Evolution. Evolutionary
  Anthropology 12, 123-135.
Morison, P. and Gardener, H. (1978). Dragons and dinosaurs: The child’s capacity to differenti-
  ate fantasy from reality. Child Development 49, 642-648.
Norenzayan, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J. and Schaller, M. (2006). Memory and mystery: The
  cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science 30, 531-553.
Skolnick, D. and Bloom, P. (2006). What does Batman think about SpongeBob? Children’s
  understanding of the fantasy/fantasy distinction. Cognition 101, B9-B18.
Swanson, G. E. (1960). The birth of the gods. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.
Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children who create them. Oxford University
  Press, New York, NY.
Zeus problem   why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods (gervais & henrich 2010)

More Related Content

Similar to Zeus problem why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods (gervais & henrich 2010)

Rel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docx
Rel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docxRel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docx
Rel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docxhennela
 
Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?
Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?
Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?Reginald V. Finley Sr. M.Ed.
 
The questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docx
The questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docxThe questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docx
The questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docxKomlin1
 
capstone presentation.pptx
capstone presentation.pptxcapstone presentation.pptx
capstone presentation.pptxDJKerns
 
Faith and Science.pptx
Faith and Science.pptxFaith and Science.pptx
Faith and Science.pptxCaleb450631
 
Narcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religious
Narcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religiousNarcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religious
Narcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religiousLex Pit
 
Culture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docx
Culture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docxCulture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docx
Culture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docxfaithxdunce63732
 
1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE .docx
1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE  .docx1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE  .docx
1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE .docxvickeryr87
 

Similar to Zeus problem why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods (gervais & henrich 2010) (15)

Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
 
Rel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docx
Rel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docxRel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docx
Rel 376 Spring, 2017. Exam #2.  Worth up to 100 pointsThe ques.docx
 
Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god cross-cultural evidenc...
Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god   cross-cultural evidenc...Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god   cross-cultural evidenc...
Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god cross-cultural evidenc...
 
Spirituality
SpiritualitySpirituality
Spirituality
 
R&G21st
R&G21stR&G21st
R&G21st
 
Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?
Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?
Biblical Inerrancy: A Barrier to The Public Acceptance of Science?
 
The questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docx
The questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docxThe questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docx
The questions for this exam are intended to give you a chance to mak.docx
 
capstone presentation.pptx
capstone presentation.pptxcapstone presentation.pptx
capstone presentation.pptx
 
Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...
Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...
Divine intuition — cognitive style influences belief in god (shenhav et al. 2...
 
Faith and Science.pptx
Faith and Science.pptxFaith and Science.pptx
Faith and Science.pptx
 
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information reply to ...
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information   reply to ...Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information   reply to ...
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information reply to ...
 
Theistic percepts in other species can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
Theistic percepts in other species   can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...Theistic percepts in other species   can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
Theistic percepts in other species can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
 
Narcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religious
Narcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religiousNarcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religious
Narcissism impairs ethical judgment even among the highly religious
 
Culture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docx
Culture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docxCulture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docx
Culture and StrategyAn organization’s culture can be defined as .docx
 
1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE .docx
1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE  .docx1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE  .docx
1THE BASIC QUESTIONSCIENCE OR RELIGION, OR SCIENCE .docx
 

More from SPK División Gráfica Digital de Surpack S.A.

More from SPK División Gráfica Digital de Surpack S.A. (20)

Religion explained (boyer 2001) ----- complete book
Religion explained (boyer 2001)  ----- complete bookReligion explained (boyer 2001)  ----- complete book
Religion explained (boyer 2001) ----- complete book
 
In gods we trust the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
In gods we trust   the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...In gods we trust   the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
In gods we trust the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
 
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995) ----- complete book
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995)  ----- complete bookFaces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995)  ----- complete book
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995) ----- complete book
 
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
 
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
 
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
 
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
 
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
 
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
 
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
 
The sense of agency and the illusion of the self
The sense of agency and the illusion of the selfThe sense of agency and the illusion of the self
The sense of agency and the illusion of the self
 
The power of charisma perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
The power of charisma   perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...The power of charisma   perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
The power of charisma perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
 
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
 
The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)
The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)
The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)
 
Supernaturalizing social life religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
Supernaturalizing social life   religion and the evolution of human cooperati...Supernaturalizing social life   religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
Supernaturalizing social life religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
 
Supernatural agents why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)
Supernatural agents   why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)Supernatural agents   why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)
Supernatural agents why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)
 
Supernatural agents citas
Supernatural agents   citasSupernatural agents   citas
Supernatural agents citas
 
Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)
Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)
Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)
 
Science and god an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...
Science and god   an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...Science and god   an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...
Science and god an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...
 
Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)
Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)
Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)
 

Recently uploaded

Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxUse of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack
"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack
"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek SchlawackFwdays
 
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr BaganFwdays
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024BookNet Canada
 
Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!
Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!
Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!Manik S Magar
 
Generative AI for Technical Writer or Information Developers
Generative AI for Technical Writer or Information DevelopersGenerative AI for Technical Writer or Information Developers
Generative AI for Technical Writer or Information DevelopersRaghuram Pandurangan
 
Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365
Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365
Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 3652toLead Limited
 
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine TuningDSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine TuningLars Bell
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024BookNet Canada
 
Sample pptx for embedding into website for demo
Sample pptx for embedding into website for demoSample pptx for embedding into website for demo
Sample pptx for embedding into website for demoHarshalMandlekar2
 
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024BookNet Canada
 
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfUnraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfAlex Barbosa Coqueiro
 
What is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdf
What is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdfWhat is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdf
What is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdfMounikaPolabathina
 
DevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache Maven
DevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache MavenDevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache Maven
DevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache MavenHervé Boutemy
 
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQLDeveloper Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQLScyllaDB
 
Advanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An IntroductionAdvanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An IntroductionDilum Bandara
 
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .Alan Dix
 
Gen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdf
Gen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdfGen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdf
Gen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdfAddepto
 
Passkey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Passkey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxPasskey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Passkey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxA Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxUse of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack
"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack
"Subclassing and Composition – A Pythonic Tour of Trade-Offs", Hynek Schlawack
 
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
 
Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!
Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!
Anypoint Exchange: It’s Not Just a Repo!
 
Generative AI for Technical Writer or Information Developers
Generative AI for Technical Writer or Information DevelopersGenerative AI for Technical Writer or Information Developers
Generative AI for Technical Writer or Information Developers
 
Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365
Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365
Ensuring Technical Readiness For Copilot in Microsoft 365
 
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine TuningDSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
 
Sample pptx for embedding into website for demo
Sample pptx for embedding into website for demoSample pptx for embedding into website for demo
Sample pptx for embedding into website for demo
 
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: Loan Stars - Tech Forum 2024
 
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfUnraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
 
What is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdf
What is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdfWhat is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdf
What is DBT - The Ultimate Data Build Tool.pdf
 
DevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache Maven
DevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache MavenDevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache Maven
DevoxxFR 2024 Reproducible Builds with Apache Maven
 
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQLDeveloper Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
 
Advanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An IntroductionAdvanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
 
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
 
Gen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdf
Gen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdfGen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdf
Gen AI in Business - Global Trends Report 2024.pdf
 
Passkey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Passkey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxPasskey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Passkey Providers and Enabling Portability: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxA Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 

Zeus problem why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods (gervais & henrich 2010)

  • 1. Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 brill.nl/jocc The Zeus Problem: Why Representational Content Biases Cannot Explain Faith in Gods Will M. Gervais* and Joseph Henrich Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T1Z4 * Corresponding author, e-mail: will@psych.ubc.ca Abstract In a recent article, Barrett (2008) argued that a collection of five representational content features can explain both why people believe in God and why people do not believe in Santa Claus or Mickey Mouse. In this model – and within the cognitive science of religion as a whole – it is argued that representational content biases are central to belief. In the present paper, we challenge the notion that representational content biases can explain the epidemiology of belief. Instead, we propose that representational content biases might explain why some concepts become widespread, but that context biases in cultural transmission are necessary to explain why people come to believe in some counterintuitive agents rather than others. Many supernatural agents, including those worshipped by other cultural groups, meet Barrett’s criteria. Nevertheless, people do not come to believe in the gods of their neighbors. This raises a new challenge for the cognitive science of religion: the Zeus Problem. Zeus contains all of the features of successful gods, and was once a target for widespread belief, worship, and commitment. But Zeus is no longer a target for widespread belief and commitment, despite having the requisite content to fulfill Barrett’s criteria. We analyze Santa Claus, God, and Zeus with both content and context biases, finding that context – not content – explains belief. We argue that a successful cognitive science of religious belief needs to move beyond simplistic notions of cultural evolution that only include representational content biases. Keywords Religious beliefs, cultural transmission, cognitive science of religion, god concepts The canonical approach to the cognitive science of religion has illuminated many of the features that make religious concepts prevalent across cultures (Boyer, 2001; Atran and Norenzayan, 2004; Barrett, 2004). In particular, researchers have focused on supernatural concepts that systematically violate ontological intuitions. These minimally counterintuitive concepts enjoy enhanced memorability – a bias that could have important effects when applied over successive generations (Barrett and Nyhof, 2001; Boyer and © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2010 DOI: 10.1163/156853710X531249
  • 2. 384 W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 Ramble, 2001; Norenzayan et al., 2006). The Mickey Mouse problem (we have not cited anyone for this handy label because when we systematically pursued its origins, we found contradictory perspectives from trusted sources) challenges the relative importance of minimal counterintuitiveness to the explanation of religion because there may be many minimally counterintuitive concepts in our cultural worlds, such as Mickey Mouse and Santa Claus, that do not inspire the faith and deep commitments associated with religious rep- resentations. Addressing this challenge, Barrett (2008) outlines five features that – he argues – combine to produce ideal representations for achieving godly status. First, successful god candidates attract attention because they violate a few routine ontological assumptions. That is, they are minimally counterintuitive. Second, they represent intentional agents. Minimally counterintuitive agents (such as talking potatoes) may be more inferentially potent than are similar non-agent concepts (such as invisible potatoes). Third, these agents possess strategic information (Boyer, 2001) that makes them relevant to peoples’ lives. Fourth, successful god representations are described as having detectable inter- actions with our world. Finally, successful god candidates have representa- tional content that motivates ritual practice that bolsters belief (Henrich, 2009). Barrett analyzed Santa Claus according to this representational content bias framework and argued that although Santa is a more suitable candidate for belief than many other counterintuitive agents, Santa ultimately fails because he lacks some of the content crucial to recruit the belief and commit- ment that is accorded to gods. Two of Barrett’s criteria that purportedly produce belief apparently presup- pose some degree of belief before they could operate on the epidemiology of representations. To our knowledge, there is no evidence that people who do not believe an agent actually exists increase their degree of belief when they learn that the agent is portrayed as holding strategic information, or as detect- ably interacting with the world. Even children show a sophisticated under- standing of the reality-fiction distinction (Morison and Gardener, 1978) and can reason about the properties, thoughts, and abilities of fictional agents (e.g., Taylor, 1999; Skolnick and Bloom, 2006), without coming to believe in their actual existence. Believers might get even more interested in their favored counterintuitive agents when they learn that these agents hold strategic infor- mation and impact the world, but if Christians learn that Zeus knows about upcoming storms (strategic information), or throws lightning bolts (detect- able action), will their Zeus belief increase? This claim awaits support. We also note that many religions possess distant high gods that do not interact in the world (Swanson, 1960), so “detectable interactions” fail to describe some gods that actually recruit belief.
  • 3. W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 385 We present a new challenge to this framework, the Zeus Problem, and pro- pose that the cognitive science of religion needs to take seriously an existing framework that looks more broadly at the cognitive and evolutionary founda- tions of culture, and integrate beyond explanations based purely on represen- tational content. The Zeus Problem implies that explanations invoking only representational content cannot explain why people place faith in some god concepts but not others, such as the gods of neighboring or intermixed cul- tures. In closing, we apply our framework to patterns of belief in three super- natural agents: Santa Claus, God and Zeus. Barrett (2008) meets the challenge of Santa and Mickey by arguing that a cognitive “sweet spot” exists for candidate god concepts that lies at the inter- section of counterintuition, agency, strategic information, verifiable activity, and action motivation. This confluence of representational content features may partially explain what sets culturally successful gods apart from Mickey Mouse and Santa Claus, but we wonder how Barrett could explain why people do not believe in other peoples’ gods? Depending on your background, these “other gods” might include Zeus, Yahweh, Baal, Thor, Ganesha, Papa Gede, or ancestor spirits. These counterintuitive agents must hit Barrett’s sweet spot, because they all held thousands of adherents for centuries and were gloried in rituals and art. Many of them still do. If they miss the sweet spot, Barrett has a bigger problem because then his explanation would be confined to a particu- lar subset of the deities that actually inspire belief. Our question is: Why doesn’t the representational content of these gods instill faith in those who hold the representational content, outside of their respective eras and cultural milieus? Barrett’s “Cognitive science of religion” fails to recognize that there is more to cultural evolution than merely representational content. In contrast, evolu- tionary approaches to cognition and culture have long emphasized both content-based learning and context-based learning (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Henrich and McElreath, 2003). Content-based mechanisms or biases arise from the interaction of mental representations with our cognitive machin- ery. Such interactions may affect a representation’s memorability, transmissi- bility, believability, or intra-psychic transformations (Henrich, forthcoming). Counterintuitiveness is an example of a content-based memory bias (Atran and Norenzayan, 2004). In contrast, context biases deal with the sources of mental representations and their integration. Evolutionary approaches suggest that learners should attend to cues of prestige, success, skill, age, sex, ethnic membership, health, and self-similarity in figuring out whom to learn from, or how to weight cul- tural information from diverse sources (Henrich and Gil-White, 2001). Learn- ers should also weight the frequency of different beliefs or behaviors using
  • 4. 386 W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 conformist transmission (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Henrich and Boyd, 1998). Learners should be especially sensitive to actions diagnostic of a mod- el’s degree of commitment to expressed beliefs, termed credibility enhancing displays. Rituals, for example, may have culturally evolved, in part, to exploit our evolved context biases for cultural learning to more effectively display commitment (Henrich, forthcoming). Content biases may explain why both religious beliefs and folk tales involve similar content, but context biases may be required to determine why people believe the former rather than the latter, or to determine which candidate god concepts are believed to exist in a given cultural context. Context biases might explain why a child raised in a Catholic village probably believes in the Trinity, but not in the Trident of Poseidon. We think context-based cultural learning better explains why people (from some societies) are more likely to have faith in God than in Zeus or Santa. This illuminates an issue that the “Cognitive science of religion” has not paid sufficient attention to: the difference between memory and recall on the one hand and belief, faith, or commitment on the other hand. The experi- ments so often pointed to as evidence for the importance of counterintuitive- ness only reveal memory and recall effects (e.g., Barrett and Nyhof, 2001; Norenzayan et al., 2006), which tell us nothing about belief. You can recall something without believing in it or placing deep faith in it. So, do the con- tent-based mechanisms of the cognitive science of religion tell us anything about the origin of faith? Our evolutionary approach suggests that humans ought to have psycho- logical mechanisms that (1) make counterintuitive representations more memorable (i.e., take note of unusual stuff) and (2) create some immunity (skepticism) against believing such representations without input from other cultural learning mechanism (Henrich, forthcoming). That is, concepts that systematically deviate from intuitive expectations may actually be less believ- able than are more intuitive concepts. In one experiment (Harris et al., 2006), children were asked whether a variety of different entities exist. The children reported that a variety of empirically non-verifiable scientific entities such as germs exist, and asserted the existence of endorsed beings like Santa Claus. Although this appears to indicate that the children came to believe in counter- intuitive agents like Santa Claus, children were more confident that scientific entities exist than that endorsed beings exist. These authors argue that children might in part be more skeptical of the endorsed beings simply because these beings violate intuitive expectations, leading the children to “conclude that the existence of special beings such as God or Santa Claus is more dubious than that of scientific entities” (p. 92). Bloom and Weisberg (2007) further
  • 5. W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 387 argue that skepticism of concepts that violate intuitive assumptions has pro- found effects for our understanding of the world as adults; science education in part is successful if it helps people override some of their intuitions. It is unclear how similar processes would produce both (1) belief in religious agents that violate intuitions, and (2) resistance to scientific concepts that violate intuitions. We think a more likely explanation is that people adhere to those non-intuitive beliefs expressed and demonstrated by members of their cultural milieu, and express skepticism in non-intuitive beliefs that stand in opposition to those they have adopted via context-based cultural learning. Santa, God and Zeus: A Preliminary Examination of Context Biases A cognitive and evolutionary approach to religious belief needs to explain why so many adults believe in God (today), but so few believe in Zeus and Santa Claus. Barrett (2008) argues that content biases adequately explain disbelief in Santa Claus. It is puzzling, however, that belief in Santa Claus wanes during late childhood even though Santa’s described content does not change. Per- haps context biases can resolve this dilemma. During early childhood, con- formist biases may reinforce belief in Santa Claus, as a child will find that his or her peers report similar experiences with Santa Claus. Prestigious individu- als in the child’s environment also evince belief in Santa Claus. Mom and dad leave out cookies and milk as a credibility enhancing display of their own Santa belief. At school, children are bombarded with testimonials about Santa. As children age, their peers and parents likely cease their credibility enhanc- ing displays. They might tell a child outright that Santa Claus is a fabrication. A change in content does not precipitate skepticism regarding Santa Claus; instead, belief in Santa Claus declines in concert with the decline of contextual cues that others believe in Santa Claus. Bolstered by the appropriate context biases, Santa Claus is a successful god among children, but a popular folktale for the rest of us. Furthermore, we do not think that Santa is much different along Barrett’s five dimensions than many a demi- or ancestor god in small- scale societies. What, then, of God and Zeus? Both are successful god candidates in Barrett’s (2008) content bias framework. Both even inspired widespread belief and commitment in their own eras. Why is belief in God a powerful force in the world today, and discussion of Zeus is relegated to mythology classes? In ancient Greece, conformist biases, prestige biases and credibility enhancing displays supported belief in Zeus, which was both widespread and popular
  • 6. 388 W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 among the elites. Ritual practices (such as animal sacrifice) and monumental architecture (such as the Temple of Zeus at Olympia) would have served as credibility enhancing displays of cultural models’ genuine commitment to and belief in Zeus, further bolstering belief among cultural learners. These factors do not persist today for Zeus, although they do for God. Supernatural concepts with the right representational content become cul- turally widespread and persistent because they recruit attention and are mem- orable, but a core assumption of the canonical version of the cognitive science of religion is that these concepts, or at least some subset of them, have content that recruits belief and commitment. We know of no support for this claim. Representational content biases may describe why some concepts become widespread, but do not adequately explain commitment. Conclusion We propose that any model of religious belief needs to solve both the Mickey Mouse problem (why are some counterintuitive concepts believed in?) and the Zeus Problem (why do people not believe in other peoples’ gods?). Represen- tational content biases are potentially powerful forces in cultural transmission, and may explain why some concepts become widespread and culturally recur- rent. However, it is far from clear how any combination of such content-based mechanisms can explain the epidemiology of commitment. It is our hope that cognitive science of religion will begin to draw more broadly on the available theoretical tools of integrative evolutionary approaches to culture. References Atran, S. and Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, com- mitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27, 713-770. Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God? AltaMira Press, Walnut Creek, CA. ——. (2008). Why Santa Claus is not a god. Journal of Cognition and Culture 8, 149-161. Barrett, J. L. and Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading nonnatural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1, 69-100. Bloom, P. and Weisberg, D. S. (2007). Childhood origins of adult resistance to science. Science 316, 996-997. Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books, New York, NY. Boyer, P. and Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science 25, 535-564.
  • 7. W. M. Gervais, J. Henrich / Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (2010) 383–389 389 Harris, P. L., Pasquini, E. S., Duke, S., Asscher, J. J. and Pons, F. (2006). Germs and angels: The role of testimony in young children’s ontology. Developmental Science 9, 76-96. Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior 30, 244-260. Henrich, J. and Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior 19, 215-242. Henrich, J. and Gil-White, F. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behav- ior 22, 165-196. Henrich, J., and McElreath, R. (2003). The Evolution of Cultural Evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 12, 123-135. Morison, P. and Gardener, H. (1978). Dragons and dinosaurs: The child’s capacity to differenti- ate fantasy from reality. Child Development 49, 642-648. Norenzayan, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J. and Schaller, M. (2006). Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science 30, 531-553. Skolnick, D. and Bloom, P. (2006). What does Batman think about SpongeBob? Children’s understanding of the fantasy/fantasy distinction. Cognition 101, B9-B18. Swanson, G. E. (1960). The birth of the gods. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI. Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children who create them. Oxford University Press, New York, NY.