SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 7
Download to read offline
Research Article
When Intentions Go Public
Does Social Reality Widen the Intention-Behavior Gap?
Peter M. Gollwitzer,1,2
Paschal Sheeran,3
Verena Michalski,2
and Andrea E. Seifert2
1
New York University, 2
Universita¨t Konstanz, and 3
University of Sheffield
ABSTRACT—Based on Lewinian goal theory in general and
self-completion theory in particular, four experiments ex-
amined the implications of other people taking notice of
one’s identity-related behavioral intentions (e.g., the in-
tention to read law periodicals regularly to reach the
identity goal of becoming a lawyer). Identity-related be-
havioral intentions that had been noticed by other people
were translated into action less intensively than those that
had been ignored (Studies 1–3). This effect was evident in
the field (persistent striving over 1 week’s time; Study 1)
and in the laboratory (jumping on opportunities to act;
Studies 2 and 3), and it held among participants with
strong but not weak commitment to the identity goal (Study
3). Study 4 showed, in addition, that when other people
take notice of an individual’s identity-related behavioral
intention, this gives the individual a premature sense of
possessing the aspired-to identity.
Are scientists more likely to write papers if they tell colleagues
about this intention than if they keep the intention private? It is
commonly assumed that whenever people make their intentions
public, the behavioral impact of these intentions is enhanced
(e.g., Staats, Harland, & Wilke, 2004). These effects are pos-
tulated to be a consequence of multiple processes. Research on
persuasion techniques points to one of these processes (Cialdini
& Trost, 1998). It is argued that a publicly stated behavioral
intention commits the individual to a certain self-view (e.g., ‘‘I
am a productive person’’) with which the person then acts con-
sistently. Indeed, individuals with a higher need for consistency
show stronger public-commitment effects (Cialdini, Wosinka,
Barrett, Butner, & Gornik-Durose, 1999). The second process is
referred to as accountability (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999). Making
intentions public is said to make a person accountable to the
addressed audience, and research has shown that various ac-
countability-related features of the audience (e.g., competence,
power) and the individual (e.g., identifiability, expectations of
having to explain oneself) affect the strength of public-com-
mitment effects.
Both of these lines of research focus on intentions in which the
specified behavior is a desired outcome in and of itself. Lewin
(1926) and his colleagues (e.g., Mahler, 1935; Ovsiankina,
1928), however, argued that people often construe behavioral
intentions in more general terms, thus allowing substitution of
means for attainment. For instance, consider a student who has
started an assigned math task with the intention to successfully
solve the required addition problems. During the process, this
student may construe the intention as being to demonstrate
mathematical skills, and this conceptually broader intention
may also be reached by solving subtraction problems (i.e., by
substitute activities). Ovsiankina and Mahler observed that a
substitute activity engenders a sense of having reached the
conceptually broader intention, given that performance of the
substitute activity has been witnessed by other people (i.e., has
become a social reality). On the basis of this line of thought—
which we explicate in the framework of self-completion theory
(SCT; Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998; Wicklund & Gollwitzer,
1982)—we propose that social recognition of an identity-rele-
vant behavioral intention may have negative effects on its en-
actment.
SCT proposes that people who are committed to identity goals
(e.g., becoming a good parent, scientist, or craftsperson) can
undertake a variety of activities to claim goal attainment. For a
scientist, such activities, or identity symbols, include engaging
in professional duties (e.g., giving lectures), making positive
self-descriptions (e.g., ‘‘I discovered a new principle!’’), exerting
identity-relevant social influence (e.g., advising students), and
acquiring skills and tools that facilitate striving for the identity
goal (e.g., programming skills, computers). However, failing to
perform an identity-relevant activity or facing the lack of an
identity symbol produces a state of incompleteness (Wicklund &
Gollwitzer, 1982). To restore completeness, the individual
makes efforts to acquire alternative identity symbols (e.g., de-
scribing oneself as having the required personality attributes:
Address correspondence to Peter M. Gollwitzer, New York Univer-
sity, Psychology Department, 6 Washington Place, 7th Floor, New
York, NY 10003, e-mail: peter.gollwitzer@nyu.edu.
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
612 Volume 20—Number 5Copyright r 2009 Association for Psychological Science
Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; engaging in identity-relevant
activities: Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996; showing off relevant
status symbols: Harmon-Jones, Schmeichel, & Harmon-Jones,
2009). Using opportunities to affirm one’s general self-integrity
or to bolster one’s self-esteem is not sufficient to offset incom-
pleteness regarding an identity goal; rather, it is necessary to
acquire specific identity symbols (Ledgerwood, Liviatan, &
Carnevale, 2007).
SCT research has also shown that an individual reaches a
higher level of completeness when his or her identity-relevant
activities are noticed by a social audience (Gollwitzer, 1986).
Moreover, research has shown that incomplete individuals are
more concerned with finding an audience for their identity
strivings, compared with complete individuals (Brunstein &
Gollwitzer, 1996). Positive self-descriptions made in public
qualify as powerful identity symbols (Gollwitzer, Wicklund, &
Hilton, 1982), and having an audience for behavioral intentions
that specify the successful performance of an identity-relevant
activity should have the same symbolic impact. The implication
is that when other people take notice of a stated identity-relevant
behavioral intention, this should engender completeness re-
garding the superordinate identity goal, and thus reaching the
identity goal by actually performing the intended behavior
should become less necessary. In other words, people should be
less likely to translate their identity-relevant behavioral inten-
tions into action when other people have taken notice of those
intentions. We conducted four experiments entailing a variety of
identity goals and behavioral intentions to test this hypothesis.
STUDY 1
In Study 1, we asked college students committed to becoming a
psychologist to form identity-relevant studying intentions. After
either taking notice of these intentions or ignoring them, we
assessed how effectively the students enacted their intentions
over the subsequent week.
Method
Forty-nine psychology students (38 women, 11 men) at a Ger-
man university were recruited after introductory lectures. Par-
ticipants were informed that they would take part in a survey
about the study intentions of first-year psychology students. A
first questionnaire assessed commitment to becoming a psy-
chologist using the following items:
 ‘‘How important is it for you to find a psychology-related job?’’
 ‘‘Suppose that you could not finish your studies of psychology
successfully. How much would that bother you?’’
 ‘‘How happy would you be in a job that is not related to
psychology?’’ (reverse-coded)
Participants responded to these items on 9-point scales ranging
from 1, not at all, to 9, very much. Responses were averaged to
form a scale (a 5 .80). Next, participants were asked to write
down their two most important study intentions for the forth-
coming week (e.g., ‘‘I will take my reading assignments more
seriously,’’ ‘‘I intend to study more statistics’’).
In the social-reality condition, the experimenter read through
each participant’s reported intentions, presumably to ensure
that the participant had understood the instructions. In the no-
social-reality condition, participants were told that the page of
the questionnaire on which participants had written down their
behavioral intentions had been wrongly included in the study,
and that this page would be discarded (i.e., the students’ in-
tentions remained unnoticed).
One week later, all participants were sent a second ques-
tionnaire via e-mail. They had to first write down the two be-
havioral intentions they had listed the previous week. Then, they
indicated on exactly which days of the past week they had acted
on each intention. Finally, participants were asked to bring their
completed questionnaire to the experimenter’s office, where they
received payment (h5) or course credit.
Results and Discussion
Overall, participants were highly committed to the identity goal
(range 5 6–9; M 5 7.32, SD 5 1.64), and there was no
significant difference in commitment scores between the
social-reality condition (M 5 7.21, SD 5 1.64) and the no-
social-reality condition (M 5 7.43, SD 5 1.08), F  1, p  .32,
d  0.16. We analyzed the number of days participants acted on
their intentions in a 2 Â 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with
social reality as the between-participants factor and the two
specified behavioral intentions as the within-participants factor.
Results showed a significant main effect of social reality, F(1,
47) 5 4.38, p  .05, d 5 0.60; participants whose intentions
remained private acted on their intentions on more days of the
week (M 5 2.70, SD 5 1.83) than did participants whose in-
tentions were noticed (M 5 1.92, SD 5 0.78). No other effects
were significant.
An apparent strength of Study 1 is that the observed negative
effects of having one’s intentions noticed cannot easily be at-
tributed to emotions that might accrue from the experimenter’s
behavior (e.g., pride). Such affective responses should vanish
quickly, whereas the effects on behavioral enactment in Study 1
were evident over a period of 1 week. An obvious weakness of
this study, however, is that enactment of intentions was assessed
via self-report.
STUDY 2
In Study 2, therefore, we observed actual enactment of inten-
tions. Participants were law students who formed the behavioral
intention to make use of identity-relevant educational oppor-
tunities. This intention was then either noticed by other people
or ignored. We then observed the degree to which participants
Volume 20—Number 5 613
P.M. Gollwitzer et al.
acted on their intention when such an educational opportunity
was actually provided.
Method
Law students at a German university were approached after a
lecture and asked to fill out a three-item commitment ques-
tionnaire adapted from Study 1 (7-point answer scales were used
this time). Only participants with high commitment to becoming
a successful jurist (score ! 5 on each item; a 5 .76) were invited
to take part in the study (N 5 32; 13 women, 19 men). The
students received h5 for their participation.
Participants were greeted individually and informed that the
experiment consisted of two independent parts. The first was
introduced as an assessment of students’ willingness to intensify
their study of law. Participants were asked to answer a four-page
questionnaire. On the first page, the following critical intention
item was presented: ‘‘I intend to make the best possible use of
educational opportunities in law.’’ Participants responded on a
9-point scale ranging from 1, definitely not, to 9, definitely yes. In
the social-reality condition, after a participant completed the
questionnaire, the experimenter looked at this item and asked
whether the number circled on the answer scale was the one the
participant actually wanted to circle. Then the experimenter
dropped the questionnaire into a box. In the no-social-reality
condition, participants were simply asked to drop the ques-
tionnaire into a prepared box. As the questionnaire was anon-
ymous, it was clear to participants in this condition that the
experimenter would never be able to link the expressed inten-
tions to individual participants.
The experimenter then turned to the supposed second part of
the experiment, which concerned the development of a com-
puter-based study package for law students. New study mate-
rials were needed, so she had prepared 20 different criminal law
cases. Participants were asked to help her find which cases to
select for the package by trying hard to solve each case. The
students were given 45 min to work on the prepared cases (plus
the time needed to finish the case they were working on when the
time limit was reached), but they were told that they could finish
earlier if they wished. The time participants spent working on
these cases was used to assess how successfully participants
translated their intention into behavior.
Results and Discussion
Only participants who intended to make the best possible use of
educational opportunities (score  5) were included (30 out of
the 32 original participants). Participants worked on the law
cases for less time if this behavioral intention was noticed than if
it was ignored by the experimenter (M 5 41.52 min, SD 5 4.42,
vs. M 5 45.65 min, SD 5 2.92), t(29) 5 3.26, p  .01, d 5 1.10.
Thus, law students—all of them highly committed to the identity
goal of being a jurist—who had stated the behavioral intention to
take advantage of educational opportunities in law acted less
intensively on this intention when it was noticed by the exper-
imenter than when it was ignored.
STUDY 3
Studies 1 and 2 were both conducted with participants who were
highly committed to the identity goal in question (i.e., psy-
chologist in Study 1, jurist in Study 2). As only individuals who
are highly committed to an identity goal can be expected to
experience self-completeness by accumulating identity symbols
(Wicklund  Gollwitzer, 1982), it follows that noncommitted
individuals should not show reduced intention enactment
whenever their identity-related behavioral intentions are no-
ticed by other people. Study 3 tested this hypothesis in two ways:
First, we compared students who wanted to become clinical
psychologists and those who wanted to become other types of
psychologists to determine whether they differed in how social
reality affected their enactment of behavioral intentions in the
service of the identity goal of clinical psychologist. Second, we
assessed the strength of participants’ commitment to the identity
goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. We decided to assess
rather than manipulate strength of identity-goal commitment, as
strong identity commitments are not easily created on the spot,
but often take years to develop (Gollwitzer  Kirchhof, 1998).
Method
Sixty-three psychology students at a German university (40
women, 23 men) participated for course credit. They were in-
formed that they would take part in two independent studies.
The first study was described as exploring students’ willingness
to intensify their studies and involved answering several short
questionnaires. The first questionnaire assessed commitment to
the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. The first
item asked, ‘‘There are different fields of specialization in psy-
chology. Which one are you trying to pursue?’’ Response options
were ‘‘child,’’ ‘‘industrial,’’ ‘‘clinical,’’ ‘‘experimental,’’ ‘‘math-
ematical,’’ and ‘‘undecided.’’ Participants who answered ‘‘clin-
ical’’ were assumed to possess this identity goal, whereas the
others were not. We assumed that strongly committed individ-
uals would be willing to take on hardships (e.g., moving to an-
other town) en route to attaining an identity goal (Gollwitzer 
Kirchhof, 1998), and the subsequent strength-of-commitment
item asked, ‘‘Would you switch universities in order to receive an
optimal education in your field of interest?’’ Participants re-
sponded to this question using a 5-point scale ranging from 1,
No, I would never do that, to 5, Yes, I would do that for sure.
A second questionnaire (one item per page) was introduced as
an inventory concerning ways of intensifying one’s studies. The
critical behavioral-intention question was the only item on the
first page: ‘‘I intend to watch videotapes of therapy sessions to
learn more about therapeutic techniques.’’ The response scale
for this item ranged from 1, definitely no, to 5, definitely yes.
614 Volume 20—Number 5
Intentions as Symbols
Upon completion of this questionnaire, participants in the so-
cial-reality condition were told that because of incomplete re-
sponses of prior participants, only the question on the first page
of the questionnaire could be analyzed. The experimenter then
studied that page, tore it off, and handed the remaining pages
back to the participant. In the no-social-reality condition, the
experimenter gave the same cover story about missing data, but
applied it to all the items on the questionnaire; then she returned
the entire questionnaire without looking at it.
Finally, the experimenter introduced participants to the sec-
ond experimenter, who told them that she was trying to find out
the extent to which making eye contact affects the quality of an
interaction. She explained that she had prepared a video
showing a conversation between a therapist and a client. Par-
ticipants’ task was to count the instances of making eye contact
and to rate the quality of interaction after each minute of the
conversation. Participants were told that the video lasted 40
min, but they should feel free to stop the video whenever they
wanted.
Results and Discussion
Thirty-one participants indicated that clinical psychology was
the field they wanted to pursue; 32 participants indicated other
fields. Because only participants with strong behavioral inten-
tions were of interest, we excluded 6 participants (2 aspiring
clinical psychologists and 4 no-goal participants) who scored 4
or less on the item ‘‘I intend to watch videotapes of therapy
sessions to learn more about therapeutic techniques.’’ A 2
(clinical-psychologist identity goal: present vs. absent) Â 2
(social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA on the amount of time
spent watching the therapy video yielded the predicted inter-
action effect, F(1, 53) 5 3.95, p 5 .05, d 5 0.95; only the
performance of aspiring clinical psychologists was affected by
social reality. As expected, aspiring clinical psychologists
whose behavioral intention to study videotaped therapy sessions
had been noticed by the experimenter invested less time in
watching the video than did aspiring clinical psychologists
whose intentions remained unnoticed (M 5 29.51 min, SD 5
6.72, vs. M 5 34.22 min, SD 5 7.19), t(28) 5 1.82, p 5 .04 (one-
tailed), d 5 0.68. None of the other comparisons were significant
(all ts  0.15, ps  .88, ds  0.06).
In an additional analysis, we examined whether strength of
commitment to the identity goal, as measured by the mobility
item, moderated the effect of social reality among the aspiring
clinical psychologists (weak commitment 3, strong commit-
ment ! 4). A 2 (strength of commitment: strong vs. weak) Â 2
(social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA revealed a main ef-
fect of social reality, F(1, 25) 5 5.50, p  .03, d 5 0.94, that was
qualified by a significant interaction with strength of commit-
ment, F(1, 25) 5 5.04, p  .04, d 5 0.90. As expected (see Table
1), participants with a strong commitment to the identity goal of
becoming a clinical psychologist spent less time (more than 11
min less) studying the videotaped therapy session if they were in
the social-reality condition than if they were in the no-social-
reality condition, t(11) 5 5.02, p  .001, d 5 2.96; in contrast,
participants with weak commitment to the identity goal spent
close to the same amount of time studying the therapy session no
matter whether they had or had not received social recognition
for their behavioral intention to study videotapes of therapy
sessions (there was only a 13.2-s difference between groups; t 5
0.09, p 5 .93, d 5 0.04).
STUDY 4
SCT (Wicklund  Gollwitzer, 1982) postulates that public rec-
ognition of an identity-relevant symbol engenders a sense of
having attained the aspired-to identity goal (i.e., self-com-
pleteness). As Studies 1 through 3 suggest that identity-relevant
behavioral intentions do qualify as identity symbols, it follows
that social recognition of such intentions should also lead to a
heightened sense of completeness. In Study 4, we tested this
hypothesis with law students committed to becoming successful
jurists.
Method
Twenty-four first-year and second-year law students (10 women,
14 men) from a German university participated in this experi-
ment in exchange for h5. They were recruited at the end of a law
seminar.
Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants met the experi-
menter and two other students (actually confederates) who were
described as fellow law students. The experimenter explained to
each group that he was conducting a study on law students’
intentions to advance their careers and then asked participants
to complete a questionnaire. The questionnaire comprised two
items. The first item measured participants’ commitment to
becoming a lawyer: ‘‘How important is a successful career in law
to you personally?’’ Responses were made on a 7-point scale
ranging from 1, not at all important, to 7, very important. The
second item asked participants to write out their three most
important behavioral intentions with respect to the goal of be-
coming a successful jurist (e.g., ‘‘I will read law periodicals
regularly’’). The importance of each intention also had to be
TABLE 1
Mean Time Spent Studying the Videotaped Therapy Session (in
Minutes) in Study 3
Commitment to identity
goal of clinical psychologist
Social reality
Present Absent
Strong 27.95 (3.11) 39.17 (4.51)
Weak 30.68 (3.62) 30.90 (6.24)
Note. Standard deviations are given in parentheses.
Volume 20—Number 5 615
P.M. Gollwitzer et al.
rated on a 7-point scale. Once participants had completed the
questionnaire, the experimenter asked them to either rate the
attractiveness of 10 pictures of landscapes (the no-social-reality
condition) or tell him and the rest of the group what intentions
they had written down (the social-reality condition). In the so-
cial-reality condition, the participants always reported their
intentions first; the confederates then reported behavioral in-
tentions derived from prior participants.
Next, a second questionnaire was handed out. This ques-
tionnaire showed a 14-cm line, above which were aligned five
pictures of the same member of the German Supreme Court
wearing the characteristic attire. The pictures varied in size,
ranging from small (1.5 Â 1 cm) to large (3.6 Â 2.4 cm). These
size gradations provided a visual analogue of the extent of
possessing the identity of being a jurist. Participants responded
to the item ‘‘How much do you feel like a jurist right now?’’ by
marking the respective point on the line. This self-assessment
manekin (SAM) rating procedure facilitates quick, nonreflective
self-evaluations (Bradley  Lang, 1994).
Results and Discussion
The commitment of students in the social-reality condition (M 5
6.00, SD 5 1.49) did not differ from that of students in the no-
social-reality condition (M 5 6.08, SD 5 0.67), t(21) 5 0.17,
p 5 .68, d 5 0.07. Ratings of the importance of the listed in-
tentions were high and also did not differ between conditions
(M 5 6.10, SD 5 0.55, vs. M 5 6.00, SD 5 0.55), t(21) 5 0.42,
p 5 .87, d 5 0.18.
We first checked the validity of our measure of felt com-
pleteness. Indeed, more semesters of law education was asso-
ciated with stronger feelings of completeness as a jurist (r 5 .54,
p 5 .005). In a hierarchical regression analysis, the social-
reality manipulation was significantly associated with feelings
of self-completeness even after number of semesters of law edu-
cation had been taken into account (bs 5 .29 and .45 for edu-
cation and condition, respectively, ps  .05), and inclusion of
this variable enhanced the fit of the model (DR2
5 .17, DF 5
5.72, p  .03). As we predicted, participants felt closer to the
identity goal of becoming a jurist when their behavioral inten-
tions were recognized than when those intentions remained pri-
vate (Ms 5 4.19 and 3.10, respectively).
CONCLUSION
When other people take notice of one’s identity-relevant be-
havioral intentions, one’s performance of the intended behaviors
is compromised. This effect occurs both when the intentions are
experimenter supplied and when they are self-generated, and is
observed in both immediate performance and performance
measured over a period of 1 week. It does not emerge when
people are not committed to the superordinate identity goal.
Other people’s taking notice of one’s identity-relevant intentions
apparently engenders a premature sense of completeness re-
garding the identity goal.
Fishbein (1980) and Ajzen (1991) showed that the strength of
a behavioral intention determines how well it is translated into
behavior (Webb  Sheeran, 2006). Moreover, a substantial lit-
erature on moderators of intention-behavior relations (e.g.,
certainty, temporal stability) has developed (Cooke  Sheeran,
2004; Sheeran, 2002). Interestingly, however, previous research
has not explored what psychological processes may intervene
between the formation of a behavioral intention and its enact-
ment. The present studies indicate that the simple matter of
identity-relevant behavioral intentions becoming public un-
dermines the realization of those intentions.
The present research is also unique in its attempt to bring
back Lewin’s work on intentions as it applies to the actual re-
alization of intentions. Most of the current research based on
Lewin’s (1926) goal theory focuses on the activation level of the
mental representation of a person’s intention (following Zei-
garnik, 1927). For instance, research has shown that the ac-
cessibility of goal-related constructs is increased as long as the
goal is active, that goal fulfillment inhibits accessibility of goal-
related constructs, and that these effects are proportional to the
strength of commitment to the goal (Fo¨rster, Liberman,  Hig-
gins, 2005; Goschke  Kuhl, 1993; Marsh, Hicks,  Bink,
1998).
Our findings are also important from an applied perspective.
Given that the effect is limited to committed individuals—those
who are most eager to reach their identity goals—an important
question is how these individuals might try to escape this effect.
Future research might address this question by exploring vari-
ous routes. First, might it suffice to increase the need for con-
sistency (Cialdini  Trost, 1998) by attending to relevant norms?
Or is it also necessary to increase perceived accountability
(Lerner  Tetlock, 1999) by considering relevant attributes of
the audience (e.g., power) or by specifying one’s behavioral in-
tention in a particular way (e.g., spelling out specific frequency
or quality standards vs. stating only that one wants to do one’s
best; Locke  Latham, 2002) so that the audience can more
easily check on its enactment? Second, might it also be effective
for one to furnish a behavioral intention with a plan for how to
enact it—that is, to form a corresponding implementation in-
tention (e.g., ‘‘If situation X is encountered, then I will perform
the intended behavior Y’’; Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer 
Sheeran, 2006)? As such if-then plans delegate the control of a
person’s behavior to situational cues, the intended behavior
should be executed when the critical cue arises, whether or not
the expression of the behavioral intention had been acknowl-
edged by other people. Third, recent research by Fishbach and
her colleagues (Fishbach  Dhar, 2005; Koo  Fishbach, 2008)
suggests that interpreting a behavioral performance in terms of
indicating commitment to a goal enhances further goal striving,
whereas conceiving of a performance in terms of progress toward
616 Volume 20—Number 5
Intentions as Symbols
a goal reduces further goal striving. This implies that a behav-
ioral intention worded to indicate a strong commitment to the
identity goal (e.g., ‘‘I want to write a paper to become a great
scientist’’) should be less negatively affected by social reality
than a behavioral intention that implies progress toward the
identity goal (e.g., ‘‘I intend to write a paper, as is done by great
scientists’’).
Finally, from a goal-systems (Kruglanski et al., 2002) or goal-
hierarchy (Vallacher  Wegner, 1987) perspective on action
control, it stands to reason that any striving for goals—and not
just identity goals—that can be attained by various behavioral
routes (means) is vulnerable to the negative effects of social
reality on the enactment of behavioral intentions. If a person is
highly committed to a superordinate goal, and if public recog-
nition of a behavioral intention specifying the use of one route to
the goal engenders a sense of goal attainment, then the enact-
ment of this very intention should be hampered. Recent research
by Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang (2006) is in line with this rea-
soning, showing that success on a subgoal (e.g., eating healthy
meals) in the service of a superordinate goal (i.e., keeping in
shape) reduces striving for alternative subgoals (e.g., going to
the gym).
REFERENCES
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
Bradley, M.M.,  Lang, P.J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-
assessment manekin and the semantic differential. Journal of
Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59.
Brunstein, J.C.,  Gollwitzer, P.M. (1996). Effects of failure on sub-
sequent performance: The importance of self-defining goals.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 395–407.
Cialdini, R.B.,  Trost, M.R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms,
conformity, and compliance. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske,  G.
Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp.
151–192). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Cialdini, R.B., Wosinka, W., Barrett, D.W., Butner, J.,  Gornik-
Durose, M. (1999). Compliance with a request in two cultures:
The differential influence of social proof and commitment/con-
sistency on collectivists and individualists. Personality and So-
cial Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1242–1253.
Cooke, R.,  Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and
cognition-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of properties of
variables from the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of
Social Psychology, 43, 159–186.
Fishbach, A.,  Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The
liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of
Consumer Research, 32, 370–377.
Fishbach, A., Dhar, R.,  Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substitutes or
complements: The role of goal accessibility. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 91, 232–242.
Fishbein, M. (1980). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications
and implications. In H. Howe  M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska sym-
posium on motivation (Vol. 27, pp. 65–116). Lincoln: University
of Nebraska Press.
Fo¨rster, J., Liberman, N.,  Higgins, E.T. (2005). Accessibility from
active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 41, 220–239.
Gollwitzer, P.M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social re-
ality of self-symbolizing. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Private self and
public self (pp. 143–159). New York: Springer.
Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of
simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Gollwitzer, P.M.,  Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity.
In J. Heckhausen  C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-
regulation across the life span (pp. 389–423). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Gollwitzer, P.M.,  Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions
and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes.
In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 38, pp. 69–119). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Gollwitzer, P.M.,  Wicklund, R.A. (1985). Self-symbolizing and the
neglect of others’ perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 48, 702–715.
Gollwitzer, P.M., Wicklund, R.A.,  Hilton, J.L. (1982). Admission of
failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 358–371.
Goschke, T.,  Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions:
Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211–1226.
Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B.J.,  Harmon-Jones, E. (2009).
Symbolic self-completion in academia: Evidence from depart-
ment web pages and email signature files. European Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 311–316.
Koo, M.,  Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation:
How (un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 183–195.
Kruglanski, A.W., Shah, J.Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W.Y.,
 Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M.P.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34,
pp. 331–378). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Ledgerwood, A., Liviatan, I.,  Carnevale, P.J. (2007). Group-identity
completion and the symbolic value of property. Psychological
Science, 18, 873–878.
Lerner, J.S.,  Tetlock, P.E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of ac-
countability. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 255–275.
Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille und Bedu¨rfnis. Psychologische For-
schung, 7, 330–385.
Locke, E.A.,  Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful
theory of goal setting and task motivation. American Psychologist,
57, 705–717.
Mahler, W. (1933). Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen Realita¨tsgrades.
Psychologische Forschung, 18, 27–89.
Marsh, R.L., Hicks, J.L.,  Bink, M.L. (1998). Activation of com-
pleted, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
24, 350–361.
Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Hand-
lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302–379.
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and
empirical review. In W. Stroebe  M. Hewstone (Eds.), European
review of social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 1–36). Chichester, En-
gland: Wiley.
Staats, H., Harland, P.,  Wilke, H.A. (2004). Effecting durable
change: A team approach to improve environmental behavior in
the household. Environment and Behavior, 36, 341–367.
Volume 20—Number 5 617
P.M. Gollwitzer et al.
Vallacher, R.R.,  Wegner, D.M. (1987). What do people think they
are doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psycho-
logical Review, 94, 3–15.
Webb, T.L.,  Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral
intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the
experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268.
Wicklund, R.A.,  Gollwitzer, P.M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Hand-
lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85.
(RECEIVED 1/14/08; REVISION ACCEPTED 10/8/08)
618 Volume 20—Number 5
Intentions as Symbols

More Related Content

What's hot

Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology PaperPersonality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology PaperJennifer Burke
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016Daniel Horsley
 
Can You Measure Up?
Can You Measure Up?Can You Measure Up?
Can You Measure Up?Lara Hogan
 
JameelGoingAbove&Beyond
JameelGoingAbove&BeyondJameelGoingAbove&Beyond
JameelGoingAbove&BeyondLeila Jameel
 

What's hot (6)

199RA_TonyWeeda
199RA_TonyWeeda199RA_TonyWeeda
199RA_TonyWeeda
 
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology PaperPersonality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
Personality, Regulatory Focus, and Political Ideology Paper
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016
 
Can You Measure Up?
Can You Measure Up?Can You Measure Up?
Can You Measure Up?
 
JameelGoingAbove&Beyond
JameelGoingAbove&BeyondJameelGoingAbove&Beyond
JameelGoingAbove&Beyond
 
Intelligence Analysis
Intelligence AnalysisIntelligence Analysis
Intelligence Analysis
 

Viewers also liked

Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...
Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...
Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...Trading Game Pty Ltd
 
English class activity 2
English class activity 2English class activity 2
English class activity 2Jaey Chai
 
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...Javier Contreras
 
20151229 112
20151229 11220151229 112
20151229 112Vetor Mil
 
Your future is participation and here’s why
Your future is participation and here’s whyYour future is participation and here’s why
Your future is participation and here’s whyBetter_Ventures
 
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to ArbitrageBiased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to ArbitrageTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
Using EEG when usability testing
Using EEG when usability testingUsing EEG when usability testing
Using EEG when usability testingCaroline Jarrett
 
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part IIntroduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part IMichel Alves
 
Bad hibernation-rooted
Bad hibernation-rootedBad hibernation-rooted
Bad hibernation-rootedRamon
 
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1Smart ERP Solutions, Inc.
 
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for businessShelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for businessBetter_Ventures
 
La etica profesional
La etica profesionalLa etica profesional
La etica profesionalYodelu
 

Viewers also liked (16)

Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...
Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...
Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfol...
 
English class activity 2
English class activity 2English class activity 2
English class activity 2
 
99
9999
99
 
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
 
Operaciones 1°e
Operaciones 1°eOperaciones 1°e
Operaciones 1°e
 
Horison singkawang
Horison singkawangHorison singkawang
Horison singkawang
 
20151229 112
20151229 11220151229 112
20151229 112
 
Your future is participation and here’s why
Your future is participation and here’s whyYour future is participation and here’s why
Your future is participation and here’s why
 
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to ArbitrageBiased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
 
Using EEG when usability testing
Using EEG when usability testingUsing EEG when usability testing
Using EEG when usability testing
 
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part IIntroduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
 
Bad hibernation-rooted
Bad hibernation-rootedBad hibernation-rooted
Bad hibernation-rooted
 
FILUM Annelida
FILUM Annelida FILUM Annelida
FILUM Annelida
 
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
 
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for businessShelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
 
La etica profesional
La etica profesionalLa etica profesional
La etica profesional
 

Similar to When Intentions Go Public

Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Ashley Fisher
 
Cross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural PsychologyCross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural PsychologyMonica Carter
 
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsStrengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsSandra Arveseth
 
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docxReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docxaudeleypearl
 
Standardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School SystemsStandardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School SystemsTiffany Sandoval
 
VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)
VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)
VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)Nur Arifaizal Basri
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxkarlhennesey
 
Difference Between Quantitative And Qualitative Research
Difference Between Quantitative And Qualitative ResearchDifference Between Quantitative And Qualitative Research
Difference Between Quantitative And Qualitative ResearchMelanie Smith
 
Tiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research ResultTiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research Resultkudozprototype
 
Observation Video Program 4Observ.docx
Observation Video Program                        4Observ.docxObservation Video Program                        4Observ.docx
Observation Video Program 4Observ.docxcherishwinsland
 
grev721 research propfinal
grev721 research propfinalgrev721 research propfinal
grev721 research propfinalemtinanalqurashi
 
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docxvickeryr87
 
Rorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning Essay
Rorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning EssayRorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning Essay
Rorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning EssayKatherine Alexander
 
Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...
Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...
Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...Camella Taylor
 
Social Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docx
Social Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docxSocial Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docx
Social Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docxjensgosney
 

Similar to When Intentions Go Public (20)

Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
 
Cross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural PsychologyCross-Cultural Psychology
Cross-Cultural Psychology
 
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsStrengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
 
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docxReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
ReflectionThis course has provided me with a different perspecti.docx
 
Standardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School SystemsStandardized Testing For Public School Systems
Standardized Testing For Public School Systems
 
VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)
VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)
VOCATIONAL INDECISION (JOHN HOLLAND)
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
 
Difference Between Quantitative And Qualitative Research
Difference Between Quantitative And Qualitative ResearchDifference Between Quantitative And Qualitative Research
Difference Between Quantitative And Qualitative Research
 
Tiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research ResultTiny Task Research Result
Tiny Task Research Result
 
Proposal Neg Campaigns
Proposal Neg CampaignsProposal Neg Campaigns
Proposal Neg Campaigns
 
Observation Video Program 4Observ.docx
Observation Video Program                        4Observ.docxObservation Video Program                        4Observ.docx
Observation Video Program 4Observ.docx
 
O Behave! Issue 20
O Behave! Issue 20O Behave! Issue 20
O Behave! Issue 20
 
Is Negative Campaigning Effective?
Is Negative Campaigning Effective?Is Negative Campaigning Effective?
Is Negative Campaigning Effective?
 
721 research propfinal
721 research propfinal721 research propfinal
721 research propfinal
 
grev721 research propfinal
grev721 research propfinalgrev721 research propfinal
grev721 research propfinal
 
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
2.1 Approaches to Sociological ResearchDerived from Approaches t.docx
 
Rorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning Essay
Rorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning EssayRorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning Essay
Rorschach Measures Of Cognition And Social Functioning Essay
 
Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...
Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...
Perfectionism As A Multidimensional Personality...
 
Social Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docx
Social Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docxSocial Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docx
Social Comparison, Social Media, and Self-EsteemErin A. Vo.docx
 
Ashleigh Vogle REU
Ashleigh Vogle REUAshleigh Vogle REU
Ashleigh Vogle REU
 

More from Trading Game Pty Ltd

Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesBullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme EcosystemAnalyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme EcosystemTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical SignificanceAge-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical SignificanceTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game TheoryA Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game TheoryTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
How Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
How Biases Affect Investor BehaviourHow Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
How Biases Affect Investor BehaviourTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision MakingThe Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision MakingTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity IncentivesARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity IncentivesTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
Behavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor Psychology
Behavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor PsychologyBehavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor Psychology
Behavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor PsychologyTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects workThe science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects workTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
What’s Going On in Your Brain?” What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
What’s Going On in Your Brain?” Trading Game Pty Ltd
 

More from Trading Game Pty Ltd (20)

2019 In Review
 2019 In Review 2019 In Review
2019 In Review
 
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesBullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
 
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme EcosystemAnalyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
 
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical SignificanceAge-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
 
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game TheoryA Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
 
Primer on Managed Futures
Primer on Managed FuturesPrimer on Managed Futures
Primer on Managed Futures
 
S&P500 Panic Attacks
S&P500 Panic AttacksS&P500 Panic Attacks
S&P500 Panic Attacks
 
How Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
How Biases Affect Investor BehaviourHow Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
How Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
 
Avoid News
Avoid NewsAvoid News
Avoid News
 
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision MakingThe Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
 
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity IncentivesARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
 
Persistent Bias In Advice Giving
Persistent Bias In Advice GivingPersistent Bias In Advice Giving
Persistent Bias In Advice Giving
 
People
PeoplePeople
People
 
Female Fund Managers 2016
Female Fund Managers 2016Female Fund Managers 2016
Female Fund Managers 2016
 
Behavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor Psychology
Behavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor PsychologyBehavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor Psychology
Behavioural Finance - An Introspection Of Investor Psychology
 
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects workThe science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
 
SPIVA Australian Scorecard
SPIVA Australian ScorecardSPIVA Australian Scorecard
SPIVA Australian Scorecard
 
The Rise of Behavioural Economics
The Rise of Behavioural EconomicsThe Rise of Behavioural Economics
The Rise of Behavioural Economics
 
Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2
Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2
Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2
 
What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
What’s Going On in Your Brain?” What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
 

Recently uploaded

Call Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls Agency
Call Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls AgencyCall Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls Agency
Call Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls Agencykojalkojal131
 
Benefits of Co working & Shared office space in India
Benefits of Co working & Shared office space in IndiaBenefits of Co working & Shared office space in India
Benefits of Co working & Shared office space in IndiaBrantfordIndia
 
English basic for beginners Future tenses .pdf
English basic for beginners Future tenses .pdfEnglish basic for beginners Future tenses .pdf
English basic for beginners Future tenses .pdfbromerom1
 
Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?
Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?
Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?Mikko Kangassalo
 
Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi
Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi
Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi OneDay18
 
integrity in personal relationship (1).pdf
integrity in personal relationship (1).pdfintegrity in personal relationship (1).pdf
integrity in personal relationship (1).pdfAmitRout25
 
The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)
The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)
The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)Shakti Savarn
 

Recently uploaded (7)

Call Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls Agency
Call Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls AgencyCall Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls Agency
Call Girls Dubai O525547819 Favor Dubai Call Girls Agency
 
Benefits of Co working & Shared office space in India
Benefits of Co working & Shared office space in IndiaBenefits of Co working & Shared office space in India
Benefits of Co working & Shared office space in India
 
English basic for beginners Future tenses .pdf
English basic for beginners Future tenses .pdfEnglish basic for beginners Future tenses .pdf
English basic for beginners Future tenses .pdf
 
Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?
Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?
Virtue ethics & Effective Altruism: What can EA learn from virtue ethics?
 
Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi
Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi
Spiritual Life Quote from Shiva Negi
 
integrity in personal relationship (1).pdf
integrity in personal relationship (1).pdfintegrity in personal relationship (1).pdf
integrity in personal relationship (1).pdf
 
The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)
The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)
The 5 sec rule - Mel Robins (Hindi Summary)
 

When Intentions Go Public

  • 1. Research Article When Intentions Go Public Does Social Reality Widen the Intention-Behavior Gap? Peter M. Gollwitzer,1,2 Paschal Sheeran,3 Verena Michalski,2 and Andrea E. Seifert2 1 New York University, 2 Universita¨t Konstanz, and 3 University of Sheffield ABSTRACT—Based on Lewinian goal theory in general and self-completion theory in particular, four experiments ex- amined the implications of other people taking notice of one’s identity-related behavioral intentions (e.g., the in- tention to read law periodicals regularly to reach the identity goal of becoming a lawyer). Identity-related be- havioral intentions that had been noticed by other people were translated into action less intensively than those that had been ignored (Studies 1–3). This effect was evident in the field (persistent striving over 1 week’s time; Study 1) and in the laboratory (jumping on opportunities to act; Studies 2 and 3), and it held among participants with strong but not weak commitment to the identity goal (Study 3). Study 4 showed, in addition, that when other people take notice of an individual’s identity-related behavioral intention, this gives the individual a premature sense of possessing the aspired-to identity. Are scientists more likely to write papers if they tell colleagues about this intention than if they keep the intention private? It is commonly assumed that whenever people make their intentions public, the behavioral impact of these intentions is enhanced (e.g., Staats, Harland, & Wilke, 2004). These effects are pos- tulated to be a consequence of multiple processes. Research on persuasion techniques points to one of these processes (Cialdini & Trost, 1998). It is argued that a publicly stated behavioral intention commits the individual to a certain self-view (e.g., ‘‘I am a productive person’’) with which the person then acts con- sistently. Indeed, individuals with a higher need for consistency show stronger public-commitment effects (Cialdini, Wosinka, Barrett, Butner, & Gornik-Durose, 1999). The second process is referred to as accountability (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999). Making intentions public is said to make a person accountable to the addressed audience, and research has shown that various ac- countability-related features of the audience (e.g., competence, power) and the individual (e.g., identifiability, expectations of having to explain oneself) affect the strength of public-com- mitment effects. Both of these lines of research focus on intentions in which the specified behavior is a desired outcome in and of itself. Lewin (1926) and his colleagues (e.g., Mahler, 1935; Ovsiankina, 1928), however, argued that people often construe behavioral intentions in more general terms, thus allowing substitution of means for attainment. For instance, consider a student who has started an assigned math task with the intention to successfully solve the required addition problems. During the process, this student may construe the intention as being to demonstrate mathematical skills, and this conceptually broader intention may also be reached by solving subtraction problems (i.e., by substitute activities). Ovsiankina and Mahler observed that a substitute activity engenders a sense of having reached the conceptually broader intention, given that performance of the substitute activity has been witnessed by other people (i.e., has become a social reality). On the basis of this line of thought— which we explicate in the framework of self-completion theory (SCT; Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998; Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982)—we propose that social recognition of an identity-rele- vant behavioral intention may have negative effects on its en- actment. SCT proposes that people who are committed to identity goals (e.g., becoming a good parent, scientist, or craftsperson) can undertake a variety of activities to claim goal attainment. For a scientist, such activities, or identity symbols, include engaging in professional duties (e.g., giving lectures), making positive self-descriptions (e.g., ‘‘I discovered a new principle!’’), exerting identity-relevant social influence (e.g., advising students), and acquiring skills and tools that facilitate striving for the identity goal (e.g., programming skills, computers). However, failing to perform an identity-relevant activity or facing the lack of an identity symbol produces a state of incompleteness (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982). To restore completeness, the individual makes efforts to acquire alternative identity symbols (e.g., de- scribing oneself as having the required personality attributes: Address correspondence to Peter M. Gollwitzer, New York Univer- sity, Psychology Department, 6 Washington Place, 7th Floor, New York, NY 10003, e-mail: peter.gollwitzer@nyu.edu. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 612 Volume 20—Number 5Copyright r 2009 Association for Psychological Science
  • 2. Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; engaging in identity-relevant activities: Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996; showing off relevant status symbols: Harmon-Jones, Schmeichel, & Harmon-Jones, 2009). Using opportunities to affirm one’s general self-integrity or to bolster one’s self-esteem is not sufficient to offset incom- pleteness regarding an identity goal; rather, it is necessary to acquire specific identity symbols (Ledgerwood, Liviatan, & Carnevale, 2007). SCT research has also shown that an individual reaches a higher level of completeness when his or her identity-relevant activities are noticed by a social audience (Gollwitzer, 1986). Moreover, research has shown that incomplete individuals are more concerned with finding an audience for their identity strivings, compared with complete individuals (Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996). Positive self-descriptions made in public qualify as powerful identity symbols (Gollwitzer, Wicklund, & Hilton, 1982), and having an audience for behavioral intentions that specify the successful performance of an identity-relevant activity should have the same symbolic impact. The implication is that when other people take notice of a stated identity-relevant behavioral intention, this should engender completeness re- garding the superordinate identity goal, and thus reaching the identity goal by actually performing the intended behavior should become less necessary. In other words, people should be less likely to translate their identity-relevant behavioral inten- tions into action when other people have taken notice of those intentions. We conducted four experiments entailing a variety of identity goals and behavioral intentions to test this hypothesis. STUDY 1 In Study 1, we asked college students committed to becoming a psychologist to form identity-relevant studying intentions. After either taking notice of these intentions or ignoring them, we assessed how effectively the students enacted their intentions over the subsequent week. Method Forty-nine psychology students (38 women, 11 men) at a Ger- man university were recruited after introductory lectures. Par- ticipants were informed that they would take part in a survey about the study intentions of first-year psychology students. A first questionnaire assessed commitment to becoming a psy- chologist using the following items: ‘‘How important is it for you to find a psychology-related job?’’ ‘‘Suppose that you could not finish your studies of psychology successfully. How much would that bother you?’’ ‘‘How happy would you be in a job that is not related to psychology?’’ (reverse-coded) Participants responded to these items on 9-point scales ranging from 1, not at all, to 9, very much. Responses were averaged to form a scale (a 5 .80). Next, participants were asked to write down their two most important study intentions for the forth- coming week (e.g., ‘‘I will take my reading assignments more seriously,’’ ‘‘I intend to study more statistics’’). In the social-reality condition, the experimenter read through each participant’s reported intentions, presumably to ensure that the participant had understood the instructions. In the no- social-reality condition, participants were told that the page of the questionnaire on which participants had written down their behavioral intentions had been wrongly included in the study, and that this page would be discarded (i.e., the students’ in- tentions remained unnoticed). One week later, all participants were sent a second ques- tionnaire via e-mail. They had to first write down the two be- havioral intentions they had listed the previous week. Then, they indicated on exactly which days of the past week they had acted on each intention. Finally, participants were asked to bring their completed questionnaire to the experimenter’s office, where they received payment (h5) or course credit. Results and Discussion Overall, participants were highly committed to the identity goal (range 5 6–9; M 5 7.32, SD 5 1.64), and there was no significant difference in commitment scores between the social-reality condition (M 5 7.21, SD 5 1.64) and the no- social-reality condition (M 5 7.43, SD 5 1.08), F 1, p .32, d 0.16. We analyzed the number of days participants acted on their intentions in a 2 Â 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with social reality as the between-participants factor and the two specified behavioral intentions as the within-participants factor. Results showed a significant main effect of social reality, F(1, 47) 5 4.38, p .05, d 5 0.60; participants whose intentions remained private acted on their intentions on more days of the week (M 5 2.70, SD 5 1.83) than did participants whose in- tentions were noticed (M 5 1.92, SD 5 0.78). No other effects were significant. An apparent strength of Study 1 is that the observed negative effects of having one’s intentions noticed cannot easily be at- tributed to emotions that might accrue from the experimenter’s behavior (e.g., pride). Such affective responses should vanish quickly, whereas the effects on behavioral enactment in Study 1 were evident over a period of 1 week. An obvious weakness of this study, however, is that enactment of intentions was assessed via self-report. STUDY 2 In Study 2, therefore, we observed actual enactment of inten- tions. Participants were law students who formed the behavioral intention to make use of identity-relevant educational oppor- tunities. This intention was then either noticed by other people or ignored. We then observed the degree to which participants Volume 20—Number 5 613 P.M. Gollwitzer et al.
  • 3. acted on their intention when such an educational opportunity was actually provided. Method Law students at a German university were approached after a lecture and asked to fill out a three-item commitment ques- tionnaire adapted from Study 1 (7-point answer scales were used this time). Only participants with high commitment to becoming a successful jurist (score ! 5 on each item; a 5 .76) were invited to take part in the study (N 5 32; 13 women, 19 men). The students received h5 for their participation. Participants were greeted individually and informed that the experiment consisted of two independent parts. The first was introduced as an assessment of students’ willingness to intensify their study of law. Participants were asked to answer a four-page questionnaire. On the first page, the following critical intention item was presented: ‘‘I intend to make the best possible use of educational opportunities in law.’’ Participants responded on a 9-point scale ranging from 1, definitely not, to 9, definitely yes. In the social-reality condition, after a participant completed the questionnaire, the experimenter looked at this item and asked whether the number circled on the answer scale was the one the participant actually wanted to circle. Then the experimenter dropped the questionnaire into a box. In the no-social-reality condition, participants were simply asked to drop the ques- tionnaire into a prepared box. As the questionnaire was anon- ymous, it was clear to participants in this condition that the experimenter would never be able to link the expressed inten- tions to individual participants. The experimenter then turned to the supposed second part of the experiment, which concerned the development of a com- puter-based study package for law students. New study mate- rials were needed, so she had prepared 20 different criminal law cases. Participants were asked to help her find which cases to select for the package by trying hard to solve each case. The students were given 45 min to work on the prepared cases (plus the time needed to finish the case they were working on when the time limit was reached), but they were told that they could finish earlier if they wished. The time participants spent working on these cases was used to assess how successfully participants translated their intention into behavior. Results and Discussion Only participants who intended to make the best possible use of educational opportunities (score 5) were included (30 out of the 32 original participants). Participants worked on the law cases for less time if this behavioral intention was noticed than if it was ignored by the experimenter (M 5 41.52 min, SD 5 4.42, vs. M 5 45.65 min, SD 5 2.92), t(29) 5 3.26, p .01, d 5 1.10. Thus, law students—all of them highly committed to the identity goal of being a jurist—who had stated the behavioral intention to take advantage of educational opportunities in law acted less intensively on this intention when it was noticed by the exper- imenter than when it was ignored. STUDY 3 Studies 1 and 2 were both conducted with participants who were highly committed to the identity goal in question (i.e., psy- chologist in Study 1, jurist in Study 2). As only individuals who are highly committed to an identity goal can be expected to experience self-completeness by accumulating identity symbols (Wicklund Gollwitzer, 1982), it follows that noncommitted individuals should not show reduced intention enactment whenever their identity-related behavioral intentions are no- ticed by other people. Study 3 tested this hypothesis in two ways: First, we compared students who wanted to become clinical psychologists and those who wanted to become other types of psychologists to determine whether they differed in how social reality affected their enactment of behavioral intentions in the service of the identity goal of clinical psychologist. Second, we assessed the strength of participants’ commitment to the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. We decided to assess rather than manipulate strength of identity-goal commitment, as strong identity commitments are not easily created on the spot, but often take years to develop (Gollwitzer Kirchhof, 1998). Method Sixty-three psychology students at a German university (40 women, 23 men) participated for course credit. They were in- formed that they would take part in two independent studies. The first study was described as exploring students’ willingness to intensify their studies and involved answering several short questionnaires. The first questionnaire assessed commitment to the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist. The first item asked, ‘‘There are different fields of specialization in psy- chology. Which one are you trying to pursue?’’ Response options were ‘‘child,’’ ‘‘industrial,’’ ‘‘clinical,’’ ‘‘experimental,’’ ‘‘math- ematical,’’ and ‘‘undecided.’’ Participants who answered ‘‘clin- ical’’ were assumed to possess this identity goal, whereas the others were not. We assumed that strongly committed individ- uals would be willing to take on hardships (e.g., moving to an- other town) en route to attaining an identity goal (Gollwitzer Kirchhof, 1998), and the subsequent strength-of-commitment item asked, ‘‘Would you switch universities in order to receive an optimal education in your field of interest?’’ Participants re- sponded to this question using a 5-point scale ranging from 1, No, I would never do that, to 5, Yes, I would do that for sure. A second questionnaire (one item per page) was introduced as an inventory concerning ways of intensifying one’s studies. The critical behavioral-intention question was the only item on the first page: ‘‘I intend to watch videotapes of therapy sessions to learn more about therapeutic techniques.’’ The response scale for this item ranged from 1, definitely no, to 5, definitely yes. 614 Volume 20—Number 5 Intentions as Symbols
  • 4. Upon completion of this questionnaire, participants in the so- cial-reality condition were told that because of incomplete re- sponses of prior participants, only the question on the first page of the questionnaire could be analyzed. The experimenter then studied that page, tore it off, and handed the remaining pages back to the participant. In the no-social-reality condition, the experimenter gave the same cover story about missing data, but applied it to all the items on the questionnaire; then she returned the entire questionnaire without looking at it. Finally, the experimenter introduced participants to the sec- ond experimenter, who told them that she was trying to find out the extent to which making eye contact affects the quality of an interaction. She explained that she had prepared a video showing a conversation between a therapist and a client. Par- ticipants’ task was to count the instances of making eye contact and to rate the quality of interaction after each minute of the conversation. Participants were told that the video lasted 40 min, but they should feel free to stop the video whenever they wanted. Results and Discussion Thirty-one participants indicated that clinical psychology was the field they wanted to pursue; 32 participants indicated other fields. Because only participants with strong behavioral inten- tions were of interest, we excluded 6 participants (2 aspiring clinical psychologists and 4 no-goal participants) who scored 4 or less on the item ‘‘I intend to watch videotapes of therapy sessions to learn more about therapeutic techniques.’’ A 2 (clinical-psychologist identity goal: present vs. absent) Â 2 (social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA on the amount of time spent watching the therapy video yielded the predicted inter- action effect, F(1, 53) 5 3.95, p 5 .05, d 5 0.95; only the performance of aspiring clinical psychologists was affected by social reality. As expected, aspiring clinical psychologists whose behavioral intention to study videotaped therapy sessions had been noticed by the experimenter invested less time in watching the video than did aspiring clinical psychologists whose intentions remained unnoticed (M 5 29.51 min, SD 5 6.72, vs. M 5 34.22 min, SD 5 7.19), t(28) 5 1.82, p 5 .04 (one- tailed), d 5 0.68. None of the other comparisons were significant (all ts 0.15, ps .88, ds 0.06). In an additional analysis, we examined whether strength of commitment to the identity goal, as measured by the mobility item, moderated the effect of social reality among the aspiring clinical psychologists (weak commitment 3, strong commit- ment ! 4). A 2 (strength of commitment: strong vs. weak) Â 2 (social reality: present vs. absent) ANOVA revealed a main ef- fect of social reality, F(1, 25) 5 5.50, p .03, d 5 0.94, that was qualified by a significant interaction with strength of commit- ment, F(1, 25) 5 5.04, p .04, d 5 0.90. As expected (see Table 1), participants with a strong commitment to the identity goal of becoming a clinical psychologist spent less time (more than 11 min less) studying the videotaped therapy session if they were in the social-reality condition than if they were in the no-social- reality condition, t(11) 5 5.02, p .001, d 5 2.96; in contrast, participants with weak commitment to the identity goal spent close to the same amount of time studying the therapy session no matter whether they had or had not received social recognition for their behavioral intention to study videotapes of therapy sessions (there was only a 13.2-s difference between groups; t 5 0.09, p 5 .93, d 5 0.04). STUDY 4 SCT (Wicklund Gollwitzer, 1982) postulates that public rec- ognition of an identity-relevant symbol engenders a sense of having attained the aspired-to identity goal (i.e., self-com- pleteness). As Studies 1 through 3 suggest that identity-relevant behavioral intentions do qualify as identity symbols, it follows that social recognition of such intentions should also lead to a heightened sense of completeness. In Study 4, we tested this hypothesis with law students committed to becoming successful jurists. Method Twenty-four first-year and second-year law students (10 women, 14 men) from a German university participated in this experi- ment in exchange for h5. They were recruited at the end of a law seminar. Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants met the experi- menter and two other students (actually confederates) who were described as fellow law students. The experimenter explained to each group that he was conducting a study on law students’ intentions to advance their careers and then asked participants to complete a questionnaire. The questionnaire comprised two items. The first item measured participants’ commitment to becoming a lawyer: ‘‘How important is a successful career in law to you personally?’’ Responses were made on a 7-point scale ranging from 1, not at all important, to 7, very important. The second item asked participants to write out their three most important behavioral intentions with respect to the goal of be- coming a successful jurist (e.g., ‘‘I will read law periodicals regularly’’). The importance of each intention also had to be TABLE 1 Mean Time Spent Studying the Videotaped Therapy Session (in Minutes) in Study 3 Commitment to identity goal of clinical psychologist Social reality Present Absent Strong 27.95 (3.11) 39.17 (4.51) Weak 30.68 (3.62) 30.90 (6.24) Note. Standard deviations are given in parentheses. Volume 20—Number 5 615 P.M. Gollwitzer et al.
  • 5. rated on a 7-point scale. Once participants had completed the questionnaire, the experimenter asked them to either rate the attractiveness of 10 pictures of landscapes (the no-social-reality condition) or tell him and the rest of the group what intentions they had written down (the social-reality condition). In the so- cial-reality condition, the participants always reported their intentions first; the confederates then reported behavioral in- tentions derived from prior participants. Next, a second questionnaire was handed out. This ques- tionnaire showed a 14-cm line, above which were aligned five pictures of the same member of the German Supreme Court wearing the characteristic attire. The pictures varied in size, ranging from small (1.5 Â 1 cm) to large (3.6 Â 2.4 cm). These size gradations provided a visual analogue of the extent of possessing the identity of being a jurist. Participants responded to the item ‘‘How much do you feel like a jurist right now?’’ by marking the respective point on the line. This self-assessment manekin (SAM) rating procedure facilitates quick, nonreflective self-evaluations (Bradley Lang, 1994). Results and Discussion The commitment of students in the social-reality condition (M 5 6.00, SD 5 1.49) did not differ from that of students in the no- social-reality condition (M 5 6.08, SD 5 0.67), t(21) 5 0.17, p 5 .68, d 5 0.07. Ratings of the importance of the listed in- tentions were high and also did not differ between conditions (M 5 6.10, SD 5 0.55, vs. M 5 6.00, SD 5 0.55), t(21) 5 0.42, p 5 .87, d 5 0.18. We first checked the validity of our measure of felt com- pleteness. Indeed, more semesters of law education was asso- ciated with stronger feelings of completeness as a jurist (r 5 .54, p 5 .005). In a hierarchical regression analysis, the social- reality manipulation was significantly associated with feelings of self-completeness even after number of semesters of law edu- cation had been taken into account (bs 5 .29 and .45 for edu- cation and condition, respectively, ps .05), and inclusion of this variable enhanced the fit of the model (DR2 5 .17, DF 5 5.72, p .03). As we predicted, participants felt closer to the identity goal of becoming a jurist when their behavioral inten- tions were recognized than when those intentions remained pri- vate (Ms 5 4.19 and 3.10, respectively). CONCLUSION When other people take notice of one’s identity-relevant be- havioral intentions, one’s performance of the intended behaviors is compromised. This effect occurs both when the intentions are experimenter supplied and when they are self-generated, and is observed in both immediate performance and performance measured over a period of 1 week. It does not emerge when people are not committed to the superordinate identity goal. Other people’s taking notice of one’s identity-relevant intentions apparently engenders a premature sense of completeness re- garding the identity goal. Fishbein (1980) and Ajzen (1991) showed that the strength of a behavioral intention determines how well it is translated into behavior (Webb Sheeran, 2006). Moreover, a substantial lit- erature on moderators of intention-behavior relations (e.g., certainty, temporal stability) has developed (Cooke Sheeran, 2004; Sheeran, 2002). Interestingly, however, previous research has not explored what psychological processes may intervene between the formation of a behavioral intention and its enact- ment. The present studies indicate that the simple matter of identity-relevant behavioral intentions becoming public un- dermines the realization of those intentions. The present research is also unique in its attempt to bring back Lewin’s work on intentions as it applies to the actual re- alization of intentions. Most of the current research based on Lewin’s (1926) goal theory focuses on the activation level of the mental representation of a person’s intention (following Zei- garnik, 1927). For instance, research has shown that the ac- cessibility of goal-related constructs is increased as long as the goal is active, that goal fulfillment inhibits accessibility of goal- related constructs, and that these effects are proportional to the strength of commitment to the goal (Fo¨rster, Liberman, Hig- gins, 2005; Goschke Kuhl, 1993; Marsh, Hicks, Bink, 1998). Our findings are also important from an applied perspective. Given that the effect is limited to committed individuals—those who are most eager to reach their identity goals—an important question is how these individuals might try to escape this effect. Future research might address this question by exploring vari- ous routes. First, might it suffice to increase the need for con- sistency (Cialdini Trost, 1998) by attending to relevant norms? Or is it also necessary to increase perceived accountability (Lerner Tetlock, 1999) by considering relevant attributes of the audience (e.g., power) or by specifying one’s behavioral in- tention in a particular way (e.g., spelling out specific frequency or quality standards vs. stating only that one wants to do one’s best; Locke Latham, 2002) so that the audience can more easily check on its enactment? Second, might it also be effective for one to furnish a behavioral intention with a plan for how to enact it—that is, to form a corresponding implementation in- tention (e.g., ‘‘If situation X is encountered, then I will perform the intended behavior Y’’; Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer Sheeran, 2006)? As such if-then plans delegate the control of a person’s behavior to situational cues, the intended behavior should be executed when the critical cue arises, whether or not the expression of the behavioral intention had been acknowl- edged by other people. Third, recent research by Fishbach and her colleagues (Fishbach Dhar, 2005; Koo Fishbach, 2008) suggests that interpreting a behavioral performance in terms of indicating commitment to a goal enhances further goal striving, whereas conceiving of a performance in terms of progress toward 616 Volume 20—Number 5 Intentions as Symbols
  • 6. a goal reduces further goal striving. This implies that a behav- ioral intention worded to indicate a strong commitment to the identity goal (e.g., ‘‘I want to write a paper to become a great scientist’’) should be less negatively affected by social reality than a behavioral intention that implies progress toward the identity goal (e.g., ‘‘I intend to write a paper, as is done by great scientists’’). Finally, from a goal-systems (Kruglanski et al., 2002) or goal- hierarchy (Vallacher Wegner, 1987) perspective on action control, it stands to reason that any striving for goals—and not just identity goals—that can be attained by various behavioral routes (means) is vulnerable to the negative effects of social reality on the enactment of behavioral intentions. If a person is highly committed to a superordinate goal, and if public recog- nition of a behavioral intention specifying the use of one route to the goal engenders a sense of goal attainment, then the enact- ment of this very intention should be hampered. Recent research by Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang (2006) is in line with this rea- soning, showing that success on a subgoal (e.g., eating healthy meals) in the service of a superordinate goal (i.e., keeping in shape) reduces striving for alternative subgoals (e.g., going to the gym). REFERENCES Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Be- havior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self- assessment manekin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59. Brunstein, J.C., Gollwitzer, P.M. (1996). Effects of failure on sub- sequent performance: The importance of self-defining goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 395–407. Cialdini, R.B., Trost, M.R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity, and compliance. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 151–192). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Cialdini, R.B., Wosinka, W., Barrett, D.W., Butner, J., Gornik- Durose, M. (1999). Compliance with a request in two cultures: The differential influence of social proof and commitment/con- sistency on collectivists and individualists. Personality and So- cial Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1242–1253. Cooke, R., Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 159–186. Fishbach, A., Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 370–377. Fishbach, A., Dhar, R., Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substitutes or complements: The role of goal accessibility. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 91, 232–242. Fishbein, M. (1980). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications and implications. In H. Howe M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska sym- posium on motivation (Vol. 27, pp. 65–116). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Fo¨rster, J., Liberman, N., Higgins, E.T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- chology, 41, 220–239. Gollwitzer, P.M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social re- ality of self-symbolizing. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Private self and public self (pp. 143–159). New York: Springer. Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. Gollwitzer, P.M., Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity. In J. Heckhausen C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self- regulation across the life span (pp. 389–423). New York: Cam- bridge University Press. Gollwitzer, P.M., Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 69–119). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Gollwitzer, P.M., Wicklund, R.A. (1985). Self-symbolizing and the neglect of others’ perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 702–715. Gollwitzer, P.M., Wicklund, R.A., Hilton, J.L. (1982). Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 358–371. Goschke, T., Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211–1226. Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B.J., Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Symbolic self-completion in academia: Evidence from depart- ment web pages and email signature files. European Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 311–316. Koo, M., Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation: How (un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 183–195. Kruglanski, A.W., Shah, J.Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W.Y., Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Ledgerwood, A., Liviatan, I., Carnevale, P.J. (2007). Group-identity completion and the symbolic value of property. Psychological Science, 18, 873–878. Lerner, J.S., Tetlock, P.E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of ac- countability. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 255–275. Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille und Bedu¨rfnis. Psychologische For- schung, 7, 330–385. Locke, E.A., Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. Mahler, W. (1933). Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen Realita¨tsgrades. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 27–89. Marsh, R.L., Hicks, J.L., Bink, M.L. (1998). Activation of com- pleted, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 350–361. Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Hand- lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302–379. Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. In W. Stroebe M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 1–36). Chichester, En- gland: Wiley. Staats, H., Harland, P., Wilke, H.A. (2004). Effecting durable change: A team approach to improve environmental behavior in the household. Environment and Behavior, 36, 341–367. Volume 20—Number 5 617 P.M. Gollwitzer et al.
  • 7. Vallacher, R.R., Wegner, D.M. (1987). What do people think they are doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psycho- logical Review, 94, 3–15. Webb, T.L., Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268. Wicklund, R.A., Gollwitzer, P.M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Hand- lungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85. (RECEIVED 1/14/08; REVISION ACCEPTED 10/8/08) 618 Volume 20—Number 5 Intentions as Symbols